fbpx
Wikipedia

Creativity

Creativity is a phenomenon whereby something new and valuable is formed. The created item may be intangible (such as an idea, a scientific theory, a musical composition, or a joke) or a physical object (such as an invention, a printed literary work, or a painting).

A picture of a lightbulb is associated with someone having an idea, an example of creativity.

Scholarly interest in creativity is found in a number of disciplines, primarily psychology, business studies, and cognitive science. However, it can also be found in education, the humanities (philosophy, the arts) and theology, social sciences (sociology, linguistics, economics), engineering, technology and mathematics. These disciplines cover the relations between creativity and general intelligence, personality type, mental and neural processes, mental health, artificial intelligence; the potential for fostering creativity through education, training, leadership and organizational practices;[1] the factors that determine how creativity is evaluated and perceived;[2] the fostering of creativity for national economic benefit; and the application of creative resources to improve the effectiveness of teaching and learning.

Etymology

The English word creativity comes from the Latin term creare, "to create, make": its derivational suffixes also come from Latin. The word "create" appeared in English as early as the 14th century, notably in Chaucer (in The Parson's Tale[3]), to indicate divine creation.[4]

However, its modern meaning as an act of human creation did not emerge until after the Enlightenment.[4]

Definition

In a summary of scientific research into creativity, Michael Mumford suggested: "Over the course of the last decade, however, we seem to have reached a general agreement that creativity involves the production of novel, useful products" (Mumford, 2003, p. 110),[5] or, in Robert Sternberg's words, the production of "something original and worthwhile".[6] Authors have diverged dramatically in their precise definitions beyond these general commonalities: Peter Meusburger estimates that over a hundred different definitions can be found in the literature, typically elaborating on the context (field, organisation, environment etc.) which determines the originality and/or appropriateness of the created object, and the processes through which it came about.[7] As an illustration, one definition given by Dr. E. Paul Torrance in the context of assessing an individual's creative ability, described it as "a process of becoming sensitive to problems, deficiencies, gaps in knowledge, missing elements, disharmonies, and so on; identifying the difficulty; searching for solutions, making guesses, or formulating hypotheses about the deficiencies: testing and retesting these hypotheses and possibly modifying and retesting them; and finally communicating the results."[8]

Creativity in general is usually distinguished from innovation in particular, where the stress is on implementation. For example, Teresa Amabile and Pratt (2016) define creativity as production of novel and useful ideas and innovation as implementation of creative ideas,[9] while the OECD and Eurostat state that "Innovation is more than a new idea or an invention. An innovation requires implementation, either by being put into active use or by being made available for use by other parties, firms, individuals or organisations."[10]

There is also an emotional creativity[11] which is described as a pattern of cognitive abilities and personality traits related to originality and appropriateness in emotional experience.[12]

Aspects

Theories of creativity (particularly investigation of why some people are more creative than others) have focused on a variety of aspects. The dominant factors are usually identified as "the four Ps" – process, product, person, and place/press, a framework first put forward by Mel Rhodes.[13] A focus on process is shown in cognitive approaches that try to describe thought mechanisms and techniques for creative thinking. Theories invoking divergent rather than convergent thinking (such as Guilford), or those describing the staging of the creative process (such as Wallas) are primarily theories of creative process. A focus on creative product usually appears in attempts to assess creative output, whether for psychometrics (see below) or in understanding why some objects are considered creative. It is from a consideration of product that the standard definition of creativity as the production of something novel and useful arises.[14] A focus on the nature of the creative person considers more general intellectual habits, such as openness, levels of ideation, autonomy, expertise, exploratory behavior, and so on. A focus on place (sometimes called press) considers the circumstances in which creativity flourishes, such as degrees of autonomy, access to resources, and the nature of gatekeepers. Creative lifestyles are characterized by nonconforming attitudes and behaviors as well as flexibility.[15]

In 2013, based on a sociocultural critique of the Four P model as individualistic, static, and decontextualised, Glǎveanu proposed a "five A's" model consisting of actor, action, artifact, audience, and affordance.[16] In this model, the actor is the person with attributes, but also located within social networks; action is the process of creativity not only in internal cognitive terms, but also external, bridging the gap between ideation and implentation; artifact emphasises how creative products typically represent cumulative innovations over time rather than abrupt discontinuities; and "press/place" is divided into audience and affordance, which consider the interdependence of the creative individual with the social and material world respectively. Although not supplanting the four Ps model in creativity research, the five As model has exerted influence over the direction of some creativity research,[17] and has been credited with bringing coherence to studies across a number of creative domains.[18]

Conceptual history

 
Greek philosophers like Plato rejected the concept of creativity, preferring to see art as a form of discovery. Asked in The Republic, "Will we say, of a painter, that he makes something?", Plato answers, "Certainly not, he merely imitates."[19]

Ancient

Most ancient cultures, including thinkers of Ancient Greece,[19] Ancient China, and Ancient India,[20] lacked the concept of creativity, seeing art as a form of discovery and not creation. The ancient Greeks had no terms corresponding to "to create" or "creator" except for the expression "poiein" ("to make"), which only applied to poiesis (poetry) and to the poietes (poet, or "maker") who made it. Plato did not believe in art as a form of creation. Asked in The Republic,[21] "Will we say, of a painter, that he makes something?", he answers, "Certainly not, he merely imitates."[19]

It is commonly argued that the notion of "creativity" originated in Western cultures through Christianity, as a matter of divine inspiration.[4] According to the historian Daniel J. Boorstin, "the early Western conception of creativity was the Biblical story of creation given in the Genesis."[22] However, this is not creativity in the modern sense, which did not arise until the Renaissance. In the Judeo-Christian tradition, creativity was the sole province of God; humans were not considered to have the ability to create something new except as an expression of God's work.[23] A concept similar to that of Christianity existed in Greek culture. For instance, Muses were seen as mediating inspiration from the Gods.[24] Romans and Greeks invoked the concept of an external creative "daemon" (Greek) or "genius" (Latin), linked to the sacred or the divine. However, none of these views are similar to the modern concept of creativity, and the rejection of creativity in favor of discovery and the belief that individual creation was a conduit of the divine would dominate the West probably until the Renaissance and even later.[23][25]

Renaissance

It was during the Renaissance that creativity was first seen, not as a conduit for the divine, but from the abilities of "great men".[25] The development of the modern concept of creativity began in the Renaissance, when creation began to be perceived as having originated from the abilities of the individual and not God. This could be attributed to the leading intellectual movement of the time, aptly named humanism, which developed an intensely human-centric outlook on the world, valuing the intellect and achievement of the individual.[26] From this philosophy arose the Renaissance man (or polymath), an individual who embodies the principals of humanism in their ceaseless courtship with knowledge and creation.[27] One of the most well-known and immensely accomplished examples is Leonardo da Vinci.

Enlightenment and thereafter

However, the shift from divine inspiration to the abilities of the individual was gradual and would not become immediately apparent until the Enlightenment.[25] By the 18th century and the Age of Enlightenment, mention of creativity (notably in aesthetics), linked with the concept of imagination, became more frequent.[28] In the writing of Thomas Hobbes, imagination became a key element of human cognition;[4] William Duff was one of the first to identify imagination as a quality of genius, typifying the separation being made between talent (productive, but breaking no new ground) and genius.[24]

As a direct and independent topic of study, creativity effectively received no attention until the 19th century.[24] Runco and Albert argue that creativity as the subject of proper study began seriously to emerge in the late 19th century with the increased interest in individual differences inspired by the arrival of Darwinism. In particular, they refer to the work of Francis Galton, who through his eugenicist outlook took a keen interest in the heritability of intelligence, with creativity taken as an aspect of genius.[4]

In the late 19th and early 20th centuries, leading mathematicians and scientists such as Hermann von Helmholtz (1896) and Henri Poincaré (1908) began to reflect on and publicly discuss their creative processes.

Modern

The insights of Poincaré and von Helmholtz were built on in early accounts of the creative process by pioneering theorists such as Graham Wallas[29] and Max Wertheimer. In his work Art of Thought, published in 1926, Wallas presented one of the first models of the creative process. In the Wallas stage model, creative insights and illuminations may be explained by a process consisting of five stages:

(i) preparation (preparatory work on a problem that focuses the individual's mind on the problem and explores the problem's dimensions),
(ii) incubation (where the problem is internalized into the unconscious mind and nothing appears externally to be happening),
(iii) intimation (the creative person gets a "feeling" that a solution is on its way),
(iv) illumination or insight (where the creative idea bursts forth from its preconscious processing into conscious awareness);
(v) verification (where the idea is consciously verified, elaborated, and then applied).

Wallas' model is often treated as four stages, with "intimation" seen as a sub-stage.

Wallas considered creativity to be a legacy of the evolutionary process, which allowed humans to quickly adapt to rapidly changing environments. Simonton[30] provides an updated perspective on this view in his book, Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity.

In 1927, Alfred North Whitehead gave the Gifford Lectures at the University of Edinburgh, later published as Process and Reality.[31] He is credited with having coined the term "creativity" to serve as the ultimate category of his metaphysical scheme: "Whitehead actually coined the term – our term, still the preferred currency of exchange among literature, science, and the arts... a term that quickly became so popular, so omnipresent, that its invention within living memory, and by Alfred North Whitehead of all people, quickly became occluded".[32]

Although psychometric studies of creativity had been conducted by The London School of Psychology as early as 1927 with the work of H. L. Hargreaves into the Faculty of Imagination,[33] the formal psychometric measurement of creativity, from the standpoint of orthodox psychological literature, is usually considered to have begun with J. P. Guilford's address to the American Psychological Association in 1950.[34] The address helped to popularize the study of creativity and to focus attention on scientific approaches to conceptualizing creativity. Statistical analyses led to the recognition of creativity (as measured) as a separate aspect of human cognition to IQ-type intelligence, into which it had previously been subsumed. Guilford's work suggested that above a threshold level of IQ, the relationship between creativity and classically measured intelligence broke down.[35]

"Four C" model

James C. Kaufman and Beghetto introduced a "four C" model of creativity; mini-c ("transformative learning" involving "personally meaningful interpretations of experiences, actions, and insights"), little-c (everyday problem solving and creative expression), Pro-C (exhibited by people who are professionally or vocationally creative though not necessarily eminent) and Big-C (creativity considered great in the given field). This model was intended to help accommodate models and theories of creativity that stressed competence as an essential component and the historical transformation of a creative domain as the highest mark of creativity. It also, the authors argued, made a useful framework for analyzing creative processes in individuals.[36]

The contrast of terms "Big C" and "Little c" has been widely used. Kozbelt, Beghetto and Runco use a little-c/Big-C model to review major theories of creativity.[35] Margaret Boden distinguishes between h-creativity (historical) and p-creativity (personal).[37]

Robinson[38] and Anna Craft[39] have focused on creativity in a general population, particularly with respect to education. Craft makes a similar distinction between "high" and "little c" creativity[39] and cites Ken Robinson as referring to "high" and "democratic" creativity. Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi[40] has defined creativity in terms of those individuals judged to have made significant creative, perhaps domain-changing contributions. Simonton has analysed the career trajectories of eminent creative people in order to map patterns and predictors of creative productivity.[41]

Process theories

There has been much empirical study in psychology and cognitive science of the processes through which creativity occurs. Interpretation of the results of these studies has led to several possible explanations of the sources and methods of creativity.

Incubation

Incubation is a temporary break from creative problem solving that can result in insight.[42] There has been some empirical research looking at whether, as the concept of "incubation" in Wallas' model implies, a period of interruption or rest from a problem may aid creative problem-solving. Early work proposed that creative solutions to problems arise mysteriously from the unconscious mind while the conscious mind is occupied on other tasks.[43] This hypothesis is discussed in Csikszentmihalyi's five-phase model of the creative process which describes incubation as a time that your unconscious takes over. This was supposed to allow for unique connections to be made without our consciousness trying to make logical order out of the problem.[44]

Ward[45] lists various hypotheses that have been advanced to explain why incubation may aid creative problem-solving, and notes how some empirical evidence is consistent with a different hypothesis: Incubation aids creative problem in that it enables "forgetting" of misleading clues. Absence of incubation may lead the problem solver to become fixated on inappropriate strategies of solving the problem.[46]

Convergent and divergent thinking

J. P. Guilford[47] drew a distinction between convergent and divergent production (commonly renamed convergent and divergent thinking). Convergent thinking involves aiming for a single, correct solution to a problem, whereas divergent thinking involves creative generation of multiple answers to a set problem. Divergent thinking is sometimes used as a synonym for creativity in psychology literature or is considered the necessary precursor to creativity.[48] Other researchers have occasionally used the terms flexible thinking or fluid intelligence, which are roughly similar to (but not synonymous with) creativity.[49]

Creative cognition approach

In 1992, Finke et al. proposed the "Geneplore" model, in which creativity takes place in two phases: a generative phase, where an individual constructs mental representations called "preinventive" structures, and an exploratory phase where those structures are used to come up with creative ideas. [50] Some evidence shows that when people use their imagination to develop new ideas, those ideas are heavily structured in predictable ways by the properties of existing categories and concepts.[51] Weisberg[52] argued, by contrast, that creativity only involves ordinary cognitive processes yielding extraordinary results.

The Explicit–Implicit Interaction (EII) theory

Helie and Sun[53] more recently proposed a unified framework for understanding creativity in problem solving, namely the Explicit–Implicit Interaction (EII) theory of creativity. This new theory constitutes an attempt at providing a more unified explanation of relevant phenomena (in part by reinterpreting/integrating various fragmentary existing theories of incubation and insight).

The EII theory relies mainly on five basic principles, namely:

  1. The co-existence of and the difference between explicit and implicit knowledge;
  2. The simultaneous involvement of implicit and explicit processes in most tasks;
  3. The redundant representation of explicit and implicit knowledge;
  4. The integration of the results of explicit and implicit processing;
  5. The iterative (and possibly bidirectional) processing.

A computational implementation of the theory was developed based on the CLARION cognitive architecture and used to simulate relevant human data. This work represents an initial step in the development of process-based theories of creativity encompassing incubation, insight, and various other related phenomena.

Conceptual blending

In The Act of Creation, Arthur Koestler introduced the concept of bisociation – that creativity arises as a result of the intersection of two quite different frames of reference.[54] This idea was later developed into conceptual blending. In the 1990s, various approaches in cognitive science that dealt with metaphor, analogy, and structure mapping have been converging, and a new integrative approach to the study of creativity in science, art and humor has emerged under the label conceptual blending.

Honing theory

Honing theory, developed principally by psychologist Liane Gabora, posits that creativity arises due to the self-organizing, self-mending nature of a worldview. The creative process is a way in which the individual hones (and re-hones) an integrated worldview. Honing theory places emphasis not only on the externally visible creative outcome but also the internal cognitive restructuring and repair of the worldview brought about by the creative process. When faced with a creatively demanding task, there is an interaction between the conception of the task and the worldview. The conception of the task changes through interaction with the worldview, and the worldview changes through interaction with the task. This interaction is reiterated until the task is complete, at which point not only is the task conceived of differently, but the worldview is subtly or drastically transformed as it follows the natural tendency of a worldview to attempt to resolve dissonance and seek internal consistency amongst its components, whether they be ideas, attitudes, or bits of knowledge.

A central feature of honing theory is the notion of a potentiality state.[55] Honing theory posits that creative thought proceeds not by searching through and randomly 'mutating' predefined possibilities, but by drawing upon associations that exist due to overlap in the distributed neural cell assemblies that participate in the encoding of experiences in memory. Midway through the creative process one may have made associations between the current task and previous experiences, but not yet disambiguated which aspects of those previous experiences are relevant to the current task. Thus the creative idea may feel 'half-baked'. It is at that point that it can be said to be in a potentiality state, because how it will actualize depends on the different internally or externally generated contexts it interacts with.

Honing theory is held to explain certain phenomena not dealt with by other theories of creativity – for example, how different works by the same creator are observed in studies to exhibit a recognizable style or 'voice', even in different creative outlets. This is not predicted by theories of creativity that emphasize chance processes or the accumulation of expertise, but it is predicted by honing theory, according to which personal style reflects the creator's uniquely structured worldview. Another example is in the environmental stimulus for creativity. Creativity is commonly considered to be fostered by a supportive, nurturing, trustworthy environment conducive to self-actualization. However, research shows that creativity is also associated with childhood adversity, which would stimulate honing.

Everyday imaginative thought

In everyday thought, people often spontaneously imagine alternatives to reality when they think "if only...".[56] Their counterfactual thinking is viewed as an example of everyday creative processes.[57] It has been proposed that the creation of counterfactual alternatives to reality depends on similar cognitive processes to rational thought.[58]

Dialectical theory of creativity

The term "dialectical theory of creativity" dates back to psychoanalyst Daniel Dervin[59] and was later developed into an interdisciplinary theory.[60] The dialectical theory of creativity starts with the antique concept that creativity takes place in an interplay between order and chaos. Similar ideas can be found in neurosciences and psychology. Neurobiologically, it can be shown that the creative process takes place in a dynamic interplay between coherence and incoherence that leads to new and usable neuronal networks. Psychology shows how the dialectics of convergent and focused thinking with divergent and associative thinking leads to new ideas and products.[61] Also, creative personality traits like the 'Big Five' seem to be dialectically intertwined in the creative process: emotional instability vs. stability, extraversion vs. introversion, openness vs. reserve, agreeableness vs. antagonism, and disinhibition vs. constraint.[62] The dialectical theory of creativity applies also to counseling and psychotherapy.[63]

Neuroeconomic framework for creative cognition

Lin and Vartanian developed a framework that provides an integrative neurobiological description of creative cognition.[64] This interdisciplinary framework integrates theoretical principles and empirical results from neuroeconomics, reinforcement learning, cognitive neuroscience, and neurotransmission research on the locus coeruleus system. It describes how decision-making processes studied by neuroeconomists as well as activity in the locus coeruleus system underlie creative cognition and the large-scale brain network dynamics associated with creativity.[65] It suggests that creativity is an optimization and utility-maximization problem that requires individuals to determine the optimal way to exploit and explore ideas (multi-armed bandit problem). This utility maximization process is thought to be mediated by the locus coeruleus system[66] and this creativity framework describes how tonic and phasic locus coerulues activity work in conjunction to facilitate the exploiting and exploring of creative ideas. This framework not only explains previous empirical results but also makes novel and falsifiable predictions at different levels of analysis (ranging from neurobiological to cognitive and personality differences).

Behaviorism theory of creativity

Skinner attributed creativity to accidental behaviors that are reinforced by the environment.[67] Spontaneous behaviors done by living creatures reflect past learned behavior.[68] In Karen Pryor's book Don't Shoot the Dog she refers to how she reinforced a dolphin to display novel behaviors. This is what one can attribute to both those who are creative and those who appreciate creativity. A behaviorist may say that prior learning caused novel behaviors to be reinforced many times over and the individual has been shaped to produce increasingly novel behaviors.[69] A creative person, according to this definition, would be someone who has been reinforced more often for novel behaviors than others. Behaviorists would also suggest that anyone can be creative, they just need to be reinforced to learn to produce novel behaviors.

Personal assessment

Psychometric approaches

J. P. Guilford's group,[47] which pioneered the modern psychometric study of creativity, constructed several performance-based tests to measure creativity in 1967:

  • Plot Titles, where participants are given the plot of a story and asked to write original titles.
  • Quick Responses is a word-association test scored for uncommonness.
  • Figure Concepts, where participants were given simple drawings of objects and individuals and asked to find qualities or features that are common by two or more drawings; these were scored for uncommonness.
  • Unusual Uses is finding unusual uses for common everyday objects such as bricks.
  • Remote Associations, where participants are asked to find a word between two given words (e.g. Hand _____ Call)
  • Remote Consequences, where participants are asked to generate a list of consequences of unexpected events (e.g. loss of gravity)

Originally, Guilford was trying to create a model for intellect as a whole, but in doing so also created a model for creativity. Guilford made an important assumption for creative research: creativity is not one abstract concept.  The idea that creativity is a category rather than one single concept opened up the ability for other researchers to look at creativity with a whole new perspective.[70][71]

Additionally, Guilford hypothesized one of the first models for the components of creativity.  He explained that creativity was a result of having:

  1. Sensitivity to problems, or the ability to recognize problems;
  2. Fluency, which encompasses
    a. Ideational fluency, or the ability rapidly to produce a variety of ideas that fulfill stated requirements;
    b. Associational fluency, or the ability to generate a list of words, each of which is associated with a given word;
    c. Expressional fluency, or the ability to organize words into larger units, such as phrases, sentences, and paragraphs;
  3. Flexibility, which encompasses
    a. Spontaneous flexibility, or the ability to demonstrate flexibility;
    b. Adaptive flexibility, or the ability to produce responses that are novel and high in quality.

This represents the base model by which several researchers would take and alter to produce their new theories of creativity years later.[70] Building on Guilford's work, tests were developed, sometimes called Divergent Thinking (DT) tests have been both supported[72] and criticized.[73] For example, Torrance[74] developed the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking in 1966.[75] They involved task of divergent thinking and other problem-solving skills, which were scored on:

  • Fluency – The total number of interpretable, meaningful, and relevant ideas generated in response to the stimulus.
  • Originality – The statistical rarity of the responses among the test subjects.
  • Elaboration – The amount of detail in the responses.

Considerable progress has been made in automated scoring of divergent thinking tests using semantic approach. When compared to human raters, NLP techniques were shown to be reliable and valid in scoring the originality.[76][77] The reported computer programs were able to achieve a correlation of 0.60 and 0.72 respectively to human graders.

Semantic networks were also used to devise originality scores that yielded significant correlations with socio-personal measures.[78] Most recently, an NSF-funded[79] team of researchers led by James C. Kaufman and Mark A. Runco[80] combined expertise in creativity research, natural language processing, computational linguistics, and statistical data analysis to devise a scalable system for computerized automated testing (SparcIt Creativity Index Testing system). This system enabled automated scoring of DT tests that is reliable, objective, and scalable, thus addressing most of the issues of DT tests that had been found and reported.[73] The resultant computer system was able to achieve a correlation of 0.73 to human graders.[81]

Social-personality approach

Some researchers have taken a social-personality approach to the measurement of creativity. In these studies, personality traits such as independence of judgement, self-confidence, attraction to complexity, aesthetic orientation, and risk-taking are used as measures of the creativity of individuals.[34] A meta-analysis by Gregory Feist showed that creative people tend to be "more open to new experiences, less conventional and less conscientious, more self-confident, self-accepting, driven, ambitious, dominant, hostile, and impulsive." Openness, conscientiousness, self-acceptance, hostility, and impulsivity had the strongest effects of the traits listed.[82] Within the framework of the Big Five model of personality, some consistent traits have emerged.[83] Openness to experience has been shown to be consistently related to a whole host of different assessments of creativity.[84] Among the other Big Five traits, research has demonstrated subtle differences between different domains of creativity. Compared to non-artists, artists tend to have higher levels of openness to experience and lower levels of conscientiousness, while scientists are more open to experience, conscientious, and higher in the confidence-dominance facets of extraversion compared to non-scientists.[82]

Self-report questionnaires

An alternative is using biographical methods. These methods use quantitative characteristics such as the number of publications, patents, or performances of a work. While this method was originally developed for highly creative personalities, today it is also available as self-report questionnaires supplemented with frequent, less outstanding creative behaviors such as writing a short story or creating your own recipes. For example, the Creative Achievement Questionnaire, a self-report test that measures creative achievement across 10 domains, was described in 2005 and shown to be reliable and valid when compared to other measures of creativity and to independent evaluation of creative output.[85] Besides the English original, it was also used in a Chinese,[86] French,[87] and German-speaking[88] version. It is the self-report questionnaire most frequently used in research.[86]

Intelligence

The potential relationship between creativity and intelligence has been of interest since the late 1900s, when a multitude of influential studies – from Getzels & Jackson,[89] Barron,[90] Wallach & Kogan,[91] and Guilford[92] – focused not only on creativity, but also on intelligence. This joint focus highlights both the theoretical and practical importance of the relationship: researchers are interested not only if the constructs are related, but also how and why.[93]

There are multiple theories accounting for their relationship, with the three main theories as follows:

  • Threshold Theory – Intelligence is a necessary, but not sufficient condition for creativity. There is a moderate positive relationship between creativity and intelligence until IQ ~120.[90][92]
  • Certification Theory – Creativity is not intrinsically related to intelligence. Instead, individuals are required to meet the requisite level intelligence in order to gain a certain level of education/work, which then in turn offers the opportunity to be creative. Displays of creativity are moderated by intelligence.[94]
  • Interference Theory – Extremely high intelligence might interfere with creative ability.[95]

Sternberg and O'Hara[96] proposed a framework of five possible relationships between creativity and intelligence:

  1. Creativity is a subset of intelligence
  2. Intelligence is a subset of creativity
  3. Creativity and intelligence are overlapping constructs
  4. Creativity and intelligence are part of the same construct (coincident sets)
  5. Creativity and intelligence are distinct constructs (disjoint sets)

Creativity as a subset of intelligence

A number of researchers include creativity, either explicitly or implicitly, as a key component of intelligence.

Examples of theories that include creativity as a subset of intelligence

  • Sternberg's Theory of Successful intelligence[95][96][97] (see Triarchic theory of intelligence) includes creativity as a main component, and comprises three sub-theories: Componential (Analytic), Contextual (Practical), and Experiential (Creative). Experiential sub-theory – the ability to use pre-existing knowledge and skills to solve new and novel problems – is directly related to creativity.
  • The Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory includes creativity as a subset of intelligence. Specifically, it is associated with the broad group factor of long-term storage and retrieval (Glr). Glr narrow abilities relating to creativity include:[98] ideational fluency, associational fluency, and originality/creativity. Silvia et al.[99] conducted a study to look at the relationship between divergent thinking and verbal fluency tests, and reported that both fluency and originality in divergent thinking were significantly affected by the broad level Glr factor. Martindale[100] extended the CHC-theory in the sense that it was proposed that those individuals who are creative are also selective in their processing speed. Martindale argues that in the creative process, larger amounts of information are processed more slowly in the early stages, and as the individual begins to understand the problem, the processing speed is increased.
  • The Dual Process Theory of Intelligence[101] posits a two-factor/type model of intelligence. Type 1 is a conscious process, and concerns goal directed thoughts, which are explained by g. Type 2 is an unconscious process, and concerns spontaneous cognition, which encompasses daydreaming and implicit learning ability. Kaufman argues that creativity occurs as a result of Type 1 and Type 2 processes working together in combination. The use of each type in the creative process can be used to varying degrees.

Intelligence as a subset of creativity

In this relationship model, intelligence is a key component in the development of creativity.

Theories of creativity that include intelligence as a subset of creativity

  • Sternberg & Lubart's Investment Theory.[102][103] Using the metaphor of a stock market, they demonstrate that creative thinkers are like good investors – they buy low and sell high (in their ideas). Like under/low-valued stock, creative individuals generate unique ideas that are initially rejected by other people. The creative individual has to persevere, and convince the others of the ideas value. After convincing the others, and thus increasing the ideas value, the creative individual 'sells high' by leaving the idea with the other people, and moves onto generating another idea. According to this theory, six distinct, but related elements contribute to successful creativity: intelligence, knowledge, thinking styles, personality, motivation, and environment. Intelligence is just one of the six factors that can either solely, or in conjunction with the other five factors, generate creative thoughts.
  • Amabile's Componential Model of Creativity.[104][105] In this model, there are three within-individual components needed for creativity – domain-relevant skills, creativity-relevant processes, and task motivation – and 1 component external to the individual: their surrounding social environment. Creativity requires a confluence of all components. High creativity will result when an individual is: intrinsically motivated, possesses both a high level of domain-relevant skills and has high skills in creative thinking, and is working in a highly creative environment.
  • Amusement Park Theoretical Model.[106] In this four-step theory, both domain-specific and generalist views are integrated into a model of creativity. The researchers make use of the metaphor of the amusement park to demonstrate that within each of these creative levels, intelligence plays a key role:
    • To get into the amusement park, there are initial requirements (e.g., time/transport to go to the park). Initial requirements (like intelligence) are necessary, but not sufficient for creativity. They are more like prerequisites for creativity, and if an individual does not possess the basic level of the initial requirement (intelligence), then they will not be able to generate creative thoughts/behaviour.
    • Secondly are the subcomponents – general thematic areas – that increase in specificity. Like choosing which type of amusement park to visit (e.g. a zoo or a water park), these areas relate to the areas in which someone could be creative (e.g. poetry).
    • Thirdly, there are specific domains. After choosing the type of park to visit e.g. waterpark, you then have to choose which specific park to go to. Within the poetry domain, there are many different types (e.g. free verse, riddles, sonnet, etc.) that have to be selected from.
    • Lastly, there are micro-domains. These are the specific tasks that reside within each domain e.g. individual lines in a free verse poem / individual rides at the waterpark.

Creativity and intelligence as overlapping yet distinct constructs

This possible relationship concerns creativity and intelligence as distinct, but intersecting constructs.

Theories that include Creativity and Intelligence as Overlapping Yet Distinct Constructs

  • Renzulli's Three-Ring Conception of Giftedness.[107] In this conceptualisation, giftedness occurs as a result from the overlap of above average intellectual ability, creativity, and task commitment. Under this view, creativity and intelligence are distinct constructs, but they do overlap under the correct conditions.
  • PASS theory of intelligence. In this theory, the planning component – relating to the ability to solve problems, make decisions and take action – strongly overlaps with the concept of creativity.[108]
  • Threshold Theory (TT). A number of previous research findings have suggested that a threshold exists in the relationship between creativity and intelligence – both constructs are moderately positively correlated up to an IQ of ~120. Above this threshold of an IQ of 120, if there is a relationship at all, it is small and weak.[89][90][109] TT posits that a moderate level of intelligence is necessary for creativity.

In support of the TT, Barron[90][110] reported finding a non-significant correlation between creativity and intelligence in a gifted sample and a significant correlation in a non-gifted sample. Yamamoto[111] in a sample of secondary school children, reported a significant correlation between creativity and intelligence of r = 0.3, and reported no significant correlation when the sample consisted of gifted children. Fuchs-Beauchamp et al.[112] in a sample of preschoolers found that creativity and intelligence correlated from r = 0.19 to r = 0.49 in the group of children who had an IQ below the threshold; and in the group above the threshold, the correlations were r = <0.12. Cho et al.[113] reported a correlation of 0.40 between creativity and intelligence in the average IQ group of a sample of adolescents and adults; and a correlation of close to r = 0.0 for the high IQ group. Jauk et al.[114] found support for the TT, but only for measures of creative potential, not creative performance.

Much modern day research reports findings against TT. Wai et al.[115] in a study using data from the longitudinal Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth – a cohort of elite students from early adolescence into adulthood – found that differences in SAT scores at age 13 were predictive of creative real-life outcomes 20 years later. Kim's[116] meta-analysis of 21 studies did not find any supporting evidence for TT, and instead negligible correlations were reported between intelligence, creativity, and divergent thinking both below and above IQ's of 120. Preckel et al.,[117] investigating fluid intelligence and creativity, reported small correlations of r = 0.3 to r = 0.4 across all levels of cognitive ability.

Creativity and intelligence as coincident sets

Under this view, researchers posit that there are no differences in the mechanisms underlying creativity in those used in normal problem solving; and in normal problem solving, there is no need for creativity. Thus, creativity and Intelligence (problem solving) are the same thing. Perkins[118] referred to this as the 'nothing-special' view.

