fbpx
Wikipedia

Big Five personality traits

The Big Five personality traits is a suggested taxonomy, or grouping, for personality traits,[1] developed from the 1980s onward in psychological trait theory.

Starting in the 1990s, the theory identified five factors by labels, for the US English speaking population, typically referred to as:

When factor analysis (a statistical technique) is applied to personality survey data, it reveals semantic associations: some words used to describe aspects of personality are often applied to the same person. For example, someone described as conscientious is more likely to be described as "always prepared" rather than "messy". These associations suggest five broad dimensions used in common language to describe the human personality, temperament and psyche.[3][4]

Those labels for the five factors may be remembered using the acronyms "OCEAN" or "CANOE". Beneath each proposed global factor, there are a number of correlated and more specific primary factors. For example, extraversion is typically associated with qualities such as gregariousness, assertiveness, excitement-seeking, warmth, activity, and positive emotions.[5] These traits are not black and white, but rather placed on continua.[6]

Development

The Big Five personality traits was the model to comprehend the relationship between personality and academic behaviors.[7] This model was defined by several independent sets of researchers who used factor analysis of verbal descriptors of human behavior.[8] These researchers began by studying relationships between a large number of verbal descriptors related to personality traits. They reduced the lists of these descriptors by 5–10 fold and then used factor analysis to group the remaining traits (using data mostly based upon people's estimations, in self-report questionnaire and peer ratings) in order to find the underlying factors of personality.[9][10][11][12][13]

The initial model was advanced by Ernest Tupes and Raymond Christal in 1961,[12] but failed to reach an academic audience until the 1980s. In 1990, J.M. Digman advanced his five-factor model of personality, which Lewis Goldberg extended to the highest level of organization.[14] These five overarching domains have been found to contain and subsume most known personality traits and are assumed to represent the basic structure behind all personality traits.[15]

At least four sets of researchers have worked independently within lexical hypothesis in personality theory for decades on this problem and have identified generally the same five factors: Tupes and Christal were first, followed by Goldberg at the Oregon Research Institute,[16][17][18][19][20] Cattell at the University of Illinois,[11][21][22][23] and Costa and McCrae.[24][25][26][27] These four sets of researchers used somewhat different methods in finding the five traits, and thus each set of five factors has somewhat different names and definitions. However, all have been found to be highly inter-correlated and factor-analytically aligned.[28][29][30][31][32] Studies indicate that the Big Five traits are not nearly as powerful in predicting and explaining actual behavior as are the more numerous facets or primary traits.[33][34]

Each of the Big Five personality traits contains two separate, but correlated, aspects reflecting a level of personality below the broad domains but above the many facet scales that are also part of the Big Five.[35] The aspects are labeled as follows: Volatility and Withdrawal for Neuroticism; Enthusiasm and Assertiveness for Extraversion; Intellect and Openness for Openness to Experience; Industriousness and Orderliness for Conscientiousness; and Compassion and Politeness for Agreeableness.[35] People who do not exhibit a clear predisposition to a single factor in each dimension above are considered adaptable, moderate and reasonable, yet they can also be perceived as unprincipled, inscrutable and calculating.[36]

Descriptions of the particular personality traits

Openness to experience

Openness to experience is a general appreciation for art, emotion, adventure, unusual ideas, imagination, curiosity, and variety of experience. People who are open to experience are intellectually curious, open to emotion, sensitive to beauty and willing to try new things. They tend to be, when compared to closed people, more creative and more aware of their feelings. They are also more likely to hold unconventional beliefs. High openness can be perceived as unpredictability or lack of focus, and more likely to engage in risky behavior or drug-taking.[37] Moreover, individuals with high openness are said to pursue self-actualization specifically by seeking out intense, euphoric experiences. Conversely, those with low openness seek to gain fulfillment through perseverance and are characterized as pragmatic and data-driven – sometimes even perceived to be dogmatic and closed-minded. Some disagreement remains about how to interpret and contextualize the openness factor as there is a lack of biological support for this particular trait. Openness has not shown a significant association with any brain regions as opposed to the other four traits which did when using brain imaging to detect changes in volume associated with each trait.[38]

Sample items

  • I have a rich vocabulary.
  • I have a vivid imagination.
  • I have excellent ideas.
  • I am quick to understand things.
  • I use difficult words.
  • I spend time reflecting on things.
  • I am full of ideas.
  • I have difficulty understanding abstract ideas. (Reversed)
  • I am not interested in abstract ideas. (Reversed)
  • I do not have a good imagination. (Reversed)[39]

Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness is a tendency to display self-discipline, act dutifully, and strive for achievement against measures or outside expectations. It is related to the way in which people control, regulate, and direct their impulses. High conscientiousness is often perceived as being stubborn and focused. Low conscientiousness is associated with flexibility and spontaneity, but can also appear as sloppiness and lack of reliability.[36] High scores on conscientiousness indicate a preference for planned rather than spontaneous behavior.[40] The average level of conscientiousness rises among young adults and then declines among older adults.[41]

Sample items

  • I am always prepared.
  • I pay attention to details.
  • I get chores done right away.
  • I like order.
  • I follow a schedule.
  • I am exacting in my work.
  • I leave my belongings around. (Reversed)
  • I make a mess of things. (Reversed)
  • I often forget to put things back in their proper place. (Reversed)
  • I shirk my duties. (Reversed)[39]

Extraversion

Extraversion is characterized by breadth of activities (as opposed to depth), surgency from external activity/situations, and energy creation from external means.[42] The trait is marked by pronounced engagement with the external world. Extraverts enjoy interacting with people, and are often perceived as full of energy. They tend to be enthusiastic, action-oriented individuals. They possess high group visibility, like to talk, and assert themselves. Extraverted people may appear more dominant in social settings, as opposed to introverted people in this setting.[43]

Introverts have lower social engagement and energy levels than extraverts. They tend to seem quiet, low-key, deliberate, and less involved in the social world. Their lack of social involvement should not be interpreted as shyness or depression; instead they are more independent of their social world than extraverts. Introverts need less stimulation, and more time alone than extraverts. This does not mean that they are unfriendly or antisocial; rather, they are reserved in social situations.[1]

Generally, people are a combination of extraversion and introversion, with personality psychologist Hans Eysenck suggesting a model by which individual neurological differences produce these traits.[43]: 106 

Sample items

  • I am the life of the party.
  • I feel comfortable around people.
  • I start conversations.
  • I talk to a lot of different people at parties.
  • I do not mind being the center of attention.
  • I do not talk a lot. (Reversed)
  • I keep in the background. (Reversed)
  • I have little to say. (Reversed)
  • I do not like to draw attention to myself. (Reversed)
  • I am quiet around strangers. (Reversed)

[39]

Agreeableness

The agreeableness trait reflects individual differences in general concern for social harmony. Agreeable individuals value getting along with others. They are generally considerate, kind, generous, trusting and trustworthy, helpful, and willing to compromise their interests with others.[1] Agreeable people also have an optimistic view of human nature.

Disagreeable individuals place self-interest above getting along with others. They are generally unconcerned with others' well-being and are less likely to extend themselves for other people. Sometimes their skepticism about others' motives causes them to be suspicious, unfriendly, and uncooperative.[44] Low agreeableness personalities are often competitive or challenging people, which can be seen as argumentative or untrustworthy.[36]

Because agreeableness is a social trait, research has shown that one's agreeableness positively correlates with the quality of relationships with one's team members. Agreeableness also positively predicts transformational leadership skills. In a study conducted among 169 participants in leadership positions in a variety of professions, individuals were asked to take a personality test and have two evaluations completed by directly supervised subordinates. Leaders with high levels of agreeableness were more likely to be considered transformational rather than transactional. Although the relationship was not strong (r=0.32, β=0.28, p<0.01), it was the strongest of the Big Five traits. However, the same study showed no predictive power of leadership effectiveness as evaluated by the leader's direct supervisor.[45]

Conversely, agreeableness has been found to be negatively related to transactional leadership in the military. A study of Asian military units showed leaders with a high level of agreeableness to be more likely to receive a low rating for transformational leadership skills.[46] Therefore, with further research, organizations may be able to determine an individual's potential for performance based on their personality traits. For instance,[47] in their journal article "Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace?" Paul Sackett and Philip Walmsley claim that conscientiousness and agreeableness are "important to success across many different jobs."

Sample items

  • I am interested in people.
  • I sympathize with others' feelings.
  • I have a soft heart.
  • I take time out for others.
  • I feel others' emotions.
  • I make people feel at ease.
  • I am not really interested in others. (Reversed)
  • I insult people. (Reversed)
  • I am not interested in other people's problems. (Reversed)
  • I feel little concern for others. (Reversed)

[39]

Neuroticism

Neuroticism is the tendency to experience negative emotions, such as anger, anxiety, or depression.[48] It is sometimes called emotional instability, or is reversed and referred to as emotional stability. According to Hans Eysenck's (1967) theory of personality, neuroticism is interlinked with low tolerance for stress or aversive stimuli.[49] Neuroticism is a classic temperament trait that has been studied in temperament research for decades, before it was adapted by the Five Factors Model.[50] Those who score high in neuroticism are emotionally reactive and vulnerable to stress. They are more likely to interpret ordinary situations as threatening. They can perceive minor frustrations as hopelessly difficult. They also tend to be flippant in the way they express emotions. Their negative emotional reactions tend to persist for unusually long periods of time, which means they are often in a bad mood. For instance, neuroticism is connected to a pessimistic approach toward work, to certainty that work impedes personal relationships, and to higher levels of anxiety from the pressures at work.[51] Furthermore, those who score high on neuroticism may display more skin-conductance reactivity than those who score low on neuroticism.[49][52] These problems in emotional regulation can diminish the ability of a person scoring high on neuroticism to think clearly, make decisions, and cope effectively with stress. Lacking contentment in one's life achievements can correlate with high neuroticism scores and increase one's likelihood of falling into clinical depression. Moreover, individuals high in neuroticism tend to experience more negative life events,[48][53] but neuroticism also changes in response to positive and negative life experiences.[48][53] Also, individuals with higher levels of neuroticism tend to have worse psychological well-being.[54]

At the other end of the scale, individuals who score low in neuroticism are less easily upset and are less emotionally reactive. They tend to be calm, emotionally stable, and free from persistent negative feelings. Freedom from negative feelings does not mean that low scorers experience a lot of positive feelings.[55]

Neuroticism is similar but not identical to being neurotic in the Freudian sense (i.e., neurosis). Some psychologists[who?] prefer to call neuroticism by the term emotional instability to differentiate it from the term neurotic in a career test.

Sample items

  • I get stressed out easily.
  • I worry about things.
  • I am easily disturbed.
  • I get upset easily.
  • I change my mood a lot.
  • I have frequent mood swings.
  • I get irritated easily.
  • I often feel blue.
  • I am relaxed most of the time. (Reversed)
  • I seldom feel blue. (Reversed)

[39]

History

Early trait research

Historically preceding the Big Five personality traits (B5) or the Five Factors Model (FFM) was Hippocrates's four types of temperament: sanguine, phlegmatic, choleric, and melancholic. The sanguine type is most closely related to emotional stability and extraversion, the phlegmatic type is also stable but introverted, the choleric type is unstable and extraverted, and the melancholic type is unstable and introverted.[56]

In 1884, Sir Francis Galton was the first person who is known to have investigated the hypothesis that it is possible to derive a comprehensive taxonomy of human personality traits by sampling language: the lexical hypothesis.[9]

In 1936, Gordon Allport and S. Odbert put Sir Francis Galton's hypothesis into practice by extracting 4,504 adjectives which they believed were descriptive of observable and relatively permanent traits from the dictionaries at that time.[57] In 1940, Raymond Cattell retained the adjectives, and eliminated synonyms to reduce the total to 171.[11] He constructed a self-report instrument for the clusters of personality traits he found from the adjectives, which he called the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire.

In 1949, the first systematic multivariate research of personality was conducted by Joy P. Guilford. Guilford analyzed ten factors of personality, which he measured by the Guilford-Zimmerman Temperament Survey. These scales included general activity (energy vs inactivity); restraint (seriousness vs impulsiveness); ascendance (social boldness vs submissiveness); sociability (social interest vs shyness); emotional stability (evenness vs fluctuation of mood); objectivity (thick-skinned vs hypersensitive); friendliness (agreeableness vs belligerence); thoughtfulness (reflective vs disconnected), personal relations (tolerance vs hypercritical); masculinity (hard-boiled vs sympathetic).[56] These overlapping scales were later further analyzed by Guilford et al., and condensed into three dimensions: social activity (general activity, ascendence, sociability), introversion-extraversion (restraint, thoughtfulness), and emotional health (emotional stability, objectivity, friendliness, personal relations).[56]

Based on a subset of only 20 of the 36 dimensions that Cattell had originally discovered, Ernest Tupes and Raymond Christal (1961) claimed to have found just five broad factors which they labeled: "surgency", "agreeableness", "dependability", "emotional stability", and "culture".[12] Warren Norman subsequently relabeled "dependability" as "conscientiousness".[13]

Hiatus in research

During the late 1960s to 1970s, the changing zeitgeist made publication of personality research difficult. In his 1968 book Personality and Assessment, Walter Mischel asserted that personality instruments could not predict behavior with a correlation of more than 0.3. Social psychologists like Mischel argued that attitudes and behavior were not stable, but varied with the situation. Predicting behavior from personality instruments was claimed to be impossible.[by whom?]

Renewed attention

The paradigm shift back to acceptance of the five-factor model came in the early 1980s. In a 1980 symposium in Honolulu, four prominent researchers, Lewis Goldberg, Naomi Takemoto-Chock, Andrew Comrey, and John M. Digman, reviewed the available personality instruments of the day.[58] This event was followed by widespread acceptance of the five-factor model among personality researchers during the 1980s.

By 1983, experiments had demonstrated that the predictions of personality models correlated better with real-life behavior under stressful emotional conditions, as opposed to typical survey administration under neutral emotional conditions.[59]Peter Saville and his team included the five-factor "Pentagon" model with the original Occupational Personality Questionnaires (OPQ) in 1984. The Pentagon model was closely followed by the NEO PI-R (NEO) five-factor personality inventory, published by Costa and McCrae in 1985. However, the methodology employed in constructing the NEO instrument has been subject to critical scrutiny (see section below).[60]: 431–33 

Emerging methodologies increasingly confirmed personality theories during the 1980s. Though generally failing to predict single instances of behavior, researchers found that they could predict patterns of behavior by aggregating large numbers of observations.[61] As a result, correlations between personality and behavior increased substantially, and it was clear that "personality" did in fact exist.[62]

Personality and social psychologists now generally agree that both personal and situational variables are needed to account for human behavior.[63] Trait theories amassed favorable evidence, and there was a resurgence of interest in this area.[64] In the 1980s, Lewis Goldberg started his own lexical project, again emphasizing five broad factors[65] which he later labeled the "Big Five".

In 2007, Colin G. DeYoung, Lena C. Quilty and Jordan B. Peterson concluded that the 10 aspects of the Big Five may have distinct biological substrates.[66] This was derived through factor analyses of two data samples with the International Personality Item Pool (L. R. Goldberg, 1999),[67] followed by cross-correlation with scores derived from 10 genetic factors identified as underlying the shared variance among the Revised NEO Personality Inventory facets (K. L. Jang et al., 2002).[68]

Colin G. DeYoung et al. (2016) researched the Big Five model and how the five broad factors are compatible with the 25 scales of the Personality Inventory (PID-5) for the DSM-5. DeYoung et al. considers the PID-5 to measure facet-level traits.[69] Because the Big Five factors are broader than the 25 scales of the PID-5, there is disagreement in personality psychology relating to the number of factors within the Big Five. According to DeYoung et al. (2016), "the number of valid facets might be limited only by the number of traits that can be shown to have discriminant validity."[69]

The FFM-associated test was used by Cambridge Analytica, and was part of the "psychographic profiling"[70] controversy during the 2016 US presidential election.[71][72]

Biological and developmental factors

There of course are factors that influence a personality and these are called the determinants of personality. These factors determine the traits which a person develops in the course of development from a child.

Temperament vis-à-vis personality

There are debates between researchers of temperament and researchers of personality as to whether or not biologically based differences define a concept of temperament or a part of personality. The presence of such differences in pre-cultural individuals (such as animals or young infants) suggests that they belong to temperament since personality is a socio-cultural concept. For this reason developmental psychologists generally interpret individual differences in children as an expression of temperament rather than personality.[73] Some researchers argue that temperaments and personality traits are age-specific manifestations of virtually the same latent qualities.[74][75] Some believe that early childhood temperaments may become adolescent and adult personality traits as individuals' basic genetic characteristics actively, reactively, and passively interact with their changing environments.[73][74][76][clarification needed]

Researchers of adult temperament point out that, similarly to sex, age, and mental illness, temperament is based on biochemical systems whereas personality is a product of socialization of an individual possessing these four types of features. Temperament interacts with social-cultural factors, but still cannot be controlled or easily changed by these factors.[77][78][79][80] Therefore, it is suggested that temperament should be kept as an independent concept for further studies and not be conflated with personality.[81] Moreover, temperament refers to dynamical features of behavior (energetic, tempo, sensitivity and emotionality-related), whereas personality is to be considered a psycho-social construct comprising the content characteristics of human behavior (such as values, attitudes, habits, preferences, personal history, self-image).[78][79][80] Temperament researchers point out that the lack of attention to extant temperament research by the developers of the Big Five model led to an overlap between its dimensions and dimensions described in multiple temperament models much earlier. For example, neuroticism reflects the traditional temperament dimension of emotionality, extraversion the temperament dimension of "energy" or "activity", and openness to experience the temperament dimension of sensation-seeking.[80][82]

Heritability

 
Personality research often uses twin studies to determine how much heritable and environmental factors contribute to the Big Five personality traits.

A 1996 behavioural genetics study of twins suggested that heritability and environmental factors both influence all five factors to the same degree.[83] Among four twin studies examined in 2003, the mean percentage for heritability was calculated for each personality and it was concluded that heritability influenced the five factors broadly. The self-report measures were as follows: openness to experience was estimated to have a 57% genetic influence, extraversion 54%, conscientiousness 49%, neuroticism 48%, and agreeableness 42%.[84]

Non-humans

 
The Big 5 personality traits can be seen in chimpanzees.

The Big Five personality traits have been assessed in some non-human species but methodology is debatable. In one series of studies, human ratings of chimpanzees using the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire, revealed factors of extraversion, conscientiousness and agreeableness – as well as an additional factor of dominance – across hundreds of chimpanzees in zoological parks, a large naturalistic sanctuary, and a research laboratory. Neuroticism and openness factors were found in an original zoo sample, but were not replicated in a new zoo sample or in other settings (perhaps reflecting the design of the CPQ).[85] A study review found that markers for the three dimensions extraversion, neuroticism, and agreeableness were found most consistently across different species, followed by openness; only chimpanzees showed markers for conscientious behavior.[86]

A study completed in 2020 concluded that dolphins have some similar personality traits to humans. Both are large brained intelligent animals but have evolved separately for millions of years.[87]

Development during childhood and adolescence

Research on the Big Five, and personality in general, has focused primarily on individual differences in adulthood, rather than in childhood and adolescence, and often include temperament traits.[73][74][76] Recently, there has been growing recognition of the need to study child and adolescent personality trait development in order to understand how traits develop and change throughout the lifespan.[88]

Recent studies have begun to explore the developmental origins and trajectories of the Big Five among children and adolescents, especially those that relate to temperament.[73][74][76] Many researchers have sought to distinguish between personality and temperament.[89] Temperament often refers to early behavioral and affective characteristics that are thought to be driven primarily by genes.[89] Models of temperament often include four trait dimensions: surgency/sociability, negative emotionality, persistence/effortful control, and activity level.[89] Some of these differences in temperament are evident at, if not before, birth.[73][74] For example, both parents and researchers recognize that some newborn infants are peaceful and easily soothed while others are comparatively fussy and hard to calm.[74] Unlike temperament, however, many researchers view the development of personality as gradually occurring throughout childhood.[89] Contrary to some researchers who question whether children have stable personality traits, Big Five or otherwise,[90] most researchers contend that there are significant psychological differences between children that are associated with relatively stable, distinct, and salient behavior patterns.[73][74][76]

The structure, manifestations, and development of the Big Five in childhood and adolescence have been studied using a variety of methods, including parent- and teacher-ratings,[91][92][93] preadolescent and adolescent self- and peer-ratings,[94][95][96] and observations of parent-child interactions.[76] Results from these studies support the relative stability of personality traits across the human lifespan, at least from preschool age through adulthood.[74][76][97][98] More specifically, research suggests that four of the Big Five – namely Extraversion, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness – reliably describe personality differences in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.[74][76][97][98] However, some evidence suggests that Openness may not be a fundamental, stable part of childhood personality. Although some researchers have found that Openness in children and adolescents relates to attributes such as creativity, curiosity, imagination, and intellect,[99] many researchers have failed to find distinct individual differences in Openness in childhood and early adolescence.[74][76] Potentially, Openness may (a) manifest in unique, currently unknown ways in childhood or (b) may only manifest as children develop socially and cognitively.[74][76] Other studies have found evidence for all of the Big Five traits in childhood and adolescence as well as two other child-specific traits: Irritability and Activity.[100] Despite these specific differences, the majority of findings suggest that personality traits – particularly Extraversion, Neuroticism, Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness – are evident in childhood and adolescence and are associated with distinct social-emotional patterns of behavior that are largely consistent with adult manifestations of those same personality traits.[74][76][97][98] Some researchers have proposed the youth personality trait is best described by six trait dimensions: neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and activity.[101] Despite some preliminary evidence for this "Little Six" model,[89][101] research in this area has been delayed by a lack of available measures.

Previous research has found evidence that most adults become more agreeable, conscientious, and less neurotic as they age.[102] This has been referred to as the maturation effect.[75] Many researchers have sought to investigate how trends in adult personality development compare to trends in youth personality development.[101] Two main population-level indices have been important in this area of research: rank-order consistency and mean-level consistency. Rank-order consistency indicates the relative placement of individuals within a group.[103] Mean-level consistency indicates whether groups increase or decrease on certain traits throughout the lifetime.[102]

Findings from these studies indicate that, consistent with adult personality trends, youth personality becomes increasingly more stable in terms of rank-order throughout childhood.[101] Unlike adult personality research, which indicates that people become agreeable, conscientious, and emotionally stable with age,[102] some findings in youth personality research have indicated that mean levels of agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience decline from late childhood to late adolescence.[101] The disruption hypothesis, which proposes that biological, social, and psychological changes experienced during youth result in temporary dips in maturity, has been proposed to explain these findings.[89][101]

Extraversion/positive emotionality

In Big Five studies, extraversion has been associated with surgency.[73] Children with high Extraversion are energetic, talkative, social, and dominant with children and adults; whereas, children with low Extraversion tend to be quiet, calm, inhibited, and submissive to other children and adults.[74] Individual differences in Extraversion first manifest in infancy as varying levels of positive emotionality.[104] These differences in turn predict social and physical activity during later childhood and may represent, or be associated with, the behavioral activation system.[73][74] In children, Extraversion/Positive Emotionality includes four sub-traits: three traits that are similar to the previously described traits of temperament – activity, sociability, shyness,[105][50] and the trait of dominance.

