fbpx
Wikipedia

Situated cognition

Situated cognition is a theory that posits that knowing is inseparable from doing[1] by arguing that all knowledge is situated in activity bound to social, cultural and physical contexts.[2]

Situativity theorists suggest a model of knowledge and learning that requires thinking on the fly rather than the storage and retrieval of conceptual knowledge. In essence, cognition cannot be separated from the context. Instead knowing exists, in situ, inseparable from context, activity, people, culture, and language. Therefore, learning is seen in terms of an individual's increasingly effective performance across situations rather than in terms of an accumulation of knowledge, since what is known is co-determined by the agent and the context.

History Edit

While situated cognition gained recognition in the field of educational psychology in the late twentieth century,[3] it shares many principles with older fields such as critical theory,[4][5] anthropology (Jean Lave & Etienne Wenger, 1991), philosophy (Martin Heidegger, 1968), critical discourse analysis (Fairclough, 1989), and sociolinguistics theories (Bakhtin, 1981) that rejected the notion of truly objective knowledge and the principles of Kantian empiricism.

Lucy Suchman's work on situated action at Xerox Labs[6] was instrumental in popularizing the idea that an actor's understanding of how to perform work results from reflecting on interactions with the social and material (e.g. technology-mediated) situation in which she or he acts.[7] More recent perspectives of situated cognition have focused on and draw from the concept of identity formation[8] as people negotiate meaning through interactions within communities of practice.[9][10] Situated cognition perspectives have been adopted in education,[11] instructional design,[12] online communities and artificial intelligence (see Brooks, Clancey). Grounded Cognition, concerned with the role of simulations and embodiment in cognition, encompasses Cognitive Linguistics, Situated Action, Simulation and Social Simulation theories. Research has contributed to the understanding of embodied language, memory, and the representation of knowledge.[13]

Situated cognition draws a variety of perspectives, from an anthropological study of human behavior in the context of technology-mediated work,[6] or within communities of practice[14] to the ecological psychology of the perception-action cycle[15] and intentional dynamics,[16] and even research on robotics with work on autonomous agents at NASA and elsewhere (e.g., work by W. J. Clancey). Early attempts to define situated cognition focused on contrasting the emerging theory with information processing theories dominant in cognitive psychology.[17]

Recently theorists have recognized a natural affinity between situated cognition, New Literacy Studies and new literacies research (Gee, 2010). This connection is made by understanding that situated cognition maintains that individuals learn through experiences. It could be stated that these experiences, and more importantly the mediators that affect attention during these experiences is affected by the tools, technologies and languages used by a socio-cultural group and the meanings given to these by the collective group. New literacies research examines the context and contingencies that language and tool use by individuals and how this changes as the Internet and other communication technologies affect literacy.[18]

Glossary Edit

Term Definition
affordance properties of the environment, specified in the information array (flow field) of the individual, that present possibilities for action and are available for an agent to perceive directly and act upon
attention and intention Once an intention (goal) is adopted, the agent's perception (attention) is attuned to the affordances of the environment.
attunement attunement is a persisting state of awareness of the affordances in the environment and how they may be acted upon
community of practice The concept of a community of practice (often abbreviated as CoP) refers to the process of social learning that occurs and shared sociocultural practices that emerge and evolve when people who have common goals interact as they strive towards those goals.
detection of invariants perception of what doesn't change across different situations
direct perception (pick up) describes the way an agent in an environment senses affordances without the need for computation or symbolic representation
effectivities The agents ability to recognize and use affordances of the environment.
embodiment as an explanation of cognition emphasizes first that the body exists as part of the world. In a dynamic process, perception and action occurring through and because of the body being in the world, interact to allow for the processes of simulation and representation.
legitimate peripheral participation the initial stage(s) of a person's active membership in a community of practice to which he or she has access and the opportunity to become a full participant.
perceiving and acting cycle Gibson (1986) described a continuous perception-action cycle, which is dynamic and ongoing. Agents perceive and act with intentionality in the environment at all times.

Key principles Edit

Affordances/effectivities Edit

James J. Gibson introduced the idea of affordances as part of a relational account of perception.[19] Perception should not be considered solely as the encoding of environmental features into the perceiver's mind, but as an element of an individual's interaction with her environment (Gibson, 1977). Central to his proposal of an ecological psychology was the notion of affordances. Gibson proposed that in any interaction between an agent and the environment, inherent conditions or qualities of the environment allow the agent to perform certain actions with the environment.[20] He defined the term as properties in the environment that presented possibilities for action and were available for an agent to perceive directly and act upon.[21] Gibson focused on the affordances of physical objects, such as doorknobs and chairs, and suggested that these affordances were directly perceived by an individual instead of mediated by mental representations such as mental models. It is important to note that Gibson's notion of direct perception as an unmediated process of noticing, perceiving, and encoding specific attributes from the environment, has long been challenged by proponents of a more category-based model of perception.[who?]

This focus on agent-situation interactions in ecological psychology was consistent with the situated cognition program of researchers such as James G. Greeno (1994, 1998), who appreciated Gibson's apparent rejection of the factoring assumptions underlying experimental psychology. The situated cognition perspective focused on "perception-action instead of memory and retrieval…A perceiving/acting agent is coupled with a developing/adapting environment and what matters is how the two interact".[22] Greeno (1994) also suggested that affordances are "preconditions for activity," and that while they do not determine behavior, they increase the likelihood that a certain action or behavior will occur.

Shaw, Turvey, & Mace (as cited by Greeno, 1994) later introduced the term effectivities, the abilities of the agent that determined what the agent could do, and consequently, the interaction that could take place. Perception and action were co-determined by the effectivities and affordances, which acted 'in the moment' together.[23] Therefore, the agent directly perceived and interacted with the environment, determining what affordances could be picked up, based on his effectivities. This view is consistent with Norman's (1988) theory of "perceived affordances," which emphasizes the agent's perception of an object's utility as opposed to focusing on the object itself.

An interesting question is the relationship between affordances and mental representations as set forth in a more cognitivist perspective. While Greeno (1998) argues that attunements to affordances are superior to constructs such as schemata and mental models, Glenberg & Robertson (1999) suggested that affordances are the building blocks of mental models.

Perception (variance/invariance) Edit

The work of Gibson (1986) in the field of visual perception greatly influences situated cognition.[20] Gibson argued that visual perception is not a matter of the eye translating inputs into symbolic representation in the brain. Instead the viewer perceives and picks up on the infinite amount of information available in the environment. Specifically, an agent perceives affordances by discovering the variants, what changes, and more importantly the invariants, what does not change across different situations. Given a specific intention (or intentional set),[clarification needed] perceptions of invariants are co-determined by the agent and the affordances of the environment, and are then built upon over time.[clarification needed]

Memory Edit

Situated cognition and ecological psychology perspectives emphasize perception and propose that memory plays a significantly diminished role in the learning process. Rather, focus is on the continuous tuning of perceptions and actions across situations based on the affordances of the environment and the interaction of the agent within that environment (Greeno, 1994). Representations are not stored and checked against past knowledge, but are created and interpreted in activity (Clancey, 1990).

Situated cognition understands memory as an interaction with the world, bounded by meaningful situations, that brings an agent toward a specified goal (intention). Thus, perception and action are co-determined by the effectivities and affordances, which act 'in the moment' together.[24] Therefore, the agent directly perceives and interacts with the environment, determining what affordances can be picked up, based on his effectivities, and does not simply recall stored symbolic representations.

Knowing Edit

Situativity theorists recast knowledge not as an entity, thing, or noun, but as knowing as an action or verb.[20] It is not an entity which can be collected as in knowledge acquisition models. Instead knowing is reciprocally co-determined between the agent and environment.[25] This reciprocal interaction can not be separated from the context and its cultural and historical constructions.[14] Therefore, knowing isn't a matter of arriving at any single truth but instead it is a particular stance that emerges from the agent-environment interaction.[25]

Knowing emerges as individuals develop intentions[26] through goal-directed activities within cultural contexts which may in turn have larger goals and claims of truth. The adoption of intentions relates to the direction of the agent's attention to the detection of affordances in the environment that will lead to accomplishment of desired goals. Knowing is expressed in the agent's ability to act as an increasingly competent participant in a community of practice. As agents participate more fully within specific communities of practice, what constitutes knowing continuously evolves.[14] For example, a novice environmentalist may not look at water quality by examining oxygen levels but may consider the color and smell.[25] Through participation and enculturation within different communities, agents express knowing through action.

