fbpx
Wikipedia

Language family

A language family is a group of languages related through descent from a common ancestral language or parental language, called the proto-language of that family. The term "family" reflects the tree model of language origination in historical linguistics, which makes use of a metaphor comparing languages to people in a biological family tree, or in a subsequent modification, to species in a phylogenetic tree of evolutionary taxonomy. Linguists therefore describe the daughter languages within a language family as being genetically related.[1] The divergence of a proto-language into daughter languages typically occurs through geographical separation, with different regional dialects of the proto-language spoken by different speech communities undergoing different language changes and thus becoming distinct languages from each other.[2]

Contemporary distribution (2005 map) of the world's major language families (in some cases geographic groups of families). This map includes only primary families i.e. branches are excluded.
See Distribution of languages on Earth for greater detail.

The language families with the most speakers are the Indo-European family, which includes many widely spoken languages native to Europe (such as English and Spanish) and South Asia (such as Hindi, Urdu and Bengali); and the Sino-Tibetan family, mainly due to the many speakers of Mandarin Chinese in China.[3] A language family may contain any number of languages: some families, such as the Austronesian and Niger-Congo families, contain hundreds of different languages,[3] while some languages, termed isolates, are not known to be related to any other languages and therefore constitute a family consisting of only one language.

Membership of languages in a language family is established by research in comparative linguistics. Genealogically related languages can be identified by their shared retentions; that is, they share systematic similarities that cannot be explained as due to chance, or to effects of language contact (such as borrowing or convergence), and therefore must be features inherited from their shared common ancestor. However, some sets of languages may in fact be derived from a common ancestor but have diverged enough from each other that their relationship is no longer detectable; and some languages have not been studied in enough detail to be classified, and therefore their family membership is unknown.

Major language families edit

Estimates of the number of language families in the world may vary widely. According to Ethnologue there are 7,151 living human languages distributed in 142 different language families.[4][5] Lyle Campbell (2019) identifies a total of 406 independent language families, including isolates.[6]

Ethnologue 24 (2021) lists the following families that contain at least 1% of the 7,139 known languages in the world:[7]

  1. Niger–Congo (1,542 languages) (21.7%)
  2. Austronesian (1,257 languages) (17.7%)
  3. Trans–New Guinea (482 languages) (6.8%)
  4. Sino-Tibetan (455 languages) (6.4%)
  5. Indo-European (448 languages) (6.3%)
  6. Australian (381 languages) (5.4%)
  7. Afro-Asiatic (377 languages) (5.3%)
  8. Nilo-Saharan (206 languages) (2.9%)
  9. Oto-Manguean (178 languages) (2.5%)
  10. Austroasiatic (167 languages) (2.3%)
  11. Tai–Kadai (91 languages) (1.3%)
  12. Dravidian (86 languages) (1.2%)
  13. Tupian (76 languages) (1.1%)

Glottolog 4.7 (2022) lists the following as the largest families, of 8,565 languages (other than sign languages, pidgins, and unclassifiable languages):[8]

  1. Atlantic–Congo (1,408 languages)
  2. Austronesian (1,273 languages)
  3. Indo-European (584 languages)
  4. Sino-Tibetan (501 languages)
  5. Afro-Asiatic (379 languages)
  6. Nuclear Trans–New Guinea (317 languages)
  7. Pama–Nyungan (250 languages)
  8. Oto-Manguean (181 languages)
  9. Austroasiatic (158 languages)
  10. Tai–Kadai (95 languages)
  11. Dravidian (82 languages)
  12. Arawakan (77 languages)
  13. Mande (75 languages)
  14. Tupian (71 languages)

Language counts can vary significantly depending on what is considered a dialect; for example Lyle Campbell counts only 27 Otomanguean languages, although he, Ethnologue and Glottolog also disagree as to which languages belong in the family.

Genetic relationship edit

Two languages have a genetic relationship, and belong to the same language family, if both are descended from a common ancestor through the process of language change, or one is descended from the other. The term and the process of language evolution are independent of, and not reliant on, the terminology, understanding, and theories related to genetics in the biological sense, so, to avoid confusion, some linguists prefer the term genealogical relationship.[9][10]: 184 

An example of linguistic genetic relationship would be among the Romance languages, such as Spanish, French, Italian, Portuguese, Romanian and many others, all descended from the spoken Latin of ancient Rome.[note 1][11]

There is a remarkably similar pattern shown by the linguistic tree and the genetic tree of human ancestry[12] that was verified statistically.[13] Languages interpreted in terms of the putative phylogenetic tree of human languages are transmitted to a great extent vertically (by ancestry) as opposed to horizontally (by spatial diffusion).[14]

Establishment edit

In some cases, the shared derivation of a group of related languages from a common ancestor is directly attested in the historical record. For example, this is the case for the Romance language family, wherein Spanish, Italian, Portuguese, Romanian, and French are all descended from Latin, as well as for the North Germanic language family, including Danish, Swedish, Norwegian and Icelandic, which have shared descent from Ancient Norse. Latin and ancient Norse are both attested in written records, as are many intermediate stages between those ancestral languages and their modern descendants.

In other cases, genetic relationships between languages are not directly attested. For instance, the Romance languages and the North Germanic languages are also related to each other, being subfamilies of the Indo-European language family, since both Latin and Old Norse are believed to be descended from an even more ancient language, Proto-Indo-European; however, no direct evidence of Proto-Indo-European or its divergence into its descendant languages survives. In cases such as these, genetic relationships are established through use of the comparative method of linguistic analysis.

