fbpx
Wikipedia

Rhythm

Rhythm (from Greek ῥυθμός, rhythmos, "any regular recurring motion, symmetry"[1]) generally means a "movement marked by the regulated succession of strong and weak elements, or of opposite or different conditions".[2] This general meaning of regular recurrence or pattern in time can apply to a wide variety of cyclical natural phenomena having a periodicity or frequency of anything from microseconds to several seconds (as with the riff in a rock music song); to several minutes or hours, or, at the most extreme, even over many years.

Rhythm is related to and distinguished from pulse, meter, and beats:

Rhythm may be defined as the way in which one or more unaccented beats are grouped in relation to an accented one. ... A rhythmic group can be apprehended only when its elements are distinguished from one another, rhythm...always involves an interrelationship between a single, accented (strong) beat and either one or two unaccented (weak) beats.[3]

In the performance arts, rhythm is the timing of events on a human scale; of musical sounds and silences that occur over time, of the steps of a dance, or the meter of spoken language and poetry. In some performing arts, such as hip hop music, the rhythmic delivery of the lyrics is one of the most important elements of the style. Rhythm may also refer to visual presentation, as "timed movement through space"[4] and a common language of pattern unites rhythm with geometry. For example, architects often speak of the rhythm of a building, referring to patterns in the spacing of windows, columns, and other elements of the façade.[citation needed] In recent years, rhythm and meter have become an important area of research among music scholars. Recent work in these areas includes books by Maury Yeston,[5] Fred Lerdahl and Ray Jackendoff,[6] Jonathan Kramer, Christopher Hasty,[7] Godfried Toussaint,[8] William Rothstein,[9] Joel Lester,[10] and Guerino Mazzola.

Anthropology

 
Percussion instruments have clearly defined sounds that aid the creation and perception of complex rhythms.

In his television series How Music Works, Howard Goodall presents theories that human rhythm recalls the regularity with which we walk and the heartbeat.[11] Other research suggests that it does not relate to the heartbeat directly, but rather the speed of emotional affect, which also influences heartbeat. Yet other researchers suggest that since certain features of human music are widespread, it is "reasonable to suspect that beat-based rhythmic processing has ancient evolutionary roots".[12] Justin London writes that musical metre "involves our initial perception as well as subsequent anticipation of a series of beats that we abstract from the rhythm surface of the music as it unfolds in time".[13] The "perception" and "abstraction" of rhythmic measure is the foundation of human instinctive musical participation, as when we divide a series of identical clock-ticks into "tick-tock-tick-tock".[14][15]

 
A simple [quadr]duple drum pattern, which lays a foundation of duration common in popular music.

Joseph Jordania recently suggested that the sense of rhythm was developed in the early stages of hominid evolution by the forces of natural selection.[16] Plenty of animals walk rhythmically and hear the sounds of the heartbeat in the womb, but only humans have the ability to be engaged (entrained) in rhythmically coordinated vocalizations and other activities. According to Jordania, development of the sense of rhythm was central for the achievement of the specific neurological state of the battle trance, crucial for the development of the effective defense system of early hominids. Rhythmic war cry, rhythmic drumming by shamans, rhythmic drilling of the soldiers and contemporary professional combat forces listening to the heavy rhythmic rock music[17] all use the ability of rhythm to unite human individuals into a shared collective identity where group members put the interests of the group above their individual interests and safety.

Some types of parrots can know rhythm.[18] Neurologist Oliver Sacks states that chimpanzees and other animals show no similar appreciation of rhythm yet posits that human affinity for rhythm is fundamental, so that a person's sense of rhythm cannot be lost (e.g. by stroke). "There is not a single report of an animal being trained to tap, peck, or move in synchrony with an auditory beat",[19] Sacks write, "No doubt many pet lovers will dispute this notion, and indeed many animals, from the Lipizzaner horses of the Spanish Riding School of Vienna to performing circus animals appear to 'dance' to music. It is not clear whether they are doing so or are responding to subtle visual or tactile cues from the humans around them."[20] Human rhythmic arts are possibly to some extent rooted in courtship ritual.[21]

 
Compound triple drum pattern: divides three beats into three; contains repetition on three levels

The establishment of a basic beat requires the perception of a regular sequence of distinct short-duration pulses and, as a subjective perception of loudness is relative to background noise levels, a pulse must decay to silence before the next occurs if it is to be really distinct. For this reason, the fast-transient sounds of percussion instruments lend themselves to the definition of rhythm. Musical cultures that rely upon such instruments may develop multi-layered polyrhythm and simultaneous rhythms in more than one time signature, called polymeter. Such are the cross-rhythms of Sub-Saharan Africa and the interlocking kotekan rhythms of the gamelan.

For information on rhythm in Indian music see Tala (music). For other Asian approaches to rhythm see Rhythm in Persian music, Rhythm in Arabic music and Usul—Rhythm in Turkish music and Dumbek rhythms.

Terminology

Pulse, beat and measure

 
Metric levels: beat level shown in middle with division levels above and multiple levels below.

As a piece of music unfolds, its rhythmic structure is perceived not as a series of discrete independent units strung together in a mechanical, additive, way like beads [or "pulses"], but as an organic process in which smaller rhythmic motives, whole possessing a shape and structure of their own, also function as integral parts of a larger ["architectonic"] rhythmic organization.[22]

Most music, dance and oral poetry establishes and maintains an underlying "metric level", a basic unit of time that may be audible or implied, the pulse or tactus of the mensural level,[23][6][24] or beat level, sometimes simply called the beat. This consists of a (repeating) series of identical yet distinct periodic short-duration stimuli perceived as points in time.[25] The "beat" pulse is not necessarily the fastest or the slowest component of the rhythm but the one that is perceived as fundamental: it has a tempo to which listeners entrain as they tap their foot or dance to a piece of music.[26] It is currently most often designated as a crotchet or quarter note in western notation (see time signature). Faster levels are division levels, and slower levels are multiple levels.[25] Maury Yeston clarified "Rhythms of recurrence" arise from the interaction of two levels of motion, the faster providing the pulse and the slower organizing the beats into repetitive groups.[27] "Once a metric hierarchy has been established, we, as listeners, will maintain that organization as long as minimal evidence is present".[28]

Unit and gesture

 
Rhythmic units: division level shown above and rhythmic units shown below

A durational pattern that synchronises with a pulse or pulses on the underlying metric level may be called a rhythmic unit. These may be classified as:

A rhythmic gesture is any durational pattern that, in contrast to the rhythmic unit, does not occupy a period of time equivalent to a pulse or pulses on an underlying metric level. It may be described according to its beginning and ending or by the rhythmic units it contains. Rhythms that begin on a strong pulse are thetic, those beginning on a weak pulse are anacrustic and those beginning after a rest or tied-over note are called initial rest. Endings on a strong pulse are strong, on a weak pulse, weak and those that end on a strong or weak upbeat are upbeat.[29]

Alternation and repetition

Rhythm is marked by the regulated succession of opposite elements: the dynamics of the strong and weak beat, the played beat and the inaudible but implied rest beat, or the long and short note. As well as perceiving rhythm humans must be able to anticipate it. This depends on repetition of a pattern that is short enough to memorize.

The alternation of the strong and weak beat is fundamental to the ancient language of poetry, dance and music. The common poetic term "foot" refers, as in dance, to the lifting and tapping of the foot in time. In a similar way musicians speak of an upbeat and a downbeat and of the "on" and "off" beat. These contrasts naturally facilitate a dual hierarchy of rhythm and depend on repeating patterns of duration, accent and rest forming a "pulse-group" that corresponds to the poetic foot. Normally such pulse-groups are defined by taking the most accented beat as the first and counting the pulses until the next accent.[30]Scholes 1977b A rhythm that accents another beat and de-emphasises the downbeat as established or assumed from the melody or from a preceding rhythm is called syncopated rhythm.

Normally, even the most complex of meters may be broken down into a chain of duple and triple pulses[30][14] either by addition or division. According to Pierre Boulez, beat structures beyond four, in western music, are "simply not natural".[31]

Tempo and duration

The tempo of the piece is the speed or frequency of the tactus, a measure of how quickly the beat flows. This is often measured in 'beats per minute' (bpm): 60 bpm means a speed of one beat per second, a frequency of 1 Hz. A rhythmic unit is a durational pattern that has a period equivalent to a pulse or several pulses.[32] The duration of any such unit is inversely related to its tempo.