Learning Edit

Since knowing is rooted in action and can not be decontextualized from individual, social, and historical goals[25] teaching approaches that focus on conveying facts and rules separately from the contexts within which they are meaningful in real-life do not allow for learning that is based on the detection of invariants. They are therefore considered to be impoverished methods that are unlikely to lead to transfer. Learning must involve more than the transmission of knowledge but must instead encourage the expression of effectivities and the development of attention and intention[27] through rich contexts[28] that reflect real life learning processes.[14]

Learning, more specifically literacy learning is affected by the Internet and other communication technologies as also evidenced in other segments of society. As a result of this youth are recently using affordances provided by these tools to become experts in a variety of domains.[29] These practices by youth are viewed as them becoming "pro-ams" and becoming experts in whatever they have developed a passion for.[30]

Language Edit

Individuals don't just read or write texts, they interact with them, and often these interactions involve others in various socio-cultural contexts. Since language is often the basis for monitoring and tracking learning gains in comprehension, content knowledge and tool use in and out of school the role of situated cognition in language learning activities is important. Membership and interaction in social and cultural groups is often determined by tools, technologies and discourse use for full participation. Language learning or literacy in various social and cultural groups must include how the groups work with and interact with these texts.[29] Language instruction in the context of situated cognition also involves the skilled or novice use of language by members of the group, and instruction of not only the elements of language, but what is needed to bring a student to the level of expert. Originating from emergent literacy,[31] specialist-language lessons examines the formal and informal styles and discourses of language use in socio-cultural contexts.[32] A function of specialist-language lessons includes "lucidly functional language", or complex specialist language is usually accompanied by clear and lucid language used to explain the rules, relationships or meanings existing between language and meaning.[29]

Legitimate peripheral participation Edit

According to Jean Lave and Wenger (1991) legitimate peripheral participation (LPP) provides a framework to describe how individuals ('newcomers') become part of a community of learners. Legitimate peripheral participation was central to Lave and Wenger's take on situated cognition (referred to as "situated activity") because it introduced socio-cultural and historical realizations of power and access to the way thinking and knowing are legitimated. They stated, "Hegemony over resources for learning and alienation from full participation are inherent in the shaping of the legitimacy and peripherality of participation in its historical realizations" (p. 42). Lave and Wenger's (1991) research on the phenomenon of apprenticeship in communities of practice not only provided a unit of analysis for locating an individual's multiple, changing levels and ways of participation, but also implied that all participants, through increased involvement, have access to, acquire, and use resources available to their particular community.

To illustrate the role of LPP in situated activity, Lave and Wenger (1991) examined five apprenticeship scenarios (Yucatec midwives, Vai and Gola tailors, naval quartermasters, meat cutters, and non-drinking alcoholics involved in AA). Their analysis of apprenticeship across five different communities of learners lead them to several conclusions about the situatedness of LPP and its relationship to successful learning. Key to newcomers' success included:

  • access to all that community membership entails,
  • involvement in productive activity,
  • learning the discourse(s) of the community including "talking about and talking within a practice," (p. 109), and
  • willingness of the community to capitalize on the inexperience of newcomers, "Insofar as this continual interaction of new perspectives is sanctioned, everyone's participation is legitimately peripheral in some respect. In other words, everyone can to some degree be considered a 'newcomer' to the future of a changing community" [14]

Planning vs. action Edit

Suchman's book, Plans and Situated Actions: The Problem of Human-machine Communication (1987), provided a novel approach to the study of human-computer interaction (HCI). By adopting an anthropological approach to sensemaking and interpretation, Suchman was able to demonstrate how both action and planning were situated in the context of a flow of socially- and materially-mediated activities - an idea that stimulated many of the later conceptualizations of situated cognition. Her studies contrasted the deterministic approach to planning assumed by technology designers with the situated nature of planning as people make sense of the status of their workflow and adjust their course of action accordingly. For example, a photocopier will instruct its user to reload all pages in the original order after a jam, whereas the user understands that they only need to copy the last page again. By arguing that plans were the result of ongoing processes of prospective/retrospective sense making, Suchman identified the limits of technology system access to relevant social and material resources as a major cause of limitations in how technology supports human work.[6]

This position led to a major debate with Vera and Simon (1993), who argued that cognition is based on symbolic representations and that planning must therefore be deterministic, based on a pre-determined repertoire of learned response. Most organizational theorists would now see this debate as reflecting individual/cognitive vs. socially-situated levels of analysis (requiring a similar need for paradigmatic co-existence as Wave–particle duality). Suchman (1993) argues that planning in the context of work-activity is similar to navigating a canoe through rapids: you know what point on the river you are aiming for, but you constantly adjust your course as you interact with rocks, swells, and currents on the way. As a result, many organizational theorists argue that plans can only be viewed as post-hoc justifications of action, while Suchman herself appears to view plans and actions as interrelated in the moment of action.[7]

Representation, symbols, and schemata Edit

In situated theories, the term "representation" refers to external forms in the environment that are created through social interactions to express meaning (language, art, gestures, etc.) and are perceived and acted upon in the first person sense. "Representing" in the first person sense is conceived as an act of re-experiencing in the imagination that involves the dialectic of ongoing perceiving and acting in coordination with the activation of neural structures and processes. This form of reflective representation is considered to be a secondary type of learning, while the primary form of learning is found in the "adaptive recoordination that occurs with every behavior".[33] Conceptualizing is considered to be a "prelinguistic" act, while "knowing" involves creative interaction with symbols in both their interpretation and use for expression. "Schema" develop as neural connections become biased through repeated activations to reactivate in situations that are perceived and conceived as temporally and compositionally similar to previous generalized situations.[33]

Goals, intention, and attention Edit

 
Young-Barab Model (1997)

The Young-Barab Model (1997) pictured to the left, illustrates the dynamics of intentions and intentional dynamics involved in the agent's interaction with his environment when problem solving.

Dynamics of Intentions:[34] goal (intention) adoption from among all possible goals (ontological descent). This describes how the learner decides whether or not to adopt a particular goal when presented with a problem. Once a goal is adopted, the learner proceeds by interacting with their environment through intentional dynamics. There are many levels of intentions, but at the moment of a particular occasion, the agent has just one intention, and that intention constrains his behavior until it is fulfilled or annihilated.

Intentional Dynamics:[34] dynamics that unfold when the agent has only one intention (goal) and begins to act towards it, perceiving and acting.[21] It is a trajectory towards the achievement of a solution or goal, the process of tuning one's perception (attention). Each intention is meaningfully bounded, where the dynamics of that intention inform the agent of whether or not he is getting closer to achieving his goal. If the agent is not getting closer to his goal, he will take corrective action, and then continue forward. This is the agent's intentional dynamics, and continues on until he achieves his goal.

Transfer Edit

There are various definition of transfer found within the situated cognition umbrella. Researchers interested in social practice often define transfer as increased participation.[14] Ecological psychology perspectives define transfer as the detection of invariance across different situations.[35] Furthermore, transfer can only "occur when there is a confluence of an individual's goals and objectives, their acquired abilities to act, and a set of affordances for action".[36]

Embodied cognition Edit

The traditional cognition approach assumes that perception and motor systems are merely peripheral input and output devices.[37] However, embodied cognition posits that the mind and body interact 'on the fly' as a single entity. An example of embodied cognition is seen in the area of robotics, where movements are not based on internal representations, rather, they are based on the robot's direct and immediate interaction with its environment.[38] Additionally, research has shown that embodied facial expressions influence judgments,[39] and arm movements are related to a person's evaluation of a word or concept.[40] In the latter example, the individual would pull or push a lever towards his name at a faster rate for positive words, than for negative words. These results appeal to the embodied nature of situated cognition, where knowledge is the achievement of the whole body in its interaction with the world.

Externalism Edit

As to the mind, by and large, situated cognition paves the way to various form of externalism. The issue is whether the situated aspect of cognition has only a practical value or it is somehow constitutive of cognition and perhaps of consciousness itself. As to the latter possibility, there are different positions. David Chalmers and Andy Clark, who developed the hugely debated model of the extended mind, explicitly rejected the externalization of consciousness.[41] For them, only cognition is extended. On the other hand, others, like Riccardo Manzotti[41] or Teed Rockwell,[42] explicitly considered the possibility to situate conscious experience in the environment.

Pedagogical implications Edit

Since situated cognition views knowing as an action within specific contexts and views direct instruction models of knowledge transmission as impoverished, there are significant implications for pedagogical practices. First, curriculum requires instructional design that draws on apprenticeship models common in real life.[3] Second, curricular design should rely on contextual narratives that situate concepts in practice. Classroom practices such as project-based learning and problem-based learning would qualify as consistent with the situated learning perspective, as would techniques such as Case Base Learning, Anchored Instruction, and cognitive apprenticeship.

Cognitive apprenticeship Edit

Cognitive apprenticeships were one of the earliest pedagogical designs to incorporate the theories of situated cognition (Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989). Cognitive apprenticeship uses four dimensions (e.g., content, methods, sequence, sociology) to embed learning in activity and make deliberate the use of the social and physical contexts present in the classroom (Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989; Collins, Brown, & Newman, 1989). Cognitive apprenticeship includes the enculturation of students into authentic practices through activity and social interaction (Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989). The technique draws on the principles of Legitimate Peripheral Participation (Lave & Wenger, 1991) and reciprocal teaching (Palincsar & Brown, 1984; 1989) in that a more knowledgeable other, i.e. a teacher, engages in a task with a more novice other, i.e. a learner, by describing their own thoughts as they work on the task, providing "just in time" scaffolding, modeling expert behaviors, and encouraging reflection.[43] The reflection process includes having students alternate between novice and expert strategies in a problem-solving context, sensitizing them to specifics of an expert performance, and adjustments that may be made to their own performance to get them to the expert level (Collins & Brown, 1988; Collins, Brown, & Newman, 1989). Thus, the function of reflection indicates "co-investigation" and/or abstracted replay by students.[44]

Collins, Brown, and Newman (1989) emphasized six critical features of a cognitive apprenticeship that included observation, coaching, scaffolding, modeling, fading, and reflection. Using these critical features, expert(s) guided students on their journey to acquire the cognitive and metacognitive processes and skills necessary to handle a variety of tasks, in a range of situations[45] Reciprocal teaching, a form of cognitive apprenticeship, involves the modeling and coaching of various comprehension skills as teacher and students take turns in assuming the role of instructor.

Anchored instruction Edit

Anchored instruction is grounded in a story or narrative that presents a realistic (but fictional) situation and raises an overarching question or problem (compare with an essential question posed by a teacher). This approach is designed to 1) engage the learner with a problem or series of related problems, 2) require the learner to develop goals and discover subgoals related to solving the problem(s), and 3) provide the learner with extensive and diverse opportunities to explore the problem(s) in a shared context with classmates. For example, a Spanish teacher uses a video drama series focused on the murder of a main character. Students work in small groups to summarize parts of the story, to create hypotheses about the murderer and motive, and to create a presentation of their solution to the class. Stories are often paired so that across the set students can detect the invariant structure of the underlying knowledge (so 2 episodes about distance-rate-time, one about boats and one about planes, so students can perceive how the distance-rate-time relationship holds across differences in vehicles). The ideal smallest set of instances needed provide students the opportunity to detect invariant structure has been referred to as a "generator set" of situations.

