fbpx
Wikipedia

Primate

Primates are a diverse order of mammals. They are divided into the strepsirrhines, which include the lemurs, galagos, and lorisids, and the haplorhines, which include the tarsiers and the simians (monkeys, including apes and humans). Primates arose 85–55 million years ago first from small terrestrial mammals, which adapted to living in the trees of tropical forests: many primate characteristics represent adaptations to life in this challenging environment, including large brains, visual acuity, color vision, a shoulder girdle allowing a large degree of movement in the shoulder joint, and dextrous hands. Primates range in size from Madame Berthe's mouse lemur, which weighs 30 g (1 oz), to the eastern gorilla, weighing over 200 kg (440 lb). There are 376–524 species of living primates, depending on which classification is used. New primate species continue to be discovered: over 25 species were described in the 2000s, 36 in the 2010s, and three in the 2020s.

Primates
Temporal range: 65.9–0 Ma Early Paleocene to Present
Aye-ayeRing-tailed lemurCapuchin monkeySpider monkeyGibbonTarsierRed slender lorisLion tamarinHamadryas baboonCommon chimpanzee
Some primate families, from top to bottom: Daubentoniidae, Tarsiidae, Lemuridae, Lorisidae, Cebidae, Callitrichidae, Atelidae, Cercopithecidae, Hylobatidae, Hominidae
Scientific classification
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Mirorder: Primatomorpha
Order: Primates
Linnaeus, 1758[1]
Suborders

sister: Dermoptera

Range of the non-human primates (green)
Synonyms

Plesiadapiformes (cladistically including crown primates[2])

Primates have large brains (relative to body size) compared to other mammals, as well as an increased reliance on visual acuity at the expense of the sense of smell, which is the dominant sensory system in most mammals. These features are more developed in monkeys and apes, and noticeably less so in lorises and lemurs. Most primates also have opposable thumbs. Some primates, including gorillas, humans, and baboons, are primarily terrestrial rather than arboreal, but all species have adaptations for climbing trees. Arboreal locomotion techniques used include leaping from tree to tree and swinging between branches of trees (brachiation); terrestrial locomotion techniques include walking on two limbs (bipedalism) and modified walking on four limbs (knuckle-walking).

Primates are among the most social of animals, forming pairs or family groups, uni-male harems, and multi-male/multi-female groups. Non-human primates have at least four types of social systems, many defined by the amount of movement by adolescent females between groups. Primates have slower rates of development than other similarly sized mammals, reach maturity later, and have longer lifespans. Primates are also the most intelligent animals and non-human primates are recorded to use tools. They may communicate using facial and hand gestures, smells and vocalizations.

Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create opportunities for the transmission of zoonotic diseases, especially virus diseases, including herpes, measles, ebola, rabies, and hepatitis. Thousands of non-human primates are used in research around the world because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction. Common threats include deforestation, forest fragmentation, monkey drives, and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing for agriculture most threatens primates.

Etymology

The English name primates is derived from Old French or French primat, from a noun use of Latin primat-, from primus ('prime, first rank').[3] The name was given by Carl Linnaeus because he thought this the "highest" order of animals.[4] The relationships among the different groups of primates were not clearly understood until relatively recently, so the commonly used terms are somewhat confused. For example, ape has been used either as an alternative for monkey or for any tailless, relatively human-like primate.[5]

Sir Wilfrid Le Gros Clark was one of the primatologists who developed the idea of trends in primate evolution and the methodology of arranging the living members of an order into an "ascending series" leading to humans.[6] Commonly used names for groups of primates such as prosimians, monkeys, lesser apes, and great apes reflect this methodology. According to our current understanding of the evolutionary history of the primates, several of these groups are paraphyletic, or rather they do not include all the descendants of a common ancestor.[7]

In contrast with Clark's methodology, modern classifications typically identify (or name) only those groupings that are monophyletic; that is, such a named group includes all the descendants of the group's common ancestor.[8]

The cladogram below shows one possible classification sequence of the living primates:[9][10] groups that use common (traditional) names are shown on the right.

Primatomorpha

 Dermoptera  

 Primates 
 Haplorhini 
 Simiiformes 
 Catarrhini 
 Hominoidea 
 Hominidae 
 Homininae 
 Hominini 

humans (genus Homo)  

chimpanzees and bonobos (genus Pan) 

gorillas (tribe Gorillini)  

orangutans (subfamily Ponginae)  

gibbons (family Hylobatidae)  

Old World monkeys (superfamily Cercopithecoidea)  

New World monkeys (parvorder Platyrrhini)  

 Tarsiiformes 

tarsiers (superfamily Tarsioidea)  

 Strepsirrhini 
Lemuriformes[a] 

lemurs (superfamily Lemuroidea)  

lorises and allies (superfamily Lorisoidea)  

prosimians
monkeys
great apes
humans
lesser apes

All groups with scientific names are clades, or monophyletic groups, and the sequence of scientific classification reflects the evolutionary history of the related lineages. Groups that are traditionally named are shown on the right; they form an "ascending series" (per Clark, see above), and several groups are paraphyletic:

  • Prosimians contain two monophyletic groups (the suborder Strepsirrhini, or lemurs, lorises and allies, as well as the tarsiers of the suborder Haplorhini); it is a paraphyletic grouping because it excludes the Simiiformes, which also are descendants of the common ancestor Primates.
  • Monkeys comprise two monophyletic groups, New World monkeys and Old World monkeys, but is paraphyletic because it excludes hominoids, superfamily Hominoidea, also descendants of the common ancestor Simiiformes.
  • Apes as a whole, and the great apes, are paraphyletic if the terms are used such that they exclude humans.

Thus, the members of the two sets of groups, and hence names, do not match, which causes problems in relating scientific names to common (usually traditional) names. Consider the superfamily Hominoidea: In terms of the common names on the right, this group consists of apes and humans and there is no single common name for all the members of the group. One remedy is to create a new common name, in this case hominoids. Another possibility is to expand the use of one of the traditional names. For example, in his 2005 book, the vertebrate palaeontologist Benton wrote, "The apes, Hominoidea, today include the gibbons and orangutan ... the gorilla and chimpanzee ... and humans";[11] thereby Benton was using apes to mean hominoids. In that case, the group heretofore called apes must now be identified as the non-human apes.

As of 2021, there is no consensus as to whether to accept traditional (that is, common), but paraphyletic, names or to use monophyletic names only; or to use 'new' common names or adaptations of old ones. Both competing approaches can be found in biological sources, often in the same work, and sometimes by the same author. Thus, Benton defines apes to include humans, then he repeatedly uses ape-like to mean 'like an ape rather than a human'; and when discussing the reaction of others to a new fossil he writes of "claims that Orrorin ... was an ape rather than a human".[12]

Classification of living primates

 
A 1927 drawing of chimpanzees, a gibbon (top right) and two orangutans (center and bottom center): The chimpanzee in the upper left is brachiating; the orangutan at the bottom center is knuckle-walking.
 
Homo sapiens is the only living primate species that is fully bipedal.
 
Nilgiri langur (Trachypithecus johnii), an Old World monkey

A list of the families of the living primates is given below, together with one possible classification into ranks between order and family.[1][9][13][14] Other classifications are also used. For example, an alternative classification of the living Strepsirrhini divides them into two infraorders, Lemuriformes and Lorisiformes.[15]

Order Primates was established by Carl Linnaeus in 1758, in the tenth edition of his book Systema Naturae,[18] for the genera Homo (humans), Simia (other apes and monkeys), Lemur (prosimians) and Vespertilio (bats). In the first edition of the same book (1735), he had used the name Anthropomorpha for Homo, Simia and Bradypus (sloths).[19] In 1839, Henri Marie Ducrotay de Blainville, following Linnaeus and imitating his nomenclature, established the orders Secundates (including the suborders Chiroptera, Insectivora and Carnivora), Tertiates (or Glires) and Quaternates (including Gravigrada, Pachydermata and Ruminantia),[20] but these new taxa were not accepted.

Before Anderson and Jones introduced the classification of Strepsirrhini and Haplorhini in 1984,[21] (followed by McKenna and Bell's 1997 work Classification of Mammals: Above the species level),[22] Primates was divided into two superfamilies: Prosimii and Anthropoidea.[23] Prosimii included all of the prosimians: Strepsirrhini plus the tarsiers. Anthropoidea contained all of the simians.

Phylogeny and genetics

Euarchontoglires  
Glires 

Rodentia (rodents)

Lagomorpha (rabbits, hares, pikas)

 Euarchonta 

Scandentia (treeshrews)

Primatomorpha

Dermoptera (colugos)

Primates

Plesiadapiformes

crown primates

Order Primates is part of the clade Euarchontoglires, which is nested within the clade Eutheria of Class Mammalia. Recent molecular genetic research on primates, colugos, and treeshrews has shown that the two species of colugos are more closely related to primates than to treeshrews,[24] even though treeshrews were at one time considered primates.[25] These three orders make up the clade Euarchonta. The combination of this clade with the clade Glires (composed of Rodentia and Lagomorpha) forms the clade Euarchontoglires. Variously, both Euarchonta and Euarchontoglires are ranked as superorders. Some scientists consider Dermoptera to be a suborder of Primates and use the suborder Euprimates for the "true" primates.[26]

Evolution

The primate lineage is thought to go back at least near the Cretaceous–Paleogene boundary or around 63–74 (mya).[27][28][29][30][31] The earliest possible primate/proto-primate may be Purgatorius, which dates back to Early Paleocene of North America ~66mya.[32][33] The oldest known primates from the fossil record date to the Late Paleocene of Africa, c.57 mya (Altiatlasius)[34] or the Paleocene-Eocene transition in the northern continents, c. 55 mya (Cantius, Donrussellia, Altanius, Plesiadapis and Teilhardina).[35][36][32] Other studies, including molecular clock studies, have estimated the origin of the primate branch to have been in the mid-Cretaceous period, around 85 mya.[37][38][39]

By modern cladistic reckoning, the order Primates is monophyletic. The suborder Strepsirrhini, the "wet-nosed" primates, is generally thought to have split off from the primitive primate line about 63 mya,[40] although earlier dates are also supported.[41] The seven strepsirrhine families are the five related lemur families and the two remaining families that include the lorisids and the galagos.[1][13] Older classification schemes wrap Lepilemuridae into Lemuridae and Galagidae into Lorisidae, yielding a four-one family distribution instead of five-two as presented here.[1] During the Eocene, most of the northern continents were dominated by two groups, the adapiforms and the omomyids.[42][43] The former are considered members of Strepsirrhini, but did not have a toothcomb like modern lemurs; recent analysis has demonstrated that Darwinius masillae fits into this grouping.[44] The latter was closely related to tarsiers, monkeys, and apes. How these two groups relate to extant primates is unclear. Omomyids perished about 30 mya,[43] while adapiforms survived until about 10 mya.[45]

According to genetic studies, the lemurs of Madagascar diverged from the lorisoids approximately 75 mya.[41] These studies, as well as chromosomal and molecular evidence, also show that lemurs are more closely related to each other than to other strepsirrhine primates.[41][46] However, Madagascar split from Africa 160 mya and from India 90 mya.[47] To account for these facts, a founding lemur population of a few individuals is thought to have reached Madagascar from Africa via a single rafting event between 50 and 80 mya.[41][46][47] Other colonization options have been suggested, such as multiple colonizations from Africa and India,[42] but none are supported by the genetic and molecular evidence.[41]

Until recently, the aye-aye has been difficult to place within Strepsirrhini.[1] Theories had been proposed that its family, Daubentoniidae, was either a lemuriform primate (meaning its ancestors split from the lemur line more recently than lemurs and lorises split) or a sister group to all the other strepsirrhines. In 2008, the aye-aye family was confirmed to be most closely related to the other Malagasy lemurs, likely having descended from the same ancestral population that colonized the island.[41]

Suborder Haplorhini, the simple-nosed or "dry-nosed" primates, is composed of two sister clades.[1] Prosimian tarsiers in the family Tarsiidae (monotypic in its own infraorder Tarsiiformes), represent the most basal division, originating about 58 mya.[48][49] The earliest known haplorhine skeleton, that of 55 MA old tarsier-like Archicebus, was found in central China,[50] supporting an already suspected Asian origin for the group.[51] The infraorder Simiiformes (simian primates, consisting of monkeys and apes) emerged about 40 mya,[43] possibly also in Asia; if so, they dispersed across the Tethys Sea from Asia to Africa soon afterwards.[52] There are two simian clades, both parvorders: Catarrhini, which developed in Africa, consisting of Old World monkeys, humans and the other apes, and Platyrrhini, which developed in South America, consisting of New World monkeys.[1] A third clade, which included the eosimiids, developed in Asia, but became extinct millions of years ago.[53]

As in the case of lemurs, the origin of New World monkeys is unclear. Molecular studies of concatenated nuclear sequences have yielded a widely varying estimated date of divergence between platyrrhines and catarrhines, ranging from 33 to 70 mya, while studies based on mitochondrial sequences produce a narrower range of 35 to 43 mya.[36][54] The anthropoid primates possibly traversed the Atlantic Ocean from Africa to South America during the Eocene by island hopping, facilitated by Atlantic Ocean ridges and a lowered sea level.[42] Alternatively, a single rafting event may explain this transoceanic colonization. Due to continental drift, the Atlantic Ocean was not nearly as wide at the time as it is today.[42] Research suggests that a small 1 kg (2.2 lb) primate could have survived 13 days on a raft of vegetation.[55] Given estimated current and wind speeds, this would have provided enough time to make the voyage between the continents.

Apes and monkeys spread from Africa into Europe and Asia starting in the Miocene.[56] Soon after, the lorises and tarsiers made the same journey. The first hominin fossils were discovered in northern Africa and date back 5–8 mya.[43] Old World monkeys disappeared from Europe about 1.8 mya.[57] Molecular and fossil studies generally show that modern humans originated in Africa 100,000–200,000 years ago.[58]

Although primates are well studied in comparison to other animal groups, several new species have been discovered recently, and genetic tests have revealed previously unrecognised species in known populations. Primate Taxonomy listed about 350 species of primates in 2001;[10] the author, Colin Groves, increased that number to 376 for his contribution to the third edition of Mammal Species of the World (MSW3).[1] However, publications since the taxonomy in MSW3 was compiled in 2003 have pushed the number to 522 species, or 708 including subspecies.[59]

Hybrids

Primate hybrids usually arise in captivity,[60] but there have also been examples in the wild.[61][62] Hybridization occurs where two species' range overlap to form hybrid zones; hybrids may be created by humans when animals are placed in zoos or due to environmental pressures such as predation.[61] Intergeneric hybridizations, hybrids of different genera, have also been found in the wild. Although they belong to genera that have been distinct for several million years, interbreeding still occurs between the gelada and the hamadryas baboon.[63]

Clones

On 24 January 2018, scientists in China reported in the journal Cell the creation of two crab-eating macaque clones, named Zhong Zhong and Hua Hua, using the complex DNA transfer method that produced Dolly the sheep, for the first time.[64][65][66][67][68]

Anatomy and physiology

Head

 
Primate skulls showing postorbital bar, and increasing brain sizes

The primate skull has a large, domed cranium, which is particularly prominent in anthropoids. The cranium protects the large brain, a distinguishing characteristic of this group.[69] The endocranial volume (the volume within the skull) is three times greater in humans than in the greatest nonhuman primate, reflecting a larger brain size.[70] The mean endocranial volume is 1,201 cubic centimeters in humans, 469 cm3 in gorillas, 400 cm3 in chimpanzees and 397 cm3 in orangutans.[70] The primary evolutionary trend of primates has been the elaboration of the brain, in particular the neocortex (a part of the cerebral cortex), which is involved with sensory perception, generation of motor commands, spatial reasoning, conscious thought and, in humans, language.[71] While other mammals rely heavily on their sense of smell, the arboreal life of primates has led to a tactile, visually dominant sensory system,[71] a reduction in the olfactory region of the brain and increasingly complex social behavior.[72]

Primates have forward-facing eyes on the front of the skull; binocular vision allows accurate distance perception, useful for the brachiating ancestors of all great apes.[69] A bony ridge above the eye sockets reinforces weaker bones in the face, which are put under strain during chewing. Strepsirrhines have a postorbital bar, a bone around the eye socket, to protect their eyes; in contrast, the higher primates, haplorhines, have evolved fully enclosed sockets.[73]

 
An 1893 drawing of the hands and feet of various primates

Primates show an evolutionary trend towards a reduced snout.[74] Technically, Old World monkeys are distinguished from New World monkeys by the structure of the nose, and from apes by the arrangement of their teeth.[72] In New World monkeys, the nostrils face sideways; in Old World monkeys, they face downwards.[72] Dental pattern in primates vary considerably; although some have lost most of their incisors, all retain at least one lower incisor.[72] In most strepsirrhines, the lower incisors form a toothcomb, which is used in grooming and sometimes foraging.[72][75] Old World monkeys have eight premolars, compared with 12 in New World monkeys. The Old World species are divided into apes and monkeys depending on the number of cusps on their molars: monkeys have four, apes have five[72] - although humans may have four or five.[76] The main hominid molar cusp (hypocone) evolved in early primate history, while the cusp of the corresponding primitive lower molar (paraconid) was lost. Prosimians are distinguished by their immobilized upper lips, the moist tip of their noses and forward-facing lower front teeth.

Body

 
Vervet hindfoot showing fingerprint ridges on the sole

Primates generally have five digits on each limb (pentadactyly), with a characteristic type of keratin fingernail on the end of each finger and toe. The bottom sides of the hands and feet have sensitive pads on the fingertips. Most have opposable thumbs, a characteristic primate feature most developed in humans, though not limited to this order (opossums and koalas, for example, also have them).[69] Thumbs allow some species to use tools. In primates, the combination of opposing thumbs, short fingernails (rather than claws) and long, inward-closing fingers is a relict of the ancestral practice of gripping branches, and has, in part, allowed some species to develop brachiation (swinging by the arms from tree limb to tree limb) as a significant means of locomotion. Prosimians have clawlike nails on the second toe of each foot, called toilet-claws, which they use for grooming.[69]

The primate collar bone is a prominent element of the pectoral girdle; this allows the shoulder joint broad mobility.[74] Compared to Old World monkeys, apes have more mobile shoulder joints and arms due to the dorsal position of the scapula, broad ribcages that are flatter front-to-back, a shorter, less mobile spine, and with lower vertebrae greatly reduced - resulting in tail loss in some species. Prehensile tails are found in the New World atelids, including the howler, spider, woolly spider, woolly monkeys; and in capuchins.[77][78] Male primates have a low-hanging penis and testes descended into a scrotum.[79][75]

Sexual dimorphism

 
Distinct sexual size dimorphism can be seen between the male and female mountain gorilla.

Sexual dimorphism is often exhibited in simians, though to a greater degree in Old World species (apes and some monkeys) than New World species. Recent studies involve comparing DNA to examine both the variation in the expression of the dimorphism among primates and the fundamental causes of sexual dimorphism. Primates usually have dimorphism in body mass[80][81] and canine tooth size[82][83] along with pelage and skin color.[84] The dimorphism can be attributed to and affected by different factors, including mating system,[85] size,[85] habitat and diet.[86]

Comparative analyses have generated a more complete understanding of the relationship between sexual selection, natural selection, and mating systems in primates. Studies have shown that dimorphism is the product of changes in both male and female traits.[87] Ontogenetic scaling, where relative extension of a common growth trajectory occurs, may give some insight into the relationship between sexual dimorphism and growth patterns.[88] Some evidence from the fossil record suggests that there was convergent evolution of dimorphism, and some extinct hominids probably had greater dimorphism than any living primate.[87]

Locomotion

 
Diademed sifaka, a lemur that is a vertical clinger and leaper

Primate species move by brachiation, bipedalism, leaping, arboreal and terrestrial quadrupedalism, climbing, knuckle-walking or by a combination of these methods. Several prosimians are primarily vertical clingers and leapers. These include many bushbabies, all indriids (i.e., sifakas, avahis and indris), sportive lemurs, and all tarsiers.[89] Other prosimians are arboreal quadrupeds and climbers. Some are also terrestrial quadrupeds, while some are leapers. Most monkeys are both arboreal and terrestrial quadrupeds and climbers. Gibbons, muriquis and spider monkeys all brachiate extensively,[57] with gibbons sometimes doing so in remarkably acrobatic fashion. Woolly monkeys also brachiate at times.[90] Orangutans use a similar form of locomotion called quadramanous climbing, in which they use their arms and legs to carry their heavy bodies through the trees.[57] Chimpanzees and gorillas knuckle walk,[57] and can move bipedally for short distances. Although numerous species, such as australopithecines and early hominids, have exhibited fully bipedal locomotion, humans are the only extant species with this trait.[91]

Vision

 
The tapetum lucidum of a northern greater galago, typical of prosimians, reflects the light of the photographer's flash.

The evolution of color vision in primates is unique among most eutherian mammals. While the remote vertebrate ancestors of the primates possessed three color vision (trichromaticism), the nocturnal, warm-blooded, mammalian ancestors lost one of three cones in the retina during the Mesozoic era. Fish, reptiles and birds are therefore trichromatic or tetrachromatic, while all mammals, with the exception of some primates and marsupials,[92] are dichromats or monochromats (totally color blind).[75] Nocturnal primates, such as the night monkeys and bush babies, are often monochromatic. Catarrhines are routinely trichromatic due to a gene duplication of the red-green opsin gene at the base of their lineage, 30 to 40 million years ago.[75][93] Platyrrhines, on the other hand, are trichromatic in a few cases only.[94] Specifically, individual females must be heterozygous for two alleles of the opsin gene (red and green) located on the same locus of the X chromosome.[75] Males, therefore, can only be dichromatic, while females can be either dichromatic or trichromatic. Color vision in strepsirrhines is not as well understood; however, research indicates a range of color vision similar to that found in platyrrhines.[75]

Like catarrhines, howler monkeys (a family of platyrrhines) show routine trichromatism that has been traced to an evolutionarily recent gene duplication.[95] Howler monkeys are one of the most specialized leaf-eaters of the New World monkeys; fruits are not a major part of their diets,[90] and the type of leaves they prefer to consume (young, nutritive, and digestible) are detectable only by a red-green signal. Field work exploring the dietary preferences of howler monkeys suggests that routine trichromaticism was selected by environment.[94]

Behavior

Social systems

Richard Wrangham stated that social systems of primates are best classified by the amount of movement by females occurring between groups.[96] He proposed four categories:

  • Female transfer systems – females move away from the group in which they were born. Females of a group will not be closely related whereas males will have remained with their natal groups, and this close association may be influential in social behavior. The groups formed are generally quite small.[96] This organization can be seen in chimpanzees, where the males, who are typically related, will cooperate in defense of the group's territory.[97] Evidence of this social system has also been found among Neanderthal remains in Spain[98] and in remains of Australopithecus and Paranthropus robustus groups in southern Africa.[99][100] Among New World Monkeys, spider monkeys and muriquis use this system.[101]
 
A social huddle of ring-tailed lemurs. The two individuals on the right exposing their white ventral surface are sunning themselves.
  • Male transfer systems – while the females remain in their natal groups, the males will emigrate as adolescents. Polygynous and multi-male societies are classed in this category. Group sizes are usually larger.[96] This system is common among the ring-tailed lemur, capuchin monkeys and cercopithecine monkeys.[57]
  • Monogamous species – a male–female bond, sometimes accompanied by a juvenile offspring. There is shared responsibility of parental care and territorial defense. The offspring leaves the parents' territory during adolescence.[96] Gibbons essentially use this system, although "monogamy" in this context does not necessarily mean absolute sexual fidelity.[102] These species do not live in larger groups.
  • Solitary species – often males who defend territories that include the home ranges of several females.[96] This type of organization is found in the prosimians such as the slow loris.[103] Orangutans do not defend their territory but effectively have this organization.[104]

Other systems are known to occur as well. For example, with howler monkeys and gorillas both the males and females typically transfer from their natal group on reaching sexual maturity, resulting in groups in which neither the males nor females are typically related.[90][105] Some prosimians, colobine monkeys and callitrichid monkeys also use this system.[57]

The transfer of females or males from their native group is likely an adaptation for avoiding inbreeding.[106] An analysis of breeding records of captive primate colonies representing numerous different species indicates that the infant mortality of inbred young is generally higher than that of non-inbred young.[106][107] This effect of inbreeding on infant mortality is probably largely a result of increased expression of deleterious recessive alleles (see Inbreeding depression).

