fbpx
Wikipedia

Iatmul language

Iatmul is the language of the Iatmul people, spoken around the Sepik River in the East Sepik Province, northern Papua New Guinea.[2] The Iatmul, however, do not refer to their language by the term Iatmul, but call it gepmakudi ("village language", from gepma = "village" and kudi "speech"; pronounced as [ŋɡɛpmaɡundi]).

Iatmul
gepmakudi
Native toPapua New Guinea
RegionSepik River basin
EthnicityIatmul people
Native speakers
8,400 (2003)[1]
Dialects
  • Nyaula or Nyaura (Western)
  • Pali'bei or Palimbei (Central)
  • Waliakwi (Eastern)
  • Maligwat (Northern)
Language codes
ISO 639-3ian
Glottologiatm1242
ELPIatmul
East Sepik Province in Papua New Guinea

There are about 8,400 Iatmul traditionally organized in villages, whereas a total amount of 46,000 speakers is estimated.[3] The inhabitants of the villages are trilingual, being fluent with Tok Pisin, good with Iatmul and having some knowledge of English. Tok Pisin is also the first language of the youngest children, despite efforts to reverse this trend.[4]

An extensive grammar of Iatmul has been recently written by Gerd Jendraschek as a postdoctoral thesis.[2]

Typological profile edit

Iatmul is part of the Ndu language family, which consists of at least six languages in which ndu is the word for 'man'. Together with Manambu it is the southernmost language of the Ndu family, spoken along the Sepik River.[5] Iatmul is perhaps the best known Ndu language of them all.[6]

Iatmul is a moderately agglutinative and nearly isolating language. Flexion is predominantly suffixed and very regular, whereas the phonological processes are the most complex ones within the language. Stems often change their form while multiple-morpheme structures can become so coalescent that they are difficult to segment.[7]

Iatmul has masculine and feminine gender marking as well as singular, dual and plural numbers.[8] Nouns and verbs are the only two major classes in Iatmul with only little derivation across them.[8] Also, there is not a strong distinction between modifiers and nouns as many roots can be used as nouns, adjectives or adverbs.[9] Smaller word classes include personal pronouns, demonstratives, postpositions, quantifiers, interrogatives as well as proclauses (yes, no), while there are no clause-linking conjunctions.[10]

Phonological system edit

Vowel and consonant phonemes edit

The phonological system of Iatmul is a matter of controversy among scholars. There is no consensus about how many vowels Iatmul has and about which realisations are to be considered as phonemes or allophones. There were attempts of analysing the language as consisting of only 1-3 vowels by Staalsen (1966) and Laycock (1991).[11] Jendraschek (2012) in contrast describes 12 phonemic monophthongs and 7 phonemic diphthongs.[12]

Monophthongs of Iatmul[13]
Front Central Back
High i ⟨i⟩ ɨ ⟨i'⟩ u ⟨u⟩, ⟨uu⟩
Mid e ⟨e⟩, () ⟨ee⟩ o ⟨o⟩
Low plain a ⟨a⟩, ⟨aa⟩  ɑ ⟨â⟩3
glottalised ⟨a'⟩ ɑˀ ⟨â'⟩
Diphthongs of Iatmul[14]
Front Back
High [ɨi̯] ⟨wi⟩6
Mid ɛɪ̯ ⟨ei⟩ ou̯ ⟨ou⟩
Low plain ai̯~ɒi̯ ⟨ai⟩ aːi̯ ⟨aai⟩ aʊ̯ ⟨au⟩ aːʊ̯ ⟨aau⟩
glottalised aˀi̯ ⟨a'i⟩
Consonant Phonemes of Iatmul[15]
  Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar
Stop fortis ⟨pp⟩ ⟨tt⟩ ⟨kk⟩
nasal (pᵐ) ⟨p'⟩5 kᵑ ⟨k'⟩4
oral lenis p ⟨p⟩ t ⟨t⟩ k ⟨k⟩
prenasalized lenis ᵐb ⟨b⟩ ⁿd ⟨d⟩ ᵑɡ ⟨g⟩
Affricate t͡ʃ ⟨ch⟩ ⁿd͡ʒ ⟨j⟩
Fricative β ⟨v⟩ s ⟨s⟩
Nasal m ⟨m⟩ n ⟨n⟩ ɲ ⟨ny⟩ (ŋ) ⟨n'⟩1
Approximant w ⟨w⟩ ɾ~l ⟨l⟩2 j ⟨j⟩

In Iatmul, the contrast between the fortis, lenis, and prenasalized stops exists only intervocalically. The fully oral stops can be voiced (to /b/, /d/, /g/), and as such they have voiced allophones in certain contexts. The latter also tend to be more open than the fortis stops, which means that they can become taps, fricatives, or approximants in post-sonorant position. Prenasalized stops are inherently voiced. As such, the distinctive feature between the fortis and oral series is not voicing, but rather voiceability.[7]

  1. /g/ and /k/+/n/ are identical phonetically, both producing [ŋɡ], and the only way to distinguish them is morphology. [n] and [ŋ] are also in free variation word-finally, which is maintained when suffixes are added. Thus, [ŋ] is written as n'.
  2. /l/ and /ɾ/ are in free variation, they are both written as l. The lateral is usually the predominant realisation, other than in western Iatmul, where the tap is dominant.
  3. Â is only written with the circumflex if a contrasting word with /a/ exists.
  4. /kᵑ/ mainly occurs as an allophone of /k/ before vowel-initial suffixes, according to Jendraschek there are only three words where it is phonemic.
  5. /pᵐ/ only occurs as an allophone of /p/ before vowel-initial suffixes. It is indicated orthographically.
  6. [ɨi̯] is underlying /wi/, and occurs after /m/. It can also tend towards [ʉi̯].

Variation of oral stops edit

Source: Jendraschek[7]
Phoneme Initial Post-Sonorant/
Intervocalic
Post-nasal
fortis Labial // n/a [p] n/a
Alveolar // n/a [t] n/a
Alveolar // n/a [k] n/a
oral Labial /p/ [p] [p~β]2 [p]
Alveolar /t/ [t~ɾ]1 [ɾ~d] [d]
Alveolar /k/ [k] [g~ɣ~ɰ] [g]
nasal Labial /ᵐb/ [ᵐb] [b]
Alveolar /ⁿd/ [ⁿd] [d]
Alveolar /ᵑɡ/ [ᵑɡ] [g]
  1. /t/'s alternation to /ɾ/ is always indicated in the orthography
  2. Unlike /t/ and /k/, /p/ is sometimes "resistant" to lenition. Lenited /p/ is also always written as v.

Segmental phonology edit

Syllable structure edit

The canonical syllable structure of Iatmul is C(A)V(C), where the first consonant can be any consonant other than the "geminates". Only approximants/liquids can be in the second onset position. Possible codas are only [p], [t], [k], [m], [n], and [ɲ] (the lenis plosives and nasals). Most words begin with the nasal consonants [m], [n] or the plosive consonants [p], [k].[16] Excluded from this rule are about 5% of the words in Iatmul, which begin with the vowels [a] or [a:].[17]

Assimilation edit

A very common phenomenon in Iatmul is regressive assimilation.[16]

(1)

kuvin

straighten

-bi'k

dual suffix

 

[ˈkuβimbɨk]

'you/they both straightened'

kuvin -bi'k → [ˈkuβimbɨk]

straighten {dual suffix} {} {'you/they both straightened'}

Assimilation can be blocked to avoid ambiguity. Whereas (2a) is ambiguous due to assimilation of bâk to bap, example (2b) is not ambiguous as assimilation was blocked in favour of a clear meaning.[18]

(2) a.

wugi

D3.SG.M

bap-ba

moon-LOC

ki’-di

eat-3PL

wugi bap-ba ki’-di

D3.SG.M moon-LOC eat-3PL

'they were eating (during) that month' or
'they were eating (from) that pig’

(2) b.

wugi

D3.SG.M

bâk-ba

pig-LOC

ki’-di

eat-3PL

wugi bâk-ba ki’-di

D3.SG.M pig-LOC eat-3PL

'they were eating (from) that pig'[18]

Elision and fusion edit

Elision can also be encountered very frequently. In the following example, both vowel fusion and onset elision are operating at the same time, making the meaning of the final form hard to recognize.[12]

(3)

wa-ka-a

say-PRS-SR

 

wa-a

say-PRS.SR

 

wa:

 

wa-ka-a → wa-a → wa:

say-PRS-SR {} say-PRS.SR {} {}

Epenthesis edit

Epenthesis can be observed when for example focus is marked with the suffix -a:

(4)

kada

who

-a

FOC(3.SG.M)

 

kada-n-a

 

kada -a → kada-n-a

who FOC(3.SG.M) {} {}

Reduction edit

Reduction mostly happens quantitatively, so that the vowels are shorted in their length. Qualitative reduction happens on a rare occasion and mainly occurs during monophthongization.[20]

Lexical stress edit

In Iatmul, words are not differentiated via pitch or accentuation. The meanings of words are not autonomous, but influenced by factors like vocal length, syllable structure and speed of enunciation. The following rules are the most important ones in descending order:[21]

  • Long vowels are stressed (ti’baadi’ [tɨˈmbaːndɨ], 'he can stay')
  • CVC syllables are stressed (ti’kali’ka [tɨˈɡaɾɡa], 'meanwhile')
  • Word-initial syllables are stressed.[21]

In compounds, these rules can be randomly applied to the first or to the second word.[21] As diphthongs are not stressed, there are no long monophthongized diphthongs.[22]ref>

Sentence intonation edit

A phonological utterance ends with falling intonation. Simple sentences are marked via pauses. Complex sentences end with falling intonation. Questions are marked via variations in pitch. Focus is marked by initial high intonation and a subsequent fall of the pitch.[23]

Morphological properties edit

Verbal structure edit

Iatmul is a moderately agglutinative language in which suffixes are dominating.[7] Therefore, much information is being expressed morphologically instead of syntactically, which is true especially for the category of the verb. Information regarding actions like the direction, the manner or temporal relations are expressed via derivation of the verbs. There are many affixes specifying the manner in which an action is performed, like the means or the amount of control over the action.[24]

