fbpx
Wikipedia

Stress (linguistics)

In linguistics, and particularly phonology, stress or accent is the relative emphasis or prominence given to a certain syllable in a word or to a certain word in a phrase or sentence. That emphasis is typically caused by such properties as increased loudness and vowel length, full articulation of the vowel, and changes in tone.[1][2] The terms stress and accent are often used synonymously in that context but are sometimes distinguished. For example, when emphasis is produced through pitch alone, it is called pitch accent, and when produced through length alone, it is called quantitative accent.[3] When caused by a combination of various intensified properties, it is called stress accent or dynamic accent; English uses what is called variable stress accent.

Primary stress
ˈ◌
IPA Number501
Encoding
Entity (decimal)ˈ
Unicode (hex)U+02C8
Secondary stress
ˌ◌
IPA Number502
Encoding
Entity (decimal)​ˌ
Unicode (hex) U+02CC

Since stress can be realised through a wide range of phonetic properties, such as loudness, vowel length, and pitch (which are also used for other linguistic functions), it is difficult to define stress solely phonetically.

The stress placed on syllables within words is called word stress. Some languages have fixed stress, meaning that the stress on virtually any multisyllable word falls on a particular syllable, such as the penultimate (e.g. Polish) or the first (e.g. Finnish). Other languages, like English and Russian, have lexical stress, where the position of stress in a word is not predictable in that way but lexically encoded. Sometimes more than one level of stress, such as primary stress and secondary stress, may be identified.

Stress is not necessarily a feature of all languages: some, such as French and Mandarin, are sometimes analyzed as lacking lexical stress entirely.

The stress placed on words within sentences is called sentence stress or prosodic stress. That is one of the three components of prosody, along with rhythm and intonation. It includes phrasal stress (the default emphasis of certain words within phrases or clauses), and contrastive stress (used to highlight an item, a word or part of a word, that is given particular focus).

Phonetic realization

There are various ways in which stress manifests itself in the speech stream, and they depend to some extent on which language is being spoken. Stressed syllables are often louder than non-stressed syllables, and they may have a higher or lower pitch. They may also sometimes be pronounced longer. There are sometimes differences in place or manner of articulation. In particular, vowels in unstressed syllables may have a more central (or "neutral") articulation, and those in stressed syllables have a more peripheral articulation. Stress may be realized to varying degrees on different words in a sentence; sometimes, the difference is minimal between the acoustic signals of stressed and those of unstressed syllables.

Those particular distinguishing features of stress, or types of prominence in which particular features are dominant, are sometimes referred to as particular types of accent: dynamic accent in the case of loudness, pitch accent in the case of pitch (although that term usually has more specialized meanings), quantitative accent in the case of length,[3] and qualitative accent in the case of differences in articulation. They can be compared to the various types of accent in music theory. In some contexts, the term stress or stress accent specifically means dynamic accent (or as an antonym to pitch accent in its various meanings).

A prominent syllable or word is said to be accented or tonic; the latter term does not imply that it carries phonemic tone. Other syllables or words are said to be unaccented or atonic. Syllables are frequently said to be in pretonic or post-tonic position, and certain phonological rules apply specifically to such positions. For instance, in American English, /t/ and /d/ are flapped in post-tonic position.

In Mandarin Chinese, which is a tonal language, stressed syllables have been found to have tones that are realized with a relatively large swing in fundamental frequency, and unstressed syllables typically have smaller swings.[4] (See also Stress in Standard Chinese.)

Stressed syllables are often perceived as being more forceful than non-stressed syllables.

Word stress

Word stress, or sometimes lexical stress, is the stress placed on a given syllable in a word. The position of word stress in a word may depend on certain general rules applicable in the language or dialect in question, but in other languages, it must be learned for each word, as it is largely unpredictable. In some cases, classes of words in a language differ in their stress properties; for example, loanwords into a language with fixed stress may preserve stress placement from the source language, or the special pattern for Turkish placenames.

Non-phonemic stress

In some languages, the placement of stress can be determined by rules. It is thus not a phonemic property of the word, because it can always be predicted by applying the rules.

Languages in which the position of the stress can usually be predicted by a simple rule are said to have fixed stress. For example, in Czech, Finnish, Icelandic and Hungarian, the stress almost always comes on the first syllable of a word. In Armenian the stress is on the last syllable of a word.[5] In Quechua, Esperanto, and Polish, the stress is almost always on the penult (second-last syllable). In Macedonian, it is on the antepenult (third-last syllable).

Other languages have stress placed on different syllables but in a predictable way, as in Classical Arabic and Latin, where stress is conditioned by the structure of particular syllables. They are said to have a regular stress rule.

Statements about the position of stress are sometimes affected by the fact that when a word is spoken in isolation, prosodic factors (see below) come into play, which do not apply when the word is spoken normally within a sentence. French words are sometimes said to be stressed on the final syllable, but that can be attributed to the prosodic stress that is placed on the last syllable (unless it is a schwa, when stress is placed on the second-last syllable) of any string of words in that language. Thus, it is on the last syllable of a word analyzed in isolation. The situation is similar in Standard Chinese. French (some authors add Chinese[6]) can be considered to have no real lexical stress.

Phonemic stress

Languages such as English, Russian, Italian, Portuguese and Spanish, in which the position of stress in a word is not fully predictable, are said to have phonemic stress. Stress in these languages is usually truly lexical and must be memorized as part of the pronunciation of an individual word. In some languages, such as Spanish, Portuguese, Lakota and, to some extent, Italian, stress is even represented in writing using diacritical marks, for example in the Spanish words célebre and celebré. Sometimes, stress is fixed for all forms of a particular word, or it can fall on different syllables in different inflections of the same word.

