fbpx
Wikipedia

Computational neuroscience

Computational neuroscience (also known as theoretical neuroscience or mathematical neuroscience) is a branch of neuroscience which employs mathematical models, computer simulations, theoretical analysis and abstractions of the brain to understand the principles that govern the development, structure, physiology and cognitive abilities of the nervous system.[1][2][3][4]

Computational neuroscience employs computational simulations to validate and solve mathematical models, and so can be seen as a sub-field of theoretical neuroscience; however, the two fields are often synonymous.[5] The term mathematical neuroscience is also used sometimes, to stress the quantitative nature of the field.[6]

Computational neuroscience focuses on the description of biologically plausible neurons (and neural systems) and their physiology and dynamics, and it is therefore not directly concerned with biologically unrealistic models used in connectionism, control theory, cybernetics, quantitative psychology, machine learning, artificial neural networks, artificial intelligence and computational learning theory;[7] [8][9] although mutual inspiration exists and sometimes there is no strict limit between fields,[10][11][12] with model abstraction in computational neuroscience depending on research scope and the granularity at which biological entities are analyzed.

Models in theoretical neuroscience are aimed at capturing the essential features of the biological system at multiple spatial-temporal scales, from membrane currents, and chemical coupling via network oscillations, columnar and topographic architecture, nuclei, all the way up to psychological faculties like memory, learning and behavior. These computational models frame hypotheses that can be directly tested by biological or psychological experiments.

History

The term 'computational neuroscience' was introduced by Eric L. Schwartz, who organized a conference, held in 1985 in Carmel, California, at the request of the Systems Development Foundation to provide a summary of the current status of a field which until that point was referred to by a variety of names, such as neural modeling, brain theory and neural networks. The proceedings of this definitional meeting were published in 1990 as the book Computational Neuroscience.[13] The first of the annual open international meetings focused on Computational Neuroscience was organized by James M. Bower and John Miller in San Francisco, California in 1989.[14] The first graduate educational program in computational neuroscience was organized as the Computational and Neural Systems Ph.D. program at the California Institute of Technology in 1985.

The early historical roots of the field can be traced to the work of people including Louis Lapicque, Hodgkin & Huxley, Hubel and Wiesel, and David Marr. Lapicque introduced the integrate and fire model of the neuron in a seminal article published in 1907,[15] a model still popular for artificial neural networks studies because of its simplicity (see a recent review[16]).

About 40 years later, Hodgkin and Huxley developed the voltage clamp and created the first biophysical model of the action potential. Hubel and Wiesel discovered that neurons in the primary visual cortex, the first cortical area to process information coming from the retina, have oriented receptive fields and are organized in columns.[17] David Marr's work focused on the interactions between neurons, suggesting computational approaches to the study of how functional groups of neurons within the hippocampus and neocortex interact, store, process, and transmit information. Computational modeling of biophysically realistic neurons and dendrites began with the work of Wilfrid Rall, with the first multicompartmental model using cable theory.

Major topics

Research in computational neuroscience can be roughly categorized into several lines of inquiry. Most computational neuroscientists collaborate closely with experimentalists in analyzing novel data and synthesizing new models of biological phenomena.

Single-neuron modeling

Even a single neuron has complex biophysical characteristics and can perform computations (e.g.[18]). Hodgkin and Huxley's original model only employed two voltage-sensitive currents (Voltage sensitive ion channels are glycoprotein molecules which extend through the lipid bilayer, allowing ions to traverse under certain conditions through the axolemma), the fast-acting sodium and the inward-rectifying potassium. Though successful in predicting the timing and qualitative features of the action potential, it nevertheless failed to predict a number of important features such as adaptation and shunting. Scientists now believe that there are a wide variety of voltage-sensitive currents, and the implications of the differing dynamics, modulations, and sensitivity of these currents is an important topic of computational neuroscience.[19]

The computational functions of complex dendrites are also under intense investigation. There is a large body of literature regarding how different currents interact with geometric properties of neurons.[20]

Some models are also tracking biochemical pathways at very small scales such as dendritic spines[21][22] or synaptic clefts.[23]

There are many software packages, such as GENESIS and NEURON, that allow rapid and systematic in silico modeling of realistic neurons. Blue Brain, a project founded by Henry Markram from the École Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne, aims to construct a biophysically detailed simulation of a cortical column on the Blue Gene supercomputer.

Modeling the richness of biophysical properties on the single-neuron scale can supply mechanisms that serve as the building blocks for network dynamics.[24] However, detailed neuron descriptions are computationally expensive and this computing cost can limit the pursuit of realistic network investigations, where many neurons need to be simulated. As a result, researchers that study large neural circuits typically represent each neuron and synapse with an artificially simple model, ignoring much of the biological detail. Hence there is a drive to produce simplified neuron models that can retain significant biological fidelity at a low computational overhead. Algorithms have been developed to produce faithful, faster running, simplified surrogate neuron models from computationally expensive, detailed neuron models.[25]

Modeling Neuron-glia interactions

Glial cells participate significantly to the regulation of neuronal activity at a cellular but also at a network level. Modeling this interaction allows to clarify the potassium cycle,[26][27] so important for maintaining homeostatis and to prevent epileptic seizures. Modeling reveals the role of glial protrusions that can penetrate in some cases the synaptic cleft to interfere with the synpatic transmission and thus control synaptic communication.[28]

Development, axonal patterning, and guidance

Computational neuroscience aims to address a wide array of questions. How do axons and dendrites form during development? How do axons know where to target and how to reach these targets? How do neurons migrate to the proper position in the central and peripheral systems? How do synapses form? We know from molecular biology that distinct parts of the nervous system release distinct chemical cues, from growth factors to hormones that modulate and influence the growth and development of functional connections between neurons.

Theoretical investigations into the formation and patterning of synaptic connection and morphology are still nascent. One hypothesis that has recently garnered some attention is the minimal wiring hypothesis, which postulates that the formation of axons and dendrites effectively minimizes resource allocation while maintaining maximal information storage.[29]

Sensory processing

Early models on sensory processing understood within a theoretical framework are credited to Horace Barlow. Somewhat similar to the minimal wiring hypothesis described in the preceding section, Barlow understood the processing of the early sensory systems to be a form of efficient coding, where the neurons encoded information which minimized the number of spikes. Experimental and computational work have since supported this hypothesis in one form or another. For the example of visual processing, efficient coding is manifested in the forms of efficient spatial coding, color coding, temporal/motion coding, stereo coding, and combinations of them.[30]

Further along the visual pathway, even the efficiently coded visual information is too much for the capacity of the information bottleneck, the visual attentional bottleneck.[31] A subsequent theory, V1 Saliency Hypothesis (V1SH), has been developed on exogenous attentional selection of a fraction of visual input for further processing, guided by a bottom-up saliency map in the primary visual cortex.[32]

Current research in sensory processing is divided among a biophysical modelling of different subsystems and a more theoretical modelling of perception. Current models of perception have suggested that the brain performs some form of Bayesian inference and integration of different sensory information in generating our perception of the physical world.[33][34]

Motor control

Many models of the way the brain controls movement have been developed. This includes models of processing in the brain such as the cerebellum's role for error correction, skill learning in motor cortex and the basal ganglia, or the control of the vestibulo ocular reflex. This also includes many normative models, such as those of the Bayesian or optimal control flavor which are built on the idea that the brain efficiently solves its problems.

