fbpx
Wikipedia

Behavioral modernity

Behavioral modernity is a suite of behavioral and cognitive traits that distinguishes current Homo sapiens from other anatomically modern humans, hominins, and primates.[1] Most scholars agree that modern human behavior can be characterized by abstract thinking, planning depth, symbolic behavior (e.g., art, ornamentation), music and dance, exploitation of large game, and blade technology, among others.[2][3][need quotation to verify] Underlying these behaviors and technological innovations are cognitive and cultural foundations that have been documented experimentally and ethnographically by evolutionary and cultural anthropologists. These human universal patterns include cumulative cultural adaptation, social norms, language, and extensive help and cooperation beyond close kin.[4][5]

Upper Paleolithic (16,000-year-old) cave painting from Lascaux cave in France

Within the tradition of evolutionary anthropology and related disciplines, it has been argued that the development of these modern behavioral traits, in combination with the climatic conditions of the Last Glacial Period and Last Glacial Maximum causing population bottlenecks, contributed to the evolutionary success of Homo sapiens worldwide relative to Neanderthals, Denisovans, and other archaic humans.[3][6][need quotation to verify]

Arising from differences in the archaeological record, debate continues as to whether anatomically modern humans were behaviorally modern as well. There are many theories on the evolution of behavioral modernity. These generally fall into two camps: gradualist and cognitive approaches. The Later Upper Paleolithic Model theorises that modern human behavior arose through cognitive, genetic changes in Africa abruptly around 40,000–50,000 years ago around the time of the Out-of-Africa migration, prompting the movement of modern humans out of Africa and across the world.[7] Other models focus on how modern human behavior may have arisen through gradual steps, with the archaeological signatures of such behavior appearing only through demographic or subsistence-based changes. Many cite evidence of behavioral modernity earlier (by at least about 150,000–75,000 years ago and possibly earlier) namely in the African Middle Stone Age.[8][3][9][10][11] Sally McBrearty and Alison S. Brooks are notable proponents of gradualism, challenging European-centric models by situating more change in the Middle Stone Age of African pre-history, though this version of the story is more difficult to develop in concrete terms due to a thinning fossil record as one goes further back in time.

Definition

To classify what should be included in modern human behavior, it is necessary to define behaviors that are universal among living human groups. Some examples of these human universals are abstract thought, planning, trade, cooperative labor, body decoration, and the control and use of fire. Along with these traits, humans possess much reliance on social learning.[12][13] This cumulative cultural change or cultural "ratchet" separates human culture from social learning in animals. In addition, a reliance on social learning may be responsible in part for humans' rapid adaptation to many environments outside of Africa. Since cultural universals are found in all cultures including some of the most isolated indigenous groups, these traits must have evolved or have been invented in Africa prior to the exodus.[14][15][16]

Archaeologically, a number of empirical traits have been used as indicators of modern human behavior. While these are often debated[17] a few are generally agreed upon. Archaeological evidence of behavioral modernity includes:[3][7]

Critiques

Several critiques have been placed against the traditional concept of behavioral modernity, both methodologically and philosophically.[3][17] Shea (2011) outlines a variety of problems with this concept, arguing instead for "behavioral variability", which, according to the author, better describes the archaeological record. The use of trait lists, according to Shea (2011), runs the risk of taphonomic bias, where some sites may yield more artifacts than others despite similar populations; as well, trait lists can be ambiguous in how behaviors may be empirically recognized in the archaeological record.[17] Shea (2011) in particular cautions that population pressure, cultural change, or optimality models, like those in human behavioral ecology, might better predict changes in tool types or subsistence strategies than a change from "archaic" to "modern" behavior.[17] Some researchers argue that a greater emphasis should be placed on identifying only those artifacts which are unquestionably, or purely, symbolic as a metric for modern human behavior.[3]

Theories and models

Late Upper Paleolithic Model or "Upper Paleolithic Revolution"

The Late Upper Paleolithic Model, or Upper Paleolithic Revolution, refers to the idea that, though anatomically modern humans first appear around 150,000 years ago (as was once believed), they were not cognitively or behaviorally "modern" until around 50,000 years ago, leading to their expansion out of Africa and into Europe and Asia.[7][18][19] These authors note that traits used as a metric for behavioral modernity do not appear as a package until around 40–50,000 years ago. Klein (1995) specifically describes evidence of fishing, bone shaped as a tool, hearths, significant artifact diversity, and elaborate graves are all absent before this point.[7][18] According to these authors, art only becomes common beyond this switching point, signifying a change from archaic to modern humans.[7] Most researchers argue that a neurological or genetic change, perhaps one enabling complex language, such as FOXP2, caused this revolutionary change in humans.[7][19] The role of FOXP2 as a driver of evolutionary selection has been called into question following recent research results. [20]

Building on the FOXP2 gene hypothesis, the late cognitive scientist Philip Lieberman has argued that proto-language behaviour has existed prior to 50,000 BP but in a more primitive form. Lieberman demonstrates, with fossil evidence; examining neck and throat dimensions, that so-called “anatomically modern” humans from 100,000 BP continued to evolve their SVT (supralaryngeal vocal tract) which already possessed a horizontal portion (SVTh) capable of producing many phonemes which were mostly consonants. Neanderthals and early Homo Sapiens would have been able to communicate using sounds and gestures.

From 100,000 BP Homo Sapiens necks continued to lengthen to the point by 50,000 BP where Homo Sapiens necks were long enough to accommodate a vertical portion to their SVT (SVTv) which is now a universal trait among humans. This SVTv enabled the enunciation of quantal vowels: [i], [u], and [a]. These quantal vowels could then be immediately put to use by the already sophisticated neuro-motor-control features of the FOXP2 gene to generate more nuanced sounds and in effect increase by orders of magnitude the number of distinct sounds that can be produced allowing for fully symbolic language.[21]

Goody (1986) draws an analogy between the development of spoken language and that of writing: the shift from pictographic or ideographic symbols into a fully abstract logographic writing system (such as hieroglyphics), or from a logoprahic system into an abjad or alphabet, led to dramatic changes in human civilization.[22]

Alternative models

Contrasted with this view of a spontaneous leap in cognition among ancient humans, some authors like Alison S. Brooks, primarily working in African archaeology, point to the gradual accumulation of "modern" behaviors, starting well before the 50,000-year benchmark of the Upper Paleolithic Revolution models.[8][3][23] Howiesons Poort, Blombos, and other South African archaeological sites, for example, show evidence of marine resource acquisition, trade, the making of bone tools, blade and microlith technology, and abstract ornamentation at least by 80,000 years ago.[8][9] Given evidence from Africa and the Middle East, a variety of hypotheses have been put forth to describe an earlier, gradual transition from simple to more complex human behavior. Some authors have pushed back the appearance of fully modern behavior to around 80,000 years ago or earlier in order to incorporate the South African data.[23]

Others focus on the slow accumulation of different technologies and behaviors across time. These researchers describe how anatomically modern humans could have been cognitively the same, and what we define as behavioral modernity is just the result of thousands of years of cultural adaptation and learning.[8][3] D'Errico and others have looked at Neanderthal culture, rather than early human behavior exclusively, for clues into behavioral modernity.[6] Noting that Neanderthal assemblages often portray traits similar to those listed for modern human behavior, researchers stress that the foundations for behavioral modernity may in fact, lie deeper in our hominin ancestors.[24] If both modern humans and Neanderthals express abstract art and complex tools then "modern human behavior" cannot be a derived trait for our species. They argue that the original "human revolution" theory reflects a profound Eurocentric bias. Recent archaeological evidence, they argue, proves that humans evolving in Africa some 300,000 or even 400,000 years ago were already becoming cognitively and behaviourally "modern". These features include blade and microlithic technology, bone tools, increased geographic range, specialized hunting, the use of aquatic resources, long-distance trade, systematic processing and use of pigment, and art and decoration. These items do not occur suddenly together as predicted by the "human revolution" model, but at sites that are widely separated in space and time. This suggests a gradual assembling of the package of modern human behaviours in Africa, and its later export to other regions of the Old World.

Between these extremes is the view – currently supported by archaeologists Chris Henshilwood,[25] Curtis Marean,[3] Ian Watts[26] and others – that there was indeed some kind of 'human revolution' but that it occurred in Africa and spanned tens of thousands of years. The term "revolution" in this context would mean not a sudden mutation but a historical development along the lines of "the industrial revolution" or "the Neolithic revolution".[27] In other words, it was a relatively accelerated process, too rapid for ordinary Darwinian "descent with modification" yet too gradual to be attributed to a single genetic or other sudden event. These archaeologists point in particular to the relatively explosive emergence of ochre crayons and shell necklaces, apparently used for cosmetic purposes. These archaeologists see symbolic organisation of human social life as the key transition in modern human evolution. Recently discovered at sites such as Blombos Cave and Pinnacle Point, South Africa, pierced shells, pigments and other striking signs of personal ornamentation have been dated within a time-window of 70,000–160,000 years ago in the African Middle Stone Age, suggesting that the emergence of Homo sapiens coincided, after all, with the transition to modern cognition and behaviour.[28] While viewing the emergence of language as a 'revolutionary' development, this school of thought generally attributes it to cumulative social, cognitive and cultural evolutionary processes as opposed to a single genetic mutation.[29]

A further view, taken by archaeologists such as Francesco D'Errico[24] and João Zilhão,[30] is a multi-species perspective arguing that evidence for symbolic culture in the form of utilised pigments and pierced shells are also found in Neanderthal sites, independently of any "modern" human influence.

