fbpx
Wikipedia

Linguistic typology

Linguistic typology (or language typology) is a field of linguistics that studies and classifies languages according to their structural features to allow their comparison. Its aim is to describe and explain the structural diversity and the common properties of the world's languages.[1] Its subdisciplines include, but are not limited to phonological typology, which deals with sound features; syntactic typology, which deals with word order and form; lexical typology, which deals with language vocabulary; and theoretical typology, which aims to explain the universal tendencies.[2]

Linguistic typology is contrasted with genealogical linguistics on the grounds that typology groups languages or their grammatical features based on formal similarities rather than historic descendence.[3] The issue of genealogical relation is however relevant to typology because modern data sets aim to be representative and unbiased. Samples are collected evenly from different language families, emphasizing the importance of exotic languages in gaining insight into human language.[4]

History

Speculations of the existence of a (logical) general or universal grammar underlying all languages were published in the Middle Ages, especially by the Modistae school. At the time, Latin was the model language of linguistics although transcribing Irish and Icelandic into the Latin alphabet was found problematic. The cross-linguistic dimension of linguistics was established in the Renaissance period. For example, Grammaticae quadrilinguis partitiones (1544) by Johannes Drosaeus compared French and the three ‘holy languages’, Hebrew, Greek, and Latin. The approach was expanded by the Port-Royal Grammar (1660) of Antoine Arnauld and Claude Lancelot, who added Spanish, Italian, German and Arabic. Nicolas Beauzée's 1767 book includes examples of English, Swedish, Lappish, Irish, Welsh, Basque, Quechua, and Chinese.[5]

The conquest and conversion of the world by Europeans gave rise to 'missionary linguistics' producing first-hand word lists and grammatical descriptions of exotic languages. Such work is accounted for in the ‘Catalogue of the Languages of the Populations We Know’, 1800, by the Spanish Jesuit Lorenzo Hervás. Johann Christoph Adelung collected the first large language sample with the Lord's prayer in almost five hundred languages (posthumous 1817).[5]

More developed nineteenth-century comparative works include Franz Bopp's 'Conjugation System' (1816) and Wilhelm von Humboldt's ‘On the Difference in Human Linguistic Structure and Its Influence on the Intellectual Development of Mankind’ (posthumous 1836). In 1818, August Wilhelm Schlegel made a classification of the world's languages into three types: (i) languages lacking grammatical structure, e.g. Chinese; (ii) agglutinative languages, e.g. Turkish; and (iii) inflectional languages, which can be synthetic like Latin and Ancient Greek, or analytic like French. This idea was later developed by others including August Schleicher, Heymann Steinthal, Franz Misteli, Franz Nicolaus Finck, and Max Müller.[3]

The word 'typology' was proposed by Georg von der Gabelentz in his Sprachwissenschaft (1891). Louis Hjelmslev proposed typology as a large-scale empirical-analytical endeavour of comparing grammatical features to uncover the essence of language. Such a project begins from the 1961 conference on language universals at Dobbs Ferry. Speakers included Roman Jakobson, Charles F. Hockett, and Joseph Greenberg who proposed forty-five different types of linguistic universals based on his data sets from thirty languages. Greenberg's findings were mostly known from the nineteenth-century grammarians, but his systematic presentation of them would serve as a model for modern typology.[3] Winfred P. Lehmann introduced Greenbergian typological theory to Indo-European studies in the 1970s.[6]

During the twentieth century, typology based on missionary linguistics became centered around SIL International, which today hosts its catalogue of living languages, Ethnologue, as an online database. The Greenbergian or universalist approach is accounted for by the World Atlas of Language Structures, among others. During the early years of the twenty-first century, however, the existence of linguistic universals became questioned by linguists proposing evolutionary typology.[7]


Method

Quantitative typology deals with the distribution and co-occurrence of structural patterns in the languages of the world.[8] Major types of non-chance distribution include:

  • preferences (for instance, absolute and implicational universals, semantic maps, and hierarchies)
  • correlations (for instance, areal patterns, such as with a Sprachbund)

Linguistic universals are patterns that can be seen cross-linguistically. Universals can either be absolute, meaning that every documented language exhibits this characteristic, or statistical, meaning that this characteristic is seen in most languages or is probable in most languages. Universals, both absolute and statistical can be unrestricted, meaning that they apply to most or all languages without any additional conditions. Conversely, both absolute and statistical universals can be restricted or implicational, meaning that a characteristic will be true on the condition of something else (if Y characteristic is true, then X characteristic is true).[9] An example of an implicational hierarchy is that dual pronouns are only found in languages with plural pronouns while singular pronouns (or unspecified in terms of number) are found in all languages. The implicational hierarchy is thus singular < plural < dual (etc.).

Qualitative typology develops cross-linguistically viable notions or types that provide a framework for the description and comparison of languages.

Subfields

The main subfields of linguistic typology include the empirical fields of syntactic, phonological and lexical typology. Additionally, theoretical typology aims to explain the empirical findings, especially statistical tendencies or implicational hierarchies.