In such languages with phonemic stress, the position of stress can serve to distinguish otherwise identical words. For example, the English words insight (/ˈɪnst/) and incite (/ɪnˈst/) are distinguished in pronunciation only by the fact that the stress falls on the first syllable in the former and on the second syllable in the latter. Examples from other languages include German Tenor ([ˈteːnoːɐ̯] "gist of message" vs. [teˈnoːɐ̯] "tenor voice"); and Italian ancora ([ˈaŋkora] "anchor" vs. [aŋˈkoːra] "more, still, yet, again").

In many languages with lexical stress, it is connected with alternations in vowels and/or consonants, which means that vowel quality differs by whether vowels are stressed or unstressed. There may also be limitations on certain phonemes in the language in which stress determines whether they are allowed to occur in a particular syllable or not. That is the case with most examples in English and occurs systematically in Russian, such as за́мок ([ˈzamək], "castle") vs. замо́к ([zɐˈmok], "lock"); and in Portuguese, such as the triplet sábia ([ˈsabjɐ], "wise woman"), sabia ([sɐˈbiɐ], "knew"), sabiá ([sɐˈbja], "thrush").

Dialects of the same language may have different stress placement. For instance, the English word laboratory is stressed on the second syllable in British English (labóratory often pronounced "labóratry", the second o being silent), but the first syllable in American English, with a secondary stress on the "tor' syllable (láboratory often pronounced "lábratory"). The Spanish word video is stressed on the first syllable in Spain (vídeo) but on the second syllable in the Americas (video). The Portuguese words for Madagascar and the continent Oceania are stressed on the third syllable in European Portuguese (Madagáscar and Oceânia), but on the fourth syllable in Brazilian Portuguese (Madagascar and Oceania).

Compounds

With very few exceptions, English compound words are stressed on their first component. Even the exceptions, such as mankínd,[7] are instead often stressed on the first component by some people or in some kinds of English.[8] The same components as those of a compound word are sometimes used in a descriptive phrase with a different meaning and with stress on both words, but that descriptive phrase is then not usually considered a compound: bláck bírd (any bird that is black) and bláckbird (a specific bird species) and páper bág (a bag made of paper) and páper bag (very rarely used for a bag for carrying newspapers but is often also used for a bag made of paper).[9]

Levels of stress

Some languages are described as having both primary stress and secondary stress. A syllable with secondary stress is stressed relative to unstressed syllables but not as strongly as a syllable with primary stress. As with primary stress, the position of secondary stress may be more or less predictable depending on language. In English, it is not fully predictable, but the different secondary stress of the words organization and accumulation (on the first and second syllable, respectively) is predictable due to the same stress of the verbs órganize and accúmulate. In some analyses, for example the one found in Chomsky and Halle's The Sound Pattern of English, English has been described as having four levels of stress: primary, secondary, tertiary, and quaternary, but the treatments often disagree with one another.

Peter Ladefoged and other phoneticians have noted that it is possible to describe English with only one degree of stress, as long as prosody is recognized and unstressed syllables are phonemically distinguished for vowel reduction.[10] They find that the multiple levels posited for English, whether primary–secondary or primary–secondary–tertiary, are not phonetic stress (let alone phonemic), and that the supposed secondary/tertiary stress is not characterized by the increase in respiratory activity associated with primary/secondary stress in English and other languages. (For further detail see Stress and vowel reduction in English.)

Prosodic stress

Extra stress
ˈˈ◌

Prosodic stress, or sentence stress, refers to stress patterns that apply at a higher level than the individual word – namely within a prosodic unit. It may involve a certain natural stress pattern characteristic of a given language, but may also involve the placing of emphasis on particular words because of their relative importance (contrastive stress).

An example of a natural prosodic stress pattern is that described for French above; stress is placed on the final syllable of a string of words (or if that is a schwa, the next-to-final syllable). A similar pattern is found in English (see § Levels of stress above): the traditional distinction between (lexical) primary and secondary stress is replaced partly by a prosodic rule stating that the final stressed syllable in a phrase is given additional stress. (A word spoken alone becomes such a phrase, hence such prosodic stress may appear to be lexical if the pronunciation of words is analyzed in a standalone context rather than within phrases.)

Another type of prosodic stress pattern is quantity sensitivity – in some languages additional stress tends to be placed on syllables that are longer (moraically heavy).

Prosodic stress is also often used pragmatically to emphasize (focus attention on) particular words or the ideas associated with them. Doing this can change or clarify the meaning of a sentence; for example:

I didn't take the test yesterday. (Somebody else did.)
I didn't take the test yesterday. (I did not take it.)
I didn't take the test yesterday. (I did something else with it.)
I didn't take the test yesterday. (I took one of several. or I didn't take the specific test that would have been implied.)
I didn't take the test yesterday. (I took something else.)
I didn't take the test yesterday. (I took it some other day.)

As in the examples above, stress is normally transcribed as italics in printed text or underlining in handwriting.

In English, stress is most dramatically realized on focused or accented words. For instance, consider the dialogue

"Is it brunch tomorrow?"
"No, it's dinner tomorrow."

In it, the stress-related acoustic differences between the syllables of "tomorrow" would be small compared to the differences between the syllables of "dinner", the emphasized word. In these emphasized words, stressed syllables such as "din" in "dinner" are louder and longer.[11][12][13] They may also have a different fundamental frequency, or other properties.

The main stress within a sentence, often found on the last stressed word, is called the nuclear stress.[14]

Stress and vowel reduction

In many languages, such as Russian and English, vowel reduction may occur when a vowel changes from a stressed to an unstressed position. In English, unstressed vowels may reduce to schwa-like vowels, though the details vary with dialect (see stress and vowel reduction in English). The effect may be dependent on lexical stress (for example, the unstressed first syllable of the word photographer contains a schwa /fəˈtɒɡrəfər/, whereas the stressed first syllable of photograph does not /ˈfoʊtəˌgræf -grɑːf/), or on prosodic stress (for example, the word of is pronounced with a schwa when it is unstressed within a sentence, but not when it is stressed).

Many other languages, such as Finnish and the mainstream dialects of Spanish, do not have unstressed vowel reduction; in these languages vowels in unstressed syllables have nearly the same quality as those in stressed syllables.

