fbpx
Wikipedia

Causative

In linguistics, a causative (abbreviated CAUS) is a valency-increasing operation[1] that indicates that a subject either causes someone or something else to do or be something or causes a change in state of a non-volitional event. Normally, it brings in a new argument (the causer), A, into a transitive clause, with the original subject S becoming the object O.

All languages have ways to express causation but differ in the means. Most, if not all, languages have specific or lexical causative forms (such as English riseraise, lielay, sitset). Some languages also have morphological devices (such as inflection) that change verbs into their causative forms or change adjectives into verbs of becoming. Other languages employ periphrasis, with control verbs, idiomatic expressions or auxiliary verbs. There tends to be a link between how "compact" a causative device is and its semantic meaning.[2]

The normal English causative verb[3] or control verb used in periphrasis is make rather than cause. Linguistic terms are traditionally given names with a Romance root, which has led some to believe that cause is more prototypical. While cause is a causative, it carries some additional meaning (it implies direct causation) and is less common than make. Also, while most other English causative verbs require a to complement clause (as in "My mom caused me to eat broccoli"), make does not require one ("My mom made me eat broccoli"), at least when it is not being used in the passive voice.[5]: 36–7 

Terminology edit

Many authors have written extensively on causative constructions and have used a variety of terms, often to talk about the same things.

S, A, and O are terms used in morphosyntactic alignment to describe arguments in a sentence. The subject of an intransitive verb is S, the agent of a transitive verb is A, and the object of a transitive is O. These terms are technically not abbreviations (anymore) for "subject", "agent", and "object", though they can usually be thought of that way. P is often used instead of O in many works.

The term underlying is used to describe sentences, phrases, or words that correspond to their causative versions. Often, this underlying sentence may not be explicitly stated. For example, for the sentence "'John made Bill drive the truck'", the underlying sentence would be Bill drove the truck. This has also been called the base situation.[6]

A derived sentence would be the causativized variant of the underlying sentence.

The causer is the new argument in a causative expression that causes the action to be done. The causer is the new argument brought into a derived sentence. In the example sentence above, John is the causer.

The causee is the argument that actually does the action in a causativized sentence. It is usually present in both the underlying and derived sentences. Bill is the causee in the above example.

Devices edit

There are various ways of encoding causation, which form somewhat of a continuum of "compactness."[2]: 74–5 

Lexical edit

Lexical causatives are common in the world's languages. There are three kinds of lexical causatives, the unifying factor being that the idea of causation is part of the semantics of the verb itself.[1]: 177  (English, for example, employs all three of these kinds of lexical causatives.)

On the surface, lexical causatives look essentially the same as a regular transitive verb. There are a few reasons why this is not true. The first is that transitive verbs generally do not have an intransitive counterpart but lexical causatives do. The semantics of the verbs show the difference as well. A regular transitive verb implies a single event while a lexical causative implies a realization of an event:[8]: 511 

(a) John kicked the ice but nothing happened to it.
(b) *John melted the ice but nothing happened to it.

Sentence (b) is judged ungrammatical because it goes against the successful event implied by the verb melt.

One word edit

Some languages, including English, have ambitransitive verbs like break, burn or awake, which may either be intransitive or transitive ("The vase broke" vs. "I broke the vase.")

These are split into two varieties: agentive and patientive ambitransitives. Agentive ambitransitives (also called S=A ambitransitives) include verbs such as walk and knit because the S of the intransitive corresponds to the A of the transitive. For example:

(1a) Mary (S) is knitting.
(1b) Mary (A) is knitting a scarf (O).

This type of ambitransitive does not show a causative relationship.

For patientive ambitransitives (also called S=O ambitransitives), such as trip and spill, the S of the intransitive corresponds to the O of the transitive:

(2a) The milk (S) spilled.
(2b) Jim (A) spilled the milk (O).

These are further divided into two more types, based on speakers' intuition. Some, like spill in (2), are primarily transitive and secondarily intransitive. Other verbs like this include smash and extend. Other verbs, such as trip in (3) go the other way: they are primarily intransitive and secondarily transitive.

(3a) John (S) tripped.
(3b) Mary (A) tripped John (O).

Other examples of this type include explode, melt, dissolve, walk, and march. It is this type of ambitransitive verb that is considered a causative.[2]: 38  This is given some anecdotal evidence in that to translate (3b) above into languages with morphological causatives, a morpheme would need to be attached to the verb.

Lexical causatives are apparently constrained to involving only one agentive argument. Semantically, the causer is usually marked as the patient. In fact, it is unlikely whether any language has a lexical causative for verbs such as swim, sing, read, or kick.[7]: 3 

Irregular stem change edit

English fell (as in "Paul felled the tree") can be thought of as a lexical causative of fall ("the tree fell"), exemplifying this category.[1]: 177  This is considered a lexical change because it is not at all productive. If it were productive, it would be an internal change morphological causative (below).[1]

Two words edit

English has verb pairs such as rise and raise, eat and feed, see and show where one is essentially the causative correspondent of the other.[1]: 177 

These pairs are linked semantically by various means, usually involving translation. For example, burn as in "The grass burned" (intransitive) would translate as awa- in Yimas, while burn as in "I burned the grass" (transitive) would translate as ampu- in Yimas.[2]: 40 

Morphological edit

There are eight different morphological processes by which a causative may be marked, roughly organized by compactness:[2]: 34 

Process Basic Verb Causative Form Language
internal change tìkti 'be suitable' táikyti 'make suitable' Lithuanian
tone change nɔ̂ (high falling) 'be awake' nɔ̄ (low level) 'awaken, rouse' Lahu
consonant repetition xarab 'go bad' xarrab 'make go bad, ruin' Gulf Arabic
vowel lengthening mar 'die' ma:r 'kill' Kashmiri
reduplication bengok 'shout' be-bengok 'make shout' Javanese
prefix gǝbba 'enter' a-gǝbba 'insert' Amharic
suffix -kam- 'die' -kam-isa- 'kill' Kʼicheʼ
circumfix -č'am- 'eat' -a-č'm-ev- 'feed (make eat)' Georgian

Within morphological causatives, this degree of compactness bears an important variable when considering the semantics of the two processes. For example, mechanisms that do not change the length of the word (internal change, tone change) are shorter than those that lengthen it. Of those that lengthen it, shorter changes are more compact than longer.

Verbs can be classified into four categories, according to how susceptible they are to morphological causativization:[7]: 4–11 

  1. Inactive intransitives (faint)
  2. Middle/ingestive verbs (either intransitive or transitive such as sit down, ascend, put clothes on, eat, or learn)
  3. Active intransitives (work)
  4. Transitive verbs (carry)

This hierarchy has some exceptions, but it does generally hold true. For example, given a text of Guarani, only about 16% of causatives apply to transitives.[7]: 5  For some languages, it may not apply to transitive verbs productively and may only apply to verbs that denote abstract action or consumption of food. Additionally, within Athabaskan family, all languages can causativize inactive intransitives, but not all of them can causativize active intransitives or even transitives.[7]: 5 

Two verbs in one predicate edit

A number of languages involve a form of analytic causative that involves two verbs in a single predicate, such as French, Spanish, Italian and Catalan.[2]: 35  For example, when French faire is used as a causative, the causee noun phrase cannot occur between it and the next verb.[10]

je

1SG.A

ferai

make+FUT+1SG

manger

eat+INF

les

the

gâteaux

cakes

à

PREP

Jean

Jean

je ferai manger les gâteaux à Jean

1SG.A make+FUT+1SG eat+INF the cakes PREP Jean

"I will make Jean eat the cakes."[2]: 35 

Unlike most other Romance languages, Portuguese uses a periphrastic construction like that of English, discussed below.

Kiowa uses a similar mechanism. Verbs can be compounded with the transitive verb ɔ́m to create a causative:[11]

bé-khó-ày-ɔ́m

2SG.A-now-start.off-CAUS+IMP

bé-khó-ày-ɔ́m

2SG.A-now-start.off-CAUS+IMP

"Go ahead and run it [the tape recorder]!" (lit. "make it start off")

Periphrastic constructions edit

Some languages use a periphrastic (or analytic) construction to express causation and typically include two verbs and two clauses. English causatives prototypically use make (but other verbs such as cause, order, allow, force, compel can be used) in the main clause with the lexical verb in a subordinate clause, as in "I made him go."[2]: 35–7 

Other languages, such as Persian,[12] have the opposite syntax: the causative is in a subordinating clause and the main verb is in the main clause, as in the following example from Macushi:

[imakui'pî

bad

kupî

do

Jesus-ya]

Jesus-ERG

emapu'tî

CAUS

yonpa-'pî

try-PAST

makui-ya

Satan-ERG

teuren

FRUSTRATION

[imakui'pî kupî Jesus-ya] emapu'tî yonpa-'pî makui-ya teuren

bad do Jesus-ERG CAUS try-PAST Satan-ERG FRUSTRATION

"Satan unsuccessfully tried to make Jesus do bad."[14]

Canela-Krahô has a combination of the two in which the causee is marked twice, once in each clause:

Capi

Capi

te

PAST

[i-jōt

1SG.S-sleep

na]

SUBORD

i-to

1SG.O-CAUS

Capi te [i-jōt na] i-to

Capi PAST 1SG.S-sleep SUBORD 1SG.O-CAUS

"Capi made me sleep."[15]

Portuguese also has a periphrastic construction like that of English but unlike most other Romance languages:

Eu

1SG

fiz

make+PAST+1SG

José

José

comer

eat+INF

os

the

bolos

cakes

Eu fiz José comer os bolos

1SG make+PAST+1SG José eat+INF the cakes

"I made José eat the cakes."[16]

Analytic causatives are sometimes not considered to be valency increasing devices, but they can semantically be interpreted as such[1].: 181 

Semantics edit

A language may have one or more different formal mechanisms for expression of causation. For languages with only one, the semantic range is broad. For those with multiple, there is always a semantic difference between the two.[2]: 61  R. M. W. Dixon breaks down these semantic differences into 9 parameters, involving the verb itself, the causee, and the causer:[2]: 62–73 

(a) Parameters that relate to the verb itself
  • 1. State/Action: Can the causative apply to state and process verbs or does it apply to action verbs?
  • 2. Transitivity: Does the causative apply to only intransitives, to intransitives and some transitives, or to all verbs?
(b) Parameters that relate to the thing being caused (the original S or A)
  • 3. Control: Does the causee have control of the activity?
  • 4. Volition: Does the causee do the action willingly or unwillingly?
  • 5. Affectedness: Is the causee completely or partially affected?
(c) Parameters that relate to the causer (the new A in a causative construction)
  • 6. Directness: Does the causer act directly or indirectly?
  • 7. Intention: Is the result achieved accidentally or intentionally?
  • 8. Naturalness: Does the activity happen fairly naturally or is it with effort, violence, or force?
  • 9. Involvement: How involved was the causer in the activity?

These parameters are not mutually exclusive. Many causative constructions involve the semantics of two or more parameters. However, the difference between the causatives in a language most likely will be distinguished by one of the parameters.

Relationship between devices and semantics edit

Animacy of the object edit

There is a strong correlation between the semantics of a causative and the mechanism by which it is expressed. Generally, if a causative is more "compact" than another, it usually implies a more direct causation.

For inanimate and unconscious objects, English analytic causatives (1–3) are therefore not completely synonymous with lexical causatives (4–6):

  1. "I made the tree fall."
  2. "I made the chicken die."
  3. "I made the cup rise to my lips."
  4. "I felled the tree."
  5. "I killed the chicken."
  6. "I raised the cup to my lips."

Analytic causatives (1–3) imply that no physical contact was involved and therefore was done by some sort of magical power or telekinesis. Lexical causatives (4–6) do not imply that meaning.[17]: 784 

For animate and conscious objects, there is a different difference in meaning:

  1. "He caused them to lie down."
  2. "He laid them down."

(1) makes sense only if they are animate and awake. Barring magic, (2) makes sense only if the object is inanimate or unconscious.[17]: 784 

Finite and non-finite verbs edit

Generally, the larger the distance between the causer and the causee, the more finite the verb is. Consider the following examples from Spanish:

(a)

Montezuma

Montezuma

hizo

CAUS:3SG:PERF

comer

eat:INF

pan

bread

a

DAT

Cortés.

Cortés

Montezuma hizo comer pan a Cortés.

Montezuma CAUS:3SG:PERF eat:INF bread DAT Cortés

"Montezuma made Cortés eat bread."

(b)

Montezuma

Montezuma

hizo

CAUS:3SG:PERF

que

that

Cortés

Cortés

comiera

eat:3SG:SUB

pan.

bread

Montezuma hizo que Cortés comiera pan.

