fbpx
Wikipedia

Morphology (linguistics)

In linguistics, morphology (/mɔːrˈfɒləi/[1]) is the study of words, how they are formed, and their relationship to other words in the same language.[2][3] It analyzes the structure of words and parts of words such as stems, root words, prefixes, and suffixes. Morphology also looks at parts of speech, intonation and stress, and the ways context can change a word's pronunciation and meaning. Morphology differs from morphological typology, which is the classification of languages based on their use of words,[4] and lexicology, which is the study of words and how they make up a language's vocabulary.[5]

While words, along with clitics, are generally accepted as being the smallest units of syntax, in most languages, if not all, many words can be related to other words by rules that collectively describe the grammar for that language. For example, English speakers recognize that the words dog and dogs are closely related, differentiated only by the plurality morpheme "-s", only found bound to noun phrases. Speakers of English, a fusional language, recognize these relations from their innate knowledge of English's rules of word formation. They infer intuitively that dog is to dogs as cat is to cats; and, in similar fashion, dog is to dog catcher as dish is to dishwasher. By contrast, Classical Chinese has very little morphology, using almost exclusively unbound morphemes ("free" morphemes) and it relies on word order to convey meaning. (Most words in modern Standard Chinese ["Mandarin"], however, are compounds and most roots are bound.) These are understood as grammars that represent the morphology of the language. The rules understood by a speaker reflect specific patterns or regularities in the way words are formed from smaller units in the language they are using, and how those smaller units interact in speech. In this way, morphology is the branch of linguistics that studies patterns of word formation within and across languages and attempts to formulate rules that model the knowledge of the speakers of those languages.

Phonological and orthographic modifications between a base word and its origin may be partial to literacy skills. Studies have indicated that the presence of modification in phonology and orthography makes morphologically complex words harder to understand and that the absence of modification between a base word and its origin makes morphologically complex words easier to understand. Morphologically complex words are easier to comprehend when they include a base word.[6]

Polysynthetic languages, such as Chukchi, have words composed of many morphemes. For example, the Chukchi word "təmeyŋəlevtpəγtərkən", meaning "I have a fierce headache", is composed of eight morphemes t-ə-meyŋ-ə-levt-pəγt-ə-rkən that may be glossed. The morphology of such languages allows for each consonant and vowel to be understood as morphemes, while the grammar of the language indicates the usage and understanding of each morpheme.

The discipline that deals specifically with the sound changes occurring within morphemes is morphophonology.

History

The history of morphological analysis dates back to the ancient Indian linguist Pāṇini, who formulated the 3,959 rules of Sanskrit morphology in the text Aṣṭādhyāyī by using a constituency grammar. The Greco-Roman grammatical tradition also engaged in morphological analysis.[7] Studies in Arabic morphology, conducted by Marāḥ al-arwāḥ and Aḥmad b. ‘alī Mas‘ūd, date back to at least 1200 CE.[8]

The linguistic term "morphology" was coined by August Schleicher in 1859.[a][9]

Fundamental concepts

Lexemes and word forms

The term "word" has no well-defined meaning.[10] Instead, two related terms are used in morphology: lexeme and word-form. Generally, a lexeme is a set of inflected word-forms that is often represented with the citation form in small capitals.[11] For instance, the lexeme eat contains the word-forms eat, eats, eaten, and ate. Eat and eats are thus considered different word-forms belonging to the same lexeme eat. Eat and Eater, on the other hand, are different lexemes, as they refer to two different concepts.

Prosodic word vs. morphological word

Here are examples from other languages of the failure of a single phonological word to coincide with a single morphological word form. In Latin, one way to express the concept of 'NOUN-PHRASE1 and NOUN-PHRASE2' (as in "apples and oranges") is to suffix '-que' to the second noun phrase: "apples oranges-and". An extreme level of the theoretical quandary posed by some phonological words is provided by the Kwak'wala language.[b] In Kwak'wala, as in a great many other languages, meaning relations between nouns, including possession and "semantic case", are formulated by affixes, instead of by independent "words". The three-word English phrase, "with his club", in which 'with' identifies its dependent noun phrase as an instrument and 'his' denotes a possession relation, would consist of two words or even one word in many languages. Unlike most other languages, Kwak'wala semantic affixes phonologically attach not to the lexeme they pertain to semantically but to the preceding lexeme. Consider the following example (in Kwak'wala, sentences begin with what corresponds to an English verb):[c]

kwixʔid-i-da

clubbed-PIVOT-DETERMINER

bəgwanəmai-χ-a

man-ACCUSATIVE-DETERMINER

q'asa-s-isi

otter-INSTRUMENTAL-3SG-POSSESSIVE

t'alwagwayu

club

kwixʔid-i-da bəgwanəmai-χ-a q'asa-s-isi t'alwagwayu

clubbed-PIVOT-DETERMINER man-ACCUSATIVE-DETERMINER otter-INSTRUMENTAL-3SG-POSSESSIVE club

"the man clubbed the otter with his club."

(Notation notes:

  1. accusative case marks an entity that something is done to.
  2. determiners are words such as "the", "this", and "that".
  3. the concept of "pivot" is a theoretical construct that is not relevant to this discussion.)

That is, to a speaker of Kwak'wala, the sentence does not contain the "words" 'him-the-otter' or 'with-his-club' Instead, the markers -i-da (PIVOT-'the'), referring to "man", attaches not to the noun bəgwanəma ("man") but to the verb; the markers -χ-a (ACCUSATIVE-'the'), referring to otter, attach to bəgwanəma instead of to q'asa ('otter'), etc. In other words, a speaker of Kwak'wala does not perceive the sentence to consist of these phonological words:

kwixʔid

clubbed

i-da-bəgwanəma

PIVOT-the-mani

χ-a-q'asa

hit-the-otter

s-isi-t'alwagwayu

with-hisi-club

kwixʔid i-da-bəgwanəma χ-a-q'asa s-isi-t'alwagwayu

clubbed PIVOT-the-mani hit-the-otter with-hisi-club

A central publication on this topic is the volume edited by Dixon and Aikhenvald (2002), examining the mismatch between prosodic-phonological and grammatical definitions of "word" in various Amazonian, Australian Aboriginal, Caucasian, Eskimo, Indo-European, Native North American, West African, and sign languages. Apparently, a wide variety of languages make use of the hybrid linguistic unit clitic, possessing the grammatical features of independent words but the prosodic-phonological lack of freedom of bound morphemes. The intermediate status of clitics poses a considerable challenge to linguistic theory.[12]

Inflection vs. word formation

Given the notion of a lexeme, it is possible to distinguish two kinds of morphological rules. Some morphological rules relate to different forms of the same lexeme, but other rules relate to different lexemes. Rules of the first kind are inflectional rules, but those of the second kind are rules of word formation.[13] The generation of the English plural dogs from dog is an inflectional rule, and compound phrases and words like dog catcher or dishwasher are examples of word formation. Informally, word formation rules form "new" words (more accurately, new lexemes), and inflection rules yield variant forms of the "same" word (lexeme).