Cultural evolutionary models may also shed light on why although evidence of behavioral modernity exists before 50,000 years ago, it is not expressed consistently until that point. With small population sizes, human groups would have been affected by demographic and cultural evolutionary forces that may not have allowed for complex cultural traits.[10][11][12][13] According to some authors,[10] until population density became significantly high, complex traits could not have been maintained effectively. Some genetic evidence supports a dramatic increase in population size before human migration out of Africa.[19] High local extinction rates within a population also can significantly decrease the amount of diversity in neutral cultural traits, regardless of cognitive ability.[11]

Highly speculatively, bicameral mind theory argues for an additional, and cultural rather than genetic, shift from selfless to self-perceiving forms of human cognition and behavior very late in human history, in the Bronze Age. This is based on a literary analysis of Bronze Age texts which claims to show the first appearances of the concept of self around this time, replacing the voices of gods as the primary form of recorded human cognition.[31] This non-mainstream theory is not widely accepted but does receive serious academic interest from time to time.[32]

Archaeological evidence

Africa

Research from 2017 indicates that Homo sapiens originated in Africa between around 350,000 and 260,000 years ago.[33][34][35][36] There is some evidence for the beginning of modern behavior among early African H. sapiens around that period.[37][38][39][40]

Before the Out of Africa theory was generally accepted, there was no consensus on where the human species evolved and, consequently, where modern human behavior arose. Now, however, African archaeology has become extremely important in discovering the origins of humanity. The first Cro-Magnon expansion into Europe around 48,000 years ago is generally accepted as already "modern",[18] and it is now generally believed that behavioral modernity appeared in Africa before 50,000 years ago, either significantly earlier, or possibly as a late Upper Paleolithic "revolution" soon before which prompted migration out of Africa.

A variety of evidence of abstract imagery, widened subsistence strategies, and other "modern" behaviors have been discovered in Africa, especially South, North, and East Africa. The Blombos Cave site in South Africa, for example, is famous for rectangular slabs of ochre engraved with geometric designs. Using multiple dating techniques, the site was dated to be around 77,000 and 100,000 to 75,000 years old.[25][41] Ostrich egg shell containers engraved with geometric designs dating to 60,000 years ago were found at Diepkloof, South Africa.[42] Beads and other personal ornamentation have been found from Morocco which might be as much as 130,000 years old; as well, the Cave of Hearths in South Africa has yielded a number of beads dating from significantly prior to 50,000 years ago,[8] and shell beads dating to about 75,000 years ago have been found at Blombos Cave, South Africa.[43][44][45]

Specialized projectile weapons as well have been found at various sites in Middle Stone Age Africa, including bone and stone arrowheads at South African sites such as Sibudu Cave (along with an early bone needle also found at Sibudu) dating approximately 72,000-60,000 years ago[46][47][48][49][50] on some of which poisons may have been used,[51] and bone harpoons at the Central African site of Katanda dating to about 90,000 years ago.[52] Evidence also exists for the systematic heat treating of silcrete stone to increase its flake-ability for the purpose of toolmaking, beginning approximately 164,000 years ago at the South African site of Pinnacle Point and becoming common there for the creation of microlithic tools at about 72,000 years ago.[53][54]

In 2008, an ochre processing workshop likely for the production of paints was uncovered dating to c. 100,000 years ago at Blombos Cave, South Africa. Analysis shows that a liquefied pigment-rich mixture was produced and stored in the two abalone shells, and that ochre, bone, charcoal, grindstones, and hammer-stones also formed a composite part of the toolkits. Evidence for the complexity of the task includes procuring and combining raw materials from various sources (implying they had a mental template of the process they would follow), possibly using pyrotechnology to facilitate fat extraction from bone, using a probable recipe to produce the compound, and the use of shell containers for mixing and storage for later use.[55][56][57] Modern behaviors, such as the making of shell beads, bone tools and arrows, and the use of ochre pigment, are evident at a Kenyan site by 78,000-67,000 years ago.[58] Evidence of early stone-tipped projectile weapons (a characteristic tool of Homo sapiens), the stone tips of javelins or throwing spears, were discovered in 2013 at the Ethiopian site of Gademotta, and date to around 279,000 years ago.[37]

Expanding subsistence strategies beyond big-game hunting and the consequential diversity in tool types has been noted as signs of behavioral modernity. A number of South African sites have shown an early reliance on aquatic resources from fish to shellfish. Pinnacle Point, in particular, shows exploitation of marine resources as early as 120,000 years ago, perhaps in response to more arid conditions inland.[9] Establishing a reliance on predictable shellfish deposits, for example, could reduce mobility and facilitate complex social systems and symbolic behavior. Blombos Cave and Site 440 in Sudan both show evidence of fishing as well. Taphonomic change in fish skeletons from Blombos Cave have been interpreted as capture of live fish, clearly an intentional human behavior.[8]

Humans in North Africa (Nazlet Sabaha, Egypt) are known to have dabbled in chert mining, as early as ≈100,000 years ago, for the construction of stone tools.[59][60]

Evidence was found in 2018, dating to about 320,000 years ago, at the Kenyan site of Olorgesailie, of the early emergence of modern behaviors including: long-distance trade networks (involving goods such as obsidian), the use of pigments, and the possible making of projectile points. It is observed by the authors of three 2018 studies on the site that the evidence of these behaviors is approximately contemporary to the earliest known Homo sapiens fossil remains from Africa (such as at Jebel Irhoud and Florisbad), and they suggest that complex and modern behaviors had already begun in Africa around the time of the emergence of anatomically modern Homo sapiens.[38][39][40]

In 2019, further evidence of early complex projectile weapons in Africa was found at Aduma, Ethiopia, dated 100,000-80,000 years ago, in the form of points considered likely to belong to darts delivered by spear throwers.[61]

Olduvai Hominid 1 wore facial piercings.[62]

Europe

While traditionally described as evidence for the later Upper Paleolithic Model,[7] European archaeology has shown that the issue is more complex. A variety of stone tool technologies are present at the time of human expansion into Europe and show evidence of modern behavior. Despite the problems of conflating specific tools with cultural groups, the Aurignacian tool complex, for example, is generally taken as a purely modern human signature.[63][64] The discovery of "transitional" complexes, like "proto-Aurignacian", have been taken as evidence of human groups progressing through "steps of innovation".[63] If, as this might suggest, human groups were already migrating into eastern Europe around 40,000 years and only afterward show evidence of behavioral modernity, then either the cognitive change must have diffused back into Africa or was already present before migration.

In light of a growing body of evidence of Neanderthal culture and tool complexes some researchers have put forth a "multiple species model" for behavioral modernity.[6][24][65] Neanderthals were often cited as being an evolutionary dead-end, apish cousins who were less advanced than their human contemporaries. Personal ornaments were relegated as trinkets or poor imitations compared to the cave art produced by H. sapiens. Despite this, European evidence has shown a variety of personal ornaments and artistic artifacts produced by Neanderthals; for example, the Neanderthal site of Grotte du Renne has produced grooved bear, wolf, and fox incisors, ochre and other symbolic artifacts.[65] Although burials are few and controversial, there has been circumstantial evidence of Neanderthal ritual burials.[24] There are two options to describe this symbolic behavior among Neanderthals: they copied cultural traits from arriving modern humans or they had their own cultural traditions comparative with behavioral modernity. If they just copied cultural traditions, which is debated by several authors,[6][24] they still possessed the capacity for complex culture described by behavioral modernity. As discussed above, if Neanderthals also were "behaviorally modern" then it cannot be a species-specific derived trait.

Asia

Most debates surrounding behavioral modernity have been focused on Africa or Europe but an increasing amount of focus has been placed on East Asia. This region offers a unique opportunity to test hypotheses of multi-regionalism, replacement, and demographic effects.[66] Unlike Europe, where initial migration occurred around 50,000 years ago, human remains have been dated in China to around 100,000 years ago.[67] This early evidence of human expansion calls into question behavioral modernity as an impetus for migration.