Memory and synaptic plasticity

Earlier models of memory are primarily based on the postulates of Hebbian learning. Biologically relevant models such as Hopfield net have been developed to address the properties of associative (also known as "content-addressable") style of memory that occur in biological systems. These attempts are primarily focusing on the formation of medium- and long-term memory, localizing in the hippocampus. Models of working memory, relying on theories of network oscillations and persistent activity, have been built to capture some features of the prefrontal cortex in context-related memory.[35] Additional models look at the close relationship between the basal ganglia and the prefrontal cortex and how that contributes to working memory.[36]

One of the major problems in neurophysiological memory is how it is maintained and changed through multiple time scales. Unstable synapses are easy to train but also prone to stochastic disruption. Stable synapses forget less easily, but they are also harder to consolidate. One recent computational hypothesis involves cascades of plasticity that allow synapses to function at multiple time scales.[37] Stereochemically detailed models of the acetylcholine receptor-based synapse with the Monte Carlo method, working at the time scale of microseconds, have been built.[38] It is likely that computational tools will contribute greatly to our understanding of how synapses function and change in relation to external stimulus in the coming decades.

Behaviors of networks

Biological neurons are connected to each other in a complex, recurrent fashion. These connections are, unlike most artificial neural networks, sparse and usually specific. It is not known how information is transmitted through such sparsely connected networks, although specific areas of the brain, such as the visual cortex, are understood in some detail.[39] It is also unknown what the computational functions of these specific connectivity patterns are, if any.

The interactions of neurons in a small network can be often reduced to simple models such as the Ising model. The statistical mechanics of such simple systems are well-characterized theoretically. Some recent evidence suggests that dynamics of arbitrary neuronal networks can be reduced to pairwise interactions.[40] It is not known, however, whether such descriptive dynamics impart any important computational function. With the emergence of two-photon microscopy and calcium imaging, we now have powerful experimental methods with which to test the new theories regarding neuronal networks.

In some cases the complex interactions between inhibitory and excitatory neurons can be simplified using mean-field theory, which gives rise to the population model of neural networks.[41] While many neurotheorists prefer such models with reduced complexity, others argue that uncovering structural-functional relations depends on including as much neuronal and network structure as possible. Models of this type are typically built in large simulation platforms like GENESIS or NEURON. There have been some attempts to provide unified methods that bridge and integrate these levels of complexity.[42]

Visual attention, identification, and categorization

Visual attention can be described as a set of mechanisms that limit some processing to a subset of incoming stimuli.[43] Attentional mechanisms shape what we see and what we can act upon. They allow for concurrent selection of some (preferably, relevant) information and inhibition of other information. In order to have a more concrete specification of the mechanism underlying visual attention and the binding of features, a number of computational models have been proposed aiming to explain psychophysical findings. In general, all models postulate the existence of a saliency or priority map for registering the potentially interesting areas of the retinal input, and a gating mechanism for reducing the amount of incoming visual information, so that the limited computational resources of the brain can handle it.[44] An example theory that is being extensively tested behaviorally and physiologically is the V1 Saliency Hypothesis that a bottom-up saliency map is created in the primary visual cortex to guide attention exogenously.[32] Computational neuroscience provides a mathematical framework for studying the mechanisms involved in brain function and allows complete simulation and prediction of neuropsychological syndromes.

Cognition, discrimination, and learning

Computational modeling of higher cognitive functions has only recently[when?] begun. Experimental data comes primarily from single-unit recording in primates. The frontal lobe and parietal lobe function as integrators of information from multiple sensory modalities. There are some tentative ideas regarding how simple mutually inhibitory functional circuits in these areas may carry out biologically relevant computation.[45]

The brain seems to be able to discriminate and adapt particularly well in certain contexts. For instance, human beings seem to have an enormous capacity for memorizing and recognizing faces. One of the key goals of computational neuroscience is to dissect how biological systems carry out these complex computations efficiently and potentially replicate these processes in building intelligent machines.

The brain's large-scale organizational principles are illuminated by many fields, including biology, psychology, and clinical practice. Integrative neuroscience attempts to consolidate these observations through unified descriptive models and databases of behavioral measures and recordings. These are the bases for some quantitative modeling of large-scale brain activity.[46]

The Computational Representational Understanding of Mind (CRUM) is another attempt at modeling human cognition through simulated processes like acquired rule-based systems in decision making and the manipulation of visual representations in decision making.

Consciousness

One of the ultimate goals of psychology/neuroscience is to be able to explain the everyday experience of conscious life. Francis Crick, Giulio Tononi and Christof Koch made some attempts to formulate consistent frameworks for future work in neural correlates of consciousness (NCC), though much of the work in this field remains speculative.[47] Specifically, Crick[48] cautioned the field of neuroscience to not approach topics that are traditionally left to philosophy and religion.[49]

Computational clinical neuroscience

Computational clinical neuroscience is a field that brings together experts in neuroscience, neurology, psychiatry, decision sciences and computational modeling to quantitatively define and investigate problems in neurological and psychiatric diseases, and to train scientists and clinicians that wish to apply these models to diagnosis and treatment.[50][51]

Predictive computational neuroscience

Predictive computational neuroscience is a recent field that combines signal processing, neuroscience, clinical data and machine learning to predict the brain during coma [52]or anesthesia.[53] For exmaple, it is possible to anticipate deep brain states using the EEG signal. These states can be used to anticipate hypnotic concentration to administrate to the patient.

Computational Psychiatry

Computational psychiatry is a new emerging field that brings together experts in machine learning, neuroscience, neurology, psychiatry, psychology to provide an understanding of psychiatric disorders.[54][55][56]

Technology

Neuromorphic computing

A neuromorphic computer/chip is any device that uses physical artificial neurons (made from silicon) to do computations (See: neuromorphic computing, physical neural network). One of the advantages of using a physical model computer such as this is that it takes the computational load of the processor (in the sense that the structural and some of the functional elements don't have to be programmed since they are in hardware). In recent times,[57] neuromorphic technology has been used to build supercomputers which are used in international neuroscience collaborations. Examples include the Human Brain Project SpiNNaker supercomputer and the BrainScaleS computer.[58]