The distinction between inflection and word formation is not at all clear-cut. There are many examples for which linguists fail to agree whether a given rule is inflection or word formation. The next section will attempt to clarify the distinction.

Word formation includes a process in which one combines two complete words, but inflection allows the combination of a suffix with a verb to change the latter's form to that of the subject of the sentence. For example: in the present indefinite, ‘go’ is used with subject I/we/you/they and plural nouns, but third-person singular pronouns (he/she/it) and singular nouns causes ‘goes’ to be used. The ‘-es’ is therefore an inflectional marker that is used to match with its subject. A further difference is that in word formation, the resultant word may differ from its source word's grammatical category, but in the process of inflection, the word never changes its grammatical category.

Types of word formation

There is a further distinction between two primary kinds of morphological word formation: derivation and compounding. The latter is a process of word formation that involves combining complete word forms into a single compound form. Dog catcher, therefore, is a compound, as both dog and catcher are complete word forms in their own right but are subsequently treated as parts of one form. Derivation involves affixing bound (non-independent) forms to existing lexemes, but the addition of the affix derives a new lexeme. The word independent, for example, is derived from the word dependent by using the prefix in-, and dependent itself is derived from the verb depend. There is also word formation in the processes of clipping in which a portion of a word is removed to create a new one, blending in which two parts of different words are blended into one, acronyms in which each letter of the new word represents a specific word in the representation (NATO for North Atlantic Treaty Organization), borrowing in which words from one language are taken and used in another, and coinage in which a new word is created to represent a new object or concept.[14]

Paradigms and morphosyntax

A linguistic paradigm is the complete set of related word forms associated with a given lexeme. The familiar examples of paradigms are the conjugations of verbs and the declensions of nouns. Also, arranging the word forms of a lexeme into tables, by classifying them according to shared inflectional categories such as tense, aspect, mood, number, gender or case, organizes such. For example, the personal pronouns in English can be organized into tables by using the categories of person (first, second, third); number (singular vs. plural); gender (masculine, feminine, neuter); and case (nominative, oblique, genitive).

The inflectional categories used to group word forms into paradigms cannot be chosen arbitrarily but must be categories that are relevant to stating the syntactic rules of the language. Person and number are categories that can be used to define paradigms in English because the language has grammatical agreement rules, which require the verb in a sentence to appear in an inflectional form that matches the person and number of the subject. Therefore, the syntactic rules of English care about the difference between dog and dogs because the choice between both forms determines the form of the verb that is used. However, no syntactic rule shows the difference between dog and dog catcher, or dependent and independent. The first two are nouns, and the other two are adjectives.

An important difference between inflection and word formation is that inflected word forms of lexemes are organized into paradigms that are defined by the requirements of syntactic rules, and there are no corresponding syntactic rules for word formation.

The relationship between syntax and morphology, as well as how they interact, is called "morphosyntax";[15][16] the term is also used to underline the fact that syntax and morphology are interrelated.[17] The study of morphosyntax concerns itself with inflection and paradigms, and some approaches to morphosyntax exclude from its domain the phenomena of word formation, compounding, and derivation.[15] Within morphosyntax fall the study of agreement and government.[15]

Allomorphy

Above, morphological rules are described as analogies between word forms: dog is to dogs as cat is to cats and dish is to dishes. In this case, the analogy applies both to the form of the words and to their meaning. In each pair, the first word means "one of X", and the second "two or more of X", and the difference is always the plural form -s (or -es) affixed to the second word, which signals the key distinction between singular and plural entities.

One of the largest sources of complexity in morphology is that the one-to-one correspondence between meaning and form scarcely applies to every case in the language. In English, there are word form pairs like ox/oxen, goose/geese, and sheep/sheep whose difference between the singular and the plural is signaled in a way that departs from the regular pattern or is not signaled at all. Even cases regarded as regular, such as -s, are not so simple; the -s in dogs is not pronounced the same way as the -s in cats, and in plurals such as dishes, a vowel is added before the -s. Those cases, in which the same distinction is effected by alternative forms of a "word", constitute allomorphy.[18]

Phonological rules constrain the sounds that can appear next to each other in a language, and morphological rules, when applied blindly, would often violate phonological rules by resulting in sound sequences that are prohibited in the language in question. For example, to form the plural of dish by simply appending an -s to the end of the word would result in the form *[dɪʃs], which is not permitted by the phonotactics of English. To "rescue" the word, a vowel sound is inserted between the root and the plural marker, and [dɪʃɪz] results. Similar rules apply to the pronunciation of the -s in dogs and cats: it depends on the quality (voiced vs. unvoiced) of the final preceding phoneme.

Lexical morphology

Lexical morphology is the branch of morphology that deals with the lexicon that, morphologically conceived, is the collection of lexemes in a language. As such, it concerns itself primarily with word formation: derivation and compounding.

Models

There are three principal approaches to morphology and each tries to capture the distinctions above in different ways:

  • Morpheme-based morphology, which makes use of an item-and-arrangement approach.
  • Lexeme-based morphology, which normally makes use of an item-and-process approach.
  • Word-based morphology, which normally makes use of a word-and-paradigm approach.

While the associations indicated between the concepts in each item in that list are very strong, they are not absolute.

Morpheme-based morphology

 
Morpheme-based morphology tree of the word "independently"

In morpheme-based morphology, word forms are analyzed as arrangements of morphemes. A morpheme is defined as the minimal meaningful unit of a language. In a word such as independently, the morphemes are said to be in-, de-, pend, -ent, and -ly; pend is the (bound) root and the other morphemes are, in this case, derivational affixes.[d] In words such as dogs, dog is the root and the -s is an inflectional morpheme. In its simplest and most naïve form, this way of analyzing word forms, called "item-and-arrangement", treats words as if they were made of morphemes put after each other ("concatenated") like beads on a string. More recent and sophisticated approaches, such as distributed morphology, seek to maintain the idea of the morpheme while accommodating non-concatenated, analogical, and other processes that have proven problematic for item-and-arrangement theories and similar approaches.

