fbpx
Wikipedia

Tübatulabal language

Tübatulabal /təˈbɑːtələbɑːl/ is an Uto-Aztecan language, traditionally spoken in Kern County, California, United States. It is the traditional language of the Tübatulabal, who still speak the traditional language in addition to English.[1] The language originally had three main dialects: Bakalanchi, Pakanapul and Palegawan.

Tübatulabal
Pakaːnil
RegionKern River, California, United States
Ethnicity900 Tübatulabal (2007)
Uto-Aztecan
  • Tübatulabal
Language codes
ISO 639-3tub
Glottologtuba1278
ELPTubatulabal
This article contains IPA phonetic symbols. Without proper rendering support, you may see question marks, boxes, or other symbols instead of Unicode characters. For an introductory guide on IPA symbols, see Help:IPA.

In English, the name Tübatulabal refers to both the Tübatulabal people and their language. However, in the language itself, the term Tübatulabal refers only to the Tübatulabal people. Its origin is unclear, but it may be related to the noun stem tɨba- "pine nuts". The Tübatulabal term for the Tübatulabal language is pakaːnil.

Phonology edit

Segmental phonology edit

Vowels edit

There are six phonemic vowels in Tübatulabal:

Contrastive short and long versions of each vowel are found in both stressed and unstressed syllables. The vowels have various allophones which occur in different environments, most notably more central lax allophones when the vowels are short and occur in unstressed syllables. i and u can occur as the second member of a diphthong with any other vowel, resulting in ten possible diphthongs (Voegelin reports that ɨu is rare). Phonologically, the members of a diphthong are treated as distinct segments. For example, the common initial reduplication process, which copies the first stem vowel, copies only the first member of a diphthong, e.g.:

ʔuinul 'the sucker fish'
ʔuʔuinul 'the many suckers in one place'

Vowel length is contrastive. However, according to (Jensen 1973), in the suffixing morphology length is typically predictable. In most cases, the first suffix is short, the second suffix is long, the third suffix is short, and so on. For example, the verbal stem tɨk- 'to eat' can be expanded to tɨk-ilɔːɡ-ɔ-maːla 'let us go and pretend to eat'. In this word, each suffix alternates in length compared to its neighbors. When arranged differently, the same suffixes will have different lengths. Thus compare maːla 'let us' with the realization of the same morpheme in tɨk-al-aː-mala 'let us go eat'.

Tübatulabal consonants show a basic voicing distinction, with a corresponding alternately voiced phoneme present for almost every obstruent. Tübatulabal voiceless consonants are unaspirated, like those in English after an initial /s/, e.g. as in 'spin', 'stiff', 'skin'.

Non-contrastive allophones of all vowels occur, usually when a vowel follows a nasal consonant, and especially when it also precedes a glottal consonant.

Consonants edit

All consonants except the glottal stop can occur as geminates. Gemination is often phonologically predictable.[2] In particular, all consonants except the voiced stops and the glottal stop geminate when following a short vowel. All stops and affricates are geminated in word-final position, regardless of the length of the preceding vowel.

Prosody edit

Tübatulabal has predictable word stress, which is tied to morphological constituency and syllable weight. Primary stress falls on the final syllable of the stem. Secondary stress is assigned right to left from the final syllable, falling on every other mora:

ˌʔɨmbɨŋˌwibaˈʔat "he is wanting to roll string on his thigh" [3]
ˌjuːuˌduːˌjuːuˈdat "the fruit is mashing"

Words with the form VːCVCV will be stressed as ˌVːCVˈCV:

ˌnaːwiˈʃul "the pine-nut pole"

For the purposes of stress assignment, two identical short vowels that are separated only by a glottal stop are treated as a single vowel if and only if they belong to the same morpheme:

ˌkuʔud͡ʒuˈbil "the little one"

Morphology edit

There are three basic word types in Tübatulabal: verbs, nouns, and particles. Verbs may be formed from verbal stems or from noun stems with verbalizing morphology; similarly, nouns can be formed from noun stems or from verbal stems with nominalizing morphology. Particles have their own stems, but they have comparatively little inflection, whereas both verbs and nouns tend to be very morphologically complex.

There are four word-formation processes in Tübatulabal: suffixation, reduplication, conjunction and compounding.

Suffixation edit

Suffixation is the most common and productive process in agglutinative word-formation. Suffixes form a closed class and occur in a fixed order according to the word type.

Reduplication edit

There are two kinds of reduplication: full reduplication and partial reduplication. Full reduplication is the less common type and marks the iterative aspect in verbs.