 
Chimpanzees are social great apes.

Primatologist Jane Goodall, who studied in the Gombe Stream National Park, noted fission-fusion societies in chimpanzees.[108] There is fission when the main group splits up to forage during the day, then fusion when the group returns at night to sleep as a group. This social structure can also be observed in the hamadryas baboon,[109] spider monkeys[90] and the bonobo.[109] The gelada has a similar social structure in which many smaller groups come together to form temporary herds of up to 600 monkeys.[109] Humans also form fission-fusion societies. In hunter-gatherer societies, humans form groups which are made up of several individuals that may split up to obtain different resources.[110]

These social systems are affected by three main ecological factors: distribution of resources, group size, and predation.[111] Within a social group there is a balance between cooperation and competition. Cooperative behaviors in many primates species include social grooming (removing skin parasites and cleaning wounds), food sharing, and collective defense against predators or of a territory. Aggressive behaviors often signal competition for food, sleeping sites or mates. Aggression is also used in establishing dominance hierarchies.[111][112]

Interspecific associations

Several species of primates are known to associate in the wild. Some of these associations have been extensively studied. In the Tai Forest of Africa several species coordinate anti-predator behavior. These include the Diana monkey, Campbell's mona monkey, lesser spot-nosed monkey, western red colobus, king colobus (western black and white colobus), and sooty mangabey, which coordinate anti-predator alarm calls.[113] Among the predators of these monkeys is the common chimpanzee.[114]

The red-tailed monkey associates with several species, including the western red colobus, blue monkey, Wolf's mona monkey, mantled guereza, black crested mangabey and Allen's swamp monkey.[109] Several of these species are preyed upon by the common chimpanzee.[115]

In South America, squirrel monkeys associate with capuchin monkeys.[116] This may have more to do with foraging benefits to the squirrel monkeys than anti-predation benefits.[116]

Communication

Lemurs, lorises, tarsiers, and New World monkeys rely on olfactory signals for many aspects of social and reproductive behavior.[71] Specialized glands are used to mark territories with pheromones, which are detected by the vomeronasal organ; this process forms a large part of the communication behavior of these primates.[71] In Old World monkeys and apes this ability is mostly vestigial, having regressed as trichromatic eyes evolved to become the main sensory organ.[117] Primates also use vocalizations, gestures, and facial expressions to convey psychological state.[118][119] Facial musculature is very developed in primates, particularly in monkeys and apes, allowing for complex facial communication. Like humans, chimpanzees can distinguish the faces of familiar and unfamiliar individuals.[120] Hand and arm gestures are also important forms of communication for great apes and a single gesture can have multiple functions.[119]

Primates are a particularly vocal group of mammals.[79] Indris and black-and-white ruffed lemurs make distinctive, loud songs and choruses which maintain territories and act as alarm calls.[121] The Philippine tarsier, has a high-frequency limit of auditory sensitivity of approximately 91 kHz with a dominant frequency of 70 kHz, among the highest recorded for any terrestrial mammal. For Philippine tarsiers, these ultrasonic vocalizations might represent a private channel of communication that subverts detection by predators, prey and competitors, enhances energetic efficiency, or improves detection against low-frequency background noise.[122] Male howler monkeys are among the loudest land mammals as their roars can be heard up to 4.8 km (3.0 mi), and relate to intergroup spacing, territorial protection and possibly mate-guarding.[123][124] Roars are produced by a modified larynx and enlarged hyoid bone which contains an air sac.[125] The vervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of at least four different predators, and the reactions of other monkeys vary according to the call.[126] Male and female siamangs both possess inflatable pouches in the throat with which pair -bonds use to sing "duets" to each other.[127]

Many non-human primates have the vocal anatomy to produce human speech but lack the proper brain wiring.[128] Vowel-like vocal patterns have been recorded in baboons which has implications for the origin of speech in humans.[129] Consonant- and vowel-like sounds exist in some orangutan calls and they maintain their meaning over great distances.[130] The time range for the evolution of human language and/or its anatomical prerequisites extends, at least in principle, from the phylogenetic divergence of Homo (2.3 to 2.4 million years ago) from Pan (5 to 6 million years ago) to the emergence of full behavioral modernity some 50,000–150,000 years ago. Few dispute that Australopithecus probably lacked vocal communication significantly more sophisticated than that of great apes in general.[131]

Life history

 
A crab-eating macaque breastfeeding her baby

Primates have slower rates of development than other mammals.[57] All primate infants are breastfed by their mothers (with the exception of some human cultures and various zoo raised primates which are fed formula) and rely on them for grooming and transportation.[57] In some species, infants are protected and transported by males in the group, particularly males who may be their fathers.[57] Other relatives of the infant, such as siblings and aunts, may participate in its care as well.[57] Most primate mothers cease ovulation while breastfeeding an infant; once the infant is weaned the mother can reproduce again.[57] This often leads to weaning conflict with infants who attempt to continue breastfeeding.[57]

Infanticide is common in polygynous species such as gray langurs and gorillas. Adult males may kill dependent offspring that are not theirs so the female will return to estrus and thus they can sire offspring of their own. Social monogamy in some species may have evolved to combat this behavior.[132] Promiscuity may also lessen the risk of infanticide since paternity becomes uncertain.[133]

Primates have a longer juvenile period between weaning and sexual maturity than other mammals of similar size.[57] Some primates such as galagos and new world monkeys use tree-holes for nesting, and park juveniles in leafy patches while foraging. Other primates follow a strategy of "riding", i.e. carrying individuals on the body while feeding. Adults may construct or use nesting sites, sometimes accompanied by juveniles, for the purpose of resting, a behavior which has developed secondarily in the great apes.[134][135] During the juvenile period, primates are more susceptible than adults to predation and starvation; they gain experience in feeding and avoiding predators during this time.[57] They learn social and fighting skills, often through playing.[57] Primates, especially females, have longer lifespans than other similarly sized mammals,[57] this may be partially due to their slower metabolisms.[136] Late in life, female catarrhine primates appear to undergo a cessation of reproductive function known as menopause; other groups are less studied.[137]

Diet and feeding

 
Leaf eating mantled guereza, a species of black-and-white colobus
 
A mouse lemur holds a cut piece of fruit in its hands and eats.

Primates exploit a variety of food sources. It has been said that many characteristics of modern primates, including humans, derive from an early ancestor's practice of taking most of its food from the tropical canopy.[138] Most primates include fruit in their diets to obtain easily digested nutrients including carbohydrates and lipids for energy.[57] Primates in the suborder Strepsirrhini (non-tarsier prosimians) are able to synthesize vitamin C, like most other mammals, while primates of the suborder Haplorhini (tarsiers, monkeys and apes) have lost this ability, and require the vitamin in their diet.[139]

Many primates have anatomical specializations that enable them to exploit particular foods, such as fruit, leaves, gum or insects.[57] For example, leaf eaters such as howler monkeys, black-and-white colobuses and sportive lemurs have extended digestive tracts which enable them to absorb nutrients from leaves that can be difficult to digest.[57] Marmosets, which are gum eaters, have strong incisor teeth, enabling them to open tree bark to get to the gum, and claws rather than nails, enabling them to cling to trees while feeding.[57] The aye-aye combines rodent-like teeth with a long, thin middle finger to fill the same ecological niche as a woodpecker. It taps on trees to find insect larvae, then gnaws holes in the wood and inserts its elongated middle finger to pull the larvae out.[140] Some species have additional specializations. For example, the grey-cheeked mangabey has thick enamel on its teeth, enabling it to open hard fruits and seeds that other monkeys cannot.[57] The gelada is the only primate species that feeds primarily on grass.[141]

Hunting

 
Humans have traditionally hunted prey for subsistence.

Tarsiers are the only extant obligate carnivorous primates, exclusively eating insects, crustaceans, small vertebrates and snakes (including venomous species).[142] Capuchin monkeys can exploit many different types of plant matter, including fruit, leaves, flowers, buds, nectar and seeds, but also eat insects and other invertebrates, bird eggs, and small vertebrates such as birds, lizards, squirrels and bats.[90]

The common chimpanzee eats an omnivorous frugivorous diet. It prefers fruit above all other food items and even seeks out and eats them when they are not abundant. It also eats leaves and leaf buds, seeds, blossoms, stems, pith, bark and resin. Insects and meat make up a small proportion of their diet, estimated as 2%.[143][144] The meat consumption includes predation on other primate species, such as the western red colobus monkey.[114] The bonobo is an omnivorous frugivore – the majority of its diet is fruit, but it supplements this with leaves, meat from small vertebrates, such as anomalures, flying squirrels and duikers,[145] and invertebrates.[146] In some instances, bonobos have been shown to consume lower-order primates.[147][148]

Until the development of agriculture approximately 10,000 years ago, Homo sapiens employed a hunter-gatherer method as their sole means of food collection. This involved combining stationary food sources (such as fruits, grains, tubers, and mushrooms, insect larvae and aquatic mollusks) with wild game, which must be hunted and killed in order to be consumed.[149] It has been proposed that humans have used fire to prepare and cook food since the time of Homo erectus.[150] Around ten thousand years ago, humans developed agriculture,[151] which substantially altered their diet. This change in diet may also have altered human biology; with the spread of dairy farming providing a new and rich source of food, leading to the evolution of the ability to digest lactose in some adults.[152][153]

As prey

Predators of primates include various species of carnivorans, birds of prey, reptiles, and other primates. Even gorillas have been recorded as prey. Predators of primates have diverse hunting strategies and as such, primates have evolved several different antipredator adaptations including crypsis, alarm calls and mobbing. Several species have separate alarm calls for different predators such as air-borne or ground-dwelling predators. Predation may have shaped group size in primates as species exposed to higher predation pressures appear to live in larger groups.[154]

Intelligence and cognition

Primates have advanced cognitive abilities: some make tools and use them to acquire food and for social displays;[155][156] some can perform tasks requiring cooperation, influence and rank;[157] they are status conscious, manipulative and capable of deception;[158][159] they can recognise kin and conspecifics;[160][161] and they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax and concepts of number and numerical sequence.[162][163][164] Research in primate cognition explores problem solving, memory, social interaction, a theory of mind, and numerical, spatial, and abstract concepts.[165] Comparative studies show a trend towards higher intelligence going from prosimians to New World monkeys to Old World monkeys, and significantly higher average cognitive abilities in the great apes.[166][167] However, there is a great deal of variation in each group (e.g., among New World monkeys, both spider[166] and capuchin monkeys[167] have scored highly by some measures), as well as in the results of different studies.[166][167]

Tool use and manufacture

 
A western lowland gorilla using a stick possibly to gauge the depth of water

In 1960, Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee poking pieces of grass into a termite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth. After he left, Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing. She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws. The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to "fish" or "dip" for termites.[168] There are more limited reports of the closely related bonobo using tools in the wild; it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity.[169] It has been reported that females, both chimpanzee and bonobo, use tools more avidly than males.[170] Orangutans in Borneo scoop catfish out of small ponds. Over two years, anthropologist Anne Russon observed orangutans learning to jab sticks at catfish to scare them out of the ponds and in to their waiting hands.[171] There are few reports of gorillas using tools in the wild. An adult female western lowland gorilla used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her in crossing a pool of water. Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering, and another used a log as a bridge.[172]

The first direct observation of a non-ape primate using a tool in a wild environment occurred in 1988. Primatologist Sue Boinski watched an adult male white-faced capuchin beat a fer-de-lance snake to death with a dead branch.[173] The black-striped capuchin was the first non-ape primate for which routine tool use was documented in the wild; individuals were observed cracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone.[174] In Thailand and Myanmar, crab-eating macaques use stone tools to open nuts, oysters and other bivalves, and various types of sea snails.[175] Chacma baboons use stones as weapons; stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat to when they are threatened. Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side whereupon they tumble down the side of the cliff or fall directly to the canyon floor.[176]

Although they have not been observed to use tools in the wild, lemurs in controlled settings have been shown to be capable of understanding the functional properties of the objects they had been trained to use as tools, performing as well as tool-using haplorhines.[177]

Soon after her initial discovery of tool use, Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs, stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects. This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery. Prior to this, scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools, and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals.[168] Chimpanzees have also been observed making "sponges" out of leaves and moss that suck up water.[178] Sumatran orangutans have been observed making and using tools. They will break off a tree branch that is about 30 cm long, snap off the twigs, fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites.[179][180] In the wild, mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools. Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at Chester Zoo (UK) stripping down a twig, apparently to make it narrower, and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails.[181] Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools.[182]

Ecology

Non-human primates primarily live in the tropical latitudes of Africa, Asia, and the Americas. Species that live outside of the tropics; include the Japanese macaque which lives in the Japanese islands of Honshū and Hokkaido; the Barbary macaque which lives in North Africa and several species of langur which live in China. Primates tend to live in tropical rainforests but are also found in temperate forests, savannas, deserts, mountains and coastal areas.[183] The number of primate species within tropical areas has been shown to be positively correlated to the amount of rainfall and the amount of rain forest area.[184] Accounting for 25% to 40% of the fruit-eating animals (by weight) within tropical rainforests, primates play an important ecological role by dispersing seeds of many tree species.[185]

Primate habitats span a range of altitudes: the black snub-nosed monkey has been found living in the Hengduan Mountains at altitudes of 4,700 meters (15,400 ft),[186] the mountain gorilla can be found at 4,200 meters (13,200 ft) crossing the Virunga Mountains,[187] and the gelada has been found at elevations of up to 5,000 m (16,000 ft) in the Ethiopian Highlands.[188] Some species interact with aquatic environments and may swim or even dive, including the proboscis monkey, De Brazza's monkey and Allen's swamp monkey.[189] Some primates, such as the rhesus macaque and gray langurs, can exploit human-modified environments and even live in cities.[109][190]

Interactions between humans and other primates

Disease transmission

Close interactions between humans and non-human primates (NHPs) can create pathways for the transmission of zoonotic diseases. Viruses such as Herpesviridae (most notably Herpes B Virus), Poxviridae, measles, ebola, rabies, the Marburg virus and viral hepatitis can be transmitted to humans; in some cases the viruses produce potentially fatal diseases in both humans and non-human primates.[191]

Legal and social status

 
Slow lorises are popular in the exotic pet trade, which threatens wild populations.

Only humans are recognized as persons and protected in law by the United Nations Universal Declaration of Human Rights.[b] The legal status of NHPs, on the other hand, is the subject of much debate, with organizations such as the Great Ape Project (GAP) campaigning to award at least some of them legal rights.[193] In June 2008, Spain became the first country in the world to recognize the rights of some NHPs, when its parliament's cross-party environmental committee urged the country to comply with GAP's recommendations, which are that chimpanzees, orangutans and gorillas are not to be used for animal experiments.[194][195]

Many species of NHP are kept as pets by humans, the Allied Effort to Save Other Primates (AESOP) estimates that around 15,000 NHPs live as exotic pets in the United States.[196] The expanding Chinese middle class has increased demand for NHPs as exotic pets in recent years.[197] Although NHP import for the pet trade was banned in the U.S. in 1975, smuggling still occurs along the United States – Mexico border, with prices ranging from US$3000 for monkeys to $30,000 for apes.[198]

Primates are used as model organisms in laboratories and have been used in space missions.[199] They serve as service animals for disabled humans. Capuchin monkeys can be trained to assist quadriplegic humans; their intelligence, memory, and manual dexterity make them ideal helpers.[200]

NHPs are kept in zoos around the globe. Historically, zoos were primarily a form of entertainment, but more recently have shifted their focus towards conservation, education and research. GAP does not insist that all NHPs should be released from zoos, primarily because captive-born primates lack the knowledge and experience to survive in the wild if released.[201]

Role in scientific research

 
Sam, a rhesus macaque, was flown to the edge of space by NASA in the 1959 Little Joe 2 flight of Project Mercury.

Thousands of non-human primates are used around the world in research because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans.[202][203] In particular, the brains and eyes of NHPs more closely parallel human anatomy than those of any other animals. NHPs are commonly used in preclinical trials, neuroscience, ophthalmology studies, and toxicity studies. Rhesus macaques are often used, as are other macaques, African green monkeys, chimpanzees, baboons, squirrel monkeys, and marmosets, both wild-caught and purpose-bred.[202][204]

In 2005, GAP reported that 1,280 of the 3,100 NHPs living in captivity in the United States were used for experiments.[193] In 2004, the European Union used around 10,000 NHPs in such experiments; in 2005 in Great Britain, 4,652 experiments were conducted on 3,115 NHPs.[205] Governments of many nations have strict care requirements of NHPs kept in captivity. In the US, federal guidelines extensively regulate aspects of NHP housing, feeding, enrichment, and breeding.[206] European groups such as the European Coalition to End Animal Experiments are seeking a ban on all NHP use in experiments as part of the European Union's review of animal testing legislation.[207]

Extinction threats

 
Humans are known to hunt other primates for food, so-called bushmeat. Pictured are two men who have killed a number of silky sifaka and white-headed brown lemurs.

The International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN) lists more than a third of primates as critically endangered or vulnerable. About 60% of primate species are threatened with extinction, including: 87% of species in Madagascar, 73% in Asia, 37% in Africa, and 36% in South and Central America.[208] Additionally, 75% of primate species have decreasing populations.[208] Trade is regulated, as all species are listed by CITES in Appendix II, except 50 species and subspecies listed in Appendix I, which gain full protection from trade.[209][210]

Common threats to primate species include deforestation, forest fragmentation, monkey drives (resulting from primate crop raiding),[211] and primate hunting for use in medicines, as pets, and for food. Large-scale tropical forest clearing is widely regarded as the process that most threatens primates.[212][213][214] More than 90% of primate species occur in tropical forests.[213][215] The main cause of forest loss is clearing for agriculture, although commercial logging, subsistence harvesting of timber, mining, and dam construction also contribute to tropical forest destruction.[215] In Indonesia large areas of lowland forest have been cleared to increase palm oil production, and one analysis of satellite imagery concluded that during 1998 and 1999 there was a loss of 1,000 Sumatran orangutans per year in the Leuser Ecosystem alone.[216]

 
The critically endangered silky sifaka

Primates with a large body size (over 5 kg) are at increased extinction risk due to their greater profitability to poachers compared to smaller primates.[215] They reach sexual maturity later and have a longer period between births. Populations therefore recover more slowly after being depleted by poaching or the pet trade.[217] Data for some African cities show that half of all protein consumed in urban areas comes from the bushmeat trade.[218] Endangered primates such as guenons and the drill are hunted at levels that far exceed sustainable levels.[218] This is due to their large body size, ease of transport and profitability per animal.[218] As farming encroaches on forest habitats, primates feed on the crops, causing the farmers large economic losses.[219] Primate crop raiding gives locals a negative impression of primates, hindering conservation efforts.[220]

Madagascar, home to five endemic primate families, has experienced the greatest extinction of the recent past; since human settlement 1,500 years ago, at least eight classes and fifteen of the larger species have become extinct due to hunting and habitat destruction.[71] Among the primates wiped out were Archaeoindris (a lemur larger than a silverback gorilla) and the families Palaeopropithecidae and Archaeolemuridae.[71]

 
The critically endangered Sumatran orangutan

In Asia, Hinduism, Buddhism, and Islam prohibit eating primate meat; however, primates are still hunted for food.[215] Some smaller traditional religions allow the consumption of primate meat.[221][222] The pet trade and traditional medicine also increase demand for illegal hunting.[197][223][224] The rhesus macaque, a model organism, was protected after excessive trapping threatened its numbers in the 1960s; the program was so effective that they are now viewed as a pest throughout their range.[214]

In Central and South America forest fragmentation and hunting are the two main problems for primates. Large tracts of forest are now rare in Central America.[212][225] This increases the amount of forest vulnerable to edge effects such as farmland encroachment, lower levels of humidity and a change in plant life.[226][227] Movement restriction results in a greater amount of inbreeding, which can cause deleterious effects leading to a population bottleneck, whereby a significant percentage of the population is lost.[228][229]

There are 21 critically endangered primates, 7 of which have remained on the IUCN's "The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates" list since the year 2000: the silky sifaka, Delacour's langur, the white-headed langur, the gray-shanked douc, the Tonkin snub-nosed monkey, the Cross River gorilla and the Sumatran orangutan.[230] Miss Waldron's red colobus was recently declared extinct when no trace of the subspecies could be found from 1993 to 1999.[231] A few hunters have found and killed individuals since then, but the subspecies' prospects remain bleak.[232]

See also

Footnotes

  1. ^ a b Although the monophyletic relationship between lemurs and lorisoids is widely accepted, their clade name is not. The term "lemuriform" is used here because it derives from one popular taxonomy that clumps the clade of toothcombed primates into one infraorder and the extinct, non-toothcombed adapiforms into another, both within the suborder Strepsirrhini.[16][17] However, another popular alternative taxonomy places the lorisoids in their own infraorder, Lorisiformes.[15]
  2. ^ Article 6: Everyone has the right to recognition everywhere as a person before the law.[192]