Syntactic typology

Syntactic typology studies a vast array of grammatical phenomena from the languages of the world. Two well-known issues include dominant order and left-right symmetry.

Dominant order

One set of types reflects the basic order of subject, verb, and direct object in sentences:

These labels usually appear abbreviated as "SVO" and so forth, and may be called "typologies" of the languages to which they apply. The most commonly attested word orders are SOV and SVO while the least common orders are those that are object initial with OVS being the least common with only four attested instances.[10]

In the 1980s, linguists began to question the relevance of geographical distribution of different values for various features of linguistic structure. They may have wanted to discover whether a particular grammatical structure found in one language is likewise found in another language in the same geographic location.[11] Some languages split verbs into an auxiliary and an infinitive or participle and put the subject and/or object between them. For instance, German (Ich habe einen Fuchs im Wald gesehen - *"I have a fox in-the woods seen"), Dutch (Hans vermoedde dat Jan Marie zag leren zwemmen - *"Hans suspected that Jan Marie saw to learn to swim") and Welsh (Mae'r gwirio sillafu wedi'i gwblhau - *"Is the checking spelling after its to complete"). In this case, linguists base the typology on the non-analytic tenses (i.e. those sentences in which the verb is not split) or on the position of the auxiliary. German is thus SVO in main clauses and Welsh is VSO (and preposition phrases would go after the infinitive).

Many typologists[who?] classify both German and Dutch as V2 languages, as the verb invariantly occurs as the second element of a full clause.

Some languages allow varying degrees of freedom in their constituent order, posing a problem for their classification within the subject–verb–object schema. Languages with bound case markings for nouns, for example, tend to have more flexible word orders than languages where case is defined by position within a sentence or presence of a preposition.[example needed] To define a basic constituent order type in this case, one generally looks at frequency of different types in declarative affirmative main clauses in pragmatically neutral contexts, preferably with only old referents. Thus, for instance, Russian is widely considered an SVO language, as this is the most frequent constituent order under such conditions—all sorts of variations are possible, though, and occur in texts. In many inflected languages, such as Russian, Latin, and Greek, departures from the default word-orders are permissible but usually imply a shift in focus, an emphasis on the final element, or some special context. In the poetry of these languages, the word order may also shift freely to meet metrical demands. Additionally, freedom of word order may vary within the same language—for example, formal, literary, or archaizing varieties may have different, stricter, or more lenient constituent-order structures than an informal spoken variety of the same language.

On the other hand, when there is no clear preference under the described conditions, the language is considered to have "flexible constituent order" (a type unto itself).

An additional problem is that in languages without living speech communities, such as Latin, Ancient Greek, and Old Church Slavonic, linguists have only written evidence, perhaps written in a poetic, formalizing, or archaic style that mischaracterizes the actual daily use of the language.[12] The daily spoken language of Sophocles or Cicero might have exhibited a different or much more regular syntax than their written legacy indicates.

Theoretical issues

The below table indicates the distribution of the dominant word order pattern of over 5,000 individual languages and 366 language families. SOV is the most common type in both although much more clearly in the data of language families including isolates. 'NODOM' represents languages without a single dominant order.[13]

Type Languages % Families %
SOV 2,275 43.3% 239 65.3%
SVO 2,117 40.3% 55 15%
VSO 503 9.5% 27 7.4%
VOS 174 3.3% 15 4.1%
NODOM 124 2.3% 26 7.1%
OVS 40 0.7% 3 0.8%
OSV 19 0.3% 1 0.3%

Though the reason of dominance is sometimes considered an unsolved or unsolvable typological problem, several explanations for the distribution pattern have been proposed. Evolutionary explanations include those by Thomas Givon (1979), who suggests that all languages stem from an SOV language but are evolving into different kinds; and by Derek Bickerton (1981), who argues that the original language was SVO, which supports simpler grammar employing word order instead of case markers to differentiate between clausal roles.[14]

Universalist explanations include a model by Russell Tomlinson (1986) based on three functional principles: (i) animate before inanimate; (ii) theme before comment; and (iii) verb-object bonding. John A. Hawkins (1994) suggests that constituents are ordered from shortest to longest in VO languages, and from longest to shortest in OV languages, giving rise to the attested distribution.[14]

Left-right symmetry

A second major way of syntactic categorization is by excluding the subject from consideration. It is a well-documented typological feature that languages with a dominant OV order (object before verb), Japanese for example, tend to have postpositions. In contrast, VO languages (verb before object) like English tend to have prepositions as their main adpositional type. Several OV/VO correlations have been uncovered.[15]

Correlation VO languages OV languages
Adposition type prepositions postpositions
Order of noun and genitive noun before genitive genitive before noun
Order of adjective and standard of comparison adjective before standard standard before adjective
Order of verb and adpositional phrase verb before adpositional phrase adpositional phrase before verb
Order of verb and manner adverb verb before manner adverb manner adverb before verb
Order of copula and predicative copula before predicate predicate before copula
Order of auxiliary verb and content verb auxiliary before content verb content verb before auxiliary
Place of adverbial subordinator in clause clause-initial subordinators clause-final subordinators
Order of noun and relative clause noun before relative clause either
Theoretical issues