Musical sound may be analyzed on five different time scales, which Moravscik has arranged in order of increasing duration.[33]

  • Supershort: a single cycle of an audible wave, approximately 130110,000 second (30–10,000 Hz or more than 1,800 bpm). These, though rhythmic in nature, are not perceived as separate events but as continuous musical pitch.
  • Short: of the order of one second (1 Hz, 60 bpm, 10–100,000 audio cycles). Musical tempo is generally specified in the range 40 to 240 beats per minute. A continuous pulse cannot be perceived as a musical beat if it is faster than 8–10 per second (8–10 Hz, 480–600 bpm) or slower than 1 per 1.5–2 seconds (0.6–0.5 Hz, 40–30 bpm). Too fast a beat becomes a drone, too slow a succession of sounds seems unconnected.[34] This time frame roughly corresponds to the human heart rate and to the duration of a single step, syllable or rhythmic gesture.
  • Medium: ≥ few seconds, this median durational level "defines rhythm in music"[33] as it allows the definition of a rhythmic unit, the arrangement of an entire sequence of accented, unaccented and silent or "rest" pulses into the cells of a measure that may give rise to the "briefest intelligible and self-existent musical unit",[15] a motif or figure. This may be further organized, by repetition and variation, into a definite phrase that may characterise an entire genre of music, dance or poetry and that may be regarded as the fundamental formal unit of music.[35]
  • Long: ≥ many seconds or a minute, corresponding to a durational unit that "consists of musical phrases"[33]—which may make up a melody, a formal section, a poetic stanza or a characteristic sequence of dance moves and steps. Thus the temporal regularity of musical organisation includes the most elementary levels of musical form.[36]
  • Very long: ≥ minutes or many hours, musical compositions or subdivisions of compositions.

Curtis Roads[37] takes a wider view by distinguishing nine-time scales, this time in order of decreasing duration. The first two, the infinite and the supra musical, encompass natural periodicities of months, years, decades, centuries, and greater, while the last three, the sample and subsample, which take account of digital and electronic rates "too brief to be properly recorded or perceived", measured in millionths of seconds (microseconds), and finally the infinitesimal or infinitely brief, are again in the extra-musical domain. Roads' Macro level, encompassing "overall musical architecture or form" roughly corresponds to Moravcsik's "very long" division while his Meso level, the level of "divisions of form" including movements, sections, phrases taking seconds or minutes, is likewise similar to Moravcsik's "long" category. Roads' Sound object:[38][incomplete short citation][39][incomplete short citation] "a basic unit of musical structure" and a generalization of note (Xenakis' mini structural time scale); fraction of a second to several seconds, and his Microsound (see granular synthesis) down to the threshold of audible perception; thousandths to millionths of seconds, are similarly comparable to Moravcsik's "short" and "supershort" levels of duration.

Rhythm–tempo interaction

One difficulty in defining rhythm is the dependence of its perception on tempo, and, conversely, the dependence of tempo perception on rhythm. Furthermore, the rhythm–tempo interaction is context dependent, as explained by Andranik Tangian using an example of the leading rhythm of ″Promenade″ from Moussorgsky's Pictures at an Exhibition:([40][41]

     
     

This rhythm is perceived as it is rather than as the first three events repeated at a double tempo (denoted as R012 = repeat from 0, one time, twice faster):

     
R012

However, the motive with this rhythm in the Moussorgsky's piece

     
     

is rather perceived as a repeat

     
R012

This context-dependent perception of rhythm is explained by the principle of correlative perception, according to which data are perceived in the simplest way. From the viewpoint of Kolmogorov's complexity theory, this means such a representation of the data that minimizes the amount of memory.

The example considered suggests two alternative representations of the same rhythm: as it is, and as the rhythm-tempo interaction – a two-level representation in terms of a generative rhythmic pattern and a "tempo curve". Table 1 displays these possibilities both with and without pitch, assuming that one duration requires one byte of information, one byte is needed for the pitch of one tone, and invoking the repeat algorithm with its parameters R012 takes four bytes. As shown in the bottom row of the table, the rhythm without pitch requires fewer bytes if it is "perceived" as it is, without repetitions and tempo leaps. On the contrary, its melodic version requires fewer bytes if the rhythm is "perceived" as being repeated at a double tempo.

Complexity of representation of time events
Rhythm only Rhythm with pitch
Complete coding Coding as repeat Complete coding Coding as repeat
     
     
     
R012
     
     
     
R012
Complexity of rhythmic pattern 6 bytes 3 bytes 12 bytes 6 bytes
Complexity of its transformation 0 bytes 4 bytes 0 bytes 4 bytes
Total complexity 6 bytes 7 bytes 12 bytes 10 bytes

Thus, the loop of interdependence of rhythm and tempo is overcome due to the simplicity criterion, which "optimally" distributes the complexity of perception between rhythm and tempo. In the above example, the repetition is recognized because of additional repetition of the melodic contour, which results in a certain redundancy of the musical structure, making the recognition of the rhythmic pattern "robust" under tempo deviations. Generally speaking, the more redundant the "musical support" of a rhythmic pattern, the better its recognizability under augmentations and diminutions, that is, its distortions are perceived as tempo variations rather than rhythmic changes:

By taking into account melodic context, homogeneity of accompaniment, harmonic pulsation, and other cues, the range of admissible tempo deviations can be extended further, yet still not preventing musically normal perception. For example, Skrjabin's own performance of his Poem op. 32 no. 1 transcribed from a piano-roll recording contains tempo deviations within  . = 19/119, a span of 5.5 times.[42] Such tempo deviations are strictly prohibited, for example, in Bulgarian or Turkish music based on so-called additive rhythms with complex duration ratios, which can also be explained by the principle of correlativity of perception. If a rhythm is not structurally redundant, then even minor tempo deviations are not perceived as accelerando or ritardando but rather given an impression of a change in rhythm, which implies an inadequate perception of musical meaning.[43]

Metric structure

 
Notation of a clave rhythm pattern: Each cell of the grid corresponds to a fixed duration of time with a resolution fine enough to capture the timing of the pattern, which may be counted as two bars of four beats in divisive (metrical or symmetrical) rhythm, each beat divided into two cells. The first bar of the pattern may also usefully be counted additively (in measured or asymmetrical rhythm) as 3 + 3 + 2.

The study of rhythm, stress, and pitch in speech is called prosody (see also: prosody (music)): it is a topic in linguistics and poetics, where it means the number of lines in a verse, the number of syllables in each line and the arrangement of those syllables as long or short, accented or unaccented. Music inherited the term "meter or metre" from the terminology of poetry.[14][15][44])

The metric structure of music includes meter, tempo and all other rhythmic aspects that produce temporal regularity against which the foreground details or durational patterns of the music are projected.[45] The terminology of western music is notoriously imprecise in this area.[14] MacPherson preferred to speak of "time" and "rhythmic shape",[36] Imogen Holst of "measured rhythm".[46]

 
An early moving picture demonstrates the waltz, a dance in triple metre.

Dance music has instantly recognizable patterns of beats built upon a characteristic tempo and measure. The Imperial Society of Teachers of Dancing defines the tango, for example, as to be danced in 2
4
time at approximately 66 beats per minute. The basic slow step forwards or backwards, lasting for one beat, is called a "slow", so that a full "right–left" step is equal to one 2
4
measure.[47] (See Rhythm and dance.)

 
Notation of three measures of a clave pattern preceded by one measure of steady quarter notes. This pattern is noted in double time relative to the one above, in one instead of two four-beat measures.
Four beats followed by three clave patterns

The general classifications of metrical rhythm, measured rhythm, and free rhythm may be distinguished.[48] Metrical or divisive rhythm, by far the most common in Western music calculates each time value as a multiple or fraction of the beat. Normal accents re-occur regularly providing systematical grouping (measures). Measured rhythm (additive rhythm) also calculates each time value as a multiple or fraction of a specified time unit but the accents do not recur regularly within the cycle. Free rhythm is where there is neither,[48] such as in Christian chant, which has a basic pulse but a freer rhythm, like the rhythm of prose compared to that of verse.[15] See Free time (music).