Stone tool technology is particularly of interest in East Asia. Following Homo erectus migrations out of Africa, Acheulean technology never seems to appear beyond present-day India and into China. Analogously, Mode 3, or Levallois technology, is not apparent in China following later hominin dispersals.[68] This lack of more advanced technology has been explained by serial founder effects and low population densities out of Africa.[69] Although tool complexes comparative to Europe are missing or fragmentary, other archaeological evidence shows behavioral modernity. For example, the peopling of the Japanese archipelago offers an opportunity to investigate the early use of watercraft. Although one site, Kanedori in Honshu, does suggest the use of watercraft as early as 84,000 years ago, there is no other evidence of hominins in Japan until 50,000 years ago.[66]

The Zhoukoudian cave system near Beijing has been excavated since the 1930s and has yielded precious data on early human behavior in East Asia. Although disputed, there is evidence of possible human burials and interred remains in the cave dated to around 34–20,000 years ago.[66] These remains have associated personal ornaments in the form of beads and worked shell, suggesting symbolic behavior. Along with possible burials, numerous other symbolic objects like punctured animal teeth and beads, some dyed in red ochre, have all been found at Zhoukoudian.[66] Although fragmentary, the archaeological record of eastern Asia shows evidence of behavioral modernity before 50,000 years ago but, like the African record, it is not fully apparent until that time.