Montezuma CAUS:3SG:PERF that Cortés eat:3SG:SUB bread

"Montezuma made Cortés eat bread."

The first example implies that Montezuma was physically there and was directly involved in making Cortés eat bread. The second example implies that Montezuma was not physically there and arranged for something to happen to make Cortés eat bread, perhaps by killing all of his cattle. That could approximate the English construction "Montezuma got Cortés to eat bread." Therefore, at least in Spanish, a conjugated verb implies a less direct causation.[1]: 185 

Dixon's prototypes edit

Dixon examines this correlation cross-linguistically, and summarizes his findings in the following table.[2]: 76  In this table, L refers to lexical causatives, M1 refers to more compact morphological processes while M2 refers to less compact processes, CP refers to complex predicates (two verbs, one predicate), and P refers to periphrastic constructions. These processes are explained more clearly in the devices section above.

Parameter Meaning Mechanism Language
Causative type 1 Causative type 2 Causative type 1 Causative type 2
1 state action M1 M2 Amharic
M P Bahasa Indonesia, Malay
2 intransitive all transitive M P Austronesian languages, Mayan languages, etc.
intransitive and simple transitive ditransitive M P Basque, Abkhaz
3 causee lacking control causee having control L M Japanese
M1 M2 Creek
4 causee willing causee unwilling M1 M2 Swahili
M CP Tangkhul Naga
M P Swahili
5 causee partially affected causee fully affected M1 M2 Tariana
6 direct indirect M1 M2 Nivkh, Apalaí, Hindi, Jingpaw
M P Buru, Chrau, Alamblak, Mixtec, Korean
7 intentional accidental M CP Kammu
P M plus P Chrau
8 naturally with effort L M Fijian
L P English
M P Russian, Tariana

Parameter 9, Involvement, cannot be included in the table because the only two languages with this distinction, Nomatsiguenga and Kamayurá, the morphemes are about the same length.[2]: 75  When a larger sample of languages show this distinction, perhaps this parameter can be included in the table.

The table shows that for each of eight semantic parameters outlined in the semantics section above, more compact causative processes show one distinction while less compact processes show the other distinction. For example, Parameter 6 distinguishes between more direct and less direct causation. In Hindi, M1, or the shorter morphological process, shows direct causation while M2, the longer morphological process, shows indirect causation.

Summarizing the table, Dixon has given two prototypes for causatives:[2]: 77 

Prototype 1
  • Causer achieves the result natural, intentionally, and directly
  • Causee either lacking control or being willing and may be partially affected
  • Less transitive verbs affected
Prototype 2
  • Causer achieves the result accidentally, with effort, or acts indirectly
  • Causee is in control but unwilling and is completely affected.
  • More likely to apply to all types of verbs

All eight of the components in each prototype are never attested in a single causative. However, a single process may have two or three components. Dixon admits to these being very tentative and in need for further investigation.[2]: 77–8 

Syntax edit

R.M.W. Dixon also outlines the syntactic possibilities of causatives in the world's languages.

Intransitives edit

Since intransitive verbs have low valency, virtually any type of causative construction can apply to them productively within a language. Some constructions are only allowed with intransitive verbs and some languages (such as Arabic, Blackfoot, and Gothic) only allow causatives of intransitive verbs, with some exceptions.[7]: 5  In all cases, the original subject of the underlying intransitive verb corresponds with the object of the derived transitive verb. All languages have this construction, though some allow a semantic difference if the original subject is marked differently (such as Japanese and Hungarian).[2]: 45 

For split systems, causatives of intransitives may be treated differently.[2]: 45 

The syntax of a causative construction is almost always the same as some other type of sentence, such as a sentence with a transitive verb. Tariana, however, is an exception to this rule.[2]: 45 

Transitives edit

In the causative of a transitive verb, the new causer always becomes the new A of the sentence. What happens to the causee and the original object depend on the language. Dixon shows that there are five main types of situations:

Causative of a transitive[2]: 48–56 
type causer original A (causee) original O languages
(i) A special marking O Nivkh, Telugu
(ii) A retains A-marking O Kabardian, Trumai, Qiang
(iii) A has O-marking has O-marking Hebrew, Tariana, Amharic, Sanskrit[1]: 180 
(iv) A O non-core Javanese, Swahili, Kammu, Babungo
(v) A non-core O many languages

Within type (v) there are two main subtypes. Either the original A goes into the first empty slot in a hierarchy or it always takes a certain function.[2]: 54 

For the first subtype, there is a hierarchy involved in the language:

subject > direct object > indirect object > oblique > genitive > object of comparison.[18]

French is a language that follows this hierarchy. When a causative is employed, the original A does not get marked the same for intransitives, transitives, and ditransitives.[2]: 54  In this first example, the verb in intransitive, and with the subject slot taken, the original A becomes a direct object:

je

1SG+NOM

ferai

make+FUT+1SG

courir

run+INF

Jean

Jean

je ferai courir Jean

1SG+NOM make+FUT+1SG run+INF Jean

"I will make Jean run."

The following example has a transitive verb. The subject and direct object slots are filled (with je and les gâteaux, respectively) so the original A becomes an indirect object:

je

1SG+NOM

ferai

make+FUT+1SG

manger

eat+INF

les

the

gâteaux

cakes

à

PREP

Jean

Jean

je ferai manger les gâteaux à Jean

1SG+NOM make+FUT+1SG eat+INF the cakes PREP Jean

"I will make Jean eat the cakes."

This final French example has a ditransitive verb. The subject is je, the direct object is une lettre, and the indirect object is directeur, so the original A is marked as an oblique:

je

1SG+NOM

ferai

make+FUT+1SG

écrire

write+INF

une

a

lettre

letter

au

PREP+ART

directeur

headmaster

par

PREP

Jean

Jean

je ferai écrire une lettre au directeur par Jean

1SG+NOM make+FUT+1SG write+INF a letter PREP+ART headmaster PREP Jean

"I will make Jean write a letter to the headmaster"

While some writers have called this hierarchical causative construction the norm,[18]: 8  outside of Romance languages it is in fact rather rare.[2]: 54 

Most other languages are of the second subtype of type (v), and the original A takes on a set case or marking, regardless whether the underlying verb is intransitive or transitive:

Further divisions of type (v)[2]: 55 
causer original A (causee) original O languages
A dative O Sanuma, Apalai, Kamaiurá, Turkish, Japanese
A instrumental O Hungarian, Kannada, Marathi
A locative O Some languages of Daghestan
A allative O West Greenlandic Eskimo
A adessive O The morphological causative in Finnish.
A possessive O Tsez

Ditransitives edit

The syntactic and morphological constraints of individual language generally restrict causatives of ditransitive verbs. The underlying phrase already contains an A, O, and indirect object, and so in order to accommodate a fourth argument, languages employ a variety of constructions. They tend to be idiosyncratic and are difficult to group together into types. Additionally, data is patchy for many languages since descriptions of languages seldom include information of causatives of ditransitives.[2]: 56–9 

Double causatives edit

Some types of causative constructions essentially do not permit double causatives, e.g. it would be difficult to find a lexical double causative. Periphrastic causatives however, have the potential to always be applied iteratively (Mom made Dad make my brother make his friends leave the house.).

Many Indo-Aryan languages (such as Hindustani) have lexical double causatives.

For morphological causatives, some languages do not allow single morpheme to be applied twice on a single verb (Jarawara) while others do (Capanawa, Hungarian, Turkish, Kabardian, Karbi), though sometimes with an idiomatic meaning (Swahili's means force to do and Oromo's carries an intensive meaning). Other languages, such as Nivkh, have two different morphological mechanisms that can apply to a single verb. Still others have one morpheme that applies to intransitives and another to transitives (Apalai, Guarani). All of these examples apply to underlying intransitive verbs, yielding a ditransitive verb. So far, there are no reliable data for a morphological double causative of a transitive verb, resulting in a verb with four arguments.[2]: 59–61 

Other topics edit

Causative (repetitive) edit

Yokuts, an indigenous language spoken in California, has a morpheme, -lsaˑ, that indicates causation in addition to repetition. This is separate from the language's normal mechanisms of causation.

-'utoˑlsunhu'-

'utuˑ

play music

-lsaˑ

CAUS

-unhoˑ

AGT

'utuˑ -lsaˑ -unhoˑ

{play music} CAUS AGT

"one who makes (people) play music repeatedly"

This implies a single act by the causer, but multiple acts by the causee.[19]

Causative voice edit

The causative voice is a grammatical voice promoting the oblique argument of a transitive verb to an actor argument. When the causative voice is applied to a verb, its valency increases by one. If, after the application of the grammatical voice, there are two actor arguments, one of them is obligatorily demoted to an oblique argument.

Japanese, Turkish and Mongolian are examples of languages with the causative voice. The following are examples from Japanese:

Tanaka-kun

Tanaka

ga

NOM

atsume-ru

collect-PRES

Tanaka-kun ga atsume-ru

Tanaka NOM collect-PRES

"Tanaka collects them."

Tanaka-kun

Tanaka

ni

DAT

atsume-sase-yō

collect-CAUS-COHORT

Tanaka-kun ni atsume-sase-yō

Tanaka DAT collect-CAUS-COHORT

"Let's get Tanaka to collect them."

kodomo

children

ga

NOM

hon

book

yom-u

read-PRES

kodomo ga hon o yom-u

children NOM book ACC read-PRES

"Children read books."

kodomo

children

ni

DAT

hon

book

yom-ase-ru

read-CAUS-PRES

kodomo ni hon o yom-ase-ru

children DAT book ACC read-CAUS-PRES

"(They) make children read books."

Causal case edit

The causal or causative case (abbreviated CAUS) is a grammatical case that indicates that the marked noun is the cause or reason for something. It is found in the Dravidian languages Kannada[20] and Telugu, the Native South American language Quechua, and Northeast Caucasian Archi. It is also found in extinct Tocharian B, an Indo-European language.

Causal-final case edit

The causal-final is a grammatical case in Hungarian (and Chuvash) expressing the meaning 'for the purpose of, for the reason that',[21]: 93  and denoting price asked of or paid for goods.[21]: 116  It is formed by adding the ending suffix -ért to the end of the noun, e.g. kenyér "bread" >kenyérért "for bread", e.g. elküldtem a boltba kenyérért "I sent him to the store for bread".[21]: 115  It is not affected by vowel harmony in Hungarian.[21]: 111 

Literature edit

Shibatani edit

Shibatani[7] lists three criteria for entities and relations that must be encoded in linguistic expressions of causation:

  1. An agent causing or forcing another participant to perform an action, or to be in a certain condition
  2. The relation between [the] two events [=the causing event, and the caused performing/being event] is such that the speaker believes that the occurrence of one event, the ‟caused event," has been realized at t2, which is after t1, the time of the ‟causing event"
  3. The relation between causing event and caused event is such that the speaker believes the occurrence of the caused event depends wholly on the occurrence of the causing event—the dependency of the two events here must be to the extent that it allows the speaker a counterfactual inference that the caused event would not have taken place at a particular time if the causing event had not taken place, provided that all else had remained the same.[citation needed]

This set of definitional prerequisites allows for a broad set of types of relationships based, at least, on the lexical verb, the semantics of the causer, the semantics of the causee and the semantics of the construction explicitly encoding the causal relationship. Many analysts (Comrie (1981), Song (1996), Dixon (2000) and others) have worked to tease apart what factors (semantic or otherwise) account for the distribution of causative constructions, as well as to document what patterns actually occur cross-linguistically.

Comrie edit

Bernard Comrie[22] focuses on the typology of the syntax and semantics of causative constructions proper. Crucially, Comrie (and others to be discussed here) distinguish between the linguistic encoding of causal relations and other extra-linguistic concerns such as the nature of causation itself and questions of how humans perceive of causal relations. While certainly not irrelevant, these extra-linguistic questions will, for now, be left aside. Comrie usefully characterizes causative events in terms of two (or more) microevents perceived of composing a macroevent, and encoded in a single expression (of varying size and form). Formally, he categorizes causatives into 3 types, depending on the contiguity of the material encoding the causing event and that encoding the caused event. These are: 1) lexical causatives, in which the two events are expressed in a single lexical item, as in the well-discussed case of English kill; 2) morphological causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in a single verbal complex via causative morphology, and, prototypically, morphological marking showing the status of affected arguments. Finally, Comrie discusses analytic causatives, in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in separate clauses.