References

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h Groves, C. P. (2005). Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 111–184. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494.
  2. ^ Silcox, Mary T.; Bloch, Jonathan I.; Boyer, Doug M.; Chester, Stephen G. B.; López‐Torres, Sergi (2017). "The evolutionary radiation of plesiadapiforms". Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews. 26 (2): 74–94. doi:10.1002/evan.21526. ISSN 1520-6505. PMID 28429568.
  3. ^ "Primate". Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary. Merriam-Webster. Retrieved 2008-07-21.
  4. ^ The Book of Popular Science. 1963. p. 257.
  5. ^ Chisholm, Hugh, ed. (1911). "Ape" . Encyclopædia Britannica. Vol. 02 (11th ed.). Cambridge University Press. p. 160.
  6. ^ Dixson, A.F. (1981), The Natural History of the Gorilla, London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson, ISBN 978-0-297-77895-0
  7. ^ Definitions of paraphyly vary; for the one used here see e.g. Stace, Clive A. (2010), (PDF), Watsonia, 28: 103–122, archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-26, retrieved 2010-02-07.
  8. ^ Definitions of monophyly vary; for the one used here see e.g. Mishler, Brent D (2009), "Species are not Uniquely Real Biological Entities", in Ayala, F.J. & Arp, R. (eds.), Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Biology, pp. 110–122, doi:10.1002/9781444314922.ch6, ISBN 978-1-4443-1492-2.
  9. ^ a b Cartmill, M.; Smith, F. H. (2011). The Human Lineage. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-118-21145-8.
  10. ^ a b Groves, C. P. (2001). Primate Taxonomy. Smithsonian Institution Press. ISBN 1-56098-872-X.
  11. ^ Benton 2005, p. 371.
  12. ^ Benton 2005, pp. 378–380.
  13. ^ a b Mittermeier, R.; Ganzhorn, J.; Konstant, W.; Glander, K.; Tattersall, I.; Groves, C.; Rylands, A.; Hapke, A.; Ratsimbazafy, J.; Mayor, M.; Louis, E.; Rumpler, Y.; Schwitzer, C.; Rasoloarison, R. (December 2008). "Lemur Diversity in Madagascar" (PDF). International Journal of Primatology. 29 (6): 1607–1656. doi:10.1007/s10764-008-9317-y. S2CID 17614597.
  14. ^ Rylands, A. B. & Mittermeier, R. A. (2009). "The Diversity of the New World Primates (Platyrrhini)". In Garber, P. A.; Estrada, A.; Bicca-Marques, J. C.; Heymann, E. W. & Strier, K. B. (eds.). South American Primates: Comparative Perspectives in the Study of Behavior, Ecology, and Conservation. Springer. ISBN 978-0-387-78704-6.
  15. ^ a b Hartwig 2011, pp. 20–21.
  16. ^ Szalay & Delson 1980, p. 149.
  17. ^ Cartmill 2010, p. 15.
  18. ^ Linnaeus, C. (1758). Sistema naturae per regna tria Naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus differentiis, synonimis locis. Tomus I. Impensis direct. Laurentii Salvii, Holmia. pp. 20–32.
  19. ^ Linnaeus, C. (1735). Sistema naturae sive regna tria Naturae systematice proposita per classes, ordines, genera, & species. apud Theodorum Haak, Lugduni Batavorum. pp. s.p.
  20. ^ Blainville, H. (1839). "Nouvelle classification des Mammifères". Annales Françaises et Etrangères d'Anatomie et de Physiologie Appliquées à la Médicine et à l'Histoire Naturelle, 3. pp. 268–269.
  21. ^ Thorington, R. W. & Anderson, S. (1984). "Primates". In Anderson, S. & Jones, J. K. (eds.). Orders and Families of Recent Mammals of the World. New York: John Wiley and Sons. pp. 187–217. ISBN 978-0-471-08493-8.
  22. ^ McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997). Classification of Mammals: Above the species level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 631. ISBN 0-231-11013-8.
  23. ^ Strier, K. (2007). Primate Behavioral Ecology (Third ed.). pp. 50–53. ISBN 978-0-205-44432-8.
  24. ^ Janečka, J. E.; Miller, W.; Pringle, T. H.; Wiens, F.; Zitzmann, A.; Helgen, K. M.; Springer, M. S.; Murphy, W. J. (2 November 2007). "Molecular and Genomic Data Identify the Closest Living Relative of Primates". Science. 318 (5851): 792–794. Bibcode:2007Sci...318..792J. doi:10.1126/science.1147555. PMID 17975064. S2CID 12251814.
  25. ^ Kavanagh, M. (1983). A Complete Guide to Monkeys, Apes and Other Primates. New York: Viking Press. pp. 18. ISBN 0-670-43543-0.
  26. ^ McKenna, M. C. & Bell, S. K. (1997). Classification of Mammals Above the Species Level. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 329. ISBN 0-231-11012-X.
  27. ^ Williams, B.A.; Kay, R.F.; Kirk, E.C. (2010). "New perspectives on anthropoid origins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (11): 4797–4804. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107. PMC 2841917. PMID 20212104.
  28. ^ Stanyon, Roscoe; Springer, Mark S.; Meredith, Robert W.; Gatesy, John; Emerling, Christopher A.; Park, Jong; Rabosky, Daniel L.; Stadler, Tanja; Steiner, Cynthia; Ryder, Oliver A.; Janečka, Jan E.; Fisher, Colleen A.; Murphy, William J. (2012). "Macroevolutionary Dynamics and Historical Biogeography of Primate Diversification Inferred from a Species Supermatrix". PLOS ONE. 7 (11): e49521. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...749521S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0049521. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3500307. PMID 23166696.
  29. ^ Jameson, Natalie M.; Hou, Zhuo-Cheng; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Weckle, Amy; Goodman, Morris; Steiper, Michael E.; Wildman, Derek E. (September 2011). "Genomic data reject the hypothesis of a prosimian primate clade". Journal of Human Evolution. 61 (3): 295–305. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2011.04.004. ISSN 0047-2484. PMID 21620437.
  30. ^ Pozzi, Luca; Hodgson, Jason A.; Burrell, Andrew S.; Sterner, Kirstin N.; Raaum, Ryan L.; Disotell, Todd R. (June 2014). "Primate phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes". Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 75: 165–183. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2014.02.023. ISSN 1055-7903. PMC 4059600. PMID 24583291.
  31. ^ Stanyon, Roscoe; Finstermeier, Knut; Zinner, Dietmar; Brameier, Markus; Meyer, Matthias; Kreuz, Eva; Hofreiter, Michael; Roos, Christian (16 July 2013). "A Mitogenomic Phylogeny of Living Primates". PLOS ONE. 8 (7): e69504. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...869504F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0069504. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3713065. PMID 23874967.
  32. ^ a b O'Leary, M. A.; et al. (8 February 2013). "The placental mammal ancestor and the post–K-Pg radiation of placentals". Science. 339 (6120): 662–667. Bibcode:2013Sci...339..662O. doi:10.1126/science.1229237. hdl:11336/7302. PMID 23393258. S2CID 206544776.
  33. ^ Wilson Mantilla, Gregory P.; Chester, Stephen G. B.; Clemens, William A.; Moore, Jason R.; Sprain, Courtney J.; Hovatter, Brody T.; Mitchell, William S.; Mans, Wade W.; Mundil, Roland; Renne, Paul R. (2021). "Earliest Palaeocene purgatoriids and the initial radiation of stem primates". Royal Society Open Science. 8 (2): 210050. Bibcode:2021RSOS....810050W. doi:10.1098/rsos.210050. PMC 8074693. PMID 33972886.
  34. ^ Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Kirk, E. C. (2010). "New perspectives on anthropoid origins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (11): 4797–4804. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.4797W. doi:10.1073/pnas.0908320107. PMC 2841917. PMID 20212104.
  35. ^ Miller, E. R.; Gunnell, G. F.; Martin, R. D. (2005). "Deep Time and the Search for Anthropoid Origins" (PDF). American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 128: 60–95. doi:10.1002/ajpa.20352. PMID 16369958.
  36. ^ a b Chatterjee, Helen J; Ho, Simon Y.W.; Barnes, Ian; Groves, Colin (27 October 2009). "Estimating the phylogeny and divergence times of primates using a supermatrix approach". BMC Evolutionary Biology. 9 (1): 259. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-9-259. PMC 2774700. PMID 19860891.
  37. ^ Lee, M. (September 1999). "Molecular Clock Calibrations and Metazoan Divergence Dates". Journal of Molecular Evolution. 49 (3): 385–391. Bibcode:1999JMolE..49..385L. doi:10.1007/PL00006562. PMID 10473780. S2CID 1629316.
  38. ^ "Scientists Push Back Primate Origins From 65 Million To 85 Million Years Ago". Science Daily. Retrieved 2008-10-24.
  39. ^ Tavaré, S.; Marshall, C. R.; Will, O.; Soligo, C.; Martin R.D. (April 18, 2002). "Using the fossil record to estimate the age of the last common ancestor of extant primates". Nature. 416 (6882): 726–729. Bibcode:2002Natur.416..726T. doi:10.1038/416726a. PMID 11961552. S2CID 4368374.
  40. ^ Klonisch, T.; Froehlich, C.; Tetens, F.; Fischer, B.; Hombach-Klonisch, S. (2001). "Molecular Remodeling of Members of the Relaxin Family During Primate Evolution". Molecular Biology and Evolution. 18 (3): 393–403. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a003815. PMID 11230540.
  41. ^ a b c d e f Horvath, J.; et al. (2008). "Development and Application of a Phylogenomic Toolkit: Resolving the Evolutionary History of Madagascar's Lemurs". Genome Research. 18 (3): 489–499. doi:10.1101/gr.7265208. PMC 2259113. PMID 18245770.
  42. ^ a b c d Sellers, Bill (2000-10-20). (PDF). University of Edinburgh. pp. 13–17. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-10-29. Retrieved 2008-10-23.
  43. ^ a b c d Hartwig, W. (2007). "Primate Evolution". In Campbell, C.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. pp. 13–17. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  44. ^ Williams, B. A.; Kay, R. F.; Christopher Kirk, E.; Ross, C. F. (2010). (PDF). Journal of Human Evolution. 59 (5): 567–573, discussion 573–9. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.01.003. PMID 20188396. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2013-05-17.
  45. ^ Ciochon, R. & Fleagle, J. (1987). Primate Evolution and Human Origins. Menlo Park, California: Benjamin/Cummings. p. 72. ISBN 978-0-202-01175-2.
  46. ^ a b Garbutt, N. (2007). Mammals of Madagascar, A Complete Guide. A&C Black Publishers. pp. 85–86. ISBN 978-0-300-12550-4.
  47. ^ a b Mittermeier, R.A.; et al. (2006). Lemurs of Madagascar (2nd ed.). Conservation International. pp. 23–26. ISBN 1-881173-88-7.
  48. ^ Shekelle, M. (2005). . Archived from the original on 2008-09-07. Retrieved 2008-08-22.
  49. ^ Schmidt, T.; et al. (3 May 2005). "Rapid electrostatic evolution at the binding site for cytochrome c on cytochrome c oxidase in anthropoid primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (18): 6379–6384. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.6379S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0409714102. PMC 1088365. PMID 15851671.
  50. ^ Wade, Lizzie (June 5, 2013). . ScienceNow. Archived from the original on 2013-06-08. Retrieved 2013-06-07.
  51. ^ Kay, R. F. (2012). "Evidence for an Asian origin of stem anthropoid s". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (26): 10132–10133. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910132K. doi:10.1073/pnas.1207933109. PMC 3387095. PMID 22699505.
  52. ^ Chaimanee, Y.; Chavasseau, O.; Beard, K. C.; Kyaw, A. A.; Soe, A. N.; Sein, C.; Lazzari, V.; Marivaux, L.; Marandat, B.; Swe, M.; Rugbumrung, M.; Lwin, T.; Valentin, X.; Zin-Maung-Maung-Thein; Jaeger, J. -J. (2012). "Late Middle Eocene primate from Myanmar and the initial anthropoid colonization of Africa". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (26): 10293–10297. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10910293C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1200644109. PMC 3387043. PMID 22665790.
  53. ^ Marivaux, L.; et al. (2005-06-14). "Anthropoid primates from the Oligocene of Pakistan (Bugti Hills): Data on early anthropoid evolution and biogeography". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 102 (24): 8436–8441. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8436M. doi:10.1073/pnas.0503469102. PMC 1150860. PMID 15937103.
  54. ^ Schrago, C.G. & Russo, C.A.M. (2003). "Timing the Origin of New World Monkeys" (PDF Reprint). Molecular Biology and Evolution. 20 (10): 1620–1625. doi:10.1093/molbev/msg172. PMID 12832653.
  55. ^ Houle, A. (1999). "The origin of platyrrhines: An evaluation of the Antarctic scenario and the floating island model". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 109 (4): 541–559. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199908)109:4<541::AID-AJPA9>3.0.CO;2-N. PMID 10423268.
  56. ^ Andrews, P. & Kelley, J. (2007). "Middle Miocene Dispersals of Apes". Folia Primatologica. 78 (5–6): 328–343. doi:10.1159/000105148. PMID 17855786. S2CID 19293586.
  57. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u Strier, K. (2007). Primate Behavioral Ecology (3rd ed.). Allyn & Bacon. pp. 7, 64, 71, 77, 182–185, 273–280, 284, 287–298. ISBN 978-0-205-44432-8.
  58. ^ Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Evolution and the Emergence of Humans". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. p. 650. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  59. ^ IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (1 March 2021). "Primate diversity by region". International Union for the Conservation of Nature.
  60. ^ Tenaza, R. (1984). "Songs of hybrid gibbons (Hylobates lar × H. muelleri)". American Journal of Primatology. 8 (3): 249–253. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350080307. PMID 31986810. S2CID 84957700.
  61. ^ a b Bernsteil, I. S. (1966). "Naturally occurring primate hybrid". Science. 154 (3756): 1559–1560. Bibcode:1966Sci...154.1559B. doi:10.1126/science.154.3756.1559. PMID 4958933. S2CID 85898043.
  62. ^ Sugawara, K. (January 1979). "Sociological study of a wild group of hybrid baboons between Papio anubis and P. hamadryas in the Awash Valley, Ethiopia". Primates. 20 (1): 21–56. doi:10.1007/BF02373827. S2CID 23061688.
  63. ^ Jolly, C. J.; Woolley-Barker, Tamsin; et al. (1997). "Intergeneric Hybrid Baboons". International Journal of Primatology. 18 (4): 597–627. doi:10.1023/A:1026367307470. S2CID 27900830.
  64. ^ Liu, Zhen; et al. (24 January 2018). "Cloning of Macaque Monkeys by Somatic Cell Nuclear Transfer". Cell. 172 (4): 881–887.e7. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.01.020. PMID 29395327.
  65. ^ Normile, Dennis (24 January 2018). "These monkey twins are the first primate clones made by the method that developed Dolly". Science. doi:10.1126/science.aat1066. Retrieved 24 January 2018.
  66. ^ Cyranoski, David (24 January 2018). "First monkeys cloned with technique that made Dolly the sheep - Chinese scientists create cloned primates that could revolutionize studies of human disease". Nature. 553 (7689): 387–388. Bibcode:2018Natur.553..387C. doi:10.1038/d41586-018-01027-z. PMID 29368720.
  67. ^ Briggs, Helen (24 January 2018). "First monkey clones created in Chinese laboratory". BBC News. Retrieved 24 January 2018.
  68. ^ "Scientists Successfully Clone Monkeys; Are Humans Up Next?". The New York Times. Associated Press. 24 January 2018. Retrieved 24 January 2018.
  69. ^ a b c d Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Characteristics of Primates". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. p. 630. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  70. ^ a b Aiello, L. & Dean, C. (1990). An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy. Academic Press. p. 193. ISBN 0-12-045590-0.
  71. ^ a b c d e f "Primate". Encyclopædia Britannica Online. Encyclopædia Britannica, Inc. 2008. Retrieved 2008-07-21.
  72. ^ a b c d e f Myers, P. (1999). ""Primates" (On-line)". Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved 2008-06-03.
  73. ^ Campbell, B. G. & Loy, J. D. (2000). Humankind Emerging (8th ed.). Allyn & Bacon. p. 85. ISBN 0-673-52364-0.
  74. ^ a b White, T. & Kazlev, A. (2006-01-08). . Palaeos. Archived from the original on 2008-05-12. Retrieved 2008-06-03.
  75. ^ a b c d e f Macdonald, David (2006). "Primates". The Encyclopedia of Mammals. The Brown Reference Group plc. pp. 282–307. ISBN 0-681-45659-0.
  76. ^ Ash, M. M.; Nelson, S. J.; Wheeler, R. C. (2003). Wheeler's Dental Anatomy, Physiology, and Occlusion. W.B. Saunders. p. 12. ISBN 978-0-7216-9382-8.
  77. ^ Garber PA, Rehg JA (November 1999). "The ecological role of the prehensile tail in white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus)". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 110 (3): 325–39. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-8644(199911)110:3<325::AID-AJPA5>3.0.CO;2-D. PMID 10516564.
  78. ^ Russo GA, Young JW (November 2011). "Tail growth tracks the ontogeny of prehensile tail use in capuchin monkeys (Cebus albifrons and C. apella)". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 146 (3): 465–73. doi:10.1002/ajpa.21617. PMID 21953012.
  79. ^ a b Friderun Ankel-Simons (27 July 2010). Primate Anatomy: An Introduction. Academic Press. pp. 442, 521. ISBN 978-0-08-046911-9.
  80. ^ Ralls, K. (1976). "Mammals in Which Females are Larger Than Males". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 51 (2): 245–76. doi:10.1086/409310. PMID 785524. S2CID 25927323.
  81. ^ Lindstedtand & Boyce; Boyce, Mark S. (July 1985). "Seasonality, Fasting Endurance, and Body Size in Mammals". The American Naturalist. 125 (6): 873. doi:10.1086/284385. S2CID 84308684.
  82. ^ Frisch, J. E. (1963). "Sex-differences in the canines of the gibbon (Hylobates lar)". Primates. 4 (2): 1–10. doi:10.1007/BF01659148. S2CID 189798134.
  83. ^ Kay, R. F. (1975). "The functional adaptations of primate molar teeth". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 43 (2): 195–215. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330430207. PMID 810034.
  84. ^ Crook, J. H. (1972). "Sexual selection, dimorphism, and social organization in the primates". In Campbell, B. G. (ed.). Sexual selection and the descent of man. Aldine Transaction. pp. 246. ISBN 978-0-202-02005-1.
  85. ^ a b Cheverud, J. M.; Dow, M. M.; Leutenegger, W. (November 1985). "The quantitative assessment of phylogenetic constraints in comparative analyses: Sexual dimorphism in body weight among primates". Evolution. 39 (6): 1335–1351. doi:10.2307/2408790. JSTOR 2408790. PMID 28564267.
  86. ^ Leutenegger, W.; Cheverud, J. M. (1982). "Correlates of sexual dimorphism in primates: Ecological and size variables". International Journal of Primatology. 3 (4): 387–402. doi:10.1007/BF02693740. S2CID 38220186.
  87. ^ a b Plavcan, J. M. (2001). "Sexual dimorphism in primate evolution". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 33: 25–53. doi:10.1002/ajpa.10011. PMID 11786990. S2CID 31722173.
  88. ^ O'Higgins, P.; Collard, M. (2002). "Sexual dimorphism and facial growth in papionine monkeys". Journal of Zoology. 257 (2): 255–72. doi:10.1017/S0952836902000857.
  89. ^ Sussman, R. W. (1999). Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 1: Lorises, Lemurs and Tarsiers. Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 78, 89–90, 108, 121–123, 233. ISBN 0-536-02256-9.
  90. ^ a b c d e Sussman, R. W. (2003). Primate Ecology and Social Structure, Volume 2: New World Monkeys (Revised First ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Pearson Custom Publishing & Prentice Hall. pp. 77–80, 132–133, 141–143. ISBN 0-536-74364-9.
  91. ^ Glazier, S. D.; Flowerday, C. A. (2003). Selected Readings in the Anthropology of Religion: Theoretical and Methodological Essays. Greenwood Publishing Group. p. 53. ISBN 9780313300905.
  92. ^ Arrese, C. A.; Oddy, Alison Y.; et al. (2005). "Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials, the quokka (Setonix brachyurus) and quenda (Isoodon obesulus)". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 272 (1565): 791–6. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.3009. PMC 1599861. PMID 15888411.
  93. ^ Bowmaker, J. K.; Astell, S.; Hunt, D. M.; Mollon, J. D. (1991). "Photosensitive and photostable pigments in the retinae of Old World monkeys" (PDF). The Journal of Experimental Biology. 156 (1): 1–19. doi:10.1242/jeb.156.1.1. ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 2051127. Retrieved 2008-06-16.
  94. ^ a b Surridge, A. K. & D. Osorio (2003). "Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 18 (4): 198–205. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00012-0.
  95. ^ Lucas, P. W.; Dominy, N. J.; Riba-Hernandez, P.; Stoner, K. E.; Yamashita, N.; Loría-Calderón, E.; Petersen-Pereira, W.; Rojas-Durán, Y.; Salas-Pena, R.; Solis-Madrigal, S.; Osorio, D.; Darvell, B. W. (2003). "Evolution and function of routine trichromatic vision in primates". Evolution. 57 (11): 2636–43. doi:10.1554/03-168. PMID 14686538. S2CID 739130.
  96. ^ a b c d e Wrangham, R. W. (1982). "Mutualism, kinship and social evolution". Current Problems in Sociobiology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 269–89. ISBN 0-521-24203-7.
  97. ^ Goldberg, T. L.; Wrangham, R. W. (September 1997). "Genetic correlates of social behavior in wild chimpanzees: evidence from mitochondrial DNA". Animal Behaviour. 54 (3): 559–70. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0450. PMID 9299041. S2CID 18223362.
  98. ^ Bowdler, Neil (21 December 2010). "Neanderthal family found cannibalised in cave in Spain". BBC News.
  99. ^ Bowdler, Neil (2 June 2011). "Ancient cave women 'left childhood homes'". BBC News. Retrieved 2 June 2011.
  100. ^ Copeland, Sandi R.; et al. (1 June 2011). "Strontium isotope evidence for landscape use by early hominins". Nature. 474 (7349): 76–78. doi:10.1038/nature10149. PMID 21637256. S2CID 205225222.
  101. ^ Fiore, A. D. & Campbell, C. J. (2007). "The Atelines". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 175. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  102. ^ Bartlett, T. Q. (2007). "The Hylobatidae". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M.; Bearder, S. K. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 283. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  103. ^ Wiens, Frank (2002). (PDF) (Ph.D. thesis). Bayreuth University. pp. 31–32. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 March 2012.
  104. ^ Knott, C. D. & Kahlenberg, S. M. (2007). "Orangutans in Perspective". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 294. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  105. ^ Watts D. P. (1996). "Comparative socio-ecology of gorillas". In McGrew W. C.; Marchant L. F.; Nishida, T. (eds.). Great Ape Societies. Cambridge (England: Cambridge Univ Press. pp. 16–28. ISBN 978-0521555364.
  106. ^ a b Charpentier MJ, Widdig A, Alberts SC (December 2007). "Inbreeding depression in non-human primates: a historical review of methods used and empirical data". American Journal of Primatology. 69 (12): 1370–86. doi:10.1002/ajp.20445. PMID 17486606. S2CID 46626761.
  107. ^ Ralls K, Ballou J (1982). "Effect of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates" (PDF). International Journal of Primatology. 3 (4): 491–505. doi:10.1007/BF02693747. S2CID 10954608.
  108. ^ Constable, J. L.; Ashley, M. V.; Goodall, J.; Pusey, A. E. (May 2001). "Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees". Molecular Ecology. 10 (5): 1279–300. doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2001.01262.x. PMID 11380884. S2CID 46604532.
  109. ^ a b c d e Rowe, N. (1996). The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates. Pogonias Press. pp. 4, 139, 143, 15 185, 223. ISBN 0-9648825-0-7.
  110. ^ Couzin, Iain D.; Laidre, Mark E. (August 2009). "Fission–fusion populations". Current Biology. 19 (15): R633–R635. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.05.034. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 19674541. S2CID 13549970.
  111. ^ a b Pough, F. W.; Janis, C. M.; Heiser, J. B. (2005) [1979]. "Primate Societies". Vertebrate Life (7th ed.). Pearson. pp. 621–623. ISBN 0-13-127836-3.
  112. ^ Smuts, B.B., Cheney, D.L. Seyfarth, R.M., Wrangham, R.W., & Struhsaker, T.T. (Eds.) (1987). Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press for articles on the structure and function of various primate societies.
  113. ^ Shultz, S. & Thomsett, S. (2007). "Interactions between African Crowned Eagles and Their Prey Community". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K. & Noe, R. (eds.). Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. p. 181. ISBN 978-0-521-81633-5.
  114. ^ a b Bshary, R. (2007). "Interactions between Red Colobus Monkeys and Chimpanzees". In McGraw, W.; Zuberbuhler, K.; Noe, R. (eds.). Monkeys of Tai Forest, An African Primate Community. Cambridge University Press. pp. 155–170. ISBN 978-0-521-81633-5.
  115. ^ Stanford, C. (1998). Chimpanzee and Red Colobus : the ecology of predator and prey. Harvard University Press. pp. 130–138, 233. ISBN 0-674-00722-0.
  116. ^ a b Boinski, S. (2000). "Social Manipulation Within and Between Troops Mediates Primate Group Movement". In Boinski, S.; Garber, P. (eds.). On the Move : how and why animals travel in groups. University of Chicago Press. pp. 447–448. ISBN 0-226-06340-2.
  117. ^ Liman, E. R.; Innan, H. (2003). "Relaxed selective pressure on an essential component of pheromone transduction in primate evolution" (PDF). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 100 (6): 3328–3332. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.3328L. doi:10.1073/pnas.0636123100. PMC 152292. PMID 12631698. Retrieved 2008-07-23.
