fbpx
Wikipedia

Cognitive science

Cognitive science is the interdisciplinary, scientific study of the mind and its processes with input from linguistics, psychology, neuroscience, philosophy, computer science/artificial intelligence, and anthropology.[2] It examines the nature, the tasks, and the functions of cognition (in a broad sense). Cognitive scientists study intelligence and behavior, with a focus on how nervous systems represent, process, and transform information. Mental faculties of concern to cognitive scientists include language, perception, memory, attention, reasoning, and emotion; to understand these faculties, cognitive scientists borrow from fields such as linguistics, psychology, artificial intelligence, philosophy, neuroscience, and anthropology.[3] The typical analysis of cognitive science spans many levels of organization, from learning and decision to logic and planning; from neural circuitry to modular brain organization. One of the fundamental concepts of cognitive science is that "thinking can best be understood in terms of representational structures in the mind and computational procedures that operate on those structures."[3]

Figure illustrating the fields that contributed to the birth of cognitive science, including linguistics, neuroscience, artificial intelligence, anthropology, and psychology[1]

The goal of cognitive science is to understand and formulate the principles of intelligence with the hope that this will lead to a better comprehension of the mind and of learning. The cognitive sciences began as an intellectual movement in the 1950s often referred to as the cognitive revolution.[4]

History Edit

The cognitive sciences began as an intellectual movement in the 1950s, called the cognitive revolution. Cognitive science has a prehistory traceable back to ancient Greek philosophical texts (see Plato's Meno and Aristotle's De Anima); Modern philosophers such as Descartes, David Hume, Immanuel Kant, Benedict de Spinoza, Nicolas Malebranche, Pierre Cabanis, Leibniz and John Locke, rejected scholasticism while mostly having never read Aristotle, and they were working with an entirely different set of tools and core concepts than those of the cognitive scientist.

The modern culture of cognitive science can be traced back to the early cyberneticists in the 1930s and 1940s, such as Warren McCulloch and Walter Pitts, who sought to understand the organizing principles of the mind. McCulloch and Pitts developed the first variants of what are now known as artificial neural networks, models of computation inspired by the structure of biological neural networks.

Another precursor was the early development of the theory of computation and the digital computer in the 1940s and 1950s. Kurt Gödel, Alonzo Church, Alan Turing, and John von Neumann were instrumental in these developments. The modern computer, or Von Neumann machine, would play a central role in cognitive science, both as a metaphor for the mind, and as a tool for investigation.

The first instance of cognitive science experiments being done at an academic institution took place at MIT Sloan School of Management, established by J.C.R. Licklider working within the psychology department and conducting experiments using computer memory as models for human cognition.[5]

In 1959, Noam Chomsky published a scathing review of B. F. Skinner's book Verbal Behavior.[6] At the time, Skinner's behaviorist paradigm dominated the field of psychology within the United States. Most psychologists focused on functional relations between stimulus and response, without positing internal representations. Chomsky argued that in order to explain language, we needed a theory like generative grammar, which not only attributed internal representations but characterized their underlying order.

The term cognitive science was coined by Christopher Longuet-Higgins in his 1973 commentary on the Lighthill report, which concerned the then-current state of artificial intelligence research.[7] In the same decade, the journal Cognitive Science and the Cognitive Science Society were founded.[8] The founding meeting of the Cognitive Science Society was held at the University of California, San Diego in 1979, which resulted in cognitive science becoming an internationally visible enterprise.[9] In 1972, Hampshire College started the first undergraduate education program in Cognitive Science, led by Neil Stillings. In 1982, with assistance from Professor Stillings, Vassar College became the first institution in the world to grant an undergraduate degree in Cognitive Science.[10] In 1986, the first Cognitive Science Department in the world was founded at the University of California, San Diego.[9]

In the 1970s and early 1980s, as access to computers increased, artificial intelligence research expanded. Researchers such as Marvin Minsky would write computer programs in languages such as LISP to attempt to formally characterize the steps that human beings went through, for instance, in making decisions and solving problems, in the hope of better understanding human thought, and also in the hope of creating artificial minds. This approach is known as "symbolic AI".

Eventually the limits of the symbolic AI research program became apparent. For instance, it seemed to be unrealistic to comprehensively list human knowledge in a form usable by a symbolic computer program. The late 80s and 90s saw the rise of neural networks and connectionism as a research paradigm. Under this point of view, often attributed to James McClelland and David Rumelhart, the mind could be characterized as a set of complex associations, represented as a layered network. Critics argue that there are some phenomena which are better captured by symbolic models, and that connectionist models are often so complex as to have little explanatory power. Recently symbolic and connectionist models have been combined, making it possible to take advantage of both forms of explanation.[11][12] While both connectionism and symbolic approaches have proven useful for testing various hypotheses and exploring approaches to understanding aspects of cognition and lower level brain functions, neither are biologically realistic and therefore, both suffer from a lack of neuroscientific plausibility.[13][14][15][16][17][18][19] Connectionism has proven useful for exploring computationally how cognition emerges in development and occurs in the human brain, and has provided alternatives to strictly domain-specific / domain general approaches. For example, scientists such as Jeff Elman, Liz Bates, and Annette Karmiloff-Smith have posited that networks in the brain emerge from the dynamic interaction between them and environmental input.[20]

Principles Edit

Levels of analysis Edit

A central tenet of cognitive science is that a complete understanding of the mind/brain cannot be attained by studying only a single level. An example would be the problem of remembering a phone number and recalling it later. One approach to understanding this process would be to study behavior through direct observation, or naturalistic observation. A person could be presented with a phone number and be asked to recall it after some delay of time; then the accuracy of the response could be measured. Another approach to measure cognitive ability would be to study the firings of individual neurons while a person is trying to remember the phone number. Neither of these experiments on its own would fully explain how the process of remembering a phone number works. Even if the technology to map out every neuron in the brain in real-time were available and it were known when each neuron fired it would still be impossible to know how a particular firing of neurons translates into the observed behavior. Thus an understanding of how these two levels relate to each other is imperative. Francisco Varela, in The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience, argues that "the new sciences of the mind need to enlarge their horizon to encompass both lived human experience and the possibilities for transformation inherent in human experience".[21] On the classic cognitivist view, this can be provided by a functional level account of the process. Studying a particular phenomenon from multiple levels creates a better understanding of the processes that occur in the brain to give rise to a particular behavior. Marr[22] gave a famous description of three levels of analysis:

  1. The computational theory, specifying the goals of the computation;
  2. Representation and algorithms, giving a representation of the inputs and outputs and the algorithms which transform one into the other; and
  3. The hardware implementation, or how algorithm and representation may be physically realized.

Interdisciplinary nature Edit

Cognitive science is an interdisciplinary field with contributors from various fields, including psychology, neuroscience, linguistics, philosophy of mind, computer science, anthropology and biology. Cognitive scientists work collectively in hope of understanding the mind and its interactions with the surrounding world much like other sciences do. The field regards itself as compatible with the physical sciences and uses the scientific method as well as simulation or modeling, often comparing the output of models with aspects of human cognition. Similarly to the field of psychology, there is some doubt whether there is a unified cognitive science, which have led some researchers to prefer 'cognitive sciences' in plural.[23][24]

Many, but not all, who consider themselves cognitive scientists hold a functionalist view of the mind—the view that mental states and processes should be explained by their function – what they do. According to the multiple realizability account of functionalism, even non-human systems such as robots and computers can be ascribed as having cognition.

Cognitive science: the term Edit

The term "cognitive" in "cognitive science" is used for "any kind of mental operation or structure that can be studied in precise terms" (Lakoff and Johnson, 1999). This conceptualization is very broad, and should not be confused with how "cognitive" is used in some traditions of analytic philosophy, where "cognitive" has to do only with formal rules and truth-conditional semantics.

The earliest entries for the word "cognitive" in the OED take it to mean roughly "pertaining to the action or process of knowing". The first entry, from 1586, shows the word was at one time used in the context of discussions of Platonic theories of knowledge. Most in cognitive science, however, presumably do not believe their field is the study of anything as certain as the knowledge sought by Plato. [25]

Scope Edit

Cognitive science is a large field, and covers a wide array of topics on cognition. However, it should be recognized that cognitive science has not always been equally concerned with every topic that might bear relevance to the nature and operation of minds. Classical cognitivists have largely de-emphasized or avoided social and cultural factors, embodiment, emotion, consciousness, animal cognition, and comparative and evolutionary psychologies. However, with the decline of behaviorism, internal states such as affects and emotions, as well as awareness and covert attention became approachable again. For example, situated and embodied cognition theories take into account the current state of the environment as well as the role of the body in cognition. With the newfound emphasis on information processing, observable behavior was no longer the hallmark of psychological theory, but the modeling or recording of mental states.

Below are some of the main topics that cognitive science is concerned with. This is not an exhaustive list. See List of cognitive science topics for a list of various aspects of the field.

Artificial intelligence Edit

Artificial intelligence (AI) involves the study of cognitive phenomena in machines. One of the practical goals of AI is to implement aspects of human intelligence in computers. Computers are also widely used as a tool with which to study cognitive phenomena. Computational modeling uses simulations to study how human intelligence may be structured.[26] (See § Computational modeling.)

There is some debate in the field as to whether the mind is best viewed as a huge array of small but individually feeble elements (i.e. neurons), or as a collection of higher-level structures such as symbols, schemes, plans, and rules. The former view uses connectionism to study the mind, whereas the latter emphasizes symbolic artificial intelligence. One way to view the issue is whether it is possible to accurately simulate a human brain on a computer without accurately simulating the neurons that make up the human brain.

Attention Edit

Attention is the selection of important information. The human mind is bombarded with millions of stimuli and it must have a way of deciding which of this information to process. Attention is sometimes seen as a spotlight, meaning one can only shine the light on a particular set of information. Experiments that support this metaphor include the dichotic listening task (Cherry, 1957) and studies of inattentional blindness (Mack and Rock, 1998). In the dichotic listening task, subjects are bombarded with two different messages, one in each ear, and told to focus on only one of the messages. At the end of the experiment, when asked about the content of the unattended message, subjects cannot report it.

Bodily processes related to cognition Edit

Embodied cognition approaches to cognitive science emphasize the role of body and environment in cognition. This includes both neural and extra-neural bodily processes, and factors that range from affective and emotional processes,[27] to posture, motor control, proprioception, and kinaesthesis,[28] to autonomic processes that involve heartbeat[29] and respiration,[30] to the role of the enteric gut microbiome.[31] It also includes accounts of how the body engages with or is coupled to social and physical environments. 4E (embodied, embedded, extended and enactive) cognition[32][33] includes a broad range of views about brain-body-environment interaction, from causal embeddedness to stronger claims about how the mind extends to include tools and instruments, as well as the role of social interactions, action-oriented processes, and affordances. 4E theories range from those closer to classic cognitivism (so-called "weak" embodied cognition[34]) to stronger extended[35] and enactive versions that are sometimes referred to as radical embodied cognitive science.[36][37]

Knowledge and processing of language Edit

 
A well known example of a phrase structure tree. This is one way of representing human language that shows how different components are organized hierarchically.