The goal of anchored instruction is the engagement of intention and attention. Through authentic tasks across multiple domains, educators present situations that require students to create or adopt meaningful goals (intentions). One of the educator's objectives can be to set a goal through the use of an anchor problem.[46] A classic example of anchored instruction is the Jasper series.[47] The Jasper series includes a variety of videodisc adventures focused on problem formulation and problem solving. Each videodisc used a visual narrative to present an authentic, realistic everyday problem. The objective was for students to adopt specific goals (intentions) after viewing the story and defining a problem. These newly adopted goals guided students through the collaborative process of problem formulation and problem solving.

Perceiving and acting in avatar-based virtual worlds Edit

Virtual worlds provide unique affordances for embodied learning, i.e. hands on, interactive, spatially oriented, that ground learning in experience. Here "embodied" means acting in a virtual world enabled by an avatar.

Contextual affordances of online games and virtual environments allow learners to engage in goal-driven activity, authentic interactions, and collaborative problem-solving – all considered in situated theories of learning to be features of optimal learning. In terms of situated assessment, virtual worlds have the advantage of facilitating dynamic feedback that directs the perceiving/acting agent, through an avatar, to continually improve performance.

Research methodologies Edit

The situative perspective is focused on interactive systems in which individuals interact with one another and physical and representational systems. Research takes place in situ and in real-world settings, reflecting assumptions that knowledge is constructed within specific contexts which have specific situational affordances. Mixed methods and qualitative methodologies are the most prominently used by researchers.

In qualitative studies, methods used are varied but the focus is often on the increased participation in specific communities of practice, the affordances of the environment that are acted upon by the agent, and the distributed nature of knowing in specific communities. A major feature of quantitative methods used in situated cognition is the absence of outcome measures. Quantitative variables used in mixed methods often focus on process over product. For example, trace nodes, dribble files, and hyperlink pathways are often used to track how students interact in the environment.[48]

Critiques of situativity Edit

In "Situated Action: A Symbolic Interpretation" Vera and Simon wrote: " ... the systems usually regarded as exemplifying Situated Action are thoroughly symbolic (and representational), and, to the extent that they are limited in these respects, have doubtful prospects for extension to complex tasks"[49] Vera and Simon (1993) also claimed that the information processing view is supported by many years of research in which symbol systems simulated "broad areas of human cognition" and that there is no evidence of cognition without representation.

Anderson, Reder and Simon (1996) summarized what they considered to be the four claims of situated learning and argued against each claim from a cognitivist perspective. The claims and their arguments were:

  1. Claim: Activity and learning are bound to the specific situations in which they occur. Argument: Whether learning is bound to context or not depends on both the kind of learning and the way that it is learned.
  2. Claim: Knowledge does not transfer between tasks. Argument: There is ample evidence of successful transfer between tasks in the literature. Transfer depends on initial practice and the degree to which a successive task has similar cognitive elements to a prior task.
  3. Claim: Teaching abstractions is ineffective. Argument: Abstract instruction can be made effective by combining of abstract concepts and concrete examples.
  4. Claim: Instruction must happen in complex social contexts. Argument: Research shows value in individual learning and on focusing individually on specific skills in a skill set.

Anderson, Reder and Simons summarize their concerns when they say: "What is needed to improve learning and teaching is to continue to deepen our research into the circumstances that determine when narrower or broader contexts are required and when attention to narrower or broader skills are optimal for effective and efficient learning" (p. 10).

Considerations Edit

However, it is important to remember that a theory is neither wrong nor right but provides affordances for certain aspects of a problem.[50] Lave and Wenger recognized this in their ironic comment, "How can we purport to be working out a theoretical conception of learning without engaging in the project of abstraction [decontextualized knowledge] rejected above?" (Lave & Wenger, 1991, p. 38).

See also Edit

References Edit

Notes Edit

  1. ^ John Seely Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989; Greeno, 1989
  2. ^ Greeno & Moore, 1993
  3. ^ a b Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989
  4. ^ McLaughlin, N. (1999). Origin myths in the social sciences: Fromm, the Frankfurt School and the emergence of critical theory. Canadian Journal of Sociology/Cahiers canadiens de sociologie, 109-139.
  5. ^ Freire, P. (1996). Pedagogy of the oppressed (revised). New York: Continuum, 356, 357-358.
  6. ^ a b c Suchman, Lucy (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine communication. Cambridge MA: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521337397.
  7. ^ a b Suchman, Lucy (January 1993). "Response to Vera and Simon's Situated Action: A Symbolic Interpretation". Cognitive Science. 17: 71–75. doi:10.1111/cogs.1993.17.issue-1.
  8. ^ Lave, J. (1991). Situating learning in communities of practice.
  9. ^ Brown, J. S., & Duguid, P. (2000). Balancing act: How to capture knowledge without killing it. Harvard business review, 78(3), 73-80.
  10. ^ Clancey, G. (2004). Local memory and worldly narrative: the remote city in America and Japan. Urban Studies, 41(12), 2335-2355.
  11. ^ Brown, Collins, & Duguid, 1989)
  12. ^ Young, 2004
  13. ^ Barsalou, L. Grounded Cognition (2008) Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2008. 59:617–45
  14. ^ a b c d e f Lave & Wenger, 1991
  15. ^ James J. Gibson, 1986
  16. ^ Shaw, Kadar, Sim & Reppenger, 1992
  17. ^ Bredo, 1994
  18. ^ Leu et al., 2009
  19. ^ Gibson, 1977
  20. ^ a b c Greeno, 1994
  21. ^ a b Gibson 1979/1986
  22. ^ Young, Kulikowich, & Barab, 1997, p. 139
  23. ^ Gibson 1979/1986; Greeno, 1994; Young et al., 1997
  24. ^ Gibson 1979/1986; Greeno, 1994; Young Kulikowich, & Barab, 1997
  25. ^ a b c d Barab & Roth, 2006
  26. ^ Young, 1997
  27. ^ Young, 2004b
  28. ^ Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt, 1990
  29. ^ a b c Gee, 2010
  30. ^ Anderson, 2006; Leadbeater & Miller, 2004
  31. ^ Dickinson & Neuman, 2006; Gee, 2004
  32. ^ Gee, 2004; Gee, 2007
  33. ^ a b Clancey, 1993
  34. ^ a b Kugler et al., 1991; Shaw et al., 1992; Young et al., 1997
  35. ^ Young & McNeese, 1995
  36. ^ Young et al., 1997, p. 147
  37. ^ Niedenthal, 2007; Wilson, 2002
  38. ^ Wilson, 2002
  39. ^ Niedenthal, 2007
  40. ^ Markman, & Brendl, 2005
  41. ^ a b Clark, A., (2008), Supersizing the Mind, Oxford, Oxford University Press.
  42. ^ Rockwell, 2005 #1561
  43. ^ Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1985; Scardamalia, Bereiter. & Steinbach, 1984
  44. ^ Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1983; Collins & Brown, 1988
  45. ^ Collins et al.,1989
  46. ^ Barab & Roth, 2006; Young et al., 1997
  47. ^ The Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt, 1990; Young et al., 1997; Young & McNeese, 1995
  48. ^ Shaw, Effken, Fajen, Garret & Morris, 1997
  49. ^ Vera & Simon, 1993, p. 7
  50. ^ Sfard, A (1998). "On two metaphors for learning and the dangers of choosing just one". Educational Researcher. 27 (2): 4–13. doi:10.3102/0013189x027002004. S2CID 135448246.