Finally some music, such as some graphically scored works since the 1950s and non-European music such as Honkyoku repertoire for shakuhachi, may be considered ametric.[49] Senza misura is an Italian musical term for "without meter", meaning to play without a beat, using time to measure how long it will take to play the bar.[50]

Composite rhythm

 
Bach's Sinfonia in F minor BWV 795, mm. 1–3
Original
With composite

A composite rhythm is the durations and patterns (rhythm) produced by amalgamating all sounding parts of a musical texture. In music of the common practice period, the composite rhythm usually confirms the meter, often in metric or even-note patterns identical to the pulse on a specific metric level. White defines composite rhythm as, "the resultant overall rhythmic articulation among all the voices of a contrapuntal texture".[51] This concept was concurrently defined as "attack point rhythm" by Maury Yeston in 1976 as "the extreme rhythmic foreground of a composition – the absolute surface of articulated movement".[52]

African music

 
A Griot performs at Diffa, Niger, West Africa. The Griot is playing a Ngoni or Xalam.

In the Griot tradition of Africa everything related to music has been passed on orally. Babatunde Olatunji (1927–2003) developed a simple series of spoken sounds for teaching the rhythms of the hand-drum, using six vocal sounds, "Goon, Doon, Go, Do, Pa, Ta", for three basic sounds on the drum, each played with either the left or the right hand.[citation needed] The debate about the appropriateness of staff notation for African music is a subject of particular interest to outsiders while African scholars from Kyagambiddwa to Kongo have, for the most part, accepted the conventions and limitations of staff notation, and produced transcriptions to inform and enable discussion and debate.[53]

John Miller[54] has argued that West African music is based on the tension between rhythms, polyrhythms created by the simultaneous sounding of two or more different rhythms, generally one dominant rhythm interacting with one or more independent competing rhythms. These often oppose or complement each other and the dominant rhythm. Moral values underpin a musical system based on repetition of relatively simple patterns that meet at distant cross-rhythmic intervals and on call-and-response form. Collective utterances such as proverbs or lineages appear either in phrases translated into "drum talk" or in the words of songs. People expect musicians to stimulate participation by reacting to people dancing. Appreciation of musicians is related to the effectiveness of their upholding community values.[55]

Indian music

Indian music has also been passed on orally. Tabla players would learn to speak complex rhythm patterns and phrases before attempting to play them. Sheila Chandra, an English pop singer of Indian descent, made performances based on her singing these patterns. In Indian classical music, the Tala of a composition is the rhythmic pattern over which the whole piece is structured.

Western music

In the 20th century, composers like Igor Stravinsky, Béla Bartók, Philip Glass, and Steve Reich wrote more rhythmically complex music using odd meters, and techniques such as phasing and additive rhythm. At the same time, modernists such as Olivier Messiaen and his pupils used increased complexity to disrupt the sense of a regular beat, leading eventually to the widespread use of irrational rhythms in New Complexity. This use may be explained by a comment of John Cage's where he notes that regular rhythms cause sounds to be heard as a group rather than individually; the irregular rhythms highlight the rapidly changing pitch relationships that would otherwise be subsumed into irrelevant rhythmic groupings.[56] La Monte Young also wrote music in which the sense of a regular beat is absent because the music consists only of long sustained tones (drones). In the 1930s, Henry Cowell wrote music involving multiple simultaneous periodic rhythms and collaborated with Leon Theremin to invent the rhythmicon, the first electronic rhythm machine, in order to perform them. Similarly, Conlon Nancarrow wrote for the player piano.

Linguistics

In linguistics, rhythm or isochrony is one of the three aspects of prosody, along with stress and intonation. Languages can be categorized according to whether they are syllable-timed, mora-timed, or stress-timed. Speakers of syllable-timed languages such as Spanish and Cantonese put roughly equal time on each syllable; in contrast, speakers of stressed-timed languages such as English and Mandarin Chinese put roughly equal time lags between stressed syllables, with the timing of the unstressed syllables in between them being adjusted to accommodate the stress timing.

Narmour[57] describes three categories of prosodic rules that create rhythmic successions that are additive (same duration repeated), cumulative (short-long), or countercumulative (long-short). Cumulation is associated with closure or relaxation, countercumulation with openness or tension, while additive rhythms are open-ended and repetitive. Richard Middleton points out this method cannot account for syncopation and suggests the concept of transformation.[58]


References

  1. ^ Liddell and Scott 1996.
  2. ^ Anon. 1971, 2537.
  3. ^ Cooper and Meyer 1960, 6.
  4. ^ Jirousek 1995.
  5. ^ Yeston 1976.
  6. ^ a b Lerdahl and Jackendoff 1983.
  7. ^ Hasty 1997.
  8. ^ Toussaint 2005.
  9. ^ Rothstein 1989.
  10. ^ Lester 1986.
  11. ^ Goodall 2006, 0:03:10.
  12. ^ Patel 2014, 1.
  13. ^ London 2004, 4.
  14. ^ a b c d Scholes 1977b.
  15. ^ a b c d Scholes 1977c.
  16. ^ Jordania 2011, 99–101.
  17. ^ Pieslak 2009,[page needed].
  18. ^ Anon. 2009.
  19. ^ Patel 2006, cited in Sacks 2007, 239–240
  20. ^ Sacks 2007, 239–240.
  21. ^ Mithen 2005,[page needed].
  22. ^ Cooper and Meyer 1960, 2.
  23. ^ Berry 1987, 349.
  24. ^ Fitch and Rosenfeld 2007, 44.
  25. ^ a b Winold 1975, 213.
  26. ^ Handel 1989.
  27. ^ Yeston 1976, 50–52.
  28. ^ Lester 1986, 77.
  29. ^ Winold 1975, 239.
  30. ^ a b MacPherson 1930, 5.
  31. ^ Slatkin n.d., at 5:05.
  32. ^ Winold 1975, 237.
  33. ^ a b c Moravcsik 2002, 114.
  34. ^ Fraisse 1956[page needed]; Woodrow 1951[page needed], both quoted in Covaciu-Pogorilowski n.d.
  35. ^ MacPherson 1930,[page needed].
  36. ^ a b MacPherson 1930, 3.
  37. ^ Roads 2001.
  38. ^ Schaeffer 1959.
  39. ^ Schaeffer 1977.
  40. ^ Tanguiane 1993.
  41. ^ Tanguiane 1994, pp. 465–502.
  42. ^ Skrjabin 1960.
  43. ^ Tanguiane 1994, p. 480.
  44. ^ Latham 2002.
  45. ^ Winold 1975, 209–210.
  46. ^ Holst 1963, 17.
  47. ^ Imperial Society of Teachers of Dancing 1977,[page needed].
  48. ^ a b Cooper 1973, 30.
  49. ^ Karpinski 2000, 19.
  50. ^ Forney and Machlis 2007,[page needed].
  51. ^ White 1976, 136.
  52. ^ Yeston 1976, 41–42.
  53. ^ Agawu 2003, 52.
  54. ^ Chernoff 1979.
  55. ^ Chernoff 1979,[page needed].
  56. ^ Sandow 2004, 257.
  57. ^ Narmour 1977, cited in Winold 1975,[page needed]
  58. ^ Middleton 1990,[page needed].

