fbpx
Wikipedia

Language acquisition

Language acquisition is the process by which humans acquire the capacity to perceive and comprehend language (in other words, gain the ability to be aware of language and to understand it), as well as to produce and use words and sentences to communicate.

Language acquisition involves structures, rules, and representation. The capacity to use language successfully requires one to acquire a range of tools including phonology, morphology, syntax, semantics, and an extensive vocabulary. Language can be vocalized as in speech, or manual as in sign.[1] Human language capacity is represented in the brain. Even though human language capacity is finite, one can say and understand an infinite number of sentences, which is based on a syntactic principle called recursion. Evidence suggests that every individual has three recursive mechanisms that allow sentences to go indeterminately. These three mechanisms are: relativization, complementation and coordination.[2]

There are two main guiding principles in first-language acquisition: speech perception always precedes speech production, and the gradually evolving system by which a child learns a language is built up one step at a time, beginning with the distinction between individual phonemes.[3]

Linguists who are interested in child language acquisition have for many years questioned how language is acquired. Lidz et al. state "The question of how these structures are acquired, then, is more properly understood as the question of how a learner takes the surface forms in the input and converts them into abstract linguistic rules and representations."[4]

Language acquisition usually refers to first-language acquisition, which studies infants' acquisition of their native language, whether that be spoken language or signed language,[1] though it can also refer to bilingual first language acquisition (BFLA), which refers to an infant's simultaneous acquisition of two native languages.[5][6][7][8][9][10][11] This is distinguished from second-language acquisition, which deals with the acquisition (in both children and adults) of additional languages. In addition to speech, reading and writing a language with an entirely different script compounds the complexities of true foreign language literacy. Language acquisition is one of the quintessential human traits.[12][13]

History

Some early observation-based ideas about language acquisition were proposed by Plato, who felt that word-meaning mapping in some form was innate. Additionally, Sanskrit grammarians debated for over twelve centuries whether humans' ability to recognize the meaning of words was god-given (possibly innate) or passed down by previous generations and learned from already established conventions: a child learning the word for cow by listening to trusted speakers talking about cows.[14]

Philosophers in ancient societies were interested in how humans acquired the ability to understand and produce language well before empirical methods for testing those theories were developed, but for the most part they seemed to regard language acquisition as a subset of man's ability to acquire knowledge and learn concepts.[15]

Empiricists, like Thomas Hobbes and John Locke, argued that knowledge (and, for Locke, language) emerge ultimately from abstracted sense impressions. These arguments lean towards the "nurture" side of the argument: that language is acquired through sensory experience, which led to Rudolf Carnap's Aufbau, an attempt to learn all knowledge from sense datum, using the notion of "remembered as similar" to bind them into clusters, which would eventually map into language.[16]

Proponents of behaviorism argued that language may be learned through a form of operant conditioning. In B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior (1957), he suggested that the successful use of a sign, such as a word or lexical unit, given a certain stimulus, reinforces its "momentary" or contextual probability. Since operant conditioning is contingent on reinforcement by rewards, a child would learn that a specific combination of sounds stands for a specific thing through repeated successful associations made between the two. A "successful" use of a sign would be one in which the child is understood (for example, a child saying "up" when they want to be picked up) and rewarded with the desired response from another person, thereby reinforcing the child's understanding of the meaning of that word and making it more likely that they will use that word in a similar situation in the future. Some empiricist theories of language acquisition include the statistical learning theory. Charles F. Hockett of language acquisition, relational frame theory, functionalist linguistics, social interactionist theory, and usage-based language acquisition.

Skinner's behaviorist idea was strongly attacked by Noam Chomsky in a review article in 1959, calling it "largely mythology" and a "serious delusion."[17] Arguments against Skinner's idea of language acquisition through operant conditioning include the fact that children often ignore language corrections from adults. Instead, children typically follow a pattern of using an irregular form of a word correctly, making errors later on, and eventually returning to the proper use of the word. For example, a child may correctly learn the word "gave" (past tense of "give"), and later on use the word "gived". Eventually, the child will typically go back to using the correct word, "gave". Chomsky claimed the pattern is difficult to attribute to Skinner's idea of operant conditioning as the primary way that children acquire language. Chomsky argued that if language were solely acquired through behavioral conditioning, children would not likely learn the proper use of a word and suddenly use the word incorrectly.[18] Chomsky believed that Skinner failed to account for the central role of syntactic knowledge in language competence. Chomsky also rejected the term "learning", which Skinner used to claim that children "learn" language through operant conditioning.[19] Instead, Chomsky argued for a mathematical approach to language acquisition, based on a study of syntax.

As a typically human phenomenon

The capacity to acquire and use language is a key aspect that distinguishes humans from other beings. Although it is difficult to pin down what aspects of language are uniquely human, there are a few design features that can be found in all known forms of human language, but that are missing from forms of animal communication. For example, many animals are able to communicate with each other by signaling to the things around them, but this kind of communication lacks the arbitrariness of human vernaculars (in that there is nothing about the sound of the word "dog" that would hint at its meaning). Other forms of animal communication may utilize arbitrary sounds, but are unable to combine those sounds in different ways to create completely novel messages that can then be automatically understood by another. Hockett called this design feature of human language "productivity". It is crucial to the understanding of human language acquisition that humans are not limited to a finite set of words, but, rather, must be able to understand and utilize a complex system that allows for an infinite number of possible messages. So, while many forms of animal communication exist, they differ from human language in that they have a limited range of vocabulary tokens, and the vocabulary items are not combined syntactically to create phrases.[20]

 
Victor of Aveyron

Herbert S. Terrace conducted a study on a chimpanzee known as Nim Chimpsky in an attempt to teach him American Sign Language. This study was an attempt to further research done with a chimpanzee named Washoe, who was reportedly able to acquire American Sign Language. However, upon further inspection, Terrace concluded that both experiments were failures.[21] While Nim was able to acquire signs, he never acquired a knowledge of grammar, and was unable to combine signs in a meaningful way. Researchers noticed that "signs that seemed spontaneous were, in fact, cued by teachers",[22] and not actually productive. When Terrace reviewed Project Washoe, he found similar results. He postulated that there is a fundamental difference between animals and humans in their motivation to learn language; animals, such as in Nim's case, are motivated only by physical reward, while humans learn language in order to "create a new type of communication".[23]

In another language acquisition study, Jean-Marc-Gaspard Itard attempted to teach Victor of Aveyron, a feral child, how to speak. Victor was able to learn a few words, but ultimately never fully acquired language.[24] Slightly more successful was a study done on Genie, another child never introduced to society. She had been entirely isolated for the first thirteen years of her life by her father. Caretakers and researchers attempted to measure her ability to learn a language. She was able to acquire a large vocabulary, but never acquired grammatical knowledge. Researchers concluded that the theory of a critical period was true; Genie was too old to learn how to speak productively, although she was still able to comprehend language.[25]

General approaches

A major debate in understanding language acquisition is how these capacities are picked up by infants from the linguistic input.[26] Input in the linguistic context is defined as "All words, contexts, and other forms of language to which a learner is exposed, relative to acquired proficiency in first or second languages". Nativists such as Chomsky have focused on the hugely complex nature of human grammars, the finiteness and ambiguity of the input that children receive, and the relatively limited cognitive abilities of an infant. From these characteristics, they conclude that the process of language acquisition in infants must be tightly constrained and guided by the biologically given characteristics of the human brain. Otherwise, they argue, it is extremely difficult to explain how children, within the first five years of life, routinely master the complex, largely tacit grammatical rules of their native language.[27] Additionally, the evidence of such rules in their native language is all indirect— adult speech to children cannot encompass all of what children know by the time they have acquired their native language.[28]

Other scholars, however, have resisted the possibility that infants' routine success at acquiring the grammar of their native language requires anything more than the forms of learning seen with other cognitive skills, including such mundane motor skills as learning to ride a bike. In particular, there has been resistance to the possibility that human biology includes any form of specialization for language. This conflict is often referred to as the "nature and nurture" debate. Of course, most scholars acknowledge that certain aspects of language acquisition must result from the specific ways in which the human brain is "wired" (a "nature" component, which accounts for the failure of non-human species to acquire human languages) and that certain others are shaped by the particular language environment in which a person is raised (a "nurture" component, which accounts for the fact that humans raised in different societies acquire different languages). The as-yet unresolved question is the extent to which the specific cognitive capacities in the "nature" component are also used outside of language.

Emergentism

Emergentist theories, such as Brian MacWhinney's competition model, posit that language acquisition is a cognitive process that emerges from the interaction of biological pressures and the environment. According to these theories, neither nature nor nurture alone is sufficient to trigger language learning; both of these influences must work together in order to allow children to acquire a language. The proponents of these theories argue that general cognitive processes subserve language acquisition and that the result of these processes is language-specific phenomena, such as word learning and grammar acquisition. The findings of many empirical studies support the predictions of these theories, suggesting that language acquisition is a more complex process than many have proposed.[29]

Empiricism

Although Chomsky's theory of a generative grammar has been enormously influential in the field of linguistics since the 1950s, many criticisms of the basic assumptions of generative theory have been put forth by cognitive-functional linguists, who argue that language structure is created through language use.[30] These linguists argue that the concept of a language acquisition device (LAD) is unsupported by evolutionary anthropology, which tends to show a gradual adaptation of the human brain and vocal cords to the use of language, rather than a sudden appearance of a complete set of binary parameters delineating the whole spectrum of possible grammars ever to have existed and ever to exist.[31] On the other hand, cognitive-functional theorists use this anthropological data to show how human beings have evolved the capacity for grammar and syntax to meet our demand for linguistic symbols. (Binary parameters are common to digital computers, but may not be applicable to neurological systems such as the human brain.)[citation needed]

Further, the generative theory has several constructs (such as movement, empty categories, complex underlying structures, and strict binary branching) that cannot possibly be acquired from any amount of linguistic input. It is unclear that human language is actually anything like the generative conception of it. Since language, as imagined by nativists, is unlearnably complex,[citation needed] subscribers to this theory argue that it must, therefore, be innate.[32] Nativists hypothesize that some features of syntactic categories exist even before a child is exposed to any experience - categories on which children map words of their language as they learn their native language.[33] A different theory of language, however, may yield different conclusions. While all theories of language acquisition posit some degree of innateness, they vary in how much value they place on this innate capacity to acquire language. Empiricism places less value on the innate knowledge, arguing instead that the input, combined with both general and language-specific learning capacities, is sufficient for acquisition.[34]

Since 1980, linguists studying children, such as Melissa Bowerman and Asifa Majid,[35] and psychologists following Jean Piaget, like Elizabeth Bates[36] and Jean Mandler, came to suspect that there may indeed be many learning processes involved in the acquisition process, and that ignoring the role of learning may have been a mistake.[citation needed]

In recent years, the debate surrounding the nativist position has centered on whether the inborn capabilities are language-specific or domain-general, such as those that enable the infant to visually make sense of the world in terms of objects and actions. The anti-nativist view has many strands, but a frequent theme is that language emerges from usage in social contexts, using learning mechanisms that are a part of an innate general cognitive learning apparatus. This position has been championed by David M. W. Powers,[37] Elizabeth Bates,[38] Catherine Snow, Anat Ninio, Brian MacWhinney, Michael Tomasello,[20] Michael Ramscar,[39] William O'Grady,[40] and others. Philosophers, such as Fiona Cowie[41] and Barbara Scholz with Geoffrey Pullum[42] have also argued against certain nativist claims in support of empiricism.

The new field of cognitive linguistics has emerged as a specific counter to Chomsky's Generative Grammar and to Nativism.

Statistical learning

Some language acquisition researchers, such as Elissa Newport, Richard Aslin, and Jenny Saffran, emphasize the possible roles of general learning mechanisms, especially statistical learning, in language acquisition. The development of connectionist models that when implemented are able to successfully learn words and syntactical conventions[43] supports the predictions of statistical learning theories of language acquisition, as do empirical studies of children's detection of word boundaries.[44] In a series of connectionist model simulations, Franklin Chang has demonstrated that such a domain general statistical learning mechanism could explain a wide range of language structure acquisition phenomena.[45]

Statistical learning theory suggests that, when learning language, a learner would use the natural statistical properties of language to deduce its structure, including sound patterns, words, and the beginnings of grammar.[46] That is, language learners are sensitive to how often syllable combinations or words occur in relation to other syllables.[47][48][49] Infants between 21 and 23 months old are also able to use statistical learning to develop "lexical categories", such as an animal category, which infants might later map to newly learned words in the same category. These findings suggest that early experience listening to language is critical to vocabulary acquisition.[49]

The statistical abilities are effective, but also limited by what qualifies as input, what is done with that input, and by the structure of the resulting output.[46] One should also note that statistical learning (and more broadly, distributional learning) can be accepted as a component of language acquisition by researchers on either side of the "nature and nurture" debate. From the perspective of that debate, an important question is whether statistical learning can, by itself, serve as an alternative to nativist explanations for the grammatical constraints of human language.

Chunking

The central idea of these theories is that language development occurs through the incremental acquisition of meaningful chunks of elementary constituents, which can be words, phonemes, or syllables. Recently, this approach has been highly successful in simulating several phenomena in the acquisition of syntactic categories[50] and the acquisition of phonological knowledge.[51]

Chunking theories of language acquisition constitute a group of theories related to statistical learning theories, in that they assume that the input from the environment plays an essential role; however, they postulate different learning mechanisms.[clarification needed]

Researchers at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology have developed a computer model analyzing early toddler conversations to predict the structure of later conversations. They showed that toddlers develop their own individual rules for speaking, with 'slots' into which they put certain kinds of words. A significant outcome of this research is that rules inferred from toddler speech were better predictors of subsequent speech than traditional grammars.[52]

This approach has several features that make it unique: the models are implemented as computer programs, which enables clear-cut and quantitative predictions to be made; they learn from naturalistic input—actual child-directed utterances; and attempt to create their own utterances, the model was tested in languages including English, Spanish, and German. Chunking for this model was shown to be most effective in learning a first language but was able to create utterances learning a second language.[53]

Relational frame theory

The relational frame theory (RFT) (Hayes, Barnes-Holmes, Roche, 2001), provides a wholly selectionist/learning account of the origin and development of language competence and complexity. Based upon the principles of Skinnerian behaviorism, RFT posits that children acquire language purely through interacting with the environment. RFT theorists introduced the concept of functional contextualism in language learning, which emphasizes the importance of predicting and influencing psychological events, such as thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, by focusing on manipulable variables in their own context. RFT distinguishes itself from Skinner's work by identifying and defining a particular type of operant conditioning known as derived relational responding, a learning process that, to date, appears to occur only in humans possessing a capacity for language. Empirical studies supporting the predictions of RFT suggest that children learn language through a system of inherent reinforcements, challenging the view that language acquisition is based upon innate, language-specific cognitive capacities.[54]

Social interactionism

Social interactionist theory is an explanation of language development emphasizing the role of social interaction between the developing child and linguistically knowledgeable adults. It is based largely on the socio-cultural theories of Soviet psychologist Lev Vygotsky, and was made prominent in the Western world by Jerome Bruner.[55]

Unlike other approaches, it emphasizes the role of feedback and reinforcement in language acquisition. Specifically, it asserts that much of a child's linguistic growth stems from modeling of and interaction with parents and other adults, who very frequently provide instructive correction.[56] It is thus somewhat similar to behaviorist accounts of language learning. It differs substantially, though, in that it posits the existence of a social-cognitive model and other mental structures within children (a sharp contrast to the "black box" approach of classical behaviorism).