Partial reduplication can occur as initial or final reduplication. Final reduplication is very rare and always expresses the idea of plural allegiance. It is also apparently limited to occurring with noun stems or suffixes that end in wa. Voegelin illustrates with an example:

tɔhat͡siŋwan 'his hunting partner'
tɔhat͡siŋwawaːn 'his hunting partner (in the sense that the partner referred to, being very proficient, has many companions in hunting)'

Initial reduplication is far more productive. It is used to express collective plurality in nouns and to express aspect reversal in verbs. Initial reduplication prefixes a copy of the first vowel of the stem (as well as any immediately following nasal), preceded by a fixed ʔ. The underlying stem-initial consonant (if any) may also undergo changes, particularly in voicing and length. Some examples illustrate the reduplication process:

Base form Reduplicated form Base form gloss
tɨk- ʔɨtːɨk to eat
tana- ʔandana to get down
paːabɨ- ʔaːbaːabi to be tired
kulaːabiʃt ʔukːulaːabiʃt the duck

Conjunction edit

Conjunction involves the combination of a particle with a word of another type. According to Voegelin, the behavior of particles is similar to that of enclitics in other Uto-Aztecan languages but distinct enough from them that it should not be considered to be a kind of cliticization.

Compounding edit

Compounding appears to have been a much more productive process at an earlier stage of the language. It now has very limited productivity, and in many cases, it appears to have been completely lexicalized if it occurs.

Verb morphology edit

Each verb stem has an unpredictable inherent aspect value (either telic or atelic; by default, a bare stem is inherently atelic), and an inherent value for transitivity (transitive, intransitive or impersonal). The inherent values can be changed by morphological addition to yield a verb stem with any of the other possible values. Aspect reversal is indicated by initial reduplication. Transitivity change is indicated by the use of one (or more) of a number of derivational suffixes with which verbs are constructed.

The full verb structure can be summarized as (A) + B + (C) + (D), where B is the verb root, and the other positions (all optional) represent classes of morphemes. A indicates initial reduplication, which can occur only once per word. C indicates a class of derivational morphemes, which can be divided into ten ordered positions, each of which allows at most one morpheme per word. D is the final position; there are nine possible morphemes in final position, but only one can occur in any single word.

The C class morphemes are given with examples in the table below. When these morphemes co-occur in a word, they must occur in the order given. Transitivity changing morphemes are marked with *. They have a different effect depending on the inherent transitivity of the verb root, as well as the presence of other transitivity-changing morphology.

Suffix Gloss Example word Gloss
-(i)n causative * hɔːhinat 's/he is coughing (through the agency of a crumb)'
-(a)n benefactive * weleʔanat kɔːimi 's/he is crawling to the woman (perhaps in the sense of "he is crawling there for the erotic benefit of the woman")'
-(a)la/-(a)ɡiːm/-(a)kin/-(a)min movement ʔɨtːɨkːamin 's/he ate it here and went away'
-(i)niːnɨm distributive ʔawaʃiniːnɨm 's/he dug first here, then there'
-(i)lɔːk pretending to ʔanaŋaːlilɔːɡibaʔat 's/he wants to go along pretending he is crying'
-(i)baʔ desiderative ʔamaɡiːibaʔ 's/he is on the verge of learning about it'
-(i)ʃa future ʔapaʔaniʃa 'it will get plugged up'
-(i)w passive * weːhiwat 's/he is being licked (e.g. kitten by mother cat)'
-(i)wɨːt collective-intensive ʔapahkaniwɨːdiʃa 'they will speak Tübatulabal'
-(a)puw- similative wɨʃɨpuwat 'it seems to be ripening'

The possible verbal final morphemes (class D) are shown below. Unlike the class C morphemes, only one of these final-position morphemes can occur in any single word. Therefore, the ordering of morphemes in this table does not indicate anything about a linear relationship among the morphemes.

Suffix/suffix type Gloss Example word Gloss
Nominalizers kabobaːʔinaːnat͡siŋwajinɨʔɨŋ 'my partner in rattling for it (the dance)'
Subordinaters ʔalaːwiʔima tɨkːat 's/he is eating while talking'
Imperatives tɔhaːhai tɔhiːla 'hunt the deer after a while'
-(a)t present tense ʔɔhtatni 's/he is asking me'
-(a)ma exhortative waʃamaːala 'let's dig it'
-(a)ha permissive wɔːʔiʃɨhatd͡za 's/he might get jealous'
-(i)ukaŋ past habituative t͡saːijinaːniukaŋ 's/he used to make lace'
-(aː)haiwɨt irrealis muːdakaːhaiwɨt 's/he should have dodged'
-(a)htajat adversative pɨːminahtajat 's/he is making it full (despite the fact that the thing to be filled is very large)'

Noun morphology edit

All nouns (whether derived from verb stems or noun stems) are obligatorily marked as absolute or relative. Nouns must also be marked with one of the three basic cases: subject, object, or genitive. Relative nouns make a finer distinction between suus and ejus objects and genitives. In addition to this obligatory morphology, nouns may also receive suffixes indicating several secondary cases (inessive, ablative, allative and instrumental) as well as many other derivational suffixes.