Stress and rhythm

Some languages, such as English, are said to be stress-timed languages; that is, stressed syllables appear at a roughly constant rate and non-stressed syllables are shortened to accommodate that, which contrasts with languages that have syllable timing (e.g. Spanish) or mora timing (e.g. Japanese), whose syllables or moras are spoken at a roughly constant rate regardless of stress. For details, see isochrony.

Historical effects

It is common for stressed and unstressed syllables to behave differently as a language evolves. For example, in the Romance languages, the original Latin short vowels /e/ and /o/ have often become diphthongs when stressed. Since stress takes part in verb conjugation, that has produced verbs with vowel alternation in the Romance languages. For example, the Spanish verb volver (to return, come back) has the form volví in the past tense but vuelvo in the present tense (see Spanish irregular verbs). Italian shows the same phenomenon but with /o/ alternating with /uo/ instead. That behavior is not confined to verbs; note for example Spanish viento "wind" from Latin ventum, or Italian fuoco "fire" from Latin focum.

Stress "deafness"

An operational definition of word stress may be provided by the stress "deafness" paradigm.[15][16] The idea is that if listeners perform poorly on reproducing the presentation order of series of stimuli that minimally differ in the position of phonetic prominence (e.g. [númi]/[numí]), the language does not have word stress. The task involves a reproduction of the order of stimuli as a sequence of key strokes, whereby key "1" is associated with one stress location (e.g. [númi]) and key "2" with the other (e.g. [numí]). A trial may be from 2 to 6 stimuli in length. Thus, the order [númi-númi-numí-númi] is to be reproduced as "1121". It was found that listeners whose native language was French performed significantly worse than Spanish listeners in reproducing the stress patterns by key strokes. The explanation is that Spanish has lexically contrastive stress, as evidenced by the minimal pairs like tópo ("mole") and topó ("[he/she/it] met"), while in French, stress does not convey lexical information and there is no equivalent of stress minimal pairs as in Spanish.

An important case of stress "deafness" relates to Persian.[16] The language has generally been described as having contrastive word stress or accent as evidenced by numerous stem and stem-clitic minimal pairs such as /mɒhi/ [mɒ.hí] ("fish") and /mɒh-i/ [mɒ́.hi] ("some month"). The authors argue that the reason that Persian listeners are stress "deaf" is that their accent locations arise postlexically. Persian thus lacks stress in the strict sense.

Stress "deafness" has been studied for a number of languages, such as Polish[17] or French learners of Spanish.[18]

Spelling and notation for stress

The orthographies of some languages include devices for indicating the position of lexical stress. Some examples are listed below:

  • In Modern Greek, all polysyllables are written with an acute accent (´) over the vowel of the stressed syllable. (The acute accent is also used on some monosyllables in order to distinguish homographs, as in η ('the') and ή ('or'); here the stress of the two words is the same.)
  • In Spanish orthography, stress may be written explicitly with a single acute accent on a vowel. Stressed antepenultimate syllables are always written with that accent mark, as in árabe. If the last syllable is stressed, the accent mark is used if the word ends in the letters n, s, or a vowel, as in está. If the penultimate syllable is stressed, the accent is used if the word ends in any other letter, as in cárcel. That is, if a word is written without an accent mark, the stress is on the penult if the last letter is a vowel, n, or s, but on the final syllable if the word ends in any other letter. However, as in Greek, the acute accent is also used for some words to distinguish various syntactical uses (e.g. 'tea' vs. te a form of the pronoun 'you'; dónde 'where' as a pronoun or wh-complement, donde 'where' as an adverb). For more information, see Stress in Spanish.
  • In Portuguese, stress is sometimes indicated explicitly with an acute accent (for i, u, and open a, e, o), or circumflex (for close a, e, o). The orthography has an extensive set of rules that describe the placement of diacritics, based on the position of the stressed syllable and the surrounding letters.
  • In Italian, the grave accent is needed in words ending with an accented vowel, e.g. città, 'city', and in some monosyllabic words that might otherwise be confused with other words, like ('there') and la ('the'). It is optional for it to be written on any vowel if there is a possibility of misunderstanding, such as condomìni ('condominiums') and condòmini ('joint owners'). See Italian alphabet § Diacritics. (In this particular case, a frequent one in which diacritics present themselves, the difference of accents is caused by the fall of the second "i" from Latin in Italian, typical of the genitive, in the first noun (con/domìnìi/, meaning "of the owner"); while the second was derived from the nominative (con/dòmini/, meaning simply "owners").

Though not part of normal orthography, a number of devices exist that are used by linguists and others to indicate the position of stress (and syllabification in some cases) when it is desirable to do so. Some of these are listed here.