See also

References

  1. ^ Korisettar, Ravi (1998). Early Human Behaviour in Global Context. Routledge.
  2. ^ McBrearty, Sally; Brooks, Allison (2000). "The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior". Journal of Human Evolution. 39 (5): 453–563. doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0435. PMID 11102266. Proponents of the model known as the 'human revolution' claim that modern human behaviors arose suddenly, and nearly simultaneously, throughout the Old World ca. 40–50 ka. [...] In fact, many of the components of the 'human revolution' claimed to appear at 40–50 ka are found in the African Middle Stone Age tens of thousands of years earlier. These features include blade and microlithic technology, bone tools, increased geographic range, specialized hunting, the use of aquatic resources, long distance trade, systematic processing and use of pigment, and art and decoration.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h i Henshilwood, Christopher; Marean, Curtis (2003). "The Origin of Modern Human Behavior: Critique of the Models and Their Test Implications". Current Anthropology. 44 (5): 627–651. doi:10.1086/377665. PMID 14971366. S2CID 11081605.
  4. ^ Hill, Kim; et al. (2009). "The Emergence of Human Uniqueness: Characters Underlying Behavioral Modernity". Evolutionary Anthropology. 18 (5): 187–200. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.469.5702. doi:10.1002/evan.20224. S2CID 56384790.
  5. ^ Klein, R. G. 1999. The human career: human biological and cultural origins. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  6. ^ a b c d D'Errico, F; et al. (1998). "Neanderthal Acculturation in Western Europe? A Critical Review of the Evidence and Its Interpretation". Current Anthropology. 39 (S1): S1–S44. doi:10.1086/204689. S2CID 144799519.
  7. ^ a b c d e f g Klein, Richard (1995). "Anatomy, behavior, and modern human origins". Journal of World Prehistory. 9 (2): 167–198. doi:10.1007/bf02221838. S2CID 10402296.
  8. ^ a b c d e f McBrearty, Sally; Brooks, Allison (2000). "The revolution that wasn't: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior". Journal of Human Evolution. 39 (5): 453–563. doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0435. PMID 11102266.
  9. ^ a b c Marean, Curtis; et al. (2007). "Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene" (PDF). Nature. 449 (7164): 905–908. Bibcode:2007Natur.449..905M. doi:10.1038/nature06204. PMID 17943129. S2CID 4387442.
  10. ^ a b c Powell, Adam; et al. (2009). "Late Pleistocene Demography and the Appearance of Modern Human Behavior" (PDF). Science. 324 (5932): 1298–1301. Bibcode:2009Sci...324.1298P. doi:10.1126/science.1170165. PMID 19498164. S2CID 206518315.
  11. ^ a b c Premo, Luke; Kuhn, Steve (2010). "Modeling Effects of Local Extinctions on Culture Change and Diversity in the Paleolithic". PLOS ONE. 5 (12): e15582. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...515582P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0015582. PMC 3003693. PMID 21179418.
  12. ^ a b Boyd, Robert; Richerson, Peter (1988). Culture and the Evolutionary Process (2 ed.). University of Chicago Press. ISBN 9780226069333.
  13. ^ a b Nakahashi, Wataru (2013). "Evolution of improvement and cumulative culture". Theoretical Population Biology. 83: 30–38. doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2012.11.001. PMID 23153511.
  14. ^ Buller, David (2005). Adapting Minds: Evolutionary Psychology and the Persistent Quest for Human Nature. PMIT Press. p. 468. ISBN 978-0-262-02579-9.
  15. ^ "80,000-year-old Beads Shed Light on Early Culture". Livescience.com. June 18, 2007. Retrieved September 10, 2009.
  16. ^ "three distinct human populations". Accessexcellence.org. Retrieved September 10, 2009.
  17. ^ a b c d Shea, John (2011). "Homo sapiens Is as Homo sapiens Was". Current Anthropology. 52 (1): 1–35. doi:10.1086/658067. S2CID 142517998.
  18. ^ a b c Hoffecker, John (2009). "The spread of modern humans in Europe". PNAS. 106 (38): 16040–16045. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10616040H. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903446106. PMC 2752585. PMID 19571003.
  19. ^ a b c Tattersall, Ian (2009). "Human origins: Out of Africa". PNAS. 106 (38): 16018–16021. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10616018T. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903207106. PMC 2752574. PMID 19805256.
  20. ^ "Language Gene Dethroned". The Scientist Magazine®. Retrieved January 23, 2023.
  21. ^ Lieberman, Philip (2007). "The Evolution of Human Speech: Its Anatomical and Neural Bases". Current Anthropology. 48 (1): 39–66. doi:10.1086/509092. ISSN 0011-3204.
  22. ^ Goody, Jack (1986). The Logic of Writing and the Organization of Society. Studies in Literacy, the Family, Culture and the State. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-33962-9.
  23. ^ a b Foley, Robert; Lahr, Marta (1997). "Mode 3 Technologies and the Evolution of Modern Humans". Cambridge Archaeological Journal. 7 (1): 3–36. doi:10.1017/s0959774300001451. S2CID 163040120.
  24. ^ a b c d e D'Errico, Francesco (2003). "The Invisible Frontier A Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioral Modernity". Evolutionary Anthropology. 12 (4): 188–202. doi:10.1002/evan.10113. S2CID 1904963.
  25. ^ a b Henshilwood, Christopher; et al. (2002). "Emergence of Modern Human Behavior: Middle Stone Age Engravings from South Africa". Science. 295 (5558): 1278–1280. Bibcode:2002Sci...295.1278H. doi:10.1126/science.1067575. PMID 11786608. S2CID 31169551.
  26. ^ Watts, I. 2009. Red ochre, body painting, and language: interpreting the Blombos ochre. In R. Botha and C. Knight (eds), The Cradle of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 62-92.
  27. ^ Mellars, P. A., K. Boyle, O. Bar-Yosef and C. Stringer (eds), 2007. Rethinking the Human Revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.
  28. ^ Henshilwood, C. S. and B. Dubreuil 2009. Reading the artifacts: gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in Southern Africa. In R. Botha and C. Knight (eds), The Cradle of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 41-61.
  29. ^ Botha, R. and C. Knight (eds), The Cradle of Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  30. ^ Zilhão, J (2006). "Neandertals and moderns mixed, and it matters". Evolutionary Anthropology. 15 (5): 183–195. doi:10.1002/evan.20110. S2CID 18565967.
  31. ^ Jaynes, Julian (2000) [1976]. The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind. Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 0-618-05707-2.
  32. ^ Damasio, Antonio (2010). Self Comes to Mind. New York: Pantheon Books. p. 289. ISBN 978-0-307-37875-0. …I sympathize with Julian Jaynes's claim that something of great import may have happened to the human mind during the relatively brief interval of time between the events narrated in the Iliad and those that make up the Odyssey.
  33. ^ Schlebusch, Carina M; Malmström, Helena; Günther, Torsten; Sjödin, Per; Coutinho, Alexandra; Edlund, Hanna; Munters, Arielle R; Vicente, Mário; Steyn, Maryna; Soodyall, Himla; Lombard, Marlize; Jakobsson, Mattias (2017). "Southern African ancient genomes estimate modern human divergence to 350,000 to 260,000 years ago". Science. 358 (6363): 652–655. Bibcode:2017Sci...358..652S. doi:10.1126/science.aao6266. PMID 28971970.
  34. ^ Sample, Ian (June 7, 2017). "Oldest Homo sapiens bones ever found shake foundations of the human story". The Guardian. Retrieved June 7, 2017.
  35. ^ Zimmer, Carl (September 10, 2019). "Scientists Find the Skull of Humanity's Ancestor — on a Computer - By comparing fossils and CT scans, researchers say they have reconstructed the skull of the last common forebear of modern humans". The New York Times. Retrieved September 10, 2019.
  36. ^ Mounier, Aurélien; Lahr, Marta (2019). "Deciphering African late middle Pleistocene hominin diversity and the origin of our species". Nature Communications. 10 (1): 3406. Bibcode:2019NatCo..10.3406M. doi:10.1038/s41467-019-11213-w. PMC 6736881. PMID 31506422.
  37. ^ a b Sahle, Y.; Hutchings, W. K.; Braun, D. R.; Sealy, J. C.; Morgan, L. E.; Negash, A.; Atnafu, B. (2013). Petraglia, Michael D (ed.). "Earliest Stone-Tipped Projectiles from the Ethiopian Rift Date to >279,000 Years Ago". PLOS ONE. 8 (11): e78092. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...878092S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0078092. PMC 3827237. PMID 24236011.
  38. ^ a b Chatterjee, Rhitu (March 15, 2018). "Scientists Are Amazed By Stone Age Tools They Dug Up In Kenya". NPR. Retrieved March 15, 2018.
  39. ^ a b Yong, Ed (March 15, 2018). "A Cultural Leap at the Dawn of Humanity - New finds from Kenya suggest that humans used long-distance trade networks, sophisticated tools, and symbolic pigments right from the dawn of our species". The Atlantic. Retrieved March 15, 2018.
  40. ^ a b Brooks AS, Yellen JE, Potts R, Behrensmeyer AK, Deino AL, Leslie DE, Ambrose SH, Ferguson JR, d'Errico F, Zipkin AM, Whittaker S, Post J, Veatch EG, Foecke K, Clark JB (2018). "Long-distance stone transport and pigment use in the earliest Middle Stone Age". Science. 360 (6384): 90–94. Bibcode:2018Sci...360...90B. doi:10.1126/science.aao2646. PMID 29545508.
  41. ^ Henshilwood, Christopher S.; d'Errico, Francesco; Watts, Ian (2009). "Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa". Journal of Human Evolution. 57 (1): 27–47. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.01.005. PMID 19487016.
  42. ^ Texier, PJ; Porraz, G; Parkington, J; Rigaud, JP; Poggenpoel, C; Miller, C; Tribolo, C; Cartwright, C; Coudenneau, A; Klein, R; Steele, T; Verna, C (2010). "A Howiesons Poort tradition of engraving ostrich eggshell containers dated to 60,000 years ago at Diepkloof Rock Shelter, South Africa". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107 (14): 6180–6185. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.6180T. doi:10.1073/pnas.0913047107. PMC 2851956. PMID 20194764.
  43. ^ Henshilwood, Christopher S.; et al. (2004). "Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa". Science. 304 (5669): 404. doi:10.1126/science.1095905. PMID 15087540. S2CID 32356688.
  44. ^ d'Errico, Francesco; et al. (2005). "Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age". Journal of Human Evolution. 48 (1): 3–24. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.09.002. PMID 15656934.
  45. ^ Vanhaeren, Marian; et al. (2013). "Thinking strings: Additional evidence for personal ornament use in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa". Journal of Human Evolution. 64 (6): 500–517. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.02.001. PMID 23498114.
  46. ^ Backwell, L; d'Errico, F; Wadley, L (2008). "Middle Stone Age bone tools from the Howiesons Poort layers, Sibudu Cave, South Africa". Journal of Archaeological Science. 35 (6): 1566–1580. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2007.11.006.
  47. ^ Wadley, Lyn (2008). "The Howieson's Poort industry of Sibudu Cave". South African Archaeological Society Goodwin Series. 10.
  48. ^ Lombard M, Phillips L (2010). "Indications of bow and stone-tipped arrow use 64,000 years ago in KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa". Antiquity. 84 (325): 635–648. doi:10.1017/S0003598X00100134. S2CID 162438490.
  49. ^ Lombard M (2011). "Quartz-tipped arrows older than 60 ka: further use-trace evidence from Sibudu, Kwa-Zulu-Natal, South Africa". Journal of Archaeological Science. 38 (8): 1918–1930. doi:10.1016/j.jas.2011.04.001.
  50. ^ Backwell, L; Bradfield, J; Carlson, KJ; Jashashvili, T; Wadley, L; d'Errico, F (2018). "The antiquity of bow-and-arrow technology: evidence from Middle Stone Age layers at Sibudu Cave". Journal of Archaeological Science. 92 (362): 289–303. doi:10.15184/aqy.2018.11.
  51. ^ Lombard M (2020). "The tip cross-sectional areas of poisoned bone arrowheads from southern Africa". Journal of Archaeological Science: Reports. 33: 102477. doi:10.1016/j.jasrep.2020.102477. S2CID 224889105.
  52. ^ Yellen, JE; AS Brooks; E Cornelissen; MJ Mehlman; K Stewart (April 28, 1995). "A middle stone age worked bone industry from Katanda, Upper Semliki Valley, Zaire". Science. 268 (5210): 553–556. Bibcode:1995Sci...268..553Y. doi:10.1126/science.7725100. PMID 7725100.
  53. ^ Brown, Kyle S.; Marean, Curtis W.; Herries, Andy I.R.; Jacobs, Zenobia; Tribolo, Chantal; Braun, David; Roberts, David L.; Meyer, Michael C.; Bernatchez, J. (August 14, 2009), "Fire as an Engineering Tool of Early Modern Humans", Science, 325 (5942): 859–862, Bibcode:2009Sci...325..859B, doi:10.1126/science.1175028, hdl:11422/11102, PMID 19679810, S2CID 43916405
  54. ^ Brown, Kyle S.; Marean, Curtis W.; Jacobs, Zenobia; Schoville, Benjamin J.; Oestmo, Simen; Fisher, Erich C.; Bernatchez, Jocelyn; Karkanas, Panagiotis; Matthews, Thalassa (2012). "An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71,000 years ago in South Africa". Nature. 491 (7425): 590–3. Bibcode:2012Natur.491..590B. doi:10.1038/nature11660. PMID 23135405. S2CID 4323569.
  55. ^ Amos, Jonathan (October 13, 2011). "A Cultural Leap at the Dawn of Humanity - Ancient 'paint factory' unearthed". BBC News. Retrieved October 13, 2011.
  56. ^ Vastag, Brian (October 13, 2011). "South African cave yields paint from dawn of humanity". The Washington Post. Retrieved October 13, 2011.
  57. ^ Henshilwood, Christopher S.; et al. (2011). "A 100,000-Year-Old Ochre-Processing Workshop at Blombos Cave, South Africa". Science. 334 (6053): 219–222. Bibcode:2011Sci...334..219H. doi:10.1126/science.1211535. PMID 21998386. S2CID 40455940.
  58. ^ Shipton C, d'Errico F, Petraglia M, et al. (2018). 78,000-year-old record of Middle and Later Stone Age innovation in an East African tropical forest. Nature Communications
  59. ^ . Archived from the original on January 5, 2019. Retrieved February 20, 2018.
  60. ^ Guinness World Records (September 10, 2015). Guinness World Records 2016. Guinness World Records. p. 27. ISBN 978-1-910561-03-4.
  61. ^ Sahle Y, Brooks AS (2019). "Assessment of complex projectiles in the early Late Pleistocene at Aduma, Ethiopia". PLOS ONE. 14 (5): e0216716. Bibcode:2019PLoSO..1416716S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0216716. PMC 6508696. PMID 31071181.
  62. ^ Willman, J. C.; Hernando, R.; Matu, M.; Crevecoeur, I. (2020). "Biocultural diversity in Late Pleistocene/Early Holocene Africa: Olduvai Hominid 1 (Tanzania) biological affinity and intentional body modification" (PDF). American Journal of Physical Anthropology. 172 (4): 664–681. doi:10.1002/ajpa.24007. PMID 31944279. S2CID 210331198.
  63. ^ a b Joris, Olaf; Street, Martin (2008). "At the end of the 14C time scaledthe Middle to Upper Paleolithic record of western Eurasia". Journal of Human Evolution. 55 (5): 782–802. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.04.002. PMID 18930513.
  64. ^ Anikovich, M.; et al. (2007). "Early Upper Paleolithic in Eastern Europe and Implications for the Dispersal of Modern Humans". Science. 315 (5809): 223–226. Bibcode:2007Sci...315..223A. doi:10.1126/science.1133376. PMID 17218523. S2CID 21428180.
  65. ^ a b Abadia, Oscar Moro; Gonzalez Morales, Manuel R. (2010). "Redefining Neanderthals and Art: An Alternative Interpretation of the Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioural Modernity". Oxford Journal of Archaeology. 29 (3): 229–243. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0092.2010.00346.x.
  66. ^ a b c d Norton, Christopher; Jin, Jennie (2009). "The Evolution of Modern Human Behavior in East Asia: Current Perspectives". Evolutionary Anthropology. 18 (6): 247–260. doi:10.1002/evan.20235. S2CID 54836302.
  67. ^ Liu, Wu; et al. (2010). "Human remains from Zhirendong, South China, and modern human emergence in East Asia". PNAS. 107 (45): 19201–19206. Bibcode:2010PNAS..10719201L. doi:10.1073/pnas.1014386107. PMC 2984215. PMID 20974952.
  68. ^ Norton, Christopher; Bae, K. (2008). "The Movius Line sensu lato (Norton et al. 2006) further assessed and defined". Journal of Human Evolution. 55 (6): 1148–1150. doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.08.003. PMID 18809202.
  69. ^ Lycett, Stephen; Norton, Christopher (2010). "A demographic model for Palaeolithic technological evolution: The case of East Asia and the Movius Line". Quaternary International. 211 (1–2): 55–65. Bibcode:2010QuInt.211...55L. doi:10.1016/j.quaint.2008.12.001.