Morpheme-based morphology presumes three basic axioms:[19]

  • Baudouin’s "single morpheme" hypothesis: Roots and affixes have the same status as morphemes.
  • Bloomfield’s "sign base" morpheme hypothesis: As morphemes, they are dualistic signs, since they have both (phonological) form and meaning.
  • Bloomfield's "lexical morpheme" hypothesis: morphemes, affixes and roots alike are stored in the lexicon.

Morpheme-based morphology comes in two flavours, one Bloomfieldian[20] and one Hockettian.[21] For Bloomfield, the morpheme was the minimal form with meaning, but did not have meaning itself.[clarification needed] For Hockett, morphemes are "meaning elements", not "form elements". For him, there is a morpheme plural using allomorphs such as -s, -en and -ren. Within much morpheme-based morphological theory, the two views are mixed in unsystematic ways so a writer may refer to "the morpheme plural" and "the morpheme -s" in the same sentence.

Lexeme-based morphology

Lexeme-based morphology usually takes what is called an item-and-process approach. Instead of analyzing a word form as a set of morphemes arranged in sequence, a word form is said to be the result of applying rules that alter a word-form or stem in order to produce a new one. An inflectional rule takes a stem, changes it as is required by the rule, and outputs a word form;[22] a derivational rule takes a stem, changes it as per its own requirements, and outputs a derived stem; a compounding rule takes word forms, and similarly outputs a compound stem.

Word-based morphology

Word-based morphology is (usually) a word-and-paradigm approach. The theory takes paradigms as a central notion. Instead of stating rules to combine morphemes into word forms or to generate word forms from stems, word-based morphology states generalizations that hold between the forms of inflectional paradigms. The major point behind this approach is that many such generalizations are hard to state with either of the other approaches. Word-and-paradigm approaches are also well-suited to capturing purely morphological phenomena, such as morphomes. Examples to show the effectiveness of word-based approaches are usually drawn from fusional languages, where a given "piece" of a word, which a morpheme-based theory would call an inflectional morpheme, corresponds to a combination of grammatical categories, for example, "third-person plural". Morpheme-based theories usually have no problems with this situation since one says that a given morpheme has two categories. Item-and-process theories, on the other hand, often break down in cases like these because they all too often assume that there will be two separate rules here, one for third person, and the other for plural, but the distinction between them turns out to be artificial. The approaches treat these as whole words that are related to each other by analogical rules. Words can be categorized based on the pattern they fit into. This applies both to existing words and to new ones. Application of a pattern different from the one that has been used historically can give rise to a new word, such as older replacing elder (where older follows the normal pattern of adjectival superlatives) and cows replacing kine (where cows fits the regular pattern of plural formation).

Morphological typology

In the 19th century, philologists devised a now classic classification of languages according to their morphology. Some languages are isolating, and have little to no morphology; others are agglutinative whose words tend to have many easily separable morphemes; others yet are inflectional or fusional because their inflectional morphemes are "fused" together. That leads to one bound morpheme conveying multiple pieces of information. A standard example of an isolating language is Chinese. An agglutinative language is Turkish. Latin and Greek are prototypical inflectional or fusional languages.

It is clear that this classification is not at all clearcut, and many languages (Latin and Greek among them) do not neatly fit any one of these types, and some fit in more than one way. A continuum of complex morphology of language may be adopted.

The three models of morphology stem from attempts to analyze languages that more or less match different categories in this typology. The item-and-arrangement approach fits very naturally with agglutinative languages. The item-and-process and word-and-paradigm approaches usually address fusional languages.

As there is very little fusion involved in word formation, classical typology mostly applies to inflectional morphology. Depending on the preferred way of expressing non-inflectional notions, languages may be classified as synthetic (using word formation) or analytic (using syntactic phrases).

Examples

Pingelapese is a Micronesian language spoken on the Pingelap atoll and on two of the eastern Caroline Islands, called the high island of Pohnpei. Similar to other languages, words in Pingelapese can take different forms to add to or even change its meaning. Verbal suffixes are morphemes added at the end of a word to change its form. Prefixes are those that are added at the front. For example, the Pingelapese suffix –kin means ‘with’ or 'at.’ It is added at the end of a verb.

ius = to use → ius-kin = to use with
mwahu = to be good → mwahu-kin = to be good at

sa- is an example of a verbal prefix. It is added to the beginning of a word and means ‘not.’

pwung = to be correct → sa-pwung = to be incorrect

There are also directional suffixes that when added to the root word give the listener a better idea of where the subject is headed. The verb alu means to walk. A directional suffix can be used to give more detail.

-da = ‘up’ → aluh-da = to walk up
-di = ‘down’ → aluh-di = to walk down
-eng = ‘away from speaker and listener’ → aluh-eng = to walk away

Directional suffixes are not limited to motion verbs. When added to non-motion verbs, their meanings are a figurative one. The following table gives some examples of directional suffixes and their possible meanings.[23]

Directional suffix Motion verb Non-motion verb
-da up Onset of a state
-di down Action has been completed
-la away from Change has caused the start of a new state
-doa towards Action continued to a certain point in time
-sang from Comparative

See also

Footnotes

  1. ^ Für die lere von der wortform wäle ich das wort « morphologie», nach dem vorgange der naturwißenschaften [...] (Standard High German "Für die Lehre von der Wortform wähle ich das Wort „Morphologie“, nach dem Vorgange der Naturwissenschaften [...]", "For the science of word-formation, I choose the term "morphology"...."
  2. ^ Formerly known as Kwakiutl, Kwak'wala belongs to the Northern branch of the Wakashan language family. "Kwakiutl" is still used to refer to the tribe itself, along with other terms.
  3. ^ Example taken from Foley (1998) using a modified transcription. This phenomenon of Kwak'wala was reported by Jacobsen as cited in van Valin & LaPolla (1997).
  4. ^ The existence of words like appendix and pending in English does not mean that the English word depend is analyzed into a derivational prefix de- and a root pend. While all those were indeed once related to each other by morphological rules, that was only the case in Latin, not in English. English borrowed such words from French and Latin but not the morphological rules that allowed Latin speakers to combine de- and the verb pendere 'to hang' into the derivative dependere.