Sources

  • Agawu, Kofi. 2003. Representing African Music: Postcolonial Notes, Queries, Positions. New York: Routledge.
  • Anon. The Compact Edition of the Oxford English Dictionary II. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 1971.
  • Anon. 2009. "Parrots Have Got Rhythm, Studies Find", World-Science.net (April 30).
  • Berry, Wallace (1987). Structural Functions in Music, second edition. New York: Dover Publications. ISBN 978-0-486-25384-8.
  • Chernoff, John Miller (1979). African Rhythm and African Sensibility: Aesthetic and Social Action in African Musical Idioms. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  • Cooper, Grosvenor, and Leonard B. Meyer (1960). The Rhythmic Structure of Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-11521-6, 0-226-11522-4.
  • Cooper, Paul (1973). Perspectives in Music Theory: An Historical-Analytical Approach. New York: Dodd, Mead. ISBN 0-396-06752-2.
  • Covaciu-Pogorilowski, Andrei. n.d. "". Self-published online (archive from 18 January 2018, accessed 26 September 2019).
  • Fitch, W. Tecumseh, and Andrew J. Rosenfeld (2007). "Perception and Production of Syncopated Rhythms". Music Perception, vol. 25, issue 1, pp. 43–58. ISSN 0730-7829.
  • Fraisse, Paul (1956). Les Structures Rhythmiques, with a preface by A. Michotte. Studia Psychologica. Louvain: Publications Universitaires; Paris and Brussels: Édition Erasme; Antwerp and Amsterdam: Standaard Boekhandel.
  • Forney, Kristine, and Joseph Machlis. 2007. The Enjoyment of Music, tenth edition. New York: W. W. Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-17423-6.
  • Goodall, Howard (presenter). 2006. How Music Works with Howard Goodall,[dead link] produced by David Jeffcock. Television series, 4 episodes. Episode 2: "Rhythm" (Saturday 25 November, 6:20–7:20pm). Tiger Aspect Productions for Channel 4 Television Corporation.
  • Handel, Stephen (1989). Listening: An Introduction to the Perception of Auditory Events. MIT Press. ISBN 9780262081795.
  • Hasty, Christopher (1997). Meter as Rhythm. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-510066-2.
  • Holst, Imogen. An ABC of Music: A Short Practical Guide to the Basic Essentials of Rudiments, Harmony, and Form. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 1963.
  • Imperial Society of Teachers of Dancing (1977). Ballroom Dancing. Sevenoaks, Kent: Hodder and Stoughton; New York: David McKay
  • Jirousek, Charlotte. 1995. "Rhythm". In An Interactive Textbook, Ithaca: Cornell University website (accessed 24 July 2014).
  • Jordania, Joseph. 2011. Why do People Sing? Music in Human Evolution. Tbilisi: Logos, International Research Center for Traditional Polyphony; Melbourne: University of Melbourne, Institute of Classical Philology, Bizantyne [sic] and Modern Greek Studies.
  • Karpinski, Gary S. Aural Skills Acquisition: The Development of Listening, Reading, and Performing Skills in College-Level Musicians. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 2000. ISBN 978-0-19-511785-1.
  • Latham, Alison. 2002. "Metre", The Oxford Companion to Music, edited by Alison Latham. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-866212-2.
  • Lerdahl, Fred, and Ray Jackendoff. 1983. A Generative Theory of Tonal Music. The MIT Press Series on Cognitive Theory and Mental Representation. Cambridge: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-12094-4, 978-0-262-62107-6, 978-0-262-62049-9.
  • Lester, Joel. The Rhythms of Tonal Music. Hillsdale, New York: Pendragon Press, 1986. ISBN 978-0-8093-1282-5.
  • Liddell, Henry George, and Robert Scott. "ῥυθμός", in A Greek–English Lexicon, revised edition, combining the text of the ninth edition with an extensively revised and expanded supplement. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, 1996. Online, Perseus Project
  • London, Justin (2004). Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-516081-9.
  • Macpherson, Stewart, Form in Music, London: Joseph Williams, 1930.
  • Middleton, Richard (1990). Studying Popular Music. Philadelphia: Open University Press. ISBN 0-335-15275-9.
  • Mithen, Steven (2005). (PDF). London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson. ISBN 0-297-64317-7. Archived from the original on 2020-11-09. Retrieved 2008-02-25.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  • Moravcsik, Michael J. (2002). Musical Sound: An Introduction to the Physics of Music. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. ISBN 978-0-306-46710-3.
  • Narmour, Eugene. Beyond Schenkerism: The Need for Alternatives in Music Analysis. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press, 1977. Phoenix paperback edition 1980. ISBN 978-0-226-56847-8 (cloth); ISBN 978-0-226-56848-5 (paperback).
  • Patel, Aniruddh D. (2006). "Musical Rhythm, Linguistic Rhythm, and Human Evolution". Music Perception. Berkeley, California: University of California Press. 24 (1): 99–104. doi:10.1525/mp.2006.24.1.99. ISSN 0730-7829.
  • Patel, Aniruddh D. (25 March 2014). "The Evolutionary Biology of Musical Rhythm: Was Darwin Wrong?". PLOS Biology. 12 (3): e1001821. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1001821. PMC 3965380. PMID 24667562.
  • Pieslak, Jonathan (2009). Sound Targets: American Soldiers and Music in the Iraq War. Bloomington and London: Indiana University Press.
  • Roads, Curtis (2001). Microsound. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-18215-7, 978-0-262-68154-4
  • Rothstein, William (1989). Phrase Rhythm in Tonal Music. New York: Schirmer Books. ISBN 9780028721910.
  • Sacks, Oliver (2007). "19. Keeping Time: Rhythm and Movement". Musicophilia, Tales of Music and the Brain. New York and Toronto: Alfred A. Knopf. pp. 233–247. ISBN 978-1-4000-4081-0.
  • Sandow, Greg (2004). "A Fine Madness". In The Pleasure of Modernist Music: Listening, Meaning, Intention, Ideology, edited by Arved Mark Ashby, 253–258. ISBN 1-58046-143-3. Reprinted from The Village Voice (16 March 1982).
  • Scholes, Percy (1977b). "Metre", in The Oxford Companion to Music, 6th corrected reprint of the 10th ed. (1970), revised and reset, edited by John Owen Ward. London and New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-311306-6.
  • Scholes, Percy (1977c). "Rhythm", in The Oxford Companion to Music, 6th corrected reprint of the 10th ed. (1970), revised and reset, edited by John Owen Ward. London and New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-311306-6.
  • Slatkin, Leonard. n.d. " with Leonard Slatkin".
  • Skrjabin, Alexander (1960). Poem for piano, Op. 32, No. 1. Transcribed by P. Lobanov. Moscow: Gosudarstvennoye Muzykalnoye Izdatelstvo.
  • Tanguiane, Andranick (1993). Artificial Perception and Music Recognition. Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence. Vol. 746. Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer. ISBN 978-3-540-57394-4.
  • Tanguiane, Andranick (1994). "A principle of correlativity of perception and its application to music recognition". Music Perception. 11 (4): 465–502. doi:10.2307/40285634. JSTOR 40285634.
  • Toussaint, Godfried T. 2005. "The Geometry of Musical Rhythm". In Proceedings of the Japan Conference on Discrete and Computational Geometry, vol. 3742: Lecture Notes in Computer Science, edited by J. Akiyama, M. Kano, and X. Tan, 198–212. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer.
  • White, John David. (1976). The Analysis of Music. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. ISBN 0-13-033233-X.
  • Winold, Allen (1975). "Rhythm in Twentieth-Century Music". In Aspects of Twentieth-Century Music, edited by Gary Wittlich, 208–269. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall. ISBN 0-13-049346-5.
  • Woodrow, Herbert. "Time Perception". In A Handbook of Experimental Psychology, edited by Stanley Smith Stevens,[page needed]. New York: Wiley, 1951.
  • Yeston, Maury. 1976. The Stratification of Musical Rhythm. New Haven and London: Yale University Press. ISBN 0-300-01884-3.

Further reading

  • Giger, Peter (1993). Die Kunst des Rhythmus, Schott Music. A theoretical approach to western and non-western rhythms. ISBN 978-3-7957-1862-6
  • Gosse, Edmund William; Tovey, Donald Francis (1911). "Rhythm" . In Chisholm, Hugh (ed.). Encyclopædia Britannica. Vol. 23 (11th ed.). Cambridge University Press. pp. 277–280.
  • Honing, H. (2002). "Structure and interpretation of rhythm and timing". Tijdschrift voor Muziektheorie [Dutch Journal of Music Theory]. 7 (3): 227–232. Archived from the original on 2012-12-08.
  • Humble, M. (2002). The Development of Rhythmic Organization in Indian Classical Music, MA dissertation, School of Oriental and African Studies, University of London.
  • Lewis, Andrew (2005). Rhythm—What it is and How to Improve Your Sense of It. San Francisco: RhythmSource Press. ISBN 978-0-9754667-0-4.
  • Mazzola, Guerino (2017). The Topos of Music, Vol. I. Heidelberg: Springer. ISBN 978-3-319-64364-9.
  • Percival, Harold W. (1946). Thinking and Destiny. The Word Foundation. ISBN 978-0-911650-06-8.
  • Palmer, John (2013). Rhythm to Go, Vision Edition and CE Books. A fast-track collection of graded exercises from elementary to advanced level divided in four sections and including an additional chapter with rhythmic structures used in contemporary music. ISMN 979-0-9002315-1-2
  • Petersen, Peter (2013). Music and Rhythm: Fundamentals, History, Analysis. New York: Peter Lang. ISBN 978-3-631-64393-8
  • Scholes, Percy (1977a). "Form", in The Oxford Companion to Music, 6th corrected reprint of the 10th ed. (1970), revised and reset, edited by John Owen Ward. London and New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-311306-6.
  • Williams, C. F. A., The Aristoxenian Theory of Musical Rhythm, (Cambridge Library Collection—Music), Cambridge University Press; first edition, 2009.
  • Van Der, Horst F. (1963). Maat en Ritme, Broekmans & Van Poppel, ISBN 9789491906008. A collection of graded exercises in two volumes, from elementary to advanced level.
  • Yeston, Maury (Autumn 1975). "Rubato and the Middleground". Journal of Music Theory. 19 (2): 286–301. doi:10.2307/843592. JSTOR 843592.

