fbpx
Wikipedia

Explicit memory

Explicit memory (or declarative memory) is one of the two main types of long-term human memory, the other of which is implicit memory. Explicit memory is the conscious, intentional recollection of factual information, previous experiences, and concepts.[1] This type of memory is dependent upon three processes: acquisition, consolidation, and retrieval.[2][3] Explicit memory can be divided into two categories: episodic memory, which stores specific personal experiences, and semantic memory, which stores factual information.[4] Explicit memory requires gradual learning, with multiple presentations of a stimulus and response.

The counterpart to explicit memory is known as implicit memory, refers to memories acquired and used unconsciously such as skills (e.g. knowing how to get dressed) or perception. Unlike explicit memory, implicit memory learns rapidly, even from a single stimulus, and it is influenced by other mental systems.

Sometimes a distinction is made between explicit memory and declarative memory. In such cases, explicit memory relates to any kind of conscious memory, and declarative memory relates to any kind of memory that can be described in words; however, if it is assumed that a memory cannot be described without being conscious and vice versa, then the two concepts are identical.

Types

Episodic memory

Episodic memory consists of the storage and recollection of observational information attached to specific life-events. These can be memories that happened to the subject directly or just memories of events that happened around them. Episodic memory is what people generally think of when they talk about memory. Episodic memory allows for recalling various contextual and situational details of one's previous experiences.

Some examples of episodic memory include the memory of entering a specific classroom for the first time, the memory of storing your carry-on baggage while boarding a plane, headed to a specific destination on a specific day and time, the memory of being notified that one are being terminated from their job, or the memory of notifying a subordinate that they are being terminated from their job. The retrieval of these episodic memories can be thought of as the action of mentally reliving in detail the past events that they concern.[4] Episodic memory is believed to be the system that provides the basic support for semantic memory.

Semantic memory

Semantic memory refers to general world knowledge (facts, ideas, meaning and concepts) that can be articulated and is independent of personal experience.[5] This includes world knowledge, object knowledge, language knowledge, and conceptual priming. Semantic memory is distinct from episodic memory, which is the memory of experiences and specific events that occur during people's lives, from which they can recreate at any given point.[6] For instance, semantic memory might contain information about what a cat is, whereas episodic memory might contain a specific memory of petting a particular cat. Humans can learn about new concepts by applying their knowledge learned from things in the past.[7]

Other examples of semantic memory include types of food, capital cities of a geographic region, facts about people, dates, or the lexicon of a language, such as a person's vocabulary.[4]

Hybrid types

Autobiographical memory is a memory system consisting of episodes recollected from an individual's life, based on a combination of episodic (personal experiences and specific objects, people and events experienced at particular time and place) and semantic (general knowledge and facts about the world) memory.[8]

Spatial memory is the part of memory responsible for recording information about one's environment and its spatial orientation. For example, a person's spatial memory is required in order to navigate around a familiar city, just as a rat's spatial memory is needed to learn the location of food at the end of a maze. It is often argued that in both humans and animals, spatial memories are summarized as a cognitive map. Spatial memory has representations within working, short-term and long-term memory. Research indicates that there are specific areas of the brain associated with spatial memory. Many methods are used for measuring spatial memory in children, adults, and animals.

Examples

Long-Term Memory Subtype Description Example
Declarative (explicit) Conscious memory of facts and events
Semantic Factual information The capital of Germany is Berlin
Episodic Specific personal experiences Your 10th birthday
Non-declarative (implicit) Modes of learning that are nonconscious - one learning to perform a sequence of actions that do not necessarily invoke knowledge
Priming Also known as pattern completion, where one has the ability to complete a pattern they have once seen before. This priming differs from priming in Psychology. If you were given a picture of half of a letter from the alphabet and you recognized which letter it is, you would be able to complete the letter.
Perceptual learning Perceptual ability to differentiate sensories through experience of stimuli Differentiating between categories such as smells, colors, tastes
Category learning "...the process of establishing a memory trace that improves the efficiency of assigning novel objects to contrasting groups" [9] Movie genres, breeds of dogs, types of fruits
Emotional learning "...retention of classically conditioned emotional relationships that cannot be voluntarily recollected or reported"[citation needed] Being afraid of dogs yet you cannot explain why
Procedural learning The formation of skills and habits Learning how to ride a bike

The model of language

Declarative and procedural memory fall into two categories of human language. Declarative memory system is used by the lexicon. Declarative memory stores all arbitrary, unique word-specific knowledge, including word meanings, word sounds, and abstract representations such as word category. In other words, declarative memory is where random bits and pieces of knowledge about language that are specific and unpredictable are stored. Declarative memory includes representations of simple words (e.g. cat), bound morphemes (morphemes that have to go together), irregular morphological forms, verb complements, and idioms (or non-compositional semantic units). Irregular morphological structures fall into the declarative system; the irregularities (such as went being the past form of go or idioms) are what we have to memorize.

Declarative memory supports a superposition associative memory, which allows for generalizations across representations. For example, the memorization of phonologically similar stem-irregular past tense pairs (e.g. spring-sprung, sing-sang) may allow for memory-based generalization to new irregularities, either from real words (bring-brought) or from novel ones (spring-sprung). This ability to generalize could underlie some degree of productivity within the memory system.

While declarative memory deals with irregularities of morphology, procedural memory uses regular phonology and regular morphology. Procedural memory system is used by grammar, where grammar is defined by the building of a rule governed structure. Language's ability to use grammar comes from procedural memory, making grammar like another procedure. It underlies the learning of new, and already learned, rule-based procedures that oversee the regularities of language, particularly those procedures related to combining items into complex structures that have precedence and hierarchical relations- precedence in the sense of left to right and hierarchical in the sense of top to bottom. Procedural memory builds rule-governed structure (merging or series) of forms and representations into complex structures such as:

  1. Phonology
  2. Inflectional and derivational morphology
  3. Compositional semantics (the meaning of composition of words into complex structures)
  4. Syntax

Broca and Wernicke's Brain Region

Broca's area is important to procedural memory, because, "Broca's area is involved in the expressive aspects of spoken and written language (production of sentences constrained by the rules of grammar and syntax)."[10] Broca's area corresponds to parts of the inferior frontal gyrus, presumably Brodmann's area 44 and 45. Procedural memory is affected by Broca's aphasia. Agrammatism is apparent in Broca's aphasia patients, where a lack of fluency and omission of morphology and function words occur. While those with Broca's aphasia are still able to understand or comprehend speech, they have difficulty producing it. Speech production becomes more difficult when sentences are complex; for example, the passive voice is a grammatically complex structure that is harder for those with Broca's aphasia to comprehend. Wernicke's area is crucial for language development, focusing on the comprehension of speech, rather than speech production. Wernicke's aphasia affects declarative memory. Opposite of Broca's aphasia, paragrammatism is apparent, which causes normal or excessive fluency and use of inappropriate words (neologisms). Those with Wernicke's aphasia struggle to understand the meaning of words and may not recognize their mistakes in speech.

History

The study of human memory stretches back over the last 2000 years. An early attempt to understand memory can be found in Aristotle's major treatise, On the Soul, in which he compares the human mind to a blank slate.[11] He theorized that all humans are born free of any knowledge and are the sum of their experiences. It wasn't until the late 1800s, however, that a young German philosopher by the name of Herman Ebbinghaus developed the first scientific approach to studying memory.[12] While some of his findings have endured and remain relevant to this day (Learning Curve), his greatest contribution to the field of memory research was demonstrating that memory can be studied scientifically. In 1972, Endel Tulving proposed the distinction between episodic and semantic memory.[4] This was quickly adopted and is now widely accepted. Following this, in 1985, Daniel Schacter proposed a more general distinction between explicit (declarative) and implicit (procedural) memory[13] With the recent advances in neuroimaging technology, there have been a multitude of findings linking specific brain areas to declarative memory. Despite these advances in Cognitive psychology, there is still much to be discovered in terms of the operating mechanisms of declarative memory.[14] It is unclear whether declarative memory is mediated by a particular memory system or if it is more accurately classified as a type of knowledge and it is not known how or why declarative memory evolved to begin with.[14]

Neuropsychology

Normal brain function

Hippocampus

 
Hippocampus as seen in red

Although many psychologists believe that the entire brain is involved with memory, the hippocampus, and surrounding structures appear to be most important in declarative memory specifically.[15] The ability to retain and recall episodic memories is highly dependent on the hippocampus,[15] whereas the formation of new declarative memories relies on both the hippocampus and the parahippocampus.[16] Other studies have found that the parahippocampal cortices were related to superior recognition memory.[16]

The Three Stage Model was developed by Eichenbaum, et al. (2001), and proposes that the hippocampus does three things with episodic memory:

  1. Mediates the recording of episodic memories
  2. Identifies common features between episodes
  3. Links these common episodes in a memory space.

To support this model, a version of Piaget's Transitive Inference Task was used to show that the hippocampus is in fact used as the memory space.[15]

When experiencing an event for the first time, a link is formed in the hippocampus allowing us to recall that event in the future. Separate links are also made for features related to that event. For example, when you meet someone new, a unique link is created for them. More links are then connected to that person's link so you can remember what colour their shirt was, what the weather was like when you met them, etc. Specific episodes are made easier to remember and recall by repeatedly exposing oneself to them (which strengthens the links in the memory space) allowing for faster retrieval when remembering.[15]

Hippocampal cells (neurons) are activated depending on what information one is exposed to at that moment. Some cells are specific to spatial information, certain stimuli (smells, etc.), or behaviours as has been shown in a Radial Maze Task.[15] It is therefore the hippocampus that allows us to recognize certain situations, environments, etc. as being either distinct or similar to others. However, the Three Stage Model does not incorporate the importance of other cortical structures in memory.

The anatomy of the hippocampus is largely conserved across mammals, and the role of these areas in declarative memory are conserved across species as well. The organization and neural pathways of the hippocampus are very similar in humans and other mammal species. In humans and other mammals, a cross-section of the hippocampus shows the dentate gyrus as well as the dense cell layers of the CA fields. The intrinsic connectivity of these areas are also conserved.[17]

Results from an experiment by Davachi, Mitchell, and Wagner (2003) and subsequent research (Davachi, 2006) shows that activation in the hippocampus during encoding is related to a subject's ability to recall prior events or later relational memories. These tests did not differentiate between individual test items later seen and those forgotten.[18][19]

Prefrontal cortex

The lateral Prefrontal cortex (PFC) is essential for remembering contextual details of an experience rather than for memory formation.[16] The PFC is also more involved with episodic memory than semantic memory, although it does play a small role in semantics.[20]

Using PET studies and word stimuli, Endel Tulving found that remembering is an automatic process.[21] It is also well documented that a hemispheric asymmetry occurs in the PFC: When encoding memories, the Left Dorsolateral PFC (LPFC) is activated, and when retrieving memories, activation is seen in the Right Dorsolateral PFC (RPFC).[21]

Studies have also shown that the PFC is extremely involved with autonoetic consciousness (See Tulving's theory).[22] This is responsible for humans' recollective experiences and 'mental time travelling' abilities (characteristics of episodic memory).