References

  1. ^ Jones, Daniel (2003) [1917], Peter Roach; James Hartmann; Jane Setter (eds.), English Pronouncing Dictionary, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, ISBN 3-12-539683-2
  2. ^ Anderson, Stephen R. (n.d.). "Morphology". Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science. Macmillan Reference, Ltd., Yale University. Retrieved 30 July 2016.
  3. ^ Aronoff, Mark; Fudeman, Kirsten (n.d.). "Morphology and Morphological Analysis" (PDF). What is Morphology?. Blackwell Publishing. Retrieved 30 July 2016.
  4. ^ Brown, Dunstan (December 2012) [2010]. (PDF). In Jae Jung Song (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. pp. 487–503. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199281251.013.0023. Archived from the original (PDF) on 15 August 2016. Retrieved 30 July 2016.
  5. ^ Sankin, A.A. (1979) [1966]. "I. Introduction" (PDF). In Ginzburg, R.S.; Khidekel, S.S.; Knyazeva, G. Y.; Sankin, A.A. (eds.). A Course in Modern English Lexicology (Revised and Enlarged, Second ed.). Moscow: VYSŠAJA ŠKOLA. p. 7. Retrieved 30 July 2016.
  6. ^ Wilson-Fowler, E.B., & Apel, K. (2015). "Influence of Morphological Awareness on College Students' Literacy Skills: A path Analytic Approach". Journal of Literacy Research. 47 (3): 405–32. doi:10.1177/1086296x15619730. S2CID 142149285.
  7. ^ Beard, Robert (1995). Lexeme-Morpheme Base Morphology: A General Theory of Inflection and Word Formation. Albany: NY: State University of New York Press. pp. 2, 3. ISBN 0-7914-2471-5.
  8. ^ Åkesson 2001.
  9. ^ Schleicher, August (1859). "Zur Morphologie der Sprache". Mémoires de l'Académie Impériale des Sciences de St.-Pétersbourg. VII°. Vol. I, N.7. St. Petersburg. p. 35.
  10. ^ Haspelmath & Sims 2002, p. 15.
  11. ^ Haspelmath & Sims 2002, p. 16.
  12. ^ Word : a cross-linguistic typology. Robert M. W. Dixon, A. I︠U︡. Aĭkhenvalʹd. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 2002. ISBN 978-0-511-48624-1. OCLC 704513339.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: others (link)
  13. ^ Anderson, Stephen R. (1992). A-Morphous Morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 74, 75. ISBN 9780521378666.
  14. ^ Plag, Ingo (2003). "Word Formation in English" (PDF). Library of Congress. Cambridge. Retrieved 2016-11-30.
  15. ^ a b c Dufter and Stark (2017) Introduction - 2 Syntax and morphosyntax: some basic notions in Dufter, Andreas, and Stark, Elisabeth (eds., 2017) Manual of Romance Morphosyntax and Syntax, Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co KG
  16. ^ Emily M. Bender (2013) Linguistic Fundamentals for Natural Language Processing: 100 Essentials from Morphology and Syntax, ch.4 Morphosyntax, p.35, Morgan & Claypool Publishers
  17. ^ Van Valin, R. D., van Valin Jr, R. D., van Valin Jr, R. D., LaPolla, R. J., & LaPolla, R. J. (1997) Syntax: Structure, meaning, and function, p.2, Cambridge University Press.
  18. ^ Haspelmath, Martin; Sims, Andrea D. (2002). Understanding Morphology. London: Arnold. ISBN 0-340-76026-5.
  19. ^ Beard 1995.
  20. ^ Bloomfield 1993.
  21. ^ Hockett 1947.
  22. ^ Bybee, Joan L. (1985). Morphology: A Study of the Relation Between Meaning and Form. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 11, 13.
  23. ^ Hattori, Ryoko (2012). Preverbal Particles in Pingelapese. pp. 31–33.

Further reading

  • Aronoff, Mark (1993). Morphology by Itself. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. ISBN 9780262510721.
  • Aronoff, Mark (2009). (PDF). ReVEL. 7 (12). ISSN 1678-8931. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-06..
  • Åkesson, Joyce (2001). Arabic morphology and phonology: based on the Marāḥ al-arwāḥ by Aḥmad b. ʻAlī b. Masʻūd. Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill. ISBN 9789004120280.
  • Bauer, Laurie (2003). Introducing linguistic morphology (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: SGeorgetown University Press. ISBN 0-87840-343-4.
  • Bauer, Laurie (2004). A glossary of morphology. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.
  • Bloomfield, Leonard (1933). Language. New York: Henry Holt. OCLC 760588323.
  • Bubenik, Vit (1999). An introduction to the study of morphology. LINCOM coursebooks in linguistics, 07. Muenchen: LINCOM Europa. ISBN 3-89586-570-2.
  • Dixon, R. M. W.; Aikhenvald, Alexandra Y., eds. (2007). Word: A cross-linguistic typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Foley, William A (1998). (Speech). Voice and Grammatical Functions in Austronesian. University of Sydney. Archived from the original on 2006-09-25.
  • Hockett, Charles F. (1947). "Problems of morphemic analysis". Language. 23 (4): 321–343. doi:10.2307/410295. JSTOR 410295.
  • Fabrega, Antonio; Scalise, Sergio (2012). Morphology: from Data to Theory. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
  • Katamba, Francis (1993). Morphology. New York: St. Martin's Press. ISBN 0-312-10356-5.
  • Korsakov, Andrey Konstantinovich (1969). "The use of tenses in English". In Korsakov, Andrey Konstantinovich (ed.). Structure of Modern English pt. 1.
  • Kishorjit, N; Vidya Raj, RK; Nirmal, Y; Sivaji, B. (December 2012). Manipuri Morpheme Identification (PDF) (Speech). Proceedings of the 3rd Workshop on South and Southeast Asian Natural Language Processing (SANLP). Mumbai: COLING.
  • Matthews, Peter (1991). Morphology (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-42256-6.
  • Mel'čuk, Igor A (1993). Cours de morphologie générale (in French). Montreal: Presses de l'Université de Montréal.
  • Mel'čuk, Igor A (2006). Aspects of the theory of morphology. Berlin: Mouton.
  • Scalise, Sergio (1983). Generative Morphology. Dordrecht: Foris.
  • Singh, Rajendra; Starosta, Stanley, eds. (2003). Explorations in Seamless Morphology. SAGE. ISBN 0-7619-9594-3.
  • Spencer, Andrew (1991). Morphological theory: an introduction to word structure in generative grammar. Blackwell textbooks in linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell. ISBN 0-631-16144-9.
  • Spencer, Andrew; Zwicky, Arnold M., eds. (1998). The handbook of morphology. Blackwell handbooks in linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell. ISBN 0-631-18544-5.
  • Stump, Gregory T. (2001). Inflectional morphology: a theory of paradigm structure. Cambridge studies in linguistics. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-78047-0.
  • van Valin, Robert D.; LaPolla, Randy (1997). Syntax : Structure, Meaning And Function. Cambridge University Press.