In order to test the hypothesis that two languages are related, the comparative method begins with the collection of pairs of words that are hypothesized to be cognates: i.e., words in related languages that are derived from the same word in the shared ancestral language. Pairs of words that have similar pronunciations and meanings in the two languages are often good candidates for hypothetical cognates. The researcher must rule out the possibility that the two words are similar merely due to chance, or due to one having borrowed the words from the other (or from a language related to the other). Chance resemblance is ruled out by the existence of large collections of pairs of words between the two languages showing similar patterns of phonetic similarity. Once coincidental similarity and borrowing have been eliminated as possible explanations for similarities in sound and meaning of words, the remaining explanation is common origin: it is inferred that the similarities occurred due to descent from a common ancestor, and the words are actually cognates, implying the languages must be related.[15]

Linguistic interference and borrowing edit

When languages are in contact with one another, either of them may influence the other through linguistic interference such as borrowing. For example, French has influenced English, Arabic has influenced Persian, Sanskrit has influenced Tamil, and Chinese has influenced Japanese in this way. However, such influence does not constitute (and is not a measure of) a genetic relationship between the languages concerned. Linguistic interference can occur between languages that are genetically closely related, between languages that are distantly related (like English and French, which are distantly related Indo-European languages) and between languages that have no genetic relationship.

Complications edit

Some problems[why?] encountered by the genetic relationship group of languages include language isolates and mixed, pidgin and creole languages.

Mixed languages, pidgins and creole languages constitute special genetic types of languages. They do not descend linearly or directly from a single language and have no single ancestor.

Isolates are languages that cannot be proven to be genealogically related to any other modern language. As a corollary, every language isolate also forms its own language family — a genetic family which happens to consist of just one language. One often cited example is Basque, which forms a language family on its own; but there are many other examples outside Europe. On the global scale, the site Glottolog counts a total of 427 language families in the world, including 182 isolates.[16]

Monogenesis edit

One controversial theory concerning the genetic relationships among languages is monogenesis, the idea that all known languages, with the exceptions of creoles, pidgins and sign languages, are descendant from a single ancestral language.[17] If that is true, it would mean all languages (other than pidgins, creoles, and sign languages) are genetically related, but in many cases, the relationships may be too remote to be detectable. Alternative explanations for some basic observed commonalities between languages include developmental theories, related to the biological development of the capacity for language as the child grows from newborn.[citation needed]

Structure of a family edit

A language family is a monophyletic unit; all its members derive from a common ancestor, and all descendants of that ancestor are included in the family. Thus, the term family is analogous to the biological term clade. Language families can be divided into smaller phylogenetic units, sometimes referred to as "branches" or "subfamilies" of the family; for instance, the Germanic languages are a subfamily of the Indo-European family. Subfamilies share a more recent common ancestor than the common ancestor of the larger family; Proto-Germanic, the common ancestor of the Germanic subfamily, was itself a descendant of Proto-Indo-European, the common ancestor of the Indo-European family. Within a large family, subfamilies can be identified through "shared innovations": members of a subfamily will share features that represent retentions from their more recent common ancestor, but were not present in the overall proto-language of the larger family.

Some taxonomists restrict the term family to a certain level, but there is little consensus on how to do so. Those who affix such labels also subdivide branches into groups, and groups into complexes. A top-level (i.e., the largest) family is often called a phylum or stock. The closer the branches are to each other, the more closely the languages will be related. This means if a branch of a proto-language is four branches down and there is also a sister language to that fourth branch, then the two sister languages are more closely related to each other than to that common ancestral proto-language.

The term macrofamily or superfamily is sometimes applied to proposed groupings of language families whose status as phylogenetic units is generally considered to be unsubstantiated by accepted historical linguistic methods.

Dialect continua edit

Some close-knit language families, and many branches within larger families, take the form of dialect continua in which there are no clear-cut borders that make it possible to unequivocally identify, define, or count individual languages within the family. However, when the differences between the speech of different regions at the extremes of the continuum are so great that there is no mutual intelligibility between them, as occurs in Arabic, the continuum cannot meaningfully be seen as a single language.

A speech variety may also be considered either a language or a dialect depending on social or political considerations. Thus, different sources, especially over time, can give wildly different numbers of languages within a certain family. Classifications of the Japonic family, for example, range from one language (a language isolate with dialects) to nearly twenty—until the classification of Ryukyuan as separate languages within a Japonic language family rather than dialects of Japanese, the Japanese language itself was considered a language isolate and therefore the only language in its family.

Isolates edit

Most of the world's languages are known to be related to others. Those that have no known relatives (or for which family relationships are only tentatively proposed) are called language isolates, essentially language families consisting of a single language. There are an estimated 129 language isolates known today.[18] An example is Basque. In general, it is assumed that language isolates have relatives or had relatives at some point in their history but at a time depth too great for linguistic comparison to recover them.

A language isolate is classified based on the fact that enough is known about the isolate to compare it genetically to other languages but no common ancestry or relationship is found with any other known language.[18]

A language isolated in its own branch within a family, such as Albanian and Armenian within Indo-European, is often also called an isolate, but the meaning of the word "isolate" in such cases is usually clarified with a modifier. For instance, Albanian and Armenian may be referred to as an "Indo-European isolate". By contrast, so far as is known, the Basque language is an absolute isolate: it has not been shown to be related to any other modern language despite numerous attempts. Another well-known isolate is Mapudungun, the Mapuche language from the Araucanían language family in Chile.[clarification needed] A language may be said to be an isolate currently but not historically if related but now extinct relatives are attested. The Aquitanian language, spoken in Roman times, may have been an ancestor of Basque, but it could also have been a sister language to the ancestor of Basque. In the latter case, Basque and Aquitanian would form a small family together. Ancestors are not considered to be distinct members of a family.[citation needed]

Proto-languages edit

A proto-language can be thought of as a mother language (not to be confused with a mother tongue[19]) being the root from which all languages in the family stem. The common ancestor of a language family is seldom known directly since most languages have a relatively short recorded history. However, it is possible to recover many features of a proto-language by applying the comparative method, a reconstructive procedure worked out by 19th century linguist August Schleicher. This can demonstrate the validity of many of the proposed families in the list of language families. For example, the reconstructible common ancestor of the Indo-European language family is called Proto-Indo-European. Proto-Indo-European is not attested by written records and so is conjectured to have been spoken before the invention of writing.