Sources Edit

  • Anderson, J.R.; Reder, L.M.; Simon, H.A. (1996). "Situated learning and education". Educational Researcher. 25 (4): 5–11. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.556.7550. doi:10.3102/0013189x025004005. S2CID 54548451.
  • Bredo, E. (1994). "Reconstructing educational psychology: Situated cognition and Deweyian pragmatism". Educational Psychologist. 29 (1): 23–35. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep2901_3.
  • Brown, J. S.; Collins, A.; Duguid, S. (1989). . Educational Researcher. 18 (1): 32–42. doi:10.3102/0013189x018001032. hdl:2142/17979. S2CID 9824073. Archived from the original on 2014-10-08.
  • Clancey, W. J (1993). "Situated action: A neuropsychological interpretation response to Vera and Simon". Cognitive Science. 17: 87–116. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1701_7.
  • Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt (1990). "Anchored instruction and its relationship to situated cognition". Educational Researcher. 19 (6): 2–10. doi:10.3102/0013189x019006002. S2CID 7792800.
  • Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. (1993). Anchored instruction and situated cognition revisited. Educational Technology March Issue, 52-70.
  • Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt. (1994). From visual word problems to learning communities: Changing conceptions of cognitive research. In K. McGilly (Ed.) Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom proactice. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
  • Driscoll, M. P. (2004). Psychology of learning for instruction (3 ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Allyn & Bacon. ISBN 978-0-205-37519-6.
  • Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). "A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality". Psychological Review. 95 (2): 256–273. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.583.9142. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.256.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Engle, R.A. (2006). "Framing interactions to foster generative learning: A situative explanation of transfer in a community of learners classroom". Journal of the Learning Sciences. 15 (4): 451–498. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1504_2. S2CID 144839447.
  • Eysenck, M. W., & Keane, M. T. (2005). Cognitive psychology (5 ed.). New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-1-84169-359-0.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Gagne, R. M., Wager, W.W., Golas, K. C., & Keller, J. M. (2005). Principles of Instructional Design (5 ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning. ISBN 978-0-534-58284-5.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Gibson, James J. (1979). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 978-0-89859-959-6.
  • Glenberg, A. M.; Robertson, D. A. (1999). "Indexical understanding of instructions". Discourse Processes. 28 (1): 1–26. doi:10.1080/01638539909545067.
  • Greeno, J. G. (1989). "A perspective on thinking". American Psychologist. 44 (2): 134–141. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.44.2.134.
  • Greeno, J. G. (1994). "Gibson's affordances". Psychological Review. 101 (2): 336–342. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.2.336. hdl:20.500.12749/3070. PMID 8022965.
  • Greeno, J. G. (1998). "The situativity of knowing, learning, and research". American Psychologist. 53 (1): 5–26. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.53.1.5.
  • Greeno, J. G. (2006). "Authoritative, accountable positioning and connected, general knowing: Progressive themes in understanding transfer". Journal of the Learning Sciences. 15 (4): 539–550. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1504_4. S2CID 145101010.
  • Hart, L. A. (1990). Human Brain & Human Learning. Black Diamond, WA: Books for Educators. ISBN 978-0-9624475-9-4.
  • Kugler, P. N., Shaw, R. E., Vicente, K. J., & Kinsella-Shaw, J. (1991). The role of attractors in the self-organization of intentional systems. In R. R. Hoffman and D. S. Palermo (Eds.) Cognition and the Symbolic Processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Lave, J. (1977). "Cognitive consequences of traditional apprenticeship training in West Africa". Anthropology & Education Quarterly. 18 (3): 1776–180. doi:10.1525/aeq.1977.8.3.05x1512d.
  • Markman, A. B., & Brendl, C. M. (2005). "Constraining theories of embodied cognition". Psychological Science. 16 (1): 6–10. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.463.6440. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00772.x. PMID 15660844. S2CID 12505177.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). "Embodying emotion". Science. 316 (5827): 1002–1005. Bibcode:2007Sci...316.1002N. doi:10.1126/science.1136930. PMID 17510358. S2CID 14537829.
  • Ormrod, J. E. (2004). Human learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. ISBN 978-0-13-094199-2.
  • Palincsar, A.S., & Brown, A.L. (1984). "Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and monitoring activities". Cognition and Instruction. 1 (2): 117–175. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0102_1.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Roth, W-M (1996). "Knowledge diffusion in a grade 4-5 classroom during a unit on civil engineering: An analysis of a classroom community in terms of its changing resources and practices". Cognition and Instruction. 14 (2): 179–220. doi:10.1207/s1532690xci1402_2.
  • Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (2003). Knowledge building. In J. W. Guthrie (Ed.) Encyclopedia of Education (2nd Ed., pp. 1370–1373). New York: Macmillan Reference.
  • Sfard, A (1998). "On two metaphors for learning and the dangers of choosing just one". Educational Researcher. 27 (2): 4–13. doi:10.3102/0013189x027002004. S2CID 135448246.
  • Shadish, W. R., Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (2002). Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company. ISBN 978-0-395-61556-0.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Shaw, R.E.; Effken, J; Fajen, B.R.; Garrett, S.R.; Morris, A. (1997). "An ecological approach to the on-line assessment of problem-solving paths: Principles and applications". Instructional Science. 25 (2): 151–166. doi:10.1023/A:1002975703555. S2CID 60918257.
  • Shaw, R. E.; Kadar, E.; Sim, M.; Repperger, D. W. (1992). "The intentional spring: A strategy for modeling systems that learn to perform intentional acts". Journal of Motor Behavior. 24 (1): 3–28. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.577.9342. doi:10.1080/00222895.1992.9941598. PMID 14766495.
  • Weiner, B. (1994). "Ability versus effort revisited: The moral determinants of achievement evaluation and achievement as a moral system". Educational Psychology Review. 12: 1–14. doi:10.1023/A:1009017532121. S2CID 19527829.
  • Wilson, M. (2002). "Six views of embodied cognition". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 9 (4): 625–636. doi:10.3758/BF03196322. PMID 12613670.
  • Wilson, B.B., & Myers, K. M. (2000). Situated Cognition in Theoretical and Practical Context. In D. Jonassen, & S. Land (Eds.) Theoretical Foundations of Learning Environments. (pp. 57–88). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Young, M. F.; Kulikowich, J. M.; Barab, S. A. (1997). "The unit of analysis for situated assessment". Instructional Science. 25 (2): 133–150. doi:10.1023/A:1002971532689. S2CID 58143966.
  • Young, M., & McNeese, M. (1995). A Situated Cognition Approach to Problem Solving. In P. Hancock, J. Flach, J. Caid, & K. Vicente (Eds.) Local Applications of the Ecological Approach to Human Machine Systems. (pp. 359–391). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Young, M. (2004a). An Ecological Description of Video Games in Education. Proceedings of the International Conference on Education and Information Systems Technologies and Applications (EISTA), July 22, Orlando, FL, pp. 203–208.
  • Young, M. (2004b). An ecological psychology of instructional design: Learning and thinking by perceiving-acting systems. In D.H. Jonassen (Ed.), Handbook of Research for Educational Communications and Technology, 2nd Ed. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Citations and further reading Edit

  • Anderson, J. R.; Greeno, J. G.; Reder, L. M.; Simon, H. A. (2000). "Perspectives on learning, thinking, and activity". Educational Researcher. 29 (4): 11–13. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.139.8833. doi:10.3102/0013189x029004011. S2CID 145255457.
  • Anderson, C. (2006). The Long Tail: Why the Future of Business Is Selling Less of More. New York: Hyperion.
  • Brown, A. L. (1992). "Design experiments:Theoretical and methodological challenges in creating in complext interventions in classroom settings". Journal of the Learning Sciences. 2 (2): 141–178. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.133.5577. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls0202_2.
  • Clancey, William J. (1997). Situated Cognition: On Human Knowledge and Computer Representation. New York: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-44871-0.
  • "Special Issue: Situated Action". Cognitive Science. 17 (1). January–March 1993.
  • Collins, A., & Brown, J. (1988). The computer as a tool for learning through reflection. In H. Mandi & A. Lesgold (Eds.), Learning issues for intelligent tutoring systems (pp. 1–18). New York: Springer-Verlag.
  • Collins, A., Brown, J., & Newman, S. (1989). Cognitive Apprenticeship: Teaching the craft of reading, writing and mathematics. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert Glaser. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Dewey, J. (1938). Experience & Education. New York: Simon & Schuster. ISBN 978-0-684-83828-1.
  • Dickinson, D. K., & Neuman, S. B. (Eds.). (2006). Handbook of Early Literacy Research, Vol. 2. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Gallagher, S., Robbins, B.D., & Bradatan, C. (2007). Special issue on the situated body. Janus Head: Journal of Interdisciplinary Studies in Literature, Continental Philosophy, Phenomenological Psychology, and the Arts, 9(2). URL: http://www.janushead.org/9-2/
  • Gee, J. P. (2004). Situated Language and learning: A critique of traditional schooling. London: Routledge.
  • Gee, J. P. (2007). Good video games and good learning: Collected essays on video games, learning, and literacy. New York: Lang.
  • Gee, J. P. (2010). A Situated-Sociocultural Approach to Literacy and Technology. In E. Baker (Ed.), The New Literacies: Multiple Perspectives on Research and Practice, pp. 165–193. New York: Guilford Press.
  • Greeno, J. G. (1994). "Gibson's affordances". Psychological Review. 101 (2): 336–342. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.2.336. hdl:20.500.12749/3070. PMID 8022965.
  • Greeno, J. G. (1997). "On claims that answer the wrong question". Educational Researcher. 26 (1): 5–17. doi:10.3102/0013189X026001005. S2CID 146633367.
  • Greeno, J.G.; Middle School, Mathematics Through Applications Project Group (1998). "The situativity of knowing, learning, and research". American Psychologist. 53 (1): 5–26. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.53.1.5.
  • Greeno, J. G. (2006). "Authoritative, accountable positioning and connected, general knowing: Progressive themes in understanding transfer". Journal of the Learning Sciences. 15 (4): 539–550. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1504_4. S2CID 145101010.
  • Griffin, M. M. (1995). "You can't get there from here: Situated learning, transfer, and map skills". Contemporary Educational Psychology. 20: 65–87. doi:10.1006/ceps.1995.1004.
  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge, Mass.: The MIT Press (A Bradford Book). ISBN 978-0-262-58146-2.
  • Keller, C. & Keller, J. (1996). Cognition and Tool Use: The Blacksmith at Work. Cambridge University Press (ISBN 0-521-55239-7)
  • Kirshner, D. & Whitson, J. A. (1997) Situated Cognition: Social, semiotic, and psychological perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum (ISBN 0-8058-2038-8)
  • Kirshner, D.; Whitson, J. A. (1998). "Obstacles to understanding cognition as situated". Educational Researcher. 27 (8): 22–28. doi:10.3102/0013189X027008022. JSTOR 1177113. S2CID 6528031.
  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in Practice. ISBN 978-0-521-35015-0.
  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Pr. ISBN 978-0-521-42374-8.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  • Leadbeater, C., & Miller, P. (2004). The Pro-Am revolution: How enthusiasts are changing our society and economy. London: Demos.
  • Leu, D. J.; O'Byrne, W. I.; Zawilinski, L.; McVerry, J. G.; Everett-Cacopardo, H. (2009). "Comments on Greenhow, Robelia, and Hughes: Expanding the New Literacies Conversation". Educational Researcher. 39 (4): 264–269. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.612.6147. doi:10.3102/0013189X09336676. S2CID 145607514.
  • Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1983). Child as co-investigator: Helping children gain insight into their own mental processes. In S. Paris, G. Olson, & H. Stevenson (Eds.), Learning and motivation in the classroom (pp. 83–107). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1985). Fostering the development of self-regulation in children's knowledge processing. In S. F. Chipman, J. W. Segal, & R. Glaser (Eds.), Thinking and learning skills: Research and open questions (pp. 563–577). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Scardamalia, M.; Bereiter, C.; Steinbach, R. (1984). "Teachability of reflective processes in written composition". Cognitive Science. 8 (2): 173–190. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0802_4.
  • Suchman, L. (1987) Plans and situated actions : The Problem of Human-Machine Communication. Cambridge University Press, New York.
  • Suchman, L. (1993) Response to Vera and Simon's Situated Action: A Symbolic Interpretation. Cognitive Science, 17:71—75, 1993