Comrie's work is also noteworthy for having brought the notion of syntactic hierarchy to bear on the typology of causative constructions. A hierarchy of grammatical relations had already been formulated to help explain possibilities for relative clause formation (first presented as Keenan and Comrie's (1972) NP accessibility hierarchy; see Croft 1990: 147), and Comrie argued that a similar hierarchy was in play, at least in some constructions, in the marking of the original A argument when a base transitive clause is causativized. The hierarchy is as follows:

  • subject > direct object > indirect object > oblique > genitive

Comrie's argument was, in short, that some causativized-transitive constructions mark the new A as belonging to the leftmost available slot in the above hierarchy. Dixon (2000) fleshes out a version this analysis in more detail.

Song edit

Presenting a typology of causatives and causation based on a database of 600 languages, Song[23] is very critical of typological work that depends on statistical inference, citing data from the Niger-Congo family that contradicts some earlier claims that "languages within genera are generally fairly similar typologically".[citation needed] Song therefore culls data from every language for which adequate documentation is available to him, and categorizes the various causative constructions gleaned therefrom into three classes: COMPACT, AND and PURP.

Song employs the following terminology:

  • [Scause] – the clause which denotes a causing event
  • [Seffect] – the clause which denotes the caused event
  • [Vcause] – verbal elements of [Scause]
  • [Veffect]- verbal elements of [Seffect][23]: 20 

The major differences between Song's analysis and Comrie (1981) and Dixon (2000), is that Song lumps the range of lexical and morphological causatives together under the label COMPACT,[23]: 20  in which [Vcause] can be "less than a free morpheme" (e.g., bound morpheme [prefix, suffix, infix, circumfix, reduplication], zero-derivation, suppletion); or "a free morpheme",[23]: 28  in which [Vcause] and [Veffect] form a single grammatical unit. Most of the examples given look like serial verb constructions, and no in-depth analysis is undertaken for some of the constructions in which [Vcause] and [Veffect] are less formally contiguous. Song notes this non-contiguity, but does not undertake to explain why it might be important.

The AND causative, for Song, is any construction with a separate [Scause] and [Seffect] i.e., in which "two clauses [are] involved".[23]: 35  This, in theory, could include larger, multi-clausal expressions of causal relations which many analysts probably would not label a 'causative construction', e.g.: 'It rained yesterday, so they stayed home', but the boundaries of the AND causative category are not discussed.

One of Song's major contributions to the literature[according to whom?] is fleshing out an analysis of his PURP causative. These are constructions which encode intended causation on the part of the causer, but which do not encode any outcome: i.e., the speaker encodes [Vcause] and causer intentionality, but remains agnostic as to whether [Veffect] was felicitously effected.

Talmy edit

Leonard Talmy[24] conducts an in-depth investigation of different types of causal relations. Talmy refers to these as "lexicalization patterns," a term which remains unclear to me[who?], given that few of the examples given in his discussion are lexical items, and most interpretations of "different types of causation incorporated in the verb root" are in fact wholly dependent on other morphosyntactic material in the clause. Let us[who?] first examine his list of possible (semantic) causative types,[24]: 69–70  with examples:

  • autonomous events (non-causative) The vase broke.
  • resulting-event causation The vase broke from a ball's rolling into it.
  • causing-event causation A ball's rolling into it broke the vase.
  • instrument causation A ball broke the vase.
  • author causation (unintended) I broke the vase in rolling a ball into it.
  • agent causation (intended) I broke the vase by rolling a ball into it.
  • undergoer situation (non-causative) My arm broke (on me) when I fell.
  • self-agentive causation I walked to the store.
  • caused agency (inductive causation) I sent him to the store.

One question remaining to be explored is how this set of divisions usefully differs from other analysts' typologies of the semantics of encoding causal relations. Some overlap in the types of semantic information in play is immediately apparent, however: in cases of instrument causation ('the hammer broke the cup'), we would certainly expect the 'causer' to be acting directly [Dixon's criterion 6] and to be involved in the activity [criterion 9]; likewise, we would expect instances of caused agency to include more information on causee control on willingness [criteria 3 & 4].

Indo-European languages edit

Germanic languages edit

Proto-Germanic edit

In Proto-Germanic, the parent language of Germanic languages such as English, causative verbs are formed by adding a suffix -j/ij- to the past-tense ablaut of a strong verb, with Verner's Law voicing applied. (All of those characteristics derive from the way that causative verbs are formed in Proto-Indo-European, with an accented -éy- suffix added to the o-grade of a non-derived verb.) Here are some examples:

  • *rīsaną (I) "to rise" → *raizijaną "to raise", i.e. "to cause to rise"
  • *frawerþaną (III) "to perish" → *frawardijaną "to destroy", i.e. "to cause to perish"
  • *nesaną (V) "to survive" → *nazjaną "to save", i.e. "to cause to survive"
  • *ligjaną (V) "to lie down" → *lagjaną "to lay": "to cause to lie down"
  • *grētaną (VII) "to weep" → *grōtijaną "to cause to weep"

In English, to sit/to seat", and in German, sitzen/setzen form pairs of resultative/causative.

English edit

English uses various causative mechanisms, with varying degrees of productivity. There are a large number of lexical causatives, such as kill, open and feed.[7]: 2 

Additionally, there are several morphemes that can express causation. For example, -(i)fy can be thought of as a causative in that it is a derivation that turns an adjective or noun into a "verb of becoming":

  • simplesimplify = "to make simple", "to cause (something) to become simple"
  • objectobjectify = "to make into an object", "to cause (something) to become an object" (figuratively, that is)

en- can also be a causative. In English, adjectives (or stative verbs in other languages) can express the acquisition of a quality or changes of state with causatives, in the same way as with regular verbs. For example, if there is a stative verb to be large, the causative will mean to enlarge, to make grow. The reflexive form of the causative can then be used to mean to enlarge oneself, or even as a middle voice, to grow.

As far as lexical causatives are concerned, English has at least 49 causative verbs. Roughly half affect only sentient beings: allow, block, cause, enable, force, get, help, hinder, hold, impede, keep, leave, let, make, permit, prevent, protect, restrain, save, set, start, stimulate, stop. The others can affect either sentient or non-sentient beings: aid, bar, bribe, compel, constrain, convince, deter, discourage, dissuade, drive, have, hamper, impel, incite, induce, influence, inspire, lead, move, persuade, prompt, push, restrict, rouse, send, spur.[25]

Sanskrit edit

In Sanskrit, there is a causative form of the verb (ṇijanta), which is used when the subject of a clause forces or makes the object perform an action. The causative suffix -ay is attached to the verbal root, which may cause vowel sandhi to take place:

  • bhū "to be, exist" → bhāv-ay; for example, bhāvayati "he causes to be"
  • khad "to eat" → khād-ay; for example, khādayāmi "I cause to eat" = "I feed"

Persian edit

In Persian, the causative form of the verb is formed by adding ân(i)dan to the present stem:

  • xordan (to eat) → xor (present stem) → xorândan (to cause/make to eat)
  • xandidan (to laugh) → xand (present stem) → xandândan (to cause/make to laugh)

Lithuanian edit

In Lithuanian, the causative form of the verb is made by adding suffix -(d)in- to the present stem:

  • skraidyti (to fly) → skraidinti (to make to fly)
  • sėdėti (to sit) → sodinti (to make to sit)
  • juoktis (to laugh) → juokinti (to make to laugh)
  • plaukti (to swim) → plaukdinti (to make to swim)
  • šokti (to dance) → šokdinti (to make to dance)

Latin edit

The topic of causatives has not been studied much for Latin, mainly because of its lack of productive morphological causative.[6]: 2 

Hindustani edit

Hindustani uses the infix -(l)ā- and -(l)vā- to make verbs causative.

  • karnā "to do" → karānā "to have done" → "karvānā" → "to have someone make someone do."
  • paṛhnā "to read" → paṛhānā "to make someone read" → "paṛhvānā" "to cause someone to make someone read."
  • hilnā "to move" → hilānā "to have something moved" → hilvānā "to have someone make something move."
  • pīnā "to drink" → pilānā "to have someone drink" → pilvānā "to have someone make someone drink": "Usne naukrānī se bachchõ-ko pānī pilvāyā" - "She had the maid make the kids drink water."

Bengali edit

The causative verbs are called prayōjaka kriẏā (প্রযোজক ক্রিয়া) in Bengali. In the simplest way, the causative form of a verb can be formed by adding the suffix "-nō" নো with the verbal noun form of the given verb.

  • dēkhā দেখা 'to see' → dēkhānō দেখানো 'to show/to cause someone to see'.
  • khāōẏā খাওয়া 'to eat' → khāōẏānō খাওয়ানো 'to feed/to cause someone to eat'.

From the verbal root (dhātu ধাতু in Bengali) perspective, the formation of causatives is done by adding the suffix "-ā" -আ with the verb roots ending with a consonant, and the suffix "-ōẏā" ওয়া with those roots ending with a vowel. Thus, the verbal root transformations of the two previously mentioned verbs are:

  • dēkh দেখ্dēkhā দেখা
  • khā খাkhāōẏā খাওয়া

These verb roots are thereafter inflected with tense, aspect and mood.

Basque edit

The Basque language has two ways to form causative verbs: by using a non-ergative transitive verb in the absolute form, or by the morphological causativization. The first method is only possible with a restricted set of verbs which excludes those whose subjects take the ergative case, such as the verb eztul egin (cough -- literally "make (a) cough").[26]

ex:1

Haurrak

child.ERG

katua

cat.ABS

hil

die

du

AUX:3SG.3SG

Haurrak katua hil du

child.ERG cat.ABS die AUX:3SG.3SG

'The child killed the cat'

ex:2

Haurrak

child.ERG

katua

cat.ABS

hilarazi

die.CAU

du

AUX:3SG.3SG

Haurrak katua hilarazi du

child.ERG cat.ABS die.CAU AUX:3SG.3SG

'The child caused the cat to die'

Turkish edit

In addition to very productive morphological causatives, Turkish also has some lexical causatives: kır- "break", yırt- "split", dik- "plant", yak- "burn", sakla- "hide", aç- "open".[7]: 2 

Semitic languages edit

In most Semitic languages, there is a causative form of the verb. It is postulated that in Proto-Semitic, the causative verbal stem was formed by the š- prefix, which has become ʾa-, hi- or ī- in different languages.

  • Syriac: kəθav "he wrote" → ʾaxtev "he composed"
  • Arabic: ʿalima "he knew" → ʾaʿlama "he informed"
  • Hebrew: ṣaħak "he laughed" → hiṣħik "he made someone laugh"

Arabic also has a causative form (Form II) created by gemination of the central consonant of the triliteral root, as follows:

  • ʿalima "he knew" → ʿallama "he taught"

The ʾa- form (Form IV), while it is used in Modern Standard Arabic, is no longer productive in many of the colloquial varieties of Arabic, which uniformly prefer Form II.

Japanese edit

Japanese has lexical forms and a morphological device to signify causation. Lexical forms come in pairs of intransitive and transitive verbs, where the causee is mostly inanimate.

  • ochiru "to fall" → otosu "to drop (something) or to let fall"

However, both intransitive and transitive verbs can form the causative in a mostly regular pattern, now with the causee being mostly animate:

  • hairu "to go in" → hairaseru "to let or force (someone) in"
  • ireru "to put in" → iresaseru "to let or force (someone) put (something) in"

In the context of an intransitive verb, the syntax of Japanese causatives allows a two-way distinction in the causee's willingness to perform the action. If the new object is marked in the accusative case (o), it suggests that the causee did the action willingly, suggesting the agent allowed or requested the action rather than forcing or demanding it. However, if the object is marked in the dative case (ni), it expresses the idea that the causee was forced to perform the action. With a transitive verb, this contrast is not directly visible as a clause cannot contain two noun phrases marked as accusative.[2]: 45, 65–66 [27]

Khmer edit

Khmer has six prefixes and one infix to derive the causative form of verbs, but they vary in frequency and productiveness. The consonantal prefix p- is one of them:

  • coap "joined" → pcoap "to join"
  • cum "around" → pcum "to gather"

Uralic languages edit

Finnish edit

Causative forms are also found in the Uralic languages of Europe, such as Finnish:

  • syödä "to eat" → syöttää "to feed"
  • täysi "full" → täyttää "to fill"
  • haihtua "to evaporate" → haihduttaa "to vaporize"

The causative suffix is often used irregularly and/or because of historical reasons, as the following Finnish examples:

  • olla "to be" → olettaa "to assume", not "to make exist"
  • kirja- ancient "patterns (of embroidery or text)" but modern "book" → kirjoittaa "to write" ("transform into patterns of text"), not "to transform into books"

Hungarian edit

Hungarian marks the original subject of an intransitive differently in causative forms to convey direct causation. If the causee is marked by the accusative case, a more direct causation is implied than if the instrumental case is used.[2]: 45–6 

Austronesian languages edit

Māori edit

In Māori, an Austronesian language, the whaka- prefix can be added to a verb:

  • ako "to learn" becomes whakaako "to teach" (to cause to learn)

Philippine languages edit

In Philippine languages such as Tagalog and Ilokano, the pa- prefix is added to verbal forms and to adjectives to form causatives:

  • dakkel "big (adjective)" → padakkelen "to enlarge" (Ilokano)
  • kain "eat" → pakainin "to make eat, to feed" (Tagalog)

Malay edit

In Malay/Indonesian, causatives are formed from the prefix per- (it becomes memper- after actor focus/active prefix meng-, expected *memer- as in *memerhatikan found informally). While most languages uses their causative affix for derivational purposes, it has integrated to Malay verb inflection system.