Weisberg & Alba[119] examined problem solving by having participants complete the nine dots puzzle – where the participants are asked to connect all nine dots in the three rows of three dots using four straight lines or less, without lifting their pen or tracing the same line twice. The problem can only be solved if the lines go outside the boundaries of the square of dots. Results demonstrated that even when participants were given this insight, they still found it difficult to solve the problem, thus showing that to successfully complete the task it is not just insight (or creativity) that is required.

Creativity and intelligence as disjoint sets

In this view, creativity and intelligence are completely different, unrelated constructs.

Getzels and Jackson[89] administered five creativity measures to a group of 449 children from grades 6–12, and compared these test findings to results from previously administered (by the school) IQ tests. They found that the correlation between the creativity measures and IQ was r = 0.26. The high creativity group scored in the top 20% of the overall creativity measures, but were not included in the top 20% of IQ scorers. The high intelligence group scored the opposite: they scored in the top 20% for IQ, but were outside the top 20% scorers for creativity, thus showing that creativity and intelligence are distinct and unrelated.

However, this work has been heavily criticised. Wallach and Kogan[91] highlighted that the creativity measures were not only weakly related to one another (to the extent that they were no more related to one another than they were with IQ), but they seemed to also draw upon non-creative skills. McNemar[120] noted that there were major measurement issues, in that the IQ scores were a mixture from three different IQ tests.

Wallach and Kogan[91] administered five measures of creativity, each of which resulted in a score for originality and fluency; and 10 measures of general intelligence to 151 5th grade children. These tests were untimed, and given in a game-like manner (aiming to facilitate creativity). Inter-correlations between creativity tests were on average r = 0.41. Inter-correlations between intelligence measures were on average r = 0.51 with each other. Creativity tests and intelligence measures correlated r = 0.09.

Neuroscience

 
Distributed functional brain network associated with divergent thinking

The neuroscience of creativity looks at the operation of the brain during creative behaviour. It has been addressed[121] in the article "Creative Innovation: Possible Brain Mechanisms". The authors write that "creative innovation might require coactivation and communication between regions of the brain that ordinarily are not strongly connected." Highly creative people who excel at creative innovation tend to differ from others in three ways:

Thus, the frontal lobe appears to be the part of the cortex that is most important for creativity.

This article also explored the links between creativity and sleep, mood and addiction disorders, and depression.

In 2005, Alice Flaherty presented a three-factor model of the creative drive. Drawing from evidence in brain imaging, drug studies and lesion analysis, she described the creative drive as resulting from an interaction of the frontal lobes, the temporal lobes, and dopamine from the limbic system. The frontal lobes can be seen as responsible for idea generation, and the temporal lobes for idea editing and evaluation. Abnormalities in the frontal lobe (such as depression or anxiety) generally decrease creativity, while abnormalities in the temporal lobe often increase creativity. High activity in the temporal lobe typically inhibits activity in the frontal lobe, and vice versa. High dopamine levels increase general arousal and goal directed behaviors and reduce latent inhibition, and all three effects increase the drive to generate ideas.[122] A 2015 study on creativity found that it involves the interaction of multiple neural networks, including those that support associative thinking, along with other default mode network functions.[123]

Similarly, in 2018, Lin and Vartanian proposed a neuroeconomic framework that precisely describes norepinephrine's role in creativity and modulating large-scale brain networks associated with creativity.[64] This framework describes how neural activity in different brain regions and networks like the default mode network are tracking utility or subjective value of ideas.

In 2018, experiments showed that when the brain suppresses obvious or 'known' solutions, the outcome is solutions that are more creative. This suppression is mediated by alpha oscillations in the right temporal lobe.[124]

Working memory and the cerebellum

Vandervert[125] described how the brain's frontal lobes and the cognitive functions of the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation. Vandervert's explanation rests on considerable evidence that all processes of working memory (responsible for processing all thought[126]) are adaptively modeled for increased efficiency by the cerebellum.[127] The cerebellum (consisting of 100 billion neurons, which is more than the entirety of the rest of the brain[128]) is also widely known to adaptively model all bodily movement for efficiency. The cerebellum's adaptive models of working memory processing are then fed back to especially frontal lobe working memory control processes[129] where creative and innovative thoughts arise.[130] (Apparently, creative insight or the "aha" experience is then triggered in the temporal lobe.[131])

According to Vandervert, the details of creative adaptation begin in "forward" cerebellar models which are anticipatory/exploratory controls for movement and thought. These cerebellar processing and control architectures have been termed Hierarchical Modular Selection and Identification for Control (HMOSAIC).[132] New, hierarchically arranged levels of the cerebellar control architecture (HMOSAIC) develop as mental mulling in working memory is extended over time. These new levels of the control architecture are fed forward to the frontal lobes. Since the cerebellum adaptively models all movement and all levels of thought and emotion,[133] Vandervert's approach helps explain creativity and innovation in sports, art, music, the design of video games, technology, mathematics, the child prodigy, and thought in general.

Essentially, Vandervert has argued that when a person is confronted with a challenging new situation, visual-spatial working memory and speech-related working memory are decomposed and re-composed (fractionated) by the cerebellum and then blended in the cerebral cortex in an attempt to deal with the new situation. With repeated attempts to deal with challenging situations, the cerebro-cerebellar blending process continues to optimize the efficiency of how working memory deals with the situation or problem.[134] Most recently, he has argued that this is the same process (only involving visual-spatial working memory and pre-language vocalization) that led to the evolution of language in humans.[135] Vandervert and Vandervert-Weathers have pointed out that this blending process, because it continuously optimizes efficiencies, constantly improves prototyping attempts toward the invention or innovation of new ideas, music, art, or technology.[136] Prototyping, they argue, not only produces new products, it trains the cerebro-cerebellar pathways involved to become more efficient at prototyping itself. Further, Vandervert and Vandervert-Weathers believe that this repetitive "mental prototyping" or mental rehearsal involving the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex explains the success of the self-driven, individualized patterning of repetitions initiated by the teaching methods of the Khan Academy. The model proposed by Vandervert has, however, received incisive critique from several authors.[137][138]

REM sleep

Creativity involves the forming of associative elements into new combinations that are useful or meet some requirement. Sleep aids this process.[139] REM rather than NREM sleep appears to be responsible.[140][141] This has been suggested to be due to changes in cholinergic and noradrenergic neuromodulation that occurs during REM sleep.[140] During this period of sleep, high levels of acetylcholine in the hippocampus suppress feedback from the hippocampus to the neocortex, and lower levels of acetylcholine and norepinephrine in the neocortex encourage the spread of associational activity within neocortical areas without control from the hippocampus.[142] This is in contrast to waking consciousness, where higher levels of norepinephrine and acetylcholine inhibit recurrent connections in the neocortex. It is proposed that REM sleep aids creativity by allowing "neocortical structures to reorganize associative hierarchies, in which information from the hippocampus would be reinterpreted in relation to previous semantic representations or nodes."[140]

Affect

Some theories suggest that creativity may be particularly susceptible to affective influence. As noted in voting behavior, the term "affect" in this context can refer to liking or disliking key aspects of the subject in question. This work largely follows from findings in psychology regarding the ways in which affective states are involved in human judgment and decision-making.[143]

Positive affect relations

According to Alice Isen, positive affect has three primary effects on cognitive activity:

  1. Positive affect makes additional cognitive material available for processing, increasing the number of cognitive elements available for association;
  2. Positive affect leads to defocused attention and a more complex cognitive context, increasing the breadth of those elements that are treated as relevant to the problem;
  3. Positive affect increases cognitive flexibility, increasing the probability that diverse cognitive elements will in fact become associated. Together, these processes lead positive affect to have a positive influence on creativity.

Barbara Fredrickson in her broaden-and-build model suggests that positive emotions such as joy and love broaden a person's available repertoire of cognitions and actions, thus enhancing creativity.

According to these researchers, positive emotions increase the number of cognitive elements available for association (attention scope) and the number of elements that are relevant to the problem (cognitive scope). Day-by-day psychological experiences including emotions, perceptions, and motivation will significantly impact creative performance. Creativity is higher when emotions and perceptions are more positive and when intrinsic motivation is stronger.[144]

Various meta-analyses, such as Baas et al. (2008) of 66 studies about creativity and affect support the link between creativity and positive affect.[145][146]

Computational creativity

Jürgen Schmidhuber's formal theory of creativity[147][148] postulates that creativity, curiosity, and interestingness are by-products of a simple computational principle for measuring and optimizing learning progress. Consider an agent able to manipulate its environment and thus its own sensory inputs. The agent can use a black box optimization method such as reinforcement learning to learn (through informed trial and error) sequences of actions that maximize the expected sum of its future reward signals. There are extrinsic reward signals for achieving externally given goals, such as finding food when hungry. But Schmidhuber's objective function to be maximized also includes an additional, intrinsic term to model "wow-effects". This non-standard term motivates purely creative behavior of the agent even when there are no external goals. A wow-effect is formally defined as follows. As the agent is creating and predicting and encoding the continually growing history of actions and sensory inputs, it keeps improving the predictor or encoder, which can be implemented as an artificial neural network or some other machine learning device that can exploit regularities in the data to improve its performance over time. The improvements can be measured precisely, by computing the difference in computational costs (storage size, number of required synapses, errors, time) needed to encode new observations before and after learning. This difference depends on the encoder's present subjective knowledge, which changes over time, but the theory formally takes this into account. The cost difference measures the strength of the present "wow-effect" due to sudden improvements in data compression or computational speed. It becomes an intrinsic reward signal for the action selector. The objective function thus motivates the action optimizer to create action sequences causing more wow-effects. Irregular, random data (or noise) do not permit any wow-effects or learning progress, and thus are "boring" by nature (providing no reward). Already known and predictable regularities also are boring. Temporarily interesting are only the initially unknown, novel, regular patterns in both actions and observations. This motivates the agent to perform continual, open-ended, active, creative exploration. Schmidhuber's work is highly influential in intrinsic motivation which has emerged as a research topic in its own right as part of the study of artificial intelligence and robotics.

According to Schmidhuber, his objective function explains the activities of scientists, artists, and comedians.[149][150] For example, physicists are motivated to create experiments leading to observations obeying previously unpublished physical laws permitting better data compression. Likewise, composers receive intrinsic reward for creating non-arbitrary melodies with unexpected but regular harmonies that permit wow-effects through data compression improvements. Similarly, a comedian gets intrinsic reward for "inventing a novel joke with an unexpected punch line, related to the beginning of the story in an initially unexpected but quickly learnable way that also allows for better compression of the perceived data."[151] Schmidhuber argues that ongoing computer hardware advances will greatly scale up rudimentary artificial scientists and artists[clarification needed] based on simple implementations of the basic principle since 1990.[152] He used the theory to create low-complexity art[153] and an attractive human face.[154]

Creativity and mental health

A study by psychologist J. Philippe Rushton found creativity to correlate with intelligence and psychoticism.[155] Another study found creativity to be greater in people with schizotypal personality disorder than in people with either schizophrenia or those without mental health conditions. While divergent thinking was associated with bilateral activation of the prefrontal cortex, schizotypal individuals were found to have much greater activation of their right prefrontal cortex.[156] This study hypothesizes that such individuals are better at accessing both hemispheres, allowing them to make novel associations at a faster rate. Consistent with this hypothesis, ambidexterity is also more common in people with schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia. Three studies by Mark Batey and Adrian Furnham have demonstrated the relationships between schizotypal personalty disorder[157][158] and hypomanic personality[159] and several different measures of creativity.

Particularly strong links have been identified between creativity and mood disorders, particularly manic-depressive disorder (a.k.a. bipolar disorder) and depressive disorder (a.k.a. unipolar disorder). In Touched with Fire: Manic-Depressive Illness and the Artistic Temperament, Kay Redfield Jamison summarizes studies of mood-disorder rates in writers, poets, and artists. She also explores research that identifies mood disorders in such famous writers and artists as Ernest Hemingway (who shot himself after electroconvulsive treatment), Virginia Woolf (who drowned herself when she felt a depressive episode coming on), composer Robert Schumann (who died in a mental institution), and even the famed visual artist Michelangelo.

A study looking at 300,000 persons with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or unipolar depression, and their relatives, found overrepresentation in creative professions for those with bipolar disorder as well as for undiagnosed siblings of those with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. There was no overall overrepresentation, but overrepresentation for artistic occupations, among those diagnosed with schizophrenia. There was no association for those with unipolar depression or their relatives.[160]

Another study involving more than one million people, conducted by Swedish researchers at the Karolinska Institute, reported a number of correlations between creative occupations and mental illnesses. Writers had a higher risk of anxiety and bipolar disorders, schizophrenia, unipolar depression, and substance abuse, and were almost twice as likely as the general population to kill themselves. Dancers and photographers were also more likely to have bipolar disorder.[161]

As a group, those in the creative professions were no more likely to have psychiatric disorders than other people, although they were more likely to have a close relative with a disorder, including anorexia and, to some extent, autism, the Journal of Psychiatric Research reports.[161]

People who have worked in the arts industry throughout history have faced many environmental factors that are associated with and can sometimes influence mental illness. Including things such as poverty, persecution, social alienation, psychological trauma, substance abuse, and high stress.[162] In fact, according to psychologist Robert Epstein, PhD, creativity can be obstructed through stress.[163] So, while research has found that people are the most creative in positive moods,[164] it might be pursuing a career that causes some problems.

Conversely, research has shown that creative activities such as art therapy, poetry writing, journaling, and reminiscence can promote mental well-being.[165][166]

Bipolar Disorders and Creativity

Nancy Andreasen was one of the first known researchers to carry out a large scale study revolving around creativity and whether mental illnesses have an impact on someone's ability to be creative. Originally she had expected to find a link between creativity and schizophrenia but her research sample had no real history of schizophrenia from the book authors she pooled. Her findings instead showed that 80% of the creative group had previously had some form of mental illness episode in their lifetime.[167] When she performed follow up studies over a 15-year period, she found that 43% of the authors had bipolar disorder compared to the 1% of the general public that has the disease. In 1989 there was another study done by Kay Redfield Jamison that reaffirmed those statistics by having 38% of her sample of authors having a history of mood disorders.[168] Anthony Storr who is a prominent psychiatrist remarked that, "The creative process can be a way of protecting the individual against being overwhelmed by depression, a means of regaining a sense of mastery in those who have lost it, and, to a varying extent, a way of repairing the self-damaged by bereavement or by the loss of confidence in human relationships which accompanies depression from whatever cause."[167]

According to a study done by Shapiro and Weisberg, there appears to be a positive correlation between the manic upswings of the cycles of bipolar disorder and the ability for an individual to be more creative.[169] The data that they had collected and analyzed through multiple tests showed that it was in fact not the depressive swing that many believe to bring forth dark creative spurts, but the act of climbing out of the depressive episode that sparked creativity. The reason behind this spur of creative genius could come from the type of self-image that the person has during a time of hypomania. A hypomanic person may be feeling a bolstered sense of self-confidence, creative confidence, and sense of individualism.[169]

In reports from people who were diagnosed with bipolar disorder they noted themselves as having a larger range of emotional understanding, heightened states of perception, and an ability to connect better with those in the world around them.[170] Other reported traits include higher rates of productivity, higher senses of self-awareness, and a greater understanding of empathy. Those who have bipolar disorder also understand their own sense of heightened creativity and ability to get immense amounts of tasks done all at once. McCraw, Parker, Fletcher, & Friend (2013) report that out of 219 participants (aged 19 to 63) that have been diagnosed bipolar disorder 82% of them reported having elevated feelings of creativity during the hypomanic swings.[171]

Giannouli believes that the creativity a person diagnosed with bipolar disorder feels comes as a form of "stress management".[172] In the realm of music, one might be expressing their stress or pains through the pieces they write in order to better understand those same feelings. Famous authors and musicians along with some actors would often attribute their wild enthusiasm to something like a hypomanic state.[173] The artistic side of society has also been notorious for behaviors that are seen as maladapted to societal norms. Side effects that come with bipolar disorder match up with many of the behaviors that we see in high-profile creative personalities; these include, but are not limited to, alcohol addiction, drug abuse including stimulants, depressants, hallucinogens and dissociatives, opioids, inhalants, and cannabis, difficulties in holding regular occupations, interpersonal problems, legal issues, and a high risk of suicide.[173]

Weisberg believes that the state of mania sets "free the powers of a thinker". What he implies here is that not only has the person become more creative they have fundamentally changed the kind of thoughts they produce.[174] In a study done of poets, who seem to have especially high percentages of bipolar authors, it was found that over a period of three years those poets would have cycles of really creative and powerful works of poetry. The timelines over the three-year study looked at the poet's personal journals and their clinical records and found that the timelines between their most powerful poems matched that of their upswings in bipolar disorder.[174]

Personality

Creativity can be expressed in a number of different forms, depending on unique people and environments. A number of different theorists have suggested models of the creative person. One model suggests that there are four "Creativity Profiles" that can help produce growth, innovation, speed, etc.[175]

(i) Incubate (Long-term Development)
(ii) Imagine (Breakthrough Ideas)
(iii) Improve (Incremental Adjustments)
(iv) Invest (Short-term Goals)

Research by Dr Mark Batey of the Psychometrics at Work Research Group at Manchester Business School has suggested that the creative profile can be explained by four primary creativity traits with narrow facets within each

(i) "Idea Generation" (Fluency, Originality, Incubation and Illumination)
(ii) "Personality" (Curiosity and Tolerance for Ambiguity)
(iii) "Motivation" (Intrinsic, Extrinsic and Achievement)
(iv) "Confidence" (Producing, Sharing and Implementing)

This model was developed in a sample of 1000 working adults using the statistical techniques of Exploratory Factor Analysis followed by Confirmatory Factor Analysis by Structural Equation Modelling.[176]

An important aspect of the creativity profiling approach is to account for the tension between predicting the creative profile of an individual, as characterised by the psychometric approach, and the evidence that team creativity is founded on diversity and difference.[177]

One characteristic of creative people, as measured by some psychologists, is what is called divergent production. Divergent production is the ability of a person to generate a diverse assortment, yet an appropriate amount of responses to a given situation.[178] One way of measuring divergent production is by administering the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking.[179] The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking assesses the diversity, quantity, and appropriateness of participants responses to a variety of open-ended questions.

Other researchers of creativity see the difference in creative people as a cognitive process of dedication to problem solving and developing expertise in the field of their creative expression. Hard working people study the work of people before them and within their current area, become experts in their fields, and then have the ability to add to and build upon previous information in innovative and creative ways. In a study of projects by design students, students who had more knowledge on their subject on average had greater creativity within their projects.[180] Other researchers emphasize how creative people are better at balancing between divergent and convergent production, which depends on an individual's innate preference or ability to explore and exploit ideas.[64]

The aspect of motivation within a person's personality may predict creativity levels in the person. Motivation stems from two different sources, intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation is an internal drive within a person to participate or invest as a result of personal interest, desires, hopes, goals, etc. Extrinsic motivation is a drive from outside of a person and might take the form of payment, rewards, fame, approval from others, etc. Although extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation can both increase creativity in certain cases, strictly extrinsic motivation often impedes creativity in people.[181]

From a personality-traits perspective, there are a number of traits that are associated with creativity in people.[182] Creative people tend to be more open to new experiences, are more self-confident, are more ambitious, self-accepting, impulsive, driven, dominant, and hostile, compared to people with less creativity.

From an evolutionary perspective, creativity may be a result of the outcome of years of generating ideas. As ideas are continuously generated, the need to evolve produces a need for new ideas and developments. As a result, people have been creating and developing new, innovative, and creative ideas to build our progress as a society.[183]

In studying exceptionally creative people in history, some common traits in lifestyle and environment are often found. Creative people in history usually had supportive parents, but rigid and non-nurturing. Most had an interest in their field at an early age, and most had a highly supportive and skilled mentor in their field of interest. Often the field they chose was relatively uncharted, allowing for their creativity to be expressed more in a field with less previous information. Most exceptionally creative people devoted almost all of their time and energy into their craft, and after about a decade had a creative breakthrough of fame. Their lives were marked with extreme dedication and a cycle of hard-work and breakthroughs as a result of their determination.[184]

Another theory of creative people is the investment theory of creativity. This approach suggest that there are many individual and environmental factors that must exist in precise ways for extremely high levels of creativity opposed to average levels of creativity. In the investment sense, a person with their particular characteristics in their particular environment may see an opportunity to devote their time and energy into something that has been overlooked by others. The creative person develops an undervalued or under-recognised idea to the point that it is established as a new and creative idea. Just like in the financial world, some investments are worth the buy in, while others are less productive and do not build to the extent that the investor expected. This investment theory of creativity views creativity in a unique perspective compared to others, by asserting that creativity might rely to some extent on the right investment of effort being added to a field at the right time in the right way.[185]

Malevolent creativity

So called malevolent creativity is associated with the "dark side" of creativity.[186][187] This type of creativity is not typically accepted within society and is defined by the intention to cause harm to others through original and innovative means. Malevolent creativity should be distinguished from negative creativity in that negative creativity may unintentionally cause harm to others, whereas malevolent creativity is explicitly malevolently motivated. While it is often associated with criminal behaviour, it can also be observed in ordinary day-to-day life as lying, cheating and betrayal.[188]

Crime

Malevolent creativity is often a key contributor to crime and in its most destructive form can even manifest as terrorism. As creativity requires deviating from the conventional, there is a permanent tension between being creative and producing products that go too far and in some cases to the point of breaking the law. Aggression is a key predictor of malevolent creativity, and studies have also shown that increased levels of aggression also correlates to a higher likelihood of committing crime.[189]

Predictive factors

Although everyone shows some levels of malevolent creativity under certain conditions, those that have a higher propensity towards it have increased tendencies to deceive and manipulate others to their own gain. While malevolent creativity appears to dramatically increase when an individual is placed under unfair conditions, personality, particularly aggressiveness, is also a key predictor in anticipating levels of malevolent thinking. Researchers Harris and Reiter-Palmon investigated the role of aggression in levels of malevolent creativity, in particular levels of implicit aggression and the tendency to employ aggressive actions in response to problem solving. The personality traits of physical aggression, conscientiousness, emotional intelligence and implicit aggression all seem to be related with malevolent creativity.[187] Harris and Reiter-Palmon's research showed that when subjects were presented with a problem that triggered malevolent creativity, participants high in implicit aggression and low in premeditation expressed the largest number of malevolently-themed solutions. When presented with the more benign problem that triggered prosocial motives of helping others and cooperating, those high in implicit aggression, even if they were high in impulsiveness, were far less destructive in their imagined solutions. They concluded premeditation, more than implicit aggression controlled an individual's expression of malevolent creativity.[190]

The current measure for malevolent creativity is the 13-item test Malevolent Creativity Behaviour Scale (MCBS).[188]

Cultural differences in creativity

Creativity is viewed differently in different countries.[191] For example, cross-cultural research centered on Hong Kong found that Westerners view creativity more in terms of the individual attributes of a creative person, such as their aesthetic taste, while Chinese people view creativity more in terms of the social influence of creative people (i.e., what they can contribute to society).[192] Mpofu et al. surveyed 28 African languages and found that 27 had no word which directly translated to 'creativity' (the exception being Arabic).[193] The principle of linguistic relativity (i.e., that language can affect thought) suggests that the lack of an equivalent word for 'creativity' may affect the views of creativity among speakers of such languages. However, more research would be needed to establish this, and there is certainly no suggestion that this linguistic difference makes people any less (or more) creative; Africa has a rich heritage of creative pursuits such as music, art, and storytelling. Nevertheless, it is true that there has been very little research on creativity in Africa,[194] and there has also been very little research on creativity in Latin America.[195] Creativity has been more thoroughly researched in the northern hemisphere, but here again there are cultural differences, even between countries or groups of countries in close proximity. For example, in Scandinavian countries, creativity is seen as an individual attitude which helps in coping with life's challenges,[196] while in Germany, creativity is seen more as a process that can be applied to help solve problems.[197]

Organizational creativity

 
Training meeting in an eco-design stainless steel company in Brazil. The leaders among other things wish to cheer and encourage the workers in order to achieve a higher level of creativity.

It has been the topic of various research studies to establish that organizational effectiveness depends on the creativity of the workforce to a large extent. For any given organization, measures of effectiveness vary, depending upon its mission, environmental context, nature of work, the product or service it produces, and customer demands. Thus, the first step in evaluating organizational effectiveness is to understand the organization itself – how it functions, how it is structured, and what it emphasizes.

Amabile[198] and Sullivan and Harper[199] argued that to enhance creativity in business, three components were needed:

  • Expertise (technical, procedural and intellectual knowledge),
  • Creative thinking skills (how flexibly and imaginatively people approach problems),
  • and Motivation (especially intrinsic motivation).

There are two types of motivation:

Six managerial practices to encourage motivation are:

  • Challenge – matching people with the right assignments;
  • Freedom – giving people autonomy choosing means to achieve goals;
  • Resources – such as time, money, space, etc. There must be balance fit among resources and people;
  • Work group features – diverse, supportive teams, where members share the excitement, willingness to help, and recognize each other's talents;
  • Supervisory encouragement – recognitions, cheering, praising;
  • Organizational support – value emphasis, information sharing, collaboration.

Nonaka, who examined several successful Japanese companies, similarly saw creativity and knowledge creation as being important to the success of organizations.[200] In particular, he emphasized the role that tacit knowledge has to play in the creative process.

In business, originality is not enough. The idea must also be appropriate – useful and actionable.[198][201] Creative competitive intelligence is a new solution to solve this problem. According to Reijo Siltala it links creativity to innovation process and competitive intelligence to creative workers.

Creativity can be encouraged in people and professionals and in the workplace. It is essential for innovation, and is a factor affecting economic growth and businesses. In 2013, the sociologist Silvia Leal Martín, using the Innova 3DX method, suggested measuring the various parameters that encourage creativity and innovation: corporate culture, work environment, leadership and management, creativity, self-esteem and optimism, locus of control and learning orientation, motivation, and fear.[202]

Similarly, social psychologists, organizational scientists, and management scientists (who conduct extensive research on the factors that influence creativity and innovation in teams and organizations) have developed integrative theoretical models that emphasize the roles of team composition, team processes, and organizational culture. These theoretical models also emphasize the mutually reinforcing relationships between them in promoting innovation.[203][204][205][206]

The investigation by Sai Loo, an academic and author of research monographs,[207] on creative working in the knowledge economy brings together studies of creativity as described in this web page. It offers connections with the sections on the '"Four C" model', 'Theories of creative processes', 'Creativity as a subset of intelligence', 'Creativity and personality', and 'In organisations'. It is the last section that the investigation addresses.

Research studies of the knowledge economy may be classified into three levels: macro, meso and micro. Macro studies refer to investigations at a societal or transnational dimension. Meso studies focus on organisations. Micro investigations centre on the minutiae workings of workers. There is also an interdisciplinary dimension such as research from businesses,[208][209] economics,[210][211][212] education,[213][214] human resource management,[215] knowledge and organizational management,[216][217][218] sociology, psychology, and knowledge economy-related sectors – especially information technology (IT) software[219][220] and advertising.[221][222]

Loo studies how individual workers in the knowledge economy use their creativity and know-how in the advertising and IT software sectors. It examines this phenomenon across three developed countries of England, Japan, and Singapore to observe global perspectives. Specifically, the study uses qualitative data from semi-structured interviews of the related professionals in the roles of creative directing and copywriting (in advertising), and systems software developing and software programme managing.[207]

The study[207] offers a conceptual framework of a two-dimensional matrix of individual and collaborative working styles, and single and multi-contexts. The investigation draws on literature sources from the four disciplines of economics,[223][224] management,[225][226][227] sociology,[228][229][230][231][232] and psychology.[233][234] The themes arising from the analysis of knowledge work and creativity literature serve to create a distinct theoretical framework of creative knowledge work. These workers apply their cognitive abilities, creative personalities and skill sets in the areas of science, technology, or culture industries to invent or discover new possibilities – e.g. a medium, product or service. These work activities may be done individually or collectively. Education, training and 'encultured environments' are necessary for the performance of these creative activities. Acts of creativity are viewed as asking new questions over and above those questions asked by an intelligent person, seeking novelty when reviewing a situation,[235] and creating something that is different and novel, i.e. a 'variation' on the idea of existing ideas in a domain.[236] This framework is evidenced by the empirical chapters on the micro-workings of creative workers in the two knowledge economy sectors from global perspectives.

This investigation identifies a definition of creative work, three types of work and the necessary conditions for it to occur. These workers use a combination of creative applications including anticipatory imagination, problem-solving, problem seeking, and generating ideas and aesthetic sensibilities. Taking aesthetic sensibilities as an example, for a creative director in the advertising industry, it is a visual imagery whether still or moving via a camera lens, and for a software programmer, it is the innovative technical expertise in which the software is written. There are specific creative applications for each of the sectors such as emotional connection in the advertising sector, and the power of expression and sensitivity in the IT software sector. In addition to the creative applications, creative workers require abilities and aptitudes to carry out their roles. Passion for one's job is generic. For copywriters, this passion is identified with fun, enjoyment and happiness alongside attributes such as honesty (regarding the product), confidence, and patience in finding the appropriate copy. Knowledge is also required in the disciplines of the humanities (e.g. literature), the creative arts (e.g. painting and music) and technical-related know-how (e.g. mathematics, computer sciences and physical sciences). In the IT software, technical knowledge of computer languages (e.g. C++) is especially significant for programmers whereas the degree of technical expertise may be less for a programme manager, as only knowledge of the relevant language is necessary to understand the issues for communicating with the team of developers and testers.

There are three types of work. One is intra-sectoral (e.g. 'general sponge' and 'in tune with the zeitgeist' [advertising], and 'power of expression' and 'sensitivity' [IT software]). The second is inter-sectoral (e.g. 'integration of advertising activities' [advertising], and 'autonomous decentralized systems' [ADS] [IT software]). The third relates to changes in culture/practices in the sectors (e.g. 'three-dimensional trust' and 'green credentials' [advertising], and 'collaboration with HEIs and industry' and 'ADS system in the Tokyo train operator' [IT software]).

The necessary conditions for creative work to exist are a supportive environment such as supportive information, communications and electronic technologies (ICET) infrastructure, training, work environment and education.

This investigation has implications for lifelong learning of these workers informally and formally. Teaching institutions need to offer multi-disciplinary knowledge of humanities, arts and sciences and it has impacts on the programme structure, delivery approaches and assessments. At a macro level, governments need to offer a rich diet of cultural activities, outdoor activities and sports fixtures that inform potential creative workers in the areas of video gaming and advertising. This study has implications for work organisations that support and encourage collaborative working alongside individual working, offer opportunities to engage in continuous professional development (formally and informally), and foster an environment, which promotes experiential functioning and supports experimentation.

Team composition

Diversity between team members' backgrounds and knowledge can increase team creativity by expanding the total collection of unique information that is available to the team and by introducing different perspectives that can integrate in novel ways. However, under some conditions, diversity can also decrease team creativity by making it more difficult for team members to communicate about ideas and causing interpersonal conflicts between those with different perspectives.[237] Thus, the potential advantages of diversity must be supported by appropriate team processes and organizational cultures in order to enhance creativity.[203][204][205][206][238][239] Recent study by An Zeng et al.[240] found that studies of fresh research teams is associated with higher creativity or originality.

