fbpx
Wikipedia

Infanticide (zoology)

In animals, infanticide involves the intentional killing of young offspring by a mature animal of the same species.[2] Animal infanticide is studied in zoology, specifically in the field of ethology. Ovicide is the analogous destruction of eggs. The practice has been observed in many species throughout the animal kingdom, especially primates (primate infanticide) but including microscopic rotifers, insects, fish, amphibians, birds and mammals.[3] Infanticide can be practiced by both males and females.[4]

Lion cubs may be killed by males replacing other males in the pride.[1]

Infanticide caused by sexual conflict has the general theme of the killer (often male) becoming the new sexual partner of the victim's parent, which would otherwise be unavailable.[5] This represents a gain in fitness by the killer, and a loss in fitness by the parents of the offspring killed. This is a type of evolutionary struggle between the two sexes, in which the victim sex may have counter-adaptations that reduce the success of this practice.[5] It may also occur for other reasons, such as the struggle for food between females. In this case individuals may even kill closely related offspring.

Filial infanticide occurs when a parent kills its own offspring. This sometimes involves consumption of the young themselves, which is termed filial cannibalism. The behavior is widespread in fishes, and is seen in terrestrial animals as well. Human infanticide has been recorded in almost every culture. A unique aspect of human infanticide is sex-selective infanticide.

Background edit

Infanticide only came to be seen as a significant occurrence in nature quite recently. At the time it was first seriously treated by Yukimaru Sugiyama,[6] infanticide was attributed to stress causing factors like overcrowding and captivity, and was considered pathological and maladaptive. Classical ethology held that conspecifics (members of the same species) rarely killed each other.[7] By the 1980s it had gained much greater acceptance. Possible reasons it was not treated as a prevalent natural phenomenon include its abhorrence to people, the popular group and species selectionist notions of the time (the idea that individuals behave for the good of the group or species; compare with gene-centered view of evolution), and the fact that it is very difficult to observe in the field.[8]

Infanticide involving sexual conflict edit

This form of infanticide represents a struggle between the sexes, where one sex exploits the other, much to the latter's disadvantage. It is usually the male who benefits from this behavior, though in cases where males play similar roles to females in parental care the victim and perpetrator may be reversed (see Bateman's principle for discussion of this asymmetry).

By males edit

Hanuman langurs (or gray langurs) are Old World monkeys found in India. They are a social animal, living in groups that consist of a single dominant male and multiple females. The dominant male has a reproductive monopoly within the group, which causes sub-ordinate males to have a much lower fitness value in comparison.[9] To gain the opportunity to reproduce, sub-ordinate males try to take over the dominant role within a group, usually resulting in an aggressive struggle with the existing dominant male.[10] If successful in overthrowing the previous male, unrelated infants of the females are then killed.[11] This infanticidal period is limited to the window just after the group is taken over. Cannibalism, however, has not been observed in this species.

Infanticide not only reduces intraspecific competition between the incumbent's offspring and those of other males but also increases the parental investment afforded to their own young, and allows females to become fertile faster.[12] This is because females of this species, as well as many other mammals, do not ovulate during lactation. It then becomes easier to understand how infanticide evolved. If a male kills a female's young, she stops lactating and is able to become pregnant again.[12] Because of this, the newly dominant male is able to reproduce at a faster rate than without the act of infanticide.[10] As males are in a constant struggle to protect their group, those that express infanticidal behavior will contribute a larger portion to future gene pools (see natural selection).

Similar behavior is also seen in male lions, among other species, who also kill young cubs, thereby enabling them to impregnate the females. Unlike langurs, male lions live in small groups, which cooperate to take control of a pride from an existing group.[1] They will attempt to kill any cubs that are roughly nine months old or younger, though as in other species, the female will attempt to defend her cubs viciously. Males have, on average, only a two-year window in which to pass on their genes, and lionesses only give birth once every two years, so the selective pressure on them to conform to this behavior is strong. In fact it is estimated that a quarter of cubs dying in the first year of life are victims of infanticide.[1]

Male mice show great variation in behavior over time. After fertilizing a female, they become aggressive towards mouse pups for three weeks, killing any they come across. After this period however, their behavior changes dramatically, and they become paternal, caring for their own offspring. This lasts for almost two months, but afterwards they become infanticidal once more. It is no coincidence here that the female gestation period is three weeks as well, or that it takes roughly two months for pups to become fully weaned and leave their nest. The proximate mechanism that allows for the correct timing of these periods involves circadian rhythms (see chronobiology), each day and night cycle affecting the mouse's internal neural physiology, and disturbances in the duration of these cycles results in different periods of time between behaviors.[13] The adaptive value of this behavior switching is twofold; infanticide removes competitors for when the mouse does have offspring, and allows the female victims to be impregnated earlier than if they continued to care for their young, as mentioned above.

Gerbils, on the other hand, no longer commit infanticide once they have paired with a female, but actively kill and eat other offspring when young. The females of this species behave much like male mice, hunting down other litters except when rearing their own.[14]

Prospective infanticide edit

Prospective infanticide is a subset of sexual competition infanticide in which young born after the arrival of the new male are killed. This is less common than infanticide of existing young, but can still increase fitness in cases where the offspring could not possibly have been fathered by the new mate, i.e. one gestation or fertility period. This is known to occur in lions and langurs, and has also been observed in other species such as house wrens.[15] In birds, however, the situation is more complex, as female eggs are fertilized one at a time, with a 24-hour delay between each. Males may destroy clutches laid 12 days or more after their arrival, though their investment of around 60 days of parental care is large, so a high level of parental certainty is needed.[15]

By females edit

 
Jacana jacana females carry out infanticide.

Females are also known to display infanticidal behavior. This may appear unexpected, as the conditions described above do not apply. Males are not always an unlimited resource though—in some species, males provide parental care to their offspring, and females may compete indirectly with others by killing their offspring, freeing up the limiting resource that the males represent. This has been documented in research by Stephen Emlen and Natalie Demong on wattled jacanas (Jacana jacana), a tropical wading bird.[16] In the wattled jacana, it is exclusively the male sex that broods, while females defend their territory. In this experiment Demong and Emlen found that removing females from a territory resulted in nearby females attacking the chicks of the male in most cases, evicting them from their nest. The males then fertilized the offending females and cared for their young.[17] Emlen describes how he "shot a female one night, and ... by first light a new female was already on the turf. I saw terrible things—pecking and picking up and throwing down chicks until they were dead. Within hours she was soliciting the male, and he was mounting her the same day. The next night I shot the other female, then came out the next morning and saw the whole thing again."[18]

Infanticide is also seen in giant water bugs.[19] Lethocerus deyrollei is a large and nocturnal predatory insect found in still waters near vegetation. In this species the males take care of masses of eggs by keeping them hydrated with water from their bodies. Without a male caring for the eggs like this, they become desiccated and will not hatch. In this species, males are a scarce resource that females must sometimes compete for. Those that cannot find a free male often stab the eggs of a brooding one. As in the above case, males then fertilize this female and care for her eggs. Noritaka Ichikawa has found that males only moisten their eggs during the first 90 seconds or so, after which all of the moisture on their bodies has evaporated. However, they guard the egg masses for as long as several hours at a time, when they could be hunting prey. They do not seem to prevent further evaporation by staying guard, as males that only guarded the nest for short periods were seen to have similar hatching rates in a controlled experiment where there were no females present. It seems rather that males are more successful in avoiding infanticidal females when they are out of the water with their eggs, which might well explain the ultimate cause of this behavior.[19]

Female rats will eat the kits of strange females for a source of nutrition, and to take over the nest for her own litter.[20]

Resource competition edit

 
The Black-tailed prairie dog (Cynomys ludovicianus)

Black-tailed prairie dogs are colonially-living, harem-polygynous squirrels found mainly in the United States. Their living arrangement involves one male living with four or so females in a territory defended by all individuals, and underground nesting. Black-tails only have one litter per year, and are in estrous for only a single day around the beginning of spring.