  • baik "good" → memperbaiki (+ local transitive suffix -i) "to fix something"
  • masalah "problem" → mempermasalahkan "to regard something as a problem" (+ beneficial transitive suffix -kan)

Guaraní edit

In Guaraní, there are three causatives: one for transitive verbs and two for intransitive verbs.[28] In some texts, the first one is called "coactive."[29]

The -uka suffix (or one of its allomorphes: -yka, -ka) is added to transitive verbs:[28]

  • ajapo "I make" → japouka "I make (someone) do".

The mbo- prefix is added to intransitive oral verbs and is replaced by mo- for nasal verbs:[28][29]

  • puka[30] "to laugh" → mbopuka "to make (someone) laugh"
  • guata[30] "to walk" → mboguata "to guide"
  • pu'ã[30] "to go up" → mopu'ã "to elevate"

The guero- (rero- or just ro-) prefix can also be added to intransitive verbs. It has a comitative meaning and translates roughly as "to cause something or someone to participate in an action with the subject:"[31]

  • guata "to walk" → roguata "to make (someone) take a walk with (the subject)"

The same root (guata) can take both causatives but with different meanings.

Uto-Aztecan languages edit

Classical Nahuatl edit

Classical Nahuatl, in the Uto-Aztecan language family, has a well-developed morphological system of expressing causation by means of the suffix -tia:

  • tlacua "he eats something" → quitlacualtia "he feeds him/her/it something" the causative makes the intransitive verb "eat something" into the bitransitive verb "feed someone something," requiring a pronominal prefix, in this case qui- "him/her/it")

Causativity is often used in honorific speech in Classical Nahuatl, and rather than simply "doing," the honored person "causes himself to do."[32]

Athabaskan languages edit

Rice makes the following points about morphological causatives in Athabaskan languages:[33]: 212 

  • In all Athabaskan languages surveyed [including Hupa, for which an ample data set is presented], the causativizing morphology can causativize at least some intransitive verbs with patientive subjects.[34]: 200–2 
  • For intransitive verbs with agentive patients, the family shows a split: only some languages then allow morphological causativization.[34]: 208 
  • Koyukon (Northern Athabaskan; Alaska) was found to be the only language in the survey allowing productive morphological causativization of transitive verbs.[34]: 211 
  • Perhaps the presence of the direct object pronoun in the causative construction has something to do with whether the causee is human or animate or is capable of being regarded as such. When the causee or the verb cannot be or is not perceived as a potential controller, the pronoun is not found [in the Athabaskan languages surveyed].[citation needed]

The semantic factor of causee control, or the degree of control that that causee wields over the effecting of the caused microevent (also discussed as parameter #3 on Dixon's (2000:62) list) and which Rice (2001) finds to be a major factor in other Athabaskan causatives helps account for much of the distribution of the Hupa syntactic causative (below).

Hupa edit

Golla, in his (1970) descriptive grammar of Hupa (summarized in Sapir and Golla (2001)), describes three classes of morphologically derived causatives:

  1. causatives from descriptive neuters with ƚ-classifier (176)
    ni-whon’ 'be good, beautiful' → O ni-(w)-ƚ-whon’ 'cause O to be beautiful'
  2. causatives from primary extension neuters with ƚ- classifier (76-77, 201)
    na-…‘a’ 'O hangs' → na-O-ƚ-‘a’ 'hang O up'
  3. causatives from primary intransitive action themes (76-77, 204)
    ti-ch’id 'grow tired' → O-ti-ƚ-ch’id 'tire O out'

While Golla does not generalize about the semantics of verb themes that are compatible with causative ƚ-, several preliminary generalizations can be made. Firstly, in the three cases described by Golla, O [the undergoer] is neither controlling nor agentive; O is largely patientive in all cases. Secondly, the causer appears to be acting directly on O. Thirdly, none of the examples given (including the examples above) involve the causativization of a base-transitive theme.

Central Alaskan Yup'ik edit

Mithun (2000) lists nine causatives for Central Alaskan Yup'ik and describes each in detail.[35]: 98–102  Here is a brief description of each:

Morpheme Approximate meaning
-vkar-/-cete- 'let, allow, permit, cause, compel'
-te- 'let, allow, cause, compel'
-nar- 'cause'
-rqe- 'intentionally or deliberately cause'
-cetaar- 'try to cause'
-narqe- 'tend to cause'
-naite- 'tend not to cause'
-cir- 'let, wait for, make'
-(r/l)i - 'become or cause to become'

Bantu languages edit

Kinyarwanda edit

Kinyarwanda uses periphrastic causatives and morphological causatives.

The periphrastic causatives use the verbs -teer- and -tum-, which mean cause. With -teer-, the original S becomes the O of the main clause, leaving the original verb in the infinitive, just like in English:[36]: 160–1 

(1a.)

Ábáana

children

b-a-gii-ye.

they-PST-go-ASP

Ábáana b-a-gii-ye.

children they-PST-go-ASP

"The children left."

(1b.)

Umugabo

man

y-a-tee-ye

he-PST-cause-ASP

ábáana

children

ku-geend-a.

INF-go-ASP

Umugabo y-a-tee-ye ábáana ku-geend-a.

man he-PST-cause-ASP children INF-go-ASP

"The man caused the children to go.

With -túm-, the original S remains in the embedded clause and the original verb is still marked for person and tense:[36]: 161–2 

(2a.)

N-a-andits-e

I-PST-write-ASP

amábárúwa

letters

meênshi.

many

N-a-andits-e amábárúwa meênshi.

I-PST-write-ASP letters many

"I wrote many letters.

(2b.)

Umukoôbwa

girl

y-a-tum-ye

she-PST-cause-ASP

n-á-andik-a

I-PST-write-ASP

amábárúwa

letters

meênshi.

many

Umukoôbwa y-a-tum-ye n-á-andik-a amábárúwa meênshi.

girl she-PST-cause-ASP I-PST-write-ASP letters many

"The girl caused me to write many letters."

Derivational causatives use the -iish- morpheme, which can be applied to intransitives (3) or transitives (4):[36]: 164 

(3a.)

Ábáana

children

ba-rá-ryáam-ye.

they-PRES-sleep-ASP

Ábáana ba-rá-ryáam-ye.

children they-PRES-sleep-ASP

"The children are sleeping."

(3b.)

Umugóre

woman

a-ryaam-iish-ije

she-sleep-CAUS-ASP

ábáana

children

Umugóre a-ryaam-iish-ije ábáana

woman she-sleep-CAUS-ASP children

"The woman is putting the children to sleep."

(4a.)

Ábáana

children

ba-ra-som-a

they-PRES-read-ASP

ibitabo.

books

Ábáana ba-ra-som-a ibitabo.

children they-PRES-read-ASP books

"The children are reading the books."

(4b.)

Umugabo

man

a-ra-som-eesh-a

he-PRES-read-CAUS-ASP

ábáana

children

ibitabo.

books

Umugabo a-ra-som-eesh-a ábáana ibitabo.

man he-PRES-read-CAUS-ASP children books

"The man is making the children read the books."

The suffix -iish- implies an indirect causation (similar to English have in "I had him write a paper"), but other causatives imply a direct causation (similar to English make in "I made him write a paper").[36]: 166 

One of the more direct causation devices is the deletion of what is called a "neutral" morpheme -ik-, which indicates state or potentiality. Stems with the -ik- removed can take -iish, but the causation is then less direct:[36]: 166 

-mének- "be broken" -mén- "break" -méneesh- "have (something) broken"
-sáduk- "be cut" -sátur- "cut" -sátuz- "have (something) cut"

Another direct causation maker is -y- which is used for some verbs:[36]: 167 

(5a.)

Ámáazi

water

a-rá-shyúuh-a.

it-PRES-be warm-ASP

Ámáazi a-rá-shyúuh-a.

water {it-PRES-be warm-ASP}

"The water is being warmed."

(5b.)

Umugóre

woman

a-rá-shyúush-y-a

she-PRES-warm-CAUS-ASP

ámáazi.

water

Umugóre a-rá-shyúush-y-a ámáazi.

woman she-PRES-warm-CAUS-ASP water

"The woman is warming the water."

(5c.)

Umugabo

man

a-rá-shyúuh-iish-a

he-PRES-warm-CAUS-ASP

umugóre

woman

ámáazi.

water.

Umugabo a-rá-shyúuh-iish-a umugóre ámáazi.

man he-PRES-warm-CAUS-ASP woman water.

"The man is having the woman warm the water.

Esperanto edit

In Esperanto, the suffix -ig- can be added to any kind of word:

  • morti "to die" → mortigi "to kill"
  • pura "clean (adj)" → purigi "to clean"

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h Payne, Thomas E. (1997). Describing morphosyntax: A guide for field linguists. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 173–186.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad Dixon, R.M.W. 2000. "A typology of causatives: form, syntax and meaning". In Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity, Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds. New York: Cambridge University Press. p.30–83.
  3. ^ Celce-Murcia, Marianne; Larsen-Freeman, Diane (1999). The Grammar Book: An ESL/EFL teacher's course, 2nd ed. Heinle & Heinle. p. 646. ISBN 0-8384-4725-2. ...verbs like let, make, and have together with their complements are often called causitive verbs or constructions since one agent is (to one degree or another) 'causing' another to act.
  4. ^ a b c Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds. (2000). Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  5. ^ Dixon, R.M.W. 2000. "A typology of causatives: form, syntax and meaning". In Dixon & Aikhenvald (2000)[4] p. 30–83.
  6. ^ a b Lehmann, Christian (2013). "Latin causativization in typological perspective". In Lenoble, Muriel & Longrée, Dominique (eds.) (forthcoming), Actes du 13ème Colloque International de Linguistique Latine. Louvain: Peeters.
  7. ^ a b c d e f g h i Shibatani, M., ed. (2001) The grammar of causation and interpersonal manipulation. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
  8. ^ Velázquez-Castillo, Maura (2002). "Guaraní causative constructions." pg. 507–534 of Shibatani (2002).[7]
  9. ^ Shibatani, M., ed. (1976). Syntax and semantics, Vol VI, The grammar of causative constructions. New York: Academic Press.
  10. ^ Comrie, B. (1976). "The syntax of causative constructions: cross-language similarities and divergencies." pp. 261–312. In Shibantani 1976.[9]
  11. ^ Watkins, L.J. (1984). A grammar of Kiowa Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. p. 153. Cited in Dixon (2000).[2]: 35 
  12. ^ Mahootian, S. (1997). Persian. London: Routledge. Cited in Dixon (2000)[2]: 36 
  13. ^ a b Derbyshire, D. C. & Pullum, G. K., eds. (1986). Handbook of Amazonian languages Vol 1. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. Cited in Dixon (2000)[2]: 36 
  14. ^ Abbot, M. (1991). "Macushi" pp. 40 in Derbyshire & Pullum (1991)[13]
  15. ^ Popjes, J. & Popjes, J. (1986). "Canela-Kraho". p. 143 in Derbyshire & Pullum (1986)[13]
  16. ^ Aissen, J. (1974). "Verb raising," Linguistic Inquiry 5.325–66. Cited in Dixon (2000)[2]: 37 
  17. ^ a b Haiman, John (1983). "Iconic and Economic Motivation". Language. 59:4 pp. 781–819.
  18. ^ a b Comrie, B. (1975). "Causatives and universal grammar," Transactions of the Philological Society for 1974. p. 1–32.
  19. ^ Newman, Stanley (1944). Yokuts Language of California. Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology. New York: Johnson Reprint Corporation. pp. 94–5.
  20. ^ "Kannada Language - Structure, Writing & Alphabet - MustGo".
  21. ^ a b c d Rounds, C. (2001). Hungarian: an essential grammar. Routledge. ISBN 0-415-22612-0
  22. ^ Comrie, B. (1981). Language Universals and Linguistic Typology: Syntax and Morphology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p.158–177
  23. ^ a b c d e Song, J.J. (1996). Causatives and causation: A universal-typological perspective. London and New York: Addison Wesley Longman.
  24. ^ a b Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics Volume 2: Typology and Process in Concept Structuring. Cambridge: MIT Press. p.67–101
  25. ^ Wolff, Phillip, Grace Song, & David Driscoll (2002). "Models of causation and causal verbs." pp. 607–622 of Andronix, M., C. Ball, H. Eslton, & S. Neuval (Eds.), Papers from the 37th Metting of the Chicago Linguistics Society, Main Session, Vol. 1. Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society.
  26. ^ Oyharçabal, Beñat (15 February 2003). "Lexical causatives and causative alternation in Basque". Anuario del Seminario de Filología Vasca "Julio de Urquijo": 223–253. doi:10.1387/asju.9721 (inactive 31 January 2024). ISSN 2444-2992. Retrieved 19 October 2022.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of January 2024 (link)
  27. ^ by. "Japanese Causative Form with the particle に and を". Wasabi - Learn Japanese Online. Retrieved 2021-07-13.
  28. ^ a b c Sebastian Nordhoff: Nomen/Verb-Distinktion im Guarani, ISSN 1615-1496, version in the internet (in German)(downloaded 17. October 2012)
  29. ^ a b (in German) (downloaded 19. September 2012.)
  30. ^ a b c Dictionary (in German) (downloaded 19. September 2012)
  31. ^ Gregores, Emma & Jorge A. Suárez (1967). A Description of Colloquial Guaraní. The Hague: Mouton. p 126.
  32. ^ Karttunen, Frances. "Conventions of Polite Speech in Nahuatl." Estudios de Cultura Nahuatl 20(1990)
  33. ^ Rice, Keren. 2000. "Voice and valency in the Athabaskan family." In Dixon & Aikhenvald (2000)[4]
  34. ^ a b c Rice, Keren. 2000. "Voice and valency in the Athabaskan family." In Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds. 2000. Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  35. ^ Mithun, Marianne. (2000). "Valency-changing derivation in Central Alaskan Yup'ik." In Dixon & Aikhenvald (2000)[4]
  36. ^ a b c d e f Kinyarwanda: Kimenyi, Alexandre (1980). A Relational Grammar of Kinyarwanda. University of California Press. p. 160–72.