  118. ^ Egnor, R.; Miller, C.; Hauser, M.D. (2004). (PDF). Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (2nd ed.). Elsevier. ISBN 0-08-044299-4. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-09-10.
  119. ^ a b Pollick, A. S.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2007). "Ape gestures and language evolution". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (19): 8184–8189. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.8184P. doi:10.1073/pnas.0702624104. PMC 1876592. PMID 17470779.
  120. ^ Burrows, A. M. (2008). "The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance". BioEssays. 30 (3): 212–225. doi:10.1002/bies.20719. PMID 18293360. S2CID 205478149.
  121. ^ Geissmann, Thomas & Mutschler, Thomas (2006). "Diurnal Distribution of Loud Calls in Sympatric Wild Indris (Indri indri) and Ruffed Lemurs (Varecia variegata): Implications for Call Functions" (PDF). Primates; Journal of Primatology. 47 (4): 393–6. doi:10.1007/s10329-006-0189-5. PMID 16736264. S2CID 1586657.
  122. ^ Ramsier, M.A.; Cunningham, A.J.; Moritz, G.L.; Finneran, J.J.; Williams, C.V.; Ong, P.S.; Gursky-Doyen, S.L.; Dominy, N.J. (2012). "Primate communication in the pure ultrasound". Biology Letters. 8 (4): 508–511. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2011.1149. PMC 3391437. PMID 22319094.
  123. ^ da Cunha, R. G. T.; Byrne, R. (2006). "Roars of Black Howler Monkeys (Alouatta caraya): Evidence for a Function in Inter-Group Spacing". Behaviour. 143 (10): 1169–1199. doi:10.1163/156853906778691568. JSTOR 4536401.
  124. ^ "Black howler monkey". Smithsonian's National Zoo & Conservation Biology Institute. 25 April 2016. Retrieved 2016-07-10.
  125. ^ Kelemen, G.; Sade, J. (1960). "The vocal organ of the Howling monkey (Alouatta palliata)". Journal of Morphology. 107 (2): 123–140. doi:10.1002/jmor.1051070202. PMID 13752246. S2CID 30539292.
  126. ^ Seyfarth, R. M.; Cheney, D. L.; Marler, Peter (1980). "Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls: Semantic communication in a Free-Ranging Primate". Animal Behaviour. 28 (4): 1070–1094. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80097-2. S2CID 53165940.
  127. ^ Haimoff, E. H. (1983). "Brief report: Occurrence of anti-resonance in the song of the siamang (Hylobates syndactylus)". American Journal of Primatology. 5 (3): 249–256. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350050309. PMID 31986856. S2CID 85262432.
  128. ^ Fitch, W. T.; de Boer, B.; Mathur, N.; Ghazanfar, A. A. (2016). "Monkey vocal tracts are speech-ready". Science Advances. 2 (12): e1600723. Bibcode:2016SciA....2E0723F. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1600723. PMC 5148209. PMID 27957536.
  129. ^ Boë L.-J.; Berthommier, F.; Legou, T.; Captier, G.; Kemp, C.; Sawallis, T. R.; et al. (2017). "Evidence of a Vocalic Proto-System in the Baboon (Papio papio) Suggests Pre-Hominin Speech Precursors". PLOS ONE. 12 (1): e0169321. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1269321B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0169321. PMC 5226677. PMID 28076426.
  130. ^ Lameira, A. R.; et al. (2021). "Orangutan information broadcast via consonant-like and vowel-like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution". Biology Letters. 17 (9). doi:10.1098/rsbl.2021.0302. PMC 8478518. PMID 34582737.
  131. ^ Arcadi, AC. (Aug 2000). "Vocal responsiveness in male wild chimpanzees: implications for the evolution of language". J Hum Evol. 39 (2): 205–23. doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0415. PMID 10968929. S2CID 7403772.
  132. ^ Opie, Christopher; Atkinson, Quentin D.; Dunbarc, Robin I. M.; Shultz, Susanne (2013). "Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 110 (33): 13328–13332. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11013328O. doi:10.1073/pnas.1307903110. PMC 3746880. PMID 23898180.
  133. ^ De Ruiter, Jan R.; Van Hooff, Jan A. R. A. M. & Scheffrahn, Wolfgang (1994). "Social and genetic aspects of paternity in wild long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis)". Behaviour. 129 (3–4): 203–24. doi:10.1163/156853994x00613. JSTOR 4535195.
  134. ^ Kappeler, Peter M. (1998). "Nests, Tree Holes, and the Evolution of Primate Life Histories". American Journal of Primatology. 46 (1): 7–33. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1998)46:1<7::AID-AJP3>3.0.CO;2-#. PMID 9730211. S2CID 196589387.
  135. ^ Ross, Caroline (1991). "Park or ride? Evolution of infant carrying in primates". International Journal of Primatology. Kluwer Academic Publishing. 22 (5): 749–771. doi:10.1023/A:1012065332758. S2CID 25301078.
  136. ^ Mintz, Zoe (14 January 2014). "Humans And Primates Burn 50 Percent Fewer Calories Each Day Than Other Mammals". www.ibtimes.com. IBT Media Inc. Retrieved 2014-01-14.
  137. ^ Walker ML, Herndon JG; Herndon (2008). "Menopause in nonhuman primates?". Biology of Reproduction. 79 (3): 398–406. doi:10.1095/biolreprod.108.068536. PMC 2553520. PMID 18495681.
  138. ^ Milton, K. (1993). "Diet and Primate Evolution" (PDF). Scientific American. Vol. 269, no. 2. pp. 86–93. Bibcode:1993SciAm.269b..86M. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0893-86. PMID 8351513.
  139. ^ Pollock, J. I.; Mullin, R. J. (1986). "Vitamin C biosynthesis in prosimians: Evidence for the anthropoid affinity of Tarsius". American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 73 (1): 65–70. doi:10.1002/ajpa.1330730106. PMID 3113259. Archived from the original on 2012-06-28.
  140. ^ Milliken, G. W.; Ward, J. P.; Erickson, C. J. (1991). "Independent digit control in foraging by the aye-aye (Daubentonia madagascariensis)". Folia Primatologica. 56 (4): 219–224. doi:10.1159/000156551. PMID 1937286.
  141. ^ Hiller, C. (2000). "Theropithecus gelada". Animal Diversity Web. Retrieved 2008-08-08.
  142. ^ Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. (2003). "Introduction". In Wright, P.; Simmons, E.; Gursky, S. (eds.). Tarsiers Past, Present and Future. Rutgers University Press. p. 1. ISBN 0-8135-3236-1.
  143. ^ Goodall, Jane (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. ISBN 0-674-11649-6.
  144. ^ Guernsey, Paul. . All About Wildlife. Archived from the original on 2019-11-18. Retrieved 2013-04-22.
  145. ^ Ihobe H (1992). "Observations on the meat-eating behavior of wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba, Republic of Zaire". Primates. 33 (2): 247–250. doi:10.1007/BF02382754. S2CID 10063791.
  146. ^ Rafert, J.; Vineberg, E.O. (1997). (PDF). Bonobo Husbandry Manual. American Association of Zoos and Aquariums. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-04-25.
  147. ^ Surbeck, M; Fowler, A; Deimel, C; Hohmann, G (2008). "Evidence for the consumption of arboreal, diurnal primates by bonobos (Pan paniscus)". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (2): 171–4. doi:10.1002/ajp.20634. PMID 19058132. S2CID 32622605.
  148. ^ Surbeck M, Hohmann G; Hohmann (14 October 2008). "Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale, Salonga National Park". Current Biology. 18 (19): R906–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.040. PMID 18957233. S2CID 6708310.
  149. ^ Cordain L, Eaton SB, Sebastian A, et al. (February 2005). "Origins and evolution of the Western diet: health implications for the 21st century". Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 81 (2): 341–54. doi:10.1093/ajcn.81.2.341. PMID 15699220.
  150. ^ Ulijaszek SJ (November 2002). "Human eating behaviour in an evolutionary ecological context". Proc Nutr Soc. 61 (4): 517–26. doi:10.1079/PNS2002180. PMID 12691181.
  151. ^ Earliest agriculture in the Americas 3 June 2010 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of barley 16 February 2007 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of figs 2 June 2006 at the Wayback Machine, retrieved 19 February 2007
  152. ^ Krebs JR (September 2009). "The gourmet ape: evolution and human food preferences". Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 90 (3): 707S–11S. doi:10.3945/ajcn.2009.27462B. PMID 19656837.
  153. ^ Holden C, Mace R (October 1997). "Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults". Hum. Biol. 69 (5): 605–28. PMID 9299882.
  154. ^ Fichtel, Claudia (2012). "Predation". In Mitani, John C.; Call, Josep; Kappeler, Peter M.; Palombit, Ryne A.; Silk, Joan B. (eds.). The Evolution of Primate Societies. University of Chicago Press. pp. 169–84. ISBN 978-0-226-53172-4.
  155. ^ Boesch, C.; Boesch, H. (1990). "Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees". Folia Primatologica. 54 (1–2): 86–99. doi:10.1159/000156428. PMID 2157651.
  156. ^ Westergaard, G. C.; Lundquist, A. L.; et al. (1998). "Why some capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use probing tools (and others do not)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 207–211. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.207. PMID 9642788.
  157. ^ de Waal, F. B. M.; Davis, J. M. (2003). "Capuchin cognitive ecology: cooperation based on projected returns". Neuropsychologia. 41 (2): 221–228. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(02)00152-5. PMID 12459220. S2CID 8190458.
  158. ^ Paar, L. A.; Winslow, J. T.; Hopkins, W. D.; de Waal, F. B. M. (2000). "Recognizing facial cues: Individual discrimination by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 114 (1): 47–60. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.47. PMC 2018744. PMID 10739311.
  159. ^ Byrne, Richard; Corp, Nadia (2004). "Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 271 (1549): 1693–1699. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2780. PMC 1691785. PMID 15306289.
  160. ^ Paar, L. A.; de Waal, F. B. M. (1999). "Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees". Nature. 399 (6737): 647–648. Bibcode:1999Natur.399..647P. doi:10.1038/21345. PMID 10385114. S2CID 4424086.
  161. ^ Fujita, K.; Watanabe, K.; Widarto, T. H.; Suryobroto, B. (1997). "Discrimination of macaques by macaques: The case of sulawesi species". Primates. 38 (3): 233–245. doi:10.1007/BF02381612. S2CID 21042762.
  162. ^ Call, J. (2001). "Object permanence in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and children (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 115 (2): 159–171. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.2.159. PMID 11459163.
  163. ^ Itakura, S.; Tanaka, M. (June 1998). "Use of experimenter-given cues during object-choice tasks by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus), and human infants (Homo sapiens)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 112 (2): 119–126. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.2.119. PMID 9642782.
  164. ^ Gouteux, S.; Thinus-Blanc, C.; Vauclair, J. (2001). "Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 130 (3): 505–519. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.505. PMID 11561924.
  165. ^ Tomasello, M. & Call, J. (1997). Primate Cognition. Oxford University Press US. ISBN 978-0-19-510624-4.
  166. ^ a b c Deaner, R. O.; van Schaik, C. P.; Johnson, V. E. (2006). "Do some taxa have better domain-general cognition than others? A metaanalysis of nonhuman primate studies". Evolutionary Psychology. 4: 149–196. doi:10.1177/147470490600400114. S2CID 16702785.
  167. ^ a b c Reader, S. M.; Hager, Y.; Laland, K. N. (2011). (PDF). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 366 (1567): 1017–1027. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0342. PMC 3049098. PMID 21357224. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-10-03. Retrieved 2011-07-04.
  168. ^ a b "Toolmaking". The Jane Goodall Institute. Retrieved 2013-08-01.
  169. ^ . ApeTag. 2010. Archived from the original on 2013-11-02. Retrieved 2013-08-03.
  170. ^ Gruber, T.; Clay, Z.; Zuberbühler, K. (2010). "A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use: evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 80 (6): 1023–1033. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.09.005. S2CID 14923158.
  171. ^ Bower, B. (18 April 2011). "Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish". Wired. Retrieved 2013-08-05.
  172. ^ Breuer, T.; Ndoundou-Hockemba, M.; Fishlock, V. (2005). "First observation of tool use in wild gorillas". PLOS Biology. 3 (11): e380. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030380. PMC 1236726. PMID 16187795.
  173. ^ Boinski, S. (1988). "Use of a club by a wild white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus) to attack a venomous snake (Bothrops asper)". American Journal of Primatology. 14 (2): 177–179. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350140208. PMID 31973450. S2CID 84653622.
  174. ^ Fragaszy, D.; Izar, P.; Visalberghi, E.; Ottoni, E.B.; de Oliveira, M.G. (2004). "Wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) use anvils and stone pounding tools". American Journal of Primatology. 64 (4): 359–366. doi:10.1002/ajp.20085. PMID 15580579. S2CID 16222308.
  175. ^ Gumert, M.D.; Kluck, M.; Malaivijitnond, S. (2009). "The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand". American Journal of Primatology. 71 (7): 594–608. doi:10.1002/ajp.20694. PMID 19405083. S2CID 22384150.
  176. ^ Hamilton, W.J.; Buskirk, R.E.; Buskirk, W.H. (1975). "Defensive stoning by baboons". Nature. 256 (5517): 488–489. Bibcode:1975Natur.256..488H. doi:10.1038/256488a0. S2CID 4149862.
  177. ^ Fichtel, C.; Kappeler, P. M. (2010). "Chapter 19: Human universals and primate symplesiomorphies: Establishing the lemur baseline". In Kappeler, P. M.; Silk, J. B. (eds.). Mind the Gap: Tracing the Origins of Human Universals. Springer. ISBN 978-3-642-02724-6.
  178. ^ Sugiyama, Y. (1995). "Drinking tools of wild chimpanzees at Bossou". American Journal of Primatology. 37 (1): 263–269. doi:10.1002/ajp.1350370308. PMID 31936951. S2CID 86473603.
  179. ^ . OrangutanIslands.com. Archived from the original on 2013-11-26. Retrieved 2013-08-02.
  180. ^ van Schaik, C.; Fox, E.; Sitompul, A. (1996). "Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans". Naturwissenschaften. 83 (4): 186–188. Bibcode:1996NW.....83..186V. doi:10.1007/BF01143062. PMID 8643126. S2CID 27180148.
  181. ^ Gill, Victoria (22 July 2011). "Mandrill monkey makes 'pedicuring' tool". BBC. Retrieved 2013-08-11.
  182. ^ Vancatova, M. (2008). "Gorillas and Tools – Part I". Retrieved 2013-08-04.
  183. ^ Cowlishaw, G.; Clutton-Brock, T. (2009). "Primates". In MacDonald, D. (ed.). The Princeton Encyclopedia of Mammals. Princeton and Oxford University Press. pp. 270–280. ISBN 978-0-691-14069-8.
  184. ^ Reed, K.; Fleagle, J. (August 15, 1995). "Geographic and climatic control of primate diversity". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 92 (17): 7874–7876. Bibcode:1995PNAS...92.7874R. doi:10.1073/pnas.92.17.7874. PMC 41248. PMID 7644506.
  185. ^ Chapman, C. & Russo, S. (2007). "Primate Seed Dispersal". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 510. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  186. ^ Long, Y. C.; Kirkpatrick, R. C.; Zhong, T.; Xiao, L. (April 1994). "Report on the distribution, population, and ecology of the Yunnan snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus bieti)". Primates. 35 (2): 241–250. doi:10.1007/BF02382060. S2CID 23931368.
  187. ^ Schaller, G. B. (1963). The Mountain Gorilla: Ecology and Behavior. Chicago: University Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-73635-8.
  188. ^ Stammbach, E. (1987). "Desert, Forest, and Montane Baboons: Multilevel-Societies". In Smuts, B; Cheney, D; Seyfarth, R; Wrangham, R; Struhsaker, T. (eds.). Primate Societies. The University of Chicago Press. pp. 112–120. ISBN 978-0226767161.
  189. ^ Kemp, E. (2009). "Patterns of Water Use in Primates". Folia Primatologica. 80 (4): 275–294. doi:10.1159/000252586. PMID 19864919. S2CID 5108827.
  190. ^ Wolfe, L. D. & Fuentes, A. (2007). "Ethnoprimatology". In Campbell, C. J.; Fuentes, A.; MacKinnon, K. C.; Panger, M. & Bearder, S. K. (eds.). Primates in Perspective. Oxford University Press. p. 692. ISBN 978-0-19-517133-4.
  191. ^ Renquist, D. M. & Whitney, R. A. (1987). "Zoonoses Acquired from Pet Primates". Veterinary Clinics of North America: Small Animal Practice. 17 (1): 219–240. doi:10.1016/s0195-5616(87)50614-3. PMID 3551307. Retrieved 2008-08-11.
  192. ^ . United Nations. 1948. Archived from the original on 2014-12-08. Retrieved 2008-12-02.
  193. ^ a b Cavalieri, P. & Singer, P. . Great Ape Project. Archived from the original on 2008-08-20. Retrieved 2008-06-16.
  194. ^ Glendinning, L. (26 June 2008). "Spanish parliament approves 'human rights' for apes". The Guardian. Retrieved 2008-11-10.
  195. ^ Singer, P. (18 July 2008). "Of great apes and men". The Guardian. Retrieved 2008-11-10.
  196. ^ Mott, M. (16 September 2003). . National Geographic. Archived from the original on 2013-06-08. Retrieved 2013-02-06.
  197. ^ a b Workman, C. (June 2004). "Primate conservation in Vietnam: toward a holistic environmental narrative". American Anthropologist. 106 (2): 346–352. doi:10.1525/aa.2004.106.2.346.
  198. ^ . International Primate Protection League. 2003. Archived from the original on 2008-07-25. Retrieved 2008-08-04.
  199. ^ Bushnell, D. (1958). "The Beginnings of Research in Space Biology at the Air Force Missile Development Center, 1946–1952". History of Research in Space Biology and Biodynamics. NASA. Retrieved 2008-08-18.
  200. ^ Blumenthal, D. (1987-06-17). "Monkeys as Helpers To Quadriplegics At Home". The New York Times. Retrieved 2008-10-08.
  201. ^ Newman, James L. (2013). Encountering Gorillas: A Chronicle of Discovery, Exploitation, Understanding, and Survival. Plymouth, United Kingdom: Rowman and Littlefield. p. 173. ISBN 978-1-4422-1957-1.
  202. ^ a b . European Biomedical Research Association. 1996. Archived from the original on 2012-01-17. Retrieved 2008-08-18.
  203. ^ Chen, F. C.; Li, W. H. (February 2001). "Genomic divergences between humans and other hominoids and the effective population size of the common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees". American Journal of Human Genetics. 68 (2): 444–456. doi:10.1086/318206. PMC 1235277. PMID 11170892.
  204. ^ Conlee, K. M.; Hoffeld, E. H.; Stephens, M. L. (2004). "A Demographic Analysis of Primate Research in the United States". Alternatives to Laboratory Animals. 32 (Sup 1): 315–322. doi:10.1177/026119290403201s52. PMID 23577480. S2CID 6743973.
  205. ^ Presented to Parliament by the Secretary of State for the Home Department by Command of Her Majesty (July 2006). Statistics of scientific procedures on living animals: Great Britain 2005 (PDF). The Stationery Office. ISBN 0-10-168772-9. Retrieved 2008-06-16.
  206. ^ . Animal Welfare Information Center. United States Department of Agriculture. Archived from the original on 2008-10-03. Retrieved 2008-07-14.
  207. ^ . European Coalition to End Animal Experiments. Archived from the original on 2008-09-29. Retrieved 2008-10-08.
  208. ^ a b Estrada, Alejandro; Garber, Paul A.; Rylands, Anthony B.; Roos, Christian; Fernandez-Duque, Eduardo; Fiore, Anthony Di; Nekaris, K. Anne-Isola; Nijman, Vincent; Heymann, Eckhard W. (2017-01-01). "Impending extinction crisis of the world's primates: Why primates matter". Science Advances. 3 (1): e1600946. Bibcode:2017SciA....3E0946E. doi:10.1126/sciadv.1600946. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 5242557. PMID 28116351.
  209. ^ IFAW (2005). (PDF). International Fund for Animal Welfare. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-26. Retrieved 2011-02-26.
  210. ^ CITES (2010-10-14). "Appendices I, II and III". Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora. Retrieved 2012-04-02.
  211. ^ Grubb, P. (1998). "The Sierra Leone monkey drives". Mammals of Ghana, Sierra Leone, and the Gambia. St. Ives: Trendrine. pp. 214–219. ISBN 0-9512562-4-6.
  212. ^ a b Chapman, C. A.; Peres, C. A. (2001). "Primate conservation in the new millennium: the role of scientists". Evolutionary Anthropology. 10 (1): 16–33. doi:10.1002/1520-6505(2001)10:1<16::AID-EVAN1010>3.0.CO;2-O. S2CID 13494366.
  213. ^ a b Mittermeier, R. A. & Cheney, D. L. (1987). "Conservation of primates and their habitats". In Smuts, B. B.; Cheney, D. L.; Seyfarth, R. M.; Wrangham, R. W. & Struhsaker, T. T. (eds.). Primate Societies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 477–490.
  214. ^ a b Southwick, C. H. & Siddiqi, M. F. (2001). "Status, conservation and management of primates in India" (PDF). Envis Bulletin: Wildlife and Protected Areas. 1 (1): 81–91. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-10-01. Retrieved 2008-08-04.
  215. ^ a b c d Cowlishaw, G. & Dunbar, R. (2000). Primate Conservation Biology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-11637-2.
  216. ^ Van Schaik, C. P.; Monk, K. A.; Robertson, J. M. Y. (2001). "Dramatic decline in orangutan numbers in the Leuser Ecosystem, northern Sumatra". Oryx. 35 (1): 14–25. doi:10.1046/j.1365-3008.2001.00150.x.
  217. ^ Purvis, A.; Gittleman, J. L.; Cowlishaw, G.; Mace, G. M. (2000). "Predicting extinction risk in declining species". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 267 (1456): 1947–1952. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1234. PMC 1690772. PMID 11075706.
  218. ^ a b c Fa, J. E.; Juste, J.; Perez de Val, J.; Castroviejo, J. (1995). "Impact of market hunting on mammal species in Equatorial Guinea". Conservation Biology. 9 (5): 1107–1115. doi:10.1046/j.1523-1739.1995.9051107.x. hdl:10261/49187. PMID 34261280.
  219. ^ Hill, C. M. (1997). "Crop-raiding by wild vertebrates: The farmer's perspective in an agricultural community in western Uganda". International Journal of Pest Management. 43 (1): 77–84. doi:10.1080/096708797229022.
  220. ^ Hill, C. M. (2002). "Primate conservation and local communities: Ethical issues and debates". American Anthropologist. 104 (4): 1184–1194. doi:10.1525/aa.2002.104.4.1184.
  221. ^ Choudhury, A. (2001). "Primates in Northeast India: an overview of their distribution and conservation status" (PDF). Envis Bulletin: Wildlife and Protected Areas. 1 (1): 92–101. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-10-01. Retrieved 2008-08-04.
  222. ^ Kumara, H. N.; Singh, M. (October 2004). "Distribution and abundance of primates in rainforests of the Western Ghats, Karnataka, India and the conservation of Macaca silenus". International Journal of Primatology. 25 (5): 1001–1018. doi:10.1023/B:IJOP.0000043348.06255.7f. S2CID 30384142.
  223. ^ Nijman, V. (2004). (PDF). The Raffles Bulletin of Zoology. 52 (1): 271–280. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-09-10. Retrieved 2008-08-04.
  224. ^ O'Brien, T. G.; Kinnaird, M. F.; Nurcahyo, A.; Iqbal, M.; Rusmanto, M. (April 2004). "Abundance and distribution of sympatric gibbons in a threatened Sumatran rain forest". International Journal of Primatology. 25 (2): 267–284. doi:10.1023/B:IJOP.0000019152.83883.1c. S2CID 32472118.
  225. ^ Estrada, A.; Coates-Estrada, R.; Meritt, D. (September 1994). "Non-flying mammals and landscape changes in the tropical forest region of Los Tuxtlas, Mexico". Ecography. 17 (3): 229–241. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0587.1994.tb00098.x.
  226. ^ Marsh, L. K. (2003). "The nature of fragmentation.". In Marsh, L. K. (ed.). Primates in Fragments: Ecology and Conservation. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. pp. 1–10. ISBN 0-306-47696-7.
  227. ^ Turner, I. M. (1996). "Species loss in fragments of tropical rain forest: a review of the evidence". Journal of Applied Ecology. 33 (2): 200–209. doi:10.2307/2404743. JSTOR 2404743.
  228. ^ Chiarello, A.G. (2003). "Primates of the Brazilian Atlantic forest: the influence of forest fragmentation on survival". In Marsh, L. K. (ed.). Primates in Fragments: Ecology and Conservation. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. pp. 99–121. ISBN 978-0-306-47696-9.
  229. ^ Pope, T.R. (1996). "Socioecology, population fragmentation, and patterns of genetic loss in endangered primates". In Avise, J.; Hamrick, J. (eds.). Conservation Genetics: Case Histories from Nature. Norwell: Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 119–159. ISBN 978-0-412-05581-2.
  230. ^ Mittermeier, R.A.; Wallis, J.; Rylands, A.B.; Ganzhorn, J.U.; Oates, J.F.; Williamson, E.A.; Palacios, E.; Heymann, E.W.; Kierulff, M.C.M.; Yongcheng, L.; Supriatna, J.; Roos, C.; Walker, S.; Cortés-Ortiz, L.; Schwitzer, C., eds. (2009). Primates in Peril: The World's 25 Most Endangered Primates 2008–2010 (PDF). Illustrated by S.D. Nash. Arlington, VA.: IUCN/SSC Primate Specialist Group (PSG), International Primatological Society (IPS), and Conservation International (CI). pp. 23–26. ISBN 978-1-934151-34-1.