Another key idea within the theory of social interactionism is that of the zone of proximal development. This is a theoretical construct denoting the set of tasks a child is capable of performing with guidance but not alone.[57] As applied to language, it describes the set of linguistic tasks (for example, proper syntax, suitable vocabulary usage) that a child cannot carry out on its own at a given time, but can learn to carry out if assisted by an able adult.

Syntax, morphology, and generative grammar

As syntax began to be studied more closely in the early 20th century in relation to language learning, it became apparent to linguists, psychologists, and philosophers that knowing a language was not merely a matter of associating words with concepts, but that a critical aspect of language involves knowledge of how to put words together; sentences are usually needed in order to communicate successfully, not just isolated words.[15] A child will use short expressions such as Bye-bye Mummy or All-gone milk, which actually are combinations of individual nouns and an operator,[58] before they begin to produce gradually more complex sentences. In the 1990s, within the principles and parameters framework, this hypothesis was extended into a maturation-based structure building model of child language regarding the acquisition of functional categories. In this model, children are seen as gradually building up more and more complex structures, with lexical categories (like noun and verb) being acquired before functional-syntactic categories (like determiner and complementizer).[59] It is also often found that in acquiring a language, the most frequently used verbs are irregular verbs.[citation needed] In learning English, for example, young children first begin to learn the past tense of verbs individually. However, when they acquire a "rule", such as adding -ed to form the past tense, they begin to exhibit occasional overgeneralization errors (e.g. "runned", "hitted") alongside correct past tense forms. One influential[citation needed] proposal regarding the origin of this type of error suggests that the adult state of grammar stores each irregular verb form in memory and also includes a "block" on the use of the regular rule for forming that type of verb. In the developing child's mind, retrieval of that "block" may fail, causing the child to erroneously apply the regular rule instead of retrieving the irregular.[60][61]

A Merge (linguistics)-based Theory

In Bare-Phrase structure (Minimalist Program), since theory-internal considerations define the specifier position of an internal-merge projection (phases vP and CP) as the only type of host which could serve as potential landing-sites for move-based elements displaced from lower down within the base-generated VP structure – e.g., A-movement such as passives (["The apple was eaten by [John (ate the apple)"]]), or raising ["Some work does seem to remain [(There) does seem to remain (some work)"]])—as a consequence, any strong version of a Structure building model of child language which calls for an exclusive "external-merge/argument structure stage" prior to an "internal-merge/scope-discourse related stage" would claim that young children's stage-1 utterances lack the ability to generate and host elements derived via movement operations. In terms of a Merge-based theory of language acquisition,[62] complements and specifiers are simply notations for first-merge (= "complement-of" [head-complement]), and later second-merge (= "specifier-of" [specifier-head], with merge always forming to a head. First-merge establishes only a set {a, b} and is not an ordered pair—e.g., an {N, N}-compound of 'boat-house' would allow the ambiguous readings of either 'a kind of house' and/or 'a kind of boat'. It is only with second-merge that order is derived out of a set {a {a, b}} which yields the recursive properties of syntax—e.g., a 'house-boat' {house {house, boat}} now reads unambiguously only as a 'kind of boat'. It is this property of recursion that allows for projection and labeling of a phrase to take place;[63] in this case, that the Noun 'boat' is the Head of the compound, and 'house' acting as a kind of specifier/modifier. External-merge (first-merge) establishes substantive 'base structure' inherent to the VP, yielding theta/argument structure, and may go beyond the lexical-category VP to involve the functional-category light verb vP. Internal-merge (second-merge) establishes more formal aspects related to edge-properties of scope and discourse-related material pegged to CP. In a Phase-based theory, this twin vP/CP distinction follows the "duality of semantics" discussed within the Minimalist Program, and is further developed into a dual distinction regarding a probe-goal relation.[64] As a consequence, at the "external/first-merge-only" stage, young children would show an inability to interpret readings from a given ordered pair, since they would only have access to the mental parsing of a non-recursive set. (See Roeper for a full discussion of recursion in child language acquisition).[65] In addition to word-order violations, other more ubiquitous results of a first-merge stage would show that children's initial utterances lack the recursive properties of inflectional morphology, yielding a strict Non-inflectional stage-1, consistent with an incremental Structure-building model of child language.

Generative grammar, associated especially with the work of Noam Chomsky, is currently one of the approaches to explaining children's acquisition of syntax.[66] Its leading idea is that human biology imposes narrow constraints on the child's "hypothesis space" during language acquisition. In the principles and parameters framework, which has dominated generative syntax since Chomsky's (1980) Lectures on Government and Binding: The Pisa Lectures, the acquisition of syntax resembles ordering from a menu: the human brain comes equipped with a limited set of choices from which the child selects the correct options by imitating the parents' speech while making use of the context.[67]

An important argument which favors the generative approach, is the poverty of the stimulus argument. The child's input (a finite number of sentences encountered by the child, together with information about the context in which they were uttered) is, in principle, compatible with an infinite number of conceivable grammars. Moreover, rarely can children rely on corrective feedback from adults when they make a grammatical error; adults generally respond and provide feedback regardless of whether a child's utterance was grammatical or not, and children have no way of discerning if a feedback response was intended to be a correction. Additionally, when children do understand that they are being corrected, they don't always reproduce accurate restatements.[dubious ][68][69] Yet, barring situations of medical abnormality or extreme privation, all children in a given speech-community converge on very much the same grammar by the age of about five years. An especially dramatic example is provided by children who, for medical reasons, are unable to produce speech and, therefore, can never be corrected for a grammatical error but nonetheless, converge on the same grammar as their typically developing peers, according to comprehension-based tests of grammar.[70][71]

Considerations such as those have led Chomsky, Jerry Fodor, Eric Lenneberg and others to argue that the types of grammar the child needs to consider must be narrowly constrained by human biology (the nativist position).[72] These innate constraints are sometimes referred to as universal grammar, the human "language faculty", or the "language instinct".[73]

Representation in the brain

Recent advances in functional neuroimaging technology have allowed for a better understanding of how language acquisition is manifested physically in the brain. Language acquisition almost always occurs in children during a period of rapid increase in brain volume. At this point in development, a child has many more neural connections than he or she will have as an adult, allowing for the child to be more able to learn new things than he or she would be as an adult.[74]

Sensitive period

Language acquisition has been studied from the perspective of developmental psychology and neuroscience,[75] which looks at learning to use and understand language parallel to a child's brain development. It has been determined, through empirical research on developmentally normal children, as well as through some extreme cases of language deprivation, that there is a "sensitive period" of language acquisition in which human infants have the ability to learn any language. Several researchers have found that from birth until the age of six months, infants can discriminate the phonetic contrasts of all languages. Researchers believe that this gives infants the ability to acquire the language spoken around them. After this age, the child is able to perceive only the phonemes specific to the language being learned. The reduced phonemic sensitivity enables children to build phonemic categories and recognize stress patterns and sound combinations specific to the language they are acquiring.[76] As Wilder Penfield noted, "Before the child begins to speak and to perceive, the uncommitted cortex is a blank slate on which nothing has been written. In the ensuing years much is written, and the writing is normally never erased. After the age of ten or twelve, the general functional connections have been established and fixed for the speech cortex." According to the sensitive or critical period models, the age at which a child acquires the ability to use language is a predictor of how well he or she is ultimately able to use language.[77] However, there may be an age at which becoming a fluent and natural user of a language is no longer possible; Penfield and Roberts (1959) cap their sensitive period at nine years old.[78] The human brain may be automatically wired to learn languages,[citation needed] but this ability does not last into adulthood in the same way that it exists during childhood.[79] By around age 12, language acquisition has typically been solidified, and it becomes more difficult to learn a language in the same way a native speaker would.[citation needed] Just like children who speak, deaf children go through a critical period for learning language. Deaf children who acquire their first language later in life show lower performance in complex aspects of grammar.[80] At that point, it is usually a second language that a person is trying to acquire and not a first.[27][clarification needed]

Assuming that children are exposed to language during the critical period,[81] acquiring language is almost never missed by cognitively normal children. Humans are so well-prepared to learn language that it becomes almost impossible not to. Researchers are unable to experimentally test the effects of the sensitive period of development on language acquisition, because it would be unethical to deprive children of language until this period is over. However, case studies on abused, language-deprived children show that they exhibit extreme limitations in language skills, even after instruction.[82]

At a very young age, children can distinguish different sounds but cannot yet produce them. During infancy, children begin to babble. Deaf babies babble in the same patterns as hearing babies do, showing that babbling is not a result of babies simply imitating certain sounds, but is actually a natural part of the process of language development. Deaf babies do, however, often babble less than hearing babies, and they begin to babble later on in infancy—at approximately 11 months as compared to approximately 6 months for hearing babies.[83]

Prelinguistic language abilities that are crucial for language acquisition have been seen even earlier than infancy. There have been many different studies examining different modes of language acquisition prior to birth. The study of language acquisition in fetuses began in the late 1980s when several researchers independently discovered that very young infants could discriminate their native language from other languages. In Mehler et al. (1988),[84] infants underwent discrimination tests, and it was shown that infants as young as 4 days old could discriminate utterances in their native language from those in an unfamiliar language, but could not discriminate between two languages when neither was native to them. These results suggest that there are mechanisms for fetal auditory learning, and other researchers have found further behavioral evidence to support this notion. Fetus auditory learning through environmental habituation has been seen in a variety of different modes, such as fetus learning of familiar melodies (Hepper, 1988),[85] story fragments (DeCasper & Spence, 1986),[86] recognition of mother's voice (Kisilevsky, 2003),[87] and other studies showing evidence of fetal adaptation to native linguistic environments (Moon, Cooper & Fifer, 1993).[88]

Prosody is the property of speech that conveys an emotional state of the utterance, as well as the intended form of speech, for example, question, statement or command. Some researchers in the field of developmental neuroscience argue that fetal auditory learning mechanisms result solely from discrimination of prosodic elements. Although this would hold merit in an evolutionary psychology perspective (i.e. recognition of mother's voice/familiar group language from emotionally valent stimuli), some theorists argue that there is more than prosodic recognition in elements of fetal learning. Newer evidence shows that fetuses not only react to the native language differently from non-native languages, but that fetuses react differently and can accurately discriminate between native and non-native vowel sounds (Moon, Lagercrantz, & Kuhl, 2013).[89] Furthermore, a 2016 study showed that newborn infants encode the edges of multisyllabic sequences better than the internal components of the sequence (Ferry et al., 2016).[90] Together, these results suggest that newborn infants have learned important properties of syntactic processing in utero, as demonstrated by infant knowledge of native language vowels and the sequencing of heard multisyllabic phrases. This ability to sequence specific vowels gives newborn infants some of the fundamental mechanisms needed in order to learn the complex organization of a language. From a neuroscientific perspective, neural correlates have been found that demonstrate human fetal learning of speech-like auditory stimuli that most other studies have been analyzing[clarification needed] (Partanen et al., 2013).[91] In a study conducted by Partanen et al. (2013),[91] researchers presented fetuses with certain word variants and observed that these fetuses exhibited higher brain activity in response to certain word variants as compared to controls. In this same study, "a significant correlation existed between the amount of prenatal exposure and brain activity, with greater activity being associated with a higher amount of prenatal speech exposure," pointing to the important learning mechanisms present before birth that are fine-tuned to features in speech (Partanen et al., 2013).[91]

 
The phases of language acquisition in children

Vocabulary acquisition

Learning a new word, that is, learning to speak this word and speak it on the appropriate occasions, depends upon many factors. First, the learner needs to be able to hear what they are attempting to pronounce. Also required is the capacity to engage in speech repetition.[92][93][94][95] Children with reduced ability to repeat non-words (a marker of speech repetition abilities) show a slower rate of vocabulary expansion than children with normal ability.[96] Several computational models of vocabulary acquisition have been proposed.[97][98][99][100][101][102][103] Various studies have shown that the size of a child's vocabulary by the age of 24 months correlates with the child's future development and language skills. A lack of language richness by this age has detrimental and long-term effects on the child's cognitive development, which is why it is so important for parents to engage their infants in language[original research?]. If a child knows fifty or fewer words by the age of 24 months, he or she is classified as a late-talker, and future language development, like vocabulary expansion and the organization of grammar, is likely to be slower and stunted.[citation needed]

Two more crucial elements of vocabulary acquisition are word segmentation and statistical learning (described above). Word segmentation, or the ability to break down words into syllables from fluent speech can be accomplished by eight-month-old infants.[47] By the time infants are 17 months old, they are able to link meaning to segmented words.[48]

Recent evidence also suggests that motor skills and experiences may influence vocabulary acquisition during infancy. Specifically, learning to sit independently between 3 and 5 months of age has been found to predict receptive vocabulary at both 10 and 14 months of age,[104] and independent walking skills have been found to correlate with language skills at around 10 to 14 months of age.[105][106] These findings show that language acquisition is an embodied process that is influenced by a child's overall motor abilities and development. Studies have also shown a correlation between socioeconomic status and vocabulary acquisition.[107]

Meaning

Children learn, on average, ten to fifteen new word meanings each day, but only one of these can be accounted for by direct instruction.[108] The other nine to fourteen word meanings must have been acquired in some other way. It has been proposed that children acquire these meanings through processes modeled by latent semantic analysis; that is, when they encounter an unfamiliar word, children use contextual information to guess its rough meaning correctly.[108] A child may expand the meaning and use of certain words that are already part of its mental lexicon in order to denominate anything that is somehow related but for which it does not know the specific word. For instance, a child may broaden the use of mummy and dada in order to indicate anything that belongs to its mother or father, or perhaps every person who resembles its own parents; another example might be to say rain while meaning I don't want to go out.[109]

There is also reason to believe that children use various heuristics to infer the meaning of words properly. Markman and others have proposed that children assume words to refer to objects with similar properties ("cow" and "pig" might both be "animals") rather than to objects that are thematically related ("cow" and "milk" are probably not both "animals").[110] Children also seem to adhere to the "whole object assumption" and think that a novel label refers to an entire entity rather than to one of its parts.[110] This assumption along with other resources, such as grammar and morphological cues or lexical constraints, may help aid the child in acquiring word meaning, but conclusions based on such resources may sometimes conflict.[111]