The ability to learn and understand language is an extremely complex process. Language is acquired within the first few years of life, and all humans under normal circumstances are able to acquire language proficiently. A major driving force in the theoretical linguistic field is discovering the nature that language must have in the abstract in order to be learned in such a fashion. Some of the driving research questions in studying how the brain itself processes language include: (1) To what extent is linguistic knowledge innate or learned?, (2) Why is it more difficult for adults to acquire a second-language than it is for infants to acquire their first-language?, and (3) How are humans able to understand novel sentences?

The study of language processing ranges from the investigation of the sound patterns of speech to the meaning of words and whole sentences. Linguistics often divides language processing into orthography, phonetics, phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and pragmatics. Many aspects of language can be studied from each of these components and from their interaction.[38][better source needed]

The study of language processing in cognitive science is closely tied to the field of linguistics. Linguistics was traditionally studied as a part of the humanities, including studies of history, art and literature. In the last fifty years or so, more and more researchers have studied knowledge and use of language as a cognitive phenomenon, the main problems being how knowledge of language can be acquired and used, and what precisely it consists of.[39] Linguists have found that, while humans form sentences in ways apparently governed by very complex systems, they are remarkably unaware of the rules that govern their own speech. Thus linguists must resort to indirect methods to determine what those rules might be, if indeed rules as such exist. In any event, if speech is indeed governed by rules, they appear to be opaque to any conscious consideration.

Learning and development Edit

Learning and development are the processes by which we acquire knowledge and information over time. Infants are born with little or no knowledge (depending on how knowledge is defined), yet they rapidly acquire the ability to use language, walk, and recognize people and objects. Research in learning and development aims to explain the mechanisms by which these processes might take place.

A major question in the study of cognitive development is the extent to which certain abilities are innate or learned. This is often framed in terms of the nature and nurture debate. The nativist view emphasizes that certain features are innate to an organism and are determined by its genetic endowment. The empiricist view, on the other hand, emphasizes that certain abilities are learned from the environment. Although clearly both genetic and environmental input is needed for a child to develop normally, considerable debate remains about how genetic information might guide cognitive development. In the area of language acquisition, for example, some (such as Steven Pinker)[40] have argued that specific information containing universal grammatical rules must be contained in the genes, whereas others (such as Jeffrey Elman and colleagues in Rethinking Innateness) have argued that Pinker's claims are biologically unrealistic. They argue that genes determine the architecture of a learning system, but that specific "facts" about how grammar works can only be learned as a result of experience.

Memory Edit

Memory allows us to store information for later retrieval. Memory is often thought of as consisting of both a long-term and short-term store. Long-term memory allows us to store information over prolonged periods (days, weeks, years). We do not yet know the practical limit of long-term memory capacity. Short-term memory allows us to store information over short time scales (seconds or minutes).

Memory is also often grouped into declarative and procedural forms. Declarative memory—grouped into subsets of semantic and episodic forms of memory—refers to our memory for facts and specific knowledge, specific meanings, and specific experiences (e.g. "Are apples food?", or "What did I eat for breakfast four days ago?"). Procedural memory allows us to remember actions and motor sequences (e.g. how to ride a bicycle) and is often dubbed implicit knowledge or memory .

Cognitive scientists study memory just as psychologists do, but tend to focus more on how memory bears on cognitive processes, and the interrelationship between cognition and memory. One example of this could be, what mental processes does a person go through to retrieve a long-lost memory? Or, what differentiates between the cognitive process of recognition (seeing hints of something before remembering it, or memory in context) and recall (retrieving a memory, as in "fill-in-the-blank")?

Perception and action Edit

 
The Necker cube, an example of an optical illusion
 
An optical illusion. The square A is exactly the same shade of gray as square B. See checker shadow illusion.

Perception is the ability to take in information via the senses, and process it in some way. Vision and hearing are two dominant senses that allow us to perceive the environment. Some questions in the study of visual perception, for example, include: (1) How are we able to recognize objects?, (2) Why do we perceive a continuous visual environment, even though we only see small bits of it at any one time? One tool for studying visual perception is by looking at how people process optical illusions. The image on the right of a Necker cube is an example of a bistable percept, that is, the cube can be interpreted as being oriented in two different directions.

The study of haptic (tactile), olfactory, and gustatory stimuli also fall into the domain of perception.

Action is taken to refer to the output of a system. In humans, this is accomplished through motor responses. Spatial planning and movement, speech production, and complex motor movements are all aspects of action.

Consciousness Edit

Consciousness is the awareness of experiences within oneself. This helps the mind with having the ability to experience or feel a sense of self.

Research methods Edit

Many different methodologies are used to study cognitive science. As the field is highly interdisciplinary, research often cuts across multiple areas of study, drawing on research methods from psychology, neuroscience, computer science and systems theory.

Behavioral experiments Edit

In order to have a description of what constitutes intelligent behavior, one must study behavior itself. This type of research is closely tied to that in cognitive psychology and psychophysics. By measuring behavioral responses to different stimuli, one can understand something about how those stimuli are processed. Lewandowski & Strohmetz (2009) reviewed a collection of innovative uses of behavioral measurement in psychology including behavioral traces, behavioral observations, and behavioral choice.[41] Behavioral traces are pieces of evidence that indicate behavior occurred, but the actor is not present (e.g., litter in a parking lot or readings on an electric meter). Behavioral observations involve the direct witnessing of the actor engaging in the behavior (e.g., watching how close a person sits next to another person). Behavioral choices are when a person selects between two or more options (e.g., voting behavior, choice of a punishment for another participant).

  • Reaction time. The time between the presentation of a stimulus and an appropriate response can indicate differences between two cognitive processes, and can indicate some things about their nature. For example, if in a search task the reaction times vary proportionally with the number of elements, then it is evident that this cognitive process of searching involves serial instead of parallel processing.
  • Psychophysical responses. Psychophysical experiments are an old psychological technique, which has been adopted by cognitive psychology. They typically involve making judgments of some physical property, e.g. the loudness of a sound. Correlation of subjective scales between individuals can show cognitive or sensory biases as compared to actual physical measurements. Some examples include:
    • sameness judgments for colors, tones, textures, etc.
    • threshold differences for colors, tones, textures, etc.
  • Eye tracking. This methodology is used to study a variety of cognitive processes, most notably visual perception and language processing. The fixation point of the eyes is linked to an individual's focus of attention. Thus, by monitoring eye movements, we can study what information is being processed at a given time. Eye tracking allows us to study cognitive processes on extremely short time scales. Eye movements reflect online decision making during a task, and they provide us with some insight into the ways in which those decisions may be processed.[42]

Brain imaging Edit

 
Image of the human head with the brain. The arrow indicates the position of the hypothalamus.

Brain imaging involves analyzing activity within the brain while performing various tasks. This allows us to link behavior and brain function to help understand how information is processed. Different types of imaging techniques vary in their temporal (time-based) and spatial (location-based) resolution. Brain imaging is often used in cognitive neuroscience.

  • Single-photon emission computed tomography and positron emission tomography. SPECT and PET use radioactive isotopes, which are injected into the subject's bloodstream and taken up by the brain. By observing which areas of the brain take up the radioactive isotope, we can see which areas of the brain are more active than other areas. PET has similar spatial resolution to fMRI, but it has extremely poor temporal resolution.
  • Electroencephalography. EEG measures the electrical fields generated by large populations of neurons in the cortex by placing a series of electrodes on the scalp of the subject. This technique has an extremely high temporal resolution, but a relatively poor spatial resolution.
  • Functional magnetic resonance imaging. fMRI measures the relative amount of oxygenated blood flowing to different parts of the brain. More oxygenated blood in a particular region is assumed to correlate with an increase in neural activity in that part of the brain. This allows us to localize particular functions within different brain regions. fMRI has moderate spatial and temporal resolution.
  • Optical imaging. This technique uses infrared transmitters and receivers to measure the amount of light reflectance by blood near different areas of the brain. Since oxygenated and deoxygenated blood reflects light by different amounts, we can study which areas are more active (i.e., those that have more oxygenated blood). Optical imaging has moderate temporal resolution, but poor spatial resolution. It also has the advantage that it is extremely safe and can be used to study infants' brains.
  • Magnetoencephalography. MEG measures magnetic fields resulting from cortical activity. It is similar to EEG, except that it has improved spatial resolution since the magnetic fields it measures are not as blurred or attenuated by the scalp, meninges and so forth as the electrical activity measured in EEG is. MEG uses SQUID sensors to detect tiny magnetic fields.

Computational modeling Edit

 
An artificial neural network with two layers

Computational models require a mathematically and logically formal representation of a problem. Computer models are used in the simulation and experimental verification of different specific and general properties of intelligence. Computational modeling can help us understand the functional organization of a particular cognitive phenomenon. Approaches to cognitive modeling can be categorized as: (1) symbolic, on abstract mental functions of an intelligent mind by means of symbols; (2) subsymbolic, on the neural and associative properties of the human brain; and (3) across the symbolic–subsymbolic border, including hybrid.

  • Symbolic modeling evolved from the computer science paradigms using the technologies of knowledge-based systems, as well as a philosophical perspective (e.g. "Good Old-Fashioned Artificial Intelligence" (GOFAI)). They were developed by the first cognitive researchers and later used in information engineering for expert systems. Since the early 1990s it was generalized in systemics for the investigation of functional human-like intelligence models, such as personoids, and, in parallel, developed as the SOAR environment. Recently, especially in the context of cognitive decision-making, symbolic cognitive modeling has been extended to the socio-cognitive approach, including social and organizational cognition, interrelated with a sub-symbolic non-conscious layer.
  • Subsymbolic modeling includes connectionist/neural network models. Connectionism relies on the idea that the mind/brain is composed of simple nodes and its problem-solving capacity derives from the connections between them. Neural nets are textbook implementations of this approach. Some critics of this approach feel that while these models approach biological reality as a representation of how the system works, these models lack explanatory powers because, even in systems endowed with simple connection rules, the emerging high complexity makes them less interpretable at the connection-level than they apparently are at the macroscopic level.
  • Other approaches gaining in popularity include (1) dynamical systems theory, (2) mapping symbolic models onto connectionist models (Neural-symbolic integration or hybrid intelligent systems), and (3) and Bayesian models, which are often drawn from machine learning.

All the above approaches tend either to be generalized to the form of integrated computational models of a synthetic/abstract intelligence (i.e. cognitive architecture) in order to be applied to the explanation and improvement of individual and social/organizational decision-making and reasoning[43][44] or to focus on single simulative programs (or microtheories/"middle-range" theories) modelling specific cognitive faculties (e.g. vision, language, categorization etc.).