Visual representation edit

 
An example of a language tree, containing the Mayan languages

A common visual representation of a language family is given by a genetic language tree. The tree model is sometimes termed a dendrogram or phylogeny. The family tree shows the relationship of the languages within a family, much as a family tree of an individual shows their relationship with their relatives. There are criticisms to the family tree model. Critics focus mainly on the claim that the internal structure of the trees is subject to variation based on the criteria of classification.[20] Even among those who support the family tree model, there are debates over which languages should be included in a language family. For example, within the dubious Altaic language family, there are debates over whether the Japonic and Koreanic languages should be included or not.[21]

The wave model has been proposed as an alternative to the tree model.[10] The wave model uses isoglosses to group language varieties; unlike in the tree model, these groups can overlap. While the tree model implies a lack of contact between languages after derivation from an ancestral form, the wave model emphasizes the relationship between languages that remain in contact, which is more realistic.[10] Historical glottometry is an application of the wave model, meant to identify and evaluate genetic relations in linguistic linkages.[10][22]

Other classifications of languages edit

Sprachbund edit

A sprachbund is a geographic area having several languages that feature common linguistic structures. The similarities between those languages are caused by language contact, not by chance or common origin, and are not recognized as criteria that define a language family. An example of a sprachbund would be the Indian subcontinent.[23]

Shared innovations, acquired by borrowing or other means, are not considered genetic and have no bearing with the language family concept. It has been asserted, for example, that many of the more striking features shared by Italic languages (Latin, Oscan, Umbrian, etc.) might well be "areal features". However, very similar-looking alterations in the systems of long vowels in the West Germanic languages greatly postdate any possible notion of a proto-language innovation (and cannot readily be regarded as "areal", either, since English and continental West Germanic were not a linguistic area). In a similar vein, there are many similar unique innovations in Germanic, Baltic and Slavic that are far more likely to be areal features than traceable to a common proto-language. But legitimate uncertainty about whether shared innovations are areal features, coincidence, or inheritance from a common ancestor, leads to disagreement over the proper subdivisions of any large language family.

Contact languages edit

The concept of language families is based on the historical observation that languages develop dialects, which over time may diverge into distinct languages. However, linguistic ancestry is less clear-cut than familiar biological ancestry, in which species do not crossbreed.[24] It is more like the evolution of microbes, with extensive lateral gene transfer. Quite distantly related languages may affect each other through language contact, which in extreme cases may lead to languages with no single ancestor, whether they be creoles or mixed languages. In addition, a number of sign languages have developed in isolation and appear to have no relatives at all. Nonetheless, such cases are relatively rare and most well-attested languages can be unambiguously classified as belonging to one language family or another, even if this family's relation to other families is not known.

Language contact can lead to the development of new languages from the mixture of two or more languages for the purposes of interactions between two groups who speak different languages. Languages that arise in order for two groups to communicate with each other to engage in commercial trade or that appeared as a result of colonialism are called pidgin. Pidgins are an example of linguistic and cultural expansion caused by language contact. However, language contact can also lead to cultural divisions. In some cases, two different language speaking groups can feel territorial towards their language and do not want any changes to be made to it. This causes language boundaries and groups in contact are not willing to make any compromises to accommodate the other language.[25]