situated, cognition, theory, that, posits, that, knowing, inseparable, from, doing, arguing, that, knowledge, situated, activity, bound, social, cultural, physical, contexts, situativity, theorists, suggest, model, knowledge, learning, that, requires, thinking. Situated cognition is a theory that posits that knowing is inseparable from doing 1 by arguing that all knowledge is situated in activity bound to social cultural and physical contexts 2 Situativity theorists suggest a model of knowledge and learning that requires thinking on the fly rather than the storage and retrieval of conceptual knowledge In essence cognition cannot be separated from the context Instead knowing exists in situ inseparable from context activity people culture and language Therefore learning is seen in terms of an individual s increasingly effective performance across situations rather than in terms of an accumulation of knowledge since what is known is co determined by the agent and the context Contents 1 History 2 Glossary 3 Key principles 3 1 Affordances effectivities 3 2 Perception variance invariance 3 3 Memory 3 4 Knowing 3 5 Learning 3 6 Language 3 7 Legitimate peripheral participation 3 8 Planning vs action 3 9 Representation symbols and schemata 3 10 Goals intention and attention 3 11 Transfer 3 12 Embodied cognition 4 Externalism 5 Pedagogical implications 5 1 Cognitive apprenticeship 5 2 Anchored instruction 5 3 Perceiving and acting in avatar based virtual worlds 6 Research methodologies 7 Critiques of situativity 7 1 Considerations 8 See also 9 References 9 1 Notes 9 2 Sources 10 Citations and further readingHistory EditWhile situated cognition gained recognition in the field of educational psychology in the late twentieth century 3 it shares many principles with older fields such as critical theory 4 5 anthropology Jean Lave amp Etienne Wenger 1991 philosophy Martin Heidegger 1968 critical discourse analysis Fairclough 1989 and sociolinguistics theories Bakhtin 1981 that rejected the notion of truly objective knowledge and the principles of Kantian empiricism Lucy Suchman s work on situated action at Xerox Labs 6 was instrumental in popularizing the idea that an actor s understanding of how to perform work results from reflecting on interactions with the social and material e g technology mediated situation in which she or he acts 7 More recent perspectives of situated cognition have focused on and draw from the concept of identity formation 8 as people negotiate meaning through interactions within communities of practice 9 10 Situated cognition perspectives have been adopted in education 11 instructional design 12 online communities and artificial intelligence see Brooks Clancey Grounded Cognition concerned with the role of simulations and embodiment in cognition encompasses Cognitive Linguistics Situated Action Simulation and Social Simulation theories Research has contributed to the understanding of embodied language memory and the representation of knowledge 13 Situated cognition draws a variety of perspectives from an anthropological study of human behavior in the context of technology mediated work 6 or within communities of practice 14 to the ecological psychology of the perception action cycle 15 and intentional dynamics 16 and even research on robotics with work on autonomous agents at NASA and elsewhere e g work by W J Clancey Early attempts to define situated cognition focused on contrasting the emerging theory with information processing theories dominant in cognitive psychology 17 Recently theorists have recognized a natural affinity between situated cognition New Literacy Studies and new literacies research Gee 2010 This connection is made by understanding that situated cognition maintains that individuals learn through experiences It could be stated that these experiences and more importantly the mediators that affect attention during these experiences is affected by the tools technologies and languages used by a socio cultural group and the meanings given to these by the collective group New literacies research examines the context and contingencies that language and tool use by individuals and how this changes as the Internet and other communication technologies affect literacy 18 Glossary EditTerm Definitionaffordance properties of the environment specified in the information array flow field of the individual that present possibilities for action and are available for an agent to perceive directly and act uponattention and intention Once an intention goal is adopted the agent s perception attention is attuned to the affordances of the environment attunement attunement is a persisting state of awareness of the affordances in the environment and how they may be acted uponcommunity of practice The concept of a community of practice often abbreviated as CoP refers to the process of social learning that occurs and shared sociocultural practices that emerge and evolve when people who have common goals interact as they strive towards those goals detection of invariants perception of what doesn t change across different situationsdirect perception pick up describes the way an agent in an environment senses affordances without the need for computation or symbolic representationeffectivities The agents ability to recognize and use affordances of the environment embodiment as an explanation of cognition emphasizes first that the body exists as part of the world In a dynamic process perception and action occurring through and because of the body being in the world interact to allow for the processes of simulation and representation legitimate peripheral participation the initial stage s of a person s active membership in a community of practice to which he or she has access and the opportunity to become a full participant perceiving and acting cycle Gibson 1986 described a continuous perception action cycle which is dynamic and ongoing Agents perceive and act with intentionality in the environment at all times Key principles EditAffordances effectivities Edit James J Gibson introduced the idea of affordances as part of a relational account of perception 19 Perception should not be considered solely as the encoding of environmental features into the perceiver s mind but as an element of an individual s interaction with her environment Gibson 1977 Central to his proposal of an ecological psychology was the notion of affordances Gibson proposed that in any interaction between an agent and the environment inherent conditions or qualities of the environment allow the agent to perform certain actions with the environment 20 He defined the term as properties in the environment that presented possibilities for action and were available for an agent to perceive directly and act upon 21 Gibson focused on the affordances of physical objects such as doorknobs and chairs and suggested that these affordances were directly perceived by an individual instead of mediated by mental representations such as mental models It is important to note that Gibson s notion of direct perception as an unmediated process of noticing perceiving and encoding specific attributes from the environment has long been challenged by proponents of a more category based model of perception who This focus on agent situation interactions in ecological psychology was consistent with the situated cognition program of researchers such as James G Greeno 1994 1998 who appreciated Gibson s apparent rejection of the factoring assumptions underlying experimental psychology The situated cognition perspective focused on perception action instead of memory and retrieval A perceiving acting agent is coupled with a developing adapting environment and what matters is how the two interact 22 Greeno 1994 also suggested that affordances are preconditions for activity and that while they do not determine behavior they increase the likelihood that a certain action or behavior will occur Shaw Turvey amp Mace as cited by Greeno 1994 later introduced the term effectivities the abilities of the agent that determined what the agent could do and consequently the interaction that could take place Perception and action were co determined by the effectivities and affordances which acted in the moment together 23 Therefore the agent directly perceived and interacted with the environment determining what affordances could be picked up based on his effectivities This view is consistent with Norman s 1988 theory of perceived affordances which emphasizes the agent s perception of an object s utility as opposed to focusing on the object itself An interesting question is the relationship between affordances and mental representations as set forth in a more cognitivist perspective While Greeno 1998 argues that attunements to affordances are superior to constructs such as schemata and mental models Glenberg amp Robertson 1999 suggested that affordances are the building blocks of mental models Perception variance invariance Edit The work of Gibson 1986 in the field of visual perception greatly influences situated cognition 20 Gibson argued that visual perception is not a matter of the eye translating inputs into symbolic representation in the brain Instead the viewer perceives and picks up on the infinite amount of information available in the environment Specifically an agent perceives affordances by discovering the variants what changes and more importantly the invariants what does not change across different situations Given a specific intention or intentional set clarification needed perceptions of invariants are co determined by the agent and the affordances of the environment and are then built upon over time clarification needed Memory Edit Situated cognition and ecological psychology perspectives emphasize perception and propose that memory plays a significantly diminished role in the learning process Rather focus is on the continuous tuning of perceptions and actions across situations based on the affordances of the environment and the interaction of the agent within that environment Greeno 1994 Representations are not stored and checked against past knowledge but are created and interpreted in activity Clancey 1990 Situated cognition understands memory as an interaction with the world bounded by meaningful situations that brings an agent toward a specified goal intention Thus perception and action are co determined by the effectivities and affordances which act in the moment together 24 Therefore the agent directly perceives and interacts with the environment determining what affordances can be picked up based on his effectivities and does not simply recall stored symbolic representations Knowing Edit Situativity theorists recast knowledge not as an entity thing or noun but as knowing as an action or verb 20 It is not an entity which can be collected as in knowledge acquisition models Instead knowing is reciprocally co determined between the agent and environment 25 This reciprocal interaction can not be separated from the context and its cultural and historical constructions 14 Therefore knowing isn t a matter of arriving at any single truth but instead it is a particular stance that emerges from the agent environment interaction 25 Knowing emerges as individuals develop intentions 26 through goal directed activities within cultural contexts which may in turn have larger goals and claims of truth The adoption of intentions relates to the direction of the agent s attention to the detection of affordances in the environment that will lead to accomplishment of desired goals Knowing is expressed in the agent s ability to act as an increasingly competent participant in a community of practice As agents participate more fully within specific communities of practice what constitutes knowing continuously evolves 14 For example a novice environmentalist