Several processing explanations were proposed in the 1980s and 1990s for the above correlations. They suggest that the brain finds it easier to parse syntactic patterns that are either right or left branching, but not mixed. The most widely held such explanation is John A. Hawkins' Grammar-Performance Correspondence Hypothesis, which argues that language is a non-innate adaptation to innate cognitive mechanisms. Typological tendencies are considered as being based on language users' preference for grammars that are organized efficiently, and on their avoidance of word orderings that cause processing difficulty. Some languages, however, exhibit regular inefficient patterning. These include the VO languages Chinese, with the adpositional phrase before the verb, and Finnish, which has postpositions. But there are few other profoundly exceptional languages.[14] It is suggested more recently that the left-right orientation is limited to role-marking connectives (adpositions and subordinators), stemming directly from the semantic mapping of the sentence.[16]

Morphosyntactic alignment

Another common classification distinguishes nominative–accusative alignment patterns and ergative–absolutive ones. In a language with cases, the classification depends on whether the subject (S) of an intransitive verb has the same case as the agent (A) or the patient (P) of a transitive verb. If a language has no cases, but the word order is AVP or PVA, then a classification may reflect whether the subject of an intransitive verb appears on the same side as the agent or the patient of the transitive verb. Bickel (2011) has argued that alignment should be seen as a construction-specific property rather than a language-specific property.[17]

Many languages show mixed accusative and ergative behaviour (for example: ergative morphology marking the verb arguments, on top of an accusative syntax). Other languages (called "active languages") have two types of intransitive verbs—some of them ("active verbs") join the subject in the same case as the agent of a transitive verb, and the rest ("stative verbs") join the subject in the same case as the patient[example needed]. Yet other languages behave ergatively only in some contexts (this "split ergativity" is often based on the grammatical person of the arguments or on the tense/aspect of the verb).[18] For example, only some verbs in Georgian behave this way, and, as a rule, only while using the perfective (aorist).

Phonological typology

Linguistic typology also seeks to identify patterns in the structure and distribution of sound systems among the world's languages. This is accomplished by surveying and analyzing the relative frequencies of different phonological properties. These relative frequencies might, for example, be used to determine why contrastive voicing commonly occurs with plosives, as in English neat and need, but occurs much more rarely among fricatives, such as the English niece and knees. According to a worldwide sample of 637 languages,[19] 62% have the voicing contrast in stops but only 35% have this in fricatives. In the vast majority of those cases, the absence of voicing contrast occurs because there is a lack of voiced fricatives and because all languages have some form of plosive, but there are languages with no fricatives. Below is a chart showing the breakdown of voicing properties among languages in the aforementioned sample.

Fricative Voicing
Plosive Voicing
Yes No Total
Yes 117 218 395 (62%)
No 44 198 242 (38%)
Total 221 (35%) 416 (65%) 637

[19]

Languages worldwide also vary in the number of sounds they use. These languages can go from very small phonemic inventories (Rotokas with six consonants and five vowels) to very large inventories (!Xóõ with 128 consonants and 28 vowels). An interesting phonological observation found with this data is that the larger a consonant inventory a language has, the more likely it is to contain a sound from a defined set of complex consonants (clicks, glottalized consonants, doubly articulated labial-velar stops, lateral fricatives and affricates, uvular and pharyngeal consonants, and dental or alveolar non-sibilant fricatives). Of this list, only about 26% of languages in a survey[19] of over 600 with small inventories (less than 19 consonants) contain a member of this set, while 51% of average languages (19-25) contain at least one member and 69% of large consonant inventories (greater than 25 consonants) contain a member of this set. It is then seen that complex consonants are in proportion to the size of the inventory.

Vowels contain a more modest number of phonemes, with the average being 5–6, which 51% of the languages in the survey have. About a third of the languages have larger than average vowel inventories. Most interesting though is the lack of relationship between consonant inventory size and vowel inventory size. Below is a chart showing this lack of predictability between consonant and vowel inventory sizes in relation to each other.