Genetic and neurocognitive research

According to several linguists, neurocognitive research has confirmed many standards of language learning, such as: "learning engages the entire person (cognitive, affective, and psychomotor domains), the human brain seeks patterns in its searching for meaning, emotions affect all aspects of learning, retention and recall, past experience always affects new learning, the brain's working memory has a limited capacity, lecture usually results in the lowest degree of retention, rehearsal is essential for retention, practice [alone] does not make perfect, and each brain is unique" (Sousa, 2006, p. 274). In terms of genetics, the gene ROBO1 has been associated with phonological buffer integrity or length.[112]

Genetic research has found two major factors predicting successful language acquisition and maintenance. These include inherited intelligence, and the lack of genetic anomalies that may cause speech pathologies, such as mutations in the FOXP2 gene which cause verbal dyspraxia. The role of inherited intelligence increases with age, accounting for 20% of IQ variation in infants, and for 60% in adults. It affects a vast variety of language-related abilities, from spatio-motor skills to writing fluency. There have been debates in linguistics, philosophy, psychology, and genetics, with some scholars arguing that language is fully or mostly innate, but the research evidence points to genetic factors only working in interaction with environmental ones.[113]

Although it is difficult to determine without invasive measures which exact parts of the brain become most active and important for language acquisition, fMRI and PET technology has allowed for some conclusions to be made about where language may be centered. Kuniyoshi Sakai has proposed, based on several neuroimaging studies, that there may be a "grammar center" in the brain, whereby language is primarily processed in the left lateral premotor cortex (located near the pre central sulcus and the inferior frontal sulcus). Additionally, these studies have suggested that first language and second language acquisition may be represented differently in the cortex.[27] In a study conducted by Newman et al., the relationship between cognitive neuroscience and language acquisition was compared through a standardized procedure involving native speakers of English and native Spanish speakers who all had a similar length of exposure to the English language (averaging about 26 years). It was concluded that the brain does in fact process languages differently[clarification needed], but rather than being related to proficiency levels, language processing relates more to the function of the brain itself.[114]

During early infancy, language processing seems to occur over many areas in the brain. However, over time, it gradually becomes concentrated into two areas – Broca's area and Wernicke's area. Broca's area is in the left frontal cortex and is primarily involved in the production of the patterns in vocal and sign language. Wernicke's area is in the left temporal cortex and is primarily involved in language comprehension. The specialization of these language centers is so extensive[clarification needed] that damage to them can result in aphasia.[115]

Artificial intelligence

Some algorithms for language acquisition are based on statistical machine translation.[116] Language acquisition can be modeled as a machine learning process, which may be based on learning semantic parsers[117] or grammar induction algorithms.[118][119]

Prelingual deafness

Prelingual deafness is defined as hearing loss that occurred at birth or before an individual has learned to speak. In the United States, 2 to 3 out of every 1000 children are born deaf or hard of hearing. Even though it might be presumed that deaf children acquire language in different ways since they are not receiving the same auditory input as hearing children, many research findings indicate that deaf children acquire language in the same way that hearing children do and when given the proper language input, understand and express language just as well as their hearing peers. Babies who learn sign language produce signs or gestures that are more regular and more frequent than hearing babies acquiring spoken language. Just as hearing babies babble, deaf babies acquiring sign language will babble with their hands, otherwise known as manual babbling. Therefore, as many studies have shown, language acquisition by deaf children parallel the language acquisition of a spoken language by hearing children because humans are biologically equipped for language regardless of the modality.

Signed language acquisition

Deaf children's visual-manual language acquisition not only parallel spoken language acquisition but by the age of 30 months, most deaf children that were exposed to a visual language had a more advanced grasp with subject-pronoun copy rules than hearing children. Their vocabulary bank at the ages of 12–17 months exceed that of a hearing child's, though it does even out when they reach the two-word stage. The use of space for absent referents and the more complex handshapes in some signs prove to be difficult for children between 5 and 9 years of age because of motor development and the complexity of remembering the spatial use.

Cochlear implants

Other options besides sign language for kids with prelingual deafness include the use of hearing aids to strengthen remaining sensory cells or cochlear implants to stimulate the hearing nerve directly. Cochlear Implants are hearing devices that are placed behind the ear and contain a receiver and electrodes which are placed under the skin and inside the cochlea. Despite these developments, there is still a risk that prelingually deaf children may not develop good speech and speech reception skills. Although cochlear implants produce sounds, they are unlike typical hearing and deaf and hard of hearing people must undergo intensive therapy in order to learn how to interpret these sounds. They must also learn how to speak given the range of hearing they may or may not have. However, deaf children of deaf parents tend to do better with language, even though they are isolated from sound and speech because their language uses a different mode of communication that is accessible to them: the visual modality of language.

Although cochlear implants were initially approved for adults, now there is pressure to implant children early in order to maximize auditory skills for mainstream learning which in turn has created controversy around the topic. Due to recent advances in technology, cochlear implants allow some deaf people to acquire some sense of hearing. There are interior and exposed exterior components that are surgically implanted. Those who receive cochlear implants earlier on in life show more improvement on speech comprehension and language. Spoken language development does vary widely for those with cochlear implants though due to a number of different factors including: age at implantation, frequency, quality and type of speech training. Some evidence suggests that speech processing occurs at a more rapid pace in some prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants than those with traditional hearing aids. However, cochlear implants may not always work.

Research shows that people develop better language with a cochlear implant when they have a solid first language to rely on to understand the second language they would be learning. In the case of prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants, a signed language, like American Sign Language would be an accessible language for them to learn to help support the use of the cochlear implant as they learn a spoken language as their L2. Without a solid, accessible first language, these children run the risk of language deprivation, especially in the case that a cochlear implant fails to work. They would have no access to sound, meaning no access to the spoken language they are supposed to be learning. If a signed language was not a strong language for them to use and neither was a spoken language, they now have no access to any language and run the risk of missing their critical period.