Structure of a fully inflected verb[24]
-2 -1 1 2 3 4 5 6
Neg Manner Root(s) Directional Event-specifier Aspect Tense/Mood Subject-agreement
Derivation -------------------------------------------------------→ ←------------------------ Inflection

Person edit

Iatmul pronouns and cross-ref suffixes[25]
Number Pronouns Cross-reference (to the subject) markers
Free Right-bound Left-bound
on verbs
Left-bound on
non-verb predicates
Left and
right-bound
1sg wun wun- -wun -wun -w-
2sg masc mi'n mi'n- -mi'n -mi'n -m-
2sg fem nyi'n nyi'n- -nyi'n -nyi'n -ny-
3sg masc di' di'n- -di' -a -d-
3sg fem li' li'l- -li' -ak -l-
1dl (dual) an an- -li -li -li-
2/3dl bi'k bi'l- -bi'k -bi'k -b-
1pl ni'n ni'n- -ni'n -ni'n -n-
2pl guk gul- -guk -guk -gw-
3pl di din- -di -di -j-

Tense, mood and aspect edit

Temporal marking exists in present tense and past tense, but not in the future tense. The marker for the present tense is -ka and in some cases the allomorph -a. The past tense is unmarked. Thus, some tenses can be distinguished only by the length of a single vowel.[26]

(1) a.

kiya-a-di'

bring-PRS-3SG.M

kiya-a-di'

bring-PRS-3SG.M

'he brings'

(1) b.

kiya-Ø-di'

bring-PST-3SG.M

kiya-Ø-di'

bring-PST-3SG.M

'he brought'[26]

The expression of future is covered by the irrealis mood with the allomorphs -kiya,-ikiya (after roots ending with -a) and it' short form -ika occurring in fast speech. Besides future reference, the irrealis expresses possibility and permission and can be used in conditional constructions.[27]

(2)

ya-ikiya-wun

come-IRR-1SG

ya-ikiya-wun

come-IRR-1SG

'I will come'[27]

The imperfective aspect marker -ti'~li' occurs most often with the present tense. Therefore, in some constructions in which present cannot be marked (e.g. nominalized clauses), the imperfective expresses present time reference. Thus, it' semantics is about to shift from aspect to tense.[28]

Other aspects in Iatmul are

  • Hortative marked with -kak, -li, -lu
  • Optative marked with -ba and -ka
  • Apprehensive marked with -ka
  • Imperative, which is built using the bare stem or additionally by the suffix -li'.[29]

Event specifiers edit

A special category in Iatmul are event-specifier-suffixes. Temporal relations are expressed with

  • -jibu: event takes place during the whole night until sunrise
  • -pwali: expresses that the event is done continuously
  • -ki’va: expresses that something else is done in the middle of one action (interruption)
  • -si’bla: expresses that an event occurs as the first one before any other events (anteriority) [30]
(3)

nyiga

leaf

kasa-jibu-di

play-DAY.BREAK-3PL

nyiga kasa-jibu-di

leaf play-DAY.BREAK-3PL

'they played cards until daybreak'[31]

Other event-specifiers express the extent of the action (complete, incomplete, all of its parts) or the frustrative (attempt, failure).[32]

Case marking edit

In Iatmul, at least three cases with overlapping functions are assumed. Jendraschek argues, that it is not possible to define basic meanings for the cases, as the case markers often are polysemous.[33] In general, case marking does not depend on the head but also on pragmatic circumstances and especially semantic properties of the controlling verb and its dependent noun. Thus, there is no strict division into structural and semantic cases in Iatmul.[34]

Case marking has the whole noun phrase in its scope so that they are placed at the end of the phrase. This is also the case when the end of the word is not the head noun.[33]

(4)

waani

erima

ki'ta

tree

gepma

one

ki'ta-ba

[village one]NP-LOC

wakkai-laa

ascend:go-CONSEC

li'di'

stay-3SG.M

waani ki'ta gepma ki'ta-ba wakkai-laa li'di'

erima tree one {[village one]NP-LOC} ascend:go-CONSEC stay-3SG.M

'There was an erima tree growing in one village'[33]

Nominative case (zero marking) edit

Subjects as well as copula subjects and copula complements are always zero-marked with respect to their case. The direct object remains unmarked if it is low in animacy or definiteness.[35] Goals are not always marked with an overt case-marker, especially when they are inanimate.[36]

Dative case edit

The marker for the so-called dative is -kak with the allomorph -kat.[37] As pronouns and proper nouns are always definite, they are marked with dativ case when they are used as transitive objects.[38] When definite nouns referring to humans are transitive objects, the marker is preferred but not obligatory. The same is the case with non-human animates (like animals), whereas here the marking more likely indicates specificity of the object ('the dog' or 'one dog'):[39]

(5) a.

wa'la-kak

dog-DAT

vi'-mi'n?

see-2SG.M

wa'la-kak vi'-mi'n?

dog-DAT see-2SG.M

'Have you seen the dog?'

(5) b.

wa'la

dog

vi'-min?

see-2SG.M

wa'la vi'-min?

dog see-2SG.M

'Have you seen a/the dog?'

Whether a transitive objects has to be marked with -kak is also depending on the meaning of the predicate. Dead animals are not perceived the same way as animate, specific animals, so that marking is not employed in sentences where someone eats an animal. On the other hand, when an animal eats a human, marking is employed.[40]

Beside transitive objects, other semantic roles like recipient, addressee, theme and reason can be marked with the dative marker.[41] Animate goals are also marked with the dative case.[42]

Locative case edit

The so-called locative is marked with -(na)ba.[43] Its main function is to mark locations, in some contexts including time expressions.[44] Regarding transitive objects, its functions overlap with those of the dative marker -kak, so that in some context it is assumed that there is no difference in meaning between both markers. Still, usually it marks transitive objects with non-human referents including those who do not accept -kak.[43]

(6)

wan

D3.SG.M

gusi'ga-ba

box-LOC

ta

stay:SR

da-ba

thing-LOC

ki'-ka

eat-DEP

li'-di'

stay-3SG.M

wan gusi'ga-ba ta da-ba ki'-ka li'-di'

D3.SG.M box-LOC stay:SR thing-LOC eat-DEP stay-3SG.M

'He was already eating the things that were in that box.'[45]

Besides marking of location and transitive objects, the locative case can also mark themes, manner, material, instruments, goals, sources and animate recipients.[46] In case of the animate recipients, -ba can only be used if the recipient is perceived more as a location where something is left instead of the finial recipient.[47]

Other cases edit

  • Allative marker -ak and allomorph -alak: marks goal in the sense of purposes of activities instead of locations as well as recipients.[48]
  • Comitative marker -(a)na(la) and -akwi or (-)okwi: It is unclear according to which rules these allomorphs are distributed.[49]
  • Vocative marker -o: The only function of the vocative is to signal that a noun phrase refers to the addressee. It is not used obligatorily but rather when the name has to be repeated in case the addressee did not hear the first call.[50]

Gender edit

In Iatmul, gender is not marked on the noun but on its modifiers (demonstratives) instead as well as via subject-verb-agreement. Often this is also true for number-marking, where only some nouns can be marked with a plural suffix.[51]

(7)

kan /

D1.SG.M /

kat

D1.SG.F.

nyaan

child

wega

market

yi-di' /

go-3SG.M /

yi-li'

go-3SG.F

{kan /} kat nyaan wega {yi-di' /} yi-li'

{D1.SG.M /} D1.SG.F. child market {go-3SG.M /} go-3SG.F

'This (male/female) child went to the market.'[51]

(8)

ankwi /

D2.SG.M.NR:REM /

akkwi

D2.SG.F.NR:REM

ji'vwa

work

{ankwi /} akkwi ji'vwa

{D2.SG.M.NR:REM /} D2.SG.F.NR:REM work

'that (hard/simple) work'[51]

Referents without natural gender like inanimate entities are marked with respect to gender depending on their size. Thus, bigger referents are marked as masculine whereas smaller referents are marked as feminine. From both gender markers, masculine is perceived as the unmarked one and thus chosen if no characterisation is intended.[51]

(9)

ki'viya

mosquito

vaali'-ka-di'

bite-PRS-3SG.M

ki'viya vaali'-ka-di'

mosquito bite-PRS-3SG.M

'A large mosquito is stinging [me].'[51]

However, there are some referents in Iatmul without natural gender but still with fixed grammatical gender. Among them are nya 'sun', which is masculine, whereas bap 'moon' is feminine. Also, some animal species have specific gender regardless of their biological gender, like kaami 'fish' (masculine). Some nouns can have both genders interchangeably without a difference in meaning as di'mai 'season'.[52]

Number edit

Plural marking on nouns edit

Iatmul has singular, dual and plural number.[8] As it is with gender, number usually is not marked on the noun. Instead, number can be marked on the determiners or modifiers of the noun as well as via subject-verb-agreement.[53] Plurality can also be marked twice.[54]

(10) a.

wun-a

1SG-GEN

wudi

D3:PL

da

thing

wun-a wudi da

1SG-GEN D3:PL thing

'those things of mine'

(10) b.

wun-a-di

1SG-GEN-PL

wudi

D3:PL

da

thing

wun-a-di wudi da

1SG-GEN-PL D3:PL thing

'those things of mine'[54]

Dual or plural marking on the verb indicates the number of the subject referent.[55] However, this is only true for human referents. Non-human referents are always marked singular on the verb as they are not perceived as individuals.[56]

(11)

wa'k

crocodile

du

man

ki'-li'-ka-di

eat-IPFV-PRS-3PL

wa'k du ki'-li'-ka-di

crocodile man eat-IPFV-PRS-3PL

'the men eat the crocodile' (cannot mean: 'the crocodiles eat the men')[56]