Team processes

Team communication norms, such as respecting others' expertise, paying attention to others' ideas, expecting information sharing, tolerating disagreements, negotiating, remaining open to others' ideas, learning from others, and building on each other's ideas, increase team creativity by facilitating the social processes involved with brainstorming and problem solving. Through these processes, team members are able to access their collective pool of knowledge, reach shared understandings, identify new ways of understanding problems or tasks, and make new connections between ideas. Engaging in these social processes also promotes positive team affect, which facilitates collective creativity.[203][205][206][238]

Organizational culture

Supportive and motivational environments that create psychological safety by encouraging risk taking and tolerating mistakes increase team creativity as well.[203][204][205][206] Organizations in which help-seeking, help giving, and collaboration are rewarded promote innovation by providing opportunities and contexts in which team processes that lead to collective creativity can occur.[241] Additionally, leadership styles that downplay status hierarchies or power differences within an organization and empower people to speak up about their ideas or opinions also help to create cultures that are conducive to creativity.[203][204][205][206]

Constraints

There is a long-standing debate on how material constraints (e.g., lack of money, materials, or equipment) affect creativity. In psychological and managerial research, two competing views in this regard prevail. In one view, many scholars propose a negative effect of material constraints on innovation and claim that material constraints starve creativity.[242] The proponents of this view argue that adequate material resources are needed to engage in creative activities like experimenting with new solutions and idea exploration.[242] In an opposing view, scholars assert that people tend to stick to established routines or solutions as long as they are not forced to deviate from them by constraints.[243][244][245] In this sense, Neren posits that scarcity is an important driver of creativity.[246] Consistently, Gibbert and Scranton demonstrated how material constraints facilitated the development of jet engines in World War II.[247]

To reconcile these competing views, contingency models were proposed.[248][249][250] The rationale behind these models is that certain contingency factors (e.g., creativity climate or creativity relevant skills) influence the relationship between constraints and creativity.[248] These contingency factors reflect the need for higher levels of motivation and skills when working on creative tasks under constraints.[248] Depending on these contingency factors, there is either a positive or negative relationship between constraints and creativity.[248][249]

The sociology of creativity

Creativity research for most of the twentieth century was dominated by psychology and business studies, with little work done in sociology as a discipline. While since the turn of the millennium, there has been more attention paid by sociological researchers,[251][252] it has yet to establish itself as a specific research field, with reviews of sociological research into creativity a rarity in high impact literature.[253]

While psychology has tended to focus on the individual as the locus of creativity, sociological research is directed more at the structures and context within which creative activity takes place, primarily based in the more long-standing field of the sociology of culture, which finds its roots in the works of Marx, Durkheim, and Weber. This has meant a particular focus on the cultural and creative industries as sociological phenomena. Such research has covered a variety of areas, including the economics and production of culture, the role of creative industries in development, and the rise of the "creative class".[254]

Economic views

Economic approaches to creativity have focussed on three aspects – the impact of creativity on economic growth, methods of modelling markets for creativity, and the maximisation of economic creativity (innovation).

In the early 20th century, Joseph Schumpeter introduced the economic theory of creative destruction, to describe the way in which old ways of doing things are endogenously destroyed and replaced by the new. Some economists (such as Paul Romer) view creativity as an important element in the recombination of elements to produce new technologies and products and, consequently, economic growth. Creativity leads to capital, and creative products are protected by intellectual property laws.

Mark A. Runco and Daniel Rubenson have tried to describe a "psychoeconomic" model of creativity.[255] In such a model, creativity is the product of endowments and active investments in creativity; the costs and benefits of bringing creative activity to market determine the supply of creativity. Such an approach has been criticised for its view of creativity consumption as always having positive utility, and for the way it analyses the value of future innovations.[256]

The creative class is seen by some to be an important driver of modern economies. In his 2002 book, The Rise of the Creative Class, economist Richard Florida popularized the notion that regions with "3 T's of economic development: Technology, Talent and Tolerance" also have high concentrations of creative professionals and tend to have a higher level of economic development.[257]

Fostering creativity

Several different researchers have proposed methods of increasing the creativity of an individual. Such ideas range from the psychological-cognitive, such as Osborn-Parnes Creative Problem Solving Process, Synectics, science-based creative thinking, Purdue Creative Thinking Program, and Edward de Bono's lateral thinking; to the highly structured, such as TRIZ (the Theory of Inventive Problem-Solving) and its variant Algorithm of Inventive Problem Solving (developed by the Russian scientist Genrich Altshuller), and Computer-Aided morphological analysis.

Daniel Pink, in his 2005 book A Whole New Mind, repeating arguments posed throughout the 20th century, argues that we are entering a new age where creativity is becoming increasingly important. In this conceptual age, we will need to foster and encourage right-directed thinking (representing creativity and emotion) over left-directed thinking (representing logical, analytical thought). However, this simplification of 'right' versus 'left' brain thinking is not supported by the research data.[258]

Nickerson[259] provides a summary of the various creativity techniques that have been proposed. These include approaches that have been developed by both academia and industry:

  1. Establishing purpose and intention
  2. Building basic skills
  3. Encouraging acquisitions of domain-specific knowledge
  4. Stimulating and rewarding curiosity and exploration
  5. Building motivation, especially internal motivation
  6. Encouraging confidence and a willingness to take risks
  7. Focusing on mastery and self-competition
  8. Promoting supportable beliefs about creativity
  9. Providing opportunities for choice and discovery
  10. Developing self-management (metacognitive skills)
  11. Teaching techniques and strategies for facilitating creative performance
  12. Providing balance


Managing the need for closure

Experiments suggest the need for closure of task participants, whether as a reflection of personality or induced (through time pressure), negatively impacts creativity.[260] Accordingly, it has been suggested that reading fiction, which can reduce the cognitive need for closure, may help to encourage creativity.[261]

Education policies

Some see the conventional system of schooling as stifling of creativity and attempt (particularly in the preschool/kindergarten and early school years) to provide a creativity-friendly, rich, imagination-fostering environment for young children.[259][262][263] Researchers have seen this as important because technology is advancing our society at an unprecedented rate and creative problem solving will be needed to cope with these challenges as they arise.[263] In addition to helping with problem solving, creativity also helps students identify problems where others have failed to do so.[259][262][264] See the Waldorf School as an example of an education program that promotes creative thought.

Promoting intrinsic motivation and problem solving are two areas where educators can foster creativity in students. Students are more creative when they see a task as intrinsically motivating, valued for its own sake.[262][263][265][266] To promote creative thinking, educators need to identify what motivates their students and structure teaching around it. Providing students with a choice of activities to complete allows them to become more intrinsically motivated and therefore creative in completing the tasks.[259][267]

Teaching students to solve problems that do not have well defined answers is another way to foster their creativity. This is accomplished by allowing students to explore problems and redefine them, possibly drawing on knowledge that at first may seem unrelated to the problem in order to solve it.[259][262][263][265] In adults, mentoring individuals is another way to foster their creativity.[268] However, the benefits of mentoring creativity apply only to creative contributions considered great in a given field, not to everyday creative expression.[88]

Musical creativity is a gateway to the flow state. A place conducive to spontaneity, improvisation, and creativity. Recent studies have shown that we should emphasize students' creative side and integrate more creativity into their curriculums. A reason for this is that "Students aiming to express themselves and connect with peers through music will be able to tap into higher-order brain regions during improvisation rather than limiting themselves to the areas seen for rudimentary pattern generator" (Landau, 2017:30). Schools should teach this Buddhist philosophy to help musicians, for example, better understand what's happening when you're in a creative process and ways to realize it better. In this sense, an improvisation is a form of self-expression that can generate connectivity amongst peers and surpasses the age-old rudimentary aspects of school.

Scotland

In the Scottish education system, creativity is identified as a core skillset for learning, life and work and is defined as "a process which generates ideas that have value to the individual. It involves looking at familiar things with a fresh eye, examining problems with an open mind, making connections, learning from mistakes and using imagination to explore new possibilities."[269] The need to develop a shared language and understanding of creativity and its role across every aspect of learning, teaching and continuous improvement was identified as a necessary aim[270] and a set of four skills is used to allow educators to discuss and develop creativity skills across all subjects and sectors of education – curiosity, open-mindedness, imagination and problem solving.[271] Distinctions are made between creative learning (when learners are using their creativity skills), creative teaching (when educators are using their creativity skills) and creative change (when creativity skills are applied to planning and improvement). [1] Scotland's national Creative Learning Plan[272] supports the development of creativity skills in all learners and of educators' expertise in developing creativity skills. A range of resources have been created to support and assess this [2] including a national review of creativity across learning by Her Majesty's Inspectorate for Education.[269]