A seven-year natural experiment by John Hoogland and others from Princeton University revealed that infanticide is widespread in this species, including infanticide from invading males and immigrant females, as well as occasional cannibalism of an individual's own offspring.[3] The surprising finding of the study was that by far the most common type of infanticide involved the killing of close kin's offspring. This seems illogical, as kin selection favors behaviors that promote the well-being of closely related individuals. It was postulated that this form of infanticide is more successful than trying to kill young in nearby groups, as the whole group must be bypassed in this case, while within a group only the mother need be evaded. Marauding behavior is evidently adaptive, as infanticidal females had more and healthier young than others, and were heavier themselves as well. This behavior appears to reduce competition with other females for food, and future competition among offspring.

Similar behavior has been reported in the meerkat (Suricata suricatta), including cases of females killing their mother's, sister's, and daughter's offspring. Infanticidal raids from neighboring groups also occurred.[21]

Other edit

Bottlenose dolphins have been reported to kill their young through impact injuries.[22] Dominant male langurs tend to kill the existing young upon taking control of a harem.[23] There have been sightings of infanticide in the leopard population.[24] The males of the Stegodyphus lineatus species of spider have been known to exhibit infanticide as a way to encourage females to mate again.

In mammals, male infanticide is most often observed in non-seasonal breeders.[25] There is less fitness advantage for a conspecific to carry out infanticide if the interbirth period of the mother will not be decreased and the female will not return to estrous. In Felidae, birthing periods can happen anytime during the year, as long as there is not an unweaned offspring of that female. This is a contributor to the frequency of infanticide in carnivorous felids.[26][25] Some species of seasonal breeders have been observed to commit infanticide. Cases in the snub-nosed monkey, a seasonal breeding primate, have shown that infanticide does lessen the interbirth period of the females and can allow them to breed with the next breeding group.[27] Other cases of seasonal breeding species where the infanticidal characteristic is observed has been explained as a way of preserving the mother's resources and energy in turn increasing the reproductive success of upcoming breeding periods.[28]

Costs and defenses edit

Costs of the behavior edit

While it may be beneficial for some species to behave this way, infanticide is not without risks to the perpetrator. Having already expended energy and perhaps sustained serious wounds in a fight with another male, attacks from females who vigorously defend their offspring may be telling for harem-polygynous males, with a risk of infection. It is also energetically costly to pursue a mother's young, which may try to escape.

Costs of the behavior described in prairie dogs include the risk to an individual of losing their own young while killing another's, not to mention the fact that they are killing their own relatives. In a species where infanticide is common, perpetrators may well be victims themselves in the future, such that they come out no better off; but as long as an infanticidal individual gains in reproductive output by its behavior, it will tend to become common. Further costs of the behavior in general may be induced by counter-strategies evolved in the other sex, as described below.

As a cost of social behavior edit

Taking a broader view of the black-tailed prairie dog situation, infanticide can be seen as a cost of social living.[3] If each female were to have her own private nest away from others, she would be much less likely to have her infants killed when absent. This, and other costs such as increased spread of parasites, must be made up for by other benefits, such as group territory defense and increased awareness of predators.

An avian example published in Nature is acorn woodpeckers. Females nest together, possibly because those nesting alone have their eggs constantly destroyed by rivals. Even so, eggs are consistently removed at first by nest partners themselves, until the entire group lays on the same day. They then cooperate and incubate the eggs as a group, but by this time a significant proportion of their eggs have been lost because of this ovicidal behavior.[29]

Counter-strategies edit

Because this form of infanticide reduces the fitness of killed individuals' parents, animals have evolved a range of counter-strategies against this behavior. These may be divided into two very different classes - those that tend to prevent infanticide, and those that minimize losses.

Loss minimization edit

Some females abort or resorb their own young while they are still in development after a new male takes over; this is known as the Bruce effect.[30] This may prevent their young from being killed after birth, saving the mother wasted time and energy. However, this strategy also benefits the new male. In mice this can occur by the proximate mechanism of the female smelling the odor of the new male's urine.[31]

Preventive adaptations edit

Infanticide in burying beetles may have led to male parental care.[32] In this species males often cooperate with the female in preparing a piece of carrion, which is buried with the eggs and eaten by the larvae when they hatch. Males may also guard the site alongside the female. It is apparent from experiments that this behavior does not provide their young with any better nourishment, nor is it of any use in defending against predators. However, other burying bugs may try to take their nesting space. When this occurs, a male-female pair is over twice as successful in nest defense, preventing the ovicide of their offspring.

Female langurs may leave the group with their young alongside the outgoing male, and others may develop a false estrous and allow the male to copulate, deceiving him into thinking she is actually sexually receptive.[33] Females may also have sexual liaisons with other males. This promiscuous behavior is adaptive, because males will not know whether it is their own offspring they are killing or not, and may be more reluctant or invest less effort in infanticide attempts.[34] Lionesses cooperatively guard against scouting males, and a pair were seen to violently attack a male after it killed one of their young.[35] Resistance to infanticide is also costly, though: for instance, a female may sustain serious injuries in defending her young. At times it is simply more advantageous to submit than to fight.[36]

Infanticide, the destruction of offspring characteristic to many species, has posed so great a threat that there have been observable changes of behavior in respective female mothers; more specifically, these changes exist as preventive measures. A common behavioral mechanism by females to reduce the risk of infanticide of future offspring is through the process of paternity confusion or dilution. In theory, this implies that a female that mates with multiple males will widely spread the assumption of paternity across many males, and therefore make them less likely to kill or attack offspring that could potentially carry their genes. This theory operates under the assumption that the specific males keep a memory of past mates, under a desire to perpetuate their own genes [37] In the Japanese macaque (macaca fuscata), female mating with multiple males, or dilution of paternity, was found to inhibit male-to-infant aggression and infanticide eight times less towards infants of females with which they had previously mated.[38] Multi-male mating, or MMM, is recorded as a measure to prevent infanticide in species where young is altricial, or heavily dependent, and where there is a high turnover rate for dominant males, which leads to infanticide of the previous dominant male's young. Examples include, but are not limited to; white-footed mice, hamsters, lions, langurs, baboons, and macaques.[34] Along with mating with multiple males, the mating of females throughout the entirety of a reproductive cycle also serves a purpose for inhibiting the chance of infanticide. This theory assumes that males use information on past matings to make decisions on committing infanticide, and that females subsequently manipulate that knowledge. Females which are able to appear sexually active or receptive at all stages of their cycle, even during pregnancy with another male's offspring, can confuse the males into believing that the subsequent children are theirs.[37] This "pseudo-estrus" theory applies to females within species that do not exhibit obvious clues to each stage of their cycle, such as langurs, rhesus macaques, and gelada baboons.[37] An alternative to paternity confusion as a method of infanticide prevention is paternity concentration. This is the behavior of females to concentrate paternity to one specific dominant male as a means of protection from infanticide at the hands of less-dominant males.[34] This particularly applies to species in which a male has a very long tenure as the dominant male, and faces little instability in this hierarchy. Females choose these dominant males as the best available form of protection, and therefore mate exclusively with this male. This is especially common within small rodents.[34] An additional behavioral strategy to prevent infanticide by males may be aggressive protection of the nest along with female presence. This strategy is commonly used in species such as European rabbits.[39][40] Aggressive protection of the nest in an effort to reduce infanticide is observed with the Black Rock Skink. Egernia saxatilis live in small families and adults defend their territories against conspecifics. The small "nuclear families" live in the same permanent shelter and the parents protect their infants from infanticidal conspecifics in this way. Adults attack unrelated juveniles but not their own offspring. The presence of a parent significantly reduces the rate of infanticide because conspecific adults ignore juveniles when a parent is present, likely because another adult is more threatening to the aggressive lizard. Therefore, a juvenile living within its parents' own territory will experience far less attacks from conspecific adults.[41][42]

