fbpx
Wikipedia

Head-directionality parameter

In linguistics, head directionality is a proposed parameter that classifies languages according to whether they are head-initial (the head of a phrase precedes its complements) or head-final (the head follows its complements). The head is the element that determines the category of a phrase: for example, in a verb phrase, the head is a verb. Therefore, head initial would be "VO" languages and head final would be "OV" languages.[1]

Some languages are consistently head-initial or head-final at all phrasal levels. English is considered to be strongly head-initial (verbs precede their objects, for example), while Japanese is an example of a language that is consistently head-final. In certain other languages, such as German and Gbe, examples of both types of head direction occur. Various theories have been proposed to explain such variation.

Head directionality is connected with the type of branching that predominates in a language: head-initial structures are right-branching, while head-final structures are left-branching.[2] On the basis of these criteria, languages can be divided into head-final (rigid and non-rigid) and head-initial types. The identification of headedness is based on the following: [3]

  1. the order of subject, object, and verb
  2. the relationship between the order of the object and verb
  3. the order of an adposition and its complement
  4. the order of relative clause and head noun.

Types of phrase

In some cases, particularly with noun and adjective phrases, it is not always clear which dependents are to be classed as complements, and which as adjuncts. Although in principle the head-directionality parameter concerns the order of heads and complements only, considerations of head-initiality and head-finality sometimes take account of the position of the head in the phrase as a whole, including adjuncts. The structure of the various types of phrase is analyzed below in relation to specific languages, with a focus on the ordering of head and complement. In some cases (such as English and Japanese) this ordering is found to be the same in practically all types of phrase, whereas in others (such as German and Gbe) the pattern is less consistent. Different theoretical explanations of these inconsistencies are discussed later in the article. There are various types of phrase in which the ordering of head and complement(s) may be considered when attempting to determine the head directionality of a language, including:

  • Verb Phrase: the head of verb phrase (VP) is a verb, and the complement(s) are most commonly objects of various types. The ordering here is related to one of the chief questions in the word order typology of languages, namely the normal order of subject, verb and object within a clause (languages are classed on this basis as SVO, SOV, VSO, etc.).
 
head-initial and head-final constructions
  • Noun Phrase: the head of a noun phrase (NP) is a noun; various kinds of complementizer phrases (CPs) and adpositional phrases (PPs) can be complements.
 
head-initial and head-final constructions
  • Adjective Phrase: the head of an adjective phrase (AP) is an adjective, which can take as a complement, for example, an adverbial phrase or adpositional phrase (PP).
 
head-initial and head-final constructions
  • Adpositional Phrase: the head of an adpositional phrase (PP) is an apposition. Such phrases are called prepositional phrases if they are head-initial (i.e. headed by a preposition), or postpositional phrases if they are head-final (i.e. headed by a postposition). For more on these, see Preposition and postposition. The complement is a determiner phrase (or noun phrase, depending on analytical scheme followed).
 
head-initial and head-final constructions
  • Determiner Phrase: the head of a determiner phrase (DP) is a determiner. DPs were proposed under generative syntax;[4] not all theories of syntax agree that they exist.[5]
  • Complementizer Phrase: the head of a complementizer phrase (CP) is a complementizer, like that in English. In some cases the C head is covert (not overtly present). The complement of C is generally agreed to be a tense phrase (TP).
 
head-initial and head-final constructions
  • Tense Phrase: the head of a tense phrase (TP) is tense; these are phrases in which the head is an abstract category representing tense; the complement is a verb phrase.
  • Aspect Phrase: the head of an aspect phrase (AspP) is aspect; these are phrases in which the head is an abstract syntactic category representing aspect. In more traditional analysis the entire phrase (including any elements denoting tense or aspect) is considered to be simply a verb phrase.

Head-initial languages

English

English is a strongly[citation needed] head-initial language. In a typical verb phrase, for example, the verb precedes its complements, as in the following example:[6]

 
English VP structure
eat an apple
[VP [V eat] [DP an apple]]

The head of the phrase (the verb eat) precedes its complement (the determiner phrase an apple). Switching the order to "[VP [DP an apple] [V eat]]" would be ungrammatical.

Nouns also tend to precede any complements, as in the following example, where the relative clause (or complementizer phrase) that follows the noun may be considered to be a complement:[7]

 
English NP structure
He married a girl who is from Texas.
[NP [N girl] [CP who is from Texas]]

Nouns do not necessarily begin their phrase; they may be preceded by attributive adjectives, but these are regarded as adjuncts rather than complements. (For preceding determiners, see below.) Adjectives themselves may be preceded by adjuncts, namely adverbs, as in extremely happy.[8] However, when an adjective phrase contains a true complement, such as a prepositional phrase, the head adjective precedes it:[9]

a person happy about her work
[AP [A happy] [PP about her work]]

English adpositional phrases are also head-initial; that is, English has prepositions rather than postpositions:[10]

 
English PP structure
a majority of eligible voters
[PP [P of] [DP eligible voters]]

On the determiner phrase (DP) view, where a determiner is taken to be the head of its phrase (rather than the associated noun), English can be seen to be head-initial in this type of phrase too. In the following example[11] the head is taken to be the determiner any, and the complement is the noun (phrase) book:

 
English DP structure
any book
[DP [D any] [NP book]]

English also has head-initial complementizer phrases, as in this example[12] where the complementizer that precedes its complement, the tense phrase Mary did not swim:

We saw that Mary did not swim
[CP [C that] [TP Mary did not swim]]

Grammatical words marking tense and aspect generally precede the semantic verb. This indicates that, if finite verb phrases are analyzed as tense phrases or aspect phrases, these are again head-initial in English. In the example above, did is considered a (past) tense marker, and precedes its complement, the verb phrase not swim. In the following, has is a (perfect) aspect marker;[13] again it appears before the verb (phrase) which is its complement.

 
English AspP Structure
John has arrived
[AspP [Asp has] [VP arrived]]

The following example shows a sequence of nested phrases in which each head precedes its complement.[14] In the complementizer phrase (CP) in (a), the complementizer (C) precedes its tense phrase (TP) complement. In the tense phrase in (b), the tense-marking element (T) precedes its verb phrase (VP) complement. (The subject of the tense phrase, the girl, is a specifier, which does not need to be considered when analyzing the ordering of head and complement.) In the verb phrase in (c), the verb (V) precedes its two complements, namely the determiner phrase (DP) the book and the prepositional phrase (PP) on the table. In (d), where a picture is analyzed as a determiner phrase, the determiner (D) a precedes its noun phrase (NP) complement, while in (e), the preposition (P) on precedes its DP complement your desk.

You know that the girl will put a picture on your desk.
a. CP: [CP [C that ] [TP the girl will put a picture on your desk ] ]
b. TP: [TP [T will ] [VP put a picture on your desk ] ]
c. VP: [VP [V put ] [DP a picture ] [PP on your desk ] ]
d. DP: [DP [D a ] [NP picture ] ]
e. PP: [PP [P on ] [DP your desk ] ]

Indonesian

Indonesian is an example of an SVO head-initial language.[1][15] The characteristic of it being a head-initial language can be examined through a dependency perspective or through a word order perspective. Both approaches lead to the conclusion that Indonesian is a head-initial language.

Dependency perspective

 
Governor-Dependent relationship in SVO Head-initial languages

When examining Indonesian through a dependency perspective, it is considered head initial as the governor of both constituents are positioned before the dependent.[16]

Placing the head before a dependent minimizes the overall dependency distance, which is the distance between the two constituents.[16] Minimizing dependency distance allows for less cognitive demand as a head-final dependency requires the constituents in the dependent clause to be stored in working memory until the head is realized.[16]

 
Dependency distance between constituents in Indonesian

In Indonesian, the number of constituencies affects the dependency direction. When there are 6 constituents — which is a relatively short sentence — there is a preference for head initial relation.[16] However, when there are 11-30 constituents, there appears to be a balance of head-initial and head-final dependencies.[16] Regardless, Indonesian displays an overall head-initial preference on all levels of dependency structure as it consistently attempts to position the head as early on in the sentence even though it produces a longer dependency distance rather than placing the head after its dependents.[16] Furthermore, Indonesian has an overall preference towards head-initial when comparing head-initial and head-final relation on all levels of constituent length for both spoken and written data.[16]

Word order perspective

The subject of the sentence followed by the verb, representing SVO order.[17] The following examples demonstrate head-initial directionality in Indonesian (note that perdana menteri "prime minister" is unusually being head-final):

 
Indonesian head-initial word order in VP

Perdana

Prime

menteri

minister

sudah

already

pulang

home

Perdana menteri sudah pulang

Prime minister already home

"The Prime minister has returned home"


     [CP [DP Perdana menteri] [VP sudah pulang]]

Classifiers and partitives can function as the head nouns of noun phrases. Below is an example of the internal structure of a noun phrase and its head-initial word order.

Botol

Bottle

ini

DET-this

retak

crack

Botol ini retak

Bottle DET-this crack

“This bottle is cracked”


     [CP[DP botol ini][VP retak]]

Head-initial word order is seen in the internal structure of the verb phrase in the following example where the V is in the head position of the verb phrase and thus appears before its complement:

 
Indonesian head-initial word order in VP

Dokter

Doctor

memeriksa

checks

mata

eye

saya

PN-my

Dokter memeriksa mata saya

Doctor checks eye PN-my

"The doctor checked my eyes"


     [CP[DPDokter][VP[V memeriksa][DPmata says]]]

In Indonesian a noun can be followed by another modifying noun whose primary function is to provide more specific information about the preceding head noun, such as indicating what the head noun is made of, gender, locative sense, and what the head noun does, etc. However, no other word is able to intervene between a head noun and its following modifying noun. If a word follows the modifying noun, then it provides reference to the head noun and not the modifying noun. [17]

  • Head noun: [N guru] + Modifying noun: [N bahasa]

guru

teacher

bahasa

language

guru bahasa

teacher language

"language teacher"

  • Head noun: [N guru] + Modifying noun: [N sekolah] + Determiner [D itu]

guru

teacher

sekolah

school

itu

DET-that

guru sekolah itu

teacher school DET-that

"that schoolteacher"

  • Head noun: [Ntoko] + Modifying noun: [N buku]

toko

shop

buku

book

toko buku

shop book

"Bookshop"

  • Head noun: [Ntoko] + Modifying noun: [N buku] + Determiner phrase [DPyang besar]

toko

shop

buku

book

yang

DET-a

besar

big

toko buku yang besar

shop book DET-a big

a big bookshop

  • Head noun: [N sate] + Modifying noun: [N ayam]

sate

satay

ayam

chicken

sate ayam

satay chicken

"chicken satay"

Head-final languages

Japonic: Japanese

Japanese is an example of a strongly head-final language. This can be seen in verb phrases and tense phrases: the verb (tabe in the example) comes after its complement, while the tense marker (ru) comes after the whole verb phrase which is its complement.[6]

 
Japanese VP structure

リンゴを

ringo-o

apple-ACC

食べる

tabe-ru

eat-NPAST

リンゴを 食べる

ringo-o tabe-ru

apple-ACC eat-NPAST

"eat an apple"


     [TP [VP [DP ringo-o] [V tabe]] [T ru]]

Nouns also typically come after any complements, as in the following example where the PP New York-de-no may be regarded as a complement:[18]

 
Japanese NP structure

ジョンの

John-no

John-GEN

昨日の

kinoo-no

yesterday-GEN

ニューヨークでの

New York-de-no

New York-in-GEN

講義

koogi

lecture

ジョンの 昨日の ニューヨークでの 講義

John-no kinoo-no {New York-de-no} koogi

John-GEN yesterday-GEN {New York-in-GEN} lecture

"John's lecture in New York yesterday"


     [NP [PP New York-de-no] [N koogi]]

Adjectives also follow any complements they may have. In this example the complement of quantity, ni-juu-meetoru ("twenty meters"), precedes the head adjective takai ("tall"):[19]

この

Kono

this

ビルは

biru-wa

building-TOP

20メートル

ni-juu-meetoru

two-ten-meter

高い

takai

tall

この ビルは 20メートル 高い

Kono biru-wa ni-juu-meetoru takai

this building-TOP two-ten-meter tall

"This building is twenty meters taller."