There are some exceptional cases in which nouns can employ plural marking. Kinship terms like walaga ‘ancestor’, nyagei 'sister' or ta’kwa 'wife' can be plural marked. But if ta’kwa is used in the meaning of 'woman', it cannot be suffixed with a plural marker as it does not employ a relational meaning.[53]

(12) a.

di'n-a

3SG.M-GEN

ta'kwa-du

woman/wife-PL

sak

lake

yi-di

go-3PL

di'n-a ta'kwa-du sak yi-di

3SG.M-GEN woman/wife-PL lake go-3PL

'his wives went to the lake'

(12) b.

gepma-na

village-GEN

ta'kwa

woman/wife

sak

lake

yi-di

go-3PL

gepma-na ta'kwa sak yi-di

village-GEN woman/wife lake go-3PL

'the women from the village went to the lake'[57]

The modifiers of a kinship term can be plural-marked also when the kinship term is already plural-marked.[58]

(13)

taba

already

wun-a-di

1SG-GEN-PL

na-du

grandchild-PL

ki’nki-sakkwat-di

D1.SG.M.NR-many-PL

taba wun-a-di na-du ki’nki-sakkwat-di

already 1SG-GEN-PL grandchild-PL D1.SG.M.NR-many-PL

'I have already this many grand-children.'[58]

Reduplication edit

It is also possible to reduplicate nouns. In this case, they do not convey the usual plural meaning, but rather a distributive one referring to the group as a whole, meaning ‘every’ instead of 'more than one'.[57] Some reduplicated nouns additionally employ adverbial meaning, like kava ‘place’ as kava kava 'everywhere' and jibula 'day' as jibula jibula 'all the time'.[59] When adjectives are reduplicated, they may be understood as plural-marked or as employing an elative meaning (good -> very good) depending on the context. When the genitive suffix -na is reduplicated, it can express the plurality of the possessee.[58]

(14)

gepma

village

gepma

village

kalibi'k

carry-2/3DU

mi'na

only

kan

D1.SG.M

missionary-na~na

missionary-GEN~PL

sule

school

ki'nki-a-n

D1.M.SG.NR-3SG-M

wa-a

say-DEP

wa-di

say-3PL

gepma gepma kalibi'k mi'na kan missionary-na~na sule ki'nki-a-n wa-a wa-di

village village carry-2/3DU only D1.SG.M missionary-GEN~PL school D1.M.SG.NR-3SG-M say-DEP say-3PL

'after the two had brought it to every village, they said "these are the missionaries' schools" '[57]

Associative plural edit

The plural suffix -du which is used on kinship terms can be used on proper names to express ‘person together with people related to this person’ (associative plural). Those related people can be his kin, people living in the same house or friends of the referent.[59]

(15)

Magina-du

Magina-PL

ya-a-di

come-PRS-3PL

Magina-du ya-a-di

Magina-PL come-PRS-3PL

'Magina and her mother/family are coming'[54]

Numerals edit

Counting in Iatmul is accomplished in mixture of a quinary, decimal and vigesimal system.[60] Cardinal numerals generally follow the noun. In this case, adjectives are placed before the noun. If numerals close the noun phrase, they carry the case-markers.[61]

  • The number 1 is ki'ta
  • The number 5 is 'one hand', taba-nak (from ta'ba 'hand' and nak '(an)other (one)').
  • The numbers 6 to 9 are counted elliptically as 'plus one', si'la-ki'ta (from si'la 'plus')
  • The number 10 is 'two hands', taba-vli (vli being the short form of vi'li'li'k 'two').
  • The number 15 is not three hands 'but two hands plus one hand', taba-vli kyeli taba-nak
  • The number 20 is 'one whole man', dumi-ki'ta (from du 'man' and mi 'tree', referring to the number of all fingers and toes of a human)
  • The number 30 is 'one whole man plus two hands', dumi-ki’ta kyeli taba-vli
  • The number 100 is 'five whole men', dumi taba-nak.[60]

Ordinal numbers are built by adding wan (probably from wa 'say') to cardinal numerals.[61]

Most Papuan languages incorporated the counting system of Tok Pisin or adopted the English counting system, which are perceived as shorter and more transparent. Due to the more and more frequent use of other counting systems, many speakers of Iatmul cannot count correctly beyond 'twenty' in their own language.[62]

Syntactic properties edit

General syntactic structure edit

There is no passive voice in Iatmul.[24] The canonical sentence structure is SOV: Adverb - Subject - Adjunct - Object - Verb[63]

(1)

Babla

today

Ruth

RuthA

aguvali

opposite

gaai-ba

house-LOC

wun-kak

1SG-DAT

ni’ma

[big

ki’ki’da

food]O

kuk-kiya-li’.

do-IRR-3SG.F

Babla Ruth aguvali gaai-ba wun-kak ni’ma ki’ki’da kuk-kiya-li’.

today RuthA opposite house-LOC 1SG-DAT [big food]O do-IRR-3SG.F

'Ruth will prepare a big meal for me in the house on the other side.'[63]

Yes–no questions are not expressed syntactically but via intonation.[63]

(2)

Ana

NEG

kla-mi'n ?/.

get-2SG.M

Ana {kla-mi'n ?/.}

NEG get-2SG.M

'Didn’t you understand?' or 'You didn’t understand.'[24]

Negation edit

The expression of negation is accomplished by different morphological structures which often are etymologically unrelated.[64]

Verbs are negated by placing the particle ana before the inflected verb. In case of non-verbal predicates or predicatively used adjectives, ana is placed in front of the pronominal subject marker.[64]

(3) a.

Du-mi'n

man-2SG.M

Du-mi'n

man-2SG.M

'You're a man.'[64]

(3) b.

Du

man

ana-mi'n

NEG-2SG.M

Du ana-mi'n

man NEG-2SG.M

'You're not a man.'[64]

(4)

Gepma-ba

village-LOC

ki'-w-a

eat-1SG-SR

ki'ki'da

food

apma

good

ana-di'

NEG-3SG.M

Gepma-ba ki'-w-a ki'ki'da apma ana-di'

village-LOC eat-1SG-SR food good NEG-3SG.M

'the food I ate in the village was not good'[65]

In dependent clauses, negation on the predicate is not possible. Therefore, negation is expressed by a periphrastic construction in which the suffix -lapman 'without' is attached to the predicate and combined with the auxiliary yi 'go'.[65]

(5) a.

*Ana

NEG

vi'-w-a

see-1SG-SR

ka'ik

picture

babla

today

vi'si'makwi-kiya-di

show-IRR-3PL

*Ana vi'-w-a ka'ik babla vi'si'makwi-kiya-di

NEG see-1SG-SR picture today show-IRR-3PL

(5) b.

Vi'-lapman

see-without

yi'-w-a

go-1SG-SR

ka'ik

picture

babla

today

vi'si'makwi-kiya-di

show-IRR-3PL

Vi'-lapman yi'-w-a ka'ik babla vi'si'makwi-kiya-di

see-without go-1SG-SR picture today show-IRR-3PL

'Today they will show the film I haven't seen yet.'[65]

To deny existence, the proclause ka'i is used.[66]

(6)

Wun

1SG

saanya

money

ka'i

no

Wun saanya ka'i

1SG money no

'I don't have any money'[66]

Ke, the reduced form of ka'i, is used together with a non-finite or a subordinate form of the verb to form the prohibitive.[67]

(7)

Ke

PROH

yi-m-∅-a!

go-2SG.M-REAL-SR

Ke yi-m-∅-a!

PROH go-2SG.M-REAL-SR

'Don't go (now)!'[67]

For the irrealis, the negator wana is used which cannot be combined with realis forms.[68]

(8)

Wana

NEG.OPT

yi-m-ay-a!

go-2SG.M-IRR-SR

Wana yi-m-ay-a!

NEG.OPT go-2SG.M-IRR-SR

'Don't go (later)!'[68]

The optative on the other hand is negated by the verbal suffix -la.[68]

(9)

Yiki'nba

gently

va'i,

step

da-la-mi'n

fall-APPR-2SG.M

Yiki'nba va'i, da-la-mi'n

gently step fall-APPR-2SG.M

'Walk slowly, otherwise you might fall.'[69]

Nominalized subordinate clauses edit

One of the two types of subordinate clauses in Iatmul are nominalised clauses, which are used like attributive, adverbial or complement clauses.[70] There are rare cases of headless relative clauses, where the predicate of the relative clause becomes the head.[71] Usually this is avoided by preferring a general noun like du 'man' as a head.[72]

(10)
Relative clause (same subject)

kaami k-a

[fish eat-SR

embedded clause

du

man]

head nominal

kiya-di'

die-3SG.M

predicate

{kaami k-a} du kiya-di'

{[fish eat-SR} man] die-3SG.M

{embedded clause} {head nominal} predicate

'The man who ate fish died.'[70]

When the relative clause has a subject which is not co-referential with the head noun of the matrix clause, then the verb of the relative clause expresses this with a pronominal subject marker on the subordinate verb.[73]

(11)
Relative clause (different subjects)

buk’-w-a

[‍[tell-1SG-SR]

vaak

topic]

gawi-na

eagle-GEN

wapuchapuk

story

buk’-w-a vaak gawi-na wapuchapuk

[‍[tell-1SG-SR] topic] eagle-GEN story

'The topic I was talking about is the story about the eagle'[73]

It is important to note that some relative clauses do not have a common argument in the matrix clause so that the relation between the head noun and the relative clause becomes a matter of semantics or pragmatics rather than syntax.[74]

To express conditional or temporal relations between clauses, the suffix sequence -a-n 'SN-NR' is used. In conditional clauses, there is a distinction only between irrealis (-ay-a-n) and realis (-a-n).[75] Thus, the tense reference of the realis can be clarified only via the following clause.[76] The verb within the relative clause can further be marked with the locative to emphasise the localisation in time.[75]