may not look at water quality by examining oxygen levels but may consider the color and smell 25 Through participation and enculturation within different communities agents express knowing through action Learning Edit Since knowing is rooted in action and can not be decontextualized from individual social and historical goals 25 teaching approaches that focus on conveying facts and rules separately from the contexts within which they are meaningful in real life do not allow for learning that is based on the detection of invariants They are therefore considered to be impoverished methods that are unlikely to lead to transfer Learning must involve more than the transmission of knowledge but must instead encourage the expression of effectivities and the development of attention and intention 27 through rich contexts 28 that reflect real life learning processes 14 Learning more specifically literacy learning is affected by the Internet and other communication technologies as also evidenced in other segments of society As a result of this youth are recently using affordances provided by these tools to become experts in a variety of domains 29 These practices by youth are viewed as them becoming pro ams and becoming experts in whatever they have developed a passion for 30 Language Edit Individuals don t just read or write texts they interact with them and often these interactions involve others in various socio cultural contexts Since language is often the basis for monitoring and tracking learning gains in comprehension content knowledge and tool use in and out of school the role of situated cognition in language learning activities is important Membership and interaction in social and cultural groups is often determined by tools technologies and discourse use for full participation Language learning or literacy in various social and cultural groups must include how the groups work with and interact with these texts 29 Language instruction in the context of situated cognition also involves the skilled or novice use of language by members of the group and instruction of not only the elements of language but what is needed to bring a student to the level of expert Originating from emergent literacy 31 specialist language lessons examines the formal and informal styles and discourses of language use in socio cultural contexts 32 A function of specialist language lessons includes lucidly functional language or complex specialist language is usually accompanied by clear and lucid language used to explain the rules relationships or meanings existing between language and meaning 29 Legitimate peripheral participation Edit According to Jean Lave and Wenger 1991 legitimate peripheral participation LPP provides a framework to describe how individuals newcomers become part of a community of learners Legitimate peripheral participation was central to Lave and Wenger s take on situated cognition referred to as situated activity because it introduced socio cultural and historical realizations of power and access to the way thinking and knowing are legitimated They stated Hegemony over resources for learning and alienation from full participation are inherent in the shaping of the legitimacy and peripherality of participation in its historical realizations p 42 Lave and Wenger s 1991 research on the phenomenon of apprenticeship in communities of practice not only provided a unit of analysis for locating an individual s multiple changing levels and ways of participation but also implied that all participants through increased involvement have access to acquire and use resources available to their particular community To illustrate the role of LPP in situated activity Lave and Wenger 1991 examined five apprenticeship scenarios Yucatec midwives Vai and Gola tailors naval quartermasters meat cutters and non drinking alcoholics involved in AA Their analysis of apprenticeship across five different communities of learners lead them to several conclusions about the situatedness of LPP and its relationship to successful learning Key to newcomers success included access to all that community membership entails involvement in productive activity learning the discourse s of the community including talking about and talking within a practice p 109 and willingness of the community to capitalize on the inexperience of newcomers Insofar as this continual interaction of new perspectives is sanctioned everyone s participation is legitimately peripheral in some respect In other words everyone can to some degree be considered a newcomer to the future of a changing community 14 Planning vs action Edit Suchman s book Plans and Situated Actions The Problem of Human machine Communication 1987 provided a novel approach to the study of human computer interaction HCI By adopting an anthropological approach to sensemaking and interpretation Suchman was able to demonstrate how both action and planning were situated in the context of a flow of socially and materially mediated activities an idea that stimulated many of the later conceptualizations of situated cognition Her studies contrasted the deterministic approach to planning assumed by technology designers with the situated nature of planning as people make sense of the status of their workflow and adjust their course of action accordingly For example a photocopier will instruct its user to reload all pages in the original order after a jam whereas the user understands that they only need to copy the last page again By arguing that plans were the result of ongoing processes of prospective retrospective sense making Suchman identified the limits of technology system access to relevant social and material resources as a major cause of limitations in how technology supports human work 6 This position led to a major debate with Vera and Simon 1993 who argued that cognition is based on symbolic representations and that planning must therefore be deterministic based on a pre determined repertoire of learned response Most organizational theorists would now see this debate as reflecting individual cognitive vs socially situated levels of analysis requiring a similar need for paradigmatic co existence as Wave particle duality Suchman 1993 argues that planning in the context of work activity is similar to navigating a canoe through rapids you know what point on the river you are aiming for but you constantly adjust your course as you interact with rocks swells and currents on the way As a result many organizational theorists argue that plans can only be viewed as post hoc justifications of action while Suchman herself appears to view plans and actions as interrelated in the moment of action 7 Representation symbols and schemata Edit In situated theories the term representation refers to external forms in the environment that are created through social interactions to express meaning language art gestures etc and are perceived and acted upon in the first person sense Representing in the first person sense is conceived as an act of re experiencing in the imagination that involves the dialectic of ongoing perceiving and acting in coordination with the activation of neural structures and processes This form of reflective representation is considered to be a secondary type of learning while the primary form of learning is found in the adaptive recoordination that occurs with every behavior 33 Conceptualizing is considered to be a prelinguistic act while knowing involves creative interaction with symbols in both their interpretation and use for expression Schema develop as neural connections become biased through repeated activations to reactivate in situations that are perceived and conceived as temporally and compositionally similar to previous generalized situations 33 Goals intention and attention Edit nbsp Young Barab Model 1997 The Young Barab Model 1997 pictured to the left illustrates the dynamics of intentions and intentional dynamics involved in the agent s interaction with his environment when problem solving Dynamics of Intentions 34 goal intention adoption from among all possible goals ontological descent This describes how the learner decides whether or not to adopt a particular goal when presented with a problem Once a goal is adopted the learner proceeds by interacting with their environment through intentional dynamics There are many levels of intentions but at the moment of a particular occasion the agent has just one intention and that intention constrains his behavior until it is fulfilled or annihilated Intentional Dynamics 34 dynamics that unfold when the agent has only one intention goal and begins to act towards it perceiving and acting 21 It is a trajectory towards the achievement of a solution or goal the process of tuning one s perception attention Each intention is meaningfully bounded where the dynamics of that intention inform the agent of whether or not he is getting closer to achieving his goal If the agent is not getting closer to his goal he will take corrective action and then continue forward This is the agent s intentional dynamics and continues on until he achieves his goal Transfer Edit There are various definition of transfer found within the situated cognition umbrella Researchers interested in social practice often define transfer as increased participation 14 Ecological psychology perspectives define transfer as the detection of invariance across different situations 35 Furthermore transfer can only occur when there is a confluence of an individual s goals and objectives their acquired abilities to act and a set of affordances for action 36 Embodied cognition Edit Main article Embodied cognition The traditional cognition approach assumes that perception and motor systems are merely peripheral input and output devices 37 However embodied cognition posits that the mind and body interact on the fly as a single entity An example of embodied cognition is seen in the area of robotics where movements are not based on internal representations rather they are based on the robot s direct and immediate interaction with its environment 38 Additionally research has shown that embodied facial expressions influence judgments 39 and arm movements are related to a person s evaluation of a word or concept 40 In the latter example the individual would pull or push a lever towards his name at a faster rate for positive words than for negative words These results appeal to the embodied nature of situated cognition where knowledge is the achievement of the whole body in its interaction with the world Externalism EditAs to the mind by and large situated cognition paves the way to various form of externalism The issue is whether the situated aspect of cognition has only a practical value or it is somehow constitutive of cognition and perhaps of consciousness itself As to the latter possibility there are different positions David Chalmers and Andy Clark who developed the hugely debated model of the extended mind explicitly rejected the externalization of consciousness 41 For them only cognition is extended On the other hand others like Riccardo Manzotti 41 or Teed Rockwell 42 explicitly considered the possibility to situate conscious experience in the environment Pedagogical implications EditSince situated cognition views knowing as an action within specific contexts and views direct instruction models of knowledge transmission as impoverished there are significant implications for pedagogical practices First curriculum requires instructional design that draws on apprenticeship models common in real life 3 Second curricular design should rely on contextual narratives that situate concepts in practice Classroom practices such as project based learning and problem based learning would qualify as consistent with the situated learning perspective as would