Consonant Inventory Vowel Quality Inventory
Small Average Large Total
Small 47 153 65 265 (39%)
Average 34 105 98 237 (35%)
Large 34 87 57 178 (26%)
Total 115 (17%) 345 (51%) 220 (32%) 680

[19]

See also

Sources

References

  1. ^ Ferguson, Charles A. (1959). "Diglossia". WORD (Worcester). 15 (2): 325–340. doi:10.1080/00437956.1959.11659702. ISSN 0043-7956. S2CID 239352211 – via Tandfonline-com.
  2. ^ Plungyan, V. A. (2011). Modern linguistic typology. Herald of the Russian Academy of Sciences, 81(2), 101-113. doi:10.1134/S1019331611020158
  3. ^ a b c Graffi, Giorgio (2010). "The Pioneers of Linguistic Typology: From Gabelentz to Greenberg". In Song, Jae Jung (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Oxford University Press. pp. 25–42. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199281251.013.0003.
  4. ^ Rijkhoff, Jan (2007). "Linguistic Typology: a short history and some current issues". Tidsskrift for Sprogforskning. 5 (1): 1–18. doi:10.7146/tfs.v5i1.529. Retrieved May 19, 2022.
  5. ^ a b Ramat, Paolo (2010). "The (Early) History of Linguistic Typology". In Song, Jae Jung (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Oxford University Press. pp. 9–23. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199281251.013.0002.
  6. ^ Lehmann, Winfred P. (1974). "The Syntactic Framework". Proto-Indo-European Syntax. University of Texas Press. ISBN 9780292733411.[permanent dead link]
  7. ^ Evans, Nicholas; Levinson, Stephen C. (2009). "The myth of language universals: Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 32 (5): 420–429. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999094X. PMID 19857320.
  8. ^ Quantitative methods in typology. (2005). In R. Köhler, G. Altmann & R. G. Piotrowski (Eds.), (). Berlin • New York: Walter de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110155785.9.554
  9. ^ Moravcsik, Edith (2013). Introducing Language Typology. Cambridge, London: Cambridge University Press. p. 9. ISBN 9780521193405.
  10. ^ Gell-Mann, Murray; Ruhlen, Merritt (October 18, 2011). "The origin and evolution of word order". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (42): 17290–17295. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10817290G. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113716108. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 3198322. PMID 21987807.
  11. ^ Comrie, Bernard, et al. “Chapter Introduction.” WALS Online - Chapter Introduction, The World Atlas of Language Structures Online, 2013.
  12. ^ [1] February 11, 2021, at the Wayback Machine Issues in the Linguistic Analysis of a Dead Language, with Particular Reference to Ancient Hebrew, Holmstead, R 2006
  13. ^ Hammarström, Harald (2016). "Linguistic diversity and language evolution". Journal of Language Evolution. 1 (1): 19–29. doi:10.1093/jole/lzw002. Retrieved May 19, 2022.
  14. ^ a b c Song, Jae Jung (2012). Word Order. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9781139033930.
  15. ^ Dryer, Matthew S. (1992). "Greenbergian Word Order Correlations" (PDF). Language. 68 (1): 81–138. doi:10.2307/416370. JSTOR 416370. (PDF) from the original on July 1, 2010. Retrieved January 1, 2021.
  16. ^ Austin, Patrik (2021). "A semantic and pragmatic explanation of harmony". Acta Linguistica Hafniensia. 54 (1): 1–23. doi:10.1080/03740463.2021.1987685. S2CID 244941417. Retrieved May 19, 2022.
  17. ^ Bickel, B. "What is typology? - a short note" (PDF). www.uni-leipzig.de (in German). Retrieved March 6, 2017.
  18. ^ Legate, J. A. (2008). Morphological and abstract case. Linguistic Inquiry, 39(1), 55-101. doi:10.1162/ling.2008.39.1.55
  19. ^ a b c d Song, J.J. (ed.) (2011). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-928125-1.

Bibliography

  • Bisang, W. (2001). Aspects of typology and universals. Berlin: Akademie Verlag. ISBN 3-050-03559-5.
  • Comrie, B. (1989). Language universals and linguistic typology: Syntax and morphology. Oxford: Blackwell, 2nd edn. ISBN 0-226-11433-3.
  • Croft, W. (2002). Typology and universals. Cambridge: Cambridge UP. 2nd ed. ISBN 0-521-00499-3.
  • Cysouw, M. (2005). Quantitative methods in typology. Quantitative linguistics: an international handbook, ed. by Gabriel Altmann, Reinhard Köhler and R. Piotrowski. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3-11-015578-8.
  • Grijzenhout, J. (2009). Phonological domains : universals and deviations. Berlin; New York: Mouton de Gruyter. ISBN 3-110-20540-8.
  • Moravcsik, Edith A. (2013). Introducing language typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Nichols, J. (1992). Linguistic diversity in space and time. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-58057-1.
  • Nichols, J. (2007). What, if anything, is typology?[permanent dead link] Linguistic Typology. Volume 11, Issue 1, Pages 231–238, ISSN (Online) 1613-415X, ISSN (Print) 1430–0532, doi:10.1515/LINGTY.2007.017, July 2007
  • Song, J.J. (2001). Linguistic typology: Morphology and syntax. Harlow and London: Pearson Education (Longman). ISBN 0-582-31220-5.
  • Song, J.J. (ed.) (2011). The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-928125-1.
  • Whaley, L.J. (1997). Introduction to typology: The unity and diversity of language. Newbury Park: Sage. ISBN 0-8039-5963-X.