primate, explanation, very, similar, terms, plesiadapiformes, this, article, about, type, animal, other, uses, disambiguation, diverse, order, mammals, they, divided, into, strepsirrhines, which, include, lemurs, galagos, lorisids, haplorhines, which, include,. For an explanation of very similar terms see Plesiadapiformes This article is about the type of animal For other uses see Primate disambiguation Primates are a diverse order of mammals They are divided into the strepsirrhines which include the lemurs galagos and lorisids and the haplorhines which include the tarsiers and the simians monkeys including apes and humans Primates arose 85 55 million years ago first from small terrestrial mammals which adapted to living in the trees of tropical forests many primate characteristics represent adaptations to life in this challenging environment including large brains visual acuity color vision a shoulder girdle allowing a large degree of movement in the shoulder joint and dextrous hands Primates range in size from Madame Berthe s mouse lemur which weighs 30 g 1 oz to the eastern gorilla weighing over 200 kg 440 lb There are 376 524 species of living primates depending on which classification is used New primate species continue to be discovered over 25 species were described in the 2000s 36 in the 2010s and three in the 2020s PrimatesTemporal range 65 9 0 Ma PreꞒ Ꞓ O S D C P T J K Pg N Early Paleocene to PresentSome primate families from top to bottom Daubentoniidae Tarsiidae Lemuridae Lorisidae Cebidae Callitrichidae Atelidae Cercopithecidae Hylobatidae HominidaeScientific classificationKingdom AnimaliaPhylum ChordataClass MammaliaMirorder PrimatomorphaOrder PrimatesLinnaeus 1758 1 SubordersStrepsirrhini Haplorhini Altiatlasiussister DermopteraRange of the non human primates green SynonymsPlesiadapiformes cladistically including crown primates 2 Primates have large brains relative to body size compared to other mammals as well as an increased reliance on visual acuity at the expense of the sense of smell which is the dominant sensory system in most mammals These features are more developed in monkeys and apes and noticeably less so in lorises and lemurs Most primates also have opposable thumbs Some primates including gorillas humans and baboons are primarily terrestrial rather than arboreal but all species have adaptations for climbing trees Arboreal locomotion techniques used include leaping from tree to tree and swinging between branches of trees brachiation terrestrial locomotion techniques include walking on two limbs bipedalism and modified walking on four limbs knuckle walking Primates are among the most social of animals forming pairs or family groups uni male harems and multi male multi female groups Non human primates have at least four types of social systems many defined by the amount of movement by adolescent females between groups Primates have slower rates of development than other similarly sized mammals reach maturity later and have longer lifespans Primates are also the most intelligent animals and non human primates are recorded to use tools They may communicate using facial and hand gestures smells and vocalizations Close interactions between humans and non human primates NHPs can create opportunities for the transmission of zoonotic diseases especially virus diseases including herpes measles ebola rabies and hepatitis Thousands of non human primates are used in research around the world because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans About 60 of primate species are threatened with extinction Common threats include deforestation forest fragmentation monkey drives and primate hunting for use in medicines as pets and for food Large scale tropical forest clearing for agriculture most threatens primates Contents 1 Etymology 2 Classification of living primates 3 Phylogeny and genetics 3 1 Evolution 3 2 Hybrids 3 3 Clones 4 Anatomy and physiology 4 1 Head 4 2 Body 4 3 Sexual dimorphism 4 4 Locomotion 4 5 Vision 5 Behavior 5 1 Social systems 5 2 Interspecific associations 5 3 Communication 5 4 Life history 5 5 Diet and feeding 5 5 1 Hunting 5 6 As prey 5 7 Intelligence and cognition 5 7 1 Tool use and manufacture 6 Ecology 7 Interactions between humans and other primates 7 1 Disease transmission 7 2 Legal and social status 7 3 Role in scientific research 7 4 Extinction threats 8 See also 9 Footnotes 10 References 10 1 Literature cited 11 Further reading 12 External linksEtymology EditThe English name primates is derived from Old French or French primat from a noun use of Latin primat from primus prime first rank 3 The name was given by Carl Linnaeus because he thought this the highest order of animals 4 The relationships among the different groups of primates were not clearly understood until relatively recently so the commonly used terms are somewhat confused For example ape has been used either as an alternative for monkey or for any tailless relatively human like primate 5 Sir Wilfrid Le Gros Clark was one of the primatologists who developed the idea of trends in primate evolution and the methodology of arranging the living members of an order into an ascending series leading to humans 6 Commonly used names for groups of primates such as prosimians monkeys lesser apes and great apes reflect this methodology According to our current understanding of the evolutionary history of the primates several of these groups are paraphyletic or rather they do not include all the descendants of a common ancestor 7 In contrast with Clark s methodology modern classifications typically identify or name only those groupings that are monophyletic that is such a named group includes all the descendants of the group s common ancestor 8 The cladogram below shows one possible classification sequence of the living primates 9 10 groups that use common traditional names are shown on the right Primatomorpha Dermoptera Primates Haplorhini Simiiformes Catarrhini Hominoidea Hominidae Homininae Hominini humans genus Homo chimpanzees and bonobos genus Pan gorillas tribe Gorillini orangutans subfamily Ponginae gibbons family Hylobatidae Old World monkeys superfamily Cercopithecoidea New World monkeys parvorder Platyrrhini Tarsiiformes tarsiers superfamily Tarsioidea Strepsirrhini Lemuriformes a lemurs superfamily Lemuroidea lorises and allies superfamily Lorisoidea prosimians monkeys great apes humans lesser apesAll groups with scientific names are clades or monophyletic groups and the sequence of scientific classification reflects the evolutionary history of the related lineages Groups that are traditionally named are shown on the right they form an ascending series per Clark see above and several groups are paraphyletic Prosimians contain two monophyletic groups the suborder Strepsirrhini or lemurs lorises and allies as well as the tarsiers of the suborder Haplorhini it is a paraphyletic grouping because it excludes the Simiiformes which also are descendants of the common ancestor Primates Monkeys comprise two monophyletic groups New World monkeys and Old World monkeys but is paraphyletic because it excludes hominoids superfamily Hominoidea also descendants of the common ancestor Simiiformes Apes as a whole and the great apes are paraphyletic if the terms are used such that they exclude humans Thus the members of the two sets of groups and hence names do not match which causes problems in relating scientific names to common usually traditional names Consider the superfamily Hominoidea In terms of the common names on the right this group consists of apes and humans and there is no single common name for all the members of the group One remedy is to create a new common name in this case hominoids Another possibility is to expand the use of one of the traditional names For example in his 2005 book the vertebrate palaeontologist Benton wrote The apes Hominoidea today include the gibbons and orangutan the gorilla and chimpanzee and humans 11 thereby Benton was using apes to mean hominoids In that case the group heretofore called apes must now be identified as the non human apes As of 2021 update there is no consensus as to whether to accept traditional that is common but paraphyletic names or to use monophyletic names only or to use new common names or adaptations of old ones Both competing approaches can be found in biological sources often in the same work and sometimes by the same author Thus Benton defines apes to include humans then he repeatedly uses ape like to mean like an ape rather than a human and when discussing the reaction of others to a new fossil he writes of claims that Orrorin was an ape rather than a human 12 Classification of living primates Edit A 1927 drawing of chimpanzees a gibbon top right and two orangutans center and bottom center The chimpanzee in the upper left is brachiating the orangutan at the bottom center is knuckle walking Homo sapiens is the only living primate species that is fully bipedal Nilgiri langur Trachypithecus johnii an Old World monkey A list of the families of the living primates is given below together with one possible classification into ranks between order and family 1 9 13 14 Other classifications are also used For example an alternative classification of the living Strepsirrhini divides them into two infraorders Lemuriformes and Lorisiformes 15 Order Primates Suborder Strepsirrhini lemurs galagos and lorisids Infraorder Lemuriformes a Superfamily Lemuroidea Family Cheirogaleidae dwarf lemurs and mouse lemurs 41 species Family Daubentoniidae aye aye 1 species Family Lemuridae ring tailed lemur and allies 21 species Family Lepilemuridae sportive lemurs 26 species Family Indriidae woolly lemurs and allies 19 species Superfamily Lorisoidea Family Lorisidae lorisids 16 species Family Galagidae galagos 23 species Suborder Haplorhini tarsiers monkeys and apes Infraorder Tarsiiformes Family Tarsiidae tarsiers 14 species Infraorder Simiiformes or Anthropoidea Parvorder Platyrrhini New World monkeys Family Callitrichidae marmosets and tamarins 49 species Family Cebidae capuchins and squirrel monkeys 29 species Family Aotidae night or owl monkeys douroucoulis 11 species Family Pitheciidae titis sakis and uakaris 56 species Family Atelidae howler spider woolly spider and woolly monkeys 26 species Parvorder Catarrhini Superfamily Cercopithecoidea Family Cercopithecidae Old World monkeys 165 species Superfamily Hominoidea Family Hylobatidae gibbons or lesser apes 20 species Family Hominidae great apes including humans 8 species Order Primates was established by Carl Linnaeus in 1758 in the tenth edition of his book Systema Naturae 18 for the genera Homo humans Simia other apes and monkeys Lemur prosimians and Vespertilio bats In the first edition of the same book 1735 he had used the name Anthropomorpha for Homo Simia and Bradypus sloths 19 In 1839 Henri Marie Ducrotay de Blainville following Linnaeus and imitating his nomenclature established the orders Secundates including the suborders Chiroptera Insectivora and Carnivora Tertiates or Glires and Quaternates including Gravigrada Pachydermata and Ruminantia 20 but these new taxa were not accepted Before Anderson and Jones introduced the classification of Strepsirrhini and Haplorhini in 1984 21 followed by McKenna and Bell s 1997 work Classification of Mammals Above the species level 22 Primates was divided into two superfamilies Prosimii and Anthropoidea 23 Prosimii included all of the prosimians Strepsirrhini plus the tarsiers Anthropoidea contained all of the simians Phylogeny and genetics EditEuarchontoglires Glires Rodentia rodents Lagomorpha rabbits hares pikas Euarchonta Scandentia treeshrews Primatomorpha Dermoptera colugos Primates Plesiadapiformescrown primatesOrder Primates is part of the clade Euarchontoglires which is nested within the clade Eutheria of Class Mammalia Recent molecular genetic research on primates colugos and treeshrews has shown that the two species of colugos are more closely related to primates than to treeshrews 24 even though treeshrews were at one time considered primates 25 These three orders make up the clade Euarchonta The combination of this clade with the clade Glires composed of Rodentia and Lagomorpha forms the clade Euarchontoglires Variously both Euarchonta and Euarchontoglires are ranked as superorders Some scientists consider Dermoptera to be a suborder of Primates and use the suborder Euprimates for the true primates 26 Evolution Edit Further information Evolution of primates The primate lineage is thought to go back at least near the Cretaceous Paleogene boundary or around 63 74 mya 27 28 29 30 31 The earliest possible primate proto primate may be Purgatorius which dates back to Early Paleocene of North America 66mya 32 33 The oldest known primates from the fossil record date to the Late Paleocene of Africa c 57 mya Altiatlasius 34 or the Paleocene Eocene transition in the northern continents c 55 mya Cantius Donrussellia Altanius Plesiadapis and Teilhardina 35 36 32 Other studies including molecular clock studies have estimated the origin of the primate branch to have been in the mid Cretaceous period around 85 mya 37 38 39 By modern cladistic reckoning the order Primates is monophyletic The suborder Strepsirrhini the wet nosed primates is generally thought to have split off from the primitive primate line about 63 mya 40 although earlier dates are also supported 41 The seven strepsirrhine families are the five related lemur families and the two remaining families that include the lorisids and the galagos 1 13 Older classification schemes wrap Lepilemuridae into Lemuridae and Galagidae into Lorisidae yielding a four one family distribution instead of five two as presented here 1 During the Eocene most of the northern continents were dominated by two groups the adapiforms and the omomyids 42 43 The former are considered members of Strepsirrhini but did not have a toothcomb like modern lemurs recent analysis has demonstrated that Darwinius masillae fits into this grouping 44 The latter was closely related to tarsiers monkeys and apes How these two groups relate to extant primates is unclear Omomyids perished about 30 mya 43 while adapiforms survived until about 10 mya 45 According to genetic studies the lemurs of Madagascar diverged from the lorisoids approximately 75 mya 41 These studies as well as chromosomal and molecular evidence also show that lemurs are more closely related to each other than to other strepsirrhine primates 41 46 However Madagascar split from Africa 160 mya and from India 90 mya 47 To account for these facts a founding lemur population of a few individuals is thought to have reached Madagascar from Africa via a single rafting event between 50 and 80 mya 41 46 47 Other colonization options have been suggested such as multiple colonizations from Africa and India 42 but none are supported by the genetic and molecular evidence 41 Common brown lemur a Strepsirrhine primate Until recently the aye aye has been difficult to place within Strepsirrhini 1 Theories had been proposed that its family Daubentoniidae was either a lemuriform primate meaning its ancestors split from the lemur line more recently than lemurs and lorises split or a sister group to all the other strepsirrhines In 2008 the aye aye family was confirmed to be most closely related to the other Malagasy lemurs likely having descended from the same ancestral population that colonized the island 41 Suborder Haplorhini the simple nosed or dry nosed primates is composed of two sister clades 1 Prosimian tarsiers in the family Tarsiidae monotypic in its own infraorder Tarsiiformes represent the most basal division originating about 58 mya 48 49 The earliest known haplorhine skeleton that of 55 MA old tarsier like Archicebus was found in central China 50 supporting an already suspected Asian origin for the group 51 The infraorder Simiiformes simian primates consisting of monkeys and apes emerged about 40 mya 43 possibly also in Asia if so they dispersed across the Tethys Sea from Asia to Africa soon afterwards 52 There are two simian clades both parvorders Catarrhini which developed in Africa consisting of Old World monkeys humans and the other apes and Platyrrhini which developed in South America consisting of New World monkeys 1 A third clade which included the eosimiids developed in Asia but became extinct millions of years ago 53 As in the case of lemurs the origin of New World monkeys is unclear Molecular studies of concatenated nuclear sequences have yielded a widely varying estimated date of divergence between platyrrhines and catarrhines ranging from 33 to 70 mya while studies based on mitochondrial sequences produce a narrower range of 35 to 43 mya 36 54 The anthropoid primates possibly traversed the Atlantic Ocean from Africa to South America during the Eocene by island hopping facilitated by Atlantic Ocean ridges and a lowered sea level 42 Alternatively a single rafting event may explain this transoceanic colonization Due to continental drift the Atlantic Ocean was not nearly as wide at the time as it is today 42 Research suggests that a small 1 kg 2 2 lb primate could have survived 13 days on a raft of vegetation 55 Given estimated current and wind speeds this would have provided enough time to make the voyage between the continents Emperor tamarin a New World monkey Apes and monkeys spread from Africa into Europe and Asia starting in the Miocene 56 Soon after the lorises and tarsiers made the same journey The first hominin fossils were discovered in northern Africa and date back 5 8 mya 43 Old World monkeys disappeared from Europe about 1 8 mya 57 Molecular and fossil studies generally show that modern humans originated in Africa 100 000 200 000 years ago 58 Although primates are well studied in comparison to other animal groups several new species have been discovered recently and genetic tests have revealed previously unrecognised species in known populations Primate Taxonomy listed about 350 species of primates in 2001 10 the author Colin Groves increased that number to 376 for his contribution to the third edition of Mammal Species of the World MSW3 1 However publications since the taxonomy in MSW3 was compiled in 2003 have pushed the number to 522 species or 708 including subspecies 59 Hybrids Edit Primate hybrids usually arise in captivity 60 but there have also been examples in the wild 61 62 Hybridization occurs where two species range overlap to form hybrid zones hybrids may be created by humans when animals are placed in zoos or due to environmental pressures such as predation 61 Intergeneric hybridizations hybrids of different genera have also been found in the wild Although they belong to genera that have been distinct for several million years interbreeding still occurs between the gelada and the hamadryas baboon 63 Clones Edit On 24 January 2018 scientists in China reported in the journal Cell the creation of two crab eating macaque clones named Zhong Zhong and Hua Hua using the complex DNA transfer method that produced Dolly the sheep for the first time 64 65 66 67 68 Anatomy and physiology EditHead Edit Primate skulls showing postorbital bar and increasing brain sizes The primate skull has a large domed cranium which is particularly prominent in anthropoids The cranium protects the large brain a distinguishing characteristic of this group 69 The endocranial volume the volume within the skull is three times greater in humans than in the greatest nonhuman primate reflecting a larger brain size 70 The mean endocranial volume is 1 201 cubic centimeters in humans 469 cm3 in gorillas 400 cm3 in chimpanzees and 397 cm3 in orangutans 70 The primary evolutionary trend of primates has been the elaboration of the brain in particular the neocortex a part of the cerebral cortex which is involved with sensory perception generation of motor commands spatial reasoning conscious thought and in humans language 71 While other mammals rely heavily on their sense of smell the arboreal life of primates has led to a tactile visually dominant sensory system 71 a reduction in the olfactory region of the brain and increasingly complex social behavior 72 Primates have forward facing eyes on the front of the skull binocular vision allows accurate distance perception useful for the brachiating ancestors of all great apes 69 A bony ridge above the eye sockets reinforces weaker bones in the face which are put under strain during chewing Strepsirrhines have a postorbital bar a bone around the eye socket to protect their eyes in contrast the higher primates haplorhines have evolved fully enclosed sockets 73 An 1893 drawing of the hands and feet of various primates Primates show an evolutionary trend towards a reduced snout 74 Technically Old World monkeys are distinguished from New World monkeys by the structure of the nose and from apes by the arrangement of their teeth 72 In New World monkeys the nostrils face sideways in Old World monkeys they face downwards 72 Dental pattern in primates vary considerably although some have lost most of their incisors all retain at least one lower incisor 72 In most strepsirrhines the lower incisors form a toothcomb which is used in grooming and sometimes foraging 72 75 Old World monkeys have eight premolars compared with 12 in New World monkeys The Old World species are divided into apes and monkeys depending on the number of cusps on their molars monkeys have four apes have five 72 although humans may have four or five 76 The main hominid molar cusp hypocone evolved in early primate history while the cusp of the corresponding primitive lower molar paraconid was lost Prosimians are distinguished by their immobilized upper lips the moist tip of their noses and forward facing lower front teeth Body Edit Vervet hindfoot showing fingerprint ridges on the sole Primates generally have five digits on each limb pentadactyly with a characteristic type of keratin fingernail on the end of each finger and toe The bottom sides of the hands and feet have sensitive pads on the fingertips Most have opposable thumbs a characteristic primate feature most developed in humans though not limited to this order opossums and koalas for example also have them 69 Thumbs allow some species to use tools In primates the combination of opposing thumbs short fingernails rather than claws and long inward closing fingers is a relict of the ancestral practice of gripping branches and has in part allowed some species to develop brachiation swinging by the arms from tree limb to tree limb as a significant means of locomotion Prosimians have clawlike nails on the second toe of each foot called toilet claws which they use for grooming 69 The primate collar bone is a prominent element of the pectoral girdle this allows the shoulder joint broad mobility 74 Compared to Old World monkeys apes have more mobile shoulder joints and arms due to the dorsal position of the scapula broad ribcages that are flatter front to back a shorter less mobile spine and with lower vertebrae greatly reduced resulting in tail loss in some species Prehensile tails are found in the New World atelids including the howler spider woolly spider woolly monkeys and in capuchins 77 78 Male primates have a low hanging penis and testes descended into a scrotum 79 75 Sexual dimorphism Edit Main article Sexual dimorphism in non human primates Distinct sexual size dimorphism can be seen between the male and female mountain gorilla Sexual dimorphism is often exhibited in simians though to a greater degree in Old World species apes and some monkeys than New World species Recent studies involve comparing DNA to examine both the variation in the expression of the dimorphism among primates and the fundamental causes of sexual dimorphism Primates usually have dimorphism in body mass 80 81 and canine tooth size 82 83 along with pelage and skin color 84 The dimorphism can be attributed to and affected by different factors including mating system 85 size 85 habitat and diet 86 Comparative analyses have generated a more complete understanding of the relationship between sexual selection natural selection and mating systems in primates Studies have shown that dimorphism is the product of changes in both male and female traits 87 Ontogenetic scaling where relative extension of a common growth trajectory occurs may give some insight into the relationship between sexual dimorphism and growth patterns 88 Some evidence from the fossil record suggests that there was convergent evolution of dimorphism and some extinct hominids probably had greater dimorphism than any living primate 87 Locomotion Edit Diademed sifaka a lemur that is a vertical clinger and leaper Primate species move by brachiation bipedalism leaping arboreal and terrestrial quadrupedalism climbing knuckle walking or by a combination of these methods Several prosimians are primarily vertical clingers and leapers These include many bushbabies all indriids i e sifakas avahis and indris sportive lemurs and all tarsiers 89 Other prosimians are arboreal quadrupeds and climbers Some are also terrestrial quadrupeds while some are leapers Most monkeys are both arboreal and terrestrial quadrupeds and climbers Gibbons muriquis and spider monkeys all brachiate extensively 57 with gibbons sometimes doing so in remarkably acrobatic fashion Woolly monkeys also brachiate at times 90 Orangutans use a similar form of locomotion called quadramanous climbing in which they use their arms and legs to carry their heavy bodies through the trees 57 Chimpanzees and gorillas knuckle walk 57 and can move bipedally for short distances Although numerous species such as australopithecines and