  • Most commonly, the stress mark is placed before the beginning of the stressed syllable, where a syllable is definable. However, it is occasionally placed immediately before the vowel.[19] In the International Phonetic Alphabet (IPA), primary stress is indicated by a high vertical line (primary stress mark: ˈ) before the stressed element, secondary stress by a low vertical line (secondary stress mark: ˌ). For example, [sɪˌlæbəfɪˈkeɪʃən] or /sɪˌlæbəfɪˈkeɪʃən/. Extra stress can be indicated by doubling the symbol: ˈˈ◌.
  • Linguists frequently mark primary stress with an acute accent over the vowel, and secondary stress by a grave accent. Example: [sɪlæ̀bəfɪkéɪʃən] or /sɪlæ̀bəfɪkéɪʃən/. That has the advantage of not requiring a decision about syllable boundaries.
  • In English dictionaries that show pronunciation by respelling, stress is typically marked with a prime mark placed after the stressed syllable: /si-lab′-ə-fi-kay′-shən/.
  • In ad hoc pronunciation guides, stress is often indicated using a combination of bold text and capital letters. For example, si-lab-if-i-KAY-shun or si-LAB-if-i-KAY-shun
  • In Russian, Belarusian, and Ukrainian dictionaries, stress is indicated with marks called znaki udareniya (знаки ударения, 'stress marks'). Primary stress is indicated with an acute accent (´) on a syllable's vowel (example: вимовля́ння).[20][21] Secondary stress may be unmarked or marked with a grave accent: о̀колозе́мный. If the acute accent sign is unavailable for technical reasons, stress can be marked by making the vowel capitalized or italic.[22] In general texts, stress marks are rare, typically used either when required for disambiguation of homographs (compare в больши́х количествах 'in great quantities', and в бо́льших количествах 'in greater quantities'), or in rare words and names that are likely to be mispronounced. Materials for foreign learners may have stress marks throughout the text.[20]
  • In Dutch, ad hoc indication of stress is usually marked by an acute accent on the vowel (or, in the case of a diphthong or double vowel, the first two vowels) of the stressed syllable. Compare achterúítgang ('deterioration') and áchteruitgang ('rear exit').
  • In Biblical Hebrew, a complex system of cantillation marks is used to mark stress, as well as verse syntax and the melody according to which the verse is chanted in ceremonial Bible reading. In Modern Hebrew, there is no standardized way to mark the stress. Most often, the cantillation mark oleh (part of oleh ve-yored), which looks like a left-pointing arrow above the consonant of the stressed syllable, for example ב֫וקר bóqer ('morning') as opposed to בוק֫ר boqér ('cowboy'). That mark is usually used in books by the Academy of the Hebrew Language and is available on the standard Hebrew keyboard at AltGr-6. In some books, other marks, such as meteg, are used.[23]

See also

References

  1. ^ Fry, D.B. (1955). "Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress". Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 27 (4): 765–768. Bibcode:1955ASAJ...27..765F. doi:10.1121/1.1908022.
  2. ^ Fry, D.B. (1958). "Experiments in the perception of stress". Language and Speech. 1 (2): 126–152. doi:10.1177/002383095800100207. S2CID 141158933.
  3. ^ a b Monrad-Krohn, G. H. (1947). "The prosodic quality of speech and its disorders (a brief survey from a neurologist's point of view)". Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 22 (3–4): 255–269. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1947.tb08246.x. S2CID 146712090.
  4. ^ Kochanski, Greg; Shih, Chilin; Jing, Hongyan (2003). "Quantitative measurement of prosodic strength in Mandarin". Speech Communication. 41 (4): 625–645. doi:10.1016/S0167-6393(03)00100-6.
  5. ^ Mirakyan, Norayr (2016). "The Implications of Prosodic Differences Between English and Armenian" (PDF). Collection of Scientific Articles of YSU SSS. YSU Press. 1.3 (13): 91–96.
  6. ^ San Duanmu (2000). The Phonology of Standard Chinese. Oxford University Press. p. 134.
  7. ^ mankind in the Collins English Dictionary
  8. ^ mankind in the American Heritage Dictionary
  9. ^ "paper bag" in the Collins English Dictionary
  10. ^ Ladefoged (1975 etc.) A course in phonetics § 5.4; (1980) Preliminaries to linguistic phonetics p 83
  11. ^ Beckman, Mary E. (1986). Stress and Non-Stress Accent. Dordrecht: Foris. ISBN 90-6765-243-1.
  12. ^ R. Silipo and S. Greenberg, Automatic Transcription of Prosodic Stress for Spontaneous English Discourse, Proceedings of the XIVth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS99), San Francisco, CA, August 1999, pages 2351–2354
  13. ^ Kochanski, G.; Grabe, E.; Coleman, J.; Rosner, B. (2005). "Loudness predicts prominence: Fundamental frequency lends little". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 118 (2): 1038–1054. Bibcode:2005ASAJ..118.1038K. doi:10.1121/1.1923349. PMID 16158659. S2CID 405045.
  14. ^ Roca, Iggy (1992). Thematic Structure: Its Role in Grammar. Walter de Gruyter. p. 80.
  15. ^ Dupoux, Emmanuel; Peperkamp, Sharon; Sebastián-Gallés, Núria (2001). "A robust method to study stress "deafness"". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 110 (3): 1606–1618. Bibcode:2001ASAJ..110.1606D. doi:10.1121/1.1380437. PMID 11572370.
  16. ^ a b Rahmani, Hamed; Rietveld, Toni; Gussenhoven, Carlos (2015-12-07). "Stress "Deafness" Reveals Absence of Lexical Marking of Stress or Tone in the Adult Grammar". PLOS ONE. 10 (12): e0143968. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1043968R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0143968. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4671725. PMID 26642328.
  17. ^ 3:439, 2012, 1-15., Ulrike; Knaus, Johannes; Orzechowska, Paula; Wiese, Richard (2012). "Stress 'deafness' in a language with fixed word stress: an ERP study on Polish". Frontiers in Psychology. 3: 439. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00439. PMC 3485581. PMID 23125839.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link) CS1 maint: numeric names: authors list (link)
  18. ^ Dupoux, Emmanuel; Sebastián-Gallés, N; Navarrete, E; Peperkamp, Sharon (2008). "Persistent stress 'deafness': The case of French learners of Spanish". Cognition. 106 (2): 682–706. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.001. hdl:11577/2714082. PMID 17592731. S2CID 2632741.
  19. ^ Payne, Elinor M. (2005). "Phonetic variation in Italian consonant gemination". Journal of the International Phonetic Association. 35 (2): 153–181. doi:10.1017/S0025100305002240. S2CID 144935892.
  20. ^ a b Лопатин, Владимир Владимирович, ed. (2009). § 116. Знак ударения. Правила русской орфографии и пунктуации. Полный академический справочник (in Russian). Эксмо. ISBN 978-5-699-18553-5.
  21. ^ Some pre-revolutionary dictionaries, e.g. Dahl's Explanatory Dictionary, marked stress with an apostrophe just after the vowel (example: гла'сная). See: Dahl, Vladimir Ivanovich (1903). Boduen de Kurtene, Ivan Aleksandrovich (ed.). Толко́вый слова́рь живо́го великору́сского языка́ [Explanatory Dictionary of the Living Great Russian Language] (in Russian) (3rd ed.). Saint Petersburg: M.O. Wolf. p. 4.
  22. ^ Каплунов, Денис (2015). Бизнес-копирайтинг: Как писать серьезные тексты для серьезных людей (in Russian). p. 389. ISBN 978-5-000-57471-3.
  23. ^ Aharoni, Amir (2020-12-02). "אז איך נציין את מקום הטעם". הזירה הלשונית – רוביק רוזנטל. Retrieved 2021-11-25.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: url-status (link)