See also

Notes and references

  1. ^ Trappenberg, Thomas P. (2010). Fundamentals of Computational Neuroscience. United States: Oxford University Press Inc. pp. 2. ISBN 978-0-19-851582-1.
  2. ^ Patricia S. Churchland; Christof Koch; Terrence J. Sejnowski (1993). "What is computational neuroscience?". In Eric L. Schwartz (ed.). . MIT Press. pp. 46–55. Archived from the original on 2011-06-04. Retrieved 2009-06-11.
  3. ^ Dayan P.; Abbott, L. F. (2001). Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-04199-7.
  4. ^ Gerstner, W.; Kistler, W.; Naud, R.; Paninski, L. (2014). Neuronal Dynamics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9781107447615.
  5. ^ Thomas, Trappenberg (2010). Fundamentals of Computational Neuroscience. OUP Oxford. p. 2. ISBN 978-0199568413. Retrieved 17 January 2017.
  6. ^ Gutkin, Boris; Pinto, David; Ermentrout, Bard (2003-03-01). "Mathematical neuroscience: from neurons to circuits to systems". Journal of Physiology-Paris. Neurogeometry and visual perception. 97 (2): 209–219. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2003.09.005. ISSN 0928-4257. PMID 14766142. S2CID 10040483.
  7. ^ Kriegeskorte, Nikolaus; Douglas, Pamela K. (September 2018). "Cognitive computational neuroscience". Nature Neuroscience. 21 (9): 1148–1160. arXiv:1807.11819. Bibcode:2018arXiv180711819K. doi:10.1038/s41593-018-0210-5. ISSN 1546-1726. PMC 6706072. PMID 30127428.
  8. ^ Paolo, E. D., "Organismically-inspired robotics: homeostatic adaptation and teleology beyond the closed sensorimotor loop", Dynamical Systems Approach to Embodiment and Sociality, S2CID 15349751
  9. ^ Brooks, R.; Hassabis, D.; Bray, D.; Shashua, A. (2012-02-22). "Turing centenary: Is the brain a good model for machine intelligence?". Nature. 482 (7386): 462–463. Bibcode:2012Natur.482..462.. doi:10.1038/482462a. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 22358812. S2CID 205070106.
  10. ^ Browne, A. (1997-01-01). Neural Network Perspectives on Cognition and Adaptive Robotics. CRC Press. ISBN 9780750304559.
  11. ^ Zorzi, Marco; Testolin, Alberto; Stoianov, Ivilin P. (2013-08-20). "Modeling language and cognition with deep unsupervised learning: a tutorial overview". Frontiers in Psychology. 4: 515. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00515. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 3747356. PMID 23970869.
  12. ^ Shai, Adam; Larkum, Matthew Evan (2017-12-05). "Branching into brains". eLife. 6. doi:10.7554/eLife.33066. ISSN 2050-084X. PMC 5716658. PMID 29205152.
  13. ^ Schwartz, Eric (1990). Computational neuroscience. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-19291-0.
  14. ^ Bower, James M. (2013). 20 years of Computational neuroscience. Berlin, Germany: Springer. ISBN 978-1461414230.
  15. ^ Lapicque L (1907). "Recherches quantitatives sur l'excitation électrique des nerfs traitée comme une polarisation". J. Physiol. Pathol. Gen. 9: 620–635.
  16. ^ Brunel N, Van Rossum MC (2007). "Lapicque's 1907 paper: from frogs to integrate-and-fire". Biol. Cybern. 97 (5–6): 337–339. doi:10.1007/s00422-007-0190-0. PMID 17968583. S2CID 17816096.
  17. ^ Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1962). "Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex". J. Physiol. 160 (1): 106–54. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1962.sp006837. PMC 1359523. PMID 14449617.
  18. ^ Forrest MD (2014). "Intracellular Calcium Dynamics Permit a Purkinje Neuron Model to Perform Toggle and Gain Computations Upon its Inputs". Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 8: 86. doi:10.3389/fncom.2014.00086. PMC 4138505. PMID 25191262.
  19. ^ Wu, Samuel Miao-sin; Johnston, Daniel (1995). Foundations of cellular neurophysiology. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-10053-3.
  20. ^ Koch, Christof (1999). Biophysics of computation: information processing in single neurons. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-510491-2.
  21. ^ Basnayake, Kanishka; Mazaud, David; Bemelmans, Alexis; Rouach, Nathalie; Korkotian, Eduard; Holcman, David (2019-06-04). "Fast calcium transients in dendritic spines driven by extreme statistics". PLOS Biology. 17 (6): e2006202. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.2006202. ISSN 1545-7885. PMC 6548358. PMID 31163024.
  22. ^ Basnayake, Kanishka; Mazaud, David; Kushnireva, Lilia; Bemelmans, Alexis; Rouach, Nathalie; Korkotian, Eduard; Holcman, David (2021-09-17). "Nanoscale molecular architecture controls calcium diffusion and ER replenishment in dendritic spines". Science Advances. 7 (38): eabh1376. Bibcode:2021SciA....7.1376B. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abh1376. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 8443180. PMID 34524854.
  23. ^ Freche, Dominik; Pannasch, Ulrike; Rouach, Nathalie; Holcman, David (2011-10-03). "Synapse Geometry and Receptor Dynamics Modulate Synaptic Strength". PLOS ONE. 6 (10): e25122. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...625122F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025122. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3184958. PMID 21984900.
  24. ^ Forrest MD (2014). "Intracellular Calcium Dynamics Permit a Purkinje Neuron Model to Perform Toggle and Gain Computations Upon its Inputs". Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience. 8: 86. doi:10.3389/fncom.2014.00086. PMC 4138505. PMID 25191262.
  25. ^ Forrest MD (April 2015). "Simulation of alcohol action upon a detailed Purkinje neuron model and a simpler surrogate model that runs >400 times faster". BMC Neuroscience. 16 (27): 27. doi:10.1186/s12868-015-0162-6. PMC 4417229. PMID 25928094.
  26. ^ "Dynamics of Ion Fluxes between Neurons, Astrocytes and the Extracellular Space during Neurotransmission". cyberleninka.ru. Retrieved 2023-03-14.
  27. ^ Sibille, Jérémie; Duc, Khanh Dao; Holcman, David; Rouach, Nathalie (2015-03-31). "The Neuroglial Potassium Cycle during Neurotransmission: Role of Kir4.1 Channels". PLOS Computational Biology. 11 (3): e1004137. Bibcode:2015PLSCB..11E4137S. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004137. ISSN 1553-7358. PMC 4380507. PMID 25826753.
  28. ^ Pannasch, Ulrike; Freche, Dominik; Dallérac, Glenn; Ghézali, Grégory; Escartin, Carole; Ezan, Pascal; Cohen-Salmon, Martine; Benchenane, Karim; Abudara, Veronica; Dufour, Amandine; Lübke, Joachim H. R.; Déglon, Nicole; Knott, Graham; Holcman, David; Rouach, Nathalie (April 2014). "Connexin 30 sets synaptic strength by controlling astroglial synapse invasion". Nature Neuroscience. 17 (4): 549–558. doi:10.1038/nn.3662. ISSN 1546-1726. PMID 24584052. S2CID 554918.
  29. ^ Chklovskii DB, Mel BW, Svoboda K (October 2004). "Cortical rewiring and information storage". Nature. 431 (7010): 782–8. Bibcode:2004Natur.431..782C. doi:10.1038/nature03012. PMID 15483599. S2CID 4430167.
    Review article
  30. ^ Zhaoping L. 2014, The efficient coding principle , chapter 3, of the textbook Understanding vision: theory, models, and data
  31. ^ see visual spational attention https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Visual_spatial_attention
  32. ^ a b Li. Z. 2002 A saliency map in primary visual cortex Trends in Cognitive Sciences vol. 6, Pages 9-16, and Zhaoping, L. 2014, The V1 hypothesis—creating a bottom-up saliency map for preattentive selection and segmentation in the book Understanding Vision: Theory, Models, and Data
  33. ^ Weiss, Yair; Simoncelli, Eero P.; Adelson, Edward H. (20 May 2002). "Motion illusions as optimal percepts". Nature Neuroscience. 5 (6): 598–604. doi:10.1038/nn0602-858. PMID 12021763. S2CID 2777968.
  34. ^ Ernst, Marc O.; Bülthoff, Heinrich H. (April 2004). "Merging the senses into a robust percept". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 8 (4): 162–169. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.299.4638. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.002. PMID 15050512. S2CID 7837073.
  35. ^ Durstewitz D, Seamans JK, Sejnowski TJ (2000). "Neurocomputational models of working memory". Nat. Neurosci. 3 (Suppl): 1184–91. doi:10.1038/81460. PMID 11127836. S2CID 8204235.
  36. ^ Frank, M. J.; Loughry, B.; O'Reilly, R. C. (2001). "Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory: A computational model" (PDF). Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience. 1 (2): 137–160. doi:10.3758/cabn.1.2.137. PMID 12467110. S2CID 964100. Retrieved 2018-12-06.
  37. ^ Fusi S, Drew PJ, Abbott LF (2005). "Cascade models of synaptically stored memories". Neuron. 45 (4): 599–611. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2005.02.001. PMID 15721245. S2CID 2981869.
  38. ^ Coggan JS, Bartol TM, Esquenazi E, et al. (2005). "Evidence for ectopic neurotransmission at a neuronal synapse". Science. 309 (5733): 446–51. Bibcode:2005Sci...309..446C. doi:10.1126/science.1108239. PMC 2915764. PMID 16020730.
  39. ^ Olshausen, Bruno A.; Field, David J. (1997-12-01). "Sparse coding with an overcomplete basis set: A strategy employed by V1?". Vision Research. 37 (23): 3311–3325. doi:10.1016/S0042-6989(97)00169-7. PMID 9425546. S2CID 14208692.
  40. ^ Schneidman E, Berry MJ, Segev R, Bialek W (2006). "Weak pairwise correlations imply strongly correlated network states in a neural population". Nature. 440 (7087): 1007–12. arXiv:q-bio/0512013. Bibcode:2006Natur.440.1007S. doi:10.1038/nature04701. PMC 1785327. PMID 16625187.
  41. ^ Wilson, H. R.; Cowan, J.D. (1973). "A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic nervous tissue". Kybernetik. 13 (2): 55–80. doi:10.1007/BF00288786. PMID 4767470. S2CID 292546.
  42. ^ Anderson, Charles H.; Eliasmith, Chris (2004). Neural Engineering: Computation, Representation, and Dynamics in Neurobiological Systems (Computational Neuroscience). Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-55060-4.
  43. ^ Marvin M. Chun; Jeremy M. Wolfe; E. B. Goldstein (2001). Blackwell Handbook of Sensation and Perception. Blackwell Publishing Ltd. pp. 272–310. ISBN 978-0-631-20684-2.
  44. ^ Edmund Rolls; Gustavo Deco (2012). Computational Neuroscience of Vision. Oxford Scholarship Online. ISBN 978-0-198-52488-5.
  45. ^ Machens CK, Romo R, Brody CD (2005). "Flexible control of mutual inhibition: a neural model of two-interval discrimination". Science. 307 (5712): 1121–4. Bibcode:2005Sci...307.1121M. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.523.4396. doi:10.1126/science.1104171. PMID 15718474. S2CID 45378154.
  46. ^ Robinson PA, Rennie CJ, Rowe DL, O'Connor SC, Gordon E (2005). "Multiscale brain modelling". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 360 (1457): 1043–1050. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1638. PMC 1854922. PMID 16087447.
  47. ^ Crick F, Koch C (2003). "A framework for consciousness". Nat. Neurosci. 6 (2): 119–26. doi:10.1038/nn0203-119. PMID 12555104. S2CID 13960489.
  48. ^ Crick, Francis (1994). The Astonishing Hypothesis: The Scientific Search for the Soul. New York: Scribner.
  49. ^ Crick, Francis; Koch, Christopher (1998). Consciousness and neuroscience. Cerebral Cortex. Vol. 8. Essential Sources In The Scientific Study of Consciousness. Roscience. pp. 97–107. doi:10.1093/cercor/8.2.97. PMID 9542889.
  50. ^ Adaszewski, Stanisław; Dukart, Juergen; Kherif, Ferath; Frackowiak, Richard; Draganski, Bogdan; Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative (2013). "How early can we predict Alzheimer's disease using computational anatomy?". Neurobiol Aging. 34 (12): 2815–26. doi:10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.06.015. PMID 23890839. S2CID 1025210.
  51. ^ Friston KJ, Stephan KE, Montague R, Dolan RJ (2014). "Computational psychiatry: the brain as a phantastic organ". Lancet Psychiatry. 1 (2): 148–58. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70275-5. PMID 26360579. S2CID 15504512.
  52. ^ Floyrac, Aymeric; Doumergue, Adrien; Legriel, Stéphane; Deye, Nicolas; Megarbane, Bruno; Richard, Alexandra; Meppiel, Elodie; Masmoudi, Sana; Lozeron, Pierre; Vicaut, Eric; Kubis, Nathalie; Holcman, David (2023). "Predicting neurological outcome after cardiac arrest by combining computational parameters extracted from standard and deviant responses from auditory evoked potentials". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 17: 988394. doi:10.3389/fnins.2023.988394. ISSN 1662-453X. PMC 9975713. PMID 36875664.
  53. ^ Sun, Christophe; Holcman, David (2022-08-01). "Combining transient statistical markers from the EEG signal to predict brain sensitivity to general anesthesia". Biomedical Signal Processing and Control. 77: 103713. doi:10.1016/j.bspc.2022.103713. ISSN 1746-8094. S2CID 248488365.
  54. ^ Montague, P. Read; Dolan, Raymond J.; Friston, Karl J.; Dayan, Peter (14 Dec 2011). "Computational psychiatry". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 16 (1): 72–80. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.018. PMC 3556822. PMID 22177032.
  55. ^ Kato, Ayaka; Kunisato, Yoshihiko; Katahira, Kentaro; Okimura, Tsukasa; Yamashita, Yuichi (2020). "Computational Psychiatry Research Map (CPSYMAP): a new database for visualizing research papers". Frontiers in Psychiatry. 11 (1360): 578706. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2020.578706. PMC 7746554. PMID 33343418.
  56. ^ Huys, Quentin J M; Maia, Tiago V; Frank, Michael J (2016). "Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications". Nature Neuroscience. 19 (3): 404–413. doi:10.1038/nn.4238. PMC 5443409. PMID 26906507.
  57. ^ Russell, John (21 March 2016). "Beyond von Neumann, Neuromorphic Computing Steadily Advances".
  58. ^ Calimera, Andrea; Macii, Enrico; Poncino, Massimo (2013-08-20). "The human brain project and neuromorphic computing". Functional Neurology. 28 (3): 191–196. doi:10.11138/FNeur/2013.28.3.191 (inactive 2023-04-11). PMC 3812737. PMID 24139655.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of April 2023 (link)