early hominids have exhibited fully bipedal locomotion humans are the only extant species with this trait 91 Vision Edit The tapetum lucidum of a northern greater galago typical of prosimians reflects the light of the photographer s flash The evolution of color vision in primates is unique among most eutherian mammals While the remote vertebrate ancestors of the primates possessed three color vision trichromaticism the nocturnal warm blooded mammalian ancestors lost one of three cones in the retina during the Mesozoic era Fish reptiles and birds are therefore trichromatic or tetrachromatic while all mammals with the exception of some primates and marsupials 92 are dichromats or monochromats totally color blind 75 Nocturnal primates such as the night monkeys and bush babies are often monochromatic Catarrhines are routinely trichromatic due to a gene duplication of the red green opsin gene at the base of their lineage 30 to 40 million years ago 75 93 Platyrrhines on the other hand are trichromatic in a few cases only 94 Specifically individual females must be heterozygous for two alleles of the opsin gene red and green located on the same locus of the X chromosome 75 Males therefore can only be dichromatic while females can be either dichromatic or trichromatic Color vision in strepsirrhines is not as well understood however research indicates a range of color vision similar to that found in platyrrhines 75 Like catarrhines howler monkeys a family of platyrrhines show routine trichromatism that has been traced to an evolutionarily recent gene duplication 95 Howler monkeys are one of the most specialized leaf eaters of the New World monkeys fruits are not a major part of their diets 90 and the type of leaves they prefer to consume young nutritive and digestible are detectable only by a red green signal Field work exploring the dietary preferences of howler monkeys suggests that routine trichromaticism was selected by environment 94 Behavior EditSocial systems Edit Richard Wrangham stated that social systems of primates are best classified by the amount of movement by females occurring between groups 96 He proposed four categories Female transfer systems females move away from the group in which they were born Females of a group will not be closely related whereas males will have remained with their natal groups and this close association may be influential in social behavior The groups formed are generally quite small 96 This organization can be seen in chimpanzees where the males who are typically related will cooperate in defense of the group s territory 97 Evidence of this social system has also been found among Neanderthal remains in Spain 98 and in remains of Australopithecus and Paranthropus robustus groups in southern Africa 99 100 Among New World Monkeys spider monkeys and muriquis use this system 101 A social huddle of ring tailed lemurs The two individuals on the right exposing their white ventral surface are sunning themselves Male transfer systems while the females remain in their natal groups the males will emigrate as adolescents Polygynous and multi male societies are classed in this category Group sizes are usually larger 96 This system is common among the ring tailed lemur capuchin monkeys and cercopithecine monkeys 57 Monogamous species a male female bond sometimes accompanied by a juvenile offspring There is shared responsibility of parental care and territorial defense The offspring leaves the parents territory during adolescence 96 Gibbons essentially use this system although monogamy in this context does not necessarily mean absolute sexual fidelity 102 These species do not live in larger groups Solitary species often males who defend territories that include the home ranges of several females 96 This type of organization is found in the prosimians such as the slow loris 103 Orangutans do not defend their territory but effectively have this organization 104 Other systems are known to occur as well For example with howler monkeys and gorillas both the males and females typically transfer from their natal group on reaching sexual maturity resulting in groups in which neither the males nor females are typically related 90 105 Some prosimians colobine monkeys and callitrichid monkeys also use this system 57 The transfer of females or males from their native group is likely an adaptation for avoiding inbreeding 106 An analysis of breeding records of captive primate colonies representing numerous different species indicates that the infant mortality of inbred young is generally higher than that of non inbred young 106 107 This effect of inbreeding on infant mortality is probably largely a result of increased expression of deleterious recessive alleles see Inbreeding depression Chimpanzees are social great apes Primatologist Jane Goodall who studied in the Gombe Stream National Park noted fission fusion societies in chimpanzees 108 There is fission when the main group splits up to forage during the day then fusion when the group returns at night to sleep as a group This social structure can also be observed in the hamadryas baboon 109 spider monkeys 90 and the bonobo 109 The gelada has a similar social structure in which many smaller groups come together to form temporary herds of up to 600 monkeys 109 Humans also form fission fusion societies In hunter gatherer societies humans form groups which are made up of several individuals that may split up to obtain different resources 110 These social systems are affected by three main ecological factors distribution of resources group size and predation 111 Within a social group there is a balance between cooperation and competition Cooperative behaviors in many primates species include social grooming removing skin parasites and cleaning wounds food sharing and collective defense against predators or of a territory Aggressive behaviors often signal competition for food sleeping sites or mates Aggression is also used in establishing dominance hierarchies 111 112 Interspecific associations Edit Several species of primates are known to associate in the wild Some of these associations have been extensively studied In the Tai Forest of Africa several species coordinate anti predator behavior These include the Diana monkey Campbell s mona monkey lesser spot nosed monkey western red colobus king colobus western black and white colobus and sooty mangabey which coordinate anti predator alarm calls 113 Among the predators of these monkeys is the common chimpanzee 114 The red tailed monkey associates with several species including the western red colobus blue monkey Wolf s mona monkey mantled guereza black crested mangabey and Allen s swamp monkey 109 Several of these species are preyed upon by the common chimpanzee 115 In South America squirrel monkeys associate with capuchin monkeys 116 This may have more to do with foraging benefits to the squirrel monkeys than anti predation benefits 116 Communication Edit Further information Great ape language Indri lemur wailing source source Indri lemur wailing ogg Vorbis format Howler monkey roaring source source track Howler monkey roaring ogg Vorbis format Vervet monkey alarm call source source Vervet monkey alarm call ogg Vorbis format Siamang singing source source Siamang singing ogg Vorbis format Problems playing these files See media help Lemurs lorises tarsiers and New World monkeys rely on olfactory signals for many aspects of social and reproductive behavior 71 Specialized glands are used to mark territories with pheromones which are detected by the vomeronasal organ this process forms a large part of the communication behavior of these primates 71 In Old World monkeys and apes this ability is mostly vestigial having regressed as trichromatic eyes evolved to become the main sensory organ 117 Primates also use vocalizations gestures and facial expressions to convey psychological state 118 119 Facial musculature is very developed in primates particularly in monkeys and apes allowing for complex facial communication Like humans chimpanzees can distinguish the faces of familiar and unfamiliar individuals 120 Hand and arm gestures are also important forms of communication for great apes and a single gesture can have multiple functions 119 Primates are a particularly vocal group of mammals 79 Indris and black and white ruffed lemurs make distinctive loud songs and choruses which maintain territories and act as alarm calls 121 The Philippine tarsier has a high frequency limit of auditory sensitivity of approximately 91 kHz with a dominant frequency of 70 kHz among the highest recorded for any terrestrial mammal For Philippine tarsiers these ultrasonic vocalizations might represent a private channel of communication that subverts detection by predators prey and competitors enhances energetic efficiency or improves detection against low frequency background noise 122 Male howler monkeys are among the loudest land mammals as their roars can be heard up to 4 8 km 3 0 mi and relate to intergroup spacing territorial protection and possibly mate guarding 123 124 Roars are produced by a modified larynx and enlarged hyoid bone which contains an air sac 125 The vervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of at least four different predators and the reactions of other monkeys vary according to the call 126 Male and female siamangs both possess inflatable pouches in the throat with which pair bonds use to sing duets to each other 127 Many non human primates have the vocal anatomy to produce human speech but lack the proper brain wiring 128 Vowel like vocal patterns have been recorded in baboons which has implications for the origin of speech in humans 129 Consonant and vowel like sounds exist in some orangutan calls and they maintain their meaning over great distances 130 The time range for the evolution of human language and or its anatomical prerequisites extends at least in principle from the phylogenetic divergence of Homo 2 3 to 2 4 million years ago from Pan 5 to 6 million years ago to the emergence of full behavioral modernity some 50 000 150 000 years ago Few dispute that Australopithecus probably lacked vocal communication significantly more sophisticated than that of great apes in general 131 Life history Edit A crab eating macaque breastfeeding her baby Primates have slower rates of development than other mammals 57 All primate infants are breastfed by their mothers with the exception of some human cultures and various zoo raised primates which are fed formula and rely on them for grooming and transportation 57 In some species infants are protected and transported by males in the group particularly males who may be their fathers 57 Other relatives of the infant such as siblings and aunts may participate in its care as well 57 Most primate mothers cease ovulation while breastfeeding an infant once the infant is weaned the mother can reproduce again 57 This often leads to weaning conflict with infants who attempt to continue breastfeeding 57 Infanticide is common in polygynous species such as gray langurs and gorillas Adult males may kill dependent offspring that are not theirs so the female will return to estrus and thus they can sire offspring of their own Social monogamy in some species may have evolved to combat this behavior 132 Promiscuity may also lessen the risk of infanticide since paternity becomes uncertain 133 Primates have a longer juvenile period between weaning and sexual maturity than other mammals of similar size 57 Some primates such as galagos and new world monkeys use tree holes for nesting and park juveniles in leafy patches while foraging Other primates follow a strategy of riding i e carrying individuals on the body while feeding Adults may construct or use nesting sites sometimes accompanied by juveniles for the purpose of resting a behavior which has developed secondarily in the great apes 134 135 During the juvenile period primates are more susceptible than adults to predation and starvation they gain experience in feeding and avoiding predators during this time 57 They learn social and fighting skills often through playing 57 Primates especially females have longer lifespans than other similarly sized mammals 57 this may be partially due to their slower metabolisms 136 Late in life female catarrhine primates appear to undergo a cessation of reproductive function known as menopause other groups are less studied 137 Diet and feeding Edit Leaf eating mantled guereza a species of black and white colobus A mouse lemur holds a cut piece of fruit in its hands and eats Primates exploit a variety of food sources It has been said that many characteristics of modern primates including humans derive from an early ancestor s practice of taking most of its food from the tropical canopy 138 Most primates include fruit in their diets to obtain easily digested nutrients including carbohydrates and lipids for energy 57 Primates in the suborder Strepsirrhini non tarsier prosimians are able to synthesize vitamin C like most other mammals while primates of the suborder Haplorhini tarsiers monkeys and apes have lost this ability and require the vitamin in their diet 139 Many primates have anatomical specializations that enable them to exploit particular foods such as fruit leaves gum or insects 57 For example leaf eaters such as howler monkeys black and white colobuses and sportive lemurs have extended digestive tracts which enable them to absorb nutrients from leaves that can be difficult to digest 57 Marmosets which are gum eaters have strong incisor teeth enabling them to open tree bark to get to the gum and claws rather than nails enabling them to cling to trees while feeding 57 The aye aye combines rodent like teeth with a long thin middle finger to fill the same ecological niche as a woodpecker It taps on trees to find insect larvae then gnaws holes in the wood and inserts its elongated middle finger to pull the larvae out 140 Some species have additional specializations For example the grey cheeked mangabey has thick enamel on its teeth enabling it to open hard fruits and seeds that other monkeys cannot 57 The gelada is the only primate species that feeds primarily on grass 141 Hunting Edit Humans have traditionally hunted prey for subsistence Tarsiers are the only extant obligate carnivorous primates exclusively eating insects crustaceans small vertebrates and snakes including venomous species 142 Capuchin monkeys can exploit many different types of plant matter including fruit leaves flowers buds nectar and seeds but also eat insects and other invertebrates bird eggs and small vertebrates such as birds lizards squirrels and bats 90 The common chimpanzee eats an omnivorous frugivorous diet It prefers fruit above all other food items and even seeks out and eats them when they are not abundant It also eats leaves and leaf buds seeds blossoms stems pith bark and resin Insects and meat make up a small proportion of their diet estimated as 2 143 144 The meat consumption includes predation on other primate species such as the western red colobus monkey 114 The bonobo is an omnivorous frugivore the majority of its diet is fruit but it supplements this with leaves meat from small vertebrates such as anomalures flying squirrels and duikers 145 and invertebrates 146 In some instances bonobos have been shown to consume lower order primates 147 148 Until the development of agriculture approximately 10 000 years ago Homo sapiens employed a hunter gatherer method as their sole means of food collection This involved combining stationary food sources such as fruits grains tubers and mushrooms insect larvae and aquatic mollusks with wild game which must be hunted and killed in order to be consumed 149 It has been proposed that humans have used fire to prepare and cook food since the time of Homo erectus 150 Around ten thousand years ago humans developed agriculture 151 which substantially altered their diet This change in diet may also have altered human biology with the spread of dairy farming providing a new and rich source of food leading to the evolution of the ability to digest lactose in some adults 152 153 As prey Edit Predators of primates include various species of carnivorans birds of prey reptiles and other primates Even gorillas have been recorded as prey Predators of primates have diverse hunting strategies and as such primates have evolved several different antipredator adaptations including crypsis alarm calls and mobbing Several species have separate alarm calls for different predators such as air borne or ground dwelling predators Predation may have shaped group size in primates as species exposed to higher predation pressures appear to live in larger groups 154 Intelligence and cognition Edit Main article Primate cognition Primates have advanced cognitive abilities some make tools and use them to acquire food and for social displays 155 156 some can perform tasks requiring cooperation influence and rank 157 they are status conscious manipulative and capable of deception 158 159 they can recognise kin and conspecifics 160 161 and they can learn to use symbols and understand aspects of human language including some relational syntax and concepts of number and numerical sequence 162 163 164 Research in primate cognition explores problem solving memory social interaction a theory of mind and numerical spatial and abstract concepts 165 Comparative studies show a trend towards higher intelligence going from prosimians to New World monkeys to Old World monkeys and significantly higher average cognitive abilities in the great apes 166 167 However there is a great deal of variation in each group e g among New World monkeys both spider 166 and capuchin monkeys 167 have scored highly by some measures as well as in the results of different studies 166 167 Tool use and manufacture Edit Main article Tool use by animals A western lowland gorilla using a stick possibly to gauge the depth of water Crab eating macaques with stone tools In 1960 Jane Goodall observed a chimpanzee poking pieces of grass into a termite mound and then raising the grass to his mouth After he left Goodall approached the mound and repeated the behaviour because she was unsure what the chimpanzee was doing She found that the termites bit onto the grass with their jaws The chimpanzee had been using the grass as a tool to fish or dip for termites 168 There are more limited reports of the closely related bonobo using tools in the wild it has been claimed they rarely use tools in the wild although they use tools as readily as chimpanzees when in captivity 169 It has been reported that females both chimpanzee and bonobo use tools more avidly than males 170 Orangutans in Borneo scoop catfish out of small ponds Over two years anthropologist Anne Russon observed orangutans learning to jab sticks at catfish to scare them out of the ponds and in to their waiting hands 171 There are few reports of gorillas using tools in the wild An adult female western lowland gorilla used a branch as a walking stick apparently to test water depth and to aid her in crossing a pool of water Another adult female used a detached trunk from a small shrub as a stabilizer during food gathering and another used a log as a bridge 172 The first direct observation of a non ape primate using a tool in a wild environment occurred in 1988 Primatologist Sue Boinski watched an adult male white faced capuchin beat a fer de lance snake to death with a dead branch 173 The black striped capuchin was the first non ape primate for which routine tool use was documented in the wild individuals were observed cracking nuts by placing them on a stone anvil and hitting them with another large stone 174 In Thailand and Myanmar crab eating macaques use stone tools to open nuts oysters and other bivalves and various types of sea snails 175 Chacma baboons use stones as weapons stoning by these baboons is done from the rocky walls of the canyon where they sleep and retreat to when they are threatened Stones are lifted with one hand and dropped over the side whereupon they tumble down the side of the cliff or fall directly to the canyon floor 176 Although they have not been observed to use tools in the wild lemurs in controlled settings have been shown to be capable of understanding the functional properties of the objects they had been trained to use as tools performing as well as tool using haplorhines 177 Soon after her initial discovery of tool use Goodall observed other chimpanzees picking up leafy twigs stripping off the leaves and using the stems to fish for insects This change of a leafy twig into a tool was a major discovery Prior to this scientists thought that only humans manufactured and used tools and that this ability was what separated humans from other animals 168 Chimpanzees have also been observed making sponges out of leaves and moss that suck up water 178 Sumatran orangutans have been observed making and using tools They will break off a tree branch that is about 30 cm long snap off the twigs fray one end and then use the stick to dig in tree holes for termites 179 180 In the wild mandrills have been observed to clean their ears with modified tools Scientists filmed a large male mandrill at Chester Zoo UK stripping down a twig apparently to make it narrower and then using the modified stick to scrape dirt from underneath its toenails 181 Captive gorillas have made a variety of tools 182 Ecology EditSee also List of primates by population Rhesus macaque at Agra Fort India Non human primates primarily live in the tropical latitudes of Africa Asia and the Americas Species that live outside of the tropics include the Japanese macaque which lives in the Japanese islands of Honshu and Hokkaido the Barbary macaque which lives in North Africa and several species of langur which live in China Primates tend to live in tropical rainforests but are also found in temperate forests savannas deserts mountains and coastal areas 183 The number of primate species within tropical areas has been shown to be positively correlated to the amount of rainfall and the amount of rain forest area 184 Accounting for 25 to 40 of the fruit eating animals by weight within tropical rainforests primates play an important ecological role by dispersing seeds of many tree species 185 Primate habitats span a range of altitudes the black snub nosed monkey has been found living in the Hengduan Mountains at altitudes of 4 700 meters 15 400 ft 186 the mountain gorilla can be found at 4 200 meters 13 200 ft crossing the Virunga Mountains 187 and the gelada has been found at elevations of up to 5 000 m 16 000 ft in the Ethiopian Highlands 188 Some species interact with aquatic environments and may swim or even dive including the proboscis monkey De Brazza s monkey and Allen s swamp monkey 189 Some primates such as the rhesus macaque and gray langurs can exploit human modified environments and even live in cities 109 190 Interactions between humans and other primates EditDisease transmission Edit Close interactions between humans and non human primates NHPs can create pathways for the transmission of zoonotic diseases Viruses such as Herpesviridae most notably Herpes B Virus Poxviridae measles ebola rabies the Marburg virus and viral hepatitis can be transmitted to humans in some cases the viruses produce potentially fatal diseases in both humans and non human primates 191 Legal and social status Edit Further information Great ape personhood Slow lorises are popular in the exotic pet trade which threatens wild populations Only humans are recognized as persons and protected in law by the United Nations Universal Declaration of Human Rights b The legal status of NHPs on the other hand is the subject of much debate with organizations such as the Great Ape Project GAP campaigning to award at least some of them legal rights 193 In June 2008 Spain became the first country in the world to recognize the rights of some NHPs when its parliament s cross party environmental committee urged the country to comply with GAP s recommendations which are that chimpanzees orangutans and gorillas are not to be used for animal experiments 194 195 Many species of NHP are kept as pets by humans the Allied Effort to Save Other Primates AESOP estimates that around 15 000 NHPs live as exotic pets in the United States 196 The expanding Chinese middle class has increased demand for NHPs as exotic pets in recent years 197 Although NHP import for the pet trade was banned in the U S in 1975 smuggling still occurs along the United States Mexico border with prices ranging from US 3000 for monkeys to 30 000 for apes 198 Primates are used as model organisms in laboratories and have been used in space missions 199 They serve as service animals for disabled humans Capuchin monkeys can be trained to assist quadriplegic humans their intelligence memory and manual dexterity make them ideal helpers 200 NHPs are kept in zoos around the globe Historically zoos were primarily a form of entertainment but more recently have shifted their focus towards conservation education and research GAP does not insist that all NHPs should be released from zoos primarily because captive born primates lack the knowledge and experience