See also edit

Notes edit

References edit

  1. ^ Rowe, Bruce M.; Levine, Diane P. (2015). A Concise Introduction to Linguistics. Routledge. pp. 340–341. ISBN 978-1317349280. Retrieved 26 January 2017.
  2. ^ Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (2011). Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages. John Benjamins Publishing. p. 336. ISBN 978-9027287229. Retrieved 26 January 2017.
  3. ^ a b "What are the largest language families?". Ethnologue. 25 May 2019.
  4. ^ "How many languages are there in the world?". Ethnologue. 3 May 2016. Retrieved 26 March 2021.
  5. ^ "What are the largest language families?". Ethnologue. 25 May 2019. Retrieved 3 March 2020.
  6. ^ Campbell, Lyle (8 January 2019). "How Many Language Families are there in the World?". Anuario del Seminario de Filología Vasca "Julio de Urquijo". UPV/EHU Press. 52 (1/2): 133. doi:10.1387/asju.20195. hdl:10810/49565. ISSN 2444-2992. S2CID 166394477.
  7. ^ "Welcome to the 24th edition". Ethnologue. 22 February 2021.
  8. ^ "Glottolog 4.7 -". glottolog.org. Retrieved 25 June 2023.
  9. ^ Haspelmath, Martin (5 May 2004). "How hopeless is genealogical linguistics, and how advanced is areal linguistics? — Review of Aikhenvald & Dixon (2001): Areal diffusion and genetic inheritance". Studies in Language. 28 (1): 209–223. doi:10.1075/sl.28.1.10has. p. 222.
  10. ^ a b c d François, Alexandre (2014). "Trees, Waves and Linkages: Models of Language Diversification" (PDF). In Bowern, Claire; Evans, Bethwyn (eds.). The Routledge Handbook of Historical Linguistics. London: Routledge. pp. 161–189. ISBN 978-0-41552-789-7.
  11. ^ Lewis, M. Paul, Gary F. Simons, and Charles D. Fennig (eds.). Ethnologue: Languages of the World, Seventeenth edition. Dallas, Texas: SIL International, 2013.
  12. ^ Henn, B. M.; Cavalli-Sforza, L. L.; Feldman, M. W. (17 October 2012). "The great human expansion". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 109 (44): 17758–17764. Bibcode:2012PNAS..10917758H. doi:10.1073/pnas.1212380109. JSTOR 41829755. PMC 3497766. PMID 23077256.
  13. ^ Cavalli-Sforza, L. L.; Minch, E.; Mountain, J. L. (15 June 1992). "Coevolution of genes and languages revisited". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 89 (12): 5620–5624. Bibcode:1992PNAS...89.5620C. doi:10.1073/pnas.89.12.5620. JSTOR 2359705. PMC 49344. PMID 1608971.
  14. ^ Gell-Mann, M.; Ruhlen, M. (10 October 2011). "The origin and evolution of word order" (PDF). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (42): 17290–17295. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10817290G. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113716108. JSTOR 41352497. PMC 3198322. PMID 21987807.
  15. ^ Campbell, Lyle (2013). Historical Linguistics. MIT Press.
  16. ^ Cf. Language families, Glottolog.
  17. ^ Nichols, Johanna. Monogenesis or Polygenesis: A Single Ancestral Language for All Humanity? Ch. 58 of The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution, ed. by Maggie Tallerman and Kathleen Rita Gibson. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2012. 558–72. Print.
  18. ^ a b Campbell, Lyle (24 August 2010). "Language Isolates and Their History, or, What's Weird, Anyway?". Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. 36 (1): 16–31. doi:10.3765/bls.v36i1.3900. ISSN 2377-1666.
  19. ^ Bloomfield, Leonard (1994). Language. Motilal Banarsidass Publ. ISBN 81-208-1196-8.
  20. ^ Edzard, Lutz. Polygenesis, Convergence, and Entropy: An Alternative Model of Linguistic Evolution Applied to Semitic Linguistics. Wiesbaden: Harrassowitz, 1998. Print.
  21. ^ Georg, Stefan, Peter A. Michalove, Alexis Manaster Ramer, and Paul J. Sidwell. Telling General Linguists about Altaic. Journal of Linguistics 35.1 (1999): 65–98. Print.
  22. ^ Kalyan, Siva; François, Alexandre (2018). "Freeing the Comparative Method from the tree model: A framework for Historical Glottometry" (PDF). In Kikusawa, Ritsuko; Reid, Laurie (eds.). Let's Talk about Trees: Genetic Relationships of Languages and Their Phylogenic Representation. Senri Ethnological Studies. Vol. 98. Ōsaka: National Museum of Ethnology. pp. 59–89.
  23. ^ Joseph, Brian (2017). "The Balkan Sprachbund" (PDF). linguisticsociety.org. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
  24. ^ List, Johann-Mattis; Nelson-Sathi, Shijulal; Geisler, Hans; Martin, William (2014). "Networks of lexical borrowing and lateral gene transfer in language and genome evolution". BioEssays. 36 (2): 141–150. doi:10.1002/bies.201300096. ISSN 0265-9247. PMC 3910147. PMID 24375688.
  25. ^ "Languages in Contact | Linguistic Society of America". www.linguisticsociety.org. Retrieved 2 October 2020.

Further reading edit

  • Boas, Franz (1911). Handbook of American Indian languages. Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40. Vol. 1. Washington: Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology. ISBN 0-8032-5017-7.
  • Boas, Franz. (1922). Handbook of American Indian languages (Vol. 2). Bureau of American Ethnology, Bulletin 40. Washington, D.C.: Government Print Office (Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology).
  • Boas, Franz. (1933). Handbook of American Indian languages (Vol. 3). Native American legal materials collection, title 1227. Glückstadt: J.J. Augustin.
  • Campbell, Lyle. (1997). American Indian languages: The historical linguistics of Native America. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-509427-1.
  • Campbell, Lyle; & Mithun, Marianne (Eds.). (1979). The languages of native America: Historical and comparative assessment. Austin: University of Texas Press.
  • Goddard, Ives (Ed.). (1996). Languages. Handbook of North American Indians (W. C. Sturtevant, General Ed.) (Vol. 17). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution. ISBN 0-16-048774-9.
  • Goddard, Ives. (1999). Native languages and language families of North America (rev. and enlarged ed. with additions and corrections). [Map]. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press (Smithsonian Institution). (Updated version of the map in Goddard 1996). ISBN 0-8032-9271-6.
  • Gordon, Raymond G., Jr. (Ed.). (2005). Ethnologue: Languages of the world (15th ed.). Dallas, TX: SIL International. ISBN 1-55671-159-X. (Online version: Ethnologue: Languages of the World).
  • Greenberg, Joseph H. (1966). The Languages of Africa (2nd ed.). Bloomington: Indiana University.
  • Harrison, K. David. (2007) When Languages Die: The Extinction of the World's Languages and the Erosion of Human Knowledge. New York and London: Oxford University Press.
  • Mithun, Marianne. (1999). The languages of Native North America. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-23228-7 (hbk); ISBN 0-521-29875-X.
  • Ross, Malcolm. (2005). "Pronouns as a preliminary diagnostic for grouping Papuan languages 8 June 2004 at the Wayback Machine". In: Andrew Pawley, Robert Attenborough, Robin Hide and Jack Golson, eds, Papuan pasts: cultural, linguistic and biological histories of Papuan-speaking peoples (PDF)
  • Ruhlen, Merritt. (1987). A guide to the world's languages. Stanford: Stanford University Press.
  • Sturtevant, William C. (Ed.). (1978–present). Handbook of North American Indians (Vol. 1–20). Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution. (Vols. 1–3, 16, 18–20 not yet published).
  • Voegelin, C. F. & Voegelin, F. M. (1977). Classification and index of the world's languages. New York: Elsevier.