Further reading edit

General reading edit

  • Croft, W. 2003. Typology and Universals, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald. 2000. "Introduction". In Changing Valency: Case Studies in Transitivity, Dixon, R.M.W. and Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, eds: 1–28. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Goertz, G. et al. 2006. "Use of causatives in Navajo: Syntax and morphology." In Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics, Volume 18: Proceedings from the Ninth Workshop on American Indigenous Languages.
  • Huang, S. and Lily I-Wen Su. 2005. "Iconicity as Evidenced in Saisiyat Linguistic Coding of Causative Events." Oceanic Linguistics, Vol. 44, No. 2 (Dec., 2005): 341–356.
  • Song, J.J. (2001) Linguistic Typology: Morphology and Syntax. Harlow and London: Pearson (Longman).
  • Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a Cognitive Semantics Volume 1: Concept Structuring Systems. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Causatives of a specific language edit

  • Kinyarwanda: Kimenyi, Alexandre (1980). A Relational Grammar of Kinyarwanda. University of California Press. p. 160–72.

External links edit

  • What is a causative?
  • What is causative case?

causative, this, article, about, causative, voice, unrelated, concept, causative, mood, mood, linguistics, causative, abbreviated, caus, valency, increasing, operation, that, indicates, that, subject, either, causes, someone, something, else, something, causes. This article is about the causative voice For the unrelated concept of the causative mood see Causative mood In linguistics a causative abbreviated CAUS is a valency increasing operation 1 that indicates that a subject either causes someone or something else to do or be something or causes a change in state of a non volitional event Normally it brings in a new argument the causer A into a transitive clause with the original subject S becoming the object O All languages have ways to express causation but differ in the means Most if not all languages have specific or lexical causative forms such as English rise raise lie lay sit set Some languages also have morphological devices such as inflection that change verbs into their causative forms or change adjectives into verbs of becoming Other languages employ periphrasis with control verbs idiomatic expressions or auxiliary verbs There tends to be a link between how compact a causative device is and its semantic meaning 2 The normal English causative verb 3 or control verb used in periphrasis is make rather than cause Linguistic terms are traditionally given names with a Romance root which has led some to believe that cause is more prototypical While cause is a causative it carries some additional meaning it implies direct causation and is less common than make Also while most other English causative verbs require a to complement clause as in My mom caused me to eat broccoli make does not require one My mom made me eat broccoli at least when it is not being used in the passive voice 5 36 7 Contents 1 Terminology 2 Devices 2 1 Lexical 2 1 1 One word 2 1 2 Irregular stem change 2 1 3 Two words 2 2 Morphological 2 3 Two verbs in one predicate 2 4 Periphrastic constructions 3 Semantics 4 Relationship between devices and semantics 4 1 Animacy of the object 4 2 Finite and non finite verbs 4 3 Dixon s prototypes 5 Syntax 5 1 Intransitives 5 2 Transitives 5 3 Ditransitives 5 4 Double causatives 6 Other topics 6 1 Causative repetitive 6 2 Causative voice 6 3 Causal case 6 4 Causal final case 7 Literature 7 1 Shibatani 7 2 Comrie 7 3 Song 7 4 Talmy 8 Indo European languages 8 1 Germanic languages 8 1 1 Proto Germanic 8 1 2 English 8 2 Sanskrit 8 3 Persian 8 4 Lithuanian 8 5 Latin 8 6 Hindustani 8 7 Bengali 9 Basque 10 Turkish 11 Semitic languages 12 Japanese 13 Khmer 14 Uralic languages 14 1 Finnish 14 2 Hungarian 15 Austronesian languages 15 1 Maori 15 2 Philippine languages 15 3 Malay 16 Guarani 17 Uto Aztecan languages 17 1 Classical Nahuatl 18 Athabaskan languages 18 1 Hupa 19 Central Alaskan Yup ik 20 Bantu languages 20 1 Kinyarwanda 21 Esperanto 22 References 23 Further reading 23 1 General reading 23 2 Causatives of a specific language 24 External linksTerminology editMany authors have written extensively on causative constructions and have used a variety of terms often to talk about the same things S A and O are terms used in morphosyntactic alignment to describe arguments in a sentence The subject of an intransitive verb is S the agent of a transitive verb is A and the object of a transitive is O These terms are technically not abbreviations anymore for subject agent and object though they can usually be thought of that way P is often used instead of O in many works The term underlying is used to describe sentences phrases or words that correspond to their causative versions Often this underlying sentence may not be explicitly stated For example for the sentence John made Bill drive the truck the underlying sentence would be Bill drove the truck This has also been called the base situation 6 A derived sentence would be the causativized variant of the underlying sentence The causer is the new argument in a causative expression that causes the action to be done The causer is the new argument brought into a derived sentence In the example sentence above John is the causer The causee is the argument that actually does the action in a causativized sentence It is usually present in both the underlying and derived sentences Bill is the causee in the above example Devices editThere are various ways of encoding causation which form somewhat of a continuum of compactness 2 74 5 Lexical edit Lexical causatives are common in the world s languages There are three kinds of lexical causatives the unifying factor being that the idea of causation is part of the semantics of the verb itself 1 177 English for example employs all three of these kinds of lexical causatives On the surface lexical causatives look essentially the same as a regular transitive verb There are a few reasons why this is not true The first is that transitive verbs generally do not have an intransitive counterpart but lexical causatives do The semantics of the verbs show the difference as well A regular transitive verb implies a single event while a lexical causative implies a realization of an event 8 511 a John kicked the ice but nothing happened to it b John melted the ice but nothing happened to it Sentence b is judged ungrammatical because it goes against the successful event implied by the verb melt One word edit Main article Causative alternation Some languages including English have ambitransitive verbs like break burn or awake which may either be intransitive or transitive The vase broke vs I broke the vase These are split into two varieties agentive and patientive ambitransitives Agentive ambitransitives also called S A ambitransitives include verbs such as walk and knit because the S of the intransitive corresponds to the A of the transitive For example 1a Mary S is knitting 1b Mary A is knitting a scarf O This type of ambitransitive does not show a causative relationship For patientive ambitransitives also called S O ambitransitives such as trip and spill the S of the intransitive corresponds to the O of the transitive 2a The milk S spilled 2b Jim A spilled the milk O These are further divided into two more types based on speakers intuition Some like spill in 2 are primarily transitive and secondarily intransitive Other verbs like this include smash and extend Other verbs such as trip in 3 go the other way they are primarily intransitive and secondarily transitive 3a John S tripped 3b Mary A tripped John O Other examples of this type include explode melt dissolve walk and march It is this type of ambitransitive verb that is considered a causative 2 38 This is given some anecdotal evidence in that to translate 3b above into languages with morphological causatives a morpheme would need to be attached to the verb Lexical causatives are apparently constrained to involving only one agentive argument Semantically the causer is usually marked as the patient In fact it is unlikely whether any language has a lexical causative for verbs such as swim sing read or kick 7 3 Irregular stem change edit English fell as in Paul felled the tree can be thought of as a lexical causative of fall the tree fell exemplifying this category 1 177 This is considered a lexical change because it is not at all productive If it were productive it would be an internal change morphological causative below 1 Two words edit English has verb pairs such as rise and raise eat and feed see and show where one is essentially the causative correspondent of the other 1 177 These pairs are linked semantically by various means usually involving translation For example burn as in The grass burned intransitive would translate as awa in Yimas while burn as in I burned the grass transitive would translate as ampu in Yimas 2 40 Morphological edit There are eight different morphological processes by which a causative may be marked roughly organized by compactness 2 34 Process Basic Verb Causative Form Languageinternal change tikti be suitable taikyti make suitable Lithuaniantone change nɔ high falling be awake nɔ low level awaken rouse Lahuconsonant repetition xarab go bad xarrab make go bad ruin Gulf Arabicvowel lengthening mar die ma r kill Kashmirireduplication bengok shout be bengok make shout Javaneseprefix gǝbba enter a gǝbba insert Amharicsuffix kam die kam isa kill Kʼicheʼcircumfix c am eat a c m ev feed make eat GeorgianWithin morphological causatives this degree of compactness bears an important variable when considering the semantics of the two processes For example mechanisms that do not change the length of the word internal change tone change are shorter than those that lengthen it Of those that lengthen it shorter changes are more compact than longer Verbs can be classified into four categories according to how susceptible they are to morphological causativization 7 4 11 Inactive intransitives faint Middle ingestive verbs either intransitive or transitive such as sit down ascend put clothes on eat or learn Active intransitives work Transitive verbs carry This hierarchy has some exceptions but it does generally hold true For example given a text of Guarani only about 16 of causatives apply to transitives 7 5 For some languages it may not apply to transitive verbs productively and may only apply to verbs that denote abstract action or consumption of food Additionally within Athabaskan family all languages can causativize inactive intransitives but not all of them can causativize active intransitives or even transitives 7 5 Two verbs in one predicate edit A number of languages involve a form of analytic causative that involves two verbs in a single predicate such as French Spanish Italian and Catalan 2 35 For example when French faire is used as a causative the causee noun phrase cannot occur between it and the next verb 10 je1SG Aferaimake FUT 1SGmangereat INFlesthegateauxcakesaPREPJeanJeanje ferai manger les gateaux a Jean1SG A make FUT 1SG eat INF the cakes PREP Jean I will make Jean eat the cakes 2 35 Unlike most other Romance languages Portuguese uses a periphrastic construction like that of English discussed below Kiowa uses a similar mechanism Verbs can be compounded with the transitive verb ɔ m to create a causative 11 be kho ay ɔ m2SG A now start off CAUS IMPbe kho ay ɔ m2SG A now start off CAUS IMP Go ahead and run it the tape recorder lit make it start off Periphrastic constructions edit Some languages use a periphrastic or analytic construction to express causation and typically include two verbs and two clauses English causatives prototypically use make but other verbs such as cause order allow force compel can be used in the main clause with the lexical verb in a subordinate clause as in I made him go 2 35 7 Other languages such as Persian 12 have the opposite syntax the causative is in a subordinating clause and the main verb is in the main clause as in the following example from Macushi imakui pibadkupidoJesus ya Jesus ERGemapu tiCAUSyonpa pitry PASTmakui yaSatan ERGteurenFRUSTRATION imakui pi kupi Jesus ya emapu ti yonpa pi makui ya teurenbad do Jesus ERG CAUS try PAST Satan ERG FRUSTRATION Satan unsuccessfully tried to make Jesus do bad 14 Canela Kraho has a combination of the two in which the causee is marked twice once in each clause CapiCapitePAST i jōt1SG S sleepna SUBORDi to1SG O CAUSCapi te i jōt na i toCapi PAST 1SG S sleep SUBORD 1SG O CAUS Capi made me sleep 15 Portuguese also has a periphrastic construction like that of English but unlike most other Romance languages Eu1SGfizmake PAST 1SGJoseJosecomereat INFostheboloscakesEu fiz Jose comer os bolos1SG make PAST 1SG Jose eat INF the cakes I made Jose eat the cakes 16 Analytic causatives are sometimes not considered to be valency increasing devices but they can semantically be interpreted as such 1 181 Semantics editA language may have one or more different formal mechanisms for expression of causation For languages with only one the semantic range is broad For those with multiple there is always a semantic difference between the two 2 61 R M W Dixon breaks down these semantic differences into 9 parameters involving the verb itself the causee and the causer 2 62 73 a Parameters that relate to the verb itself1 State Action Can the causative apply to state and process verbs or does it apply to action verbs 2 Transitivity Does the causative apply to only intransitives to intransitives and some transitives or to all verbs dd b Parameters that relate to the thing being caused the original S or A 3 Control Does the causee have control of the activity 4 Volition Does the causee do the action willingly or unwillingly 5 Affectedness Is the causee completely or partially affected dd c Parameters that relate to the causer the new A in a causative construction 6 Directness Does the causer act directly or indirectly 7 Intention Is the result achieved accidentally or intentionally 8 