 
Amygdala as seen in red

Amygdala

The amygdala is believed to be involved in the encoding and retrieval of emotionally charged memories. Much of the evidence for this has come from research on a phenomenon known as flashbulb memories. These are instances in which memories of powerful emotional events are more highly detailed and enduring than regular memories (e.g. September 11 attacks, assassination of JFK). These memories have been linked to increased activation in the amygdala.[23] Recent studies of patients with damage to the amygdala suggest that it is involved in memory for general knowledge, and not for specific information.[24][25]

Other structures involved

The regions of the diencephalon have shown brain activation when a remote memory is being recovered[20] and the occipital lobe, ventral temporal lobe, and fusiform gyrus all play a role in memory formation.[16]

Lesion studies

Lesion studies are commonly used in cognitive neuroscience research. Lesions can occur naturally through trauma or disease, or they can be surgically induced by researchers. In the study of declarative memory, the hippocampus and the amygdala are two structures frequently examined using this technique.

Hippocampal lesion studies

 
The Morris water maze

The Morris water navigation task tests spatial learning in rats.[26] In this test rats learn to escape from a pool by swimming toward a platform submerged just below the surface of the water. Visual cues that surround the pool (e.g. a chair or window) help the rat to locate the platform on subsequent trials. The rats' use of specific events, cues, and places are all forms of declarative memory.[27] Two groups of rats are observed: a control group with no lesions and an experimental group with hippocampal lesions. In this task created by Morris, rats are placed in the pool at the same position for 12 trials. Each trial is timed and the path taken by the rats is recorded. Rats with hippocampal lesions successfully learn to find the platform. If the starting point is moved, the rats with hippocampal lesions typically fail to locate the platform. The control rats, however, are able to find the platform using the cues acquired during the learning trials.[26] This demonstrates the involvement of the hippocampus in declarative memory.[27]

The Odor-odor Recognition Task, devised by Bunsey and Eichenbaum, involves a social encounter between two rats (a subject and a demonstrator). The demonstrator, after eating a specific type of food, interacts with the subject rat, who then smells the food odor on the other's breath. The experimenters then present the subject rat with a decision between two food options; the food previously eaten by the demonstrator, and a novel food. The researchers found that when there was no time delay, both control rats and rats with lesions chose the familiar food. After 24 hours, however, the rats with hippocampal lesions were just as likely to eat both types of food, while control rats chose the familiar food.[28] This can be attributed to the inability to form episodic memories due to lesions in the hippocampus. The effects of this study can be observed in humans with amnesia, indicating the role of the hippocampus in developing episodic memories that can be generalized to similar situations.[27]

Henry Molaison, previously known as H.M., had parts of both his left and right medial temporal lobes (hippocampi) removed which resulted in the loss of the ability to form new memories.[29] The long-term declarative memory was crucially affected when the structures from the medial temporal lobe were removed, including the ability to form new semantic knowledge and memories.[30] The dissociation in Molaison between the acquisition of declarative memory and other kinds of learning was seen initially in motor learning.[31] Molaison's declarative memory was not functioning, as was seen when Molaison completed the task of repetition priming. His performance does improve over trials, however, his scores were inferior to those of control participants.[32] In the condition of Molaison the same results from this priming task are reflected when looking at the other basic memory functions like remembering, recall and recognizing.[29] Lesions should not be interpreted as an all-or-nothing condition, in the case of Molaison not all memory and recognition is lost, although the declarative memory is severely damaged he still has a sense of self and memories that were developed before the lesion occurred.[33]

Patient R.B. was another clinical case reinforcing the role of the hippocampus in declarative memory. After suffering an ischemic episode during a cardiac bypass operation, Patient R.B. awoke with a severe anterograde amnesic disorder. IQ and cognition were unaffected, but declarative memory deficits were observed (although not to the extent of that seen in Molaison). Upon death, an autopsy revealed that Patient R.B. had bilateral lesions of the CA1 cell region along the whole length of the hippocampus.

Amygdala lesion studies

Adolph, Cahill and Schul completed a study showing that emotional arousal facilitates the encoding of material into long term declarative memory.[34] They selected two subjects with bilateral damage to the amygdala, as well as six control subjects and six subjects with brain damage. All subjects were shown a series of twelve slides accompanied by a narrative. The slides varied in the degree to which they evoked emotion – slides 1 through 4 and slides 9 through 12 contain non-emotional content. Slides 5 through 8 contain emotional material, and the seventh slide contained the most emotionally arousing image and description (a picture of surgically repaired legs of a car crash victim).[34]

The emotionally arousing slide (slide 7) was remembered no better by the bilateral damage participants than any of the other slides. All other participants notably remembered the seventh slide the best and in most detail out of all the other slides.[34] This shows that the amygdala is necessary to facilitate encoding of declarative knowledge regarding emotionally arousing stimuli, but is not required for encoding knowledge of emotionally neutral stimuli.[35]

Factors that affect declarative memory

Stress

Stress may have an effect on the recall of declarative memories. Lupien, et al. completed a study that had 3 phases for participants to take part in. Phase 1 involved memorizing a series of words, phase 2 entailed either a stressful (public speaking) or non-stressful situation (an attention task), and phase 3 required participants to recall the words they learned in phase 1. There were signs of decreased declarative memory performance in the participants that had to complete the stressful situation after learning the words.[36] Recall performance after the stressful situation was found to be worse overall than after the non-stressful situation. It was also found that performance differed based on whether the participant responded to the stressful situation with an increase in measured levels of salivary cortisol.

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) emerges after exposure to a traumatic event eliciting fear, horror or helplessness that involves bodily injury, the threat of injury, or death to one's self or another person.[37] The chronic stress in PTSD contributes to an observed decrease in hippocampal volume and declarative memory deficits.[38]

Stress can alter memory functions, reward, immune function, metabolism and susceptibility to different diseases.[39] Disease risk is particularly pertinent to mental illnesses, whereby chronic or severe stress remains a common risk factor for several mental illnesses.[40] One system suggests there are five types of stress labeled acute time-limited stressors, brief naturalistic stressors, stressful event sequences, chronic stressors, and distant stressors. An acute time-limited stressor involves a short-term challenge, while a brief natural stressor involves an event that is normal but nevertheless challenging. A stressful event sequence is a stressor that occurs, and then continues to yield stress into the immediate future. A chronic stressor involves exposure to a long-term stressor, and a distant stressor is a stressor that is not immediate.[41]

Neurochemical factors of stress on the brain

Cortisol is the primary glucocorticoid in the human body. In the brain, it modulates the ability of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex to process memories.[42] Although the exact molecular mechanism of how glucocorticoids influence memory formation is unknown, the presence of glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex tell us these structures are some of its many targets.[42] It has been demonstrated that cortisone, a glucocorticoid, impaired blood flow in the right parahippocampal gyrus, left visual cortex and cerebellum.[42]

A study by Damoiseaux et al. (2007) evaluated the effects of glucocorticoids on hippocampal and prefrontal cortex activation during declarative memory retrieval. They found that administration of hydrocortisone (name given to cortisol when it is used as a medication) to participants one hour before retrieval of information impairs free recall of words, yet when administered before or after learning they had no effect on recall.[42] They also found that hydrocortisone decreases brain activity in the above-mentioned areas during declarative memory retrieval.[42] Therefore, naturally occurring elevations of cortisol during periods of stress lead to impairment of declarative memory.[42]

It is important to note that this study involved only male subjects, which may be significant as sex steroid hormones may have different effects in response to cortisol administration. Men and women also respond to emotional stimuli differently and this may affect cortisol levels. This was also the first Functional magnetic resonance imaging(fMRI) study done utilising glucocorticoids, therefore more research is necessary to further substantiate these findings.[42]

Consolidation during sleep

It is believed that sleep plays an active role in consolidation of declarative memory. Specifically, sleep's unique properties enhance memory consolidation, such as the reactivation of newly learned memories during sleep. For example, it has been suggested that the central mechanism for consolidation of declarative memory during sleep is the reactivation of hippocampal memory representations. This reactivation transfers information to neocortical networks where it is integrated into long-term representations.[43] Studies on rats involving maze learning found that hippocampal neuronal assemblies that are used in the encoding of spatial information are reactivated in the same temporal order.[44] Similarly, positron emission tomography (PET) has shown reactivation of the hippocampus in slow-wave sleep (SWS) after spatial learning.[45] Together these studies show that newly learned memories are reactivated during sleep and through this process new memory traces are consolidated.[46] In addition, researchers have identified three types of sleep (SWS, sleep spindle and REM) in which declarative memory is consolidated.

Slow-wave sleep, often referred to as deep sleep, plays the most important role in consolidation of declarative memory and there is a large amount of evidence to support this claim. One study found that the first 3.5 hours of sleep offer the greatest performance enhancement on memory recall tasks because the first couple of hours are dominated by SWS. Additional hours of sleep do not add to the initial level of performance. Thus this study suggests that full sleep may not be important for optimal performance of memory.[47] Another study shows that people who experience SWS during the first half of their sleep cycle compared to subjects who did not, showed better recall of information. However this is not the case for subjects who were tested for the second half of their sleep cycle, as they experience less SWS.[48]

Another key piece of evidence regarding SWS's involvement in declarative memory consolidation is a finding that people with pathological conditions of sleep, such as insomnia, exhibit both reduction in Slow-Wave Sleep and also have impaired consolidation of declarative memory during sleep.[49] Another study found that middle aged people compared to young group had a worse retrieval of memories. This in turn indicated that SWS is associated with poor declarative memory consolidation but not with age itself.[50]

Some researchers suggest that sleep spindle, a burst of brain activity occurring during stage 2 sleep, plays a role in boosting consolidation of declarative memories.[51] Critics point out that spindle activity is positively correlated with intelligence.[52] In contrast, Schabus and Gruber point out that sleep spindle activity only relates to performance on newly learned memories and not to absolute performance. This supports the hypothesis that sleep spindle helps to consolidate recent memory traces but not memory performance in general.[53] The relationship between sleep spindles and declarative memory consolidation is not yet fully understood.[53]

There is a relatively small body of evidence that supports the idea that REM sleep helps consolidate highly emotional declarative memories. For instance Wagner, et al. compared memory retention for emotional versus neutral text over two instances; early sleep that is dominated by SWS and late sleep that is dominated by REM phase.[54] This study found that sleep improved memory retention of emotional text only during late sleep phase, which was primarily REM. Similarly, Hu & Stylos-Allen, et al. performed a study with emotional versus neutral pictures and concluded that REM sleep facilitates consolidation of emotional declarative memories.[55]