Academic journals

See also

Notes

  1. ^ Anderson, Neil; Potočnik, Kristina; Zhou, Jing (July 2014). "Innovation and Creativity in Organizations: A State-of-the-Science Review, Prospective Commentary, and Guiding Framework". Journal of Management. 40 (5): 1297–1333. doi:10.1177/0149206314527128. hdl:10454/16825. ISSN 0149-2063. S2CID 44041503.
  2. ^ Zhou, Jing; Wang, Xiaoye May; Bavato, Davide; Tasselli, Stefano; Wu, Junfeng (July 2019). "Understanding the Receiving Side of Creativity: A Multidisciplinary Review and Implications for Management Research". Journal of Management. 45 (6): 2570–2595. doi:10.1177/0149206319827088. ISSN 0149-2063. S2CID 150033432.
  3. ^ "And eke Job saith, that in hell is no order of rule. And albeit that God hath created all things in right order, and nothing without order, but all things be ordered and numbered, yet nevertheless they that be damned be not in order, nor hold no order."
  4. ^ a b c d e Runco, Mark A.; Albert, Robert S. (2010). "Creativity Research". In James C. Kaufman; Robert J. Sternberg (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-73025-9.
  5. ^ Mumford, M. D. (2003). "Where have we been, where are we going? Taking stock in creativity research". Creativity Research Journal. 15 (2–3): 107–120. doi:10.1080/10400419.2003.9651403. S2CID 218546467.
  6. ^ Sternberg, Robert J. (2011). "Creativity". Cognitive Psychology (6 ed.). Cengage Learning. p. 479. ISBN 978-1-133-38701-5.
  7. ^ Meusburger, Peter (2009). "Milieus of Creativity: The Role of Places, Environments and Spatial Contexts". In Meusburger, P.; Funke, J.; Wunder, E. (eds.). Milieus of Creativity: An Interdisciplinary Approach to Spatiality of Creativity. Springer. ISBN 978-1-4020-9876-5.
  8. ^ Torrance, Paul. "Verbal Tests. Forms A and B-Figural Tests, Forms A and B.". The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking-Norms-Technical Manual Research Edition. Princeton, New Jersey: Personnel Press. p. 6.
  9. ^ Amabile, Teresa M.; Pratt, Michael G. (2016). "The dynamic componential model of creativity and innovation in organizations: Making progress, making meaning". Research in Organizational Behavior. 36: 157–183. doi:10.1016/j.riob.2016.10.001.
  10. ^ Oslo Manual 2018: Guidelines for Collecting, Reporting and Using Data on Innovation (4th ed.). Paris/Luxembourg: OECD/Eurostat. 2018. p. 44.
  11. ^ Averill, James R. (February 1999). "Individual Differences in Emotional Creativity: Structure and Correlates". Journal of Personality. 67 (2): 331–371. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00058. ISSN 0022-3506. PMID 10202807.
  12. ^ Ivcevic, Zorana; Brackett, Marc A.; Mayer, John D. (April 2007). "Emotional Intelligence and Emotional Creativity". Journal of Personality. 75 (2): 199–236. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00437.x. ISSN 0022-3506. PMID 17359237.
  13. ^ Rhodes, Mel (1961). "An Analysis of Creativity". The Phi Delta Kappan. 42 (7): 305–310. JSTOR 20342603.
  14. ^ Gruszka, Aleksandra; Tang, Min (2017). "The 4P's creativity model and its application in different fields". In Tang, Lisa Min; Werner, Christian (eds.). Handbook of the management of creativity and innovation: Theory and practice. World Scientific Publishing Company. pp. 51–71. ISBN 9789813141896.
  15. ^ Sternberg, Robert J. (2009). Jaime A. Perkins; Dan Moneypenny; Wilson Co (eds.). Cognitive Psychology. CENGAGE Learning. p. 468. ISBN 978-0-495-50629-4.
  16. ^ Glǎveanu, Vlad Petre (2013). "Rewriting the language of creativity: The five A's framework". Review of General Psychology. 17 (1): 69-81. doi:10.1037/a0029528. S2CID 143404705.
  17. ^ Mattson, David; Mathew, Katie; Katz-Buonincontro, Jen (2021). "Media Analysis of News Articles During COVID-19: Renewal, Continuity and Cultural Dimensions of Creative Action". Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 601938. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.601938. PMC 7920979. PMID 33664688.
  18. ^ Sun, Jingyan; Okada, Takeshi (2021). "The process of interactive role-making in acting training". Thinking Skills and Creativity. 41: 100860. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2021.100860.
  19. ^ a b c Władysław Tatarkiewicz, A History of Six Ideas: an Essay in Aesthetics, p. 244.
  20. ^ Albert, R. S.; Runco, M. A. (1999). "A History of Research on Creativity". In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press.
  21. ^ Plato, The Republic, Book X – wikisource:The Republic/Book X
  22. ^ Albert, R. S.; Runco, M. A. (1999). "A History of Research on Creativity". In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press. p. 5.
  23. ^ a b Niu, Weihua; Sternberg, Robert J. (2006). (PDF). Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. 26 (1–2): 18–38. doi:10.1037/h0091265. S2CID 143648016. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 December 2011. Retrieved 23 October 2010.; cf. Michel Weber, "Creativity, Efficacy and Vision: Ethics and Psychology in an Open Universe," in Michel Weber and Pierfrancesco Basile (eds.), Subjectivity, Process, and Rationality, Frankfurt/Lancaster, ontos verlag, Process Thought XIV, 2006, pp. 263-281.
  24. ^ a b c Dacey, John (1999). "Concepts of Creativity: A history". In Mark A. Runco; Steven R. Pritzer (eds.). Encyclopedia of Creativity, Vol. 1. Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-12-227076-5.
  25. ^ a b c Albert, R. S.; Runco, M. A. (1999). "A History of Research on Creativity". In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press. p. 6.
  26. ^ "Humanism - Rome Reborn: The Vatican Library & Renaissance Culture | Exhibitions - Library of Congress". www.loc.gov. 1993-01-08. Retrieved 2015-11-23.
  27. ^ "Leonardo da Vinci | Italian artist, engineer, and scientist". Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 2015-11-23.
  28. ^ Tatarkiewicz, Władysław (1980). A History of Six Ideas: an Essay in Aesthetics. Translated from the Polish by Christopher Kasparek, The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.
  29. ^ Wallas, G. (1926). Art of Thought.
  30. ^ Simonton, D. K. (1999). Origins of genius: Darwinian perspectives on creativity. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-512879-6.
  31. ^ Whitehead, Alfred North (1978). Process, and reality : an essay in cosmology ; Gifford Lectures delivered in the University of Edinburgh during the session 1927–28 (Corrected ed.). New York: Free Press. ISBN 978-0-02-934580-1.
  32. ^ Meyer, Steven (2005). "Introduction: Whitehead Now". Configurations. 1 (13): 1–33. doi:10.1353/con.2007.0010.. Cf. Michel Weber and Will Desmond (eds.). Handbook of Whiteheadian Process Thought (Frankfurt / Lancaster, Ontos Verlag, Process Thought X1 & X2, 2008) and Ronny Desmet & Michel Weber (edited by), Whitehead. The Algebra of Metaphysics. Applied Process Metaphysics Summer Institute Memorandum, Louvain-la-Neuve, Les Éditions Chromatika, 2010.
  33. ^ Hargreaves, H. L. (1927). "The faculty of imagination: An enquiry concerning the existence of a general faculty, or group factor, of imagination". British Journal of Psychology. Monograph Supplement 3: 1–74.
  34. ^ a b Sternberg, R. J.; Lubart, T. I. (1999). "The Concept of Creativity: Prospects and Paradigms". In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-57285-9.
  35. ^ a b Kozbelt, Aaron; Beghetto, Ronald A.; Runco, Mark A. (2010). "Theories of Creativity". In James C. Kaufman; Robert J. Sternberg (eds.). The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-73025-9.
  36. ^ Kaufman, James C.; Beghetto, Ronald A. (2009). "Beyond Big and Little: The Four C Model of Creativity". Review of General Psychology. 13 (1): 1–12. doi:10.1037/a0013688. S2CID 41410038.
  37. ^ Boden, Margaret (2004). The Creative Mind: Myths And Mechanisms. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-297-82069-7.
  38. ^ Robinson, Ken (1998). (PDF). National Advisory Committee on Creative and Cultural Education. Archived from the original (PDF) on 16 October 2014. Retrieved 2 October 2010.
  39. ^ a b Craft, Anna (2001). "'Little C' creativity". In Craft, A.; Jeffrey, B.; Leibling, M. (eds.). Creativity in education. Continuum International. ISBN 978-0-8264-4863-7.
  40. ^ Csíkszentmihályi, Mihály (1996). Creativity:Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention. Harper Collins. ISBN 978-0-06-092820-9.
  41. ^ Simonton, D. K. (1997). "Creative Productivity: A Predictive and Explanatory Model of Career Trajectories and Landmarks". Psychological Review. 104 (1): 66–89. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.66. S2CID 13547975.
  42. ^ Smith, S. M. (2011). "Incubation". In M. A. Runco; S. R. Pritzker (eds.). Encyclopedia of Creativity Volume I (2nd ed.). Academic Press. pp. 653–657. ISBN 978-0-12-375039-6.
  43. ^ "Anderson, J. R. (2000). Cognitive psychology and its implications. Worth Publishers. ISBN 978-0-7167-1686-0.
  44. ^ Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly (1996). Creativity: Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention. New York: Harper Perennial. ISBN 0-06-092820-4
  45. ^ Ward, T. (2003). "Creativity". In Nagel, L. (ed.). Encyclopaedia of Cognition. New York: Macmillan.
  46. ^ Smith, Steven M. (1995). "Fixation, Incubation, and Insight in Memory and Creative Thinking". In Steven M. Smith; Thomas B. Ward; Ronald A. Finke (eds.). The Creative Cognition Approach. MIT Press.
  47. ^ a b Guilford, J. P. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence.
  48. ^ Skalski, J. (2021, 10, 25). Adult Development: Creativity [Instruction of creativity and related components]. PSYCH 322 Adult Development, Brigham Young University-Idaho.
  49. ^ "The Relationship Between Individual Creativity Andcollective Intelligence in Modern Chinese Society". Knowledge Cultures. 7 (2): 35. 2019. doi:10.22381/kc7220197. ISSN 2327-5731.
  50. ^ Finke, R.; Ward, T. B.; Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-06150-6.
  51. ^ Ward, T.B. (1995). What's old about new ideas. In S. M. Smith, T. B. Ward & R. A. & Finke (Eds.) The creative cognition approach, 157–178, London: MIT Press.
  52. ^ Weisberg, R. W. (1993). Creativity: Beyond the myth of genius. Freeman. ISBN 978-0-7167-2119-2.
  53. ^ Helie S.; Sun R. (2010). "Incubation, insight, and creative problem solving: A unified theory and a connectionist model". Psychological Review. 117 (3): 994–1024. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.405.2245. doi:10.1037/a0019532. PMID 20658861.
  54. ^ Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. London: Pan Books. ISBN 978-0-330-73116-4.
  55. ^ Gabora, L. & Saab, A. (2011). Creative interference and states of potentiality in analogy problem solving. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. July 20–23, 2011, Boston MA.
  56. ^ Roese, N. J. & Olson, J. M. (1995). What Might Have Been: The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking. Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum
  57. ^ Markman, K. Klein, W. & Suhr, E. (eds) (2009). Handbook of mental simulation and the human imagination. Hove, Psychology Press
  58. ^ Byrne, R. M. J. (2005). The Rational Imagination: How People Create Counterfactual Alternatives to Reality. MIT Press. ISBN 9780262025843.
  59. ^ Dervin, Daniel (1990). Creativity and Culture: A Psychoanalytic Study of the Creative Process in the Arts, Sciences, and Culture. Fairleigh Dickinson University Press. ISBN 978-0-8386-3366-3.
  60. ^ Runco, M. & Pritzker, S.: Encyclopedia of Creativity, Academic Press, 2020
  61. ^ Holm-Hadulla, R.M. & Wendt, A.N. (2020). Dialectical Thinking. In: Encyclopedia of Creativity, 3rd edition, Runco, M. & Pritzker, S. (eds.), Academic Press
  62. ^ Widiger, T. A., Crego, C. (2019): The five factor model of personality structure. World Psychiatry, 18:3, p. 271-272
  63. ^ Holm-Hadulla R. M., Hofmann F.H. (2012): Counseling, Psychotherapy and Creativity
  64. ^ a b c Lin, Hause; Vartanian, Oshin (2018). "A Neuroeconomic Framework for Creative Cognition". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 13 (6): 655–677. doi:10.1177/1745691618794945. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 30304640. S2CID 206778956.
  65. ^ Beaty, Roger E.; Benedek, Mathias; Silvia, Paul J.; Schacter, Daniel L. (2016). "Creative Cognition and Brain Network Dynamics". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 20 (2): 87–95. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004. ISSN 1364-6613. PMC 4724474. PMID 26553223.
  66. ^ Aston-Jones, Gary; Cohen, Jonathan D. (2005). "An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain and optimal performance". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 28 (1): 403–450. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709. ISSN 0147-006X. PMID 16022602. S2CID 535645.
  67. ^ Epstein, Robert (November 1991). "Skinner, Creativity, and the Problem of Spontaneous Behavior". Psychological Science. 2 (6): 362–370. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00168.x. ISSN 0956-7976. S2CID 146725916.
  68. ^ Abra, Jock (1988). "Skinner on Creativity: A Critical Commentary". Leonardo. 21 (4): 407–412. doi:10.2307/1578703. JSTOR 1578703. S2CID 147669813.
  69. ^ Sumner, Sarah (December 2021). "How Can We Talk about Creativity?". The Psychological Record. 71 (4): 503–507. doi:10.1007/s40732-021-00505-7. ISSN 0033-2933. S2CID 245350174.
  70. ^ a b Sternberg, Robert J.; Grigorenko, Elena L. (8 June 2010). "Guilford's Structure of Intellect Model and Model of Creativity: Contributions and Limitations". Creativity Research Journal. 13 (3–4): 309–316. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1334_08. ISSN 1040-0419. S2CID 145796128.
  71. ^ Guilford, J. P. (1957). "Creative abilities in the arts". Psychological Review. 64 (2): 110–118. doi:10.1037/h0048280. ISSN 1939-1471. PMID 13420286.
  72. ^ Kim, K. H. (2006). "Can We Trust Creativity Tests? A Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT)". Creativity Research Journal. 18: 3–14. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1801_2. S2CID 17636888.
  73. ^ a b Zeng, L.; Proctor, R. W.; Salvendy, G. (2011). "Can Traditional Divergent Thinking Tests Be Trusted in Measuring and Predicting Real-World Creativity?". Creativity Research Journal. 23: 24–37. doi:10.1080/10400419.2011.545713. S2CID 11322958.
  74. ^ (Torrance, 1974)
  75. ^ Kim, Kyung Hee (2006). "Can We Trust Creativity Tests?" (PDF). Creativity Research Journal. 18 (1): 3–14. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1801_2. S2CID 17636888.
  76. ^ Forster, E. A., & Dunbar, K. N. (2009). Creativity evaluation through latent semantic analysis. In Proceedings of the 31st Annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 602–607).
  77. ^ Harbison, I. J., & Haarmann, H. (2014). Automated scoring of originality using semantic representations. In Proceedings of the 36th Annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (poster paper).
  78. ^ Acar, S.; Runco, M. A. (2014). "Assessing associative distance among ideas elicited by tests of Divergent Thinking". Creativity Research Journal. 26 (2): 229–238. doi:10.1080/10400419.2014.901095. S2CID 146788570.
  79. ^ NSF SBIR Grant Number 1315053.
  80. ^ Other members include Kenes Beketayev PhD Computer Science; Liberty Lidz, PhD Linguistics; Perman Gochyyev, PhD Statistics
  81. ^ Beketayev, K.; Runco, M. A. (2016). "Scoring Divergent Thinking Tests by Computer With a Semantics-Based Algorithm". Europe's Journal of Psychology. 12 (2): 210–220. doi:10.5964/ejop.v12i2.1127. PMC 4894287. PMID 27298632.
  82. ^ a b Feist, G. J. (1998). "A meta-analysis of the impact of personality on scientific and artistic creativity". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2 (4): 290–309. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0204_5. PMID 15647135. S2CID 24067985.
  83. ^ Batey, M.; Furnham, A. (2006). "Creativity, intelligence and personality: A critical review of the scattered literature". Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs. 132 (4): 355–429. doi:10.3200/mono.132.4.355-430. PMID 18341234. S2CID 7435403.
  84. ^ Batey, M.; Furnham, A. F.; Safiullina, X. (2010). "Intelligence, General Knowledge and Personality as Predictors of Creativity". Learning and Individual Differences. 20 (5): 532–535. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2010.04.008.
  85. ^ Carson, S. H.; Peterson, J. B.; Higgins, D. M. (2005). "Reliability, Validity, and Factor Structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire". Creativity Research Journal. 17 (1): 37–50. doi:10.1207/s15326934crj1701_4. S2CID 146304521.
  86. ^ a b Ying-Yao, Wang, Chia-Chi|Ho, Hsiao-Chi|Cheng, Chih-Ling|Cheng (2014). "Application of the Rasch Model to the Measurement of Creativity: The Creative Achievement Questionnaire". Creativity Research Journal. 26 (1). ISSN 1040-0419.
  87. ^ Bendetowicz, David; Urbanski, Marika; Aichelburg, Clarisse; Levy, Richard; Volle, Emmanuelle (January 2017). "Brain morphometry predicts individual creative potential and the ability to combine remote ideas" (PDF). Cortex. 86: 216–229. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2016.10.021. ISSN 0010-9452. PMID 27919546. S2CID 13248682.
  88. ^ a b Form, Sven; Schlichting, Kerrin; Kaernbach, Christian (November 2017). "Mentoring functions: Interpersonal tensions are associated with mentees' creative achievement". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 11 (4): 440–450. doi:10.1037/aca0000103. ISSN 1931-390X. S2CID 148927589.
  89. ^ a b c Getzels, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence: Explorations with gifted students. New York: Wiley.
  90. ^ a b c d Barron, F. (1963). Creativity and psychological health. Princeton: D. Van Nostrand Company.
  91. ^ a b c Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children: A study of the creativity-intelligence distinction. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
  92. ^ a b Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.
  93. ^ Plucker, J., & Renzulli, J. S. (1999). Psychometric approaches to the study of human creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 35–60). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  94. ^ Hayes, J. R. (1989). Cognitive processes in creativity. In J. A. Glover, R. R. Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 135–145). New York: Plenum.
  95. ^ a b Sternberg, R. J. (1996). Successful Intelligence. New York: Simon & Schuster.
  96. ^ a b Sternberg, R. J., & O'Hara, L. A. (1999). Creativity and intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 251–272). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.
  97. ^ Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2008). Applied intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  98. ^ Kaufman, J. C.; Kaufman, S. B.; Lichtenberger, E. O. (2011). "Finding creativity on intelligence tests via divergent production". Canadian Journal of School Psychology. 26 (2): 83–106. doi:10.1177/0829573511406511. S2CID 18061207.
  99. ^ Silvia, P. J.; Beaty, R. E.; Nusbaum, E. C. (2013). "Verbal fluency and creativity: General and specific contributions of broad retrieval ability (Gr) factors to divergent thinking". Intelligence. 41 (5): 328–340. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.05.004.
  100. ^ Martindale, C. (1999). Biological bases of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 137– 152). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  101. ^ Kaufman, J.C., Kaufman, S.B., & Plucker, J.A. (2013). Contemporary theories of intelligence. In J. Reisberg (Ed.), The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology (pp. 811-822). New York, NY: Oxford University Press
  102. ^ Sternberg, R. J.; Lubart, T. I. (1991). "An investment theory of creativity and its development". Human Development. 34: 1–32. doi:10.1159/000277029.
  103. ^ Sternberg, R. J.; Lubart, T. I. (1992). "Buy low and sell high: An investment approach to creativity". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 1 (1): 1–5. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.1992.tb00002.x. S2CID 143591670.
  104. ^ Amabile, T. M. (1982). "Social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 43 (5): 997–1013. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.43.5.997. S2CID 144256250.
  105. ^ Amabile, Teresa M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Routledge. ISBN 9780813330341.
  106. ^ Baer, J.; Kaufman, J. C. (2005). "Bridging generality and specificity: The Amusement Park Theoretical (APT) Model of creativity". Roeper Review. 27 (3): 158–163. doi:10.1080/02783190509554310. S2CID 33513570.
  107. ^ Renzulli, J. S. (1978). "What makes giftedness? Reexamining a definition". Phi Delta Kappan. 60: 180–261.
  108. ^ Naglieri, J. A.; Kaufman, J. C. (2001). "Understanding intelligence, giftedness, and creativity using PASS theory". Roeper Review. 23 (3): 151–156. doi:10.1080/02783190109554087. S2CID 144199243.
  109. ^ Torrance, E. P. (1962). Guiding creative talent. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  110. ^ Barron, F. (1969). Creative person and creative process. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  111. ^ Yamamoto, K (1964). "Creativity and sociometric choice among adolescents". Journal of Social Psychology. 64 (2): 249–261. doi:10.1080/00224545.1964.9919564. PMID 14238998.
  112. ^ Fuchs-Beauchamp, K. D.; Karnes, M. B.; Johnson, L. J. (1993). "Creativity and intelligence in preschoolers". Gifted Child Quarterly. 37 (3): 113–117. doi:10.1177/001698629303700303. S2CID 144005401.
  113. ^ Cho, S. H.; Nijenhuis, J. T.; van Vianen, N. E. M.; Kim, H.-B.; Lee, K. H. (2010). "The relationship between diverse components of intelligence and creativity". Journal of Creative Behavior. 44 (2): 125–137. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2010.tb01329.x.
  114. ^ Jauk, E.; Benedek, M.; Dunst, B.; Neubauer, A. C. (2013). "The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection". Intelligence. 41 (4): 212–221. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2013.03.003. PMC 3682183. PMID 23825884.
  115. ^ Wai, J.; Lubinski, D.; Benbow, C. P. (2005). "Creativity and occupational accomplishments among intellectually precocious youths: An age 13 to age 33 longitudinal study". Journal of Educational Psychology. 97 (3): 484–492. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.484. S2CID 17610985.
  116. ^ Kim, K. H. (2005). "Can only intelligent people be creative?". Journal of Secondary Gifted Education. 16 (2–3): 57–66. doi:10.4219/jsge-2005-473. S2CID 49475973.
  117. ^ Preckel, F.; Holling, H.; Wiese, M. (2006). "Relationship of intelligence and creativity in gifted and non-gifted students: An investigation of threshold theory". Personality and Individual Differences. 40: 159–170. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.06.022.
  118. ^ Perkins, D. N. (1981) The mind's best work. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  119. ^ Weisberg, R. W.; Alba, J. W. (1981). "An examination of the alleged role of "fixation" in the solution of several "insight" problems". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 110 (2): 169–192. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.110.2.169.
  120. ^ McNemar, O (1964). "Lost: Our Intelligence? Why?". American Psychologist. 19 (12): 871–882. doi:10.1037/h0042008.
  121. ^ Kenneth M Heilman, MD, Stephen E. Nadeau, MD, and David Q. Beversdorf, MD. "Creative Innovation: Possible Brain Mechanisms" Neurocase (2003) 2009-03-19 at the Wayback Machine
  122. ^ Flaherty AW (2005). "Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive". J Comp Neurol. 493 (1): 147–53. doi:10.1002/cne.20768. PMC 2571074. PMID 16254989.
  123. ^ Mayseless, Naama; Eran, Ayelet; Shamay-Tsoory, Simone G (2015). "Generating original ideas: The neural underpinning of originality". NeuroImage. 116: 232–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.030. PMID 26003860. S2CID 12973770. These results are in line with the dual model of creativity, according to which original ideas are a product of the interaction between a system that generates ideas and a control system that evaluates these ideas.
  124. ^ Di Bernardi Luft C, Zioga I, Thompson NM, Banissy MJ, Bhattacharya J (December 26, 2018). "Right temporal alpha oscillations as a neural mechanism for inhibiting obvious associations". PNAS. 115 (52): E12144–E12152. Bibcode:2018PNAS..11512144L. doi:10.1073/pnas.1811465115. PMC 6310824. PMID 30541890.
  125. ^ Vandervert 2003a, 2003b; Vandervert, Schimpf & Liu, 2007
  126. ^ Miyake & Shah, 1999
  127. ^ Schmahmann, 1997, 2004
  128. ^ Andersen, Korbo & Pakkenberg, 1992.
  129. ^ Miller & Cohen, 2001
  130. ^ Vandervert, 2003a
  131. ^ Jung-Beeman, Bowden, Haberman, Frymiare, Arambel-Liu, Greenblatt, Reber & Kounios, 2004
  132. ^ Imamizu, Kuroda, Miyauchi, Yoshioka & Kawato, 2003
  133. ^ Schmahmann, 2004,
  134. ^ Vandervert, in press-a
  135. ^ Vandervert, 2011, in press-b
  136. ^ Vandervert & Vandervert-Weathers, 2013
  137. ^ Brown, J.; et al. (2007). "On Vandervert et al. "Working memory cerebellum, and creativity"". Creat. Res. J. 19: 25–29. doi:10.1080/10400410709336875. S2CID 143457667.
  138. ^ Abraham, A. (2007). "Can a neural system geared to bring about rapid, predictive, and efficient function explain creativity?". Creat. Res. J. 19: 19–24. doi:10.1080/10400410709336874. S2CID 43976883.
  139. ^ Wagner U.; Gais S.; Haider H.; Verleger R.; Born J. (2004). "Sleep inspires insight". Nature. 427 (6972): 352–5. Bibcode:2004Natur.427..352W. doi:10.1038/nature02223. PMID 14737168. S2CID 4405704.
  140. ^ a b c Cai D. J.; Mednick S. A.; Harrison E. M.; Kanady J. C.; Mednick S. C. (2009). "REM, not incubation, improves creativity by priming associative networks". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 106 (25): 10130–10134. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10610130C. doi:10.1073/pnas.0900271106. PMC 2700890. PMID 19506253.
  141. ^ Walker MP, Liston C, Hobson JA, Stickgold R (November 2002). "Cognitive flexibility across the sleep-wake cycle: REM-sleep enhancement of anagram problem solving". Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 14 (3): 317–24. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00134-9. PMID 12421655.
  142. ^ Hasselmo ME (September 1999). "Neuromodulation: acetylcholine and memory consolidation". Trends Cogn. Sci. (Regul. Ed.). 3 (9): 351–359. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01365-0. PMID 10461198. S2CID 14725160.
  143. ^ Winkielman, P.; Knutson, B. (2007), "Affective Influence on Judgments and Decisions: Moving Towards Core Mechanisms", Review of General Psychology, 11 (2): 179–192, doi:10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.179, S2CID 15618397
  144. ^ Amabile, T. (2017). In pursuit of everyday creativity. The Journal of Creative Behavior.
  145. ^ Mark A. Davis (January 2009). "Understanding the relationship between mood and creativity: A meta-analysis". Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 100 (1): 25–38. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.04.001.
  146. ^ Baas, Matthijs; De Dreu Carsten K. W. & Nijstad, Bernard A. (November 2008). (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 134 (6): 779–806. doi:10.1037/a0012815. PMID 18954157. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-18.
  147. ^ Schmidhuber, Jürgen (2006). "Developmental Robotics, Optimal Artificial Curiosity, Creativity, Music, and the Fine Arts". Connection Science. 18 (2): 173–187. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.474.6919. doi:10.1080/09540090600768658. S2CID 2923356.
  148. ^ Schmidhuber, Jürgen (2010). "Formal Theory of Creativity, Fun, and Intrinsic Motivation (1990–2010)". IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development. 2 (3): 230–247. doi:10.1109/tamd.2010.2056368. S2CID 234198.
  149. ^ Video of Jürgen Schmidhuber's keynote at the 2011 Winter Intelligence Conference, Oxford: Universal AI and Theory of Fun and Creativity. Youtube, 2012
  150. ^ Video of Jürgen Schmidhuber's talk at the 2009 Singularity Summit, NYC: Compression Progress: The Algorithmic Principle Behind Curiosity and Creativity. Youtube, 2010
  151. ^ Kurzweil AI: Transcript of Jürgen Schmidhuber's TEDx talk (2012): When creative machines overtake man
  152. ^ Schmidhuber, J. (1991), Curious model-building control systems. In Proc. ICANN, Singapore, volume 2, pp 1458–1463. IEEE.
  153. ^ Schmidhuber, J. (2012), A Formal Theory of Creativity to Model the Creation of Art. In McCormack, Jon and M. d'Inverno (eds), Computers and Creativity, Springer 2012
  154. ^ Schmidhuber, J. (2007), Simple Algorithmic Principles of Discovery, Subjective Beauty, Selective Attention, Curiosity & Creativity. In V. Corruble, M. Takeda, E. Suzuki, eds., Proc. 10th Intl. Conf. on Discovery Science 2007 pp 26-38, LNAI 4755, Springer
  155. ^ (Rushton, 1990)
  156. ^ Folley, Bradley S.; Park, Sohee (2005). . Schizophrenia Research. 80 (2–3): 271–282. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2005.06.016. PMC 2817946. PMID 16125369. Archived from the original on 2006-02-15. Retrieved 2006-02-19.
  157. ^ Batey, M. Furnham, A. (2009). The relationship between creativity, schizotypy and intelligence. Individual Differences Research, 7, p.272-284.
  158. ^ Batey, M.; Furnham, A. (2008). "The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy". Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (8): 816–821. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.08.014.
  159. ^ Furnham, A.; Batey, M.; Anand, K.; Manfield, J. (2008). "Personality, hypomania, intelligence and creativity". Personality and Individual Differences. 44 (5): 1060–1069. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.10.035.
  160. ^ Kyaga, S.; Lichtenstein, P.; Boman, M.; Hultman, C.; Långström, N.; Landén, M. (2011). "Creativity and mental disorder: Family study of 300 000 people with severe mental disorder". The British Journal of Psychiatry. 199 (5): 373–379. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.110.085316. PMID 21653945.
  161. ^ a b Roberts, Michelle. Creativity 'closely entwined with mental illness'. https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/health-19959565. 16 October 2012.
  162. ^ Ludwig, Arnold M. (1995). The Price of Greatness: Resolving the Creativity and Madness Controversy. ISBN 978-0-89862-839-5.
  163. ^ "The science of creativity". apa.org.
  164. ^ Baas, Matthijs; De Dreu, Carsten K. W.; Nijstad, Bernard A. (2008). "A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus?". Psychological Bulletin. 134 (6): 779–806. doi:10.1037/a0012815. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 18954157.
  165. ^ Flood, Meredith; Phillips, Kenneth D. (2007). "Creativity in older adults: a plethors of possibilities". Issues in Mental Health Nursing. 28 (4): 389–411. doi:10.1080/01612840701252956. PMID 17454290. S2CID 12380062. Retrieved 27 August 2019.
  166. ^ Malchiodi, Cathy (2014). "Creative Arts Therapy and Expressive Arts Therapy". Psychology Today.
  167. ^ a b Burton, N. (2012). "Bipolar Disorder and Creativity". Psychology Today.
  168. ^ "Bipolar Disorder and Creativity". Psychology Today. Retrieved 2020-07-17.
  169. ^ a b Shapiro, Pamela J.; Weisberg, Robert W. (1999). "Creativity and Bipolar Diathesis: Common Behavioural and Cognitive Components". Cognition & Emotion. 13 (6): 741–762. doi:10.1080/026999399379069. ISSN 0269-9931.
  170. ^ Miller, Natalie; Perich, Tania; Meade, Tanya (2019). "Depression, mania and self-reported creativity in bipolar disorder". Psychiatry Research. 276: 129–133. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2019.05.006. PMID 31078791. S2CID 145024133.
  171. ^ McCraw, Stacey; Parker, Gordon; Fletcher, Kathryn; Friend, Paul (2013). "Self-reported creativity in bipolar disorder: prevalence, types and associated outcomes in mania versus hypomania". Journal of Affective Disorders. 151 (3): 831–836. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2013.07.016. ISSN 0165-0327. PMID 24084622.
  172. ^ Dimkov, Petar Radoev (2018-04-01). "The Genius of Creativity and the Creativity of Genius: The Neuro-Dynamics of Creativity in Karl Jaspers and Sigmund Freud". Journal of Genius and Eminence. 3 (Fall 2018): 83–92. doi:10.18536/jge.2018.04.3.1.07.
  173. ^ a b Kim, Bin-Na; Kwon, Seok-Man (2017). "The link between hypomania risk and creativity: The role of heightened behavioral activation system (BAS) sensitivity". Journal of Affective Disorders. 215: 9–14. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2017.02.033. PMID 28288308.
  174. ^ a b Weisberg, Robert W. (1994). "Genius and Madness?: A Quasi-Experimental Test of the Hypothesis That Manic-Depression Increases Creativity". Psychological Science. 5 (6): 361–367. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00286.x. ISSN 0956-7976. S2CID 146691937.
  175. ^ DeGraff, Jeff; Lawrence, Katherine A. (2002-10-10). Creativity at Work: Developing the Right Practices to Make Innovation Happen. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-0-7879-6653-9.
  176. ^ Irwing, Paul; Batey, Mark (2011). Me2 General Factor of Creativity: Technical Manual (Commissioned report). Altrincham: E-Metrixx.
  177. ^ Nijstad B. A.; De Dreu C. K. (2002). "Creativity and Group Innovation". Applied Psychology. 51 (3): 400–406. doi:10.1111/1464-0597.00984.
  178. ^ Guilford, Joy Paul (1950). "Creativity". American Psychologist. 5 (9): 444–454. doi:10.1037/h0063487. PMID 14771441.
  179. ^ (Torrance, 1974, 1984)
  180. ^ (Christiaans & Venselaar, 2007)
  181. ^ (Amabile, 1996; Prabhu et al., 2008)
  182. ^ (Feist, 1998, 1999; Prabhu et al., 2008; Zhang & Sternberg, 2009)
  183. ^ (Campbell, 1960)
  184. ^ (Gardner, 1993a, Policastro & Gardner, 1999)
  185. ^ (Sternberg & Lubart, 1991, 1995, 1996)
  186. ^ Cropley, David H.; Cropley, Arthur J.; Kaufman, James C.; et al., eds. (2010). The Dark Side of Creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-13960-1.
  187. ^ a b McLaren, R. B. (1993). "The dark side of creativity". Creat. Res. J. 6 (1–2): 137–144. doi:10.1080/10400419309534472.
  188. ^ a b Hao, N; Tang, M; Yang, J; Wang, Q; Runco, MA (2016). "A New Tool to Measure Malevolent Creativity: The Malevolent Creativity Behavior Scale". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 682. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00682. PMC 4870273. PMID 27242596.
  189. ^ Aggression: A social psychological analysis. Berkowitz, Leonard New York, NY, US: McGraw-Hill Aggression: A social psychological analysis. (1962). xv 361 pp
  190. ^ Harris, D. J.; Reiter-Palmon, R. (2015). "Fast and furious: The influence of implicit aggression, premeditation, and provoking situations on malevolent creativity". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 9 (1): 54–64. doi:10.1037/a0038499.
  191. ^ Sternberg RJ 'Introduction' in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ (2006) (eds) The International Handbook of Creativity pp 1-9. Cambridge University Press ISBN 0-521-54731-8
  192. ^ Niu W (2006) 'Development of Creativity Research in Chinese Societies' in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ (eds) The International Handbook of Creativity pp 386-387. Cambridge University Press ISBN 0-521-54731-8
  193. ^ Mpofu E et al (2006) 'African Perspectives on Creativity' in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ (eds) The International Handbook of Creativity p 465. Cambridge University Press ISBN 0-521-54731-8
  194. ^ Mpofu E et al (2006) 'African Perspectives on Creativity' in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ (eds) The International Handbook of Creativity p 458. Cambridge University Press ISBN 0-521-54731-8
  195. ^ Preiss DD and Strasser K (2006) 'Creativity in Latin America' in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ (eds) The International Handbook of Creativity p 46. Cambridge University Press ISBN 0-521-54731-8
  196. ^ Smith GJW and Carlsson I (2006) 'Creativity under the Northern Lights' in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ (eds) The International Handbook of Creativity p 202. Cambridge University Press ISBN 0-521-54731-8
  197. ^ Preiser S (2006) 'Creativity Research in German-Speaking Countries' in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ (eds) The International Handbook of Creativity p 175. Cambridge University Press ISBN 0-521-54731-8
  198. ^ a b Amabile, T. M. (1998). "How to kill creativity". Harvard Business Review. 76 (5): 76–87, 186. PMID 10185433.
  199. ^ Sullivan, Ceri; Harper, Grame, eds. (2009). Authors at Work: The Creative Environment. DS Brewer/The English Association. ISBN 9781843841951.
  200. ^ (Nonaka, 1991)
  201. ^ Siltala, R. 2010. Innovativity and cooperative learning in business life and teaching. University of Turku
  202. ^ Leal, S. y Urrea J. "Ingenio y Pasión" (2013), Lid Publishers (Spanish) and Forbes India Magazine http://forbesindia.com/article/ie/new-trends-in-innovation-management/33905/1#ixzz2iiuuDxVq
  203. ^ a b c d e Woodman, R. W.; Sawyer, J. E.; Griffin, R. W. (1993). "Toward a theory of organizational creativity". Academy of Management Review. 18 (2): 293–321. doi:10.5465/amr.1993.3997517. S2CID 15250032.
  204. ^ a b c d Paulus, P. B.; Dzindolet, M. (2008). "Social influence, creativity and innovation". Social Influence. 3 (4): 228–247. doi:10.1080/15534510802341082. S2CID 143485863.
  205. ^ a b c d e Salazar, M. R.; Lant, T. K.; Fiore, S. M.; Salas, E. (2012). "Facilitating innovation in diverse science teams through integrative capacity". Small Group Research. 43 (5): 527–5. doi:10.1177/1046496412453622. S2CID 643746.
  206. ^ a b c d e Harvey, S (2014). "Creative synthesis: Exploring the process of extraordinary group creativity". Academy of Management Review. 39 (3): 324–343. doi:10.5465/amr.2012.0224.
  207. ^ a b c Loo, S. (2017) Creative Working in the Knowledge Economy. Abingdon, Oxfordshire: Routledge Ltd. ISBN 9781138211391; ISBN 9781315453095. https://www.routledge.com/Creative-Working-in-the-Knowledge-Economy/Loo/p/book/9781138211391
  208. ^ Burton-Jones, Alan (1999-10-21). Knowledge Capitalism. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198296225.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-829622-5.
  209. ^ Drucker, David; Drucker, Erika (1999). ""There's no place like home" (a Victorian song title)". Le Globe. Revue genevoise de géographie. 139 (1): 77–92. doi:10.3406/globe.1999.1410. ISSN 0398-3412.
  210. ^ Cortada, J (1998), "Introducing the Knowledge Worker", Rise of the Knowledge Worker, Elsevier, pp. xiii–xix, doi:10.1016/b978-0-7506-7058-6.50004-1, ISBN 978-0-7506-7058-6, retrieved 2021-06-26
  211. ^ Stenzl, Jürg (2001). Reich, Willi. Oxford Music Online. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/gmo/9781561592630.article.23092.
  212. ^ Florida, Richard (2003-10-27), "Entrepreneurship, Creativity, and Regional Economic Growth", The Emergence of Entrepreneurship Policy, Cambridge University Press, pp. 39–58, doi:10.1017/cbo9780511610134.003, ISBN 978-0-521-82677-8, retrieved 2021-06-26
  213. ^ Farrell, Lesley; Fenwick, Tara, eds. (2007-03-12). World Yearbook of Education 2007. Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780203962664. ISBN 978-1-134-11806-9.
  214. ^ Brown, Phillip; Lauder, Hugh; Ashton, David (2010-12-03). The Global Auction. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199731688.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-973168-8.
  215. ^ Davenport, T. H. (2005). The coming commoditization of processes. Harvard business review, 83(6), 100-108.
  216. ^ Alvesson, Mats (2004). Knowledge work and knowledge-intensive firms. OUP Oxford.
  217. ^ Arthur, Michael B.; DeFillippi, Robert J.; Lindsay, Valerie J. (October 2008). "On Being a Knowledge Worker". Organizational Dynamics. 37 (4): 365–377. doi:10.1016/j.orgdyn.2008.07.005. ISSN 0090-2616.
  218. ^ Orr, Kevin Martin; Nutley, Sandra M.; Russell, Shona; Bain, Rod; Hacking, Bonnie; Moran, Clare, eds. (22 March 2016). Knowledge and practice in business and organisations. ISBN 978-1-317-37792-4. OCLC 945552692.
  219. ^ Ó Riain, Sean (2004). The Politics of High Tech Growth: Developmental Network States in the Global Economy. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-511-49960-9. OCLC 776970799.
  220. ^ Nerland, Monika (2007-12-28). "Knowledge Cultures and the Shaping of Work-based Learning: The Case of Computer Engineering". Vocations and Learning. 1 (1): 49–69. doi:10.1007/s12186-007-9002-x. ISSN 1874-785X. S2CID 61112900.
  221. ^ Grabher, Gernot (November 2004). "Temporary Architectures of Learning: Knowledge Governance in Project Ecologies". Organization Studies. 25 (9): 1491–1514. doi:10.1177/0170840604047996. ISSN 0170-8406. S2CID 145269032.
  222. ^ Lury, Celia (2004-08-12). Brands. doi:10.4324/9780203495025. ISBN 9781134529179.
  223. ^ Reich, Utz-Peter (2001), "Value Theory in the National Accounts", National Accounts and Economic value, London: Palgrave Macmillan UK, pp. 142–154, doi:10.1057/9780230512900_7, ISBN 978-1-349-40765-1, retrieved 2021-06-30
  224. ^ Quah, Danny (2002). 24/7 Competitive Innovation. LSE Economics Department.
  225. ^ Drucker, Peter (1994). The Theory of the Business.
  226. ^ Nonaka, Ikujirō (1995). The knowledge-creating company : how Japanese companies create the dynamics of innovation. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-509269-4. OCLC 782177702.
  227. ^ von Hippel, Eric (2005). Democratizing Innovation. doi:10.7551/mitpress/2333.001.0001. hdl:1721.1/118153. ISBN 9780262285636. S2CID 243335219.
  228. ^ Zuboff, Shoshana (1988). In the age of the smart machine : the future of work and power. Basic Books. ISBN 0-465-03212-5. OCLC 17480009.
  229. ^ Lash, Scott; Urry, John (2002). Economies of Signs and Space. SAGE Publications Ltd. doi:10.4135/9781446280539. ISBN 978-0-8039-8472-1.
  230. ^ Bell, Colin; Newby, Howard (2012-11-12). Sociology of Community. doi:10.4324/9780203043110. ISBN 9781136272462.
  231. ^ Castells, Manuel (2000). "Toward a Sociology of the Network Society". Contemporary Sociology. 29 (5): 693–699. doi:10.2307/2655234. ISSN 0094-3061. JSTOR 2655234.
  232. ^ Knorr Cetina, Karin; Schatzki, Theodore R.; von Savigny, Eike, eds. (2005-06-20). The Practice Turn in Contemporary Theory. Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780203977453. ISBN 9781134586295.
  233. ^ Sternberg, Robert J.; Kaufman, James C.; Pretz, Jean E. (2004). "A Propulsion Model of Creative Leadership". Creativity and Innovation Management. 13 (3): 145–153. doi:10.1111/j.0963-1690.2004.00304.x. ISSN 0963-1690.
  234. ^ Gardner, Howard (1988). "Creativity: An interdisciplinary perspective". Creativity Research Journal. 1 (1): 8–26. doi:10.1080/10400418809534284. ISSN 1040-0419.
  235. ^ Li, Jin; Gardner, Howard (1993). "How Domains Constrain Creativity". American Behavioral Scientist. 37 (1): 94–101. doi:10.1177/0002764293037001010. ISSN 0002-7642. S2CID 143591939.
  236. ^ Csikszentmihalyi, Mihaly; Csikszentmihalyi, Isabella Selega, eds. (1988-08-26). Optimal Experience. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/cbo9780511621956. ISBN 9780521342889.
  237. ^ Harvey, S (2013). "A different perspective: The multiple effects of deep level diversity on group creativity". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 49 (5): 822–832. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2013.04.004.
  238. ^ a b Paletz, S. B.; Schunn, C. D. (2010). "A social‐cognitive framework of multidisciplinary team innovation". Topics in Cognitive Science. 2 (1): 73–95. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.611.2475. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01029.x. PMID 25163622.
  239. ^ Polzer, J. T.; Milton, L. P.; Swarm, W. B. Jr. (2002). "Capitalizing on diversity: Interpersonal congruence in small work groups". Administrative Science Quarterly. 47 (2): 296–324. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.198.3908. doi:10.2307/3094807. JSTOR 3094807. S2CID 152150563.
  240. ^ Zeng, A; Fan, Y; Di, Z; Wang, Y; Havlin, S (2021). "Fresh teams are associated with original and multidisciplinary research". Nature Human Behaviour. 5 (10): 1314–1322. doi:10.1038/s41562-021-01084-x. PMID 33820976. S2CID 233036821.
  241. ^ Hargadon, A. B.; Bechky, B. A. (2006). "When collections of creatives become creative collectives: A field study of problem solving at work". Organization Science. 17 (4): 484–500. doi:10.1287/orsc.1060.0200. S2CID 6580938.
  242. ^ a b Amabile, Teresa M.; Conti, Regina; Coon, Heather; Lazenby, Jeffrey; Herron, Michael (1996). "Assessing the Work Environment for Creativity". Academy of Management Journal. 39 (5): 1154–1184. doi:10.5465/256995. ISSN 0001-4273. S2CID 144812471.
  243. ^ Ward, T.B. (1994). "Structured Imagination: the Role of Category Structure in Exemplar Generation". Cognitive Psychology. 27 (1): 1–40. doi:10.1006/cogp.1994.1010. S2CID 54276064.
  244. ^ Stokes, Patricia D. (2007). "Using constraints to generate and sustain novelty". Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts. 1 (2): 107–113. doi:10.1037/1931-3896.1.2.107. ISSN 1931-390X.
  245. ^ Moreau, C. Page; Dahl, Darren W. (2005). "Designing the Solution: The Impact of Constraints on Consumers' Creativity". Journal of Consumer Research. 32 (1): 13–22. doi:10.1086/429597. ISSN 0093-5301. S2CID 2152095.
  246. ^ Neren, Uri (2011-01-14). "The Number One Key to Innovation: Scarcity". Harvard Business Review. ISSN 0017-8012. Retrieved 2019-03-26.
  247. ^ Gibbert, Michael; Scranton, Philip (2009). "Constraints as sources of radical innovation? Insights from jet propulsion development". Management & Organizational History. 4 (4): 385–399. doi:10.1177/1744935909341781. ISSN 1744-9359. S2CID 144428010.
  248. ^ a b c d Hoegl, Martin; Gibbert, Michael; Mazursky, David (2008). "Financial constraints in innovation projects: When is less more?". Research Policy. 37 (8): 1382–1391. doi:10.1016/j.respol.2008.04.018.
  249. ^ a b Weiss, Matthias; Hoegl, Martin; Gibbert, Michael (2011). "Making Virtue of Necessity: The Role of Team Climate for Innovation in Resource-Constrained Innovation Projects". Journal of Product Innovation Management. 28 (s1): 196–207. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5885.2011.00870.x.
  250. ^ Weiss, Matthias; Hoegl, Martin; Gibbert, Michael (2017). "How Does Material Resource Adequacy Affect Innovation Project Performance? A Meta-Analysis". Journal of Product Innovation Management. 34 (6): 842–863. doi:10.1111/jpim.12368.
  251. ^ Chan, Janet (201). "Towards a sociology of creativity". In Mann, Leon; Chan, Janet (eds.). Creativity and Innovation in Business and Beyond: Social Science Perspectives and Policy Implications. Routledge.
  252. ^ Reckwitz, Andreas (2017). The Invention of Creativity. Polity Press. p. vi.
  253. ^ Godart, Frédéric; Seong, Sorah; Phillips, Damon J. (2020-07-30). "The Sociology of Creativity: Elements, Structures, and Audiences". Annual Review of Sociology. 46 (1): 489–510. doi:10.1146/annurev-soc-121919-054833. ISSN 0360-0572. S2CID 218819502.
  254. ^ Casey, Emma; O'Brien, Dave (2020). "Sociology, Sociology and the Cultural and Creative Industries". Sociology. 54 (3): 443–459. doi:10.1177/0038038520904712. S2CID 216202901.
  255. ^ Rubenson, Daniel L.; Runco, Mark (1992). "The psychoeconomic approach to creativity". New Ideas in Psychology. 10 (2): 131–147. doi:10.1016/0732-118X(92)90021-Q.
  256. ^ Diamond, Arthur M. (1992). "Creativity and Interdisciplinarity: A Response to Rubenson and Runco". New Ideas in Psychology. 10 (2): 157–160. doi:10.1016/0732-118X(92)90023-S.
  257. ^ Florida, Richard (2002). The Rise of the Creative Class: And How It's Transforming Work, Leisure, Community and Everyday Life. Basic Books. ISBN 978-0-465-02476-6.
  258. ^ "Creativity, the Brain, and Evolution". Psychology Today.
  259. ^ a b c d e Nickerson, R. S. (1999). "Enhancing creativity". In R. J. Sternberg (ed.). Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press.
  260. ^ Chirumbolo, Antonio; Livi, Stefano; Mannetti, Lucia; Pierro, Antonio; Kruglanski, Arie W (2004). "Effects of need for closure on creativity in small group interactions". European Journal of Personality. 18 (4): 265–278. doi:10.1002/per.518. S2CID 144190667.
  261. ^ Djikic, Maja; Oatley, Keith; Moldoveanu, Mihnea C. (2013). "Opening the Closed Mind: The Effect of Exposure toLiterature on the Need for Closure". Creativity Research Journal. 25 (2): 149–154. doi:10.1080/10400419.2013.783735. S2CID 143961189.
  262. ^ a b c d Csíkszentmihályi, Mihály (1999). "Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity". In R. J. Sternberg (ed.). Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press.
  263. ^ a b c d Robinson, K.; Azzam, A. M. (2009). "Why creativity now?". Educational Leadership. 67 (1): 22–26.
  264. ^ Paris, C., Edwards, N., Sheffield, E., Mutinsky, M., Olexa, T., Reilly, S., & Baer, J. (2006). How early school experiences impact creativity. In J. C. Kaufman & J. Baer (Eds.), Creativity and Reason in Cognitive Development (pp. 333–350). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  265. ^ a b Byrge, C.; Hanson. S. (2009). "The creative platform: A new paradigm for teaching creativity". Problems of Education in the 21st Century. 18: 33–50.
  266. ^ Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1993). Evolution and flow. In M. Csikszentmihalyi (Ed.), The evolving self: A psychology for the third millennium (pp. 175–206). New York: Harper Perennial.
  267. ^ National Advisory Committee on Creative and Cultural Education (1998). All our futures: Creativity, culture, and education. UK: NACCCE
  268. ^ Torrance, E. Paul (Ellis Paul) (2002). The manifesto : a guide to developing a creative career. Westport, Conn.: Ablex Pub. ISBN 978-0313011863. OCLC 52769638.
  269. ^ a b "Creativity 3-18 curriculum review (impact report) | Practice exemplars | National Improvement Hub". education.gov.scot. Retrieved 2021-10-25.
  270. ^ "Creative Learning Networks | Learning resources | National Improvement Hub". education.gov.scot. Retrieved 2021-10-25.
  271. ^ "What are creativity skills? | Learning resources | National Improvement Hub". education.gov.scot. Retrieved 2021-10-25.
  272. ^ "Scotland's Creative Learning Plan". www.creativescotland.com. Retrieved 2021-10-25.