Nouns may be divided into three basic classes according to their stem shape and morphological behavior and sometimes according to their semantic contribution as well. The basic test for classification is how the noun occurs when it is absolute. The absolute suffix has a different allomorph when it occurs with a noun from each of these classes. Class A nouns all have vowel-final stems, and add the absolute suffix as -l. Class B noun stems may be vowel-final or consonant-final, but in either case the absolute suffix is -t. Class C is a small class of nouns, many of which are kinship terms or other inalienable nouns. The absolute noun is phonologically null when it occurs with class C nouns.[4]

Each of the classes can be subdivided into two or more classes, depending on phonological differences in the noun stem that lead to divergent behavior in certain case forms. Specifically, class A is divided into A1 nouns (stems end with a long vowel) and A2 nouns (stems end with a short vowel). Class B is divided into five subclasses, depending on whether the stem ends in a short vowel, a long vowel, n, m, or a voiceless consonant. Class C is divided into C1 (nouns which take an overt relative suffix) and C2 (nouns with no overt relative suffix).

noun classes and subclasses, with all obligatory cases
Class Example stem Gloss Absolute Relative[5]
Subject Object Genitive Subject Suus object Ejus object Suus genitive Ejus genitive[6]
A1 haniː house haniːl haniːla haniːliŋ haniːn haniː haniːjin haniː haniːnin
A2 t͡ʃaːmi acorn gravy t͡ʃaːmil t͡ʃaːmila t͡ʃaːmilaʔaŋ t͡ʃaːmin t͡ʃaːmi t͡ʃaːmijin t͡ʃaːmiʔin t͡ʃaːmiʔinin
B1 pit͡ʃiliː squirrel pit͡ʃiliːt pit͡ʃiliːida pit͡ʃiliːidiŋ pit͡ʃiliːn pit͡ʃiliː pit͡ʃiliːijip pit͡ʃiliʔin pit͡ʃiliːʔinin
B2 maːaʃa sack maːaʃat maːʃata maːʃatiŋ maːaʃap maːaʃat͡s maːʃat͡sip maːʃaʔadin maːaʃaʔinin
B3 ʃulun fingernail ʃulunt ʃulunda ʃulundiŋ ʃulunin * ʃulun ʃuluninip ʃulunʔin ʃulunʔinin
B4 pɔm egg pɔmt pɔmda pɔmdiŋ pɔmin * pɔm pɔmd͡zip pɔmin * pɔminin
B5 muːʃ fish spear muːʃt muːʃta muːʃtiŋ muːʃn * muːʃ * muːʃip muːʃin muːʃinin
C1 tahambiʃ old man tahambiʃ tahambiʃi tahambiʃiŋ tahambiʃin * tahambiʃ tahambiʃin * tahambiʃʔin tahambiʃʔinin
C2 naːadɨʔ cat naːadɨʔ naːadɨʔi naːadɨʔiŋ naːadɨʔap naːadɨʔai naːadɨʔajin naːadɨʔaʔin naːadɨʔaʔinin

Particle morphology edit

Morphemes belonging to the particle class are distinguished by the fact that they undergo little or no inflection and suffixation, unlike verbs and nouns. The particle class includes two subclasses of morphemes which behave quite differently: conjunctive particles and independent particles.

Conjunctive particles resemble clitics in that they never appear independently but always lean on another word. However, unlike clitics, conjunctive particles typically bear their own stress, and they do not alter the stress of the word on which they lean. Conjunctive particles include various discourse and modal morphemes as well as the typical pronominal agreement morphemes that occur with verbs.

Independent particles are fully independent words. They include prepositional, modal and exclamatory morphemes, numerals, and one class of pronouns.

The table below shows the pronominal morphemes of Tübatulabal. Like nouns, pronouns distinguish between three cases: subject, object and possessive. (Pronouns do not make a distinction between absolute and relative entities.) Different forms exist for first-, second- and third-person entities. Second- and third-person forms distinguish only singular and plural numbers, but first-person forms distinguish between singular, dual inclusive, dual exclusive, and plural numbers. All pronouns may be expressed bu conjunctive particles. The subject pronouns are unique in that they can also be expressed by an independent particle.[7]

Subject Object Possessive
independent conjunctive conjunctive conjunctive
1sg. nik -ɡi -ni -nɨʔɨŋ
1d.inc. iŋɡila -ɡila ? ?
1d.exc. iŋɡilaʔaŋ -ɡilaʔaŋ -d͡ʒijaʔaŋ -t͡ʃ
1pl. iŋɡiluːt͡s -ɡiluːt͡s -d͡ziː -t͡s
2sg. imbi -bi -diŋ -iŋ
2pl. imbuːmu -buːmu -dulu ulu
3sg. in (-d͡za) -n
3pl. inda -da -tɨpɨ -p

The first-person subject conjunctive forms have special allomorphs when they occur with the exhortative suffix -ma:

1sg.
1d.inc. -la
1d.inc. -laʔaŋ
1pl. -luːt͡s

The third-person conjunctive form is usually null, but it is expressed by -d͡za after the exhortative or permissive suffixes. (The suffix often undergoes syncope and devoicing, yielding -t͡s.) The second-person conjunctive plural subject form may also syncopate, and medial vowel then shortens as well: -bum. The first-person conjunctive singular subject form may also syncopate, triggering devoicing but no irregular phonology: the suffix then has the form -k.