The view that sleep plays an active role in declarative memory consolidation is not shared by all researchers. For instance Ellenbogen, et al. argue that sleep actively protects declarative memory from associative interference.[56] Furthermore, Wixted believes that the sole role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation is nothing more but creating ideal conditions for memory consolidation.[57] For example, when awake, people are bombarded with mental activity which interferes with effective consolidation. However, during sleep, when interference is minimal, memories can be consolidated without associative interference. More research is needed to make a definite statement whether sleep creates favourable conditions for consolidation or it actively enhances declarative memory consolidation.[46]

Encoding and retrieval

The encoding of explicit memory depends on conceptually driven, top-down processing, in which a subject reorganizes the data to store it.[58] The subject makes associations with previously related stimuli or experiences.[59] This was termed deep encoding by Fergus Craik and Robert Lockhart.[60] This way a memory persists longer and will be remembered well. The later recall of information is thus greatly influenced by the way in which the information was originally processed.[58]

The depth-of-processing effect is the improvement in subsequent recall of an object about which a person has given thought to its meaning or shape. Simply put: To create explicit memories, you have to do something with your experiences: think about them, talk about them, write them down, study them, etc. The more you do, the better you will remember. Testing of information while learning has also shown to improve encoding in explicit memory. If a student reads a text book and then tests themselves afterward, their semantic memory of what was read is improved. This study – test method improves encoding of information. This Phenomenon is referred to as the Testing Effect.[61]

Retrieval: Because a person has played an active role in processing explicit information, the internal cues that were used in processing it can also be used to initiate spontaneous recall.[58] When someone talks about an experience, the words they use will help when they try to remember this experience at a later date. The conditions in which information is memorized can affect recall. If a person has the same surroundings or cues when the original information is presented, they are more likely to remember it. This is referred to as encoding specificity and it also applies to explicit memory. In a study where subjects were asked to perform a cued recall task participants with a high working memory did better than participants with a low working memory when the conditions were maintained. When the conditions were changed for recall both groups dropped. The subjects with higher working memory declined more.[62] This is thought to happen because matching environments activates areas of the brain known as the left inferior frontal gyrus and the hippocampus.[63]

Neural structures involved

Several neural structures are proposed to be involved in explicit memory. Most are in the temporal lobe or closely related to it, such as the amygdala, the hippocampus, the rhinal cortex in the temporal lobe, and the prefrontal cortex.[58] Nuclei in the thalamus also are included, because many connections between the prefrontal cortex and temporal cortex are made through the thalamus.[58] The regions that make up the explicit memory circuit receive input from the neocortex and from brainstem systems, including acetylcholine, serotonin, and noradrenaline systems.[64]

Traumatic brain injury

While the human brain is certainly regarded for its plasticity, there is some evidence that shows traumatic brain injury (TBI) in young children can have negative effects on explicit memory. Researchers have looked at children with TBI in early childhood (i.e. infancy) and late childhood. Findings showed that children with severe TBI in late childhood experienced impaired explicit memory while still maintaining implicit memory formation. Researchers also found that children with severe TBI in early childhood had both increased chance of having both impaired explicit memory and implicit memory. While children with severe TBI are at risk for impaired explicit memory, the chances of impaired explicit memory in adults with severe TBI is much greater.[65]

Memory loss

Alzheimer's disease has a profound effect on explicit memory. Mild cognitive impairment is an early sign of Alzheimer's disease. People with memory conditions often receive cognitive training. When an fMRI was used to view brain activity after training, it found increased activation in various neural systems that are involved with explicit memory.[66] People with Alzheimer's have problems learning new tasks. However, if the task is presented repeatedly they can learn and retain some new knowledge of the task. This effect is more apparent if the information is familiar. The person with Alzheimer's must also be guided through the task and prevented from making errors.[67] Alzheimer's also has an effect on explicit spatial memory. This means that people with Alzheimer's have difficulty remembering where items are placed in unfamiliar environments.[68] The hippocampus has been shown to become active in semantic and episodic memory.[69]

The effects of Alzheimer's disease are seen in the episodic part of explicit memory. This can lead to problems with communication. A study was conducted where Alzheimer's patients were asked to name a variety of objects from different periods. The results shown that their ability to name the object depended on frequency of use of the item and when the item was first acquired.[70] This effect on semantic memory also has an effect on music and tones. Alzheimer's patients have difficulty distinguishing between different melodies they have never heard before. People with Alzheimer's also have issues with picturing future events. This is due to a deficit in episodic future thinking.[71] There are many other reasons why adults and others may begin to have memory loss.

In popular culture

Amnesiacs are frequently portrayed in television and movies. Some of the better-known examples include:

In the romantic comedy 50 First Dates (2004), Adam Sandler plays veterinarian Henry Roth, who falls for Lucy Whitmore, played by Drew Barrymore. Having lost her short-term memory in a car crash, Lucy can only remember the current day's events until she falls asleep. When she wakes up the next morning, she has no recollection of the previous day's experiences.[72] These experiences would normally be transferred into declarative knowledge, allowing them to be recalled in the future. Although this movie is not the most accurate representation of a true amnesic patient, it is useful for informing viewers of the detrimental effects of amnesia.

Memento (2000) a film inspired by the case of Henry Molaison (H.M.).[73] Guy Pearce plays an ex-insurance investigator suffering from severe anterograde amnesia caused by a head injury. Unlike most amnesiacs, Leonard retains his identity and the memories of events that occurred before the injury, but loses all ability to form new memories. This loss of ability to form new memories indicates that the head injury affected the medial temporal lobe of the brain resulting in the inability for Leonard to form declarative memory.

Finding Nemo features a reef fish named Dory with an inability to develop declarative memory. This prevents her from learning or retaining any new information such as names or directions. The exact origin of Dory's impairment is not mentioned in the film, but her memory loss accurately portrays the difficulties facing amnesiacs.[72]