References

  • Fredrickson B. L. (2001). "The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions". American Psychologist. 56 (3): 218–26. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.218. PMC 3122271. PMID 11315248.
  • Hadamard, Jacques, The Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical Field (Dover, 1954) ISBN 0-486-20107-4
  • Helmholtz, H. v. L. (1896). Vorträge und Reden (5th ed.). Friederich Vieweg und Sohn.
  • Isen A. M.; Daubman K. A.; Nowicki G. P. (1987). "Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52 (6): 1122–31. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1122. PMID 3598858. S2CID 12776791.
  • Jeffery, G. (2005). The Creative College: building a successful learning culture in the arts. Trentham Books.
  • Johnson, D. M. (1972). Systematic introduction to the psychology of thinking. Harper & Row.
  • Jullien, F. (2004). In Praise of Blandness: Proceeding from Chinese Thought and Aesthetics. Translated by Paula M. Varsano. Zone Books, U.S. ISBN 1-890951-41-2.
  • Jung, C. G., The Collected Works of C. G. Jung. Volume 8. The Structure and Dynamics of the Psyche. (Princeton, 1981) ISBN 0-691-09774-7
  • Kanigel, Robert, The Man Who Knew Infinity: A Life of the Genius Ramanujan (Washington Square Press, 1992) ISBN 0-671-75061-5
  • Kraft, U. (2005). "Unleashing Creativity". Scientific American Mind. April: 16–23. doi:10.1038/scientificamericanmind0405-16.
  • Kolp, P.; Lammé, A.; Regnard, Fr. (2009). Rens, J. M. (ed.). "Musique et créativité". Orphée Apprenti. NS (1): 9–119. D/2009/11848/5
  • *Lehrer, Jonah (2012), Imagine: How Creativity Works.
  • McLaren, R. B. (1999). "Dark Side of Creativity". In Runco, M. A.; Pritzker, S. R. (eds.). Encyclopedia of Creativity. Academic Press.
  • McCrae, R. R. (1987). "Creativity, Divergent Thinking, and Openness to Experience". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52 (6): 1258–1265. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1258.
  • Michalko, M. (1998). Cracking Creativity: The Secrets of Creative Genius. Berkeley, Calif.: Ten Speed Press. ISBN 978-0-89815-913-4.
  • National Academy of Engineering (2005). Educating the engineer of 2020: adapting engineering education to the new century. National Academies Press. ISBN 978-0-309-09649-2.
  • Nonaka, I. (1991). "The Knowledge-Creating Company". Harvard Business Review. 69 (6): 96–104.
  • O'Hara, L. A.; Sternberg, R. J. (1999). "Creativity and Intelligence". In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). Handbook of Creativity. Cambridge University Press.
  • Pink, D. H. (2005). A Whole New Mind: Moving from the information age into the conceptual age. Allen & Unwin.
  • Poincaré, H. (1952) [1908]. "Mathematical creation". In Ghiselin, B. (ed.). The Creative Process: A Symposium. Mentor.
  • Rhodes, M. (1961). "An analysis of creativity". Phi Delta Kappan. 42: 305–311.
  • Rushton, J. P. (1990). "Creativity, intelligence, and psychoticism". Personality and Individual Differences. 11 (12): 1291–1298. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(90)90156-L.
  • Runco, M. A. (2004). "Creativity". Annual Review of Psychology. 55: 657–687. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141502. PMID 14744230.
  • Sabaneev, Leonid. The Psychology of the Musico-Creative Process // Psyche. - Vol. 9 (July 1928). - pp. 37–54.
  • Smith, S. M.; Blakenship, S.E. (1 April 1991). "Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving". American Journal of Psychology. 104 (1): 61–87. doi:10.2307/1422851. ISSN 0002-9556. JSTOR 1422851. PMID 2058758. S2CID 10359632.
  • Taylor, C. W. (1988). "Various approaches to and definitions of creativity". In Sternberg, R. J. (ed.). The nature of creativity: Contemporary psychological perspectives. Cambridge University Press.
  • Torrance, E. P. (1974). Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Personnel Press.
  • von Franz, Marie-Louise, Psyche and Matter (Shambhala, 1992) ISBN 0-87773-902-1
  • Andersen B.; Korbo L.; Pakkenberg B. (1992). "A quantitative study of the human cerebellum with unbiased stereological techniques". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 326 (4): 549–560. doi:10.1002/cne.903260405. PMID 1484123. S2CID 11492983.
  • Imamizu H.; Kuroda T.; Miyauchi S.; Yoshioka T.; Kawato M. (2003). "Modular organization of internal models of tools in the cerebellum". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 100 (9): 5461–5466. doi:10.1073/pnas.0835746100. PMC 154367. PMID 12704240.
  • Jung-Beeman, M.; Bowden, E.; Haberman, J.; Frymiare, J.; Arambel-Liu, S.; Greenblatt, R.; Reber, P.; Kounios, J. (2004). "Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight". PLOS Biology. 2 (4): 500–510. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0020097. PMC 387268. PMID 15094802.
  • Miller E.; Cohen J. (2001). "An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 24: 167–202. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. PMID 11283309. S2CID 7301474.
  • Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (Eds.). (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Schmahmann, J. (Ed.). (1997). The cerebellum and cognition. New York: Academic Press.
  • Schmahmann J (2004). "Disorders of the cerebellum: Ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome". Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 16 (3): 367–378. doi:10.1176/jnp.16.3.367. PMID 15377747.
  • Tatarkiewicz, Władysław, A History of Six Ideas: an Essay in Aesthetics, translated from the Polish by Christopher Kasparek, The Hague, Martinus Nijhoff, 1980.
  • Vandervert, L (2003a). "How working memory and cognitive modeling functions of the cerebellum contribute to discoveries in mathematics". New Ideas in Psychology. 21 (2): 159–175. doi:10.1016/s0732-118x(03)00012-6.
  • Vandervert, L. (2003b). The neurophysiological basis of innovation. In L. V. Shavinina (Ed.) The international handbook on innovation (pp. 17–30). Oxford, England: Elsevier Science.
  • Vandervert, L (2011). "The evolution of language: The cerebro-cerebellar blending of visual-spatial working memory with vocalizations". The Journal of Mind and Behavior. 32: 317–334.
  • Vandervert, L. (in press). How the blending of cerebellar internal models can explain the evolution of thought and language. Cerebellum.
  • Vandervert, L.; Schimpf, P.; Liu, H. (2007). "How working memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation [Special Issue]". Creativity Research Journal. 19 (1): 1–19. doi:10.1080/10400410709336873. S2CID 15247122.
  • Vandervert, L., & Vandervert-Weathers, K. (in press). New brain-imaging studies indicate how prototyping is related to entrepreneurial giftedness and innovation education in children. In L. Shavinina (Ed.), The International Handbook of Innovation Education. London: Routlage.
  • DeGraff, J.; Lawrence, K. (2002). Creativity at Work. Jossey-Bass. ISBN 978-0-7879-5725-4.
  • Gielen, P. (2013). Creativity and other Fundamentalisms. Mondriaan: Amsterdam.

Further reading

  • Kaufman, James C.; Sternberg, Robert J., eds. (2019). The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity (Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology) (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1316638545.
  • Glǎveanu, Vlad Petre, ed. (2019). The Creativity Reader. Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780190841713.
  • Craft, A. (2005). Creativity in Schools: tensions and dilemmas. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-32414-4.
  • Boden, Margaret (2004). The Creative Mind: Myths And Mechanisms. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-297-82069-7.
  • Amabile, Teresa M. (1996). Creativity in Context: Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity. Routledge. ISBN 9780813330341.
  • J. P. Guilford's 1950 inaugural speech to the American Psychological Association, reproduced in Guilford, Joy Paul (1950). "Creativity". American Psychologist. 5 (9): 444–454. doi:10.1037/h0063487. PMID 14771441.
  • Wallas, Graham (1926). The Art of Thought. Jonathan Cape.


