fbpx
Wikipedia

Noun phrase

A noun phrase, or nominal (phrase), is a phrase that has a noun or pronoun as its head or performs the same grammatical function as a noun.[1] Noun phrases are very common cross-linguistically, and they may be the most frequently occurring phrase type.

Noun phrases often function as verb subjects and objects, as predicative expressions and as the complements of prepositions. Noun phrases can be embedded inside each other; for instance, the noun phrase some of his constituents contains the shorter noun phrase his constituents.

In some more modern theories of grammar, noun phrases with determiners are analyzed as having the determiner as the head of the phrase, see for instance Chomsky (1995) and Hudson (1990).

Identification

Some examples of noun phrases are underlined in the sentences below. The head noun appears in bold.

This election-year's politics are annoying for many people.
Almost every sentence contains at least one noun phrase.
 
Those five beautiful shiny Arkansas Black apples is a noun phrase of which apples is the head. To test, a single pronoun can replace the whole noun phrase, as in "They look delicious".
Current economic weakness may be a result of high energy prices.

Noun phrases can be identified by the possibility of pronoun substitution, as is illustrated in the examples below.

a. This sentence contains two noun phrases.
b. It contains them.
a. The subject noun phrase that is present in this sentence is long.
b. It is long.
a. Noun phrases can be embedded in other noun phrases.
b. They can be embedded in them.

A string of words that can be replaced by a single pronoun without rendering the sentence grammatically unacceptable is a noun phrase. As to whether the string must contain at least two words, see the following section.

Status of single words as phrases

Traditionally, a phrase is understood to contain two or more words. The traditional progression in the size of syntactic units is word < phrase < clause, and in this approach a single word (such as a noun or pronoun) would not be referred to as a phrase. However, many modern schools of syntax – especially those that have been influenced by X-bar theory – make no such restriction.[2] Here many single words are judged to be phrases based on a desire for theory-internal consistency. A phrase is deemed to be a word or a combination of words that appears in a set syntactic position, for instance in subject position or object position.

On this understanding of phrases, the nouns and pronouns in bold in the following sentences are noun phrases (as well as nouns or pronouns):

He saw someone.
Milk is good.
They spoke about corruption.

The words in bold are called phrases since they appear in the syntactic positions where multiple-word phrases (i.e. traditional phrases) can appear. This practice takes the constellation to be primitive rather than the words themselves. The word he, for instance, functions as a pronoun, but within the sentence it also functions as a noun phrase. The phrase structure grammars of the Chomskyan tradition (government and binding theory and the minimalist program) are primary examples of theories that apply this understanding of phrases. Other grammars such as dependency grammars are likely to reject this approach to phrases, since they take the words themselves to be primitive. For them, phrases must contain two or more words.

Components

A typical noun phrase consists of a noun (the head of the phrase) together with zero or more dependents of various types. (These dependents, since they modify a noun, are called adnominal.) The chief types of these dependents are:

The allowability, form and position of these elements depend on the syntax of the language in question. In English, determiners, adjectives (and some adjective phrases) and noun modifiers precede the head noun, whereas the heavier units – phrases and clauses – generally follow it. This is part of a strong tendency in English to place heavier constituents to the right, making English more of a head-initial language. Head-final languages (e.g. Japanese and Turkish) are more likely to place all modifiers before the head noun. Other languages, such as French, often place even single-word adjectives after the noun.

Noun phrases can take different forms than that described above, for example when the head is a pronoun rather than a noun, or when elements are linked with a coordinating conjunction such as and, or, but. For more information about the structure of noun phrases in English, see English grammar § Phrases.

Syntactic function

Noun phrases typically bear argument functions.[3] That is, the syntactic functions that they fulfill are those of the arguments of the main clause predicate, particularly those of subject, object and predicative expression. They also function as arguments in such constructs as participial phrases and prepositional phrases. For example:

For us the news is a concern. the news is the subject argument
Have you heard the news? the news is the object argument
That is the news. the news is the predicative expression following the copula is
They are talking about the news. the news is the argument in the prepositional phrase about the news
The man reading the news is very tall. the news is the object argument in the participial phrase reading the news

Sometimes a noun phrase can also function as an adjunct of the main clause predicate, thus taking on an adverbial function, e.g.