Infanticide by parents and caregivers edit

 
Damselfish may eat their own offspring.[43]

Filial infanticide occurs when a parent kills its own offspring. Both male and female parents have been observed to do this, as well as sterile worker castes in some eusocial animals. Filial infanticide is also observed as a form of brood reduction in some birds species, such as the white stork.[44] This may be due to a lack of siblicide in this species.[45]

Maternal edit

Maternal infanticide occurs when newborn offspring are killed by their mother. This is sometimes seen in pigs,[46] a behavior known as savaging, which affects up to 5% of gilts. Similar behavior has been observed in various animals such as rabbits,[47] hamsters,[48] humans and burying beetles.[49]

Paternal edit

Paternal infanticide—where fathers eat their own offspring—may also occur. When young bass hatch from the spawn, the father guards the area, circling around them and keeping them together, as well as providing protection from would-be predators. After a few days, most of the fish will swim away. At this point the male's behavior changes: instead of defending the stragglers, he treats them as any other small prey, and eats them.[50]

Worker caste killing young edit

Honey bees may become infected with a bacterial disease called foul brood, which attacks the developing bee larva while still living in the cell. Some hives however have evolved a behavioral adaptation that resists this disease: the worker bees selectively kill the infected individuals by removing them from their cells and tossing them out of the hive, preventing it from spreading. The genetics of this behavior are quite complex. Experiments by Rothenbuhler showed that the 'hygienic' behavior of the queen was lost by crossing with a non-hygienic drone. This means that the trait must be recessive, only being expressed when both alleles contain the gene for hygienic behavior. Furthermore, the behavior is dependent on two separate loci. A backcross produced a mixed result. The hives of some offspring were hygienic, while others were not. There was also a third type of hive where workers removed the wax cap of the infected cells, but did nothing more. What was not apparent was the presence of a fourth group who threw diseased larvae out of the hive, but did not have the uncapping gene. This was suspected by Rothenbuhler however, who manually removed the caps, and found some hives proceeded to clear out infected cells.[51][52]

Humans and infanticide edit

Family structure is the most important risk factor in child abuse and infanticide. Children who live with both their natural (biological) parents are at low risk for abuse. The risk increases greatly when children live with step-parents or with a single parent. Children living without either parent (foster children) are 10 times more likely to be abused than children who live with both biological parents.[citation needed]

Children who live with a single parent that has a live-in partner are at the highest risk: they are 20 times more likely to be victims of child abuse than children living with both biological parents.[53]

Infanticide is a subject that some humans may find discomforting. Cornell University ethologist Glenn Hausfater states that "infanticide has not received much study because it's a repulsive subject [...] Many people regard it as reprehensible to even think about it." Research into infanticide in animals is in part motivated by the desire to understand human behaviors, such as child abuse. Hausfater explains that researchers are "trying to see if there's any connection between animal infanticide and child abuse, neglect and killing by humans [...] We just don't know yet what the connections are."[54]

Infanticide has been, and still is, practiced by some human cultures, groups, or individuals.[citation needed] In many past societies, certain forms of infanticide were considered permissible, whereas in most modern societies the practice is considered immoral and criminal. It still takes place in the Western world usually because of the parent's mental illness or violent behavior, in addition to some poor countries as a form of population control — sometimes with tacit societal acceptance. Female infanticide, a form of sex-selective infanticide, is more common than the killing of male offspring, especially in cultures where male children are more desirable.

See also edit

Further reading edit

  • Alcock, J. (1998). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach (6th ed.). Sinauer Associates, Inc. Sunderland, Massachusetts. ISBN 978-0-87893-009-8.
  • Parmigiani, S.; vom Saal, F.S. (1994). Infanticide and Parental Care. Harwood: London. p. 493. ISBN 978-3-7186-5505-2.
  • van Schaik, C.P.; Janson, C.H. (2000). Infanticide By Males And Its Implications. Cambridge University Press. p. 569. ISBN 978-0-521-77295-2.