to survive in the wild if released 201 Role in scientific research Edit Further information Animal testing on non human primates and International trade in primates Sam a rhesus macaque was flown to the edge of space by NASA in the 1959 Little Joe 2 flight of Project Mercury Thousands of non human primates are used around the world in research because of their psychological and physiological similarity to humans 202 203 In particular the brains and eyes of NHPs more closely parallel human anatomy than those of any other animals NHPs are commonly used in preclinical trials neuroscience ophthalmology studies and toxicity studies Rhesus macaques are often used as are other macaques African green monkeys chimpanzees baboons squirrel monkeys and marmosets both wild caught and purpose bred 202 204 In 2005 GAP reported that 1 280 of the 3 100 NHPs living in captivity in the United States were used for experiments 193 In 2004 the European Union used around 10 000 NHPs in such experiments in 2005 in Great Britain 4 652 experiments were conducted on 3 115 NHPs 205 Governments of many nations have strict care requirements of NHPs kept in captivity In the US federal guidelines extensively regulate aspects of NHP housing feeding enrichment and breeding 206 European groups such as the European Coalition to End Animal Experiments are seeking a ban on all NHP use in experiments as part of the European Union s review of animal testing legislation 207 Extinction threats Edit Humans are known to hunt other primates for food so called bushmeat Pictured are two men who have killed a number of silky sifaka and white headed brown lemurs The International Union for Conservation of Nature IUCN lists more than a third of primates as critically endangered or vulnerable About 60 of primate species are threatened with extinction including 87 of species in Madagascar 73 in Asia 37 in Africa and 36 in South and Central America 208 Additionally 75 of primate species have decreasing populations 208 Trade is regulated as all species are listed by CITES in Appendix II except 50 species and subspecies listed in Appendix I which gain full protection from trade 209 210 Common threats to primate species include deforestation forest fragmentation monkey drives resulting from primate crop raiding 211 and primate hunting for use in medicines as pets and for food Large scale tropical forest clearing is widely regarded as the process that most threatens primates 212 213 214 More than 90 of primate species occur in tropical forests 213 215 The main cause of forest loss is clearing for agriculture although commercial logging subsistence harvesting of timber mining and dam construction also contribute to tropical forest destruction 215 In Indonesia large areas of lowland forest have been cleared to increase palm oil production and one analysis of satellite imagery concluded that during 1998 and 1999 there was a loss of 1 000 Sumatran orangutans per year in the Leuser Ecosystem alone 216 The critically endangered silky sifaka Primates with a large body size over 5 kg are at increased extinction risk due to their greater profitability to poachers compared to smaller primates 215 They reach sexual maturity later and have a longer period between births Populations therefore recover more slowly after being depleted by poaching or the pet trade 217 Data for some African cities show that half of all protein consumed in urban areas comes from the bushmeat trade 218 Endangered primates such as guenons and the drill are hunted at levels that far exceed sustainable levels 218 This is due to their large body size ease of transport and profitability per animal 218 As farming encroaches on forest habitats primates feed on the crops causing the farmers large economic losses 219 Primate crop raiding gives locals a negative impression of primates hindering conservation efforts 220 Madagascar home to five endemic primate families has experienced the greatest extinction of the recent past since human settlement 1 500 years ago at least eight classes and fifteen of the larger species have become extinct due to hunting and habitat destruction 71 Among the primates wiped out were Archaeoindris a lemur larger than a silverback gorilla and the families Palaeopropithecidae and Archaeolemuridae 71 The critically endangered Sumatran orangutan In Asia Hinduism Buddhism and Islam prohibit eating primate meat however primates are still hunted for food 215 Some smaller traditional religions allow the consumption of primate meat 221 222 The pet trade and traditional medicine also increase demand for illegal hunting 197 223 224 The rhesus macaque a model organism was protected after excessive trapping threatened its numbers in the 1960s the program was so effective that they are now viewed as a pest throughout their range 214 In Central and South America forest fragmentation and hunting are the two main problems for primates Large tracts of forest are now rare in Central America 212 225 This increases the amount of forest vulnerable to edge effects such as farmland encroachment lower levels of humidity and a change in plant life 226 227 Movement restriction results in a greater amount of inbreeding which can cause deleterious effects leading to a population bottleneck whereby a significant percentage of the population is lost 228 229 There are 21 critically endangered primates 7 of which have remained on the IUCN s The World s 25 Most Endangered Primates list since the year 2000 the silky sifaka Delacour s langur the white headed langur the gray shanked douc the Tonkin snub nosed monkey the Cross River gorilla and the Sumatran orangutan 230 Miss Waldron s red colobus was recently declared extinct when no trace of the subspecies could be found from 1993 to 1999 231 A few hunters have found and killed individuals since then but the subspecies prospects remain bleak 232 See also Edit Animals portal Mammals portal Primates portalArboreal theory Great Ape Project Human evolution International Primate Day List of primates List of fossil primates Monkey Day PrimatologyFootnotes Edit a b Although the monophyletic relationship between lemurs and lorisoids is widely accepted their clade name is not The term lemuriform is used here because it derives from one popular taxonomy that clumps the clade of toothcombed primates into one infraorder and the extinct non toothcombed adapiforms into another both within the suborder Strepsirrhini 16 17 However another popular alternative taxonomy places the lorisoids in their own infraorder Lorisiformes 15 Article 6 Everyone has the right to recognition everywhere as a person before the law 192 References Edit a b c d e f g h Groves C P 2005 Wilson D E Reeder D M eds Mammal Species of the World A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference 3rd ed Baltimore Johns Hopkins University Press pp 111 184 ISBN 0 801 88221 4 OCLC 62265494 Silcox Mary T Bloch Jonathan I Boyer Doug M Chester Stephen G B Lopez Torres Sergi 2017 The evolutionary radiation of plesiadapiforms Evolutionary Anthropology Issues News and Reviews 26 2 74 94 doi 10 1002 evan 21526 ISSN 1520 6505 PMID 28429568 Primate Merriam Webster Online Dictionary Merriam Webster Retrieved 2008 07 21 The Book of Popular Science 1963 p 257 Chisholm Hugh ed 1911 Ape Encyclopaedia Britannica Vol 02 11th ed Cambridge University Press p 160 Dixson A F 1981 The Natural History of the Gorilla London Weidenfeld amp Nicolson ISBN 978 0 297 77895 0 Definitions of paraphyly vary for the one used here see e g Stace Clive A 2010 Classification by molecules What s in it for field botanists PDF Watsonia 28 103 122 archived from the original PDF on 2011 07 26 retrieved 2010 02 07 Definitions of monophyly vary for the one used here see e g Mishler Brent D 2009 Species are not Uniquely Real Biological Entities in Ayala F J amp Arp R eds Contemporary Debates in Philosophy of Biology pp 110 122 doi 10 1002 9781444314922 ch6 ISBN 978 1 4443 1492 2 a b Cartmill M Smith F H 2011 The Human Lineage John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 1 118 21145 8 a b Groves C P 2001 Primate Taxonomy Smithsonian Institution Press ISBN 1 56098 872 X Benton 2005 p 371 Benton 2005 pp 378 380 a b Mittermeier R Ganzhorn J Konstant W Glander K Tattersall I Groves C Rylands A Hapke A Ratsimbazafy J Mayor M Louis E Rumpler Y Schwitzer C Rasoloarison R December 2008 Lemur Diversity in Madagascar PDF International Journal of Primatology 29 6 1607 1656 doi 10 1007 s10764 008 9317 y S2CID 17614597 Rylands A B amp Mittermeier R A 2009 The Diversity of the New World Primates Platyrrhini In Garber P A Estrada A Bicca Marques J C Heymann E W amp Strier K B eds South American Primates Comparative Perspectives in the Study of Behavior Ecology and Conservation Springer ISBN 978 0 387 78704 6 a b Hartwig 2011 pp 20 21 Szalay amp Delson 1980 p 149 Cartmill 2010 p 15 Linnaeus C 1758 Sistema naturae per regna tria Naturae secundum classes ordines genera species cum characteribus differentiis synonimis locis Tomus I Impensis direct Laurentii Salvii Holmia pp 20 32 Linnaeus C 1735 Sistema naturae sive regna tria Naturae systematice proposita per classes ordines genera amp species apud Theodorum Haak Lugduni Batavorum pp s p Blainville H 1839 Nouvelle classification des Mammiferes Annales Francaises et Etrangeres d Anatomie et de Physiologie Appliquees a la Medicine et a l Histoire Naturelle 3 pp 268 269 Thorington R W amp Anderson S 1984 Primates In Anderson S amp Jones J K eds Orders and Families of Recent Mammals of the World New York John Wiley and Sons pp 187 217 ISBN 978 0 471 08493 8 McKenna M C amp Bell S K 1997 Classification of Mammals Above the species level New York Columbia University Press p 631 ISBN 0 231 11013 8 Strier K 2007 Primate Behavioral Ecology Third ed pp 50 53 ISBN 978 0 205 44432 8 Janecka J E Miller W Pringle T H Wiens F Zitzmann A Helgen K M Springer M S Murphy W J 2 November 2007 Molecular and Genomic Data Identify the Closest Living Relative of Primates Science 318 5851 792 794 Bibcode 2007Sci 318 792J doi 10 1126 science 1147555 PMID 17975064 S2CID 12251814 Kavanagh M 1983 A Complete Guide to Monkeys Apes and Other Primates New York Viking Press pp 18 ISBN 0 670 43543 0 McKenna M C amp Bell S K 1997 Classification of Mammals Above the Species Level New York Columbia University Press p 329 ISBN 0 231 11012 X Williams B A Kay R F Kirk E C 2010 New perspectives on anthropoid origins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 11 4797 4804 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 4797W doi 10 1073 pnas 0908320107 PMC 2841917 PMID 20212104 Stanyon Roscoe Springer Mark S Meredith Robert W Gatesy John Emerling Christopher A Park Jong Rabosky Daniel L Stadler Tanja Steiner Cynthia Ryder Oliver A Janecka Jan E Fisher Colleen A Murphy William J 2012 Macroevolutionary Dynamics and Historical Biogeography of Primate Diversification Inferred from a Species Supermatrix PLOS ONE 7 11 e49521 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 749521S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0049521 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3500307 PMID 23166696 Jameson Natalie M Hou Zhuo Cheng Sterner Kirstin N Weckle Amy Goodman Morris Steiper Michael E Wildman Derek E September 2011 Genomic data reject the hypothesis of a prosimian primate clade Journal of Human Evolution 61 3 295 305 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2011 04 004 ISSN 0047 2484 PMID 21620437 Pozzi Luca Hodgson Jason A Burrell Andrew S Sterner Kirstin N Raaum Ryan L Disotell Todd R June 2014 Primate phylogenetic relationships and divergence dates inferred from complete mitochondrial genomes Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 75 165 183 doi 10 1016 j ympev 2014 02 023 ISSN 1055 7903 PMC 4059600 PMID 24583291 Stanyon Roscoe Finstermeier Knut Zinner Dietmar Brameier Markus Meyer Matthias Kreuz Eva Hofreiter Michael Roos Christian 16 July 2013 A Mitogenomic Phylogeny of Living Primates PLOS ONE 8 7 e69504 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 869504F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0069504 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3713065 PMID 23874967 a b O Leary M A et al 8 February 2013 The placental mammal ancestor and the post K Pg radiation of placentals Science 339 6120 662 667 Bibcode 2013Sci 339 662O doi 10 1126 science 1229237 hdl 11336 7302 PMID 23393258 S2CID 206544776 Wilson Mantilla Gregory P Chester Stephen G B Clemens William A Moore Jason R Sprain Courtney J Hovatter Brody T Mitchell William S Mans Wade W Mundil Roland Renne Paul R 2021 Earliest Palaeocene purgatoriids and the initial radiation of stem primates Royal Society Open Science 8 2 210050 Bibcode 2021RSOS 810050W doi 10 1098 rsos 210050 PMC 8074693 PMID 33972886 Williams B A Kay R F Kirk E C 2010 New perspectives on anthropoid origins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 11 4797 4804 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 4797W doi 10 1073 pnas 0908320107 PMC 2841917 PMID 20212104 Miller E R Gunnell G F Martin R D 2005 Deep Time and the Search for Anthropoid Origins PDF American Journal of Physical Anthropology 128 60 95 doi 10 1002 ajpa 20352 PMID 16369958 a b Chatterjee Helen J Ho Simon Y W Barnes Ian Groves Colin 27 October 2009 Estimating the phylogeny and divergence times of primates using a supermatrix approach BMC Evolutionary Biology 9 1 259 doi 10 1186 1471 2148 9 259 PMC 2774700 PMID 19860891 Lee M September 1999 Molecular Clock Calibrations and Metazoan Divergence Dates Journal of Molecular Evolution 49 3 385 391 Bibcode 1999JMolE 49 385L doi 10 1007 PL00006562 PMID 10473780 S2CID 1629316 Scientists Push Back Primate Origins From 65 Million To 85 Million Years Ago Science Daily Retrieved 2008 10 24 Tavare S Marshall C R Will O Soligo C Martin R D April 18 2002 Using the fossil record to estimate the age of the last common ancestor of extant primates Nature 416 6882 726 729 Bibcode 2002Natur 416 726T doi 10 1038 416726a PMID 11961552 S2CID 4368374 Klonisch T Froehlich C Tetens F Fischer B Hombach Klonisch S 2001 Molecular Remodeling of Members of the Relaxin Family During Primate Evolution Molecular Biology and Evolution 18 3 393 403 doi 10 1093 oxfordjournals molbev a003815 PMID 11230540 a b c d e f Horvath J et al 2008 Development and Application of a Phylogenomic Toolkit Resolving the Evolutionary History of Madagascar s Lemurs Genome Research 18 3 489 499 doi 10 1101 gr 7265208 PMC 2259113 PMID 18245770 a b c d Sellers Bill 2000 10 20 Primate Evolution PDF University of Edinburgh pp 13 17 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 10 29 Retrieved 2008 10 23 a b c d Hartwig W 2007 Primate Evolution In Campbell C Fuentes A MacKinnon K Panger M Bearder S eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press pp 13 17 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Williams B A Kay R F Christopher Kirk E Ross C F 2010 Darwinius masillae is a strepsirrhine a reply to Franzen et al 2009 PDF Journal of Human Evolution 59 5 567 573 discussion 573 9 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2010 01 003 PMID 20188396 Archived from the original PDF on 2013 05 17 Ciochon R amp Fleagle J 1987 Primate Evolution and Human Origins Menlo Park California Benjamin Cummings p 72 ISBN 978 0 202 01175 2 a b Garbutt N 2007 Mammals of Madagascar A Complete Guide A amp C Black Publishers pp 85 86 ISBN 978 0 300 12550 4 a b Mittermeier R A et al 2006 Lemurs of Madagascar 2nd ed Conservation International pp 23 26 ISBN 1 881173 88 7 Shekelle M 2005 Evolutionary Biology of Tarsiers Archived from the original on 2008 09 07 Retrieved 2008 08 22 Schmidt T et al 3 May 2005 Rapid electrostatic evolution at the binding site for cytochrome c on cytochrome c oxidase in anthropoid primates Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 18 6379 6384 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 6379S doi 10 1073 pnas 0409714102 PMC 1088365 PMID 15851671 Wade Lizzie June 5 2013 Early Primate Weighed Less Than an Ounce ScienceNow Archived from the original on 2013 06 08 Retrieved 2013 06 07 Kay R F 2012 Evidence for an Asian origin of stem anthropoid s Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 26 10132 10133 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10910132K doi 10 1073 pnas 1207933109 PMC 3387095 PMID 22699505 Chaimanee Y Chavasseau O Beard K C Kyaw A A Soe A N Sein C Lazzari V Marivaux L Marandat B Swe M Rugbumrung M Lwin T Valentin X Zin Maung Maung Thein Jaeger J J 2012 Late Middle Eocene primate from Myanmar and the initial anthropoid colonization of Africa Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 26 10293 10297 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10910293C doi 10 1073 pnas 1200644109 PMC 3387043 PMID 22665790 Marivaux L et al 2005 06 14 Anthropoid primates from the Oligocene of Pakistan Bugti Hills Data on early anthropoid evolution and biogeography Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102 24 8436 8441 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 8436M doi 10 1073 pnas 0503469102 PMC 1150860 PMID 15937103 Schrago C G amp Russo C A M 2003 Timing the Origin of New World Monkeys PDF Reprint Molecular Biology and Evolution 20 10 1620 1625 doi 10 1093 molbev msg172 PMID 12832653 Houle A 1999 The origin of platyrrhines An evaluation of the Antarctic scenario and the floating island model American Journal of Physical Anthropology 109 4 541 559 doi 10 1002 SICI 1096 8644 199908 109 4 lt 541 AID AJPA9 gt 3 0 CO 2 N PMID 10423268 Andrews P amp Kelley J 2007 Middle Miocene Dispersals of Apes Folia Primatologica 78 5 6 328 343 doi 10 1159 000105148 PMID 17855786 S2CID 19293586 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u Strier K 2007 Primate Behavioral Ecology 3rd ed Allyn amp Bacon pp 7 64 71 77 182 185 273 280 284 287 298 ISBN 978 0 205 44432 8 Pough F W Janis C M Heiser J B 2005 1979 Primate Evolution and the Emergence of Humans Vertebrate Life 7th ed Pearson p 650 ISBN 0 13 127836 3 IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group 1 March 2021 Primate diversity by region International Union for the Conservation of Nature Tenaza R 1984 Songs of hybrid gibbons Hylobates lar H muelleri American Journal of Primatology 8 3 249 253 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350080307 PMID 31986810 S2CID 84957700 a b Bernsteil I S 1966 Naturally occurring primate hybrid Science 154 3756 1559 1560 Bibcode 1966Sci 154 1559B doi 10 1126 science 154 3756 1559 PMID 4958933 S2CID 85898043 Sugawara K January 1979 Sociological study of a wild group of hybrid baboons between Papio anubis and P hamadryas in the Awash Valley Ethiopia Primates 20 1 21 56 doi 10 1007 BF02373827 S2CID 23061688 Jolly C J Woolley Barker Tamsin et al 1997 Intergeneric Hybrid Baboons International Journal of Primatology 18 4 597 627 doi 10 1023 A 1026367307470 S2CID 27900830 Liu Zhen et al 24 January 2018 Cloning of Macaque Monkeys by Somatic Cell Nuclear Transfer Cell 172 4 881 887 e7 doi 10 1016 j cell 2018 01 020 PMID 29395327 Normile Dennis 24 January 2018 These monkey twins are the first primate clones made by the method that developed Dolly Science doi 10 1126 science aat1066 Retrieved 24 January 2018 Cyranoski David 24 January 2018 First monkeys cloned with technique that made Dolly the sheep Chinese scientists create cloned primates that could revolutionize studies of human disease Nature 553 7689 387 388 Bibcode 2018Natur 553 387C doi 10 1038 d41586 018 01027 z PMID 29368720 Briggs Helen 24 January 2018 First monkey clones created in Chinese laboratory BBC News Retrieved 24 January 2018 Scientists Successfully Clone Monkeys Are Humans Up Next The New York Times Associated Press 24 January 2018 Retrieved 24 January 2018 a b c d Pough F W Janis C M Heiser J B 2005 1979 Characteristics of Primates Vertebrate Life 7th ed Pearson p 630 ISBN 0 13 127836 3 a b Aiello L amp Dean C 1990 An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy Academic Press p 193 ISBN 0 12 045590 0 a b c d e f Primate Encyclopaedia Britannica Online Encyclopaedia Britannica Inc 2008 Retrieved 2008 07 21 a b c d e f Myers P 1999 Primates On line Animal Diversity Web Retrieved 2008 06 03 Campbell B G amp Loy J D 2000 Humankind Emerging 8th ed Allyn amp Bacon p 85 ISBN 0 673 52364 0 a b White T amp Kazlev A 2006 01 08 Archonta Primates Palaeos Archived from the original on 2008 05 12 Retrieved 2008 06 03 a b c d e f Macdonald David 2006 Primates The Encyclopedia of Mammals The Brown Reference Group plc pp 282 307 ISBN 0 681 45659 0 Ash M M Nelson S J Wheeler R C 2003 Wheeler s Dental Anatomy Physiology and Occlusion W B Saunders p 12 ISBN 978 0 7216 9382 8 Garber PA Rehg JA November 1999 The ecological role of the prehensile tail in white faced capuchins Cebus capucinus American Journal of Physical Anthropology 110 3 325 39 doi 10 1002 SICI 1096 8644 199911 110 3 lt 325 AID AJPA5 gt 3 0 CO 2 D PMID 10516564 Russo GA Young JW November 2011 Tail growth tracks the ontogeny of prehensile tail use in capuchin monkeys Cebus albifrons and C apella American Journal of Physical Anthropology 146 3 465 73 doi 10 1002 ajpa 21617 PMID 21953012 a b Friderun Ankel Simons 27 July 2010 Primate Anatomy An Introduction Academic Press pp 442 521 ISBN 978 0 08 046911 9 Ralls K 1976 Mammals in Which Females are Larger Than Males The Quarterly Review of Biology 51 2 245 76 doi 10 1086 409310 PMID 785524 S2CID 25927323 Lindstedtand amp Boyce Boyce Mark S July 1985 Seasonality Fasting Endurance and Body Size in Mammals The American Naturalist 125 6 873 doi 10 1086 284385 S2CID 84308684 Frisch J E 1963 Sex differences in the canines of the gibbon Hylobates lar Primates 4 2 1 10 doi 10 1007 BF01659148 S2CID 189798134 Kay R F 1975 The functional adaptations of primate molar teeth American Journal of Physical Anthropology 43 2 195 215 doi 10 1002 ajpa 1330430207 PMID 810034 Crook J H 1972 Sexual selection dimorphism and social organization in the primates In Campbell B G ed Sexual selection and the descent of man Aldine Transaction pp 246 ISBN 978 0 202 02005 1 a b Cheverud J M Dow M M Leutenegger W November 1985 The quantitative assessment of phylogenetic constraints in comparative analyses Sexual dimorphism in body weight among primates Evolution 39 6 1335 1351 doi 10 2307 2408790 JSTOR 2408790 PMID 28564267 Leutenegger W Cheverud J M 1982 Correlates of sexual dimorphism in primates Ecological and size variables International Journal of Primatology 3 4 387 402 doi 10 1007 BF02693740 S2CID 38220186 a b Plavcan J M 2001 Sexual dimorphism in primate evolution American Journal of Physical Anthropology 33 25 53 doi 10 1002 ajpa 10011 PMID 11786990 S2CID 31722173 O Higgins P Collard M 2002 Sexual dimorphism and facial growth in papionine monkeys Journal of Zoology 257 2 255 72 doi 10 1017 S0952836902000857 Sussman R W 1999 Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 1 Lorises Lemurs and Tarsiers Needham Heights MA Pearson Custom Publishing amp Prentice Hall pp 78 89 90 108 121 123 233 ISBN 0 536 02256 9 a b c d e Sussman R W 2003 Primate Ecology and Social Structure Volume 2 New World Monkeys Revised First ed Needham Heights MA Pearson Custom Publishing amp Prentice Hall pp 77 80 132 133 141 143 ISBN 0 536 74364 9 Glazier S D Flowerday C A 2003 Selected Readings in the Anthropology of Religion Theoretical and Methodological Essays Greenwood Publishing Group p 53 ISBN 9780313300905 Arrese C A Oddy Alison Y et al 2005 Cone topography and spectral sensitivity in two potentially trichromatic marsupials the quokka Setonix brachyurus and quenda Isoodon obesulus Proceedings of the Royal Society B 272 1565 791 6 doi 10 1098 rspb 2004 3009 PMC 1599861 PMID 15888411 Bowmaker J K Astell S Hunt D M Mollon J D 1991 Photosensitive and photostable pigments in the retinae of Old World monkeys PDF The Journal of Experimental Biology 156 1 1 19 doi 10 1242 jeb 156 1 1 ISSN 0022 0949 PMID 2051127 Retrieved 2008 06 16 a b Surridge A K amp D Osorio 2003 Evolution and selection of trichromatic vision in primates Trends in Ecology and Evolution 18 4 198 205 doi 10 1016 S0169 5347 03 00012 0 Lucas P W Dominy N J Riba Hernandez P Stoner K E Yamashita N Loria Calderon E Petersen Pereira W Rojas Duran Y Salas Pena R Solis Madrigal S Osorio D Darvell B W 2003 Evolution and function of routine trichromatic vision in primates Evolution 57 11 2636 43 doi 10 1554 03 168 PMID 14686538 S2CID 739130 a b c d e Wrangham R W 1982 Mutualism kinship and social evolution Current Problems in Sociobiology Cambridge University Press pp 269 89 ISBN 0 521 24203 7 Goldberg T L Wrangham R W September 1997 Genetic correlates of social behavior in wild chimpanzees evidence from mitochondrial DNA Animal Behaviour 54 3 559 70 doi 10 1006 anbe 1996 0450 PMID 9299041 S2CID 18223362 Bowdler Neil 21 December 2010 Neanderthal family found cannibalised in cave in Spain BBC News Bowdler Neil 2 June 2011 Ancient cave women left childhood homes BBC News Retrieved 2 June 2011 Copeland Sandi R et al 1 June 2011 Strontium isotope evidence for landscape use by early hominins Nature 474 7349 76 78 doi 10 1038 nature10149 PMID 21637256 S2CID 205225222 Fiore A D amp Campbell C J 2007 The Atelines