(12)
Conditional clause

gu

water

ki’-j-ay-a-n-ba

eat-3PL-IRR-SR-NR-LOC

si’laam

noise

si’-kiya-di

shoot-IRR-3PL

gu ki’-j-ay-a-n-ba si’laam si’-kiya-di

water eat-3PL-IRR-SR-NR-LOC noise shoot-IRR-3PL

'If they drink, they will make a lot of noise'[75]

Switch-reference edit

The second type of subordinate clauses in Iatmul are realised by switch-reference and allow for clause chains. With switch-reference markers on a verb it is possible to express whether the subject of one verb is also the subject of another verb. Thus, it is possible to keep track of the subjects within clause chains consisting of several subordinate clauses. Switch-reference is a special feature of the syntax of Iatmul.[77]

If the subject of both clauses is the same, person is marked once and a non-finite verb form is mandatory in the subordinate clause. If the subjects are different, it is indicated by person-marking in both clauses and by the absence of tense-marking and non-final intonation.[78] In tenseless clauses, semantic relations are expressed in same-subject and different-subject clauses by marking the dependent verb in the following way:[79]

Semantic relation same subject different subject
contextual -ka 'DEP' subject marking only
consecutive -taa 'CONSEC' subject marking + particle mi'na
simultaneous -kakwi 'SIM' imperfective aspect -ti' + subject marking

It is important to note that -ka 'DEP' is unmarked and thus can also cover the other two relations.[79]

(14) a.
Same subject

ki’ki’da

[food

kuk-ka

do-DEP]

yaki

[tobacco

ki’-li’-li

eat-IPFV-3.SG.F]

ki’ki’da kuk-ka yaki ki’-li’-li

[food do-DEP] [tobacco eat-IPFV-3.SG.F]

'She was smoking (while) preparing food'[78]

(14) b.
Different subject (simultaneous relation)

ki’ki’da

[food

kut-ti’-li’

do-IPFV-3.SG.F]

yaki

[tobacco

ki’-li’-li’

eat-IPFV-3.SG.F]

ki’ki’da kut-ti’-li’ yaki ki’-li’-li’

[food do-IPFV-3.SG.F] [tobacco eat-IPFV-3.SG.F]

'(While) shei was preparing food, she*i/j was smoking'[78]

Switch-reference clauses can be used to chronologically order the narrated event or to express temporal overlap, manner of the action or causality.[80]

Tail-head linkage edit

A phenomenon typical for Papuan languages is tail-head linkage. It is the repetition of the last part of the sentence in the beginning of a new sentence. It frequently occurs together with switch-reference and is used to structure communicated information.[81]

(15) a.

Di’-kat

3SG.M-DAT

yi-ka

go-DEP

ta’bak

hand

yi-ka

go-DEP

di’n-a

3SG.M-GEN

gusa-maan-ba

paddle-leg-LOC

kut-di’.

touch-3SG.M

Di’-kat yi-ka ta’bak yi-ka di’n-a gusa-maan-ba kut-di’.

3SG.M-DAT go-DEP hand go-DEP 3SG.M-GEN paddle-leg-LOC touch-3SG.M

(The dog) going to him touching (him) he touched the back of his leg.[82]

(15) b.

Kut-di’

touch-3SG.M

di’-kak

3SG.M-DAT

vi’-di’

see-3SG.M

avayabi’

path

pi’li’-ka

run-DEP

wakwai-ka-di’

advance-PRS-3SG.M

Kut-di’ di’-kak vi’-di’ avayabi’ pi’li’-ka wakwai-ka-di’

touch-3SG.M 3SG.M-DAT see-3SG.M path run-DEP advance-PRS-3SG.M

He (=the dog) touched he (=the boy) saw him (=the dog) he (=the dog) advanced running to the path[82]

Information structural properties edit

Focus in declaratives edit

In a neutral sentence, the subject and the non-referential object are unmarked with respect to information structure.[83]

(1)

Joachim

Joachim

yuwisi’k

rice

kut-di’

do-3SG.M

Joachim yuwisi’k kut-di’

Joachim rice do-3SG.M

'Joachim has cooked rice'[83]

When the subject is focused, the focus marker -a (masculine, -ak for feminine) marks the masculine subject. The verb in contrary misses markers for person and grammatical gender and is marked with the focus marker -a instead. Even though both focus markers have the same form, their origin is different and they have different allomorphs. The following sentence is the answer to the question 'Who cooked rice?'.[83]

(2)

Joachim-a

Joachim-FOC

yuwisi’k

rice

kuk'-a

do-FOC

Joachim-a yuwisi’k kuk'-a

Joachim-FOC rice do-FOC

'Joachim has cooked rice'[84]

When a non-subject is focused, the verb is marked with respect to person and grammatical gender of the subject in addition to focus. The focus constituent is focus-marked and precedes the verb, whereas non-focused constituents can follow the verb (subject as afterthought). The following sentence is the answer to the question 'What did Joachim cook?'.[84]

(3)

Joachim

Joachim

yuwisi’k'-a

rice-FOC

kut-d-a

do-3SG.M-FOC

Joachim yuwisi’k'-a kut-d-a

Joachim rice-FOC do-3SG.M-FOC

'Joachim has cooked rice'[84]

In all these cases, the marking of noun and verb indicates that the focus constructions must have emerged from cleft-constructions (hence the alternative glossing in the following examples with 3SG and SR).[85] Sentences marked with respect to focus generally have a smaller potential for inflection than neutral sentences. Therefore, some grammatical categories (like optative, imperative) which are expressed by suffixation can not be realised when the sentence is marked with respect to focus.[86]

Focus in questions edit

Questions which are asking for the subject or direct object need to be focus-marked, while the answers can be focused or neutral.[87]

(4)
Focused question after the subject

kada-na

who-3SG.M

yuwisi’k

rice

kuk’-a?

do-SR

kada-na yuwisi’k kuk’-a?

who-3SG.M rice do-SR

'Who has cooked rice?'[87]

(5)
Focused question after the direct object

Joachim

Joachim

mi’da-na

what-FOC

kut-d-a?

do-3SG.M-FOC

Joachim mi’da-na kut-d-a?

Joachim what-FOC do-3SG.M-FOC

'What has Joachim done?'[87]

To mark the questions, the allomorphs -na (masc.) and -lak (fem.) are used for focus marking. Inanimate referents are always marked with -na. This can be explained by assuming that the question words used to be marked twice with the demonstrative pronouns -(a)n (masc.) and -(a)t (fem.), which due to phonological processes evolved to the current focus suffixes.[88]

(6) kada-an > kadan > kadan-an > kadana[88]
(7) kada-at > kadat > kadat-at > kadalat > kadalak[88]

If it is not asked for the subject or the direct object, another possibility is a neutral question.[89]

(8)
Focused question

kada-na

who-3SG.M

kwi-m-a?

give-2SG.M-SR

kada-na kwi-m-a?

who-3SG.M give-2SG.M-SR

'Whom did you give it to?'[89]

(9)
Neutral Question

kada-kak

who-DAT

kwi-mi'n?

give-2SG.M

kada-kak kwi-mi'n?

who-DAT give-2SG.M

'Whom did you give it to?'[89]

Negated focus sentences edit

In negated sentences, focus marking causes extraordinary structures. The negation particle ana which is placed before the verb receives a congruency marker. The following examples illustrate this with focus on the subject.[90]

(10) a.
Negation in neutral questions:

Kerol

Kerol

ana

NEG

ya-a-li'

come-PRS-3SG.F

Kerol ana ya-a-li'

Kerol NEG come-PRS-3SG.F

'Kerol is not coming.'[90]

(10) b.
Focused sentence (subject)

Kerol-ak

Kerol-3SG.F

ya-li’-ka

come-IPFV-PRS:S

Kerol-ak ya-li’-ka

Kerol-3SG.F come-IPFV-PRS:S

'Kerol is coming.'[91]

(10) c.
Negated focus sentence

Kerol

Kerol

ana-li'

NEG-3SG.F

ya-li’-ka

come-IPFV-PRS:SR

Kerol ana-li' ya-li’-ka

Kerol NEG-3SG.F come-IPFV-PRS:SR

'Kerol is coming.'[91]

In sentences, in which the predicate is negated, the negation is carried out periphrastically with -lapman 'without'.[91]

(11)
Negation of the predicate while subject is focused

Kerol-ak

Kerol-3SG.F

ya-lapman

come-without

yi-li’-l-a

go-IPFV-3SG.F-SR

Kerol-ak ya-lapman yi-li’-l-a

Kerol-3SG.F come-without go-IPFV-3SG.F-SR

'Kerol has not come (yet)’ or ‘It’s Kerol who has not come.'[91]