See also

References

  1. ^ Ullman, MT (2004). "Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model". Cognition. 92 (1–2): 231–70. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.008. PMID 15037131. S2CID 14611894.
  2. ^ Lazzarim, Mayla K, Targa, Adriano, Sardi, Natalia F, et al. Pain impairs consolidation, but not acquisition or retrieval of a declarative memory. Behavioural Pharmacology. 2020;31(8):707-715. doi:10.1097/FBP.0000000000000576.
  3. ^ Kandel ER, Dudai, Y, Mayford MR. (2014). The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell157:163–186
  4. ^ a b c d Tulving E. 1972. Episodic and semantic memory. In Organization of Memory, ed. E Tulving, W Donaldson, pp. 381–403. New York: Academic
  5. ^ McRae, Ken; Jones, Michael (2013). Reisberg, Daniel (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. pp. 206–216. ISBN 9780195376746.
  6. ^ Tulving, Endel (2002). "Episodic Memory: From Mind to Brain". Annual Review of Psychology. 53: 1–25. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114. PMID 11752477.
  7. ^ Saumier, D.; Chertkow, H. (2002). "Semantic Memory". Current Science. 2 (6): 516–522. doi:10.1007/s11910-002-0039-9. PMID 12359106. S2CID 14184578.
  8. ^ Williams, H. L., Conway, M. A., & Cohen, G. (2008). Autobiographical memory. In G. Cohen & M. A. Conway (Eds.), Memory in the Real World (3rd ed., pp. 21-90). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
  9. ^ Ell, Shawn; Zilioli, Monica (2012), "Categorical Learning", in Seel, Norbert M. (ed.), Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning, Springer US, pp. 509–512, doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-1428-6_98, ISBN 978-1-4419-1428-6
  10. ^ "Broca's Area - an overview | ScienceDirect Topics". www.sciencedirect.com. Retrieved 2019-12-17.
  11. ^ Aristotle, On the Soul (De Anima), in Aristotle, Volume 4, Loeb Classical Library, William Heinemann, London, UK, 1936.
  12. ^ Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology. Teachers College, Columbia University
  13. ^ Graf, P.; Schacter, D. L. (1985). "Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 11 (3): 501–518. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.11.3.501. PMID 3160813.
  14. ^ a b Eichenbaum, Howard (1997). "Declarative memory: Insights from cognitive neurobiology". Annual Review of Psychology. 48: 547–572. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.48.1.547. PMID 9046568.
  15. ^ a b c d e Eichenbaum, Howard (2001). "The Hippocampus and Declarative Memory: Cognitive Mechanisms and Neural Codes". Behavioural Brain Research. 127 (1–2): 199–207. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(01)00365-5. PMID 11718892. S2CID 20843130.
  16. ^ a b c d Gabrieli, J.; Kao, Y. (2007). "Development of the Declarative Memory System in the Human Brain". Nature Neuroscience. 10 (9): 1198–1205. doi:10.1038/nn1950. PMID 17676059. S2CID 15637865.
  17. ^ Manns, Joseph; Eichenbaum, Howard (September 2006). "Evolution of Declarative Memory". Hippocampus. 16 (9): 795–808. doi:10.1002/hipo.20205. PMID 16881079. S2CID 39081299.
  18. ^ Davachi, L.; Mitchell, J.P.; Wagner, A.D. (2003). "Multiple routes to memory: Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 100 (4): 2157–2162. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.2157D. doi:10.1073/pnas.0337195100. PMC 149975. PMID 12578977.
  19. ^ Davachi, Dobbins (2008). "Declarative Memory". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (2): 112–118. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00559.x. PMC 2790294. PMID 20011622.
  20. ^ a b Graham, S.; Levine, B. (2004). "The Fundamental Neuroanatomy of Episodic and Semantic Autobiographical Remembering: A Prospective Functional MRI Study". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 16 (9): 1633–1646. doi:10.1162/0898929042568587. PMID 15601525. S2CID 32682464.
  21. ^ a b Craik, F.I.M.; Houle, S. (1994). "Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Human Episodic Memory: Lessons From PET Studies". Biol. Psychiatry. 42: 75S–76S. doi:10.1016/s0006-3223(97)87185-5. S2CID 54399679.
  22. ^ Stuss, D.T.; Tulving, E. (1997). "Toward a Theory of Episodic Memory: The Frontal Lobes & Autonoetic Consciousness". Psychological Bulletin. 121 (3): 331–354. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.331. PMID 9136640.
  23. ^ Sharot, T; Martorella, EA; Delgado, MR; Phelps, EA (2007). "How personal experience modulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11". Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 104 (1): 389–394. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104..389S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0609230103. PMC 1713166. PMID 17182739.
  24. ^ Adolphs, R; Tranel, D; Buchanan, TW (2005). "Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli" (PDF). Nat Neurosci. 8 (4): 512–518. doi:10.1038/nn1413. PMID 15735643. S2CID 205430564.
  25. ^ Adolphs, R; Denburg, NL; Tranel, D (2001). "The amygdala's role in long-term declarative memory for gist and detail". Behav Neurosci. 115 (5): 983–992. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.115.5.983. PMID 11584931.
  26. ^ a b Eichenbaum, H.; Stewart, C.; Morris, R. G. M. (1990). "Hippocampal representation in spatial learning". J. Neurosci. 10 (11): 331–339. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.10-11-03531.1990. PMC 6570096. PMID 2230943.
  27. ^ a b c Eichenbaum, H (2000). "A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 1 (1): 41–50. doi:10.1038/35036213. PMID 11252767. S2CID 5242470.
  28. ^ Bunsey, M.; Eichenbaum, H. (1995). "Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus-stimulus association". Hippocampus. 5 (6): 546–556. doi:10.1002/hipo.450050606. PMID 8646281. S2CID 37972779.
  29. ^ a b Corkin, S (2002). "Perspectives: What's new with the amnesic patient H.M.?". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 3 (2): 153–160. doi:10.1038/nrn726. PMID 11836523. S2CID 5429133.
  30. ^ Gabrieli, J. D. E.; Cohen, N. J.; Corkin, S. (1988). "The impaired learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal-lobe resection". Brain Cogn. 7 (2): 157–177. doi:10.1016/0278-2626(88)90027-9. PMID 3377896. S2CID 23026117.
  31. ^ Corkin, S (1968). "Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal-lobe excision". Neuropsychologia. 6 (3): 255–264. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90024-9.
  32. ^ Milner, B.; Corkin, S.; Teuber, H.-L. (1968). "Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H.M.". Neuropsychologia. 6 (3): 215–234. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90021-3.
  33. ^ Aggleton, J. P.; Brown, M. W. (1999). "Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal–anterior thalamic axis" (PDF). Behav. Brain Sci. 22 (3): 425–489. doi:10.1017/s0140525x99002034. PMID 11301518. S2CID 11258997.
  34. ^ a b c Adolphs, R.; Cahill, L.; Schul, R.; Babinsky, R. (1997). "Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans". Learning and Memory. 4 (3): 291–300. doi:10.1101/lm.4.3.291. PMID 10456070.
  35. ^ Babinsky, R.; Calabrese, P.; Durwen, H.; Markowitsch, H.; Brechtelsbuauer, D. (1993). "The possible contribution of the amygdala to memory". Behav. Neurol. 6 (3): 167–170. doi:10.1155/1993/684234. PMID 24487116.
  36. ^ Lupien, S.; Gaudreau, S.; Tchiteya, B.; Maheu, F.; Sharma, S.; Nair, N.; et al. (1997). "Stress-Induced Declarative Memory Impairment in Healthy Elderly Subjects: Relationship to Cortisol Reactivity". The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism. 82 (7): 2070–2075. doi:10.1210/jcem.82.7.4075. PMID 9215274.
  37. ^ Cabeza, R.; LaBar, K.S. (2006). "Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 7 (1): 54–64. doi:10.1038/nrn1825. PMID 16371950. S2CID 1829420.
  38. ^ Baker, D.G.; et al. (2005). "Higher levels of basal CSF cortisol in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder". American Journal of Psychiatry. 162 (5): 992–994. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.5.992. PMID 15863803.
  39. ^ Stephens, Mary Ann C.; Wand, Gary (2012). "Stress and the HPA Axis". Alcohol Research: Current Reviews. 34 (4): 468–483. ISSN 2168-3492. PMC 3860380. PMID 23584113.
  40. ^ Notaras, Michael; van den Buuse, Maarten (2020-01-03). "Neurobiology of BDNF in fear memory, sensitivity to stress, and stress-related disorders". Molecular Psychiatry. 25 (10): 2251–2274. doi:10.1038/s41380-019-0639-2. ISSN 1476-5578. PMID 31900428. S2CID 209540967.
  41. ^ Segerstrom, Suzanne C.; Miller, Gregory E. (July 2004). "Psychological Stress and the Human Immune System: A Meta-Analytic Study of 30 Years of Inquiry". Psychological Bulletin. 130 (4): 601–630. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.601. ISSN 0033-2909. PMC 1361287. PMID 15250815.
  42. ^ a b c d e f g Damoiseaux, J.S.; Elzinga, B.M. (2007). "Glucocorticoids Decrease Hippocampal and Prefrontal Activation during Declarative Memory Retrieval in Young Men". Brain Imaging and Behaviour. 1 (1–2): 31–41. doi:10.1007/s11682-007-9003-2. PMC 2780685. PMID 19946603.
  43. ^ McClelland, J.L.; McNaughton, B.L.; O'Reilly, R.C. (1995). "Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory". Psychol. Rev. 102 (3): 419–457. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.102.3.419. PMID 7624455.
  44. ^ Ji, D.; Wilson, M. A. (2007). "Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep". Nat. Neurosci. 10 (1): 100–7. doi:10.1038/nn1825. PMID 17173043. S2CID 205431067.
  45. ^ Peigneux, P.; et al. (2004). "Are Spatial Memories Strengthened in the Human Hippocampus during Slow Wave Sleep?". Neuron. 44 (3): 535–45. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.10.007. PMID 15504332. S2CID 1424898.
  46. ^ a b Ellenbogen, J; Payne, D; et al. (2006). "The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation: passive, permissive, active or none?". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 16 (6): 716–722. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.10.006. PMID 17085038. S2CID 15514443.
  47. ^ Tucker A, Fishbein W, (2009) The Impact of sleep duration and subject intelligence on declarative and motor memory performance: how much is enough? J. Sleep Res., 304-312
  48. ^ Plihal, W; Born, J (1997). "Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory". J Cogn Neurosci. 9 (4): 534–547. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.4.534. PMID 23968216. S2CID 3300300.
  49. ^ Backhaus, J.; Junghanns, K.; Born, J.; Hohaus, K.; Faasch, F.; Hohagen, F. (2006). "Impaired declarative memory consolidation during sleep in patients with primary insomnia: Influence of sleep architecture and nocturnal cortisol release". Biol. Psychiatry. 60 (12): 1324–1330. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.051. PMID 16876140. S2CID 32826396.
  50. ^ Backhaus, J; Born, J; et al. (2007). "Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep". Learning & Memory. 14 (5): 336–341. doi:10.1101/lm.470507. PMC 1876757. PMID 17522024.
  51. ^ Gais, S; Molle, M; Helms, K; Born, J (2002). "Learning-dependent increases in sleep spindle density". J Neurosci. 22 (15): 6830–6834. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-15-06830.2002. PMC 6758170. PMID 12151563.
  52. ^ Schabus, M; Hodlmoser, K; Gruber, G; Sauter, C; Anderer, P; Klosch, G; Parapatics, S; Saletu, B; Klimesch, W; Zeitlhofer, J (2006). "Sleep spindle-related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities". Eur J Neurosci. 23 (7): 1738–1746. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04694.x. PMID 16623830. S2CID 3168339.
  53. ^ a b Schabus, M; Gruber, G; Parapatics, S; et al. (2004). "Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation". Sleep. 27 (8): 1479–85. doi:10.1093/sleep/27.7.1479. PMID 15683137.
  54. ^ Wagner, U; Gais, S; Born, J (2001). "Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep". Learn Mem. 8 (2): 112–119. doi:10.1101/lm.36801. PMC 311359. PMID 11274257.
  55. ^ Hu, P; Stylos-Allen, M; Walker, MP (2006). "Sleep facilitates consolidation of emotionally arousing declarative memory". Psychol Sci. 17 (10): 891–8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01799.x. PMID 17100790. S2CID 13535490.
  56. ^ Ellenbogen, J; Hulbert, J; et al. (2006). ""Interfering with Theories of Sleep and Memory " Sleep, Declarative Memory, and Associative Interference". Current Biology. 16 (13): 1290–1294. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.05.024. PMID 16824917. S2CID 10114241.
  57. ^ Wixted, JT (2004). "The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting". Annu Rev Psychol. 55: 235–269. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555. PMID 14744216.
  58. ^ a b c d e Kolb, Bryan; Whishaw, Ian Q. (2003). Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology. Worth Publishers. pp. 454–455. ISBN 978-071675300-1.
  59. ^ Kandel, E.R.; Schwartz, J.H.; Jessell, T.M.; Siegelbaum, S.A.; Hudspeth, A.J. (2013). Principles of neural science. McGraw Hill Medical. ISBN 978-0-07-139011-8.
  60. ^ Craik, F.I.M.; Lockhart, R.S. (1972). "Levels of processing: a framework for memory research". J Verb Learn Verb Behav. 11 (6): 671–684. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X.
  61. ^ Einstein, G.O.; Mullet, H.G.; Harrison, T.L. (2012). "The testing effect: Illustrating a fundamental concept and changing study strategies". Teaching of Psychology. 39 (3): 190–193. doi:10.1177/0098628312450432. S2CID 14352611.
  62. ^ Unsworth, N.; Brewer, G. A.; Spillers, G. J. (2011). "Variation in working memory capacity and episodic memory: Examining the importance of encoding specificity". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 18 (6): 1113–1118. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0165-y. PMID 21912997.
  63. ^ Staresina, B. P.; Gray, J. C.; Davachi, L. (2009). "Event congruency enhances episodic memory encoding through semantic elaboration and relational binding". Cerebral Cortex. 19 (5): 1198–1207. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn165. PMC 2665161. PMID 18820289.
  64. ^ H.L. Petri and M. Mishkin: Behaviorism, cognitivism, and the neuropsychology of memory, in: American scientist, 82:30-37, 1994
  65. ^ Lah, S.; Epps, A.; Levick, W.; Parry, L. (2011). "Implicit and explicit memory outcome in children who have sustained severe traumatic brain injury: Impact of age at injury (preliminary findings)". Brain Injury. 25 (1): 44–52. doi:10.3109/02699052.2010.531693. PMID 21117914. S2CID 11678379.
  66. ^ Hampstead, B. M.; Stringer, A.Y.; Stilla, R.F.; Deshpande, G.; Hu, X.; Moore, A.; Sathian, K.K. (2011). "Activation and effective connectivity changes following explicit-memory training for face–name pairs in patients with mild cognitive impairment: A pilot study". Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair. 25 (3): 210–222. doi:10.1177/1545968310382424. PMC 3595021. PMID 20935339.
  67. ^ Metzler-Baddeley, C.; Snowden, J. S. (2005). "Brief report: Errorless versus errorful learning as a memory rehabilitation approach in Alzheimer's disease". Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 27 (8): 1070–1079. doi:10.1080/13803390490919164. PMID 16207625. S2CID 26207056.
  68. ^ Kessels, R. C.; Feijen, J. J.; Postma, A. A. (2005). "Implicit and Explicit Memory for Spatial Information in Alzheimer's Disease". Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders. 20 (2–3): 184–191. doi:10.1159/000087233. PMID 16088143. S2CID 1855801.
  69. ^ Hoscheidt, S. M.; Nadel, L.; Payne, J.; Ryan, L. (2010). "Hippocampal activation during retrieval of spatial context from episodic and semantic memory". Behavioural Brain Research. 212 (2): 121–132. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.04.010. PMID 20385169. S2CID 12588078.
  70. ^ Small, J. A.; Sandhu, N. (2008). "Episodic and semantic memory influences on picture naming in Alzheimer's disease". Brain and Language. 104 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.12.002. PMID 17223189. S2CID 5866604.
  71. ^ Irish, M.; Addis, D.; Hodges, J. R.; Piguet, O. (2012). "Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking: Evidence from semantic dementia". Brain. 135 (7): 2178–2191. doi:10.1093/brain/aws119. PMID 22614246.
  72. ^ a b Baxtendale, S (2004). "Memories aren't made of this: Amnesia at the movies". British Medical Journal. 329 (7480): 1480–1483. doi:10.1136/bmj.329.7480.1480. PMC 535990. PMID 15604191.
  73. ^ Chun, M (2005). "Drug-induced amnesia impairs implicit relational memory". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 9 (8): 355–357. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.06.015. PMID 16006177. S2CID 6053734.