External links

  • Steven Mithen (1999), The Prehistory of the Mind: The Cognitive Origins of Art, Religion and Science, Thames & Hudson, ISBN 978-0-500-28100-0.
  • Artifacts in Africa Suggest An Earlier Modern Human
  • Tools point to African origin for human behaviour
  • Key Human Traits Tied to Shellfish Remains, nytimes 2007/10/18
  • "Python Cave" Reveals Oldest Human Ritual, Scientists Suggest
  • Human Timeline (Interactive) – Smithsonian, National Museum of Natural History (August 2016).

behavioral, modernity, suite, behavioral, cognitive, traits, that, distinguishes, current, homo, sapiens, from, other, anatomically, modern, humans, hominins, primates, most, scholars, agree, that, modern, human, behavior, characterized, abstract, thinking, pl. Behavioral modernity is a suite of behavioral and cognitive traits that distinguishes current Homo sapiens from other anatomically modern humans hominins and primates 1 Most scholars agree that modern human behavior can be characterized by abstract thinking planning depth symbolic behavior e g art ornamentation music and dance exploitation of large game and blade technology among others 2 3 need quotation to verify Underlying these behaviors and technological innovations are cognitive and cultural foundations that have been documented experimentally and ethnographically by evolutionary and cultural anthropologists These human universal patterns include cumulative cultural adaptation social norms language and extensive help and cooperation beyond close kin 4 5 Upper Paleolithic 16 000 year old cave painting from Lascaux cave in France Within the tradition of evolutionary anthropology and related disciplines it has been argued that the development of these modern behavioral traits in combination with the climatic conditions of the Last Glacial Period and Last Glacial Maximum causing population bottlenecks contributed to the evolutionary success of Homo sapiens worldwide relative to Neanderthals Denisovans and other archaic humans 3 6 need quotation to verify Arising from differences in the archaeological record debate continues as to whether anatomically modern humans were behaviorally modern as well There are many theories on the evolution of behavioral modernity These generally fall into two camps gradualist and cognitive approaches The Later Upper Paleolithic Model theorises that modern human behavior arose through cognitive genetic changes in Africa abruptly around 40 000 50 000 years ago around the time of the Out of Africa migration prompting the movement of modern humans out of Africa and across the world 7 Other models focus on how modern human behavior may have arisen through gradual steps with the archaeological signatures of such behavior appearing only through demographic or subsistence based changes Many cite evidence of behavioral modernity earlier by at least about 150 000 75 000 years ago and possibly earlier namely in the African Middle Stone Age 8 3 9 10 11 Sally McBrearty and Alison S Brooks are notable proponents of gradualism challenging European centric models by situating more change in the Middle Stone Age of African pre history though this version of the story is more difficult to develop in concrete terms due to a thinning fossil record as one goes further back in time Contents 1 Definition 1 1 Critiques 2 Theories and models 2 1 Late Upper Paleolithic Model or Upper Paleolithic Revolution 2 2 Alternative models 3 Archaeological evidence 3 1 Africa 3 2 Europe 3 3 Asia 4 See also 5 References 6 External linksDefinition EditTo classify what should be included in modern human behavior it is necessary to define behaviors that are universal among living human groups Some examples of these human universals are abstract thought planning trade cooperative labor body decoration and the control and use of fire Along with these traits humans possess much reliance on social learning 12 13 This cumulative cultural change or cultural ratchet separates human culture from social learning in animals In addition a reliance on social learning may be responsible in part for humans rapid adaptation to many environments outside of Africa Since cultural universals are found in all cultures including some of the most isolated indigenous groups these traits must have evolved or have been invented in Africa prior to the exodus 14 15 16 Archaeologically a number of empirical traits have been used as indicators of modern human behavior While these are often debated 17 a few are generally agreed upon Archaeological evidence of behavioral modernity includes 3 7 Burial Fishing Figurative art cave paintings petroglyphs dendroglyphs figurines Systematic use of pigment such as ochre and jewelry for decoration or self ornamentation Using bone material for tools Transport of resources over long distances Blade technology Diversity standardization and regionally distinct artifacts Hearths Composite toolsCritiques Edit Several critiques have been placed against the traditional concept of behavioral modernity both methodologically and philosophically 3 17 Shea 2011 outlines a variety of problems with this concept arguing instead for behavioral variability which according to the author better describes the archaeological record The use of trait lists according to Shea 2011 runs the risk of taphonomic bias where some sites may yield more artifacts than others despite similar populations as well trait lists can be ambiguous in how behaviors may be empirically recognized in the archaeological record 17 Shea 2011 in particular cautions that population pressure cultural change or optimality models like those in human behavioral ecology might better predict changes in tool types or subsistence strategies than a change from archaic to modern behavior 17 Some researchers argue that a greater emphasis should be placed on identifying only those artifacts which are unquestionably or purely symbolic as a metric for modern human behavior 3 Theories and models EditLate Upper Paleolithic Model or Upper Paleolithic Revolution Edit The Late Upper Paleolithic Model or Upper Paleolithic Revolution refers to the idea that though anatomically modern humans first appear around 150 000 years ago as was once believed they were not cognitively or behaviorally modern until around 50 000 years ago leading to their expansion out of Africa and into Europe and Asia 7 18 19 These authors note that traits used as a metric for behavioral modernity do not appear as a package until around 40 50 000 years ago Klein 1995 specifically describes evidence of fishing bone shaped as a tool hearths significant artifact diversity and elaborate graves are all absent before this point 7 18 According to these authors art only becomes common beyond this switching point signifying a change from archaic to modern humans 7 Most researchers argue that a neurological or genetic change perhaps one enabling complex language such as FOXP2 caused this revolutionary change in humans 7 19 The role of FOXP2 as a driver of evolutionary selection has been called into question following recent research results 20 Building on the FOXP2 gene hypothesis the late cognitive scientist Philip Lieberman has argued that proto language behaviour has existed prior to 50 000 BP but in a more primitive form Lieberman demonstrates with fossil evidence examining neck and throat dimensions that so called anatomically modern humans from 100 000 BP continued to evolve their SVT supralaryngeal vocal tract which already possessed a horizontal portion SVTh capable of producing many phonemes which were mostly consonants Neanderthals and early Homo Sapiens would have been able to communicate using sounds and gestures From 100 000 BP Homo Sapiens necks continued to lengthen to the point by 50 000 BP where Homo Sapiens necks were long enough to accommodate a vertical portion to their SVT SVTv which is now a universal trait among humans This SVTv enabled the enunciation of quantal vowels i u and a These quantal vowels could then be immediately put to use by the already sophisticated neuro motor control features of the FOXP2 gene to generate more nuanced sounds and in effect increase by orders of magnitude the number of distinct sounds that can be produced allowing for fully symbolic language 21 Goody 1986 draws an analogy between the development of spoken language and that of writing the shift from pictographic or ideographic symbols into a fully abstract logographic writing system such as hieroglyphics or from a logoprahic system into an abjad or alphabet led to dramatic changes in human civilization 22 Alternative models Edit Contrasted with this view of a spontaneous leap in cognition among ancient humans some authors like Alison S Brooks primarily working in African archaeology point to the gradual accumulation of modern behaviors starting well before the 50 000 year benchmark of the Upper Paleolithic Revolution models 8 3 23 Howiesons Poort Blombos and other South African archaeological sites for example show evidence of marine resource acquisition trade the making of bone tools blade and microlith technology and abstract ornamentation at least by 80 000 years ago 8 9 Given evidence from Africa and the Middle East a variety of hypotheses have been put forth to describe an earlier gradual transition from simple to more complex human behavior Some authors have pushed back the appearance of fully modern behavior to around 80 000 years ago or earlier in order to incorporate the South African data 23 Others focus on the slow accumulation of different technologies and behaviors across time These researchers describe how anatomically modern humans could have been cognitively the same and what we define as behavioral modernity is just the result of thousands of years of cultural adaptation and learning 8 3 D Errico and others have looked at Neanderthal culture rather than early human behavior exclusively