Neurobiological methods Edit

Research methods borrowed directly from neuroscience and neuropsychology can also help us to understand aspects of intelligence. These methods allow us to understand how intelligent behavior is implemented in a physical system.

Key findings Edit

Cognitive science has given rise to models of human cognitive bias and risk perception, and has been influential in the development of behavioral finance, part of economics. It has also given rise to a new theory of the philosophy of mathematics (related to denotational mathematics), and many theories of artificial intelligence, persuasion and coercion. It has made its presence known in the philosophy of language and epistemology as well as constituting a substantial wing of modern linguistics. Fields of cognitive science have been influential in understanding the brain's particular functional systems (and functional deficits) ranging from speech production to auditory processing and visual perception. It has made progress in understanding how damage to particular areas of the brain affect cognition, and it has helped to uncover the root causes and results of specific dysfunction, such as dyslexia, anopia, and hemispatial neglect.

Notable researchers Edit

Name Year of birth Year of contribution Contribution(s)
David Chalmers 1966[45] 1995[46] Dualism, hard problem of consciousness
Daniel Dennett 1942[47] 1987 Offered a computational systems perspective (Multiple drafts model)
John Searle 1932[48] 1980 Chinese room
Douglas Hofstadter 1945 1979[49] Gödel, Escher, Bach[50]
Jerry Fodor 1935[51] 1968, 1975 Functionalism
Alan Baddeley 1934[52] 1974 Baddeley's model of working memory
Marvin Minsky 1927[53] 1970s, early 1980s Wrote computer programs in languages such as LISP to attempt to formally characterize the steps that human beings go through, such as making decisions and solving problems
Christopher Longuet-Higgins 1923[54] 1973 Coined the term cognitive science
Noam Chomsky 1928[55] 1959 Published a review of B.F. Skinner's book Verbal Behavior which began cognitivism against then-dominant behaviorism[6]
George Miller 1920 1956 Wrote about the capacities of human thinking through mental representations
Herbert Simon 1916 1956 Co-created Logic Theory Machine and General Problem Solver with Allen Newell, EPAM (Elementary Perceiver and Memorizer) theory, organizational decision-making
John McCarthy 1927 1955 Coined the term artificial intelligence and organized the famous Dartmouth conference in Summer 1956, which started AI as a field
McCulloch and Pitts 1930s–1940s Developed early artificial neural networks
J. C. R. Licklider 1915[56] Established MIT Sloan School of Management
Lila R. Gleitman 1929 1970s-2010s Wide-ranging contributions to understanding the cognition of language acquisition, including syntactic bootstrapping theory[57]
Eleanor Rosch 1938 1976 Development of the Prototype Theory of categorisation[58]
Philip N. Johnson-Laird 1936 1980 Introduced the idea of mental models in cognitive science[59]
Dedre Gentner 1944 1983 Development of the Structure-mapping Theory of analogical reasoning[60]
Allen Newell 1927 1990 Development of the field of Cognitive architecture in cognitive modelling and artificial intelligence[61]
Annette Karmiloff-Smith 1938 1992 Integrating neuroscience and computational modelling into theories of cognitive development[62]
David Marr (neuroscientist) 1945 1990 Proponent of the Three-Level Hypothesis of levels of analysis of computational systems[63]
Peter Gärdenfors 1949 2000 Creator of the conceptual space framework used in cognitive modelling and artificial intelligence.
Linda B. Smith 1951 1993 Together with Esther Thelen, created a dynamical systems approach to understanding cognitive development[64]

Some of the more recognized names in cognitive science are usually either the most controversial or the most cited. Within philosophy, some familiar names include Daniel Dennett, who writes from a computational systems perspective,[65] John Searle, known for his controversial Chinese room argument,[66] and Jerry Fodor, who advocates functionalism.[67]

Others include David Chalmers, who advocates Dualism and is also known for articulating the hard problem of consciousness, and Douglas Hofstadter, famous for writing Gödel, Escher, Bach, which questions the nature of words and thought.

In the realm of linguistics, Noam Chomsky and George Lakoff have been influential (both have also become notable as political commentators). In artificial intelligence, Marvin Minsky, Herbert A. Simon, and Allen Newell are prominent.

Popular names in the discipline of psychology include George A. Miller, James McClelland, Philip Johnson-Laird, Lawrence Barsalou, Vittorio Guidano, Howard Gardner and Steven Pinker. Anthropologists Dan Sperber, Edwin Hutchins, Bradd Shore, James Wertsch and Scott Atran, have been involved in collaborative projects with cognitive and social psychologists, political scientists and evolutionary biologists in attempts to develop general theories of culture formation, religion, and political association.

Computational theories (with models and simulations) have also been developed, by David Rumelhart, James McClelland and Philip Johnson-Laird.

Epistemics Edit

Epistemics is a term coined in 1969 by the University of Edinburgh with the foundation of its School of Epistemics. Epistemics is to be distinguished from epistemology in that epistemology is the philosophical theory of knowledge, whereas epistemics signifies the scientific study of knowledge.

Christopher Longuet-Higgins has defined it as "the construction of formal models of the processes (perceptual, intellectual, and linguistic) by which knowledge and understanding are achieved and communicated."[68] In his 1978 essay "Epistemics: The Regulative Theory of Cognition",[69] Alvin I. Goldman claims to have coined the term "epistemics" to describe a reorientation of epistemology. Goldman maintains that his epistemics is continuous with traditional epistemology and the new term is only to avoid opposition. Epistemics, in Goldman's version, differs only slightly from traditional epistemology in its alliance with the psychology of cognition; epistemics stresses the detailed study of mental processes and information-processing mechanisms that lead to knowledge or beliefs.

In the mid-1980s, the School of Epistemics was renamed as The Centre for Cognitive Science (CCS). In 1998, CCS was incorporated into the University of Edinburgh's School of Informatics.[70]

Binding problem in cognitive science Edit

One of the core aims of cognitive science is to achieve an integrated theory of cognition. This requires integrative mechanisms explaining how the information processing that occurs simultaneously in spatially segregated (sub-)cortical areas in the brain is coordinated and bound together to give rise to coherent perceptual and symbolic representations. One approach is to solve this "Binding problem"[71][72][73] (that is, the problem of dynamically representing conjunctions of informational elements, from the most basic perceptual representations ("feature binding") to the most complex cognitive representations, like symbol structures ("variable binding")), by means of integrative synchronization mechanisms. In other words, one of the coordinating mechanisms appears to be the temporal (phase) synchronization of neural activity based on dynamical self-organizing processes in neural networks, described by the Binding-by-synchrony (BBS) Hypothesis from neurophysiology.[74][75][76][77] Connectionist cognitive neuroarchitectures have been developed that use integrative synchronization mechanisms to solve this binding problem in perceptual cognition and in language cognition.[78][79][80] In perceptual cognition the problem is to explain how elementary object properties and object relations, like the object color or the object form, can be dynamically bound together or can be integrated to a representation of this perceptual object by means of a synchronization mechanism ("feature binding", "feature linking"). In language cognition the problem is to explain how semantic concepts and syntactic roles can be dynamically bound together or can be integrated to complex cognitive representations like systematic and compositional symbol structures and propositions by means of a synchronization mechanism ("variable binding") (see also the "Symbolism vs. connectionism debate" in connectionism).

See also Edit

Outlines
  • Outline of human intelligence – topic tree presenting the traits, capacities, models, and research fields of human intelligence, and more.
  • Outline of thought – topic tree that identifies many types of thoughts, types of thinking, aspects of thought, related fields, and more.