     [AP [Q ni-juu-meetoru] [A takai]]

Japanese uses postpositions rather than prepositions, so its adpositional phrases are again head-final:[20]

 
Japanese PP structure

僕が

Boku-ga

I-NOM

高須村に

Takasu-mura-ni

Takasu-village-in

住んでいる

sunde-iru

live-PRES

僕が 高須村に 住んでいる

Boku-ga Takasu-mura-ni sunde-iru

I-NOM Takasu-village-in live-PRES

"I live in Takasu village."


     [PP [DP Takasu-mura] [P ni]]

Determiner phrases are head-final as well:[11]

 
Japanese DP structure

dare

person

mo

any

誰 も

dare mo

person any

"anyone"


     [DP [NP dare] [D mo]]

A complementizer (here to, equivalent to English "that") comes after its complement (here a tense phrase meaning "Mary did not swim"), thus Japanese complementizer phrases are head-final:[12]

メリーが

Mary-ga

Mary-NOM

泳がなかったこと

oyog-ana-katta-koto

swim-NEG-PAST-that

メリーが 泳がなかったこと

Mary-ga oyog-ana-katta-koto

Mary-NOM swim-NEG-PAST-that

"that Mary did not swim"


     [CP [TP Mary-ga oyog-ana-katta] [C to]]

Turkic: Turkish

Turkish is an agglutinative, head-final, and left-branching language that uses a SOV word order.[21] As such, Turkish complements and adjuncts typically precede their head under neutral prosody, and adpositions are postpositional. Turkish employs a case marking system[22] which affixes to the right boundary of the word it is modifying. As such, all case markings in Turkish are suffixes. For example, the set of accusative case marking suffixes -(y)ı-, -(y)i-, -(y)u-, -(y)ü- in Turkish indicate that it is the direct object of a verb. Additionally, while some kinds of definite determiners and postpositions in Turkish can be marked by case, other types also exist as free morphemes.[23] In the following examples, Turkish case marker suffixes are analyzed as complements to the head.

Head-final Tense Phrase

 
Turkish TP structure

In Turkish, tense is denoted by a case marking suffix on the verb.[24]

Ahmet

Ahmet

anne-sin-i

mother-3SG-ACC

ziyaret

visit

et-ti

do-PAST

Ahmet anne-sin-i ziyaret et-ti

Ahmet mother-3SG-ACC visit do-PAST

'Ahmet visited his mother.'


     [TP [VP et][T -ti]]

Head-final Verb Phrase

 
Turkish VP structure

In neutral prosody, Turkish verb phrases are primarily head-final, as the verb comes after its complement. Variation in object-verb ordering is not strictly rigid. However, constructions where the verb precedes the object are less common.[25]

Çocuk-lar

child-PL

çikolata

chocolate

sever

like

Çocuk-lar çikolata sever

child-PL chocolate like

'Children like chocolate.'


     [VP [DP çikolata][V sever]]

Head-final Determiner Phrase

 
Turkish DP structure

In Turkish, definite determiners may be marked with a case marker suffix on the noun, such as when the noun is the direct object of a verb. They may also exist as free morphemes that attach to a head-initial determiner phrase, such as when the determiner is a demonstrative. Like other case markers in Turkish, when the morpheme carrying the demonstrative meaning is a case marker, they attach at the end of the word. As such, the head of the phrase, in this case the determiner, follows its complement like in the example below:[26]

Dün

yesteray

çok

very

garip

strange

kitap-lar-ı

book-PL-ACC

oku-du-m

read-PAST-1SG

Dün çok garip kitap-lar-ı oku-du-m

yesteray very strange book-PL-ACC read-PAST-1SG

'Yesterday I read the very strange books.'


     [DP [NP kitap-lar][D -ı]]

Head-final Postpositional Phrase

 
Turkish PP Structure

Turkish adpositions are postpositions that can affix as a case marker at the end of a word. They can also be a separate word that attaches to the head-final postpositional phrase, as is the case in the example below:[25]

Bu

This

kitab-ı

book-ACC

Ahmet

Ahmet

için

for

al-dı-m

buy-PAST-1SG

Bu kitab-ı Ahmet için al-dı-m

This book-ACC Ahmet for buy-PAST-1SG

'I bought this book for Ahmet.'


     [PP [DP Ahmet][P için]]

Word order variation in matrix clauses

Turkish employs a case marking system that allows some constituents in Turkish clauses to participate in permutations of its canonical SOV word order, thereby in some ways exhibiting a 'free' word order. Specifically, constituents of an independent clause can be moved around and constituents of phrasal categories can occur outside of the projections they are elements of. As a result, it is possible for the major case-marked constituents of a clause in Turkish to appear in all possible orders in a sentence, such that SOV, SVO, OSV, OVS, VSO, and VOS word orders are acceptable.[27]

This free word order allows for the verbal phrase to occur in any position in an independent clause, unlike other head-final languages (such as Japanese and Korean, in which any variation in word order must occur in the preverbal domain and the verb remains at the end of the clause (see § Japonic: Japanese, above)). Because of this relatively high degree of variation in word order in Turkish, its status as a head-final language is generally considered to be less strict and not absolute like Japanese or Korean, since while embedded clauses must remain verb-final, matrix clauses can show variability in word order.[27]

In the canonical word order of Turkish, as is typical in a head-final language, subjects come at the beginning of the sentence, then objects, with verbs coming in last:

 
Canonical SOV word order in Turkish

1. Subject-Object-Verb (SOV, canonical word order)

Yazar

author

makale-yi

article-ACC

bitir-di

finish-PAST

Yazar makale-yi bitir-di

author article-ACC finish-PAST

'The author finished the article.'

However, several variations on this order can occur on matrix clauses, such that the subject, object, and verb can occupy all different positions within a sentence. Because Turkish uses a case-marking system to denote how each word functions in a sentence in relation to the rest, case-marked elements can be moved around without a loss in meaning. These variations, also called permutations,[28][27] can change the discourse focus of the constituents in the sentence:

2. Object-Subject-Verb (OSV)

Makale-yi

article-ACC

yazar

author

bitir-di

finish-PAST

Makale-yi yazar bitir-di

article-ACC author finish-PAST

'The author finished the article.'

In this variation, the object moves to the beginning of the sentence, the subject follows, and the verb remains in final position.

3. Object-Verb-Subject (OVS)

Makale-yi

article-ACC

bitir-di

finish-PAST

yazar

author

Makale-yi bitir-di yazar

article-ACC finish-PAST author

'The author finished the article.'

In this variation, the subject moves to end of the sentence. This is an example of how verbs in Turkish can move to other positions in the clause, even though other head-final languages, such as Japanese and Korean, typically see verbs coming only at the end of the sentence.

4. Subject-Verb-Object (SVO)

Yazar

author

bitir-di

finish-PAST

makale-yi

article-ACC

Yazar bitir-di makale-yi

author finish-PAST article-ACC

'The author finished the article.'

In this variation, the object moves to the end of the sentence and the verb phrase now directly precedes the subject, which remains at the beginning of the sentence. This word order is akin to English word order.

5. Verb-Subject-Object (VSO)

Bitir-di

finish-PAST

yazar

author

makale-yi

article-ACC

Bitir-di yazar makale-yi

finish-PAST author article-ACC

'The author finished the article.'

In this variation, the verb phrase moves from the end of the sentence to the beginning of the sentence.

6. Verb-Object-Subject (VOS)

Bitir-di

finish-PAST

makale-yi

article-ACC

yazar

author

Bitir-di makale-yi yazar

finish-PAST article-ACC author

'The author finished the article.'

In this variation, the verb phrase moves to the beginning of the sentence, the object moves so that it directly following the verb, and the subject is at the end of the sentence.

Mixed word-order languages

Indo-European: German

German, while being predominantly head-initial, is less conclusively so than in the case of English. German also features certain head-final structures. For example, in a nonfinite verb phrase the verb is final. In a finite verb phrase (or tense/aspect phrase) the verb (tense/aspect) is initial, although it may move to final position in a subordinate clause. In the following example,[29] the non-finite verb phrase es finden is head-final, whereas in the tensed main clause ich werde es finden (headed by the auxiliary verb werde indicating future tense), the finite auxiliary precedes its complement (as an instance of a verb-second construction; in the example below, this V2-position is called "T").

 
German VP structure

Ich

I

werde

will

es

it

finden

find

Ich werde es finden

I will it find

"I will find it."

[TP [DP Ich] [T werde] [VP [DP es] [V finden]]]

Noun phrases containing complements are head-initial; in this example[30] the complement, the CP der den Befehl überbrachte, follows the head noun Boten.

Man

one

beschimpfte

insulted

den

the

Boten,

messenger

der

who

den

the

Befehl

command

überbrachte

delivered

Man beschimpfte den Boten, der den Befehl überbrachte

one insulted the messenger who the command delivered

"The messenger, who delivered the command, was insulted."

[NP [N Boten] [CP der den Befehl überbrachte]]

Adjective phrases may be head-final or head-initial. In the next example the adjective (stolze) follows its complement (auf seine Kinder).[31]

 
German head-final AP Structure

der

the

auf

of

seine

his

Kinder

children

stolze

proud

Vater

father

der auf seine Kinder stolze Vater

the of his children proud father

"the father (who is) proud of his children"

[AP [PP auf seine Kinder] [A stolze]]

However, when essentially the same adjective phrase is used predicatively rather than attributively, it can also be head-initial:[32]

 
German head-initial AP Structure

weil

since

er

he

stolz

proud

auf

of

seine

his

Kinder

children

ist

is

weil er stolz auf seine Kinder ist

since he proud of his children is

"since he is proud of his children"

[AP [A stolz] [PP auf seine Kinder]]

Most adpositional phrases are head-initial (as German has mostly prepositions rather than postpositions), as in the following example, where auf comes before its complement den Tisch:[33]

 
German head-initial PP structure

Peter

Peter

legt

puts

das

the

Buch

book

auf

on

den

the.ACC

Tisch

table

Peter legt das Buch auf den Tisch

Peter puts the book on the.ACC table

"Peter puts the book on the table."