creativity, other, uses, disambiguation, this, article, lead, section, short, adequately, summarize, points, please, consider, expanding, lead, provide, accessible, overview, important, aspects, article, december, 2019, phenomenon, whereby, something, valuable. For other uses see Creativity disambiguation This article s lead section may be too short to adequately summarize the key points Please consider expanding the lead to provide an accessible overview of all important aspects of the article December 2019 Creativity is a phenomenon whereby something new and valuable is formed The created item may be intangible such as an idea a scientific theory a musical composition or a joke or a physical object such as an invention a printed literary work or a painting A picture of a lightbulb is associated with someone having an idea an example of creativity Scholarly interest in creativity is found in a number of disciplines primarily psychology business studies and cognitive science However it can also be found in education the humanities philosophy the arts and theology social sciences sociology linguistics economics engineering technology and mathematics These disciplines cover the relations between creativity and general intelligence personality type mental and neural processes mental health artificial intelligence the potential for fostering creativity through education training leadership and organizational practices 1 the factors that determine how creativity is evaluated and perceived 2 the fostering of creativity for national economic benefit and the application of creative resources to improve the effectiveness of teaching and learning Contents 1 Etymology 2 Definition 3 Aspects 4 Conceptual history 4 1 Ancient 4 2 Renaissance 4 3 Enlightenment and thereafter 4 4 Modern 5 Four C model 6 Process theories 6 1 Incubation 6 2 Convergent and divergent thinking 6 3 Creative cognition approach 6 4 The Explicit Implicit Interaction EII theory 6 5 Conceptual blending 6 6 Honing theory 6 7 Everyday imaginative thought 6 8 Dialectical theory of creativity 6 9 Neuroeconomic framework for creative cognition 6 10 Behaviorism theory of creativity 7 Personal assessment 7 1 Psychometric approaches 7 2 Social personality approach 7 3 Self report questionnaires 8 Intelligence 8 1 Creativity as a subset of intelligence 8 2 Intelligence as a subset of creativity 8 3 Creativity and intelligence as overlapping yet distinct constructs 8 4 Creativity and intelligence as coincident sets 8 5 Creativity and intelligence as disjoint sets 9 Neuroscience 9 1 Working memory and the cerebellum 9 2 REM sleep 10 Affect 10 1 Positive affect relations 11 Computational creativity 12 Creativity and mental health 12 1 Bipolar Disorders and Creativity 13 Personality 14 Malevolent creativity 14 1 Crime 14 2 Predictive factors 15 Cultural differences in creativity 16 Organizational creativity 16 1 Team composition 16 2 Team processes 16 3 Organizational culture 16 4 Constraints 17 The sociology of creativity 18 Economic views 19 Fostering creativity 19 1 Managing the need for closure 20 Education policies 20 1 Scotland 21 Academic journals 22 See also 23 Notes 24 References 25 Further readingEtymology EditThe English word creativity comes from the Latin term creare to create make its derivational suffixes also come from Latin The word create appeared in English as early as the 14th century notably in Chaucer in The Parson s Tale 3 to indicate divine creation 4 However its modern meaning as an act of human creation did not emerge until after the Enlightenment 4 Definition EditIn a summary of scientific research into creativity Michael Mumford suggested Over the course of the last decade however we seem to have reached a general agreement that creativity involves the production of novel useful products Mumford 2003 p 110 5 or in Robert Sternberg s words the production of something original and worthwhile 6 Authors have diverged dramatically in their precise definitions beyond these general commonalities Peter Meusburger estimates that over a hundred different definitions can be found in the literature typically elaborating on the context field organisation environment etc which determines the originality and or appropriateness of the created object and the processes through which it came about 7 As an illustration one definition given by Dr E Paul Torrance in the context of assessing an individual s creative ability described it as a process of becoming sensitive to problems deficiencies gaps in knowledge missing elements disharmonies and so on identifying the difficulty searching for solutions making guesses or formulating hypotheses about the deficiencies testing and retesting these hypotheses and possibly modifying and retesting them and finally communicating the results 8 Creativity in general is usually distinguished from innovation in particular where the stress is on implementation For example Teresa Amabile and Pratt 2016 define creativity as production of novel and useful ideas and innovation as implementation of creative ideas 9 while the OECD and Eurostat state that Innovation is more than a new idea or an invention An innovation requires implementation either by being put into active use or by being made available for use by other parties firms individuals or organisations 10 There is also an emotional creativity 11 which is described as a pattern of cognitive abilities and personality traits related to originality and appropriateness in emotional experience 12 Aspects EditTheories of creativity particularly investigation of why some people are more creative than others have focused on a variety of aspects The dominant factors are usually identified as the four Ps process product person and place press a framework first put forward by Mel Rhodes 13 A focus on process is shown in cognitive approaches that try to describe thought mechanisms and techniques for creative thinking Theories invoking divergent rather than convergent thinking such as Guilford or those describing the staging of the creative process such as Wallas are primarily theories of creative process A focus on creative product usually appears in attempts to assess creative output whether for psychometrics see below or in understanding why some objects are considered creative It is from a consideration of product that the standard definition of creativity as the production of something novel and useful arises 14 A focus on the nature of the creative person considers more general intellectual habits such as openness levels of ideation autonomy expertise exploratory behavior and so on A focus on place sometimes called press considers the circumstances in which creativity flourishes such as degrees of autonomy access to resources and the nature of gatekeepers Creative lifestyles are characterized by nonconforming attitudes and behaviors as well as flexibility 15 In 2013 based on a sociocultural critique of the Four P model as individualistic static and decontextualised Glǎveanu proposed a five A s model consisting of actor action artifact audience and affordance 16 In this model the actor is the person with attributes but also located within social networks action is the process of creativity not only in internal cognitive terms but also external bridging the gap between ideation and implentation artifact emphasises how creative products typically represent cumulative innovations over time rather than abrupt discontinuities and press place is divided into audience and affordance which consider the interdependence of the creative individual with the social and material world respectively Although not supplanting the four Ps model in creativity research the five As model has exerted influence over the direction of some creativity research 17 and has been credited with bringing coherence to studies across a number of creative domains 18 Conceptual history EditMain article History of the concept of creativity Greek philosophers like Plato rejected the concept of creativity preferring to see art as a form of discovery Asked in The Republic Will we say of a painter that he makes something Plato answers Certainly not he merely imitates 19 Ancient Edit Most ancient cultures including thinkers of Ancient Greece 19 Ancient China and Ancient India 20 lacked the concept of creativity seeing art as a form of discovery and not creation The ancient Greeks had no terms corresponding to to create or creator except for the expression poiein to make which only applied to poiesis poetry and to the poietes poet or maker who made it Plato did not believe in art as a form of creation Asked in The Republic 21 Will we say of a painter that he makes something he answers Certainly not he merely imitates 19 It is commonly argued that the notion of creativity originated in Western cultures through Christianity as a matter of divine inspiration 4 According to the historian Daniel J Boorstin the early Western conception of creativity was the Biblical story of creation given in the Genesis 22 However this is not creativity in the modern sense which did not arise until the Renaissance In the Judeo Christian tradition creativity was the sole province of God humans were not considered to have the ability to create something new except as an expression of God s work 23 A concept similar to that of Christianity existed in Greek culture For instance Muses were seen as mediating inspiration from the Gods 24 Romans and Greeks invoked the concept of an external creative daemon Greek or genius Latin linked to the sacred or the divine However none of these views are similar to the modern concept of creativity and the rejection of creativity in favor of discovery and the belief that individual creation was a conduit of the divine would dominate the West probably until the Renaissance and even later 23 25 Renaissance Edit It was during the Renaissance that creativity was first seen not as a conduit for the divine but from the abilities of great men 25 The development of the modern concept of creativity began in the Renaissance when creation began to be perceived as having originated from the abilities of the individual and not God This could be attributed to the leading intellectual movement of the time aptly named humanism which developed an intensely human centric outlook on the world valuing the intellect and achievement of the individual 26 From this philosophy arose the Renaissance man or polymath an individual who embodies the principals of humanism in their ceaseless courtship with knowledge and creation 27 One of the most well known and immensely accomplished examples is Leonardo da Vinci Enlightenment and thereafter Edit However the shift from divine inspiration to the abilities of the individual was gradual and would not become immediately apparent until the Enlightenment 25 By the 18th century and the Age of Enlightenment mention of creativity notably in aesthetics linked with the concept of imagination became more frequent 28 In the writing of Thomas Hobbes imagination became a key element of human cognition 4 William Duff was one of the first to identify imagination as a quality of genius typifying the separation being made between talent productive but breaking no new ground and genius 24 As a direct and independent topic of study creativity effectively received no attention until the 19th century 24 Runco and Albert argue that creativity as the subject of proper study began seriously to emerge in the late 19th century with the increased interest in individual differences inspired by the arrival of Darwinism In particular they refer to the work of Francis Galton who through his eugenicist outlook took a keen interest in the heritability of intelligence with creativity taken as an aspect of genius 4 In the late 19th and early 20th centuries leading mathematicians and scientists such as Hermann von Helmholtz 1896 and Henri Poincare 1908 began to reflect on and publicly discuss their creative processes Modern Edit The insights of Poincare and von Helmholtz were built on in early accounts of the creative process by pioneering theorists such as Graham Wallas 29 and Max Wertheimer In his work Art of Thought published in 1926 Wallas presented one of the first models of the creative process In the Wallas stage model creative insights and illuminations may be explained by a process consisting of five stages i preparation preparatory work on a problem that focuses the individual s mind on the problem and explores the problem s dimensions ii incubation where the problem is internalized into the unconscious mind and nothing appears externally to be happening iii intimation the creative person gets a feeling that a solution is on its way iv illumination or insight where the creative idea bursts forth from its preconscious processing into conscious awareness v verification where the idea is consciously verified elaborated and then applied Wallas model is often treated as four stages with intimation seen as a sub stage Wallas considered creativity to be a legacy of the evolutionary process which allowed humans to quickly adapt to rapidly changing environments Simonton 30 provides an updated perspective on this view in his book Origins of genius Darwinian perspectives on creativity In 1927 Alfred North Whitehead gave the Gifford Lectures at the University of Edinburgh later published as Process and Reality 31 He is credited with having coined the term creativity to serve as the ultimate category of his metaphysical scheme Whitehead actually coined the term our term still the preferred currency of exchange among literature science and the arts a term that quickly became so popular so omnipresent that its invention within living memory and by Alfred North Whitehead of all people quickly became occluded 32 Although psychometric studies of creativity had been conducted by The London School of Psychology as early as 1927 with the work of H L Hargreaves into the Faculty of Imagination 33 the formal psychometric measurement of creativity from the standpoint of orthodox psychological literature is usually considered to have begun with J P Guilford s address to the American Psychological Association in 1950 34 The address helped to popularize the study of creativity and to focus attention on scientific approaches to conceptualizing creativity Statistical analyses led to the recognition of creativity as measured as a separate aspect of human cognition to IQ type intelligence into which it had previously been subsumed Guilford s work suggested that above a threshold level of IQ the relationship between creativity and classically measured intelligence broke down 35 Four C model EditJames C Kaufman and Beghetto introduced a four C model of creativity mini c transformative learning involving personally meaningful interpretations of experiences actions and insights little c everyday problem solving and creative expression Pro C exhibited by people who are professionally or vocationally creative though not necessarily eminent and Big C creativity considered great in the given field This model was intended to help accommodate models and theories of creativity that stressed competence as an essential component and the historical transformation of a creative domain as the highest mark of creativity It also the authors argued made a useful framework for analyzing creative processes in individuals 36 The contrast of terms Big C and Little c has been widely used Kozbelt Beghetto and Runco use a little c Big C model to review major theories of creativity 35 Margaret Boden distinguishes between h creativity historical and p creativity personal 37 Robinson 38 and Anna Craft 39 have focused on creativity in a general population particularly with respect to education Craft makes a similar distinction between high and little c creativity 39 and cites Ken Robinson as referring to high and democratic creativity Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi 40 has defined creativity in terms of those individuals judged to have made significant creative perhaps domain changing contributions Simonton has analysed the career trajectories of eminent creative people in order to map patterns and predictors of creative productivity 41 Process theories EditThere has been much empirical study in psychology and cognitive science of the processes through which creativity occurs Interpretation of the results of these studies has led to several possible explanations of the sources and methods of creativity Incubation Edit Main article Incubation psychology Incubation is a temporary break from creative problem solving that can result in insight 42 There has been some empirical research looking at whether as the concept of incubation in Wallas model implies a period of interruption or rest from a problem may aid creative problem solving Early work proposed that creative solutions to problems arise mysteriously from the unconscious mind while the conscious mind is occupied on other tasks 43 This hypothesis is discussed in Csikszentmihalyi s five phase model of the creative process which describes incubation as a time that your unconscious takes over This was supposed to allow for unique connections to be made without our consciousness trying to make logical order out of the problem 44 Ward 45 lists various hypotheses that have been advanced to explain why incubation may aid creative problem solving and notes how some empirical evidence is consistent with a different hypothesis Incubation aids creative problem in that it enables forgetting of misleading clues Absence of incubation may lead the problem solver to become fixated on inappropriate strategies of solving the problem 46 Convergent and divergent thinking Edit J P Guilford 47 drew a distinction between convergent and divergent production commonly renamed convergent and divergent thinking Convergent thinking involves aiming for a single correct solution to a problem whereas divergent thinking involves creative generation of multiple answers to a set problem Divergent thinking is sometimes used as a synonym for creativity in psychology literature or is considered the necessary precursor to creativity 48 Other researchers have occasionally used the terms flexible thinking or fluid intelligence which are roughly similar to but not synonymous with creativity 49 Creative cognition approach Edit In 1992 Finke et al proposed the Geneplore model in which creativity takes place in two phases a generative phase where an individual constructs mental representations called preinventive structures and an exploratory phase where those structures are used to come up with creative ideas 50 Some evidence shows that when people use their imagination to develop new ideas those ideas are heavily structured in predictable ways by the properties of existing categories and concepts 51 Weisberg 52 argued by contrast that creativity only involves ordinary cognitive processes yielding extraordinary results The Explicit Implicit Interaction EII theory Edit Helie and Sun 53 more recently proposed a unified framework for understanding creativity in problem solving namely the Explicit Implicit Interaction EII theory of creativity This new theory constitutes an attempt at providing a more unified explanation of relevant phenomena in part by reinterpreting integrating various fragmentary existing theories of incubation and insight The EII theory relies mainly on five basic principles namely The co existence of and the difference between explicit and implicit knowledge The simultaneous involvement of implicit and explicit processes in most tasks The redundant representation of explicit and implicit knowledge The integration of the results of explicit and implicit processing The iterative and possibly bidirectional processing A computational implementation of the theory was developed based on the CLARION cognitive architecture and used to simulate relevant human data This work represents an initial step in the development of process based theories of creativity encompassing incubation insight and various other related phenomena Conceptual blending Edit Main article Conceptual blending In The Act of Creation Arthur Koestler introduced the concept of bisociation that creativity arises as a result of the intersection of two quite different frames of reference 54 This idea was later developed into conceptual blending In the 1990s various approaches in cognitive science that dealt with metaphor analogy and structure mapping have been converging and a new integrative approach to the study of creativity in science art and humor has emerged under the label conceptual blending Honing theory Edit Honing theory developed principally by psychologist Liane Gabora posits that creativity arises due to the self organizing self mending nature of a worldview The creative process is a way in which the individual hones and re hones an integrated worldview Honing theory places emphasis not only on the externally visible creative outcome but also the internal cognitive restructuring and repair of the worldview brought about by the creative process When faced with a creatively demanding task there is an interaction between the conception of the task and the worldview The conception of the task changes through interaction with the worldview and the worldview changes through interaction with the task This interaction is reiterated until the task is complete at which point not only is the task conceived of differently but the worldview is subtly or drastically transformed as it follows the natural tendency of a worldview to attempt to resolve dissonance and seek internal consistency amongst its components whether they be ideas attitudes or bits of knowledge A central feature of honing theory is the notion of a potentiality state 55 Honing theory posits that creative thought proceeds not by searching through and randomly mutating predefined possibilities but by drawing upon associations that exist due to overlap in the distributed neural cell assemblies that participate in the encoding of experiences in memory Midway through the creative process one may have made associations between the current task and previous experiences but not yet disambiguated which aspects of those previous experiences are relevant to the current task Thus the creative idea may feel half baked It is at that point that it can be said to be in a potentiality state because how it will actualize depends on the different internally or externally generated contexts it interacts with Honing theory is held to explain certain phenomena not dealt with by other theories of creativity for example how different works by the same creator are observed in studies to exhibit a recognizable style or voice even in different creative outlets This is not predicted by theories of creativity that emphasize chance processes or the accumulation of expertise but it is predicted by honing theory according to which personal style reflects the creator s uniquely structured worldview Another example is in the environmental stimulus for creativity Creativity is commonly considered to be fostered by a supportive nurturing trustworthy environment conducive to self actualization However research shows that creativity is also associated with childhood adversity which would stimulate honing Everyday imaginative thought Edit In everyday thought people often spontaneously imagine alternatives to reality when they think if only 56 Their counterfactual thinking is viewed as an example of everyday creative processes 57 It has been proposed that the creation of counterfactual alternatives to reality depends on similar cognitive processes to rational thought 58 Dialectical theory of creativity Edit The term dialectical theory of creativity dates back to psychoanalyst Daniel Dervin 59 and was later developed into an interdisciplinary theory 60 The dialectical theory of creativity starts with the antique concept that creativity takes place in an interplay between order and chaos Similar ideas can be found in neurosciences and psychology Neurobiologically it can be shown that the creative process takes place in a dynamic interplay between coherence and incoherence that leads to new and usable neuronal networks Psychology shows how the dialectics of convergent and focused thinking with divergent and associative thinking leads to new ideas and products 61 Also creative personality traits like the Big Five seem to be dialectically intertwined in the creative process emotional instability vs stability extraversion vs introversion openness vs reserve agreeableness vs antagonism and disinhibition vs constraint 62 The dialectical theory of creativity applies also to counseling and psychotherapy 63 Neuroeconomic framework for creative cognition Edit Lin and Vartanian developed a framework that provides an integrative neurobiological description of creative cognition 64 This interdisciplinary framework integrates theoretical principles and empirical results from neuroeconomics reinforcement learning cognitive neuroscience and neurotransmission research on the locus coeruleus system It describes how decision making processes studied by neuroeconomists as well as activity in the locus coeruleus system underlie creative cognition and the large scale brain network dynamics associated with creativity 65 It suggests that creativity is an optimization and utility maximization problem that requires individuals to determine the optimal way to exploit and explore ideas multi armed bandit problem This utility maximization process is thought to be mediated by the locus coeruleus system 66 and this creativity framework describes how tonic and phasic locus coerulues activity work in conjunction to facilitate the exploiting and exploring of creative ideas This framework not only explains previous empirical results but also makes novel and falsifiable predictions at different levels of analysis ranging from neurobiological to cognitive and personality differences Behaviorism theory of creativity Edit Skinner attributed creativity to accidental behaviors that are reinforced by the environment 67 Spontaneous behaviors done by living creatures reflect past learned behavior 68 In Karen Pryor s book Don t Shoot the Dog she refers to how she reinforced a dolphin to display novel behaviors This is what one can attribute to both those who are creative and those who appreciate creativity A behaviorist may say that prior learning caused novel behaviors to be reinforced many times over and the individual has been shaped to produce increasingly novel behaviors 69 A creative person according to this definition would be someone who has been reinforced more often for novel behaviors than others Behaviorists would also suggest that anyone can be creative they just need to be reinforced to learn to produce novel behaviors Personal assessment EditPsychometric approaches Edit J P Guilford s group 47 which pioneered the modern psychometric study of creativity constructed several performance based tests to measure creativity in 1967 Plot Titles where participants are given the plot of a story and asked to write original titles Quick Responses is a word association test scored for uncommonness Figure Concepts where participants were given simple drawings of objects and individuals and asked to find qualities or features that are common by two or more drawings these were scored for uncommonness Unusual Uses is finding unusual uses for common everyday objects such as bricks Remote Associations where participants are asked to find a word between two given words e g Hand Call Remote Consequences where participants are asked to generate a list of consequences of unexpected events e g loss of gravity Originally Guilford was trying to create a model for intellect as a whole but in doing so also created a model for creativity Guilford made an important assumption for creative research creativity is not one abstract concept The idea that creativity is a category rather than one single concept opened up the ability for other researchers to look at creativity with a whole new perspective 70 71 Additionally Guilford hypothesized one of the first models for the components of creativity He explained that creativity was a result of having Sensitivity to problems or the ability to recognize problems Fluency which encompasses a Ideational fluency or the ability rapidly to produce a variety of ideas that fulfill stated requirements b Associational fluency or the ability to generate a list of words each of which is associated with a given word c Expressional fluency or the ability to organize words into larger units such as phrases sentences and paragraphs Flexibility which encompasses a Spontaneous flexibility or the ability to demonstrate flexibility b Adaptive flexibility or the ability to produce responses that are novel and high in quality This represents the base model by which several researchers would take and alter to produce their new theories of creativity years later 70 Building on Guilford s work tests were developed sometimes called Divergent Thinking DT tests have been both supported 72 and criticized 73 For example Torrance 74 developed the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking in 1966 75 They involved task of divergent thinking and other problem solving skills which were scored on Fluency The total number of interpretable meaningful and relevant ideas generated in response to the stimulus Originality The statistical rarity of the responses among the test subjects Elaboration The amount of detail in the responses Considerable progress has been made in automated scoring of divergent thinking tests using semantic approach When compared to human raters NLP techniques were shown to be reliable and valid in scoring the originality 76 77 The reported computer programs were able to achieve a correlation of 0 60 and 0 72 respectively to human graders Semantic networks were also used to devise originality scores that yielded significant correlations with socio personal measures 78 Most recently an NSF funded 79 team of researchers led by James C Kaufman and Mark A Runco 80 combined expertise in creativity research natural language processing computational linguistics and statistical data analysis to devise a scalable system for computerized automated testing SparcIt Creativity Index Testing system This system enabled automated scoring of DT tests that is reliable objective and scalable thus addressing most of the issues of DT tests that had been found and reported 73 The resultant computer system was able to achieve a correlation of 0 73 to human graders 81 Social personality approach Edit Some researchers have taken a social personality approach to the measurement of creativity In these studies personality traits such as independence of judgement self confidence attraction to complexity aesthetic orientation and risk taking are used as measures of the creativity of individuals 34 A meta analysis by Gregory Feist showed that creative people tend to be more open to new experiences less conventional and less conscientious more self confident self accepting driven ambitious dominant hostile and impulsive Openness conscientiousness self acceptance hostility and impulsivity had the strongest effects of the traits listed 82 Within the framework of the Big Five model of personality some consistent traits have emerged 83 Openness to experience has been shown to be consistently related to a whole host of different assessments of creativity 84 Among the other Big Five traits research has demonstrated subtle differences between different domains of creativity Compared to non artists artists tend to have higher levels of openness to experience and lower levels of conscientiousness while scientists are more open to experience conscientious and higher in the confidence dominance facets of extraversion compared to non scientists 82 Self report questionnaires Edit An alternative is using biographical methods These methods use quantitative characteristics such as the number of publications patents or performances of a work While this method was originally developed for highly creative personalities today it is also available as self report questionnaires supplemented with frequent less outstanding creative behaviors such as writing a short story or creating your own recipes For example the Creative Achievement Questionnaire a self report test that measures creative achievement across 10 domains was described in 2005 and shown to be reliable and valid when compared to other measures of creativity and to independent evaluation of creative output 85 Besides the English original it was also used in a Chinese 86 French 87 and German speaking 88 version It is the self report questionnaire most frequently used in research 86 Intelligence EditThe potential relationship between creativity and intelligence has been of interest since the late 1900s when a multitude of influential studies from Getzels amp Jackson 89 Barron 90 Wallach amp Kogan 91 and Guilford 92 focused not only on creativity but also on intelligence This joint focus highlights both the theoretical and practical importance of the relationship researchers are interested not only if the constructs are related but also how and why 93 There are multiple theories accounting for their relationship with the three main theories as follows Threshold Theory Intelligence is a necessary but not sufficient condition for creativity There is a moderate positive relationship between creativity and intelligence until IQ 120 90 92 Certification Theory Creativity is not intrinsically related to intelligence Instead individuals are required to meet the requisite level intelligence in order to gain a certain level of education work which then in turn offers the opportunity to be creative Displays of creativity are moderated by intelligence 94 Interference Theory Extremely high intelligence might interfere with creative ability 95 Sternberg and O Hara 96 proposed a framework of five possible relationships between creativity and intelligence Creativity is a subset of intelligence Intelligence is a subset of creativity Creativity and intelligence are overlapping constructs Creativity and intelligence are part of the same construct coincident sets Creativity and intelligence are distinct constructs disjoint sets Creativity as a subset of intelligence Edit A number of researchers include creativity either explicitly or implicitly as a key component of intelligence Examples of theories that include creativity as a subset of intelligence Sternberg s Theory of Successful intelligence 95 96 97 see Triarchic theory of intelligence includes creativity as a main component and comprises three sub theories Componential Analytic Contextual Practical and Experiential Creative Experiential sub theory the ability to use pre existing knowledge and skills to solve new and novel problems is directly related to creativity The Cattell Horn Carroll theory includes creativity as a subset of intelligence Specifically it is associated with the broad group factor of long term storage and retrieval Glr Glr narrow abilities relating to creativity include 98 ideational fluency associational fluency and originality creativity Silvia et al 99 conducted a study to look at the relationship between divergent thinking and verbal fluency tests and reported that both fluency and originality in divergent thinking were significantly affected by the broad level Glr factor Martindale 100 extended the CHC theory in the sense that it was proposed that those individuals who are creative are also selective in their processing speed Martindale argues that in the creative process larger amounts of information are processed more slowly in the early stages and as the individual begins to understand the problem the processing speed is increased The Dual Process Theory of Intelligence 101 posits a two factor type model of intelligence Type 1 is a conscious process and concerns goal directed thoughts which are explained by g Type 2 is an unconscious process and concerns spontaneous cognition which encompasses daydreaming and implicit learning ability Kaufman argues that creativity occurs as a result of Type 1 and Type 2 processes working together in combination The use of each type in the creative process can be used to varying degrees Intelligence as a subset of creativity Edit In this relationship model intelligence is a key component in the development of creativity Theories of creativity that include intelligence as a subset of creativity Sternberg amp Lubart s Investment Theory 102 103 Using the metaphor of a stock market they demonstrate that creative thinkers are like good investors they buy low and sell high in their ideas Like under low valued stock creative individuals generate unique ideas that are initially rejected by other people The creative individual has to persevere and convince the others of the ideas value After convincing the others and thus increasing the ideas value the creative individual sells high by leaving the idea with the other people and moves onto generating another idea According to this theory six distinct but related elements contribute to successful creativity intelligence knowledge thinking styles personality motivation and environment Intelligence is just one of the six factors that can either solely or in conjunction with the other five factors generate creative thoughts Amabile s Componential Model of Creativity 104 105 In this model there are three within individual components needed for creativity domain relevant skills creativity relevant processes and task motivation and 1 component external to the individual their surrounding social environment Creativity requires a confluence of all components High creativity will result when an individual is intrinsically motivated possesses both a high level of domain relevant skills and has high skills in creative thinking and is working in a highly creative environment Amusement Park Theoretical Model 106 In this four step theory both domain specific and generalist views are integrated into a model of creativity The researchers make use of the metaphor of the amusement park to demonstrate that within each of these creative levels intelligence plays a key role To get into the amusement park there are initial requirements e g time transport to go to the park Initial requirements like intelligence are necessary but not sufficient for creativity They are more like prerequisites for creativity and if an individual does not possess the basic level of the initial requirement intelligence then they will not be able to generate creative thoughts behaviour Secondly are the subcomponents general thematic areas that increase in specificity Like choosing which type of amusement park to visit e g a zoo or a water park these areas relate to the areas in which someone could be creative e g poetry Thirdly there are specific domains After choosing the type of park to visit e g waterpark you then have to choose which specific park to go to Within the poetry domain there are many different types e g free verse riddles sonnet etc that have to be selected from Lastly there are micro domains These are the specific tasks that reside within each domain e g individual lines in a free verse poem individual rides at the waterpark Creativity and intelligence as overlapping yet distinct constructs Edit This possible relationship concerns creativity and intelligence as distinct but intersecting constructs Theories that include Creativity and Intelligence as Overlapping Yet Distinct Constructs Renzulli s Three Ring Conception of Giftedness 107 In this conceptualisation giftedness occurs as a result from the overlap of above average intellectual ability creativity and task commitment Under this view creativity and intelligence are distinct constructs but they do overlap under the correct conditions PASS theory of intelligence In this theory the planning component relating to the ability to solve problems make decisions and take action strongly overlaps with the concept of creativity 108 Threshold Theory TT A number of previous research findings have suggested that a threshold exists in the relationship between creativity and intelligence both constructs are moderately positively correlated up to an IQ of 120 Above this threshold of an IQ of 120 if there is a relationship at all it is small and weak 89 90 109 TT posits that a moderate level of intelligence is necessary for creativity In support of the TT Barron 90 110 reported finding a non significant correlation between creativity and intelligence in a gifted sample and a significant correlation in a non gifted sample Yamamoto 111 in a sample of secondary school children reported a significant correlation between creativity and intelligence of r 0 3 and reported no significant correlation when the sample consisted of gifted children Fuchs Beauchamp et al 112 in a sample of preschoolers found that creativity and intelligence correlated from r 0 19 to r 0 49 in the group of children who had an IQ below the threshold and in the group above the threshold the correlations were r lt 0 12 Cho et al 113 reported a correlation of 0 40 between creativity and intelligence in the average IQ group of a sample of adolescents and adults and a correlation of close to r 0 0 for the high IQ group Jauk et al 114 found support for the TT but only for measures of creative potential not creative performance Much modern day research reports findings against TT Wai et al 115 in a study using data from the longitudinal Study of Mathematically Precocious Youth a cohort of elite students from early adolescence into adulthood found that differences in SAT scores at age 13 were predictive of creative real life outcomes 20 years later Kim s 116 meta analysis of 21 studies did not find any supporting evidence for TT and instead negligible correlations were reported between intelligence creativity and divergent thinking both below and above IQ s of 120 Preckel et al 117 investigating fluid intelligence and creativity reported small correlations of r 0 3 to r 0 4 across all levels of cognitive ability Creativity and intelligence as coincident sets Edit Under this view researchers posit that there are no differences in the mechanisms underlying creativity in those used in normal problem solving and in normal problem solving there is no need for creativity Thus creativity and Intelligence problem solving are the same thing Perkins 118 referred to this as the nothing special view Weisberg amp Alba 119 examined problem solving by having participants complete the nine dots puzzle where the participants are asked to connect all nine dots in the three rows of three dots using four straight lines or less without lifting their pen or tracing the same line twice The problem can only be solved if the lines go outside the boundaries of the square of dots Results demonstrated that even when participants were given this insight they still found it difficult to solve the problem thus showing that to successfully complete the task it is not just insight or creativity that is required Creativity and intelligence as disjoint sets Edit In this view creativity and intelligence are completely different unrelated constructs Getzels and Jackson 89 administered five creativity measures to a group of 449 children from grades 6 12 and compared these test findings to results from previously administered by the school IQ tests They found that the correlation between the creativity measures and IQ was r 0 26 The high creativity group scored in the top 20 of the overall creativity measures but were not included in the top 20 of IQ scorers The high intelligence group scored the opposite they scored in the top 20 for IQ but were outside the top 20 scorers for creativity thus showing that creativity and intelligence are distinct and unrelated However this work has been heavily criticised Wallach and Kogan 91 highlighted that the creativity measures were not only weakly related to one another to the extent that they were no more related to one another than they were with IQ but they seemed to also draw upon non creative skills McNemar 120 noted that there were major measurement issues in that the IQ scores were a mixture from three different IQ tests Wallach and Kogan 91 administered five measures of creativity each of which resulted in a score for originality and fluency and 10 measures of general intelligence to 151 5th grade children These tests were untimed and given in a game like manner aiming to facilitate creativity Inter correlations between creativity tests were on average r 0 41 Inter correlations between intelligence measures were on average r 0 51 with each other Creativity tests and intelligence measures correlated r 0 09 Neuroscience Edit Distributed functional brain network associated with divergent thinking The neuroscience of creativity looks at the operation of the brain during creative behaviour It has been addressed 121 in the article Creative Innovation Possible Brain Mechanisms The authors write that creative innovation might require coactivation and communication between regions of the brain that ordinarily are not strongly connected Highly creative people who excel at creative innovation tend to differ from others in three ways they have a high level of specialized knowledge they are capable of divergent thinking mediated by the frontal lobe and they are able to modulate neurotransmitters such as norepinephrine in their frontal lobe Thus the frontal lobe appears to be the part of the cortex that is most important for creativity This article also explored the links between creativity and sleep mood and addiction disorders and depression In 2005 Alice Flaherty presented a three factor model of the creative drive Drawing from evidence in brain imaging drug studies and lesion analysis she described the creative drive as resulting from an interaction of the frontal lobes the temporal lobes and dopamine from the limbic system The frontal lobes can be seen as responsible for idea generation and the temporal lobes for idea editing and evaluation Abnormalities in the frontal lobe such as depression or anxiety generally decrease creativity while abnormalities in the temporal lobe often increase creativity High activity in the temporal lobe typically inhibits activity in the frontal lobe and vice versa High dopamine levels increase general arousal and goal directed behaviors and reduce latent inhibition and all three effects increase the drive to generate ideas 122 A 2015 study on creativity found that it involves the interaction of multiple neural networks including those that support associative thinking along with other default mode network functions 123 Similarly in 2018 Lin and Vartanian proposed a neuroeconomic framework that precisely describes norepinephrine s role in creativity and modulating large scale brain networks associated with creativity 64 This framework describes how neural activity in different brain regions and networks like the default mode network are tracking utility or subjective value of ideas In 2018 experiments showed that when the brain suppresses obvious or known solutions the outcome is solutions that are more creative This suppression is mediated by alpha oscillations in the right temporal lobe 124 Working memory and the cerebellum Edit Vandervert 125 described how the brain s frontal lobes and the cognitive functions of the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation Vandervert s explanation rests on considerable evidence that all processes of working memory responsible for processing all thought 126 are adaptively modeled for increased efficiency by the cerebellum 127 The cerebellum consisting of 100 billion neurons which is more than the entirety of the rest of the brain 128 is also widely known to adaptively model all bodily movement for efficiency The cerebellum s adaptive models of working memory processing are then fed back to especially frontal lobe working memory control processes 129 where creative and innovative thoughts arise 130 Apparently creative insight or the aha experience is then triggered in the temporal lobe 131 According to Vandervert the details of creative adaptation begin in forward cerebellar models which are anticipatory exploratory controls for movement and thought These cerebellar processing and control architectures have been termed Hierarchical Modular Selection and Identification for Control HMOSAIC 132 New hierarchically arranged levels of the cerebellar control architecture HMOSAIC develop as mental mulling in working memory is extended over time These new levels of the control architecture are fed forward to the frontal lobes Since the cerebellum adaptively models all movement and all levels of thought and emotion 133 Vandervert s approach helps explain creativity and innovation in sports art music the design of video games technology mathematics the child prodigy and thought in general Essentially Vandervert has argued that when a person is confronted with a challenging new situation visual spatial working memory and speech related working memory are decomposed and re composed fractionated by the cerebellum and then blended in the cerebral cortex in an attempt to deal with the new situation With repeated attempts to deal with challenging situations the cerebro cerebellar blending process continues to optimize the efficiency