External links

  • 'Rhythm of Prose', William Morrison Patterson ,Columbia University Press 1917
  • Melodyhound has a "Query by Tapping" search that allows users to identify music based on rhythm
  • Louis Hébert, "A Little Semiotics of Rhythm. Elements of Rhythmology", in Signo

rhythm, other, uses, disambiguation, from, greek, ῥυθμός, rhythmos, regular, recurring, motion, symmetry, generally, means, movement, marked, regulated, succession, strong, weak, elements, opposite, different, conditions, this, general, meaning, regular, recur. For other uses see Rhythm disambiguation Rhythm from Greek ῥy8mos rhythmos any regular recurring motion symmetry 1 generally means a movement marked by the regulated succession of strong and weak elements or of opposite or different conditions 2 This general meaning of regular recurrence or pattern in time can apply to a wide variety of cyclical natural phenomena having a periodicity or frequency of anything from microseconds to several seconds as with the riff in a rock music song to several minutes or hours or at the most extreme even over many years Rhythm is related to and distinguished from pulse meter and beats Rhythm may be defined as the way in which one or more unaccented beats are grouped in relation to an accented one A rhythmic group can be apprehended only when its elements are distinguished from one another rhythm always involves an interrelationship between a single accented strong beat and either one or two unaccented weak beats 3 In the performance arts rhythm is the timing of events on a human scale of musical sounds and silences that occur over time of the steps of a dance or the meter of spoken language and poetry In some performing arts such as hip hop music the rhythmic delivery of the lyrics is one of the most important elements of the style Rhythm may also refer to visual presentation as timed movement through space 4 and a common language of pattern unites rhythm with geometry For example architects often speak of the rhythm of a building referring to patterns in the spacing of windows columns and other elements of the facade citation needed In recent years rhythm and meter have become an important area of research among music scholars Recent work in these areas includes books by Maury Yeston 5 Fred Lerdahl and Ray Jackendoff 6 Jonathan Kramer Christopher Hasty 7 Godfried Toussaint 8 William Rothstein 9 Joel Lester 10 and Guerino Mazzola Contents 1 Anthropology 2 Terminology 2 1 Pulse beat and measure 2 2 Unit and gesture 2 3 Alternation and repetition 2 4 Tempo and duration 2 5 Rhythm tempo interaction 2 6 Metric structure 3 Composite rhythm 3 1 African music 3 2 Indian music 3 3 Western music 4 Linguistics 5 References 5 1 Sources 6 Further reading 7 External linksAnthropology Edit Percussion instruments have clearly defined sounds that aid the creation and perception of complex rhythms In his television series How Music Works Howard Goodall presents theories that human rhythm recalls the regularity with which we walk and the heartbeat 11 Other research suggests that it does not relate to the heartbeat directly but rather the speed of emotional affect which also influences heartbeat Yet other researchers suggest that since certain features of human music are widespread it is reasonable to suspect that beat based rhythmic processing has ancient evolutionary roots 12 Justin London writes that musical metre involves our initial perception as well as subsequent anticipation of a series of beats that we abstract from the rhythm surface of the music as it unfolds in time 13 The perception and abstraction of rhythmic measure is the foundation of human instinctive musical participation as when we divide a series of identical clock ticks into tick tock tick tock 14 15 A simple quadr duple drum pattern which lays a foundation of duration common in popular music source source source Joseph Jordania recently suggested that the sense of rhythm was developed in the early stages of hominid evolution by the forces of natural selection 16 Plenty of animals walk rhythmically and hear the sounds of the heartbeat in the womb but only humans have the ability to be engaged entrained in rhythmically coordinated vocalizations and other activities According to Jordania development of the sense of rhythm was central for the achievement of the specific neurological state of the battle trance crucial for the development of the effective defense system of early hominids Rhythmic war cry rhythmic drumming by shamans rhythmic drilling of the soldiers and contemporary professional combat forces listening to the heavy rhythmic rock music 17 all use the ability of rhythm to unite human individuals into a shared collective identity where group members put the interests of the group above their individual interests and safety Some types of parrots can know rhythm 18 Neurologist Oliver Sacks states that chimpanzees and other animals show no similar appreciation of rhythm yet posits that human affinity for rhythm is fundamental so that a person s sense of rhythm cannot be lost e g by stroke There is not a single report of an animal being trained to tap peck or move in synchrony with an auditory beat 19 Sacks write No doubt many pet lovers will dispute this notion and indeed many animals from the Lipizzaner horses of the Spanish Riding School of Vienna to performing circus animals appear to dance to music It is not clear whether they are doing so or are responding to subtle visual or tactile cues from the humans around them 20 Human rhythmic arts are possibly to some extent rooted in courtship ritual 21 Compound triple drum pattern divides three beats into three contains repetition on three levels source source source The establishment of a basic beat requires the perception of a regular sequence of distinct short duration pulses and as a subjective perception of loudness is relative to background noise levels a pulse must decay to silence before the next occurs if it is to be really distinct For this reason the fast transient sounds of percussion instruments lend themselves to the definition of rhythm Musical cultures that rely upon such instruments may develop multi layered polyrhythm and simultaneous rhythms in more than one time signature called polymeter Such are the cross rhythms of Sub Saharan Africa and the interlocking kotekan rhythms of the gamelan For information on rhythm in Indian music see Tala music For other Asian approaches to rhythm see Rhythm in Persian music Rhythm in Arabic music and Usul Rhythm in Turkish music and Dumbek rhythms Terminology EditPulse beat and measure Edit Further information Pulse music and Beat music Metric levels beat level shown in middle with division levels above and multiple levels below As a piece of music unfolds its rhythmic structure is perceived not as a series of discrete independent units strung together in a mechanical additive way like beads or pulses but as an organic process in which smaller rhythmic motives whole possessing a shape and structure of their own also function as integral parts of a larger architectonic rhythmic organization 22 Most music dance and oral poetry establishes and maintains an underlying metric level a basic unit of time that may be audible or implied the pulse or tactus of the mensural level 23 6 24 or beat level sometimes simply called the beat This consists of a repeating series of identical yet distinct periodic short duration stimuli perceived as points in time 25 The beat pulse is not necessarily the fastest or the slowest component of the rhythm but the one that is perceived as fundamental it has a tempo to which listeners entrain as they tap their foot or dance to a piece of music 26 It is currently most often designated as a crotchet or quarter note in western notation see time signature Faster levels are division levels and slower levels are multiple levels 25 Maury Yeston clarified Rhythms of recurrence arise from the interaction of two levels of motion the faster providing the pulse and the slower organizing the beats into repetitive groups 27 Once a metric hierarchy has been established we as listeners will maintain that organization as long as minimal evidence is present 28 Unit and gesture Edit Rhythmic units division level shown above and rhythmic units shown below source source source A durational pattern that synchronises with a pulse or pulses on the underlying metric level may be called a rhythmic unit These may be classified as Metric even patterns such as steady eighth notes or pulses Intrametric confirming patterns such as dotted eighth sixteenth note and swing patterns Contrametric non confirming or syncopated patterns and Extrametric irregular patterns such as tuplets A rhythmic gesture is any durational pattern that in contrast to the rhythmic unit does not occupy a period of time equivalent to a pulse or pulses on an underlying metric level It may be described according to its beginning and ending or by the rhythmic units it contains Rhythms that begin on a strong pulse are thetic those beginning on a weak pulse are anacrustic and those beginning after a rest or tied over note are called initial rest Endings on a strong pulse are strong on a weak pulse weak and those that end on a