[PP [P auf] [DP den Tisch]]

German also has some postpositions, however (such as gegenüber "opposite"), and so adpositional phrases can also sometimes be head-final. Another example is provided by the analysis of the following sentence:[34]

Die

the

Schnecke

snail

kroch

crept

das

the

Dach

roof

hinauf

up

Die Schnecke kroch das Dach hinauf

the snail crept the roof up

"The snail crept up the roof"

[PP [DP das Dach] [P hinauf]]

Like in English, determiner phrases and complementizer phrases in German are head-initial. The next example is of a determiner phrase, headed by the article der:[35]

 
German CP Structure

der

the

Mann

man

der Mann

the man

"the man"

[DP [D der] [NP Mann]]

In the following example, the complementizer dass precedes the tense phrase which serves as its complement:[36]

dass

that

Lisa

Lisa

eine

a

Blume

flower

gepflanzt

planted

hat

has

dass Lisa eine Blume gepflanzt hat

that Lisa a flower planted has

"that Lisa planted a flower"

[CP [C dass] [TP Lisa eine Blume gepflanzt hat]]

Sino-Tibetan: Chinese

Standard Chinese (whose syntax is typical of Chinese varieties generally) features a mixture of head-final and head-initial structures. Noun phrases are head-final. Modifiers virtually always precede the noun they modify. For examples of this involving relative clauses, see Relative clause § Mandarin.

In the case of strict head/complement ordering, however, Chinese appears to be head-initial. Verbs normally precede their objects. Both prepositions and postpositions are reported, but the postpositions can be analyzed as a type of noun (the prepositions are often called coverbs). For more details and examples of the relevant structures, see Chinese grammar. For a head-direction analysis of Chinese aspect phrases, see the theoretical section below.

Niger-Congo: Gbe

In Gbe, a mixture of head-initial and head-final structures is found. For example, a verb may appear after or before its complement, which means that both head-initial and head-final verb phrases occur.[37] In the first example the verb for "use" appears after its complement:

Kɔ̀jó

Kojo

IMPERF

àmí

oil

lɔ́

DET

zân

use

Kɔ̀jó tó àmí lɔ́ zân

Kojo IMPERF oil DET use

"Kojo is using the oil."

[VP [DP àmí lɔ́] [V zân]]

In the second example the verb precedes the complement:

Kɔ̀jó

Kojo

nɔ̀

HAB

zán

use-PERF

àmí

oil

lɔ́

DET

Kɔ̀jó nɔ̀ zán àmí lɔ́

Kojo HAB use-PERF oil DET

"Kojo habitually used the oil/Kojo habitually uses the oil."

[VP [V zán] [DP àmí lɔ́]]

It has been debated whether the first example is due to object movement to the left side of the verb [38] or whether the lexical entry of the verb simply allows head-initial and head-final structures.[39]

Tense phrases and aspect phrases are head-initial since aspect markers (such as and nɔ̀ above) and tense markers (such as the future marker in the following example, but that does not apply to tense markers shown by verb inflection) come before the verb phrase.[40]

dàwé

man

lɔ̀

DET

FUT

xɔ̀

buy

kɛ̀kɛ́

bicycle

dàwé lɔ̀ ná xɔ̀ kɛ̀kɛ́

man DET FUT buy bicycle

"The man will buy a bicycle."

[TP [T ] [VP xɔ̀ kɛ̀kɛ́]]

Gbe noun phrases are typically head-final, as in this example:[41]

 
Gbe NP structure

Kɔ̀kú

Koku

sín

CASE

ɖìdè

sketch

lɛ̀

PL

Kɔ̀kú sín ɖìdè lɛ̀

Koku CASE sketch PL

"sketches of Koku"

[NP [KP Kɔ̀kú sín] [N ɖìdè]]

In the following example of an adjective phrase, Gbe follows a head-initial pattern, as the head precedes the intensifier tàùú.[42]

 
Gbe AP structure

àǔn

dog

black

tàùú

INT

àǔn yù tàùú

dog black INT

"really black dogs"

[AP [A ] [Int tàùú]]

Gbe adpositional phrases are head-initial, with prepositions preceding their complement:[43]

Kòfi

Kofi

take-PERF

kwɛ́

money

xlán

to

Àsíbá

Asiba

Kòfi zé kwɛ́ xlán Àsíbá

Kofi take-PERF money to Asiba

"Kofi sent money to Asiba."

[PP [P xlán] [DP Àsíbá]]

Determiner phrases, however, are head-final:[44]

 
Gbe CP structure

Asíbá

Asiba

xɔ̀

buy-PERF

àvɔ̀

cloth

àmàmú

green

màtàn-màtàn

odd

ɖé

DEF

Asíbá xɔ̀ àvɔ̀ àmàmú màtàn-màtàn ɖé

Asiba buy-PERF cloth green odd DEF

"Asiba bought a specific ugly green cloth"

[DP [NP àvɔ̀ àmàmú màtàn-màtàn] [D ɖé]]

Complementizer phrases are head-initial:[45]

ɖé

that

Dòsà

Dosa

gbá

build-PERF

xwé

house

ɔ̀

DEF

ɔ̀

DET

ɖé Dòsà gbá xwé ɔ̀ ɔ̀

that Dosa build-PERF house DEF DET

"that Dosa built the house"

[CP [C ɖé] [TP Dòsà gb xwé ɔ̀ ɔ̀]]

Theoretical views

Tesnière: dependency grammar

The idea that syntactic structures reduce to binary relations was introduced by Lucien Tesnière in 1959 within the framework of dependency theory, which was further developed in the 1960s. Tesnière distinguished two structures that differ in the placement of the structurally governing element (head):[46] centripetal structures, in which heads precede their dependents, and centrifugal structures, in which heads follow their dependents. Dependents here may include complements, adjuncts, and specifiers.

Greenberg: typology

Joseph Greenberg, who worked in the field of language typology, put forward an implicational theory of word order, whereby:[47]

  • If a language has VO (verb-before-object) ordering, then it will also have prepositions (rather than postpositions), and genitives and adjectives will be placed after the noun they modify.
  • If a language has OV ordering, then it will also have postpositions, and genitives and adjectives will be placed before the noun they modify.

The first set of properties make heads come at the start of their phrases, while the second set make heads come at the end. However, it has been claimed that many languages (such as Basque) do not fulfill the above conditions, and that Greenberg's theory fails to predict the exceptions.[48]

Lehmann: Fundamental Principle of Placement

Winfred P. Lehmann, expanding upon Greenberg's theory, proposed a Fundamental Principle of Placement (FPP) in 1973. The FPP states that the order of object and verb relative to each other in a language determines other features of that language's typology, beyond the features that Greenberg identified.

Features associated with head-directionality according to Lehmann (1973)[49]
Feature OV languages VO languages
Morphological typology Agglutinative Inflectional or analytic
Position of negation and interrogative markers After verb root Before verb root
Position of sentence function markers End of sentence Beginning of sentence
Affixation Strictly suffixing Suffixing or prefixing
Relative and reflexive pronouns Absent Present
Syllable codas Open syllables preferred Closed syllables preferred
Vowel harmony directionality Left-to-right Right-to-left
Accent type Pitch accent Stress accent

Lehmann also believed that the subject is not a primary element of a sentence, and that the traditional six-order typology of languages should be reduced to just two, VO and OV, based on head-directionality alone. Thus, for example, SVO and VSO would be considered the same type in Lehmann’s classification system.

Chomsky: principles and parameters

Noam Chomsky's Principles and Parameters theory in the 1980s[50] introduced the idea that a small number of innate principles are common to every human language (e.g. phrases are oriented around heads), and that these general principles are subject to parametric variation (e.g. the order of heads and other phrasal components may differ). In this theory, the dependency relation between heads, complements, specifiers, and adjuncts is regulated by X-bar theory, proposed by Jackendoff[51] in the 1970s. The complement is sister to the head, and they can be ordered in one of two ways. A head-complement order is called a head-initial structure, while a complement-head order is called a head-final structure. These are special cases of Tesnière's centripetal and centrifugal structures, since here only complements are considered, whereas Tesnière considered all types of dependents.

In the principles and parameters theory, a head-directionality parameter is proposed as a way of classifying languages. A language which has head-initial structures is considered to be a head-initial language, and one which has head-final structures is considered to be a head-final language. It is found, however, that very few, if any, languages are entirely one direction or the other. Linguists have come up with a number of theories to explain the inconsistencies, sometimes positing a more consistent underlying order, with the phenomenon of phrasal movement being used to explain the surface deviations.

Kayne: antisymmetry

According to the Antisymmetry theory proposed by Richard S. Kayne, there is no head-directionality parameter as such: it is claimed that at an underlying level, all languages are head-initial. In fact, it is argued that all languages have the underlying order Specifier-Head-Complement. Deviations from this order are accounted for by different syntactic movements applied by languages. Kayne argues that a theory that allows both directionalities would imply an absence of asymmetries between languages, whereas in fact languages fail to be symmetrical in many respects. Kayne argues using the concept of a probe-goal search (based on the ideas of the Minimalist program), whereby a head acts as a probe and looks for a goal, namely its complement. Kayne proposes that the direction of the probe-goal search must share the direction of language parsing and production.[52] Parsing and production proceed in a left-to-right direction: the beginning of sentence is heard or spoken first, and the end of the sentence is heard or spoken last. This implies (according to the theory) an ordering whereby probe comes before goal, i.e. head precedes complement.

Some linguists have rejected the conclusions of the Antisymmetry approach. Some have pointed out that in predominantly head-final languages such as Japanese and Basque, the change from an underlying head-initial form to a largely head-final surface form would involve complex and massive leftward movement, which is not in accordance with the ideal of grammatical simplicity.[48] Some take a "surface true" viewpoint: that analysis of head direction must take place at the level of surface derivations, or even the Phonetic Form (PF), i.e. the order in which sentences are pronounced in natural speech. This rejects the idea of an underlying ordering which is then subject to movement, as posited in Antisymmetry and in certain other approaches. It has been argued that a head parameter must only reside at PF, as it is unmaintainable in its original form as a structural parameter.[53]

Some linguists have provided evidence which may be taken to support Kayne's scheme, such as Lin,[54] who considered Standard Chinese sentences with the sentence-final particle le. Certain restrictions on movement from within verb phrases preceding such a particle are found (if various other assumptions from the literature are accepted) to be consistent with the idea that the verb phrase has moved from its underlying position after its head (the particle le here being taken as the head of an aspect phrase). However, Takita (2009) observes that similar restrictions do not apply in Japanese, in spite of its surface head-final character, concluding that if Lin's assumptions are correct, then Japanese must be considered to be a true head-final language, contrary to the main tenet of Antisymmetry.[55] More details about these arguments can be found in the Antisymmetry article.

Gradient classification

Some scholars, such as Tesnière, argue that there are no absolute head-initial or head-final languages. According to this approach, it is true that some languages have more head-initial or head-final elements than other languages do, but almost any language contains both head-initial and head-final elements. Therefore, rather than being classifiable into fixed categories, languages can be arranged on a continuum with head-initial and head-final as the extremes, based on the frequency distribution of their dependency directions. This view was supported in a study by Haitao Liu (2010), who investigated 20 languages using a dependency treebank-based method.[56] For instance, Japanese is close to the head-final end of the continuum, while English and German, which have mixed head-initial and head-final dependencies, are plotted in relatively intermediate positions on the continuum.