Naturalness Does the activity happen fairly naturally or is it with effort violence or force 9 Involvement How involved was the causer in the activity dd These parameters are not mutually exclusive Many causative constructions involve the semantics of two or more parameters However the difference between the causatives in a language most likely will be distinguished by one of the parameters Relationship between devices and semantics editAnimacy of the object edit There is a strong correlation between the semantics of a causative and the mechanism by which it is expressed Generally if a causative is more compact than another it usually implies a more direct causation For inanimate and unconscious objects English analytic causatives 1 3 are therefore not completely synonymous with lexical causatives 4 6 I made the tree fall I made the chicken die I made the cup rise to my lips I felled the tree I killed the chicken I raised the cup to my lips Analytic causatives 1 3 imply that no physical contact was involved and therefore was done by some sort of magical power or telekinesis Lexical causatives 4 6 do not imply that meaning 17 784 For animate and conscious objects there is a different difference in meaning He caused them to lie down He laid them down 1 makes sense only if they are animate and awake Barring magic 2 makes sense only if the object is inanimate or unconscious 17 784 Finite and non finite verbs edit Generally the larger the distance between the causer and the causee the more finite the verb is Consider the following examples from Spanish a MontezumaMontezumahizoCAUS 3SG PERFcomereat INFpanbreadaDATCortes CortesMontezuma hizo comer pan a Cortes Montezuma CAUS 3SG PERF eat INF bread DAT Cortes Montezuma made Cortes eat bread b MontezumaMontezumahizoCAUS 3SG PERFquethatCortesCortescomieraeat 3SG SUBpan breadMontezuma hizo que Cortes comiera pan Montezuma CAUS 3SG PERF that Cortes eat 3SG SUB bread Montezuma made Cortes eat bread The first example implies that Montezuma was physically there and was directly involved in making Cortes eat bread The second example implies that Montezuma was not physically there and arranged for something to happen to make Cortes eat bread perhaps by killing all of his cattle That could approximate the English construction Montezuma got Cortes to eat bread Therefore at least in Spanish a conjugated verb implies a less direct causation 1 185 Dixon s prototypes edit Dixon examines this correlation cross linguistically and summarizes his findings in the following table 2 76 In this table L refers to lexical causatives M1 refers to more compact morphological processes while M2 refers to less compact processes CP refers to complex predicates two verbs one predicate and P refers to periphrastic constructions These processes are explained more clearly in the devices section above Parameter Meaning Mechanism LanguageCausative type 1 Causative type 2 Causative type 1 Causative type 21 state action M1 M2 AmharicM P Bahasa Indonesia Malay2 intransitive all transitive M P Austronesian languages Mayan languages etc intransitive and simple transitive ditransitive M P Basque Abkhaz3 causee lacking control causee having control L M JapaneseM1 M2 Creek4 causee willing causee unwilling M1 M2 SwahiliM CP Tangkhul NagaM P Swahili5 causee partially affected causee fully affected M1 M2 Tariana6 direct indirect M1 M2 Nivkh Apalai Hindi JingpawM P Buru Chrau Alamblak Mixtec Korean7 intentional accidental M CP KammuP M plus P Chrau8 naturally with effort L M FijianL P EnglishM P Russian TarianaParameter 9 Involvement cannot be included in the table because the only two languages with this distinction Nomatsiguenga and Kamayura the morphemes are about the same length 2 75 When a larger sample of languages show this distinction perhaps this parameter can be included in the table The table shows that for each of eight semantic parameters outlined in the semantics section above more compact causative processes show one distinction while less compact processes show the other distinction For example Parameter 6 distinguishes between more direct and less direct causation In Hindi M1 or the shorter morphological process shows direct causation while M2 the longer morphological process shows indirect causation Summarizing the table Dixon has given two prototypes for causatives 2 77 Prototype 1Causer achieves the result natural intentionally and directly Causee either lacking control or being willing and may be partially affected Less transitive verbs affected dd Prototype 2Causer achieves the result accidentally with effort or acts indirectly Causee is in control but unwilling and is completely affected More likely to apply to all types of verbs dd All eight of the components in each prototype are never attested in a single causative However a single process may have two or three components Dixon admits to these being very tentative and in need for further investigation 2 77 8 Syntax editR M W Dixon also outlines the syntactic possibilities of causatives in the world s languages Intransitives edit Since intransitive verbs have low valency virtually any type of causative construction can apply to them productively within a language Some constructions are only allowed with intransitive verbs and some languages such as Arabic Blackfoot and Gothic only allow causatives of intransitive verbs with some exceptions 7 5 In all cases the original subject of the underlying intransitive verb corresponds with the object of the derived transitive verb All languages have this construction though some allow a semantic difference if the original subject is marked differently such as Japanese and Hungarian 2 45 For split systems causatives of intransitives may be treated differently 2 45 The syntax of a causative construction is almost always the same as some other type of sentence such as a sentence with a transitive verb Tariana however is an exception to this rule 2 45 Transitives edit In the causative of a transitive verb the new causer always becomes the new A of the sentence What happens to the causee and the original object depend on the language Dixon shows that there are five main types of situations Causative of a transitive 2 48 56 type causer original A causee original O languages i A special marking O Nivkh Telugu ii A retains A marking O Kabardian Trumai Qiang iii A has O marking has O marking Hebrew Tariana Amharic Sanskrit 1 180 iv A O non core Javanese Swahili Kammu Babungo v A non core O many languagesWithin type v there are two main subtypes Either the original A goes into the first empty slot in a hierarchy or it always takes a certain function 2 54 For the first subtype there is a hierarchy involved in the language subject gt direct object gt indirect object gt oblique gt genitive gt object of comparison 18 French is a language that follows this hierarchy When a causative is employed the original A does not get marked the same for intransitives transitives and ditransitives 2 54 In this first example the verb in intransitive and with the subject slot taken the original A becomes a direct object je1SG NOMferaimake FUT 1SGcourirrun INFJeanJeanje ferai courir Jean1SG NOM make FUT 1SG run INF Jean I will make Jean run The following example has a transitive verb The subject and direct object slots are filled with je and les gateaux respectively so the original A becomes an indirect object je1SG NOMferaimake FUT 1SGmangereat INFlesthegateauxcakesaPREPJeanJeanje ferai manger les gateaux a Jean1SG NOM make FUT 1SG eat INF the cakes PREP Jean I will make Jean eat the cakes This final French example has a ditransitive verb The subject is je the direct object is une lettre and the indirect object is directeur so the original A is marked as an oblique je1SG NOMferaimake FUT 1SGecrirewrite INFunealettreletterauPREP ARTdirecteurheadmasterparPREPJeanJeanje ferai ecrire une lettre au directeur par Jean1SG NOM make FUT 1SG write INF a letter PREP ART headmaster PREP Jean I will make Jean write a letter to the headmaster While some writers have called this hierarchical causative construction the norm 18 8 outside of Romance languages it is in fact rather rare 2 54 Most other languages are of the second subtype of type v and the original A takes on a set case or marking regardless whether the underlying verb is intransitive or transitive Further divisions of type v 2 55 causer original A causee original O languagesA dative O Sanuma Apalai Kamaiura Turkish JapaneseA instrumental O Hungarian Kannada MarathiA locative O Some languages of DaghestanA allative O West Greenlandic EskimoA adessive O The morphological causative in Finnish A possessive O TsezDitransitives edit The syntactic and morphological constraints of individual language generally restrict causatives of ditransitive verbs The underlying phrase already contains an A O and indirect object and so in order to accommodate a fourth argument languages employ a variety of constructions They tend to be idiosyncratic and are difficult to group together into types Additionally data is patchy for many languages since descriptions of languages seldom include information of causatives of ditransitives 2 56 9 Double causatives edit Some types of causative constructions essentially do not permit double causatives e g it would be difficult to find a lexical double causative Periphrastic causatives however have the potential to always be applied iteratively Mom made Dad make my brother make his friends leave the house Many Indo Aryan languages such as Hindustani have lexical double causatives For morphological causatives some languages do not allow single morpheme to be applied twice on a single verb Jarawara while others do Capanawa Hungarian Turkish Kabardian Karbi though sometimes with an idiomatic meaning Swahili s means force to do and Oromo s carries an intensive meaning Other languages such as Nivkh have two different morphological mechanisms that can apply to a single verb Still others have one morpheme that applies to intransitives and another to transitives Apalai Guarani All of these examples apply to underlying intransitive verbs yielding a ditransitive verb So far there are no reliable data for a morphological double causative of a transitive verb resulting in a verb with four arguments 2 59 61 Other topics editCausative repetitive edit Yokuts an indigenous language spoken in California has a morpheme lsaˑ that indicates causation in addition to repetition This is separate from the language s normal mechanisms of causation utoˑlsunhu utuˑplay music lsaˑCAUS unhoˑAGT utuˑ lsaˑ unhoˑ play music CAUS AGT one who makes people play music repeatedly This implies a single act by the causer but multiple acts by the causee 19 Causative voice edit This section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed April 2014 Learn how and when to remove this template message The causative voice is a grammatical voice promoting the oblique argument of a transitive verb to an actor argument When the causative voice is applied to a verb its valency increases by one If after the application of the grammatical voice there are two actor arguments one of them is obligatorily demoted to an oblique argument Japanese Turkish and Mongolian are examples of languages with the causative voice The following are examples from Japanese Tanaka kunTanakagaNOMatsume rucollect PRESTanaka kun ga atsume ruTanaka NOM collect PRES Tanaka collects them Tanaka kunTanakaniDATatsume sase yōcollect CAUS COHORTTanaka kun ni atsume sase yōTanaka DAT collect CAUS COHORT Let s get Tanaka to collect them kodomochildrengaNOMhonbookoACCyom uread PRESkodomo ga hon o yom uchildren NOM book ACC read PRES Children read books kodomochildrenniDAThonbookoACCyom ase ruread CAUS PRESkodomo ni hon o yom ase ruchildren DAT book ACC read CAUS PRES They make children read books Causal case edit This section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed April 2014 Learn how and when to remove this template message The causal or causative case abbreviated CAUS is a grammatical case that indicates that the marked noun is the cause or reason for something It is found in the Dravidian languages Kannada 20 and Telugu the Native South American language Quechua and Northeast Caucasian Archi It is also found in extinct Tocharian B an Indo European language Causal final case edit The causal final is a grammatical case in Hungarian and Chuvash expressing the meaning for the purpose of for the reason that 21 93 and denoting price asked of or paid for goods 21 116 It is formed by adding the ending suffix ert to the end of the noun e g kenyer bread gt kenyerert for bread e g elkuldtem a boltba kenyerert I sent him to the store for bread 21 115 It is not affected by vowel harmony in Hungarian 21 111 Literature editShibatani edit Shibatani 7 lists three criteria for entities and relations that must be encoded in linguistic expressions of causation An agent causing or forcing another participant to perform an action or to be in a certain condition The relation between the two events the causing event and the caused performing being event is such that the speaker believes that the occurrence of one event the caused event has been realized at t2 which is after t1 the time of the causing event The relation between causing event and caused event is such that the speaker believes the occurrence of the caused event depends wholly on the occurrence of the causing event the dependency of the two events here must be to the extent that it allows the speaker a counterfactual inference that the caused event would not have taken place at a particular time if the causing event had not taken place provided that all else had remained the same citation needed This set of definitional prerequisites allows for a broad set of types of relationships based at least on the lexical verb the semantics of the causer the semantics of the causee and the semantics of the construction explicitly encoding the causal relationship Many analysts Comrie 1981 Song 1996 Dixon 2000 and others have worked to tease apart what factors semantic or otherwise account for the