References Edit

  1. ^ Miller, George A (1 March 2003). "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (3): 141–144. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9. ISSN 1364-6613. PMID 12639696. S2CID 206129621. from the original on 21 November 2018. Retrieved 5 February 2023.
  2. ^ "Ask the Cognitive Scientist". American Federation of Teachers. 8 August 2014. from the original on 17 September 2014. Retrieved 25 December 2013. Cognitive science is an interdisciplinary field of researchers from Linguistics, psychology, neuroscience, philosophy, computer science, and anthropology that seek to understand the mind.
  3. ^ a b Thagard, Paul, Cognitive Science 15 July 2018 at the Wayback Machine, The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2008 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.).
  4. ^ Miller, George A. (2003). "The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (3): 141–144. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9. PMID 12639696. S2CID 206129621.
  5. ^ Hafner, K.; Lyon, M. (1996). Where wizards stay up late: The origins of the Internet. New York: Simon & Schuster. p. 32. ISBN 0-684-81201-0.
  6. ^ a b Chomsky, Noam (1959). "Review of Verbal behavior". Language. 35 (1): 26–58. doi:10.2307/411334. ISSN 0097-8507. JSTOR 411334.
  7. ^ Longuet-Higgins, H. C. (1973). "Comments on the Lighthill Report and the Sutherland Reply". Artificial Intelligence: a paper symposium. Science Research Council. pp. 35–37. ISBN 0-901660-18-3.
  8. ^ Cognitive Science Society 17 July 2010 at the Wayback Machine
  9. ^ a b . Archived from the original on 9 July 2015. Retrieved 8 July 2015.
  10. ^ Box 729. "About - Cognitive Science - Vassar College". Cogsci.vassar.edu. from the original on 17 September 2012. Retrieved 15 August 2012.
  11. ^ d'Avila Garcez, Artur S.; Lamb, Luis C.; Gabbay, Dov M. (2008). Neural-Symbolic Cognitive Reasoning. Cognitive Technologies. Springer. ISBN 978-3-540-73245-7.
  12. ^ Sun, Ron; Bookman, Larry, eds. (1994). Computational Architectures Integrating Neural and Symbolic Processes. Needham, MA: Kluwer Academic. ISBN 0-7923-9517-4.
  13. ^ "Encephalos Journal". www.encephalos.gr. from the original on 25 June 2011. Retrieved 20 February 2018.
  14. ^ Wilson, Elizabeth A. (4 February 2016). Neural Geographies: Feminism and the Microstructure of Cognition. Routledge. ISBN 9781317958765. from the original on 5 February 2023. Retrieved 16 April 2018.
  15. ^ Di Paolo, Ezequiel A. (2003). "Organismically-inspired robotics: homeostatic adaptation and teleology beyond the closed sensorimotor loop". In Murase, Kazuyuki; Asakura, Toshiyuki (eds.). Dynamic Systems Approach for Embodiment and Sociality: From Ecological Psychology to Robotics. Advanced Knowledge International. pp. 19–42. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.62.4813. ISBN 978-0-9751004-1-7. S2CID 15349751.
  16. ^ Zorzi, Marco; Testolin, Alberto; Stoianov, Ivilin P. (20 August 2013). "Modeling language and cognition with deep unsupervised learning: a tutorial overview". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 515. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00515. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 3747356. PMID 23970869.
  17. ^ Tieszen, Richard (15 July 2011). "Analytic and Continental Philosophy, Science, and Global Philosophy". Comparative Philosophy. 2 (2). doi:10.31979/2151-6014(2011).020206.
  18. ^ Browne, A. (1997). Neural Network Perspectives on Cognition and Adaptive Robotics. CRC Press. ISBN 0-7503-0455-3. from the original on 5 February 2023. Retrieved 16 April 2018.
  19. ^ Pfeifer, R.; Schreter, Z.; Fogelman-Soulié, F.; Steels, L. (1989). Connectionism in Perspective. Elsevier. ISBN 0-444-59876-6. from the original on 5 February 2023. Retrieved 16 April 2018.
  20. ^ Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2015). "An alternative to domain-general or domain-specific frameworks for theorizing about human evolution and ontogenesis". AIMS Neuroscience. 2 (2): 91–104. doi:10.3934/Neuroscience.2015.2.91. PMC 4678597. PMID 26682283.
  21. ^ Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press.
  22. ^ Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information. W. H. Freeman.
  23. ^ Miller, G. A. (2003). "The cognitive revolution: a historical perspective". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (3): 141–144. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00029-9. PMID 12639696. S2CID 206129621.
  24. ^ Ferrés, Joan; Masanet, Maria-Jose (2017). "Communication Efficiency in Education: Increasing Emotions and Storytelling". Comunicar (in Spanish). 25 (52): 51–60. doi:10.3916/c52-2017-05. ISSN 1134-3478.
  25. ^ Shields, Christopher (2020), "Aristotle's Psychology", in Zalta, Edward N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2020 ed.), Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, from the original on 20 October 2022, retrieved 20 October 2022
  26. ^ Sun, Ron (ed.) (2008). The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology. Cambridge University Press, New York.
  27. ^ Colombetti, G. and, Krueger, J. (2015). "Scaffoldings of the affective mind". Philosophical Psychology. 28 (8): 1157–1176. doi:10.1080/09515089.2014.976334. hdl:10871/15680. S2CID 73617860.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  28. ^ Gallagher, Shaun (2005). How the Body Shapes the Mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  29. ^ Garfinkel, S. N.; Barrett, A. B.; Minati, L.; Dolan, R. J.; Seth, A. K.; Critchley, H. D. (2013). "What the heart forgets: Cardiac timing influences memory for words and is modulated by metacognition and interoceptive sensitivity". Psychophysiology. 50 (6): 505–512. doi:10.1111/psyp.12039. PMC 4340570. PMID 23521494.
  30. ^ Varga, S.; Heck, D. H. (2017). "Rhythms of the body, rhythms of the brain: respiration, neural oscillations, and embodied cognition". Consciousness and Cognition. 56: 77–90. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2017.09.008. PMID 29073509. S2CID 8448790.
  31. ^ Davidson, G. L.; Cooke, A. C.; Johnson, C. N.; Quinn, J. L. (2018). "The gut microbiome as a driver of individual variation in cognition and functional behaviour". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 373 (1756): 20170286. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0286. PMC 6107574. PMID 30104431.
  32. ^ Newen, A.; De Bruin, L.; Gallagher, S., eds. (2018). The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  33. ^ Menary, Richard (2010). "Special Issue on 4E Cognition". Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences. 9 (4). doi:10.1007/s11097-010-9187-6. S2CID 143621939.
  34. ^ Alsmith, A. J. T.; De Vignemont, F. (2012). "Embodying the mind and representing the body" (PDF). Review of Philosophy and Psychology. 3 (1): 1–13. doi:10.1007/s13164-012-0085-4. S2CID 5823387.
  35. ^ Clark, Andy (2011). "Précis of Supersizing the mind: embodiment, action, and cognitive extension". Philosophical Studies. 152 (3): 413–416. doi:10.1007/s11098-010-9597-x. S2CID 170708745.
  36. ^ Chemero, Anthony (2011). Radical embodied cognitive science. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  37. ^ Hutto, Daniel D. & Erik Myin. (2012). Radicalizing enactivism: Basic minds without content. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  38. ^ "Linguistics: Semantics, Phonetics, Pragmatics, and Human Communication". Decoded Science. 16 February 2014. from the original on 6 June 2017. Retrieved 7 February 2018.
  39. ^ Isac, Daniela; Charles Reiss (2013). . Oxford University Press. p. 5. ISBN 978-0199660179. Archived from the original on 6 July 2011. Retrieved 29 July 2008.
  40. ^ Pinker S., Bloom P. (1990). "Natural language and natural selection". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 13 (4): 707–784. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.116.4044. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00081061. S2CID 6167614.
  41. ^ Lewandowski, Gary; Strohmetz, David (2009). "Actions can speak as loud as words: Measuring behavior in psychological science". Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 3 (6): 992–1002. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00229.x.
  42. ^ König, Peter; Wilming, Niklas; Kietzmann, Tim C.; Ossandón, Jose P.; Onat, Selim; Ehinger, Benedikt V.; Gameiro, Ricardo R.; Kaspar, Kai (1 December 2016). "Eye movements as a window to cognitive processes". Journal of Eye Movement Research. 9 (5). doi:10.16910/jemr.9.5.3.
  43. ^ Lieto, Antonio (2021). Cognitive Design for Artificial Minds. London, UK: Routledge, Taylor & Francis. ISBN 9781138207929.
  44. ^ Sun, Ron (ed.), Grounding Social Sciences in Cognitive Sciences. MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. 2012.
  45. ^ "David Chalmers". www.informationphilosopher.com. from the original on 24 April 2017. Retrieved 24 April 2017.
  46. ^ "Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness". consc.net. from the original on 8 April 2011. Retrieved 24 April 2017.
  47. ^ "Daniel C. Dennett | American philosopher". Encyclopædia Britannica. from the original on 23 July 2016. Retrieved 3 May 2017.
  48. ^ "John Searle". www.informationphilosopher.com. from the original on 24 April 2017. Retrieved 3 May 2017.
  49. ^ "Gödel, Escher, Bach". Goodreads. from the original on 22 May 2017. Retrieved 3 May 2017.
  50. ^ Somers, James. "The Man Who Would Teach Machines to Think". The Atlantic. from the original on 17 September 2016. Retrieved 3 May 2017.
  51. ^ "Fodor, Jerry | Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy". www.iep.utm.edu. from the original on 17 October 2015. Retrieved 3 May 2017.
  52. ^ "Alan Baddeley in International Directory of Psychologists". 1966. from the original on 5 February 2023. Retrieved 10 March 2022.
  53. ^ "Marvin Minsky | American scientist". Encyclopædia Britannica. from the original on 28 March 2017. Retrieved 27 March 2017.
  54. ^ Darwin, Chris (9 June 2004). "Christopher Longuet-Higgins". The Guardian. ISSN 0261-3077. from the original on 21 December 2016. Retrieved 27 March 2017.
  55. ^ "Noam Chomsky". chomsky.info. from the original on 25 April 2017. Retrieved 24 April 2017.
  56. ^ "J.C.R. Licklider | Internet Hall of Fame". internethalloffame.org. from the original on 28 March 2017. Retrieved 24 April 2017.
  57. ^ Gleitman, Lila (January 1990). "The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings". Language Acquisition. 1 (1): 3–55. doi:10.1207/s15327817la0101_2. S2CID 144713838.
  58. ^ Rosch, Eleanor; Mervis, Carolyn B; Gray, Wayne D; Johnson, David M; Boyes-Braem, Penny (July 1976). "Basic objects in natural categories". Cognitive Psychology. 8 (3): 382–439. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.149.3392. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(76)90013-X. S2CID 5612467.
  59. ^ Johnsonlaird, P (March 1981). "Mental models in cognitive science". Cognitive Science. 4 (1): 71–115. doi:10.1016/S0364-0213(81)80005-5.
  60. ^ Gentner, Dedre (April 1983). "Structure-Mapping: A Theoretical Framework for Analogy". Cognitive Science. 7 (2): 155–170. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0702_3. S2CID 5371492.
  61. ^ Newell, Allen. 1990. Unified Theories of Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.[page needed]
  62. ^ Karmiloff-Smith, Annette (1992). Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. MIT Press. ISBN 9780262111690.[page needed]
  63. ^ Marr, D.; Poggio, T. (1976). From Understanding Computation to Understanding Neural Circuitry. Artificial Intelligence Laboratory (Report). A.I. Memo. Massachusetts Institute of Technology. hdl:1721.1/5782. AIM-357.
  64. ^ Smith, Linda B; Thelen, Esther (1993). A Dynamic systems approach to development: applications. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-585-03867-4. OCLC 42854628.[page needed]
  65. ^ Rescorla, Michael (1 January 2017). Zalta, Edward N. (ed.). The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Spring 2017 ed.). Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University. from the original on 18 March 2019. Retrieved 27 March 2017.
  66. ^ Hauser, Larry. "Chinese Room Argument". Internet Enclclopedia of Philosophy. from the original on 27 April 2016. Retrieved 27 March 2017.
  67. ^ "Fodor, Jerry | Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy". www.iep.utm.edu. from the original on 17 October 2015. Retrieved 27 March 2017.
  68. ^ Longuet-Higgins, Christopher (1977) [1969], "Epistemics", in A. Bullock & O. Stallybrass (ed.), Fontana dictionary of modern thought, London, UK: Fontana, p. 209, ISBN 9780002161497
  69. ^ Goldman, Alvin I. (1978). "Epistemics: The Regulative Theory of Cognition". The Journal of Philosophy. 75 (10): 509–23. doi:10.2307/2025838. JSTOR 2025838.
  70. ^ "The old WWW.cogsci.ed.ac.uk server". from the original on 19 October 2010. Retrieved 27 August 2019.
  71. ^ Hardcastle, V.G. (1998). "The binding problem". In W. Bechtel & G. Graham (eds.), A Companion to Cognitive Science (pp. 555-565). Blackwell Publisher, Malden/MA, Oxford/UK.
  72. ^ Hummel, J. (1999). "Binding problem". In: R.A. Wilson & F.C. Keil, The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences (pp. 85-86). Cambridge, MA, London: The MIT Press.
  73. ^ Malsburg, C. von der (1999). "The what and why of binding: The modeler's perspective". Neuron. 24: 95-104.
  74. ^ Gray, C. M. & Singer, W. (1989). "Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 86: 1698-1702.
  75. ^ Singer, W. (1999b). "Neuronal synchrony: A versatile code for the definition of relations". Neuron. 24: 49-65.
  76. ^ Singer, W. & A. Lazar. (2016). "Does the cerebral cortex exploit high-dimensional, non-linear dynamics for information processing?" Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience.10: 99.
  77. ^ Singer, W. (2018). "Neuronal oscillations: unavoidable and useful?" European Journal of Neuroscience. 48: 2389-2399.
  78. ^ Maurer, H. (2021). "Cognitive science: Integrative synchronization mechanisms in cognitive neuroarchitectures of the modern connectionism". CRC Press, Boca Raton/FL, ISBN 978-1-351-04352-6. https://doi.org/10.1201/9781351043526 5 February 2023 at the Wayback Machine
  79. ^ Maurer, H. (2016). "Integrative synchronization mechanisms in connectionist cognitive Neuroarchitectures". Computational Cognitive Science. 2: 3. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40469-016-0010-8 5 February 2023 at the Wayback Machine
  80. ^ Marcus, G.F. (2001). "The algebraic mind. Integrating connectionism and cognitive science". Bradford Book, The MIT Press, Cambridge, ISBN 0-262-13379-2.