explicit, memory, declarative, memory, main, types, long, term, human, memory, other, which, implicit, memory, conscious, intentional, recollection, factual, information, previous, experiences, concepts, this, type, memory, dependent, upon, three, processes, a. Explicit memory or declarative memory is one of the two main types of long term human memory the other of which is implicit memory Explicit memory is the conscious intentional recollection of factual information previous experiences and concepts 1 This type of memory is dependent upon three processes acquisition consolidation and retrieval 2 3 Explicit memory can be divided into two categories episodic memory which stores specific personal experiences and semantic memory which stores factual information 4 Explicit memory requires gradual learning with multiple presentations of a stimulus and response The counterpart to explicit memory is known as implicit memory refers to memories acquired and used unconsciously such as skills e g knowing how to get dressed or perception Unlike explicit memory implicit memory learns rapidly even from a single stimulus and it is influenced by other mental systems Sometimes a distinction is made between explicit memory and declarative memory In such cases explicit memory relates to any kind of conscious memory and declarative memory relates to any kind of memory that can be described in words however if it is assumed that a memory cannot be described without being conscious and vice versa then the two concepts are identical Contents 1 Types 1 1 Episodic memory 1 2 Semantic memory 1 3 Hybrid types 1 4 Examples 2 The model of language 2 1 Broca and Wernicke s Brain Region 3 History 4 Neuropsychology 4 1 Normal brain function 4 1 1 Hippocampus 4 1 2 Prefrontal cortex 4 1 3 Amygdala 4 1 4 Other structures involved 4 2 Lesion studies 4 2 1 Hippocampal lesion studies 4 2 2 Amygdala lesion studies 5 Factors that affect declarative memory 5 1 Stress 5 1 1 Neurochemical factors of stress on the brain 6 Consolidation during sleep 7 Encoding and retrieval 8 Neural structures involved 9 Traumatic brain injury 10 Memory loss 11 In popular culture 12 See also 13 ReferencesTypes EditEpisodic memory Edit Main article Episodic memory Episodic memory consists of the storage and recollection of observational information attached to specific life events These can be memories that happened to the subject directly or just memories of events that happened around them Episodic memory is what people generally think of when they talk about memory Episodic memory allows for recalling various contextual and situational details of one s previous experiences Some examples of episodic memory include the memory of entering a specific classroom for the first time the memory of storing your carry on baggage while boarding a plane headed to a specific destination on a specific day and time the memory of being notified that one are being terminated from their job or the memory of notifying a subordinate that they are being terminated from their job The retrieval of these episodic memories can be thought of as the action of mentally reliving in detail the past events that they concern 4 Episodic memory is believed to be the system that provides the basic support for semantic memory Semantic memory Edit Main article Semantic memory Semantic memory refers to general world knowledge facts ideas meaning and concepts that can be articulated and is independent of personal experience 5 This includes world knowledge object knowledge language knowledge and conceptual priming Semantic memory is distinct from episodic memory which is the memory of experiences and specific events that occur during people s lives from which they can recreate at any given point 6 For instance semantic memory might contain information about what a cat is whereas episodic memory might contain a specific memory of petting a particular cat Humans can learn about new concepts by applying their knowledge learned from things in the past 7 Other examples of semantic memory include types of food capital cities of a geographic region facts about people dates or the lexicon of a language such as a person s vocabulary 4 Hybrid types Edit Autobiographical memory is a memory system consisting of episodes recollected from an individual s life based on a combination of episodic personal experiences and specific objects people and events experienced at particular time and place and semantic general knowledge and facts about the world memory 8 Spatial memory is the part of memory responsible for recording information about one s environment and its spatial orientation For example a person s spatial memory is required in order to navigate around a familiar city just as a rat s spatial memory is needed to learn the location of food at the end of a maze It is often argued that in both humans and animals spatial memories are summarized as a cognitive map Spatial memory has representations within working short term and long term memory Research indicates that there are specific areas of the brain associated with spatial memory Many methods are used for measuring spatial memory in children adults and animals Examples Edit Long Term Memory Subtype Description ExampleDeclarative explicit Conscious memory of facts and eventsSemantic Factual information The capital of Germany is BerlinEpisodic Specific personal experiences Your 10th birthdayNon declarative implicit Modes of learning that are nonconscious one learning to perform a sequence of actions that do not necessarily invoke knowledgePriming Also known as pattern completion where one has the ability to complete a pattern they have once seen before This priming differs from priming in Psychology If you were given a picture of half of a letter from the alphabet and you recognized which letter it is you would be able to complete the letter Perceptual learning Perceptual ability to differentiate sensories through experience of stimuli Differentiating between categories such as smells colors tastesCategory learning the process of establishing a memory trace that improves the efficiency of assigning novel objects to contrasting groups 9 Movie genres breeds of dogs types of fruitsEmotional learning retention of classically conditioned emotional relationships that cannot be voluntarily recollected or reported citation needed Being afraid of dogs yet you cannot explain whyProcedural learning The formation of skills and habits Learning how to ride a bikeThe model of language EditDeclarative and procedural memory fall into two categories of human language Declarative memory system is used by the lexicon Declarative memory stores all arbitrary unique word specific knowledge including word meanings word sounds and abstract representations such as word category In other words declarative memory is where random bits and pieces of knowledge about language that are specific and unpredictable are stored Declarative memory includes representations of simple words e g cat bound morphemes morphemes that have to go together irregular morphological forms verb complements and idioms or non compositional semantic units Irregular morphological structures fall into the declarative system the irregularities such as went being the past form of go or idioms are what we have to memorize Declarative memory supports a superposition associative memory which allows for generalizations across representations For example the memorization of phonologically similar stem irregular past tense pairs e g spring sprung sing sang may allow for memory based generalization to new irregularities either from real words bring brought or from novel ones spring sprung This ability to generalize could underlie some degree of productivity within the memory system While declarative memory deals with irregularities of morphology procedural memory uses regular phonology and regular morphology Procedural memory system is used by grammar where grammar is defined by the building of a rule governed structure Language s ability to use grammar comes from procedural memory making grammar like another procedure It underlies the learning of new and already learned rule based procedures that oversee the regularities of language particularly those procedures related to combining items into complex structures that have precedence and hierarchical relations precedence in the sense of left to right and hierarchical in the sense of top to bottom Procedural memory builds rule governed structure merging or series of forms and representations into complex structures such as Phonology Inflectional and derivational morphology Compositional semantics the meaning of composition of words into complex structures SyntaxBroca and Wernicke s Brain Region Edit Broca s area is important to procedural memory because Broca s area is involved in the expressive aspects of spoken and written language production of sentences constrained by the rules of grammar and syntax 10 Broca s area corresponds to parts of the inferior frontal gyrus presumably Brodmann s area 44 and 45 Procedural memory is affected by Broca s aphasia Agrammatism is apparent in Broca s aphasia patients where a lack of fluency and omission of morphology and function words occur While those with Broca s aphasia are still able to understand or comprehend speech they have difficulty producing it Speech production becomes more difficult when sentences are complex for example the passive voice is a grammatically complex structure that is harder for those with Broca s aphasia to comprehend Wernicke s area is crucial for language development focusing on the comprehension of speech rather than speech production Wernicke s aphasia affects declarative memory Opposite of Broca s aphasia paragrammatism is apparent which causes normal or excessive fluency and use of inappropriate words neologisms Those with Wernicke s aphasia struggle to understand the meaning of words and may not recognize their mistakes in speech History EditThe study of human memory stretches back over the last 2000 years An early attempt to understand memory can be found in Aristotle s major treatise On the Soul in which he compares the human mind to a blank slate 11 He theorized that all humans are born free of any knowledge and are the sum of their experiences It wasn t until the late 1800s however that a young German philosopher by the name of Herman Ebbinghaus developed the first scientific approach to studying memory 12 While some of his findings have endured and remain relevant to this day Learning Curve his greatest contribution to the field of memory research was demonstrating that memory can be studied scientifically In 1972 Endel Tulving proposed the distinction between episodic and semantic memory 4 This was quickly adopted and is now widely accepted Following this in 1985 Daniel Schacter proposed a more general distinction between explicit declarative and implicit procedural memory 13 With the recent advances in neuroimaging technology there have been a multitude of findings linking specific brain areas to declarative memory Despite these advances in Cognitive psychology there is still much to be discovered in terms of the operating mechanisms of declarative memory 14 It is unclear whether declarative memory is mediated by a particular memory system or if it is more accurately classified as a type of knowledge and it is not known how or why declarative memory evolved to begin with 14 Neuropsychology EditNormal brain function Edit Hippocampus Edit Hippocampus as seen in red Although many psychologists believe that the entire brain is involved with memory the hippocampus and surrounding structures appear to be most important in declarative memory specifically 15 The ability to retain and recall episodic memories is highly dependent on the hippocampus 15 whereas the formation of new declarative memories relies on both the hippocampus and the parahippocampus 16 Other studies have found that the parahippocampal cortices were related to superior recognition memory 16 The Three Stage Model was developed by Eichenbaum et al 2001 and proposes that the hippocampus does three things with episodic memory Mediates the recording of episodic memories Identifies common features between episodes Links these common episodes in a memory space To support this model a version of Piaget s Transitive Inference Task was used to show that the hippocampus is in fact used as the memory space 15 When experiencing an event for the first time a link is formed in the hippocampus allowing us to recall that event in the future Separate links are also made for features related to that event For example when you meet someone new a unique link is created for them More links are then connected to that person s link so you can remember what colour their shirt was what the weather was like when you met them