External links edit

  • Linguistic maps (from Muturzikin)
  • Ethnologue
  • The Multitree Project
  • Lenguas del mundo (World Languages)
  • Comparative Swadesh list tables of various language families (from Wiktionary)
  • Most similar languages

language, family, language, family, group, languages, related, through, descent, from, common, ancestral, language, parental, language, called, proto, language, that, family, term, family, reflects, tree, model, language, origination, historical, linguistics, . A language family is a group of languages related through descent from a common ancestral language or parental language called the proto language of that family The term family reflects the tree model of language origination in historical linguistics which makes use of a metaphor comparing languages to people in a biological family tree or in a subsequent modification to species in a phylogenetic tree of evolutionary taxonomy Linguists therefore describe the daughter languages within a language family as being genetically related 1 The divergence of a proto language into daughter languages typically occurs through geographical separation with different regional dialects of the proto language spoken by different speech communities undergoing different language changes and thus becoming distinct languages from each other 2 Contemporary distribution 2005 map of the world s major language families in some cases geographic groups of families This map includes only primary families i e branches are excluded See Distribution of languages on Earth for greater detail The language families with the most speakers are the Indo European family which includes many widely spoken languages native to Europe such as English and Spanish and South Asia such as Hindi Urdu and Bengali and the Sino Tibetan family mainly due to the many speakers of Mandarin Chinese in China 3 A language family may contain any number of languages some families such as the Austronesian and Niger Congo families contain hundreds of different languages 3 while some languages termed isolates are not known to be related to any other languages and therefore constitute a family consisting of only one language Membership of languages in a language family is established by research in comparative linguistics Genealogically related languages can be identified by their shared retentions that is they share systematic similarities that cannot be explained as due to chance or to effects of language contact such as borrowing or convergence and therefore must be features inherited from their shared common ancestor However some sets of languages may in fact be derived from a common ancestor but have diverged enough from each other that their relationship is no longer detectable and some languages have not been studied in enough detail to be classified and therefore their family membership is unknown Contents 1 Major language families 2 Genetic relationship 2 1 Establishment 2 1 1 Linguistic interference and borrowing 2 2 Complications 2 3 Monogenesis 3 Structure of a family 3 1 Dialect continua 3 2 Isolates 3 3 Proto languages 4 Visual representation 5 Other classifications of languages 5 1 Sprachbund 5 2 Contact languages 6 See also 7 Notes 8 References 9 Further reading 10 External linksMajor language families editMain article List of language families Estimates of the number of language families in the world may vary widely According to Ethnologue there are 7 151 living human languages distributed in 142 different language families 4 5 Lyle Campbell 2019 identifies a total of 406 independent language families including isolates 6 Ethnologue 24 2021 lists the following families that contain at least 1 of the 7 139 known languages in the world 7 Niger Congo 1 542 languages 21 7 Austronesian 1 257 languages 17 7 Trans New Guinea 482 languages 6 8 Sino Tibetan 455 languages 6 4 Indo European 448 languages 6 3 Australian 381 languages 5 4 Afro Asiatic 377 languages 5 3 Nilo Saharan 206 languages 2 9 Oto Manguean 178 languages 2 5 Austroasiatic 167 languages 2 3 Tai Kadai 91 languages 1 3 Dravidian 86 languages 1 2 Tupian 76 languages 1 1 Glottolog 4 7 2022 lists the following as the largest families of 8 565 languages other than sign languages pidgins and unclassifiable languages 8 Atlantic Congo 1 408 languages Austronesian 1 273 languages Indo European 584 languages Sino Tibetan 501 languages Afro Asiatic 379 languages Nuclear Trans New Guinea 317 languages Pama Nyungan 250 languages Oto Manguean 181 languages Austroasiatic 158 languages Tai Kadai 95 languages Dravidian 82 languages Arawakan 77 languages Mande 75 languages Tupian 71 languages Language counts can vary significantly depending on what is considered a dialect for example Lyle Campbell counts only 27 Otomanguean languages although he Ethnologue and Glottolog also disagree as to which languages belong in the family Genetic relationship editTwo languages have a genetic relationship and belong to the same language family if both are descended from a common ancestor through the process of language change or one is descended from the other The term and the process of language evolution are independent of and not reliant on the terminology understanding and theories related to genetics in the biological sense so to avoid confusion some linguists prefer the term genealogical relationship 9 10 184 An example of linguistic genetic relationship would be among the Romance languages such as Spanish French Italian Portuguese Romanian and many others all descended from the spoken Latin of ancient Rome note 1 11 There is a remarkably similar pattern shown by the linguistic tree and the genetic tree of human ancestry 12 that was verified statistically 13 Languages interpreted in terms of the putative phylogenetic tree of human languages are transmitted to a great extent vertically by ancestry as opposed to horizontally by spatial diffusion 14 Establishment edit Main article Comparative method In some cases the shared derivation of a group of related languages from a common ancestor is directly attested in the historical record For example this is the case for the Romance language family wherein Spanish Italian Portuguese Romanian and French are all descended from Latin as well as for the North Germanic language family including Danish Swedish Norwegian and Icelandic which have shared descent from Ancient Norse Latin and ancient Norse are both attested in written records as are many intermediate stages between those ancestral languages and their modern descendants In other cases genetic relationships between languages are not directly attested For instance the Romance languages and the North Germanic languages are also related to each other being subfamilies of the Indo European language family since both Latin and Old Norse are believed to be descended from an even more ancient language Proto Indo European however no direct evidence of Proto Indo European or its divergence into its descendant languages survives In cases such as these genetic relationships are established through use of the comparative method of linguistic analysis In order to test the hypothesis