techniques such as Case Base Learning Anchored Instruction and cognitive apprenticeship Cognitive apprenticeship Edit Cognitive apprenticeships were one of the earliest pedagogical designs to incorporate the theories of situated cognition Brown Collins amp Duguid 1989 Cognitive apprenticeship uses four dimensions e g content methods sequence sociology to embed learning in activity and make deliberate the use of the social and physical contexts present in the classroom Brown Collins amp Duguid 1989 Collins Brown amp Newman 1989 Cognitive apprenticeship includes the enculturation of students into authentic practices through activity and social interaction Brown Collins amp Duguid 1989 The technique draws on the principles of Legitimate Peripheral Participation Lave amp Wenger 1991 and reciprocal teaching Palincsar amp Brown 1984 1989 in that a more knowledgeable other i e a teacher engages in a task with a more novice other i e a learner by describing their own thoughts as they work on the task providing just in time scaffolding modeling expert behaviors and encouraging reflection 43 The reflection process includes having students alternate between novice and expert strategies in a problem solving context sensitizing them to specifics of an expert performance and adjustments that may be made to their own performance to get them to the expert level Collins amp Brown 1988 Collins Brown amp Newman 1989 Thus the function of reflection indicates co investigation and or abstracted replay by students 44 Collins Brown and Newman 1989 emphasized six critical features of a cognitive apprenticeship that included observation coaching scaffolding modeling fading and reflection Using these critical features expert s guided students on their journey to acquire the cognitive and metacognitive processes and skills necessary to handle a variety of tasks in a range of situations 45 Reciprocal teaching a form of cognitive apprenticeship involves the modeling and coaching of various comprehension skills as teacher and students take turns in assuming the role of instructor Anchored instruction Edit Anchored instruction is grounded in a story or narrative that presents a realistic but fictional situation and raises an overarching question or problem compare with an essential question posed by a teacher This approach is designed to 1 engage the learner with a problem or series of related problems 2 require the learner to develop goals and discover subgoals related to solving the problem s and 3 provide the learner with extensive and diverse opportunities to explore the problem s in a shared context with classmates For example a Spanish teacher uses a video drama series focused on the murder of a main character Students work in small groups to summarize parts of the story to create hypotheses about the murderer and motive and to create a presentation of their solution to the class Stories are often paired so that across the set students can detect the invariant structure of the underlying knowledge so 2 episodes about distance rate time one about boats and one about planes so students can perceive how the distance rate time relationship holds across differences in vehicles The ideal smallest set of instances needed provide students the opportunity to detect invariant structure has been referred to as a generator set of situations The goal of anchored instruction is the engagement of intention and attention Through authentic tasks across multiple domains educators present situations that require students to create or adopt meaningful goals intentions One of the educator s objectives can be to set a goal through the use of an anchor problem 46 A classic example of anchored instruction is the Jasper series 47 The Jasper series includes a variety of videodisc adventures focused on problem formulation and problem solving Each videodisc used a visual narrative to present an authentic realistic everyday problem The objective was for students to adopt specific goals intentions after viewing the story and defining a problem These newly adopted goals guided students through the collaborative process of problem formulation and problem solving Perceiving and acting in avatar based virtual worlds Edit Virtual worlds provide unique affordances for embodied learning i e hands on interactive spatially oriented that ground learning in experience Here embodied means acting in a virtual world enabled by an avatar Contextual affordances of online games and virtual environments allow learners to engage in goal driven activity authentic interactions and collaborative problem solving all considered in situated theories of learning to be features of optimal learning In terms of situated assessment virtual worlds have the advantage of facilitating dynamic feedback that directs the perceiving acting agent through an avatar to continually improve performance Research methodologies EditThe situative perspective is focused on interactive systems in which individuals interact with one another and physical and representational systems Research takes place in situ and in real world settings reflecting assumptions that knowledge is constructed within specific contexts which have specific situational affordances Mixed methods and qualitative methodologies are the most prominently used by researchers In qualitative studies methods used are varied but the focus is often on the increased participation in specific communities of practice the affordances of the environment that are acted upon by the agent and the distributed nature of knowing in specific communities A major feature of quantitative methods used in situated cognition is the absence of outcome measures Quantitative variables used in mixed methods often focus on process over product For example trace nodes dribble files and hyperlink pathways are often used to track how students interact in the environment 48 Critiques of situativity EditIn Situated Action A Symbolic Interpretation Vera and Simon wrote the systems usually regarded as exemplifying Situated Action are thoroughly symbolic and representational and to the extent that they are limited in these respects have doubtful prospects for extension to complex tasks 49 Vera and Simon 1993 also claimed that the information processing view is supported by many years of research in which symbol systems simulated broad areas of human cognition and that there is no evidence of cognition without representation Anderson Reder and Simon 1996 summarized what they considered to be the four claims of situated learning and argued against each claim from a cognitivist perspective The claims and their arguments were Claim Activity and learning are bound to the specific situations in which they occur Argument Whether learning is bound to context or not depends on both the kind of learning and the way that it is learned Claim Knowledge does not transfer between tasks Argument There is ample evidence of successful transfer between tasks in the literature Transfer depends on initial practice and the degree to which a successive task has similar cognitive elements to a prior task Claim Teaching abstractions is ineffective Argument Abstract instruction can be made effective by combining of abstract concepts and concrete examples Claim Instruction must happen in complex social contexts Argument Research shows value in individual learning and on focusing individually on specific skills in a skill set Anderson Reder and Simons summarize their concerns when they say What is needed to improve learning and teaching is to continue to deepen our research into the circumstances that determine when narrower or broader contexts are required and when attention to narrower or broader skills are optimal for effective and efficient learning p 10 Considerations Edit However it is important to remember that a theory is neither wrong nor right but provides affordances for certain aspects of a problem 50 Lave and Wenger recognized this in their ironic comment How can we purport to be working out a theoretical conception of learning without engaging in the project of abstraction decontextualized knowledge rejected above Lave amp Wenger 1991 p 38 See also EditAction specific perception Activity theory Cognitive biology Cognitive psychology Computer supported collaborative learning Cultural historical activity theory CHAT Distributed cognition Dynamical systems models of cognition Embodied cognition Extended cognition Enactivism Group cognition Pragmatism Situational awarenessReferences EditNotes Edit John Seely Brown Collins amp Duguid 1989 Greeno 1989 Greeno amp Moore 1993 a b Brown Collins amp Duguid 1989 McLaughlin N 1999 Origin myths in the social sciences Fromm the Frankfurt School and the emergence of critical theory Canadian Journal of Sociology Cahiers canadiens de sociologie 109 139 Freire P 1996 Pedagogy of the oppressed revised New York Continuum 356 357 358 a b c Suchman Lucy 1987 Plans and situated actions The problem of human machine communication Cambridge MA Cambridge University Press ISBN 9780521337397 a b Suchman Lucy January 1993 Response to Vera and Simon s Situated Action A Symbolic Interpretation Cognitive Science 17 71 75 doi 10 1111 cogs 1993 17 issue 1 Lave J 1991 Situating learning in communities of practice Brown J S amp Duguid P 2000 Balancing act How to capture knowledge without killing it Harvard business review 78 3 73 80 Clancey G 2004 Local memory and worldly narrative the remote city in America and Japan Urban Studies 41 12 2335 2355 Brown Collins amp Duguid 1989 Young 2004 Barsalou L Grounded Cognition 2008 Annu Rev Psychol 2008 59 617 45 a b c d e f Lave amp Wenger 1991 James J Gibson 1986 Shaw Kadar Sim amp Reppenger 1992 Bredo 1994 Leu et al 2009 Gibson 1977 a b c Greeno 1994 a b Gibson 1979 1986 Young Kulikowich amp Barab 1997 p 139 Gibson 1979 1986 Greeno 1994 Young et al 1997 Gibson 1979 1986 Greeno 1994 Young Kulikowich amp Barab 1997 a b c d Barab amp Roth 2006 Young 1997 Young 2004b Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt 1990 a b c Gee 2010 Anderson 2006 Leadbeater amp Miller 2004 Dickinson amp Neuman 2006 Gee 2004 Gee 2004 Gee 2007 a b Clancey 1993 a b Kugler et al 1991 Shaw et al 1992 Young et al 1997 Young amp McNeese 1995 Young et al 1997 p 147 Niedenthal 2007 Wilson 2002 Wilson 2002 Niedenthal 2007 Markman amp Brendl 2005 a b Clark A 2008 Supersizing the Mind Oxford Oxford University Press Rockwell 2005 1561 Scardamalia amp Bereiter 1985 Scardamalia Bereiter amp Steinbach 1984 Scardamalia amp Bereiter 1983 Collins amp Brown 1988 Collins et al 1989 Barab amp Roth 2006 Young et al 1997 The Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt 1990 Young et al 1997 Young amp McNeese 1995 Shaw Effken Fajen Garret amp Morris 1997 Vera amp Simon 1993 p 7 Sfard A 1998 On two metaphors for learning and the dangers of choosing just one Educational Researcher 27 2 4 13 doi 10 3102 0013189x027002004 S2CID 135448246 Sources Edit Anderson J R Reder L M Simon H A 1996 Situated learning and education Educational Researcher 25 4 5 11 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 556 7550 doi 10 3102 0013189x025004005 S2CID 54548451 Bredo E 1994 Reconstructing educational psychology Situated cognition and Deweyian pragmatism Educational Psychologist 29 1 23 35 doi 10 1207 s15326985ep2901 3 Brown J S Collins A Duguid S 1989 Situated cognition and the culture of learning Educational Researcher 18 1 32 42 doi 10 3102 0013189x018001032 hdl 2142 17979 S2CID 9824073 Archived from the original on 2014 10 08 Clancey W J 1993 Situated action A neuropsychological interpretation response to Vera and Simon Cognitive Science 17 87 116 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog1701 7 Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt 1990 Anchored instruction and its relationship to situated cognition