Subject pronouns typically lean on verbs (if conjunctive) and correspond to grammatical subject: iwikkːɨki "I discarded (it)" (with devoicing); anabaːhaʃta "they can throw it" (with metathesis of the components of the affricate and a change of s > ʃ).

Object pronouns also lean on verbs and indicate any non-possessive oblique function, including transitive objects, ditransitive objects or benefactives, objects of imperative verbs, and subjects of subordinate verbs if not equivalent to the subject of the matrix verb.

Possessive pronouns typically lean on the possessum: haniːnɨʔɨŋ "my house"; ʃɔːɔjin "his wife".

Syntax edit

Word order is generally flexible. According to (Voegelin 1935), "Word-order in general is stylistic rather than obligatory." (p. 185)

Orthography edit

Transcriptions in this article follow the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA). Much published material concerning Tübatulabal uses the Americanist orthography. In addition, the most important linguistic work on Tübatulabal, the original grammatical description of the language, (Voegelin 1935) uses a somewhat different orthography.

Voegelin writes ɨ as ⟨ï⟩ and ɔ as ⟨ô⟩. He also writes ʃ as ⟨c⟩, t͡ʃ as ⟨tc⟩, ʔ as ⟨‘⟩, d͡ʒ as ⟨dž⟩ and j as ⟨y⟩. He also uses a number of special symbols for vocalic allomorphs. ⟨ι⟩ is an allomorph of ⟨i⟩, ⟨μ⟩ is an allomorph of ⟨u⟩, ⟨o⟩ is an allomorph of ⟨ô⟩ (IPA ɔ), and ⟨ŏ⟩ is an allomorph of both ⟨a⟩ and ⟨ô⟩.

The letter ⟨ü⟩ in the name Tübatulabal represents the central unrounded vowel ɨ.

Notes edit

  1. ^ "Pahka'anil (Tübatulabal) Text Project - Home". web.csulb.edu. Retrieved 2023-05-05.
  2. ^ See Jensen for discussion of the role of "2.6 Gemination" in Tubatulabal phonology. (1973:61 et seq)
  3. ^ See Jensen for discussion of the arbitrary behavior of glottal stops in stress assignment. The glottal stop, which is not otherwise counted as a mora, is counted as a mora for the purpose of stress assignment. (Jensen 1973, pp. 76–76).
  4. ^ The morphological differences between noun classes can probably be traced back to a simple case of allomorphy. At a certain point in the history of an ancestor of Tübatulabal, all class A nouns ended in a vowel, while all class B nouns ended in a consonant. (Class C forms are few in number and have a more complicated origin.) The absolute suffix in this language was *-t, which lenited intervocalically, leading to -l in Tübatulabal class A nouns (compare cognates ʈ͡ʂ in Serrano, r in Tongva.) See (Voegelin 1935)[which?] and (Manaster Ramer 1992).
  5. ^ Relative nouns typically require a suffix indicating the possessor or entity to which the suffixed noun is related. Forms in this table are those for a third-person singular possessor -n. The exception is suus forms, which do not allow a possessive suffix (since they are inherently possessed by/related to the grammatical subject of the clause).
  6. ^ The ejus forms are not given in (Voegelin 1935).[which?] The forms in this table are hypothesized on the basis of the suffix charts that Voegelin gives. With a few exceptions, all other forms in this table are taken directly from (Voegelin 1935).[which?] The few forms that are hypothetical are marked with *.
  7. ^ Cells marked with -- are phonologically null. Cells marked with ? are forms missing without explanation in (Voegelin 1935).

Bibliography edit

  • Aion, Nora (2003). Selected Topics in Nootka and Tübatulabal Phonology. PhD dissertation: City University of New York.
  • Arvidson, Lucy. Alaawich (Our Language): First Book of Words in the Tübatulabal Language of Southern California
  • Crowhurst, Megan (1991). "Demorification in Tübatulabal: Evidence from Initial Reduplication and Stress". NELS. 21: 49–63.
  • Gifford, Edward Winslow (1917). Tübatulabal and Kawaiisu kinship terms. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Retrieved 2012-08-26.
  • Heath, Jeffrey (1981). "Tübatulabal Phonology". Harvard Studies in Phonology. 2.
  • Jensen, James R. (1973). Stress and the Verbal Phonology of Tübatulabal (PhD dissertation). Indiana University.
  • Manaster Ramer, Alexis (1992). "Proto-Uto-Aztecan Phonology: Evidence From Tübatulabal Noun Morphophonemics". International Journal of American Linguistics. 58 (4): 436–446. doi:10.1086/ijal.58.4.3519778. JSTOR 3519778. S2CID 147964960.
  • Voegelin, Charles F. (1935a). "Tübatulabal Grammar". University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology. 34: 55–190.
  • Voegelin, Charles F. (1935b). "Tubatulabal Texts". University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology. 34: 191–246.
  • Voegelin, Charles F. (1958). "Working Dictionary of Tübatulabal". International Journal of American Linguistics. 24 (3): 221–228. doi:10.1086/464459. S2CID 145758965.