etc Specific episodes are made easier to remember and recall by repeatedly exposing oneself to them which strengthens the links in the memory space allowing for faster retrieval when remembering 15 Hippocampal cells neurons are activated depending on what information one is exposed to at that moment Some cells are specific to spatial information certain stimuli smells etc or behaviours as has been shown in a Radial Maze Task 15 It is therefore the hippocampus that allows us to recognize certain situations environments etc as being either distinct or similar to others However the Three Stage Model does not incorporate the importance of other cortical structures in memory The anatomy of the hippocampus is largely conserved across mammals and the role of these areas in declarative memory are conserved across species as well The organization and neural pathways of the hippocampus are very similar in humans and other mammal species In humans and other mammals a cross section of the hippocampus shows the dentate gyrus as well as the dense cell layers of the CA fields The intrinsic connectivity of these areas are also conserved 17 Results from an experiment by Davachi Mitchell and Wagner 2003 and subsequent research Davachi 2006 shows that activation in the hippocampus during encoding is related to a subject s ability to recall prior events or later relational memories These tests did not differentiate between individual test items later seen and those forgotten 18 19 Prefrontal cortex Edit The lateral Prefrontal cortex PFC is essential for remembering contextual details of an experience rather than for memory formation 16 The PFC is also more involved with episodic memory than semantic memory although it does play a small role in semantics 20 Using PET studies and word stimuli Endel Tulving found that remembering is an automatic process 21 It is also well documented that a hemispheric asymmetry occurs in the PFC When encoding memories the Left Dorsolateral PFC LPFC is activated and when retrieving memories activation is seen in the Right Dorsolateral PFC RPFC 21 Studies have also shown that the PFC is extremely involved with autonoetic consciousness See Tulving s theory 22 This is responsible for humans recollective experiences and mental time travelling abilities characteristics of episodic memory Amygdala as seen in red Amygdala Edit The amygdala is believed to be involved in the encoding and retrieval of emotionally charged memories Much of the evidence for this has come from research on a phenomenon known as flashbulb memories These are instances in which memories of powerful emotional events are more highly detailed and enduring than regular memories e g September 11 attacks assassination of JFK These memories have been linked to increased activation in the amygdala 23 Recent studies of patients with damage to the amygdala suggest that it is involved in memory for general knowledge and not for specific information 24 25 Other structures involved Edit The regions of the diencephalon have shown brain activation when a remote memory is being recovered 20 and the occipital lobe ventral temporal lobe and fusiform gyrus all play a role in memory formation 16 Lesion studies Edit Lesion studies are commonly used in cognitive neuroscience research Lesions can occur naturally through trauma or disease or they can be surgically induced by researchers In the study of declarative memory the hippocampus and the amygdala are two structures frequently examined using this technique Hippocampal lesion studies Edit The Morris water maze The Morris water navigation task tests spatial learning in rats 26 In this test rats learn to escape from a pool by swimming toward a platform submerged just below the surface of the water Visual cues that surround the pool e g a chair or window help the rat to locate the platform on subsequent trials The rats use of specific events cues and places are all forms of declarative memory 27 Two groups of rats are observed a control group with no lesions and an experimental group with hippocampal lesions In this task created by Morris rats are placed in the pool at the same position for 12 trials Each trial is timed and the path taken by the rats is recorded Rats with hippocampal lesions successfully learn to find the platform If the starting point is moved the rats with hippocampal lesions typically fail to locate the platform The control rats however are able to find the platform using the cues acquired during the learning trials 26 This demonstrates the involvement of the hippocampus in declarative memory 27 The Odor odor Recognition Task devised by Bunsey and Eichenbaum involves a social encounter between two rats a subject and a demonstrator The demonstrator after eating a specific type of food interacts with the subject rat who then smells the food odor on the other s breath The experimenters then present the subject rat with a decision between two food options the food previously eaten by the demonstrator and a novel food The researchers found that when there was no time delay both control rats and rats with lesions chose the familiar food After 24 hours however the rats with hippocampal lesions were just as likely to eat both types of food while control rats chose the familiar food 28 This can be attributed to the inability to form episodic memories due to lesions in the hippocampus The effects of this study can be observed in humans with amnesia indicating the role of the hippocampus in developing episodic memories that can be generalized to similar situations 27 Henry Molaison previously known as H M had parts of both his left and right medial temporal lobes hippocampi removed which resulted in the loss of the ability to form new memories 29 The long term declarative memory was crucially affected when the structures from the medial temporal lobe were removed including the ability to form new semantic knowledge and memories 30 The dissociation in Molaison between the acquisition of declarative memory and other kinds of learning was seen initially in motor learning 31 Molaison s declarative memory was not functioning as was seen when Molaison completed the task of repetition priming His performance does improve over trials however his scores were inferior to those of control participants 32 In the condition of Molaison the same results from this priming task are reflected when looking at the other basic memory functions like remembering recall and recognizing 29 Lesions should not be interpreted as an all or nothing condition in the case of Molaison not all memory and recognition is lost although the declarative memory is severely damaged he still has a sense of self and memories that were developed before the lesion occurred 33 Patient R B was another clinical case reinforcing the role of the hippocampus in declarative memory After suffering an ischemic episode during a cardiac bypass operation Patient R B awoke with a severe anterograde amnesic disorder IQ and cognition were unaffected but declarative memory deficits were observed although not to the extent of that seen in Molaison Upon death an autopsy revealed that Patient R B had bilateral lesions of the CA1 cell region along the whole length of the hippocampus Amygdala lesion studies Edit Adolph Cahill and Schul completed a study showing that emotional arousal facilitates the encoding of material into long term declarative memory 34 They selected two subjects with bilateral damage to the amygdala as well as six control subjects and six subjects with brain damage All subjects were shown a series of twelve slides accompanied by a narrative The slides varied in the degree to which they evoked emotion slides 1 through 4 and slides 9 through 12 contain non emotional content Slides 5 through 8 contain emotional material and the seventh slide contained the most emotionally arousing image and description a picture of surgically repaired legs of a car crash victim 34 The emotionally arousing slide slide 7 was remembered no better by the bilateral damage participants than any of the other slides All other participants notably remembered the seventh slide the best and in most detail out of all the other slides 34 This shows that the amygdala is necessary to facilitate encoding of declarative knowledge regarding emotionally arousing stimuli but is not required for encoding knowledge of emotionally neutral stimuli 35 Factors that affect declarative memory EditFurther information Neurobiological effects of physical exercise Cognitive control and memory Stress Edit Stress may have an effect on the recall of declarative memories Lupien et al completed a study that had 3 phases for participants to take part in Phase 1 involved memorizing a series of words phase 2 entailed either a stressful public speaking or non stressful situation an attention task and phase 3 required participants to recall the words they learned in phase 1 There were signs of decreased declarative memory performance in the participants that had to complete the stressful situation after learning the words 36 Recall performance after the stressful situation was found to be worse overall than after the non stressful situation It was also found that performance differed based on whether the participant responded to the stressful situation with an increase in measured levels of salivary cortisol Posttraumatic stress disorder PTSD emerges after exposure to a traumatic event eliciting fear horror or helplessness that involves bodily injury the threat of injury or death to one s self or another person 37 The chronic stress in PTSD contributes to an observed decrease in hippocampal volume and declarative memory deficits 38 Stress can alter memory functions reward immune function metabolism and susceptibility to different diseases 39 Disease risk is particularly pertinent to mental illnesses whereby chronic or severe stress remains a common risk factor for several mental illnesses 40 One system suggests there are five types of stress labeled acute time limited stressors brief naturalistic stressors stressful event sequences chronic stressors and distant stressors An acute time limited stressor involves a short term challenge while a brief natural stressor involves an event that is normal but nevertheless challenging A stressful event sequence is a stressor that occurs and then continues to yield stress into the immediate future A chronic stressor involves exposure to a long term stressor and a distant stressor is a stressor that is not immediate 41 Neurochemical factors of stress on the brain Edit Cortisol is the primary glucocorticoid in the human body In the brain it modulates the ability of the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex to process memories 42 Although the exact molecular mechanism of how glucocorticoids influence memory formation is unknown the presence of glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex tell us these structures are some of its many targets 42 It has been demonstrated that cortisone a glucocorticoid impaired blood flow in the right parahippocampal gyrus left visual cortex and cerebellum 42 A study by Damoiseaux et al 2007 evaluated the effects of glucocorticoids on hippocampal and prefrontal cortex activation during declarative memory retrieval They found that administration of hydrocortisone name given to cortisol when it is used as a medication to participants one hour before retrieval of information impairs free recall of words yet when administered before or after learning they had no effect on recall 42 They also found that hydrocortisone decreases brain activity in the above mentioned areas during declarative memory retrieval 42 Therefore naturally occurring elevations of cortisol during periods of stress lead to impairment of declarative memory 42 It is important to note that this study involved only male subjects which may be significant as sex steroid hormones may have different effects in response to cortisol administration Men and women also respond to emotional stimuli differently and this may affect cortisol levels This was also the first Functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI study done utilising glucocorticoids therefore more research is necessary to further substantiate these findings 42 Consolidation during sleep EditIt is believed that sleep plays an active role in consolidation of declarative memory Specifically sleep s unique properties enhance memory consolidation such as the reactivation of newly learned memories during sleep For example it has been suggested that the central mechanism for consolidation of declarative memory during sleep is the reactivation of hippocampal memory representations This reactivation transfers information to neocortical networks where it is integrated into long term representations 43 Studies on rats involving maze learning