See also

References

  1. ^ a b Pichler, Chen (2015). "Language Learning through the Eye and Ear Webcast". Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center. Gallaudet University. Retrieved 15 December 2020.
  2. ^ Lightfoot, David (2010). "Language acquisition and language change". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 1 (5): 677–684. doi:10.1002/wcs.39. ISSN 1939-5078. PMID 26271652.
  3. ^ Fry, Dennis (1977). Homo loquens, Man as a talking animal. Cambridge University Press. pp. 107–108. ISBN 978-0-521-29239-9.
  4. ^ Lidz, Jeffrey; Waxman (16 April 2003). (PDF). Cognition 89 (2003) B65-B73. Archived from the original (PDF) on 12 December 2017. Retrieved 12 December 2017 – via Elsevier science.
  5. ^ Bergman, Coral Rhodes (1976). "Interference vs. independent development in infant bilingualism". In Keller, Gary D.; Teschner, Richard V.; Viera, Silvia (eds.). Bilingualism in the Bicentennial and Beyond. Bilingual Press. pp. 86–96. ISBN 9780916950019.
  6. ^ Genesee, Fred (1989). "Early bilingual development: One language or two?". Journal of Child Language. 16 (1): 161–179. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013490. PMID 2647777. S2CID 20680592.
  7. ^ de Houwer, Annick (1990). The Acquisition of Two Languages from Birth. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780511519789.
  8. ^ de Houwer, Annick (1996). "Bilingual Language Acquisition". In Fletcher, Paul; MacWhinney, Brian (eds.). The Handbook of Child Language. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-20312-4.
  9. ^ Hulk, Aafke; Müller, Natascha (December 2000). "Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics". Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 3 (3): 227–244. doi:10.1017/S1366728900000353.
  10. ^ Paradis, Johanne; Genesee, Fred (1996). "SYNTACTIC ACQUISITION IN BILINGUAL CHILDREN: Autonomous or Interdependent?" (PDF). Studies in Second Language Acquisition. 18 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1017/S0272263100014662. JSTOR 44487857. S2CID 143994688.
  11. ^ Serratrice, Ludovica; Sorace, Antonella; Paoli, Sandra (2004). "Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax–pragmatics interface: Subjects and objects in English–Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition" (PDF). Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 7 (3): 183–205. doi:10.1017/S1366728904001610. S2CID 38643617.
  12. ^ Friederici, AD. (Oct 2011). "The brain basis of language processing: from structure to function". Physiol Rev. 91 (4): 1357–92. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.385.5620. doi:10.1152/physrev.00006.2011. PMID 22013214.
  13. ^ Kosslyn, Stephen M.; Osherson, Daniel N. (1995). An invitation to cognitive science. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-65045-8. OCLC 613819557.
  14. ^ Matilal, Bimal Krishna (1990). The word and the world: India's contribution to the study of language. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-562515-8. OCLC 24041690.
  15. ^ a b Innateness and Language. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. 2017.
  16. ^ Kendra A. Palmer (2009). "Understanding Human Language: An In-Depth Exploration of the Human Facility for Language". StudentPulse.com. Retrieved 22 August 2012.
  17. ^ Noam, Chomsky; Skinner, B. F. (1959). "A Review of B. F. Skinner's Verbal Behavior". Language. 35 (1): 26–58. doi:10.2307/411334. JSTOR 411334.
  18. ^ Harley, Trevor A. (2010). Talking the Talk: Language, Psychology and Science. New York, NY: Psychology Press. pp. 68–71. ISBN 978-1-84169-339-2.
  19. ^ Harris, Margaret (1992). Language Experience and Early Language Development: From Input to Uptake. UK: Psychology Press. ISBN 978-0863772382.
  20. ^ a b Tomasello, Michael (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-20177-3. OCLC 439979810.
  21. ^ Carey, Benedict (2007-11-01). "Washoe, a Chimp of Many Words, Dies at 42". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 2020-09-30.
  22. ^ "Nim Chimpsky and Noam Chomsky". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2020-09-28.
  23. ^ "How Infants Learn to Use Words". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2020-09-28.
  24. ^ . The Layman's Linguist. 2019-10-19. Archived from the original on 2021-04-15. Retrieved 2020-09-30.
  25. ^ "The Feral Child Nicknamed Genie". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2020-09-28.
  26. ^ Kennison, Shelia M. (2013-07-30). Introduction to language development. Los Angeles: SAGE Publications. ISBN 978-1-4129-9606-8. OCLC 830837502.
  27. ^ a b c Sakai, Kuniyoshi L. (2005). "Language Acquisition and Brain Development". Science. 310 (5749): 815–819. Bibcode:2005Sci...310..815S. doi:10.1126/science.1113530. PMID 16272114. S2CID 20714845.
  28. ^ Lust, Barbara (2006). Child Language Acquisition and Growth. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 28–29. ISBN 9780511803413.
  29. ^ Brian MacWhinney, ed. (1999). The Emergence of Language. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 978-0-8058-3010-1. OCLC 44958022.
  30. ^ Tomasello, Michael (2003). Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-01030-7. OCLC 62782600.
  31. ^ Mameli, M.; Bateson, P. (Feb 2011). "An evaluation of the concept of innateness". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366 (1563): 436–43. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0174. PMC 3013469. PMID 21199847.
  32. ^ Lidz, Jeffrey; Lasnik, Howard (Dec 2016). Roberts, Ian (ed.). "The Argument from the Poverty of the Stimulus". The Oxford Handbook of Universal Grammar. 1: 220–248. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199573776.013.10. ISBN 9780199573776.
  33. ^ L., Bavin, Edith (2009). The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 15–34. ISBN 9780511576164. OCLC 798060196.
  34. ^ Tomasello, Michael (2000). "First Steps Toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition". Cognitive Linguistics. 11 (1–2): 61–82. doi:10.1515/cogl.2001.012.
  35. ^ Majid, Asifa; Bowerman, Melissa; Staden, Miriam van; Boster, James S (2007). "The semantic categories of cutting and breaking events: A crosslinguistic perspective" (PDF). Cognitive Linguistics. 18 (2). CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1014.4819. doi:10.1515/COG.2007.005. hdl:2066/104711. ISSN 0936-5907. S2CID 33506231.
  36. ^ Bates, E.; D'Amico, S.; Jacobsen, T.; Székely, A.; Andonova, E.; Devescovi, A.; Herron, D.; Lu, CC.; et al. (Jun 2003). (PDF). Psychon Bull Rev. 10 (2): 344–80. doi:10.3758/BF03196494. PMC 3392189. PMID 12921412. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-08-13. Retrieved 2013-12-27.
  37. ^ Powers, David M. W.; Turk, Christopher. (1989). Machine learning of natural language. London ; New York: Springer-Verlag. ISBN 978-0-387-19557-5. OCLC 20263032.
  38. ^ Bates, E; Elman, J; Johnson, M; Karmiloff-Smith, A; Parisi, D; Plunkett, K (1999). "Innateness and emergentism". In Graham, George; Bechtel, William (eds.). A companion to cognitive science. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 590–601. ISBN 978-0-631-21851-7. OCLC 47008353.
  39. ^ Ramscar, Michael; Gitcho, Nicole (2007). "Developmental change and the nature of learning in childhood". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (7): 274–9. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.007. PMID 17560161. S2CID 6513545.
  40. ^ O'Grady, William (2008). "Innateness, universal grammar, and emergentism" (PDF). Lingua. 118 (4): 620–631. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2007.03.005.
  41. ^ Cowie, F. (1999) What's Within? Nativism Reconsidered (Oxford University Press, New York).
  42. ^ Barbara Scholz; Geoffrey Pullum (2006). Robert J. Stainton (ed.). "Irrational Nativist Exuberance". Contemporary Debates in Cognitive Science: 59–80. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.401.2561.
  43. ^ Seidenberg, Mark S.; J.L. McClelland (1989). "A distributed developmental model of word recognition and naming". Psychological Review. 96 (4): 523–568. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.127.3083. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.4.523. PMID 2798649.
  44. ^ Saffran, Jenny; R.N.Aslin; E.L. Newport (1996). "Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants". Science. 274 (5294): 1926–1928. Bibcode:1996Sci...274.1926S. doi:10.1126/science.274.5294.1926. PMID 8943209. S2CID 13321604.
  45. ^ Chang, Franklin; Dell, Gary S.; Bock, Kathryn (2006). "Becoming syntactic". Psychological Review. 113 (2): 234–272. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.113.2.234. ISSN 1939-1471. PMID 16637761.
  46. ^ a b Saffran, Jenny R. (2003). "Statistical language learning: mechanisms and constraints". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 12 (4): 110–114. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01243. ISSN 0963-7214. S2CID 146485087.
  47. ^ a b Saffran, Jenny; Aslin, Newport (1996). "Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants". Science. 274 (5294): 1926–1928. Bibcode:1996Sci...274.1926S. doi:10.1126/science.274.5294.1926. PMID 8943209. S2CID 13321604.
  48. ^ a b Estes, Katharine Graf; Evans, Julia L.; Alibali, Martha W.; Saffran, Jenny R. (2007). "Can Infants Map Meaning to Newly Segmented Words?". Psychological Science. SAGE Publications. 18 (3): 254–260. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01885.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMC 3864753. PMID 17444923.
  49. ^ a b Lany, Jill; Saffran (January 2010). "From Statistics to Meaning: Infants' Acquisition of Lexical Categories". Psychological Science. 21 (2): 284–91. doi:10.1177/0956797609358570. PMC 3865606. PMID 20424058.
  50. ^ Freudenthal, Daniel; J.M. Pine; F. Gobet (2005). "Modelling the development of children's use of optional infinitives in English and Dutch using MOSAIC" (PDF). Cognitive Science. 30 (2): 277–310. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_47. PMID 21702816. Retrieved 2 April 2009.
  51. ^ Jones, Gary; F. Gobet; J.M. Pine (2007). "Linking working memory and long-term memory: A computational model of the learning of new words" (PDF). Developmental Science. 10 (6): 853–873. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00638.x. PMID 17973801. Retrieved 2 April 2009.
  52. ^ Bannard C, Lieven E, Tomasello M (October 2009). "Modeling children's early grammatical knowledge". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106 (41): 17284–9. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10617284B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905638106. PMC 2765208. PMID 19805057.
  53. ^ McCauley, Stewart M.; Christiansen, Morten H. (2017). "Computational Investigations of Multiword Chunks in Language Learning". Topics in Cognitive Science. 9 (3): 637–652. doi:10.1111/tops.12258. ISSN 1756-8765. PMID 28481476.
  54. ^ Steven C. Hayes; Dermot Barnes-Holmes; Brian Roche, eds. (2001). Relational Frame Theory: A Post-Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition (Hardcover). Plenum Press. ISBN 978-0-306-46600-7. OCLC 51896575.
  55. ^ Bruner, J. (1983). Child's Talk: Learning to Use Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  56. ^ Moerk, E.L. (1994). . International Journal of Psycholinguistics. 10: 33–58. Archived from the original on 2019-08-29. Retrieved 2019-08-29.
  57. ^ Vygotskii [Vygotsky], L.S. 1935. "Dinamika umstvennogo razvitiia shkol'nika v sviazi s obucheniem." In Umstvennoe razvitie detei v protsesse obucheniia, pp. 33–52. Moscow-Leningrad: Gosuchpedgiz.
  58. ^ Fry, Dennis (1977). Homo loquens, Man as a talking animal. Cambridge University Press. pp. 117. ISBN 978-0-521-29239-9.
  59. ^ Radford, Andrew (1990). Syntactic Theory and the Acquisition of English Syntax. Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-631-16358-9.
  60. ^ Marcus G, Pinker S, Ullman M, Hollander M, Rosen TJ, Xu F (1992). "Overregularization in language acquisition" (PDF). Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Serial No. 228. 57 (4): 1–182. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5834.1992.tb00313.x. PMID 1518508.
  61. ^ Carlson, Neil; Heth, Donald (2007). Psychology the Science of Behaviour. Pearson Education:New Jersey.
  62. ^ Galasso, Joseph (2016). From Merge to Move: A Minimalist Perspective on the Design of Language and its Role in Early Child Syntax. LINCOM Studies in Theoretical Linguistics 59.).
  63. ^ Moro, A. (2000). Dynamic Antisymmetry, Linguistic Inquiry Monograph Series 38. MIT Press.).
  64. ^ Miyagawa, Shigeru (2010). Why Agree? Why Move?. MIT Press.
  65. ^ Roeper, Tom (2007). The Prism of Grammar: How child language illuminates humanism. MIT Press.).
  66. ^ Lillo-Martin, Diane C.; Crain, Stephen (1999). An introduction to linguistic theory and language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers. ISBN 978-0-631-19536-8. OCLC 799714148.
  67. ^ Baker, Mark Raphael (2002). The atoms of language. Oxford [Oxfordshire]: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-860632-1. OCLC 66740160.
  68. ^ Marcus, Gary F. (1993). "Negative evidence in language acquisition" (PDF). Cognition. 46 (1): 53–85. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.466.3904. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90022-n. PMID 8432090. S2CID 23458757.
  69. ^ Brown, Roger; Camile Hanlon (1970). "Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech". In J. R. Hayes (ed.). Cognition and the development of language. New York: Wiley.
  70. ^ Lenneberg, Eric (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York: Wiley.
  71. ^ Stromswold, Karin (11 December 2009). Lessons from a mute child. Rich Languages from Poor Inputs: A Workshop in Honor of Carol Chomsky. MIT, Cambridge, MA.
  72. ^ Chomsky, N. (1975). Reflections on Language. New York: Pantheon Books.
  73. ^ Pinker, Steven (2007). The Language Instinct: How the Mind Creates Language (P.S.). Harper Perennial Modern Classics. ISBN 978-0-06-133646-1. OCLC 778413074.
  74. ^ Nadia, Steve. . Archived from the original on 2019-06-30. Retrieved 2016-05-01.
  75. ^ White, EJ.; Hutka, SA.; Williams, LJ.; Moreno, S. (2013). "Learning, neural plasticity and sensitive periods: implications for language acquisition, music training and transfer across the lifespan". Front Syst Neurosci. 7: 90. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00090. PMC 3834520. PMID 24312022.
  76. ^ Kuhl P, Stevens E, Hayashi A, Deguchi T, Kiritani S, Iverson P (February 2006). "Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months". Developmental Science. 9 (2): F13–F21. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00468.x. PMID 16472309.
  77. ^ Pallier, Cristophe. "Critical periods in language acquisition and language attrition" (PDF).
  78. ^ Penfield, Wilder (1959). Speech and Brain-mechanisms. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. p. 242. ISBN 9781400854677.
  79. ^ Singleton, David; Ryan, Lisa (2004-12-31). Language Acquisition. Bristol, Blue Ridge Summit: Multilingual Matters. doi:10.21832/9781853597596. ISBN 978-1-85359-759-6.
  80. ^ Newport, Elissa (1990). "Maturational constraints on language learning". Cognitive Science. 14: 11–28. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2. S2CID 207056257.
  81. ^ Purves, Dale; Augustine, George J.; Fitzpatrick, David; Katz, Lawrence C.; LaMantia, Anthony-Samuel; McNamara, James O.; Williams, S. Mark (2001-01-01). "The Development of Language: A Critical Period in Humans". Neuroscience (2nd ed.).
  82. ^ Curtiss, Susan (1977). Genie: a psycholinguistic study of a modern-day "wild child". Boston: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-196350-7. OCLC 3073433.
  83. ^ Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Wegner, Daniel M. (2011) [2009]. "9". Psychology [Second Edition] (Second ed.). United States of America: Worth Publishers. pp. 351–352. ISBN 9781429237192.
  84. ^ Mehler, Jacques; Jusczyk, Peter; Lambertz, Ghislaine; Halsted, Nilofar; Bertoncini, Josiane; Amiel-Tison, Claudine (1988). "A precursor to language acquisition in young infants". Cognition. 29 (2): 143–178. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(88)90035-2. PMID 3168420. S2CID 43126875.
  85. ^ Hepper, Peter (11 June 1988). "Fetal "Soap" Addiction". Lancet. 331 (8598): 1347–1348. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(88)92170-8. PMID 2897602. S2CID 5350836.
  86. ^ DeCasper, Anthony; Spence, Melanie (1986). "Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns' perception of speech sounds". Infant Behavior and Development. 9 (2): 133–150. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(86)90025-1.
  87. ^ Kisilevsky, Barbara; Hains, Sylvia; Lee, Kang; Xie, Xing; Huang, Hefeng; Ye, Hai; Zhang, Ke; We, Zengping (2003). "Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition". Psychological Science. 14 (3): 220–224. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.02435. PMID 12741744. S2CID 11219888.
  88. ^ Moon, Christine; Cooper, Robin; Fifer, William (1993). "Two-day-olds prefer their native language". Infant Behavior and Development. 16 (4): 495–500. doi:10.1016/0163-6383(93)80007-U.
  89. ^ Moon, Christine; Lagercrantz, Hugo; Kuhl, Patricia (2013). "Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth: A two-country study". Acta Paediatr. 102 (2): 156–160. doi:10.1111/apa.12098. PMC 3543479. PMID 23173548.
  90. ^ Ferry, Alissa; Fló, Ana; Brusini, Perrine; Cattarossi, Luigi; Macagno, Francesco; Nespor, Marina; Mehler, Jacques (2016). "On the edge of language acquisition: inherent constraints on encoding multisyllabic sequences in the neonate brain". Developmental Science. 19 (3): 488–503. doi:10.1111/desc.12323. PMID 26190466.
  91. ^ a b c Partanen, Eino; Kujala, Teija; Näätänen, Risto; Litola, Auli; Sambeth, Anke; Huotilainen, Minna (2013). "Learning-induced neural plasticity of speech processing before birth". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (37): 15145–15150. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11015145P. doi:10.1073/pnas.1302159110. PMC 3773755. PMID 23980148.
  92. ^ Bloom L.; Hood L.; Lichtbown P. (1974). "Imitation in language: If, when, and why". Cognitive Psychology. 6 (3): 380–420. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90018-8. OCLC 65013247.
  93. ^ Miller, George A. (1977). Spontaneous apprentices: children and language. New York: Seabury Press. ISBN 978-0-8164-9330-2. OCLC 3002566.
  94. ^ Masur EF (1995). "Infants' early verbal imitation and their later lexical development". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 41 (3): 286–306. OCLC 89395784.
  95. ^ Gathercole SE, Baddeley AD (1989). . Journal of Memory and Language. 28 (2): 200–213. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2. Archived from the original on 2012-08-17. Retrieved 2009-12-20.
  96. ^ Gathercole SE (2006). (PDF). Applied Psycholinguistics. 27 (4): 513–543. doi:10.1017/S0142716406060383. S2CID 145633911. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-06-05.
  97. ^ Gupta Prahlad; MacWhinney Brian (1997). "Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short-term memory: Computational and neural bases". Brain and Language. 59 (2): 267–333. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1819. PMID 9299067. S2CID 16443213.
  98. ^ Regier Terry (2003). "Emergent constraints on word-learning: A computational review". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (6): 263–268. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.330.5309. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00108-6. PMID 12804693. S2CID 18524556.
  99. ^ Regier, T. (Nov 2005). "The emergence of words: attentional learning in form and meaning". Cogn Sci. 29 (6): 819–65. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.531.1228. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_31. PMID 21702796.
  100. ^ Hadzibeganovic Tarik; Cannas Sergio A (2009). "A Tsallis' statistics based neural network model for novel word learning". Physica A. 388 (5): 732–746. Bibcode:2009PhyA..388..732H. doi:10.1016/j.physa.2008.10.042.
  101. ^ Roy Deb K.; Pentland Alex P. (2002). "Learning words from sights and sounds: A computational model". Cognitive Science. 26: 113–146. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.61.2924. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2601_4.
  102. ^ Fazly Afsaneh; Alishahi Afra; Stevenson Suzanne (2010). "A Probabilistic Computational Model of Cross-Situational Word Learning". Cognitive Science. 34 (6): 1017–1063. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.639.8919. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01104.x. PMID 21564243.
  103. ^ Yu Chen; Ballard Dana H (2007). "A unified model of early word learning: Integrating statistical and social cues". Neurocomputing. 70 (13–15): 2149–2165. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.218.7981. doi:10.1016/j.neucom.2006.01.034.
  104. ^ Libertus Klaus; Violi Dominic A (2016). "Sit to talk: Relation between motor skills and language development in infancy". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 475. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00475. PMC 4815289. PMID 27065934.
  105. ^ Walle Eric A, Campos Joe J (2014). . Developmental Psychology. 50 (2): 336–348. doi:10.1037/a0033238. PMID 23750505. S2CID 46531535. Archived from the original on 2020-07-27.
  106. ^ He M, Walle Eric A, Campos Joe J (2015). . Infancy. 20 (3): 283–305. doi:10.1111/infa.12071. S2CID 956306. Archived from the original on 2020-06-23.
  107. ^ Letts, Carolyn (March 2, 2013). "Socio-economic status and language acquisition: children's performance on the new Reynell Developmental Language Scales" (PDF). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders. 48 (2): 131–143. doi:10.1111/1460-6984.12004. PMID 23472954.
  108. ^ a b Landauer, TK; Dumais, ST. (1997). "A solution to Plato's problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition" (PDF). Psychological Review. 104 (2): 211–240. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.184.4759. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.104.2.211.
  109. ^ Fry, Dennis (1977). Homo loquens, Man as a talking animal. Cambridge University Press. pp. 115–116. ISBN 978-0-521-29239-9.
  110. ^ a b Markman, Ellen M. (1990). "Constraints Children Place on Word Meanings". Cognitive Science. 14 (1): 57–77. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog1401_4.
  111. ^ Hansen, Mikkel B.; Markman, Ellen M. (2009). "Children's use of mutual exclusivity to learn labels for parts of objects". Developmental Psychology. 45 (2): 592–596. doi:10.1037/a0014838. PMID 19271842.
  112. ^ Bates, TC.; Luciano, M.; Medland, SE.; Montgomery, GW.; Wright, MJ.; Martin, NG. (Jan 2011). "Genetic variance in a component of the language acquisition device: ROBO1 polymorphisms associated with phonological buffer deficits". Behav Genet. 41 (1): 50–7. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9402-9. PMID 20949370. S2CID 13129473.
  113. ^ Mountford, Hayley S.; Newbury, Dianne F. (2019). "The genetics of language acquisition". In Horst, Jessica S.; von Koss Torkildsen, Janne (eds.). International Handbook of Language Acquisition. Routledge. pp. 33–50. doi:10.4324/9781315110622-3. ISBN 9781315110622. S2CID 195460306. Retrieved 2021-08-26.
  114. ^ Newman, A. J.; Tremblay, A.; Nichols, E. S.; Neville, H. J.; Ullman, M. T. (2012). "The influence of language proficiency on lexical semantic processing in native and late learners of english". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 24 (5): 1205–1223. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00143. PMC 4447492. PMID 21981676.
  115. ^ Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel Todd.; Wegner, Daniel M. (2011). Psycholog. New York, NY: Worth Publishers. p. 357. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2. OCLC 696604625.
  116. ^ Och, Franz Josef; Ney, Hermann (2004). "The Alignment Template Approach to Statistical Machine Translation". Computational Linguistics. 30 (4): 417–449. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.136.1291. doi:10.1162/0891201042544884. S2CID 1272090.
  117. ^ Chen, David L., and Raymond J. Mooney. "Learning to sportscast: a test of grounded language acquisition 2015-10-23 at the Wayback Machine." Proceedings of the 25th international conference on Machine learning. ACM, 2008.
  118. ^ Chater, Nick; Manning, Christopher D. (2006). "Probabilistic models of language processing and acquisition" (PDF). Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 10 (7): 335–344. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.006. PMID 16784883. S2CID 1923520.
  119. ^ Zuidema, Willem H. "How the poverty of the stimulus solves the poverty of the stimulus." Advances in neural information processing systems. 2003.