External links edit

  • Tubatulabal language overview at the Survey of California and Other Indian Languages
  • "Tubatulabal Language" at native-languages.org
  • Recordings of Tübatulabal recorded by JP Harrington
  • OLAC resources in and about the Tübatulabal language
  • Audio recordings of word lists in Tübatulabal from the Northern Uto-Aztecan Collection of Megan Crowhurst at AILLA.
  • Tübatulabal, California Language Archives

tübatulabal, language, tübatulabal, ɑː, ɑː, aztecan, language, traditionally, spoken, kern, county, california, united, states, traditional, language, tübatulabal, still, speak, traditional, language, addition, english, language, originally, three, main, diale. Tubatulabal t e ˈ b ɑː t e l e b ɑː l is an Uto Aztecan language traditionally spoken in Kern County California United States It is the traditional language of the Tubatulabal who still speak the traditional language in addition to English 1 The language originally had three main dialects Bakalanchi Pakanapul and Palegawan TubatulabalPakaːnilRegionKern River California United StatesEthnicity900 Tubatulabal 2007 Language familyUto Aztecan TubatulabalLanguage codesISO 639 3 a href https iso639 3 sil org code tub class extiw title iso639 3 tub tub a Glottologtuba1278ELPTubatulabalThis article contains IPA phonetic symbols Without proper rendering support you may see question marks boxes or other symbols instead of Unicode characters For an introductory guide on IPA symbols see Help IPA In English the name Tubatulabal refers to both the Tubatulabal people and their language However in the language itself the term Tubatulabal refers only to the Tubatulabal people Its origin is unclear but it may be related to the noun stem tɨba pine nuts The Tubatulabal term for the Tubatulabal language is pakaːnil Contents 1 Phonology 1 1 Segmental phonology 1 1 1 Vowels 1 1 2 Consonants 1 2 Prosody 2 Morphology 2 1 Suffixation 2 2 Reduplication 2 3 Conjunction 2 4 Compounding 2 5 Verb morphology 2 6 Noun morphology 2 7 Particle morphology 3 Syntax 4 Orthography 5 Notes 6 Bibliography 7 External linksPhonology editSegmental phonology edit Vowels edit There are six phonemic vowels in Tubatulabal Front Central BackHigh i ɨ uMid e ɔLow aContrastive short and long versions of each vowel are found in both stressed and unstressed syllables The vowels have various allophones which occur in different environments most notably more central lax allophones when the vowels are short and occur in unstressed syllables i and u can occur as the second member of a diphthong with any other vowel resulting in ten possible diphthongs Voegelin reports that ɨu is rare Phonologically the members of a diphthong are treated as distinct segments For example the common initial reduplication process which copies the first stem vowel copies only the first member of a diphthong e g ʔuinul the sucker fish ʔuʔuinul the many suckers in one place Vowel length is contrastive However according to Jensen 1973 in the suffixing morphology length is typically predictable In most cases the first suffix is short the second suffix is long the third suffix is short and so on For example the verbal stem tɨk to eat can be expanded to tɨk ilɔːɡ ɔ maːla let us go and pretend to eat In this word each suffix alternates in length compared to its neighbors When arranged differently the same suffixes will have different lengths Thus compare maːla let us with the realization of the same morpheme in tɨk al aː mala let us go eat Tubatulabal consonants show a basic voicing distinction with a corresponding alternately voiced phoneme present for almost every obstruent Tubatulabal voiceless consonants are unaspirated like those in English after an initial s e g as in spin stiff skin Non contrastive allophones of all vowels occur usually when a vowel follows a nasal consonant and especially when it also precedes a glottal consonant Consonants edit Labial Alveolar Palatal Velar GlottalNasal m n ŋPlosive voiceless p t ts tʃ k ʔvoiced b d dz dʒ ɡFricative ʃ hApproximant l j wAll consonants except the glottal stop can occur as geminates Gemination is often phonologically predictable 2 In particular all consonants except the voiced stops and the glottal stop geminate when following a short vowel All stops and affricates are geminated in word final position regardless of the length of the preceding vowel Prosody edit Tubatulabal has predictable word stress which is tied to morphological constituency and syllable weight Primary stress falls on the final syllable of the stem Secondary stress is assigned right to left from the final syllable falling on every other mora ˌʔɨmbɨŋˌwibaˈʔat he is wanting to roll string on his thigh 3 ˌjuːuˌduːˌjuːuˈdat the fruit is mashing Words with the form VːCVCV will be stressed as ˌVːCVˈCV ˌnaːwiˈʃul the pine nut pole For the purposes of stress assignment two identical short vowels that are separated only by a glottal stop are treated as a single vowel if and only if they belong to the same morpheme ˌkuʔud ʒuˈbil the little one Morphology editThere are three basic word types in Tubatulabal verbs nouns and particles Verbs may be formed from verbal stems or from noun