External links Edit

  •   Media related to Cognitive science at Wikimedia Commons
  •   Quotations related to Cognitive science at Wikiquote
  •   Learning materials related to Cognitive science at Wikiversity
  • "Cognitive Science" on the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy
  • Cognitive Science Society
  • Cognitive Science Movie Index: A broad list of movies showcasing themes in the Cognitive Sciences 4 September 2015 at the Wayback Machine

cognitive, science, journal, cognitive, science, journal, interdisciplinary, scientific, study, mind, processes, with, input, from, linguistics, psychology, neuroscience, philosophy, computer, science, artificial, intelligence, anthropology, examines, nature, . For the journal see Cognitive Science journal Cognitive science is the interdisciplinary scientific study of the mind and its processes with input from linguistics psychology neuroscience philosophy computer science artificial intelligence and anthropology 2 It examines the nature the tasks and the functions of cognition in a broad sense Cognitive scientists study intelligence and behavior with a focus on how nervous systems represent process and transform information Mental faculties of concern to cognitive scientists include language perception memory attention reasoning and emotion to understand these faculties cognitive scientists borrow from fields such as linguistics psychology artificial intelligence philosophy neuroscience and anthropology 3 The typical analysis of cognitive science spans many levels of organization from learning and decision to logic and planning from neural circuitry to modular brain organization One of the fundamental concepts of cognitive science is that thinking can best be understood in terms of representational structures in the mind and computational procedures that operate on those structures 3 Figure illustrating the fields that contributed to the birth of cognitive science including linguistics neuroscience artificial intelligence anthropology and psychology 1 The goal of cognitive science is to understand and formulate the principles of intelligence with the hope that this will lead to a better comprehension of the mind and of learning The cognitive sciences began as an intellectual movement in the 1950s often referred to as the cognitive revolution 4 Contents 1 History 2 Principles 2 1 Levels of analysis 2 2 Interdisciplinary nature 2 3 Cognitive science the term 3 Scope 3 1 Artificial intelligence 3 2 Attention 3 3 Bodily processes related to cognition 3 4 Knowledge and processing of language 3 5 Learning and development 3 6 Memory 3 7 Perception and action 3 8 Consciousness 4 Research methods 4 1 Behavioral experiments 4 2 Brain imaging 4 3 Computational modeling 4 4 Neurobiological methods 5 Key findings 6 Notable researchers 7 Epistemics 8 Binding problem in cognitive science 9 See also 10 References 11 External linksHistory EditThe cognitive sciences began as an intellectual movement in the 1950s called the cognitive revolution Cognitive science has a prehistory traceable back to ancient Greek philosophical texts see Plato s Meno and Aristotle s De Anima Modern philosophers such as Descartes David Hume Immanuel Kant Benedict de Spinoza Nicolas Malebranche Pierre Cabanis Leibniz and John Locke rejected scholasticism while mostly having never read Aristotle and they were working with an entirely different set of tools and core concepts than those of the cognitive scientist The modern culture of cognitive science can be traced back to the early cyberneticists in the 1930s and 1940s such as Warren McCulloch and Walter Pitts who sought to understand the organizing principles of the mind McCulloch and Pitts developed the first variants of what are now known as artificial neural networks models of computation inspired by the structure of biological neural networks Another precursor was the early development of the theory of computation and the digital computer in the 1940s and 1950s Kurt Godel Alonzo Church Alan Turing and John von Neumann were instrumental in these developments The modern computer or Von Neumann machine would play a central role in cognitive science both as a metaphor for the mind and as a tool for investigation The first instance of cognitive science experiments being done at an academic institution took place at MIT Sloan School of Management established by J C R Licklider working within the psychology department and conducting experiments using computer memory as models for human cognition 5 In 1959 Noam Chomsky published a scathing review of B F Skinner s book Verbal Behavior 6 At the time Skinner s behaviorist paradigm dominated the field of psychology within the United States Most psychologists focused on functional relations between stimulus and response without positing internal representations Chomsky argued that in order to explain language we needed a theory like generative grammar which not only attributed internal representations but characterized their underlying order The term cognitive science was coined by Christopher Longuet Higgins in his 1973 commentary on the Lighthill report which concerned the then current state of artificial intelligence research 7 In the same decade the journal Cognitive Science and the Cognitive Science Society were founded 8 The founding meeting of the Cognitive Science Society was held at the University of California San Diego in 1979 which resulted in cognitive science becoming an internationally visible enterprise 9 In 1972 Hampshire College started the first undergraduate education program in Cognitive Science led by Neil Stillings In 1982 with assistance from Professor Stillings Vassar College became the first institution in the world to grant an undergraduate degree in Cognitive Science 10 In 1986 the first Cognitive Science Department in the world was founded at the University of California San Diego 9 In the 1970s and early 1980s as access to computers increased artificial intelligence research expanded Researchers such as Marvin Minsky would write computer programs in languages such as LISP to attempt to formally characterize the steps that human beings went through for instance in making decisions and solving problems in the hope of better understanding human thought and also in the hope of creating artificial minds This approach is known as symbolic AI Eventually the limits of the symbolic AI research program became apparent For instance it seemed to be unrealistic to comprehensively list human knowledge in a form usable by a symbolic computer program The late 80s and 90s saw the rise of neural networks and connectionism as a research paradigm Under this point of view often attributed to James McClelland and David Rumelhart the mind could be characterized as a set of complex associations represented as a layered network Critics argue that there are some phenomena which are better captured by symbolic models and that connectionist models are often so complex as to have little explanatory power Recently symbolic and connectionist models have been combined making it possible to take advantage of both forms of explanation 11 12 While both connectionism and symbolic approaches have proven useful for testing various hypotheses and exploring approaches to understanding aspects of cognition and lower level brain functions neither are biologically realistic and therefore both suffer from a lack of neuroscientific plausibility 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 Connectionism has proven useful for exploring computationally how cognition emerges in development and occurs in the human brain and has provided alternatives to strictly domain specific domain general approaches For example scientists such as Jeff Elman Liz Bates and Annette Karmiloff Smith have posited that networks in the brain emerge from the dynamic interaction between them and environmental input 20 Principles EditLevels of analysis Edit See also Functionalism philosophy of mind and functionalism A central tenet of cognitive science is that a complete understanding of the mind brain cannot be attained by studying only a single level An example would be the problem of remembering a phone number and recalling it later One approach to understanding this process would be to study behavior through direct observation or naturalistic observation A person could be presented with a phone number and be asked to recall it after some delay of time then the accuracy of the response could be measured Another approach to measure cognitive ability would be to study the firings of individual neurons while a person is trying to remember the phone number Neither of these experiments on its own would fully explain how the process of remembering a phone number works Even if the technology to map out every neuron in the brain in real time were available and it were known when each neuron fired it would still be impossible to know how a particular firing of neurons translates into the observed behavior Thus an understanding of how these two levels relate to each other is imperative Francisco Varela in The Embodied Mind Cognitive Science and Human Experience argues that the new sciences of the mind need to enlarge their horizon to encompass both lived human experience and the possibilities for transformation inherent in human experience 21 On the classic cognitivist view this can be provided by a functional level account of the process Studying a particular phenomenon from multiple levels creates a better understanding of the processes that occur in the brain to give rise to a particular behavior Marr 22 gave a famous description of three levels of analysis The computational theory specifying the goals of the computation Representation and algorithms giving a representation of the inputs and outputs and the algorithms which transform one into the other and The hardware implementation or how algorithm and representation may be physically realized Interdisciplinary nature Edit Cognitive science is an interdisciplinary field with contributors from various fields including psychology neuroscience linguistics philosophy of mind computer science anthropology and biology Cognitive scientists work collectively in hope of understanding the mind and its interactions with the surrounding world much like other sciences do The field regards itself as compatible with the physical sciences and uses the scientific method as well as simulation or modeling often comparing the output of models with aspects of human cognition Similarly to the field of psychology there is some doubt whether there is a unified cognitive science which have led some researchers to prefer cognitive sciences in plural 23 24 Many but not all who consider themselves cognitive scientists hold a functionalist view of the mind the view that mental states and processes should be explained by their function what they do According to the multiple realizability account of functionalism even non human systems such as robots and computers can be ascribed as having cognition Cognitive science the term Edit The term cognitive in cognitive science is used for any kind of mental operation or structure that can be studied in precise terms Lakoff and Johnson 1999 This conceptualization is very broad and should not be confused with how cognitive is used in some traditions of analytic philosophy where cognitive has to do only with formal rules and truth conditional semantics The earliest entries for the word cognitive in the OED take it to mean roughly pertaining to the action or process of knowing The first entry from 1586 shows the word was at one time used in the context of discussions of Platonic theories of knowledge Most in cognitive science however presumably do not believe their field is the study of anything as certain as the knowledge sought by Plato 25 Scope EditCognitive science is a large field and covers a wide array of topics on cognition However it should be recognized that cognitive science has not always been equally concerned with every topic that might bear relevance to the nature and operation of minds Classical cognitivists have largely de emphasized or avoided social and cultural factors embodiment emotion consciousness animal cognition and comparative and evolutionary psychologies However with the decline of behaviorism internal states such as affects and emotions as well as awareness and covert attention became approachable again For example situated and embodied cognition theories take into account the current state of the environment as well as the role of the body in cognition With the newfound emphasis on information processing observable behavior was no longer the hallmark of psychological theory but the modeling or recording of mental states Below are some of the main topics that cognitive science is concerned with This is not an exhaustive list See List of cognitive science topics for a list of various aspects of the field Artificial intelligence Edit Main articles Artificial intelligence and Outline of artificial intelligence Artificial intelligence AI involves the study of cognitive phenomena in machines One of the practical goals of AI is to implement aspects of human intelligence in computers Computers are also widely used as a tool with which to study cognitive phenomena Computational modeling uses simulations to study how human intelligence may be structured 26 See Computational modeling There is some debate in the field as to