External links

  • Association for Linguistic Typology
  • Ivan G. Iliev. On the Nature of Grammatical Case, Language Typology, and on the Origin of Cognate Objects and Subjects. [2]
  • Plank, F. Themes in Typology: Basic Reading List.
  • Bickel, B. (2001). What is typology? - a short note. [4]
  • Bickel, B. (2005). Typology in the 21st century: major developments. [5]
  • "Linguistic typology" (PDF). (275 KiB), chapter 4 of Halvor Eifring & Rolf Theil: Linguistics for Students of Asian and African Languages
  • World Atlas of Language Structures
  • Linguistic Typology

linguistic, typology, language, typology, field, linguistics, that, studies, classifies, languages, according, their, structural, features, allow, their, comparison, describe, explain, structural, diversity, common, properties, world, languages, subdisciplines. Linguistic typology or language typology is a field of linguistics that studies and classifies languages according to their structural features to allow their comparison Its aim is to describe and explain the structural diversity and the common properties of the world s languages 1 Its subdisciplines include but are not limited to phonological typology which deals with sound features syntactic typology which deals with word order and form lexical typology which deals with language vocabulary and theoretical typology which aims to explain the universal tendencies 2 Linguistic typology is contrasted with genealogical linguistics on the grounds that typology groups languages or their grammatical features based on formal similarities rather than historic descendence 3 The issue of genealogical relation is however relevant to typology because modern data sets aim to be representative and unbiased Samples are collected evenly from different language families emphasizing the importance of exotic languages in gaining insight into human language 4 Contents 1 History 2 Method 3 Subfields 3 1 Syntactic typology 3 1 1 Dominant order 3 1 1 1 Theoretical issues 3 1 2 Left right symmetry 3 1 2 1 Theoretical issues 3 1 3 Morphosyntactic alignment 3 2 Phonological typology 4 See also 5 Sources 5 1 References 5 2 Bibliography 6 External linksHistory EditSpeculations of the existence of a logical general or universal grammar underlying all languages were published in the Middle Ages especially by the Modistae school At the time Latin was the model language of linguistics although transcribing Irish and Icelandic into the Latin alphabet was found problematic The cross linguistic dimension of linguistics was established in the Renaissance period For example Grammaticae quadrilinguis partitiones 1544 by Johannes Drosaeus compared French and the three holy languages Hebrew Greek and Latin The approach was expanded by the Port Royal Grammar 1660 of Antoine Arnauld and Claude Lancelot who added Spanish Italian German and Arabic Nicolas Beauzee s 1767 book includes examples of English Swedish Lappish Irish Welsh Basque Quechua and Chinese 5 The conquest and conversion of the world by Europeans gave rise to missionary linguistics producing first hand word lists and grammatical descriptions of exotic languages Such work is accounted for in the Catalogue of the Languages of the Populations We Know 1800 by the Spanish Jesuit Lorenzo Hervas Johann Christoph Adelung collected the first large language sample with the Lord s prayer in almost five hundred languages posthumous 1817 5 More developed nineteenth century comparative works include Franz Bopp s Conjugation System 1816 and Wilhelm von Humboldt s On the Difference in Human Linguistic Structure and Its Influence on the Intellectual Development of Mankind posthumous 1836 In 1818 August Wilhelm Schlegel made a classification of the world s languages into three types i languages lacking grammatical structure e g Chinese ii agglutinative languages e g Turkish and iii inflectional languages which can be synthetic like Latin and Ancient Greek or analytic like French This idea was later developed by others including August Schleicher Heymann Steinthal Franz Misteli Franz Nicolaus Finck and Max Muller 3 The word typology was proposed by Georg von der Gabelentz in his Sprachwissenschaft 1891 Louis Hjelmslev proposed typology as a large scale empirical analytical endeavour of comparing grammatical features to uncover the essence of language Such a project begins from the 1961 conference on language universals at Dobbs Ferry Speakers included Roman Jakobson Charles F Hockett and Joseph Greenberg who proposed forty five different types of linguistic universals based on his data sets from thirty languages Greenberg s findings were mostly known from the nineteenth century grammarians but his systematic presentation of them would serve as a model for modern typology 3 Winfred P Lehmann introduced Greenbergian typological theory to Indo European studies in the 1970s 6 During the twentieth century typology based on missionary linguistics became centered around SIL International which today hosts its catalogue of living languages Ethnologue as an online database The Greenbergian or universalist approach is accounted for by the World Atlas of Language Structures among others During the early years of the twenty first century however the existence of linguistic universals became questioned by linguists proposing evolutionary typology 7 Method EditQuantitative typology deals with the distribution and co occurrence of structural patterns in the languages of the world 8 Major types of non chance distribution include preferences for instance absolute and implicational universals semantic maps and hierarchies correlations for instance areal patterns such as with a Sprachbund Linguistic universals are patterns that can be seen cross linguistically Universals can either be absolute meaning that every documented language exhibits this characteristic or statistical meaning that this characteristic is seen in most languages