  • Activity: Similarly to findings in temperament research, children with high activity tend to have high energy levels and more intense and frequent motor activity compared to their peers.[74][91][106] Salient differences in activity reliably manifest in infancy, persist through adolescence, and fade as motor activity decreases in adulthood[107] or potentially develops into talkativeness.[74][108]
  • Dominance: Children with high dominance tend to influence the behavior of others, particularly their peers, to obtain desirable rewards or outcomes.[74][109][110] Such children are generally skilled at organizing activities and games[111] and deceiving others by controlling their nonverbal behavior.[112]
  • Shyness: Children with high shyness are generally socially withdrawn, nervous, and inhibited around strangers.[74] In time, such children may become fearful even around "known others", especially if their peers reject them.[74][113] Similar pattern was described in temperament longitudinal studies of shyness[50]
  • Sociability: Children with high sociability generally prefer to be with others rather than alone.[74][114] During middle childhood, the distinction between low sociability and high shyness becomes more pronounced, particularly as children gain greater control over how and where they spend their time.[74][115][116]

Development throughout adulthood

Many studies of longitudinal data, which correlate people's test scores over time, and cross-sectional data, which compare personality levels across different age groups, show a high degree of stability in personality traits during adulthood, especially Neuroticism that is often regarded as a temperament trait [117] similarly to longitudinal research in temperament for the same traits.[50] It is shown that the personality stabilizes for working-age individuals within about four years after starting working. There is also little evidence that adverse life events can have any significant impact on the personality of individuals.[118] More recent research and meta-analyses of previous studies, however, indicate that change occurs in all five traits at various points in the lifespan. The new research shows evidence for a maturation effect. On average, levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness typically increase with time, whereas extraversion, neuroticism, and openness tend to decrease.[119] Research has also demonstrated that changes in Big Five personality traits depend on the individual's current stage of development. For example, levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness demonstrate a negative trend during childhood and early adolescence before trending upwards during late adolescence and into adulthood.[88] In addition to these group effects, there are individual differences: different people demonstrate unique patterns of change at all stages of life.[120]

In addition, some research (Fleeson, 2001) suggests that the Big Five should not be conceived of as dichotomies (such as extraversion vs. introversion) but as continua. Each individual has the capacity to move along each dimension as circumstances (social or temporal) change. He is or she is therefore not simply on one end of each trait dichotomy but is a blend of both, exhibiting some characteristics more often than others:[121]

Research regarding personality with growing age has suggested that as individuals enter their elder years (79–86), those with lower IQ see a raise in extraversion, but a decline in conscientiousness and physical well-being.[122]

Group differences

Gender differences

Some cross-cultural research has shown some patterns of gender differences on responses to the NEO-PI-R and the Big Five Inventory.[123][124] For example, women consistently report higher Neuroticism, Agreeableness, warmth (an extraversion facet) and openness to feelings, and men often report higher assertiveness (a facet of extraversion) and openness to ideas as assessed by the NEO-PI-R.[125]

A study of gender differences in 55 nations using the Big Five Inventory found that women tended to be somewhat higher than men in neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness. The difference in neuroticism was the most prominent and consistent, with significant differences found in 49 of the 55 nations surveyed.[126]

Gender differences in personality traits are largest in prosperous, healthy, and more gender-egalitarian nations. The explanation for this given by the researchers of a 2001 paper is that acts by women in individualistic, egalitarian countries are more likely to be attributed to their personality, rather than being attributed to ascribed gender roles within collectivist, traditional countries.[125]

Measured differences in the magnitude of sex differences between more or less developed world regions were caused by the changes in the measured personalities of men, not women, in these respective regions. That is, men in highly developed world regions were less neurotic, less extraverted, less conscientious and less agreeable compared to men in less developed world regions. Women, on the other hand tended not to differ in personality traits across regions.[126]

The authors of this 2008 study speculated that resource-poor environments (that is, countries with low levels of development) may inhibit the development of gender differences, whereas resource-rich environments facilitate them. This may be because males require more resources than females in order to reach their full personality potential of less conscientious, less agreeable, less neurotic, and less extraverted. The authors also speculated in their discussion that due to different evolutionary pressures, men may have evolved to be more risk taking and socially dominant, whereas women evolved to be more cautious and nurturing. The authors further posited that ancient hunter-gatherer societies may have been more egalitarian than later agriculturally oriented societies. Hence, the development of gender inequalities may have acted to constrain the development of gender differences in personality that originally evolved in hunter-gatherer societies. As modern societies have become more egalitarian, again, it may be that innate sex differences are no longer constrained and hence manifest more fully than in less-wealthy cultures.[126] This is one interpretation of the results among other possible interpretations.

Birth-order differences

Frank Sulloway argues that firstborns are more conscientious, more socially dominant, less agreeable, and less open to new ideas compared to siblings that were born later. Large-scale studies using random samples and self-report personality tests, however, have found milder effects than Sulloway claimed, or no significant effects of birth order on personality.[127][128] A study using the Project Talent data, which is a large-scale representative survey of American high school students, with 272,003 eligible participants, found statistically significant but very small effects (the average absolute correlation between birth order and personality was .02) of birth order on personality, such that firstborns were slightly more conscientious, dominant, and agreeable, while also being less neurotic and less sociable.[129] Parental socioeconomic status and participant gender had much larger correlations with personality.

In 2002, the Journal of Psychology posted a Big Five Personality Trait Difference; where researchers explored the relationship between the five-factor model and the Universal-Diverse Orientation (UDO) in counselor trainees. (Thompson, R., Brossart, D., and Mivielle, A., 2002). UDO is known as one social attitude that produces a strong awareness and/or acceptance towards the similarities and differences among individuals. (Miville, M., Romas, J., Johnson, J., and Lon, R. 2002) The study found that the counselor trainees that are more open to the idea of creative expression (a facet of Openness to Experience, Openness to Aesthetics) among individuals are more likely to work with a diverse group of clients, and feel comfortable in their role.[130]

Cultural differences

The Big Five have been pursued in a variety of languages and cultures, such as German,[131] Chinese,[132] and Indian.[133][134] For example, Thompson has claimed to find the Big Five structure across several cultures using an international English language scale.[135] Cheung, van de Vijver, and Leong (2011) suggest, however, that the Openness factor is particularly unsupported in Asian countries and that a different fifth factor is identified.[136]

Recent work has found relationships between Geert Hofstede's cultural factors, Individualism, Power Distance, Masculinity, and Uncertainty Avoidance, with the average Big Five scores in a country.[137] For instance, the degree to which a country values individualism correlates with its average extraversion, whereas people living in cultures which are accepting of large inequalities in their power structures tend to score somewhat higher on conscientiousness.[138][139]

Personality differences around the world might even have contributed to the emergence of different political systems. A recent study has found that countries' average personality trait levels are correlated with their political systems: countries with higher average trait Openness tended to have more democratic institutions, an association that held even after factoring out other relevant influences such as economic development.[140]

Attempts to replicate the Big Five in other countries with local dictionaries have succeeded in some countries but not in others. Apparently, for instance, Hungarians do not appear to have a single agreeableness factor.[141] Other researchers have found evidence for agreeableness but not for other factors.[142] It is important to recognize that individual differences in traits are relevant in a specific cultural context, and that the traits do not have their effects outside of that context.[43]: 189 

Health

Personality change from disease

Some diseases cause changes in personality. For example, although gradual memory impairment is the hallmark feature of Alzheimer's disease, a systematic review of personality changes in Alzheimer's disease by Robins Wahlin and Byrne, published in 2011, found systematic and consistent trait changes mapped to the Big Five. The largest change observed was a decrease in conscientiousness. The next most significant changes were an increase in Neuroticism and decrease in Extraversion, but Openness and Agreeableness were also decreased. These changes in personality could assist with early diagnosis.[143]

Personality disorders

As of 2002, there were over fifty published studies relating the FFM to personality disorders.[144] Since that time, quite a number of additional studies have expanded on this research base and provided further empirical support for understanding the DSM personality disorders in terms of the FFM domains.[145]

In her review of the personality disorder literature published in 2007, Lee Anna Clark asserted that "the five-factor model of personality is widely accepted as representing the higher-order structure of both normal and abnormal personality traits".[146] However, other researchers disagree that this model is widely accepted (see the section Critique below) and suggest that it simply replicates early temperament research.[80][147] Noticeably, FFM publications never compare their findings to temperament models even though temperament and mental disorders (especially personality disorders) are thought to be based on the same neurotransmitter imbalances, just to varying degrees.[80][148][149][150]

The five-factor model was claimed to significantly predict all ten personality disorder symptoms and outperform the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI) in the prediction of borderline, avoidant, and dependent personality disorder symptoms.[151] However, most predictions related to an increase in Neuroticism and a decrease in Agreeableness, and therefore did not differentiate between the disorders very well.[152]

Common mental disorders

 
Average deviation of five factor personality profile of heroin users from the population mean.[153] N stands for Neuroticism, E for Extraversion, O for Openness to experience, A for Agreeableness and C for Conscientiousness.

Converging evidence from several nationally representative studies has established three classes of mental disorders which are especially common in the general population: Depressive disorders (e.g., major depressive disorder (MDD), dysthymic disorder),[154] anxiety disorders (e.g., generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), panic disorder, agoraphobia, specific phobia, and social phobia),[154] and substance use disorders (SUDs).[155][156] The Five Factor personality profiles of users of different drugs may be different.[157] For example, the typical profile for heroin users is  , whereas for ecstasy users the high level of N is not expected but E is higher:  .[157]

These common mental disorders (CMDs) have been empirically linked to the Big Five personality traits, neuroticism in particular. Numerous studies have found that having high scores of neuroticism significantly increases one's risk for developing a common mental disorder.[158][159] A large-scale meta-analysis (n > 75,000) examining the relationship between all of the Big Five personality traits and common mental disorders found that low conscientiousness yielded consistently strong effects for each common mental disorder examined (i.e., MDD, dysthymic disorder, GAD, PTSD, panic disorder, agoraphobia, social phobia, specific phobia, and SUD).[160] This finding parallels research on physical health, which has established that conscientiousness is the strongest personality predictor of reduced mortality, and is highly negatively correlated with making poor health choices.[161][162] In regards to the other personality domains, the meta-analysis found that all common mental disorders examined were defined by high neuroticism, most exhibited low extraversion, only SUD was linked to agreeableness (negatively), and no disorders were associated with Openness.[160] A meta-analysis of 59 longitudinal studies showed that high neuroticism predicted the development of anxiety, depression, substance abuse, psychosis, schizophrenia, and non-specific mental distress, also after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history.[163]

The personality-psychopathology models

Five major models have been posed to explain the nature of the relationship between personality and mental illness. There is currently no single "best model", as each of them has received at least some empirical support. It is also important to note that these models are not mutually exclusive – more than one may be operating for a particular individual and various mental disorders may be explained by different models.[163][164]

  • The Vulnerability/Risk Model: According to this model, personality contributes to the onset or etiology of various common mental disorders. In other words, pre-existing personality traits either cause the development of CMDs directly or enhance the impact of causal risk factors.[160][165][166][167] There is strong support for neuroticism being a robust vulnerability factor.[163]
  • The Pathoplasty Model: This model proposes that premorbid personality traits impact the expression, course, severity, and/or treatment response of a mental disorder.[160][166][6] An example of this relationship would be a heightened likelihood of committing suicide in a depressed individual who also has low levels of constraint.[166]
  • The Common Cause Model: According to the common cause model, personality traits are predictive of CMDs because personality and psychopathology have shared genetic and environmental determinants which result in non-causal associations between the two constructs.[160][165]
  • The Spectrum Model: This model proposes that associations between personality and psychopathology are found because these two constructs both occupy a single domain or spectrum and psychopathology is simply a display of the extremes of normal personality function.[160][165][166][167] Support for this model is provided by an issue of criterion overlap. For instance, two of the primary facet scales of neuroticism in the NEO-PI-R are "depression" and "anxiety". Thus the fact that diagnostic criteria for depression, anxiety, and neuroticism assess the same content increases the correlations between these domains.[167]
  • The Scar Model: According to the scar model, episodes of a mental disorder 'scar' an individual's personality, changing it in significant ways from premorbid functioning.[160][165][166][167] An example of a scar effect would be a decrease in openness to experience following an episode of PTSD.[166]

Physical health

To examine how the Big Five personality traits are related to subjective health outcomes (positive and negative mood, physical symptoms, and general health concern) and objective health conditions (chronic illness, serious illness, and physical injuries), Jasna Hudek-Knezevic and Igor Kardum conducted a study from a sample of 822 healthy volunteers (438 women and 384 men).[168] Out of the Big Five personality traits, they found neuroticism most related to worse subjective health outcomes and optimistic control to better subjective health outcomes. When relating to objective health conditions, connections drawn were presented weak, except that neuroticism significantly predicted chronic illness, whereas optimistic control was more closely related to physical injuries caused by accident.[168]

Being highly conscientious may add as much as five years to one's life.[vague][162] The Big Five personality traits also predict positive health outcomes.[169][170] In an elderly Japanese sample, conscientiousness, extraversion, and openness were related to lower risk of mortality.[171]

Higher conscientiousness is associated with lower obesity risk. In already obese individuals, higher conscientiousness is associated with a higher likelihood of becoming non-obese over a five-year period.[172]

Effect of personality traits through life

Education

Academic achievement

Personality plays an important role in academic achievement. A study of 308 undergraduates who completed the Five Factor Inventory Processes and reported their GPA suggested that conscientiousness and agreeableness have a positive relationship with all types of learning styles (synthesis-analysis, methodical study, fact retention, and elaborative processing), whereas neuroticism shows an inverse relationship. Moreover, extraversion and openness were proportional to elaborative processing. The Big Five personality traits accounted for 14% of the variance in GPA, suggesting that personality traits make some contributions to academic performance. Furthermore, reflective learning styles (synthesis-analysis and elaborative processing) were able to mediate the relationship between openness and GPA. These results indicate that intellectual curiosity significantly enhances academic performance if students combine their scholarly interest with thoughtful information processing.[173]

A recent study of Israeli high-school students found that those in the gifted program systematically scored higher on openness and lower on neuroticism than those not in the gifted program. While not a measure of the Big Five, gifted students also reported less state anxiety than students not in the gifted program.[174] Specific Big Five personality traits predict learning styles in addition to academic success.

Studies conducted on college students have concluded that hope, which is linked to agreeableness,[175] conscientiousness, neuroticism, and openness,[175] has a positive effect on psychological well-being. Individuals high in neurotic tendencies are less likely to display hopeful tendencies and are negatively associated with well-being.[176] Personality can sometimes be flexible and measuring the big five personality for individuals as they enter certain stages of life may predict their educational identity. Recent studies have suggested the likelihood of an individual's personality affecting their educational identity.[177]

Learning styles

Learning styles have been described as "enduring ways of thinking and processing information".[173]

In 2008, the Association for Psychological Science (APS) commissioned a report that concludes that no significant evidence exists that learning-style assessments should be included in the education system.[178] Thus it is premature, at best, to conclude that the evidence links the Big Five to "learning styles", or "learning styles" to learning itself.

However, the APS report also suggested that all existing learning styles have not been exhausted and that there could exist learning styles worthy of being included in educational practices. There are studies that conclude that personality and thinking styles may be intertwined in ways that link thinking styles to the Big Five personality traits.[179] There is no general consensus on the number or specifications of particular learning styles, but there have been many different proposals.

As one example, Schmeck, Ribich, and Ramanaiah (1997) defined four types of learning styles:[180]

  • synthesis analysis
  • methodical study
  • fact retention
  • elaborative processing

When all four facets are implicated within the classroom, they will each likely improve academic achievement.[173] This model asserts that students develop either agentic/shallow processing or reflective/deep processing. Deep processors are more often found to be more conscientious, intellectually open, and extraverted than shallow processors. Deep processing is associated with appropriate study methods (methodical study) and a stronger ability to analyze information (synthesis analysis), whereas shallow processors prefer structured fact retention learning styles and are better suited for elaborative processing.[173] The main functions of these four specific learning styles are as follows:

Name Function
Synthesis analysis: processing information, forming categories, and organizing them into hierarchies. This is the only one of the learning styles that has explained a significant impact on academic performance.[173]
Methodical study: methodical behavior while completing academic assignments
Fact retention: focusing on the actual result instead of understanding the logic behind something
Elaborative processing: connecting and applying new ideas to existing knowledge

Openness has been linked to learning styles that often lead to academic success and higher grades like synthesis analysis and methodical study. Because conscientiousness and openness have been shown to predict all four learning styles, it suggests that individuals who possess characteristics like discipline, determination, and curiosity are more likely to engage in all of the above learning styles.[173]

According to the research carried out by Komarraju, Karau, Schmeck & Avdic (2011), conscientiousness and agreeableness are positively related with all four learning styles, whereas neuroticism was negatively related with those four. Furthermore, extraversion and openness were only positively related to elaborative processing, and openness itself correlated with higher academic achievement.[173]

In addition, a previous study by psychologist Mikael Jensen has shown relationships between the Big Five personality traits, learning, and academic achievement. According to Jensen, all personality traits, except neuroticism, are associated with learning goals and motivation. Openness and conscientiousness influence individuals to learn to a high degree unrecognized, while extraversion and agreeableness have similar effects.[181] Conscientiousness and neuroticism also influence individuals to perform well in front of others for a sense of credit and reward, while agreeableness forces individuals to avoid this strategy of learning.[181] Jensen's study concludes that individuals who score high on the agreeableness trait will likely learn just to perform well in front of others.[181]

Besides openness, all Big Five personality traits helped predict the educational identity of students. Based on these findings, scientists are beginning to see that the Big Five traits might have a large influence of on academic motivation that leads to predicting a student's academic performance.[177]

Some authors suggested that Big Five personality traits combined with learning styles can help predict some variations in the academic performance and the academic motivation of an individual which can then influence their academic achievements.[182] This may be seen because individual differences in personality represent stable approaches to information processing. For instance, conscientiousness has consistently emerged as a stable predictor of success in exam performance, largely because conscientious students experience fewer study delays.[177] Conscientiousness shows a positive association with the four learning styles because students with high levels of conscientiousness develop focused learning strategies and appear to be more disciplined and achievement-oriented.

Personality and learning styles are both likely to play significant roles in influencing academic achievement. College students (308 undergraduates) completed the Five Factor Inventory and the Inventory of Learning Processes and reported their grade point average. Two of the Big Five traits, conscientiousness and agreeableness, were positively related with all four learning styles (synthesis analysis, methodical study, fact retention, and elaborative processing), whereas neuroticism was negatively related with all four learning styles. In addition, extraversion and openness were positively related with elaborative processing. The Big Five together explained 14% of the variance in grade point average (GPA), and learning styles explained an additional 3%, suggesting that both personality traits and learning styles contribute to academic performance. Further, the relationship between openness and GPA was mediated by reflective learning styles (synthesis-analysis and elaborative processing). These latter results suggest that being intellectually curious fully enhances academic performance when students combine this scholarly interest with thoughtful information processing. Implications of these results are discussed in the context of teaching techniques and curriculum design.

— M Komarraju[173]

Distance Learning

When the relationship between the five-factor personality traits and academic achievement in distance education settings was examined in brief, the openness personality trait was found to be the most important variable that has a positive relationship with academic achievement in distance education environments. In addition, it was found that self-discipline, extraversion, and adaptability personality traits are generally in a positive relationship with academic achievement. The most important personality trait that has a negative relationship with academic achievement has emerged as neuroticism. The results generally show that individuals who are organized, planned, determined, who are oriented to new ideas and independent thinking have increased success in distance education environments. On the other hand, it can be said that individuals with anxiety and stress tendencies generally have lower academic success.[183][184][185]

Employment

Occupation and personality fit

 
The Vocations Map - many people in the same role share similar personality traits.

Researchers have long suggested that work is more likely to be fulfilling to the individual and beneficial to society when there is alignment between the person and their occupation.[186] For instance, software programmers and scientists were generally more open to experiencing a variety of new activities, were intellectually curious, tended to think in symbols and abstractions, and found repetition boring.[187]

Work success

 
Controversy exists as to whether or not the Big 5 personality traits are correlated with success in the workplace.

It is believed that the Big Five traits are predictors of future performance outcomes. Job outcome measures include job and training proficiency and personnel data.[188] However, research demonstrating such prediction has been criticized, in part because of the apparently low correlation coefficients characterizing the relationship between personality and job performance. In a 2007 article[189] co-authored by six current or former editors of psychological journals, Dr. Kevin Murphy, Professor of Psychology at Pennsylvania State University and Editor of the Journal of Applied Psychology (1996–2002), states:

The problem with personality tests is ... that the validity of personality measures as predictors of job performance is often disappointingly low. The argument for using personality tests to predict performance does not strike me as convincing in the first place.

Such criticisms were put forward by Walter Mischel,[190] whose publication caused a two-decades' long crisis in personality psychometrics. However, later work demonstrated (1) that the correlations obtained by psychometric personality researchers were actually very respectable by comparative standards,[191] and (2) that the economic value of even incremental increases in prediction accuracy was exceptionally large, given the vast difference in performance by those who occupy complex job positions.[192]

There have been studies that link national innovation to openness to experience and conscientiousness. Those who express these traits have showed leadership and beneficial ideas towards the country of origin.[193]

Some businesses, organizations, and interviewers assess individuals based on the Big Five personality traits. Research has suggested that individuals who are considered leaders typically exhibit lower amounts of neurotic traits, maintain higher levels of openness (envisioning success), balanced levels of conscientiousness (well-organized), and balanced levels of extraversion (outgoing, but not excessive).[194] Further studies have linked professional burnout to neuroticism, and extraversion to enduring positive work experience.[195] When it comes to making money, research has suggested that those who are high in agreeableness (especially men) are not as successful in accumulating income.[196]

Some research suggests that vocational outcomes are correlated to Big Five personality traits. Conscientiousness predicts job performance in general. Conscientiousness is considered as top-ranked in overall job performance,[47] research further categorized the Big 5 behaviors into 3 perspectives: task performance, organizational citizenship behavior, and counterproductive work behavior. Task performance is the set of activity that a worker is hired to complete, and results showed that Extraversion ranked second after the Conscientiousness, with Emotional Stability tied with Agreeableness ranked third. For organizational citizenship behavior, relatively less tied to the specific task core but benefits an organization by contributing to its social and psychological environment, Agreeableness and Emotional Stability ranked second and third. Lastly, Agreeableness tied with Conscientiousness as top ranked for Counterproductive work behavior, which refers to intentional behavior that is counter to the legitimate interests of the organization or its members.[47]

In addition, research has demonstrated that agreeableness is negatively related to salary. Those high in agreeableness make less, on average, than those low in the same trait. Neuroticism is also negatively related to salary while conscientiousness and extraversion are positive predictors of salary.[197] Occupational self-efficacy has also been shown to be positively correlated with conscientiousness and negatively correlated with neuroticism. Significant predictors of career-advancement goals are: extraversion, conscientiousness, and agreeableness.[197] Some research has also suggested that the Conscientiousness of a supervisor is positively associated with an employee's perception of abusive supervision.[198] While others have suggested that those with low agreeableness and high neuroticism are traits more related to abusive supervision.[199]

A 2019 study of Canadian adults found conscientiousness to be positively associated with wages, while agreeableness, extraversion, and neuroticism were negatively associated with wages. In the United States, by contrast, no negative correlation between extraversion and wages has been found. Also, the magnitudes found for agreeableness and conscientiousness in this study were higher for women than for men (i.e. there was a higher negative penalty for greater agreeableness in women, as well as a higher positive reward for greater conscientiousness).[200]

Research designed to investigate the individual effects of Big Five personality traits on work performance via worker completed surveys and supervisor ratings of work performance has implicated individual traits in several different work roles performances. A "work role" is defined as the responsibilities an individual has while they are working. Nine work roles have been identified, which can be classified in three broader categories: proficiency (the ability of a worker to effectively perform their work duties), adaptivity (a workers ability to change working strategies in response to changing work environments), and proactivity (extent to which a worker will spontaneously put forth effort to change the work environment). These three categories of behavior can then be directed towards three different levels: either the individual, team, or organizational level leading to the nine different work role performance possibilities.[201]

  • Openness is positively related to proactivity at the individual and the organizational levels and is negatively related to team and organizational proficiency. These effects were found to be completely independent of one another. This is also counter-conscientious and has a negative correlation to Conscientiousness.[202]
  • Agreeableness is negatively related to individual task proactivity. Typically this is associated with lower career success and being less able to cope with conflict.[202]
  • Extraversion is negatively related to individual task proficiency. Extraversion has a higher job and life satisfaction but more impulsive behaviors.[202]
  • Conscientiousness is positively related to all forms of work role performance. This has a higher leadership effectiveness and lower deviance behaviors but also lower learning in skill acquisition.[202]
  • Neuroticism is negatively related to all forms of work role performance. This has a trend to engage in more risky behaviors [202][201]

Two theories have been integrated in an attempt to account for these differences in work role performance. Trait activation theory posits that within a person trait levels predict future behavior, that trait levels differ between people, and that work-related cues activate traits which leads to work relevant behaviors. Role theory suggests that role senders provide cues to elicit desired behaviors. In this context, role senders (i.e.: supervisors, managers, etc.) provide workers with cues for expected behaviors, which in turn activates personality traits and work relevant behaviors. In essence, expectations of the role sender lead to different behavioral outcomes depending on the trait levels of individual workers and because people differ in trait levels, responses to these cues will not be universal.[201]

Romantic relationships

The Big Five model of personality was used for attempts to predict satisfaction in romantic relationships, relationship quality in dating, engaged, and married couples.[203]

Dating couples

  • Self-reported relationship quality is negatively related to partner-reported neuroticism and positively related to both self- and partner-reported conscientiousness[203]

Engaged couples

  • Self-reported relationship quality was higher among those high in partner-reported openness, agreeableness and conscientiousness.
  • Self-reported relationship quality was higher among those high in self-reported extraversion and agreeableness.
  • Self-reported relationship quality is negatively related to both self- and partner-reported neuroticism
  • Observers rated the relationship quality higher if the participating partner's self-reported extraversion was high[203]

Married couples

  • High self-reported neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness are related to high levels of self-reported relationship quality
  • Partner-reported agreeableness is related to observed relationship quality.[203]

These reports are, however, rare and not conclusive.