Bibliography

  • Chklovskii DB (2004). "Synaptic connectivity and neuronal morphology: two sides of the same coin". Neuron. 43 (5): 609–17. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.08.012. PMID 15339643. S2CID 16217065.
  • Sejnowski, Terrence J.; Churchland, Patricia Smith (1992). The computational brain. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-03188-2.
  • Gerstner, W.; Kistler, W.; Naud, R.; Paninski, L. (2014). Neuronal Dynamics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9781107447615.
  • Dayan P.; Abbott, L. F. (2001). Theoretical neuroscience: computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-04199-7.
  • Eliasmith, Chris; Anderson, Charles H. (2003). Neural engineering: Representation, computation, and dynamics in neurobiological systems. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-05071-5.
  • Hodgkin AL, Huxley AF (28 August 1952). "A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve". J. Physiol. 117 (4): 500–44. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1952.sp004764. PMC 1392413. PMID 12991237.
  • William Bialek; Rieke, Fred; David Warland; Rob de Ruyter van Steveninck (1999). Spikes: exploring the neural code. Cambridge, Mass: MIT. ISBN 978-0-262-68108-7.
  • Schutter, Erik de (2001). Computational neuroscience: realistic modeling for experimentalists. Boca Raton: CRC. ISBN 978-0-8493-2068-2.
  • Sejnowski, Terrence J.; Hemmen, J. L. van (2006). 23 problems in systems neuroscience. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-514822-0.
  • Michael A. Arbib; Shun-ichi Amari; Prudence H. Arbib (2002). The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks. Cambridge, Massachusetts: The MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-01197-6.
  • Zhaoping, Li (2014). Understanding vision: theory, models, and data. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0199564668.

See also

Software

  • BRIAN, a Python based simulator
  • Budapest Reference Connectome, web based 3D visualization tool to browse connections in the human brain
  • Emergent, neural simulation software.
  • GENESIS, a general neural simulation system.
  • NEST is a simulator for spiking neural network models that focuses on the dynamics, size and structure of neural systems rather than on the exact morphology of individual neurons.

External links

Journals

  • Journal of Mathematical Neuroscience
  • Journal of Computational Neuroscience
  • Neural Computation
  • Cognitive Neurodynamics
  • Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience
  • PLoS Computational Biology
  • Frontiers in Neuroinformatics

Conferences

  • Computational and Systems Neuroscience (COSYNE) – a computational neuroscience meeting with a systems neuroscience focus.
  • Annual Computational Neuroscience Meeting (CNS) – a yearly computational neuroscience meeting.
  • Computational Cognitive Neuroscience - a yearly computational neuroscience meeting with a focus on cognitive phenomena.
  • Neural Information Processing Systems (NIPS)– a leading annual conference covering mostly machine learning.
  • Cognitive Computational Neuroscience (CCN) – a computational neuroscience meeting focusing on computational models capable of cognitive tasks.
  • – a yearly conference.
  • – a yearly conference, focused on mathematical aspects.
  • – a yearly computational neuroscience conference ].
  • AREADNE Conferences– a biennial meeting that includes theoretical and experimental results.