that two languages are related the comparative method begins with the collection of pairs of words that are hypothesized to be cognates i e words in related languages that are derived from the same word in the shared ancestral language Pairs of words that have similar pronunciations and meanings in the two languages are often good candidates for hypothetical cognates The researcher must rule out the possibility that the two words are similar merely due to chance or due to one having borrowed the words from the other or from a language related to the other Chance resemblance is ruled out by the existence of large collections of pairs of words between the two languages showing similar patterns of phonetic similarity Once coincidental similarity and borrowing have been eliminated as possible explanations for similarities in sound and meaning of words the remaining explanation is common origin it is inferred that the similarities occurred due to descent from a common ancestor and the words are actually cognates implying the languages must be related 15 Linguistic interference and borrowing edit When languages are in contact with one another either of them may influence the other through linguistic interference such as borrowing For example French has influenced English Arabic has influenced Persian Sanskrit has influenced Tamil and Chinese has influenced Japanese in this way However such influence does not constitute and is not a measure of a genetic relationship between the languages concerned Linguistic interference can occur between languages that are genetically closely related between languages that are distantly related like English and French which are distantly related Indo European languages and between languages that have no genetic relationship Complications edit Some problems why encountered by the genetic relationship group of languages include language isolates and mixed pidgin and creole languages Mixed languages pidgins and creole languages constitute special genetic types of languages They do not descend linearly or directly from a single language and have no single ancestor Isolates are languages that cannot be proven to be genealogically related to any other modern language As a corollary every language isolate also forms its own language family a genetic family which happens to consist of just one language One often cited example is Basque which forms a language family on its own but there are many other examples outside Europe On the global scale the site Glottolog counts a total of 427 language families in the world including 182 isolates 16 Monogenesis edit One controversial theory concerning the genetic relationships among languages is monogenesis the idea that all known languages with the exceptions of creoles pidgins and sign languages are descendant from a single ancestral language 17 If that is true it would mean all languages other than pidgins creoles and sign languages are genetically related but in many cases the relationships may be too remote to be detectable Alternative explanations for some basic observed commonalities between languages include developmental theories related to the biological development of the capacity for language as the child grows from newborn citation needed Structure of a family editThis section needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources in this section Unsourced material may be challenged and removed May 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message A language family is a monophyletic unit all its members derive from a common ancestor and all descendants of that ancestor are included in the family Thus the term family is analogous to the biological term clade Language families can be divided into smaller phylogenetic units sometimes referred to as branches or subfamilies of the family for instance the Germanic languages are a subfamily of the Indo European family Subfamilies share a more recent common ancestor than the common ancestor of the larger family Proto Germanic the common ancestor of the Germanic subfamily was itself a descendant of Proto Indo European the common ancestor of the Indo European family Within a large family subfamilies can be identified through shared innovations members of a subfamily will share features that represent retentions from their more recent common ancestor but were not present in the overall proto language of the larger family Some taxonomists restrict the term family to a certain level but there is little consensus on how to do so Those who affix such labels also subdivide branches into groups and groups into complexes A top level i e the largest family is often called a phylum or stock The closer the branches are to each other the more closely the languages will be related This means if a branch of a proto language is four branches down and there is also a sister language to that fourth branch then the two sister languages are more closely related to each other than to that common ancestral proto language The term macrofamily or superfamily is sometimes applied to proposed groupings of language families whose status as phylogenetic units is generally considered to be unsubstantiated by accepted historical linguistic methods Dialect continua edit Main article Dialect continuum Some close knit language families and many branches within larger families take the form of dialect continua in which there are no clear cut borders that make it possible to unequivocally identify define or count individual languages within the family However when the differences between the speech of different regions at the extremes of the continuum are so great that there is no mutual intelligibility between them as occurs in Arabic the continuum cannot meaningfully be seen as a single language A speech variety may also be considered either a language or a dialect depending on social or political considerations Thus different sources especially over time can give wildly different numbers of languages within a certain family Classifications of the Japonic family for example range from one language a language isolate with dialects to nearly twenty until the classification of Ryukyuan as separate languages within a Japonic language family rather than dialects of Japanese the Japanese language itself was considered a language isolate and therefore the only language in its family Isolates edit Main article Language isolate Most of the world s languages are known to be related to others Those that have no known relatives or for which family relationships are only tentatively proposed are called language isolates essentially language families consisting of a single language There are an estimated 129 language isolates known today 18 An example is Basque In general it is assumed that language isolates have relatives or had relatives at some point in their history but at a time depth too great for linguistic comparison to recover them A language isolate is classified based on the fact that enough is known about the isolate to compare it genetically to other languages but no common ancestry or relationship is found with any other known language 18 A language