Political identification

The Big Five Personality Model also has applications in the study of political psychology. Studies have been finding links between the big five personality traits and political identification. It has been found by several studies that individuals who score high in Conscientiousness are more likely to possess a right-wing political identification.[204][205][206] On the opposite end of the spectrum, a strong correlation was identified between high scores in Openness to Experience and a left-leaning ideology.[204][207][208] While the traits of agreeableness, extraversion, and neuroticism have not been consistently linked to either conservative or liberal ideology, with studies producing mixed results, such traits are promising when analyzing the strength of an individual's party identification.[207][208] However, correlations between the Big Five and political beliefs, while present, tend to be small, with one study finding correlations ranged from 0.14 to 0.24.[209]

Scope of predictive power

The predictive effects of the Big Five personality traits relate mostly to social functioning and rules-driven behavior and are not very specific for prediction of particular aspects of behavior. For example, it was noted that high neuroticism precedes the development of all common mental disorders[163] and is not associated with personality by all temperament researchers.[81] Further evidence is required to fully uncover the nature and differences between personality traits, temperament and life outcomes. Social and contextual parameters also play a role in outcomes and the interaction between the two is not yet fully understood.[210]

Religiosity

Though the effect sizes are small: Of the Big Five personality traits high Agreeableness, Conscientiousness and Extraversion relate to general religiosity, while Openness relate negatively to religious fundamentalism and positively to spirituality. High Neuroticism may be related to extrinsic religiosity, whereas intrinsic religiosity and spirituality reflect Emotional Stability.[211]

Measurements

Several measures of the Big Five exist:

The most frequently used measures of the Big Five comprise either items that are self-descriptive sentences[142] or, in the case of lexical measures, items that are single adjectives.[214] Due to the length of sentence-based and some lexical measures, short forms have been developed and validated for use in applied research settings where questionnaire space and respondent time are limited, such as the 40-item balanced International English Big-Five Mini-Markers[135] or a very brief (10 item) measure of the Big Five domains.[217] Research has suggested that some methodologies in administering personality tests are inadequate in length and provide insufficient detail to truly evaluate personality. Usually, longer, more detailed questions will give a more accurate portrayal of personality.[218] The five factor structure has been replicated in peer reports.[219] However, many of the substantive findings rely on self-reports.

Much of the evidence on the measures of the Big 5 relies on self-report questionnaires, which makes self-report bias and falsification of responses difficult to deal with and account for.[215] It has been argued that the Big Five tests do not create an accurate personality profile because the responses given on these tests are not true in all cases and can be falsified.[220] For example, questionnaires are answered by potential employees who might choose answers that paint them in the best light.[221]

Research suggests that a relative-scored Big Five measure in which respondents had to make repeated choices between equally desirable personality descriptors may be a potential alternative to traditional Big Five measures in accurately assessing personality traits, especially when lying or biased responding is present.[216] When compared with a traditional Big Five measure for its ability to predict GPA and creative achievement under both normal and "fake good"-bias response conditions, the relative-scored measure significantly and consistently predicted these outcomes under both conditions; however, the Likert questionnaire lost its predictive ability in the faking condition. Thus, the relative-scored measure proved to be less affected by biased responding than the Likert measure of the Big Five.

Andrew H. Schwartz analyzed 700 million words, phrases, and topic instances collected from the Facebook messages of 75,000 volunteers, who also took standard personality tests, and found striking variations in language with personality, gender, and age.[222]

Critique

The proposed Big Five model has been subjected to considerable critical scrutiny in a number of published studies.[223][224][225][226][227][228][60][229][81] One prominent critic of the model has been Jack Block at the University of California, Berkeley. In response to Block, the model was defended in a paper published by Costa and McCrae.[230] This was followed by a number of published critical replies from Block.[231][232][233]

It has been argued that there are limitations to the scope of the Big Five model as an explanatory or predictive theory.[60][229] It has also been argued that measures of the Big Five account for only 56% of the normal personality trait sphere alone (not even considering the abnormal personality trait sphere).[60] Also, the static Big Five[234] is not theory driven, it is merely a statistically driven investigation of certain descriptors that tend to cluster together often based on less-than-optimal factor analytic procedures.[60]: 431–33 [81] Measures of the Big Five constructs appear to show some consistency in interviews, self-descriptions and observations, and this static five-factor structure seems to be found across a wide range of participants of different ages and cultures.[235] However, while genotypic temperament trait dimensions might appear across different cultures, the phenotypic expression of personality traits differs profoundly across different cultures as a function of the different socio-cultural conditioning and experiential learning that takes place within different cultural settings.[236]

Moreover, the fact that the Big Five model was based on lexical hypothesis (i.e. on the verbal descriptors of individual differences) indicated strong methodological flaws in this model, especially related to its main factors, Extraversion and Neuroticism. First, there is a natural pro-social bias of language in people's verbal evaluations. After all, language is an invention of group dynamics that was developed to facilitate socialization and the exchange of information and to synchronize group activity. This social function of language therefore creates a sociability bias in verbal descriptors of human behavior: there are more words related to social than physical or even mental aspects of behavior. The sheer number of such descriptors will cause them to group into the largest factor in any language, and such grouping has nothing to do with the way that core systems of individual differences are set up. Second, there is also a negativity bias in emotionality (i.e. most emotions have negative affectivity), and there are more words in language to describe negative rather than positive emotions. Such asymmetry in emotional valence creates another bias in language. Experiments using the lexical hypothesis approach indeed demonstrated that the use of lexical material skews the resulting dimensionality according to a sociability bias of language and a negativity bias of emotionality, grouping all evaluations around these two dimensions.[227] This means that the two largest dimensions in the Big Five model might be just an artifact of the lexical approach that this model employed.

Limited scope

One common criticism is that the Big Five does not explain all of human personality. Some psychologists have dissented from the model precisely because they feel it neglects other domains of personality, such as religiosity, manipulativeness/machiavellianism, honesty, sexiness/seductiveness, thriftiness, conservativeness, masculinity/femininity, snobbishness/egotism, sense of humour, and risk-taking/thrill-seeking.[228][237] Dan P. McAdams has called the Big Five a "psychology of the stranger", because they refer to traits that are relatively easy to observe in a stranger; other aspects of personality that are more privately held or more context-dependent are excluded from the Big Five.[238]

There may be debate as to what counts as personality and what does not and the nature of the questions in the survey greatly influence outcome. Multiple particularly broad question databases have failed to produce the Big Five as the top five traits.[239]

In many studies, the five factors are not fully orthogonal to one another; that is, the five factors are not independent.[240][241] Orthogonality is viewed as desirable by some researchers because it minimizes redundancy between the dimensions. This is particularly important when the goal of a study is to provide a comprehensive description of personality with as few variables as possible.

Methodological issues

Factor analysis, the statistical method used to identify the dimensional structure of observed variables, lacks a universally recognized basis for choosing among solutions with different numbers of factors.[242] A five factor solution depends on some degree of interpretation by the analyst. A larger number of factors may underlie these five factors. This has led to disputes about the "true" number of factors. Big Five proponents have responded that although other solutions may be viable in a single data set, only the five-factor structure consistently replicates across different studies.[243]

Surveys in studies are often online surveys of college students. Results do not always replicate when run on other populations or in other languages.[244]

Moreover, the factor analysis that this model is based on is a linear method incapable of capturing nonlinear, feedback and contingent relationships between core systems of individual differences.[227]

Theoretical status

A frequent criticism is that the Big Five is not based on any underlying theory; it is merely an empirical finding that certain descriptors cluster together under factor analysis.[242] Although this does not mean that these five factors do not exist, the underlying causes behind them are unknown.

Jack Block's final published work before his death in January 2010 drew together his lifetime perspective on the five-factor model.[245]

He summarized his critique of the model in terms of:

  • the atheoretical nature of the five-factors.
  • their "cloudy" measurement.
  • the model's inappropriateness for studying early childhood.
  • the use of factor analysis as the exclusive paradigm for conceptualizing personality.
  • the continuing non-consensual understandings of the five-factors.
  • the existence of unrecognized but successful efforts to specify aspects of character not subsumed by the five-factors.

He went on to suggest that repeatedly observed higher order factors hierarchically above the proclaimed Big Five personality traits may promise deeper biological understanding of the origins and implications of these superfactors.