References edit

  1. ^ a b c Pusey, A.E.; Packer, C. (1994). "Infanticide in lions". In Parmigiani, S.; vom Saal, F.S. (eds.). Infanticide and Parental Care. Harwood Academic Press, Chur, Switzerland. ISBN 9783718655052.
  2. ^ Logos, Aleksandar A. (2022). "Jasenovac in Croatia or a short story about a war and mass killing in it". p. 10. Retrieved 2022-09-11.
  3. ^ a b c Hoogland, J. L. (1985). "Infanticide in prairie dogs: Lactating females kill offspring of close kin". Science. 230 (4729): 1037–1040. Bibcode:1985Sci...230.1037H. doi:10.1126/science.230.4729.1037. PMID 17814930. S2CID 23653101.
  4. ^ Logos, Aleksandar A. (2022). "Jasenovac in Croatia or a short story about a war and mass killing in it". pp. 10 and note 28. Retrieved 2022-09-11.
  5. ^ a b Palombit, Ryne A. (2015-06-01). "Infanticide as Sexual Conflict: Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies". Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology. 7 (6): a017640. doi:10.1101/cshperspect.a017640. ISSN 1943-0264. PMC 4448612. PMID 25986557.
  6. ^ Sugiyama, Y. (1965). "On the social change of Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus) in their natural conditions". Primates. 6 (3–4): 381–417. doi:10.1007/BF01730356. S2CID 26758190.
  7. ^ Lorenz, K. (1966). On Aggression. New York: Harcourt, Brace and Worl.
  8. ^ Hausfater, G.; S.B. Hrdy (1984). Infanticide: Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives. New York, Aldine. ISBN 978-0-202-02022-8.
  9. ^ "Mating conflict in primates: infanticide, sexual harassment and female sexuality". {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  10. ^ a b Lukas, Dieter; Huchard, Elise (2014). "The evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian societies" (PDF). Science. 346 (6211): 841–4. Bibcode:2014Sci...346..841L. doi:10.1126/science.1257226. PMID 25395534. S2CID 206559886.
  11. ^ Hrdy, D. B. (1977). "Infanticide as a primate reproductive strategy". American Scientist. 65 (1): 40–49. Bibcode:1977AmSci..65...40B. PMID 402873.
  12. ^ a b Hausfater, Glenn (1984-08-01). "Infanticide: Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives". Current Anthropology. 25 (4): 500–502. doi:10.1086/203169. ISSN 0011-3204. S2CID 84753908.
  13. ^ Perrigo, G.; Bryant, W. C.; Vomsaal, F (1990). "A unique neural timing system prevents male mice from harming their own offspring". Animal Behaviour. 39 (3): 535–539. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80419-1. S2CID 53200704.
  14. ^ Hausfater, G. (1984). "Infanticide: Comparative and evolutionary perspectives". Current Anthropology. 25 (4): 500–502. doi:10.1086/203169. JSTOR 2742911. S2CID 84753908.
  15. ^ a b Freed, L. A. (1987). "Prospective infanticide and protection of genetic paternity in tropical house wrens". The American Naturalist. 130 (6): 948–954. doi:10.1086/284758. S2CID 84236418.
  16. ^ Emlen, S. T.; Demong, N. J.; Emlen, D. J. (1989). "Experimental induction of infanticide in female wattled jacanas" (PDF). Auk. 106 (1): 1–7. doi:10.2307/4087750. JSTOR 4087750.
  17. ^ For footage of this, see Attenborough, D. (1990) The Trials of Life, Episode 12, or Evolution, Episode 3 "Why Sex?".
  18. ^ Zimmer, Carl (1996-09-01). "First, kill the babies". Discover. Retrieved 2008-01-19.
  19. ^ a b Ichikawa, N. (1995). "Male counterstrategy against infanticide of the female giant water bug Lethocerus deyrollei (Hemiptera: Belostomatidae)". Journal of Insect Behavior. 8 (2): 181–186. doi:10.1007/BF01988903. S2CID 41306483.
  20. ^ Infanticide in Norway rats. ratbehavior.org
  21. ^ Clutton-Brock, T. H.; P. N. M., B.; Smith, R.; McIlrath, G. M.; Kansky, R.; Gaynor, D.; O'Riain, M. J.; Skinner, J. D. (1998). "Infanticide and expulsion of females in a cooperative mammal". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 265 (1412): 2291–2295. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0573. JSTOR 51272. PMC 1689533. PMID 9881475.
  22. ^ Milius, S. (July 18, 1998). "Infanticide reported in dolphins". Science News. Vol. 154, no. 3. p. 36. doi:10.2307/4010610. JSTOR 4010610. Retrieved 2007-05-22.
  23. ^ "The evolution of infanticidal mechanisms in male langurs" (PDF). Retrieved 2007-05-22.[permanent dead link]
  24. ^ Fildes, Jonathan (2007-05-29). "Cheating cheetahs caught by DNA". BBC News. Retrieved 2007-05-30.
  25. ^ a b Lukas, Dieter; Huchard, Elise (2014-11-14). "The evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian societies" (PDF). Science. 346 (6211): 841–844. Bibcode:2014Sci...346..841L. doi:10.1126/science.1257226. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 25395534. S2CID 206559886.
  26. ^ Balme, Guy A.; Hunter, Luke T.B. (2013-10-01). "Why leopards commit infanticide". Animal Behaviour. 86 (4): 791–799. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.07.019. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 53266285.
  27. ^ Yao, Hui; Yu, Huiliang; Yang, Banghe; Yang, Wangji; Xu, Haiqing; Grueter, Cyril C.; Li, Ming; Xiang, Zuofu (2016-02-02). "Male Infanticide in the Golden Snub-Nosed Monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana), a Seasonally Breeding Primate". International Journal of Primatology. 37 (2): 175–184. doi:10.1007/s10764-016-9892-2. ISSN 0164-0291. S2CID 14544830.
  28. ^ Hrdy, Sarah; Hausfater, G (1984-01-01). "Comparative and evolutionary perspectives on infanticide: An introduction and overview". Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives.
  29. ^ Mumme, R. L.; Koenig, W. D.; Pitelka, F. A. (1983). "Reproductive competition in the communal acorn woodpecker: Sisters destroy each other's eggs". Nature. 306 (5943): 583–584. Bibcode:1983Natur.306..583M. doi:10.1038/306583a0. S2CID 19299353.
  30. ^ Bruce, H. M. (1959). "An exteroceptive block to pregnancy in the mouse". Nature. 184 (4680): 105. Bibcode:1959Natur.184..105B. doi:10.1038/184105a0. PMID 13805128. S2CID 4200823.
  31. ^ Labov, J. B. (1981). "Pregnancy blocking in rodents: Adaptive advantages for females". American Naturalist. 118 (3): 361–371. doi:10.1086/283828. JSTOR 2460637. S2CID 85020158.
  32. ^ Scott, M. P. (1990). "Brood guarding and the evolution of male parental care in burying beetles". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 26 (1): 31–40. doi:10.1007/BF00174022. JSTOR 4600371. S2CID 23748510.
  33. ^ Hrdy, S.B. (1977). The Langurs of Abu. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. ISBN 978-0674510586.
  34. ^ a b c d Agrell, J.; Wolff, J.; Ylönen, H.; Ylonen, Hannu (1998). "Counter-strategies to infanticide in mammals: Costs and consequences". Oikos. 83 (3): 507–517. doi:10.2307/3546678. JSTOR 3546678.
  35. ^ Packer, C.; Pusey, A. (1983). "Adaptations of female lions to infanticide by incoming males". The American Naturalist. 121 (5): 716–728. doi:10.1086/284097. S2CID 84927815.
  36. ^ Yamamura, N.; Hasegawa, T.; Ito, Y. (1990). "Why mothers do not resist infanticide: A cost-benefit genetic model" (PDF). Evolution. 44 (5): 1346–1357. doi:10.2307/2409294. JSTOR 2409294. PMID 28563893.
  37. ^ a b c Hrdy, S. B. (1979). "Infanticide among animals: A review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females". Ethology and Sociobiology. 1: 13–18. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(79)90004-9.
  38. ^ Soltis, J.; Thomsen, R.; Matsubayashi, K.; Takenaka, O. (2000). "Infanticide by resident males and female counter-strategies in wild Japanese macaques ( Macaca fuscata )". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 48 (3): 195. doi:10.1007/s002650000224. S2CID 22485349.
  39. ^ Rödel, H.G; Starkloff, A (2008). "Infanticide and maternal offspring defence in european rabbits under natural breeding conditions". Ethology. 114 (1): 22–31. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01447.x.
  40. ^ Yöken, H.; Haapakoski, A. (2017). "How do infanticidal male bank voles (myodes glarelus) find the next with pups?". Ethology. 123 (2): 105–112. doi:10.1111/eth.12579.
  41. ^ O'Connor D, Shine R (2004) Parental care protects against infanticide in the lizard Egernia saxatilis(Scincidae). Animal Behaviour 68, 1361–1369.
  42. ^ O'Connor, D.; Shine, R. (2003). "Lizards in 'nuclear families': A novel reptilian social system in Egernia saxatilis (Scincidae)". Molecular Ecology. 12 (3): 743–752. doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2003.01777.x. PMID 12675829. S2CID 45090454.
  43. ^ Payne, A. G.; Smith, C.; Campbell, A. C. (2002). "Filial cannibalism improves survival and development of Beaugregory damselfish embryos". Proceedings: Biological Sciences. 269 (1505): 2095–2102. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2144. PMC 1691142. PMID 12396483.
  44. ^ Tortosa, Francisco S.; Redondo, Tomas (1992). "Motives for Parental Infanticide in White Storks Ciconia ciconia". Ornis Scandinavica (Scandinavian Journal of Ornithology). 23 (2): 185–189. doi:10.2307/3676447. JSTOR 3676447.
  45. ^ Zieliński, Piotr (December 2002). "Brood Reduction and Parental Infanticide — are the White StorkCiconia ciconiaand the Black StorkC. nigraexceptional?". Acta Ornithologica. 37 (2): 113–119. doi:10.3161/068.037.0207. ISSN 0001-6454.
  46. ^ North Carolina Pork Conference 2007-08-20 at the Wayback Machine notes. 2002. North Carolina State University.
  47. ^ Boyd, I. L. (1985). "Investment in growth by pregnant wild rabbits in relation to litter size and sex of the offspring". The Journal of Animal Ecology. 54 (1): 137–147. doi:10.2307/4626. JSTOR 4626.
  48. ^ "What Can You Do to Keep Your Hamster From Eating Its Own Babies?". The Spruce Pets. Retrieved 2022-06-15.
  49. ^ Trumbo, S. (1994). "Interspecific competition, brood parasitism, and the evolution of biparental cooperation in Burying beetles". Oikos. 69 (2): 241–249. doi:10.2307/3546144. JSTOR 3546144.
  50. ^ Elgar, M.A.; Crespi, B.J., eds. (1992). Cannibalism: Ecology and Evolution of Cannibalism among Diverse Taxa. Oxford University Press, New York. ISBN 978-0-19-854650-4.
  51. ^ Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-286092-7.
  52. ^ Rothenbuhler, W. C. (1964). "Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees. IV. Responses of F1 and backcross generations to disease killed brood". American Zoologist. 4 (2): 111–123. doi:10.1093/icb/4.2.111. PMID 14172721.
  53. ^ Sedlak, A.J., Mettenburg, J., Basena, M., Petta, I., McPherson, K., Greene, A., and Li, S. (2010). Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS–4): Report to Congress, Executive Summary. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families.
  54. ^ Bayard Webster (1982-08-17). "Infanticide: Animal behavior scrutinized for clues to humans". The New York Times. Retrieved 2008-01-18.