External links

  • Lecture 7 Morphology in Linguistics 001 by Mark Liberman, ling.upenn.edu
  • Intro to Linguistics – Morphology by Jirka Hana, ufal.mff.cuni.cz
  • Morphology by Stephen R. Anderson, part of Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, cowgill.ling.yale.edu
  • Introduction to Linguistic Theory - Morphology: The Words of Language by Adam Szczegielniak, scholar.harvard.edu
  • LIGN120: Introduction to Morphology by Farrell Ackerman and Henry Beecher, grammar.ucsd.edu
  • Morphological analysis by P.J.Hancox, cs.bham.ac.uk

morphology, linguistics, confused, with, morphological, typology, linguistics, morphology, ɔːr, study, words, they, formed, their, relationship, other, words, same, language, analyzes, structure, words, parts, words, such, stems, root, words, prefixes, suffixe. Not to be confused with Morphological typology In linguistics morphology m ɔːr ˈ f ɒ l e dʒ i 1 is the study of words how they are formed and their relationship to other words in the same language 2 3 It analyzes the structure of words and parts of words such as stems root words prefixes and suffixes Morphology also looks at parts of speech intonation and stress and the ways context can change a word s pronunciation and meaning Morphology differs from morphological typology which is the classification of languages based on their use of words 4 and lexicology which is the study of words and how they make up a language s vocabulary 5 While words along with clitics are generally accepted as being the smallest units of syntax in most languages if not all many words can be related to other words by rules that collectively describe the grammar for that language For example English speakers recognize that the words dog and dogs are closely related differentiated only by the plurality morpheme s only found bound to noun phrases Speakers of English a fusional language recognize these relations from their innate knowledge of English s rules of word formation They infer intuitively that dog is to dogs as cat is to cats and in similar fashion dog is to dog catcher as dish is to dishwasher By contrast Classical Chinese has very little morphology using almost exclusively unbound morphemes free morphemes and it relies on word order to convey meaning Most words in modern Standard Chinese Mandarin however are compounds and most roots are bound These are understood as grammars that represent the morphology of the language The rules understood by a speaker reflect specific patterns or regularities in the way words are formed from smaller units in the language they are using and how those smaller units interact in speech In this way morphology is the branch of linguistics that studies patterns of word formation within and across languages and attempts to formulate rules that model the knowledge of the speakers of those languages Phonological and orthographic modifications between a base word and its origin may be partial to literacy skills Studies have indicated that the presence of modification in phonology and orthography makes morphologically complex words harder to understand and that the absence of modification between a base word and its origin makes morphologically complex words easier to understand Morphologically complex words are easier to comprehend when they include a base word 6 Polysynthetic languages such as Chukchi have words composed of many morphemes For example the Chukchi word temeyŋelevtpegterken meaning I have a fierce headache is composed of eight morphemes t e meyŋ e levt pegt e rken that may be glossed The morphology of such languages allows for each consonant and vowel to be understood as morphemes while the grammar of the language indicates the usage and understanding of each morpheme The discipline that deals specifically with the sound changes occurring within morphemes is morphophonology Contents 1 History 2 Fundamental concepts 2 1 Lexemes and word forms 2 1 1 Prosodic word vs morphological word 2 2 Inflection vs word formation 2 3 Types of word formation 2 4 Paradigms and morphosyntax 2 5 Allomorphy 2 6 Lexical morphology 3 Models 3 1 Morpheme based morphology 3 2 Lexeme based morphology 3 3 Word based morphology 4 Morphological typology 5 Examples 6 See also 7 Footnotes 8 References 9 Further reading 10 External linksHistory EditThe history of morphological analysis dates back to the ancient Indian linguist Paṇini who formulated the 3 959 rules of Sanskrit morphology in the text Aṣṭadhyayi by using a constituency grammar The Greco Roman grammatical tradition also engaged in morphological analysis 7 Studies in Arabic morphology conducted by Maraḥ al arwaḥ and Aḥmad b ali Mas ud date back to at least 1200 CE 8 The linguistic term morphology was coined by August Schleicher in 1859 a 9 Fundamental concepts EditLexemes and word forms Edit The term word has no well defined meaning 10 Instead two related terms are used in morphology lexeme and word form Generally a lexeme is a set of inflected word forms that is often represented with the citation form in small capitals 11 For instance the lexeme eat contains the word forms eat eats eaten and ate Eat and eats are thus considered different word forms belonging to the same lexeme eat Eat and Eater on the other hand are different lexemes as they refer to two different concepts Prosodic word vs morphological word Edit Here are examples from other languages of the failure of a single phonological word to coincide with a single morphological word form In Latin one way to express the concept of NOUN PHRASE 1 and NOUN PHRASE 2 as in apples and oranges is to suffix que to the second noun phrase apples oranges and An extreme level of the theoretical quandary posed by some phonological words is provided by the Kwak wala language b In Kwak wala as in a great many other languages meaning relations between nouns including possession and semantic case are formulated by affixes instead of by independent words The three word English phrase with his club in which with identifies its dependent noun phrase as an instrument and his denotes a possession relation would consist of two words or even one word in many languages Unlike most other languages Kwak wala semantic affixes phonologically attach not to the lexeme they pertain to semantically but to the preceding lexeme Consider the following example in Kwak wala sentences begin with what corresponds to an English verb c kwixʔid i daclubbed PIVOT DETERMINERbegwanemai x aman ACCUSATIVE DETERMINERq asa s isiotter INSTRUMENTAL 3SG POSSESSIVEt alwagwayuclubkwixʔid i da begwanemai x a q asa s isi t alwagwayuclubbed PIVOT DETERMINER man ACCUSATIVE DETERMINER otter INSTRUMENTAL 3SG POSSESSIVE club the man clubbed the otter with his club Notation notes accusative case marks an entity that something is done to determiners are words such as the this and that the concept of pivot is a theoretical construct that is not relevant to this discussion That is to a speaker of Kwak wala the sentence does not contain the words him the otter or with his club Instead the markers i da PIVOT the referring to man attaches not to the noun begwanema man but to the verb the markers x a ACCUSATIVE the referring to otter attach to begwanema instead of to q asa otter etc In other words a speaker of Kwak wala does not perceive the sentence to consist of these phonological words kwixʔidclubbedi da begwanemaPIVOT the manix a q asahit the otters isi t alwagwayuwith hisi clubkwixʔid i da begwanema x a q asa s isi t alwagwayuclubbed PIVOT the mani hit