of how working memory deals with the situation or problem 134 Most recently he has argued that this is the same process only involving visual spatial working memory and pre language vocalization that led to the evolution of language in humans 135 Vandervert and Vandervert Weathers have pointed out that this blending process because it continuously optimizes efficiencies constantly improves prototyping attempts toward the invention or innovation of new ideas music art or technology 136 Prototyping they argue not only produces new products it trains the cerebro cerebellar pathways involved to become more efficient at prototyping itself Further Vandervert and Vandervert Weathers believe that this repetitive mental prototyping or mental rehearsal involving the cerebellum and the cerebral cortex explains the success of the self driven individualized patterning of repetitions initiated by the teaching methods of the Khan Academy The model proposed by Vandervert has however received incisive critique from several authors 137 138 REM sleep Edit Creativity involves the forming of associative elements into new combinations that are useful or meet some requirement Sleep aids this process 139 REM rather than NREM sleep appears to be responsible 140 141 This has been suggested to be due to changes in cholinergic and noradrenergic neuromodulation that occurs during REM sleep 140 During this period of sleep high levels of acetylcholine in the hippocampus suppress feedback from the hippocampus to the neocortex and lower levels of acetylcholine and norepinephrine in the neocortex encourage the spread of associational activity within neocortical areas without control from the hippocampus 142 This is in contrast to waking consciousness where higher levels of norepinephrine and acetylcholine inhibit recurrent connections in the neocortex It is proposed that REM sleep aids creativity by allowing neocortical structures to reorganize associative hierarchies in which information from the hippocampus would be reinterpreted in relation to previous semantic representations or nodes 140 Affect EditSome theories suggest that creativity may be particularly susceptible to affective influence As noted in voting behavior the term affect in this context can refer to liking or disliking key aspects of the subject in question This work largely follows from findings in psychology regarding the ways in which affective states are involved in human judgment and decision making 143 Positive affect relations Edit According to Alice Isen positive affect has three primary effects on cognitive activity Positive affect makes additional cognitive material available for processing increasing the number of cognitive elements available for association Positive affect leads to defocused attention and a more complex cognitive context increasing the breadth of those elements that are treated as relevant to the problem Positive affect increases cognitive flexibility increasing the probability that diverse cognitive elements will in fact become associated Together these processes lead positive affect to have a positive influence on creativity Barbara Fredrickson in her broaden and build model suggests that positive emotions such as joy and love broaden a person s available repertoire of cognitions and actions thus enhancing creativity According to these researchers positive emotions increase the number of cognitive elements available for association attention scope and the number of elements that are relevant to the problem cognitive scope Day by day psychological experiences including emotions perceptions and motivation will significantly impact creative performance Creativity is higher when emotions and perceptions are more positive and when intrinsic motivation is stronger 144 Various meta analyses such as Baas et al 2008 of 66 studies about creativity and affect support the link between creativity and positive affect 145 146 Computational creativity EditMain article Computational creativity Jurgen Schmidhuber s formal theory of creativity 147 148 postulates that creativity curiosity and interestingness are by products of a simple computational principle for measuring and optimizing learning progress Consider an agent able to manipulate its environment and thus its own sensory inputs The agent can use a black box optimization method such as reinforcement learning to learn through informed trial and error sequences of actions that maximize the expected sum of its future reward signals There are extrinsic reward signals for achieving externally given goals such as finding food when hungry But Schmidhuber s objective function to be maximized also includes an additional intrinsic term to model wow effects This non standard term motivates purely creative behavior of the agent even when there are no external goals A wow effect is formally defined as follows As the agent is creating and predicting and encoding the continually growing history of actions and sensory inputs it keeps improving the predictor or encoder which can be implemented as an artificial neural network or some other machine learning device that can exploit regularities in the data to improve its performance over time The improvements can be measured precisely by computing the difference in computational costs storage size number of required synapses errors time needed to encode new observations before and after learning This difference depends on the encoder s present subjective knowledge which changes over time but the theory formally takes this into account The cost difference measures the strength of the present wow effect due to sudden improvements in data compression or computational speed It becomes an intrinsic reward signal for the action selector The objective function thus motivates the action optimizer to create action sequences causing more wow effects Irregular random data or noise do not permit any wow effects or learning progress and thus are boring by nature providing no reward Already known and predictable regularities also are boring Temporarily interesting are only the initially unknown novel regular patterns in both actions and observations This motivates the agent to perform continual open ended active creative exploration Schmidhuber s work is highly influential in intrinsic motivation which has emerged as a research topic in its own right as part of the study of artificial intelligence and robotics According to Schmidhuber his objective function explains the activities of scientists artists and comedians 149 150 For example physicists are motivated to create experiments leading to observations obeying previously unpublished physical laws permitting better data compression Likewise composers receive intrinsic reward for creating non arbitrary melodies with unexpected but regular harmonies that permit wow effects through data compression improvements Similarly a comedian gets intrinsic reward for inventing a novel joke with an unexpected punch line related to the beginning of the story in an initially unexpected but quickly learnable way that also allows for better compression of the perceived data 151 Schmidhuber argues that ongoing computer hardware advances will greatly scale up rudimentary artificial scientists and artists clarification needed based on simple implementations of the basic principle since 1990 152 He used the theory to create low complexity art 153 and an attractive human face 154 Creativity and mental health EditMain article Creativity and mental health A study by psychologist J Philippe Rushton found creativity to correlate with intelligence and psychoticism 155 Another study found creativity to be greater in people with schizotypal personality disorder than in people with either schizophrenia or those without mental health conditions While divergent thinking was associated with bilateral activation of the prefrontal cortex schizotypal individuals were found to have much greater activation of their right prefrontal cortex 156 This study hypothesizes that such individuals are better at accessing both hemispheres allowing them to make novel associations at a faster rate Consistent with this hypothesis ambidexterity is also more common in people with schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia Three studies by Mark Batey and Adrian Furnham have demonstrated the relationships between schizotypal personalty disorder 157 158 and hypomanic personality 159 and several different measures of creativity Particularly strong links have been identified between creativity and mood disorders particularly manic depressive disorder a k a bipolar disorder and depressive disorder a k a unipolar disorder In Touched with Fire Manic Depressive Illness and the Artistic Temperament Kay Redfield Jamison summarizes studies of mood disorder rates in writers poets and artists She also explores research that identifies mood disorders in such famous writers and artists as Ernest Hemingway who shot himself after electroconvulsive treatment Virginia Woolf who drowned herself when she felt a depressive episode coming on composer Robert Schumann who died in a mental institution and even the famed visual artist Michelangelo A study looking at 300 000 persons with schizophrenia bipolar disorder or unipolar depression and their relatives found overrepresentation in creative professions for those with bipolar disorder as well as for undiagnosed siblings of those with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder There was no overall overrepresentation but overrepresentation for artistic occupations among those diagnosed with schizophrenia There was no association for those with unipolar depression or their relatives 160 Another study involving more than one million people conducted by Swedish researchers at the Karolinska Institute reported a number of correlations between creative occupations and mental illnesses Writers had a higher risk of anxiety and bipolar disorders schizophrenia unipolar depression and substance abuse and were almost twice as likely as the general population to kill themselves Dancers and photographers were also more likely to have bipolar disorder 161 As a group those in the creative professions were no more likely to have psychiatric disorders than other people although they were more likely to have a close relative with a disorder including anorexia and to some extent autism the Journal of Psychiatric Research reports 161 People who have worked in the arts industry throughout history have faced many environmental factors that are associated with and can sometimes influence mental illness Including things such as poverty persecution social alienation psychological trauma substance abuse and high stress 162 In fact according to psychologist Robert Epstein PhD creativity can be obstructed through stress 163 So while research has found that people are the most creative in positive moods 164 it might be pursuing a career that causes some problems Conversely research has shown that creative activities such as art therapy poetry writing journaling and reminiscence can promote mental well being 165 166 Bipolar Disorders and Creativity Edit Nancy Andreasen was one of the first known researchers to carry out a large scale study revolving around creativity and whether mental illnesses have an impact on someone s ability to be creative Originally she had expected to find a link between creativity and schizophrenia but her research sample had no real history of schizophrenia from the book authors she pooled Her findings instead showed that 80 of the creative group had previously had some form of mental illness episode in their lifetime 167 When she performed follow up studies over a 15 year period she found that 43 of the authors had bipolar disorder compared to the 1 of the general public that has the disease In 1989 there was another study done by Kay Redfield Jamison that reaffirmed those statistics by having 38 of her sample of authors having a history of mood disorders 168 Anthony Storr who is a prominent psychiatrist remarked that The creative process can be a way of protecting the individual against being overwhelmed by depression a means of regaining a sense of mastery in those who have lost it and to a varying extent a way of repairing the self damaged by bereavement or by the loss of confidence in human relationships which accompanies depression from whatever cause 167 According to a study done by Shapiro and Weisberg there appears to be a positive correlation between the manic upswings of the cycles of bipolar disorder and the ability for an individual to be more creative 169 The data that they had collected and analyzed through multiple tests showed that it was in fact not the depressive swing that many believe to bring forth dark creative spurts but the act of climbing out of the depressive episode that sparked creativity The reason behind this spur of creative genius could come from the type of self image that the person has during a time of hypomania A hypomanic person may be feeling a bolstered sense of self confidence creative confidence and sense of individualism 169 In reports from people who were diagnosed with bipolar disorder they noted themselves as having a larger range of emotional understanding heightened states of perception and an ability to connect better with those in the world around them 170 Other reported traits include higher rates of productivity higher senses of self awareness and a greater understanding of empathy Those who have bipolar disorder also understand their own sense of heightened creativity and ability to get immense amounts of tasks done all at once McCraw Parker Fletcher amp Friend 2013 report that out of 219 participants aged 19 to 63 that have been diagnosed bipolar disorder 82 of them reported having elevated feelings of creativity during the hypomanic swings 171 Giannouli believes that the creativity a person diagnosed with bipolar disorder feels comes as a form of stress management 172 In the realm of music one might be expressing their stress or pains through the pieces they write in order to better understand those same feelings Famous authors and musicians along with some actors would often attribute their wild enthusiasm to something like a hypomanic state 173 The artistic side of society has also been notorious for behaviors that are seen as maladapted to societal norms Side effects that come with bipolar disorder match up with many of the behaviors that we see in high profile creative personalities these include but are not limited to alcohol addiction drug abuse including stimulants depressants hallucinogens and dissociatives opioids inhalants and cannabis difficulties in holding regular occupations interpersonal problems legal issues and a high risk of suicide 173 Weisberg believes that the state of mania sets free the powers of a thinker What he implies here is that not only has the person become more creative they have fundamentally changed the kind of thoughts they produce 174 In a study done of poets who seem to have especially high percentages of bipolar authors it was found that over a period of three years those poets would have cycles of really creative and powerful works of poetry The timelines over the three year study looked at the poet s personal journals and their clinical records and found that the timelines between their most powerful poems matched that of their upswings in bipolar disorder 174 Personality EditCreativity can be expressed in a number of different forms depending on unique people and environments A number of different theorists have suggested models of the creative person One model suggests that there are four Creativity Profiles that can help produce growth innovation speed etc 175 i Incubate Long term Development ii Imagine Breakthrough Ideas iii Improve Incremental Adjustments iv Invest Short term Goals Research by Dr Mark Batey of the Psychometrics at Work Research Group at Manchester Business School has suggested that the creative profile can be explained by four primary creativity traits with narrow facets within each i Idea Generation Fluency Originality Incubation and Illumination ii Personality Curiosity and Tolerance for Ambiguity iii Motivation Intrinsic Extrinsic and Achievement iv Confidence Producing Sharing and Implementing This model was developed in a sample of 1000 working adults using the statistical techniques of Exploratory Factor Analysis followed by Confirmatory Factor Analysis by Structural Equation Modelling 176 An important aspect of the creativity profiling approach is to account for the tension between predicting the creative profile of an individual as characterised by the psychometric approach and the evidence that team creativity is founded on diversity and difference 177 One characteristic of creative people as measured by some psychologists is what is called divergent production Divergent production is the ability of a person to generate a diverse assortment yet an appropriate amount of responses to a given situation 178 One way of measuring divergent production is by administering the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking 179 The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking assesses the diversity quantity and appropriateness of participants responses to a variety of open ended questions Other researchers of creativity see the difference in creative people as a cognitive process of dedication to problem solving and developing expertise in the field of their creative expression Hard working people study the work of people before them and within their current area become experts in their fields and then have the ability to add to and build upon previous information in innovative and creative ways In a study of projects by design students students who had more knowledge on their subject on average had greater creativity within their projects 180 Other researchers emphasize how creative people are better at balancing between divergent and convergent production which depends on an individual s innate preference or ability to explore and exploit ideas 64 The aspect of motivation within a person s personality may predict creativity levels in the person Motivation stems from two different sources intrinsic and extrinsic motivation Intrinsic motivation is an internal drive within a person to participate or invest as a result of personal interest desires hopes goals etc Extrinsic motivation is a drive from outside of a person and might take the form of payment rewards fame approval from others etc Although extrinsic motivation and intrinsic motivation can both increase creativity in certain cases strictly extrinsic motivation often impedes creativity in people 181 From a personality traits perspective there are a number of traits that are associated with creativity in people 182 Creative people tend to be more open to new experiences are more self confident are more ambitious self accepting impulsive driven dominant and hostile compared to people with less creativity From an evolutionary perspective creativity may be a result of the outcome of years of generating ideas As ideas are continuously generated the need to evolve produces a need for new ideas and developments As a result people have been creating and developing new innovative and creative ideas to build our progress as a society 183 In studying exceptionally creative people in history some common traits in lifestyle and environment are often found Creative people in history usually had supportive parents but rigid and non nurturing Most had an interest in their field at an early age and most had a highly supportive and skilled mentor in their field of interest Often the field they chose was relatively uncharted allowing for their creativity to be expressed more in a field with less previous information Most exceptionally creative people devoted almost all of their time and energy into their craft and after about a decade had a creative breakthrough of fame Their lives were marked with extreme dedication and a cycle of hard work and breakthroughs as a result of their determination 184 Another theory of creative people is the investment theory of creativity This approach suggest that there are many individual and environmental factors that must exist in precise ways for extremely high levels of creativity opposed to average levels of creativity In the investment sense a person with their particular characteristics in their particular environment may see an opportunity to devote their time and energy into something that has been overlooked by others The creative person develops an undervalued or under recognised idea to the point that it is established as a new and creative idea Just like in the financial world some investments are worth the buy in while others are less productive and do not build to the extent that the investor expected This investment theory of creativity views creativity in a unique perspective compared to others by asserting that creativity might rely to some extent on the right investment of effort being added to a field at the right time in the right way 185 Malevolent creativity EditMain article Malevolent creativity So called malevolent creativity is associated with the dark side of creativity 186 187 This type of creativity is not typically accepted within society and is defined by the intention to cause harm to others through original and innovative means Malevolent creativity should be distinguished from negative creativity in that negative creativity may unintentionally cause harm to others whereas malevolent creativity is explicitly malevolently motivated While it is often associated with criminal behaviour it can also be observed in ordinary day to day life as lying cheating and betrayal 188 Crime Edit Malevolent creativity is often a key contributor to crime and in its most destructive form can even manifest as terrorism As creativity requires deviating from the conventional there is a permanent tension between being creative and producing products that go too far and in some cases to the point of breaking the law Aggression is a key predictor of malevolent creativity and studies have also shown that increased levels of aggression also correlates to a higher likelihood of committing crime 189 Predictive factors Edit Although everyone shows some levels of malevolent creativity under certain conditions those that have a higher propensity towards it have increased tendencies to deceive and manipulate others to their own gain While malevolent creativity appears to dramatically increase when an individual is placed under unfair conditions personality particularly aggressiveness is also a key predictor in anticipating levels of malevolent thinking Researchers Harris and Reiter Palmon investigated the role of aggression in levels of malevolent creativity in particular levels of implicit aggression and the tendency to employ aggressive actions in response to problem solving The personality traits of physical aggression conscientiousness emotional intelligence and implicit aggression all seem to be related with malevolent creativity 187 Harris and Reiter Palmon s research showed that when subjects were presented with a problem that triggered malevolent creativity participants high in implicit aggression and low in premeditation expressed the largest number of malevolently themed solutions When presented with the more benign problem that triggered prosocial motives of helping others and cooperating those high in implicit aggression even if they were high in impulsiveness were far less destructive in their imagined solutions They concluded premeditation more than implicit aggression controlled an individual s expression of malevolent creativity 190 The current measure for malevolent creativity is the 13 item test Malevolent Creativity Behaviour Scale MCBS 188 Cultural differences in creativity EditCreativity is viewed differently in different countries 191 For example cross cultural research centered on Hong Kong found that Westerners view creativity more in terms of the individual attributes of a creative person such as their aesthetic taste while Chinese people view creativity more in terms of the social influence of creative people i e what they can contribute to society 192 Mpofu et al surveyed 28 African languages and found that 27 had no word which directly translated to creativity the exception being Arabic 193 The principle of linguistic relativity i e that language can affect thought suggests that the lack of an equivalent word for creativity may affect the views of creativity among speakers of such languages However more research would be needed to establish this and there is certainly no suggestion that this linguistic difference makes people any less or more creative Africa has a rich heritage of creative pursuits such as music art and storytelling Nevertheless it is true that there has been very little research on creativity in Africa 194 and there has also been very little research on creativity in Latin America 195 Creativity has been more thoroughly researched in the northern hemisphere but here again there are cultural differences even between countries or groups of countries in close proximity For example in Scandinavian countries creativity is seen as an individual attitude which helps in coping with life s challenges 196 while in Germany creativity is seen more as a process that can be applied to help solve problems 197 Organizational creativity Edit Training meeting in an eco design stainless steel company in Brazil The leaders among other things wish to cheer and encourage the workers in order to achieve a higher level of creativity It has been the topic of various research studies to establish that organizational effectiveness depends on the creativity of the workforce to a large extent For any given organization measures of effectiveness vary depending upon its mission environmental context nature of work the product or service it produces and customer demands Thus the first step in evaluating organizational effectiveness is to understand the organization itself how it functions how it is structured and what it emphasizes Amabile 198 and Sullivan and Harper 199 argued that to enhance creativity in business three components were needed Expertise technical procedural and intellectual knowledge Creative thinking skills how flexibly and imaginatively people approach problems and Motivation especially intrinsic motivation There are two types of motivation extrinsic motivation external factors for example threats of being fired or money as a reward intrinsic motivation comes from inside an individual satisfaction enjoyment of work etc Six managerial practices to encourage motivation are Challenge matching people with the right assignments Freedom giving people autonomy choosing means to achieve goals Resources such as time money space etc There must be balance fit among resources and people Work group features diverse supportive teams where members share the excitement willingness to help and recognize each other s talents Supervisory encouragement recognitions cheering praising Organizational support value emphasis information sharing collaboration Nonaka who examined several successful Japanese companies similarly saw creativity and knowledge creation as being important to the success of organizations 200 In particular he emphasized the role that tacit knowledge has to play in the creative process In business originality is not enough The idea must also be appropriate useful and actionable 198 201 Creative competitive intelligence is a new solution to solve this problem According to Reijo Siltala it links creativity to innovation process and competitive intelligence to creative workers Creativity can be encouraged in people and professionals and in the workplace It is essential for innovation and is a factor affecting economic growth and businesses In 2013 the sociologist Silvia Leal Martin using the Innova 3DX method suggested measuring the various parameters that encourage creativity and innovation corporate culture work environment leadership and management creativity self esteem and optimism locus of control and learning orientation motivation and fear 202 Similarly social psychologists organizational scientists and management scientists who conduct extensive research on the factors that influence creativity and innovation in teams and organizations have developed integrative theoretical models that emphasize the roles of team composition team processes and organizational culture These theoretical models also emphasize the mutually reinforcing relationships between them in promoting innovation 203 204 205 206 The investigation by Sai Loo an academic and author of research monographs 207 on creative working in the knowledge economy brings together studies of creativity as described in this web page It offers connections with the sections on the Four C model Theories of creative processes Creativity as a subset of intelligence Creativity and personality and In organisations It is the last section that the investigation addresses Research studies of the knowledge economy may be classified into three levels macro meso and micro Macro studies refer to investigations at a societal or transnational dimension Meso studies focus on organisations Micro investigations centre on the minutiae workings of workers There is also an interdisciplinary dimension such as research from businesses 208 209 economics 210 211 212 education 213 214 human resource management 215 knowledge and organizational management 216 217 218 sociology psychology and knowledge economy related sectors especially information technology IT software 219 220 and advertising 221 222 Loo studies how individual workers in the knowledge economy use their creativity and know how in the advertising and IT software sectors It examines this phenomenon across three developed countries of England Japan and Singapore to observe global perspectives Specifically the study uses qualitative data from semi structured interviews of the related professionals in the roles of creative directing and copywriting in advertising and systems software developing and software programme managing 207 The study 207 offers a conceptual framework of a two dimensional matrix of individual and collaborative working styles and single and multi contexts The investigation draws on literature sources from the four disciplines of economics 223 224 management 225 226 227 sociology 228 229 230 231 232 and psychology 233 234 The themes arising from the analysis of knowledge work and creativity literature serve to create a distinct theoretical framework of creative knowledge work These workers apply their cognitive abilities creative personalities and skill sets in the areas of science technology or culture industries to invent or discover new possibilities e g a medium product or service These work activities may be done individually or collectively Education training and encultured environments are necessary for the performance of these creative activities Acts of creativity are viewed as asking new questions over and above those questions asked by an intelligent person seeking novelty when reviewing a situation 235 and creating something that is different and novel i e a variation on the idea of existing ideas in a domain 236 This framework is evidenced by the empirical chapters on the micro workings of creative workers in the two knowledge economy sectors from global perspectives This investigation identifies a definition of creative work three types of work and the necessary conditions for it to occur These workers use a combination of creative applications including anticipatory imagination problem solving problem seeking and generating ideas and aesthetic sensibilities Taking aesthetic sensibilities as an example for a creative director in the advertising industry it is a visual imagery whether still or moving via a camera lens and for a software programmer it is the innovative technical expertise in which the software is written There are specific creative applications for each of the sectors such as emotional connection in the advertising sector and the power of expression and sensitivity in the IT software sector In addition to the creative applications creative workers require abilities and aptitudes to carry out their roles Passion for one s job is generic For copywriters this passion is identified with fun enjoyment and happiness alongside attributes such as honesty regarding the product confidence and patience in finding the appropriate copy Knowledge is also required in the disciplines of the humanities e g literature the creative arts e g painting and music and technical related know how e g mathematics computer sciences and physical sciences In the IT software technical knowledge of computer languages e g C is especially significant for programmers whereas the degree of technical expertise may be less for a programme manager as only knowledge of the relevant language is necessary to understand the issues for communicating with the team of developers and testers There are three types of work One is intra sectoral e g general sponge and in tune with the zeitgeist advertising and power of expression and sensitivity IT software The second is inter sectoral e g integration of advertising activities advertising and autonomous decentralized systems ADS IT software The third relates to changes in culture practices in the sectors e g three dimensional trust and green credentials advertising and collaboration with HEIs and industry and ADS system in the Tokyo train operator IT software The necessary conditions for creative work to exist are a supportive environment such as supportive information communications and electronic technologies ICET infrastructure training work environment and education This investigation has implications for lifelong learning of these workers informally and formally Teaching institutions need to offer multi disciplinary knowledge of humanities arts and sciences and it has impacts on the programme structure delivery approaches and assessments At a macro level governments need to offer a rich diet of cultural activities outdoor activities and sports fixtures that inform potential creative workers in the areas of video gaming and advertising This study has implications for work organisations that support and encourage collaborative working alongside individual working offer opportunities to engage in continuous professional development formally and informally and foster an environment which promotes experiential functioning and supports experimentation Team composition Edit Diversity between team members backgrounds and knowledge can increase team creativity by expanding the total collection of unique information that is available to the team and by introducing different perspectives that can integrate in novel ways However under some conditions diversity can also decrease team creativity by making it more difficult for team members to communicate about ideas and causing interpersonal conflicts between those with different perspectives 237 Thus the potential advantages of diversity must be supported by appropriate team processes and organizational cultures in order to enhance creativity 203 204 205 206 238 239 Recent study by An Zeng et al 240 found that studies of fresh research teams is associated with higher creativity or originality Team processes Edit Team communication norms such as respecting others expertise paying attention to others ideas expecting information sharing tolerating disagreements negotiating remaining open to others ideas learning from others and building on each other s ideas increase team creativity by facilitating the social processes involved with brainstorming and problem solving Through these processes team members are able to access their collective pool of knowledge reach shared understandings identify new ways of understanding problems or tasks and make new connections between ideas Engaging in these social processes also promotes positive team affect which facilitates collective creativity 203 205 206 238 Organizational culture Edit Supportive and motivational environments that create psychological safety by encouraging risk taking and tolerating mistakes increase team creativity as well 203 204 205 206 Organizations in which help seeking help giving and collaboration are rewarded promote innovation by providing opportunities and contexts in which team processes that lead to collective creativity can occur 241 Additionally leadership styles that downplay status hierarchies or power differences within an organization and empower people to speak up about their ideas or opinions also help to create cultures that are conducive to creativity 203 204 205 206 Constraints Edit Main article Creative limitation There is a long standing debate on how material constraints e g lack of money materials or equipment affect creativity In psychological and managerial research two competing views in this regard prevail In one view many scholars propose a negative effect of material constraints on innovation and claim that material constraints starve creativity 242 The proponents of this view argue that adequate material resources are needed to engage in creative activities like experimenting with new solutions and idea exploration 242 In an opposing view scholars assert that people tend to stick to established routines or solutions as long as they are not forced to deviate from them by constraints 243 244 245 In this sense Neren posits that scarcity is an important driver of creativity 246 Consistently Gibbert and Scranton demonstrated how material constraints facilitated the development of jet engines in World War II 247 To reconcile these competing views contingency models were proposed 248 249 250 The rationale behind these models is that certain contingency factors e g creativity climate or creativity relevant skills influence the relationship between constraints and creativity 248 These contingency factors reflect the need for higher levels of motivation and skills when working on creative tasks under constraints 248 Depending on these contingency factors there is either a positive or negative relationship between constraints and creativity 248 249 The sociology of creativity EditCreativity research for most of the twentieth century was dominated by psychology and business studies with little work done in sociology as a discipline While since the turn of the millennium there has been more attention paid by sociological researchers 251 252 it has yet to establish itself as a specific research field with reviews of sociological research into creativity a rarity in high impact literature 253 While psychology has tended to focus on the individual as the locus of creativity sociological research is directed more at the structures and context within which creative activity takes place primarily based in the more long standing field of the sociology of culture which finds its roots in the works of Marx Durkheim and Weber This has meant a particular focus on the cultural and creative industries as sociological phenomena Such research has covered a variety of areas including the economics and production of culture the role of creative industries in development and the rise of the creative class 254 Economic views EditEconomic approaches to creativity have focussed on three aspects the impact of creativity on economic growth methods of modelling markets for creativity and the maximisation of economic creativity innovation In the early 20th century Joseph Schumpeter introduced the economic theory of creative destruction to describe the way in which old ways of doing things are endogenously destroyed and replaced by the new Some economists such as Paul Romer view creativity as an important element in the recombination of elements to produce new technologies and products and consequently economic growth Creativity leads to capital and creative products are protected by intellectual property laws Mark A Runco and Daniel Rubenson have tried to describe a psychoeconomic model of creativity 255 In such a model creativity is the product of endowments and active investments in creativity the costs and benefits of bringing creative activity to market determine the supply of creativity Such an approach has been criticised for its view of creativity consumption as always having positive utility and for the way it analyses the value of future innovations 256 The creative class is seen by some to be an important driver of modern economies In his 2002 book The Rise of the Creative Class economist Richard Florida popularized the notion that regions with 3 T s of economic development Technology Talent and Tolerance also have high concentrations of creative professionals and tend to have a higher level of economic development 257 Fostering creativity EditMain article Creativity techniques Several different researchers have proposed methods of increasing the creativity of an individual Such ideas range from the psychological cognitive such as Osborn Parnes Creative Problem Solving Process Synectics science based creative thinking Purdue Creative Thinking Program and Edward de Bono s lateral thinking to the highly structured such as TRIZ the Theory of Inventive Problem Solving and its variant Algorithm of Inventive Problem Solving developed by the Russian scientist Genrich Altshuller and Computer Aided morphological analysis Daniel Pink in his 2005 book A Whole New Mind repeating arguments posed throughout the 20th century argues that we are entering a new age where creativity is becoming increasingly important In this conceptual age we will need to foster and encourage right directed thinking representing creativity and emotion over left directed thinking representing logical analytical thought However this simplification of right versus left brain thinking is not supported by the research data 258 Nickerson 259 provides a summary of the various creativity techniques that have been proposed These include approaches that have been developed by both academia and industry Establishing purpose and intention Building basic skills Encouraging acquisitions of domain specific knowledge Stimulating and rewarding curiosity and exploration Building motivation especially internal motivation Encouraging confidence and a willingness to take risks Focusing on mastery and self competition Promoting supportable beliefs about creativity Providing opportunities for choice and discovery Developing self management metacognitive skills Teaching techniques and strategies for facilitating creative performance Providing balance Managing the need for closure Edit Experiments suggest the need for closure of task participants whether as a reflection of personality or induced through time pressure negatively impacts creativity 260 Accordingly it has been suggested that reading fiction which can reduce the cognitive need for closure may help to encourage creativity 261 Education policies EditSome see the conventional system of schooling as stifling of creativity and attempt particularly in the preschool kindergarten and early school years to provide a creativity friendly rich imagination fostering environment for young children 259 262 263 Researchers have seen this as important because technology is advancing our society at an unprecedented rate and creative problem solving will be needed to cope with these challenges as they arise 263 In addition to helping with problem solving creativity also helps students identify problems where others have failed to do so 259 262 264 See the Waldorf School as an example of an education program that promotes creative thought Promoting intrinsic motivation and problem solving are two areas where educators can foster creativity in students Students are more creative when they see a task as intrinsically motivating valued for its own sake 262 263 265 266 To promote creative thinking educators need to identify what motivates their students and structure teaching around it Providing students with a choice of activities to complete allows them to become more intrinsically motivated and therefore creative in completing the tasks 259 267 Teaching students to solve problems that do not have well defined answers is another way to foster their creativity This is accomplished by allowing students to explore problems and redefine them possibly drawing on knowledge that at first may seem unrelated to the problem in order to solve it 259 262 263 265 In adults mentoring individuals is another way to foster their creativity 268 However the benefits of mentoring creativity apply only to creative contributions considered great in a given field not to everyday creative expression 88 Musical creativity is a gateway to the flow state A place conducive to spontaneity improvisation and creativity Recent studies have shown that we should emphasize students creative side and integrate more creativity into their curriculums A reason for this is that Students aiming to express themselves and connect with peers through music will be able to tap into higher order brain regions during improvisation rather than limiting themselves to the areas seen for rudimentary pattern generator Landau 2017 30 Schools should teach this Buddhist philosophy to help musicians for example better understand what s happening when you re in a creative process and ways to realize it better In this sense an improvisation is a form of self expression that can generate connectivity amongst peers and surpasses the age old rudimentary aspects of school Scotland Edit In the Scottish education system creativity is identified as a core skillset for learning life and work and is defined as a process which generates ideas that have value to the individual It involves looking at familiar things with a fresh eye examining problems with an open mind making connections learning from mistakes and using imagination to explore new possibilities 269 The need to develop a shared language and understanding of creativity and its role across every aspect of learning teaching and continuous improvement was identified as a necessary aim 270 and a set of four skills is used to allow educators to discuss and develop creativity skills across all subjects and sectors of education curiosity open mindedness imagination and problem solving 271 Distinctions are made between creative learning when learners are using their creativity skills creative teaching when educators are using their creativity skills and creative change when creativity skills are applied to planning and improvement 1 Scotland s national Creative Learning Plan 272 supports the development of creativity skills in all learners and of educators expertise in developing creativity skills A range of resources have been created to support and assess this 2 including a national review of creativity across learning by Her Majesty s Inspectorate for Education 269 Academic journals EditCreativity Research Journal International Journal of Creative Computing Journal of Creative Behavior Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts Thinking Skills and CreativitySee also Edit Philosophy portal Psychology portalAdaptive performance Brainstorming Computational creativity Confabulation neural networks Content creation Creativity techniques Daydreaming E scape Fantasy prone personality Genius Guided visualization Heroic theory of invention and scientific development History of the concept of creativity Innovation Invention such as artistic invention in the visual arts Lateral thinking Learned industriousness Multiple discovery Music therapy Musical improvisation Openness to experience Originality Why Man Creates film Notes Edit Anderson Neil Potocnik Kristina Zhou Jing July 2014 Innovation and Creativity in Organizations A State of the Science Review Prospective Commentary and Guiding Framework Journal of Management 40 5 1297 1333 doi 10 1177 0149206314527128 hdl 10454 16825 ISSN 0149 2063 S2CID 44041503 Zhou Jing Wang Xiaoye May Bavato Davide Tasselli Stefano Wu Junfeng July 2019 Understanding the Receiving Side of Creativity A Multidisciplinary Review and Implications for Management Research Journal of Management 45 6 2570 2595 doi 10 1177 0149206319827088 ISSN 0149 2063 S2CID 150033432 And eke Job saith that in hell is no order of rule And albeit that God hath created all things in right order and nothing without order but all things be ordered and numbered yet nevertheless they that be damned be not in order nor hold no order a b c d e Runco Mark A Albert Robert S 2010 Creativity Research In James C Kaufman Robert J Sternberg eds The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 73025 9 Mumford M D 2003 Where have we been where are we going Taking stock in creativity research Creativity Research Journal 15 2 3 107 120 doi 10 1080 10400419 2003 9651403 S2CID 218546467 Sternberg Robert J 2011 Creativity Cognitive Psychology 6 ed Cengage Learning p 479 ISBN 978 1 133 38701 5 Meusburger Peter 2009 Milieus of Creativity The Role of Places Environments and Spatial Contexts In Meusburger P Funke J Wunder E eds Milieus of Creativity An Interdisciplinary Approach to Spatiality of Creativity Springer ISBN 978 1 4020 9876 5 Torrance Paul Verbal Tests Forms A and B Figural Tests Forms A and B The Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Norms