for clues into behavioral modernity 6 Noting that Neanderthal assemblages often portray traits similar to those listed for modern human behavior researchers stress that the foundations for behavioral modernity may in fact lie deeper in our hominin ancestors 24 If both modern humans and Neanderthals express abstract art and complex tools then modern human behavior cannot be a derived trait for our species They argue that the original human revolution theory reflects a profound Eurocentric bias Recent archaeological evidence they argue proves that humans evolving in Africa some 300 000 or even 400 000 years ago were already becoming cognitively and behaviourally modern These features include blade and microlithic technology bone tools increased geographic range specialized hunting the use of aquatic resources long distance trade systematic processing and use of pigment and art and decoration These items do not occur suddenly together as predicted by the human revolution model but at sites that are widely separated in space and time This suggests a gradual assembling of the package of modern human behaviours in Africa and its later export to other regions of the Old World Between these extremes is the view currently supported by archaeologists Chris Henshilwood 25 Curtis Marean 3 Ian Watts 26 and others that there was indeed some kind of human revolution but that it occurred in Africa and spanned tens of thousands of years The term revolution in this context would mean not a sudden mutation but a historical development along the lines of the industrial revolution or the Neolithic revolution 27 In other words it was a relatively accelerated process too rapid for ordinary Darwinian descent with modification yet too gradual to be attributed to a single genetic or other sudden event These archaeologists point in particular to the relatively explosive emergence of ochre crayons and shell necklaces apparently used for cosmetic purposes These archaeologists see symbolic organisation of human social life as the key transition in modern human evolution Recently discovered at sites such as Blombos Cave and Pinnacle Point South Africa pierced shells pigments and other striking signs of personal ornamentation have been dated within a time window of 70 000 160 000 years ago in the African Middle Stone Age suggesting that the emergence of Homo sapiens coincided after all with the transition to modern cognition and behaviour 28 While viewing the emergence of language as a revolutionary development this school of thought generally attributes it to cumulative social cognitive and cultural evolutionary processes as opposed to a single genetic mutation 29 A further view taken by archaeologists such as Francesco D Errico 24 and Joao Zilhao 30 is a multi species perspective arguing that evidence for symbolic culture in the form of utilised pigments and pierced shells are also found in Neanderthal sites independently of any modern human influence Cultural evolutionary models may also shed light on why although evidence of behavioral modernity exists before 50 000 years ago it is not expressed consistently until that point With small population sizes human groups would have been affected by demographic and cultural evolutionary forces that may not have allowed for complex cultural traits 10 11 12 13 According to some authors 10 until population density became significantly high complex traits could not have been maintained effectively Some genetic evidence supports a dramatic increase in population size before human migration out of Africa 19 High local extinction rates within a population also can significantly decrease the amount of diversity in neutral cultural traits regardless of cognitive ability 11 Highly speculatively bicameral mind theory argues for an additional and cultural rather than genetic shift from selfless to self perceiving forms of human cognition and behavior very late in human history in the Bronze Age This is based on a literary analysis of Bronze Age texts which claims to show the first appearances of the concept of self around this time replacing the voices of gods as the primary form of recorded human cognition 31 This non mainstream theory is not widely accepted but does receive serious academic interest from time to time 32 Archaeological evidence EditAfrica Edit See also Early expansions of hominins out of Africa Early human migrations and Recent African origin of modern humans Research from 2017 indicates that Homo sapiens originated in Africa between around 350 000 and 260 000 years ago 33 34 35 36 There is some evidence for the beginning of modern behavior among early African H sapiens around that period 37 38 39 40 Before the Out of Africa theory was generally accepted there was no consensus on where the human species evolved and consequently where modern human behavior arose Now however African archaeology has become extremely important in discovering the origins of humanity The first Cro Magnon expansion into Europe around 48 000 years ago is generally accepted as already modern 18 and it is now generally believed that behavioral modernity appeared in Africa before 50 000 years ago either significantly earlier or possibly as a late Upper Paleolithic revolution soon before which prompted migration out of Africa A variety of evidence of abstract imagery widened subsistence strategies and other modern behaviors have been discovered in Africa especially South North and East Africa The Blombos Cave site in South Africa for example is famous for rectangular slabs of ochre engraved with geometric designs Using multiple dating techniques the site was dated to be around 77 000 and 100 000 to 75 000 years old 25 41 Ostrich egg shell containers engraved with geometric designs dating to 60 000 years ago were found at Diepkloof South Africa 42 Beads and other personal ornamentation have been found from Morocco which might be as much as 130 000 years old as well the Cave of Hearths in South Africa has yielded a number of beads dating from significantly prior to 50 000 years ago 8 and shell beads dating to about 75 000 years ago have been found at Blombos Cave South Africa 43 44 45 Specialized projectile weapons as well have been found at various sites in Middle Stone Age Africa including bone and stone arrowheads at South African sites such as Sibudu Cave along with an early bone needle also found at Sibudu dating approximately 72 000 60 000 years ago 46 47 48 49 50 on some of which poisons may have been used 51 and bone harpoons at the Central African site of Katanda dating to about 90 000 years ago 52 Evidence also exists for the systematic heat treating of silcrete stone to increase its flake ability for the purpose of toolmaking beginning approximately 164 000 years ago at the South African site of Pinnacle Point and becoming common there for the creation of microlithic tools at about 72 000 years ago 53 54 In 2008 an ochre processing workshop likely for the production of paints was uncovered dating to c 100 000 years ago at Blombos Cave South Africa Analysis shows that a liquefied pigment rich mixture was produced and stored in the two abalone shells and that ochre bone charcoal grindstones and hammer stones also formed a composite part of the toolkits Evidence for the complexity of the task includes procuring and combining raw materials from various sources implying they had a mental template of the process they would follow possibly using pyrotechnology to facilitate fat extraction from bone using a probable recipe to produce the compound and the use of shell containers for mixing and storage for later use 55 56 57 Modern behaviors such as the making of shell beads bone tools and arrows and the use of ochre pigment are evident at a Kenyan site by 78 000 67 000 years ago 58 Evidence of early stone tipped projectile weapons a characteristic tool of Homo sapiens the stone tips of javelins or throwing spears were discovered in 2013 at the Ethiopian site of Gademotta and date to around 279 000 years ago 37 Expanding subsistence strategies beyond big game hunting and the consequential diversity in tool types has been noted as signs of behavioral modernity A number of South African sites have shown an early reliance on aquatic resources from fish to shellfish Pinnacle Point in particular shows exploitation of marine resources as early as 120 000 years ago perhaps in response to more arid conditions inland 9 Establishing a reliance on predictable shellfish deposits for example could reduce mobility and facilitate complex social systems and symbolic behavior Blombos Cave and Site 440 in Sudan both show evidence of fishing as well Taphonomic change in fish skeletons from Blombos Cave have been interpreted as capture of live fish clearly an intentional human behavior 8 Humans in North Africa Nazlet Sabaha Egypt are known to have dabbled in chert mining as early as 100 000 years ago for the construction of stone tools 59 60 Evidence was found in 2018 dating to about 320 000 years ago at the Kenyan site of Olorgesailie of the early emergence of modern behaviors including long distance trade networks involving goods such as obsidian the use of pigments and the possible making of projectile points It is observed by the authors of three 2018 studies on the site that the evidence of these behaviors is approximately contemporary to the earliest known Homo sapiens fossil remains from Africa such as at Jebel Irhoud and Florisbad and they suggest that complex and modern behaviors had already begun in Africa around the time of the emergence of anatomically modern Homo sapiens 38 39 40 In 2019 further evidence of early complex projectile weapons in Africa was found at Aduma Ethiopia dated 100 000 80 000 years ago in the form of points considered likely to belong to darts delivered by spear throwers 61 Olduvai Hominid 1 wore facial piercings 62 Europe Edit While traditionally