In Campbell C J Fuentes A MacKinnon K C Panger M amp Bearder S K eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press p 175 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Bartlett T Q 2007 The Hylobatidae In Campbell C J Fuentes A MacKinnon K C Panger M Bearder S K eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press p 283 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Wiens Frank 2002 Behavior and ecology of wild slow lorises Nycticebus coucang social organization infant care system and diet PDF Ph D thesis Bayreuth University pp 31 32 Archived from the original PDF on 9 March 2012 Knott C D amp Kahlenberg S M 2007 Orangutans in Perspective In Campbell C J Fuentes A MacKinnon K C Panger M amp Bearder S K eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press p 294 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Watts D P 1996 Comparative socio ecology of gorillas In McGrew W C Marchant L F Nishida T eds Great Ape Societies Cambridge England Cambridge Univ Press pp 16 28 ISBN 978 0521555364 a b Charpentier MJ Widdig A Alberts SC December 2007 Inbreeding depression in non human primates a historical review of methods used and empirical data American Journal of Primatology 69 12 1370 86 doi 10 1002 ajp 20445 PMID 17486606 S2CID 46626761 Ralls K Ballou J 1982 Effect of inbreeding on infant mortality in captive primates PDF International Journal of Primatology 3 4 491 505 doi 10 1007 BF02693747 S2CID 10954608 Constable J L Ashley M V Goodall J Pusey A E May 2001 Noninvasive paternity assignment in Gombe chimpanzees Molecular Ecology 10 5 1279 300 doi 10 1046 j 1365 294X 2001 01262 x PMID 11380884 S2CID 46604532 a b c d e Rowe N 1996 The Pictorial Guide to the Living Primates Pogonias Press pp 4 139 143 15 185 223 ISBN 0 9648825 0 7 Couzin Iain D Laidre Mark E August 2009 Fission fusion populations Current Biology 19 15 R633 R635 doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 05 034 ISSN 0960 9822 PMID 19674541 S2CID 13549970 a b Pough F W Janis C M Heiser J B 2005 1979 Primate Societies Vertebrate Life 7th ed Pearson pp 621 623 ISBN 0 13 127836 3 Smuts B B Cheney D L Seyfarth R M Wrangham R W amp Struhsaker T T Eds 1987 Primate Societies Chicago University of Chicago Press for articles on the structure and function of various primate societies Shultz S amp Thomsett S 2007 Interactions between African Crowned Eagles and Their Prey Community In McGraw W Zuberbuhler K amp Noe R eds Monkeys of Tai Forest An African Primate Community Cambridge University Press p 181 ISBN 978 0 521 81633 5 a b Bshary R 2007 Interactions between Red Colobus Monkeys and Chimpanzees In McGraw W Zuberbuhler K Noe R eds Monkeys of Tai Forest An African Primate Community Cambridge University Press pp 155 170 ISBN 978 0 521 81633 5 Stanford C 1998 Chimpanzee and Red Colobus the ecology of predator and prey Harvard University Press pp 130 138 233 ISBN 0 674 00722 0 a b Boinski S 2000 Social Manipulation Within and Between Troops Mediates Primate Group Movement In Boinski S Garber P eds On the Move how and why animals travel in groups University of Chicago Press pp 447 448 ISBN 0 226 06340 2 Liman E R Innan H 2003 Relaxed selective pressure on an essential component of pheromone transduction in primate evolution PDF Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100 6 3328 3332 Bibcode 2003PNAS 100 3328L doi 10 1073 pnas 0636123100 PMC 152292 PMID 12631698 Retrieved 2008 07 23 Egnor R Miller C Hauser M D 2004 Nonhuman Primate Communication PDF Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics 2nd ed Elsevier ISBN 0 08 044299 4 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 09 10 a b Pollick A S de Waal F B M 2007 Ape gestures and language evolution Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 19 8184 8189 Bibcode 2007PNAS 104 8184P doi 10 1073 pnas 0702624104 PMC 1876592 PMID 17470779 Burrows A M 2008 The facial expression musculature in primates and its evolutionary significance BioEssays 30 3 212 225 doi 10 1002 bies 20719 PMID 18293360 S2CID 205478149 Geissmann Thomas amp Mutschler Thomas 2006 Diurnal Distribution of Loud Calls in Sympatric Wild Indris Indri indri and Ruffed Lemurs Varecia variegata Implications for Call Functions PDF Primates Journal of Primatology 47 4 393 6 doi 10 1007 s10329 006 0189 5 PMID 16736264 S2CID 1586657 Ramsier M A Cunningham A J Moritz G L Finneran J J Williams C V Ong P S Gursky Doyen S L Dominy N J 2012 Primate communication in the pure ultrasound Biology Letters 8 4 508 511 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2011 1149 PMC 3391437 PMID 22319094 da Cunha R G T Byrne R 2006 Roars of Black Howler Monkeys Alouatta caraya Evidence for a Function in Inter Group Spacing Behaviour 143 10 1169 1199 doi 10 1163 156853906778691568 JSTOR 4536401 Black howler monkey Smithsonian s National Zoo amp Conservation Biology Institute 25 April 2016 Retrieved 2016 07 10 Kelemen G Sade J 1960 The vocal organ of the Howling monkey Alouatta palliata Journal of Morphology 107 2 123 140 doi 10 1002 jmor 1051070202 PMID 13752246 S2CID 30539292 Seyfarth R M Cheney D L Marler Peter 1980 Vervet Monkey Alarm Calls Semantic communication in a Free Ranging Primate Animal Behaviour 28 4 1070 1094 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 80 80097 2 S2CID 53165940 Haimoff E H 1983 Brief report Occurrence of anti resonance in the song of the siamang Hylobates syndactylus American Journal of Primatology 5 3 249 256 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350050309 PMID 31986856 S2CID 85262432 Fitch W T de Boer B Mathur N Ghazanfar A A 2016 Monkey vocal tracts are speech ready Science Advances 2 12 e1600723 Bibcode 2016SciA 2E0723F doi 10 1126 sciadv 1600723 PMC 5148209 PMID 27957536 Boe L J Berthommier F Legou T Captier G Kemp C Sawallis T R et al 2017 Evidence of a Vocalic Proto System in the Baboon Papio papio Suggests Pre Hominin Speech Precursors PLOS ONE 12 1 e0169321 Bibcode 2017PLoSO 1269321B doi 10 1371 journal pone 0169321 PMC 5226677 PMID 28076426 Lameira A R et al 2021 Orangutan information broadcast via consonant like and vowel like calls breaches mathematical models of linguistic evolution Biology Letters 17 9 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2021 0302 PMC 8478518 PMID 34582737 Arcadi AC Aug 2000 Vocal responsiveness in male wild chimpanzees implications for the evolution of language J Hum Evol 39 2 205 23 doi 10 1006 jhev 2000 0415 PMID 10968929 S2CID 7403772 Opie Christopher Atkinson Quentin D Dunbarc Robin I M Shultz Susanne 2013 Male infanticide leads to social monogamy in primates Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110 33 13328 13332 Bibcode 2013PNAS 11013328O doi 10 1073 pnas 1307903110 PMC 3746880 PMID 23898180 De Ruiter Jan R Van Hooff Jan A R A M amp Scheffrahn Wolfgang 1994 Social and genetic aspects of paternity in wild long tailed macaques Macaca fascicularis Behaviour 129 3 4 203 24 doi 10 1163 156853994x00613 JSTOR 4535195 Kappeler Peter M 1998 Nests Tree Holes and the Evolution of Primate Life Histories American Journal of Primatology 46 1 7 33 doi 10 1002 SICI 1098 2345 1998 46 1 lt 7 AID AJP3 gt 3 0 CO 2 PMID 9730211 S2CID 196589387 Ross Caroline 1991 Park or ride Evolution of infant carrying in primates International Journal of Primatology Kluwer Academic Publishing 22 5 749 771 doi 10 1023 A 1012065332758 S2CID 25301078 Mintz Zoe 14 January 2014 Humans And Primates Burn 50 Percent Fewer Calories Each Day Than Other Mammals www ibtimes com IBT Media Inc Retrieved 2014 01 14 Walker ML Herndon JG Herndon 2008 Menopause in nonhuman primates Biology of Reproduction 79 3 398 406 doi 10 1095 biolreprod 108 068536 PMC 2553520 PMID 18495681 Milton K 1993 Diet and Primate Evolution PDF Scientific American Vol 269 no 2 pp 86 93 Bibcode 1993SciAm 269b 86M doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0893 86 PMID 8351513 Pollock J I Mullin R J 1986 Vitamin C biosynthesis in prosimians Evidence for the anthropoid affinity of Tarsius American Journal of Physical Anthropology 73 1 65 70 doi 10 1002 ajpa 1330730106 PMID 3113259 Archived from the original on 2012 06 28 Milliken G W Ward J P Erickson C J 1991 Independent digit control in foraging by the aye aye Daubentonia madagascariensis Folia Primatologica 56 4 219 224 doi 10 1159 000156551 PMID 1937286 Hiller C 2000 Theropithecus gelada Animal Diversity Web Retrieved 2008 08 08 Wright P Simmons E Gursky S 2003 Introduction In Wright P Simmons E Gursky S eds Tarsiers Past Present and Future Rutgers University Press p 1 ISBN 0 8135 3236 1 Goodall Jane 1986 The Chimpanzees of Gombe Patterns of Behavior ISBN 0 674 11649 6 Guernsey Paul WHAT DO CHIMPS EAT All About Wildlife Archived from the original on 2019 11 18 Retrieved 2013 04 22 Ihobe H 1992 Observations on the meat eating behavior of wild bonobos Pan paniscus at Wamba Republic of Zaire Primates 33 2 247 250 doi 10 1007 BF02382754 S2CID 10063791 Rafert J Vineberg E O 1997 Bonobo Nutrition relation of captive diet to wild diet PDF Bonobo Husbandry Manual American Association of Zoos and Aquariums Archived from the original PDF on 2012 04 25 Surbeck M Fowler A Deimel C Hohmann G 2008 Evidence for the consumption of arboreal diurnal primates by bonobos Pan paniscus American Journal of Primatology 71 2 171 4 doi 10 1002 ajp 20634 PMID 19058132 S2CID 32622605 Surbeck M Hohmann G Hohmann 14 October 2008 Primate hunting by bonobos at LuiKotale Salonga National Park Current Biology 18 19 R906 7 doi 10 1016 j cub 2008 08 040 PMID 18957233 S2CID 6708310 Cordain L Eaton SB Sebastian A et al February 2005 Origins and evolution of the Western diet health implications for the 21st century Am J Clin Nutr 81 2 341 54 doi 10 1093 ajcn 81 2 341 PMID 15699220 Ulijaszek SJ November 2002 Human eating behaviour in an evolutionary ecological context Proc Nutr Soc 61 4 517 26 doi 10 1079 PNS2002180 PMID 12691181 Earliest agriculture in the Americas Archived 3 June 2010 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of barley Archived 16 February 2007 at the Wayback Machine Earliest cultivation of figs Archived 2 June 2006 at the Wayback Machine retrieved 19 February 2007 Krebs JR September 2009 The gourmet ape evolution and human food preferences Am J Clin Nutr 90 3 707S 11S doi 10 3945 ajcn 2009 27462B PMID 19656837 Holden C Mace R October 1997 Phylogenetic analysis of the evolution of lactose digestion in adults Hum Biol 69 5 605 28 PMID 9299882 Fichtel Claudia 2012 Predation In Mitani John C Call Josep Kappeler Peter M Palombit Ryne A Silk Joan B eds The Evolution of Primate Societies University of Chicago Press pp 169 84 ISBN 978 0 226 53172 4 Boesch C Boesch H 1990 Tool Use and Tool Making in Wild Chimpanzees Folia Primatologica 54 1 2 86 99 doi 10 1159 000156428 PMID 2157651 Westergaard G C Lundquist A L et al 1998 Why some capuchin monkeys Cebus apella use probing tools and others do not Journal of Comparative Psychology 112 2 207 211 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 112 2 207 PMID 9642788 de Waal F B M Davis J M 2003 Capuchin cognitive ecology cooperation based on projected returns Neuropsychologia 41 2 221 228 doi 10 1016 S0028 3932 02 00152 5 PMID 12459220 S2CID 8190458 Paar L A Winslow J T Hopkins W D de Waal F B M 2000 Recognizing facial cues Individual discrimination by chimpanzees Pan troglodytes and rhesus monkeys Macaca mulatta Journal of Comparative Psychology 114 1 47 60 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 114 1 47 PMC 2018744 PMID 10739311 Byrne Richard Corp Nadia 2004 Neocortex size predicts deception rate in primates Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 271 1549 1693 1699 doi 10 1098 rspb 2004 2780 PMC 1691785 PMID 15306289 Paar L A de Waal F B M 1999 Visual kin recognition in chimpanzees Nature 399 6737 647 648 Bibcode 1999Natur 399 647P doi 10 1038 21345 PMID 10385114 S2CID 4424086 Fujita K Watanabe K Widarto T H Suryobroto B 1997 Discrimination of macaques by macaques The case of sulawesi species Primates 38 3 233 245 doi 10 1007 BF02381612 S2CID 21042762 Call J 2001 Object permanence in orangutans Pongo pygmaeus chimpanzees Pan troglodytes and children Homo sapiens Journal of Comparative Psychology 115 2 159 171 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 115 2 159 PMID 11459163 Itakura S Tanaka M June 1998 Use of experimenter given cues during object choice tasks by chimpanzees Pan troglodytes an orangutan Pongo pygmaeus and human infants Homo sapiens Journal of Comparative Psychology 112 2 119 126 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 112 2 119 PMID 9642782 Gouteux S Thinus Blanc C Vauclair J 2001 Rhesus monkeys use geometric and nongeometric information during a reorientation task PDF Journal of Experimental Psychology General 130 3 505 519 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 130 3 505 PMID 11561924 Tomasello M amp Call J 1997 Primate Cognition Oxford University Press US ISBN 978 0 19 510624 4 a b c Deaner R O van Schaik C P Johnson V E 2006 Do some taxa have better domain general cognition than others A metaanalysis of nonhuman primate studies Evolutionary Psychology 4 149 196 doi 10 1177 147470490600400114 S2CID 16702785 a b c Reader S M Hager Y Laland K N 2011 The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence PDF Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366 1567 1017 1027 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0342 PMC 3049098 PMID 21357224 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 10 03 Retrieved 2011 07 04 a b Toolmaking The Jane Goodall Institute Retrieved 2013 08 01 Bonobos ApeTag 2010 Archived from the original on 2013 11 02 Retrieved 2013 08 03 Gruber T Clay Z Zuberbuhler K 2010 A comparison of bonobo and chimpanzee tool use evidence for a female bias in the Pan lineage PDF Animal Behaviour 80 6 1023 1033 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2010 09 005 S2CID 14923158 Bower B 18 April 2011 Orangutans use simple tools to catch fish Wired Retrieved 2013 08 05 Breuer T Ndoundou Hockemba M Fishlock V 2005 First observation of tool use in wild gorillas PLOS Biology 3 11 e380 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0030380 PMC 1236726 PMID 16187795 Boinski S 1988 Use of a club by a wild white faced capuchin Cebus capucinus to attack a venomous snake Bothrops asper American Journal of Primatology 14 2 177 179 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350140208 PMID 31973450 S2CID 84653622 Fragaszy D Izar P Visalberghi E Ottoni E B de Oliveira M G 2004 Wild capuchin monkeys Cebus libidinosus use anvils and stone pounding tools American Journal of Primatology 64 4 359 366 doi 10 1002 ajp 20085 PMID 15580579 S2CID 16222308 Gumert M D Kluck M Malaivijitnond S 2009 The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand American Journal of Primatology 71 7 594 608 doi 10 1002 ajp 20694 PMID 19405083 S2CID 22384150 Hamilton W J Buskirk R E Buskirk W H 1975 Defensive stoning by baboons Nature 256 5517 488 489 Bibcode 1975Natur 256 488H doi 10 1038 256488a0 S2CID 4149862 Fichtel C Kappeler P M 2010 Chapter 19 Human universals and primate symplesiomorphies Establishing the lemur baseline In Kappeler P M Silk J B eds Mind the Gap Tracing the Origins of Human Universals Springer ISBN 978 3 642 02724 6 Sugiyama Y 1995 Drinking tools of wild chimpanzees at Bossou American Journal of Primatology 37 1 263 269 doi 10 1002 ajp 1350370308 PMID 31936951 S2CID 86473603 Sumatran orangutans OrangutanIslands com Archived from the original on 2013 11 26 Retrieved 2013 08 02 van Schaik C Fox E Sitompul A 1996 Manufacture and use of tools in wild Sumatran orangutans Naturwissenschaften 83 4 186 188 Bibcode 1996NW 83 186V doi 10 1007 BF01143062 PMID 8643126 S2CID 27180148 Gill Victoria 22 July 2011 Mandrill monkey makes pedicuring tool BBC Retrieved 2013 08 11 Vancatova M 2008 Gorillas and Tools Part I Retrieved 2013 08 04 Cowlishaw G Clutton Brock T 2009 Primates In MacDonald D ed The Princeton Encyclopedia of Mammals Princeton and Oxford University Press pp 270 280 ISBN 978 0 691 14069 8 Reed K Fleagle J August 15 1995 Geographic and climatic control of primate diversity Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 92 17 7874 7876 Bibcode 1995PNAS 92 7874R doi 10 1073 pnas 92 17 7874 PMC 41248 PMID 7644506 Chapman C amp Russo S 2007 Primate Seed Dispersal In Campbell C J Fuentes A MacKinnon K C Panger M amp Bearder S K eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press p 510 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Long Y C Kirkpatrick R C Zhong T Xiao L April 1994 Report on the distribution population and ecology of the Yunnan snub nosed monkey Rhinopithecus bieti Primates 35 2 241 250 doi 10 1007 BF02382060 S2CID 23931368 Schaller G B 1963 The Mountain Gorilla Ecology and Behavior Chicago University Chicago Press ISBN 978 0 226 73635 8 Stammbach E 1987 Desert Forest and Montane Baboons Multilevel Societies In Smuts B Cheney D Seyfarth R Wrangham R Struhsaker T eds Primate Societies The University of Chicago Press pp 112 120 ISBN 978 0226767161 Kemp E 2009 Patterns of Water Use in Primates Folia Primatologica 80 4 275 294 doi 10 1159 000252586 PMID 19864919 S2CID 5108827 Wolfe L D amp Fuentes A 2007 Ethnoprimatology In Campbell C J Fuentes A MacKinnon K C Panger M amp Bearder S K eds Primates in Perspective Oxford University Press p 692 ISBN 978 0 19 517133 4 Renquist D M amp Whitney R A 1987 Zoonoses Acquired from Pet Primates Veterinary Clinics of North America Small Animal Practice 17 1 219 240 doi 10 1016 s0195 5616 87 50614 3 PMID 3551307 Retrieved 2008 08 11 The Universal Declaration of Human Rights United Nations 1948 Archived from the original on 2014 12 08 Retrieved 2008 12 02 a b Cavalieri P amp Singer P Declaration on Great Apes Great Ape Project Archived from the original on 2008 08 20 Retrieved 2008 06 16 Glendinning L 26 June 2008 Spanish parliament approves human rights for apes The Guardian Retrieved 2008 11 10 Singer P 18 July 2008 Of great apes and men The Guardian Retrieved 2008 11 10 Mott M 16 September 2003 The Perils of Keeping Monkeys as Pets National Geographic Archived from the original on 2013 06 08 Retrieved 2013 02 06 a b Workman C June 2004 Primate conservation in Vietnam toward a holistic environmental narrative American Anthropologist 106 2 346 352 doi 10 1525 aa 2004 106 2 346 IPPL News The US Pet Monkey Trade International Primate Protection League 2003 Archived from the original on 2008 07 25 Retrieved 2008 08 04 Bushnell D 1958 The Beginnings of Research in Space Biology at the Air Force Missile Development Center 1946 1952 History of Research in Space Biology and Biodynamics NASA Retrieved 2008 08 18 Blumenthal D 1987 06 17 Monkeys as Helpers To Quadriplegics At Home The New York Times Retrieved 2008 10 08 Newman James L 2013 Encountering Gorillas A Chronicle of Discovery Exploitation Understanding and Survival Plymouth United Kingdom Rowman and Littlefield p 173 ISBN 978 1 4422 1957 1 a b The supply and use of primates in the EU European Biomedical Research Association 1996 Archived from the original on 2012 01 17 Retrieved 2008 08 18 Chen F C Li W H February 2001 Genomic divergences between humans and other hominoids and the effective population size of the common ancestor of humans and chimpanzees American Journal of Human Genetics 68 2 444 456 doi 10 1086 318206 PMC 1235277 PMID 11170892 Conlee K M Hoffeld E H Stephens M L 2004 A Demographic Analysis of Primate Research in the United States Alternatives to Laboratory Animals 32 Sup 1 315 322 doi 10 1177 026119290403201s52 PMID 23577480 S2CID 6743973 Presented to Parliament by the Secretary of State for the Home Department by Command of Her Majesty July 2006 Statistics of scientific procedures on living animals Great Britain 2005 PDF The Stationery Office ISBN 0 10 168772 9 Retrieved 2008 06 16 Nonhuman Primates Research Animals Animal Welfare Information Center United States Department of Agriculture Archived from the original on 2008 10 03 Retrieved 2008 07 14 Directive 86 609 European Coalition to End Animal Experiments Archived from the original on 2008 09 29 Retrieved 2008 10 08 a b Estrada Alejandro Garber Paul A Rylands Anthony B Roos Christian Fernandez Duque Eduardo Fiore Anthony Di Nekaris K Anne Isola Nijman Vincent Heymann Eckhard W 2017 01 01 Impending extinction crisis of the world s primates Why primates matter Science Advances 3 1 e1600946 Bibcode 2017SciA 3E0946E doi 10 1126 sciadv 1600946 ISSN 2375 2548 PMC 5242557 PMID 28116351 IFAW 2005 Born to be wild Primates are not pets PDF International Fund for Animal Welfare Archived from the original PDF on 2011 07 26 Retrieved 2011 02 26 CITES 2010 10 14 Appendices I II and III Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of Wild Fauna and Flora Retrieved 2012 04 02 Grubb P 1998 The Sierra Leone monkey drives Mammals of Ghana Sierra Leone and the Gambia St Ives Trendrine pp 214 219 ISBN 0 9512562 4 6 a b Chapman C A Peres C A 2001 Primate conservation in the new millennium the role of scientists Evolutionary Anthropology 10 1 16 33 doi 10 1002 1520 6505 2001 10 1 lt 16 AID EVAN1010 gt 3 0 CO 2 O S2CID 13494366 a b Mittermeier R A amp Cheney D L 1987 Conservation of primates and their habitats In Smuts B B Cheney D L Seyfarth R M Wrangham R W amp Struhsaker T T eds Primate Societies Chicago University of Chicago Press pp 477 490 a b Southwick C H amp Siddiqi M F 2001 Status conservation and management of primates in India PDF Envis Bulletin Wildlife and Protected Areas 1 1 81 91 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 10 01 Retrieved 2008 08 04 a b c d Cowlishaw G amp Dunbar R 2000 Primate Conservation Biology Chicago University of Chicago Press ISBN 978 0 226 11637 2 Van Schaik C P Monk K A Robertson J M Y 2001 Dramatic decline in orangutan numbers in the Leuser Ecosystem northern Sumatra Oryx 35 1 14 25 doi 10 1046 j 1365 3008 2001 00150 x Purvis A Gittleman J L Cowlishaw G Mace G M 2000 Predicting extinction risk in declining species Proceedings of the Royal Society B 267 1456 1947 1952 doi 10 1098 rspb 2000 1234 PMC 1690772 PMID 11075706 a b c Fa J E Juste J Perez de Val J Castroviejo J 1995 Impact of market hunting on mammal species in Equatorial Guinea Conservation Biology 9 5 1107 1115 doi 10 1046 j 1523 1739 1995 9051107 x hdl 10261 49187 PMID 34261280 Hill C M 1997 Crop raiding by wild vertebrates The farmer s perspective in an agricultural community in western Uganda International Journal of Pest Management 43 1 77 84 doi 10 1080 096708797229022 Hill C M 2002 Primate conservation and local communities Ethical issues and debates American Anthropologist 104 4 1184 1194 doi 10 1525 aa 2002 104 4 1184 Choudhury A 2001 Primates in Northeast India an overview of their distribution and conservation status PDF Envis Bulletin Wildlife and Protected Areas 1 1 92 101 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 10 01 Retrieved 2008 08 04 Kumara H N Singh M October 2004 Distribution and abundance of primates in rainforests of the Western Ghats Karnataka India and the conservation of Macaca silenus International Journal of Primatology 25 5 1001 1018 doi 10 1023 B IJOP 0000043348 06255 7f S2CID 30384142 Nijman V 2004 Conservation of the Javan gibbon Hylobates moloch population estimates local extinction and conservation priorities PDF The Raffles Bulletin of Zoology 52 1 271 280 Archived from the original PDF on 2008 09 10 Retrieved 2008 08 04 O Brien T G Kinnaird M F Nurcahyo A Iqbal M Rusmanto M April 2004 Abundance and distribution of sympatric gibbons in a threatened Sumatran rain forest International Journal of Primatology 25 2 267 284 doi 10 1023 B IJOP 0000019152 83883 1c S2CID 32472118 Estrada A Coates Estrada R Meritt D September 1994 Non flying mammals and landscape changes in the tropical forest region of Los Tuxtlas Mexico Ecography 17 3 229 241 doi 10 1111 j 1600 0587 1994 tb00098 x Marsh L K 2003 The nature of fragmentation In Marsh L K ed Primates in Fragments Ecology and Conservation New York Kluwer Academic Plenum Publishers pp 1 10 ISBN 0 306 47696 7 Turner I M 1996 Species loss in fragments of tropical rain forest a review of the evidence Journal of Applied Ecology 33 2 200 209 doi 10 2307 2404743 JSTOR 2404743 Chiarello A G 2003 Primates of the Brazilian Atlantic forest the influence of forest fragmentation on survival In Marsh L K ed Primates in Fragments Ecology and Conservation New York Kluwer Academic Plenum Publishers pp 99 121 ISBN 978 0 306 47696 9 Pope T R 1996 Socioecology population fragmentation and patterns of genetic loss in endangered primates In Avise J Hamrick J eds Conservation Genetics Case Histories from Nature Norwell Kluwer Academic Publishers pp 119 159 ISBN 978 0 412 05581 2 Mittermeier R A Wallis J Rylands A B Ganzhorn J U Oates J F Williamson E A Palacios E Heymann E W Kierulff M C M Yongcheng L Supriatna J Roos C Walker S Cortes Ortiz L Schwitzer C eds 2009 Primates in Peril The World s 25 Most Endangered Primates 2008 2010 PDF Illustrated by S D Nash Arlington VA IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group PSG International Primatological Society IPS and Conservation International CI pp 23 26 ISBN 978 1 934151 34 1 span, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.