found that hippocampal neuronal assemblies that are used in the encoding of spatial information are reactivated in the same temporal order 44 Similarly positron emission tomography PET has shown reactivation of the hippocampus in slow wave sleep SWS after spatial learning 45 Together these studies show that newly learned memories are reactivated during sleep and through this process new memory traces are consolidated 46 In addition researchers have identified three types of sleep SWS sleep spindle and REM in which declarative memory is consolidated Slow wave sleep often referred to as deep sleep plays the most important role in consolidation of declarative memory and there is a large amount of evidence to support this claim One study found that the first 3 5 hours of sleep offer the greatest performance enhancement on memory recall tasks because the first couple of hours are dominated by SWS Additional hours of sleep do not add to the initial level of performance Thus this study suggests that full sleep may not be important for optimal performance of memory 47 Another study shows that people who experience SWS during the first half of their sleep cycle compared to subjects who did not showed better recall of information However this is not the case for subjects who were tested for the second half of their sleep cycle as they experience less SWS 48 Another key piece of evidence regarding SWS s involvement in declarative memory consolidation is a finding that people with pathological conditions of sleep such as insomnia exhibit both reduction in Slow Wave Sleep and also have impaired consolidation of declarative memory during sleep 49 Another study found that middle aged people compared to young group had a worse retrieval of memories This in turn indicated that SWS is associated with poor declarative memory consolidation but not with age itself 50 Some researchers suggest that sleep spindle a burst of brain activity occurring during stage 2 sleep plays a role in boosting consolidation of declarative memories 51 Critics point out that spindle activity is positively correlated with intelligence 52 In contrast Schabus and Gruber point out that sleep spindle activity only relates to performance on newly learned memories and not to absolute performance This supports the hypothesis that sleep spindle helps to consolidate recent memory traces but not memory performance in general 53 The relationship between sleep spindles and declarative memory consolidation is not yet fully understood 53 There is a relatively small body of evidence that supports the idea that REM sleep helps consolidate highly emotional declarative memories For instance Wagner et al compared memory retention for emotional versus neutral text over two instances early sleep that is dominated by SWS and late sleep that is dominated by REM phase 54 This study found that sleep improved memory retention of emotional text only during late sleep phase which was primarily REM Similarly Hu amp Stylos Allen et al performed a study with emotional versus neutral pictures and concluded that REM sleep facilitates consolidation of emotional declarative memories 55 The view that sleep plays an active role in declarative memory consolidation is not shared by all researchers For instance Ellenbogen et al argue that sleep actively protects declarative memory from associative interference 56 Furthermore Wixted believes that the sole role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation is nothing more but creating ideal conditions for memory consolidation 57 For example when awake people are bombarded with mental activity which interferes with effective consolidation However during sleep when interference is minimal memories can be consolidated without associative interference More research is needed to make a definite statement whether sleep creates favourable conditions for consolidation or it actively enhances declarative memory consolidation 46 Encoding and retrieval EditThe encoding of explicit memory depends on conceptually driven top down processing in which a subject reorganizes the data to store it 58 The subject makes associations with previously related stimuli or experiences 59 This was termed deep encoding by Fergus Craik and Robert Lockhart 60 This way a memory persists longer and will be remembered well The later recall of information is thus greatly influenced by the way in which the information was originally processed 58 The depth of processing effect is the improvement in subsequent recall of an object about which a person has given thought to its meaning or shape Simply put To create explicit memories you have to do something with your experiences think about them talk about them write them down study them etc The more you do the better you will remember Testing of information while learning has also shown to improve encoding in explicit memory If a student reads a text book and then tests themselves afterward their semantic memory of what was read is improved This study test method improves encoding of information This Phenomenon is referred to as the Testing Effect 61 Retrieval Because a person has played an active role in processing explicit information the internal cues that were used in processing it can also be used to initiate spontaneous recall 58 When someone talks about an experience the words they use will help when they try to remember this experience at a later date The conditions in which information is memorized can affect recall If a person has the same surroundings or cues when the original information is presented they are more likely to remember it This is referred to as encoding specificity and it also applies to explicit memory In a study where subjects were asked to perform a cued recall task participants with a high working memory did better than participants with a low working memory when the conditions were maintained When the conditions were changed for recall both groups dropped The subjects with higher working memory declined more 62 This is thought to happen because matching environments activates areas of the brain known as the left inferior frontal gyrus and the hippocampus 63 Neural structures involved EditSeveral neural structures are proposed to be involved in explicit memory Most are in the temporal lobe or closely related to it such as the amygdala the hippocampus the rhinal cortex in the temporal lobe and the prefrontal cortex 58 Nuclei in the thalamus also are included because many connections between the prefrontal cortex and temporal cortex are made through the thalamus 58 The regions that make up the explicit memory circuit receive input from the neocortex and from brainstem systems including acetylcholine serotonin and noradrenaline systems 64 Traumatic brain injury EditWhile the human brain is certainly regarded for its plasticity there is some evidence that shows traumatic brain injury TBI in young children can have negative effects on explicit memory Researchers have looked at children with TBI in early childhood i e infancy and late childhood Findings showed that children with severe TBI in late childhood experienced impaired explicit memory while still maintaining implicit memory formation Researchers also found that children with severe TBI in early childhood had both increased chance of having both impaired explicit memory and implicit memory While children with severe TBI are at risk for impaired explicit memory the chances of impaired explicit memory in adults with severe TBI is much greater 65 Memory loss EditAlzheimer s disease has a profound effect on explicit memory Mild cognitive impairment is an early sign of Alzheimer s disease People with memory conditions often receive cognitive training When an fMRI was used to view brain activity after training it found increased activation in various neural systems that are involved with explicit memory 66 People with Alzheimer s have problems learning new tasks However if the task is presented repeatedly they can learn and retain some new knowledge of the task This effect is more apparent if the information is familiar The person with Alzheimer s must also be guided through the task and prevented from making errors 67 Alzheimer s also has an effect on explicit spatial memory This means that people with Alzheimer s have difficulty remembering where items are placed in unfamiliar environments 68 The hippocampus has been shown to become active in semantic and episodic memory 69 The effects of Alzheimer s disease are seen in the episodic part of explicit memory This can lead to problems with communication A study was conducted where Alzheimer s patients were asked to name a variety of objects from different periods The results shown that their ability to name the object depended on frequency of use of the item and when the item was first acquired 70 This effect on semantic memory also has an effect on music and tones Alzheimer s patients have difficulty distinguishing between different melodies they have never heard before People with Alzheimer s also have issues with picturing future events This is due to a deficit in episodic future thinking 71 There are many other reasons why adults and others may begin to have memory loss In popular culture EditAmnesiacs are frequently portrayed in television and movies Some of the better known examples include In the romantic comedy 50 First Dates 2004 Adam Sandler plays veterinarian Henry Roth who falls for Lucy Whitmore played by Drew Barrymore Having lost her short term memory in a car crash Lucy can only remember the current day s events until she falls asleep When she wakes up the next morning she has no recollection of the previous day s experiences 72 These experiences would normally be transferred into declarative knowledge allowing them to be recalled in the future Although this movie is not the most accurate representation of a true amnesic patient it is useful for informing viewers of the detrimental effects of amnesia Memento 2000 a film inspired by the case of Henry Molaison H M 73 Guy Pearce plays an ex insurance investigator suffering from severe anterograde amnesia caused by a head injury Unlike most amnesiacs Leonard retains his identity and the memories of events that occurred before the injury but loses all ability to form new memories This loss of ability to form new memories indicates that the head injury affected the medial temporal lobe of the brain resulting in the inability for Leonard to form declarative memory Finding Nemo features a reef fish named Dory with an inability to develop declarative memory This prevents her from learning or retaining any new information such as names or directions The exact origin of Dory s impairment is not mentioned in the film but her memory loss accurately portrays the difficulties facing amnesiacs 72 See also EditGollin figure testReferences Edit Ullman MT 2004 Contributions of memory circuits to language the declarative procedural model Cognition 92 1 2 231 70 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2003 10 008 PMID 15037131 S2CID 14611894 Lazzarim Mayla K Targa Adriano Sardi Natalia F et al Pain impairs consolidation but not acquisition or retrieval of a declarative memory Behavioural Pharmacology 2020 31 8 707 715 doi 10 1097 FBP 0000000000000576 Kandel ER Dudai Y Mayford MR 2014 The molecular and systems biology of memory Cell157 163 186 a b c d Tulving E 1972 Episodic and semantic memory In Organization of Memory ed E Tulving W Donaldson pp 381 403 New York Academic McRae Ken Jones Michael 2013 Reisberg Daniel ed The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology New York NY Oxford University Press pp 206 216 ISBN 9780195376746 Tulving Endel 2002 Episodic Memory From Mind to Brain Annual Review of Psychology 53 1 25 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 53 100901 135114 PMID 11752477 Saumier D Chertkow H 2002 Semantic Memory Current Science 2 6 516 522 doi 10 1007 s11910 002 0039 9 PMID 12359106 S2CID 14184578 Williams H L Conway M A amp Cohen G 2008 Autobiographical memory In G Cohen amp M A Conway Eds Memory in the Real World 3rd ed pp 21 90 Hove UK Psychology Press Ell Shawn Zilioli Monica 2012 Categorical Learning in Seel Norbert M ed Encyclopedia of the Sciences of Learning Springer US pp 509 512 doi 10 1007 978 1 4419 1428 6 98 ISBN 978 1 4419 1428 6 Broca s Area an overview ScienceDirect Topics www sciencedirect com Retrieved 2019 12 17 Aristotle On the Soul De Anima in Aristotle Volume 4 Loeb Classical Library William Heinemann London UK 1936 Ebbinghaus H 1885 Memory A Contribution to Experimental Psychology Teachers College Columbia University Graf P Schacter D L 1985 Implicit and explicit memory for new associations in normal and amnesic subjects Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 11 3 501 518 doi 10 1037 0278 7393 11 3 501 PMID 