whether the mind is best viewed as a huge array of small but individually feeble elements i e neurons or as a collection of higher level structures such as symbols schemes plans and rules The former view uses connectionism to study the mind whereas the latter emphasizes symbolic artificial intelligence One way to view the issue is whether it is possible to accurately simulate a human brain on a computer without accurately simulating the neurons that make up the human brain Attention Edit Main article Attention Attention is the selection of important information The human mind is bombarded with millions of stimuli and it must have a way of deciding which of this information to process Attention is sometimes seen as a spotlight meaning one can only shine the light on a particular set of information Experiments that support this metaphor include the dichotic listening task Cherry 1957 and studies of inattentional blindness Mack and Rock 1998 In the dichotic listening task subjects are bombarded with two different messages one in each ear and told to focus on only one of the messages At the end of the experiment when asked about the content of the unattended message subjects cannot report it Bodily processes related to cognition Edit Embodied cognition approaches to cognitive science emphasize the role of body and environment in cognition This includes both neural and extra neural bodily processes and factors that range from affective and emotional processes 27 to posture motor control proprioception and kinaesthesis 28 to autonomic processes that involve heartbeat 29 and respiration 30 to the role of the enteric gut microbiome 31 It also includes accounts of how the body engages with or is coupled to social and physical environments 4E embodied embedded extended and enactive cognition 32 33 includes a broad range of views about brain body environment interaction from causal embeddedness to stronger claims about how the mind extends to include tools and instruments as well as the role of social interactions action oriented processes and affordances 4E theories range from those closer to classic cognitivism so called weak embodied cognition 34 to stronger extended 35 and enactive versions that are sometimes referred to as radical embodied cognitive science 36 37 Knowledge and processing of language Edit nbsp A well known example of a phrase structure tree This is one way of representing human language that shows how different components are organized hierarchically Main articles Theoretical linguistics Cognitive linguistics Language Linguistics and Psycholinguistics The ability to learn and understand language is an extremely complex process Language is acquired within the first few years of life and all humans under normal circumstances are able to acquire language proficiently A major driving force in the theoretical linguistic field is discovering the nature that language must have in the abstract in order to be learned in such a fashion Some of the driving research questions in studying how the brain itself processes language include 1 To what extent is linguistic knowledge innate or learned 2 Why is it more difficult for adults to acquire a second language than it is for infants to acquire their first language and 3 How are humans able to understand novel sentences The study of language processing ranges from the investigation of the sound patterns of speech to the meaning of words and whole sentences Linguistics often divides language processing into orthography phonetics phonology morphology syntax semantics and pragmatics Many aspects of language can be studied from each of these components and from their interaction 38 better source needed The study of language processing in cognitive science is closely tied to the field of linguistics Linguistics was traditionally studied as a part of the humanities including studies of history art and literature In the last fifty years or so more and more researchers have studied knowledge and use of language as a cognitive phenomenon the main problems being how knowledge of language can be acquired and used and what precisely it consists of 39 Linguists have found that while humans form sentences in ways apparently governed by very complex systems they are remarkably unaware of the rules that govern their own speech Thus linguists must resort to indirect methods to determine what those rules might be if indeed rules as such exist In any event if speech is indeed governed by rules they appear to be opaque to any conscious consideration Learning and development Edit Main articles Learning and Developmental psychology Learning and development are the processes by which we acquire knowledge and information over time Infants are born with little or no knowledge depending on how knowledge is defined yet they rapidly acquire the ability to use language walk and recognize people and objects Research in learning and development aims to explain the mechanisms by which these processes might take place A major question in the study of cognitive development is the extent to which certain abilities are innate or learned This is often framed in terms of the nature and nurture debate The nativist view emphasizes that certain features are innate to an organism and are determined by its genetic endowment The empiricist view on the other hand emphasizes that certain abilities are learned from the environment Although clearly both genetic and environmental input is needed for a child to develop normally considerable debate remains about how genetic information might guide cognitive development In the area of language acquisition for example some such as Steven Pinker 40 have argued that specific information containing universal grammatical rules must be contained in the genes whereas others such as Jeffrey Elman and colleagues in Rethinking Innateness have argued that Pinker s claims are biologically unrealistic They argue that genes determine the architecture of a learning system but that specific facts about how grammar works can only be learned as a result of experience Memory Edit Main article Memory Memory allows us to store information for later retrieval Memory is often thought of as consisting of both a long term and short term store Long term memory allows us to store information over prolonged periods days weeks years We do not yet know the practical limit of long term memory capacity Short term memory allows us to store information over short time scales seconds or minutes Memory is also often grouped into declarative and procedural forms Declarative memory grouped into subsets of semantic and episodic forms of memory refers to our memory for facts and specific knowledge specific meanings and specific experiences e g Are apples food or What did I eat for breakfast four days ago Procedural memory allows us to remember actions and motor sequences e g how to ride a bicycle and is often dubbed implicit knowledge or memory Cognitive scientists study memory just as psychologists do but tend to focus more on how memory bears on cognitive processes and the interrelationship between cognition and memory One example of this could be what mental processes does a person go through to retrieve a long lost memory Or what differentiates between the cognitive process of recognition seeing hints of something before remembering it or memory in context and recall retrieving a memory as in fill in the blank Perception and action Edit nbsp The Necker cube an example of an optical illusion nbsp An optical illusion The square A is exactly the same shade of gray as square B See checker shadow illusion Main article Perception Perception is the ability to take in information via the senses and process it in some way Vision and hearing are two dominant senses that allow us to perceive the environment Some questions in the study of visual perception for example include 1 How are we able to recognize objects 2 Why do we perceive a continuous visual environment even though we only see small bits of it at any one time One tool for studying visual perception is by looking at how people process optical illusions The image on the right of a Necker cube is an example of a bistable percept that is the cube can be interpreted as being oriented in two different directions The study of haptic tactile olfactory and gustatory stimuli also fall into the domain of perception Action is taken to refer to the output of a system In humans this is accomplished through motor responses Spatial planning and movement speech production and complex motor movements are all aspects of action Consciousness Edit Main article Consciousness Consciousness is the awareness of experiences within oneself This helps the mind with having the ability to experience or feel a sense of self Research methods EditMany different methodologies are used to study cognitive science As the field is highly interdisciplinary research often cuts across multiple areas of study drawing on research methods from psychology neuroscience computer science and systems theory Behavioral experiments Edit In order to have a description of what constitutes intelligent behavior one must study behavior itself This type of research is closely tied to that in cognitive psychology and psychophysics By measuring behavioral responses to different stimuli one can understand something about how those stimuli are processed Lewandowski amp Strohmetz 2009 reviewed a collection of innovative uses of behavioral measurement in psychology including behavioral traces behavioral observations and behavioral choice 41 Behavioral traces are pieces of evidence that indicate behavior occurred but the actor is not present e g litter in a parking lot or readings on an electric meter Behavioral observations involve the direct witnessing of the actor engaging in the behavior e g watching how close a person sits next to another person Behavioral choices are when a person selects between two or more options e g voting behavior choice of a punishment for another participant Reaction time The time between the presentation of a stimulus and an appropriate response can indicate differences between two cognitive processes and can indicate some things about their nature For example if in a search task the reaction times vary proportionally with the number of elements then it is evident that this cognitive process of searching involves serial instead of parallel processing Psychophysical responses Psychophysical experiments are an old psychological technique which has been adopted by cognitive psychology They typically involve making judgments of some physical property e g the loudness of a sound Correlation of subjective scales between individuals can show cognitive or sensory biases as compared to actual physical measurements Some examples include sameness judgments for colors tones textures etc threshold differences for colors tones textures etc Eye tracking This methodology is used to study a variety of cognitive processes most notably visual perception and language processing The fixation point of the eyes is linked to an individual s focus of attention Thus by monitoring eye movements we can study what information is being processed at a given time Eye tracking allows us to study cognitive processes on extremely short time scales Eye movements reflect online decision making during a task and they provide us with some insight into the ways in which those decisions may be processed 42 Brain imaging Edit Main article Neuroimaging nbsp Image of the human head with the brain The arrow indicates the position of the hypothalamus Brain imaging involves analyzing activity within the brain while performing various tasks This allows us to link behavior and brain function to help understand how information is processed Different types of imaging techniques vary in their temporal time based and spatial location based resolution Brain imaging is often used in cognitive neuroscience Single photon emission computed tomography and positron emission tomography SPECT and PET use radioactive isotopes which are injected into the subject s bloodstream and taken up by the brain By observing which areas of the brain take up the radioactive isotope we can see which areas of the brain are more active than other areas PET has similar spatial resolution to fMRI but it has extremely poor temporal resolution Electroencephalography EEG measures the electrical fields generated by large populations of neurons in the cortex by placing a series of electrodes on the scalp of the subject This technique has an extremely high temporal resolution but a relatively poor spatial resolution Functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI measures the relative amount of oxygenated blood flowing to different parts of the brain More oxygenated blood in a particular region is assumed to correlate with an increase in neural activity in that part of the brain This allows us to localize particular functions within different brain regions fMRI has moderate spatial and temporal resolution Optical imaging This technique uses infrared transmitters and receivers to measure the amount of light reflectance by blood near different areas of the brain Since oxygenated and deoxygenated blood reflects light by different amounts we can study which areas are more active i e those that have more oxygenated blood Optical imaging has moderate temporal resolution but poor spatial resolution It also has the advantage that it