strong or weak upbeat are upbeat 29 Alternation and repetition Edit Rhythm is marked by the regulated succession of opposite elements the dynamics of the strong and weak beat the played beat and the inaudible but implied rest beat or the long and short note As well as perceiving rhythm humans must be able to anticipate it This depends on repetition of a pattern that is short enough to memorize The alternation of the strong and weak beat is fundamental to the ancient language of poetry dance and music The common poetic term foot refers as in dance to the lifting and tapping of the foot in time In a similar way musicians speak of an upbeat and a downbeat and of the on and off beat These contrasts naturally facilitate a dual hierarchy of rhythm and depend on repeating patterns of duration accent and rest forming a pulse group that corresponds to the poetic foot Normally such pulse groups are defined by taking the most accented beat as the first and counting the pulses until the next accent 30 Scholes 1977b A rhythm that accents another beat and de emphasises the downbeat as established or assumed from the melody or from a preceding rhythm is called syncopated rhythm Normally even the most complex of meters may be broken down into a chain of duple and triple pulses 30 14 either by addition or division According to Pierre Boulez beat structures beyond four in western music are simply not natural 31 Tempo and duration Edit Further information Tempo and Duration music The tempo of the piece is the speed or frequency of the tactus a measure of how quickly the beat flows This is often measured in beats per minute bpm 60 bpm means a speed of one beat per second a frequency of 1 Hz A rhythmic unit is a durational pattern that has a period equivalent to a pulse or several pulses 32 The duration of any such unit is inversely related to its tempo Musical sound may be analyzed on five different time scales which Moravscik has arranged in order of increasing duration 33 Supershort a single cycle of an audible wave approximately 1 30 1 10 000 second 30 10 000 Hz or more than 1 800 bpm These though rhythmic in nature are not perceived as separate events but as continuous musical pitch Short of the order of one second 1 Hz 60 bpm 10 100 000 audio cycles Musical tempo is generally specified in the range 40 to 240 beats per minute A continuous pulse cannot be perceived as a musical beat if it is faster than 8 10 per second 8 10 Hz 480 600 bpm or slower than 1 per 1 5 2 seconds 0 6 0 5 Hz 40 30 bpm Too fast a beat becomes a drone too slow a succession of sounds seems unconnected 34 This time frame roughly corresponds to the human heart rate and to the duration of a single step syllable or rhythmic gesture Medium few seconds this median durational level defines rhythm in music 33 as it allows the definition of a rhythmic unit the arrangement of an entire sequence of accented unaccented and silent or rest pulses into the cells of a measure that may give rise to the briefest intelligible and self existent musical unit 15 a motif or figure This may be further organized by repetition and variation into a definite phrase that may characterise an entire genre of music dance or poetry and that may be regarded as the fundamental formal unit of music 35 Long many seconds or a minute corresponding to a durational unit that consists of musical phrases 33 which may make up a melody a formal section a poetic stanza or a characteristic sequence of dance moves and steps Thus the temporal regularity of musical organisation includes the most elementary levels of musical form 36 Very long minutes or many hours musical compositions or subdivisions of compositions Curtis Roads 37 takes a wider view by distinguishing nine time scales this time in order of decreasing duration The first two the infinite and the supra musical encompass natural periodicities of months years decades centuries and greater while the last three the sample and subsample which take account of digital and electronic rates too brief to be properly recorded or perceived measured in millionths of seconds microseconds and finally the infinitesimal or infinitely brief are again in the extra musical domain Roads Macro level encompassing overall musical architecture or form roughly corresponds to Moravcsik s very long division while his Meso level the level of divisions of form including movements sections phrases taking seconds or minutes is likewise similar to Moravcsik s long category Roads Sound object 38 incomplete short citation 39 incomplete short citation a basic unit of musical structure and a generalization of note Xenakis mini structural time scale fraction of a second to several seconds and his Microsound see granular synthesis down to the threshold of audible perception thousandths to millionths of seconds are similarly comparable to Moravcsik s short and supershort levels of duration Rhythm tempo interaction Edit This section relies largely or entirely on a single source Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page Please help improve this article by introducing citations to additional sources Find sources Rhythm news newspapers books scholar JSTOR January 2021 This section may lend undue weight to certain ideas incidents or controversies Please help to create a more balanced presentation Discuss and resolve this issue before removing this message January 2021 One difficulty in defining rhythm is the dependence of its perception on tempo and conversely the dependence of tempo perception on rhythm Furthermore the rhythm tempo interaction is context dependent as explained by Andranik Tangian using an example of the leading rhythm of Promenade from Moussorgsky s Pictures at an Exhibition 40 41 This rhythm is perceived as it is rather than as the first three events repeated at a double tempo denoted as R012 repeat from 0 one time twice faster R012 However the motive with this rhythm in the Moussorgsky s piece is rather perceived as a repeat R012 This context dependent perception of rhythm is explained by the principle of correlative perception according to which data are perceived in the simplest way From the viewpoint of Kolmogorov s complexity theory this means such a representation of the data that minimizes the amount of memory The example considered suggests two alternative representations of the same rhythm as it is and as the rhythm tempo interaction a two level representation in terms of a generative rhythmic pattern and a tempo curve Table 1 displays these possibilities both with and without pitch assuming that one duration requires one byte of information one byte is needed for the pitch of one tone and invoking the repeat algorithm with its parameters R012 takes four bytes As shown in the bottom row of the table the rhythm without pitch requires fewer bytes if it is perceived as it is without repetitions and tempo leaps On the contrary its melodic version requires fewer bytes if the rhythm is perceived as being repeated at a double tempo Complexity of representation of time events Rhythm only Rhythm with pitchComplete coding Coding as repeat Complete coding Coding as repeat R012 R012Complexity of rhythmic pattern 6 bytes 3 bytes 12 bytes 6 bytesComplexity of its transformation 0 bytes 4 bytes 0 bytes 4 bytesTotal complexity 6 bytes 7 bytes 12 bytes 10 bytesThus the loop of interdependence of rhythm and tempo is overcome due to the simplicity criterion which optimally distributes the complexity of perception between rhythm and tempo In the above example the repetition is recognized because of additional repetition of the melodic contour which results in a certain redundancy of the musical structure making the recognition of the rhythmic pattern robust under tempo deviations Generally speaking the more redundant the musical support of a rhythmic pattern the better its recognizability under augmentations and diminutions that is its distortions are perceived as tempo variations rather than rhythmic changes By taking into account melodic context homogeneity of accompaniment harmonic pulsation and other cues the range of admissible tempo deviations can be extended further yet still not preventing musically normal perception For example Skrjabin s own performance of his Poem op 32 no 1 transcribed from a piano roll recording contains tempo deviations within 19 119 a span of 5 5 times 42 Such tempo deviations are strictly prohibited for example in Bulgarian or Turkish music based on so called additive rhythms with complex duration ratios which can also be explained by the principle of correlativity of perception If a rhythm is not structurally redundant then even minor tempo deviations are not perceived as accelerando or ritardando but rather given an impression of a change in rhythm which implies an inadequate perception of musical meaning 43 Metric structure Edit Further information Metre music Bar music and Metre poetry Notation of a clave rhythm pattern Each cell of the grid corresponds to a fixed duration of time with a resolution fine enough to capture the timing of the pattern which may be counted as two bars of four beats in divisive metrical or symmetrical rhythm each beat divided into two cells The first bar of the pattern may also usefully be counted