Evidence for six factors rather than five

It has been noted that even though early lexical studies in the English language indicated five large groups of personality traits, more recent, and more comprehensive, cross-language studies have provided evidence for six large groups rather than five, [246] with the sixth factor being Honesty-Humility. These six groups form the basis of the HEXACO model of personality structure. Based on these findings it has been suggested that the Big Five system should be replaced by HEXACO, or revised to better align with lexical evidence.[247]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c Rothmann S, Coetzer EP (24 October 2003). "The big five personality dimensions and job performance". SA Journal of Industrial Psychology. 29. doi:10.4102/sajip.v29i1.88.
  2. ^ Roccas, Sonia; Sagiv, Lilach; Schwartz, Shalom H.; Knafo, Ariel (2002). "The Big Five Personality Factors and Personal Values". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28 (6): 789–801. doi:10.1177/0146167202289008. S2CID 144611052.
  3. ^ Goldberg LR (January 1993). "The structure of phenotypic personality traits". American Psychologist. 48 (1): 26–34. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.48.1.26. PMID 8427480.
  4. ^ Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO-PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) manual. Odessa, Florida: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  5. ^ Matthews G, Deary IJ, Whiteman MC (2003). (PDF) (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-83107-9. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-12-05.
  6. ^ a b De Bolle M, Beyers W, De Clercq B, De Fruyt F (November 2012). "General personality and psychopathology in referred and nonreferred children and adolescents: an investigation of continuity, pathoplasty, and complication models". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 121 (4): 958–70. doi:10.1037/a0027742. PMID 22448741. S2CID 33228527.
  7. ^ Poropat AE (March 2009). "A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic performance". Psychological Bulletin. 135 (2): 322–38. doi:10.1037/a0014996. hdl:10072/30324. PMID 19254083.
  8. ^ Digman JM (1990). "Personality structure: Emergence of the five-factor model". Annual Review of Psychology. 41: 417–40. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.002221.
  9. ^ a b Shrout PE, Fiske ST (1995). Personality research, methods, and theory. Psychology Press.
  10. ^ Allport GW, Odbert HS (1936). "Trait names: A psycholexical study". Psychological Monographs. 47: 211. doi:10.1037/h0093360.
  11. ^ a b c Bagby RM, Marshall MB, Georgiades S (February 2005). "Dimensional personality traits and the prediction of DSM-IV personality disorder symptom counts in a nonclinical sample". Journal of Personality Disorders. 19 (1): 53–67. doi:10.1521/pedi.19.1.53.62180. PMID 15899720.
  12. ^ a b c Tupes EC, Christal RE (1961). "Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings". USAF ASD Tech. Rep. 60 (61–97): 225–51. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00973.x. PMID 1635043.
  13. ^ a b Norman WT (June 1963). "Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes: replicated factors structure in peer nomination personality ratings". Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 66 (6): 574–83. doi:10.1037/h0040291. PMID 13938947.
  14. ^ Goldberg LR (January 1993). "The structure of phenotypic personality traits". The American Psychologist. 48 (1): 26–34. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.48.1.26. PMID 8427480.
  15. ^ O'Connor BP (June 2002). "A quantitative review of the comprehensiveness of the five-factor model in relation to popular personality inventories". Assessment. 9 (2): 188–203. doi:10.1177/1073191102092010. PMID 12066834. S2CID 145580837.
  16. ^ Goldberg LR (1982). "From Ace to Zombie: Some explorations in the language of personality". In Spielberger CD, Butcher JN (eds.). Advances in personality assessment. Vol. 1. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 201–34.
  17. ^ Norman WT, Goldberg LR (1966). "Raters, ratees, and randomness in personality structure". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 4 (6): 681–91. doi:10.1037/h0024002.
  18. ^ Peabody D, Goldberg LR (September 1989). "Some determinants of factor structures from personality-trait descriptors". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 57 (3): 552–67. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.3.552. PMID 2778639.
  19. ^ Saucier G, Goldberg LR (1996). "The language of personality: Lexical perspectives on the five-factor model". In Wiggins JS (ed.). The five-factor model of personality: Theoretical perspectives. New York: Guilford.[page needed]
  20. ^ Digman JM (June 1989). "Five robust trait dimensions: development, stability, and utility". Journal of Personality. 57 (2): 195–214. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00480.x. PMID 2671337.
  21. ^ Karson S, O'Dell JW (1976). A guide to the clinical use of the 16PF (Report). Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality & Ability Testing.
  22. ^ Krug SE, Johns EF (1986). "A large scale cross-validation of second-order personality structure defined by the 16PF". Psychological Reports. 59 (2): 683–93. doi:10.2466/pr0.1986.59.2.683. S2CID 145610003.
  23. ^ Cattell HE, Mead AD (2007). "The 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire (16PF)". In Boyle GJ, Matthews G, Saklofske DH (eds.). Handbook of personality theory and testing, Volume 2: Personality measurement and assessment. London: Sage.[page needed]
  24. ^ Costa PT, McCrae RR (September 1976). "Age differences in personality structure: a cluster analytic approach". Journal of Gerontology. 31 (5): 564–70. doi:10.1093/geronj/31.5.564. PMID 950450.
  25. ^ Costa PT, McCrae RR (1985). The NEO Personality Inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  26. ^ McCrae RR, Costa PT (January 1987). "Validation of the five-factor model of personality across instruments and observers". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52 (1): 81–90. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.81. PMID 3820081.
  27. ^ McCrae RR, John OP (June 1992). "An introduction to the five-factor model and its applications". Journal of Personality. 60 (2): 175–215. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.470.4858. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00970.x. PMID 1635039. S2CID 10596836.
  28. ^ "International Personality Item Pool (IPIP)". The Society for Judgment and Decision Making.
  29. ^ Goldberg LR, Johnson JA, Eber HW, Hogan R, Ashton MC, Cloninger CR, Gough HG (February 2006). "The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures". Journal of Research in Personality. 40 (1): 84–96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007.
  30. ^ Conn S, Rieke M (1994). The 16PF Fifth Edition technical manual. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality & Ability Testing.
  31. ^ Cattell HE (1996). "The original big five: A historical perspective". European Review of Applied Psychology. 46: 5–14.
  32. ^ Grucza RA, Goldberg LR (October 2007). "The comparative validity of 11 modern personality inventories: predictions of behavioral acts, informant reports, and clinical indicators". Journal of Personality Assessment. 89 (2): 167–87. doi:10.1080/00223890701468568. PMID 17764394. S2CID 42394327.
  33. ^ Mershon B, Gorsuch RL (1988). "Number of factors in the personality sphere: does increase in factors increase predictability of real-life criteria?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 55 (4): 675–80. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.675.
  34. ^ Paunonen SV, Ashton MS (2001). "Big Five factors and facets and the prediction of behavior". Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 81 (3): 524–39. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.3.524. PMID 11554651.
  35. ^ a b DeYoung CG, Quilty LC, Peterson JB (November 2007). "Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880–96. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID 17983306. S2CID 8261816.
  36. ^ a b c Toegel G, Barsoux JL (2012). "How to become a better leader". MIT Sloan Management Review. 53 (3): 51–60.
  37. ^ Ambridge B (2014). Psy-Q: You know your IQ – now test your psychological intelligence. Profile. p. 11. ISBN 978-1782830238 – via Google Books.
  38. ^ DeYoung, Colin G.; Hirsh, Jacob B.; Shane, Matthew S.; Papademetris, Xenophon; Rajeevan, Nallakkandi; Gray, Jeremy R. (2010). "Testing Predictions From Personality Neuroscience: Brain Structure and the Big Five". Psychological Science. 21 (6): 820–828. doi:10.1177/0956797610370159. ISSN 0956-7976. JSTOR 41062296. PMC 3049165. PMID 20435951.
  39. ^ a b c d e The 50-item IPIP representation of the Goldberg (1992) markers for the Big-Five structure at ipip.ori.org.
  40. ^ Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992). Neo PI-R professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.[page needed][ISBN missing]
  41. ^ "Research Reports on Science from Michigan State University Provide New Insights". Science Letter. Gale Student Resource in Context. Retrieved 4 April 2012.
  42. ^ Laney MO (2002). The Introvert Advantage. Canada: Thomas Allen & Son Limited. pp. 28, 35. ISBN 978-0761123699.
  43. ^ a b c Friedman H, Schustack M (2016). Personality: Classic Theories and Modern Research (Sixth ed.). Pearson Education Inc. ISBN 978-0205997930.[page needed]
  44. ^ Bartneck C, Van der Hoek M, Mubin O, Al Mahmud A (March 2007). ""Daisy, daisy, give me your answer do!" switching off a robot". Eindhoven, Netherlands: Dept. of Ind. Design, Eindhoven Univ. of Technol. pp. 217–22. Retrieved 6 February 2013.
  45. ^ Judge TA, Bono JE (October 2000). "Five-factor model of personality and transformational leadership". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 85 (5): 751–65. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.85.5.751. PMID 11055147.
  46. ^ Lim BC, Ployhart RE (August 2004). "Transformational leadership: relations to the five-factor model and team performance in typical and maximum contexts". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 89 (4): 610–21. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.89.4.610. PMID 15327348.
  47. ^ a b c Sackett PR, Walmsley PT (2014). "Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace?". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 9 (5): 538–51. doi:10.1177/1745691614543972. PMID 26186756. S2CID 21245818.
  48. ^ a b c Jeronimus BF, Riese H, Sanderman R, Ormel J (October 2014). "Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences: a five-wave, 16-year study to test reciprocal causation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 107 (4): 751–64. doi:10.1037/a0037009. PMID 25111305.
  49. ^ a b Norris CJ, Larsen JT, Cacioppo JT (September 2007). "Neuroticism is associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to emotionally evocative pictures" (PDF). Psychophysiology. 44 (5): 823–26. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00551.x. PMID 17596178.
  50. ^ a b c d Kagan J, Snidman N (2009). The Long Shadow of Temperament. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.[ISBN missing][page needed]
  51. ^ Fiske ST, Gilbert DT, Lindzey G (2009). Handbook of Social Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.[ISBN missing][page needed]
  52. ^ Reynaud E, El Khoury-Malhame M, Rossier J, Blin O, Khalfa S (2012). "Neuroticism modifies psycho physiological responses to fearful films". PLOS ONE. 7 (3): e32413. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...732413R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0032413. PMC 3316522. PMID 22479326.
  53. ^ a b Jeronimus BF, Ormel J, Aleman A, Penninx BW, Riese H (November 2013). "Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism". Psychological Medicine. 43 (11): 2403–15. doi:10.1017/s0033291713000159. PMID 23410535. S2CID 43717734.
  54. ^ Dwan T, Ownsworth T (2019). "The Big Five personality factors and psychological well-being following stroke: a systematic review". Disability and Rehabilitation. 41 (10): 1119–30. doi:10.1080/09638288.2017.1419382. PMID 29272953. S2CID 7300458.
  55. ^ Dolan SL (2006). Stress, Self-Esteem, Health and Work. p. 76.[ISBN missing]
  56. ^ a b c Musek, Janek (2017). The General Factor of Personality. London: Academic Press. pp. 14–25. ISBN 978-0-12-811249-6.
  57. ^ Allport GW, Odbert HS (1936). "Trait names: A psycholexical study". Psychological Monographs. 47: 211. doi:10.1037/h0093360.
  58. ^ Goldberg LR (May 1980). Some ruminations about the structure of individual differences: Developing a common lexicon for the major characteristics of human personality. Symposium presentation at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association (Report). Honolulu, HI.[page needed]
  59. ^ Boyle GJ (November 1983). "Effects on academic learning of manipulating emotional states and motivational dynamics". The British Journal of Educational Psychology. 53 (3): 347–57. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8279.1983.tb02567.x. PMID 6652035.
  60. ^ a b c d e Boyle GJ, Stankov L, Cattell RB (1995). "Measurement and statistical models in the study of personality and intelligence". In Saklofske DH, Zeidner M (eds.). International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence. pp. 417–46.[ISBN missing]
  61. ^ Epstein S, O'Brien EJ (November 1985). "The person-situation debate in historical and current perspective". Psychological Bulletin. 98 (3): 513–37. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.3.513. PMID 4080897.
  62. ^ Kenrick DT, Funder DC (January 1988). "Profiting from controversy. Lessons from the person-situation debate". The American Psychologist. 43 (1): 23–34. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.43.1.23. PMID 3279875.
  63. ^ Lucas RE, Donnellan MB (2009). "If the person-situation debate is really over, why does it still generate so much negative affect?". Journal of Research in Personality. 43 (3): 146–49. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.009.
  64. ^ Eysenck MW, Eysenck JS (1980). "Mischel and the concept of personality". British Journal of Psychology. 71 (2): 191–204. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1980.tb01737.x.
  65. ^ Goldberg LR (1981). "Language and individual differences: The search for universals in personality lexicons". In Wheeler (ed.). Review of Personality and social psychology. Vol. 1. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. pp. 141–65.[ISBN missing]
  66. ^ Deyoung, C. G.; Quilty, L. C.; Peterson, J. B. (2007). "Between Facets and Domains: 10 Aspects of the Big Five". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880–896. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.513.2517. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID 17983306. S2CID 8261816.
  67. ^ Goldberg, Lewis R.; Johnson, John A.; Eber, Herbert W.; Hogan, Robert; Ashton, Michael C.; Cloninger, C. Robert; Gough, Harrison G. (2006). "The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures" (PDF). Journal of Research in Personality. 40: 84–96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007 – via Elsevier.
  68. ^ "The revised NEO personality inventory (NEO-PI-R)". The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment. 2: 223–257 – via Researchgate.
  69. ^ a b DeYoung CG, Carey BE, Krueger RF, Ross SR (April 2016). "Ten aspects of the Big Five in the Personality Inventory for DSM-5". Personality Disorders. 7 (2): 113–23. doi:10.1037/per0000170. PMC 4818974. PMID 27032017.
  70. ^ Alexander, Nix (2017-03-03). "From Mad Men to Math Men. Verbatim report of the presentation of Alexander Nix, Cambridge Analytica CEO, at Online Marketing Rockstars Festival 2017". freud.online. Retrieved 2022-10-23.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: url-status (link)
  71. ^ . Cambridge Analytica. Archived from the original on 16 February 2016. Retrieved 27 December 2015.
  72. ^ Sellers FS (19 October 2015). "Cruz campaign paid $750,000 to 'psychographic profiling' company". The Washington Post. Retrieved 7 February 2016.
  73. ^ a b c d e f g h Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000). "Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 122–35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.122. PMID 10653510.
  74. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u Shiner R, Caspi A (January 2003). "Personality differences in childhood and adolescence: measurement, development, and consequences". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 44 (1): 2–32. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00101. PMID 12553411.
  75. ^ a b McCrae RR, Costa PT, Ostendorf F, Angleitner A, Hrebícková M, Avia MD, Sanz J, Sánchez-Bernardos ML, Kusdil ME, Woodfield R, Saunders PR, Smith PB (January 2000). "Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (1): 173–86. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.173. PMID 10653513.
  76. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Markey PM, Markey CN, Tinsley BJ (April 2004). "Children's behavioral manifestations of the five-factor model of personality". Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 30 (4): 423–32. doi:10.1177/0146167203261886. PMID 15070472. S2CID 33684001.
  77. ^ Rusalov VM (1989). "Motor and communicative aspects of human temperament: a new questionnaire of the structure of temperament". Personality and Individual Differences. 10 (8): 817–27. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(89)90017-2.
  78. ^ a b Strelau J (1998). Temperament: A Psychological Perspective. New York: Plenum.
  79. ^ a b Rusalov VM, Trofimova IN (2007). Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement. Toronto, Canada: Psychological Services Press.[page needed]
  80. ^ a b c d e Trofimova IN (2016). "The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament". In MC (ed.). Temperaments: Individual Differences, Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life. New York: Nova Science Publishers. pp. 77–147.
  81. ^ a b c d Trofimova I, Robbins TW, Sulis WH, Uher J (April 2018). "Taxonomies of psychological individual differences: biological perspectives on millennia-long challenges". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 373 (1744): 20170152. doi:10.1098/rstb.2017.0152. PMC 5832678. PMID 29483338.
  82. ^ Trofimova IN (2010). "An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality". American Journal of Psychology. 123 (4): 467–80. doi:10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0467. PMID 21291163.
  83. ^ Jang KL, Livesley WJ, Vernon PA (September 1996). "Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: a twin study". Journal of Personality. 64 (3): 577–91. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00522.x. PMID 8776880.
  84. ^ Bouchard TJ, McGue M (January 2003). "Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences". Journal of Neurobiology. 54 (1): 4–45. doi:10.1002/neu.10160. PMID 12486697.
  85. ^ Weiss A, King JE, Hopkins WD (November 2007). "A cross-setting study of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) personality structure and development: zoological parks and Yerkes National Primate Research Center". American Journal of Primatology. 69 (11): 1264–77. doi:10.1002/ajp.20428. PMC 2654334. PMID 17397036.
  86. ^ Gosling SD, John OP (1999). (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8 (3): 69–75. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00017. S2CID 145716504. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-09-28. Retrieved 2016-12-05.
  87. ^ Morton FB, Robinson LM, Brando S, Weiss A (2021). "Personality structure in bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 135 (2): 219–231. doi:10.1037/com0000259. PMID 33464108. S2CID 231642036.
  88. ^ a b Soto CJ, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J (February 2011). "Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65: Big Five domains and facets in a large cross-sectional sample". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 100 (2): 330–48. doi:10.1037/a0021717. PMID 21171787.
  89. ^ a b c d e f Soto CJ (August 2016). "The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood: Mean-Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents' Reports". Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 409–22. doi:10.1111/jopy.12168. PMID 25728032.
  90. ^ Lewis M (2001). "Issues in the study of personality development". Psychological Inquiry. 12 (2): 67–83. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1202_02. S2CID 144557981.
  91. ^ a b Goldberg LR (2001). "Analyses of Digman's child- personality data: Derivation of Big Five Factor Scores from each of six samples". Journal of Personality. 69 (5): 709–43. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.695161. PMID 11575511.
  92. ^ Mervielde I, De Fruyt F (1999). "Construction of the Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children (Hi- PIC).". In Mervielde ID, De Fruyt F, Ostendorf F (eds.). Personality psychology in Europe: Proceedings of the Eighth European Conference on Personality. Tilburg University Press. pp. 107–27.
  93. ^ Resing WC, Bleichrodt N, Dekker PH (1999). "Measuring personality traits in the classroom" (PDF). European Journal of Personality. 13 (6): 493–509. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0984(199911/12)13:6<493::aid-per355>3.0.co;2-v. hdl:1871/18675. S2CID 56322465.
  94. ^ Markey PM, Markey CN, Ericksen AJ, Tinsley BJ (2002). "A preliminary validation of preadolescents' self-reports using the Five-Factor Model of personality". Journal of Research in Personality. 36 (2): 173–81. doi:10.1006/jrpe.2001.2341.
  95. ^ Scholte RH, van Aken MA, van Lieshout CF (December 1997). "Adolescent personality factors in self-ratings and peer nominations and their prediction of peer acceptance and peer rejection". Journal of Personality Assessment. 69 (3): 534–54. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6903_8. PMID 9501483.
  96. ^ van Lieshout CF, Haselager GJ (1994). "The Big Five personality factors in Q-sort descriptions of children and adolescents.". In Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP (eds.). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 293–318.
  97. ^ a b c Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP, eds. (1994). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  98. ^ a b c Kohnstamm GA, Halverson Jr CF, Mervielde I, Havill VL, eds. (1998). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the Big Five?. Psychology Press.[ISBN missing][page needed]
  99. ^ Mervielde I, De Fruyt F, Jarmuz S (May 1998). "Linking openness and intellect in childhood and adulthood.". In Kohnstamm GA, Halverson CF, Mervielde I, Havill VL (eds.). Parental descriptions of child personality: Developmental antecedents of the Big Five. Mahway, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 105–26. ISBN 9780805823011.
  100. ^ John OP, Srivastava S (1999). "The Big-Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and theoretical perspectives" (PDF). In Pervin LA, John OP (eds.). Handbook of personality: Theory and research. Vol. 2. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 102–38.
  101. ^ a b c d e f Soto C, Tackett J (2015). "Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence: Structure, Development, and Outcomes" (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24: 358–62. doi:10.1177/0963721415589345. S2CID 29475747.
  102. ^ a b c Roberts BW, Walton KE, Viechtbauer W (January 2006). "Patterns of mean-level change in personality traits across the life course: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies". Psychological Bulletin. 132 (1): 1–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.1.1. PMID 16435954.
  103. ^ Roberts BW, DelVecchio WF (January 2000). "The rank-order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age: a quantitative review of longitudinal studies" (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 126 (1): 3–25. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.1.3. PMID 10668348.
  104. ^ Lemery KS, Goldsmith HH, Klinnert MD, Mrazek DA (January 1999). "Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament". Developmental Psychology. 35 (1): 189–204. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.189. PMID 9923474.
  105. ^ Buss A, Plomin R (1984). Temperament: early developing personality trait. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.
  106. ^ Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL, Fisher P (2001). "Investigations of temperament at three to seven years: the Children's Behavior Questionnaire". Child Development. 72 (5): 1394–408. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.398.8830. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00355. PMID 11699677.
  107. ^ John OP, Caspi A, Robins RW, Moffitt TE, Stouthamer-Loeber M (February 1994). "The "little five": exploring the nomological network of the five-factor model of personality in adolescent boys". Child Development. 65 (1): 160–78. doi:10.2307/1131373. JSTOR 1131373. PMID 8131645.
  108. ^ Eaton WO (1994). "Temperament, development, and the Five-Factor Model: Lessons from activity level". In Halverson CF, Kohnstamm GA, Martin RP (eds.). The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 173–87.
  109. ^ Hawley PH (1999). "The ontogenesis of social dominance: A strategy-based evolutionary perspective". Developmental Review. 19: 97–132. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.459.4755. doi:10.1006/drev.1998.0470.
  110. ^ Hawley PH, Little TD (1999). "On winning some and losing some: A social relations approach to social dominance in toddlers". Merrill Palmer Quarterly. 45: 185–214.
  111. ^ Sherif M, Harvey O, White BJ, Hood WR, Sherif C (1961). Intergroup conflict and cooperation: The robbers' cave experiment. Norman, OK: University of Oklahoma Press. OCLC 953442127.
  112. ^ Keating CF, Heltman KR (1994). "Dominance and deception in children and adults: Are leaders the best misleaders?". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 20 (3): 312–21. doi:10.1177/0146167294203009. S2CID 19252480.
  113. ^ Asendorpf JB (1990). "Development of inhibition during childhood: Evidence for situational specificity and a two-factor model". Developmental Psychology. 26 (5): 721–30. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.5.721.
  114. ^ Asendorpf JB, Meier GH (1993). "Personality effects on children's speech in everyday life: Sociability-mediated exposure and shyness-mediated re-activity to social situations". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 64 (6): 1072–83. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.1072. PMID 8326470.
  115. ^ Harrist AW, Zaia AF, Bates JE, Dodge KA, Pettit GS (April 1997). "Subtypes of social withdrawal in early childhood: sociometric status and social-cognitive differences across four years". Child Development. 68 (2): 278–94. doi:10.2307/1131850. JSTOR 1131850. PMID 9180002.
  116. ^ Mathiesen KS, Tambs K (March 1999). "The EAS temperament questionnaire – factor structure, age trends, reliability, and stability in a Norwegian sample". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 40 (3): 431–39. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00460. PMID 10190344.
  117. ^ McCrae RR, Costa PT (1990). Personality in adulthood. New York: The Guildford Press.[page needed]
  118. ^ Cobb-Clark DA, Schurer S (2012). "The stability of big-five personality traits" (PDF). Economics Letters. 115 (2): 11–15. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2011.11.015. S2CID 12086995.
  119. ^ Srivastava S, John OP, Gosling SD, Potter J (May 2003). "Development of personality in early and middle adulthood: set like plaster or persistent change?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84 (5): 1041–53. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.499.4124. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.5.1041. PMID 12757147.
  120. ^ Roberts BW, Mroczek D (February 2008). "Personality Trait Change in Adulthood". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (1): 31–35. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00543.x. PMC 2743415. PMID 19756219.
  121. ^ Fleeson W (2001). "Towards a structure- and process-integrated view of personality: Traits as density distributions of states". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (6): 1011–27. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.1011. PMID 11414368.
  122. ^ Mõttus R, Johnson W, Starr JM, Dearya IJ (June 2012). "Correlates of personality trait levels and their changes in very old age: The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921" (PDF). Journal of Research in Personality. 46 (3): 271–78. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.02.004. hdl:20.500.11820/b6b6961d-902f-48e0-bf25-f505a659a056.
  123. ^ Cavallera G, Passerini A, Pepe A (2013). "Personality and gender in swimmers in indoor practice at leisure level". Social Behavior and Personality. 41 (4): 693–704. doi:10.2224/sbp.2013.41.4.693.
  124. ^ Falk, Armin; Hermle, Johannes (2018-10-19). "Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality". Science. 362 (6412): eaas9899. doi:10.1126/science.aas9899. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 30337384.
  125. ^ a b Costa PT, Terracciano A, McCrae RR (August 2001). "Gender differences in personality traits across cultures: robust and surprising findings". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 81 (2): 322–31. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.322. PMID 11519935.
  126. ^ a b c Schmitt DP, Realo A, Voracek M, Allik J (January 2008). "Why can't a man be more like a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 94 (1): 168–82. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.168. PMID 18179326.
  127. ^ Harris, J. R. (2006). No two alike: Human nature and human individuality. WW Norton & Company.
  128. ^ Jefferson T, Herbst JH, McCrae RR (1998). "Associations between birth order and personality traits: Evidence from self-reports and observer ratings". Journal of Research in Personality. 32 (4): 498–509. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1998.2233.
  129. ^ Damian RI, Roberts BW (October 2015). "The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of U.S. high school students". Journal of Research in Personality. 58: 96–105. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2015.05.005.
  130. ^ Thompson RL, Brossart DF, Carlozzi AF, Miville ML (September 2002). "Five-factor model (Big Five) personality traits and universal-diverse orientation in counselor trainees". The Journal of Psychology. 136 (5): 561–72. doi:10.1080/00223980209605551. PMID 12431039. S2CID 22076221.
  131. ^ Ostendorf, F. (1990). Sprache und Persoenlichkeitsstruktur: Zur Validitaet des Funf-Factoren-Modells der Persoenlichkeit. Regensburg, Germany: S. Roderer Verlag.[page needed]
  132. ^ Trull TJ, Geary DC (October 1997). "Comparison of the big-five factor structure across samples of Chinese and American adults". Journal of Personality Assessment. 69 (2): 324–41. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6902_6. PMID 9392894.
  133. ^ Lodhi PH, Deo S, Belhekar VM (2002). "The Five-Factor model of personality in Indian context: measurement and correlates.". In McCrae RR, Allik J (eds.). The Five-Factor model of personality across cultures. New York: Kluwer Academic Publisher. pp. 227–48.
  134. ^ McCrae RR (2002). "NEO-PI-R data from 36 cultures: Further Intercultural comparisons.". In McCrae RR, Allik J (eds.). The Five-Factor model of personality across cultures. New York: Kluwer Academic Publisher. pp. 105–25.
  135. ^ a b Thompson ER (2008). "Development and validation of an international English big-five mini-markers". Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (6): 542–48. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013.
  136. ^ Cheung FM, van de Vijver FJ, Leong FT (October 2011). . The American Psychologist. 66 (7): 593–603. doi:10.1037/a0022389. PMID 21261408. Archived from the original on 2013-05-18. Retrieved 2013-01-16.
  137. ^ McCrae RR, Terracciano A (September 2005). "Personality profiles of cultures: aggregate personality traits" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 89 (3): 407–25. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.407. PMID 16248722.
  138. ^ Hofstede, Geert; Bond, Michael H. (1984). "Hofstede's Culture Dimensions: An Independent Validation Using Rokeach's Value Survey". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 15 (4): 417–433. doi:10.1177/0022002184015004003. ISSN 0022-0221. S2CID 145651845.
  139. ^ Mooradian, Todd A.; Swan, K. Scott (2006-06-01). "Personality-and-culture: The case of national extraversion and word-of-mouth". Journal of Business Research. Special Section - The 2005 La Londe Seminar. 59 (6): 778–785. doi:10.1016/j.jbusres.2006.01.015. ISSN 0148-2963.