stems with verbalizing morphology similarly nouns can be formed from noun stems or from verbal stems with nominalizing morphology Particles have their own stems but they have comparatively little inflection whereas both verbs and nouns tend to be very morphologically complex There are four word formation processes in Tubatulabal suffixation reduplication conjunction and compounding Suffixation edit Suffixation is the most common and productive process in agglutinative word formation Suffixes form a closed class and occur in a fixed order according to the word type Reduplication edit There are two kinds of reduplication full reduplication and partial reduplication Full reduplication is the less common type and marks the iterative aspect in verbs Partial reduplication can occur as initial or final reduplication Final reduplication is very rare and always expresses the idea of plural allegiance It is also apparently limited to occurring with noun stems or suffixes that end in wa Voegelin illustrates with an example tɔhat siŋwan his hunting partner tɔhat siŋwawaːn his hunting partner in the sense that the partner referred to being very proficient has many companions in hunting Initial reduplication is far more productive It is used to express collective plurality in nouns and to express aspect reversal in verbs Initial reduplication prefixes a copy of the first vowel of the stem as well as any immediately following nasal preceded by a fixed ʔ The underlying stem initial consonant if any may also undergo changes particularly in voicing and length Some examples illustrate the reduplication process Base form Reduplicated form Base form glosstɨk ʔɨtːɨk to eattana ʔandana to get downpaːabɨ ʔaːbaːabi to be tiredkulaːabiʃt ʔukːulaːabiʃt the duckConjunction edit Conjunction involves the combination of a particle with a word of another type According to Voegelin the behavior of particles is similar to that of enclitics in other Uto Aztecan languages but distinct enough from them that it should not be considered to be a kind of cliticization Compounding edit Compounding appears to have been a much more productive process at an earlier stage of the language It now has very limited productivity and in many cases it appears to have been completely lexicalized if it occurs Verb morphology edit Each verb stem has an unpredictable inherent aspect value either telic or atelic by default a bare stem is inherently atelic and an inherent value for transitivity transitive intransitive or impersonal The inherent values can be changed by morphological addition to yield a verb stem with any of the other possible values Aspect reversal is indicated by initial reduplication Transitivity change is indicated by the use of one or more of a number of derivational suffixes with which verbs are constructed The full verb structure can be summarized as A B C D where B is the verb root and the other positions all optional represent classes of morphemes A indicates initial reduplication which can occur only once per word C indicates a class of derivational morphemes which can be divided into ten ordered positions each of which allows at most one morpheme per word D is the final position there are nine possible morphemes in final position but only one can occur in any single word The C class morphemes are given with examples in the table below When these morphemes co occur in a word they must occur in the order given Transitivity changing morphemes are marked with They have a different effect depending on the inherent transitivity of the verb root as well as the presence of other transitivity changing morphology Suffix Gloss Example word Gloss i n causative hɔːhinat s he is coughing through the agency of a crumb a n benefactive weleʔanat kɔːimi s he is crawling to the woman perhaps in the sense of he is crawling there for the erotic benefit of the woman a la a ɡiːm a kin a min movement ʔɨtːɨkːamin s he ate it here and went away i niːnɨm distributive ʔawaʃiniːnɨm s he dug first here then there i lɔːk pretending to ʔanaŋaːlilɔːɡibaʔat s he wants to go along pretending he is crying i baʔ desiderative ʔamaɡiːibaʔ s he is on the verge of learning about it i ʃa future ʔapaʔaniʃa it will get plugged up i w passive weːhiwat s he is being licked e g kitten by mother cat i wɨːt collective intensive ʔapahkaniwɨːdiʃa they will speak Tubatulabal a puw similative wɨʃɨpuwat it seems to be ripening The possible verbal final morphemes class D are shown below Unlike the class C morphemes only one of these final position morphemes can occur in any single word Therefore the ordering of morphemes in this table does not indicate anything about a linear relationship among the morphemes Suffix suffix type Gloss Example word GlossNominalizers kabobaːʔinaːnat siŋwajinɨʔɨŋ my partner in rattling for it the dance Subordinaters ʔalaːwiʔima tɨkːat s he is eating while talking Imperatives tɔhaːhai tɔhiːla hunt the deer after a while a t present tense ʔɔhtatni s he is asking me a ma exhortative waʃamaːala let s dig it a ha permissive wɔːʔiʃɨhatd za s he might get jealous i ukaŋ past habituative t saːijinaːniukaŋ s he used to make lace aː haiwɨt irrealis muːdakaːhaiwɨt s he