Sample Text edit

"The Market"[92]
Iatmul Wegani’ba yika vaala kuka kawika wakkaika, glasi’k wudi da klaka gi’li’laa, nyiga vicholaa kaami taakadi. Sawasi’ lattaa kaami kladi mi’na, Sawasi’ nau kukka, glasi’k wudi da kukka, ni’nadi nyimeikak kwika gi’li’ka wega gi’li’di’, vaala kulaa valayakiyadi. Valayalaa ki’ki’da kukka gi’li’laa ki’ka gi’li’laa, gla’bu jwaakkiyadi gepma. Ak taba gi’li’kali’.
IPA [weᵑganɨᵐba jiga βaːla kuga kawiga wakai̯ga, ᵑglasɨk wuⁿdi ⁿda klaga ᵑgɨlɨlaː, ɲiᵑga βit͡ʃolaː kaːmi taːgaⁿdi. sawasɨ lataː kaːmi klaⁿdi mɨna, sawasɨ naʊ̯ kuka, ᵑglasɨk wuⁿdi ⁿda kuka, nɨnaⁿdi ɲimei̯gak kwiga ᵑgɨlɨga weᵑga ᵑgɨlɨⁿdɨ, βaːla kulaː βalajagijaⁿdi. βalayalaː kɨgɨⁿda kuka ᵑgɨlɨlaː kɨga ᵑgɨlɨaː, ᵑglaˀᵐbu ⁿd͡ʒaːkijaⁿdi ᵑgepma. ak taᵐba ᵑgɨlɨgalɨ.]
English The market people get into their canoes, paddle to the other shore, and go up [to the market], after they finish buying fruits and vegetables, they put a mat on the ground and display their fish. The Sawos get up, and once they’ve got their fish, the Sawos take the sago, they take the fruits, and once they finish giving them to our mothers, the market is over, and [people] will get into their canoes and return. When they’ve arrived, they finish preparing food and finish eating, they will walk around in the village in the afternoon. That’s how it ends.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Iatmul at Ethnologue (18th ed., 2015) (subscription required)
  2. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 1.
  3. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 3.
  4. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 8f.
  5. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 7.
  6. ^ Foley 1986.
  7. ^ a b c d Jendraschek 2012, p. 21.
  8. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 22.
  9. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 23.
  10. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 23ff.
  11. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 25f.
  12. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 80.
  13. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 47.
  14. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 47–49.
  15. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 52.
  16. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 74.
  17. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 69.
  18. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 78.
  19. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 84.
  20. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 85ff.
  21. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, pp. 91f.
  22. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 93.
  23. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 121ff.
  24. ^ a b c d Jendraschek 2012, p. 169.
  25. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 102.
  26. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, pp. 191f.
  27. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, pp. 193f.
  28. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 195f.
  29. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 201–206.
  30. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 184f.
  31. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 185.
  32. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 184.
  33. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 221.
  34. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. }261f.
  35. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 221f.
  36. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 227.
  37. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 234.
  38. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 227f.
  39. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 229.
  40. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 229f.
  41. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 233f.
  42. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 236.
  43. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, pp. 237f.
  44. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 242.
  45. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 237.
  46. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 237, 240, 245, 248, 250.
  47. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 240.
  48. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 252f.
  49. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 254.
  50. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 260.
  51. ^ a b c d e Jendraschek 2012, p. 125.
  52. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 127.
  53. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 129.
  54. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 133.
  55. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 135.
  56. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 136.
  57. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 130.
  58. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 134.
  59. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 132.
  60. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, pp. 137f.
  61. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 139.
  62. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 140.
  63. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 168.
  64. ^ a b c d Jendraschek 2012, pp. 321f.
  65. ^ a b c d Jendraschek 2012, p. 322.
  66. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 323.
  67. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 324.
  68. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 325.
  69. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 326.
  70. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 327.
  71. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 339.
  72. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 328.
  73. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 331.
  74. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 338.
  75. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 341.
  76. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 342.
  77. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 353.
  78. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 357.
  79. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 358.
  80. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 354–356.
  81. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 359f.
  82. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 361.
  83. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 377.
  84. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 378.
  85. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 378ff.
  86. ^ Jendraschek 2012, pp. 387f.
  87. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 381.
  88. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, pp. 381f.
  89. ^ a b c Jendraschek 2012, p. 382.
  90. ^ a b Jendraschek 2012, p. 384.
  91. ^ a b c d Jendraschek 2012, p. 385.
  92. ^ Jendraschek 2012, p. 488.

SR:subordinator CONSEC:consecutive NP:noun phrase D1:demonstrative, deictic degree 1 D2:demonstrative, deictic degree 2 D3:demonstrative, deictic degree 3 DEP:dependent verb form

Sources edit

  • Jendraschek, Gerd (2012). A grammar of Iatmul. University of Regensburg. OCLC 816072585.
  • Foley, William (1986). The Papuan Languages of New Guinea. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-28621-2.