Educational Researcher 19 6 2 10 doi 10 3102 0013189x019006002 S2CID 7792800 Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt 1993 Anchored instruction and situated cognition revisited Educational Technology March Issue 52 70 Cognition and Technology Group at Vanderbilt 1994 From visual word problems to learning communities Changing conceptions of cognitive research In K McGilly Ed Classroom lessons Integrating cognitive theory and classroom proactice Cambridge MA MIT Press Driscoll M P 2004 Psychology of learning for instruction 3 ed Upper Saddle River NJ Allyn amp Bacon ISBN 978 0 205 37519 6 Dweck C S amp Leggett E L 1988 A social cognitive approach to motivation and personality Psychological Review 95 2 256 273 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 583 9142 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 95 2 256 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Engle R A 2006 Framing interactions to foster generative learning A situative explanation of transfer in a community of learners classroom Journal of the Learning Sciences 15 4 451 498 doi 10 1207 s15327809jls1504 2 S2CID 144839447 Eysenck M W amp Keane M T 2005 Cognitive psychology 5 ed New York Psychology Press ISBN 978 1 84169 359 0 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Gagne R M Wager W W Golas K C amp Keller J M 2005 Principles of Instructional Design 5 ed Belmont CA Wadsworth Thomson Learning ISBN 978 0 534 58284 5 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Gibson James J 1979 The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception Boston Houghton Mifflin ISBN 978 0 89859 959 6 Glenberg A M Robertson D A 1999 Indexical understanding of instructions Discourse Processes 28 1 1 26 doi 10 1080 01638539909545067 Greeno J G 1989 A perspective on thinking American Psychologist 44 2 134 141 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 44 2 134 Greeno J G 1994 Gibson s affordances Psychological Review 101 2 336 342 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 101 2 336 hdl 20 500 12749 3070 PMID 8022965 Greeno J G 1998 The situativity of knowing learning and research American Psychologist 53 1 5 26 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 53 1 5 Greeno J G 2006 Authoritative accountable positioning and connected general knowing Progressive themes in understanding transfer Journal of the Learning Sciences 15 4 539 550 doi 10 1207 s15327809jls1504 4 S2CID 145101010 Hart L A 1990 Human Brain amp Human Learning Black Diamond WA Books for Educators ISBN 978 0 9624475 9 4 Kugler P N Shaw R E Vicente K J amp Kinsella Shaw J 1991 The role of attractors in the self organization of intentional systems In R R Hoffman and D S Palermo Eds Cognition and the Symbolic Processes Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum Lave J 1977 Cognitive consequences of traditional apprenticeship training in West Africa Anthropology amp Education Quarterly 18 3 1776 180 doi 10 1525 aeq 1977 8 3 05x1512d Markman A B amp Brendl C M 2005 Constraining theories of embodied cognition Psychological Science 16 1 6 10 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 463 6440 doi 10 1111 j 0956 7976 2005 00772 x PMID 15660844 S2CID 12505177 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Niedenthal P M 2007 Embodying emotion Science 316 5827 1002 1005 Bibcode 2007Sci 316 1002N doi 10 1126 science 1136930 PMID 17510358 S2CID 14537829 Ormrod J E 2004 Human learning 4th ed Upper Saddle River NJ Pearson ISBN 978 0 13 094199 2 Palincsar A S amp Brown A L 1984 Reciprocal teaching of comprehension fostering and monitoring activities Cognition and Instruction 1 2 117 175 doi 10 1207 s1532690xci0102 1 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Roth W M 1996 Knowledge diffusion in a grade 4 5 classroom during a unit on civil engineering An analysis of a classroom community in terms of its changing resources and practices Cognition and Instruction 14 2 179 220 doi 10 1207 s1532690xci1402 2 Scardamalia M amp Bereiter C 2003 Knowledge building In J W Guthrie Ed Encyclopedia of Education 2nd Ed pp 1370 1373 New York Macmillan Reference Sfard A 1998 On two metaphors for learning and the dangers of choosing just one Educational Researcher 27 2 4 13 doi 10 3102 0013189x027002004 S2CID 135448246 Shadish W R Cook T D amp Campbell D T 2002 Experimental and quasi experimental designs for generalized causal inference Boston Houghton Mifflin Company ISBN 978 0 395 61556 0 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Shaw R E Effken J Fajen B R Garrett S R Morris A 1997 An ecological approach to the on line assessment of problem solving paths Principles and applications Instructional Science 25 2 151 166 doi 10 1023 A 1002975703555 S2CID 60918257 Shaw R E Kadar E Sim M Repperger D W 1992 The intentional spring A strategy for modeling systems that learn to perform intentional acts Journal of Motor Behavior 24 1 3 28 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 577 9342 doi 10 1080 00222895 1992 9941598 PMID 14766495 Weiner B 1994 Ability versus effort revisited The moral determinants of achievement evaluation and achievement as a moral system Educational Psychology Review 12 1 14 doi 10 1023 A 1009017532121 S2CID 19527829 Wilson M 2002 Six views of embodied cognition Psychonomic Bulletin amp Review 9 4 625 636 doi 10 3758 BF03196322 PMID 12613670 Wilson B B amp Myers K M 2000 Situated Cognition in Theoretical and Practical Context In D Jonassen amp S Land Eds Theoretical Foundations of Learning Environments pp 57 88 Mahway NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Young M F Kulikowich J M Barab S A 1997 The unit of analysis for situated assessment Instructional Science 25 2 133 150 doi 10 1023 A 1002971532689 S2CID 58143966 Young M amp McNeese M 1995 A Situated Cognition Approach to Problem Solving In P Hancock J Flach J Caid amp K Vicente Eds Local Applications of the Ecological Approach to Human Machine Systems pp 359 391 Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum Young M 2004a An Ecological Description of Video Games in Education Proceedings of the International Conference on Education and Information Systems Technologies and Applications EISTA July 22 Orlando FL pp 203 208 Young M 2004b An ecological psychology of instructional design Learning and thinking by perceiving acting systems In D H Jonassen Ed Handbook of Research for Educational Communications and Technology 2nd Ed Mahwah NJ Erlbaum Citations and further reading EditAnderson J R Greeno J G Reder L M Simon H A 2000 Perspectives on learning thinking and activity Educational Researcher 29 4 11 13 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 139 8833 doi 10 3102 0013189x029004011 S2CID 145255457 Anderson C 2006 The Long Tail Why the Future of Business Is Selling Less of More New York Hyperion Brown A L 1992 Design experiments Theoretical and methodological challenges in creating in complext interventions in classroom settings Journal of the Learning Sciences 2 2 141 178 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 133 5577 doi 10 1207 s15327809jls0202 2 Clancey William J 1997 Situated Cognition On Human Knowledge and Computer Representation New York Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 44871 0 Special Issue Situated Action Cognitive Science 17 1 January March 1993 Collins A amp Brown J 1988 The computer as a tool for learning through reflection In H Mandi amp A Lesgold Eds Learning issues for intelligent tutoring systems pp 1 18 New York Springer Verlag Collins A Brown J amp Newman S 1989 Cognitive Apprenticeship Teaching the craft of reading writing and mathematics In L B Resnick Ed Knowing learning and instruction Essays in honor of Robert Glaser Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum Dewey J 1938 Experience amp Education New York Simon amp Schuster ISBN 978 0 684 83828 1 Dickinson D K amp Neuman S B Eds 2006 Handbook of Early Literacy Research Vol 2 New York Guilford Press Gallagher S Robbins B D amp Bradatan C 2007 Special issue on the situated body Janus Head Journal of Interdisciplinary Studies in Literature Continental Philosophy Phenomenological Psychology and the Arts 9 2 URL http www janushead org 9 2 Gee J P 2004 Situated Language and learning A critique of traditional schooling London Routledge Gee J P 2007 Good video games and good learning Collected essays on video games learning and literacy New York Lang Gee J P 2010 A Situated Sociocultural Approach to Literacy and Technology In E Baker Ed The New Literacies Multiple Perspectives on Research and Practice pp 165 193 New York Guilford Press Greeno J G 1994 Gibson s affordances Psychological Review 101 2 336 342 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 101 2 336 hdl 20 500 12749 3070 PMID 8022965 Greeno J G 1997 On claims that answer the wrong question Educational Researcher 26 1 5 17 doi 10 3102 0013189X026001005 S2CID 146633367 Greeno J G Middle School Mathematics Through Applications Project Group 1998 The situativity of knowing learning and research American Psychologist 53 1 5 26 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 53 1 5 Greeno J G 2006 Authoritative accountable positioning and connected general knowing Progressive themes in understanding transfer Journal of the Learning Sciences 15 4 539 550 doi 10 1207 s15327809jls1504 4 S2CID 145101010 Griffin M M 1995 You can t get there from here Situated learning transfer and map skills Contemporary Educational Psychology 20 65 87 doi 10 1006 ceps 1995 1004 Hutchins E 1995 Cognition in the Wild Cambridge Mass The MIT Press A Bradford Book ISBN 978 0 262 58146 2 Keller C amp Keller J 1996 Cognition and Tool Use The Blacksmith at Work Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 55239 7 Kirshner D amp Whitson J A 1997 Situated Cognition Social semiotic and psychological perspectives Mahwah NJ Erlbaum ISBN 0 8058 2038 8 Kirshner D Whitson J A 1998 Obstacles to understanding cognition as situated Educational Researcher 27 8 22 28 doi 10 3102 0013189X027008022 JSTOR 1177113 S2CID 6528031 Lave J 1988 Cognition in Practice ISBN 978 0 521 35015 0 Lave J amp Wenger E 1991 Situated Learning Legitimate Peripheral Participation Cambridge Cambridge Univ Pr ISBN 978 0 521 42374 8 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Leadbeater C amp Miller P 2004 The Pro Am revolution How enthusiasts are changing our society and economy London Demos Leu D J O Byrne W I Zawilinski L McVerry J G Everett Cacopardo H 2009 Comments on Greenhow Robelia and Hughes Expanding the New Literacies Conversation Educational Researcher 39 4 264 269 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 612 6147 doi 10 3102 0013189X09336676 S2CID 145607514 Scardamalia M amp Bereiter C 1983 Child as co investigator Helping children gain insight into their own mental processes In S Paris G Olson amp H Stevenson Eds Learning and motivation in the classroom pp 83 107 Hillsdale NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Scardamalia M amp Bereiter C 1985 Fostering the development of self regulation in children s knowledge processing In S F Chipman J W Segal amp R Glaser Eds Thinking and learning skills Research and open questions pp 563 577 Hillsdale NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Scardamalia M Bereiter C Steinbach R 1984 Teachability of reflective processes in written composition Cognitive Science 8 2 173 190 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog0802 4 Suchman L 1987 Plans and situated actions The Problem of Human Machine Communication Cambridge University Press New York Suchman L 1993 Response to Vera and Simon s Situated Action A Symbolic Interpretation Cognitive Science 17 71 75 1993 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Situated cognition amp oldid 1170032148, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.