Most days I read the newspaper.
She has been studying all night.

With and without determiners

In some languages, including English, noun phrases are required to be "completed" with a determiner in many contexts, and thus a distinction is made in syntactic analysis between phrases that have received their required determiner (such as the big house), and those in which the determiner is lacking (such as big house).

The situation is complicated by the fact that in some contexts a noun phrase may nonetheless be used without a determiner (as in I like big houses); in this case the phrase may be described as having a "null determiner". (Situations in which this is possible depend on the rules of the language in question; for English, see English articles.)

In the original X-bar theory, the two respective types of entity are called noun phrase (NP) and N-bar (N, N′). Thus in the sentence Here is the big house, both house and big house are N-bars, while the big house is a noun phrase. In the sentence I like big houses, both houses and big houses are N-bars, but big houses also functions as a noun phrase (in this case without an explicit determiner).

In some modern theories of syntax, however, what are called "noun phrases" above are no longer considered to be headed by a noun, but by the determiner (which may be null), and they are thus called determiner phrases (DP) instead of noun phrases. (In some accounts that take this approach, the constituent lacking the determiner – that called N-bar above – may be referred to as a noun phrase.)

This analysis of noun phrases is widely referred to as the DP hypothesis. It has been the preferred analysis of noun phrases in the minimalist program from its start (since the early 1990s), though the arguments in its favor tend to be theory-internal. By taking the determiner, a function word, to be head over the noun, a structure is established that is analogous to the structure of the finite clause, with a complementizer. Apart from the minimalist program, however, the DP hypothesis is rejected by most other modern theories of syntax and grammar, in part because these theories lack the relevant functional categories.[4] Dependency grammars, for instance, almost all assume the traditional NP analysis of noun phrases.

For illustrations of different analyses of noun phrases depending on whether the DP hypothesis is rejected or accepted, see the next section.

Tree representations

The representation of noun phrases using parse trees depends on the basic approach to syntactic structure adopted. The layered trees of many phrase structure grammars grant noun phrases an intricate structure that acknowledges a hierarchy of functional projections. Dependency grammars, in contrast, since the basic architecture of dependency places a major limitation on the amount of structure that the theory can assume, produce simple, relatively flat structures for noun phrases.

The representation also depends on whether the noun or the determiner is taken to be the head of the phrase (see the discussion of the DP hypothesis in the previous section).

Below are some possible trees for the two noun phrases the big house and big houses (as in the sentences Here is the big house and I like big houses).

1. Phrase-structure trees, first using the original X-bar theory, then using the current DP approach:

 NP NP | DP DP / \ | | / \ | det N' N' | det NP NP | / \ / \ | | / \ / \ the adj N' adj N' | the adj NP adj NP | | | | | | | | | big N big N | big N big N | | | | | house houses | house houses 

2. Dependency trees, first using the traditional NP approach, then using the DP approach:

 house houses | the (null) / / / | \ \ / / big | house houses the big | / / | big big 

The following trees represent a more complex phrase. For simplicity, only dependency-based trees are given.[5]

The first tree is based on the traditional assumption that nouns, rather than determiners, are the heads of phrases.

 

The head noun picture has the four dependents the, old, of Fred, and that I found in the drawer. The tree shows how the lighter dependents appear as pre-dependents (preceding their head) and the heavier ones as post-dependents (following their head).

The second tree assumes the DP hypothesis, namely that determiners rather than nouns serve as phrase heads.

 

The determiner the is now depicted as the head of the entire phrase, thus making the phrase a determiner phrase. Note that there is still a noun phrase present (old picture of Fred that I found in the drawer) but this phrase is below the determiner.