additively in measured or asymmetrical rhythm as 3 3 2 The study of rhythm stress and pitch in speech is called prosody see also prosody music it is a topic in linguistics and poetics where it means the number of lines in a verse the number of syllables in each line and the arrangement of those syllables as long or short accented or unaccented Music inherited the term meter or metre from the terminology of poetry 14 15 44 The metric structure of music includes meter tempo and all other rhythmic aspects that produce temporal regularity against which the foreground details or durational patterns of the music are projected 45 The terminology of western music is notoriously imprecise in this area 14 MacPherson preferred to speak of time and rhythmic shape 36 Imogen Holst of measured rhythm 46 An early moving picture demonstrates the waltz a dance in triple metre Dance music has instantly recognizable patterns of beats built upon a characteristic tempo and measure The Imperial Society of Teachers of Dancing defines the tango for example as to be danced in 24 time at approximately 66 beats per minute The basic slow step forwards or backwards lasting for one beat is called a slow so that a full right left step is equal to one 24 measure 47 See Rhythm and dance Notation of three measures of a clave pattern preceded by one measure of steady quarter notes This pattern is noted in double time relative to the one above in one instead of two four beat measures source source Four beats followed by three clave patterns The general classifications of metrical rhythm measured rhythm and free rhythm may be distinguished 48 Metrical or divisive rhythm by far the most common in Western music calculates each time value as a multiple or fraction of the beat Normal accents re occur regularly providing systematical grouping measures Measured rhythm additive rhythm also calculates each time value as a multiple or fraction of a specified time unit but the accents do not recur regularly within the cycle Free rhythm is where there is neither 48 such as in Christian chant which has a basic pulse but a freer rhythm like the rhythm of prose compared to that of verse 15 See Free time music Finally some music such as some graphically scored works since the 1950s and non European music such as Honkyoku repertoire for shakuhachi may be considered ametric 49 Senza misura is an Italian musical term for without meter meaning to play without a beat using time to measure how long it will take to play the bar 50 Composite rhythm Edit Bach s Sinfonia in F minor BWV 795 mm 1 3 source source source Original source source source With composite A composite rhythm is the durations and patterns rhythm produced by amalgamating all sounding parts of a musical texture In music of the common practice period the composite rhythm usually confirms the meter often in metric or even note patterns identical to the pulse on a specific metric level White defines composite rhythm as the resultant overall rhythmic articulation among all the voices of a contrapuntal texture 51 This concept was concurrently defined as attack point rhythm by Maury Yeston in 1976 as the extreme rhythmic foreground of a composition the absolute surface of articulated movement 52 African music Edit A Griot performs at Diffa Niger West Africa The Griot is playing a Ngoni or Xalam In the Griot tradition of Africa everything related to music has been passed on orally Babatunde Olatunji 1927 2003 developed a simple series of spoken sounds for teaching the rhythms of the hand drum using six vocal sounds Goon Doon Go Do Pa Ta for three basic sounds on the drum each played with either the left or the right hand citation needed The debate about the appropriateness of staff notation for African music is a subject of particular interest to outsiders while African scholars from Kyagambiddwa to Kongo have for the most part accepted the conventions and limitations of staff notation and produced transcriptions to inform and enable discussion and debate 53 John Miller 54 has argued that West African music is based on the tension between rhythms polyrhythms created by the simultaneous sounding of two or more different rhythms generally one dominant rhythm interacting with one or more independent competing rhythms These often oppose or complement each other and the dominant rhythm Moral values underpin a musical system based on repetition of relatively simple patterns that meet at distant cross rhythmic intervals and on call and response form Collective utterances such as proverbs or lineages appear either in phrases translated into drum talk or in the words of songs People expect musicians to stimulate participation by reacting to people dancing Appreciation of musicians is related to the effectiveness of their upholding community values 55 Indian music Edit See also Bol music Indian music has also been passed on orally Tabla players would learn to speak complex rhythm patterns and phrases before attempting to play them Sheila Chandra an English pop singer of Indian descent made performances based on her singing these patterns In Indian classical music the Tala of a composition is the rhythmic pattern over which the whole piece is structured Western music Edit In the 20th century composers like Igor Stravinsky Bela Bartok Philip Glass and Steve Reich wrote more rhythmically complex music using odd meters and techniques such as phasing and additive rhythm At the same time modernists such as Olivier Messiaen and his pupils used increased complexity to disrupt the sense of a regular beat leading eventually to the widespread use of irrational rhythms in New Complexity This use may be explained by a comment of John Cage s where he notes that regular rhythms cause sounds to be heard as a group rather than individually the irregular rhythms highlight the rapidly changing pitch relationships that would otherwise be subsumed into irrelevant rhythmic groupings 56 La Monte Young also wrote music in which the sense of a regular beat is absent because the music consists only of long sustained tones drones In the 1930s Henry Cowell wrote music involving multiple simultaneous periodic rhythms and collaborated with Leon Theremin to invent the rhythmicon the first electronic rhythm machine in order to perform them Similarly Conlon Nancarrow wrote for the player piano Linguistics EditMain article Isochrony In linguistics rhythm or isochrony is one of the three aspects of prosody along with stress and intonation Languages can be categorized according to whether they are syllable timed mora timed or stress timed Speakers of syllable timed languages such as Spanish and Cantonese put roughly equal time on each syllable in contrast speakers of stressed timed languages such as English and Mandarin Chinese put roughly equal time lags between stressed syllables with the timing of the unstressed syllables in between them being adjusted to accommodate the stress timing Narmour 57 describes three categories of prosodic rules that create rhythmic successions that are additive same duration repeated cumulative short long or countercumulative long short Cumulation is associated with closure or relaxation countercumulation with openness or tension while additive rhythms are open ended and repetitive Richard Middleton points out this method cannot account for syncopation and suggests the concept of transformation 58 References Edit Liddell and Scott 1996 Anon 1971 2537 Cooper and Meyer 1960 6 Jirousek 1995 Yeston 1976 a b Lerdahl and Jackendoff 1983 Hasty 1997 Toussaint 2005 Rothstein 1989 Lester 1986 Goodall 2006 0 03 10 Patel 2014 1 London 2004 4 a b c d Scholes 1977b a b c d Scholes 1977c Jordania 2011 99 101 Pieslak 2009 page needed Anon 2009 Patel 2006 cited in Sacks 2007 239 240 Sacks 2007 239 240 Mithen 2005 page needed Cooper and Meyer 1960 2 Berry 1987 349 Fitch and Rosenfeld 2007 44 a b Winold 1975 213 Handel 1989 Yeston 1976 50 52 Lester 1986 77 Winold 1975 239 a b MacPherson 1930 5 Slatkin n d at 5 05 Winold 1975 237 a b c Moravcsik 2002 114 Fraisse 1956 page needed Woodrow 1951 page needed both quoted in Covaciu Pogorilowski n d MacPherson 1930 page needed a b MacPherson 1930 3 Roads 2001 Schaeffer 1959 sfn error no target CITEREFSchaeffer1959 help Schaeffer 1977 sfn error no target CITEREFSchaeffer1977 help Tanguiane 1993 Tanguiane 1994 pp 465 502 Skrjabin 1960 Tanguiane 1994 p 480 Latham 2002 Winold 1975 209 210 Holst 1963 17 Imperial Society of Teachers of Dancing 1977 page needed a b Cooper 1973 30 Karpinski 2000 19 Forney and Machlis 2007 page needed White 1976 136 Yeston 1976 41 42 Agawu 2003 52 Chernoff 1979 Chernoff 1979 page needed Sandow 2004 257 Narmour 1977 cited in Winold 1975 page needed Middleton 1990 page needed Sources Edit Agawu Kofi 2003 Representing African Music Postcolonial Notes Queries Positions New York Routledge Anon The Compact Edition of the Oxford English Dictionary II Oxford and New York Oxford University Press 1971 Anon 2009 Parrots Have Got Rhythm Studies Find World Science net April 30 Berry Wallace 1987 Structural Functions in Music second edition New York Dover Publications ISBN 978 0 486 25384 8 Chernoff John Miller 1979 African Rhythm and African Sensibility Aesthetic and Social Action in African