should have dodged a htajat adversative pɨːminahtajat s he is making it full despite the fact that the thing to be filled is very large Noun morphology edit All nouns whether derived from verb stems or noun stems are obligatorily marked as absolute or relative Nouns must also be marked with one of the three basic cases subject object or genitive Relative nouns make a finer distinction between suus and ejus objects and genitives In addition to this obligatory morphology nouns may also receive suffixes indicating several secondary cases inessive ablative allative and instrumental as well as many other derivational suffixes Nouns may be divided into three basic classes according to their stem shape and morphological behavior and sometimes according to their semantic contribution as well The basic test for classification is how the noun occurs when it is absolute The absolute suffix has a different allomorph when it occurs with a noun from each of these classes Class A nouns all have vowel final stems and add the absolute suffix as l Class B noun stems may be vowel final or consonant final but in either case the absolute suffix is t Class C is a small class of nouns many of which are kinship terms or other inalienable nouns The absolute noun is phonologically null when it occurs with class C nouns 4 Each of the classes can be subdivided into two or more classes depending on phonological differences in the noun stem that lead to divergent behavior in certain case forms Specifically class A is divided into A1 nouns stems end with a long vowel and A2 nouns stems end with a short vowel Class B is divided into five subclasses depending on whether the stem ends in a short vowel a long vowel n m or a voiceless consonant Class C is divided into C1 nouns which take an overt relative suffix and C2 nouns with no overt relative suffix noun classes and subclasses with all obligatory cases Class Example stem Gloss Absolute Relative 5 Subject Object Genitive Subject Suus object Ejus object Suus genitive Ejus genitive 6 A1 haniː house haniːl haniːla haniːliŋ haniːn haniː haniːjin haniː haniːninA2 t ʃaːmi acorn gravy t ʃaːmil t ʃaːmila t ʃaːmilaʔaŋ t ʃaːmin t ʃaːmi t ʃaːmijin t ʃaːmiʔin t ʃaːmiʔininB1 pit ʃiliː squirrel pit ʃiliːt pit ʃiliːida pit ʃiliːidiŋ pit ʃiliːn pit ʃiliː pit ʃiliːijip pit ʃiliʔin pit ʃiliːʔininB2 maːaʃa sack maːaʃat maːʃata maːʃatiŋ maːaʃap maːaʃat s maːʃat sip maːʃaʔadin maːaʃaʔininB3 ʃulun fingernail ʃulunt ʃulunda ʃulundiŋ ʃulunin ʃulun ʃuluninip ʃulunʔin ʃulunʔininB4 pɔm egg pɔmt pɔmda pɔmdiŋ pɔmin pɔm pɔmd zip pɔmin pɔmininB5 muːʃ fish spear muːʃt muːʃta muːʃtiŋ muːʃn muːʃ muːʃip muːʃin muːʃininC1 tahambiʃ old man tahambiʃ tahambiʃi tahambiʃiŋ tahambiʃin tahambiʃ tahambiʃin tahambiʃʔin tahambiʃʔininC2 naːadɨʔ cat naːadɨʔ naːadɨʔi naːadɨʔiŋ naːadɨʔap naːadɨʔai naːadɨʔajin naːadɨʔaʔin naːadɨʔaʔininParticle morphology edit Morphemes belonging to the particle class are distinguished by the fact that they undergo little or no inflection and suffixation unlike verbs and nouns The particle class includes two subclasses of morphemes which behave quite differently conjunctive particles and independent particles Conjunctive particles resemble clitics in that they never appear independently but always lean on another word However unlike clitics conjunctive particles typically bear their own stress and they do not alter the stress of the word on which they lean Conjunctive particles include various discourse and modal morphemes as well as the typical pronominal agreement morphemes that occur with verbs Independent particles are fully independent words They include prepositional modal and exclamatory morphemes numerals and one class of pronouns The table below shows the pronominal morphemes of Tubatulabal Like nouns pronouns distinguish between three cases subject object and possessive Pronouns do not make a distinction between absolute and relative entities Different forms exist for first second and third person entities Second and third person forms distinguish only singular and plural numbers but first person forms distinguish between singular dual inclusive dual exclusive and plural numbers All pronouns may be expressed bu conjunctive particles The subject pronouns are unique in that they can also be expressed by an independent particle 7 Subject Object Possessiveindependent conjunctive conjunctive conjunctive1sg nik ɡi ni nɨʔɨŋ1d inc iŋɡila ɡila 1d exc iŋɡilaʔaŋ ɡilaʔaŋ d ʒijaʔaŋ t ʃ1pl iŋɡiluːt s ɡiluːt s d ziː t s2sg imbi bi diŋ iŋ2pl imbuːmu buːmu dulu ulu3sg in d za n3pl inda da tɨpɨ pThe first person subject conjunctive forms have special allomorphs when they occur with the exhortative suffix ma 1sg 1d inc la1d inc laʔaŋ1pl luːt sThe third person conjunctive form is usually null but it is expressed by d za after the exhortative or permissive suffixes The suffix often undergoes syncope and devoicing yielding t s The second person conjunctive plural subject form may also syncopate and medial vowel then shortens as well bum The first person conjunctive singular subject form may also syncopate triggering devoicing but no irregular phonology the suffix then has the form k Subject pronouns typically lean