infanticide, zoology, animals, infanticide, involves, intentional, killing, young, offspring, mature, animal, same, species, animal, infanticide, studied, zoology, specifically, field, ethology, ovicide, analogous, destruction, eggs, practice, been, observed, . In animals infanticide involves the intentional killing of young offspring by a mature animal of the same species 2 Animal infanticide is studied in zoology specifically in the field of ethology Ovicide is the analogous destruction of eggs The practice has been observed in many species throughout the animal kingdom especially primates primate infanticide but including microscopic rotifers insects fish amphibians birds and mammals 3 Infanticide can be practiced by both males and females 4 Lion cubs may be killed by males replacing other males in the pride 1 Infanticide caused by sexual conflict has the general theme of the killer often male becoming the new sexual partner of the victim s parent which would otherwise be unavailable 5 This represents a gain in fitness by the killer and a loss in fitness by the parents of the offspring killed This is a type of evolutionary struggle between the two sexes in which the victim sex may have counter adaptations that reduce the success of this practice 5 It may also occur for other reasons such as the struggle for food between females In this case individuals may even kill closely related offspring Filial infanticide occurs when a parent kills its own offspring This sometimes involves consumption of the young themselves which is termed filial cannibalism The behavior is widespread in fishes and is seen in terrestrial animals as well Human infanticide has been recorded in almost every culture A unique aspect of human infanticide is sex selective infanticide Contents 1 Background 2 Infanticide involving sexual conflict 2 1 By males 2 1 1 Prospective infanticide 2 2 By females 3 Resource competition 4 Other 5 Costs and defenses 5 1 Costs of the behavior 5 2 As a cost of social behavior 5 3 Counter strategies 5 3 1 Loss minimization 5 3 2 Preventive adaptations 6 Infanticide by parents and caregivers 6 1 Maternal 6 2 Paternal 6 3 Worker caste killing young 7 Humans and infanticide 8 See also 9 Further reading 10 ReferencesBackground editInfanticide only came to be seen as a significant occurrence in nature quite recently At the time it was first seriously treated by Yukimaru Sugiyama 6 infanticide was attributed to stress causing factors like overcrowding and captivity and was considered pathological and maladaptive Classical ethology held that conspecifics members of the same species rarely killed each other 7 By the 1980s it had gained much greater acceptance Possible reasons it was not treated as a prevalent natural phenomenon include its abhorrence to people the popular group and species selectionist notions of the time the idea that individuals behave for the good of the group or species compare with gene centered view of evolution and the fact that it is very difficult to observe in the field 8 Infanticide involving sexual conflict editThis form of infanticide represents a struggle between the sexes where one sex exploits the other much to the latter s disadvantage It is usually the male who benefits from this behavior though in cases where males play similar roles to females in parental care the victim and perpetrator may be reversed see Bateman s principle for discussion of this asymmetry By males edit Hanuman langurs or gray langurs are Old World monkeys found in India They are a social animal living in groups that consist of a single dominant male and multiple females The dominant male has a reproductive monopoly within the group which causes sub ordinate males to have a much lower fitness value in comparison 9 To gain the opportunity to reproduce sub ordinate males try to take over the dominant role within a group usually resulting in an aggressive struggle with the existing dominant male 10 If successful in overthrowing the previous male unrelated infants of the females are then killed 11 This infanticidal period is limited to the window just after the group is taken over Cannibalism however has not been observed in this species Infanticide not only reduces intraspecific competition between the incumbent s offspring and those of other males but also increases the parental investment afforded to their own young and allows females to become fertile faster 12 This is because females of this species as well as many other mammals do not ovulate during lactation It then becomes easier to understand how infanticide evolved If a male kills a female s young she stops lactating and is able to become pregnant again 12 Because of this the newly dominant male is able to reproduce at a faster rate than without the act of infanticide 10 As males are in a constant struggle to protect their group those that express infanticidal behavior will contribute a larger portion to future gene pools see natural selection Similar behavior is also seen in male lions among other species who also kill young cubs thereby enabling them to impregnate the females Unlike langurs male lions live in small groups which cooperate to take control of a pride from an existing group 1 They will attempt to kill any cubs that are roughly nine months old or younger though as in other species the female will attempt to defend her cubs viciously Males have on average only a two year window in which to pass on their genes and lionesses only give birth once every two years so the selective pressure on them to conform to this behavior is strong In fact it is estimated that a quarter of cubs dying in the first year of life are victims of infanticide 1 Male mice show great variation in behavior over time After fertilizing a female they become aggressive towards mouse pups for three weeks killing any they come across After this period however their behavior changes dramatically and they become paternal caring for their own offspring This lasts for almost two months but afterwards they become infanticidal once more It is no coincidence here that the female gestation period is three weeks as well or that it takes roughly two months for pups to become fully weaned and leave their nest The proximate mechanism that allows for the correct timing of these periods involves circadian rhythms see chronobiology each day and night cycle affecting the mouse s internal neural physiology and disturbances in the duration of these cycles results in different periods of time between behaviors 13 The adaptive value of this behavior switching is twofold infanticide removes competitors for when the mouse does have offspring and allows the female victims to be impregnated earlier than if they continued to care for their young as mentioned above Gerbils on the other hand no longer commit infanticide once they have paired with a female but actively kill and eat other offspring when young The females of this species behave much like male mice hunting down other litters except when rearing their own 14 Prospective infanticide edit Prospective infanticide is a subset of sexual competition infanticide in which young born after the arrival of the new male are killed This is less common than infanticide of existing young but can still increase fitness in cases where the offspring could not possibly have been fathered by the new mate i e one gestation or fertility period This is known to occur in lions and langurs and has also been observed in other species such as house wrens 15 In birds however the situation is more complex as female eggs are fertilized one at a time with a 24 hour delay between each Males may destroy clutches laid 12 days or more after their arrival though their investment of around 60 days of parental care is large so a high level of parental certainty is needed 15 By females edit nbsp Jacana jacana females carry out infanticide Females are also known to display infanticidal behavior This may appear unexpected as the conditions described above do not apply Males are not always an unlimited resource though in some species males provide parental care to their offspring and females may compete indirectly with others by killing their offspring freeing up the limiting resource that the males represent This has been documented in research by Stephen Emlen and Natalie Demong on wattled jacanas Jacana jacana a tropical wading bird 16 In the wattled jacana it is exclusively the male sex that broods while females defend their territory In this experiment Demong and Emlen found that removing females from a territory resulted in nearby females attacking the chicks of the male in most cases evicting them from their nest The males then fertilized the offending females and cared for their young 17 Emlen describes how he shot a female one night and by first light a new female was already on the turf I saw terrible things pecking and picking