Further reading

  • Fitch, WT. (Feb 2011). "Unity and diversity in human language". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366 (1563): 376–88. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0223. PMC 3013471. PMID 21199842.
  • Kuhl PK (September 2010). "Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition". Neuron. 67 (5): 713–27. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.038. PMC 2947444. PMID 20826304.
  • Arias-Trejo N, Plunkett K (December 2009). "Lexical-semantic priming effects during infancy". Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 364 (1536): 3633–47. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0146. PMC 2846315. PMID 19933137.
  • Hickok G, Poeppel D (2004). "Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language". Cognition. 92 (1–2): 67–99. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.011. PMID 15037127. S2CID 635860.
  • Kennison, S. (2013). Introduction to language development. Los Angeles, CA: Sage.
  • Kuperberg GR (May 2007). "Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: challenges to syntax". Brain Res. 1146: 23–49. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2006.12.063. PMID 17400197. S2CID 17364244.
  • Pickering MJ, Ferreira VS (May 2008). "Structural priming: a critical review". Psychol Bull. 134 (3): 427–59. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.427. PMC 2657366. PMID 18444704.
  • Pinker, Steven (2004). "Why Nature & Nurture Won't Go Away". Daedalus. 133 (4): 5–17. doi:10.1162/0011526042365591. S2CID 57568640.
  • Richardson FM, Price CJ (October 2009). "Structural MRI studies of language function in the undamaged brain". Brain Struct Funct. 213 (6): 511–23. doi:10.1007/s00429-009-0211-y. PMC 2749930. PMID 19618210.
  • Sousa, David A. (2011). How the brain learns. Thousand Oaks, Calif.: Corwin Press. ISBN 978-1-4129-9797-3. OCLC 769765510.

External links

  • Language acquisition in American Sign Language Rich, detailed documentation of language acquisition in ASL.
  • Innateness and Language, Encyclopedia Entry
  • Beth Skwarecki, "Babies Learn to Recognize Words in the Womb", Science, 26 August 2013 [1]