Polinsky (2012) identified the following five head-directionality sub-types:

She identified a strong correlation between the head-directionality type of a language and the ratio of verbs to nouns in the lexical inventory. Languages with a scarcity of simple verbs tend to be rigidly head-final, as in the case of Japanese, whereas verb-rich languages tend to be head-initial languages.[57]

See also

Notes

  1. ^ a b Contemporary Linguistic Parameters : Contemporary Studies in Linguistics. Bloomsbury Academic. 2015. doi:10.5040/9781474219549.ch-004. ISBN 978-1-4725-3393-7.
  2. ^ Dryer 2009.
  3. ^ Polinsky, Maria; Magyar, Lilla (March 2020). "Headedness and the Lexicon: The Case of Verb-to-Noun Ratios". Languages. 5 (1): 5. doi:10.3390/languages5010009.
  4. ^ Szabolcsi 1983.
  5. ^ Van Langendonck 1994.
  6. ^ a b Fukui 1994, p. 4.
  7. ^ Smith 1964, p. 6.
  8. ^ Sadler & Arnold 1994, pp. 28–34.
  9. ^ Sadler & Arnold 1994, pp. 28.
  10. ^ Gillion 1992, p. 15.
  11. ^ a b Takahashi 2002, p. 2.
  12. ^ a b Sells 1995, p. 4.
  13. ^ Lin 2003, p. 2.
  14. ^ Sportiche, Koopman & Stabler 2014.
  15. ^ Graf, T; Paperno, D; Szabolcsi, A; Tellings, J (2012). Theories of Everything. In Honour of Ed Keenan (PDF). UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics. pp. 348–359.
  16. ^ a b c d e f g Ansari, Lalitia; Suhardiianto, Totok (November 2018). "Where is the Head Positioned in Indonesian Language?: A Corpus Study of Head Directionality from a Dependency Perspective". 2018 International Conference on Asian Language Processing (IALP). Bandung, Indonesia: IEEE: 171–177. doi:10.1109/IALP.2018.8629214. ISBN 978-1-7281-1175-9. S2CID 59554177.
  17. ^ a b Sneddon, James N (15 April 2010). Indonesian: A Comprehensive Grammar. London: Routledge. pp. 131–147. doi:10.4324/9780203720882. ISBN 9780203720882. Retrieved 10 December 2021.
  18. ^ Naoki 1993, pp. 15–16.
  19. ^ Watanabe 2011, pp. 9–10.
  20. ^ Vinka 2009, p. 4.
  21. ^ Arik, Engin (October 2019). "An Experimental Approach to Basic Word Order in Turkish Intransitives". Psychology of Language and Communication. 20 (1): 73–91. doi:10.1515/plc-2016-0004. S2CID 152036877.
  22. ^ van Schaaik, Gerjan (September 2020). The Oxford Turkish Grammar. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780198851509.
  23. ^ van Schaaik, Gerjan (September 2020). The Oxford Turkish Grammar. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780198851509.
  24. ^ Kornfilt, Jaklin (2013). Turkish. London: Routledge. pp. 1–211. ISBN 9781315823652.
  25. ^ a b Göksel, Aslı; Kerslake, Celia (2011). Turkish an Essential Grammar. Routledge. p. 368. ISBN 9780415462693.
  26. ^ van Schaaik, Gerjan (September 2020). The Oxford Turkish Grammar. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780198851509.
  27. ^ a b c A. Sumru Özsoy, ed. (2019). Word Order in Turkish. Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing AG. p. 313. ISBN 9783030113841.
  28. ^ Taylan, Eser Erguvanlı (1984). The Function of Word Order in Turkish Grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press. p. 179. ISBN 0520099559.
  29. ^ Dopke 1998, p. 6.
  30. ^ Berthold 2005, p. 6.
  31. ^ Hinterhölzl 2010, p. 4.
  32. ^ Hinterhölzl 2010, p. 5.
  33. ^ Van Riemsdijk 2007, p. 3.
  34. ^ Van Riemsdijk 2007, p. 1.
  35. ^ Bianchi 1999, p. 251.
  36. ^ Kathol 2001, p. 1.
  37. ^ Aboh 2001, pp. 1–2.
  38. ^ Aboh 2001, p. 2.
  39. ^ Kinyalolo 1992, pp. 1–16.
  40. ^ Aboh 2001, p. 34.
  41. ^ Brousseau & Lumsden 1992, p. 4.
  42. ^ Aboh 2004.
  43. ^ Aboh 2001, p. 117.
  44. ^ Aboh 2001, p. 100.
  45. ^ Aboh 2001, p. 348.
  46. ^ Graffi 2001, pp. 197–198.
  47. ^ Elordieta 2014, p. 2.
  48. ^ a b Elordieta 2014, p. 5.
  49. ^ Lehmann, Winfred P. (March 1973). "A Structural Principle of Language and Its Implications". Language. Linguistic Society of America. 49 (1): 47–66. doi:10.2307/412102. JSTOR 412102.
  50. ^ Chomsky 1981.
  51. ^ Jackendoff 1977.
  52. ^ Kayne 2011, p. 12.
  53. ^ Richards 2008, p. 283.
  54. ^ Lin 2006.
  55. ^ Takita 2009, pp. 59.
  56. ^ Liu 2010, pp. 1567–1578.
  57. ^ Polinsky 2012, pp. 348–359.

Bibliography

  • Aboh, Enoch (2001). "Object shift and verb movement in Gbe" (PDF). Generative Grammar in Geneva. 2: 1–13. Retrieved 29 October 2014.
  • Aboh, Enoch (2004). The morphosyntax of complement-head sequences: Clause structure and word order patterns in kwa. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Bianchi, Valentina (1999). Consequences of antisymmetry: Headed relative clauses. Walter de Gruyter.
  • Broekhuis, H (2013). Syntax of Dutch: Adjectives and Adjective Phrases. Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
  • Brousseau, Anne-Marie; Lumsden, John S (1992). "Nominal structures in Fongbe". Journal of West African Languages. 22 (1): 1–22.
  • Chomsky, Noam. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Foris Publications.
  • Comrie, Bernard (2008). "Pronominal relative clauses in verb-object languages" (PDF). Language and Linguistics. 9 (4): 723–733.
  • Courtney, Ellen H. (2011). "Learning to produce Quechua relative clauses". Acquisition of Relative Clauses : Processing, typology and function. John Benjamins Publishing Company. pp. 141–172. ISBN 9789027283405. Retrieved 13 November 2014.
  • Crysmann, Berthold (2005). "Relative Clause Extraposition in German: an efficient and portable implementation". Research on Language and Computation. 3 (1): 61–82. doi:10.1007/s11168-005-1287-z. S2CID 17250437.
  • Dopke, Susanne (1998). "Competing language structures: the acquisition of verb placement by bilingual German-English children". Journal of Child Language. 22 (3): 555–584. doi:10.1017/S0305000998003584. PMID 10095325. S2CID 11270532.
  • Dryer, Matthew S. (2009). The Branching Direction Theory of Word Order Correlations Revisited. In S. Scalise, E. Magni, & A. Bisetto (Eds.), Universals of Language Today (pp. 185–207). Rotterdam, Netherlands: Springer.
  • Elordieta, Arantzazu (2014). Biberauer, T.; Sheehan, M. (eds.). On the relevance of the Head Parameter in a mixed OV language. Theoretical Approaches to Disharmonic Word Order. Oxford Scholarship Online. pp. 306–329. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199684359.003.0011. ISBN 9780199684359.
  • Fukui, Naoki (1993). "Parameters and optionality". Linguistic Inquiry. 24 (3): 399–420. JSTOR 4178821.
  • Gillion, Brendan (1992). "Towards a Common Semantics for English Count and Mass Nouns". Linguistics and Philosophy. 15 (6): 597–639. doi:10.1007/BF00628112. S2CID 50730562.
  • Graffi, Giorgio (2001). 200 Years of Syntax: A critical survey. John Benjamins Publishing.
  • Hinterhölzl, Roland (2010). "Collapsing the Head Final Filter and the Head Complement Parameter". Linguistics. 20: 35–66.
  • Hinterhölzl, Roland; Svetlana, Petrova (2009). Information structure and language change: New approaches to word order variation in Germanic. Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Huang, C-T. J. (1982). "Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar". PhD Dissertation, MIT.
  • Jackendoff, Ray. (1977). X Syntax: A Study of Phrase Structure. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Kathol, Andreas (2001). "Positional Effects in a Monostratal Grammar of German". Journal of Linguistics. 37 (1): 35–66. doi:10.1017/s0022226701008805.
  • Kayne, Richard S. (2003). "Antisymmetry and Japanese". English Linguistics. 20: 1–40. doi:10.9793/elsj1984.20.1.
  • Kayne, Richard S. (2011). "Why Are There No Directionality Parameters?" (PDF). Proceedings of the 28th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Somerville: Cascadilla Press. pp. 1–23. Retrieved 13 November 2014.
  • Kinyalolo, Kasangati (1992). "A note on word order in the progressive and prospective in Fon". Journal of West African Languages. 22 (1): 37–51. Retrieved 29 October 2014.
  • Kratzer, Angelika. "More structural analogies between pronouns and tenses". Proceedings of SALT. 8 (92–110).
  • Kroch, Anthony. "Morphosyntactic Variation". Proceedings of the Thirtieth Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society. 2: 180–210.
  • Lasnik, Howard (2010). "Government-binding/principles and parameters theory". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 1 (1): 40–50. doi:10.1002/wcs.35. PMID 26272837.
  • Lin, Chienjer (2003). "Aspect is result: Mandarin resultative constructions and aspect incorporation". Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Linguistics (WECOL).
  • Lin, Tzong-Hong J. "Complement-to-specifier movement in Mandarin Chinese". Ms., National Tsing Hua University.
  • Liu, Haitao (2010). "Dependency direction as a means of word-order typology: A method based on dependency treebanks". Lingua. 120 (6): 1567–1578. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2009.10.001.
  • Nolda, Andreas (2004). Topics Detached to the Left: On 'Left Dislocation', 'Hanging Topic', and Related Constructions in German (PDF). Berlin: ZAS Papers in Linguistics. pp. 423–448. Retrieved 13 November 2014.
  • Onozuka, Hiromi (2008). "On the Resultative Reading of the Imperfective Aspect in English". Australian Journal of Linguistics. 28 (1): 1–16. doi:10.1080/07268600701877465. S2CID 56964107.
  • Ozeki, Hiromi (2011). "The acquisition of relative clauses in Japanese". Acquisition of Relative Clauses : Processing, typology and function. John Benjamins Publishing Company. pp. 173–196. ISBN 9789027283405. Retrieved 13 November 2014.
  • Ozeki, Hiromi (2011). "The acquisition of relative clauses in Japanese". Acquisition of Relative Clauses : Processing, typology and function. John Benjamins Publishing Company. pp. 173–196. ISBN 9789027283405. Retrieved 13 November 2014.
  • Polinsky, Maria (2012). "Headness, again" (PDF). UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics. 17: 348–359.
  • Richards, Marc D. (2008). "Desymmetrization: Parametric variation at the PF-Interface". The Canadian Journal of Linguistics. 53 (2–3): 275–300. doi:10.1353/cjl.0.0024. S2CID 170810562. ProQuest 85703588.
  • Sadler, Louisa; Arnold, Douglas (1994). "Prenominal adjectives and the phrase/lexical distinction". Journal of Linguistics. 30 (1): 187–226. doi:10.1017/s0022226700016224. JSTOR 4176260. S2CID 144681113.
  • Sells, Peter (1995). "Korean and Japanese morphology from a lexical perspective". Linguistic Inquiry. 26 (2): 277–325. JSTOR 4178898.
  • Shirai, Yasuhiro; Kurono, Atsuko (1998). "The acquisition of tense-aspect marking in Japanese as a second language" (PDF). Language Learning. 48 (2): 245–279. doi:10.1111/1467-9922.00041. Retrieved 26 October 2014.
  • Dominique Sportiche; Hilda Koopman; Edward Stabler (2014). An Introduction to Syntactic Analysis and Theory. Chichester: Wiley Blackwell.
  • Szabolcsi, Anna (1983). "The possessor that ran away from home". The Linguistic Review. 3 (1): 89–102. doi:10.1515/tlir.1983.3.1.89. S2CID 170588159.
  • Takahashi, Daiko (2002). "Determiner raising and scope shift". Linguistic Inquiry. 33 (4): 575–615. doi:10.1162/002438902762731772. S2CID 57564972.
  • Takita, Kensuke (2009). "If Chinese is head-initial, Japanese cannot be". Journal of East Asian Linguistics. 18 (1): 41–61. doi:10.1007/s10831-009-9038-z. JSTOR 40345242. S2CID 121661611.
  • Tokizaki, Hisao (2011). "The nature of linear information in the morphosyntax-PF interface". English Linguistics. 28 (2): 227–257. doi:10.9793/elsj.28.2_227.
  • Van Langendonck, Willy (1994). "Determiners as heads?". Cognitive Linguistics. 5 (1): 243–260. doi:10.1515/cogl.1994.5.3.243. S2CID 201654317.
  • Van Riemsdijk, Henk (2007). "Case in spatial adpositional phrases: The dative-accusative alternation in German". Pitar Mos: A Building with a View. Festschrift for Alexandra Cornilescu: 1–23.
  • Vinka, Mikael (2009). "The syntax of three Japanese postpositions". Lund Working Papers in Linguistics. 40: 229–250.
  • Watanabe, Akira (2011). "Adjectival inflection and the position of measure phrases". Linguistic Inquiry. 42 (3): 490–507. doi:10.1162/ling_a_00055. S2CID 57565727.