Technical Manual Research Edition Princeton New Jersey Personnel Press p 6 Amabile Teresa M Pratt Michael G 2016 The dynamic componential model of creativity and innovation in organizations Making progress making meaning Research in Organizational Behavior 36 157 183 doi 10 1016 j riob 2016 10 001 Oslo Manual 2018 Guidelines for Collecting Reporting and Using Data on Innovation 4th ed Paris Luxembourg OECD Eurostat 2018 p 44 Averill James R February 1999 Individual Differences in Emotional Creativity Structure and Correlates Journal of Personality 67 2 331 371 doi 10 1111 1467 6494 00058 ISSN 0022 3506 PMID 10202807 Ivcevic Zorana Brackett Marc A Mayer John D April 2007 Emotional Intelligence and Emotional Creativity Journal of Personality 75 2 199 236 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 2007 00437 x ISSN 0022 3506 PMID 17359237 Rhodes Mel 1961 An Analysis of Creativity The Phi Delta Kappan 42 7 305 310 JSTOR 20342603 Gruszka Aleksandra Tang Min 2017 The 4P s creativity model and its application in different fields In Tang Lisa Min Werner Christian eds Handbook of the management of creativity and innovation Theory and practice World Scientific Publishing Company pp 51 71 ISBN 9789813141896 Sternberg Robert J 2009 Jaime A Perkins Dan Moneypenny Wilson Co eds Cognitive Psychology CENGAGE Learning p 468 ISBN 978 0 495 50629 4 Glǎveanu Vlad Petre 2013 Rewriting the language of creativity The five A s framework Review of General Psychology 17 1 69 81 doi 10 1037 a0029528 S2CID 143404705 Mattson David Mathew Katie Katz Buonincontro Jen 2021 Media Analysis of News Articles During COVID 19 Renewal Continuity and Cultural Dimensions of Creative Action Frontiers in Psychology 11 601938 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2020 601938 PMC 7920979 PMID 33664688 Sun Jingyan Okada Takeshi 2021 The process of interactive role making in acting training Thinking Skills and Creativity 41 100860 doi 10 1016 j tsc 2021 100860 a b c Wladyslaw Tatarkiewicz A History of Six Ideas an Essay in Aesthetics p 244 Albert R S Runco M A 1999 A History of Research on Creativity In Sternberg R J ed Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press Plato The Republic Book X wikisource The Republic Book X Albert R S Runco M A 1999 A History of Research on Creativity In Sternberg R J ed Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press p 5 a b Niu Weihua Sternberg Robert J 2006 The Philosophical Roots of Western and Eastern Conceptions of Creativity PDF Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 26 1 2 18 38 doi 10 1037 h0091265 S2CID 143648016 Archived from the original PDF on 19 December 2011 Retrieved 23 October 2010 cf Michel Weber Creativity Efficacy and Vision Ethics and Psychology in an Open Universe in Michel Weber and Pierfrancesco Basile eds Subjectivity Process and Rationality Frankfurt Lancaster ontos verlag Process Thought XIV 2006 pp 263 281 a b c Dacey John 1999 Concepts of Creativity A history In Mark A Runco Steven R Pritzer eds Encyclopedia of Creativity Vol 1 Elsevier ISBN 978 0 12 227076 5 a b c Albert R S Runco M A 1999 A History of Research on Creativity In Sternberg R J ed Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press p 6 Humanism Rome Reborn The Vatican Library amp Renaissance Culture Exhibitions Library of Congress www loc gov 1993 01 08 Retrieved 2015 11 23 Leonardo da Vinci Italian artist engineer and scientist Encyclopaedia Britannica Retrieved 2015 11 23 Tatarkiewicz Wladyslaw 1980 A History of Six Ideas an Essay in Aesthetics Translated from the Polish by Christopher Kasparek The Hague Martinus Nijhoff Wallas G 1926 Art of Thought Simonton D K 1999 Origins of genius Darwinian perspectives on creativity Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 512879 6 Whitehead Alfred North 1978 Process and reality an essay in cosmology Gifford Lectures delivered in the University of Edinburgh during the session 1927 28 Corrected ed New York Free Press ISBN 978 0 02 934580 1 Meyer Steven 2005 Introduction Whitehead Now Configurations 1 13 1 33 doi 10 1353 con 2007 0010 Cf Michel Weber and Will Desmond eds Handbook of Whiteheadian Process Thought Frankfurt Lancaster Ontos Verlag Process Thought X1 amp X2 2008 and Ronny Desmet amp Michel Weber edited by Whitehead The Algebra of Metaphysics Applied Process Metaphysics Summer Institute Memorandum Louvain la Neuve Les Editions Chromatika 2010 Hargreaves H L 1927 The faculty of imagination An enquiry concerning the existence of a general faculty or group factor of imagination British Journal of Psychology Monograph Supplement 3 1 74 a b Sternberg R J Lubart T I 1999 The Concept of Creativity Prospects and Paradigms In Sternberg R J ed Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 57285 9 a b Kozbelt Aaron Beghetto Ronald A Runco Mark A 2010 Theories of Creativity In James C Kaufman Robert J Sternberg eds The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 73025 9 Kaufman James C Beghetto Ronald A 2009 Beyond Big and Little The Four C Model of Creativity Review of General Psychology 13 1 1 12 doi 10 1037 a0013688 S2CID 41410038 Boden Margaret 2004 The Creative Mind Myths And Mechanisms Routledge ISBN 978 0 297 82069 7 Robinson Ken 1998 All our futures Creativity culture education PDF National Advisory Committee on Creative and Cultural Education Archived from the original PDF on 16 October 2014 Retrieved 2 October 2010 a b Craft Anna 2001 Little C creativity In Craft A Jeffrey B Leibling M eds Creativity in education Continuum International ISBN 978 0 8264 4863 7 Csikszentmihalyi Mihaly 1996 Creativity Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention Harper Collins ISBN 978 0 06 092820 9 Simonton D K 1997 Creative Productivity A Predictive and Explanatory Model of Career Trajectories and Landmarks Psychological Review 104 1 66 89 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 104 1 66 S2CID 13547975 Smith S M 2011 Incubation In M A Runco S R Pritzker eds Encyclopedia of Creativity Volume I 2nd ed Academic Press pp 653 657 ISBN 978 0 12 375039 6 Anderson J R 2000 Cognitive psychology and its implications Worth Publishers ISBN 978 0 7167 1686 0 Csikszentmihalyi Mihaly 1996 Creativity Flow and the Psychology of Discovery and Invention New York Harper Perennial ISBN 0 06 092820 4 Ward T 2003 Creativity In Nagel L ed Encyclopaedia of Cognition New York Macmillan Smith Steven M 1995 Fixation Incubation and Insight in Memory and Creative Thinking In Steven M Smith Thomas B Ward Ronald A Finke eds The Creative Cognition Approach MIT Press a b Guilford J P 1967 The Nature of Human Intelligence Skalski J 2021 10 25 Adult Development Creativity Instruction of creativity and related components PSYCH 322 Adult Development Brigham Young University Idaho The Relationship Between Individual Creativity Andcollective Intelligence in Modern Chinese Society Knowledge Cultures 7 2 35 2019 doi 10 22381 kc7220197 ISSN 2327 5731 Finke R Ward T B Smith S M 1992 Creative cognition Theory research and applications MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 06150 6 Ward T B 1995 What s old about new ideas In S M Smith T B Ward amp R A amp Finke Eds The creative cognition approach 157 178 London MIT Press Weisberg R W 1993 Creativity Beyond the myth of genius Freeman ISBN 978 0 7167 2119 2 Helie S Sun R 2010 Incubation insight and creative problem solving A unified theory and a connectionist model Psychological Review 117 3 994 1024 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 405 2245 doi 10 1037 a0019532 PMID 20658861 Koestler A 1964 The Act of Creation London Pan Books ISBN 978 0 330 73116 4 Gabora L amp Saab A 2011 Creative interference and states of potentiality in analogy problem solving Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society July 20 23 2011 Boston MA Roese N J amp Olson J M 1995 What Might Have Been The Social Psychology of Counterfactual Thinking Mahwah New Jersey Erlbaum Markman K Klein W amp Suhr E eds 2009 Handbook of mental simulation and the human imagination Hove Psychology Press Byrne R M J 2005 The Rational Imagination How People Create Counterfactual Alternatives to Reality MIT Press ISBN 9780262025843 Dervin Daniel 1990 Creativity and Culture A Psychoanalytic Study of the Creative Process in the Arts Sciences and Culture Fairleigh Dickinson University Press ISBN 978 0 8386 3366 3 Runco M amp Pritzker S Encyclopedia of Creativity Academic Press 2020 Holm Hadulla R M amp Wendt A N 2020 Dialectical Thinking In Encyclopedia of Creativity 3rd edition Runco M amp Pritzker S eds Academic Press Widiger T A Crego C 2019 The five factor model of personality structure World Psychiatry 18 3 p 271 272 Holm Hadulla R M Hofmann F H 2012 Counseling Psychotherapy and Creativity a b c Lin Hause Vartanian Oshin 2018 A Neuroeconomic Framework for Creative Cognition Perspectives on Psychological Science 13 6 655 677 doi 10 1177 1745691618794945 ISSN 1745 6916 PMID 30304640 S2CID 206778956 Beaty Roger E Benedek Mathias Silvia Paul J Schacter Daniel L 2016 Creative Cognition and Brain Network Dynamics Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20 2 87 95 doi 10 1016 j tics 2015 10 004 ISSN 1364 6613 PMC 4724474 PMID 26553223 Aston Jones Gary Cohen Jonathan D 2005 An integrative theory of locus coeruleus norepinephrine function Adaptive gain and optimal performance Annual Review of Neuroscience 28 1 403 450 doi 10 1146 annurev neuro 28 061604 135709 ISSN 0147 006X PMID 16022602 S2CID 535645 Epstein Robert November 1991 Skinner Creativity and the Problem of Spontaneous Behavior Psychological Science 2 6 362 370 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 1991 tb00168 x ISSN 0956 7976 S2CID 146725916 Abra Jock 1988 Skinner on Creativity A Critical Commentary Leonardo 21 4 407 412 doi 10 2307 1578703 JSTOR 1578703 S2CID 147669813 Sumner Sarah December 2021 How Can We Talk about Creativity The Psychological Record 71 4 503 507 doi 10 1007 s40732 021 00505 7 ISSN 0033 2933 S2CID 245350174 a b Sternberg Robert J Grigorenko Elena L 8 June 2010 Guilford s Structure of Intellect Model and Model of Creativity Contributions and Limitations Creativity Research Journal 13 3 4 309 316 doi 10 1207 s15326934crj1334 08 ISSN 1040 0419 S2CID 145796128 Guilford J P 1957 Creative abilities in the arts Psychological Review 64 2 110 118 doi 10 1037 h0048280 ISSN 1939 1471 PMID 13420286 Kim K H 2006 Can We Trust Creativity Tests A Review of the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking TTCT Creativity Research Journal 18 3 14 doi 10 1207 s15326934crj1801 2 S2CID 17636888 a b Zeng L Proctor R W Salvendy G 2011 Can Traditional Divergent Thinking Tests Be Trusted in Measuring and Predicting Real World Creativity Creativity Research Journal 23 24 37 doi 10 1080 10400419 2011 545713 S2CID 11322958 Torrance 1974 Kim Kyung Hee 2006 Can We Trust Creativity Tests PDF Creativity Research Journal 18 1 3 14 doi 10 1207 s15326934crj1801 2 S2CID 17636888 Forster E A amp Dunbar K N 2009 Creativity evaluation through latent semantic analysis In Proceedings of the 31st Annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society pp 602 607 Harbison I J amp Haarmann H 2014 Automated scoring of originality using semantic representations In Proceedings of the 36th Annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society poster paper Acar S Runco M A 2014 Assessing associative distance among ideas elicited by tests of Divergent Thinking Creativity Research Journal 26 2 229 238 doi 10 1080 10400419 2014 901095 S2CID 146788570 NSF SBIR Grant Number 1315053 Other members include Kenes Beketayev PhD Computer Science Liberty Lidz PhD Linguistics Perman Gochyyev PhD Statistics Beketayev K Runco M A 2016 Scoring Divergent Thinking Tests by Computer With a Semantics Based Algorithm Europe s Journal of Psychology 12 2 210 220 doi 10 5964 ejop v12i2 1127 PMC 4894287 PMID 27298632 a b Feist G J 1998 A meta analysis of the impact of personality on scientific and artistic creativity Personality and Social Psychology Review 2 4 290 309 doi 10 1207 s15327957pspr0204 5 PMID 15647135 S2CID 24067985 Batey M Furnham A 2006 Creativity intelligence and personality A critical review of the scattered literature Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs 132 4 355 429 doi 10 3200 mono 132 4 355 430 PMID 18341234 S2CID 7435403 Batey M Furnham A F Safiullina X 2010 Intelligence General Knowledge and Personality as Predictors of Creativity Learning and Individual Differences 20 5 532 535 doi 10 1016 j lindif 2010 04 008 Carson S H Peterson J B Higgins D M 2005 Reliability Validity and Factor Structure of the Creative Achievement Questionnaire Creativity Research Journal 17 1 37 50 doi 10 1207 s15326934crj1701 4 S2CID 146304521 a b Ying Yao Wang Chia Chi Ho Hsiao Chi Cheng Chih Ling Cheng 2014 Application of the Rasch Model to the Measurement of Creativity The Creative Achievement Questionnaire Creativity Research Journal 26 1 ISSN 1040 0419 Bendetowicz David Urbanski Marika Aichelburg Clarisse Levy Richard Volle Emmanuelle January 2017 Brain morphometry predicts individual creative potential and the ability to combine remote ideas PDF Cortex 86 216 229 doi 10 1016 j cortex 2016 10 021 ISSN 0010 9452 PMID 27919546 S2CID 13248682 a b Form Sven Schlichting Kerrin Kaernbach Christian November 2017 Mentoring functions Interpersonal tensions are associated with mentees creative achievement Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 11 4 440 450 doi 10 1037 aca0000103 ISSN 1931 390X S2CID 148927589 a b c Getzels J W amp Jackson P W 1962 Creativity and intelligence Explorations with gifted students New York Wiley a b c d Barron F 1963 Creativity and psychological health Princeton D Van Nostrand Company a b c Wallach M A amp Kogan N 1965 Modes of thinking in young children A study of the creativity intelligence distinction New York Holt Rinehart and Winston a b Guilford J P 1967 The nature of human intelligence New York McGraw Hill Plucker J amp Renzulli J S 1999 Psychometric approaches to the study of human creativity In R J Sternberg Ed Handbook of creativity pp 35 60 New York Cambridge University Press Hayes J R 1989 Cognitive processes in creativity In J A Glover R R Ronning amp C R Reynolds Eds Handbook of creativity pp 135 145 New York Plenum a b Sternberg R J 1996 Successful Intelligence New York Simon amp Schuster a b Sternberg R J amp O Hara L A 1999 Creativity and intelligence In R J Sternberg Ed Handbook of creativity pp 251 272 Cambridge MA Cambridge University Press Sternberg R J Kaufman J C amp Grigorenko E L 2008 Applied intelligence Cambridge Cambridge University Press Kaufman J C Kaufman S B Lichtenberger E O 2011 Finding creativity on intelligence tests via divergent production Canadian Journal of School Psychology 26 2 83 106 doi 10 1177 0829573511406511 S2CID 18061207 Silvia P J Beaty R E Nusbaum E C 2013 Verbal fluency and creativity General and specific contributions of broad retrieval ability Gr factors to divergent thinking Intelligence 41 5 328 340 doi 10 1016 j intell 2013 05 004 Martindale C 1999 Biological bases of creativity In R J Sternberg Ed Handbook of creativity pp 137 152 New York Cambridge University Press Kaufman J C Kaufman S B amp Plucker J A 2013 Contemporary theories of intelligence In J Reisberg Ed The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Psychology pp 811 822 New York NY Oxford University Press Sternberg R J Lubart T I 1991 An investment theory of creativity and its development Human Development 34 1 32 doi 10 1159 000277029 Sternberg R J Lubart T I 1992 Buy low and sell high An investment approach to creativity Current Directions in Psychological Science 1 1 1 5 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 1992 tb00002 x S2CID 143591670 Amabile T M 1982 Social psychology of creativity A consensual assessment technique Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43 5 997 1013 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 43 5 997 S2CID 144256250 Amabile Teresa M 1996 Creativity in Context Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity Routledge ISBN 9780813330341 Baer J Kaufman J C 2005 Bridging generality and specificity The Amusement Park Theoretical APT Model of creativity Roeper Review 27 3 158 163 doi 10 1080 02783190509554310 S2CID 33513570 Renzulli J S 1978 What makes giftedness Reexamining a definition Phi Delta Kappan 60 180 261 Naglieri J A Kaufman J C 2001 Understanding intelligence giftedness and creativity using PASS theory Roeper Review 23 3 151 156 doi 10 1080 02783190109554087 S2CID 144199243 Torrance E P 1962 Guiding creative talent Englewood Cliffs NJ Prentice Hall Barron F 1969 Creative person and creative process New York Holt Rinehart amp Winston Yamamoto K 1964 Creativity and sociometric choice among adolescents Journal of Social Psychology 64 2 249 261 doi 10 1080 00224545 1964 9919564 PMID 14238998 Fuchs Beauchamp K D Karnes M B Johnson L J 1993 Creativity and intelligence in preschoolers Gifted Child Quarterly 37 3 113 117 doi 10 1177 001698629303700303 S2CID 144005401 Cho S H Nijenhuis J T van Vianen N E M Kim H B Lee K H 2010 The relationship between diverse components of intelligence and creativity Journal of Creative Behavior 44 2 125 137 doi 10 1002 j 2162 6057 2010 tb01329 x Jauk E Benedek M Dunst B Neubauer A C 2013 The relationship between intelligence and creativity New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection Intelligence 41 4 212 221 doi 10 1016 j intell 2013 03 003 PMC 3682183 PMID 23825884 Wai J Lubinski D Benbow C P 2005 Creativity and occupational accomplishments among intellectually precocious youths An age 13 to age 33 longitudinal study Journal of Educational Psychology 97 3 484 492 doi 10 1037 0022 0663 97 3 484 S2CID 17610985 Kim K H 2005 Can only intelligent people be creative Journal of Secondary Gifted Education 16 2 3 57 66 doi 10 4219 jsge 2005 473 S2CID 49475973 Preckel F Holling H Wiese M 2006 Relationship of intelligence and creativity in gifted and non gifted students An investigation of threshold theory Personality and Individual Differences 40 159 170 doi 10 1016 j paid 2005 06 022 Perkins D N 1981 The mind s best work Cambridge MA Harvard University Press Weisberg R W Alba J W 1981 An examination of the alleged role of fixation in the solution of several insight problems Journal of Experimental Psychology General 110 2 169 192 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 110 2 169 McNemar O 1964 Lost Our Intelligence Why American Psychologist 19 12 871 882 doi 10 1037 h0042008 Kenneth M Heilman MD Stephen E Nadeau MD and David Q Beversdorf MD Creative Innovation Possible Brain Mechanisms Neurocase 2003 Archived 2009 03 19 at the Wayback Machine Flaherty AW 2005 Frontotemporal and dopaminergic control of idea generation and creative drive J Comp Neurol 493 1 147 53 doi 10 1002 cne 20768 PMC 2571074 PMID 16254989 Mayseless Naama Eran Ayelet Shamay Tsoory Simone G 2015 Generating original ideas The neural underpinning of originality NeuroImage 116 232 9 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2015 05 030 PMID 26003860 S2CID 12973770 These results are in line with the dual model of creativity according to which original ideas are a product of the interaction between a system that generates ideas and a control system that evaluates these ideas Di Bernardi Luft C Zioga I Thompson NM Banissy MJ Bhattacharya J December 26 2018 Right temporal alpha oscillations as a neural mechanism for inhibiting obvious associations PNAS 115 52 E12144 E12152 Bibcode 2018PNAS 11512144L doi 10 1073 pnas 1811465115 PMC 6310824 PMID 30541890 Vandervert 2003a 2003b Vandervert Schimpf amp Liu 2007 Miyake amp Shah 1999 Schmahmann 1997 2004 Andersen Korbo amp Pakkenberg 1992 Miller amp Cohen 2001 Vandervert 2003a Jung Beeman Bowden Haberman Frymiare Arambel Liu Greenblatt Reber amp Kounios 2004 Imamizu Kuroda Miyauchi Yoshioka amp Kawato 2003 Schmahmann 2004 Vandervert in press a Vandervert 2011 in press b Vandervert amp Vandervert Weathers 2013 Brown J et al 2007 On Vandervert et al Working memory cerebellum and creativity Creat Res J 19 25 29 doi 10 1080 10400410709336875 S2CID 143457667 Abraham A 2007 Can a neural system geared to bring about rapid predictive and efficient function explain creativity Creat Res J 19 19 24 doi 10 1080 10400410709336874 S2CID 43976883 Wagner U Gais S Haider H Verleger R Born J 2004 Sleep inspires insight Nature 427 6972 352 5 Bibcode 2004Natur 427 352W doi 10 1038 nature02223 PMID 14737168 S2CID 4405704 a b c Cai D J Mednick S A Harrison E M Kanady J C Mednick S C 2009 REM not incubation improves creativity by priming associative networks Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106 25 10130 10134 Bibcode 2009PNAS 10610130C doi 10 1073 pnas 0900271106 PMC 2700890 PMID 19506253 Walker MP Liston C Hobson JA Stickgold R November 2002 Cognitive flexibility across the sleep wake cycle REM sleep enhancement of anagram problem solving Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 14 3 317 24 doi 10 1016 S0926 6410 02 00134 9 PMID 12421655 Hasselmo ME September 1999 Neuromodulation acetylcholine and memory consolidation Trends Cogn Sci Regul Ed 3 9 351 359 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 99 01365 0 PMID 10461198 S2CID 14725160 Winkielman P Knutson B 2007 Affective Influence on Judgments and Decisions Moving Towards Core Mechanisms Review of General Psychology 11 2 179 192 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 11 2 179 S2CID 15618397 Amabile T 2017 In pursuit of everyday creativity The Journal of Creative Behavior Mark A Davis January 2009 Understanding the relationship between mood and creativity A meta analysis Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 100 1 25 38 doi 10 1016 j obhdp 2008 04 001 Baas Matthijs De Dreu Carsten K W amp Nijstad Bernard A November 2008 A meta analysis of 25 years of mood creativity research Hedonic tone activation or regulatory focus PDF Psychological Bulletin 134 6 779 806 doi 10 1037 a0012815 PMID 18954157 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 07 18 Schmidhuber Jurgen 2006 Developmental Robotics Optimal Artificial Curiosity Creativity Music and the Fine Arts Connection Science 18 2 173 187 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 474 6919 doi 10 1080 09540090600768658 S2CID 2923356 Schmidhuber Jurgen 2010 Formal Theory of Creativity Fun and Intrinsic Motivation 1990 2010 IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development 2 3 230 247 doi 10 1109 tamd 2010 2056368 S2CID 234198 Video of Jurgen Schmidhuber s keynote at the 2011 Winter Intelligence Conference Oxford Universal AI and Theory of Fun and Creativity Youtube 2012 Video of Jurgen Schmidhuber s talk at the 2009 Singularity Summit NYC Compression Progress The Algorithmic Principle Behind Curiosity and Creativity Youtube 2010 Kurzweil AI Transcript of Jurgen Schmidhuber s TEDx talk 2012 When creative machines overtake man Schmidhuber J 1991 Curious model building control systems In Proc ICANN Singapore volume 2 pp 1458 1463 IEEE Schmidhuber J 2012 A Formal Theory of Creativity to Model the Creation of Art In McCormack Jon and M d Inverno eds Computers and Creativity Springer 2012 Schmidhuber J 2007 Simple Algorithmic Principles of Discovery Subjective Beauty Selective Attention Curiosity amp Creativity In V Corruble M Takeda E Suzuki eds Proc 10th Intl Conf on Discovery Science 2007 pp 26 38 LNAI 4755 Springer Rushton 1990 Folley Bradley S Park Sohee 2005 Verbal creativity and schizotypal personality in relation to prefrontal hemispheric laterality A behavioral and near infrared optical imaging study Schizophrenia Research 80 2 3 271 282 doi 10 1016 j schres 2005 06 016 PMC 2817946 PMID 16125369 Archived from the original on 2006 02 15 Retrieved 2006 02 19 Batey M Furnham A 2009 The relationship between creativity schizotypy and intelligence Individual Differences Research 7 p 272 284 Batey M Furnham A 2008 The relationship between measures of creativity and schizotypy Personality and Individual Differences 45 8 816 821 doi 10 1016 j paid 2008 08 014 Furnham A Batey M Anand K Manfield J 2008 Personality hypomania intelligence and creativity Personality and Individual Differences 44 5 1060 1069 doi 10 1016 j paid 2007 10 035 Kyaga S Lichtenstein P Boman M Hultman C Langstrom N Landen M 2011 Creativity and mental disorder Family study of 300 000 people with severe mental disorder The British Journal of Psychiatry 199 5 373 379 doi 10 1192 bjp bp 110 085316 PMID 21653945 a b Roberts Michelle Creativity closely entwined with mental illness https www bbc co uk news health 19959565 16 October 2012 Ludwig Arnold M 1995 The Price of Greatness Resolving the Creativity and Madness Controversy ISBN 978 0 89862 839 5 The science of creativity apa org Baas Matthijs De Dreu Carsten K W Nijstad Bernard A 2008 A meta analysis of 25 years of mood creativity research Hedonic tone activation or regulatory focus Psychological Bulletin 134 6 779 806 doi 10 1037 a0012815 ISSN 1939 1455 PMID 18954157 Flood Meredith Phillips Kenneth D 2007 Creativity in older adults a plethors of possibilities Issues in Mental Health Nursing 28 4 389 411 doi 10 1080 01612840701252956 PMID 17454290 S2CID 12380062 Retrieved 27 August 2019 Malchiodi Cathy 2014 Creative Arts Therapy and Expressive Arts Therapy Psychology Today a b Burton N 2012 Bipolar Disorder and Creativity Psychology Today Bipolar Disorder and Creativity Psychology Today Retrieved 2020 07 17 a b Shapiro Pamela J Weisberg Robert W 1999 Creativity and Bipolar Diathesis Common Behavioural and Cognitive Components Cognition amp Emotion 13 6 741 762 doi 10 1080 026999399379069 ISSN 0269 9931 Miller Natalie Perich Tania Meade Tanya 2019 Depression mania and self reported creativity in bipolar disorder Psychiatry Research 276 129 133 doi 10 1016 j psychres 2019 05 006 PMID 31078791 S2CID 145024133 McCraw Stacey Parker Gordon Fletcher Kathryn Friend Paul 2013 Self reported creativity in bipolar disorder prevalence types and associated outcomes in mania versus hypomania Journal of Affective Disorders 151 3 831 836 doi 10 1016 j jad 2013 07 016 ISSN 0165 0327 PMID 24084622 Dimkov Petar Radoev 2018 04 01 The Genius of Creativity and the Creativity of Genius The Neuro Dynamics of Creativity in Karl Jaspers and Sigmund Freud Journal of Genius and Eminence 3 Fall 2018 83 92 doi 10 18536 jge 2018 04 3 1 07 a b Kim Bin Na Kwon Seok Man 2017 The link between hypomania risk and creativity The role of heightened behavioral activation system BAS sensitivity Journal of Affective Disorders 215 9 14 doi 10 1016 j jad 2017 02 033 PMID 28288308 a b Weisberg Robert W 1994 Genius and Madness A Quasi Experimental Test of the Hypothesis That Manic Depression Increases Creativity Psychological Science 5 6 361 367 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 1994 tb00286 x ISSN 0956 7976 S2CID 146691937 DeGraff Jeff Lawrence Katherine A 2002 10 10 Creativity at Work Developing the Right Practices to Make Innovation Happen John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 0 7879 6653 9 Irwing Paul Batey Mark 2011 Me2 General Factor of Creativity Technical Manual Commissioned report Altrincham E Metrixx Nijstad B A De Dreu C K 2002 Creativity and Group Innovation Applied Psychology 51 3 400 406 doi 10 1111 1464 0597 00984 Guilford Joy Paul 1950 Creativity American Psychologist 5 9 444 454 doi 10 1037 h0063487 PMID 14771441 Torrance 1974 1984 Christiaans amp Venselaar 2007 Amabile 1996 Prabhu et al 2008 Feist 1998 1999 Prabhu et al 2008 Zhang amp Sternberg 2009 Campbell 1960 Gardner 1993a Policastro amp Gardner 1999 Sternberg amp Lubart 1991 1995 1996 Cropley David H Cropley Arthur J Kaufman James C et al eds 2010 The Dark Side of Creativity Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 13960 1 a b McLaren R B 1993 The dark side of creativity Creat Res J 6 1 2 137 144 doi 10 1080 10400419309534472 a b Hao N Tang M Yang J Wang Q Runco MA 2016 A New Tool to Measure Malevolent Creativity The Malevolent Creativity Behavior Scale Frontiers in Psychology 7 682 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2016 00682 PMC 4870273 PMID 27242596 Aggression A social psychological analysis Berkowitz Leonard New York NY US McGraw Hill Aggression A social psychological analysis 1962 xv 361 pp Harris D J Reiter Palmon R 2015 Fast and furious The influence of implicit aggression premeditation and provoking situations on malevolent creativity Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 9 1 54 64 doi 10 1037 a0038499 Sternberg RJ Introduction in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ 2006 eds The International Handbook of Creativity pp 1 9 Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 54731 8 Niu W 2006 Development of Creativity Research in Chinese Societies in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ eds The International Handbook of Creativity pp 386 387 Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 54731 8 Mpofu E et al 2006 African Perspectives on Creativity in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ eds The International Handbook of Creativity p 465 Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 54731 8 Mpofu E et al 2006 African Perspectives on Creativity in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ eds The International Handbook of Creativity p 458 Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 54731 8 Preiss DD and Strasser K 2006 Creativity in Latin America in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ eds The International Handbook of Creativity p 46 Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 54731 8 Smith GJW and Carlsson I 2006 Creativity under the Northern Lights in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ eds The International Handbook of Creativity p 202 Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 54731 8 Preiser S 2006 Creativity Research in German Speaking Countries in Kaufman JC and Sternberg RJ eds The International Handbook of Creativity p 175 Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 54731 8 a b Amabile T M 1998 How to kill creativity Harvard Business Review 76 5 76 87 186 PMID 10185433 Sullivan Ceri Harper Grame eds 2009 Authors at Work The Creative Environment DS Brewer The English Association ISBN 9781843841951 Nonaka 1991 Siltala R 2010 Innovativity and cooperative learning in business life and teaching University of Turku Leal S y Urrea J Ingenio y Pasion 2013 Lid Publishers Spanish and Forbes India Magazine http forbesindia com article ie new trends in innovation management 33905 1 ixzz2iiuuDxVq a b c d e Woodman R W Sawyer J E Griffin R W 1993 Toward a theory of organizational creativity Academy of Management Review 18 2 293 321 doi 10 5465 amr 1993 3997517 S2CID 15250032 a b c d Paulus P B Dzindolet M 2008 Social influence creativity and innovation Social Influence 3 4 228 247 doi 10 1080 15534510802341082 S2CID 143485863 a b c d e Salazar M R Lant T K Fiore S M Salas E 2012 Facilitating innovation in diverse science teams through integrative capacity Small Group Research 43 5 527 5 doi 10 1177 1046496412453622 S2CID 643746 a b c d e Harvey S 2014 Creative synthesis Exploring the process of extraordinary group creativity Academy of Management Review 39 3 324 343 doi 10 5465 amr 2012 0224 a b c Loo S 2017 Creative Working in the Knowledge Economy Abingdon Oxfordshire Routledge Ltd ISBN 9781138211391 ISBN 9781315453095 https www routledge com Creative Working in the Knowledge Economy Loo p book 9781138211391 Burton Jones Alan 1999 10 21 Knowledge Capitalism Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780198296225 001 0001 ISBN 978 0 19 829622 5 Drucker David Drucker Erika 1999 There s no place like home a Victorian song title Le Globe Revue genevoise de geographie 139 1 77 92 doi 10 3406 globe 1999 1410 ISSN 0398 3412 Cortada J 1998 Introducing the Knowledge Worker Rise of the Knowledge Worker Elsevier pp xiii xix doi 10 1016 b978 0 7506 7058 6 50004 1 ISBN 978 0 7506 7058 6 retrieved 2021 06 26 Stenzl Jurg 2001 Reich Willi Oxford Music Online Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 gmo 9781561592630 article 23092 Florida Richard 2003 10 27 Entrepreneurship Creativity and Regional Economic Growth The Emergence of Entrepreneurship Policy Cambridge University Press pp 39 58 doi 10 1017 cbo9780511610134 003 ISBN 978 0 521 82677 8 retrieved 2021 06 26 Farrell Lesley Fenwick Tara eds 2007 03 12 World Yearbook of Education 2007 Routledge doi 10 4324 9780203962664 ISBN 978 1 134 11806 9 Brown Phillip Lauder Hugh Ashton David 2010 12 03 The Global Auction Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199731688 001 0001 ISBN 978 0 19 973168 8 Davenport T H 2005 The coming commoditization of processes Harvard business review 83 6 100 108 Alvesson Mats 2004 Knowledge work and knowledge intensive firms OUP Oxford Arthur Michael B DeFillippi Robert J Lindsay Valerie J October 2008 On Being a Knowledge Worker Organizational Dynamics 37 4 365 377 doi 10 1016 j orgdyn 2008 07 005 ISSN 0090 2616 Orr Kevin Martin Nutley Sandra M Russell Shona Bain Rod Hacking Bonnie Moran Clare eds 22 March 2016 Knowledge and practice in business and organisations ISBN 978 1 317 37792 4 OCLC 945552692 o Riain Sean 2004 The Politics of High Tech Growth Developmental Network States in the Global Economy Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 511 49960 9 OCLC 776970799 Nerland Monika 2007 12 28 Knowledge Cultures and the Shaping of Work based Learning The Case of Computer Engineering Vocations and Learning 1 1 49 69 doi 10 1007 s12186 007 9002 x ISSN 1874 785X S2CID 61112900 Grabher Gernot November 2004 Temporary Architectures of Learning Knowledge Governance in Project Ecologies Organization Studies 25 9 1491 1514 doi 10 1177 0170840604047996 ISSN 0170 8406 S2CID 145269032 Lury Celia 2004 08 12 Brands doi 10 4324 9780203495025 ISBN 9781134529179 Reich Utz Peter 2001 Value Theory in the National Accounts National Accounts and Economic value London Palgrave Macmillan UK pp 142 154 doi 10 1057 9780230512900 7 ISBN 978 1 349 40765 1 retrieved 2021 06 30 Quah Danny 2002 24 7 Competitive Innovation LSE Economics Department Drucker Peter 1994 The Theory of the Business Nonaka Ikujirō 1995 The knowledge creating company how Japanese companies create the dynamics of innovation Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 509269 4 OCLC 782177702 von Hippel Eric 2005 Democratizing Innovation doi 10 7551 mitpress 2333 001 0001 hdl 1721 1 118153 ISBN 9780262285636 S2CID 243335219 Zuboff Shoshana 1988 In the age of the smart machine the future of work and power Basic Books ISBN 0 465 03212 5 OCLC 17480009 Lash Scott Urry John 2002 Economies of Signs and Space SAGE Publications Ltd doi 10 4135 9781446280539 ISBN 978 0 8039 8472 1 Bell Colin Newby Howard 2012 11 12 Sociology of Community doi 10 4324 9780203043110 ISBN 9781136272462 Castells Manuel 2000 Toward a Sociology of the Network Society Contemporary Sociology 29 5 693 699 doi 10 2307 2655234 ISSN 0094 3061 JSTOR 2655234 Knorr Cetina Karin Schatzki Theodore R von Savigny Eike eds 2005 06 20 The Practice Turn in Contemporary Theory Routledge doi 10 4324 9780203977453 ISBN 9781134586295 Sternberg Robert J Kaufman James C Pretz Jean E 2004 A Propulsion Model of Creative Leadership Creativity and Innovation Management 13 3 145 153 doi 10 1111 j 0963 1690 2004 00304 x ISSN 0963 1690 Gardner Howard 1988 Creativity An interdisciplinary perspective Creativity Research Journal 1 1 8 26 doi 10 1080 10400418809534284 ISSN 1040 0419 Li Jin Gardner Howard 1993 How Domains Constrain Creativity American Behavioral Scientist 37 1 94 101 doi 10 1177 0002764293037001010 ISSN 0002 7642 S2CID 143591939 Csikszentmihalyi Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi Isabella Selega eds 1988 08 26 Optimal Experience Cambridge University Press doi 10 1017 cbo9780511621956 ISBN 9780521342889 Harvey S 2013 A different perspective The multiple effects of deep level diversity on group creativity Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 49 5 822 832 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2013 04 004 a b Paletz S B Schunn C D 2010 A social cognitive framework of multidisciplinary team innovation Topics in Cognitive Science 2 1 73 95 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 611 2475 doi 10 1111 j 1756 8765 2009 01029 x PMID 25163622 Polzer J T Milton L P Swarm W B Jr 2002 Capitalizing on diversity Interpersonal congruence in small work groups Administrative Science Quarterly 47 2 296 324 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 198 3908 doi 10 2307 3094807 JSTOR 3094807 S2CID 152150563 Zeng A Fan Y Di Z Wang Y Havlin S 2021 Fresh teams are associated with original and multidisciplinary research Nature Human Behaviour 5 10 1314 1322 doi 10 1038 s41562 021 01084 x PMID 33820976 S2CID 233036821 Hargadon A B Bechky B A 2006 When collections of creatives become creative collectives A field study of problem solving at work Organization Science 17 4 484 500 doi 10 1287 orsc 1060 0200 S2CID 6580938 a b Amabile Teresa M Conti Regina Coon Heather Lazenby Jeffrey Herron Michael 1996 Assessing the Work Environment for Creativity Academy of Management Journal 39 5 1154 1184 doi 10 5465 256995 ISSN 0001 4273 S2CID 144812471 Ward T B 1994 Structured Imagination the Role of Category Structure in Exemplar Generation Cognitive Psychology 27 1 1 40 doi 10 1006 cogp 1994 1010 S2CID 54276064 Stokes Patricia D 2007 Using constraints to generate and sustain novelty Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts 1 2 107 113 doi 10 1037 1931 3896 1 2 107 ISSN 1931 390X Moreau C Page Dahl Darren W 2005 Designing the Solution The Impact of Constraints on Consumers Creativity Journal of Consumer Research 32 1 13 22 doi 10 1086 429597 ISSN 0093 5301 S2CID 2152095 Neren Uri 2011 01 14 The Number One Key to Innovation Scarcity Harvard Business Review ISSN 0017 8012 Retrieved 2019 03 26 Gibbert Michael Scranton Philip 2009 Constraints as sources of radical innovation Insights from jet propulsion development Management amp Organizational History 4 4 385 399 doi 10 1177 1744935909341781 ISSN 1744 9359 S2CID 144428010 a b c d Hoegl Martin Gibbert Michael Mazursky David 2008 Financial constraints in innovation projects When is less more Research Policy 37 8 1382 1391 doi 10 1016 j respol 2008 04 018 a b Weiss Matthias Hoegl Martin Gibbert Michael 2011 Making Virtue of Necessity The Role of Team Climate for Innovation in Resource Constrained Innovation Projects Journal of Product Innovation Management 28 s1 196 207 doi 10 1111 j 1540 5885 2011 00870 x Weiss Matthias Hoegl Martin Gibbert Michael 2017 How Does Material Resource Adequacy Affect Innovation Project Performance A Meta Analysis Journal of Product Innovation Management 34 6 842 863 doi 10 1111 jpim 12368 Chan Janet 201 Towards a sociology of creativity In Mann Leon Chan Janet eds Creativity and Innovation in Business and Beyond Social Science Perspectives and Policy Implications Routledge Reckwitz Andreas 2017 The Invention of Creativity Polity Press p vi Godart Frederic Seong Sorah Phillips Damon J 2020 07 30 The Sociology of Creativity Elements Structures and Audiences Annual Review of Sociology 46 1 489 510 doi 10 1146 annurev soc 121919 054833 ISSN 0360 0572 S2CID 218819502 Casey Emma O Brien Dave 2020 Sociology Sociology and the Cultural and Creative Industries Sociology 54 3 443 459 doi 10 1177 0038038520904712 S2CID 216202901 Rubenson Daniel L Runco Mark 1992 The psychoeconomic approach to creativity New Ideas in Psychology 10 2 131 147 doi 10 1016 0732 118X 92 90021 Q Diamond Arthur M 1992 Creativity and Interdisciplinarity A Response to Rubenson and Runco New Ideas in Psychology 10 2 157 160 doi 10 1016 0732 118X 92 90023 S Florida Richard 2002 The Rise of the Creative Class And How It s Transforming Work Leisure Community and Everyday Life Basic Books ISBN 978 0 465 02476 6 Creativity the Brain and Evolution Psychology Today a b c d e Nickerson R S 1999 Enhancing creativity In R J Sternberg ed Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press Chirumbolo Antonio Livi Stefano Mannetti Lucia Pierro Antonio Kruglanski Arie W 2004 Effects of need for closure on creativity in small group interactions European Journal of Personality 18 4 265 278 doi 10 1002 per 518 S2CID 144190667 Djikic Maja Oatley Keith Moldoveanu Mihnea C 2013 Opening the Closed Mind The Effect of Exposure toLiterature on the Need for Closure Creativity Research Journal 25 2 149 154 doi 10 1080 10400419 2013 783735 S2CID 143961189 a b c d Csikszentmihalyi Mihaly 1999 Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity In R J Sternberg ed Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press a b c d Robinson K Azzam A M 2009 Why creativity now Educational Leadership 67 1 22 26 Paris C Edwards N Sheffield E Mutinsky M Olexa T Reilly S amp Baer J 2006 How early school experiences impact creativity In J C Kaufman amp J Baer Eds Creativity and Reason in Cognitive Development pp 333 350 New York NY Cambridge University Press a b Byrge C Hanson S 2009 The creative platform A new paradigm for teaching creativity Problems of Education in the 21st Century 18 33 50 Csikszentmihalyi M 1993 Evolution and flow In M Csikszentmihalyi Ed The evolving self A psychology for the third millennium pp 175 206 New York Harper Perennial National Advisory Committee on Creative and Cultural Education 1998 All our futures Creativity culture and education UK NACCCE Torrance E Paul Ellis Paul 2002 The manifesto a guide to developing a creative career Westport Conn Ablex Pub ISBN 978 0313011863 OCLC 52769638 a b Creativity 3 18 curriculum review impact report Practice exemplars National Improvement Hub education gov scot Retrieved 2021 10 25 Creative Learning Networks Learning resources National Improvement Hub education gov scot Retrieved 2021 10 25 What are creativity skills Learning resources National Improvement Hub education gov scot Retrieved 2021 10 25 Scotland s Creative Learning Plan www creativescotland com Retrieved 2021 10 25 References EditFredrickson B L 2001 The role of positive emotions in positive psychology The broaden and build theory of positive emotions American Psychologist 56 3 218 26 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 56 3 218 PMC 3122271 PMID 11315248 Hadamard Jacques The Psychology of Invention in the Mathematical Field Dover 1954 ISBN 0 486 20107 4 Helmholtz H v L 1896 Vortrage und Reden 5th ed Friederich Vieweg und Sohn Isen A M Daubman K A Nowicki G P 1987 Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 6 1122 31 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 52 6 1122 PMID 3598858 S2CID 12776791 Jeffery G 2005 The Creative College building a successful learning culture in the arts Trentham Books Johnson D M 1972 Systematic introduction to the psychology of thinking Harper amp Row Jullien F 2004 In Praise of Blandness Proceeding from Chinese Thought and Aesthetics Translated by Paula M Varsano Zone Books U S ISBN 1 890951 41 2 Jung C G The Collected Works of C G Jung Volume 8 The Structure and Dynamics of the Psyche Princeton 1981 ISBN 0 691 09774 7 Kanigel Robert The Man Who Knew Infinity A Life of the Genius Ramanujan Washington Square Press 1992 ISBN 0 671 75061 5 Kraft U 2005 Unleashing Creativity Scientific American Mind April 16 23 doi 10 1038 scientificamericanmind0405 16 Kolp P Lamme A Regnard Fr 2009 Rens J M ed Musique et creativite Orphee Apprenti NS 1 9 119 D 2009 11848 5 Lehrer Jonah 2012 Imagine How Creativity Works McLaren R B 1999 Dark Side of Creativity In Runco M A Pritzker S R eds Encyclopedia of Creativity Academic Press McCrae R R 1987 Creativity Divergent Thinking and Openness to Experience Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 6 1258 1265 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 52 6 1258 Michalko M 1998 Cracking Creativity The Secrets of Creative Genius Berkeley Calif Ten Speed Press ISBN 978 0 89815 913 4 National Academy of Engineering 2005 Educating the engineer of 2020 adapting engineering education to the new century National Academies Press ISBN 978 0 309 09649 2 Nonaka I 1991 The Knowledge Creating Company Harvard Business Review 69 6 96 104 O Hara L A Sternberg R J 1999 Creativity and Intelligence In Sternberg R J ed Handbook of Creativity Cambridge University Press Pink D H 2005 A Whole New Mind Moving from the information age into the conceptual age Allen amp Unwin Poincare H 1952 1908 Mathematical creation In Ghiselin B ed The Creative Process A Symposium Mentor Rhodes M 1961 An analysis of creativity Phi Delta Kappan 42 305 311 Rushton J P 1990 Creativity intelligence and psychoticism Personality and Individual Differences 11 12 1291 1298 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 90 90156 L Runco M A 2004 Creativity Annual Review of Psychology 55 657 687 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 55 090902 141502 PMID 14744230 Sabaneev Leonid The Psychology of the Musico Creative Process Psyche Vol 9 July 1928 pp 37 54 Smith S M Blakenship S E 1 April 1991 Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving American Journal of Psychology 104 1 61 87 doi 10 2307 1422851 ISSN 0002 9556 JSTOR 1422851 PMID 2058758 S2CID 10359632 Taylor C W 1988 Various approaches to and definitions of creativity In Sternberg R J ed The nature of creativity Contemporary psychological perspectives Cambridge University Press Torrance E P 1974 Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking Personnel Press von Franz Marie Louise Psyche and Matter Shambhala 1992 ISBN 0 87773 902 1 Andersen B Korbo L Pakkenberg B 1992 A quantitative study of the human cerebellum with unbiased stereological techniques The Journal of Comparative Neurology 326 4 549 560 doi 10 1002 cne 903260405 PMID 1484123 S2CID 11492983 Imamizu H Kuroda T Miyauchi S Yoshioka T Kawato M 2003 Modular organization of internal models of tools in the cerebellum Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100 9 5461 5466 doi 10 1073 pnas 0835746100 PMC 154367 PMID 12704240 Jung Beeman M Bowden E Haberman J Frymiare J Arambel Liu S Greenblatt R Reber P Kounios J 2004 Neural activity when people solve verbal problems with insight PLOS Biology 2 4 500 510 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0020097 PMC 387268 PMID 15094802 Miller E Cohen J 2001 An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function Annual Review of Neuroscience 24 167 202 doi 10 1146 annurev neuro 24 1 167 PMID 11283309 S2CID 7301474 Miyake A amp Shah P Eds 1999 Models of working memory Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control New York Cambridge University Press Schmahmann J Ed 1997 The cerebellum and cognition New York Academic Press Schmahmann J 2004 Disorders of the cerebellum Ataxia dysmetria of thought and the cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 16 3 367 378 doi 10 1176 jnp 16 3 367 PMID 15377747 Tatarkiewicz Wladyslaw A History of Six Ideas an Essay in Aesthetics translated from the Polish by Christopher Kasparek The Hague Martinus Nijhoff 1980 Vandervert L 2003a How working memory and cognitive modeling functions of the cerebellum contribute to discoveries in mathematics New Ideas in Psychology 21 2 159 175 doi 10 1016 s0732 118x 03 00012 6 Vandervert L 2003b The neurophysiological basis of innovation In L V Shavinina Ed The international handbook on innovation pp 17 30 Oxford England Elsevier Science Vandervert L 2011 The evolution of language The cerebro cerebellar blending of visual spatial working memory with vocalizations The Journal of Mind and Behavior 32 317 334 Vandervert L in press How the blending of cerebellar internal models can explain the evolution of thought and language Cerebellum Vandervert L Schimpf P Liu H 2007 How working memory and the cerebellum collaborate to produce creativity and innovation Special Issue Creativity Research Journal 19 1 1 19 doi 10 1080 10400410709336873 S2CID 15247122 Vandervert L amp Vandervert Weathers K in press New brain imaging studies indicate how prototyping is related to entrepreneurial giftedness and innovation education in children In L Shavinina Ed The International Handbook of Innovation Education London Routlage DeGraff J Lawrence K 2002 Creativity at Work Jossey Bass ISBN 978 0 7879 5725 4 Gielen P 2013 Creativity and other Fundamentalisms Mondriaan Amsterdam Further reading EditKaufman James C Sternberg Robert J eds 2019 The Cambridge Handbook of Creativity Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology 2nd ed Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 1316638545 Glǎveanu Vlad Petre ed 2019 The Creativity Reader Oxford University Press ISBN 9780190841713 Craft A 2005 Creativity in Schools tensions and dilemmas Routledge ISBN 978 0 415 32414 4 Boden Margaret 2004 The Creative Mind Myths And Mechanisms Routledge ISBN 978 0 297 82069 7 Amabile Teresa M 1996 Creativity in Context Update to the Social Psychology of Creativity Routledge ISBN 9780813330341 J P Guilford s 1950 inaugural speech to the American Psychological Association reproduced in Guilford Joy Paul 1950 Creativity American Psychologist 5 9 444 454 doi 10 1037 h0063487 PMID 14771441 Wallas Graham 1926 The Art of Thought Jonathan Cape Wikiquote has quotations related to Creativity Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Creativity amp oldid 1149130676, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.