External links

  • "Feet and Metrical Stress", The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology
  • "Word stress in English: Six Basic Rules", Linguapress
  • Word Stress Rules: A Guide to Word and Sentence Stress Rules for English Learners and Teachers, based on affixation

stress, linguistics, confused, with, accent, sociolinguistics, other, uses, stress, linguistics, particularly, phonology, stress, accent, relative, emphasis, prominence, given, certain, syllable, word, certain, word, phrase, sentence, that, emphasis, typically. Not to be confused with Accent sociolinguistics For other uses see Stress In linguistics and particularly phonology stress or accent is the relative emphasis or prominence given to a certain syllable in a word or to a certain word in a phrase or sentence That emphasis is typically caused by such properties as increased loudness and vowel length full articulation of the vowel and changes in tone 1 2 The terms stress and accent are often used synonymously in that context but are sometimes distinguished For example when emphasis is produced through pitch alone it is called pitch accent and when produced through length alone it is called quantitative accent 3 When caused by a combination of various intensified properties it is called stress accent or dynamic accent English uses what is called variable stress accent Primary stressˈ IPA Number501EncodingEntity decimal amp 712 Unicode hex U 02C8Secondary stressˌ IPA Number502EncodingEntity decimal amp 716 Unicode hex U 02CCThis article contains phonetic transcriptions in the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA For an introductory guide on IPA symbols see Help IPA For the distinction between and see IPA Brackets and transcription delimiters Since stress can be realised through a wide range of phonetic properties such as loudness vowel length and pitch which are also used for other linguistic functions it is difficult to define stress solely phonetically The stress placed on syllables within words is called word stress Some languages have fixed stress meaning that the stress on virtually any multisyllable word falls on a particular syllable such as the penultimate e g Polish or the first e g Finnish Other languages like English and Russian have lexical stress where the position of stress in a word is not predictable in that way but lexically encoded Sometimes more than one level of stress such as primary stress and secondary stress may be identified Stress is not necessarily a feature of all languages some such as French and Mandarin are sometimes analyzed as lacking lexical stress entirely The stress placed on words within sentences is called sentence stress or prosodic stress That is one of the three components of prosody along with rhythm and intonation It includes phrasal stress the default emphasis of certain words within phrases or clauses and contrastive stress used to highlight an item a word or part of a word that is given particular focus Contents 1 Phonetic realization 2 Word stress 2 1 Non phonemic stress 2 2 Phonemic stress 2 3 Compounds 2 4 Levels of stress 3 Prosodic stress 4 Stress and vowel reduction 5 Stress and rhythm 6 Historical effects 7 Stress deafness 8 Spelling and notation for stress 9 See also 10 References 11 External linksPhonetic realization EditThere are various ways in which stress manifests itself in the speech stream and they depend to some extent on which language is being spoken Stressed syllables are often louder than non stressed syllables and they may have a higher or lower pitch They may also sometimes be pronounced longer There are sometimes differences in place or manner of articulation In particular vowels in unstressed syllables may have a more central or neutral articulation and those in stressed syllables have a more peripheral articulation Stress may be realized to varying degrees on different words in a sentence sometimes the difference is minimal between the acoustic signals of stressed and those of unstressed syllables Those particular distinguishing features of stress or types of prominence in which particular features are dominant are sometimes referred to as particular types of accent dynamic accent in the case of loudness pitch accent in the case of pitch although that term usually has more specialized meanings quantitative accent in the case of length 3 and qualitative accent in the case of differences in articulation They can be compared to the various types of accent in music theory In some contexts the term stress or stress accent specifically means dynamic accent or as an antonym to pitch accent in its various meanings A prominent syllable or word is said to be accented or tonic the latter term does not imply that it carries phonemic tone Other syllables or words are said to be unaccented or atonic Syllables are frequently said to be in pretonic or post tonic position and certain phonological rules apply specifically to such positions For instance in American English t and d are flapped in post tonic position In Mandarin Chinese which is a tonal language stressed syllables have been found to have tones that are realized with a relatively large swing in fundamental frequency and unstressed syllables typically have smaller swings 4 See also Stress in Standard Chinese Stressed syllables are often perceived as being more forceful than non stressed syllables Word stress EditWord stress or sometimes lexical stress is the stress placed on a given syllable in a word The position of word stress in a word may depend on certain general rules applicable in the language or dialect in question but in other languages it must be learned for each word as it is largely unpredictable In some cases classes of words in a language differ in their stress properties for example loanwords into a language with fixed stress may preserve stress placement from the source language or the special pattern for Turkish placenames Non phonemic stress Edit In some languages the placement of stress can be determined by rules It is thus not a phonemic property of the word because it can always be predicted by applying the rules Languages in which the position of the stress can usually be predicted by a simple rule are said to have fixed stress For example in Czech Finnish Icelandic and Hungarian the stress almost always comes on the first syllable of a word In Armenian the stress is on the last syllable of a word 5 In Quechua Esperanto and Polish the stress is almost always on the penult second last syllable In Macedonian it is on the antepenult third last syllable Other languages have stress placed on different syllables but in a predictable way as in Classical Arabic and Latin where stress is conditioned by the structure of particular syllables They are said to have a regular stress rule Statements about the position of stress are sometimes affected by the fact that when a word is spoken in isolation prosodic factors see below come into play which do not apply when the word is spoken normally within a sentence French words are sometimes said to be