described as evidence for the later Upper Paleolithic Model 7 European archaeology has shown that the issue is more complex A variety of stone tool technologies are present at the time of human expansion into Europe and show evidence of modern behavior Despite the problems of conflating specific tools with cultural groups the Aurignacian tool complex for example is generally taken as a purely modern human signature 63 64 The discovery of transitional complexes like proto Aurignacian have been taken as evidence of human groups progressing through steps of innovation 63 If as this might suggest human groups were already migrating into eastern Europe around 40 000 years and only afterward show evidence of behavioral modernity then either the cognitive change must have diffused back into Africa or was already present before migration In light of a growing body of evidence of Neanderthal culture and tool complexes some researchers have put forth a multiple species model for behavioral modernity 6 24 65 Neanderthals were often cited as being an evolutionary dead end apish cousins who were less advanced than their human contemporaries Personal ornaments were relegated as trinkets or poor imitations compared to the cave art produced by H sapiens Despite this European evidence has shown a variety of personal ornaments and artistic artifacts produced by Neanderthals for example the Neanderthal site of Grotte du Renne has produced grooved bear wolf and fox incisors ochre and other symbolic artifacts 65 Although burials are few and controversial there has been circumstantial evidence of Neanderthal ritual burials 24 There are two options to describe this symbolic behavior among Neanderthals they copied cultural traits from arriving modern humans or they had their own cultural traditions comparative with behavioral modernity If they just copied cultural traditions which is debated by several authors 6 24 they still possessed the capacity for complex culture described by behavioral modernity As discussed above if Neanderthals also were behaviorally modern then it cannot be a species specific derived trait Asia Edit Most debates surrounding behavioral modernity have been focused on Africa or Europe but an increasing amount of focus has been placed on East Asia This region offers a unique opportunity to test hypotheses of multi regionalism replacement and demographic effects 66 Unlike Europe where initial migration occurred around 50 000 years ago human remains have been dated in China to around 100 000 years ago 67 This early evidence of human expansion calls into question behavioral modernity as an impetus for migration Stone tool technology is particularly of interest in East Asia Following Homo erectus migrations out of Africa Acheulean technology never seems to appear beyond present day India and into China Analogously Mode 3 or Levallois technology is not apparent in China following later hominin dispersals 68 This lack of more advanced technology has been explained by serial founder effects and low population densities out of Africa 69 Although tool complexes comparative to Europe are missing or fragmentary other archaeological evidence shows behavioral modernity For example the peopling of the Japanese archipelago offers an opportunity to investigate the early use of watercraft Although one site Kanedori in Honshu does suggest the use of watercraft as early as 84 000 years ago there is no other evidence of hominins in Japan until 50 000 years ago 66 The Zhoukoudian cave system near Beijing has been excavated since the 1930s and has yielded precious data on early human behavior in East Asia Although disputed there is evidence of possible human burials and interred remains in the cave dated to around 34 20 000 years ago 66 These remains have associated personal ornaments in the form of beads and worked shell suggesting symbolic behavior Along with possible burials numerous other symbolic objects like punctured animal teeth and beads some dyed in red ochre have all been found at Zhoukoudian 66 Although fragmentary the archaeological record of eastern Asia shows evidence of behavioral modernity before 50 000 years ago but like the African record it is not fully apparent until that time See also EditAnatomically modern human Archaic Homo sapiens Blombos Cave Cultural universal Dawn of Humanity film Evolution of human intelligence Female cosmetic coalitions FOXP2 and human evolution Human evolution List of Stone Age art Origin of language Origins of society Prehistoric art Prehistoric music Paleolithic religion Recent African origin Sibudu Cave Sociocultural evolution Symbolism disambiguation Symbolic culture Timeline of evolutionReferences Edit Korisettar Ravi 1998 Early Human Behaviour in Global Context Routledge McBrearty Sally Brooks Allison 2000 The revolution that wasn t a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior Journal of Human Evolution 39 5 453 563 doi 10 1006 jhev 2000 0435 PMID 11102266 Proponents of the model known as the human revolution claim that modern human behaviors arose suddenly and nearly simultaneously throughout the Old World ca 40 50 ka In fact many of the components of the human revolution claimed to appear at 40 50 ka are found in the African Middle Stone Age tens of thousands of years earlier These features include blade and microlithic technology bone tools increased geographic range specialized hunting the use of aquatic resources long distance trade systematic processing and use of pigment and art and decoration a b c d e f g h i Henshilwood Christopher Marean Curtis 2003 The Origin of Modern Human Behavior Critique of the Models and Their Test Implications Current Anthropology 44 5 627 651 doi 10 1086 377665 PMID 14971366 S2CID 11081605 Hill Kim et al 2009 The Emergence of Human Uniqueness Characters Underlying Behavioral Modernity Evolutionary Anthropology 18 5 187 200 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 469 5702 doi 10 1002 evan 20224 S2CID 56384790 Klein R G 1999 The human career human biological and cultural origins Chicago University of Chicago Press a b c d D Errico F et al 1998 Neanderthal Acculturation in Western Europe A Critical Review of the Evidence and Its Interpretation Current Anthropology 39 S1 S1 S44 doi 10 1086 204689 S2CID 144799519 a b c d e f g Klein Richard 1995 Anatomy behavior and modern human origins Journal of World Prehistory 9 2 167 198 doi 10 1007 bf02221838 S2CID 10402296 a b c d e f McBrearty Sally Brooks Allison 2000 The revolution that wasn t a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior Journal of Human Evolution 39 5 453 563 doi 10 1006 jhev 2000 0435 PMID 11102266 a b c Marean Curtis et al 2007 Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene PDF Nature 449 7164 905 908 Bibcode 2007Natur 449 905M doi 10 1038 nature06204 PMID 17943129 S2CID 4387442 a b c Powell Adam et al 2009 Late Pleistocene Demography and the Appearance of Modern Human Behavior PDF Science 324 5932 1298 1301 Bibcode 2009Sci 324 1298P doi 10 1126 science 1170165 PMID 19498164 S2CID 206518315 a b c Premo Luke Kuhn Steve 2010 Modeling Effects of Local Extinctions on Culture Change and Diversity in the Paleolithic PLOS ONE 5 12 e15582 Bibcode 2010PLoSO 515582P doi 10 1371 journal pone 0015582 PMC 3003693 PMID 21179418 a b Boyd Robert Richerson Peter 1988 Culture and the Evolutionary Process 2 ed University of Chicago Press ISBN 9780226069333 a b Nakahashi Wataru 2013 Evolution of improvement and cumulative culture Theoretical Population Biology 83 30 38 doi 10 1016 j tpb 2012 11 001 PMID 23153511 Buller David 2005 Adapting Minds Evolutionary Psychology and the Persistent Quest for Human Nature PMIT Press p 468 ISBN 978 0 262 02579 9 80 000 year old Beads Shed Light on Early Culture Livescience com June 18 2007 Retrieved September 10 2009 three distinct human populations Accessexcellence org Retrieved September 10 2009 a b c d Shea John 2011 Homo sapiens Is as Homo sapiens Was Current Anthropology 52 1 1 35 doi 10 1086 658067 S2CID 142517998 a b c Hoffecker John 2009 The spread of modern humans in Europe PNAS 106 38 16040 16045 Bibcode 2009PNAS 10616040H doi 10 1073 pnas 0903446106 PMC 2752585 PMID 19571003 a b c Tattersall Ian 2009 Human origins Out of Africa PNAS 106 38 16018 16021 Bibcode 2009PNAS 10616018T doi 10 1073 pnas 0903207106 PMC 2752574 PMID 19805256 Language Gene Dethroned The Scientist Magazine Retrieved January 23 2023 Lieberman Philip 2007 The Evolution of Human Speech Its Anatomical and Neural Bases Current Anthropology 48 1 39 66 doi 10 1086 509092 ISSN 0011 3204 Goody Jack 1986 The Logic of Writing and the Organization of Society Studies in Literacy the Family Culture and the State Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 33962 9 a b Foley Robert Lahr Marta 1997 Mode 3 Technologies and the Evolution of Modern Humans Cambridge Archaeological Journal 7 1 3 36 doi 10 1017 s0959774300001451 S2CID 163040120 a b c d e D Errico Francesco 2003 The Invisible Frontier A Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioral Modernity Evolutionary Anthropology 12 4 188 202 doi 10 1002 evan 10113 S2CID 1904963 a b Henshilwood Christopher et al 2002 Emergence of Modern Human Behavior Middle Stone Age Engravings from South Africa Science 295 5558 1278 1280 Bibcode 2002Sci 295 1278H doi 10 1126 science 1067575 PMID 11786608 S2CID 31169551 Watts I 2009 Red ochre body painting and language interpreting the Blombos ochre In R Botha and C Knight eds The Cradle of Language Oxford Oxford University Press pp 62 92 Mellars P A K Boyle O Bar Yosef and C Stringer eds 2007 Rethinking the Human Revolution new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans Cambridge McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research Henshilwood C S and B Dubreuil 2009 Reading the artifacts gleaning language skills from the Middle Stone Age in Southern Africa In R