Websites

  • Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience, part of Scholarpedia, an online expert curated encyclopedia on computational neuroscience and dynamical systems

computational, neuroscience, this, article, technical, most, readers, understand, please, help, improve, make, understandable, experts, without, removing, technical, details, march, 2014, learn, when, remove, this, template, message, also, known, theoretical, . This article may be too technical for most readers to understand Please help improve it to make it understandable to non experts without removing the technical details March 2014 Learn how and when to remove this template message Computational neuroscience also known as theoretical neuroscience or mathematical neuroscience is a branch of neuroscience which employs mathematical models computer simulations theoretical analysis and abstractions of the brain to understand the principles that govern the development structure physiology and cognitive abilities of the nervous system 1 2 3 4 Computational neuroscience employs computational simulations to validate and solve mathematical models and so can be seen as a sub field of theoretical neuroscience however the two fields are often synonymous 5 The term mathematical neuroscience is also used sometimes to stress the quantitative nature of the field 6 Computational neuroscience focuses on the description of biologically plausible neurons and neural systems and their physiology and dynamics and it is therefore not directly concerned with biologically unrealistic models used in connectionism control theory cybernetics quantitative psychology machine learning artificial neural networks artificial intelligence and computational learning theory 7 8 9 although mutual inspiration exists and sometimes there is no strict limit between fields 10 11 12 with model abstraction in computational neuroscience depending on research scope and the granularity at which biological entities are analyzed Models in theoretical neuroscience are aimed at capturing the essential features of the biological system at multiple spatial temporal scales from membrane currents and chemical coupling via network oscillations columnar and topographic architecture nuclei all the way up to psychological faculties like memory learning and behavior These computational models frame hypotheses that can be directly tested by biological or psychological experiments Contents 1 History 2 Major topics 2 1 Single neuron modeling 2 2 Modeling Neuron glia interactions 2 3 Development axonal patterning and guidance 2 4 Sensory processing 2 5 Motor control 2 6 Memory and synaptic plasticity 2 7 Behaviors of networks 2 8 Visual attention identification and categorization 2 9 Cognition discrimination and learning 2 10 Consciousness 2 11 Computational clinical neuroscience 2 12 Predictive computational neuroscience 2 13 Computational Psychiatry 3 Technology 3 1 Neuromorphic computing 4 See also 5 Notes and references 6 Bibliography 7 See also 7 1 Software 8 External links 8 1 Journals 8 2 Conferences 8 3 WebsitesHistory EditThe term computational neuroscience was introduced by Eric L Schwartz who organized a conference held in 1985 in Carmel California at the request of the Systems Development Foundation to provide a summary of the current status of a field which until that point was referred to by a variety of names such as neural modeling brain theory and neural networks The proceedings of this definitional meeting were published in 1990 as the book Computational Neuroscience 13 The first of the annual open international meetings focused on Computational Neuroscience was organized by James M Bower and John Miller in San Francisco California in 1989 14 The first graduate educational program in computational neuroscience was organized as the Computational and Neural Systems Ph D program at the California Institute of Technology in 1985 The early historical roots of the field can be traced to the work of people including Louis Lapicque Hodgkin amp Huxley Hubel and Wiesel and David Marr Lapicque introduced the integrate and fire model of the neuron in a seminal article published in 1907 15 a model still popular for artificial neural networks studies because of its simplicity see a recent review 16 About 40 years later Hodgkin and Huxley developed the voltage clamp and created the first biophysical model of the action potential Hubel and Wiesel discovered that neurons in the primary visual cortex the first cortical area to process information coming from the retina have oriented receptive fields and are organized in columns 17 David Marr s work focused on the interactions between neurons suggesting computational approaches to the study of how functional groups of neurons within the hippocampus and neocortex interact store process and transmit information Computational modeling of biophysically realistic neurons and dendrites began with the work of Wilfrid Rall with the first multicompartmental model using cable theory Major topics EditResearch in computational neuroscience can be roughly categorized into several lines of inquiry Most computational neuroscientists collaborate closely with experimentalists in analyzing novel data and synthesizing new models of biological phenomena Single neuron modeling Edit Main article Biological neuron models Even a single neuron has complex biophysical characteristics and can perform computations e g 18 Hodgkin and Huxley s original model only employed two voltage sensitive currents Voltage sensitive ion channels are glycoprotein molecules which extend through the lipid bilayer allowing ions to traverse under certain conditions through the axolemma the fast acting sodium and the inward rectifying potassium Though successful in predicting the timing and qualitative features of the action potential it nevertheless failed to predict a number of important features such as adaptation and shunting Scientists now believe that there are a wide variety of voltage sensitive currents and the implications of the differing dynamics modulations and sensitivity of these currents is an important topic of computational neuroscience 19 The computational functions of complex dendrites are also under intense investigation There is a large body of literature regarding how different currents interact with geometric properties of neurons 20 Some models are also tracking biochemical pathways at very small scales such as dendritic spines 21 22 or synaptic clefts 23 There are many software packages such as GENESIS and NEURON that allow rapid and systematic in silico modeling of realistic neurons Blue Brain a project founded by Henry Markram from the Ecole Polytechnique Federale de Lausanne aims to construct a biophysically detailed simulation of a cortical column on the Blue Gene supercomputer Modeling the richness of biophysical properties on the single neuron scale can supply mechanisms that serve as the building blocks for network dynamics 24 However detailed neuron descriptions are computationally expensive and this computing cost can limit the pursuit of realistic network investigations where many neurons need to be simulated As a result researchers that study large neural circuits typically represent each neuron and synapse with an artificially simple model ignoring much of the biological detail Hence there is a drive to produce simplified neuron models that can retain significant biological fidelity at a low computational overhead Algorithms have been developed to produce faithful faster running simplified surrogate neuron models from computationally expensive detailed neuron models 25 Modeling Neuron glia interactions Edit Glial cells participate significantly to the regulation of neuronal activity at a cellular but also at a network level Modeling this interaction allows to clarify the potassium cycle 26 27 so important for maintaining homeostatis and to prevent epileptic seizures Modeling reveals the role of glial protrusions that can penetrate in some cases the synaptic cleft to interfere with the synpatic transmission and thus control synaptic communication 28 Development axonal patterning and guidance Edit Computational neuroscience aims to address a wide array of questions How do axons and dendrites form during development How do axons know where to target and how to reach these targets How do neurons migrate to the proper position in the central and peripheral systems How do synapses form We know from molecular biology that distinct parts of the nervous system release distinct chemical cues from growth factors to hormones that modulate and influence the growth and development of functional connections between neurons Theoretical investigations into the formation and patterning of synaptic connection and morphology are still nascent One hypothesis that has recently garnered some attention is the minimal wiring hypothesis which postulates that the formation