is extremely safe and can be used to study infants brains Magnetoencephalography MEG measures magnetic fields resulting from cortical activity It is similar to EEG except that it has improved spatial resolution since the magnetic fields it measures are not as blurred or attenuated by the scalp meninges and so forth as the electrical activity measured in EEG is MEG uses SQUID sensors to detect tiny magnetic fields Computational modeling Edit See also Computational cognition and Cognitive model nbsp An artificial neural network with two layersComputational models require a mathematically and logically formal representation of a problem Computer models are used in the simulation and experimental verification of different specific and general properties of intelligence Computational modeling can help us understand the functional organization of a particular cognitive phenomenon Approaches to cognitive modeling can be categorized as 1 symbolic on abstract mental functions of an intelligent mind by means of symbols 2 subsymbolic on the neural and associative properties of the human brain and 3 across the symbolic subsymbolic border including hybrid Symbolic modeling evolved from the computer science paradigms using the technologies of knowledge based systems as well as a philosophical perspective e g Good Old Fashioned Artificial Intelligence GOFAI They were developed by the first cognitive researchers and later used in information engineering for expert systems Since the early 1990s it was generalized in systemics for the investigation of functional human like intelligence models such as personoids and in parallel developed as the SOAR environment Recently especially in the context of cognitive decision making symbolic cognitive modeling has been extended to the socio cognitive approach including social and organizational cognition interrelated with a sub symbolic non conscious layer Subsymbolic modeling includes connectionist neural network models Connectionism relies on the idea that the mind brain is composed of simple nodes and its problem solving capacity derives from the connections between them Neural nets are textbook implementations of this approach Some critics of this approach feel that while these models approach biological reality as a representation of how the system works these models lack explanatory powers because even in systems endowed with simple connection rules the emerging high complexity makes them less interpretable at the connection level than they apparently are at the macroscopic level Other approaches gaining in popularity include 1 dynamical systems theory 2 mapping symbolic models onto connectionist models Neural symbolic integration or hybrid intelligent systems and 3 and Bayesian models which are often drawn from machine learning All the above approaches tend either to be generalized to the form of integrated computational models of a synthetic abstract intelligence i e cognitive architecture in order to be applied to the explanation and improvement of individual and social organizational decision making and reasoning 43 44 or to focus on single simulative programs or microtheories middle range theories modelling specific cognitive faculties e g vision language categorization etc Neurobiological methods Edit Research methods borrowed directly from neuroscience and neuropsychology can also help us to understand aspects of intelligence These methods allow us to understand how intelligent behavior is implemented in a physical system Single unit recording Direct brain stimulation Animal models Postmortem studiesKey findings EditCognitive science has given rise to models of human cognitive bias and risk perception and has been influential in the development of behavioral finance part of economics It has also given rise to a new theory of the philosophy of mathematics related to denotational mathematics and many theories of artificial intelligence persuasion and coercion It has made its presence known in the philosophy of language and epistemology as well as constituting a substantial wing of modern linguistics Fields of cognitive science have been influential in understanding the brain s particular functional systems and functional deficits ranging from speech production to auditory processing and visual perception It has made progress in understanding how damage to particular areas of the brain affect cognition and it has helped to uncover the root causes and results of specific dysfunction such as dyslexia anopia and hemispatial neglect Notable researchers EditThis section needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources in this section Unsourced material may be challenged and removed August 2012 Learn how and when to remove this template message See also List of cognitive scientists Name Year of birth Year of contribution Contribution s David Chalmers 1966 45 1995 46 Dualism hard problem of consciousnessDaniel Dennett 1942 47 1987 Offered a computational systems perspective Multiple drafts model John Searle 1932 48 1980 Chinese roomDouglas Hofstadter 1945 1979 49 Godel Escher Bach 50 Jerry Fodor 1935 51 1968 1975 FunctionalismAlan Baddeley 1934 52 1974 Baddeley s model of working memoryMarvin Minsky 1927 53 1970s early 1980s Wrote computer programs in languages such as LISP to attempt to formally characterize the steps that human beings go through such as making decisions and solving problemsChristopher Longuet Higgins 1923 54 1973 Coined the term cognitive scienceNoam Chomsky 1928 55 1959 Published a review of B F Skinner s book Verbal Behavior which began cognitivism against then dominant behaviorism 6 George Miller 1920 1956 Wrote about the capacities of human thinking through mental representationsHerbert Simon 1916 1956 Co created Logic Theory Machine and General Problem Solver with Allen Newell EPAM Elementary Perceiver and Memorizer theory organizational decision makingJohn McCarthy 1927 1955 Coined the term artificial intelligence and organized the famous Dartmouth conference in Summer 1956 which started AI as a fieldMcCulloch and Pitts 1930s 1940s Developed early artificial neural networksJ C R Licklider 1915 56 Established MIT Sloan School of ManagementLila R Gleitman 1929 1970s 2010s Wide ranging contributions to understanding the cognition of language acquisition including syntactic bootstrapping theory 57 Eleanor Rosch 1938 1976 Development of the Prototype Theory of categorisation 58 Philip N Johnson Laird 1936 1980 Introduced the idea of mental models in cognitive science 59 Dedre Gentner 1944 1983 Development of the Structure mapping Theory of analogical reasoning 60 Allen Newell 1927 1990 Development of the field of Cognitive architecture in cognitive modelling and artificial intelligence 61 Annette Karmiloff Smith 1938 1992 Integrating neuroscience and computational modelling into theories of cognitive development 62 David Marr neuroscientist 1945 1990 Proponent of the Three Level Hypothesis of levels of analysis of computational systems 63 Peter Gardenfors 1949 2000 Creator of the conceptual space framework used in cognitive modelling and artificial intelligence Linda B Smith 1951 1993 Together with Esther Thelen created a dynamical systems approach to understanding cognitive development 64 Some of the more recognized names in cognitive science are usually either the most controversial or the most cited Within philosophy some familiar names include Daniel Dennett who writes from a computational systems perspective 65 John Searle known for his controversial Chinese room argument 66 and Jerry Fodor who advocates functionalism 67 Others include David Chalmers who advocates Dualism and is also known for articulating the hard problem of consciousness and Douglas Hofstadter famous for writing Godel Escher Bach which questions the nature of words and thought In the realm of linguistics Noam Chomsky and George Lakoff have been influential both have also become notable as political commentators In artificial intelligence Marvin Minsky Herbert A Simon and Allen Newell are prominent Popular names in the discipline of psychology include George A Miller James McClelland Philip Johnson Laird Lawrence Barsalou Vittorio Guidano Howard Gardner and Steven Pinker Anthropologists Dan Sperber Edwin Hutchins Bradd Shore James Wertsch and Scott Atran have been involved in collaborative projects with cognitive and social psychologists political scientists and evolutionary biologists in attempts to develop general theories of culture formation religion and political association Computational theories with models and simulations have also been developed by David Rumelhart James McClelland and Philip Johnson Laird Epistemics EditEpistemics is a term coined in 1969 by the University of Edinburgh with the foundation of its School of Epistemics Epistemics is to be distinguished from epistemology in that epistemology is the philosophical theory of knowledge whereas epistemics signifies the scientific study of knowledge Christopher Longuet Higgins has defined it as the construction of formal models of the processes perceptual intellectual and linguistic by which knowledge and understanding are achieved and communicated 68 In his 1978 essay Epistemics The Regulative Theory of Cognition 69 Alvin I Goldman claims to have coined the term epistemics to describe a reorientation of epistemology Goldman maintains that his epistemics is continuous with traditional epistemology and the new term is only to avoid opposition Epistemics in Goldman s version differs only slightly from traditional epistemology in its alliance with the psychology of cognition epistemics stresses the detailed study of mental processes and information processing mechanisms that lead to knowledge or beliefs In the mid 1980s the School of Epistemics was renamed as The Centre for Cognitive Science CCS In 1998 CCS was incorporated into the University of Edinburgh s School of Informatics 70 Binding problem in cognitive science EditOne of the core aims of cognitive science is to achieve an integrated theory of cognition This requires integrative mechanisms explaining how the information processing that occurs simultaneously in spatially segregated sub cortical areas in the brain is coordinated and bound together to give rise to coherent perceptual and symbolic representations One approach is to solve this Binding problem 71 72 73 that is the problem of dynamically representing conjunctions of informational elements from the most basic perceptual representations feature binding to the most complex cognitive representations like symbol structures variable binding by means of integrative synchronization mechanisms In other words one of the coordinating mechanisms appears to be the temporal phase synchronization of neural activity based on dynamical self organizing processes in neural networks described by the Binding by synchrony BBS Hypothesis from neurophysiology 74 75 76 77 Connectionist cognitive neuroarchitectures have been developed that use integrative synchronization mechanisms to solve this binding problem in perceptual cognition and in language cognition 78 79 80 In perceptual cognition the problem is to explain how elementary object properties and object relations like the object color or the object form can be dynamically bound together or can be integrated to a representation of this perceptual object by means of a synchronization mechanism feature binding feature linking In language cognition the problem is to explain how semantic concepts and syntactic roles can be dynamically bound together or can be integrated to complex cognitive representations like systematic and compositional symbol structures and propositions by means of a synchronization mechanism variable binding see also the Symbolism vs connectionism debate in connectionism See also EditAffective science Cognitive anthropology Cognitive biology Cognitive computing Cognitive ethology Cognitive linguistics Cognitive neuropsychology Cognitive neuroscience Cognitive psychology Cognitive science of religion Computational neuroscience Computational representational understanding of mind Concept mining Decision field theory Decision theory Dynamicism Educational neuroscience Educational psychology Embodied cognition Embodied cognitive science Enactivism Epistemology Folk psychology Heterophenomenology Human Cognome Project Human computer interaction Indiana Archives of Cognitive Science Informatics academic field List of cognitive scientists List of psychology awards Malleable intelligence Neural Darwinism Noogenesis Personal information management PIM Qualia Quantum cognition Simulated consciousness Situated cognition Society of Mind theory Spatial cognition Speech language pathology nbsp Philosophy portal nbsp Psychology portalOutlinesOutline of human intelligence topic tree presenting the traits capacities models and research fields of human intelligence and more Outline of thought topic tree that identifies many types of thoughts types of thinking aspects of thought related fields and more References Edit Miller George A 1 March 2003 The cognitive revolution a historical perspective Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 3 141 144 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 03 00029 9 ISSN 1364 6613 PMID 12639696 S2CID 206129621 Archived from the original on 21 November 2018 Retrieved 5 February 2023 Ask the Cognitive Scientist American Federation of Teachers 8 August 2014 Archived from the original