language, acquisition, language, learning, redirects, here, other, uses, language, learning, disambiguation, process, which, humans, acquire, capacity, perceive, comprehend, language, other, words, gain, ability, aware, language, understand, well, produce, wor. Language learning redirects here For other uses see Language learning disambiguation Language acquisition is the process by which humans acquire the capacity to perceive and comprehend language in other words gain the ability to be aware of language and to understand it as well as to produce and use words and sentences to communicate Language acquisition involves structures rules and representation The capacity to use language successfully requires one to acquire a range of tools including phonology morphology syntax semantics and an extensive vocabulary Language can be vocalized as in speech or manual as in sign 1 Human language capacity is represented in the brain Even though human language capacity is finite one can say and understand an infinite number of sentences which is based on a syntactic principle called recursion Evidence suggests that every individual has three recursive mechanisms that allow sentences to go indeterminately These three mechanisms are relativization complementation and coordination 2 There are two main guiding principles in first language acquisition speech perception always precedes speech production and the gradually evolving system by which a child learns a language is built up one step at a time beginning with the distinction between individual phonemes 3 Linguists who are interested in child language acquisition have for many years questioned how language is acquired Lidz et al state The question of how these structures are acquired then is more properly understood as the question of how a learner takes the surface forms in the input and converts them into abstract linguistic rules and representations 4 Language acquisition usually refers to first language acquisition which studies infants acquisition of their native language whether that be spoken language or signed language 1 though it can also refer to bilingual first language acquisition BFLA which refers to an infant s simultaneous acquisition of two native languages 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 This is distinguished from second language acquisition which deals with the acquisition in both children and adults of additional languages In addition to speech reading and writing a language with an entirely different script compounds the complexities of true foreign language literacy Language acquisition is one of the quintessential human traits 12 13 Contents 1 History 2 As a typically human phenomenon 3 General approaches 3 1 Emergentism 3 2 Empiricism 3 2 1 Statistical learning 3 2 2 Chunking 3 3 Relational frame theory 3 4 Social interactionism 3 5 Syntax morphology and generative grammar 3 6 A Merge linguistics based Theory 4 Representation in the brain 4 1 Sensitive period 5 Vocabulary acquisition 6 Meaning 7 Genetic and neurocognitive research 8 Artificial intelligence 9 Prelingual deafness 9 1 Signed language acquisition 9 2 Cochlear implants 10 See also 11 References 12 Further reading 13 External linksHistory EditSome early observation based ideas about language acquisition were proposed by Plato who felt that word meaning mapping in some form was innate Additionally Sanskrit grammarians debated for over twelve centuries whether humans ability to recognize the meaning of words was god given possibly innate or passed down by previous generations and learned from already established conventions a child learning the word for cow by listening to trusted speakers talking about cows 14 Philosophers in ancient societies were interested in how humans acquired the ability to understand and produce language well before empirical methods for testing those theories were developed but for the most part they seemed to regard language acquisition as a subset of man s ability to acquire knowledge and learn concepts 15 Empiricists like Thomas Hobbes and John Locke argued that knowledge and for Locke language emerge ultimately from abstracted sense impressions These arguments lean towards the nurture side of the argument that language is acquired through sensory experience which led to Rudolf Carnap s Aufbau an attempt to learn all knowledge from sense datum using the notion of remembered as similar to bind them into clusters which would eventually map into language 16 Proponents of behaviorism argued that language may be learned through a form of operant conditioning In B F Skinner s Verbal Behavior 1957 he suggested that the successful use of a sign such as a word or lexical unit given a certain stimulus reinforces its momentary or contextual probability Since operant conditioning is contingent on reinforcement by rewards a child would learn that a specific combination of sounds stands for a specific thing through repeated successful associations made between the two A successful use of a sign would be one in which the child is understood for example a child saying up when they want to be picked up and rewarded with the desired response from another person thereby reinforcing the child s understanding of the meaning of that word and making it more likely that they will use that word in a similar situation in the future Some empiricist theories of language acquisition include the statistical learning theory Charles F Hockett of language acquisition relational frame theory functionalist linguistics social interactionist theory and usage based language acquisition Skinner s behaviorist idea was strongly attacked by Noam Chomsky in a review article in 1959 calling it largely mythology and a serious delusion 17 Arguments against Skinner s idea of language acquisition through operant conditioning include the fact that children often ignore language corrections from adults Instead children typically follow a pattern of using an irregular form of a word correctly making errors later on and eventually returning to the proper use of the word For example a child may correctly learn the word gave past tense of give and later on use the word gived Eventually the child will typically go back to using the correct word gave Chomsky claimed the pattern is difficult to attribute to Skinner s idea of operant conditioning as the primary way that children acquire language Chomsky argued that if language were solely acquired through behavioral conditioning children would not likely learn the proper use of a word and suddenly use the word incorrectly 18 Chomsky believed that Skinner failed to account for the central role of syntactic knowledge in language competence Chomsky also rejected the term learning which Skinner used to claim that children learn language through operant conditioning 19 Instead Chomsky argued for a mathematical approach to language acquisition based on a study of syntax As a typically human phenomenon EditThe capacity to acquire and use language is a key aspect that distinguishes humans from other beings Although it is difficult to pin down what aspects of language are uniquely human there are a few design features that can be found in all known forms of human language but that are missing from forms of animal communication For example many animals are able to communicate with each other by signaling to the things around them but this kind of communication lacks the arbitrariness of human vernaculars in that there is nothing about the sound of the word dog that would hint at its meaning Other forms of animal communication may utilize arbitrary sounds but are unable to combine those sounds in different ways to create completely novel messages that can then be automatically understood by another Hockett called this design feature of human language productivity It is crucial to the understanding of human language acquisition that humans are not limited to a finite set of words but rather must be able to understand and utilize a complex system that allows for an infinite number of possible messages So while many forms of animal communication exist they differ from human language in that they have a limited range of vocabulary tokens and the vocabulary items are not combined syntactically to create phrases 20 Victor of Aveyron Herbert S Terrace conducted a study on a chimpanzee known as Nim Chimpsky in an attempt to teach him American Sign Language This study was an attempt to further research done with a chimpanzee named Washoe who was reportedly able to acquire American Sign Language However upon further inspection Terrace concluded that both experiments were failures 21 While Nim was able to acquire signs he never acquired a knowledge of grammar and was unable to combine signs in a meaningful way Researchers noticed that signs that seemed spontaneous were in fact cued by teachers 22 and not actually productive When Terrace reviewed Project Washoe he found similar results He postulated that there is a fundamental difference between animals and humans in their motivation to learn language animals such as in Nim s case are motivated only by physical reward while humans learn language in order to create a new type of communication 23 In another language acquisition study Jean Marc Gaspard Itard attempted to teach Victor of Aveyron a feral child how to speak Victor was able to learn a few words but ultimately never fully acquired language 24 Slightly more successful was a study done on Genie another child never introduced to society She had been entirely isolated for the first thirteen years of her life by her father Caretakers and researchers attempted to measure her ability to learn a language She was able to acquire a large vocabulary but never acquired grammatical knowledge Researchers concluded that the theory of a critical period was true Genie was too old to learn how to speak productively although she was still able to comprehend language 25 General approaches EditA major debate in understanding language acquisition is how these capacities are picked up by infants from the linguistic input 26 Input in the linguistic context is defined as All words contexts and other forms of language to which a learner is exposed relative to acquired proficiency in first or second languages Nativists such as Chomsky have focused on the hugely complex nature of human grammars the finiteness and ambiguity of the input that children receive and the relatively limited cognitive abilities of an infant From these characteristics they conclude that the process of language acquisition in infants must be tightly constrained and guided by the biologically given characteristics of the human brain Otherwise they argue it is extremely difficult to explain how children within the first five years of life routinely master the complex largely tacit grammatical rules of their native language 27 Additionally the evidence of such rules in their native language is all indirect adult speech to children cannot encompass all of what children know by the time they have acquired their native language 28 Other scholars however have resisted the possibility that infants routine success at acquiring the grammar of their native language requires anything more than the forms of learning seen with other cognitive skills including such mundane motor skills as learning to ride a bike In particular there has been resistance to the possibility that human biology includes any form of specialization for language This conflict is often referred to as the nature and nurture debate Of course most scholars acknowledge that certain aspects of language acquisition must result from the specific ways in which the human brain is wired a nature component which accounts for the failure of non human species to acquire human languages and that certain others are shaped by the particular language environment in which a person is raised a nurture component which accounts for the fact that humans raised in different societies acquire different languages The as yet unresolved question is the extent to which the specific cognitive capacities in the nature component are also used outside of language Emergentism Edit Emergentist theories such as Brian MacWhinney s competition model posit that language acquisition is a cognitive process that emerges from the interaction of biological pressures and the environment According to these theories neither nature nor nurture alone is sufficient to trigger language learning both of these influences must work together in order to allow children to acquire a language The proponents of these theories argue that general cognitive processes subserve language acquisition and that the result of these processes is language specific phenomena such as word learning and grammar acquisition The findings of many empirical studies support the predictions of these theories suggesting that language acquisition is a more complex process than many have proposed 29 Empiricism Edit Although Chomsky s theory of a generative grammar has been enormously influential in the field of linguistics since the 1950s many criticisms of the basic assumptions of generative theory have been put forth by cognitive functional linguists who argue that language structure is created through language use 30 These linguists argue that the concept of a language acquisition device LAD is unsupported by evolutionary anthropology which tends to show a gradual adaptation of the human brain and vocal cords to the use of language rather than a sudden appearance of a complete set of binary parameters delineating the whole spectrum of possible grammars ever to have existed and ever to exist 31 On the other hand cognitive functional theorists use this anthropological data to show how human beings have evolved the capacity for grammar and syntax to meet our demand for linguistic symbols Binary parameters are common to digital computers but may not be applicable to neurological systems such as the human brain citation needed Further the generative theory has several constructs such as movement empty categories complex underlying structures and strict binary branching that cannot possibly be acquired from any amount of linguistic input It is unclear that human language is actually anything like the generative conception of it Since language as imagined by nativists is unlearnably complex citation needed subscribers to this theory argue that it must therefore be innate 32 Nativists hypothesize that some features of syntactic categories exist even before a child is exposed to any experience categories on which children map words of their language as they learn their native language 33 A different theory of language however may yield different conclusions While all theories of language acquisition posit some degree of innateness they vary in how much value they place on this innate capacity to acquire language Empiricism places less value on the innate knowledge arguing instead that the input combined with both general and language specific learning capacities is sufficient for acquisition 34 Since 1980 linguists studying children such as Melissa Bowerman and Asifa Majid 35 and psychologists following Jean Piaget like Elizabeth Bates 36 and Jean Mandler came to suspect that there may indeed be many learning processes involved in the acquisition process and that ignoring the role of learning may have been a mistake citation needed In recent years the debate surrounding the nativist position has centered on whether the inborn capabilities are language specific or domain general such as those that enable the infant to visually make sense of the world in terms of objects and actions The anti nativist view has many strands but a frequent theme is that language emerges from usage in social contexts using learning mechanisms that are a part of an innate general cognitive learning apparatus This position has been championed by David M W Powers 37 Elizabeth Bates 38 Catherine Snow Anat Ninio Brian MacWhinney Michael Tomasello 20 Michael Ramscar 39 William O Grady 40 and others Philosophers such as Fiona Cowie 41 and Barbara Scholz with Geoffrey Pullum 42 have also argued against certain nativist claims in support of empiricism The new field of cognitive linguistics has emerged as a specific counter to Chomsky s Generative Grammar and to Nativism Statistical learning Edit Main article Statistical learning in language acquisition Some language acquisition researchers such as Elissa Newport Richard Aslin and Jenny Saffran emphasize the possible roles of general learning mechanisms especially statistical learning in language acquisition The development of connectionist models that when implemented are able to successfully learn words and syntactical conventions 43 supports the predictions of statistical learning theories of language acquisition as do empirical studies of children s detection of word boundaries 44 In a series of connectionist model simulations Franklin Chang has demonstrated that such a domain general statistical learning mechanism could explain a wide range of language structure acquisition phenomena 45 Statistical learning theory suggests that when learning language a learner would use the natural statistical properties of language to deduce its structure including sound patterns words and the beginnings of grammar 46 That is language learners are sensitive to how often syllable combinations or words occur in relation to other syllables 47 48 49 Infants between 21 and 23 months old are also able to use statistical learning to develop lexical categories such as an animal category which infants might later map to newly learned words in the same category These findings suggest that early experience listening to language is critical to vocabulary acquisition 49 The statistical abilities are effective but also limited by what qualifies as input what is done with that input and by the structure of the resulting output 46 One should also note that statistical learning and more broadly distributional learning can be accepted as a component of language acquisition by researchers on either side of the nature and nurture debate From the perspective of that debate an important question is whether statistical learning can by itself serve as an alternative to nativist explanations for the grammatical constraints of human language Chunking Edit The central idea of these theories is that language development occurs through the incremental acquisition of meaningful chunks of elementary constituents which can be words phonemes or syllables Recently this approach has been highly successful in simulating several phenomena in the acquisition of syntactic categories 50 and the acquisition of phonological knowledge 51 Chunking theories of language acquisition constitute a group of theories related to statistical learning theories in that they assume that the input from the environment plays an essential role however they postulate different learning mechanisms clarification needed Researchers at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology have developed a computer model analyzing early toddler conversations to predict the structure of later conversations They showed that toddlers develop their own individual rules for speaking with slots into which they put certain kinds of words A significant outcome of this research is that rules inferred from toddler speech were better predictors of subsequent speech than traditional grammars 52 This approach has several features that make it unique the models are implemented as computer programs which enables clear cut and quantitative predictions to be made they learn from naturalistic input actual child directed utterances and attempt to create their own utterances the model was tested in languages including English Spanish and German Chunking for this model was shown to be most effective in learning a first language but was able to create utterances learning a second language 53 Relational frame theory Edit Main article Relational frame theory The relational frame theory RFT Hayes Barnes Holmes Roche 2001 provides a wholly selectionist learning account of the origin and development of language competence and complexity Based upon the principles of Skinnerian behaviorism RFT posits that children acquire language purely through interacting with the environment RFT theorists introduced the concept of functional contextualism in language learning which emphasizes the importance of predicting and influencing psychological events such as thoughts feelings and behaviors by focusing on manipulable variables in their own context RFT distinguishes itself from Skinner s work by identifying and defining a particular type of operant conditioning known as derived relational responding a learning process that to date appears to occur only in humans possessing a capacity for language Empirical studies supporting the predictions of RFT suggest that children learn language through a system of inherent reinforcements challenging the view that language acquisition is based upon innate language specific cognitive capacities 54 Social interactionism Edit Main article Social interactionist theory Social interactionist theory is an explanation of language development emphasizing the role of social interaction between the developing child and linguistically knowledgeable adults It is based largely on the socio cultural theories of Soviet psychologist Lev Vygotsky and was made prominent in the Western world by Jerome Bruner 55 Unlike other approaches it emphasizes the role of feedback and reinforcement in language acquisition Specifically it asserts that much of a child s linguistic growth stems from modeling of and interaction with parents and other adults who very frequently provide instructive correction 56 It is thus somewhat similar to behaviorist accounts of language learning It differs substantially though in that it posits the existence of a social cognitive model and other mental structures within children a sharp contrast to the black box approach of classical behaviorism Another key idea within the theory of social interactionism is that of the zone of proximal development This is a theoretical construct denoting the set of tasks a child is capable of performing with guidance but not alone 57 As applied to language it describes the set of linguistic tasks for example proper syntax suitable vocabulary usage that a child cannot carry out on its own at a given time but can learn to carry out if assisted by an able adult Syntax morphology and generative grammar Edit As syntax began to be studied more closely in the early 20th century in relation to language learning it became apparent to linguists psychologists and philosophers that knowing a language was not merely a matter of associating words with concepts but that a critical aspect of language involves knowledge of how to put words together sentences are usually needed in order to communicate successfully not just isolated words 15 A child will use short expressions such as Bye bye Mummy or All gone milk which actually are combinations of individual nouns and an operator 58 before they begin to produce gradually more complex sentences In the 1990s within the principles and parameters framework this hypothesis was extended into a maturation based structure building model of child language regarding the acquisition of functional categories In this model children are seen as gradually building up more and more complex structures with lexical categories like noun and verb being acquired before functional syntactic categories like determiner and complementizer 59 It is also often found that in acquiring a language the most frequently used verbs are irregular verbs citation needed In learning English for example young children first begin to learn the past tense of verbs individually However when they acquire a rule such as adding ed to form the past tense they begin to exhibit occasional overgeneralization errors e g runned hitted alongside correct past tense forms One influential citation needed proposal regarding the origin of this type of error suggests that the adult state of grammar stores each irregular verb form in memory and also includes a block on the use of the regular rule for forming that type of verb In the developing child s mind retrieval of that block may fail causing the child to erroneously apply the regular rule instead of retrieving the irregular 60 61 A Merge linguistics based Theory Edit Further information Merge linguistics In Bare Phrase structure Minimalist Program since