distribution of causative constructions as well as to document what patterns actually occur cross linguistically Comrie edit Bernard Comrie 22 focuses on the typology of the syntax and semantics of causative constructions proper Crucially Comrie and others to be discussed here distinguish between the linguistic encoding of causal relations and other extra linguistic concerns such as the nature of causation itself and questions of how humans perceive of causal relations While certainly not irrelevant these extra linguistic questions will for now be left aside Comrie usefully characterizes causative events in terms of two or more microevents perceived of composing a macroevent and encoded in a single expression of varying size and form Formally he categorizes causatives into 3 types depending on the contiguity of the material encoding the causing event and that encoding the caused event These are 1 lexical causatives in which the two events are expressed in a single lexical item as in the well discussed case of English kill 2 morphological causatives in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in a single verbal complex via causative morphology and prototypically morphological marking showing the status of affected arguments Finally Comrie discusses analytic causatives in which the causing event and the caused event are encoded in separate clauses Comrie s work is also noteworthy for having brought the notion of syntactic hierarchy to bear on the typology of causative constructions A hierarchy of grammatical relations had already been formulated to help explain possibilities for relative clause formation first presented as Keenan and Comrie s 1972 NP accessibility hierarchy see Croft 1990 147 and Comrie argued that a similar hierarchy was in play at least in some constructions in the marking of the original A argument when a base transitive clause is causativized The hierarchy is as follows subject gt direct object gt indirect object gt oblique gt genitiveComrie s argument was in short that some causativized transitive constructions mark the new A as belonging to the leftmost available slot in the above hierarchy Dixon 2000 fleshes out a version this analysis in more detail Song edit Presenting a typology of causatives and causation based on a database of 600 languages Song 23 is very critical of typological work that depends on statistical inference citing data from the Niger Congo family that contradicts some earlier claims that languages within genera are generally fairly similar typologically citation needed Song therefore culls data from every language for which adequate documentation is available to him and categorizes the various causative constructions gleaned therefrom into three classes COMPACT AND and PURP Song employs the following terminology Scause the clause which denotes a causing event Seffect the clause which denotes the caused event Vcause verbal elements of Scause Veffect verbal elements of Seffect 23 20 The major differences between Song s analysis and Comrie 1981 and Dixon 2000 is that Song lumps the range of lexical and morphological causatives together under the label COMPACT 23 20 in which Vcause can be less than a free morpheme e g bound morpheme prefix suffix infix circumfix reduplication zero derivation suppletion or a free morpheme 23 28 in which Vcause and Veffect form a single grammatical unit Most of the examples given look like serial verb constructions and no in depth analysis is undertaken for some of the constructions in which Vcause and Veffect are less formally contiguous Song notes this non contiguity but does not undertake to explain why it might be important The AND causative for Song is any construction with a separate Scause and Seffect i e in which two clauses are involved 23 35 This in theory could include larger multi clausal expressions of causal relations which many analysts probably would not label a causative construction e g It rained yesterday so they stayed home but the boundaries of the AND causative category are not discussed One of Song s major contributions to the literature according to whom is fleshing out an analysis of his PURP causative These are constructions which encode intended causation on the part of the causer but which do not encode any outcome i e the speaker encodes Vcause and causer intentionality but remains agnostic as to whether Veffect was felicitously effected Talmy edit Leonard Talmy 24 conducts an in depth investigation of different types of causal relations Talmy refers to these as lexicalization patterns a term which remains unclear to me who given that few of the examples given in his discussion are lexical items and most interpretations of different types of causation incorporated in the verb root are in fact wholly dependent on other morphosyntactic material in the clause Let us who first examine his list of possible semantic causative types 24 69 70 with examples autonomous events non causative The vase broke resulting event causation The vase broke from a ball s rolling into it causing event causation A ball s rolling into it broke the vase instrument causation A ball broke the vase author causation unintended I broke the vase in rolling a ball into it agent causation intended I broke the vase by rolling a ball into it undergoer situation non causative My arm broke on me when I fell self agentive causation I walked to the store caused agency inductive causation I sent him to the store One question remaining to be explored is how this set of divisions usefully differs from other analysts typologies of the semantics of encoding causal relations Some overlap in the types of semantic information in play is immediately apparent however in cases of instrument causation the hammer broke the cup we would certainly expect the causer to be acting directly Dixon s criterion 6 and to be involved in the activity criterion 9 likewise we would expect instances of caused agency to include more information on causee control on willingness criteria 3 amp 4 Indo European languages editGermanic languages edit Proto Germanic edit In Proto Germanic the parent language of Germanic languages such as English causative verbs are formed by adding a suffix j ij to the past tense ablaut of a strong verb with Verner s Law voicing applied All of those characteristics derive from the way that causative verbs are formed in Proto Indo European with an accented ey suffix added to the o grade of a non derived verb Here are some examples risana I to rise raizijana to raise i e to cause to rise frawerthana III to perish frawardijana to destroy i e to cause to perish nesana V to survive nazjana to save i e to cause to survive ligjana V to lie down lagjana to lay to cause to lie down gretana VII to weep grōtijana to cause to weep In English to sit to seat and in German sitzen setzen form pairs of resultative causative English edit English uses various causative mechanisms with varying degrees of productivity There are a large number of lexical causatives such as kill open and feed 7 2 Additionally there are several morphemes that can express causation For example i fy can be thought of as a causative in that it is a derivation that turns an adjective or noun into a verb of becoming simple simplify to make simple to cause something to become simple object objectify to make into an object to cause something to become an object figuratively that is en can also be a causative In English adjectives or stative verbs in other languages can express the acquisition of a quality or changes of state with causatives in the same way as with regular verbs For example if there is a stative verb to be large the causative will mean to enlarge to make grow The reflexive form of the causative can then be used to mean to enlarge oneself or even as a middle voice to grow As far as lexical causatives are concerned English has at least 49 causative verbs Roughly half affect only sentient beings allow block cause enable force get help hinder hold impede keep leave let make permit prevent protect restrain save set start stimulate stop The others can affect either sentient or non sentient beings aid bar bribe compel constrain convince deter discourage dissuade drive have hamper impel incite induce influence inspire lead move persuade prompt push restrict rouse send spur 25 Sanskrit edit In Sanskrit there is a causative form of the verb ṇijanta which is used when the subject of a clause forces or makes the object perform an action The causative suffix ay is attached to the verbal root which may cause vowel sandhi to take place bhu to be exist bhav ay for example bhavayati he causes to be khad to eat khad ay for example khadayami I cause to eat I feed Persian edit In Persian the causative form of the verb is formed by adding an i dan to the present stem xordan to eat xor present stem xorandan to cause make to eat xandidan to laugh xand present stem xandandan to cause make to laugh Lithuanian edit In Lithuanian the causative form of the verb is made by adding suffix d in to the present stem skraidyti to fly skraidinti to make to fly sedeti to sit sodinti to make to sit juoktis to laugh juokinti to make to laugh plaukti to swim plaukdinti to make to swim sokti to dance sokdinti to make to dance Latin edit The topic of causatives has not been studied much for Latin mainly because of its lack of productive morphological causative 6 2 Hindustani edit Main article Hindustani Grammar Hindustani uses the infix l a and l va to make verbs causative karna to do karana to have done karvana to have someone make someone do paṛhna to read paṛhana to make someone read paṛhvana to cause someone to make someone read hilna to move hilana to have something moved hilvana to have someone make something move pina to drink pilana to have someone drink pilvana to have someone make someone drink Usne naukrani se bachcho ko pani pilvaya She had the maid make the kids drink water Bengali edit The causative verbs are called prayōjaka kriẏa প রয জক ক র য in Bengali In the simplest way the causative form of a verb can be formed by adding the suffix nō ন with the verbal noun form of the given verb dekha দ খ to see dekhanō দ খ ন to show to cause someone to see khaōẏa খ ওয to eat khaōẏanō খ ওয ন to feed to cause someone to eat From the verbal root dhatu ধ ত in Bengali perspective the formation of causatives is done by adding the suffix a আ with the verb roots ending with a consonant and the suffix ōẏa ওয with those roots ending with a vowel Thus the verbal root transformations of the two previously mentioned verbs are dekh দ খ dekha দ খ kha খ khaōẏa খ ওয These verb roots are thereafter inflected with tense aspect and mood Basque editThe Basque language has two ways to form causative verbs by using a non ergative transitive verb in the absolute form or by the morphological causativization The first method is only possible with a restricted set of verbs which excludes those whose subjects take the ergative case such as the verb eztul egin cough literally make a cough 26 ex 1Haurrakchild ERGkatuacat ABShildieduAUX 3SG 3SGHaurrak katua hil duchild ERG cat ABS die AUX 3SG 3SG The child killed the cat ex 2Haurrakchild ERGkatuacat ABShilarazidie CAUduAUX 3SG 3SGHaurrak katua hilarazi duchild ERG cat ABS die CAU AUX 3SG 3SG The child caused the cat to die Turkish editIn addition to very productive morphological causatives Turkish also has some lexical causatives kir break yirt split dik plant yak burn sakla hide ac open 7 2 Semitic languages editIn most Semitic languages there is a causative form of the verb It is postulated that in Proto Semitic the causative verbal stem was formed by the s prefix which has become ʾa hi or i in different languages Syriac ke8av he wrote ʾaxtev he composed Arabic ʿalima he knew ʾaʿlama he informed Hebrew ṣaħak he laughed hiṣħik he made someone laugh Arabic also has a causative form Form II created by gemination of the central consonant of the triliteral root as follows ʿalima he knew ʿallama he taught The ʾa form Form IV while it is used in Modern Standard Arabic is no longer productive in many of the colloquial varieties of Arabic which uniformly prefer Form II Japanese editJapanese has lexical forms and a morphological device to signify causation Lexical forms come in pairs of intransitive and transitive verbs where the causee is mostly inanimate ochiru to fall otosu to drop something or to let fall However both intransitive and transitive verbs can form the causative in a mostly regular pattern now with the causee being mostly animate hairu to go in hairaseru to let or force someone in ireru to put in iresaseru to let or force someone put something in In the context of an intransitive verb the syntax of Japanese causatives allows a two way distinction in the causee s willingness to perform the action If the new object is marked in the accusative case o it suggests that the causee did the action willingly suggesting the agent allowed or requested the action rather than forcing or demanding it However if the object is marked in the dative case ni it expresses the idea that the causee was forced to perform the action With a transitive verb this contrast is not directly visible as a clause cannot contain two noun phrases marked as accusative 2 45 65 66 27 Khmer editKhmer has six prefixes and one infix to derive the causative form of verbs but they vary in frequency and productiveness The consonantal prefix p is one of them coap joined pcoap to join cum around pcum to gather Uralic languages editFinnish edit Causative forms are also found in the Uralic languages of Europe such as Finnish syoda to eat syottaa to feed taysi full tayttaa to fill haihtua to evaporate haihduttaa to vaporize The causative suffix is often used irregularly and or because of historical reasons as the following Finnish examples olla to be olettaa to assume not to make exist kirja ancient patterns of embroidery or text but modern book kirjoittaa to write transform into patterns of text not to transform into books Hungarian edit Hungarian marks the original subject of an intransitive differently in causative forms to convey