  140. ^ Barceló J (2017). "National Personality Traits and Regime Type: A Cross-National Study of 47 Countries". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 48 (2): 195–216. doi:10.1177/0022022116678324. S2CID 151607260.
  141. ^ Szirmak Z, De Raad B (1994). "Taxonomy and structure of Hungarian personality traits". European Journal of Personality. 8 (2): 95–117. doi:10.1002/per.2410080203. S2CID 145275826.
  142. ^ a b c De Fruyt F, McCrae RR, Szirmák Z, Nagy J (September 2004). "The Five-factor Personality Inventory as a measure of the Five-factor Model: Belgian, American, and Hungarian comparisons with the NEO-PI-R". Assessment. 11 (3): 207–15. doi:10.1177/1073191104265800. PMID 15358876. S2CID 29733250.
  143. ^ Robins Wahlin TB, Byrne GJ (October 2011). "Personality changes in Alzheimer's disease: a systematic review". International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 26 (10): 1019–29. doi:10.1002/gps.2655. PMID 21905097. S2CID 40949990.
  144. ^ Widiger TA, Costa PT. Jr. "Five-Factor model personality disorder research". In: Costa Paul T Jr, Widiger Thomas A., editors. Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality. 2nd. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2002. pp. 59–87. 2002.[ISBN missing]
  145. ^ Mullins-Sweatt SN, Widiger TA (2006). "The five-factor model of personality disorder: A translation across science and practice.". In Krueger R, Tackett J (eds.). Personality and psychopathology: Building bridges. New York: Guilford. pp. 39–70.[ISBN missing]
  146. ^ Clark LA (2007). "Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorder: perennial issues and an emerging reconceptualization". Annual Review of Psychology. 58: 227–57. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190200. PMID 16903806.
  147. ^ Trofimova I, Robbins TW (May 2016). "Temperament and arousal systems: A new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 64: 382–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.008. hdl:11375/26202. PMID 26969100. S2CID 13937324.
  148. ^ Trofimova I, Sulis W (2016). "Benefits of Distinguishing between Physical and Social-Verbal Aspects of Behavior: An Example of Generalized Anxiety". Frontiers in Psychology. 7: 338. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00338. PMC 4789559. PMID 27014146.
  149. ^ Trofimova I, Christiansen J (April 2016). "Coupling of Temperament with Mental Illness in Four Age Groups". Psychological Reports. 118 (2): 387–412. doi:10.1177/0033294116639430. PMID 27154370. S2CID 24465522.
  150. ^ Depue R, Fu Y (2012). "Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults". In Zentner M, Shiner R (eds.). Handbook of Temperament. New York: Guilford Publications. pp. 368–99.
  151. ^ Bagby RM, Sellbom M, Costa PT, Widiger TA (April 2008). "PredictingDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV personality disorders with the five-factor model of personality and the personality psychopathology five". Personality and Mental Health. 2 (2): 55–69. doi:10.1002/pmh.33.
  152. ^ "The five-factor model and personality disorder empirical literature: A meta-analytic review." LM Saulsman, AC Page, Clinical Psychology Review, 2004 – Elsevier Science[ISBN missing][page needed]
  153. ^ Fehrman E, Muhammad AK, Mirkes EM, Egan V, Gorban AN (2015). "The Five Factor Model of personality and evaluation of drug consumption risk". arXiv:1506.06297 [stat.AP].
  154. ^ a b Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE (June 2005). "Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication". Archives of General Psychiatry. 62 (6): 617–27. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617. PMC 2847357. PMID 15939839.
  155. ^ Compton WM, Conway KP, Stinson FS, Colliver JD, Grant BF (June 2005). "Prevalence, correlates, and comorbidity of DSM-IV antisocial personality syndromes and alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 66 (6): 677–85. doi:10.4088/jcp.v66n0602. PMID 15960559.
  156. ^ Hasin DS, Goodwin RD, Stinson FS, Grant BF (October 2005). "Epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcoholism and Related Conditions". Archives of General Psychiatry. 62 (10): 1097–106. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.10.1097. PMID 16203955.
  157. ^ a b Fehrman, Elaine; Egan, Vincent; Gorban, Alexander N.; Levesley, Jeremy; Mirkes, Evgeny M.; Muhammad, Awaz K. (2019). Personality Traits and Drug Consumption. A Story Told by Data. Springer, Cham. arXiv:2001.06520. doi:10.1007/978-3-030-10442-9. ISBN 978-3-030-10441-2. S2CID 151160405.
  158. ^ Khan AA, Jacobson KC, Gardner CO, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (March 2005). "Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders". The British Journal of Psychiatry. 186 (3): 190–96. doi:10.1192/bjp.186.3.190. PMID 15738498.
  159. ^ Cuijpers P, Smit F, Penninx BW, de Graaf R, ten Have M, Beekman AT (October 2010). "Economic costs of neuroticism: a population-based study". Archives of General Psychiatry. 67 (10): 1086–93. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.130. PMID 20921124.
  160. ^ a b c d e f g Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D (September 2010). "Linking "big" personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 136 (5): 768–821. doi:10.1037/a0020327. PMID 20804236.
  161. ^ Bogg T, Roberts BW (November 2004). "Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: a meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality". Psychological Bulletin. 130 (6): 887–919. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.130.6.887. PMID 15535742.
  162. ^ a b Roberts BW, Kuncel NR, Shiner R, Caspi A, Goldberg LR (December 2007). "The Power of Personality: The Comparative Validity of Personality Traits, Socioeconomic Status, and Cognitive Ability for Predicting Important Life Outcomes" (PDF). Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2 (4): 313–45. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00047.x. PMC 4499872. PMID 26151971.
  163. ^ a b c d Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Ormel J (October 2016). "Neuroticism's prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history, but the adjusted association hardly decays with time: a meta-analysis on 59 longitudinal/prospective studies with 443 313 participants". Psychological Medicine. 46 (14): 2883–906. doi:10.1017/S0033291716001653. PMID 27523506. S2CID 23548727.
  164. ^ Livesley WJ (2001). Handbook of Personality Disorders. New York: The Guildford Press. pp. 84–104. ISBN 978-1-57230-629-5. OCLC 783011161.
  165. ^ a b c d Ormel J, Jeronimus BF, Kotov R, Riese H, Bos EH, Hankin B, Rosmalen JG, Oldehinkel AJ (July 2013). "Neuroticism and common mental disorders: meaning and utility of a complex relationship". Clinical Psychology Review. 33 (5): 686–97. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2013.04.003. PMC 4382368. PMID 23702592.
  166. ^ a b c d e f Millon T, Krueger R, Simonsen E (2011). Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology: Scientific Foundations of the DSM-IV and ICD-11. Guilford Press.
  167. ^ a b c d Krueger R, Tackett L (2006). Personality and Psychopathology. Guilford Press.[page needed][ISBN missing]
  168. ^ a b Hudek-Knezević J, Kardum I (August 2009). "Five-factor personality dimensions and 3 health-related personality constructs as predictors of health". Croatian Medical Journal. 50 (4): 394–402. doi:10.3325/cmj.2009.50.394. PMC 2728392. PMID 19673040.
  169. ^ Jerram, Kathryn L.; Coleman, Peter G. (1999). "The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age". British Journal of Health Psychology. 4 (2): 181–92. doi:10.1348/135910799168560. ISSN 2044-8287.
  170. ^ Jerram, Kathryn (16 December 2010). "The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age". British Journal of Health Psychology. 4 (2): 181–192. doi:10.1348/135910799168560 – via Wiley.
  171. ^ Iwasa H, Masui Y, Gondo Y, Inagaki H, Kawaai C, Suzuki T (May 2008). "Personality and all-cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community: a five-year population-based prospective cohort study". The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 16 (5): 399–405. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181662ac9. PMID 18403571.
  172. ^ Jokela M, Hintsanen M, Hakulinen C, Batty GD, Nabi H, Singh-Manoux A, Kivimäki M (April 2013). "Association of personality with the development and persistence of obesity: a meta-analysis based on individual-participant data". Obesity Reviews. 14 (4): 315–23. doi:10.1111/obr.12007. PMC 3717171. PMID 23176713.
  173. ^ a b c d e f g h i Komarraju M, Karau SJ, Schmeck RR, Avdic A (September 2011). "The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement". Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (4): 472–77. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019.
  174. ^ Zeidner M, Shani-Zinovich I (11 October 2011). "Do academically gifted and nongifted students differ on the Big-Five and adaptive status? Some recent data and conclusions". Personality and Individual Differences. 51 (5): 566–70. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.007.
  175. ^ a b Mutlu, Tansu; Balbag, Zafer; Cemrek, Fatih (2010-01-01). "The role of self-esteem, locus of control and big five personality traits in predicting hopelessness". Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences. World Conference on Learning, Teaching and Administration Papers. 9: 1788–1792. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.12.401. ISSN 1877-0428.
  176. ^ Singh AK (2012). "Does trait predict psychological well-being among students of professional courses?". Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology. 38 (2): 234–41.
  177. ^ a b c Klimstra TA, Luyckx K, Germeijs V, Meeus WH, Goossens L (March 2012). "Personality traits and educational identity formation in late adolescents: longitudinal associations and academic progress" (PDF). Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 41 (3): 346–61. doi:10.1007/s10964-011-9734-7. PMID 22147120. S2CID 33747401.
  178. ^ Pashler H, McDaniel M, Rohrer D, Bjork R (December 2008). "Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence". Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 9 (3): 105–19. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01038.x. PMID 26162104.
  179. ^ Zhang L (6 September 2001). "Measuring thinking styles in addition to measuring personality traits?". Personality and Individual Differences. 33 (3): 445–58. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00166-0.
  180. ^ Schmeck RR, Ribich F, Ramainah N (1997). "Development of a Self-Report inventory for assessing Individual Differences in Learning Processes". Applied Psychological Measurement. 1 (3): 413–31. doi:10.1177/014662167700100310. S2CID 143890188.
  181. ^ a b c Jensen, Mikael (2015). "Personality Traits, Learning and Academic Achievements". Journal of Education and Learning. 4 (4): 91. doi:10.5539/jel.v4n4p91.
  182. ^ De Feyter T, Caers R, Vigna C, Berings D (22 March 2012). "Unraveling the impact of the Big Five personality traits on academic performance: The moderating and mediating effects of self-efficacy and academic motivation". Learning and Individual Differences. 22 (4): 439–48. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.03.013.
  183. ^ Vedel A (2014). "The Big Five and tertiary academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis" (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 71: 66–76. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.07.011.
  184. ^ Trapmann, S., Hell, B., Hirn, J.-O. W. ve Schuler, H. (2007). Meta-analysis of the relationship between the Big Five and academic success at university. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 215(2), 132–51.
  185. ^ Bartolic-Zlomislic, Bates A (1999). "Investing in On-line Learning: Potential Benefits and Limitations". Canadian Journal of Communication. 24 (3). doi:10.22230/CJC.1999V24N3A1111.
  186. ^ Holland, J. L. (1966). The Psychology of Vocational Choice: A Theory of Personality Types and Model Environments. Oxford: Blaisdell.
  187. ^ Armitage, Catherine. "Scientists are curious and passionate and ready to argue". Retrieved 9 June 2021.
  188. ^ Mount MK, Barrick MR (1998). "Five reasons why the "big five" article has been frequently cited". Personnel Psychology. 51 (4): 849–57. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1998.tb00743.x.
  189. ^ Morgeson FP, Campion MA, Dipboye RL, Hollenbeck JR, Murphy K, Schmitt N (2007). "Reconsidering the use of personality tests in personnel selection contexts". Personnel Psychology. 60 (3): 683–729. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.493.5981. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2007.00089.x.
  190. ^ Mischel W (1968). Personality and assessment. London: Wiley. ISBN 978-0-8058-2330-1.
  191. ^ Rosenthal R (1990). "How are we doing in soft psychology?". American Psychologist. 45 (6): 775–77. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.45.6.775.
  192. ^ Hunter JE, Schmidt FL, Judiesch MK (1990). "Individual differences in output variability as a function of job complexity". Journal of Applied Psychology. 75: 28–42. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.75.1.28. S2CID 144507523.
  193. ^ Fairweather J (2012). "Personality, nations, and innovation: Relationships between personality traits and national innovation scores". Cross-Cultural Research. 46: 3–30. doi:10.1177/1069397111409124. S2CID 144015495.
  194. ^ Judge TA, Bono JE, Ilies R, Gerhardt MW (August 2002). "Personality and leadership: a qualitative and quantitative review". The Journal of Applied Psychology. 87 (4): 765–80. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.4.765. PMID 12184579.
  195. ^ Mehta P (2012). "Personality as a predictor of burnout among managers of manufacturing industries.". Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology. 32: 321–28.
  196. ^ Judge TA, Livingston BA, Hurst C (February 2012). "Do nice guys--and gals--really finish last? The joint effects of sex and agreeableness on income". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 102 (2): 390–407. doi:10.1037/a0026021. PMID 22121889.
  197. ^ a b Spurk D, Abele AE (16 June 2010). "Who Earns More and Why? A Multiple Mediation Model from Personality to Salary". Journal of Business and Psychology. 26: 87–103. doi:10.1007/s10869-010-9184-3. S2CID 144290202.
  198. ^ Camps J, Stouten J, Euwema M (February 2016). "The relation between supervisors' big five personality traits and employees' experiences of abusive supervision". Frontiers in Psychology. 10 (7): 112. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00112. PMC 4748047. PMID 26903919.
  199. ^ Tepper BJ (June 2007). "Abusive supervision in work organizations: Review, synthesis, and research agenda". Journal of Management. 33 (3): 261–89. doi:10.1177/0149206307300812. S2CID 143934380.
  200. ^ McLean, Dawson; Bouaissa, Mohsen; Rainville, Bruno; Auger, Ludovic (2019-12-04). "Non-Cognitive Skills: How Much Do They Matter for Earnings in Canada?". American Journal of Management. 19 (4). doi:10.33423/ajm.v19i4.2392. ISSN 2165-7998.
  201. ^ a b c Neal A, Yeo G, Koy A, Xiao T (26 January 2011). "Predicting the Form and Direction of Work Role Performance From the Big 5 Model of Personality Traits". Journal of Organizational Behavior. 33 (2): 175–92. doi:10.1002/job.742.
  202. ^ a b c d e Judge & LePine, "Bright and Dark Sides…" Research Companion to the Dysfunctional Workplace, 2007
  203. ^ a b c d Holland AS, Roisman GI (October 2008). (PDF). Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 25 (5): 811–29. doi:10.1177/0265407508096697. S2CID 28388979. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 March 2013. Retrieved 12 April 2012.
  204. ^ a b Gerber AS, et al. (2010). "Personality and Political Attitudes: Relationships across Issue Domains and Political Contexts". The American Political Science Review. 104: 111–133. doi:10.1017/S0003055410000031. S2CID 6208090.
  205. ^ Sweetser KD (2014). "Partisan Personality: The Psychological Differences Between Democrats and Republicans, and Independents Somewhere in Between". American Behavioral Scientist. 58 (9): 1183–94. doi:10.1177/0002764213506215. S2CID 145674720.
  206. ^ Fatke M (2017). "Personality Traits and Political Ideology: A First Global Assessment". Political Psychology. 38 (5): 881–99. doi:10.1111/pops.12347.
  207. ^ a b Bakker BN, et al. (2015). "Personality Traits and Party Identification over Time". European Journal of Political Research. 54 (2): 197–215. doi:10.1111/1475-6765.12070.
  208. ^ a b Gerber AS, et al. (2012). "Personality and the Strength and Direction of Partisan Identification". Political Behavior. 34 (4): 653–688. doi:10.1007/s11109-011-9178-5. S2CID 144317734.
  209. ^ Löwe, Konstantin Felix. "Is Politics Downstream from Personality? The Five Factor Model's Effect on Political Orientation in Sweden." (2019). http://lup.lub.lu.se/student-papers/record/8992021 Thesis
  210. ^ Roberts, p. 338
  211. ^ Saroglou, Vassilis (2002). "Religion and the five-factors of personality: A meta-analytic review". Personality and Individual Differences. 32: 15–25. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00233-6.
  212. ^ "IPIP Home". ipip.ori.org. Retrieved 2017-07-01.
  213. ^ Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB (2003). "A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains". Journal of Research in Personality. 37 (6): 504–28. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1013.6925. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1. ISSN 0092-6566.
  214. ^ a b Goldberg LR (1992). "The development of markers for the Big-five factor structure". Psychological Assessment. 4 (1): 26–42. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26.
  215. ^ a b Donaldson SI, Grant-Vallone EJ (2002). "Understanding self-report bias in organizational behavior research". Journal of Business and Psychology. 17 (2): 245–60. doi:10.1023/A:1019637632584. JSTOR 25092818. S2CID 10464760.
  216. ^ a b Hirsh JB, Peterson JB (October 2008). "Predicting creativity and academic success with a 'Fake-Proof' measure of the Big Five". Journal of Research in Personality. 42 (5): 1323–33. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2008.04.006. S2CID 18849547.
  217. ^ Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB (2003). "A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains". Journal of Research in Personality. 37 (6): 504–28. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1013.6925. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1.
  218. ^ Credé M, Harms P, Niehorster S, Gaye-Valentine A (April 2012). "An evaluation of the consequences of using short measures of the Big Five personality traits". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 102 (4): 874–88. doi:10.1037/a0027403. PMID 22352328.
  219. ^ Goldberg LR (December 1990). "An alternative "description of personality": the big-five factor structure". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 59 (6): 1216–29. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1216. PMID 2283588.
  220. ^ McFarland, Lynn A.; Ryan, Ann Marie (2000). "Variance in faking across noncognitive measures". Journal of Applied Psychology. 85 (5): 812–21. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.85.5.812. ISSN 1939-1854. PMID 11055152.
  221. ^ "Big Five Personality Tests, traits and background". Personality and Aptitude Career Tests. Retrieved 2017-07-01.
  222. ^ Schwartz HA, Eichstaedt JC, Kern ML, Dziurzynski L, Ramones SM, Agrawal M, Shah A, Kosinski M, Stillwell D, Seligman ME, Ungar LH (2013). "Personality, gender, and age in the language of social media: the open-vocabulary approach". PLOS ONE. 8 (9): e73791. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...873791S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0073791. PMC 3783449. PMID 24086296.
  223. ^ Block J (March 1995). "A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description". Psychological Bulletin. 117 (2): 187–215. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187. PMID 7724687.
  224. ^ Eysenck HJ (1991). "Dimensions of personality: 16, 5, 3?". Personality and Individual Differences. 12 (8): 773–90. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90144-z.
  225. ^ Eysenck HJ (1992). "Four ways five factors are not basic". Personality and Individual Differences. 13 (6): 667–73. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90237-j.
  226. ^ Cattell RB (May 1995). "The fallacy of five factors in the personality sphere". The Psychologist: 207–08.
  227. ^ a b c Trofimova I (2014). "Observer bias: an interaction of temperament traits with biases in the semantic perception of lexical material". PLOS ONE. 9 (1): e85677. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...985677T. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0085677. PMC 3903487. PMID 24475048.   Text was copied from this source, which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
  228. ^ a b Paunonen SV, Jackson DN (2000). (PDF). Journal of Personality. 68 (October 2000): 821–35. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00117. PMID 11001150. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2019-02-14. Retrieved 2012-01-15.
  229. ^ a b Boyle GJ (2008). "Critique of Five-Factor Model (FFM).". In Boyle GJ, Matthews G, Saklofske DH (eds.). The Sage handbook of personality theory and assessment, Vol. 1 – Personality theories and models. Los Angeles, CA: Sage. ISBN 978-1-4129-4651-3.
  230. ^ Costa PT, McCrae RR (1995). "Solid ground in the wetlands of personality: A reply to Block". Psychological Bulletin. 117 (2): 216–20. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.216. PMID 7724688.
  231. ^ Block J (1995b). "Going beyond the five factors given: Rejoinder to Costa and McCrae and Goldberg and Saucier". Psychological Bulletin. 117 (2): 226–29. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.226.
  232. ^ Block J (2001). "Millennial contrarianism". Journal of Research in Personality. 35: 98–107. doi:10.1006/jrpe.2000.2293. S2CID 40747837.
  233. ^ Block J (2010). "The Five-Factor framing of personality and beyond: Some ruminations". Psychological Inquiry. 21: 2–25. doi:10.1080/10478401003596626. S2CID 26355524.
  234. ^ Cattell RB, Boyle GJ, Chant D (2002). . Psychological Review (Submitted manuscript). 109 (1): 202–05. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.109.1.202. PMID 11863038. Archived from the original on 2015-01-23. Retrieved 2018-10-25.
  235. ^ Schacter DL, Gilbert DT, Wegner DM (2011). Psychology (2nd ed.). Worth. pp. 474–75. ISBN 978-1-4292-3719-2.
  236. ^ Piekkola B (2011). "Traits across cultures: A neo-Allportian perspective". Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology. 31: 2–24. doi:10.1037/a0022478.
  237. ^ Paunonen SV, Haddock G, Forsterling F, Keinonen M (2003). "Broad versus Narrow Personality Measures and the Prediction of Behaviour Across Cultures". European Journal of Personality. 17 (6): 413–33. doi:10.1002/per.496. S2CID 143671349.
  238. ^ McAdams DP (1995). "What do we know when we know a person?". Journal of Personality. 63 (3): 365–96. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.527.6832. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1995.tb00500.x.
  239. ^ Saucier, Gerard; Srivastava, Sanjay (2015), "What makes a good structural model of personality? Evaluating the big five and alternatives.", APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Volume 4: Personality processes and individual differences., Washington: American Psychological Association, pp. 283–305, doi:10.1037/14343-013, ISBN 978-1-4338-1704-5, retrieved 2021-01-03
  240. ^ Musek J (2007). "A general factor of personality: Evidence for the Big One in the five-factor model". Journal of Research in Personality. 41 (6): 1213–33. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.02.003.
  241. ^ Van der Linden D, te Nijenhuis J, Bakker AB (2010). (PDF). Journal of Research in Personality. 44 (3): 315–27. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.03.003. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-07-11. Retrieved 2012-06-17.
  242. ^ a b Eysenck HJ (1992). (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 13 (8): 667–73. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90237-j. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-11-07. Retrieved 2012-06-17.
  243. ^ Costa PT, McCrae RR (1992). "Reply to Eysenck". Personality and Individual Differences. 13 (8): 861–65. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(92)90002-7.
  244. ^ Gurven, M.; von Rueden, C.; Massenkoff, M.; Kaplan, H.; Lero Vie, M. (2013). "APA PsycNet". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 104 (2): 354–370. doi:10.1037/a0030841. PMC 4104167. PMID 23245291.
  245. ^ Block J (2010). "The five-factor framing of personality and beyond: Some ruminations". Psychological Inquiry. 21 (1): 2–25. doi:10.1080/10478401003596626. S2CID 26355524.
  246. ^ Ashton MC, Lee K, Goldberg LR (November 2004). "A hierarchical analysis of 1,710 English personality-descriptive adjectives". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (5): 707–21. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.707. PMID 15535781.
  247. ^ Ashton MC, Lee K, de Vries RE (May 2014). "The HEXACO Honesty-Humility, Agreeableness, and Emotionality factors: a review of research and theory". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 18 (2): 139–52. doi:10.1177/1088868314523838. PMID 24577101. S2CID 38312803.
five, personality, traits, suggested, taxonomy, grouping, personality, traits, developed, from, 1980s, onward, psychological, trait, theory, starting, 1990s, theory, identified, five, factors, labels, english, speaking, population, typically, referred, opennes. The Big Five personality traits is a suggested taxonomy or grouping for personality traits 1 developed from the 1980s onward in psychological trait theory Starting in the 1990s the theory identified five factors by labels for the US English speaking population typically referred to as openness to experience inventive curious vs consistent cautious conscientiousness efficient organized vs extravagant careless extraversion outgoing energetic vs solitary reserved agreeableness friendly compassionate vs critical rational neuroticism sensitive nervous vs resilient confident 2 When factor analysis a statistical technique is applied to personality survey data it reveals semantic associations some words used to describe aspects of personality are often applied to the same person For example someone described as conscientious is more likely to be described as always prepared rather than messy These associations suggest five broad dimensions used in common language to describe the human personality temperament and psyche 3 4 Those labels for the five factors may be remembered using the acronyms OCEAN or CANOE Beneath each proposed global factor there are a number of correlated and more specific primary factors For example extraversion is typically associated with qualities such as gregariousness assertiveness excitement seeking warmth activity and positive emotions 5 These traits are not black and white but rather placed on continua 6 Contents 1 Development 2 Descriptions of the particular personality traits 2 1 Openness to experience 2 1 1 Sample items 2 2 Conscientiousness 2 2 1 Sample items 2 3 Extraversion 2 3 1 Sample items 2 4 Agreeableness 2 4 1 Sample items 2 5 Neuroticism 2 5 1 Sample items 3 History 3 1 Early trait research 3 2 Hiatus in research 3 3 Renewed attention 4 Biological and developmental factors 4 1 Temperament vis a vis personality 4 2 Heritability 4 3 Non humans 4 4 Development during childhood and adolescence 4 4 1 Extraversion positive emotionality 4 5 Development throughout adulthood 5 Group differences 5 1 Gender differences 5 2 Birth order differences 6 Cultural differences 7 Health 7 1 Personality change from disease 7 2 Personality disorders 7 3 Common mental disorders 7 3 1 The personality psychopathology models 7 4 Physical health 8 Effect of personality traits through life 8 1 Education 8 1 1 Academic achievement 8 1 2 Learning styles 8 1 3 Distance Learning 8 2 Employment 8 3 Occupation and personality fit 8 4 Work success 8 5 Romantic relationships 8 6 Political identification 8 7 Scope of predictive power 8 8 Religiosity 9 Measurements 10 Critique 10 1 Limited scope 10 2 Methodological issues 10 3 Theoretical status 10 4 Evidence for six factors rather than five 11 See also 12 References 13 External linksDevelopment EditThe Big Five personality traits was the model to comprehend the relationship between personality and academic behaviors 7 This model was defined by several independent sets of researchers who used factor analysis of verbal descriptors of human behavior 8 These researchers began by studying relationships between a large number of verbal descriptors related to personality traits They reduced the lists of these descriptors by 5 10 fold and then used factor analysis to group the remaining traits using data mostly based upon people s estimations in self report questionnaire and peer ratings in order to find the underlying factors of personality 9 10 11 12 13 The initial model was advanced by Ernest Tupes and Raymond Christal in 1961 12 but failed to reach an academic audience until the 1980s In 1990 J M Digman advanced his five factor model of personality which Lewis Goldberg extended to the highest level of organization 14 These five overarching domains have been found to contain and subsume most known personality traits and are assumed to represent the basic structure behind all personality traits 15 At least four sets of researchers have worked independently within lexical hypothesis in personality theory for decades on this problem and have identified generally the same five factors Tupes and Christal were first followed by Goldberg at the Oregon Research Institute 16 17 18 19 20 Cattell at the University of Illinois 11 21 22 23 and Costa and McCrae 24 25 26 27 These four sets of researchers used somewhat different methods in finding the five traits and thus each set of five factors has somewhat different names and definitions However all have been found to be highly inter correlated and factor analytically aligned 28 29 30 31 32 Studies indicate that the Big Five traits are not nearly as powerful in predicting and explaining actual behavior as are the more numerous facets or primary traits 33 34 Each of the Big Five personality traits contains two separate but correlated aspects reflecting a level of personality below the broad domains but above the many facet scales that are also part of the Big Five 35 The aspects are labeled as follows Volatility and Withdrawal for Neuroticism Enthusiasm and Assertiveness for Extraversion Intellect and Openness for Openness to Experience Industriousness and Orderliness for Conscientiousness and Compassion and Politeness for Agreeableness 35 People who do not exhibit a clear predisposition to a single factor in each dimension above are considered adaptable moderate and reasonable yet they can also be perceived as unprincipled inscrutable and calculating 36 Descriptions of the particular personality traits EditOpenness to experience Edit Openness to experience is a general appreciation for art emotion adventure unusual ideas imagination curiosity and variety of experience People who are open to experience are intellectually curious open to emotion sensitive to beauty and willing to try new things They tend to be when compared to closed people more creative and more aware of their feelings They are also more likely to hold unconventional beliefs High openness can be perceived as unpredictability or lack of focus and more likely to engage in risky behavior or drug taking 37 Moreover individuals with high openness are said to pursue self actualization specifically by seeking out intense euphoric experiences Conversely those with low openness seek to gain fulfillment through perseverance and are characterized as pragmatic and data driven sometimes even perceived to be dogmatic and closed minded Some disagreement remains about how to interpret and contextualize the openness factor as there is a lack of biological support for this particular trait Openness has not shown a significant association with any brain regions as opposed to the other four traits which did when using brain imaging to detect changes in volume associated with each trait 38 Sample items Edit I have a rich vocabulary I have a vivid imagination I have excellent ideas I am quick to understand things I use difficult words I spend time reflecting on things I am full of ideas I have difficulty understanding abstract ideas Reversed I am not interested in abstract ideas Reversed I do not have a good imagination Reversed 39 Conscientiousness