up and throwing down chicks until they were dead Within hours she was soliciting the male and he was mounting her the same day The next night I shot the other female then came out the next morning and saw the whole thing again 18 Infanticide is also seen in giant water bugs 19 Lethocerus deyrollei is a large and nocturnal predatory insect found in still waters near vegetation In this species the males take care of masses of eggs by keeping them hydrated with water from their bodies Without a male caring for the eggs like this they become desiccated and will not hatch In this species males are a scarce resource that females must sometimes compete for Those that cannot find a free male often stab the eggs of a brooding one As in the above case males then fertilize this female and care for her eggs Noritaka Ichikawa has found that males only moisten their eggs during the first 90 seconds or so after which all of the moisture on their bodies has evaporated However they guard the egg masses for as long as several hours at a time when they could be hunting prey They do not seem to prevent further evaporation by staying guard as males that only guarded the nest for short periods were seen to have similar hatching rates in a controlled experiment where there were no females present It seems rather that males are more successful in avoiding infanticidal females when they are out of the water with their eggs which might well explain the ultimate cause of this behavior 19 Female rats will eat the kits of strange females for a source of nutrition and to take over the nest for her own litter 20 Resource competition edit nbsp The Black tailed prairie dog Cynomys ludovicianus Black tailed prairie dogs are colonially living harem polygynous squirrels found mainly in the United States Their living arrangement involves one male living with four or so females in a territory defended by all individuals and underground nesting Black tails only have one litter per year and are in estrous for only a single day around the beginning of spring A seven year natural experiment by John Hoogland and others from Princeton University revealed that infanticide is widespread in this species including infanticide from invading males and immigrant females as well as occasional cannibalism of an individual s own offspring 3 The surprising finding of the study was that by far the most common type of infanticide involved the killing of close kin s offspring This seems illogical as kin selection favors behaviors that promote the well being of closely related individuals It was postulated that this form of infanticide is more successful than trying to kill young in nearby groups as the whole group must be bypassed in this case while within a group only the mother need be evaded Marauding behavior is evidently adaptive as infanticidal females had more and healthier young than others and were heavier themselves as well This behavior appears to reduce competition with other females for food and future competition among offspring Similar behavior has been reported in the meerkat Suricata suricatta including cases of females killing their mother s sister s and daughter s offspring Infanticidal raids from neighboring groups also occurred 21 Other editBottlenose dolphins have been reported to kill their young through impact injuries 22 Dominant male langurs tend to kill the existing young upon taking control of a harem 23 There have been sightings of infanticide in the leopard population 24 The males of the Stegodyphus lineatus species of spider have been known to exhibit infanticide as a way to encourage females to mate again In mammals male infanticide is most often observed in non seasonal breeders 25 There is less fitness advantage for a conspecific to carry out infanticide if the interbirth period of the mother will not be decreased and the female will not return to estrous In Felidae birthing periods can happen anytime during the year as long as there is not an unweaned offspring of that female This is a contributor to the frequency of infanticide in carnivorous felids 26 25 Some species of seasonal breeders have been observed to commit infanticide Cases in the snub nosed monkey a seasonal breeding primate have shown that infanticide does lessen the interbirth period of the females and can allow them to breed with the next breeding group 27 Other cases of seasonal breeding species where the infanticidal characteristic is observed has been explained as a way of preserving the mother s resources and energy in turn increasing the reproductive success of upcoming breeding periods 28 Costs and defenses editCosts of the behavior edit While it may be beneficial for some species to behave this way infanticide is not without risks to the perpetrator Having already expended energy and perhaps sustained serious wounds in a fight with another male attacks from females who vigorously defend their offspring may be telling for harem polygynous males with a risk of infection It is also energetically costly to pursue a mother s young which may try to escape Costs of the behavior described in prairie dogs include the risk to an individual of losing their own young while killing another s not to mention the fact that they are killing their own relatives In a species where infanticide is common perpetrators may well be victims themselves in the future such that they come out no better off but as long as an infanticidal individual gains in reproductive output by its behavior it will tend to become common Further costs of the behavior in general may be induced by counter strategies evolved in the other sex as described below As a cost of social behavior edit Taking a broader view of the black tailed prairie dog situation infanticide can be seen as a cost of social living 3 If each female were to have her own private nest away from others she would be much less likely to have her infants killed when absent This and other costs such as increased spread of parasites must be made up for by other benefits such as group territory defense and increased awareness of predators An avian example published in Nature is acorn woodpeckers Females nest together possibly because those nesting alone have their eggs constantly destroyed by rivals Even so eggs are consistently removed at first by nest partners themselves until the entire group lays on the same day They then cooperate and incubate the eggs as a group but by this time a significant proportion of their eggs have been lost because of this ovicidal behavior 29 Counter strategies edit Because this form of infanticide reduces the fitness of killed individuals parents animals have evolved a range of counter strategies against this behavior These may be divided into two very different classes those that tend to prevent infanticide and those that minimize losses Loss minimization edit Some females abort or resorb their own young while they are still in development after a new male takes over this is known as the Bruce effect 30 This may prevent their young from being killed after birth saving the mother wasted time and energy However this strategy also benefits the new male In mice this can occur by the proximate mechanism of the female smelling the odor of the new male s urine 31 Preventive adaptations edit Infanticide in burying beetles may have led to male parental care 32 In this species males often cooperate with the female in preparing a piece of carrion which is buried with the eggs and eaten by the larvae when they hatch Males may also guard the site alongside the female It is apparent from experiments that this behavior does not provide their young with any better nourishment nor is it of any use in defending against predators However other burying bugs may try to take their nesting space When this occurs a male female pair is over twice as successful in nest defense preventing the ovicide of their offspring Female langurs may leave the group with their young alongside the outgoing male and others may develop a false estrous and allow the male to copulate deceiving him into thinking she is actually sexually receptive 33 Females may also have sexual liaisons with other males This promiscuous behavior is adaptive because males will not know whether it is their own offspring they are killing or not and may be more reluctant or invest less effort in infanticide attempts 34 Lionesses cooperatively guard against scouting males and a pair were seen to violently attack a male after it killed one of their young 35 Resistance to infanticide is also costly though for instance a female may sustain serious injuries in defending her young At times it is simply more advantageous to submit than to fight 36 Infanticide the destruction of offspring characteristic to many species has posed so great a threat that there have been observable changes of behavior in respective female mothers more specifically