of axons and dendrites effectively minimizes resource allocation while maintaining maximal information storage 29 Sensory processing Edit Early models on sensory processing understood within a theoretical framework are credited to Horace Barlow Somewhat similar to the minimal wiring hypothesis described in the preceding section Barlow understood the processing of the early sensory systems to be a form of efficient coding where the neurons encoded information which minimized the number of spikes Experimental and computational work have since supported this hypothesis in one form or another For the example of visual processing efficient coding is manifested in the forms of efficient spatial coding color coding temporal motion coding stereo coding and combinations of them 30 Further along the visual pathway even the efficiently coded visual information is too much for the capacity of the information bottleneck the visual attentional bottleneck 31 A subsequent theory V1 Saliency Hypothesis V1SH has been developed on exogenous attentional selection of a fraction of visual input for further processing guided by a bottom up saliency map in the primary visual cortex 32 Current research in sensory processing is divided among a biophysical modelling of different subsystems and a more theoretical modelling of perception Current models of perception have suggested that the brain performs some form of Bayesian inference and integration of different sensory information in generating our perception of the physical world 33 34 Motor control Edit Many models of the way the brain controls movement have been developed This includes models of processing in the brain such as the cerebellum s role for error correction skill learning in motor cortex and the basal ganglia or the control of the vestibulo ocular reflex This also includes many normative models such as those of the Bayesian or optimal control flavor which are built on the idea that the brain efficiently solves its problems Memory and synaptic plasticity Edit Main article Synaptic plasticity Earlier models of memory are primarily based on the postulates of Hebbian learning Biologically relevant models such as Hopfield net have been developed to address the properties of associative also known as content addressable style of memory that occur in biological systems These attempts are primarily focusing on the formation of medium and long term memory localizing in the hippocampus Models of working memory relying on theories of network oscillations and persistent activity have been built to capture some features of the prefrontal cortex in context related memory 35 Additional models look at the close relationship between the basal ganglia and the prefrontal cortex and how that contributes to working memory 36 One of the major problems in neurophysiological memory is how it is maintained and changed through multiple time scales Unstable synapses are easy to train but also prone to stochastic disruption Stable synapses forget less easily but they are also harder to consolidate One recent computational hypothesis involves cascades of plasticity that allow synapses to function at multiple time scales 37 Stereochemically detailed models of the acetylcholine receptor based synapse with the Monte Carlo method working at the time scale of microseconds have been built 38 It is likely that computational tools will contribute greatly to our understanding of how synapses function and change in relation to external stimulus in the coming decades Behaviors of networks Edit Biological neurons are connected to each other in a complex recurrent fashion These connections are unlike most artificial neural networks sparse and usually specific It is not known how information is transmitted through such sparsely connected networks although specific areas of the brain such as the visual cortex are understood in some detail 39 It is also unknown what the computational functions of these specific connectivity patterns are if any The interactions of neurons in a small network can be often reduced to simple models such as the Ising model The statistical mechanics of such simple systems are well characterized theoretically Some recent evidence suggests that dynamics of arbitrary neuronal networks can be reduced to pairwise interactions 40 It is not known however whether such descriptive dynamics impart any important computational function With the emergence of two photon microscopy and calcium imaging we now have powerful experimental methods with which to test the new theories regarding neuronal networks In some cases the complex interactions between inhibitory and excitatory neurons can be simplified using mean field theory which gives rise to the population model of neural networks 41 While many neurotheorists prefer such models with reduced complexity others argue that uncovering structural functional relations depends on including as much neuronal and network structure as possible Models of this type are typically built in large simulation platforms like GENESIS or NEURON There have been some attempts to provide unified methods that bridge and integrate these levels of complexity 42 Visual attention identification and categorization Edit Visual attention can be described as a set of mechanisms that limit some processing to a subset of incoming stimuli 43 Attentional mechanisms shape what we see and what we can act upon They allow for concurrent selection of some preferably relevant information and inhibition of other information In order to have a more concrete specification of the mechanism underlying visual attention and the binding of features a number of computational models have been proposed aiming to explain psychophysical findings In general all models postulate the existence of a saliency or priority map for registering the potentially interesting areas of the retinal input and a gating mechanism for reducing the amount of incoming visual information so that the limited computational resources of the brain can handle it 44 An example theory that is being extensively tested behaviorally and physiologically is the V1 Saliency Hypothesis that a bottom up saliency map is created in the primary visual cortex to guide attention exogenously 32 Computational neuroscience provides a mathematical framework for studying the mechanisms involved in brain function and allows complete simulation and prediction of neuropsychological syndromes Cognition discrimination and learning Edit Computational modeling of higher cognitive functions has only recently when begun Experimental data comes primarily from single unit recording in primates The frontal lobe and parietal lobe function as integrators of information from multiple sensory modalities There are some tentative ideas regarding how simple mutually inhibitory functional circuits in these areas may carry out biologically relevant computation 45 The brain seems to be able to discriminate and adapt particularly well in certain contexts For instance human beings seem to have an enormous capacity for memorizing and recognizing faces One of the key goals of computational neuroscience is to dissect how biological systems carry out these complex computations efficiently and potentially replicate these processes in building intelligent machines The brain s large scale organizational principles are illuminated by many fields including biology psychology and clinical practice Integrative neuroscience attempts to consolidate these observations through unified descriptive models and databases of behavioral measures and recordings These are the bases for some quantitative modeling of large scale brain activity 46 The Computational Representational Understanding of Mind CRUM is another attempt at modeling human cognition through simulated processes like acquired rule based systems in decision making and the manipulation of visual representations in decision making Consciousness Edit One of the ultimate goals of psychology neuroscience is to be able to explain the everyday experience of conscious life Francis Crick Giulio Tononi and Christof Koch made some attempts to formulate consistent frameworks for future work in neural correlates of consciousness NCC though much of the work in this field remains speculative 47 Specifically Crick 48 cautioned the field of neuroscience to not approach topics that are traditionally left to philosophy and religion 49 Computational clinical neuroscience Edit Computational clinical neuroscience is a field that brings together experts in neuroscience neurology psychiatry decision sciences and computational modeling to quantitatively define and investigate problems in neurological and psychiatric diseases and to train scientists and clinicians that wish to apply these models to diagnosis and treatment 50 51 Predictive computational neuroscience Edit Predictive computational neuroscience is a recent field that combines signal