Edit Conscientiousness is a tendency to display self discipline act dutifully and strive for achievement against measures or outside expectations It is related to the way in which people control regulate and direct their impulses High conscientiousness is often perceived as being stubborn and focused Low conscientiousness is associated with flexibility and spontaneity but can also appear as sloppiness and lack of reliability 36 High scores on conscientiousness indicate a preference for planned rather than spontaneous behavior 40 The average level of conscientiousness rises among young adults and then declines among older adults 41 Sample items Edit I am always prepared I pay attention to details I get chores done right away I like order I follow a schedule I am exacting in my work I leave my belongings around Reversed I make a mess of things Reversed I often forget to put things back in their proper place Reversed I shirk my duties Reversed 39 Extraversion Edit Extraversion is characterized by breadth of activities as opposed to depth surgency from external activity situations and energy creation from external means 42 The trait is marked by pronounced engagement with the external world Extraverts enjoy interacting with people and are often perceived as full of energy They tend to be enthusiastic action oriented individuals They possess high group visibility like to talk and assert themselves Extraverted people may appear more dominant in social settings as opposed to introverted people in this setting 43 Introverts have lower social engagement and energy levels than extraverts They tend to seem quiet low key deliberate and less involved in the social world Their lack of social involvement should not be interpreted as shyness or depression instead they are more independent of their social world than extraverts Introverts need less stimulation and more time alone than extraverts This does not mean that they are unfriendly or antisocial rather they are reserved in social situations 1 Generally people are a combination of extraversion and introversion with personality psychologist Hans Eysenck suggesting a model by which individual neurological differences produce these traits 43 106 Sample items Edit I am the life of the party I feel comfortable around people I start conversations I talk to a lot of different people at parties I do not mind being the center of attention I do not talk a lot Reversed I keep in the background Reversed I have little to say Reversed I do not like to draw attention to myself Reversed I am quiet around strangers Reversed 39 Agreeableness Edit The agreeableness trait reflects individual differences in general concern for social harmony Agreeable individuals value getting along with others They are generally considerate kind generous trusting and trustworthy helpful and willing to compromise their interests with others 1 Agreeable people also have an optimistic view of human nature Disagreeable individuals place self interest above getting along with others They are generally unconcerned with others well being and are less likely to extend themselves for other people Sometimes their skepticism about others motives causes them to be suspicious unfriendly and uncooperative 44 Low agreeableness personalities are often competitive or challenging people which can be seen as argumentative or untrustworthy 36 Because agreeableness is a social trait research has shown that one s agreeableness positively correlates with the quality of relationships with one s team members Agreeableness also positively predicts transformational leadership skills In a study conducted among 169 participants in leadership positions in a variety of professions individuals were asked to take a personality test and have two evaluations completed by directly supervised subordinates Leaders with high levels of agreeableness were more likely to be considered transformational rather than transactional Although the relationship was not strong r 0 32 b 0 28 p lt 0 01 it was the strongest of the Big Five traits However the same study showed no predictive power of leadership effectiveness as evaluated by the leader s direct supervisor 45 Conversely agreeableness has been found to be negatively related to transactional leadership in the military A study of Asian military units showed leaders with a high level of agreeableness to be more likely to receive a low rating for transformational leadership skills 46 Therefore with further research organizations may be able to determine an individual s potential for performance based on their personality traits For instance 47 in their journal article Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace Paul Sackett and Philip Walmsley claim that conscientiousness and agreeableness are important to success across many different jobs Sample items Edit I am interested in people I sympathize with others feelings I have a soft heart I take time out for others I feel others emotions I make people feel at ease I am not really interested in others Reversed I insult people Reversed I am not interested in other people s problems Reversed I feel little concern for others Reversed 39 Neuroticism Edit Neuroticism is the tendency to experience negative emotions such as anger anxiety or depression 48 It is sometimes called emotional instability or is reversed and referred to as emotional stability According to Hans Eysenck s 1967 theory of personality neuroticism is interlinked with low tolerance for stress or aversive stimuli 49 Neuroticism is a classic temperament trait that has been studied in temperament research for decades before it was adapted by the Five Factors Model 50 Those who score high in neuroticism are emotionally reactive and vulnerable to stress They are more likely to interpret ordinary situations as threatening They can perceive minor frustrations as hopelessly difficult They also tend to be flippant in the way they express emotions Their negative emotional reactions tend to persist for unusually long periods of time which means they are often in a bad mood For instance neuroticism is connected to a pessimistic approach toward work to certainty that work impedes personal relationships and to higher levels of anxiety from the pressures at work 51 Furthermore those who score high on neuroticism may display more skin conductance reactivity than those who score low on neuroticism 49 52 These problems in emotional regulation can diminish the ability of a person scoring high on neuroticism to think clearly make decisions and cope effectively with stress Lacking contentment in one s life achievements can correlate with high neuroticism scores and increase one s likelihood of falling into clinical depression Moreover individuals high in neuroticism tend to experience more negative life events 48 53 but neuroticism also changes in response to positive and negative life experiences 48 53 Also individuals with higher levels of neuroticism tend to have worse psychological well being 54 At the other end of the scale individuals who score low in neuroticism are less easily upset and are less emotionally reactive They tend to be calm emotionally stable and free from persistent negative feelings Freedom from negative feelings does not mean that low scorers experience a lot of positive feelings 55 Neuroticism is similar but not identical to being neurotic in the Freudian sense i e neurosis Some psychologists who prefer to call neuroticism by the term emotional instability to differentiate it from the term neurotic in a career test Sample items Edit I get stressed out easily I worry about things I am easily disturbed I get upset easily I change my mood a lot I have frequent mood swings I get irritated easily I often feel blue I am relaxed most of the time Reversed I seldom feel blue Reversed 39 History EditEarly trait research Edit Historically preceding the Big Five personality traits B5 or the Five Factors Model FFM was Hippocrates s four types of temperament sanguine phlegmatic choleric and melancholic The sanguine type is most closely related to emotional stability and extraversion the phlegmatic type is also stable but introverted the choleric type is unstable and extraverted and the melancholic type is unstable and introverted 56 In 1884 Sir Francis Galton was the first person who is known to have investigated the hypothesis that it is possible to derive a comprehensive taxonomy of human personality traits by sampling language the lexical hypothesis 9 In 1936 Gordon Allport and S Odbert put Sir Francis Galton s hypothesis into practice by extracting 4 504 adjectives which they believed were descriptive of observable and relatively permanent traits from the dictionaries at that time 57 In 1940 Raymond Cattell retained the adjectives and eliminated synonyms to reduce the total to 171 11 He constructed a self report instrument for the clusters of personality traits he found from the adjectives which he called the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire In 1949 the first systematic multivariate research of personality was conducted by Joy P Guilford Guilford analyzed ten factors of personality which he measured by the Guilford Zimmerman Temperament Survey These scales included general activity energy vs inactivity restraint seriousness vs impulsiveness ascendance social boldness vs submissiveness sociability social interest vs shyness emotional stability evenness vs fluctuation of mood objectivity thick skinned vs hypersensitive friendliness agreeableness vs belligerence thoughtfulness reflective vs disconnected personal relations tolerance vs hypercritical masculinity hard boiled vs sympathetic 56 These overlapping scales were later further analyzed by Guilford et al and condensed into three dimensions social activity general activity ascendence sociability introversion extraversion restraint thoughtfulness and emotional health emotional stability objectivity friendliness personal relations 56 Based on a subset of only 20 of the 36 dimensions that Cattell had originally discovered Ernest Tupes and Raymond Christal 1961 claimed to have found just five broad factors which they labeled surgency agreeableness dependability emotional stability and culture 12 Warren Norman subsequently relabeled dependability as conscientiousness 13 Hiatus in research Edit During the late 1960s to 1970s the changing zeitgeist made publication of personality research difficult In his 1968 book Personality and Assessment Walter Mischel asserted that personality instruments could not predict behavior with a correlation of more than 0 3 Social psychologists like Mischel argued that attitudes and behavior were not stable but varied with the situation Predicting behavior from personality instruments was claimed to be impossible by whom Renewed attention Edit The paradigm shift back to acceptance of the five factor model came in the early 1980s In a 1980 symposium in Honolulu four prominent researchers Lewis Goldberg Naomi Takemoto Chock Andrew Comrey and John M Digman reviewed the available personality instruments of the day 58 This event was followed by widespread acceptance of the five factor model among personality researchers during the 1980s By 1983 experiments had demonstrated that the predictions of personality models correlated better with real life behavior under stressful emotional conditions as opposed to typical survey administration under neutral emotional conditions 59 Peter Saville and his team included the five factor Pentagon model with the original Occupational Personality Questionnaires OPQ in 1984 The Pentagon model was closely followed by the NEO PI R NEO five factor personality inventory published by Costa and McCrae in 1985 However the methodology employed in constructing the NEO instrument has been subject to critical scrutiny see section below 60 431 33 Emerging methodologies increasingly confirmed personality theories during the 1980s Though generally failing to predict single instances of behavior researchers found that they could predict patterns of behavior by aggregating large numbers of observations 61 As a result correlations between personality and behavior increased substantially and it was clear that personality did in fact exist 62 Personality and social psychologists now generally agree that both personal and situational variables are needed to account for human behavior 63 Trait theories amassed favorable evidence and there was a resurgence of interest in this area 64 In the 1980s Lewis Goldberg started his own lexical project again emphasizing five broad factors 65 which he later labeled the Big Five In 2007 Colin G DeYoung Lena C Quilty and Jordan B Peterson concluded that the 10 aspects of the Big Five may have distinct biological substrates 66 This was derived through factor analyses of two data samples with the International Personality Item Pool L R Goldberg 1999 67 followed by cross correlation with scores derived from 10 genetic factors identified as underlying the shared variance among the Revised NEO Personality Inventory facets K L Jang et al 2002 68 Colin G DeYoung et al 2016 researched the Big Five model and how the five broad factors are compatible with the 25 scales of the Personality Inventory PID 5 for the DSM 5 DeYoung et al considers the PID 5 to measure facet level traits 69 Because the Big Five factors are broader than the 25 scales of the PID 5 there is disagreement in personality psychology relating to the number of factors within the Big Five According to DeYoung et al 2016 the number of valid facets might be limited only by the number of traits that can be shown to have discriminant validity 69 The FFM associated test was used by Cambridge Analytica and was part of the psychographic profiling 70 controversy during the 2016 US presidential election 71 72 Biological and developmental factors EditThere of course are factors that influence a personality and these are called the determinants of personality These factors determine the traits which a person develops in the course of development from a child Temperament vis a vis personality Edit There are debates between researchers of temperament and researchers of personality as to whether or not biologically based differences define a concept of temperament or a part of personality The presence of such differences in pre cultural individuals such as animals or young infants suggests that they belong to temperament since personality is a socio cultural concept For this reason developmental psychologists generally interpret individual differences in children as an expression of temperament rather than personality 73 Some researchers argue that temperaments and personality traits are age specific manifestations of virtually the same latent qualities 74 75 Some believe that early childhood temperaments may become adolescent and adult personality traits as individuals basic genetic characteristics actively reactively and passively interact with their changing environments 73 74 76 clarification needed Researchers of adult temperament point out that similarly to sex age and mental illness temperament is based on biochemical systems whereas personality is a product of socialization of an individual possessing these four types of features Temperament interacts with social cultural factors but still cannot be controlled or easily changed by these factors 77 78 79 80 Therefore it is suggested that temperament should be kept as an independent concept for further studies and not be conflated with personality 81 Moreover temperament refers to dynamical features of behavior energetic tempo sensitivity and emotionality related whereas personality is to be considered a psycho social construct comprising the content characteristics of human behavior such as values attitudes habits preferences personal history self image 78 79 80 Temperament researchers point out that the lack of attention to extant temperament research by the developers of the Big Five model led to an overlap between its dimensions and dimensions described in multiple temperament models much earlier For example neuroticism reflects the traditional temperament dimension of emotionality extraversion the temperament dimension of energy or activity and openness to experience the temperament dimension of sensation seeking 80 82 Heritability Edit Personality research often uses twin studies to determine how much heritable and environmental factors contribute to the Big Five personality traits A 1996 behavioural genetics study of twins suggested that heritability and environmental factors both influence all five factors to the same degree 83 Among four twin studies examined in 2003 the mean percentage for heritability was calculated for each personality and it was concluded that heritability influenced the five factors broadly The self report measures were as follows openness to experience was estimated to have a 57 genetic influence extraversion 54 conscientiousness 49 neuroticism 48 and agreeableness 42 84 Non humans Edit The Big 5 personality traits can be seen in chimpanzees The Big Five personality traits have been assessed in some non human species but methodology is debatable In one series of studies human ratings of chimpanzees using the Hominoid Personality Questionnaire revealed factors of extraversion conscientiousness and agreeableness as well as an additional factor of dominance across hundreds of chimpanzees in zoological parks a large naturalistic sanctuary and a research laboratory Neuroticism and openness factors were found in an original zoo sample but were not replicated in a new zoo sample or in other settings perhaps reflecting the design of the CPQ 85 A study review found that markers for the three dimensions extraversion neuroticism and agreeableness were found most consistently across different species followed by openness only chimpanzees showed markers for conscientious behavior 86 A study completed in 2020 concluded that dolphins have some similar personality traits to humans Both are large brained intelligent animals but have evolved separately for millions of years 87 Development during childhood and adolescence Edit Research on the Big Five and personality in general has focused primarily on individual differences in adulthood rather than in childhood and adolescence and often include temperament traits 73 74 76 Recently there has been growing recognition of the need to study child and adolescent personality trait development in order to understand how traits develop and change throughout the lifespan 88 Recent studies have begun to explore the developmental origins and trajectories of the Big Five among children and adolescents especially those that relate to temperament 73 74 76 Many researchers have sought to distinguish between personality and temperament 89 Temperament often refers to early behavioral and affective characteristics that are thought to be driven primarily by genes 89 Models of temperament often include four trait dimensions surgency sociability negative emotionality persistence effortful control and activity level 89 Some of these differences in temperament are evident at if not before birth 73 74 For example both parents and researchers recognize that some newborn infants are peaceful and easily soothed while others are comparatively fussy and hard to calm 74 Unlike temperament however many researchers view the development of personality as gradually occurring throughout childhood 89 Contrary to some researchers who question whether children have stable personality traits Big Five or otherwise 90 most researchers contend that there are significant psychological differences between children that are associated with relatively stable distinct and salient behavior patterns 73 74 76 The structure manifestations and development of the Big Five in childhood and adolescence have been studied using a variety of methods including parent and teacher ratings 91 92 93 preadolescent and adolescent self and peer ratings 94 95 96 and observations of parent child interactions 76 Results from these studies support the relative stability of personality traits across the human lifespan at least from preschool age through adulthood 74 76 97 98 More specifically research suggests that four of the Big Five namely Extraversion Neuroticism Conscientiousness and Agreeableness reliably describe personality differences in childhood adolescence and adulthood 74 76 97 98 However some evidence suggests that Openness may not be a fundamental stable part of childhood personality Although some researchers have found that Openness in children and adolescents relates to attributes such as creativity curiosity imagination and intellect 99 many researchers have failed to find distinct individual differences in Openness in childhood and early adolescence 74 76 Potentially Openness may a manifest in unique currently unknown ways in childhood or b may only manifest as children develop socially and cognitively 74 76 Other studies have found evidence for all of the Big Five traits in childhood and adolescence as well as two other child specific traits Irritability and Activity 100 Despite these specific differences the majority of findings suggest that personality traits particularly Extraversion Neuroticism Conscientiousness and Agreeableness are evident in childhood and adolescence and are associated with distinct social emotional patterns of behavior that are largely consistent with adult manifestations of those same personality traits 74 76 97 98 Some researchers have proposed the youth personality trait is best described by six trait dimensions neuroticism extraversion openness to experience agreeableness conscientiousness and activity 101 Despite some preliminary evidence for this Little Six model 89 101 research in this area has been delayed by a lack of available measures Previous research has found evidence that most adults become more agreeable conscientious and less neurotic as they age 102 This has been referred to as the maturation effect 75 Many researchers have sought to investigate how trends in adult personality development compare to trends in youth personality development 101 Two main population level indices have been important in this area of research rank order consistency and mean level consistency Rank order consistency indicates the relative placement of individuals within a group 103 Mean level consistency indicates whether groups increase or decrease on certain traits throughout the lifetime 102 Findings from these studies indicate that consistent with adult personality trends youth personality becomes increasingly more stable in terms of rank order throughout childhood 101 Unlike adult personality research which indicates that people become agreeable conscientious and emotionally stable with age 102 some findings in youth personality research have indicated that mean levels of agreeableness conscientiousness and openness to experience decline from late childhood to late adolescence 101 The disruption hypothesis which proposes that biological social and psychological changes experienced during youth result in temporary dips in maturity has been proposed to explain these findings 89 101 Extraversion positive emotionality Edit In Big Five studies extraversion has been associated with surgency 73 Children with high Extraversion are energetic talkative social and dominant with children and adults whereas children with low Extraversion tend to be quiet calm inhibited and submissive to other children and adults 74 Individual differences in Extraversion first manifest in infancy as varying levels of positive emotionality 104 These differences in turn predict social and physical activity during later childhood and may represent or be associated with the behavioral activation system 73 74 In children Extraversion Positive Emotionality includes four sub traits three traits that are similar to the previously described traits of temperament activity sociability shyness 105 50 and the trait of dominance Activity Similarly to findings in temperament research children with high activity tend to have high energy levels and more intense and frequent motor activity compared to their peers 74 91 106 Salient differences in activity reliably manifest in infancy persist through adolescence and fade as motor activity decreases in adulthood 107 or potentially develops into talkativeness 74 108 Dominance Children with high dominance tend to influence the behavior of others particularly their peers to obtain desirable rewards or outcomes 74 109 110 Such children are generally skilled at organizing activities and games 111 and deceiving others by controlling their nonverbal behavior 112 Shyness Children with high shyness are generally socially withdrawn nervous and inhibited around strangers 74 In time such children may become fearful even around known others especially if their peers reject them 74 113 Similar pattern was described in temperament longitudinal studies of shyness 50 Sociability Children with high sociability generally prefer to be with others rather than alone 74 114 During middle childhood the distinction between low sociability and high shyness becomes more pronounced particularly as children gain greater control over how and where they spend their time 74 115 116 Development throughout adulthood Edit Many studies of longitudinal data which correlate people s test scores over time and cross sectional data which compare personality levels across different age groups show a high degree of stability in personality traits during adulthood especially Neuroticism that is often regarded as a temperament trait 117 similarly to longitudinal research in temperament for the same traits 50 It is shown that the personality stabilizes for working age individuals within about four years after starting working There is also little evidence that adverse life events can have any significant impact on the personality of individuals 118 More recent research and meta analyses of previous studies however indicate that change occurs in all five traits at various points in the lifespan The new research shows evidence for a maturation effect On average levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness typically increase with time whereas extraversion neuroticism and openness tend to decrease 119 Research has also demonstrated that changes in Big Five personality traits depend on the individual s current stage of development For example levels of agreeableness and conscientiousness demonstrate a negative trend during childhood and early adolescence before trending upwards during late adolescence and into adulthood 88 In addition to these group effects there are individual differences different people demonstrate unique patterns of change at all stages of life 120 In addition some research Fleeson 2001 suggests that the Big Five should not be conceived of as dichotomies such as extraversion vs introversion but as continua Each individual has the capacity to move along each dimension as circumstances social or temporal change He is or she is therefore not simply on one end of each trait dichotomy but is a blend of both exhibiting some characteristics more often than others 121 Research regarding personality with growing age has suggested that as individuals enter their elder years 79 86 those with lower IQ see a raise in extraversion but a decline in conscientiousness and physical well being 122 Group differences EditGender differences Edit Some cross cultural research has shown some patterns of gender differences on responses to the NEO PI R and the Big Five Inventory 123 124 For example women consistently report higher Neuroticism Agreeableness warmth an extraversion facet and openness to feelings and men often report higher assertiveness a facet of extraversion and openness to ideas as assessed by the NEO PI R 125 A study of gender differences in 55 nations using the Big Five Inventory found that women tended to be somewhat higher than men in neuroticism extraversion agreeableness and conscientiousness The difference in neuroticism was the most prominent and consistent with significant differences found in 49 of the 55 nations surveyed 126 Gender differences in personality traits are largest in prosperous healthy and more gender egalitarian nations The explanation for this given by the researchers of a 2001 paper is that acts by women in individualistic egalitarian countries are more likely to be attributed to their personality rather than being attributed to ascribed gender roles within collectivist traditional countries 125 Measured differences in the magnitude of sex differences between more or less developed world regions were caused by the changes in the measured personalities of men not women in these respective regions That is men in highly developed world regions were less neurotic less extraverted less conscientious and less agreeable compared to men in less developed world regions Women on the other hand tended not to differ in personality traits across regions 126 The authors of this 2008 study speculated that resource poor environments that is countries with low levels of development may inhibit the development of gender differences whereas resource rich environments facilitate them This may be because males require more resources than females in order to reach their full personality potential of less conscientious less agreeable less neurotic and less extraverted The authors also speculated in their discussion that due to different evolutionary pressures men may have evolved to be more risk taking and socially dominant whereas women evolved to be more cautious and nurturing The authors further posited that ancient hunter gatherer societies may have been more egalitarian than later agriculturally oriented societies Hence the development of gender inequalities may have acted to constrain the development of gender differences in personality that originally evolved in hunter gatherer societies As modern societies have become more egalitarian again it may be that innate sex differences are no longer constrained and hence manifest more fully than in less wealthy cultures 126 This is one interpretation of the results among other possible interpretations Birth order differences Edit Main article Birth order Frank Sulloway argues that firstborns are more conscientious more socially dominant less agreeable and less open to new ideas compared to siblings that were born later Large scale studies using random samples and self report personality tests however have found milder effects than Sulloway claimed or no significant effects of birth order on personality 127 128 A study using the Project Talent data which is a large scale representative survey of American high school students with 272 003 eligible participants found statistically significant but very small effects the average absolute correlation between birth order and personality was 02 of birth order on personality such that firstborns were slightly more conscientious dominant and agreeable while also being less neurotic and less sociable 129 Parental socioeconomic status and participant gender had much larger correlations with personality In 2002 the Journal of Psychology posted a Big Five Personality Trait Difference where researchers explored the relationship between the five factor model and the Universal Diverse Orientation UDO in counselor trainees Thompson R Brossart D and Mivielle A 2002 UDO is known as one social attitude that produces a strong awareness and or acceptance towards the similarities and differences among individuals Miville M Romas J Johnson J and Lon R 2002 The study found that the counselor trainees that are more open to the idea of creative expression a facet of Openness to Experience Openness to Aesthetics among individuals are more likely to work with a diverse group of clients and feel comfortable in their role 130 Cultural differences EditMain article Big Five personality traits and culture The Big Five have been pursued in a variety of languages and cultures such as German 131 Chinese 132 and Indian 133 134 For example Thompson has claimed to find the Big Five structure across several cultures using an international English language scale 135 Cheung van de Vijver and Leong 2011 suggest however that the Openness factor is particularly unsupported in Asian countries and that a different fifth factor is identified 136 Recent work has found relationships between Geert Hofstede s cultural factors Individualism Power Distance Masculinity and Uncertainty Avoidance with the average Big Five scores in a country 137 For instance the degree to which a country values individualism correlates with its average extraversion whereas people living in cultures which are accepting of large inequalities in their power structures tend to score somewhat higher on conscientiousness 138 139 Personality differences around the world might even have contributed to the emergence of different political systems A recent study has