iatmul, language, iatmul, language, iatmul, people, spoken, around, sepik, river, east, sepik, province, northern, papua, guinea, iatmul, however, refer, their, language, term, iatmul, call, gepmakudi, village, language, from, gepma, village, kudi, speech, pro. Iatmul is the language of the Iatmul people spoken around the Sepik River in the East Sepik Province northern Papua New Guinea 2 The Iatmul however do not refer to their language by the term Iatmul but call it gepmakudi village language from gepma village and kudi speech pronounced as ŋɡɛpmaɡundi IatmulgepmakudiNative toPapua New GuineaRegionSepik River basinEthnicityIatmul peopleNative speakers8 400 2003 1 Language familySepik Middle SepikNduIatmulDialectsNyaula or Nyaura Western Pali bei or Palimbei Central Waliakwi Eastern Maligwat Northern Language codesISO 639 3 a href https iso639 3 sil org code ian class extiw title iso639 3 ian ian a Glottologiatm1242ELPIatmul East Sepik Province in Papua New Guinea There are about 8 400 Iatmul traditionally organized in villages whereas a total amount of 46 000 speakers is estimated 3 The inhabitants of the villages are trilingual being fluent with Tok Pisin good with Iatmul and having some knowledge of English Tok Pisin is also the first language of the youngest children despite efforts to reverse this trend 4 An extensive grammar of Iatmul has been recently written by Gerd Jendraschek as a postdoctoral thesis 2 Contents 1 Typological profile 2 Phonological system 2 1 Vowel and consonant phonemes 2 1 1 Variation of oral stops 2 2 Segmental phonology 2 2 1 Syllable structure 2 2 2 Assimilation 2 2 3 Elision and fusion 2 2 4 Epenthesis 2 2 5 Reduction 2 3 Lexical stress 2 4 Sentence intonation 3 Morphological properties 3 1 Verbal structure 3 1 1 Person 3 1 2 Tense mood and aspect 3 1 3 Event specifiers 3 2 Case marking 3 2 1 Nominative case zero marking 3 2 2 Dative case 3 2 3 Locative case 3 2 4 Other cases 3 3 Gender 3 4 Number 3 4 1 Plural marking on nouns 3 4 2 Reduplication 3 4 3 Associative plural 3 4 4 Numerals 4 Syntactic properties 4 1 General syntactic structure 4 2 Negation 4 3 Nominalized subordinate clauses 4 4 Switch reference 4 5 Tail head linkage 5 Information structural properties 5 1 Focus in declaratives 5 2 Focus in questions 5 3 Negated focus sentences 6 Sample Text 7 See also 8 References 9 SourcesTypological profile editIatmul is part of the Ndu language family which consists of at least six languages in which ndu is the word for man Together with Manambu it is the southernmost language of the Ndu family spoken along the Sepik River 5 Iatmul is perhaps the best known Ndu language of them all 6 Iatmul is a moderately agglutinative and nearly isolating language Flexion is predominantly suffixed and very regular whereas the phonological processes are the most complex ones within the language Stems often change their form while multiple morpheme structures can become so coalescent that they are difficult to segment 7 Iatmul has masculine and feminine gender marking as well as singular dual and plural numbers 8 Nouns and verbs are the only two major classes in Iatmul with only little derivation across them 8 Also there is not a strong distinction between modifiers and nouns as many roots can be used as nouns adjectives or adverbs 9 Smaller word classes include personal pronouns demonstratives postpositions quantifiers interrogatives as well as proclauses yes no while there are no clause linking conjunctions 10 Phonological system editVowel and consonant phonemes edit The phonological system of Iatmul is a matter of controversy among scholars There is no consensus about how many vowels Iatmul has and about which realisations are to be considered as phonemes or allophones There were attempts of analysing the language as consisting of only 1 3 vowels by Staalsen 1966 and Laycock 1991 11 Jendraschek 2012 in contrast describes 12 phonemic monophthongs and 7 phonemic diphthongs 12 Monophthongs of Iatmul 13 Front Central Back High i i ɨ i u u uː uu Mid e e eː ee o o Low plain a a aː aa ɑ a 3 glottalised aˀ a ɑˀ a Diphthongs of Iatmul 14 Front Back High ɨi wi 6 Mid ɛɪ ei ou ou Low plain ai ɒi ai aːi aai aʊ au aːʊ aau glottalised aˀi a i Consonant Phonemes of Iatmul 15 Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar Stop fortis pː pp tː tt kː kk nasal pᵐ p 5 kᵑ k 4 oral lenis p p t t k k prenasalized lenis ᵐb b ⁿd d ᵑɡ g Affricate t ʃ ch ⁿd ʒ j Fricative b v s s Nasal m m n n ɲ ny ŋ n 1 Approximant w w ɾ l l 2 j j In Iatmul the contrast between the fortis lenis and prenasalized stops exists only intervocalically The fully oral stops can be voiced to b d g and as such they have voiced allophones in certain contexts The latter also tend to be more open than the fortis stops which means that they can become taps fricatives or approximants in post sonorant position Prenasalized stops are inherently voiced As such the distinctive feature between the fortis and oral series is not voicing but rather voiceability 7 g and k n are identical phonetically both producing ŋɡ and the only way to distinguish them is morphology n and ŋ are also in free variation word finally which is maintained when suffixes are added Thus ŋ is written as n l and ɾ are in free variation they are both written as l The lateral is usually the predominant realisation other than in western Iatmul where the tap is dominant A is only written with the circumflex if a contrasting word with a exists kᵑ mainly occurs as an allophone of k before vowel initial suffixes according to Jendraschek there are only three words where it is phonemic pᵐ only occurs as an allophone of p before vowel initial suffixes It is indicated orthographically ɨi is underlying wi and occurs after m It can also tend towards ʉi Variation of oral stops edit Source Jendraschek 7 Phoneme Initial Post Sonorant Intervocalic Post nasal fortis Labial pː n a p n a Alveolar tː n a t n a Alveolar kː n a k n a oral Labial p p p b 2 p Alveolar t t ɾ 1 ɾ d d Alveolar k k g ɣ ɰ g nasal Labial ᵐb ᵐb b Alveolar ⁿd ⁿd d Alveolar ᵑɡ ᵑɡ g t s alternation to ɾ is always indicated in the orthography Unlike t and k p is sometimes resistant to lenition Lenited p is also always written as v Segmental phonology edit Syllable structure edit The canonical syllable structure of Iatmul is C A V C where the first consonant can be any consonant other than the geminates Only approximants liquids can be in the second onset position Possible codas are only p t k m n and ɲ the lenis plosives and nasals Most words begin with the nasal consonants m n or the plosive consonants p k 16 Excluded from this rule are about 5 of the words in Iatmul which begin with the vowels a or a 17 Assimilation edit A very common phenomenon in Iatmul is regressive assimilation 16 1 kuvinstraighten bi kdual suffix ˈkubimbɨk you they both straightened 16 kuvin bi k ˈkubimbɨk straighten dual suffix you they both straightened Assimilation can be blocked to avoid ambiguity Whereas 2a is ambiguous due to assimilation of bak to bap example 2b is not ambiguous as assimilation was blocked in favour of a clear meaning 18 2 a wugiD3 SG Mbap bamoon LOCki dieat 3PLwugi bap ba ki diD3 SG M moon LOC eat 3PL they were eating during that month or they were eating from that pig 2 b wugiD3 SG Mbak bapig LOCki dieat 3PLwugi bak ba ki diD3 SG M pig LOC eat 3PL they were eating from that pig 18 Elision and fusion edit Elision can also be encountered very frequently In the following example both vowel fusion and onset elision are operating at the same time making the meaning of the final form hard to recognize 12 3 wa ka asay PRS SR wa asay PRS SR wa 12 wa ka a wa a wa say PRS SR say PRS SR Epenthesis edit Epenthesis can be observed when for example focus is marked with the suffix a 4 kadawho aFOC 3 SG M kada n a 19 kada a kada n awho FOC 3 SG M Reduction edit Reduction mostly happens quantitatively so that the vowels are shorted in their length Qualitative reduction happens on a rare occasion and mainly occurs during monophthongization 20 Lexical stress edit In Iatmul words are not differentiated via pitch or accentuation The meanings of words are not autonomous but influenced by factors like vocal length syllable structure and speed of enunciation The following rules are the most important ones in descending order 21 Long vowels are stressed ti baadi tɨˈmbaːndɨ he can stay CVC syllables are stressed ti kali ka tɨˈɡaɾɡa meanwhile Word initial syllables are stressed 21 In compounds these rules can be randomly applied to the first or to the second word 21 As diphthongs are not stressed there are no long monophthongized diphthongs 22 ref gt Sentence intonation edit A phonological utterance ends with falling intonation Simple sentences are marked via pauses Complex sentences end with falling intonation Questions are marked via variations in pitch Focus is marked by initial high intonation and a subsequent fall of the pitch 23 Morphological properties editVerbal structure edit Iatmul is a moderately agglutinative language in which suffixes are dominating 7 Therefore much information is being expressed morphologically instead of syntactically which is true especially for the category of the verb Information regarding actions like the direction the manner or temporal relations are expressed via derivation of the verbs There are many affixes specifying the manner in which an action is performed like the means or the amount of control over the action 24 Structure of a fully inflected verb 24 2 1 1 2 3 4 5 6 Neg Manner Root s Directional Event specifier Aspect Tense Mood Subject agreement Derivation Inflection Person edit Iatmul pronouns and cross ref suffixes 25 Number Pronouns Cross reference to the subject markers Free Right bound Left boundon verbs Left bound onnon verb predicates Left andright bound 1sg wun wun wun wun w 2sg masc mi n mi n mi n mi n m 2sg fem nyi n nyi n nyi n nyi n ny 3sg masc di di n di a d 3sg fem li li l li ak l 1dl dual an an li li li 2 3dl bi k bi l bi k bi k b 1pl ni n ni n ni n ni n n 2pl guk gul guk guk gw 3pl di din di di j Tense mood and aspect edit Temporal marking exists in present tense and past tense but not in the future tense The marker for the present tense is ka and in some cases the allomorph a The past tense is unmarked Thus some tenses can be distinguished only by the length of a single vowel 26 1 a kiya a di bring PRS 3SG Mkiya a di bring PRS 3SG M he brings 1 b kiya O di bring PST 3SG Mkiya O di bring PST 3SG M he brought 26 The expression of future is covered by the irrealis mood with the allomorphs kiya ikiya after roots ending with a and it short form ika occurring in fast speech Besides future reference the irrealis expresses possibility and permission and can be used in conditional constructions 27 2 ya ikiya wuncome IRR 1SGya ikiya wuncome IRR 1SG I will come 27 The imperfective aspect marker ti li occurs most often with the present tense Therefore in some constructions in which present cannot be marked e g nominalized clauses the imperfective expresses present time reference Thus it semantics is about to shift from aspect to tense 28 Other aspects in Iatmul are Hortative marked with kak li lu Optative marked with ba and ka Apprehensive marked with ka Imperative which is built using the bare stem or additionally by the suffix li 29 Event specifiers edit A special category in Iatmul are event specifier suffixes Temporal relations are expressed with jibu event takes place during the whole night until sunrise pwali expresses that the event is done continuously ki va expresses that something else is done in the middle of one action interruption si bla expresses that an event occurs as the first one before any other events anteriority 30 3 nyigaleafkasa jibu diplay DAY BREAK 3PLnyiga kasa jibu dileaf play DAY BREAK 3PL they played cards until daybreak 31 Other event specifiers express the extent of the action complete incomplete all of its parts or the frustrative attempt failure 32 Case marking edit In Iatmul at least three cases with overlapping functions are assumed Jendraschek argues that it is not possible to define basic meanings for the cases as the case markers often are polysemous 33 In general case marking does not depend on the head but also on pragmatic circumstances and especially semantic properties of the controlling verb and its dependent noun Thus there is no strict division into structural and semantic cases in Iatmul 34 Case marking has the whole noun phrase in its scope so that they are placed at the end of the phrase This is also the case when the end of the word is not the head noun 33 4 waanierimaki tatreegepmaoneki ta ba village