these changes exist as preventive measures A common behavioral mechanism by females to reduce the risk of infanticide of future offspring is through the process of paternity confusion or dilution In theory this implies that a female that mates with multiple males will widely spread the assumption of paternity across many males and therefore make them less likely to kill or attack offspring that could potentially carry their genes This theory operates under the assumption that the specific males keep a memory of past mates under a desire to perpetuate their own genes 37 In the Japanese macaque macaca fuscata female mating with multiple males or dilution of paternity was found to inhibit male to infant aggression and infanticide eight times less towards infants of females with which they had previously mated 38 Multi male mating or MMM is recorded as a measure to prevent infanticide in species where young is altricial or heavily dependent and where there is a high turnover rate for dominant males which leads to infanticide of the previous dominant male s young Examples include but are not limited to white footed mice hamsters lions langurs baboons and macaques 34 Along with mating with multiple males the mating of females throughout the entirety of a reproductive cycle also serves a purpose for inhibiting the chance of infanticide This theory assumes that males use information on past matings to make decisions on committing infanticide and that females subsequently manipulate that knowledge Females which are able to appear sexually active or receptive at all stages of their cycle even during pregnancy with another male s offspring can confuse the males into believing that the subsequent children are theirs 37 This pseudo estrus theory applies to females within species that do not exhibit obvious clues to each stage of their cycle such as langurs rhesus macaques and gelada baboons 37 An alternative to paternity confusion as a method of infanticide prevention is paternity concentration This is the behavior of females to concentrate paternity to one specific dominant male as a means of protection from infanticide at the hands of less dominant males 34 This particularly applies to species in which a male has a very long tenure as the dominant male and faces little instability in this hierarchy Females choose these dominant males as the best available form of protection and therefore mate exclusively with this male This is especially common within small rodents 34 An additional behavioral strategy to prevent infanticide by males may be aggressive protection of the nest along with female presence This strategy is commonly used in species such as European rabbits 39 40 Aggressive protection of the nest in an effort to reduce infanticide is observed with the Black Rock Skink Egernia saxatilis live in small families and adults defend their territories against conspecifics The small nuclear families live in the same permanent shelter and the parents protect their infants from infanticidal conspecifics in this way Adults attack unrelated juveniles but not their own offspring The presence of a parent significantly reduces the rate of infanticide because conspecific adults ignore juveniles when a parent is present likely because another adult is more threatening to the aggressive lizard Therefore a juvenile living within its parents own territory will experience far less attacks from conspecific adults 41 42 Infanticide by parents and caregivers editSee also Filial cannibalism nbsp Damselfish may eat their own offspring 43 Filial infanticide occurs when a parent kills its own offspring Both male and female parents have been observed to do this as well as sterile worker castes in some eusocial animals Filial infanticide is also observed as a form of brood reduction in some birds species such as the white stork 44 This may be due to a lack of siblicide in this species 45 Maternal edit Maternal infanticide occurs when newborn offspring are killed by their mother This is sometimes seen in pigs 46 a behavior known as savaging which affects up to 5 of gilts Similar behavior has been observed in various animals such as rabbits 47 hamsters 48 humans and burying beetles 49 Paternal edit Paternal infanticide where fathers eat their own offspring may also occur When young bass hatch from the spawn the father guards the area circling around them and keeping them together as well as providing protection from would be predators After a few days most of the fish will swim away At this point the male s behavior changes instead of defending the stragglers he treats them as any other small prey and eats them 50 Worker caste killing young edit Honey bees may become infected with a bacterial disease called foul brood which attacks the developing bee larva while still living in the cell Some hives however have evolved a behavioral adaptation that resists this disease the worker bees selectively kill the infected individuals by removing them from their cells and tossing them out of the hive preventing it from spreading The genetics of this behavior are quite complex Experiments by Rothenbuhler showed that the hygienic behavior of the queen was lost by crossing with a non hygienic drone This means that the trait must be recessive only being expressed when both alleles contain the gene for hygienic behavior Furthermore the behavior is dependent on two separate loci A backcross produced a mixed result The hives of some offspring were hygienic while others were not There was also a third type of hive where workers removed the wax cap of the infected cells but did nothing more What was not apparent was the presence of a fourth group who threw diseased larvae out of the hive but did not have the uncapping gene This was suspected by Rothenbuhler however who manually removed the caps and found some hives proceeded to clear out infected cells 51 52 Humans and infanticide editMain article Infanticide Family structure is the most important risk factor in child abuse and infanticide Children who live with both their natural biological parents are at low risk for abuse The risk increases greatly when children live with step parents or with a single parent Children living without either parent foster children are 10 times more likely to be abused than children who live with both biological parents citation needed Children who live with a single parent that has a live in partner are at the highest risk they are 20 times more likely to be victims of child abuse than children living with both biological parents 53 Infanticide is a subject that some humans may find discomforting Cornell University ethologist Glenn Hausfater states that infanticide has not received much study because it s a repulsive subject Many people regard it as reprehensible to even think about it Research into infanticide in animals is in part motivated by the desire to understand human behaviors such as child abuse Hausfater explains that researchers are trying to see if there s any connection between animal infanticide and child abuse neglect and killing by humans We just don t know yet what the connections are 54 Infanticide has been and still is practiced by some human cultures groups or individuals citation needed In many past societies certain forms of infanticide were considered permissible whereas in most modern societies the practice is considered immoral and criminal It still takes place in the Western world usually because of the parent s mental illness or violent behavior in addition to some poor countries as a form of population control sometimes with tacit societal acceptance Female infanticide a form of sex selective infanticide is more common than the killing of male offspring especially in cultures where male children are more desirable See also editHuman infanticide Infanticide in carnivores Infanticide in primates Infanticide in rodents Parent offspring conflict Parricide Paternal care Runt Sexual cannibalism Sexual selection SiblicideFurther reading editAlcock J 1998 Animal Behavior An Evolutionary Approach 6th ed Sinauer Associates Inc Sunderland Massachusetts ISBN 978 0 87893 009 8 Parmigiani S vom Saal F S 1994 Infanticide and Parental Care Harwood London p 493 ISBN 978 3 7186 5505 2 van Schaik C P Janson C H 2000 Infanticide By Males And Its Implications Cambridge University Press p 569 ISBN 978 0 521 77295 2 References edit a b c Pusey A E Packer C 1994 Infanticide in lions In Parmigiani S vom Saal F S eds Infanticide and Parental Care Harwood Academic Press Chur Switzerland ISBN 9783718655052 Logos Aleksandar A 2022 Jasenovac in Croatia or a short story about a war and mass killing in it p 10 Retrieved 2022 09 11 a b c Hoogland J L 1985 Infanticide in prairie dogs Lactating females kill offspring of close kin Science 230 4729 1037 1040 Bibcode 1985Sci 230 1037H doi 10 1126 science 230 4729 1037 PMID 17814930 S2CID 23653101 