the otter with hisi club A central publication on this topic is the volume edited by Dixon and Aikhenvald 2002 examining the mismatch between prosodic phonological and grammatical definitions of word in various Amazonian Australian Aboriginal Caucasian Eskimo Indo European Native North American West African and sign languages Apparently a wide variety of languages make use of the hybrid linguistic unit clitic possessing the grammatical features of independent words but the prosodic phonological lack of freedom of bound morphemes The intermediate status of clitics poses a considerable challenge to linguistic theory 12 Inflection vs word formation Edit Given the notion of a lexeme it is possible to distinguish two kinds of morphological rules Some morphological rules relate to different forms of the same lexeme but other rules relate to different lexemes Rules of the first kind are inflectional rules but those of the second kind are rules of word formation 13 The generation of the English plural dogs from dog is an inflectional rule and compound phrases and words like dog catcher or dishwasher are examples of word formation Informally word formation rules form new words more accurately new lexemes and inflection rules yield variant forms of the same word lexeme The distinction between inflection and word formation is not at all clear cut There are many examples for which linguists fail to agree whether a given rule is inflection or word formation The next section will attempt to clarify the distinction Word formation includes a process in which one combines two complete words but inflection allows the combination of a suffix with a verb to change the latter s form to that of the subject of the sentence For example in the present indefinite go is used with subject I we you they and plural nouns but third person singular pronouns he she it and singular nouns causes goes to be used The es is therefore an inflectional marker that is used to match with its subject A further difference is that in word formation the resultant word may differ from its source word s grammatical category but in the process of inflection the word never changes its grammatical category Types of word formation Edit Main article Word formation There is a further distinction between two primary kinds of morphological word formation derivation and compounding The latter is a process of word formation that involves combining complete word forms into a single compound form Dog catcher therefore is a compound as both dog and catcher are complete word forms in their own right but are subsequently treated as parts of one form Derivation involves affixing bound non independent forms to existing lexemes but the addition of the affix derives a new lexeme The word independent for example is derived from the word dependent by using the prefix in and dependent itself is derived from the verb depend There is also word formation in the processes of clipping in which a portion of a word is removed to create a new one blending in which two parts of different words are blended into one acronyms in which each letter of the new word represents a specific word in the representation NATO for North Atlantic Treaty Organization borrowing in which words from one language are taken and used in another and coinage in which a new word is created to represent a new object or concept 14 Paradigms and morphosyntax Edit A linguistic paradigm is the complete set of related word forms associated with a given lexeme The familiar examples of paradigms are the conjugations of verbs and the declensions of nouns Also arranging the word forms of a lexeme into tables by classifying them according to shared inflectional categories such as tense aspect mood number gender or case organizes such For example the personal pronouns in English can be organized into tables by using the categories of person first second third number singular vs plural gender masculine feminine neuter and case nominative oblique genitive The inflectional categories used to group word forms into paradigms cannot be chosen arbitrarily but must be categories that are relevant to stating the syntactic rules of the language Person and number are categories that can be used to define paradigms in English because the language has grammatical agreement rules which require the verb in a sentence to appear in an inflectional form that matches the person and number of the subject Therefore the syntactic rules of English care about the difference between dog and dogs because the choice between both forms determines the form of the verb that is used However no syntactic rule shows the difference between dog and dog catcher or dependent and independent The first two are nouns and the other two are adjectives An important difference between inflection and word formation is that inflected word forms of lexemes are organized into paradigms that are defined by the requirements of syntactic rules and there are no corresponding syntactic rules for word formation The relationship between syntax and morphology as well as how they interact is called morphosyntax 15 16 the term is also used to underline the fact that syntax and morphology are interrelated 17 The study of morphosyntax concerns itself with inflection and paradigms and some approaches to morphosyntax exclude from its domain the phenomena of word formation compounding and derivation 15 Within morphosyntax fall the study of agreement and government 15 Allomorphy Edit Above morphological rules are described as analogies between word forms dog is to dogs as cat is to cats and dish is to dishes In this case the analogy applies both to the form of the words and to their meaning In each pair the first word means one of X and the second two or more of X and the difference is always the plural form s or es affixed to the second word which signals the key distinction between singular and plural entities One of the largest sources of complexity in morphology is that the one to one correspondence between meaning and form scarcely applies to every case in the language In English there are word form pairs like ox oxen goose geese and sheep sheep whose difference between the singular and the plural is signaled in a way that departs from the regular pattern or is not signaled at all Even cases regarded as regular such as s are not so simple the s in dogs is not pronounced the same way as the s in cats and in plurals such as dishes a vowel is added before the s Those cases in which the same distinction is effected by alternative forms of a word constitute allomorphy 18 Phonological rules constrain the sounds that can appear next to each other in a language and morphological rules when applied blindly would often violate phonological rules by resulting in sound sequences that are prohibited in the language in question For example to form the plural of dish by simply appending an s to the end of the word would result in the form dɪʃs which is not permitted by the phonotactics of English To rescue the word a vowel sound is inserted between the root and the plural marker and dɪʃɪz results Similar rules apply to the pronunciation of the s in dogs and cats it depends on the quality voiced vs unvoiced of the final preceding phoneme Lexical morphology Edit Lexical morphology is the branch of morphology that deals with the lexicon that morphologically conceived is the collection of lexemes in a language As such it concerns itself primarily with word formation derivation and compounding