theory internal considerations define the specifier position of an internal merge projection phases vP and CP as the only type of host which could serve as potential landing sites for move based elements displaced from lower down within the base generated VP structure e g A movement such as passives The apple was eaten by John ate the apple or raising Some work does seem to remain There does seem to remain some work as a consequence any strong version of a Structure building model of child language which calls for an exclusive external merge argument structure stage prior to an internal merge scope discourse related stage would claim that young children s stage 1 utterances lack the ability to generate and host elements derived via movement operations In terms of a Merge based theory of language acquisition 62 complements and specifiers are simply notations for first merge complement of head complement and later second merge specifier of specifier head with merge always forming to a head First merge establishes only a set a b and is not an ordered pair e g an N N compound of boat house would allow the ambiguous readings of either a kind of house and or a kind of boat It is only with second merge that order is derived out of a set a a b which yields the recursive properties of syntax e g a house boat house house boat now reads unambiguously only as a kind of boat It is this property of recursion that allows for projection and labeling of a phrase to take place 63 in this case that the Noun boat is the Head of the compound and house acting as a kind of specifier modifier External merge first merge establishes substantive base structure inherent to the VP yielding theta argument structure and may go beyond the lexical category VP to involve the functional category light verb vP Internal merge second merge establishes more formal aspects related to edge properties of scope and discourse related material pegged to CP In a Phase based theory this twin vP CP distinction follows the duality of semantics discussed within the Minimalist Program and is further developed into a dual distinction regarding a probe goal relation 64 As a consequence at the external first merge only stage young children would show an inability to interpret readings from a given ordered pair since they would only have access to the mental parsing of a non recursive set See Roeper for a full discussion of recursion in child language acquisition 65 In addition to word order violations other more ubiquitous results of a first merge stage would show that children s initial utterances lack the recursive properties of inflectional morphology yielding a strict Non inflectional stage 1 consistent with an incremental Structure building model of child language Generative grammar associated especially with the work of Noam Chomsky is currently one of the approaches to explaining children s acquisition of syntax 66 Its leading idea is that human biology imposes narrow constraints on the child s hypothesis space during language acquisition In the principles and parameters framework which has dominated generative syntax since Chomsky s 1980 Lectures on Government and Binding The Pisa Lectures the acquisition of syntax resembles ordering from a menu the human brain comes equipped with a limited set of choices from which the child selects the correct options by imitating the parents speech while making use of the context 67 An important argument which favors the generative approach is the poverty of the stimulus argument The child s input a finite number of sentences encountered by the child together with information about the context in which they were uttered is in principle compatible with an infinite number of conceivable grammars Moreover rarely can children rely on corrective feedback from adults when they make a grammatical error adults generally respond and provide feedback regardless of whether a child s utterance was grammatical or not and children have no way of discerning if a feedback response was intended to be a correction Additionally when children do understand that they are being corrected they don t always reproduce accurate restatements dubious discuss 68 69 Yet barring situations of medical abnormality or extreme privation all children in a given speech community converge on very much the same grammar by the age of about five years An especially dramatic example is provided by children who for medical reasons are unable to produce speech and therefore can never be corrected for a grammatical error but nonetheless converge on the same grammar as their typically developing peers according to comprehension based tests of grammar 70 71 Considerations such as those have led Chomsky Jerry Fodor Eric Lenneberg and others to argue that the types of grammar the child needs to consider must be narrowly constrained by human biology the nativist position 72 These innate constraints are sometimes referred to as universal grammar the human language faculty or the language instinct 73 Representation in the brain EditRecent advances in functional neuroimaging technology have allowed for a better understanding of how language acquisition is manifested physically in the brain Language acquisition almost always occurs in children during a period of rapid increase in brain volume At this point in development a child has many more neural connections than he or she will have as an adult allowing for the child to be more able to learn new things than he or she would be as an adult 74 Sensitive period Edit Main articles Sensitive periods Language and Critical period hypothesis Language acquisition has been studied from the perspective of developmental psychology and neuroscience 75 which looks at learning to use and understand language parallel to a child s brain development It has been determined through empirical research on developmentally normal children as well as through some extreme cases of language deprivation that there is a sensitive period of language acquisition in which human infants have the ability to learn any language Several researchers have found that from birth until the age of six months infants can discriminate the phonetic contrasts of all languages Researchers believe that this gives infants the ability to acquire the language spoken around them After this age the child is able to perceive only the phonemes specific to the language being learned The reduced phonemic sensitivity enables children to build phonemic categories and recognize stress patterns and sound combinations specific to the language they are acquiring 76 As Wilder Penfield noted Before the child begins to speak and to perceive the uncommitted cortex is a blank slate on which nothing has been written In the ensuing years much is written and the writing is normally never erased After the age of ten or twelve the general functional connections have been established and fixed for the speech cortex According to the sensitive or critical period models the age at which a child acquires the ability to use language is a predictor of how well he or she is ultimately able to use language 77 However there may be an age at which becoming a fluent and natural user of a language is no longer possible Penfield and Roberts 1959 cap their sensitive period at nine years old 78 The human brain may be automatically wired to learn languages citation needed but this ability does not last into adulthood in the same way that it exists during childhood 79 By around age 12 language acquisition has typically been solidified and it becomes more difficult to learn a language in the same way a native speaker would citation needed Just like children who speak deaf children go through a critical period for learning language Deaf children who acquire their first language later in life show lower performance in complex aspects of grammar 80 At that point it is usually a second language that a person is trying to acquire and not a first 27 clarification needed Assuming that children are exposed to language during the critical period 81 acquiring language is almost never missed by cognitively normal children Humans are so well prepared to learn language that it becomes almost impossible not to Researchers are unable to experimentally test the effects of the sensitive period of development on language acquisition because it would be unethical to deprive children of language until this period is over However case studies on abused language deprived children show that they exhibit extreme limitations in language skills even after instruction 82 At a very young age children can distinguish different sounds but cannot yet produce them During infancy children begin to babble Deaf babies babble in the same patterns as hearing babies do showing that babbling is not a result of babies simply imitating certain sounds but is actually a natural part of the process of language development Deaf babies do however often babble less than hearing babies and they begin to babble later on in infancy at approximately 11 months as compared to approximately 6 months for hearing babies 83 Prelinguistic language abilities that are crucial for language acquisition have been seen even earlier than infancy There have been many different studies examining different modes of language acquisition prior to birth The study of language acquisition in fetuses began in the late 1980s when several researchers independently discovered that very young infants could discriminate their native language from other languages In Mehler et al 1988 84 infants underwent discrimination tests and it was shown that infants as young as 4 days old could discriminate utterances in their native language from those in an unfamiliar language but could not discriminate between two languages when neither was native to them These results suggest that there are mechanisms for fetal auditory learning and other researchers have found further behavioral evidence to support this notion Fetus auditory learning through environmental habituation has been seen in a variety of different modes such as fetus learning of familiar melodies Hepper 1988 85 story fragments DeCasper amp Spence 1986 86 recognition of mother s voice Kisilevsky 2003 87 and other studies showing evidence of fetal adaptation to native linguistic environments Moon Cooper amp Fifer 1993 88 Prosody is the property of speech that conveys an emotional state of the utterance as well as the intended form of speech for example question statement or command Some researchers in the field of developmental neuroscience argue that fetal auditory learning mechanisms result solely from discrimination of prosodic elements Although this would hold merit in an evolutionary psychology perspective i e recognition of mother s voice familiar group language from emotionally valent stimuli some theorists argue that there is more than prosodic recognition in elements of fetal learning Newer evidence shows that fetuses not only react to the native language differently from non native languages but that fetuses react differently and can accurately discriminate between native and non native vowel sounds Moon Lagercrantz amp Kuhl 2013 89 Furthermore a 2016 study showed that newborn infants encode the edges of multisyllabic sequences better than the internal components of the sequence Ferry et al 2016 90 Together these results suggest that newborn infants have learned important properties of syntactic processing in utero as demonstrated by infant knowledge of native language vowels and the sequencing of heard multisyllabic phrases This ability to sequence specific vowels gives newborn infants some of the fundamental mechanisms needed in order to learn the complex organization of a language From a neuroscientific perspective neural correlates have been found that demonstrate human fetal learning of speech like auditory stimuli that most other studies have been analyzing clarification needed Partanen et al 2013 91 In a study conducted by Partanen et al 2013 91 researchers presented fetuses with certain word variants and observed that these fetuses exhibited higher brain activity in response to certain word variants as compared to controls In this same study a significant correlation existed between the amount of prenatal exposure and brain activity with greater activity being associated with a higher amount of prenatal speech exposure pointing to the important learning mechanisms present before birth that are fine tuned to features in speech Partanen et al 2013 91 The phases of language acquisition in childrenVocabulary acquisition EditMain article Vocabulary learning Learning a new word that is learning to speak this word and speak it on the appropriate occasions depends upon many factors First the learner needs to be able to hear what they are attempting to pronounce Also required is the capacity to engage in speech repetition 92 93 94 95 Children with reduced ability to repeat non words a marker of speech repetition abilities show a slower rate of vocabulary expansion than children with normal ability 96 Several computational models of vocabulary acquisition have been proposed 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 Various studies have shown that the size of a child s vocabulary by the age of 24 months correlates with the child s future development and language skills A lack of language richness by this age has detrimental and long term effects on the child s cognitive development which is why it is so important for parents to engage their infants in language original research If a child knows fifty or fewer words by the age of 24 months he or she is classified as a late talker and future language development like vocabulary expansion and the organization of grammar is likely to be slower and stunted citation needed Two more crucial elements of vocabulary acquisition are word segmentation and statistical learning described above Word segmentation or the ability to break down words into syllables from fluent speech can be accomplished by eight month old infants 47 By the time infants are 17 months old they are able to link meaning to segmented words 48 Recent evidence also suggests that motor skills and experiences may influence vocabulary acquisition during infancy Specifically learning to sit independently between 3 and 5 months of age has been found to predict receptive vocabulary at both 10 and 14 months of age 104 and independent walking skills have been found to correlate with language skills at around 10 to 14 months of age 105 106 These findings show that language acquisition is an embodied process that is influenced by a child s overall motor abilities and development Studies have also shown a correlation between socioeconomic status and vocabulary acquisition 107 Meaning EditChildren learn on average ten to fifteen new word meanings each day but only one of these can be accounted for by direct instruction 108 The other nine to fourteen word meanings must have been acquired in some other way It has been proposed that children acquire these meanings through processes modeled by latent semantic analysis that is when they encounter an unfamiliar word children use contextual information to guess its rough meaning correctly 108 A child may expand the meaning and use of certain words that are already part of its mental lexicon in order to denominate anything that is somehow related but for which it does not know the specific word For instance a child may broaden the use of mummy and dada in order to indicate anything that belongs to its mother or father or perhaps every person who resembles its own parents another example might be to say rain while meaning I don t want to go out 109 There is also reason to believe that children use various heuristics to infer the meaning of words properly Markman and others have proposed that children assume words to refer to objects with similar properties cow and pig might both be animals rather than to objects that are thematically related cow and milk are probably not both animals 110 Children also seem to adhere to the whole object assumption and think that a novel label refers to an entire entity rather than to one of its parts 110 This assumption along with other resources such as grammar and morphological cues or lexical constraints may help aid the child in acquiring word meaning but conclusions based on such resources may sometimes conflict 111 Genetic and neurocognitive research EditAccording to several linguists neurocognitive research has confirmed many standards of language learning such as learning engages the entire person cognitive affective and psychomotor domains the human brain seeks patterns in its searching for meaning emotions affect all aspects of learning retention and recall past experience always affects new learning the brain s working memory has a limited capacity lecture usually results in the lowest degree of retention rehearsal is essential for retention practice alone does not make perfect and each brain is unique Sousa 2006 p 274 In terms of genetics the gene ROBO1 has been associated with phonological buffer integrity or length 112 Genetic research has found two major factors predicting successful language acquisition and maintenance These include inherited intelligence and the lack of genetic anomalies that may cause speech pathologies such as mutations in the FOXP2 gene which cause verbal dyspraxia The role of inherited intelligence increases with age accounting for 20 of IQ variation in infants and for 60 in adults It affects a vast variety of language related abilities from spatio motor skills to writing fluency There have been debates in linguistics philosophy psychology and genetics with some scholars arguing that language is fully or mostly innate but the research evidence points to genetic factors only working in interaction with environmental ones 113 Although it is difficult to determine without invasive measures which exact parts of the brain become most active and important for language acquisition fMRI and PET technology has allowed for some conclusions to be made about where language may be centered Kuniyoshi Sakai has proposed based on several neuroimaging studies that there may be a grammar center in the brain whereby language is primarily processed in the left lateral premotor cortex located near the pre central sulcus and the inferior frontal sulcus Additionally these studies have suggested that first language and second language acquisition may be represented differently in the cortex 27 In a study conducted by Newman et al the relationship between cognitive neuroscience and language acquisition was compared through a standardized procedure involving native speakers of English and native Spanish speakers who all had a similar length of exposure to the English language averaging about 26 years It was concluded that the brain does in fact process languages differently clarification needed but rather than being related to proficiency levels language processing relates more to the function of the brain itself 114 During early infancy language processing seems to occur over many areas in the brain However over time it gradually becomes concentrated into two areas Broca s area and Wernicke s area Broca s area is in the left frontal cortex and is primarily involved in the production of the patterns in vocal and sign language Wernicke s area is in the left temporal cortex and is primarily involved in language comprehension The specialization of these language centers is so extensive clarification needed that damage to them can result in aphasia 115 Artificial intelligence EditFurther information Computational models of language acquisition Some algorithms for language acquisition are based on statistical machine translation 116 Language acquisition can be modeled as a machine learning process which may be based on learning semantic parsers 117 or grammar induction algorithms 118 119 Prelingual deafness EditMain article Prelingual deafnessThis section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed June 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message Prelingual deafness is defined as hearing loss that occurred at birth or before an individual has learned to speak In the United States 2 to 3 out of every 1000 children are born deaf or hard of hearing Even though it might be presumed that deaf children acquire language in different ways since they are not receiving the same auditory input as hearing children many research findings indicate that deaf children acquire language in the same way that hearing children do and when given the proper language input understand and express language just as well as their hearing peers Babies who learn sign language produce signs or gestures that are more regular and more frequent than hearing babies acquiring spoken language Just as hearing babies babble deaf babies acquiring sign language will babble with their hands otherwise known as manual babbling Therefore as many studies have shown language acquisition by deaf children parallel the language acquisition of a spoken language by hearing children because humans are biologically equipped for language regardless of the modality Signed language acquisition Edit Deaf children s visual manual language acquisition not only parallel spoken language acquisition but by the age of 30 months most deaf children that were exposed to a visual language had a more advanced grasp with subject pronoun copy rules than hearing children Their vocabulary bank at the ages of 12 17 months exceed that of a hearing child s though it does even out when they reach the two word stage The use of space for absent referents and the more complex handshapes in some signs prove to be difficult for children between 5 and 9 years of age because of motor development and the complexity of remembering the spatial use Cochlear implants Edit Other options besides sign language for kids with prelingual deafness include the use of hearing aids to strengthen remaining sensory cells or cochlear implants to stimulate the hearing nerve directly Cochlear Implants are hearing devices that are placed behind the ear and contain a receiver and electrodes which are placed under the skin and inside the cochlea Despite these developments there is still a risk that prelingually deaf children may not develop good speech and speech reception skills Although cochlear implants produce sounds they are unlike typical hearing and deaf and hard of hearing people must undergo intensive therapy in order to learn how to interpret these sounds They must also learn how to speak given the range of hearing they may or may not have However deaf children of deaf parents tend to do better with language even though they are isolated from sound and speech because their language uses a different mode of communication that is accessible to them the visual modality of language Although cochlear implants were initially approved for adults now there is pressure to implant children early in order to maximize auditory skills for mainstream learning which in turn has created controversy around the topic Due to recent advances in technology cochlear implants allow some deaf people to acquire some sense of hearing There are interior and exposed exterior components that are surgically implanted Those who receive cochlear implants earlier on in life show more improvement on speech comprehension and language Spoken language development does vary widely for those with cochlear implants though due to a number of different factors including age at implantation frequency quality and type of speech training Some evidence suggests that speech processing occurs at a more rapid pace in some prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants than those with traditional hearing aids However cochlear implants may not always work Research shows that people develop better language with a cochlear implant when they have a solid first language to rely on to understand the second language they would be learning In the case of prelingually deaf children with cochlear implants a signed language like American Sign Language would be an accessible language for them to learn to help support the use of the cochlear implant as they learn a spoken language as their L2 Without a solid accessible first language these children run the risk of language deprivation especially in the case that a cochlear implant fails to work They would have no access to sound meaning no access to the spoken language they are supposed to be learning If a signed language was not a