head, directionality, parameter, main, article, principles, parameters, linguistics, head, directionality, proposed, parameter, that, classifies, languages, according, whether, they, head, initial, head, phrase, precedes, complements, head, final, head, follow. Main article Principles and parameters In linguistics head directionality is a proposed parameter that classifies languages according to whether they are head initial the head of a phrase precedes its complements or head final the head follows its complements The head is the element that determines the category of a phrase for example in a verb phrase the head is a verb Therefore head initial would be VO languages and head final would be OV languages 1 Some languages are consistently head initial or head final at all phrasal levels English is considered to be strongly head initial verbs precede their objects for example while Japanese is an example of a language that is consistently head final In certain other languages such as German and Gbe examples of both types of head direction occur Various theories have been proposed to explain such variation Head directionality is connected with the type of branching that predominates in a language head initial structures are right branching while head final structures are left branching 2 On the basis of these criteria languages can be divided into head final rigid and non rigid and head initial types The identification of headedness is based on the following 3 the order of subject object and verb the relationship between the order of the object and verb the order of an adposition and its complement the order of relative clause and head noun Contents 1 Types of phrase 2 Head initial languages 2 1 English 2 2 Indonesian 2 2 1 Dependency perspective 2 2 2 Word order perspective 3 Head final languages 3 1 Japonic Japanese 3 2 Turkic Turkish 3 2 1 Head final Tense Phrase 3 2 2 Head final Verb Phrase 3 2 3 Head final Determiner Phrase 3 2 4 Head final Postpositional Phrase 3 2 5 Word order variation in matrix clauses 4 Mixed word order languages 4 1 Indo European German 4 2 Sino Tibetan Chinese 4 3 Niger Congo Gbe 5 Theoretical views 5 1 Tesniere dependency grammar 5 2 Greenberg typology 5 3 Lehmann Fundamental Principle of Placement 5 4 Chomsky principles and parameters 5 5 Kayne antisymmetry 5 6 Gradient classification 6 See also 7 Notes 8 BibliographyTypes of phrase EditIn some cases particularly with noun and adjective phrases it is not always clear which dependents are to be classed as complements and which as adjuncts Although in principle the head directionality parameter concerns the order of heads and complements only considerations of head initiality and head finality sometimes take account of the position of the head in the phrase as a whole including adjuncts The structure of the various types of phrase is analyzed below in relation to specific languages with a focus on the ordering of head and complement In some cases such as English and Japanese this ordering is found to be the same in practically all types of phrase whereas in others such as German and Gbe the pattern is less consistent Different theoretical explanations of these inconsistencies are discussed later in the article There are various types of phrase in which the ordering of head and complement s may be considered when attempting to determine the head directionality of a language including Verb Phrase the head of verb phrase VP is a verb and the complement s are most commonly objects of various types The ordering here is related to one of the chief questions in the word order typology of languages namely the normal order of subject verb and object within a clause languages are classed on this basis as SVO SOV VSO etc head initial and head final constructions Noun Phrase the head of a noun phrase NP is a noun various kinds of complementizer phrases CPs and adpositional phrases PPs can be complements head initial and head final constructions Adjective Phrase the head of an adjective phrase AP is an adjective which can take as a complement for example an adverbial phrase or adpositional phrase PP head initial and head final constructions Adpositional Phrase the head of an adpositional phrase PP is an apposition Such phrases are called prepositional phrases if they are head initial i e headed by a preposition or postpositional phrases if they are head final i e headed by a postposition For more on these see Preposition and postposition The complement is a determiner phrase or noun phrase depending on analytical scheme followed head initial and head final constructions Determiner Phrase the head of a determiner phrase DP is a determiner DPs were proposed under generative syntax 4 not all theories of syntax agree that they exist 5 Complementizer Phrase the head of a complementizer phrase CP is a complementizer like that in English In some cases the C head is covert not overtly present The complement of C is generally agreed to be a tense phrase TP head initial and head final constructions Tense Phrase the head of a tense phrase TP is tense these are phrases in which the head is an abstract category representing tense the complement is a verb phrase Aspect Phrase the head of an aspect phrase AspP is aspect these are phrases in which the head is an abstract syntactic category representing aspect In more traditional analysis the entire phrase including any elements denoting tense or aspect is considered to be simply a verb phrase Head initial languages EditEnglish Edit English is a strongly citation needed head initial language In a typical verb phrase for example the verb precedes its complements as in the following example 6 English VP structure eat an apple VP V eat DP an apple The head of the phrase the verb eat precedes its complement the determiner phrase an apple Switching the order to VP DP an apple V eat would be ungrammatical Nouns also tend to precede any complements as in the following example where the relative clause or complementizer phrase that follows the noun may be considered to be a complement 7 English NP structure He married a girl who is from Texas NP N girl CP who is from Texas Nouns do not necessarily begin their phrase they may be preceded by attributive adjectives but these are regarded as adjuncts rather than complements For preceding determiners see below Adjectives themselves may be preceded by adjuncts namely adverbs as in extremely happy 8 However when an adjective phrase contains a true complement such as a prepositional phrase the head adjective precedes it 9 a person happy about her work AP A happy PP about her work English adpositional phrases are also head initial that is English has prepositions rather than postpositions 10 English PP structure a majority of eligible voters PP P of DP eligible voters On the determiner phrase DP view where a determiner is taken to be the head of its phrase rather than the associated noun English can be seen to be head initial in this type of phrase too In the following example 11 the head is taken to be the determiner any and the complement is the noun phrase book English DP structure any book DP D any NP book English also has head initial complementizer phrases as in this example 12 where the complementizer that precedes its complement the tense phrase Mary did not swim We saw that Mary did not swim CP C that TP Mary did not swim Grammatical words marking tense and aspect generally precede the semantic verb This indicates that if finite verb phrases are analyzed as tense phrases or aspect phrases these are again head initial in English In the example above did is considered a past tense marker and precedes its complement the verb phrase not swim In the following has is a perfect aspect marker 13 again it appears before the verb phrase which is its complement English AspP Structure John has arrived AspP Asp has VP arrived The following example shows a sequence of nested phrases in which each head precedes its complement 14 In the complementizer phrase CP in a the complementizer C precedes its tense phrase TP complement In the tense phrase in b the tense marking element T precedes its verb phrase VP complement The subject of the tense phrase the girl is a specifier which does not need to be considered when analyzing the ordering of head and complement In the verb phrase in c the verb V precedes its two complements namely the determiner phrase DP the book and the prepositional phrase PP on the table In d where a picture is analyzed as a determiner phrase the determiner D a precedes its noun phrase NP complement while in e the preposition P on precedes its DP complement your desk You know that the girl will put a picture on your desk a CP CP C that TP the girl will put a picture on your desk b TP TP T will VP put a picture on your desk c VP VP V put DP a picture PP on your desk d DP DP D a NP picture e PP PP P on DP your desk Indonesian Edit Indonesian is an example of an SVO head initial language 1 15 The characteristic of it being a head initial language can be examined through a dependency perspective or through a word order perspective Both approaches lead to the conclusion that Indonesian is a head initial language Dependency perspective Edit Governor Dependent relationship in SVO Head initial languages When examining Indonesian through a dependency perspective it is considered head initial as the governor of both constituents are positioned before the dependent 16 Placing the head before a dependent minimizes the overall dependency distance which is the distance between the two constituents 16 Minimizing dependency distance allows for less cognitive demand as a head final dependency requires the constituents in the dependent clause to be stored in working memory until the head is realized 16 Dependency distance between constituents in Indonesian In Indonesian the number of constituencies affects the dependency direction When there are 6 constituents which is a relatively short sentence there is a preference for head initial relation 16 However when there are 11 30 constituents there appears to be a balance of head initial and head final dependencies 16 Regardless Indonesian displays an overall head initial preference on all levels of dependency structure as it consistently attempts to position the head as early on in the sentence even though it produces a longer dependency distance rather than placing the head after its dependents 16 Furthermore Indonesian has an overall preference towards head initial when comparing head initial and head final relation on all levels of constituent length for both spoken and written data 16 Word order perspective Edit The subject of the sentence followed by the verb representing SVO order 17 The following examples demonstrate head initial directionality in Indonesian note that perdana menteri prime minister is unusually being head final Indonesian head initial word order in VP PerdanaPrimementeriministersudahalreadypulanghomePerdana menteri sudah pulangPrime minister already home The Prime minister has returned home CP DP Perdana menteri VP sudah pulang Classifiers and partitives can function as the head nouns of noun phrases Below is an example of the internal structure of a noun phrase and its head initial word order BotolBottleiniDET thisretakcrackBotol ini retakBottle DET this crack This bottle is cracked CP DP botol ini VP retak Head initial word order is seen in the internal structure of the verb phrase in the following example where the V is in the head position of the verb phrase and thus appears before its complement Indonesian head initial word order in VP DokterDoctormemeriksachecksmataeyesayaPN myDokter memeriksa mata sayaDoctor checks eye PN my The doctor checked my eyes CP DPDokter VP V memeriksa DPmata says In Indonesian a noun can be followed by another modifying noun whose primary function