or is probable in most languages Universals both absolute and statistical can be unrestricted meaning that they apply to most or all languages without any additional conditions Conversely both absolute and statistical universals can be restricted or implicational meaning that a characteristic will be true on the condition of something else if Y characteristic is true then X characteristic is true 9 An example of an implicational hierarchy is that dual pronouns are only found in languages with plural pronouns while singular pronouns or unspecified in terms of number are found in all languages The implicational hierarchy is thus singular lt plural lt dual etc Qualitative typology develops cross linguistically viable notions or types that provide a framework for the description and comparison of languages Subfields EditThe main subfields of linguistic typology include the empirical fields of syntactic phonological and lexical typology Additionally theoretical typology aims to explain the empirical findings especially statistical tendencies or implicational hierarchies Syntactic typology Edit Main article Word order Syntactic typology studies a vast array of grammatical phenomena from the languages of the world Two well known issues include dominant order and left right symmetry Dominant order Edit One set of types reflects the basic order of subject verb and direct object in sentences Object subject verb OSV Object verb subject OVS Subject verb object SVO Subject object verb SOV Verb subject object VSO Verb object subject VOS These labels usually appear abbreviated as SVO and so forth and may be called typologies of the languages to which they apply The most commonly attested word orders are SOV and SVO while the least common orders are those that are object initial with OVS being the least common with only four attested instances 10 In the 1980s linguists began to question the relevance of geographical distribution of different values for various features of linguistic structure They may have wanted to discover whether a particular grammatical structure found in one language is likewise found in another language in the same geographic location 11 Some languages split verbs into an auxiliary and an infinitive or participle and put the subject and or object between them For instance German Ich habe einen Fuchs im Wald gesehen I have a fox in the woods seen Dutch Hans vermoedde dat Jan Marie zag leren zwemmen Hans suspected that Jan Marie saw to learn to swim and Welsh Mae r gwirio sillafu wedi i gwblhau Is the checking spelling after its to complete In this case linguists base the typology on the non analytic tenses i e those sentences in which the verb is not split or on the position of the auxiliary German is thus SVO in main clauses and Welsh is VSO and preposition phrases would go after the infinitive Many typologists who classify both German and Dutch as V2 languages as the verb invariantly occurs as the second element of a full clause Some languages allow varying degrees of freedom in their constituent order posing a problem for their classification within the subject verb object schema Languages with bound case markings for nouns for example tend to have more flexible word orders than languages where case is defined by position within a sentence or presence of a preposition example needed To define a basic constituent order type in this case one generally looks at frequency of different types in declarative affirmative main clauses in pragmatically neutral contexts preferably with only old referents Thus for instance Russian is widely considered an SVO language as this is the most frequent constituent order under such conditions all sorts of variations are possible though and occur in texts In many inflected languages such as Russian Latin and Greek departures from the default word orders are permissible but usually imply a shift in focus an emphasis on the final element or some special context In the poetry of these languages the word order may also shift freely to meet metrical demands Additionally freedom of word order may vary within the same language for example formal literary or archaizing varieties may have different stricter or more lenient constituent order structures than an informal spoken variety of the same language On the other hand when there is no clear preference under the described conditions the language is considered to have flexible constituent order a type unto itself An additional problem is that in languages without living speech communities such as Latin Ancient Greek and Old Church Slavonic linguists have only written evidence perhaps written in a poetic formalizing or archaic style that mischaracterizes the actual daily use of the language 12 The daily spoken language of Sophocles or Cicero might have exhibited a different or much more regular syntax than their written legacy indicates Theoretical issues Edit The below table indicates the distribution of the dominant word order pattern of over 5 000 individual languages and 366 language families SOV is the most common type in both although much more clearly in the data of language families including isolates NODOM represents languages without a single dominant order 13 Type Languages Families SOV 2 275 43 3 239 65 3 SVO 2 117 40 3 55 15 VSO 503 9 5 27 7 4 VOS 174 3 3 15 4 1 NODOM 124 2 3 26 7 1 OVS 40 0 7 3 0 8 OSV 19 0 3 1 0 3 Though the reason of dominance is sometimes considered an unsolved or unsolvable typological problem several explanations for the distribution pattern have been proposed Evolutionary explanations include those by Thomas Givon 1979 who suggests that all languages stem from an SOV language but are evolving into different kinds and by Derek Bickerton 1981 who argues that the original language was SVO which supports simpler grammar employing word order instead of case markers to differentiate between clausal roles 14 Universalist explanations