stressed on the final syllable but that can be attributed to the prosodic stress that is placed on the last syllable unless it is a schwa when stress is placed on the second last syllable of any string of words in that language Thus it is on the last syllable of a word analyzed in isolation The situation is similar in Standard Chinese French some authors add Chinese 6 can be considered to have no real lexical stress Phonemic stress Edit Languages such as English Russian Italian Portuguese and Spanish in which the position of stress in a word is not fully predictable are said to have phonemic stress Stress in these languages is usually truly lexical and must be memorized as part of the pronunciation of an individual word In some languages such as Spanish Portuguese Lakota and to some extent Italian stress is even represented in writing using diacritical marks for example in the Spanish words celebre and celebre Sometimes stress is fixed for all forms of a particular word or it can fall on different syllables in different inflections of the same word In such languages with phonemic stress the position of stress can serve to distinguish otherwise identical words For example the English words insight ˈ ɪ n s aɪ t and incite ɪ n ˈ s aɪ t are distinguished in pronunciation only by the fact that the stress falls on the first syllable in the former and on the second syllable in the latter Examples from other languages include German Tenor ˈteːnoːɐ gist of message vs teˈnoːɐ tenor voice and Italian ancora ˈaŋkora anchor vs aŋˈkoːra more still yet again In many languages with lexical stress it is connected with alternations in vowels and or consonants which means that vowel quality differs by whether vowels are stressed or unstressed There may also be limitations on certain phonemes in the language in which stress determines whether they are allowed to occur in a particular syllable or not That is the case with most examples in English and occurs systematically in Russian such as za mok ˈzamek castle vs zamo k zɐˈmok lock and in Portuguese such as the triplet sabia ˈsabjɐ wise woman sabia sɐˈbiɐ knew sabia sɐˈbja thrush Dialects of the same language may have different stress placement For instance the English word laboratory is stressed on the second syllable in British English laboratory often pronounced laboratry the second o being silent but the first syllable in American English with a secondary stress on the tor syllable laboratory often pronounced labratory The Spanish word video is stressed on the first syllable in Spain video but on the second syllable in the Americas video The Portuguese words for Madagascar and the continent Oceania are stressed on the third syllable in European Portuguese Madagascar and Oceania but on the fourth syllable in Brazilian Portuguese Madagascar and Oceania Compounds Edit With very few exceptions English compound words are stressed on their first component Even the exceptions such as mankind 7 are instead often stressed on the first component by some people or in some kinds of English 8 The same components as those of a compound word are sometimes used in a descriptive phrase with a different meaning and with stress on both words but that descriptive phrase is then not usually considered a compound black bird any bird that is black and blackbird a specific bird species and paper bag a bag made of paper and paper bag very rarely used for a bag for carrying newspapers but is often also used for a bag made of paper 9 Levels of stress Edit Further information Secondary stress Some languages are described as having both primary stress and secondary stress A syllable with secondary stress is stressed relative to unstressed syllables but not as strongly as a syllable with primary stress As with primary stress the position of secondary stress may be more or less predictable depending on language In English it is not fully predictable but the different secondary stress of the words organization and accumulation on the first and second syllable respectively is predictable due to the same stress of the verbs organize and accumulate In some analyses for example the one found in Chomsky and Halle s The Sound Pattern of English English has been described as having four levels of stress primary secondary tertiary and quaternary but the treatments often disagree with one another Peter Ladefoged and other phoneticians have noted that it is possible to describe English with only one degree of stress as long as prosody is recognized and unstressed syllables are phonemically distinguished for vowel reduction 10 They find that the multiple levels posited for English whether primary secondary or primary secondary tertiary are not phonetic stress let alone phonemic and that the supposed secondary tertiary stress is not characterized by the increase in respiratory activity associated with primary secondary stress in English and other languages For further detail see Stress and vowel reduction in English Prosodic stress EditExtra stressˈˈ Prosodic stress or sentence stress refers to stress patterns that apply at a higher level than the individual word namely within a prosodic unit It may involve a certain natural stress pattern characteristic of a given language but may also involve the placing of emphasis on particular words because of their relative importance contrastive stress An example of a natural prosodic stress pattern is that described for French above stress is placed on the final syllable of a string of words or if that is a schwa the next to final syllable A similar pattern is found in English see Levels of stress above the traditional distinction between lexical primary and secondary stress is replaced partly by a prosodic rule stating that the final stressed syllable in a phrase is given additional stress A word spoken alone becomes such a phrase hence such prosodic stress may appear to be lexical if the pronunciation of words is analyzed in a standalone context rather than within phrases Another type of prosodic stress pattern is quantity sensitivity in some languages additional stress tends to be placed on syllables that are longer moraically heavy Prosodic stress is also often used pragmatically to emphasize focus attention on particular words or the ideas associated with them Doing this can change or clarify the meaning of a sentence for example I didn t take the test yesterday Somebody else did I didn t take the test yesterday I did not take it I didn t take the test yesterday I did something else with it I didn t take the test yesterday I took one of several or I didn t take the specific test that would have been implied I didn t take the test yesterday I took something else I didn t take the test yesterday I took it some other day As in the examples above stress is normally transcribed as italics in printed text or underlining in handwriting In English stress is most dramatically realized on focused or accented words For instance consider the dialogue Is it brunch tomorrow No it s dinner tomorrow In it the stress related acoustic differences between