processing neuroscience clinical data and machine learning to predict the brain during coma 52 or anesthesia 53 For exmaple it is possible to anticipate deep brain states using the EEG signal These states can be used to anticipate hypnotic concentration to administrate to the patient Computational Psychiatry Edit Computational psychiatry is a new emerging field that brings together experts in machine learning neuroscience neurology psychiatry psychology to provide an understanding of psychiatric disorders 54 55 56 Technology EditNeuromorphic computing Edit This section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Computational neuroscience news newspapers books scholar JSTOR October 2021 Learn how and when to remove this template message Main article Neuromorphic engineering A neuromorphic computer chip is any device that uses physical artificial neurons made from silicon to do computations See neuromorphic computing physical neural network One of the advantages of using a physical model computer such as this is that it takes the computational load of the processor in the sense that the structural and some of the functional elements don t have to be programmed since they are in hardware In recent times 57 neuromorphic technology has been used to build supercomputers which are used in international neuroscience collaborations Examples include the Human Brain Project SpiNNaker supercomputer and the BrainScaleS computer 58 See also EditAction potential Biological neuron models Bayesian brain Brain simulation Computational anatomy Connectomics Differentiable programming Electrophysiology FitzHugh Nagumo model Galves Locherbach model Goldman equation Hodgkin Huxley model Information theory Mathematical model Nonlinear dynamics Neural coding Neural decoding Neural oscillation Neuroinformatics Neuroplasticity Neurophysiology Noogenesis Systems neuroscience Theoretical biology Theta modelNotes and references Edit Trappenberg Thomas P 2010 Fundamentals of Computational Neuroscience United States Oxford University Press Inc pp 2 ISBN 978 0 19 851582 1 Patricia S Churchland Christof Koch Terrence J Sejnowski 1993 What is computational neuroscience In Eric L Schwartz ed Computational Neuroscience MIT Press pp 46 55 Archived from the original on 2011 06 04 Retrieved 2009 06 11 Dayan P Abbott L F 2001 Theoretical neuroscience computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 04199 7 Gerstner W Kistler W Naud R Paninski L 2014 Neuronal Dynamics Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press ISBN 9781107447615 Thomas Trappenberg 2010 Fundamentals of Computational Neuroscience OUP Oxford p 2 ISBN 978 0199568413 Retrieved 17 January 2017 Gutkin Boris Pinto David Ermentrout Bard 2003 03 01 Mathematical neuroscience from neurons to circuits to systems Journal of Physiology Paris Neurogeometry and visual perception 97 2 209 219 doi 10 1016 j jphysparis 2003 09 005 ISSN 0928 4257 PMID 14766142 S2CID 10040483 Kriegeskorte Nikolaus Douglas Pamela K September 2018 Cognitive computational neuroscience Nature Neuroscience 21 9 1148 1160 arXiv 1807 11819 Bibcode 2018arXiv180711819K doi 10 1038 s41593 018 0210 5 ISSN 1546 1726 PMC 6706072 PMID 30127428 Paolo E D Organismically inspired robotics homeostatic adaptation and teleology beyond the closed sensorimotor loop Dynamical Systems Approach to Embodiment and Sociality S2CID 15349751 Brooks R Hassabis D Bray D Shashua A 2012 02 22 Turing centenary Is the brain a good model for machine intelligence Nature 482 7386 462 463 Bibcode 2012Natur 482 462 doi 10 1038 482462a ISSN 0028 0836 PMID 22358812 S2CID 205070106 Browne A 1997 01 01 Neural Network Perspectives on Cognition and Adaptive Robotics CRC Press ISBN 9780750304559 Zorzi Marco Testolin Alberto Stoianov Ivilin P 2013 08 20 Modeling language and cognition with deep unsupervised learning a tutorial overview Frontiers in Psychology 4 515 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2013 00515 ISSN 1664 1078 PMC 3747356 PMID 23970869 Shai Adam Larkum Matthew Evan 2017 12 05 Branching into brains eLife 6 doi 10 7554 eLife 33066 ISSN 2050 084X PMC 5716658 PMID 29205152 Schwartz Eric 1990 Computational neuroscience Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 19291 0 Bower James M 2013 20 years of Computational neuroscience Berlin Germany Springer ISBN 978 1461414230 Lapicque L 1907 Recherches quantitatives sur l excitation electrique des nerfs traitee comme une polarisation J Physiol Pathol Gen 9 620 635 Brunel N Van Rossum MC 2007 Lapicque s 1907 paper from frogs to integrate and fire Biol Cybern 97 5 6 337 339 doi 10 1007 s00422 007 0190 0 PMID 17968583 S2CID 17816096 Hubel DH Wiesel TN 1962 Receptive fields binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat s visual cortex J Physiol 160 1 106 54 doi 10 1113 jphysiol 1962 sp006837 PMC 1359523 PMID 14449617 Forrest MD 2014 Intracellular Calcium Dynamics Permit a Purkinje Neuron Model to Perform Toggle and Gain Computations Upon its Inputs Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 8 86 doi 10 3389 fncom 2014 00086 PMC 4138505 PMID 25191262 Wu Samuel Miao sin Johnston Daniel 1995 Foundations of cellular neurophysiology Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 10053 3 Koch Christof 1999 Biophysics of computation information processing in single neurons Oxford Oxfordshire Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 510491 2 Basnayake Kanishka Mazaud David Bemelmans Alexis Rouach Nathalie Korkotian Eduard Holcman David 2019 06 04 Fast calcium transients in dendritic spines driven by extreme statistics PLOS Biology 17 6 e2006202 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 2006202 ISSN 1545 7885 PMC 6548358 PMID 31163024 Basnayake Kanishka Mazaud David Kushnireva Lilia Bemelmans Alexis Rouach Nathalie Korkotian Eduard Holcman David 2021 09 17 Nanoscale molecular architecture controls calcium diffusion and ER replenishment in dendritic spines Science Advances 7 38 eabh1376 Bibcode 2021SciA 7 1376B doi 10 1126 sciadv abh1376 ISSN 2375 2548 PMC 8443180 PMID 34524854 Freche Dominik Pannasch Ulrike Rouach Nathalie Holcman David 2011 10 03 Synapse Geometry and Receptor Dynamics Modulate Synaptic Strength PLOS ONE 6 10 e25122 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 625122F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0025122 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3184958 PMID 21984900 Forrest MD 2014 Intracellular Calcium Dynamics Permit a Purkinje Neuron Model to Perform Toggle and Gain Computations Upon its Inputs Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience 8 86 doi 10 3389 fncom 2014 00086 PMC 4138505 PMID 25191262 Forrest MD April 2015 Simulation of alcohol action upon a detailed Purkinje neuron model and a simpler surrogate model that runs gt 400 times faster BMC Neuroscience 16 27 27 doi 10 1186 s12868 015 0162 6 PMC 4417229 PMID 25928094 Dynamics of Ion Fluxes between Neurons Astrocytes and the Extracellular Space during Neurotransmission cyberleninka ru Retrieved 2023 03 14 Sibille Jeremie Duc Khanh Dao Holcman David Rouach Nathalie 2015 03 31 The Neuroglial Potassium Cycle during Neurotransmission Role of Kir4 1 Channels PLOS Computational Biology 11 3 e1004137 Bibcode 2015PLSCB 11E4137S doi 10 1371 journal pcbi 1004137 ISSN 1553 7358 PMC 4380507 PMID 25826753 Pannasch Ulrike Freche Dominik Dallerac Glenn Ghezali Gregory Escartin Carole Ezan Pascal Cohen Salmon Martine Benchenane Karim Abudara Veronica Dufour Amandine Lubke Joachim H R Deglon Nicole Knott Graham Holcman David Rouach Nathalie April 2014 Connexin 30 sets synaptic strength by controlling astroglial synapse invasion Nature Neuroscience 17 4 549 558 doi 10 1038 nn 3662 ISSN 1546 1726 PMID 24584052 S2CID 554918 Chklovskii DB Mel BW Svoboda K October 2004 Cortical rewiring and information storage Nature 431 7010 782 8 Bibcode 2004Natur 431 782C doi 10 1038 nature03012 PMID 15483599 S2CID 4430167 Review article Zhaoping L 2014 The efficient coding principle chapter 3 of the textbook Understanding vision theory models and data see visual spational attention https en wikipedia org wiki Visual spatial attention a b Li Z 2002 A saliency map in primary visual cortex Trends in Cognitive Sciences vol 6 Pages 9 16 and Zhaoping L 2014 The V1 hypothesis creating a bottom up saliency map for preattentive selection and segmentation in the book Understanding Vision Theory Models and Data Weiss Yair Simoncelli Eero P Adelson Edward H 20 May 2002 Motion illusions as optimal percepts Nature Neuroscience 5 6 598 604 doi 10 1038 nn0602 858 PMID 12021763 S2CID 2777968 Ernst Marc O Bulthoff Heinrich H April 2004 Merging the senses into a robust percept Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8 4 162 169 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 299 4638 doi 10 1016 j tics 2004 02 002 PMID 15050512 S2CID 7837073 Durstewitz D Seamans JK Sejnowski TJ 2000 Neurocomputational models of