is to provide more specific information about the preceding head noun such as indicating what the head noun is made of gender locative sense and what the head noun does etc However no other word is able to intervene between a head noun and its following modifying noun If a word follows the modifying noun then it provides reference to the head noun and not the modifying noun 17 Head noun N guru Modifying noun N bahasa guruteacherbahasalanguageguru bahasateacher language language teacher Head noun N guru Modifying noun N sekolah Determiner D itu guruteachersekolahschoolituDET thatguru sekolah ituteacher school DET that that schoolteacher Head noun Ntoko Modifying noun N buku tokoshopbukubooktoko bukushop book Bookshop Head noun Ntoko Modifying noun N buku Determiner phrase DPyang besar tokoshopbukubookyangDET abesarbigtoko buku yang besarshop book DET a biga big bookshop Head noun N sate Modifying noun N ayam satesatayayamchickensate ayamsatay chicken chicken satay Head final languages EditJaponic Japanese Edit Japanese is an example of a strongly head final language This can be seen in verb phrases and tense phrases the verb tabe in the example comes after its complement while the tense marker ru comes after the whole verb phrase which is its complement 6 Japanese VP structure リンゴをringo oapple ACC食べるtabe rueat NPASTリンゴを 食べるringo o tabe ruapple ACC eat NPAST eat an apple TP VP DP ringo o V tabe T ru Nouns also typically come after any complements as in the following example where the PP New York de no may be regarded as a complement 18 Japanese NP structure ジョンのJohn noJohn GEN昨日のkinoo noyesterday GENニューヨークでのNew York de noNew York in GEN講義koogilectureジョンの 昨日の ニューヨークでの 講義John no kinoo no New York de no koogiJohn GEN yesterday GEN New York in GEN lecture John s lecture in New York yesterday NP PP New York de no N koogi Adjectives also follow any complements they may have In this example the complement of quantity ni juu meetoru twenty meters precedes the head adjective takai tall 19 このKonothisビルはbiru wabuilding TOP20メートルni juu meetorutwo ten meter高いtakaitallこの ビルは 20メートル 高いKono biru wa ni juu meetoru takaithis building TOP two ten meter tall This building is twenty meters taller AP Q ni juu meetoru A takai Japanese uses postpositions rather than prepositions so its adpositional phrases are again head final 20 Japanese PP structure 僕がBoku gaI NOM高須村にTakasu mura niTakasu village in住んでいるsunde irulive PRES僕が 高須村に 住んでいるBoku ga Takasu mura ni sunde iruI NOM Takasu village in live PRES I live in Takasu village PP DP Takasu mura P ni Determiner phrases are head final as well 11 Japanese DP structure 誰darepersonもmoany誰 もdare moperson any anyone DP NP dare D mo A complementizer here to equivalent to English that comes after its complement here a tense phrase meaning Mary did not swim thus Japanese complementizer phrases are head final 12 メリーがMary gaMary NOM泳がなかったことoyog ana katta kotoswim NEG PAST thatメリーが 泳がなかったことMary ga oyog ana katta kotoMary NOM swim NEG PAST that that Mary did not swim CP TP Mary ga oyog ana katta C to Turkic Turkish Edit Turkish is an agglutinative head final and left branching language that uses a SOV word order 21 As such Turkish complements and adjuncts typically precede their head under neutral prosody and adpositions are postpositional Turkish employs a case marking system 22 which affixes to the right boundary of the word it is modifying As such all case markings in Turkish are suffixes For example the set of accusative case marking suffixes y i y i y u y u in Turkish indicate that it is the direct object of a verb Additionally while some kinds of definite determiners and postpositions in Turkish can be marked by case other types also exist as free morphemes 23 In the following examples Turkish case marker suffixes are analyzed as complements to the head Head final Tense Phrase Edit Turkish TP structure In Turkish tense is denoted by a case marking suffix on the verb 24 AhmetAhmetanne sin imother 3SG ACCziyaretvisitet tido PASTAhmet anne sin i ziyaret et tiAhmet mother 3SG ACC visit do PAST Ahmet visited his mother TP VP et T ti Head final Verb Phrase Edit Turkish VP structure In neutral prosody Turkish verb phrases are primarily head final as the verb comes after its complement Variation in object verb ordering is not strictly rigid However constructions where the verb precedes the object are less common 25 Cocuk larchild PLcikolatachocolateseverlikeCocuk lar cikolata severchild PL chocolate like Children like chocolate VP DP cikolata V sever Head final Determiner Phrase Edit Turkish DP structure In Turkish definite determiners may be marked with a case marker suffix on the noun such as when the noun is the direct object of a verb They may also exist as free morphemes that attach to a head initial determiner phrase such as when the determiner is a demonstrative Like other case markers in Turkish when the morpheme carrying the demonstrative meaning is a case marker they attach at the end of the word As such the head of the phrase in this case the determiner follows its complement like in the example below 26 Dunyesteraycokverygaripstrangekitap lar ibook PL ACCoku du mread PAST 1SGDun cok garip kitap lar i oku du myesteray very strange book PL ACC read PAST 1SG Yesterday I read the very strange books DP NP kitap lar D i Head final Postpositional Phrase Edit Turkish PP Structure Turkish adpositions are postpositions that can affix as a case marker at the end of a word They can also be a separate word that attaches to the head final postpositional phrase as is the case in the example below 25 BuThiskitab ibook ACCAhmetAhmeticinforal di mbuy PAST 1SGBu kitab i Ahmet icin al di mThis book ACC Ahmet for buy PAST 1SG I bought this book for Ahmet PP DP Ahmet P icin Word order variation in matrix clauses Edit Turkish employs a case marking system that allows some constituents in Turkish clauses to participate in permutations of its canonical SOV word order thereby in some ways exhibiting a free word order Specifically constituents of an independent clause can be moved around and constituents of phrasal categories can occur outside of the projections they are elements of As a result it is possible for the major case marked constituents of a clause in Turkish to appear in all possible orders in a sentence such that SOV SVO OSV OVS VSO and VOS word orders are acceptable 27 This free word order allows for the verbal phrase to occur in any position in an independent clause unlike other head final languages such as Japanese and Korean in which any variation in word order must occur in the preverbal domain and the verb remains at the end of the clause see Japonic Japanese above Because of this relatively high degree of variation in word order in Turkish its status as a head final language is generally considered to be less strict and not absolute like Japanese or Korean since while embedded clauses must remain verb final matrix clauses can show variability in word order 27 In the canonical word order of Turkish as is typical in a head final language subjects come at the beginning of the sentence then objects with verbs coming in last Canonical SOV word order in Turkish 1 Subject Object Verb SOV canonical word order Yazarauthormakale yiarticle ACCbitir difinish PASTYazar makale yi bitir diauthor article ACC finish PAST The author finished the article However several variations on this order can occur on matrix clauses such that the subject object and verb can occupy all different positions within a sentence Because Turkish uses a case marking system to denote how each word functions in a sentence in relation to the rest case marked elements can be moved around without a loss in meaning These variations also called permutations 28 27 can change the discourse focus of the constituents in the sentence 2 Object Subject Verb OSV Makale yiarticle ACCyazarauthorbitir difinish PASTMakale yi yazar bitir diarticle ACC author finish PAST The author finished the article In this variation the object moves to the beginning of the sentence the subject follows and the verb remains in final position 3 Object Verb Subject OVS Makale yiarticle ACCbitir difinish PASTyazarauthorMakale yi bitir di yazararticle ACC finish PAST author The author finished the article In this variation the subject moves to end of the sentence This is an example of how verbs in Turkish can move to other positions in the clause even though other head final languages such as Japanese and Korean typically see verbs coming only at the end of the sentence 4 Subject Verb Object SVO Yazarauthorbitir difinish PASTmakale yiarticle ACCYazar bitir di makale yiauthor finish PAST article ACC The author finished the article In this variation the object moves to the end of the sentence and the verb phrase now directly precedes the subject which remains at the beginning of the sentence This word order is akin to English word order 5 Verb Subject Object VSO Bitir difinish PASTyazarauthormakale yiarticle ACCBitir di yazar makale yifinish PAST author article ACC The author finished the article In this variation the verb phrase moves from the end of the sentence to the beginning of the sentence 6 Verb Object Subject VOS Bitir difinish PASTmakale yiarticle ACCyazarauthorBitir di makale yi yazarfinish PAST article ACC author The author finished the article In this variation the verb phrase moves to the beginning of the sentence the object moves so that it directly following the verb and the subject is at the end of the sentence Mixed word order languages EditIndo European German Edit This section may require cleanup to meet Wikipedia s quality standards The specific problem is See talk page section Mistakes Please help improve this section if you can January 2019 Learn how and when to remove this template message German while being predominantly head initial is less conclusively so than in the case of English German also features certain head final structures For example in a nonfinite verb phrase the verb is final In a finite verb phrase or tense aspect phrase the verb tense aspect is initial although it may move to final position in a subordinate clause In the following example 29 the non finite verb phrase es finden is head final whereas in the tensed main clause ich werde es finden headed by the auxiliary verb werde indicating future tense the finite auxiliary precedes its complement as an instance of a verb second construction in the example below this V2 position is called T German VP structure IchIwerdewillesitfindenfindIch werde es findenI will it find I will find it TP DP Ich T werde VP DP es V finden Noun phrases containing complements are head initial in this example 30 the complement the CP der den Befehl uberbrachte follows the head noun Boten ManonebeschimpfteinsulteddentheBoten messengerderwhodentheBefehlcommanduberbrachtedeliveredMan beschimpfte den Boten der den Befehl uberbrachteone insulted the messenger who the command delivered The messenger who delivered the command was insulted NP N Boten CP der den Befehl uberbrachte Adjective phrases may be head final or head initial In the next example the adjective stolze follows its complement auf seine Kinder 31 German head final AP Structure dertheaufofseinehisKinderchildrenstolzeproudVaterfatherder auf seine Kinder stolze Vaterthe of his children proud father the father who is proud of his children AP PP auf seine Kinder A stolze However when essentially the same adjective phrase is used predicatively rather than attributively it can also be head initial 32 German head initial AP Structure weilsinceerhestolzproudaufofseinehisKinderchildrenistisweil er stolz auf seine Kinder istsince he proud of his children is since he is proud of his children AP A stolz PP auf seine Kinder Most adpositional phrases are head initial as German has mostly prepositions rather than postpositions