isolated in its own branch within a family such as Albanian and Armenian within Indo European is often also called an isolate but the meaning of the word isolate in such cases is usually clarified with a modifier For instance Albanian and Armenian may be referred to as an Indo European isolate By contrast so far as is known the Basque language is an absolute isolate it has not been shown to be related to any other modern language despite numerous attempts Another well known isolate is Mapudungun the Mapuche language from the Araucanian language family in Chile clarification needed A language may be said to be an isolate currently but not historically if related but now extinct relatives are attested The Aquitanian language spoken in Roman times may have been an ancestor of Basque but it could also have been a sister language to the ancestor of Basque In the latter case Basque and Aquitanian would form a small family together Ancestors are not considered to be distinct members of a family citation needed Proto languages edit Main article Proto language A proto language can be thought of as a mother language not to be confused with a mother tongue 19 being the root from which all languages in the family stem The common ancestor of a language family is seldom known directly since most languages have a relatively short recorded history However it is possible to recover many features of a proto language by applying the comparative method a reconstructive procedure worked out by 19th century linguist August Schleicher This can demonstrate the validity of many of the proposed families in the list of language families For example the reconstructible common ancestor of the Indo European language family is called Proto Indo European Proto Indo European is not attested by written records and so is conjectured to have been spoken before the invention of writing Visual representation edit nbsp An example of a language tree containing the Mayan languagesA common visual representation of a language family is given by a genetic language tree The tree model is sometimes termed a dendrogram or phylogeny The family tree shows the relationship of the languages within a family much as a family tree of an individual shows their relationship with their relatives There are criticisms to the family tree model Critics focus mainly on the claim that the internal structure of the trees is subject to variation based on the criteria of classification 20 Even among those who support the family tree model there are debates over which languages should be included in a language family For example within the dubious Altaic language family there are debates over whether the Japonic and Koreanic languages should be included or not 21 The wave model has been proposed as an alternative to the tree model 10 The wave model uses isoglosses to group language varieties unlike in the tree model these groups can overlap While the tree model implies a lack of contact between languages after derivation from an ancestral form the wave model emphasizes the relationship between languages that remain in contact which is more realistic 10 Historical glottometry is an application of the wave model meant to identify and evaluate genetic relations in linguistic linkages 10 22 Other classifications of languages editThis section needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources in this section Unsourced material may be challenged and removed May 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message Sprachbund edit Main article Sprachbund A sprachbund is a geographic area having several languages that feature common linguistic structures The similarities between those languages are caused by language contact not by chance or common origin and are not recognized as criteria that define a language family An example of a sprachbund would be the Indian subcontinent 23 Shared innovations acquired by borrowing or other means are not considered genetic and have no bearing with the language family concept It has been asserted for example that many of the more striking features shared by Italic languages Latin Oscan Umbrian etc might well be areal features However very similar looking alterations in the systems of long vowels in the West Germanic languages greatly postdate any possible notion of a proto language innovation and cannot readily be regarded as areal either since English and continental West Germanic were not a linguistic area In a similar vein there are many similar unique innovations in Germanic Baltic and Slavic that are far more likely to be areal features than traceable to a common proto language But legitimate uncertainty about whether shared innovations are areal features coincidence or inheritance from a common ancestor leads to disagreement over the proper subdivisions of any large language family Contact languages edit Main articles Mixed language Creole language and Pidgin The concept of language families is based on the historical observation that languages develop dialects which over time may diverge into distinct languages However linguistic ancestry is less clear cut than familiar biological ancestry in which species do not crossbreed 24 It is more like the evolution of microbes with extensive lateral gene transfer Quite distantly related languages may affect each other through language contact which in extreme cases may lead to languages with no single ancestor whether they be creoles or mixed languages In addition a number of sign languages have developed in isolation and appear to have no relatives at all Nonetheless such cases are relatively rare and most well attested languages can be unambiguously classified as belonging to one language family or another even if this family s relation to other families is not known Language contact can lead to the development of new languages from the mixture of two or more languages for the purposes of interactions between two groups who speak different languages Languages that arise in order for two groups to communicate with each other to engage in commercial trade or that appeared as a result of colonialism are called pidgin Pidgins are an example of linguistic and cultural expansion caused by language contact However language contact can also lead to cultural divisions In some cases two different language speaking groups can feel territorial towards their language and do not want any changes to be made to it This causes language boundaries and groups in contact are not willing to make any compromises to accommodate the other language 25 See also editBackground colors used on Wikipedia for various language families and groupsAfro Asiatic Nilo Saharan Niger Congo Khoisan areal Indo European Caucasian areal Uralic Dravidian Altaic areal Paleosiberian areal Sino Tibetan Hmong Mien Kra Dai AustroasiaticAustronesian Papuan areal Australian areal Andamanese areal Eskaleut Algic Uto Aztecan Na Dene and Dene Yeniseian American areal Creole Pidgin Mixed Language isolate Sign language Constructed language UnclassifiedComparative linguistics Constructed language Endangered language Extinct language Language death Language isolate List of revived