Models EditThere are three principal approaches to morphology and each tries to capture the distinctions above in different ways Morpheme based morphology which makes use of an item and arrangement approach Lexeme based morphology which normally makes use of an item and process approach Word based morphology which normally makes use of a word and paradigm approach While the associations indicated between the concepts in each item in that list are very strong they are not absolute Morpheme based morphology Edit Morpheme based morphology tree of the word independently In morpheme based morphology word forms are analyzed as arrangements of morphemes A morpheme is defined as the minimal meaningful unit of a language In a word such as independently the morphemes are said to be in de pend ent and ly pend is the bound root and the other morphemes are in this case derivational affixes d In words such as dogs dog is the root and the s is an inflectional morpheme In its simplest and most naive form this way of analyzing word forms called item and arrangement treats words as if they were made of morphemes put after each other concatenated like beads on a string More recent and sophisticated approaches such as distributed morphology seek to maintain the idea of the morpheme while accommodating non concatenated analogical and other processes that have proven problematic for item and arrangement theories and similar approaches Morpheme based morphology presumes three basic axioms 19 Baudouin s single morpheme hypothesis Roots and affixes have the same status as morphemes Bloomfield s sign base morpheme hypothesis As morphemes they are dualistic signs since they have both phonological form and meaning Bloomfield s lexical morpheme hypothesis morphemes affixes and roots alike are stored in the lexicon Morpheme based morphology comes in two flavours one Bloomfieldian 20 and one Hockettian 21 For Bloomfield the morpheme was the minimal form with meaning but did not have meaning itself clarification needed For Hockett morphemes are meaning elements not form elements For him there is a morpheme plural using allomorphs such as s en and ren Within much morpheme based morphological theory the two views are mixed in unsystematic ways so a writer may refer to the morpheme plural and the morpheme s in the same sentence Lexeme based morphology Edit Lexeme based morphology usually takes what is called an item and process approach Instead of analyzing a word form as a set of morphemes arranged in sequence a word form is said to be the result of applying rules that alter a word form or stem in order to produce a new one An inflectional rule takes a stem changes it as is required by the rule and outputs a word form 22 a derivational rule takes a stem changes it as per its own requirements and outputs a derived stem a compounding rule takes word forms and similarly outputs a compound stem Word based morphology Edit Word based morphology is usually a word and paradigm approach The theory takes paradigms as a central notion Instead of stating rules to combine morphemes into word forms or to generate word forms from stems word based morphology states generalizations that hold between the forms of inflectional paradigms The major point behind this approach is that many such generalizations are hard to state with either of the other approaches Word and paradigm approaches are also well suited to capturing purely morphological phenomena such as morphomes Examples to show the effectiveness of word based approaches are usually drawn from fusional languages where a given piece of a word which a morpheme based theory would call an inflectional morpheme corresponds to a combination of grammatical categories for example third person plural Morpheme based theories usually have no problems with this situation since one says that a given morpheme has two categories Item and process theories on the other hand often break down in cases like these because they all too often assume that there will be two separate rules here one for third person and the other for plural but the distinction between them turns out to be artificial The approaches treat these as whole words that are related to each other by analogical rules Words can be categorized based on the pattern they fit into This applies both to existing words and to new ones Application of a pattern different from the one that has been used historically can give rise to a new word such as older replacing elder where older follows the normal pattern of adjectival superlatives and cows replacing kine where cows fits the regular pattern of plural formation Morphological typology EditMain article Morphological typology In the 19th century philologists devised a now classic classification of languages according to their morphology Some languages are isolating and have little to no morphology others are agglutinative whose words tend to have many easily separable morphemes others yet are inflectional or fusional because their inflectional morphemes are fused together That leads to one bound morpheme conveying multiple pieces of information A standard example of an isolating language is Chinese An agglutinative language is Turkish Latin and Greek are prototypical inflectional or fusional languages It is clear that this classification is not at all clearcut and many languages Latin and Greek among them do not neatly fit any one of these types and some fit in more than one way A continuum of complex morphology of language may be adopted The three models of morphology stem from attempts to analyze languages that more or less match different categories in this typology The item and arrangement approach fits very naturally with agglutinative languages The item and process and word and paradigm approaches usually address fusional languages As there is very little fusion involved in word formation classical typology mostly applies to inflectional morphology Depending on the preferred way of expressing non inflectional notions languages may be classified as synthetic using word formation or analytic using syntactic phrases Examples EditPingelapese is a Micronesian language spoken on the Pingelap atoll and on two of the eastern Caroline Islands called the high island of Pohnpei Similar to other languages words in Pingelapese can take different forms to add to or even change its meaning Verbal suffixes are morphemes added at the end of a word to change its form Prefixes are those that are added at the front For example the Pingelapese suffix kin means with or at It is added at the end of a verb ius to use ius kin to use with mwahu to be good mwahu kin to be good atsa is an example of a verbal prefix It is added to the beginning of a word and means not pwung to be correct sa pwung to be incorrectThere are also directional suffixes that when added to the root word give the listener a better idea of where the subject is headed The verb alu means to walk A directional suffix can be used to give more detail da up aluh da to walk up di down aluh di to walk down eng away from speaker and listener aluh eng to walk awayDirectional suffixes are not limited to motion verbs When added to non motion verbs their meanings are a figurative one The following table gives some examples of directional suffixes and their possible meanings 23 Directional suffix Motion verb Non motion verb da up Onset of a state di down Action has been completed la away from Change has caused the start of a new state doa towards Action continued to a certain