on verbs if conjunctive and correspond to grammatical subject iwikkːɨki I discarded it with devoicing anabaːhaʃta they can throw it with metathesis of the components of the affricate and a change of s gt ʃ Object pronouns also lean on verbs and indicate any non possessive oblique function including transitive objects ditransitive objects or benefactives objects of imperative verbs and subjects of subordinate verbs if not equivalent to the subject of the matrix verb Possessive pronouns typically lean on the possessum haniːnɨʔɨŋ my house ʃɔːɔjin his wife Syntax editWord order is generally flexible According to Voegelin 1935 harv error no target CITEREFVoegelin1935 help Word order in general is stylistic rather than obligatory p 185 Orthography editTranscriptions in this article follow the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA Much published material concerning Tubatulabal uses the Americanist orthography In addition the most important linguistic work on Tubatulabal the original grammatical description of the language Voegelin 1935 harv error no target CITEREFVoegelin1935 help uses a somewhat different orthography Voegelin writes ɨ as i and ɔ as o He also writes ʃ as c t ʃ as tc ʔ as d ʒ as dz and j as y He also uses a number of special symbols for vocalic allomorphs i is an allomorph of i m is an allomorph of u o is an allomorph of o IPA ɔ and ŏ is an allomorph of both a and o The letter u in the name Tubatulabal represents the central unrounded vowel ɨ Notes edit Pahka anil Tubatulabal Text Project Home web csulb edu Retrieved 2023 05 05 See Jensen for discussion of the role of 2 6 Gemination in Tubatulabal phonology 1973 61 et seq See Jensen for discussion of the arbitrary behavior of glottal stops in stress assignment The glottal stop which is not otherwise counted as a mora is counted as a mora for the purpose of stress assignment Jensen 1973 pp 76 76 The morphological differences between noun classes can probably be traced back to a simple case of allomorphy At a certain point in the history of an ancestor of Tubatulabal all class A nouns ended in a vowel while all class B nouns ended in a consonant Class C forms are few in number and have a more complicated origin The absolute suffix in this language was t which lenited intervocalically leading to l in Tubatulabal class A nouns compare cognates ʈ ʂ in Serrano r in Tongva See Voegelin 1935 harv error no target CITEREFVoegelin1935 help which and Manaster Ramer 1992 Relative nouns typically require a suffix indicating the possessor or entity to which the suffixed noun is related Forms in this table are those for a third person singular possessor n The exception is suus forms which do not allow a possessive suffix since they are inherently possessed by related to the grammatical subject of the clause The ejus forms are not given in Voegelin 1935 harv error no target CITEREFVoegelin1935 help which The forms in this table are hypothesized on the basis of the suffix charts that Voegelin gives With a few exceptions all other forms in this table are taken directly from Voegelin 1935 harv error no target CITEREFVoegelin1935 help which The few forms that are hypothetical are marked with Cells marked with are phonologically null Cells marked with are forms missing without explanation in Voegelin 1935 harv error no target CITEREFVoegelin1935 help Bibliography editAion Nora 2003 Selected Topics in Nootka and Tubatulabal Phonology PhD dissertation City University of New York Arvidson Lucy Alaawich Our Language First Book of Words in the Tubatulabal Language of Southern California Crowhurst Megan 1991 Demorification in Tubatulabal Evidence from Initial Reduplication and Stress NELS 21 49 63 Gifford Edward Winslow 1917 Tubatulabal and Kawaiisu kinship terms Berkeley CA University of California Press Retrieved 2012 08 26 Heath Jeffrey 1981 Tubatulabal Phonology Harvard Studies in Phonology 2 Jensen James R 1973 Stress and the Verbal Phonology of Tubatulabal PhD dissertation Indiana University Manaster Ramer Alexis 1992 Proto Uto Aztecan Phonology Evidence From Tubatulabal Noun Morphophonemics International Journal of American Linguistics 58 4 436 446 doi 10 1086 ijal 58 4 3519778 JSTOR 3519778 S2CID 147964960 Voegelin Charles F 1935a Tubatulabal Grammar University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology 34 55 190 Voegelin Charles F 1935b Tubatulabal Texts University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology 34 191 246 Voegelin Charles F 1958 Working Dictionary of Tubatulabal International Journal of American Linguistics 24 3 221 228 doi 10 1086 464459 S2CID 145758965 External links editTubatulabal language overview at the Survey of California and Other Indian Languages Tubatulabal Language at native languages org Recordings of Tubatulabal recorded by JP Harrington OLAC resources in and about the Tubatulabal language Audio recordings of word lists in Tubatulabal from the Northern Uto Aztecan Collection of Megan Crowhurst at AILLA Tubatulabal California Language Archives Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Tubatulabal language amp oldid 1165805511, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.