See also

Footnotes

  1. ^ For definitions and discussions of the noun (nominal) phrase that point to the presence of a head noun, see for instance Crystal (1997:264), Lockwood (2002:3), and Radford (2004: 14, 348).
  2. ^ For direct examples of approaches that obscure the distinction between nouns and pronouns on the one hand and noun phrases on the other, see for instance Matthews (1981:160f.) and (Lockwood (2002:3).
  3. ^ Concerning how noun phrases function, see for instance Stockwell (1977:55ff.).
  4. ^ For discussion and criticism of the DP analysis of noun phrases, see Matthews (2007:12ff.).
  5. ^ For a dependency grammar analysis of noun phrases similar to the one represented by the trees here, see for instance Starosta (1988:219ff.). For an example of a relatively "flat" analysis of NP structure like the one produced here, but in a phrase structure grammar, see Culicover and Jackendoff (2005:140).

References

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Crystal, D. (1997). (PDF). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers. ISBN 978-1-405-15296-9. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2015-07-24. Retrieved 2015-04-28.
  • Culicover, P. and R. Jackendoff (2005). Simpler syntax. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • Hudson, R. (1990). English Word Grammar. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
  • Lockwood, D. 2002. Syntactic analysis and description: A constructional approach. London: Continuum.
  • Matthews, P. (1981). Syntax. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Matthews, P. (2007). Syntactic relations: A critical survey. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-60829-9.
  • Radford, A. 2004. English syntax: An introduction. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Starosta, S. (1988). The case for lexicase. London: Pinter Publishers. ISBN 0-86187-639-3.
  • Stockwell, P. 1977. Foundations of syntactic theory Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc.

See also:

  • Rijkhoff, Jan. 2008. Descriptive and discourse-referential modifiers in a layered model of the noun phrase. Linguistics 46-4, 789–829.
  • Rijkhoff, Jan (2002). The Noun Phrase. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198237822.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-823782-2.
  • Rijkhoff, Jan (2015). "Word Order". International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (PDF). pp. 644–656. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.53031-1. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • García Velasco, Daniel and Jan Rijkhoff (eds.).2008. The Noun Phrase in Functional Discourse Grammar (Trends in Linguistics. Studies and Monographs [TiLSM] 195). Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