found that countries average personality trait levels are correlated with their political systems countries with higher average trait Openness tended to have more democratic institutions an association that held even after factoring out other relevant influences such as economic development 140 Attempts to replicate the Big Five in other countries with local dictionaries have succeeded in some countries but not in others Apparently for instance Hungarians do not appear to have a single agreeableness factor 141 Other researchers have found evidence for agreeableness but not for other factors 142 It is important to recognize that individual differences in traits are relevant in a specific cultural context and that the traits do not have their effects outside of that context 43 189 Health EditPersonality change from disease Edit Some diseases cause changes in personality For example although gradual memory impairment is the hallmark feature of Alzheimer s disease a systematic review of personality changes in Alzheimer s disease by Robins Wahlin and Byrne published in 2011 found systematic and consistent trait changes mapped to the Big Five The largest change observed was a decrease in conscientiousness The next most significant changes were an increase in Neuroticism and decrease in Extraversion but Openness and Agreeableness were also decreased These changes in personality could assist with early diagnosis 143 Personality disorders Edit Main article Personality disorders As of 2002 update there were over fifty published studies relating the FFM to personality disorders 144 Since that time quite a number of additional studies have expanded on this research base and provided further empirical support for understanding the DSM personality disorders in terms of the FFM domains 145 In her review of the personality disorder literature published in 2007 Lee Anna Clark asserted that the five factor model of personality is widely accepted as representing the higher order structure of both normal and abnormal personality traits 146 However other researchers disagree that this model is widely accepted see the section Critique below and suggest that it simply replicates early temperament research 80 147 Noticeably FFM publications never compare their findings to temperament models even though temperament and mental disorders especially personality disorders are thought to be based on the same neurotransmitter imbalances just to varying degrees 80 148 149 150 The five factor model was claimed to significantly predict all ten personality disorder symptoms and outperform the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory MMPI in the prediction of borderline avoidant and dependent personality disorder symptoms 151 However most predictions related to an increase in Neuroticism and a decrease in Agreeableness and therefore did not differentiate between the disorders very well 152 Common mental disorders Edit Average deviation of five factor personality profile of heroin users from the population mean 153 N stands for Neuroticism E for Extraversion O for Openness to experience A for Agreeableness and C for Conscientiousness Converging evidence from several nationally representative studies has established three classes of mental disorders which are especially common in the general population Depressive disorders e g major depressive disorder MDD dysthymic disorder 154 anxiety disorders e g generalized anxiety disorder GAD post traumatic stress disorder PTSD panic disorder agoraphobia specific phobia and social phobia 154 and substance use disorders SUDs 155 156 The Five Factor personality profiles of users of different drugs may be different 157 For example the typical profile for heroin users is N O A C displaystyle rm N Uparrow rm O Uparrow rm A Downarrow rm C Downarrow whereas for ecstasy users the high level of N is not expected but E is higher E O A C displaystyle rm E Uparrow rm O Uparrow rm A Downarrow rm C Downarrow 157 These common mental disorders CMDs have been empirically linked to the Big Five personality traits neuroticism in particular Numerous studies have found that having high scores of neuroticism significantly increases one s risk for developing a common mental disorder 158 159 A large scale meta analysis n gt 75 000 examining the relationship between all of the Big Five personality traits and common mental disorders found that low conscientiousness yielded consistently strong effects for each common mental disorder examined i e MDD dysthymic disorder GAD PTSD panic disorder agoraphobia social phobia specific phobia and SUD 160 This finding parallels research on physical health which has established that conscientiousness is the strongest personality predictor of reduced mortality and is highly negatively correlated with making poor health choices 161 162 In regards to the other personality domains the meta analysis found that all common mental disorders examined were defined by high neuroticism most exhibited low extraversion only SUD was linked to agreeableness negatively and no disorders were associated with Openness 160 A meta analysis of 59 longitudinal studies showed that high neuroticism predicted the development of anxiety depression substance abuse psychosis schizophrenia and non specific mental distress also after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history 163 The personality psychopathology models Edit Five major models have been posed to explain the nature of the relationship between personality and mental illness There is currently no single best model as each of them has received at least some empirical support It is also important to note that these models are not mutually exclusive more than one may be operating for a particular individual and various mental disorders may be explained by different models 163 164 The Vulnerability Risk Model According to this model personality contributes to the onset or etiology of various common mental disorders In other words pre existing personality traits either cause the development of CMDs directly or enhance the impact of causal risk factors 160 165 166 167 There is strong support for neuroticism being a robust vulnerability factor 163 The Pathoplasty Model This model proposes that premorbid personality traits impact the expression course severity and or treatment response of a mental disorder 160 166 6 An example of this relationship would be a heightened likelihood of committing suicide in a depressed individual who also has low levels of constraint 166 The Common Cause Model According to the common cause model personality traits are predictive of CMDs because personality and psychopathology have shared genetic and environmental determinants which result in non causal associations between the two constructs 160 165 The Spectrum Model This model proposes that associations between personality and psychopathology are found because these two constructs both occupy a single domain or spectrum and psychopathology is simply a display of the extremes of normal personality function 160 165 166 167 Support for this model is provided by an issue of criterion overlap For instance two of the primary facet scales of neuroticism in the NEO PI R are depression and anxiety Thus the fact that diagnostic criteria for depression anxiety and neuroticism assess the same content increases the correlations between these domains 167 The Scar Model According to the scar model episodes of a mental disorder scar an individual s personality changing it in significant ways from premorbid functioning 160 165 166 167 An example of a scar effect would be a decrease in openness to experience following an episode of PTSD 166 Physical health Edit To examine how the Big Five personality traits are related to subjective health outcomes positive and negative mood physical symptoms and general health concern and objective health conditions chronic illness serious illness and physical injuries Jasna Hudek Knezevic and Igor Kardum conducted a study from a sample of 822 healthy volunteers 438 women and 384 men 168 Out of the Big Five personality traits they found neuroticism most related to worse subjective health outcomes and optimistic control to better subjective health outcomes When relating to objective health conditions connections drawn were presented weak except that neuroticism significantly predicted chronic illness whereas optimistic control was more closely related to physical injuries caused by accident 168 Being highly conscientious may add as much as five years to one s life vague 162 The Big Five personality traits also predict positive health outcomes 169 170 In an elderly Japanese sample conscientiousness extraversion and openness were related to lower risk of mortality 171 Higher conscientiousness is associated with lower obesity risk In already obese individuals higher conscientiousness is associated with a higher likelihood of becoming non obese over a five year period 172 Effect of personality traits through life EditEducation Edit Academic achievement Edit Personality plays an important role in academic achievement A study of 308 undergraduates who completed the Five Factor Inventory Processes and reported their GPA suggested that conscientiousness and agreeableness have a positive relationship with all types of learning styles synthesis analysis methodical study fact retention and elaborative processing whereas neuroticism shows an inverse relationship Moreover extraversion and openness were proportional to elaborative processing The Big Five personality traits accounted for 14 of the variance in GPA suggesting that personality traits make some contributions to academic performance Furthermore reflective learning styles synthesis analysis and elaborative processing were able to mediate the relationship between openness and GPA These results indicate that intellectual curiosity significantly enhances academic performance if students combine their scholarly interest with thoughtful information processing 173 A recent study of Israeli high school students found that those in the gifted program systematically scored higher on openness and lower on neuroticism than those not in the gifted program While not a measure of the Big Five gifted students also reported less state anxiety than students not in the gifted program 174 Specific Big Five personality traits predict learning styles in addition to academic success GPA and exam performance are both predicted by conscientiousness neuroticism is negatively related to academic success openness predicts utilizing synthesis analysis and elaborative processing learning styles neuroticism negatively correlates with learning styles in general openness and extraversion both predict all four learning styles 173 Studies conducted on college students have concluded that hope which is linked to agreeableness 175 conscientiousness neuroticism and openness 175 has a positive effect on psychological well being Individuals high in neurotic tendencies are less likely to display hopeful tendencies and are negatively associated with well being 176 Personality can sometimes be flexible and measuring the big five personality for individuals as they enter certain stages of life may predict their educational identity Recent studies have suggested the likelihood of an individual s personality affecting their educational identity 177 Learning styles Edit Learning styles have been described as enduring ways of thinking and processing information 173 In 2008 the Association for Psychological Science APS commissioned a report that concludes that no significant evidence exists that learning style assessments should be included in the education system 178 Thus it is premature at best to conclude that the evidence links the Big Five to learning styles or learning styles to learning itself However the APS report also suggested that all existing learning styles have not been exhausted and that there could exist learning styles worthy of being included in educational practices There are studies that conclude that personality and thinking styles may be intertwined in ways that link thinking styles to the Big Five personality traits 179 There is no general consensus on the number or specifications of particular learning styles but there have been many different proposals As one example Schmeck Ribich and Ramanaiah 1997 defined four types of learning styles 180 synthesis analysis methodical study fact retention elaborative processingWhen all four facets are implicated within the classroom they will each likely improve academic achievement 173 This model asserts that students develop either agentic shallow processing or reflective deep processing Deep processors are more often found to be more conscientious intellectually open and extraverted than shallow processors Deep processing is associated with appropriate study methods methodical study and a stronger ability to analyze information synthesis analysis whereas shallow processors prefer structured fact retention learning styles and are better suited for elaborative processing 173 The main functions of these four specific learning styles are as follows Name FunctionSynthesis analysis processing information forming categories and organizing them into hierarchies This is the only one of the learning styles that has explained a significant impact on academic performance 173 Methodical study methodical behavior while completing academic assignmentsFact retention focusing on the actual result instead of understanding the logic behind somethingElaborative processing connecting and applying new ideas to existing knowledgeOpenness has been linked to learning styles that often lead to academic success and higher grades like synthesis analysis and methodical study Because conscientiousness and openness have been shown to predict all four learning styles it suggests that individuals who possess characteristics like discipline determination and curiosity are more likely to engage in all of the above learning styles 173 According to the research carried out by Komarraju Karau Schmeck amp Avdic 2011 conscientiousness and agreeableness are positively related with all four learning styles whereas neuroticism was negatively related with those four Furthermore extraversion and openness were only positively related to elaborative processing and openness itself correlated with higher academic achievement 173 In addition a previous study by psychologist Mikael Jensen has shown relationships between the Big Five personality traits learning and academic achievement According to Jensen all personality traits except neuroticism are associated with learning goals and motivation Openness and conscientiousness influence individuals to learn to a high degree unrecognized while extraversion and agreeableness have similar effects 181 Conscientiousness and neuroticism also influence individuals to perform well in front of others for a sense of credit and reward while agreeableness forces individuals to avoid this strategy of learning 181 Jensen s study concludes that individuals who score high on the agreeableness trait will likely learn just to perform well in front of others 181 Besides openness all Big Five personality traits helped predict the educational identity of students Based on these findings scientists are beginning to see that the Big Five traits might have a large influence of on academic motivation that leads to predicting a student s academic performance 177 Some authors suggested that Big Five personality traits combined with learning styles can help predict some variations in the academic performance and the academic motivation of an individual which can then influence their academic achievements 182 This may be seen because individual differences in personality represent stable approaches to information processing For instance conscientiousness has consistently emerged as a stable predictor of success in exam performance largely because conscientious students experience fewer study delays 177 Conscientiousness shows a positive association with the four learning styles because students with high levels of conscientiousness develop focused learning strategies and appear to be more disciplined and achievement oriented Personality and learning styles are both likely to play significant roles in influencing academic achievement College students 308 undergraduates completed the Five Factor Inventory and the Inventory of Learning Processes and reported their grade point average Two of the Big Five traits conscientiousness and agreeableness were positively related with all four learning styles synthesis analysis methodical study fact retention and elaborative processing whereas neuroticism was negatively related with all four learning styles In addition extraversion and openness were positively related with elaborative processing The Big Five together explained 14 of the variance in grade point average GPA and learning styles explained an additional 3 suggesting that both personality traits and learning styles contribute to academic performance Further the relationship between openness and GPA was mediated by reflective learning styles synthesis analysis and elaborative processing These latter results suggest that being intellectually curious fully enhances academic performance when students combine this scholarly interest with thoughtful information processing Implications of these results are discussed in the context of teaching techniques and curriculum design M Komarraju 173 Distance Learning Edit When the relationship between the five factor personality traits and academic achievement in distance education settings was examined in brief the openness personality trait was found to be the most important variable that has a positive relationship with academic achievement in distance education environments In addition it was found that self discipline extraversion and adaptability personality traits are generally in a positive relationship with academic achievement The most important personality trait that has a negative relationship with academic achievement has emerged as neuroticism The results generally show that individuals who are organized planned determined who are oriented to new ideas and independent thinking have increased success in distance education environments On the other hand it can be said that individuals with anxiety and stress tendencies generally have lower academic success 183 184 185 Employment Edit Occupation and personality fit Edit The Vocations Map many people in the same role share similar personality traits Researchers have long suggested that work is more likely to be fulfilling to the individual and beneficial to society when there is alignment between the person and their occupation 186 For instance software programmers and scientists were generally more open to experiencing a variety of new activities were intellectually curious tended to think in symbols and abstractions and found repetition boring 187 Work success Edit Controversy exists as to whether or not the Big 5 personality traits are correlated with success in the workplace It is believed that the Big Five traits are predictors of future performance outcomes Job outcome measures include job and training proficiency and personnel data 188 However research demonstrating such prediction has been criticized in part because of the apparently low correlation coefficients characterizing the relationship between personality and job performance In a 2007 article 189 co authored by six current or former editors of psychological journals Dr Kevin Murphy Professor of Psychology at Pennsylvania State University and Editor of the Journal of Applied Psychology 1996 2002 states The problem with personality tests is that the validity of personality measures as predictors of job performance is often disappointingly low The argument for using personality tests to predict performance does not strike me as convincing in the first place Such criticisms were put forward by Walter Mischel 190 whose publication caused a two decades long crisis in personality psychometrics However later work demonstrated 1 that the correlations obtained by psychometric personality researchers were actually very respectable by comparative standards 191 and 2 that the economic value of even incremental increases in prediction accuracy was exceptionally large given the vast difference in performance by those who occupy complex job positions 192 There have been studies that link national innovation to openness to experience and conscientiousness Those who express these traits have showed leadership and beneficial ideas towards the country of origin 193 Some businesses organizations and interviewers assess individuals based on the Big Five personality traits Research has suggested that individuals who are considered leaders typically exhibit lower amounts of neurotic traits maintain higher levels of openness envisioning success balanced levels of conscientiousness well organized and balanced levels of extraversion outgoing but not excessive 194 Further studies have linked professional burnout to neuroticism and extraversion to enduring positive work experience 195 When it comes to making money research has suggested that those who are high in agreeableness especially men are not as successful in accumulating income 196 Some research suggests that vocational outcomes are correlated to Big Five personality traits Conscientiousness predicts job performance in general Conscientiousness is considered as top ranked in overall job performance 47 research further categorized the Big 5 behaviors into 3 perspectives task performance organizational citizenship behavior and counterproductive work behavior Task performance is the set of activity that a worker is hired to complete and results showed that Extraversion ranked second after the Conscientiousness with Emotional Stability tied with Agreeableness ranked third For organizational citizenship behavior relatively less tied to the specific task core but benefits an organization by contributing to its social and psychological environment Agreeableness and Emotional Stability ranked second and third Lastly Agreeableness tied with Conscientiousness as top ranked for Counterproductive work behavior which refers to intentional behavior that is counter to the legitimate interests of the organization or its members 47 In addition research has demonstrated that agreeableness is negatively related to salary Those high in agreeableness make less on average than those low in the same trait Neuroticism is also negatively related to salary while conscientiousness and extraversion are positive predictors of salary 197 Occupational self efficacy has also been shown to be positively correlated with conscientiousness and negatively correlated with neuroticism Significant predictors of career advancement goals are extraversion conscientiousness and agreeableness 197 Some research has also suggested that the Conscientiousness of a supervisor is positively associated with an employee s perception of abusive supervision 198 While others have suggested that those with low agreeableness and high neuroticism are traits more related to abusive supervision 199 A 2019 study of Canadian adults found conscientiousness to be positively associated with wages while agreeableness extraversion and neuroticism were negatively associated with wages In the United States by contrast no negative correlation between extraversion and wages has been found Also the magnitudes found for agreeableness and conscientiousness in this study were higher for women than for men i e there was a higher negative penalty for greater agreeableness in women as well as a higher positive reward for greater conscientiousness 200 Research designed to investigate the individual effects of Big Five personality traits on work performance via worker completed surveys and supervisor ratings of work performance has implicated individual traits in several different work roles performances A work role is defined as the responsibilities an individual has while they are working Nine work roles have been identified which can be classified in three broader categories proficiency the ability of a worker to effectively perform their work duties adaptivity a workers ability to change working strategies in response to changing work environments and proactivity extent to which a worker will spontaneously put forth effort to change the work environment These three categories of behavior can then be directed towards three different levels either the individual team or organizational level leading to the nine different work role performance possibilities 201 Openness is positively related to proactivity at the individual and the organizational levels and is negatively related to team and organizational proficiency These effects were found to be completely independent of one another This is also counter conscientious and has a negative correlation to Conscientiousness 202 Agreeableness is negatively related to individual task proactivity Typically this is associated with lower career success and being less able to cope with conflict 202 Extraversion is negatively related to individual task proficiency Extraversion has a higher job and life satisfaction but more impulsive behaviors 202 Conscientiousness is positively related to all forms of work role performance This has a higher leadership effectiveness and lower deviance behaviors but also lower learning in skill acquisition 202 Neuroticism is negatively related to all forms of work role performance This has a trend to engage in more risky behaviors 202 201 Two theories have been integrated in an attempt to account for these differences in work role performance Trait activation theory posits that within a person trait levels predict future behavior that trait levels differ between people and that work related cues activate traits which leads to work relevant behaviors Role theory suggests that role senders provide cues to elicit desired behaviors In this context role senders i e supervisors managers etc provide workers with cues for expected behaviors which in turn activates personality traits and work relevant behaviors In essence expectations of the role sender lead to different behavioral outcomes depending on the trait levels of individual workers and because people differ in trait levels responses to these cues will not be universal 201 Romantic relationships Edit The Big Five model of personality was used for attempts to predict satisfaction in romantic relationships relationship quality in dating engaged and married couples 203 Dating couples Self reported relationship quality is negatively related to partner reported neuroticism and positively related to both self and partner reported conscientiousness 203 Engaged couples Self reported relationship quality was higher among those high in partner reported openness agreeableness and conscientiousness Self reported relationship quality was higher among those high in self reported extraversion and agreeableness Self reported relationship quality is negatively related to both self and partner reported neuroticism Observers rated the relationship quality higher if the participating partner s self reported extraversion was high 203 Married couples High self reported neuroticism extraversion and agreeableness are related to high levels of self reported relationship quality Partner reported agreeableness is related to observed relationship quality 203 These reports are however rare and not conclusive Political identification Edit The Big Five Personality Model also has applications in the study of political psychology Studies have been finding links between the big five personality traits and political identification It has been found by several studies that individuals who score high in Conscientiousness are more likely to possess a right wing political identification 204 205 206 On the opposite end of the spectrum a strong correlation was identified between high scores in Openness to Experience and a left leaning ideology 204 207 208 While the traits of agreeableness extraversion and neuroticism have not been consistently linked to either conservative or liberal ideology with studies producing mixed results such traits are promising when analyzing the strength of an individual s party identification 207 208 However correlations between the Big Five and political beliefs while present tend to be small with one study finding correlations ranged from 0 14 to 0 24 209 Scope of predictive power Edit The predictive effects of the Big Five personality traits relate mostly to social functioning and rules driven behavior and are not very specific for prediction of particular aspects of behavior For example it was noted that high neuroticism precedes the development of all common mental disorders 163 and is not associated with personality by all temperament researchers 81 Further evidence is required to fully uncover the nature and differences between personality traits temperament and life outcomes Social and contextual parameters also play a role in outcomes and the interaction between the two is not yet fully understood 210 Religiosity Edit Though the effect sizes are small Of the Big Five personality traits high Agreeableness Conscientiousness and Extraversion relate to general religiosity while Openness relate negatively to religious fundamentalism and positively to spirituality High Neuroticism may be related to extrinsic religiosity whereas intrinsic religiosity and spirituality reflect Emotional Stability 211 Measurements EditSeveral measures of the Big Five exist International Personality Item Pool IPIP 212 NEO PI R The Ten Item Personality Inventory TIPI and the Five Item Personality Inventory FIPI are very abbreviated rating forms of the Big Five personality traits 213 Self descriptive sentence questionnaires 142 Lexical questionnaires 214 Self report questionnaires 215 Relative scored Big 5 measure 216 The most frequently used measures of the Big Five comprise either items that are self descriptive sentences 142 or in the case of lexical measures items that are single adjectives 214 Due to the length of sentence based and some lexical measures short forms have been developed and validated for use in applied research settings where questionnaire space and respondent time are limited such as the 40 item balanced International English Big Five Mini Markers 135 or a very brief 10 item measure of the Big Five domains 217 Research has suggested that some methodologies in administering personality tests are inadequate in length and provide insufficient detail to truly evaluate personality Usually longer more detailed questions will give a more accurate portrayal of personality 218 The five factor structure has been replicated in peer reports 219 However many of the substantive findings rely on self reports Much of the evidence on the measures of the Big 5 relies on self report questionnaires which makes self report bias and falsification of responses difficult to deal with and account for 215 It has been argued that the Big Five tests do not create an accurate personality profile because the responses given on these tests are not true in all cases and can be falsified 220 For example questionnaires are answered by potential employees who might choose answers that paint them in the best light 221 Research suggests that a relative scored Big Five measure in which respondents had to make repeated choices between equally desirable personality descriptors may be a potential alternative to traditional Big Five measures in accurately assessing personality traits especially when lying or biased responding is present 216 When compared with a traditional Big Five measure for its ability to predict GPA and creative achievement under both normal and fake good bias response conditions the relative scored measure significantly and consistently predicted these outcomes under both conditions however the Likert questionnaire lost its predictive ability in the faking condition Thus the relative scored measure proved to be less affected by biased responding than the Likert measure of the Big Five Andrew H Schwartz analyzed 700 million words phrases and topic instances collected from the Facebook messages of 75 000 volunteers who also took standard personality tests and found striking variations in language with personality gender and age 222 Critique EditThe proposed Big Five model has been subjected to considerable critical scrutiny in a number of published studies 223 224 225 226 227 228 60 229 81 One prominent critic of the model has been Jack Block at the University of California Berkeley In response to Block the model was defended in a paper published by Costa and McCrae 230 This was followed by a number of published critical replies from Block 231 232 233 It has been argued that there are limitations to the scope of the Big Five model as an explanatory or predictive theory 60 