the syllables of tomorrow would be small compared to the differences between the syllables of dinner the emphasized word In these emphasized words stressed syllables such as din in dinner are louder and longer 11 12 13 They may also have a different fundamental frequency or other properties The main stress within a sentence often found on the last stressed word is called the nuclear stress 14 Stress and vowel reduction EditIn many languages such as Russian and English vowel reduction may occur when a vowel changes from a stressed to an unstressed position In English unstressed vowels may reduce to schwa like vowels though the details vary with dialect see stress and vowel reduction in English The effect may be dependent on lexical stress for example the unstressed first syllable of the word photographer contains a schwa f e ˈ t ɒ ɡ r e f er whereas the stressed first syllable of photograph does not ˈfoʊteˌgraef grɑːf or on prosodic stress for example the word of is pronounced with a schwa when it is unstressed within a sentence but not when it is stressed Many other languages such as Finnish and the mainstream dialects of Spanish do not have unstressed vowel reduction in these languages vowels in unstressed syllables have nearly the same quality as those in stressed syllables Stress and rhythm EditSome languages such as English are said to be stress timed languages that is stressed syllables appear at a roughly constant rate and non stressed syllables are shortened to accommodate that which contrasts with languages that have syllable timing e g Spanish or mora timing e g Japanese whose syllables or moras are spoken at a roughly constant rate regardless of stress For details see isochrony Historical effects EditIt is common for stressed and unstressed syllables to behave differently as a language evolves For example in the Romance languages the original Latin short vowels e and o have often become diphthongs when stressed Since stress takes part in verb conjugation that has produced verbs with vowel alternation in the Romance languages For example the Spanish verb volver to return come back has the form volvi in the past tense but vuelvo in the present tense see Spanish irregular verbs Italian shows the same phenomenon but with o alternating with uo instead That behavior is not confined to verbs note for example Spanish viento wind from Latin ventum or Italian fuoco fire from Latin focum Stress deafness EditAn operational definition of word stress may be provided by the stress deafness paradigm 15 16 The idea is that if listeners perform poorly on reproducing the presentation order of series of stimuli that minimally differ in the position of phonetic prominence e g numi numi the language does not have word stress The task involves a reproduction of the order of stimuli as a sequence of key strokes whereby key 1 is associated with one stress location e g numi and key 2 with the other e g numi A trial may be from 2 to 6 stimuli in length Thus the order numi numi numi numi is to be reproduced as 1121 It was found that listeners whose native language was French performed significantly worse than Spanish listeners in reproducing the stress patterns by key strokes The explanation is that Spanish has lexically contrastive stress as evidenced by the minimal pairs like topo mole and topo he she it met while in French stress does not convey lexical information and there is no equivalent of stress minimal pairs as in Spanish An important case of stress deafness relates to Persian 16 The language has generally been described as having contrastive word stress or accent as evidenced by numerous stem and stem clitic minimal pairs such as mɒhi mɒ hi fish and mɒh i mɒ hi some month The authors argue that the reason that Persian listeners are stress deaf is that their accent locations arise postlexically Persian thus lacks stress in the strict sense Stress deafness has been studied for a number of languages such as Polish 17 or French learners of Spanish 18 Spelling and notation for stress EditThe orthographies of some languages include devices for indicating the position of lexical stress Some examples are listed below In Modern Greek all polysyllables are written with an acute accent over the vowel of the stressed syllable The acute accent is also used on some monosyllables in order to distinguish homographs as in h the and h or here the stress of the two words is the same In Spanish orthography stress may be written explicitly with a single acute accent on a vowel Stressed antepenultimate syllables are always written with that accent mark as in arabe If the last syllable is stressed the accent mark is used if the word ends in the letters n s or a vowel as in esta If the penultimate syllable is stressed the accent is used if the word ends in any other letter as in carcel That is if a word is written without an accent mark the stress is on the penult if the last letter is a vowel n or s but on the final syllable if the word ends in any other letter However as in Greek the acute accent is also used for some words to distinguish various syntactical uses e g te tea vs te a form of the pronoun tu you donde where as a pronoun or wh complement donde where as an adverb For more information see Stress in Spanish In Portuguese stress is sometimes indicated explicitly with an acute accent for i u and open a e o or circumflex for close a e o The orthography has an extensive set of rules that describe the placement of diacritics based on the position of the stressed syllable and the surrounding letters In Italian the grave accent is needed in words ending with an accented vowel e g citta city and in some monosyllabic words that might otherwise be confused with other words like la there and la the It is optional for it to be written on any vowel if there is a possibility of misunderstanding such as condomini condominiums and condomini joint owners See Italian alphabet Diacritics In this particular case a frequent one in which diacritics present themselves the difference of accents is caused by the fall of the second i from Latin in Italian typical of the genitive in the first noun con dominii meaning of the owner while the second was derived from the nominative con domini meaning simply owners Though not part of normal orthography a number of devices exist that are used by linguists and others to indicate the position of stress and syllabification in some cases when it is desirable to do so Some of these are listed here Most commonly the stress mark is placed before the beginning of the stressed syllable where a syllable is definable However it is occasionally placed immediately before the vowel 19 In the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA primary stress is indicated by a high vertical line primary stress mark span title Representation in the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA class IPA lang und Latn fonipa ˈ span before the stressed element secondary stress by a low vertical line secondary stress mark span title Representation in the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA class IPA lang und Latn fonipa ˌ span For example sɪˌlaebefɪˈkeɪʃen or sɪˌlaebefɪˈkeɪʃen Extra stress can be indicated by doubling the symbol ˈˈ Linguists frequently mark primary stress with an acute accent over the vowel and secondary stress by a grave accent Example sɪlae befɪkeɪʃen or sɪlae befɪkeɪʃen That has the advantage of not requiring a decision about syllable boundaries In English dictionaries that show pronunciation by respelling stress is typically marked with a prime mark placed after the stressed syllable si lab e fi kay shen In ad hoc pronunciation guides stress is often indicated using a combination of bold text and capital letters For example si lab if i KAY shun or si LAB if i KAY shun In Russian Belarusian and Ukrainian dictionaries stress is indicated with marks called znaki udareniya znaki udareniya stress marks Primary stress is indicated with an acute accent on a syllable s vowel example vimovlya nnya 20 21 Secondary stress may be unmarked or marked with a grave accent o koloze mnyj If the acute accent sign is unavailable for technical reasons stress can be marked by making the vowel capitalized or italic 22 In general texts stress marks are rare typically used either when required for disambiguation of homographs compare v bolshi h kolichestvah in great quantities and v bo lshih kolichestvah in greater quantities or in rare words and names that are likely to be mispronounced Materials for foreign learners may have stress marks throughout the text 20 In Dutch ad hoc indication of stress is usually marked by an acute accent on the vowel or in the case of a diphthong or double vowel the first two vowels of the stressed syllable Compare achteruitgang deterioration and achteruitgang rear exit In Biblical Hebrew a complex system of cantillation marks is used to mark stress as well as verse syntax and the melody according to which the verse is chanted in ceremonial Bible reading In Modern Hebrew there is no standardized way to mark the stress Most often the cantillation mark oleh part of oleh ve yored which looks like a left pointing arrow above the consonant of the stressed syllable for example ב וקר boqer morning as opposed to בוק ר boqer cowboy That mark is usually used in books by the Academy of the Hebrew Language and is available on the standard Hebrew keyboard at AltGr 6 In some books other marks such as meteg are used 23 See also EditAccent poetry Accent music Foot prosody Initial stress derived noun Pitch accent intonation Rhythm Syllable weightReferences Edit Fry D B 1955 Duration and intensity as physical correlates of linguistic stress Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 27 4 765 768 Bibcode 1955ASAJ 27 765F doi 10 1121 1 1908022 Fry D B 1958 Experiments in the perception of stress Language and Speech 1 2 126 152 doi 10 1177 002383095800100207 S2CID 141158933 a b Monrad Krohn G H 1947 The prosodic quality of speech and its disorders a brief survey from a neurologist s point of view Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 22 3 4 255 269 doi 10 1111 j 1600 0447 1947 tb08246 x S2CID 146712090 Kochanski Greg Shih Chilin Jing Hongyan 2003 Quantitative measurement of prosodic strength in Mandarin Speech Communication 41 4 625 645 doi 10 1016 S0167 6393 03 00100 6 Mirakyan Norayr 2016 The Implications of Prosodic Differences Between English and Armenian PDF Collection of Scientific Articles of YSU SSS YSU Press 1 3 13 91 96 San Duanmu 2000 The Phonology of Standard Chinese Oxford University Press p 134 mankind in the Collins English Dictionary mankind in the American Heritage Dictionary paper bag in the Collins English Dictionary Ladefoged 1975 etc A course in phonetics 5 4 1980 Preliminaries to linguistic phonetics p 83 Beckman Mary E 1986 Stress and Non Stress Accent Dordrecht Foris ISBN 90 6765 243 1 R Silipo and S Greenberg Automatic Transcription of Prosodic Stress for Spontaneous English Discourse Proceedings of the XIVth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences ICPhS99 San Francisco CA August 1999 pages 2351 2354 Kochanski G Grabe E Coleman J Rosner B 2005 Loudness predicts prominence Fundamental frequency lends little The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 118 2 1038 1054 Bibcode 2005ASAJ 118 1038K doi 10 1121 1 1923349 PMID 16158659 S2CID 405045 Roca Iggy 1992 Thematic Structure Its Role in Grammar Walter de Gruyter p 80 Dupoux Emmanuel Peperkamp Sharon Sebastian Galles Nuria 2001 A robust method to study stress deafness The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 110 3 1606 1618 Bibcode 2001ASAJ 110 1606D doi 10 1121 1 1380437 PMID 11572370 a b Rahmani Hamed Rietveld Toni Gussenhoven Carlos 2015 12 07 Stress Deafness Reveals Absence of Lexical Marking of Stress or Tone in the Adult Grammar PLOS ONE 10 12 e0143968 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1043968R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0143968 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4671725 PMID 26642328 3 439 2012 1 15 Ulrike Knaus Johannes Orzechowska Paula Wiese Richard 2012 Stress deafness in a language with fixed word stress an ERP study on Polish Frontiers in Psychology 3 439 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2012 00439 PMC 3485581 PMID 23125839 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link CS1 maint numeric names authors list link Dupoux Emmanuel Sebastian Galles N Navarrete E Peperkamp Sharon 2008 Persistent stress deafness The case of French learners of Spanish Cognition 106 2 682 706 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2007 04 001 hdl 11577 2714082 PMID 17592731 S2CID 2632741 Payne Elinor M 2005 Phonetic variation in Italian consonant gemination Journal of the International Phonetic Association 35 2 153 181 doi 10 1017 S0025100305002240 S2CID 144935892 a b Lopatin Vladimir Vladimirovich ed 2009 116 Znak udareniya Pravila russkoj orfografii i punktuacii Polnyj akademicheskij spravochnik in Russian Eksmo ISBN 978 5 699 18553 5 Some pre revolutionary dictionaries e g Dahl s Explanatory Dictionary marked stress with an apostrophe just after the vowel example gla snaya See Dahl Vladimir Ivanovich 1903 Boduen de Kurtene Ivan Aleksandrovich ed Tolko vyj slova r zhivo go velikoru sskogo yazyka Explanatory Dictionary of the Living Great Russian Language in Russian 3rd ed Saint Petersburg M O Wolf p 4 Kaplunov Denis 2015 Biznes kopirajting Kak pisat sereznye teksty dlya sereznyh lyudej in Russian p 389 ISBN 978 5 000 57471 3 Aharoni Amir 2020 12 02 אז איך נציין את מקום הטעם הזירה הלשונית רוביק רוזנטל Retrieved 2021 11 25 a href Template Cite web html title Template Cite web cite web a CS1 maint url status link External links Edit Feet and Metrical Stress The Cambridge Handbook of Phonology Word stress in English Six Basic Rules Linguapress Word Stress Rules A Guide to Word and Sentence Stress Rules for English Learners and Teachers based on affixation Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Stress linguistics amp oldid 1128621049, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.