noun, phrase, this, article, includes, list, general, references, lacks, sufficient, corresponding, inline, citations, please, help, improve, this, article, introducing, more, precise, citations, 2022, learn, when, remove, this, template, message, noun, phrase. This article includes a list of general references but it lacks sufficient corresponding inline citations Please help to improve this article by introducing more precise citations May 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message A noun phrase or nominal phrase is a phrase that has a noun or pronoun as its head or performs the same grammatical function as a noun 1 Noun phrases are very common cross linguistically and they may be the most frequently occurring phrase type Noun phrases often function as verb subjects and objects as predicative expressions and as the complements of prepositions Noun phrases can be embedded inside each other for instance the noun phrase some of his constituents contains the shorter noun phrase his constituents In some more modern theories of grammar noun phrases with determiners are analyzed as having the determiner as the head of the phrase see for instance Chomsky 1995 and Hudson 1990 Contents 1 Identification 2 Status of single words as phrases 3 Components 4 Syntactic function 5 With and without determiners 6 Tree representations 7 See also 8 Footnotes 9 ReferencesIdentification EditSome examples of noun phrases are underlined in the sentences below The head noun appears in bold This election year s politics are annoying for many people Almost every sentence contains at least one noun phrase Those five beautiful shiny Arkansas Black apples is a noun phrase of which apples is the head To test a single pronoun can replace the whole noun phrase as in They look delicious Current economic weakness may be a result of high energy prices dd Noun phrases can be identified by the possibility of pronoun substitution as is illustrated in the examples below a This sentence contains two noun phrases b It contains them a The subject noun phrase that is present in this sentence is long b It is long a Noun phrases can be embedded in other noun phrases b They can be embedded in them dd A string of words that can be replaced by a single pronoun without rendering the sentence grammatically unacceptable is a noun phrase As to whether the string must contain at least two words see the following section Status of single words as phrases EditTraditionally a phrase is understood to contain two or more words The traditional progression in the size of syntactic units is word lt phrase lt clause and in this approach a single word such as a noun or pronoun would not be referred to as a phrase However many modern schools of syntax especially those that have been influenced by X bar theory make no such restriction 2 Here many single words are judged to be phrases based on a desire for theory internal consistency A phrase is deemed to be a word or a combination of words that appears in a set syntactic position for instance in subject position or object position On this understanding of phrases the nouns and pronouns in bold in the following sentences are noun phrases as well as nouns or pronouns He saw someone Milk is good They spoke about corruption dd The words in bold are called phrases since they appear in the syntactic positions where multiple word phrases i e traditional phrases can appear This practice takes the constellation to be primitive rather than the words themselves The word he for instance functions as a pronoun but within the sentence it also functions as a noun phrase The phrase structure grammars of the Chomskyan tradition government and binding theory and the minimalist program are primary examples of theories that apply this understanding of phrases Other grammars such as dependency grammars are likely to reject this approach to phrases since they take the words themselves to be primitive For them phrases must contain two or more words Components EditA typical noun phrase consists of a noun the head of the phrase together with zero or more dependents of various types These dependents since they modify a noun are called adnominal The chief types of these dependents are determiners such as the this my some Jane s attributive adjectives such as large beautiful sweeter adjective phrases and participial phrases such as extremely large hard as nails made of wood sitting on the step noun adjuncts such as college in the noun phrase a college student nouns in certain oblique cases in languages which have them such as German des Mannes of the man genitive form prepositional phrases such as in the drawing room of his aunt adnominal adverbs and adverbials such as over there in the noun phrase the man over there relative clauses such as which we noticed other clauses serving as complements to the noun such as that God exists in the noun phrase the belief that God exists infinitive phrases such as to sing well and to beat in the noun phrases a desire to sing well and the man to beatThe allowability form and position of these elements depend on the syntax of the language in question In English determiners adjectives and some adjective phrases and noun modifiers precede the head noun whereas the heavier units phrases and clauses generally follow it This is part of a strong tendency in English to place heavier constituents to the right making English more of a head initial language Head final languages e g Japanese and Turkish are more likely to place all modifiers before the head noun Other languages such as French often place even single word adjectives after the noun Noun phrases can take different forms than that described above for example when the head is a pronoun rather than a noun or when elements are linked with a coordinating conjunction such as and or but For more information about the structure of noun phrases in English see English grammar Phrases Syntactic function EditNoun phrases typically bear argument functions 3 That is the syntactic functions that they fulfill are those of the arguments of the main clause predicate particularly those of subject object and predicative expression They also function as arguments in such constructs as participial phrases and prepositional phrases For example For us the news is a concern the news is the subject argument dd Have you heard the news the news is the object argument dd That is the news the news is the predicative expression following the copula is dd They are talking about the news the news is the argument in the prepositional phrase about the news dd The man reading the news is very tall the news is the object argument in the participial phrase reading the news dd Sometimes a noun phrase can also function as an adjunct of the main clause predicate thus taking on an adverbial function e g Most days I read the newspaper dd She has been studying all night dd With and without