3160813 a b Eichenbaum Howard 1997 Declarative memory Insights from cognitive neurobiology Annual Review of Psychology 48 547 572 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 48 1 547 PMID 9046568 a b c d e Eichenbaum Howard 2001 The Hippocampus and Declarative Memory Cognitive Mechanisms and Neural Codes Behavioural Brain Research 127 1 2 199 207 doi 10 1016 s0166 4328 01 00365 5 PMID 11718892 S2CID 20843130 a b c d Gabrieli J Kao Y 2007 Development of the Declarative Memory System in the Human Brain Nature Neuroscience 10 9 1198 1205 doi 10 1038 nn1950 PMID 17676059 S2CID 15637865 Manns Joseph Eichenbaum Howard September 2006 Evolution of Declarative Memory Hippocampus 16 9 795 808 doi 10 1002 hipo 20205 PMID 16881079 S2CID 39081299 Davachi L Mitchell J P Wagner A D 2003 Multiple routes to memory Distinct medial temporal lobe processes build item and source memories Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100 4 2157 2162 Bibcode 2003PNAS 100 2157D doi 10 1073 pnas 0337195100 PMC 149975 PMID 12578977 Davachi Dobbins 2008 Declarative Memory Current Directions in Psychological Science 17 2 112 118 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2008 00559 x PMC 2790294 PMID 20011622 a b Graham S Levine B 2004 The Fundamental Neuroanatomy of Episodic and Semantic Autobiographical Remembering A Prospective Functional MRI Study Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 16 9 1633 1646 doi 10 1162 0898929042568587 PMID 15601525 S2CID 32682464 a b Craik F I M Houle S 1994 Role of Prefrontal Cortex in Human Episodic Memory Lessons From PET Studies Biol Psychiatry 42 75S 76S doi 10 1016 s0006 3223 97 87185 5 S2CID 54399679 Stuss D T Tulving E 1997 Toward a Theory of Episodic Memory The Frontal Lobes amp Autonoetic Consciousness Psychological Bulletin 121 3 331 354 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 121 3 331 PMID 9136640 Sharot T Martorella EA Delgado MR Phelps EA 2007 How personal experience modulates the neural circuitry of memories of September 11 Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 104 1 389 394 Bibcode 2007PNAS 104 389S doi 10 1073 pnas 0609230103 PMC 1713166 PMID 17182739 Adolphs R Tranel D Buchanan TW 2005 Amygdala damage impairs emotional memory for gist but not details of complex stimuli PDF Nat Neurosci 8 4 512 518 doi 10 1038 nn1413 PMID 15735643 S2CID 205430564 Adolphs R Denburg NL Tranel D 2001 The amygdala s role in long term declarative memory for gist and detail Behav Neurosci 115 5 983 992 doi 10 1037 0735 7044 115 5 983 PMID 11584931 a b Eichenbaum H Stewart C Morris R G M 1990 Hippocampal representation in spatial learning J Neurosci 10 11 331 339 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 10 11 03531 1990 PMC 6570096 PMID 2230943 a b c Eichenbaum H 2000 A cortical hippocampal system for declarative memory Nature Reviews Neuroscience 1 1 41 50 doi 10 1038 35036213 PMID 11252767 S2CID 5242470 Bunsey M Eichenbaum H 1995 Selective damage to the hippocampal region blocks long term retention of a natural and nonspatial stimulus stimulus association Hippocampus 5 6 546 556 doi 10 1002 hipo 450050606 PMID 8646281 S2CID 37972779 a b Corkin S 2002 Perspectives What s new with the amnesic patient H M Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3 2 153 160 doi 10 1038 nrn726 PMID 11836523 S2CID 5429133 Gabrieli J D E Cohen N J Corkin S 1988 The impaired learning of semantic knowledge following bilateral medial temporal lobe resection Brain Cogn 7 2 157 177 doi 10 1016 0278 2626 88 90027 9 PMID 3377896 S2CID 23026117 Corkin S 1968 Acquisition of motor skill after bilateral medial temporal lobe excision Neuropsychologia 6 3 255 264 doi 10 1016 0028 3932 68 90024 9 Milner B Corkin S Teuber H L 1968 Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome 14 year follow up study of H M Neuropsychologia 6 3 215 234 doi 10 1016 0028 3932 68 90021 3 Aggleton J P Brown M W 1999 Episodic memory amnesia and the hippocampal anterior thalamic axis PDF Behav Brain Sci 22 3 425 489 doi 10 1017 s0140525x99002034 PMID 11301518 S2CID 11258997 a b c Adolphs R Cahill L Schul R Babinsky R 1997 Impaired declarative memory for emotional material following bilateral amygdala damage in humans Learning and Memory 4 3 291 300 doi 10 1101 lm 4 3 291 PMID 10456070 Babinsky R Calabrese P Durwen H Markowitsch H Brechtelsbuauer D 1993 The possible contribution of the amygdala to memory Behav Neurol 6 3 167 170 doi 10 1155 1993 684234 PMID 24487116 Lupien S Gaudreau S Tchiteya B Maheu F Sharma S Nair N et al 1997 Stress Induced Declarative Memory Impairment in Healthy Elderly Subjects Relationship to Cortisol Reactivity The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology amp Metabolism 82 7 2070 2075 doi 10 1210 jcem 82 7 4075 PMID 9215274 Cabeza R LaBar K S 2006 Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7 1 54 64 doi 10 1038 nrn1825 PMID 16371950 S2CID 1829420 Baker D G et al 2005 Higher levels of basal CSF cortisol in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder American Journal of Psychiatry 162 5 992 994 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 162 5 992 PMID 15863803 Stephens Mary Ann C Wand Gary 2012 Stress and the HPA Axis Alcohol Research Current Reviews 34 4 468 483 ISSN 2168 3492 PMC 3860380 PMID 23584113 Notaras Michael van den Buuse Maarten 2020 01 03 Neurobiology of BDNF in fear memory sensitivity to stress and stress related disorders Molecular Psychiatry 25 10 2251 2274 doi 10 1038 s41380 019 0639 2 ISSN 1476 5578 PMID 31900428 S2CID 209540967 Segerstrom Suzanne C Miller Gregory E July 2004 Psychological Stress and the Human Immune System A Meta Analytic Study of 30 Years of Inquiry Psychological Bulletin 130 4 601 630 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 130 4 601 ISSN 0033 2909 PMC 1361287 PMID 15250815 a b c d e f g Damoiseaux J S Elzinga B M 2007 Glucocorticoids Decrease Hippocampal and Prefrontal Activation during Declarative Memory Retrieval in Young Men Brain Imaging and Behaviour 1 1 2 31 41 doi 10 1007 s11682 007 9003 2 PMC 2780685 PMID 19946603 McClelland J L McNaughton B L O Reilly R C 1995 Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex Insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory Psychol Rev 102 3 419 457 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 102 3 419 PMID 7624455 Ji D Wilson M A 2007 Coordinated memory replay in the visual cortex and hippocampus during sleep Nat Neurosci 10 1 100 7 doi 10 1038 nn1825 PMID 17173043 S2CID 205431067 Peigneux P et al 2004 Are Spatial Memories Strengthened in the Human Hippocampus during Slow Wave Sleep Neuron 44 3 535 45 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2004 10 007 PMID 15504332 S2CID 1424898 a b Ellenbogen J Payne D et al 2006 The role of sleep in declarative memory consolidation passive permissive active or none Current Opinion in Neurobiology 16 6 716 722 doi 10 1016 j conb 2006 10 006 PMID 17085038 S2CID 15514443 Tucker A Fishbein W 2009 The Impact of sleep duration and subject intelligence on declarative and motor memory performance how much is enough J Sleep Res 304 312 Plihal W Born J 1997 Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory J Cogn Neurosci 9 4 534 547 doi 10 1162 jocn 1997 9 4 534 PMID 23968216 S2CID 3300300 Backhaus J Junghanns K Born J Hohaus K Faasch F Hohagen F 2006 Impaired declarative memory consolidation during sleep in patients with primary insomnia Influence of sleep architecture and nocturnal cortisol release Biol Psychiatry 60 12 1324 1330 doi 10 1016 j biopsych 2006 03 051 PMID 16876140 S2CID 32826396 Backhaus J Born J et al 2007 Midlife decline in declarative memory consolidation is correlated with a decline in slow wave sleep Learning amp Memory 14 5 336 341 doi 10 1101 lm 470507 PMC 1876757 PMID 17522024 Gais S Molle M Helms K Born J 2002 Learning dependent increases in sleep spindle density J Neurosci 22 15 6830 6834 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 22 15 06830 2002 PMC 6758170 PMID 12151563 Schabus M Hodlmoser K Gruber G Sauter C Anderer P Klosch G Parapatics S Saletu B Klimesch W Zeitlhofer J 2006 Sleep spindle related activity in the human EEG and its relation to general cognitive and learning abilities Eur J Neurosci 23 7 1738 1746 doi 10 1111 j 1460 9568 2006 04694 x PMID 16623830 S2CID 3168339 a b Schabus M Gruber G Parapatics S et al 2004 Sleep spindles and their significance for declarative memory consolidation Sleep 27 8 1479 85 doi 10 1093 sleep 27 7 1479 PMID 15683137 Wagner U Gais S Born J 2001 Emotional memory formation is enhanced across sleep intervals with high amounts of rapid eye movement sleep Learn Mem 8 2 112 119 doi 10 1101 lm 36801 PMC 311359 PMID 11274257 Hu P Stylos Allen M Walker MP 2006 Sleep facilitates consolidation of emotionally arousing declarative memory Psychol Sci 17 10 891 8 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 2006 01799 x PMID 17100790 S2CID 13535490 Ellenbogen J Hulbert J et al 2006 Interfering with Theories of Sleep and Memory Sleep Declarative Memory and Associative Interference Current Biology 16 13 1290 1294 doi 10 1016 j cub 2006 05 024 PMID 16824917 S2CID 10114241 Wixted JT 2004 The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting Annu Rev Psychol 55 235 269 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 55 090902 141555 PMID 14744216 a b c d e Kolb Bryan Whishaw Ian Q 2003 Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology Worth Publishers pp 454 455 ISBN 978 071675300 1 Kandel E R Schwartz J H Jessell T M Siegelbaum S A Hudspeth A J 2013 Principles of neural science McGraw Hill Medical ISBN 978 0 07 139011 8 Craik F I M Lockhart R S 1972 Levels of processing a framework for memory research J Verb Learn Verb Behav 11 6 671 684 doi 10 1016 S0022 5371 72 80001 X Einstein G O Mullet H G Harrison T L 2012 The testing effect Illustrating a fundamental concept and changing study strategies Teaching of Psychology 39 3 190 193 doi 10 1177 0098628312450432 S2CID 14352611 Unsworth N Brewer G A Spillers G J 2011 Variation in working memory capacity and episodic memory Examining the importance of encoding specificity Psychonomic Bulletin amp Review 18 6 1113 1118 doi 10 3758 s13423 011 0165 y PMID 21912997 Staresina B P Gray J C Davachi L 2009 Event congruency enhances episodic memory encoding through semantic elaboration and relational binding Cerebral Cortex 19 5 1198 1207 doi 10 1093 cercor bhn165 PMC 2665161 PMID 18820289 H L Petri and M Mishkin Behaviorism cognitivism and the neuropsychology of memory in American scientist 82 30 37 1994 Lah S Epps A Levick W Parry L 2011 Implicit and explicit memory outcome in children who have sustained severe traumatic brain injury Impact of age at injury preliminary findings Brain Injury 25 1 44 52 doi 10 3109 02699052 2010 531693 PMID 21117914 S2CID 11678379 Hampstead B M Stringer A Y Stilla R F Deshpande G Hu X Moore A Sathian K K 2011 Activation and effective connectivity changes following explicit memory training for face name pairs in patients with mild cognitive impairment A pilot study Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 25 3 210 222 doi 10 1177 1545968310382424 PMC 3595021 PMID 20935339 Metzler Baddeley C Snowden J S 2005 Brief report Errorless versus errorful learning as a memory rehabilitation approach in Alzheimer s disease Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 27 8 1070 1079 doi 10 1080 13803390490919164 PMID 16207625 S2CID 26207056 Kessels R C Feijen J J Postma A A 2005 Implicit and Explicit Memory for Spatial Information in Alzheimer s Disease Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders 20 2 3 184 191 doi 10 1159 000087233 PMID 16088143 S2CID 1855801 Hoscheidt S M Nadel L Payne J Ryan L 2010 Hippocampal activation during retrieval of spatial context from episodic and semantic memory Behavioural Brain Research 212 2 121 132 doi 10 1016 j bbr 2010 04 010 PMID 20385169 S2CID 12588078 Small J A Sandhu N 2008 Episodic and semantic memory influences on picture naming in Alzheimer s disease Brain and Language 104 1 1 9 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2006 12 002 PMID 17223189 S2CID 5866604 Irish M Addis D Hodges J R Piguet O 2012 Considering the role of semantic memory in episodic future thinking Evidence from semantic dementia Brain 135 7 2178 2191 doi 10 1093 brain aws119 PMID 22614246 a b Baxtendale S 2004 Memories aren t made of this Amnesia at the movies British Medical Journal 329 7480 1480 1483 doi 10 1136 bmj 329 7480 1480 PMC 535990 PMID 15604191 Chun M 2005 Drug induced amnesia impairs implicit relational memory Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 8 355 357 doi 10 1016 j tics 2005 06 015 PMID 16006177 S2CID 6053734 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Explicit memory amp oldid 1131367726, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.