as in the following example where auf comes before its complement den Tisch 33 German head initial PP structure PeterPeterlegtputsdastheBuchbookaufondenthe ACCTischtablePeter legt das Buch auf den TischPeter puts the book on the ACC table Peter puts the book on the table PP P auf DP den Tisch German also has some postpositions however such as gegenuber opposite and so adpositional phrases can also sometimes be head final Another example is provided by the analysis of the following sentence 34 DietheSchneckesnailkrochcreptdastheDachroofhinaufupDie Schnecke kroch das Dach hinaufthe snail crept the roof up The snail crept up the roof PP DP das Dach P hinauf Like in English determiner phrases and complementizer phrases in German are head initial The next example is of a determiner phrase headed by the article der 35 German CP Structure dertheMannmander Mannthe man the man DP D der NP Mann In the following example the complementizer dass precedes the tense phrase which serves as its complement 36 dassthatLisaLisaeineaBlumeflowergepflanztplantedhathasdass Lisa eine Blume gepflanzt hatthat Lisa a flower planted has that Lisa planted a flower CP C dass TP Lisa eine Blume gepflanzt hat Sino Tibetan Chinese Edit Standard Chinese whose syntax is typical of Chinese varieties generally features a mixture of head final and head initial structures Noun phrases are head final Modifiers virtually always precede the noun they modify For examples of this involving relative clauses see Relative clause Mandarin In the case of strict head complement ordering however Chinese appears to be head initial Verbs normally precede their objects Both prepositions and postpositions are reported but the postpositions can be analyzed as a type of noun the prepositions are often called coverbs For more details and examples of the relevant structures see Chinese grammar For a head direction analysis of Chinese aspect phrases see the theoretical section below Niger Congo Gbe Edit In Gbe a mixture of head initial and head final structures is found For example a verb may appear after or before its complement which means that both head initial and head final verb phrases occur 37 In the first example the verb for use appears after its complement Kɔ joKojotoIMPERFamioillɔ DETzanuseKɔ jo to ami lɔ zanKojo IMPERF oil DET use Kojo is using the oil VP DP ami lɔ V zan In the second example the verb precedes the complement Kɔ joKojonɔ HABzanuse PERFamioillɔ DETKɔ jo nɔ zan ami lɔ Kojo HAB use PERF oil DET Kojo habitually used the oil Kojo habitually uses the oil VP V zan DP ami lɔ It has been debated whether the first example is due to object movement to the left side of the verb 38 or whether the lexical entry of the verb simply allows head initial and head final structures 39 Tense phrases and aspect phrases are head initial since aspect markers such as to and nɔ above and tense markers such as the future marker na in the following example but that does not apply to tense markers shown by verb inflection come before the verb phrase 40 dawemanlɔ DETnaFUTxɔ buykɛ kɛ bicycledawe lɔ na xɔ kɛ kɛ man DET FUT buy bicycle The man will buy a bicycle TP T na VP xɔ kɛ kɛ Gbe noun phrases are typically head final as in this example 41 Gbe NP structure Kɔ kuKokusinCASEɖidesketchlɛ PLKɔ ku sin ɖide lɛ Koku CASE sketch PL sketches of Koku NP KP Kɔ ku sin N ɖide In the following example of an adjective phrase Gbe follows a head initial pattern as the head yu precedes the intensifier tauu 42 Gbe AP structure aǔndogyublacktauuINTaǔn yu tauudog black INT really black dogs AP A yu Int tauu Gbe adpositional phrases are head initial with prepositions preceding their complement 43 KofiKofizetake PERFkwɛ moneyxlantoAsibaAsibaKofi ze kwɛ xlan AsibaKofi take PERF money to Asiba Kofi sent money to Asiba PP P xlan DP Asiba Determiner phrases however are head final 44 Gbe CP structure AsibaAsibaxɔ buy PERFavɔ clothamamugreenmatan matanoddɖeDEFAsiba xɔ avɔ amamu matan matan ɖeAsiba buy PERF cloth green odd DEF Asiba bought a specific ugly green cloth DP NP avɔ amamu matan matan D ɖe Complementizer phrases are head initial 45 ɖethatDosaDosagbabuild PERFxwehouseɔ DEFɔ DETɖe Dosa gba xwe ɔ ɔ that Dosa build PERF house DEF DET that Dosa built the house CP C ɖe TP Dosa gb xwe ɔ ɔ Theoretical views EditTesniere dependency grammar Edit The idea that syntactic structures reduce to binary relations was introduced by Lucien Tesniere in 1959 within the framework of dependency theory which was further developed in the 1960s Tesniere distinguished two structures that differ in the placement of the structurally governing element head 46 centripetal structures in which heads precede their dependents and centrifugal structures in which heads follow their dependents Dependents here may include complements adjuncts and specifiers Greenberg typology Edit Joseph Greenberg who worked in the field of language typology put forward an implicational theory of word order whereby 47 If a language has VO verb before object ordering then it will also have prepositions rather than postpositions and genitives and adjectives will be placed after the noun they modify If a language has OV ordering then it will also have postpositions and genitives and adjectives will be placed before the noun they modify The first set of properties make heads come at the start of their phrases while the second set make heads come at the end However it has been claimed that many languages such as Basque do not fulfill the above conditions and that Greenberg s theory fails to predict the exceptions 48 Lehmann Fundamental Principle of Placement Edit Winfred P Lehmann expanding upon Greenberg s theory proposed a Fundamental Principle of Placement FPP in 1973 The FPP states that the order of object and verb relative to each other in a language determines other features of that language s typology beyond the features that Greenberg identified Features associated with head directionality according to Lehmann 1973 49 Feature OV languages VO languagesMorphological typology Agglutinative Inflectional or analyticPosition of negation and interrogative markers After verb root Before verb rootPosition of sentence function markers End of sentence Beginning of sentenceAffixation Strictly suffixing Suffixing or prefixingRelative and reflexive pronouns Absent PresentSyllable codas Open syllables preferred Closed syllables preferredVowel harmony directionality Left to right Right to leftAccent type Pitch accent Stress accentLehmann also believed that the subject is not a primary element of a sentence and that the traditional six order typology of languages should be reduced to just two VO and OV based on head directionality alone Thus for example SVO and VSO would be considered the same type in Lehmann s classification system Chomsky principles and parameters Edit Noam Chomsky s Principles and Parameters theory in the 1980s 50 introduced the idea that a small number of innate principles are common to every human language e g phrases are oriented around heads and that these general principles are subject to parametric variation e g the order of heads and other phrasal components may differ In this theory the dependency relation between heads complements specifiers and adjuncts is regulated by X bar theory proposed by Jackendoff 51 in the 1970s The complement is sister to the head and they can be ordered in one of two ways A head complement order is called a head initial structure while a complement head order is called a head final structure These are special cases of Tesniere s centripetal and centrifugal structures since here only complements are considered whereas Tesniere considered all types of dependents In the principles and parameters theory a head directionality parameter is proposed as a way of classifying languages A language which has head initial structures is considered to be a head initial language and one which has head final structures is considered to be a head final language It is found however that very few if any languages are entirely one direction or the other Linguists have come up with a number of theories to explain the inconsistencies sometimes positing a more consistent underlying order with the phenomenon of phrasal movement being used to explain the surface deviations Kayne antisymmetry Edit According to the Antisymmetry theory proposed by Richard S Kayne there is no head directionality parameter as such it is claimed that at an underlying level all languages are head initial In fact it is argued that all languages have the underlying order Specifier Head Complement Deviations from this order are accounted for by different syntactic movements applied by languages Kayne argues that a theory that allows both directionalities would imply an absence of asymmetries between languages whereas in fact languages fail to be symmetrical in many respects Kayne argues using the concept of a probe goal search based on the ideas of the Minimalist program whereby a head acts as a probe and looks for a goal namely its complement Kayne proposes that the direction of the probe goal search must share the direction of language parsing and production 52 Parsing and production proceed in a left to right direction the beginning of sentence is heard or spoken first and the end of the sentence is heard or spoken last This implies according to the theory an ordering whereby probe comes before goal i e head precedes complement Some linguists have rejected the conclusions of the Antisymmetry approach Some have pointed out that in predominantly head final languages such as Japanese and Basque the change from an underlying head initial form to a largely head final surface form would involve complex and massive leftward movement which is not in accordance with the ideal of grammatical simplicity 48 Some take a surface true viewpoint that analysis of head direction must take place at the level of surface derivations or even the Phonetic Form PF i e the order in which sentences are pronounced in natural speech This rejects the idea of an underlying ordering which is then subject to movement as posited in Antisymmetry and in certain other approaches It has been argued that a head parameter must only reside at PF as it is unmaintainable in its original form as a structural parameter 53 Some linguists have provided evidence which may be taken to support Kayne s scheme such as Lin 54 who considered Standard Chinese sentences with the sentence final particle le Certain restrictions on movement from within verb phrases preceding such a particle are found if various other assumptions from the literature are accepted to be consistent with the idea that the verb phrase has moved from its underlying position after its head the particle le here being taken as the head of an aspect phrase However Takita 2009 observes that similar restrictions do not apply in Japanese in spite of its surface head final character concluding that if Lin s assumptions are correct then Japanese must be considered to be a true head final language contrary to the main tenet of Antisymmetry 55 More details about these arguments can be found in the Antisymmetry article Gradient classification Edit Some scholars such as Tesniere argue that there are no absolute head initial or head final languages According to this approach it is true that some languages have more head initial or head final elements than other languages do but almost any language contains both head initial and head final elements Therefore rather than being classifiable into fixed categories languages can be arranged on a continuum with head initial and head final as the extremes based on the frequency distribution of their dependency directions This view was supported in a study by Haitao Liu 2010 who investigated 20 languages using a dependency treebank based method 56 For instance Japanese