direct causation If the causee is marked by the accusative case a more direct causation is implied than if the instrumental case is used 2 45 6 Austronesian languages editMaori edit In Maori an Austronesian language the whaka prefix can be added to a verb ako to learn becomes whakaako to teach to cause to learn Philippine languages edit In Philippine languages such as Tagalog and Ilokano the pa prefix is added to verbal forms and to adjectives to form causatives dakkel big adjective padakkelen to enlarge Ilokano kain eat pakainin to make eat to feed Tagalog Malay edit In Malay Indonesian causatives are formed from the prefix per it becomes memper after actor focus active prefix meng expected memer as in memerhatikan found informally While most languages uses their causative affix for derivational purposes it has integrated to Malay verb inflection system baik good memperbaiki local transitive suffix i to fix something masalah problem mempermasalahkan to regard something as a problem beneficial transitive suffix kan Guarani editIn Guarani there are three causatives one for transitive verbs and two for intransitive verbs 28 In some texts the first one is called coactive 29 The uka suffix or one of its allomorphes yka ka is added to transitive verbs 28 ajapo I make japouka I make someone do The mbo prefix is added to intransitive oral verbs and is replaced by mo for nasal verbs 28 29 puka 30 to laugh mbopuka to make someone laugh guata 30 to walk mboguata to guide pu a 30 to go up mopu a to elevate The guero rero or just ro prefix can also be added to intransitive verbs It has a comitative meaning and translates roughly as to cause something or someone to participate in an action with the subject 31 guata to walk roguata to make someone take a walk with the subject The same root guata can take both causatives but with different meanings Uto Aztecan languages editClassical Nahuatl edit Classical Nahuatl in the Uto Aztecan language family has a well developed morphological system of expressing causation by means of the suffix tia tlacua he eats something quitlacualtia he feeds him her it something the causative makes the intransitive verb eat something into the bitransitive verb feed someone something requiring a pronominal prefix in this case qui him her it Causativity is often used in honorific speech in Classical Nahuatl and rather than simply doing the honored person causes himself to do 32 Athabaskan languages editRice makes the following points about morphological causatives in Athabaskan languages 33 212 In all Athabaskan languages surveyed including Hupa for which an ample data set is presented the causativizing morphology can causativize at least some intransitive verbs with patientive subjects 34 200 2 For intransitive verbs with agentive patients the family shows a split only some languages then allow morphological causativization 34 208 Koyukon Northern Athabaskan Alaska was found to be the only language in the survey allowing productive morphological causativization of transitive verbs 34 211 Perhaps the presence of the direct object pronoun in the causative construction has something to do with whether the causee is human or animate or is capable of being regarded as such When the causee or the verb cannot be or is not perceived as a potential controller the pronoun is not found in the Athabaskan languages surveyed citation needed The semantic factor of causee control or the degree of control that that causee wields over the effecting of the caused microevent also discussed as parameter 3 on Dixon s 2000 62 list and which Rice 2001 finds to be a major factor in other Athabaskan causatives helps account for much of the distribution of the Hupa syntactic causative below Hupa edit Golla in his 1970 descriptive grammar of Hupa summarized in Sapir and Golla 2001 describes three classes of morphologically derived causatives causatives from descriptive neuters with ƚ classifier 176 ni whon be good beautiful O ni w ƚ whon cause O to be beautiful causatives from primary extension neuters with ƚ classifier 76 77 201 na a O hangs na O ƚ a hang O up causatives from primary intransitive action themes 76 77 204 ti ch id grow tired O ti ƚ ch id tire O out While Golla does not generalize about the semantics of verb themes that are compatible with causative ƚ several preliminary generalizations can be made Firstly in the three cases described by Golla O the undergoer is neither controlling nor agentive O is largely patientive in all cases Secondly the causer appears to be acting directly on O Thirdly none of the examples given including the examples above involve the causativization of a base transitive theme Central Alaskan Yup ik editMithun 2000 lists nine causatives for Central Alaskan Yup ik and describes each in detail 35 98 102 Here is a brief description of each Morpheme Approximate meaning vkar cete let allow permit cause compel te let allow cause compel nar cause rqe intentionally or deliberately cause cetaar try to cause narqe tend to cause naite tend not to cause cir let wait for make r l i become or cause to become Bantu languages editKinyarwanda edit Kinyarwanda uses periphrastic causatives and morphological causatives The periphrastic causatives use the verbs teer and tum which mean cause With teer the original S becomes the O of the main clause leaving the original verb in the infinitive just like in English 36 160 1 1a Abaanachildrenb a gii ye they PST go ASPAbaana b a gii ye children they PST go ASP The children left 1b Umugabomany a tee yehe PST cause ASPabaanachildrenku geend a INF go ASPUmugabo y a tee ye abaana ku geend a man he PST cause ASP children INF go ASP The man caused the children to go With tum the original S remains in the embedded clause and the original verb is still marked for person and tense 36 161 2 2a N a andits eI PST write ASPamabaruwalettersmeenshi manyN a andits e amabaruwa meenshi I PST write ASP letters many I wrote many letters 2b Umukoobwagirly a tum yeshe PST cause ASPn a andik aI PST write ASPamabaruwalettersmeenshi manyUmukoobwa y a tum ye n a andik a amabaruwa meenshi girl she PST cause ASP I PST write ASP letters many The girl caused me to write many letters Derivational causatives use the iish morpheme which can be applied to intransitives 3 or transitives 4 36 164 3a Abaanachildrenba ra ryaam ye they PRES sleep ASPAbaana ba ra ryaam ye children they PRES sleep ASP The children are sleeping 3b Umugorewomana ryaam iish ijeshe sleep CAUS ASPabaanachildrenUmugore a ryaam iish ije abaanawoman she sleep CAUS ASP children The woman is putting the children to sleep 4a Abaanachildrenba ra som athey PRES read ASPibitabo booksAbaana ba ra som a ibitabo children they PRES read ASP books The children are reading the books 4b Umugabomana ra som eesh ahe PRES read CAUS ASPabaanachildrenibitabo booksUmugabo a ra som eesh a abaana ibitabo man he PRES read CAUS ASP children books The man is making the children read the books The suffix iish implies an indirect causation similar to English have in I had him write a paper but other causatives imply a direct causation similar to English make in I made him write a paper 36 166 One of the more direct causation devices is the deletion of what is called a neutral morpheme ik which indicates state or potentiality Stems with the ik removed can take iish but the causation is then less direct 36 166 menek be broken men break meneesh have something broken saduk be cut satur cut satuz have something cut Another direct causation maker is y which is used for some verbs 36 167 5a Amaaziwatera ra shyuuh a it PRES be warm ASPAmaazi a ra shyuuh a water it PRES be warm ASP The water is being warmed 5b Umugorewomana ra shyuush y ashe PRES warm CAUS ASPamaazi waterUmugore a ra shyuush y a amaazi woman she PRES warm CAUS ASP water The woman is warming the water 5c Umugabomana ra shyuuh iish ahe PRES warm CAUS ASPumugorewomanamaazi water Umugabo a ra shyuuh iish a umugore amaazi man he PRES warm CAUS ASP woman water The man is having the woman warm the water Esperanto editIn Esperanto the suffix ig can be added to any kind of word morti to die mortigi to kill pura clean adj purigi to clean References edit a b c d e f g h Payne Thomas E 1997 Describing morphosyntax A guide for field linguists Cambridge Cambridge University Press p 173 186 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad Dixon R M W 2000 A typology of causatives form syntax and meaning In Changing Valency Case Studies in Transitivity Dixon R M W and Alexandra Y Aikhenvald eds New York Cambridge University Press p 30 83 Celce Murcia Marianne Larsen Freeman Diane 1999 The Grammar Book An ESL EFL teacher s course 2nd ed Heinle amp Heinle p 646 ISBN 0 8384 4725 2 verbs like let make and have together with their complements are often called causitive verbs or constructions since one agent is to one degree or another causing another to act a b c Dixon R M W and Alexandra Y Aikhenvald eds 2000 Changing Valency Case Studies in Transitivity New York Cambridge University Press Dixon R M W 2000 A typology of causatives form syntax and meaning In Dixon amp Aikhenvald 2000 4 p 30 83 a b Lehmann Christian 2013 Latin causativization in typological perspective In Lenoble Muriel amp Longree Dominique eds forthcoming Actes du 13eme Colloque International de Linguistique Latine Louvain Peeters a b c d e f g h i Shibatani M ed 2001 The grammar of causation and interpersonal manipulation Amsterdam and Philadelphia John Benjamins Velazquez Castillo Maura 2002 Guarani causative constructions pg 507 534 of Shibatani 2002 7 Shibatani M ed 1976 Syntax and semantics Vol VI The grammar of causative constructions New York Academic Press Comrie B 1976 The syntax of causative constructions cross language similarities and divergencies pp 261 312 In Shibantani 1976 9 Watkins L J 1984 A grammar of Kiowa Lincoln University of Nebraska Press p 153 Cited in Dixon 2000 2 35 Mahootian S 1997 Persian London Routledge Cited in Dixon 2000 2 36 a b Derbyshire D C amp Pullum G K eds 1986 Handbook of Amazonian languages Vol 1 Berlin Mouton de Gruyter Cited in Dixon 2000 2 36 Abbot M 1991 Macushi pp 40 in Derbyshire amp Pullum 1991 13 Popjes J amp Popjes J 1986 Canela Kraho p 143 in Derbyshire amp Pullum 1986 13 Aissen J 1974 Verb raising Linguistic Inquiry 5 325 66 Cited in Dixon 2000 2 37 a b Haiman John 1983 Iconic and Economic Motivation Language 59 4 pp 781 819 a b Comrie B 1975 Causatives and universal grammar Transactions of the Philological Society for 1974 p 1 32 Newman Stanley 1944 Yokuts Language of California Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology New York Johnson Reprint Corporation pp 94 5 Kannada Language Structure Writing amp Alphabet MustGo a b c d Rounds C 2001 Hungarian an essential grammar Routledge ISBN 0 415 22612 0 Comrie B 1981 Language Universals and Linguistic Typology Syntax and Morphology Chicago University of Chicago Press p 158 177 a b c d e Song J J 1996 Causatives and causation A universal typological perspective London and New York Addison Wesley Longman a b Talmy L 2000 Toward a Cognitive Semantics Volume 2 Typology and Process in Concept Structuring Cambridge MIT Press p 67 101 Wolff Phillip Grace Song amp David Driscoll 2002 Models of causation and causal verbs pp 607 622 of Andronix M C Ball H Eslton amp S Neuval Eds Papers from the 37th Metting of the Chicago Linguistics Society Main Session Vol 1 Chicago Chicago Linguistics Society Oyharcabal Benat 15 February 2003 Lexical causatives and causative alternation in Basque Anuario del Seminario de Filologia Vasca Julio de Urquijo 223 253 doi 10 1387 asju 9721 inactive 31 January 2024 ISSN 2444 2992 Retrieved 19 October 2022 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint DOI inactive as of January 2024 link by Japanese Causative Form with the particle に and を Wasabi Learn Japanese Online Retrieved 2021 07 13 a b c Sebastian Nordhoff Nomen Verb Distinktion im Guarani ISSN 1615 1496 version in the internet in German downloaded 17 October 2012 a b Description of the language in German downloaded 19 September 2012 a b c Dictionary in German downloaded 19 September 2012 Gregores Emma amp Jorge A Suarez 1967 A Description of Colloquial Guarani The Hague Mouton p 126 Karttunen Frances Conventions of Polite Speech in Nahuatl Estudios de Cultura Nahuatl 20 1990 Rice Keren 2000 Voice and valency in the Athabaskan family In Dixon amp Aikhenvald 2000 4 a b c Rice Keren 2000 Voice and valency in the Athabaskan family In Dixon R M W and Alexandra Y Aikhenvald eds 2000 Changing Valency Case Studies in Transitivity New York Cambridge University Press Mithun Marianne 2000 Valency changing derivation in Central Alaskan Yup ik In Dixon amp Aikhenvald 2000 4 a b c d e f Kinyarwanda Kimenyi Alexandre 1980 A Relational Grammar of Kinyarwanda University of California Press p 160 72 Further reading editGeneral reading edit Croft W 2003 Typology and Universals 2nd ed New York Cambridge University Press Dixon R M W and Alexandra Y Aikhenvald 2000 Introduction In Changing Valency Case Studies in Transitivity Dixon R M W and Alexandra Y Aikhenvald eds 1 28 New York Cambridge University Press Goertz G et al 2006 Use of causatives in Navajo Syntax and morphology In Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics Volume 18 Proceedings from the Ninth Workshop on American Indigenous Languages Huang S and Lily I Wen Su 2005 Iconicity as Evidenced in Saisiyat Linguistic Coding of Causative Events Oceanic Linguistics Vol 44 No 2 Dec 2005 341 356 Song J J 2001 Linguistic Typology Morphology and Syntax Harlow and London Pearson Longman Talmy L 2000 Toward a Cognitive Semantics Volume 1 Concept Structuring Systems Cambridge MIT Press Causatives of a specific language edit Kinyarwanda Kimenyi Alexandre 1980 A Relational Grammar of Kinyarwanda University of California Press p 160 72 External links edit nbsp Look up causative or factitive in Wiktionary the free dictionary What is a causative What is causative case Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Causative amp oldid 1207877275, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.