strong language for them to use and neither was a spoken language they now have no access to any language and run the risk of missing their critical period See also EditChunking Creole language Evolutionary linguistics Evolutionary psychology of language Fis phenomenon FOXP2 Gestures in language acquisition Glossary of language teaching terms and ideas Identity and language learning KE family Language attrition Language transfer List of children s speech corpora List of language acquisition researchers Metalinguistic awareness Natural language processing Non native speech database Origin of language Passive speaker language Second language attrition Spoken languageReferences Edit a b Pichler Chen 2015 Language Learning through the Eye and Ear Webcast Laurent Clerc National Deaf Education Center Gallaudet University Retrieved 15 December 2020 Lightfoot David 2010 Language acquisition and language change Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science 1 5 677 684 doi 10 1002 wcs 39 ISSN 1939 5078 PMID 26271652 Fry Dennis 1977 Homo loquens Man as a talking animal Cambridge University Press pp 107 108 ISBN 978 0 521 29239 9 Lidz Jeffrey Waxman 16 April 2003 What infants know about syntax but couldn t have learned experimental evidence for syntactic structure at 18 months PDF Cognition 89 2003 B65 B73 Archived from the original PDF on 12 December 2017 Retrieved 12 December 2017 via Elsevier science Bergman Coral Rhodes 1976 Interference vs independent development in infant bilingualism In Keller Gary D Teschner Richard V Viera Silvia eds Bilingualism in the Bicentennial and Beyond Bilingual Press pp 86 96 ISBN 9780916950019 Genesee Fred 1989 Early bilingual development One language or two Journal of Child Language 16 1 161 179 doi 10 1017 S0305000900013490 PMID 2647777 S2CID 20680592 de Houwer Annick 1990 The Acquisition of Two Languages from Birth Cambridge University Press ISBN 9780511519789 de Houwer Annick 1996 Bilingual Language Acquisition In Fletcher Paul MacWhinney Brian eds The Handbook of Child Language Wiley Blackwell ISBN 978 0 631 20312 4 Hulk Aafke Muller Natascha December 2000 Bilingual first language acquisition at the interface between syntax and pragmatics Bilingualism Language and Cognition 3 3 227 244 doi 10 1017 S1366728900000353 Paradis Johanne Genesee Fred 1996 SYNTACTIC ACQUISITION IN BILINGUAL CHILDREN Autonomous or Interdependent PDF Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18 1 1 25 doi 10 1017 S0272263100014662 JSTOR 44487857 S2CID 143994688 Serratrice Ludovica Sorace Antonella Paoli Sandra 2004 Crosslinguistic influence at the syntax pragmatics interface Subjects and objects in English Italian bilingual and monolingual acquisition PDF Bilingualism Language and Cognition 7 3 183 205 doi 10 1017 S1366728904001610 S2CID 38643617 Friederici AD Oct 2011 The brain basis of language processing from structure to function Physiol Rev 91 4 1357 92 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 385 5620 doi 10 1152 physrev 00006 2011 PMID 22013214 Kosslyn Stephen M Osherson Daniel N 1995 An invitation to cognitive science Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 65045 8 OCLC 613819557 Matilal Bimal Krishna 1990 The word and the world India s contribution to the study of language Oxford Oxfordshire Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 562515 8 OCLC 24041690 a b Innateness and Language Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy 2017 Kendra A Palmer 2009 Understanding Human Language An In Depth Exploration of the Human Facility for Language StudentPulse com Retrieved 22 August 2012 Noam Chomsky Skinner B F 1959 A Review of B F Skinner s Verbal Behavior Language 35 1 26 58 doi 10 2307 411334 JSTOR 411334 Harley Trevor A 2010 Talking the Talk Language Psychology and Science New York NY Psychology Press pp 68 71 ISBN 978 1 84169 339 2 Harris Margaret 1992 Language Experience and Early Language Development From Input to Uptake UK Psychology Press ISBN 978 0863772382 a b Tomasello Michael 2008 Origins of human communication Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 20177 3 OCLC 439979810 Carey Benedict 2007 11 01 Washoe a Chimp of Many Words Dies at 42 The New York Times ISSN 0362 4331 Retrieved 2020 09 30 Nim Chimpsky and Noam Chomsky Psychology Today Retrieved 2020 09 28 How Infants Learn to Use Words Psychology Today Retrieved 2020 09 28 The Wild Child of Aveyron amp Critical Periods of Learning The Layman s Linguist 2019 10 19 Archived from the original on 2021 04 15 Retrieved 2020 09 30 The Feral Child Nicknamed Genie Psychology Today Retrieved 2020 09 28 Kennison Shelia M 2013 07 30 Introduction to language development Los Angeles SAGE Publications ISBN 978 1 4129 9606 8 OCLC 830837502 a b c Sakai Kuniyoshi L 2005 Language Acquisition and Brain Development Science 310 5749 815 819 Bibcode 2005Sci 310 815S doi 10 1126 science 1113530 PMID 16272114 S2CID 20714845 Lust Barbara 2006 Child Language Acquisition and Growth Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 28 29 ISBN 9780511803413 Brian MacWhinney ed 1999 The Emergence of Language Lawrence Erlbaum Associates ISBN 978 0 8058 3010 1 OCLC 44958022 Tomasello Michael 2003 Constructing a language a usage based theory of language acquisition Cambridge Harvard University Press ISBN 978 0 674 01030 7 OCLC 62782600 Mameli M Bateson P Feb 2011 An evaluation of the concept of innateness Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366 1563 436 43 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0174 PMC 3013469 PMID 21199847 Lidz Jeffrey Lasnik Howard Dec 2016 Roberts Ian ed The Argument from the Poverty of the Stimulus The Oxford Handbook of Universal Grammar 1 220 248 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780199573776 013 10 ISBN 9780199573776 L Bavin Edith 2009 The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 15 34 ISBN 9780511576164 OCLC 798060196 Tomasello Michael 2000 First Steps Toward a usage based theory of language acquisition Cognitive Linguistics 11 1 2 61 82 doi 10 1515 cogl 2001 012 Majid Asifa Bowerman Melissa Staden Miriam van Boster James S 2007 The semantic categories of cutting and breaking events A crosslinguistic perspective PDF Cognitive Linguistics 18 2 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 1014 4819 doi 10 1515 COG 2007 005 hdl 2066 104711 ISSN 0936 5907 S2CID 33506231 Bates E D Amico S Jacobsen T Szekely A Andonova E Devescovi A Herron D Lu CC et al Jun 2003 Timed picture naming in seven languages PDF Psychon Bull Rev 10 2 344 80 doi 10 3758 BF03196494 PMC 3392189 PMID 12921412 Archived from the original PDF on 2017 08 13 Retrieved 2013 12 27 Powers David M W Turk Christopher 1989 Machine learning of natural language London New York Springer Verlag ISBN 978 0 387 19557 5 OCLC 20263032 Bates E Elman J Johnson M Karmiloff Smith A Parisi D Plunkett K 1999 Innateness and emergentism In Graham George Bechtel William eds A companion to cognitive science Oxford Blackwell pp 590 601 ISBN 978 0 631 21851 7 OCLC 47008353 Ramscar Michael Gitcho Nicole 2007 Developmental change and the nature of learning in childhood Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 7 274 9 doi 10 1016 j tics 2007 05 007 PMID 17560161 S2CID 6513545 O Grady William 2008 Innateness universal grammar and emergentism PDF Lingua 118 4 620 631 doi 10 1016 j lingua 2007 03 005 Cowie F 1999 What s Within Nativism Reconsidered Oxford University Press New York Barbara Scholz Geoffrey Pullum 2006 Robert J Stainton ed Irrational Nativist Exuberance Contemporary Debates in Cognitive Science 59 80 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 401 2561 Seidenberg Mark S J L McClelland 1989 A distributed developmental model of word recognition and naming Psychological Review 96 4 523 568 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 127 3083 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 96 4 523 PMID 2798649 Saffran Jenny R N Aslin E L Newport 1996 Statistical learning by 8 month old infants Science 274 5294 1926 1928 Bibcode 1996Sci 274 1926S doi 10 1126 science 274 5294 1926 PMID 8943209 S2CID 13321604 Chang Franklin Dell Gary S Bock Kathryn 2006 Becoming syntactic Psychological Review 113 2 234 272 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 113 2 234 ISSN 1939 1471 PMID 16637761 a b Saffran Jenny R 2003 Statistical language learning mechanisms and constraints Current Directions in Psychological Science 12 4 110 114 doi 10 1111 1467 8721 01243 ISSN 0963 7214 S2CID 146485087 a b Saffran Jenny Aslin Newport 1996 Statistical learning by 8 month old infants Science 274 5294 1926 1928 Bibcode 1996Sci 274 1926S doi 10 1126 science 274 5294 1926 PMID 8943209 S2CID 13321604 a b Estes Katharine Graf Evans Julia L Alibali Martha W Saffran Jenny R 2007 Can Infants Map Meaning to Newly Segmented Words Psychological Science SAGE Publications 18 3 254 260 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 2007 01885 x ISSN 0956 7976 PMC 3864753 PMID 17444923 a b Lany Jill Saffran January 2010 From Statistics to Meaning Infants Acquisition of Lexical Categories Psychological Science 21 2 284 91 doi 10 1177 0956797609358570 PMC 3865606 PMID 20424058 Freudenthal Daniel J M Pine F Gobet 2005 Modelling the development of children s use of optional infinitives in English and Dutch using MOSAIC PDF Cognitive Science 30 2 277 310 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog0000 47 PMID 21702816 Retrieved 2 April 2009 Jones Gary F Gobet J M Pine 2007 Linking working memory and long term memory A computational model of the learning of new words PDF Developmental Science 10 6 853 873 doi 10 1111 j 1467 7687 2007 00638 x PMID 17973801 Retrieved 2 April 2009 Bannard C Lieven E Tomasello M October 2009 Modeling children s early grammatical knowledge Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106 41 17284 9 Bibcode 2009PNAS 10617284B doi 10 1073 pnas 0905638106 PMC 2765208 PMID 19805057 McCauley Stewart M Christiansen Morten H 2017 Computational Investigations of Multiword Chunks in Language Learning Topics in Cognitive Science 9 3 637 652 doi 10 1111 tops 12258 ISSN 1756 8765 PMID 28481476 Steven C Hayes Dermot Barnes Holmes Brian Roche eds 2001 Relational Frame Theory A Post Skinnerian Account of Human Language and Cognition Hardcover Plenum Press ISBN 978 0 306 46600 7 OCLC 51896575 Bruner J 1983 Child s Talk Learning to Use Language Oxford Oxford University Press Moerk E L 1994 Corrections in first language acquisition Theoretical controversies and factual evidence International Journal of Psycholinguistics 10 33 58 Archived from the original on 2019 08 29 Retrieved 2019 08 29 Vygotskii Vygotsky L S 1935 Dinamika umstvennogo razvitiia shkol nika v sviazi s obucheniem In Umstvennoe razvitie detei v protsesse obucheniia pp 33 52 Moscow Leningrad Gosuchpedgiz Fry Dennis 1977 Homo loquens Man as a talking animal Cambridge University Press pp 117 ISBN 978 0 521 29239 9 Radford Andrew 1990 Syntactic Theory and the Acquisition of English Syntax Blackwell ISBN 978 0 631 16358 9 Marcus G Pinker S Ullman M Hollander M Rosen TJ Xu F 1992 Overregularization in language acquisition PDF Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development Serial No 228 57 4 1 182 doi 10 1111 j 1540 5834 1992 tb00313 x PMID 1518508 Carlson Neil Heth Donald 2007 Psychology the Science of Behaviour Pearson Education New Jersey Galasso Joseph 2016 From Merge to Move A Minimalist Perspective on the Design of Language and its Role in Early Child Syntax LINCOM Studies in Theoretical Linguistics 59 Moro A 2000 Dynamic Antisymmetry Linguistic Inquiry Monograph Series 38 MIT Press Miyagawa Shigeru 2010 Why Agree Why Move MIT Press Roeper Tom 2007 The Prism of Grammar How child language illuminates humanism MIT Press Lillo Martin Diane C Crain Stephen 1999 An introduction to linguistic theory and language acquisition Cambridge MA Blackwell Publishers ISBN 978 0 631 19536 8 OCLC 799714148 Baker Mark Raphael 2002 The atoms of language Oxford Oxfordshire Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 860632 1 OCLC 66740160 Marcus Gary F 1993 Negative evidence in language acquisition PDF Cognition 46 1 53 85 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 466 3904 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 93 90022 n PMID 8432090 S2CID 23458757 Brown Roger Camile Hanlon 1970 Derivational complexity and order of acquisition in child speech In J R Hayes ed Cognition and the development of language New York Wiley Lenneberg Eric 1967 Biological Foundations of Language New York Wiley Stromswold Karin 11 December 2009 Lessons from a mute child Rich Languages from Poor Inputs A Workshop in Honor of Carol Chomsky MIT Cambridge MA Chomsky N 1975 Reflections on Language New York Pantheon Books Pinker Steven 2007 The Language Instinct How the Mind Creates Language P S Harper Perennial Modern Classics ISBN 978 0 06 133646 1 OCLC 778413074 Nadia Steve Kid s Brain Power Archived from the original on 2019 06 30 Retrieved 2016 05 01 White EJ Hutka SA Williams LJ Moreno S 2013 Learning neural plasticity and sensitive periods implications for language acquisition music training and transfer across the lifespan Front Syst Neurosci 7 90 doi 10 3389 fnsys 2013 00090 PMC 3834520 PMID 24312022 Kuhl P Stevens E Hayashi A Deguchi T Kiritani S Iverson P February 2006 Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months Developmental Science 9 2 F13 F21 doi 10 1111 j 1467 7687 2006 00468 x PMID 16472309 Pallier Cristophe Critical periods in language acquisition and language attrition PDF Penfield Wilder 1959 Speech and Brain mechanisms Princeton NJ Princeton University Press p 242 ISBN 9781400854677 Singleton David Ryan Lisa 2004 12 31 Language Acquisition Bristol Blue Ridge Summit Multilingual Matters doi 10 21832 9781853597596 ISBN 978 1 85359 759 6 Newport Elissa 1990 Maturational constraints on language learning Cognitive Science 14 11 28 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog1401 2 S2CID 207056257 Purves Dale Augustine George J Fitzpatrick David Katz Lawrence C LaMantia Anthony Samuel McNamara James O Williams S Mark 2001 01 01 The Development of Language A Critical Period in Humans Neuroscience 2nd ed Curtiss Susan 1977 Genie a psycholinguistic study of a modern day wild child Boston Academic Press ISBN 978 0 12 196350 7 OCLC 3073433 Schacter Daniel L Gilbert Daniel T Wegner Daniel M 2011 2009 9 Psychology Second Edition Second ed United States of America Worth Publishers pp 351 352 ISBN 9781429237192 Mehler Jacques Jusczyk Peter Lambertz Ghislaine Halsted Nilofar Bertoncini Josiane Amiel Tison Claudine 1988 A precursor to language acquisition in young infants Cognition 29 2 143 178 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 88 90035 2 PMID 3168420 S2CID 43126875 Hepper Peter 11 June 1988 Fetal Soap Addiction Lancet 331 8598 1347 1348 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 88 92170 8 PMID 2897602 S2CID 5350836 DeCasper Anthony Spence Melanie 1986 Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns perception of speech sounds Infant Behavior and Development 9 2 133 150 doi 10 1016 0163 6383 86 90025 1 Kisilevsky Barbara Hains Sylvia Lee Kang Xie Xing Huang Hefeng Ye Hai Zhang Ke We Zengping 2003 Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition Psychological Science 14 3 220 224 doi 10 1111 1467 9280 02435 PMID 12741744 S2CID 11219888 Moon Christine Cooper Robin Fifer William 1993 Two day olds prefer their native language Infant Behavior and Development 16 4 495 500 doi 10 1016 0163 6383 93 80007 U Moon Christine Lagercrantz Hugo Kuhl Patricia 2013 Language experienced in utero affects vowel perception after birth A two country study Acta Paediatr 102 2 156 160 doi 10 1111 apa 12098 PMC 3543479 PMID 23173548 Ferry Alissa Flo Ana Brusini Perrine Cattarossi Luigi Macagno Francesco Nespor Marina Mehler Jacques 2016 On the edge of language acquisition inherent constraints on encoding multisyllabic sequences in the neonate brain Developmental Science 19 3 488 503 doi 10 1111 desc 12323 PMID 26190466 a b c Partanen Eino Kujala Teija Naatanen Risto Litola Auli Sambeth Anke Huotilainen Minna 2013 Learning induced neural plasticity of speech processing before birth Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110 37 15145 15150 Bibcode 2013PNAS 11015145P doi 10 1073 pnas 1302159110 PMC 3773755 PMID 23980148 Bloom L Hood L Lichtbown P 1974 Imitation in language If when and why Cognitive Psychology 6 3 380 420 doi 10 1016 0010 0285 74 90018 8 OCLC 65013247 Miller George A 1977 Spontaneous apprentices children and language New York Seabury Press ISBN 978 0 8164 9330 2 OCLC 3002566 Masur EF 1995 Infants early verbal imitation and their later lexical development Merrill Palmer Quarterly 41 3 286 306 OCLC 89395784 Gathercole SE Baddeley AD 1989 Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children A longitudinal study Journal of Memory and Language 28 2 200 213 doi 10 1016 0749 596X 89 90044 2 Archived from the original on 2012 08 17 Retrieved 2009 12 20 Gathercole SE 2006 Nonword repetition and word learning The nature of the relationship PDF Applied Psycholinguistics 27 4 513 543 doi 10 1017 S0142716406060383 S2CID 145633911 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 06 05 Gupta Prahlad MacWhinney Brian 1997 Vocabulary acquisition and verbal short term memory Computational and neural bases Brain and Language 59 2 267 333 doi 10 1006 brln 1997 1819 PMID 9299067 S2CID 16443213 Regier Terry 2003 Emergent constraints on word learning A computational review Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7 6 263 268 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 330 5309 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 03 00108 6 PMID 12804693 S2CID 18524556 Regier T Nov 2005 The emergence of words attentional learning in form and meaning Cogn Sci 29 6 819 65 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 531 1228 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog0000 31 PMID 21702796 Hadzibeganovic Tarik Cannas Sergio A 2009 A Tsallis statistics based neural network model for novel word learning Physica A 388 5 732 746 Bibcode 2009PhyA 388 732H doi 10 1016 j physa 2008 10 042 Roy Deb K Pentland Alex P 2002 Learning words from sights and sounds A computational model Cognitive Science 26 113 146 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 61 2924 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog2601 4 Fazly Afsaneh Alishahi Afra Stevenson Suzanne 2010 A Probabilistic Computational Model of Cross Situational Word Learning Cognitive Science 34 6 1017 1063 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 639 8919 doi 10 1111 j 1551 6709 2010 01104 x PMID 21564243 Yu Chen Ballard Dana H 2007 A unified model of early word learning Integrating statistical and social cues Neurocomputing 70 13 15 2149 2165 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 218 7981 doi 10 1016 j neucom 2006 01 034 Libertus Klaus Violi Dominic A 2016 Sit to talk Relation between motor skills and language development in infancy Frontiers in Psychology 7 475 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2016 00475 PMC 4815289 PMID 27065934 Walle Eric A Campos Joe J 2014 Infant language development is related to the acquisition of walking Developmental Psychology 50 2 336 348 doi 10 1037 a0033238 PMID 23750505 S2CID 46531535 Archived from the original on 2020 07 27 He M Walle Eric A Campos Joe J 2015 A cross national investigation of the relationship between infant walking and language development Infancy 20 3 283 305 doi 10 1111 infa 12071 S2CID 956306 Archived from the original on 2020 06 23 Letts Carolyn March 2 2013 Socio economic status and language acquisition children s performance on the new Reynell Developmental Language Scales PDF International Journal of Language amp Communication Disorders 48 2 131 143 doi 10 1111 1460 6984 12004 PMID 23472954 a b Landauer TK Dumais ST 1997 A solution to Plato s problem The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition PDF Psychological Review 104 2 211 240 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 184 4759 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 104 2 211 Fry Dennis 1977 Homo loquens Man as a talking animal Cambridge University Press pp 115 116 ISBN 978 0 521 29239 9 a b Markman Ellen M 1990 Constraints Children Place on Word Meanings Cognitive Science 14 1 57 77 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog1401 4 Hansen Mikkel B Markman Ellen M 2009 Children s use of mutual exclusivity to learn labels for parts of objects Developmental Psychology 45 2 592 596 doi 10 1037 a0014838 PMID 19271842 Bates TC Luciano M Medland SE Montgomery GW Wright MJ Martin NG Jan 2011 Genetic variance in a component of the language acquisition device ROBO1 polymorphisms associated with phonological buffer deficits Behav Genet 41 1 50 7 doi 10 1007 s10519 010 9402 9 PMID 20949370 S2CID 13129473 Mountford Hayley S Newbury Dianne F 2019 The genetics of language acquisition In Horst Jessica S von Koss Torkildsen Janne eds International Handbook of Language Acquisition Routledge pp 33 50 doi 10 4324 9781315110622 3 ISBN 9781315110622 S2CID 195460306 Retrieved 2021 08 26 Newman A J Tremblay A Nichols E S Neville H J Ullman M T 2012 The influence of language proficiency on lexical semantic processing in native and late learners of english Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 24 5 1205 1223 doi 10 1162 jocn a 00143 PMC 4447492 PMID 21981676 Schacter Daniel L Gilbert Daniel Todd Wegner Daniel M 2011 Psycholog New York NY Worth Publishers p 357 ISBN 978 1 4292 3719 2 OCLC 696604625 Och Franz Josef Ney Hermann 2004 The Alignment Template Approach to Statistical Machine Translation Computational Linguistics 30 4 417 449 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 136 1291 doi 10 1162 0891201042544884 S2CID 1272090 Chen David L and Raymond J Mooney Learning to sportscast a test of grounded language acquisition Archived 2015 10 23 at the Wayback Machine Proceedings of the 25th international conference on Machine learning ACM 2008 Chater Nick Manning Christopher D 2006 Probabilistic models of language processing and acquisition PDF Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10 7 335 344 doi 10 1016 j tics 2006 05 006 PMID 16784883 S2CID 1923520 Zuidema Willem H How the poverty of the stimulus solves the poverty of the stimulus Advances in neural information processing systems 2003 Further reading EditFitch WT Feb 2011 Unity and diversity in human language Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366 1563 376 88 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0223 PMC 3013471 PMID 21199842 Kuhl PK September 2010 Brain mechanisms in early language acquisition Neuron 67 5 713 27 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2010 08 038 PMC 2947444 PMID 20826304 Arias Trejo N Plunkett K December 2009 Lexical semantic priming effects during infancy Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364 1536 3633 47 doi 10 1098 rstb 2009 0146 PMC 2846315 PMID 19933137 Hickok G Poeppel D 2004 Dorsal and ventral streams a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language Cognition 92 1 2 67 99 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2003 10 011 PMID 15037127 S2CID 635860 Kennison S 2013 Introduction to language development Los Angeles CA Sage Kuperberg GR May 2007 Neural mechanisms of language comprehension challenges to syntax Brain Res 1146 23 49 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2006 12 063 PMID 17400197 S2CID 17364244 Pickering MJ Ferreira VS May 2008 Structural priming a critical review Psychol Bull 134 3 427 59 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 134 3 427 PMC 2657366 PMID 18444704 Pinker Steven 2004 Why Nature amp Nurture Won t Go Away Daedalus 133 4 5 17 doi 10 1162 0011526042365591 S2CID 57568640 Richardson FM Price CJ October 2009 Structural MRI studies of language function in the undamaged brain Brain Struct Funct 213 6 511 23 doi 10 1007 s00429 009 0211 y PMC 2749930 PMID 19618210 Sousa David A 2011 How the brain learns Thousand Oaks Calif Corwin Press ISBN 978 1 4129 9797 3 OCLC 769765510 External links EditLanguage acquisition in American Sign Language Rich detailed documentation of language acquisition in ASL Innateness and Language Encyclopedia Entry Beth Skwarecki Babies Learn to Recognize Words in the Womb Science 26 August 2013 1 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Language acquisition amp oldid 1142127970, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.