is close to the head final end of the continuum while English and German which have mixed head initial and head final dependencies are plotted in relatively intermediate positions on the continuum Polinsky 2012 identified the following five head directionality sub types Rigid head final languages including Japanese Korean and Tamil Non rigid head final languages including Latin German Persian Basque Tsez and Avar Clearly head initial languages including Irish Malagasy Tongan and most Mayan languages SVO head initial languages including Indonesian and Yucatec Mayan SVO sundry including English Russian the Romance languages and Bantu languages She identified a strong correlation between the head directionality type of a language and the ratio of verbs to nouns in the lexical inventory Languages with a scarcity of simple verbs tend to be rigidly head final as in the case of Japanese whereas verb rich languages tend to be head initial languages 57 See also EditDependency grammar Dependent marking language Double marking language Government linguistics Government and binding theory Head linguistics Head driven phrase structure grammar Head marking language Minimalist grammar Transformational grammar Word order Zero marking language Polish notationNotes Edit a b Contemporary Linguistic Parameters Contemporary Studies in Linguistics Bloomsbury Academic 2015 doi 10 5040 9781474219549 ch 004 ISBN 978 1 4725 3393 7 Dryer 2009 sfn error no target CITEREFDryer2009 help Polinsky Maria Magyar Lilla March 2020 Headedness and the Lexicon The Case of Verb to Noun Ratios Languages 5 1 5 doi 10 3390 languages5010009 Szabolcsi 1983 Van Langendonck 1994 a b Fukui 1994 p 4 sfn error no target CITEREFFukui1994 help Smith 1964 p 6 sfn error no target CITEREFSmith1964 help Sadler amp Arnold 1994 pp 28 34 sfn error no target CITEREFSadler amp Arnold1994 help Sadler amp Arnold 1994 pp 28 sfn error no target CITEREFSadler amp Arnold1994 help Gillion 1992 p 15 a b Takahashi 2002 p 2 a b Sells 1995 p 4 Lin 2003 p 2 Sportiche Koopman amp Stabler 2014 sfn error no target CITEREFSporticheKoopmanStabler2014 help Graf T Paperno D Szabolcsi A Tellings J 2012 Theories of Everything In Honour of Ed Keenan PDF UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics pp 348 359 a b c d e f g Ansari Lalitia Suhardiianto Totok November 2018 Where is the Head Positioned in Indonesian Language A Corpus Study of Head Directionality from a Dependency Perspective 2018 International Conference on Asian Language Processing IALP Bandung Indonesia IEEE 171 177 doi 10 1109 IALP 2018 8629214 ISBN 978 1 7281 1175 9 S2CID 59554177 a b Sneddon James N 15 April 2010 Indonesian A Comprehensive Grammar London Routledge pp 131 147 doi 10 4324 9780203720882 ISBN 9780203720882 Retrieved 10 December 2021 Naoki 1993 pp 15 16 sfn error no target CITEREFNaoki1993 help Watanabe 2011 pp 9 10 Vinka 2009 p 4 Arik Engin October 2019 An Experimental Approach to Basic Word Order in Turkish Intransitives Psychology of Language and Communication 20 1 73 91 doi 10 1515 plc 2016 0004 S2CID 152036877 van Schaaik Gerjan September 2020 The Oxford Turkish Grammar Oxford University Press ISBN 9780198851509 van Schaaik Gerjan September 2020 The Oxford Turkish Grammar Oxford University Press ISBN 9780198851509 Kornfilt Jaklin 2013 Turkish London Routledge pp 1 211 ISBN 9781315823652 a b Goksel Asli Kerslake Celia 2011 Turkish an Essential Grammar Routledge p 368 ISBN 9780415462693 van Schaaik Gerjan September 2020 The Oxford Turkish Grammar Oxford University Press ISBN 9780198851509 a b c A Sumru Ozsoy ed 2019 Word Order in Turkish Cham Switzerland Springer International Publishing AG p 313 ISBN 9783030113841 Taylan Eser Erguvanli 1984 The Function of Word Order in Turkish Grammar Berkeley University of California Press p 179 ISBN 0520099559 Dopke 1998 p 6 Berthold 2005 p 6 sfn error no target CITEREFBerthold2005 help Hinterholzl 2010 p 4 Hinterholzl 2010 p 5 Van Riemsdijk 2007 p 3 Van Riemsdijk 2007 p 1 Bianchi 1999 p 251 Kathol 2001 p 1 Aboh 2001 pp 1 2 Aboh 2001 p 2 Kinyalolo 1992 pp 1 16 Aboh 2001 p 34 Brousseau amp Lumsden 1992 p 4 sfn error no target CITEREFBrousseau amp Lumsden1992 help Aboh 2004 Aboh 2001 p 117 Aboh 2001 p 100 Aboh 2001 p 348 Graffi 2001 pp 197 198 Elordieta 2014 p 2 a b Elordieta 2014 p 5 Lehmann Winfred P March 1973 A Structural Principle of Language and Its Implications Language Linguistic Society of America 49 1 47 66 doi 10 2307 412102 JSTOR 412102 Chomsky 1981 sfn error no target CITEREFChomsky1981 help Jackendoff 1977 sfn error no target CITEREFJackendoff1977 help Kayne 2011 p 12 Richards 2008 p 283 Lin 2006 sfn error no target CITEREFLin2006 help Takita 2009 pp 59 Liu 2010 pp 1567 1578 Polinsky 2012 pp 348 359 Bibliography EditAboh Enoch 2001 Object shift and verb movement in Gbe PDF Generative Grammar in Geneva 2 1 13 Retrieved 29 October 2014 Aboh Enoch 2004 The morphosyntax of complement head sequences Clause structure and word order patterns in kwa New York Oxford University Press Bianchi Valentina 1999 Consequences of antisymmetry Headed relative clauses Walter de Gruyter Broekhuis H 2013 Syntax of Dutch Adjectives and Adjective Phrases Amsterdam Amsterdam University Press Brousseau Anne Marie Lumsden John S 1992 Nominal structures in Fongbe Journal of West African Languages 22 1 1 22 Chomsky Noam 1981 Lectures on Government and Binding Foris Publications Comrie Bernard 2008 Pronominal relative clauses in verb object languages PDF Language and Linguistics 9 4 723 733 Courtney Ellen H 2011 Learning to produce Quechua relative clauses Acquisition of Relative Clauses Processing typology and function John Benjamins Publishing Company pp 141 172 ISBN 9789027283405 Retrieved 13 November 2014 Crysmann Berthold 2005 Relative Clause Extraposition in German an efficient and portable implementation Research on Language and Computation 3 1 61 82 doi 10 1007 s11168 005 1287 z S2CID 17250437 Dopke Susanne 1998 Competing language structures the acquisition of verb placement by bilingual German English children Journal of Child Language 22 3 555 584 doi 10 1017 S0305000998003584 PMID 10095325 S2CID 11270532 Dryer Matthew S 2009 The Branching Direction Theory of Word Order Correlations Revisited In S Scalise E Magni amp A Bisetto Eds Universals of Language Today pp 185 207 Rotterdam Netherlands Springer Elordieta Arantzazu 2014 Biberauer T Sheehan M eds On the relevance of the Head Parameter in a mixed OV language Theoretical Approaches to Disharmonic Word Order Oxford Scholarship Online pp 306 329 doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199684359 003 0011 ISBN 9780199684359 Fukui Naoki 1993 Parameters and optionality Linguistic Inquiry 24 3 399 420 JSTOR 4178821 Gillion Brendan 1992 Towards a Common Semantics for English Count and Mass Nouns Linguistics and Philosophy 15 6 597 639 doi 10 1007 BF00628112 S2CID 50730562 Graffi Giorgio 2001 200 Years of Syntax A critical survey John Benjamins Publishing Hinterholzl Roland 2010 Collapsing the Head Final Filter and the Head Complement Parameter Linguistics 20 35 66 Hinterholzl Roland Svetlana Petrova 2009 Information structure and language change New approaches to word order variation in Germanic Mouton de Gruyter Huang C T J 1982 Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar PhD Dissertation MIT Jackendoff Ray 1977 X Syntax A Study of Phrase Structure Cambridge MIT Press Kathol Andreas 2001 Positional Effects in a Monostratal Grammar of German Journal of Linguistics 37 1 35 66 doi 10 1017 s0022226701008805 Kayne Richard S 2003 Antisymmetry and Japanese English Linguistics 20 1 40 doi 10 9793 elsj1984 20 1 Kayne Richard S 2011 Why Are There No Directionality Parameters PDF Proceedings of the 28th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics Somerville Cascadilla Press pp 1 23 Retrieved 13 November 2014 Kinyalolo Kasangati 1992 A note on word order in the progressive and prospective in Fon Journal of West African Languages 22 1 37 51 Retrieved 29 October 2014 Kratzer Angelika More structural analogies between pronouns and tenses Proceedings of SALT 8 92 110 Kroch Anthony Morphosyntactic Variation Proceedings of the Thirtieth Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society 2 180 210 Lasnik Howard 2010 Government binding principles and parameters theory Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science 1 1 40 50 doi 10 1002 wcs 35 PMID 26272837 Lin Chienjer 2003 Aspect is result Mandarin resultative constructions and aspect incorporation Proceedings of the West Coast Conference on Linguistics WECOL Lin Tzong Hong J Complement to specifier movement in Mandarin Chinese Ms National Tsing Hua University Liu Haitao 2010 Dependency direction as a means of word order typology A method based on dependency treebanks Lingua 120 6 1567 1578 doi 10 1016 j lingua 2009 10 001 Nolda Andreas 2004 Topics Detached to the Left On Left Dislocation Hanging Topic and Related Constructions in German PDF Berlin ZAS Papers in Linguistics pp 423 448 Retrieved 13 November 2014 Onozuka Hiromi 2008 On the Resultative Reading of the Imperfective Aspect in English Australian Journal of Linguistics 28 1 1 16 doi 10 1080 07268600701877465 S2CID 56964107 Ozeki Hiromi 2011 The acquisition of relative clauses in Japanese Acquisition of Relative Clauses Processing typology and function John Benjamins Publishing Company pp 173 196 ISBN 9789027283405 Retrieved 13 November 2014 Ozeki Hiromi 2011 The acquisition of relative clauses in Japanese Acquisition of Relative Clauses Processing typology and function John Benjamins Publishing Company pp 173 196 ISBN 9789027283405 Retrieved 13 November 2014 Polinsky Maria 2012 Headness again PDF UCLA Working Papers in Linguistics 17 348 359 Richards Marc D 2008 Desymmetrization Parametric variation at the PF Interface The Canadian Journal of Linguistics 53 2 3 275 300 doi 10 1353 cjl 0 0024 S2CID 170810562 ProQuest 85703588 Sadler Louisa Arnold Douglas 1994 Prenominal adjectives and the phrase lexical distinction Journal of Linguistics 30 1 187 226 doi 10 1017 s0022226700016224 JSTOR 4176260 S2CID 144681113 Sells Peter 1995 Korean and Japanese morphology from a lexical perspective Linguistic Inquiry 26 2 277 325 JSTOR 4178898 Shirai Yasuhiro Kurono Atsuko 1998 The acquisition of tense aspect marking in Japanese as a second language PDF Language Learning 48 2 245 279 doi 10 1111 1467 9922 00041 Retrieved 26 October 2014 Dominique Sportiche Hilda Koopman Edward Stabler 2014 An Introduction to Syntactic Analysis and Theory Chichester Wiley Blackwell Szabolcsi Anna 1983 The possessor that ran away from home The Linguistic Review 3 1 89 102 doi 10 1515 tlir 1983 3 1 89 S2CID 170588159 Takahashi Daiko 2002 Determiner raising and scope shift Linguistic Inquiry 33 4 575 615 doi 10 1162 002438902762731772 S2CID 57564972 Takita Kensuke 2009 If Chinese is head initial Japanese cannot be Journal of East Asian Linguistics 18 1 41 61 doi 10 1007 s10831 009 9038 z JSTOR 40345242 S2CID 121661611 Tokizaki Hisao 2011 The nature of linear information in the morphosyntax PF interface English Linguistics 28 2 227 257 doi 10 9793 elsj 28 2 227 Van Langendonck Willy 1994 Determiners as heads Cognitive Linguistics 5 1 243 260 doi 10 1515 cogl 1994 5 3 243 S2CID 201654317 Van Riemsdijk Henk 2007 Case in spatial adpositional phrases The dative accusative alternation in German Pitar Mos A Building with a View Festschrift for Alexandra Cornilescu 1 23 Vinka Mikael 2009 The syntax of three Japanese postpositions Lund Working Papers in Linguistics 40 229 250 Watanabe Akira 2011 Adjectival inflection and the position of measure phrases Linguistic Inquiry 42 3 490 507 doi 10 1162 ling a 00055 S2CID 57565727 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Head directionality parameter amp oldid 1141093766, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.