include a model by Russell Tomlinson 1986 based on three functional principles i animate before inanimate ii theme before comment and iii verb object bonding John A Hawkins 1994 suggests that constituents are ordered from shortest to longest in VO languages and from longest to shortest in OV languages giving rise to the attested distribution 14 Left right symmetry Edit A second major way of syntactic categorization is by excluding the subject from consideration It is a well documented typological feature that languages with a dominant OV order object before verb Japanese for example tend to have postpositions In contrast VO languages verb before object like English tend to have prepositions as their main adpositional type Several OV VO correlations have been uncovered 15 Correlation VO languages OV languagesAdposition type prepositions postpositionsOrder of noun and genitive noun before genitive genitive before nounOrder of adjective and standard of comparison adjective before standard standard before adjectiveOrder of verb and adpositional phrase verb before adpositional phrase adpositional phrase before verbOrder of verb and manner adverb verb before manner adverb manner adverb before verbOrder of copula and predicative copula before predicate predicate before copulaOrder of auxiliary verb and content verb auxiliary before content verb content verb before auxiliaryPlace of adverbial subordinator in clause clause initial subordinators clause final subordinatorsOrder of noun and relative clause noun before relative clause eitherTheoretical issues Edit Several processing explanations were proposed in the 1980s and 1990s for the above correlations They suggest that the brain finds it easier to parse syntactic patterns that are either right or left branching but not mixed The most widely held such explanation is John A Hawkins Grammar Performance Correspondence Hypothesis which argues that language is a non innate adaptation to innate cognitive mechanisms Typological tendencies are considered as being based on language users preference for grammars that are organized efficiently and on their avoidance of word orderings that cause processing difficulty Some languages however exhibit regular inefficient patterning These include the VO languages Chinese with the adpositional phrase before the verb and Finnish which has postpositions But there are few other profoundly exceptional languages 14 It is suggested more recently that the left right orientation is limited to role marking connectives adpositions and subordinators stemming directly from the semantic mapping of the sentence 16 Morphosyntactic alignment Edit Main article Morphosyntactic alignment Another common classification distinguishes nominative accusative alignment patterns and ergative absolutive ones In a language with cases the classification depends on whether the subject S of an intransitive verb has the same case as the agent A or the patient P of a transitive verb If a language has no cases but the word order is AVP or PVA then a classification may reflect whether the subject of an intransitive verb appears on the same side as the agent or the patient of the transitive verb Bickel 2011 has argued that alignment should be seen as a construction specific property rather than a language specific property 17 Many languages show mixed accusative and ergative behaviour for example ergative morphology marking the verb arguments on top of an accusative syntax Other languages called active languages have two types of intransitive verbs some of them active verbs join the subject in the same case as the agent of a transitive verb and the rest stative verbs join the subject in the same case as the patient example needed Yet other languages behave ergatively only in some contexts this split ergativity is often based on the grammatical person of the arguments or on the tense aspect of the verb 18 For example only some verbs in Georgian behave this way and as a rule only while using the perfective aorist Phonological typology Edit Main article Phonology Linguistic typology also seeks to identify patterns in the structure and distribution of sound systems among the world s languages This is accomplished by surveying and analyzing the relative frequencies of different phonological properties These relative frequencies might for example be used to determine why contrastive voicing commonly occurs with plosives as in English neat and need but occurs much more rarely among fricatives such as the English niece and knees According to a worldwide sample of 637 languages 19 62 have the voicing contrast in stops but only 35 have this in fricatives In the vast majority of those cases the absence of voicing contrast occurs because there is a lack of voiced fricatives and because all languages have some form of plosive but there are languages with no fricatives Below is a chart showing the breakdown of voicing properties among languages in the aforementioned sample Fricative VoicingPlosive Voicing Yes No TotalYes 117 218 395 62 No 44 198 242 38 Total 221 35 416 65 637 19 Languages worldwide also vary in the number of sounds they use These languages can go from very small phonemic inventories Rotokas with six consonants and five vowels to very large inventories Xoo with 128 consonants and 28 vowels An interesting phonological observation found with this data is that the larger a consonant inventory a language has the more likely it is to contain a sound from a defined set of complex consonants clicks glottalized consonants doubly articulated labial velar stops lateral fricatives and affricates uvular and pharyngeal consonants and dental or alveolar non sibilant fricatives Of this list only about 26 of languages in a survey 19 of over 600 with small inventories less than 19 consonants contain a member of this set while 51 of average languages 19 25 contain at least one member and 69 of large consonant inventories greater