working memory Nat Neurosci 3 Suppl 1184 91 doi 10 1038 81460 PMID 11127836 S2CID 8204235 Frank M J Loughry B O Reilly R C 2001 Interactions between frontal cortex and basal ganglia in working memory A computational model PDF Cognitive Affective amp Behavioral Neuroscience 1 2 137 160 doi 10 3758 cabn 1 2 137 PMID 12467110 S2CID 964100 Retrieved 2018 12 06 Fusi S Drew PJ Abbott LF 2005 Cascade models of synaptically stored memories Neuron 45 4 599 611 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2005 02 001 PMID 15721245 S2CID 2981869 Coggan JS Bartol TM Esquenazi E et al 2005 Evidence for ectopic neurotransmission at a neuronal synapse Science 309 5733 446 51 Bibcode 2005Sci 309 446C doi 10 1126 science 1108239 PMC 2915764 PMID 16020730 Olshausen Bruno A Field David J 1997 12 01 Sparse coding with an overcomplete basis set A strategy employed by V1 Vision Research 37 23 3311 3325 doi 10 1016 S0042 6989 97 00169 7 PMID 9425546 S2CID 14208692 Schneidman E Berry MJ Segev R Bialek W 2006 Weak pairwise correlations imply strongly correlated network states in a neural population Nature 440 7087 1007 12 arXiv q bio 0512013 Bibcode 2006Natur 440 1007S doi 10 1038 nature04701 PMC 1785327 PMID 16625187 Wilson H R Cowan J D 1973 A mathematical theory of the functional dynamics of cortical and thalamic nervous tissue Kybernetik 13 2 55 80 doi 10 1007 BF00288786 PMID 4767470 S2CID 292546 Anderson Charles H Eliasmith Chris 2004 Neural Engineering Computation Representation and Dynamics in Neurobiological Systems Computational Neuroscience Cambridge Mass The MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 55060 4 Marvin M Chun Jeremy M Wolfe E B Goldstein 2001 Blackwell Handbook of Sensation and Perception Blackwell Publishing Ltd pp 272 310 ISBN 978 0 631 20684 2 Edmund Rolls Gustavo Deco 2012 Computational Neuroscience of Vision Oxford Scholarship Online ISBN 978 0 198 52488 5 Machens CK Romo R Brody CD 2005 Flexible control of mutual inhibition a neural model of two interval discrimination Science 307 5712 1121 4 Bibcode 2005Sci 307 1121M CiteSeerX 10 1 1 523 4396 doi 10 1126 science 1104171 PMID 15718474 S2CID 45378154 Robinson PA Rennie CJ Rowe DL O Connor SC Gordon E 2005 Multiscale brain modelling Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 360 1457 1043 1050 doi 10 1098 rstb 2005 1638 PMC 1854922 PMID 16087447 Crick F Koch C 2003 A framework for consciousness Nat Neurosci 6 2 119 26 doi 10 1038 nn0203 119 PMID 12555104 S2CID 13960489 Crick Francis 1994 The Astonishing Hypothesis The Scientific Search for the Soul New York Scribner Crick Francis Koch Christopher 1998 Consciousness and neuroscience Cerebral Cortex Vol 8 Essential Sources In The Scientific Study of Consciousness Roscience pp 97 107 doi 10 1093 cercor 8 2 97 PMID 9542889 Adaszewski Stanislaw Dukart Juergen Kherif Ferath Frackowiak Richard Draganski Bogdan Alzheimer s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative 2013 How early can we predict Alzheimer s disease using computational anatomy Neurobiol Aging 34 12 2815 26 doi 10 1016 j neurobiolaging 2013 06 015 PMID 23890839 S2CID 1025210 Friston KJ Stephan KE Montague R Dolan RJ 2014 Computational psychiatry the brain as a phantastic organ Lancet Psychiatry 1 2 148 58 doi 10 1016 S2215 0366 14 70275 5 PMID 26360579 S2CID 15504512 Floyrac Aymeric Doumergue Adrien Legriel Stephane Deye Nicolas Megarbane Bruno Richard Alexandra Meppiel Elodie Masmoudi Sana Lozeron Pierre Vicaut Eric Kubis Nathalie Holcman David 2023 Predicting neurological outcome after cardiac arrest by combining computational parameters extracted from standard and deviant responses from auditory evoked potentials Frontiers in Neuroscience 17 988394 doi 10 3389 fnins 2023 988394 ISSN 1662 453X PMC 9975713 PMID 36875664 Sun Christophe Holcman David 2022 08 01 Combining transient statistical markers from the EEG signal to predict brain sensitivity to general anesthesia Biomedical Signal Processing and Control 77 103713 doi 10 1016 j bspc 2022 103713 ISSN 1746 8094 S2CID 248488365 Montague P Read Dolan Raymond J Friston Karl J Dayan Peter 14 Dec 2011 Computational psychiatry Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 1 72 80 doi 10 1016 j tics 2011 11 018 PMC 3556822 PMID 22177032 Kato Ayaka Kunisato Yoshihiko Katahira Kentaro Okimura Tsukasa Yamashita Yuichi 2020 Computational Psychiatry Research Map CPSYMAP a new database for visualizing research papers Frontiers in Psychiatry 11 1360 578706 doi 10 3389 fpsyt 2020 578706 PMC 7746554 PMID 33343418 Huys Quentin J M Maia Tiago V Frank Michael J 2016 Computational psychiatry as a bridge from neuroscience to clinical applications Nature Neuroscience 19 3 404 413 doi 10 1038 nn 4238 PMC 5443409 PMID 26906507 Russell John 21 March 2016 Beyond von Neumann Neuromorphic Computing Steadily Advances Calimera Andrea Macii Enrico Poncino Massimo 2013 08 20 The human brain project and neuromorphic computing Functional Neurology 28 3 191 196 doi 10 11138 FNeur 2013 28 3 191 inactive 2023 04 11 PMC 3812737 PMID 24139655 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint DOI inactive as of April 2023 link Bibliography EditChklovskii DB 2004 Synaptic connectivity and neuronal morphology two sides of the same coin Neuron 43 5 609 17 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2004 08 012 PMID 15339643 S2CID 16217065 Sejnowski Terrence J Churchland Patricia Smith 1992 The computational brain Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 03188 2 Gerstner W Kistler W Naud R Paninski L 2014 Neuronal Dynamics Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press ISBN 9781107447615 Dayan P Abbott L F 2001 Theoretical neuroscience computational and mathematical modeling of neural systems Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 04199 7 Eliasmith Chris Anderson Charles H 2003 Neural engineering Representation computation and dynamics in neurobiological systems Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 05071 5 Hodgkin AL Huxley AF 28 August 1952 A quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve J Physiol 117 4 500 44 doi 10 1113 jphysiol 1952 sp004764 PMC 1392413 PMID 12991237 William Bialek Rieke Fred David Warland Rob de Ruyter van Steveninck 1999 Spikes exploring the neural code Cambridge Mass MIT ISBN 978 0 262 68108 7 Schutter Erik de 2001 Computational neuroscience realistic modeling for experimentalists Boca Raton CRC ISBN 978 0 8493 2068 2 Sejnowski Terrence J Hemmen J L van 2006 23 problems in systems neuroscience Oxford Oxfordshire Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 514822 0 Michael A Arbib Shun ichi Amari Prudence H Arbib 2002 The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks Cambridge Massachusetts The MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 01197 6 Zhaoping Li 2014 Understanding vision theory models and data Oxford UK Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0199564668 See also EditSoftware Edit BRIAN a Python based simulator Budapest Reference Connectome web based 3D visualization tool to browse connections in the human brain Emergent neural simulation software GENESIS a general neural simulation system NEST is a simulator for spiking neural network models that focuses on the dynamics size and structure of neural systems rather than on the exact morphology of individual neurons External links Edit Wikimedia Commons has media related to Computational neuroscience Journals Edit Journal of Mathematical Neuroscience Journal of Computational Neuroscience Neural Computation Cognitive Neurodynamics Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience PLoS Computational Biology Frontiers in NeuroinformaticsConferences Edit Computational and Systems Neuroscience COSYNE a computational neuroscience meeting with a systems neuroscience focus Annual Computational Neuroscience Meeting CNS a yearly computational neuroscience meeting Computational Cognitive Neuroscience a yearly computational neuroscience meeting with a focus on cognitive phenomena Neural Information Processing Systems NIPS a leading annual conference covering mostly machine learning Cognitive Computational Neuroscience CCN a computational neuroscience meeting focusing on computational models capable of cognitive tasks International Conference on Cognitive Neurodynamics ICCN a yearly conference UK Mathematical Neurosciences Meeting a yearly conference focused on mathematical aspects Bernstein Conference on Computational Neuroscience BCCN a yearly computational neuroscience conference AREADNE Conferences a biennial meeting that includes theoretical and experimental results Websites Edit Encyclopedia of Computational Neuroscience part of Scholarpedia an online expert curated encyclopedia on computational neuroscience and dynamical systems Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Computational neuroscience amp oldid 1150285776, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.