determiners EditIn some languages including English noun phrases are required to be completed with a determiner in many contexts and thus a distinction is made in syntactic analysis between phrases that have received their required determiner such as the big house and those in which the determiner is lacking such as big house The situation is complicated by the fact that in some contexts a noun phrase may nonetheless be used without a determiner as in I like big houses in this case the phrase may be described as having a null determiner Situations in which this is possible depend on the rules of the language in question for English see English articles In the original X bar theory the two respective types of entity are called noun phrase NP and N bar N N Thus in the sentence Here is the big house both house and big house are N bars while the big house is a noun phrase In the sentence I like big houses both houses and big houses are N bars but big houses also functions as a noun phrase in this case without an explicit determiner In some modern theories of syntax however what are called noun phrases above are no longer considered to be headed by a noun but by the determiner which may be null and they are thus called determiner phrases DP instead of noun phrases In some accounts that take this approach the constituent lacking the determiner that called N bar above may be referred to as a noun phrase This analysis of noun phrases is widely referred to as the DP hypothesis It has been the preferred analysis of noun phrases in the minimalist program from its start since the early 1990s though the arguments in its favor tend to be theory internal By taking the determiner a function word to be head over the noun a structure is established that is analogous to the structure of the finite clause with a complementizer Apart from the minimalist program however the DP hypothesis is rejected by most other modern theories of syntax and grammar in part because these theories lack the relevant functional categories 4 Dependency grammars for instance almost all assume the traditional NP analysis of noun phrases For illustrations of different analyses of noun phrases depending on whether the DP hypothesis is rejected or accepted see the next section Tree representations EditThe representation of noun phrases using parse trees depends on the basic approach to syntactic structure adopted The layered trees of many phrase structure grammars grant noun phrases an intricate structure that acknowledges a hierarchy of functional projections Dependency grammars in contrast since the basic architecture of dependency places a major limitation on the amount of structure that the theory can assume produce simple relatively flat structures for noun phrases The representation also depends on whether the noun or the determiner is taken to be the head of the phrase see the discussion of the DP hypothesis in the previous section Below are some possible trees for the two noun phrases the big house and big houses as in the sentences Here is the big house and I like big houses 1 Phrase structure trees first using the original X bar theory then using the current DP approach NP NP DP DP det N N det NP NP the adj N adj N the adj NP adj NP big N big N big N big N house houses house houses 2 Dependency trees first using the traditional NP approach then using the DP approach house houses the null big house houses the big big big The following trees represent a more complex phrase For simplicity only dependency based trees are given 5 The first tree is based on the traditional assumption that nouns rather than determiners are the heads of phrases dd The head noun picture has the four dependents the old of Fred and that I found in the drawer The tree shows how the lighter dependents appear as pre dependents preceding their head and the heavier ones as post dependents following their head The second tree assumes the DP hypothesis namely that determiners rather than nouns serve as phrase heads dd The determiner the is now depicted as the head of the entire phrase thus making the phrase a determiner phrase Note that there is still a noun phrase present old picture of Fred that I found in the drawer but this phrase is below the determiner See also EditChunking computational linguistics Conservativity Nominal group functional grammar Footnotes Edit For definitions and discussions of the noun nominal phrase that point to the presence of a head noun see for instance Crystal 1997 264 Lockwood 2002 3 and Radford 2004 14 348 For direct examples of approaches that obscure the distinction between nouns and pronouns on the one hand and noun phrases on the other see for instance Matthews 1981 160f and Lockwood 2002 3 Concerning how noun phrases function see for instance Stockwell 1977 55ff For discussion and criticism of the DP analysis of noun phrases see Matthews 2007 12ff For a dependency grammar analysis of noun phrases similar to the one represented by the trees here see for instance Starosta 1988 219ff For an example of a relatively flat analysis of NP structure like the one produced here but in a phrase structure grammar see Culicover and Jackendoff 2005 140 References EditChomsky N 1995 The Minimalist Program Cambridge MA The MIT Press Crystal D 1997 A dictionary of linguistics and phonetics PDF Oxford UK Blackwell Publishers ISBN 978 1 405 15296 9 Archived from the original PDF on 2015 07 24 Retrieved 2015 04 28 Culicover P and R Jackendoff 2005 Simpler syntax Oxford UK Oxford University Press Hudson R 1990 English Word Grammar Oxford Basil Blackwell Lockwood D 2002 Syntactic analysis and description A constructional approach London Continuum Matthews P 1981 Syntax Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press Matthews P 2007 Syntactic relations A critical survey Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 60829 9 Radford A 2004 English syntax An introduction Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press Starosta S 1988 The case for lexicase London Pinter Publishers ISBN 0 86187 639 3 Stockwell P 1977 Foundations of syntactic theory Englewood Cliffs NJ Prentice Hall Inc See also Rijkhoff Jan 2008 Descriptive and discourse referential modifiers in a layered model of the noun phrase Linguistics 46 4 789 829 Rijkhoff Jan 2002 The Noun Phrase doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780198237822 001 0001 ISBN 978 0 19 823782 2 Rijkhoff Jan 2015 Word Order International Encyclopedia of the Social amp Behavioral Sciences PDF pp 644 656 doi 10 1016 B978 0 08 097086 8 53031 1 ISBN 978 0 08 097087 5 Garcia Velasco Daniel and Jan Rijkhoff eds 2008 The Noun Phrase in Functional Discourse Grammar Trends in Linguistics Studies and Monographs TiLSM 195 Berlin and New York Mouton de Gruyter Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Noun phrase amp oldid 1131848466, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.