Logos Aleksandar A 2022 Jasenovac in Croatia or a short story about a war and mass killing in it pp 10 and note 28 Retrieved 2022 09 11 a b Palombit Ryne A 2015 06 01 Infanticide as Sexual Conflict Coevolution of Male Strategies and Female Counterstrategies Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Biology 7 6 a017640 doi 10 1101 cshperspect a017640 ISSN 1943 0264 PMC 4448612 PMID 25986557 Sugiyama Y 1965 On the social change of Hanuman langurs Presbytis entellus in their natural conditions Primates 6 3 4 381 417 doi 10 1007 BF01730356 S2CID 26758190 Lorenz K 1966 On Aggression New York Harcourt Brace and Worl Hausfater G S B Hrdy 1984 Infanticide Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives New York Aldine ISBN 978 0 202 02022 8 Mating conflict in primates infanticide sexual harassment and female sexuality a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a Cite journal requires journal help a b Lukas Dieter Huchard Elise 2014 The evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian societies PDF Science 346 6211 841 4 Bibcode 2014Sci 346 841L doi 10 1126 science 1257226 PMID 25395534 S2CID 206559886 Hrdy D B 1977 Infanticide as a primate reproductive strategy American Scientist 65 1 40 49 Bibcode 1977AmSci 65 40B PMID 402873 a b Hausfater Glenn 1984 08 01 Infanticide Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives Current Anthropology 25 4 500 502 doi 10 1086 203169 ISSN 0011 3204 S2CID 84753908 Perrigo G Bryant W C Vomsaal F 1990 A unique neural timing system prevents male mice from harming their own offspring Animal Behaviour 39 3 535 539 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 05 80419 1 S2CID 53200704 Hausfater G 1984 Infanticide Comparative and evolutionary perspectives Current Anthropology 25 4 500 502 doi 10 1086 203169 JSTOR 2742911 S2CID 84753908 a b Freed L A 1987 Prospective infanticide and protection of genetic paternity in tropical house wrens The American Naturalist 130 6 948 954 doi 10 1086 284758 S2CID 84236418 Emlen S T Demong N J Emlen D J 1989 Experimental induction of infanticide in female wattled jacanas PDF Auk 106 1 1 7 doi 10 2307 4087750 JSTOR 4087750 For footage of this see Attenborough D 1990 The Trials of Life Episode 12 or Evolution Episode 3 Why Sex Zimmer Carl 1996 09 01 First kill the babies Discover Retrieved 2008 01 19 a b Ichikawa N 1995 Male counterstrategy against infanticide of the female giant water bug Lethocerus deyrollei Hemiptera Belostomatidae Journal of Insect Behavior 8 2 181 186 doi 10 1007 BF01988903 S2CID 41306483 Infanticide in Norway rats ratbehavior org Clutton Brock T H P N M B Smith R McIlrath G M Kansky R Gaynor D O Riain M J Skinner J D 1998 Infanticide and expulsion of females in a cooperative mammal Proceedings Biological Sciences 265 1412 2291 2295 doi 10 1098 rspb 1998 0573 JSTOR 51272 PMC 1689533 PMID 9881475 Milius S July 18 1998 Infanticide reported in dolphins Science News Vol 154 no 3 p 36 doi 10 2307 4010610 JSTOR 4010610 Retrieved 2007 05 22 The evolution of infanticidal mechanisms in male langurs PDF Retrieved 2007 05 22 permanent dead link Fildes Jonathan 2007 05 29 Cheating cheetahs caught by DNA BBC News Retrieved 2007 05 30 a b Lukas Dieter Huchard Elise 2014 11 14 The evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian societies PDF Science 346 6211 841 844 Bibcode 2014Sci 346 841L doi 10 1126 science 1257226 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 25395534 S2CID 206559886 Balme Guy A Hunter Luke T B 2013 10 01 Why leopards commit infanticide Animal Behaviour 86 4 791 799 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2013 07 019 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 53266285 Yao Hui Yu Huiliang Yang Banghe Yang Wangji Xu Haiqing Grueter Cyril C Li Ming Xiang Zuofu 2016 02 02 Male Infanticide in the Golden Snub Nosed Monkey Rhinopithecus roxellana a Seasonally Breeding Primate International Journal of Primatology 37 2 175 184 doi 10 1007 s10764 016 9892 2 ISSN 0164 0291 S2CID 14544830 Hrdy Sarah Hausfater G 1984 01 01 Comparative and evolutionary perspectives on infanticide An introduction and overview Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives Mumme R L Koenig W D Pitelka F A 1983 Reproductive competition in the communal acorn woodpecker Sisters destroy each other s eggs Nature 306 5943 583 584 Bibcode 1983Natur 306 583M doi 10 1038 306583a0 S2CID 19299353 Bruce H M 1959 An exteroceptive block to pregnancy in the mouse Nature 184 4680 105 Bibcode 1959Natur 184 105B doi 10 1038 184105a0 PMID 13805128 S2CID 4200823 Labov J B 1981 Pregnancy blocking in rodents Adaptive advantages for females American Naturalist 118 3 361 371 doi 10 1086 283828 JSTOR 2460637 S2CID 85020158 Scott M P 1990 Brood guarding and the evolution of male parental care in burying beetles Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 26 1 31 40 doi 10 1007 BF00174022 JSTOR 4600371 S2CID 23748510 Hrdy S B 1977 The Langurs of Abu Harvard University Press Cambridge MA ISBN 978 0674510586 a b c d Agrell J Wolff J Ylonen H Ylonen Hannu 1998 Counter strategies to infanticide in mammals Costs and consequences Oikos 83 3 507 517 doi 10 2307 3546678 JSTOR 3546678 Packer C Pusey A 1983 Adaptations of female lions to infanticide by incoming males The American Naturalist 121 5 716 728 doi 10 1086 284097 S2CID 84927815 Yamamura N Hasegawa T Ito Y 1990 Why mothers do not resist infanticide A cost benefit genetic model PDF Evolution 44 5 1346 1357 doi 10 2307 2409294 JSTOR 2409294 PMID 28563893 a b c Hrdy S B 1979 Infanticide among animals A review classification and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females Ethology and Sociobiology 1 13 18 doi 10 1016 0162 3095 79 90004 9 Soltis J Thomsen R Matsubayashi K Takenaka O 2000 Infanticide by resident males and female counter strategies in wild Japanese macaques Macaca fuscata Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 48 3 195 doi 10 1007 s002650000224 S2CID 22485349 Rodel H G Starkloff A 2008 Infanticide and maternal offspring defence in european rabbits under natural breeding conditions Ethology 114 1 22 31 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 2007 01447 x Yoken H Haapakoski A 2017 How do infanticidal male bank voles myodes glarelus find the next with pups Ethology 123 2 105 112 doi 10 1111 eth 12579 O Connor D Shine R 2004 Parental care protects against infanticide in the lizard Egernia saxatilis Scincidae Animal Behaviour 68 1361 1369 O Connor D Shine R 2003 Lizards in nuclear families A novel reptilian social system in Egernia saxatilis Scincidae Molecular Ecology 12 3 743 752 doi 10 1046 j 1365 294X 2003 01777 x PMID 12675829 S2CID 45090454 Payne A G Smith C Campbell A C 2002 Filial cannibalism improves survival and development of Beaugregory damselfish embryos Proceedings Biological Sciences 269 1505 2095 2102 doi 10 1098 rspb 2002 2144 PMC 1691142 PMID 12396483 Tortosa Francisco S Redondo Tomas 1992 Motives for Parental Infanticide in White Storks Ciconia ciconia Ornis Scandinavica Scandinavian Journal of Ornithology 23 2 185 189 doi 10 2307 3676447 JSTOR 3676447 Zielinski Piotr December 2002 Brood Reduction and Parental Infanticide are the White StorkCiconia ciconiaand the Black StorkC nigraexceptional Acta Ornithologica 37 2 113 119 doi 10 3161 068 037 0207 ISSN 0001 6454 North Carolina Pork Conference Archived 2007 08 20 at the Wayback Machine notes 2002 North Carolina State University Boyd I L 1985 Investment in growth by pregnant wild rabbits in relation to litter size and sex of the offspring The Journal of Animal Ecology 54 1 137 147 doi 10 2307 4626 JSTOR 4626 What Can You Do to Keep Your Hamster From Eating Its Own Babies The Spruce Pets Retrieved 2022 06 15 Trumbo S 1994 Interspecific competition brood parasitism and the evolution of biparental cooperation in Burying beetles Oikos 69 2 241 249 doi 10 2307 3546144 JSTOR 3546144 Elgar M A Crespi B J eds 1992 Cannibalism Ecology and Evolution of Cannibalism among Diverse Taxa Oxford University Press New York ISBN 978 0 19 854650 4 Dawkins R 1976 The Selfish Gene Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 286092 7 Rothenbuhler W C 1964 Behavior genetics of nest cleaning in honey bees IV Responses of F1 and backcross generations to disease killed brood American Zoologist 4 2 111 123 doi 10 1093 icb 4 2 111 PMID 14172721 Sedlak A J Mettenburg J Basena M Petta I McPherson K Greene A and Li S 2010 Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect NIS 4 Report to Congress Executive Summary Washington DC U S Department of Health and Human Services Administration for Children and Families Bayard Webster 1982 08 17 Infanticide Animal behavior scrutinized for clues to humans The New York Times Retrieved 2008 01 18 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Infanticide zoology amp oldid 1192716577, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.