than 25 consonants contain a member of this set It is then seen that complex consonants are in proportion to the size of the inventory Vowels contain a more modest number of phonemes with the average being 5 6 which 51 of the languages in the survey have About a third of the languages have larger than average vowel inventories Most interesting though is the lack of relationship between consonant inventory size and vowel inventory size Below is a chart showing this lack of predictability between consonant and vowel inventory sizes in relation to each other Consonant Inventory Vowel Quality InventorySmall Average Large TotalSmall 47 153 65 265 39 Average 34 105 98 237 35 Large 34 87 57 178 26 Total 115 17 345 51 220 32 680 19 See also EditGrammaticalizationSources EditReferences Edit Ferguson Charles A 1959 Diglossia WORD Worcester 15 2 325 340 doi 10 1080 00437956 1959 11659702 ISSN 0043 7956 S2CID 239352211 via Tandfonline com Plungyan V A 2011 Modern linguistic typology Herald of the Russian Academy of Sciences 81 2 101 113 doi 10 1134 S1019331611020158 a b c Graffi Giorgio 2010 The Pioneers of Linguistic Typology From Gabelentz to Greenberg In Song Jae Jung ed The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology Oxford University Press pp 25 42 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780199281251 013 0003 Rijkhoff Jan 2007 Linguistic Typology a short history and some current issues Tidsskrift for Sprogforskning 5 1 1 18 doi 10 7146 tfs v5i1 529 Retrieved May 19 2022 a b Ramat Paolo 2010 The Early History of Linguistic Typology In Song Jae Jung ed The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology Oxford University Press pp 9 23 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780199281251 013 0002 Lehmann Winfred P 1974 The Syntactic Framework Proto Indo European Syntax University of Texas Press ISBN 9780292733411 permanent dead link Evans Nicholas Levinson Stephen C 2009 The myth of language universals Language diversity and its importance for cognitive science Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 5 420 429 doi 10 1017 S0140525X0999094X PMID 19857320 Quantitative methods in typology 2005 In R Kohler G Altmann amp R G Piotrowski Eds Berlin New York Walter de Gruyter doi 10 1515 9783110155785 9 554 Moravcsik Edith 2013 Introducing Language Typology Cambridge London Cambridge University Press p 9 ISBN 9780521193405 Gell Mann Murray Ruhlen Merritt October 18 2011 The origin and evolution of word order Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 42 17290 17295 Bibcode 2011PNAS 10817290G doi 10 1073 pnas 1113716108 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 3198322 PMID 21987807 Comrie Bernard et al Chapter Introduction WALS Online Chapter Introduction The World Atlas of Language Structures Online 2013 1 Archived February 11 2021 at the Wayback Machine Issues in the Linguistic Analysis of a Dead Language with Particular Reference to Ancient Hebrew Holmstead R 2006 Hammarstrom Harald 2016 Linguistic diversity and language evolution Journal of Language Evolution 1 1 19 29 doi 10 1093 jole lzw002 Retrieved May 19 2022 a b c Song Jae Jung 2012 Word Order Cambridge University Press ISBN 9781139033930 Dryer Matthew S 1992 Greenbergian Word Order Correlations PDF Language 68 1 81 138 doi 10 2307 416370 JSTOR 416370 Archived PDF from the original on July 1 2010 Retrieved January 1 2021 Austin Patrik 2021 A semantic and pragmatic explanation of harmony Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 54 1 1 23 doi 10 1080 03740463 2021 1987685 S2CID 244941417 Retrieved May 19 2022 Bickel B What is typology a short note PDF www uni leipzig de in German Retrieved March 6 2017 Legate J A 2008 Morphological and abstract case Linguistic Inquiry 39 1 55 101 doi 10 1162 ling 2008 39 1 55 a b c d Song J J ed 2011 The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 928125 1 Bibliography Edit Bisang W 2001 Aspects of typology and universals Berlin Akademie Verlag ISBN 3 050 03559 5 Comrie B 1989 Language universals and linguistic typology Syntax and morphology Oxford Blackwell 2nd edn ISBN 0 226 11433 3 Croft W 2002 Typology and universals Cambridge Cambridge UP 2nd ed ISBN 0 521 00499 3 Cysouw M 2005 Quantitative methods in typology Quantitative linguistics an international handbook ed by Gabriel Altmann Reinhard Kohler and R Piotrowski Berlin Mouton de Gruyter ISBN 3 11 015578 8 Grijzenhout J 2009 Phonological domains universals and deviations Berlin New York Mouton de Gruyter ISBN 3 110 20540 8 Moravcsik Edith A 2013 Introducing language typology Cambridge Cambridge University Press Nichols J 1992 Linguistic diversity in space and time Chicago University of Chicago Press ISBN 0 226 58057 1 Nichols J 2007 What if anything is typology permanent dead link Linguistic Typology Volume 11 Issue 1 Pages 231 238 ISSN Online 1613 415X ISSN Print 1430 0532 doi 10 1515 LINGTY 2007 017 July 2007 Song J J 2001 Linguistic typology Morphology and syntax Harlow and London Pearson Education Longman ISBN 0 582 31220 5 Song J J ed 2011 The Oxford Handbook of Linguistic Typology Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 928125 1 Whaley L J 1997 Introduction to typology The unity and diversity of language Newbury Park Sage ISBN 0 8039 5963 X External links EditAssociation for Linguistic Typology Ivan G Iliev On the Nature of Grammatical Case Language Typology and on the Origin of Cognate Objects and Subjects 2 Plank F Themes in Typology Basic Reading List 3 Bickel B 2001 What is typology a short note 4 Bickel B 2005 Typology in the 21st century major developments 5 Linguistic typology PDF 275 KiB chapter 4 of Halvor Eifring amp Rolf Theil Linguistics for Students of Asian and African Languages World Atlas of Language Structures Linguistic Typology Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Linguistic typology amp oldid 1126427031, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.