Musical Idioms Chicago The University of Chicago Press Cooper Grosvenor and Leonard B Meyer 1960 The Rhythmic Structure of Music Chicago University of Chicago Press ISBN 0 226 11521 6 0 226 11522 4 Cooper Paul 1973 Perspectives in Music Theory An Historical Analytical Approach New York Dodd Mead ISBN 0 396 06752 2 Covaciu Pogorilowski Andrei n d Musical Time Theory and a Manifesto Self published online archive from 18 January 2018 accessed 26 September 2019 Fitch W Tecumseh and Andrew J Rosenfeld 2007 Perception and Production of Syncopated Rhythms Music Perception vol 25 issue 1 pp 43 58 ISSN 0730 7829 Fraisse Paul 1956 Les Structures Rhythmiques with a preface by A Michotte Studia Psychologica Louvain Publications Universitaires Paris and Brussels Edition Erasme Antwerp and Amsterdam Standaard Boekhandel Forney Kristine and Joseph Machlis 2007 The Enjoyment of Music tenth edition New York W W Norton ISBN 978 0 393 17423 6 Goodall Howard presenter 2006 How Music Works with Howard Goodall dead link produced by David Jeffcock Television series 4 episodes Episode 2 Rhythm Saturday 25 November 6 20 7 20pm Tiger Aspect Productions for Channel 4 Television Corporation Handel Stephen 1989 Listening An Introduction to the Perception of Auditory Events MIT Press ISBN 9780262081795 Hasty Christopher 1997 Meter as Rhythm Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 510066 2 Holst Imogen An ABC of Music A Short Practical Guide to the Basic Essentials of Rudiments Harmony and Form Oxford and New York Oxford University Press 1963 Imperial Society of Teachers of Dancing 1977 Ballroom Dancing Sevenoaks Kent Hodder and Stoughton New York David McKay Jirousek Charlotte 1995 Rhythm In An Interactive Textbook Ithaca Cornell University website accessed 24 July 2014 Jordania Joseph 2011 Why do People Sing Music in Human Evolution Tbilisi Logos International Research Center for Traditional Polyphony Melbourne University of Melbourne Institute of Classical Philology Bizantyne sic and Modern Greek Studies Karpinski Gary S Aural Skills Acquisition The Development of Listening Reading and Performing Skills in College Level Musicians Oxford and New York Oxford University Press 2000 ISBN 978 0 19 511785 1 Latham Alison 2002 Metre The Oxford Companion to Music edited by Alison Latham Oxford and New York Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 866212 2 Lerdahl Fred and Ray Jackendoff 1983 A Generative Theory of Tonal Music The MIT Press Series on Cognitive Theory and Mental Representation Cambridge MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 12094 4 978 0 262 62107 6 978 0 262 62049 9 Lester Joel The Rhythms of Tonal Music Hillsdale New York Pendragon Press 1986 ISBN 978 0 8093 1282 5 Liddell Henry George and Robert Scott ῥy8mos in A Greek English Lexicon revised edition combining the text of the ninth edition with an extensively revised and expanded supplement Oxford and New York Oxford University Press 1996 Online Perseus Project London Justin 2004 Hearing in Time Psychological Aspects of Musical Meter Oxford and New York Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 516081 9 Macpherson Stewart Form in Music London Joseph Williams 1930 Middleton Richard 1990 Studying Popular Music Philadelphia Open University Press ISBN 0 335 15275 9 Mithen Steven 2005 The Singing Neanderthals The Origins of Music Language Mind and Body PDF London Weidenfeld amp Nicolson ISBN 0 297 64317 7 Archived from the original on 2020 11 09 Retrieved 2008 02 25 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint unfit URL link Moravcsik Michael J 2002 Musical Sound An Introduction to the Physics of Music New York Kluwer Academic Plenum Publishers ISBN 978 0 306 46710 3 Narmour Eugene Beyond Schenkerism The Need for Alternatives in Music Analysis Chicago and London University of Chicago Press 1977 Phoenix paperback edition 1980 ISBN 978 0 226 56847 8 cloth ISBN 978 0 226 56848 5 paperback Patel Aniruddh D 2006 Musical Rhythm Linguistic Rhythm and Human Evolution Music Perception Berkeley California University of California Press 24 1 99 104 doi 10 1525 mp 2006 24 1 99 ISSN 0730 7829 Patel Aniruddh D 25 March 2014 The Evolutionary Biology of Musical Rhythm Was Darwin Wrong PLOS Biology 12 3 e1001821 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 1001821 PMC 3965380 PMID 24667562 Pieslak Jonathan 2009 Sound Targets American Soldiers and Music in the Iraq War Bloomington and London Indiana University Press Roads Curtis 2001 Microsound Cambridge Massachusetts MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 18215 7 978 0 262 68154 4 Rothstein William 1989 Phrase Rhythm in Tonal Music New York Schirmer Books ISBN 9780028721910 Sacks Oliver 2007 19 Keeping Time Rhythm and Movement Musicophilia Tales of Music and the Brain New York and Toronto Alfred A Knopf pp 233 247 ISBN 978 1 4000 4081 0 Sandow Greg 2004 A Fine Madness In The Pleasure of Modernist Music Listening Meaning Intention Ideology edited by Arved Mark Ashby 253 258 ISBN 1 58046 143 3 Reprinted from The Village Voice 16 March 1982 Scholes Percy 1977b Metre in The Oxford Companion to Music 6th corrected reprint of the 10th ed 1970 revised and reset edited by John Owen Ward London and New York Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 311306 6 Scholes Percy 1977c Rhythm in The Oxford Companion to Music 6th corrected reprint of the 10th ed 1970 revised and reset edited by John Owen Ward London and New York Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 311306 6 Slatkin Leonard n d Discovering Music Rhythm with Leonard Slatkin Skrjabin Alexander 1960 Poem for piano Op 32 No 1 Transcribed by P Lobanov Moscow Gosudarstvennoye Muzykalnoye Izdatelstvo Tanguiane Andranick 1993 Artificial Perception and Music Recognition Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence Vol 746 Berlin Heidelberg Springer ISBN 978 3 540 57394 4 Tanguiane Andranick 1994 A principle of correlativity of perception and its application to music recognition Music Perception 11 4 465 502 doi 10 2307 40285634 JSTOR 40285634 Toussaint Godfried T 2005 The Geometry of Musical Rhythm In Proceedings of the Japan Conference on Discrete and Computational Geometry vol 3742 Lecture Notes in Computer Science edited by J Akiyama M Kano and X Tan 198 212 Berlin Heidelberg Springer White John David 1976 The Analysis of Music Englewood Cliffs New Jersey Prentice Hall ISBN 0 13 033233 X Winold Allen 1975 Rhythm in Twentieth Century Music In Aspects of Twentieth Century Music edited by Gary Wittlich 208 269 Englewood Cliffs New Jersey Prentice Hall ISBN 0 13 049346 5 Woodrow Herbert Time Perception In A Handbook of Experimental Psychology edited by Stanley Smith Stevens page needed New York Wiley 1951 Yeston Maury 1976 The Stratification of Musical Rhythm New Haven and London Yale University Press ISBN 0 300 01884 3 Further reading EditGiger Peter 1993 Die Kunst des Rhythmus Schott Music A theoretical approach to western and non western rhythms ISBN 978 3 7957 1862 6 Gosse Edmund William Tovey Donald Francis 1911 Rhythm In Chisholm Hugh ed Encyclopaedia Britannica Vol 23 11th ed Cambridge University Press pp 277 280 Honing H 2002 Structure and interpretation of rhythm and timing Tijdschrift voor Muziektheorie Dutch Journal of Music Theory 7 3 227 232 Archived from the original on 2012 12 08 Humble M 2002 The Development of Rhythmic Organization in Indian Classical Music MA dissertation School of Oriental and African Studies University of London Lewis Andrew 2005 Rhythm What it is and How to Improve Your Sense of It San Francisco RhythmSource Press ISBN 978 0 9754667 0 4 Mazzola Guerino 2017 The Topos of Music Vol I Heidelberg Springer ISBN 978 3 319 64364 9 Percival Harold W 1946 Thinking and Destiny The Word Foundation ISBN 978 0 911650 06 8 Palmer John 2013 Rhythm to Go Vision Edition and CE Books A fast track collection of graded exercises from elementary to advanced level divided in four sections and including an additional chapter with rhythmic structures used in contemporary music ISMN 979 0 9002315 1 2 Petersen Peter 2013 Music and Rhythm Fundamentals History Analysis New York Peter Lang ISBN 978 3 631 64393 8 Scholes Percy 1977a Form in The Oxford Companion to Music 6th corrected reprint of the 10th ed 1970 revised and reset edited by John Owen Ward London and New York Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 311306 6 Williams C F A The Aristoxenian Theory of Musical Rhythm Cambridge Library Collection Music Cambridge University Press first edition 2009 Van Der Horst F 1963 Maat en Ritme Broekmans amp Van Poppel ISBN 9789491906008 A collection of graded exercises in two volumes from elementary to advanced level Yeston Maury Autumn 1975 Rubato and the Middleground Journal of Music Theory 19 2 286 301 doi 10 2307 843592 JSTOR 843592 External links Edit Look up rhythm in Wiktionary the free dictionary Rhythm of Prose William Morrison Patterson Columbia University Press 1917 Melodyhound has a Query by Tapping search that allows users to identify music based on rhythm Louis Hebert A Little Semiotics of Rhythm Elements of Rhythmology in Signo Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Rhythm amp oldid 1144983871, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.