Botha and C Knight eds The Cradle of Language Oxford Oxford University Press pp 41 61 Botha R and C Knight eds The Cradle of Language Oxford Oxford University Press Zilhao J 2006 Neandertals and moderns mixed and it matters Evolutionary Anthropology 15 5 183 195 doi 10 1002 evan 20110 S2CID 18565967 Jaynes Julian 2000 1976 The Origin of Consciousness in the Breakdown of the Bicameral Mind Houghton Mifflin ISBN 0 618 05707 2 Damasio Antonio 2010 Self Comes to Mind New York Pantheon Books p 289 ISBN 978 0 307 37875 0 I sympathize with Julian Jaynes s claim that something of great import may have happened to the human mind during the relatively brief interval of time between the events narrated in the Iliad and those that make up the Odyssey Schlebusch Carina M Malmstrom Helena Gunther Torsten Sjodin Per Coutinho Alexandra Edlund Hanna Munters Arielle R Vicente Mario Steyn Maryna Soodyall Himla Lombard Marlize Jakobsson Mattias 2017 Southern African ancient genomes estimate modern human divergence to 350 000 to 260 000 years ago Science 358 6363 652 655 Bibcode 2017Sci 358 652S doi 10 1126 science aao6266 PMID 28971970 Sample Ian June 7 2017 Oldest Homo sapiens bones ever found shake foundations of the human story The Guardian Retrieved June 7 2017 Zimmer Carl September 10 2019 Scientists Find the Skull of Humanity s Ancestor on a Computer By comparing fossils and CT scans researchers say they have reconstructed the skull of the last common forebear of modern humans The New York Times Retrieved September 10 2019 Mounier Aurelien Lahr Marta 2019 Deciphering African late middle Pleistocene hominin diversity and the origin of our species Nature Communications 10 1 3406 Bibcode 2019NatCo 10 3406M doi 10 1038 s41467 019 11213 w PMC 6736881 PMID 31506422 a b Sahle Y Hutchings W K Braun D R Sealy J C Morgan L E Negash A Atnafu B 2013 Petraglia Michael D ed Earliest Stone Tipped Projectiles from the Ethiopian Rift Date to gt 279 000 Years Ago PLOS ONE 8 11 e78092 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 878092S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0078092 PMC 3827237 PMID 24236011 a b Chatterjee Rhitu March 15 2018 Scientists Are Amazed By Stone Age Tools They Dug Up In Kenya NPR Retrieved March 15 2018 a b Yong Ed March 15 2018 A Cultural Leap at the Dawn of Humanity New finds from Kenya suggest that humans used long distance trade networks sophisticated tools and symbolic pigments right from the dawn of our species The Atlantic Retrieved March 15 2018 a b Brooks AS Yellen JE Potts R Behrensmeyer AK Deino AL Leslie DE Ambrose SH Ferguson JR d Errico F Zipkin AM Whittaker S Post J Veatch EG Foecke K Clark JB 2018 Long distance stone transport and pigment use in the earliest Middle Stone Age Science 360 6384 90 94 Bibcode 2018Sci 360 90B doi 10 1126 science aao2646 PMID 29545508 Henshilwood Christopher S d Errico Francesco Watts Ian 2009 Engraved ochres from the Middle Stone Age levels at Blombos Cave South Africa Journal of Human Evolution 57 1 27 47 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2009 01 005 PMID 19487016 Texier PJ Porraz G Parkington J Rigaud JP Poggenpoel C Miller C Tribolo C Cartwright C Coudenneau A Klein R Steele T Verna C 2010 A Howiesons Poort tradition of engraving ostrich eggshell containers dated to 60 000 years ago at Diepkloof Rock Shelter South Africa Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 14 6180 6185 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 6180T doi 10 1073 pnas 0913047107 PMC 2851956 PMID 20194764 Henshilwood Christopher S et al 2004 Middle Stone Age shell beads from South Africa Science 304 5669 404 doi 10 1126 science 1095905 PMID 15087540 S2CID 32356688 d Errico Francesco et al 2005 Nassarius kraussianus shell beads from Blombos Cave evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age Journal of Human Evolution 48 1 3 24 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2004 09 002 PMID 15656934 Vanhaeren Marian et al 2013 Thinking strings Additional evidence for personal ornament use in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave South Africa Journal of Human Evolution 64 6 500 517 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2013 02 001 PMID 23498114 Backwell L d Errico F Wadley L 2008 Middle Stone Age bone tools from the Howiesons Poort layers Sibudu Cave South Africa Journal of Archaeological Science 35 6 1566 1580 doi 10 1016 j jas 2007 11 006 Wadley Lyn 2008 The Howieson s Poort industry of Sibudu Cave South African Archaeological Society Goodwin Series 10 Lombard M Phillips L 2010 Indications of bow and stone tipped arrow use 64 000 years ago in KwaZulu Natal South Africa Antiquity 84 325 635 648 doi 10 1017 S0003598X00100134 S2CID 162438490 Lombard M 2011 Quartz tipped arrows older than 60 ka further use trace evidence from Sibudu Kwa Zulu Natal South Africa Journal of Archaeological Science 38 8 1918 1930 doi 10 1016 j jas 2011 04 001 Backwell L Bradfield J Carlson KJ Jashashvili T Wadley L d Errico F 2018 The antiquity of bow and arrow technology evidence from Middle Stone Age layers at Sibudu Cave Journal of Archaeological Science 92 362 289 303 doi 10 15184 aqy 2018 11 Lombard M 2020 The tip cross sectional areas of poisoned bone arrowheads from southern Africa Journal of Archaeological Science Reports 33 102477 doi 10 1016 j jasrep 2020 102477 S2CID 224889105 Yellen JE AS Brooks E Cornelissen MJ Mehlman K Stewart April 28 1995 A middle stone age worked bone industry from Katanda Upper Semliki Valley Zaire Science 268 5210 553 556 Bibcode 1995Sci 268 553Y doi 10 1126 science 7725100 PMID 7725100 Brown Kyle S Marean Curtis W Herries Andy I R Jacobs Zenobia Tribolo Chantal Braun David Roberts David L Meyer Michael C Bernatchez J August 14 2009 Fire as an Engineering Tool of Early Modern Humans Science 325 5942 859 862 Bibcode 2009Sci 325 859B doi 10 1126 science 1175028 hdl 11422 11102 PMID 19679810 S2CID 43916405 Brown Kyle S Marean Curtis W Jacobs Zenobia Schoville Benjamin J Oestmo Simen Fisher Erich C Bernatchez Jocelyn Karkanas Panagiotis Matthews Thalassa 2012 An early and enduring advanced technology originating 71 000 years ago in South Africa Nature 491 7425 590 3 Bibcode 2012Natur 491 590B doi 10 1038 nature11660 PMID 23135405 S2CID 4323569 Amos Jonathan October 13 2011 A Cultural Leap at the Dawn of Humanity Ancient paint factory unearthed BBC News Retrieved October 13 2011 Vastag Brian October 13 2011 South African cave yields paint from dawn of humanity The Washington Post Retrieved October 13 2011 Henshilwood Christopher S et al 2011 A 100 000 Year Old Ochre Processing Workshop at Blombos Cave South Africa Science 334 6053 219 222 Bibcode 2011Sci 334 219H doi 10 1126 science 1211535 PMID 21998386 S2CID 40455940 Shipton C d Errico F Petraglia M et al 2018 78 000 year old record of Middle and Later Stone Age innovation in an East African tropical forest Nature Communications 5 Oldest Mines in the World A Casual Survey Archived from the original on January 5 2019 Retrieved February 20 2018 Guinness World Records September 10 2015 Guinness World Records 2016 Guinness World Records p 27 ISBN 978 1 910561 03 4 Sahle Y Brooks AS 2019 Assessment of complex projectiles in the early Late Pleistocene at Aduma Ethiopia PLOS ONE 14 5 e0216716 Bibcode 2019PLoSO 1416716S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0216716 PMC 6508696 PMID 31071181 Willman J C Hernando R Matu M Crevecoeur I 2020 Biocultural diversity in Late Pleistocene Early Holocene Africa Olduvai Hominid 1 Tanzania biological affinity and intentional body modification PDF American Journal of Physical Anthropology 172 4 664 681 doi 10 1002 ajpa 24007 PMID 31944279 S2CID 210331198 a b Joris Olaf Street Martin 2008 At the end of the 14C time scaledthe Middle to Upper Paleolithic record of western Eurasia Journal of Human Evolution 55 5 782 802 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2008 04 002 PMID 18930513 Anikovich M et al 2007 Early Upper Paleolithic in Eastern Europe and Implications for the Dispersal of Modern Humans Science 315 5809 223 226 Bibcode 2007Sci 315 223A doi 10 1126 science 1133376 PMID 17218523 S2CID 21428180 a b Abadia Oscar Moro Gonzalez Morales Manuel R 2010 Redefining Neanderthals and Art An Alternative Interpretation of the Multiple Species Model for the Origin of Behavioural Modernity Oxford Journal of Archaeology 29 3 229 243 doi 10 1111 j 1468 0092 2010 00346 x a b c d Norton Christopher Jin Jennie 2009 The Evolution of Modern Human Behavior in East Asia Current Perspectives Evolutionary Anthropology 18 6 247 260 doi 10 1002 evan 20235 S2CID 54836302 Liu Wu et al 2010 Human remains from Zhirendong South China and modern human emergence in East Asia PNAS 107 45 19201 19206 Bibcode 2010PNAS 10719201L doi 10 1073 pnas 1014386107 PMC 2984215 PMID 20974952 Norton Christopher Bae K 2008 The Movius Line sensu lato Norton et al 2006 further assessed and defined Journal of Human Evolution 55 6 1148 1150 doi 10 1016 j jhevol 2008 08 003 PMID 18809202 Lycett Stephen Norton Christopher 2010 A demographic model for Palaeolithic technological evolution The case of East Asia and the Movius Line Quaternary International 211 1 2 55 65 Bibcode 2010QuInt 211 55L doi 10 1016 j quaint 2008 12 001 External links EditSteven Mithen 1999 The Prehistory of the Mind The Cognitive Origins of Art Religion and Science Thames amp Hudson ISBN 978 0 500 28100 0 Artifacts in Africa Suggest An Earlier Modern Human Tools point to African origin for human behaviour Key Human Traits Tied to Shellfish Remains nytimes 2007 10 18 Python Cave Reveals Oldest Human Ritual Scientists Suggest Human Timeline Interactive Smithsonian National Museum of Natural History August 2016 Portals Evolutionary biology Science Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Behavioral modernity amp oldid 1141341538, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.