one NP LOCwakkai laaascend go CONSECli di stay 3SG Mwaani ki ta gepma ki ta ba wakkai laa li di erima tree one village one NP LOC ascend go CONSEC stay 3SG M There was an erima tree growing in one village 33 Nominative case zero marking edit Subjects as well as copula subjects and copula complements are always zero marked with respect to their case The direct object remains unmarked if it is low in animacy or definiteness 35 Goals are not always marked with an overt case marker especially when they are inanimate 36 Dative case edit The marker for the so called dative is kak with the allomorph kat 37 As pronouns and proper nouns are always definite they are marked with dativ case when they are used as transitive objects 38 When definite nouns referring to humans are transitive objects the marker is preferred but not obligatory The same is the case with non human animates like animals whereas here the marking more likely indicates specificity of the object the dog or one dog 39 5 a wa la kakdog DATvi mi n see 2SG Mwa la kak vi mi n dog DAT see 2SG M Have you seen the dog 5 b wa ladogvi min see 2SG Mwa la vi min dog see 2SG M Have you seen a the dog Whether a transitive objects has to be marked with kak is also depending on the meaning of the predicate Dead animals are not perceived the same way as animate specific animals so that marking is not employed in sentences where someone eats an animal On the other hand when an animal eats a human marking is employed 40 Beside transitive objects other semantic roles like recipient addressee theme and reason can be marked with the dative marker 41 Animate goals are also marked with the dative case 42 Locative case edit The so called locative is marked with na ba 43 Its main function is to mark locations in some contexts including time expressions 44 Regarding transitive objects its functions overlap with those of the dative marker kak so that in some context it is assumed that there is no difference in meaning between both markers Still usually it marks transitive objects with non human referents including those who do not accept kak 43 6 wanD3 SG Mgusi ga babox LOCtastay SRda bathing LOCki kaeat DEPli di stay 3SG Mwan gusi ga ba ta da ba ki ka li di D3 SG M box LOC stay SR thing LOC eat DEP stay 3SG M He was already eating the things that were in that box 45 Besides marking of location and transitive objects the locative case can also mark themes manner material instruments goals sources and animate recipients 46 In case of the animate recipients ba can only be used if the recipient is perceived more as a location where something is left instead of the finial recipient 47 Other cases edit Allative marker ak and allomorph alak marks goal in the sense of purposes of activities instead of locations as well as recipients 48 Comitative marker a na la and akwi or okwi It is unclear according to which rules these allomorphs are distributed 49 Vocative marker o The only function of the vocative is to signal that a noun phrase refers to the addressee It is not used obligatorily but rather when the name has to be repeated in case the addressee did not hear the first call 50 Gender edit In Iatmul gender is not marked on the noun but on its modifiers demonstratives instead as well as via subject verb agreement Often this is also true for number marking where only some nouns can be marked with a plural suffix 51 7 kan D1 SG M katD1 SG F nyaanchildwegamarketyi di go 3SG M yi li go 3SG F kan kat nyaan wega yi di yi li D1 SG M D1 SG F child market go 3SG M go 3SG F This male female child went to the market 51 8 ankwi D2 SG M NR REM akkwiD2 SG F NR REMji vwawork ankwi akkwi ji vwa D2 SG M NR REM D2 SG F NR REM work that hard simple work 51 Referents without natural gender like inanimate entities are marked with respect to gender depending on their size Thus bigger referents are marked as masculine whereas smaller referents are marked as feminine From both gender markers masculine is perceived as the unmarked one and thus chosen if no characterisation is intended 51 9 ki viyamosquitovaali ka di bite PRS 3SG Mki viya vaali ka di mosquito bite PRS 3SG M A large mosquito is stinging me 51 However there are some referents in Iatmul without natural gender but still with fixed grammatical gender Among them are nya sun which is masculine whereas bap moon is feminine Also some animal species have specific gender regardless of their biological gender like kaami fish masculine Some nouns can have both genders interchangeably without a difference in meaning as di mai season 52 Number edit Plural marking on nouns edit Iatmul has singular dual and plural number 8 As it is with gender number usually is not marked on the noun Instead number can be marked on the determiners or modifiers of the noun as well as via subject verb agreement 53 Plurality can also be marked twice 54 10 a wun a1SG GENwudiD3 PLdathingwun a wudi da1SG GEN D3 PL thing those things of mine 10 b wun a di1SG GEN PLwudiD3 PLdathingwun a di wudi da1SG GEN PL D3 PL thing those things of mine 54 Dual or plural marking on the verb indicates the number of the subject referent 55 However this is only true for human referents Non human referents are always marked singular on the verb as they are not perceived as individuals 56 11 wa kcrocodiledumanki li ka dieat IPFV PRS 3PLwa k du ki li ka dicrocodile man eat IPFV PRS 3PL the men eat the crocodile cannot mean the crocodiles eat the men 56 There are some exceptional cases in which nouns can employ plural marking Kinship terms like walaga ancestor nyagei sister or ta kwa wife can be plural marked But if ta kwa is used in the meaning of woman it cannot be suffixed with a plural marker as it does not employ a relational meaning 53 12 a di n a3SG M GENta kwa duwoman wife PLsaklakeyi digo 3PLdi n a ta kwa du sak yi di3SG M GEN woman wife PL lake go 3PL his wives went to the lake 12 b gepma navillage GENta kwawoman wifesaklakeyi digo 3PLgepma na ta kwa sak yi divillage GEN woman wife lake go 3PL the women from the village went to the lake 57 The modifiers of a kinship term can be plural marked also when the kinship term is already plural marked 58 13 tabaalreadywun a di1SG GEN PLna dugrandchild PLki nki sakkwat diD1 SG M NR many PLtaba wun a di na du ki nki sakkwat dialready 1SG GEN PL grandchild PL D1 SG M NR many PL I have already this many grand children 58 Reduplication edit It is also possible to reduplicate nouns In this case they do not convey the usual plural meaning but rather a distributive one referring to the group as a whole meaning every instead of more than one 57 Some reduplicated nouns additionally employ adverbial meaning like kava place as kava kava everywhere and jibula day as jibula jibula all the time 59 When adjectives are reduplicated they may be understood as plural marked or as employing an elative meaning good gt very good depending on the context When the genitive suffix na is reduplicated it can express the plurality of the possessee 58 14 gepmavillagegepmavillagekalibi kcarry 2 3DUmi naonlykanD1 SG Mmissionary na namissionary GEN PLsuleschoolki nki a nD1 M SG NR 3SG Mwa asay DEPwa disay 3PLgepma gepma kalibi k mi na kan missionary na na sule ki nki a n wa a wa divillage village carry 2 3DU only D1 SG M missionary GEN PL school D1 M SG NR 3SG M say DEP say 3PL after the two had brought it to every village they said these are the missionaries schools 57 Associative plural edit The plural suffix du which is used on kinship terms can be used on proper names to express person together with people related to this person associative plural Those related people can be his kin people living in the same house or friends of the referent 59 15 Magina duMagina PLya a dicome PRS 3PLMagina du ya a diMagina PL come PRS 3PL Magina and her mother family are coming 54 Numerals edit Counting in Iatmul is accomplished in mixture of a quinary decimal and vigesimal system 60 Cardinal numerals generally follow the noun In this case adjectives are placed before the noun If numerals close the noun phrase they carry the case markers 61 The number 1 is ki ta The number 5 is one hand taba nak from ta ba hand and nak an other one The numbers 6 to 9 are counted elliptically as plus one si la ki ta from si la plus The number 10 is two hands taba vli vli being the short form of vi li li k two The number 15 is not three hands but two hands plus one hand taba vli kyeli taba nak The number 20 is one whole man dumi ki ta from du man and mi tree referring to the number of all fingers and toes of a human The number 30 is one whole man plus two hands dumi ki ta kyeli taba vli The number 100 is five whole men dumi taba nak 60 Ordinal numbers are built by adding wan probably from wa say to cardinal numerals 61 Most Papuan languages incorporated the counting system of Tok Pisin or adopted the English counting system which are perceived as shorter and more transparent Due to the more and more frequent use of other counting systems many speakers of Iatmul cannot count correctly beyond twenty in their own language 62 Syntactic properties editGeneral syntactic structure edit There is no passive voice in Iatmul 24 The canonical sentence structure is SOV Adverb Subject Adjunct Object Verb 63 1 BablatodayRuthRuthAaguvalioppositegaai bahouse LOCwun kak1SG DATni ma bigki ki dafood Okuk kiya li do IRR 3SG FBabla Ruth aguvali gaai ba wun kak ni ma ki ki da kuk kiya li today RuthA opposite house LOC 1SG DAT big food O do IRR 3SG F Ruth will prepare a big meal for me in the house on the other side 63 Yes no questions are not expressed syntactically but via intonation 63 2 AnaNEGkla mi n get 2SG MAna kla mi n NEG get 2SG M Didn t you understand or You didn t understand 24 Negation edit The expression of negation is accomplished by different morphological structures which often are etymologically unrelated 64 Verbs are negated by placing the particle ana before the inflected verb In case of non verbal predicates or predicatively used adjectives ana is placed in front of the pronominal subject marker 64 3 a Du mi nman 2SG MDu mi nman 2SG M You re a man 64 3 b Dumanana mi nNEG 2SG MDu ana mi nman NEG 2SG M You re not a man 64 4 Gepma bavillage LOCki w aeat 1SG SRki ki dafoodapmagoodana di NEG 3SG MGepma ba ki w a ki ki da apma ana di village LOC eat 1SG SR food good NEG 3SG M the food I ate in the village was not good 65 In dependent clauses negation on the predicate is not possible Therefore negation is expressed by a periphrastic construction in which the suffix lapman without is attached to the predicate and combined with the auxiliary yi go 65 5 a AnaNEGvi w asee 1SG SRka ikpicturebablatodayvi si makwi kiya dishow IRR 3PL 65 Ana vi w a ka ik babla vi si makwi kiya diNEG see 1SG SR picture today show IRR 3PL 5 b Vi lapmansee withoutyi w ago 1SG SRka ikpicturebablatodayvi si makwi kiya dishow IRR 3PLVi lapman yi w a ka ik babla vi si makwi kiya disee without go 1SG SR picture today show IRR 3PL Today they will show the film I haven t seen yet 65 To deny existence the proclause ka i is used 66 6 Wun1SGsaanyamoneyka inoWun saanya ka i1SG money no I don t have any money 66 Ke the reduced form of ka i is used together with a non finite or a subordinate form of the verb to form the prohibitive 67 7 KePROHyi m a go 2SG M REAL SRKe yi m a PROH go 2SG M REAL SR Don t go now 67 For the irrealis the negator wana is used which cannot be combined with realis forms 68 8 WanaNEG OPTyi m ay a go 2SG M IRR SRWana yi m ay a NEG OPT go 2SG M IRR SR Don t go later 68 The optative on the other hand is negated by the verbal suffix la 68 9 Yiki nbagentlyva i stepda la mi nfall APPR 2SG MYiki nba va i da la mi ngently step fall APPR 2SG M Walk slowly otherwise you might fall 69 Nominalized subordinate clauses edit One of the two types of subordinate clauses in Iatmul are nominalised clauses which are used like attributive adverbial or complement clauses 70 There are rare cases of headless relative clauses where the predicate of the relative clause becomes the head 71 Usually this is avoided by preferring a general noun like du man as a head 72 10 Relative clause same subject kaami k a fish eat SRembedded clauseduman head nominalkiya di die 3SG Mpredicate kaami k a du kiya di fish eat SR man die 3SG M embedded clause head nominal predicate The man who ate fish died 70 When the relative clause has a subject which is not co referential with the head noun of the matrix clause then the verb of the relative clause expresses this with a pronominal subject marker on the subordinate verb 73 11 Relative clause different subjects buk w a tell 1SG SR vaaktopic gawi naeagle GENwapuchapukstorybuk w a vaak