point in time sang from ComparativeSee also EditMorphome linguistics Footnotes Edit Fur die lere von der wortform wale ich das wort morphologie nach dem vorgange der naturwissenschaften Standard High German Fur die Lehre von der Wortform wahle ich das Wort Morphologie nach dem Vorgange der Naturwissenschaften For the science of word formation I choose the term morphology Formerly known as Kwakiutl Kwak wala belongs to the Northern branch of the Wakashan language family Kwakiutl is still used to refer to the tribe itself along with other terms Example taken from Foley 1998 using a modified transcription This phenomenon of Kwak wala was reported by Jacobsen as cited in van Valin amp LaPolla 1997 The existence of words like appendix and pending in English does not mean that the English word depend is analyzed into a derivational prefix de and a root pend While all those were indeed once related to each other by morphological rules that was only the case in Latin not in English English borrowed such words from French and Latin but not the morphological rules that allowed Latin speakers to combine de and the verb pendere to hang into the derivative dependere References Edit Jones Daniel 2003 1917 Peter Roach James Hartmann Jane Setter eds English Pronouncing Dictionary Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 3 12 539683 2 Anderson Stephen R n d Morphology Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science Macmillan Reference Ltd Yale University Retrieved 30 July 2016 Aronoff Mark Fudeman Kirsten n d Morphology and Morphological Analysis PDF What is Morphology Blackwell Publishing Retrieved 30 July 2016 Brown Dunstan December 2012 2010 Morphological Typology PDF In Jae Jung Song ed The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology pp 487 503 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780199281251 013 0023 Archived from the original PDF on 15 August 2016 Retrieved 30 July 2016 Sankin A A 1979 1966 I Introduction PDF In Ginzburg R S Khidekel S S Knyazeva G Y Sankin A A eds A Course in Modern English Lexicology Revised and Enlarged Second ed Moscow VYSSAJA SKOLA p 7 Retrieved 30 July 2016 Wilson Fowler E B amp Apel K 2015 Influence of Morphological Awareness on College Students Literacy Skills A path Analytic Approach Journal of Literacy Research 47 3 405 32 doi 10 1177 1086296x15619730 S2CID 142149285 Beard Robert 1995 Lexeme Morpheme Base Morphology A General Theory of Inflection and Word Formation Albany NY State University of New York Press pp 2 3 ISBN 0 7914 2471 5 Akesson 2001 Schleicher August 1859 Zur Morphologie der Sprache Memoires de l Academie Imperiale des Sciences de St Petersbourg VII Vol I N 7 St Petersburg p 35 Haspelmath amp Sims 2002 p 15 Haspelmath amp Sims 2002 p 16 Word a cross linguistic typology Robert M W Dixon A I U Aĭkhenvalʹd Cambridge Cambridge University Press 2002 ISBN 978 0 511 48624 1 OCLC 704513339 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint others link Anderson Stephen R 1992 A Morphous Morphology Cambridge Cambridge University Press p 74 75 ISBN 9780521378666 Plag Ingo 2003 Word Formation in English PDF Library of Congress Cambridge Retrieved 2016 11 30 a b c Dufter and Stark 2017 Introduction 2 Syntax and morphosyntax some basic notions in Dufter Andreas and Stark Elisabeth eds 2017 Manual of Romance Morphosyntax and Syntax Walter de Gruyter GmbH amp Co KG Emily M Bender 2013 Linguistic Fundamentals for Natural Language Processing 100 Essentials from Morphology and Syntax ch 4 Morphosyntax p 35 Morgan amp Claypool Publishers Van Valin R D van Valin Jr R D van Valin Jr R D LaPolla R J amp LaPolla R J 1997 Syntax Structure meaning and function p 2 Cambridge University Press Haspelmath Martin Sims Andrea D 2002 Understanding Morphology London Arnold ISBN 0 340 76026 5 Beard 1995 Bloomfield 1993 sfn error no target CITEREFBloomfield1993 help Hockett 1947 Bybee Joan L 1985 Morphology A Study of the Relation Between Meaning and Form Amsterdam John Benjamins pp 11 13 Hattori Ryoko 2012 Preverbal Particles in Pingelapese pp 31 33 Further reading EditAronoff Mark 1993 Morphology by Itself Cambridge MA MIT Press ISBN 9780262510721 Aronoff Mark 2009 Morphology an interview with Mark Aronoff PDF ReVEL 7 12 ISSN 1678 8931 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 07 06 Akesson Joyce 2001 Arabic morphology and phonology based on the Maraḥ al arwaḥ by Aḥmad b ʻAli b Masʻud Leiden The Netherlands Brill ISBN 9789004120280 Bauer Laurie 2003 Introducing linguistic morphology 2nd ed Washington DC SGeorgetown University Press ISBN 0 87840 343 4 Bauer Laurie 2004 A glossary of morphology Washington DC Georgetown University Press Bloomfield Leonard 1933 Language New York Henry Holt OCLC 760588323 Bubenik Vit 1999 An introduction to the study of morphology LINCOM coursebooks in linguistics 07 Muenchen LINCOM Europa ISBN 3 89586 570 2 Dixon R M W Aikhenvald Alexandra Y eds 2007 Word A cross linguistic typology Cambridge Cambridge University Press Foley William A 1998 Symmetrical Voice Systems and Precategoriality in Philippine Languages Speech Voice and Grammatical Functions in Austronesian University of Sydney Archived from the original on 2006 09 25 Hockett Charles F 1947 Problems of morphemic analysis Language 23 4 321 343 doi 10 2307 410295 JSTOR 410295 Fabrega Antonio Scalise Sergio 2012 Morphology from Data to Theory Edinburgh Edinburgh University Press Katamba Francis 1993 Morphology New York St Martin s Press ISBN 0 312 10356 5 Korsakov Andrey Konstantinovich 1969 The use of tenses in English In Korsakov Andrey Konstantinovich ed Structure of Modern English pt 1 Kishorjit N Vidya Raj RK Nirmal Y Sivaji B December 2012 Manipuri Morpheme Identification PDF Speech Proceedings of the 3rd Workshop on South and Southeast Asian Natural Language Processing SANLP Mumbai COLING Matthews Peter 1991 Morphology 2nd ed Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 42256 6 Mel cuk Igor A 1993 Cours de morphologie generale in French Montreal Presses de l Universite de Montreal Mel cuk Igor A 2006 Aspects of the theory of morphology Berlin Mouton Scalise Sergio 1983 Generative Morphology Dordrecht Foris Singh Rajendra Starosta Stanley eds 2003 Explorations in Seamless Morphology SAGE ISBN 0 7619 9594 3 Spencer Andrew 1991 Morphological theory an introduction to word structure in generative grammar Blackwell textbooks in linguistics Oxford Blackwell ISBN 0 631 16144 9 Spencer Andrew Zwicky Arnold M eds 1998 The handbook of morphology Blackwell handbooks in linguistics Oxford Blackwell ISBN 0 631 18544 5 Stump Gregory T 2001 Inflectional morphology a theory of paradigm structure Cambridge studies in linguistics Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 78047 0 van Valin Robert D LaPolla Randy 1997 Syntax Structure Meaning And Function Cambridge University Press External links EditLecture 7 Morphology in Linguistics 001 by Mark Liberman ling upenn edu Intro to Linguistics Morphology by Jirka Hana ufal mff cuni cz Morphology by Stephen R Anderson part of Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science cowgill ling yale edu Introduction to Linguistic Theory Morphology The Words of Language by Adam Szczegielniak scholar harvard edu LIGN120 Introduction to Morphology by Farrell Ackerman and Henry Beecher grammar ucsd edu Morphological analysis by P J Hancox cs bham ac uk Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Morphology linguistics amp oldid 1133307759, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.