on 17 September 2014 Retrieved 25 December 2013 Cognitive science is an interdisciplinary field of researchers from Linguistics psychology neuroscience philosophy computer science and anthropology that seek to understand the mind a b Thagard Paul Cognitive Science Archived 15 July 2018 at the Wayback Machine The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Fall 2008 Edition Edward N Zalta ed Miller George A 2003 The cognitive revolution A historical perspective Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 3 141 144 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 03 00029 9 PMID 12639696 S2CID 206129621 Hafner K Lyon M 1996 Where wizards stay up late The origins of the Internet New York Simon amp Schuster p 32 ISBN 0 684 81201 0 a b Chomsky Noam 1959 Review of Verbal behavior Language 35 1 26 58 doi 10 2307 411334 ISSN 0097 8507 JSTOR 411334 Longuet Higgins H C 1973 Comments on the Lighthill Report and the Sutherland Reply Artificial Intelligence a paper symposium Science Research Council pp 35 37 ISBN 0 901660 18 3 Cognitive Science Society Archived 17 July 2010 at the Wayback Machine a b UCSD Cognitive Science UCSD Cognitive Science Archived from the original on 9 July 2015 Retrieved 8 July 2015 Box 729 About Cognitive Science Vassar College Cogsci vassar edu Archived from the original on 17 September 2012 Retrieved 15 August 2012 d Avila Garcez Artur S Lamb Luis C Gabbay Dov M 2008 Neural Symbolic Cognitive Reasoning Cognitive Technologies Springer ISBN 978 3 540 73245 7 Sun Ron Bookman Larry eds 1994 Computational Architectures Integrating Neural and Symbolic Processes Needham MA Kluwer Academic ISBN 0 7923 9517 4 Encephalos Journal www encephalos gr Archived from the original on 25 June 2011 Retrieved 20 February 2018 Wilson Elizabeth A 4 February 2016 Neural Geographies Feminism and the Microstructure of Cognition Routledge ISBN 9781317958765 Archived from the original on 5 February 2023 Retrieved 16 April 2018 Di Paolo Ezequiel A 2003 Organismically inspired robotics homeostatic adaptation and teleology beyond the closed sensorimotor loop In Murase Kazuyuki Asakura Toshiyuki eds Dynamic Systems Approach for Embodiment and Sociality From Ecological Psychology to Robotics Advanced Knowledge International pp 19 42 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 62 4813 ISBN 978 0 9751004 1 7 S2CID 15349751 Zorzi Marco Testolin Alberto Stoianov Ivilin P 20 August 2013 Modeling language and cognition with deep unsupervised learning a tutorial overview Frontiers in Psychology 4 515 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2013 00515 ISSN 1664 1078 PMC 3747356 PMID 23970869 Tieszen Richard 15 July 2011 Analytic and Continental Philosophy Science and Global Philosophy Comparative Philosophy 2 2 doi 10 31979 2151 6014 2011 020206 Browne A 1997 Neural Network Perspectives on Cognition and Adaptive Robotics CRC Press ISBN 0 7503 0455 3 Archived from the original on 5 February 2023 Retrieved 16 April 2018 Pfeifer R Schreter Z Fogelman Soulie F Steels L 1989 Connectionism in Perspective Elsevier ISBN 0 444 59876 6 Archived from the original on 5 February 2023 Retrieved 16 April 2018 Karmiloff Smith A 2015 An alternative to domain general or domain specific frameworks for theorizing about human evolution and ontogenesis AIMS Neuroscience 2 2 91 104 doi 10 3934 Neuroscience 2015 2 91 PMC 4678597 PMID 26682283 Varela F J Thompson E amp Rosch E 1991 The embodied mind cognitive science and human experience Cambridge Massachusetts MIT Press Marr D 1982 Vision A Computational Investigation into the Human Representation and Processing of Visual Information W H Freeman Miller G A 2003 The cognitive revolution a historical perspective Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 3 141 144 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 03 00029 9 PMID 12639696 S2CID 206129621 Ferres Joan Masanet Maria Jose 2017 Communication Efficiency in Education Increasing Emotions and Storytelling Comunicar in Spanish 25 52 51 60 doi 10 3916 c52 2017 05 ISSN 1134 3478 Shields Christopher 2020 Aristotle s Psychology in Zalta Edward N ed The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Winter 2020 ed Metaphysics Research Lab Stanford University archived from the original on 20 October 2022 retrieved 20 October 2022 Sun Ron ed 2008 The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology Cambridge University Press New York Colombetti G and Krueger J 2015 Scaffoldings of the affective mind Philosophical Psychology 28 8 1157 1176 doi 10 1080 09515089 2014 976334 hdl 10871 15680 S2CID 73617860 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Gallagher Shaun 2005 How the Body Shapes the Mind Oxford Oxford University Press Garfinkel S N Barrett A B Minati L Dolan R J Seth A K Critchley H D 2013 What the heart forgets Cardiac timing influences memory for words and is modulated by metacognition and interoceptive sensitivity Psychophysiology 50 6 505 512 doi 10 1111 psyp 12039 PMC 4340570 PMID 23521494 Varga S Heck D H 2017 Rhythms of the body rhythms of the brain respiration neural oscillations and embodied cognition Consciousness and Cognition 56 77 90 doi 10 1016 j concog 2017 09 008 PMID 29073509 S2CID 8448790 Davidson G L Cooke A C Johnson C N Quinn J L 2018 The gut microbiome as a driver of individual variation in cognition and functional behaviour Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 373 1756 20170286 doi 10 1098 rstb 2017 0286 PMC 6107574 PMID 30104431 Newen A De Bruin L Gallagher S eds 2018 The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition Oxford Oxford University Press Menary Richard 2010 Special Issue on 4E Cognition Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 9 4 doi 10 1007 s11097 010 9187 6 S2CID 143621939 Alsmith A J T De Vignemont F 2012 Embodying the mind and representing the body PDF Review of Philosophy and Psychology 3 1 1 13 doi 10 1007 s13164 012 0085 4 S2CID 5823387 Clark Andy 2011 Precis of Supersizing the mind embodiment action and cognitive extension Philosophical Studies 152 3 413 416 doi 10 1007 s11098 010 9597 x S2CID 170708745 Chemero Anthony 2011 Radical embodied cognitive science Cambridge MIT Press Hutto Daniel D amp Erik Myin 2012 Radicalizing enactivism Basic minds without content Cambridge MIT Press Linguistics Semantics Phonetics Pragmatics and Human Communication Decoded Science 16 February 2014 Archived from the original on 6 June 2017 Retrieved 7 February 2018 Isac Daniela Charles Reiss 2013 I language An Introduction to Linguistics as Cognitive Science 2nd edition Oxford University Press p 5 ISBN 978 0199660179 Archived from the original on 6 July 2011 Retrieved 29 July 2008 Pinker S Bloom P 1990 Natural language and natural selection Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13 4 707 784 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 116 4044 doi 10 1017 S0140525X00081061 S2CID 6167614 Lewandowski Gary Strohmetz David 2009 Actions can speak as loud as words Measuring behavior in psychological science Social and Personality Psychology Compass 3 6 992 1002 doi 10 1111 j 1751 9004 2009 00229 x Konig Peter Wilming Niklas Kietzmann Tim C Ossandon Jose P Onat Selim Ehinger Benedikt V Gameiro Ricardo R Kaspar Kai 1 December 2016 Eye movements as a window to cognitive processes Journal of Eye Movement Research 9 5 doi 10 16910 jemr 9 5 3 Lieto Antonio 2021 Cognitive Design for Artificial Minds London UK Routledge Taylor amp Francis ISBN 9781138207929 Sun Ron ed Grounding Social Sciences in Cognitive Sciences MIT Press Cambridge Massachusetts 2012 David Chalmers www informationphilosopher com Archived from the original on 24 April 2017 Retrieved 24 April 2017 Facing Up to the Problem of Consciousness consc net Archived from the original on 8 April 2011 Retrieved 24 April 2017 Daniel C Dennett American philosopher Encyclopaedia Britannica Archived from the original on 23 July 2016 Retrieved 3 May 2017 John Searle www informationphilosopher com Archived from the original on 24 April 2017 Retrieved 3 May 2017 Godel Escher Bach Goodreads Archived from the original on 22 May 2017 Retrieved 3 May 2017 Somers James The Man Who Would Teach Machines to Think The Atlantic Archived from the original on 17 September 2016 Retrieved 3 May 2017 Fodor Jerry Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy www iep utm edu Archived from the original on 17 October 2015 Retrieved 3 May 2017 Alan Baddeley in International Directory of Psychologists 1966 Archived from the original on 5 February 2023 Retrieved 10 March 2022 Marvin Minsky American scientist Encyclopaedia Britannica Archived from the original on 28 March 2017 Retrieved 27 March 2017 Darwin Chris 9 June 2004 Christopher Longuet Higgins The Guardian ISSN 0261 3077 Archived from the original on 21 December 2016 Retrieved 27 March 2017 Noam Chomsky chomsky info Archived from the original on 25 April 2017 Retrieved 24 April 2017 J C R Licklider Internet Hall of Fame internethalloffame org Archived from the original on 28 March 2017 Retrieved 24 April 2017 Gleitman Lila January 1990 The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings Language Acquisition 1 1 3 55 doi 10 1207 s15327817la0101 2 S2CID 144713838 Rosch Eleanor Mervis Carolyn B Gray Wayne D Johnson David M Boyes Braem Penny July 1976 Basic objects in natural categories Cognitive Psychology 8 3 382 439 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 149 3392 doi 10 1016 0010 0285 76 90013 X S2CID 5612467 Johnsonlaird P March 1981 Mental models in cognitive science Cognitive Science 4 1 71 115 doi 10 1016 S0364 0213 81 80005 5 Gentner Dedre April 1983 Structure Mapping A Theoretical Framework for Analogy Cognitive Science 7 2 155 170 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog0702 3 S2CID 5371492 Newell Allen 1990 Unified Theories of Cognition Harvard University Press Cambridge Massachusetts page needed Karmiloff Smith Annette 1992 Beyond Modularity A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science MIT Press ISBN 9780262111690 page needed Marr D Poggio T 1976 From Understanding Computation to Understanding Neural Circuitry Artificial Intelligence Laboratory Report A I Memo Massachusetts Institute of Technology hdl 1721 1 5782 AIM 357 Smith Linda B Thelen Esther 1993 A Dynamic systems approach to development applications MIT Press ISBN 978 0 585 03867 4 OCLC 42854628 page needed Rescorla Michael 1 January 2017 Zalta Edward N ed The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Spring 2017 ed Metaphysics Research Lab Stanford University Archived from the original on 18 March 2019 Retrieved 27 March 2017 Hauser Larry Chinese Room Argument Internet Enclclopedia of Philosophy Archived from the original on 27 April 2016 Retrieved 27 March 2017 Fodor Jerry Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy www iep utm edu Archived from the original on 17 October 2015 Retrieved 27 March 2017 Longuet Higgins Christopher 1977 1969 Epistemics in A Bullock amp O Stallybrass ed Fontana dictionary of modern thought London UK Fontana p 209 ISBN 9780002161497 Goldman Alvin I 1978 Epistemics The Regulative Theory of Cognition The Journal of Philosophy 75 10 509 23 doi 10 2307 2025838 JSTOR 2025838 The old WWW cogsci ed ac uk server Archived from the original on 19 October 2010 Retrieved 27 August 2019 Hardcastle V G 1998 The binding problem In W Bechtel amp G Graham eds A Companion to Cognitive Science pp 555 565 Blackwell Publisher Malden MA Oxford UK Hummel J 1999 Binding problem In R A Wilson amp F C Keil The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences pp 85 86 Cambridge MA London The MIT Press Malsburg C von der 1999 The what and why of binding The modeler s perspective Neuron 24 95 104 Gray C M amp Singer W 1989 Stimulus specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 86 1698 1702 Singer W 1999b Neuronal synchrony A versatile code for the definition of relations Neuron 24 49 65 Singer W amp A Lazar 2016 Does the cerebral cortex exploit high dimensional non linear dynamics for information processing Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 10 99 Singer W 2018 Neuronal oscillations unavoidable and useful European Journal of Neuroscience 48 2389 2399 Maurer H 2021 Cognitive science Integrative synchronization mechanisms in cognitive neuroarchitectures of the modern connectionism CRC Press Boca Raton FL ISBN 978 1 351 04352 6 https doi org 10 1201 9781351043526 Archived 5 February 2023 at the Wayback Machine Maurer H 2016 Integrative synchronization mechanisms in connectionist cognitive Neuroarchitectures Computational Cognitive Science 2 3 https doi org 10 1186 s40469 016 0010 8 Archived 5 February 2023 at the Wayback Machine Marcus G F 2001 The algebraic mind Integrating connectionism and cognitive science Bradford Book The MIT Press Cambridge ISBN 0 262 13379 2 External links Edit nbsp Media related to Cognitive science at Wikimedia Commons nbsp Quotations related to Cognitive science at Wikiquote nbsp Learning materials related to Cognitive science at Wikiversity Cognitive Science on the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Cognitive Science Society Cognitive Science Movie Index A broad list of movies showcasing themes in the Cognitive Sciences Archived 4 September 2015 at the Wayback Machine List of leading thinkers in cognitive science Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Cognitive science amp oldid 1180112712, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.