gawi na wapuchapuk tell 1SG SR topic eagle GEN story The topic I was talking about is the story about the eagle 73 It is important to note that some relative clauses do not have a common argument in the matrix clause so that the relation between the head noun and the relative clause becomes a matter of semantics or pragmatics rather than syntax 74 To express conditional or temporal relations between clauses the suffix sequence a n SN NR is used In conditional clauses there is a distinction only between irrealis ay a n and realis a n 75 Thus the tense reference of the realis can be clarified only via the following clause 76 The verb within the relative clause can further be marked with the locative to emphasise the localisation in time 75 12 Conditional clauseguwaterki j ay a n baeat 3PL IRR SR NR LOCsi laamnoisesi kiya dishoot IRR 3PLgu ki j ay a n ba si laam si kiya diwater eat 3PL IRR SR NR LOC noise shoot IRR 3PL If they drink they will make a lot of noise 75 Switch reference edit The second type of subordinate clauses in Iatmul are realised by switch reference and allow for clause chains With switch reference markers on a verb it is possible to express whether the subject of one verb is also the subject of another verb Thus it is possible to keep track of the subjects within clause chains consisting of several subordinate clauses Switch reference is a special feature of the syntax of Iatmul 77 If the subject of both clauses is the same person is marked once and a non finite verb form is mandatory in the subordinate clause If the subjects are different it is indicated by person marking in both clauses and by the absence of tense marking and non final intonation 78 In tenseless clauses semantic relations are expressed in same subject and different subject clauses by marking the dependent verb in the following way 79 Semantic relation same subject different subject contextual ka DEP subject marking only consecutive taa CONSEC subject marking particle mi na simultaneous kakwi SIM imperfective aspect ti subject marking It is important to note that ka DEP is unmarked and thus can also cover the other two relations 79 14 a Same subjectki ki da foodkuk kado DEP yaki tobaccoki li li eat IPFV 3 SG F ki ki da kuk ka yaki ki li li food do DEP tobacco eat IPFV 3 SG F She was smoking while preparing food 78 14 b Different subject simultaneous relation ki ki da foodkut ti li do IPFV 3 SG F yaki tobaccoki li li eat IPFV 3 SG F ki ki da kut ti li yaki ki li li food do IPFV 3 SG F tobacco eat IPFV 3 SG F While shei was preparing food she i j was smoking 78 Switch reference clauses can be used to chronologically order the narrated event or to express temporal overlap manner of the action or causality 80 Tail head linkage edit A phenomenon typical for Papuan languages is tail head linkage It is the repetition of the last part of the sentence in the beginning of a new sentence It frequently occurs together with switch reference and is used to structure communicated information 81 15 a Di kat3SG M DATyi kago DEPta bakhandyi kago DEPdi n a3SG M GENgusa maan bapaddle leg LOCkut di touch 3SG MDi kat yi ka ta bak yi ka di n a gusa maan ba kut di 3SG M DAT go DEP hand go DEP 3SG M GEN paddle leg LOC touch 3SG M The dog going to him touching him he touched the back of his leg 82 15 b Kut di touch 3SG Mdi kak3SG M DATvi di see 3SG Mavayabi pathpi li karun DEPwakwai ka di advance PRS 3SG MKut di di kak vi di avayabi pi li ka wakwai ka di touch 3SG M 3SG M DAT see 3SG M path run DEP advance PRS 3SG MHe the dog touched he the boy saw him the dog he the dog advanced running to the path 82 Information structural properties editFocus in declaratives edit In a neutral sentence the subject and the non referential object are unmarked with respect to information structure 83 1 JoachimJoachimyuwisi kricekut di do 3SG MJoachim yuwisi k kut di Joachim rice do 3SG M Joachim has cooked rice 83 When the subject is focused the focus marker a masculine ak for feminine marks the masculine subject The verb in contrary misses markers for person and grammatical gender and is marked with the focus marker a instead Even though both focus markers have the same form their origin is different and they have different allomorphs The following sentence is the answer to the question Who cooked rice 83 2 Joachim aJoachim FOCyuwisi kricekuk ado FOCJoachim a yuwisi k kuk aJoachim FOC rice do FOC Joachim has cooked rice 84 When a non subject is focused the verb is marked with respect to person and grammatical gender of the subject in addition to focus The focus constituent is focus marked and precedes the verb whereas non focused constituents can follow the verb subject as afterthought The following sentence is the answer to the question What did Joachim cook 84 3 JoachimJoachimyuwisi k arice FOCkut d ado 3SG M FOCJoachim yuwisi k a kut d aJoachim rice FOC do 3SG M FOC Joachim has cooked rice 84 In all these cases the marking of noun and verb indicates that the focus constructions must have emerged from cleft constructions hence the alternative glossing in the following examples with 3SG and SR 85 Sentences marked with respect to focus generally have a smaller potential for inflection than neutral sentences Therefore some grammatical categories like optative imperative which are expressed by suffixation can not be realised when the sentence is marked with respect to focus 86 Focus in questions edit Questions which are asking for the subject or direct object need to be focus marked while the answers can be focused or neutral 87 4 Focused question after the subjectkada nawho 3SG Myuwisi kricekuk a do SRkada na yuwisi k kuk a who 3SG M rice do SR Who has cooked rice 87 5 Focused question after the direct objectJoachimJoachimmi da nawhat FOCkut d a do 3SG M FOCJoachim mi da na kut d a Joachim what FOC do 3SG M FOC What has Joachim done 87 To mark the questions the allomorphs na masc and lak fem are used for focus marking Inanimate referents are always marked with na This can be explained by assuming that the question words used to be marked twice with the demonstrative pronouns a n masc and a t fem which due to phonological processes evolved to the current focus suffixes 88 6 kada an gt kadan gt kadan an gt kadana 88 7 kada at gt kadat gt kadat at gt kadalat gt kadalak 88 If it is not asked for the subject or the direct object another possibility is a neutral question 89 8 Focused questionkada nawho 3SG Mkwi m a give 2SG M SRkada na kwi m a who 3SG M give 2SG M SR Whom did you give it to 89 9 Neutral Questionkada kakwho DATkwi mi n give 2SG Mkada kak kwi mi n who DAT give 2SG M Whom did you give it to 89 Negated focus sentences edit In negated sentences focus marking causes extraordinary structures The negation particle ana which is placed before the verb receives a congruency marker The following examples illustrate this with focus on the subject 90 10 a Negation in neutral questions KerolKerolanaNEGya a li come PRS 3SG FKerol ana ya a li Kerol NEG come PRS 3SG F Kerol is not coming 90 10 b Focused sentence subject Kerol akKerol 3SG Fya li kacome IPFV PRS SKerol ak ya li kaKerol 3SG F come IPFV PRS S Kerol is coming 91 10 c Negated focus sentenceKerolKerolana li NEG 3SG Fya li kacome IPFV PRS SRKerol ana li ya li kaKerol NEG 3SG F come IPFV PRS SR Kerol is coming 91 In sentences in which the predicate is negated the negation is carried out periphrastically with lapman without 91 11 Negation of the predicate while subject is focusedKerol akKerol 3SG Fya lapmancome withoutyi li l ago IPFV 3SG F SRKerol ak ya lapman yi li l aKerol 3SG F come without go IPFV 3SG F SR Kerol has not come yet or It s Kerol who has not come 91 Sample Text edit The Market 92 Iatmul Wegani ba yika vaala kuka kawika wakkaika glasi k wudi da klaka gi li laa nyiga vicholaa kaami taakadi Sawasi lattaa kaami kladi mi na Sawasi nau kukka glasi k wudi da kukka ni nadi nyimeikak kwika gi li ka wega gi li di vaala kulaa valayakiyadi Valayalaa ki ki da kukka gi li laa ki ka gi li laa gla bu jwaakkiyadi gepma Ak taba gi li kali IPA weᵑganɨᵐba jiga baːla kuga kawiga wakai ga ᵑglasɨk wuⁿdi ⁿda klaga ᵑgɨlɨlaː ɲiᵑga bit ʃolaː kaːmi taːgaⁿdi sawasɨ lataː kaːmi klaⁿdi mɨna sawasɨ naʊ kuka ᵑglasɨk wuⁿdi ⁿda kuka nɨnaⁿdi ɲimei gak kwiga ᵑgɨlɨga weᵑga ᵑgɨlɨⁿdɨ baːla kulaː balajagijaⁿdi balayalaː kɨgɨⁿda kuka ᵑgɨlɨlaː kɨga ᵑgɨlɨaː ᵑglaˀᵐbu ⁿd ʒaːkijaⁿdi ᵑgepma ak taᵐba ᵑgɨlɨgalɨ English The market people get into their canoes paddle to the other shore and go up to the market after they finish buying fruits and vegetables they put a mat on the ground and display their fish The Sawos get up and once they ve got their fish the Sawos take the sago they take the fruits and once they finish giving them to our mothers the market is over and people will get into their canoes and return When they ve arrived they finish preparing food and finish eating they will walk around in the village in the afternoon That s how it ends See also editIatmul people Ndu languages Yimas Iatmul Pidgin Gerd JendraschekReferences edit Iatmul at Ethnologue 18th ed 2015 subscription required a b Jendraschek 2012 p 1 Jendraschek 2012 p 3 Jendraschek 2012 pp 8f Jendraschek 2012 p 7 Foley 1986 a b c d Jendraschek 2012 p 21 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 22 Jendraschek 2012 p 23 Jendraschek 2012 pp 23ff Jendraschek 2012 pp 25f a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 80 Jendraschek 2012 p 47 Jendraschek 2012 pp 47 49 Jendraschek 2012 p 52 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 74 Jendraschek 2012 p 69 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 78 Jendraschek 2012 p 84 Jendraschek 2012 pp 85ff a b c Jendraschek 2012 pp 91f Jendraschek 2012 p 93 Jendraschek 2012 pp 121ff a b c d Jendraschek 2012 p 169 Jendraschek 2012 p 102 a b Jendraschek 2012 pp 191f a b Jendraschek 2012 pp 193f Jendraschek 2012 pp 195f Jendraschek 2012 pp 201 206 Jendraschek 2012 pp 184f Jendraschek 2012 p 185 Jendraschek 2012 p 184 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 221 Jendraschek 2012 pp 261f Jendraschek 2012 pp 221f Jendraschek 2012 p 227 Jendraschek 2012 p 234 Jendraschek 2012 pp 227f Jendraschek 2012 p 229 Jendraschek 2012 pp 229f Jendraschek 2012 pp 233f Jendraschek 2012 p 236 a b Jendraschek 2012 pp 237f Jendraschek 2012 p 242 Jendraschek 2012 p 237 Jendraschek 2012 pp 237 240 245 248 250 Jendraschek 2012 p 240 Jendraschek 2012 pp 252f Jendraschek 2012 p 254 Jendraschek 2012 p 260 a b c d e Jendraschek 2012 p 125 Jendraschek 2012 p 127 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 129 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 133 Jendraschek 2012 p 135 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 136 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 130 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 134 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 132 a b Jendraschek 2012 pp 137f a b Jendraschek 2012 p 139 Jendraschek 2012 p 140 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 168 a b c d Jendraschek 2012 pp 321f a b c d Jendraschek 2012 p 322 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 323 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 324 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 325 Jendraschek 2012 p 326 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 327 Jendraschek 2012 p 339 Jendraschek 2012 p 328 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 331 Jendraschek 2012 p 338 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 341 Jendraschek 2012 p 342 Jendraschek 2012 p 353 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 357 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 358 Jendraschek 2012 pp 354 356 Jendraschek 2012 pp 359f a b Jendraschek 2012 p 361 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 377 a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 378 Jendraschek 2012 pp 378ff Jendraschek 2012 pp 387f a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 381 a b c Jendraschek 2012 pp 381f a b c Jendraschek 2012 p 382 a b Jendraschek 2012 p 384 a b c d Jendraschek 2012 p 385 Jendraschek 2012 p 488 SR subordinator CONSEC consecutive NP noun phrase D1 demonstrative deictic degree 1 D2 demonstrative deictic degree 2 D3 demonstrative deictic degree 3 DEP dependent verb formSources editJendraschek Gerd 2012 A grammar of Iatmul University of Regensburg OCLC 816072585 Foley William 1986 The Papuan Languages of New Guinea Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 28621 2 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Iatmul language amp oldid 1212010081, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.