languages Global language system ISO 639 5 Linguist List List of language families List of languages by number of native speakers Origin of language Proto language Proto Human language Sprachbund Tree model Unclassified language Father Tongue hypothesis Farming language dispersal hypothesisNotes edit Vernacular Latin as opposed to the classical literary language References edit Rowe Bruce M Levine Diane P 2015 A Concise Introduction to Linguistics Routledge pp 340 341 ISBN 978 1317349280 Retrieved 26 January 2017 Dimmendaal Gerrit J 2011 Historical Linguistics and the Comparative Study of African Languages John Benjamins Publishing p 336 ISBN 978 9027287229 Retrieved 26 January 2017 a b What are the largest language families Ethnologue 25 May 2019 How many languages are there in the world Ethnologue 3 May 2016 Retrieved 26 March 2021 What are the largest language families Ethnologue 25 May 2019 Retrieved 3 March 2020 Campbell Lyle 8 January 2019 How Many Language Families are there in the World Anuario del Seminario de Filologia Vasca Julio de Urquijo UPV EHU Press 52 1 2 133 doi 10 1387 asju 20195 hdl 10810 49565 ISSN 2444 2992 S2CID 166394477 Welcome to the 24th edition Ethnologue 22 February 2021 Glottolog 4 7 glottolog org Retrieved 25 June 2023 Haspelmath Martin 5 May 2004 How hopeless is genealogical linguistics and how advanced is areal linguistics Review of Aikhenvald amp Dixon 2001 Areal diffusion and genetic inheritance Studies in Language 28 1 209 223 doi 10 1075 sl 28 1 10has p 222 a b c d Francois Alexandre 2014 Trees Waves and Linkages Models of Language Diversification PDF In Bowern Claire Evans Bethwyn eds The Routledge Handbook of Historical Linguistics London Routledge pp 161 189 ISBN 978 0 41552 789 7 Lewis M Paul Gary F Simons and Charles D Fennig eds Ethnologue Languages of the World Seventeenth edition Dallas Texas SIL International 2013 Henn B M Cavalli Sforza L L Feldman M W 17 October 2012 The great human expansion Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 109 44 17758 17764 Bibcode 2012PNAS 10917758H doi 10 1073 pnas 1212380109 JSTOR 41829755 PMC 3497766 PMID 23077256 Cavalli Sforza L L Minch E Mountain J L 15 June 1992 Coevolution of genes and languages revisited Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 89 12 5620 5624 Bibcode 1992PNAS 89 5620C doi 10 1073 pnas 89 12 5620 JSTOR 2359705 PMC 49344 PMID 1608971 Gell Mann M Ruhlen M 10 October 2011 The origin and evolution of word order PDF Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 42 17290 17295 Bibcode 2011PNAS 10817290G doi 10 1073 pnas 1113716108 JSTOR 41352497 PMC 3198322 PMID 21987807 Campbell Lyle 2013 Historical Linguistics MIT Press Cf Language families Glottolog Nichols Johanna Monogenesis or Polygenesis A Single Ancestral Language for All Humanity Ch 58 of The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution ed by Maggie Tallerman and Kathleen Rita Gibson Oxford Oxford UP 2012 558 72 Print a b Campbell Lyle 24 August 2010 Language Isolates and Their History or What s Weird Anyway Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society 36 1 16 31 doi 10 3765 bls v36i1 3900 ISSN 2377 1666 Bloomfield Leonard 1994 Language Motilal Banarsidass Publ ISBN 81 208 1196 8 Edzard Lutz Polygenesis Convergence and Entropy An Alternative Model of Linguistic Evolution Applied to Semitic Linguistics Wiesbaden Harrassowitz 1998 Print Georg Stefan Peter A Michalove Alexis Manaster Ramer and Paul J Sidwell Telling General Linguists about Altaic Journal of Linguistics 35 1 1999 65 98 Print Kalyan Siva Francois Alexandre 2018 Freeing the Comparative Method from the tree model A framework for Historical Glottometry PDF In Kikusawa Ritsuko Reid Laurie eds Let s Talk about Trees Genetic Relationships of Languages and Their Phylogenic Representation Senri Ethnological Studies Vol 98 Ōsaka National Museum of Ethnology pp 59 89 Joseph Brian 2017 The Balkan Sprachbund PDF linguisticsociety org Retrieved 2 October 2020 List Johann Mattis Nelson Sathi Shijulal Geisler Hans Martin William 2014 Networks of lexical borrowing and lateral gene transfer in language and genome evolution BioEssays 36 2 141 150 doi 10 1002 bies 201300096 ISSN 0265 9247 PMC 3910147 PMID 24375688 Languages in Contact Linguistic Society of America www linguisticsociety org Retrieved 2 October 2020 Further reading editBoas Franz 1911 Handbook of American Indian languages Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 40 Vol 1 Washington Smithsonian Institution Bureau of American Ethnology ISBN 0 8032 5017 7 Boas Franz 1922 Handbook of American Indian languages Vol 2 Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin 40 Washington D C Government Print Office Smithsonian Institution Bureau of American Ethnology Boas Franz 1933 Handbook of American Indian languages Vol 3 Native American legal materials collection title 1227 Gluckstadt J J Augustin Campbell Lyle 1997 American Indian languages The historical linguistics of Native America New York Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 509427 1 Campbell Lyle amp Mithun Marianne Eds 1979 The languages of native America Historical and comparative assessment Austin University of Texas Press Goddard Ives Ed 1996 Languages Handbook of North American Indians W C Sturtevant General Ed Vol 17 Washington D C Smithsonian Institution ISBN 0 16 048774 9 Goddard Ives 1999 Native languages and language families of North America rev and enlarged ed with additions and corrections Map Lincoln NE University of Nebraska Press Smithsonian Institution Updated version of the map in Goddard 1996 ISBN 0 8032 9271 6 Gordon Raymond G Jr Ed 2005 Ethnologue Languages of the world 15th ed Dallas TX SIL International ISBN 1 55671 159 X Online version Ethnologue Languages of the World Greenberg Joseph H 1966 The Languages of Africa 2nd ed Bloomington Indiana University Harrison K David 2007 When Languages Die The Extinction of the World s Languages and the Erosion of Human Knowledge New York and London Oxford University Press Mithun Marianne 1999 The languages of Native North America Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 23228 7 hbk ISBN 0 521 29875 X Ross Malcolm 2005 Pronouns as a preliminary diagnostic for grouping Papuan languages Archived 8 June 2004 at the Wayback Machine In Andrew Pawley Robert Attenborough Robin Hide and Jack Golson eds Papuan pasts cultural linguistic and biological histories of Papuan speaking peoples PDF Ruhlen Merritt 1987 A guide to the world s languages Stanford Stanford University Press Sturtevant William C Ed 1978 present Handbook of North American Indians Vol 1 20 Washington D C Smithsonian Institution Vols 1 3 16 18 20 not yet published Voegelin C F amp Voegelin F M 1977 Classification and index of the world s languages New York Elsevier External links editLinguistic maps from Muturzikin Ethnologue The Multitree Project Lenguas del mundo World Languages Comparative Swadesh list tables of various language families from Wiktionary Most similar languages Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Language family amp oldid 1202866117, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.