229 It has also been argued that measures of the Big Five account for only 56 of the normal personality trait sphere alone not even considering the abnormal personality trait sphere 60 Also the static Big Five 234 is not theory driven it is merely a statistically driven investigation of certain descriptors that tend to cluster together often based on less than optimal factor analytic procedures 60 431 33 81 Measures of the Big Five constructs appear to show some consistency in interviews self descriptions and observations and this static five factor structure seems to be found across a wide range of participants of different ages and cultures 235 However while genotypic temperament trait dimensions might appear across different cultures the phenotypic expression of personality traits differs profoundly across different cultures as a function of the different socio cultural conditioning and experiential learning that takes place within different cultural settings 236 Moreover the fact that the Big Five model was based on lexical hypothesis i e on the verbal descriptors of individual differences indicated strong methodological flaws in this model especially related to its main factors Extraversion and Neuroticism First there is a natural pro social bias of language in people s verbal evaluations After all language is an invention of group dynamics that was developed to facilitate socialization and the exchange of information and to synchronize group activity This social function of language therefore creates a sociability bias in verbal descriptors of human behavior there are more words related to social than physical or even mental aspects of behavior The sheer number of such descriptors will cause them to group into the largest factor in any language and such grouping has nothing to do with the way that core systems of individual differences are set up Second there is also a negativity bias in emotionality i e most emotions have negative affectivity and there are more words in language to describe negative rather than positive emotions Such asymmetry in emotional valence creates another bias in language Experiments using the lexical hypothesis approach indeed demonstrated that the use of lexical material skews the resulting dimensionality according to a sociability bias of language and a negativity bias of emotionality grouping all evaluations around these two dimensions 227 This means that the two largest dimensions in the Big Five model might be just an artifact of the lexical approach that this model employed Limited scope Edit One common criticism is that the Big Five does not explain all of human personality Some psychologists have dissented from the model precisely because they feel it neglects other domains of personality such as religiosity manipulativeness machiavellianism honesty sexiness seductiveness thriftiness conservativeness masculinity femininity snobbishness egotism sense of humour and risk taking thrill seeking 228 237 Dan P McAdams has called the Big Five a psychology of the stranger because they refer to traits that are relatively easy to observe in a stranger other aspects of personality that are more privately held or more context dependent are excluded from the Big Five 238 There may be debate as to what counts as personality and what does not and the nature of the questions in the survey greatly influence outcome Multiple particularly broad question databases have failed to produce the Big Five as the top five traits 239 In many studies the five factors are not fully orthogonal to one another that is the five factors are not independent 240 241 Orthogonality is viewed as desirable by some researchers because it minimizes redundancy between the dimensions This is particularly important when the goal of a study is to provide a comprehensive description of personality with as few variables as possible Methodological issues Edit Factor analysis the statistical method used to identify the dimensional structure of observed variables lacks a universally recognized basis for choosing among solutions with different numbers of factors 242 A five factor solution depends on some degree of interpretation by the analyst A larger number of factors may underlie these five factors This has led to disputes about the true number of factors Big Five proponents have responded that although other solutions may be viable in a single data set only the five factor structure consistently replicates across different studies 243 Surveys in studies are often online surveys of college students Results do not always replicate when run on other populations or in other languages 244 Moreover the factor analysis that this model is based on is a linear method incapable of capturing nonlinear feedback and contingent relationships between core systems of individual differences 227 Theoretical status Edit A frequent criticism is that the Big Five is not based on any underlying theory it is merely an empirical finding that certain descriptors cluster together under factor analysis 242 Although this does not mean that these five factors do not exist the underlying causes behind them are unknown Jack Block s final published work before his death in January 2010 drew together his lifetime perspective on the five factor model 245 He summarized his critique of the model in terms of the atheoretical nature of the five factors their cloudy measurement the model s inappropriateness for studying early childhood the use of factor analysis as the exclusive paradigm for conceptualizing personality the continuing non consensual understandings of the five factors the existence of unrecognized but successful efforts to specify aspects of character not subsumed by the five factors He went on to suggest that repeatedly observed higher order factors hierarchically above the proclaimed Big Five personality traits may promise deeper biological understanding of the origins and implications of these superfactors Evidence for six factors rather than five Edit It has been noted that even though early lexical studies in the English language indicated five large groups of personality traits more recent and more comprehensive cross language studies have provided evidence for six large groups rather than five 246 with the sixth factor being Honesty Humility These six groups form the basis of the HEXACO model of personality structure Based on these findings it has been suggested that the Big Five system should be replaced by HEXACO or revised to better align with lexical evidence 247 See also EditCore self evaluations Dark triad DISC assessment Facet Genomics of personality traits Goal orientation HEXACO model of personality structure Moral foundations theory Myers Briggs Type Indicator Personality psychology Szondi test Trait theoryReferences Edit a b c Rothmann S Coetzer EP 24 October 2003 The big five personality dimensions and job performance SA Journal of Industrial Psychology 29 doi 10 4102 sajip v29i1 88 Roccas Sonia Sagiv Lilach Schwartz Shalom H Knafo Ariel 2002 The Big Five Personality Factors and Personal Values Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 6 789 801 doi 10 1177 0146167202289008 S2CID 144611052 Goldberg LR January 1993 The structure of phenotypic personality traits American Psychologist 48 1 26 34 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 48 1 26 PMID 8427480 Costa PT McCrae RR 1992 Revised NEO Personality Inventory NEO PI R and NEO Five Factor Inventory NEO FFI manual Odessa Florida Psychological Assessment Resources Matthews G Deary IJ Whiteman MC 2003 Personality Traits PDF 2nd ed Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 83107 9 Archived from the original PDF on 2014 12 05 a b De Bolle M Beyers W De Clercq B De Fruyt F November 2012 General personality and psychopathology in referred and nonreferred children and adolescents an investigation of continuity pathoplasty and complication models Journal of Abnormal Psychology 121 4 958 70 doi 10 1037 a0027742 PMID 22448741 S2CID 33228527 Poropat AE March 2009 A meta analysis of the five factor model of personality and academic performance Psychological Bulletin 135 2 322 38 doi 10 1037 a0014996 hdl 10072 30324 PMID 19254083 Digman JM 1990 Personality structure Emergence of the five factor model Annual Review of Psychology 41 417 40 doi 10 1146 annurev ps 41 020190 002221 a b Shrout PE Fiske ST 1995 Personality research methods and theory Psychology Press Allport GW Odbert HS 1936 Trait names A psycholexical study Psychological Monographs 47 211 doi 10 1037 h0093360 a b c Bagby RM Marshall MB Georgiades S February 2005 Dimensional personality traits and the prediction of DSM IV personality disorder symptom counts in a nonclinical sample Journal of Personality Disorders 19 1 53 67 doi 10 1521 pedi 19 1 53 62180 PMID 15899720 a b c Tupes EC Christal RE 1961 Recurrent personality factors based on trait ratings USAF ASD Tech Rep 60 61 97 225 51 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 1992 tb00973 x PMID 1635043 a b Norman WT June 1963 Toward an adequate taxonomy of personality attributes replicated factors structure in peer nomination personality ratings Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 66 6 574 83 doi 10 1037 h0040291 PMID 13938947 Goldberg LR January 1993 The structure of phenotypic personality traits The American Psychologist 48 1 26 34 doi 10 1037 0003 066X 48 1 26 PMID 8427480 O Connor BP June 2002 A quantitative review of the comprehensiveness of the five factor model in relation to popular personality inventories Assessment 9 2 188 203 doi 10 1177 1073191102092010 PMID 12066834 S2CID 145580837 Goldberg LR 1982 From Ace to Zombie Some explorations in the language of personality In Spielberger CD Butcher JN eds Advances in personality assessment Vol 1 Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum pp 201 34 Norman WT Goldberg LR 1966 Raters ratees and randomness in personality structure Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4 6 681 91 doi 10 1037 h0024002 Peabody D Goldberg LR September 1989 Some determinants of factor structures from personality trait descriptors Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57 3 552 67 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 57 3 552 PMID 2778639 Saucier G Goldberg LR 1996 The language of personality Lexical perspectives on the five factor model In Wiggins JS ed The five factor model of personality Theoretical perspectives New York Guilford page needed Digman JM June 1989 Five robust trait dimensions development stability and utility Journal of Personality 57 2 195 214 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 1989 tb00480 x PMID 2671337 Karson S O Dell JW 1976 A guide to the clinical use of the 16PF Report Champaign IL Institute for Personality amp Ability Testing Krug SE Johns EF 1986 A large scale cross validation of second order personality structure defined by the 16PF Psychological Reports 59 2 683 93 doi 10 2466 pr0 1986 59 2 683 S2CID 145610003 Cattell HE Mead AD 2007 The 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire 16PF In Boyle GJ Matthews G Saklofske DH eds Handbook of personality theory and testing Volume 2 Personality measurement and assessment London Sage page needed Costa PT McCrae RR September 1976 Age differences in personality structure a cluster analytic approach Journal of Gerontology 31 5 564 70 doi 10 1093 geronj 31 5 564 PMID 950450 Costa PT McCrae RR 1985 The NEO Personality Inventory manual Odessa FL Psychological Assessment Resources McCrae RR Costa PT January 1987 Validation of the five factor model of personality across instruments and observers Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 1 81 90 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 52 1 81 PMID 3820081 McCrae RR John OP June 1992 An introduction to the five factor model and its applications Journal of Personality 60 2 175 215 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 470 4858 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 1992 tb00970 x PMID 1635039 S2CID 10596836 International Personality Item Pool IPIP The Society for Judgment and Decision Making Goldberg LR Johnson JA Eber HW Hogan R Ashton MC Cloninger CR Gough HG February 2006 The international personality item pool and the future of public domain personality measures Journal of Research in Personality 40 1 84 96 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2005 08 007 Conn S Rieke M 1994 The 16PF Fifth Edition technical manual Champaign IL Institute for Personality amp Ability Testing Cattell HE 1996 The original big five A historical perspective European Review of Applied Psychology 46 5 14 Grucza RA Goldberg LR October 2007 The comparative validity of 11 modern personality inventories predictions of behavioral acts informant reports and clinical indicators Journal of Personality Assessment 89 2 167 87 doi 10 1080 00223890701468568 PMID 17764394 S2CID 42394327 Mershon B Gorsuch RL 1988 Number of factors in the personality sphere does increase in factors increase predictability of real life criteria Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 55 4 675 80 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 55 4 675 Paunonen SV Ashton MS 2001 Big Five factors and facets and the prediction of behavior Journal of Personality amp Social Psychology 81 3 524 39 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 3 524 PMID 11554651 a b DeYoung CG Quilty LC Peterson JB November 2007 Between facets and domains 10 aspects of the Big Five Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93 5 880 96 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 93 5 880 PMID 17983306 S2CID 8261816 a b c Toegel G Barsoux JL 2012 How to become a better leader MIT Sloan Management Review 53 3 51 60 Ambridge B 2014 Psy Q You know your IQ now test your psychological intelligence Profile p 11 ISBN 978 1782830238 via Google Books DeYoung Colin G Hirsh Jacob B Shane Matthew S Papademetris Xenophon Rajeevan Nallakkandi Gray Jeremy R 2010 Testing Predictions From Personality Neuroscience Brain Structure and the Big Five Psychological Science 21 6 820 828 doi 10 1177 0956797610370159 ISSN 0956 7976 JSTOR 41062296 PMC 3049165 PMID 20435951 a b c d e The 50 item IPIP representation of the Goldberg 1992 markers for the Big Five structure at ipip ori org Costa PT McCrae RR 1992 Neo PI R professional manual Odessa FL Psychological Assessment Resources page needed ISBN missing Research Reports on Science from Michigan State University Provide New Insights Science Letter Gale Student Resource in Context Retrieved 4 April 2012 Laney MO 2002 The Introvert Advantage Canada Thomas Allen amp Son Limited pp 28 35 ISBN 978 0761123699 a b c Friedman H Schustack M 2016 Personality Classic Theories and Modern Research Sixth ed Pearson Education Inc ISBN 978 0205997930 page needed Bartneck C Van der Hoek M Mubin O Al Mahmud A March 2007 Daisy daisy give me your answer do switching off a robot Eindhoven Netherlands Dept of Ind Design Eindhoven Univ of Technol pp 217 22 Retrieved 6 February 2013 Judge TA Bono JE October 2000 Five factor model of personality and transformational leadership The Journal of Applied Psychology 85 5 751 65 doi 10 1037 0021 9010 85 5 751 PMID 11055147 Lim BC Ployhart RE August 2004 Transformational leadership relations to the five factor model and team performance in typical and maximum contexts The Journal of Applied Psychology 89 4 610 21 doi 10 1037 0021 9010 89 4 610 PMID 15327348 a b c Sackett PR Walmsley PT 2014 Which Personality Attributes Are Most Important in the Workplace Perspectives on Psychological Science 9 5 538 51 doi 10 1177 1745691614543972 PMID 26186756 S2CID 21245818 a b c Jeronimus BF Riese H Sanderman R Ormel J October 2014 Mutual reinforcement between neuroticism and life experiences a five wave 16 year study to test reciprocal causation Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 107 4 751 64 doi 10 1037 a0037009 PMID 25111305 a b Norris CJ Larsen JT Cacioppo JT September 2007 Neuroticism is associated with larger and more prolonged electrodermal responses to emotionally evocative pictures PDF Psychophysiology 44 5 823 26 doi 10 1111 j 1469 8986 2007 00551 x PMID 17596178 a b c d Kagan J Snidman N 2009 The Long Shadow of Temperament Cambridge MA Harvard University Press ISBN missing page needed Fiske ST Gilbert DT Lindzey G 2009 Handbook of Social Psychology Hoboken NJ Wiley ISBN missing page needed Reynaud E El Khoury Malhame M Rossier J Blin O Khalfa S 2012 Neuroticism modifies psycho physiological responses to fearful films PLOS ONE 7 3 e32413 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 732413R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0032413 PMC 3316522 PMID 22479326 a b Jeronimus BF Ormel J Aleman A Penninx BW Riese H November 2013 Negative and positive life events are associated with small but lasting change in neuroticism Psychological Medicine 43 11 2403 15 doi 10 1017 s0033291713000159 PMID 23410535 S2CID 43717734 Dwan T Ownsworth T 2019 The Big Five personality factors and psychological well being following stroke a systematic review Disability and Rehabilitation 41 10 1119 30 doi 10 1080 09638288 2017 1419382 PMID 29272953 S2CID 7300458 Dolan SL 2006 Stress Self Esteem Health and Work p 76 ISBN missing a b c Musek Janek 2017 The General Factor of Personality London Academic Press pp 14 25 ISBN 978 0 12 811249 6 Allport GW Odbert HS 1936 Trait names A psycholexical study Psychological Monographs 47 211 doi 10 1037 h0093360 Goldberg LR May 1980 Some ruminations about the structure of individual differences Developing a common lexicon for the major characteristics of human personality Symposium presentation at the meeting of the Western Psychological Association Report Honolulu HI page needed Boyle GJ November 1983 Effects on academic learning of manipulating emotional states and motivational dynamics The British Journal of Educational Psychology 53 3 347 57 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8279 1983 tb02567 x PMID 6652035 a b c d e Boyle GJ Stankov L Cattell RB 1995 Measurement and statistical models in the study of personality and intelligence In Saklofske DH Zeidner M eds International Handbook of Personality and Intelligence pp 417 46 ISBN missing Epstein S O Brien EJ November 1985 The person situation debate in historical and current perspective Psychological Bulletin 98 3 513 37 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 98 3 513 PMID 4080897 Kenrick DT Funder DC January 1988 Profiting from controversy Lessons from the person situation debate The American Psychologist 43 1 23 34 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 43 1 23 PMID 3279875 Lucas RE Donnellan MB 2009 If the person situation debate is really over why does it still generate so much negative affect Journal of Research in Personality 43 3 146 49 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2009 02 009 Eysenck MW Eysenck JS 1980 Mischel and the concept of personality British Journal of Psychology 71 2 191 204 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8295 1980 tb01737 x Goldberg LR 1981 Language and individual differences The search for universals in personality lexicons In Wheeler ed Review of Personality and social psychology Vol 1 Beverly Hills CA Sage pp 141 65 ISBN missing Deyoung C G Quilty L C Peterson J B 2007 Between Facets and Domains 10 Aspects of the Big Five Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93 5 880 896 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 513 2517 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 93 5 880 PMID 17983306 S2CID 8261816 Goldberg Lewis R Johnson John A Eber Herbert W Hogan Robert Ashton Michael C Cloninger C Robert Gough Harrison G 2006 The international personality item pool and the future of public domain personality measures PDF Journal of Research in Personality 40 84 96 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2005 08 007 via Elsevier The revised NEO personality inventory NEO PI R The SAGE Handbook of Personality Theory and Assessment 2 223 257 via Researchgate a b DeYoung CG Carey BE Krueger RF Ross SR April 2016 Ten aspects of the Big Five in the Personality Inventory for DSM 5 Personality Disorders 7 2 113 23 doi 10 1037 per0000170 PMC 4818974 PMID 27032017 Alexander Nix 2017 03 03 From Mad Men to Math Men Verbatim report of the presentation of Alexander Nix Cambridge Analytica CEO at Online Marketing Rockstars Festival 2017 freud online Retrieved 2022 10 23 a href Template Cite web html title Template Cite web cite web a CS1 maint url status link About Us Cambridge Analytica Archived from the original on 16 February 2016 Retrieved 27 December 2015 Sellers FS 19 October 2015 Cruz campaign paid 750 000 to psychographic profiling company The Washington Post Retrieved 7 February 2016 a b c d e f g h Rothbart MK Ahadi SA Evans DE 2000 Temperament and personality Origins and outcomes Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 1 122 35 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 78 1 122 PMID 10653510 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u Shiner R Caspi A January 2003 Personality differences in childhood and adolescence measurement development and consequences Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 44 1 2 32 doi 10 1111 1469 7610 00101 PMID 12553411 a b McCrae RR Costa PT Ostendorf F Angleitner A Hrebickova M Avia MD Sanz J Sanchez Bernardos ML Kusdil ME Woodfield R Saunders PR Smith PB January 2000 Nature over nurture temperament personality and life span development Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 1 173 86 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 78 1 173 PMID 10653513 a b c d e f g h i j Markey PM Markey CN Tinsley BJ April 2004 Children s behavioral manifestations of the five factor model of personality Personality amp Social Psychology Bulletin 30 4 423 32 doi 10 1177 0146167203261886 PMID 15070472 S2CID 33684001 Rusalov VM 1989 Motor and communicative aspects of human temperament a new questionnaire of the structure of temperament Personality and Individual Differences 10 8 817 27 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 89 90017 2 a b Strelau J 1998 Temperament A Psychological Perspective New York Plenum a b Rusalov VM Trofimova IN 2007 Structure of Temperament and Its Measurement Toronto Canada Psychological Services Press page needed a b c d e Trofimova IN 2016 The interlocking between functional aspects of activities and a neurochemical model of adult temperament In MC ed Temperaments Individual Differences Social and Environmental Influences and Impact on Quality of Life New York Nova Science Publishers pp 77 147 a b c d Trofimova I Robbins TW Sulis WH Uher J April 2018 Taxonomies of psychological individual differences biological perspectives on millennia long challenges Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 373 1744 20170152 doi 10 1098 rstb 2017 0152 PMC 5832678 PMID 29483338 Trofimova IN 2010 An investigation into differences between the structure of temperament and the structure of personality American Journal of Psychology 123 4 467 80 doi 10 5406 amerjpsyc 123 4 0467 PMID 21291163 Jang KL Livesley WJ Vernon PA September 1996 Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets a twin study Journal of Personality 64 3 577 91 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 1996 tb00522 x PMID 8776880 Bouchard TJ McGue M January 2003 Genetic and environmental influences on human psychological differences Journal of Neurobiology 54 1 4 45 doi 10 1002 neu 10160 PMID 12486697 Weiss A King JE Hopkins WD November 2007 A cross setting study of chimpanzee Pan troglodytes personality structure and development zoological parks and Yerkes National Primate Research Center American Journal of Primatology 69 11 1264 77 doi 10 1002 ajp 20428 PMC 2654334 PMID 17397036 Gosling SD John OP 1999 Personality Dimensions in Nonhuman Animals A Cross Species Review PDF Current Directions in Psychological Science 8 3 69 75 doi 10 1111 1467 8721 00017 S2CID 145716504 Archived from the original PDF on 2018 09 28 Retrieved 2016 12 05 Morton FB Robinson LM Brando S Weiss A 2021 Personality structure in bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus Journal of Comparative Psychology 135 2 219 231 doi 10 1037 com0000259 PMID 33464108 S2CID 231642036 a b Soto CJ John OP Gosling SD Potter J February 2011 Age differences in personality traits from 10 to 65 Big Five domains and facets in a large cross sectional sample Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 100 2 330 48 doi 10 1037 a0021717 PMID 21171787 a b c d e f Soto CJ August 2016 The Little Six Personality Dimensions From Early Childhood to Early Adulthood Mean Level Age and Gender Differences in Parents Reports Journal of Personality 84 4 409 22 doi 10 1111 jopy 12168 PMID 25728032 Lewis M 2001 Issues in the study of personality development Psychological Inquiry 12 2 67 83 doi 10 1207 s15327965pli1202 02 S2CID 144557981 a b Goldberg LR 2001 Analyses of Digman s child personality data Derivation of Big Five Factor Scores from each of six samples Journal of Personality 69 5 709 43 doi 10 1111 1467 6494 695161 PMID 11575511 Mervielde I De Fruyt F 1999 Construction of the Hierarchical Personality Inventory for Children Hi PIC In Mervielde ID De Fruyt F Ostendorf F eds Personality psychology in Europe Proceedings of the Eighth European Conference on Personality Tilburg University Press pp 107 27 Resing WC Bleichrodt N Dekker PH 1999 Measuring personality traits in the classroom PDF European Journal of Personality 13 6 493 509 doi 10 1002 sici 1099 0984 199911 12 13 6 lt 493 aid per355 gt 3 0 co 2 v hdl 1871 18675 S2CID 56322465 Markey PM Markey CN Ericksen AJ Tinsley BJ 2002 A preliminary validation of preadolescents self reports using the Five Factor Model of personality Journal of Research in Personality 36 2 173 81 doi 10 1006 jrpe 2001 2341 Scholte RH van Aken MA van Lieshout CF December 1997 Adolescent personality factors in self ratings and peer nominations and their prediction of peer acceptance and peer rejection Journal of Personality Assessment 69 3 534 54 doi 10 1207 s15327752jpa6903 8 PMID 9501483 van Lieshout CF Haselager GJ 1994 The Big Five personality factors in Q sort descriptions of children and adolescents In Halverson CF Kohnstamm GA Martin RP eds The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum pp 293 318 a b c Halverson CF Kohnstamm GA Martin RP eds 1994 The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum a b c Kohnstamm GA Halverson Jr CF Mervielde I Havill VL eds 1998 Parental descriptions of child personality Developmental antecedents of the Big Five Psychology Press ISBN missing page needed Mervielde I De Fruyt F Jarmuz S May 1998 Linking openness and intellect in childhood and adulthood In Kohnstamm GA Halverson CF Mervielde I Havill VL eds Parental descriptions of child personality Developmental antecedents of the Big Five Mahway NJ Erlbaum pp 105 26 ISBN 9780805823011 John OP Srivastava S 1999 The Big Five trait taxonomy history measurement and theoretical perspectives PDF In Pervin LA John OP eds Handbook of personality Theory and research Vol 2 New York Guilford Press pp 102 38 a b c d e f Soto C Tackett J 2015 Personality Traits in Childhood and Adolescence Structure Development and Outcomes PDF Current Directions in Psychological Science 24 358 62 doi 10 1177 0963721415589345 S2CID 29475747 a b c Roberts BW Walton KE Viechtbauer W January 2006 Patterns of mean level change in personality traits across the life course a meta analysis of longitudinal studies Psychological Bulletin 132 1 1 25 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 132 1 1 PMID 16435954 Roberts BW DelVecchio WF January 2000 The rank order consistency of personality traits from childhood to old age a quantitative review of longitudinal studies PDF Psychological Bulletin 126 1 3 25 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 126 1 3 PMID 10668348 Lemery KS Goldsmith HH Klinnert MD Mrazek DA January 1999 Developmental models of infant and childhood temperament Developmental Psychology 35 1 189 204 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 35 1 189 PMID 9923474 Buss A Plomin R 1984 Temperament early developing personality trait Hillsdale Erlbaum Rothbart MK Ahadi SA Hershey KL Fisher P 2001 Investigations of temperament at three to seven years the Children s Behavior Questionnaire Child Development 72 5 1394 408 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 398 8830 doi 10 1111 1467 8624 00355 PMID 11699677 John OP Caspi A Robins RW Moffitt TE Stouthamer Loeber M February 1994 The little five exploring the nomological network of the five factor model of personality in adolescent boys Child Development 65 1 160 78 doi 10 2307 1131373 JSTOR 1131373 PMID 8131645 Eaton WO 1994 Temperament development and the Five Factor Model Lessons from activity level In Halverson CF Kohnstamm GA Martin RP eds The developing structure of temperament and personality from infancy to adulthood Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum pp 173 87 Hawley PH 1999 The ontogenesis of social dominance A strategy based evolutionary perspective Developmental Review 19 97 132 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 459 4755 doi 10 1006 drev 1998 0470 Hawley PH Little TD 1999 On winning some and losing some A social relations approach to social dominance in toddlers Merrill Palmer Quarterly 45 185 214 Sherif M Harvey O White BJ Hood WR Sherif C 1961 Intergroup conflict and cooperation The robbers cave experiment Norman OK University of Oklahoma Press OCLC 953442127 Keating CF Heltman KR 1994 Dominance and deception in children and adults Are leaders the best misleaders Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 20 3 312 21 doi 10 1177 0146167294203009 S2CID 19252480 Asendorpf JB 1990 Development of inhibition during childhood Evidence for situational specificity and a two factor model Developmental Psychology 26 5 721 30 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 26 5 721 Asendorpf JB Meier GH 1993 Personality effects on children s speech in everyday life Sociability mediated exposure and shyness mediated re activity to social situations Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64 6 1072 83 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 64 6 1072 PMID 8326470 Harrist AW Zaia AF Bates JE Dodge KA Pettit GS April 1997 Subtypes of social withdrawal in early childhood sociometric status and social cognitive differences across four years Child Development 68 2 278 94 doi 10 2307 1131850 JSTOR 1131850 PMID 9180002 Mathiesen KS Tambs K March 1999 The EAS temperament questionnaire factor structure age trends reliability and stability in a Norwegian sample Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 40 3 431 39 doi 10 1111 1469 7610 00460 PMID 10190344 McCrae RR Costa PT 1990 Personality in adulthood New York The Guildford Press page needed Cobb Clark DA Schurer S 2012 The stability of big five personality traits PDF Economics Letters 115 2 11 15 doi 10 1016 j econlet 2011 11 015 S2CID 12086995 Srivastava S John OP Gosling SD Potter J May 2003 Development of personality in early and middle adulthood set like plaster or persistent change Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84 5 1041 53 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 499 4124 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 84 5 1041 PMID 12757147 Roberts BW Mroczek D February 2008 Personality Trait Change in Adulthood Current Directions in Psychological Science 17 1 31 35 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2008 00543 x PMC 2743415 PMID 19756219 Fleeson W 2001 Towards a structure and process integrated view of personality Traits as density distributions of states Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80 6 1011 27 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 80 6 1011 PMID 11414368 Mottus R Johnson W Starr JM Dearya IJ June 2012 Correlates of personality trait levels and their changes in very old age The Lothian Birth Cohort 1921 PDF Journal of Research in Personality 46 3 271 78 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2012 02 004 hdl 20 500 11820 b6b6961d 902f 48e0 bf25 f505a659a056 Cavallera G Passerini A Pepe A 2013 Personality and gender in swimmers in indoor practice at leisure level Social Behavior and Personality 41 4 693 704 doi 10 2224 sbp 2013 41 4 693 Falk Armin Hermle Johannes 2018 10 19 Relationship of gender differences in preferences to economic development and gender equality Science 362 6412 eaas9899 doi 10 1126 science aas9899 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 30337384 a b Costa PT Terracciano A McCrae RR August 2001 Gender differences in personality traits across cultures robust and surprising findings Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81 2 322 31 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 81 2 322 PMID 11519935 a b c Schmitt DP Realo A Voracek M Allik J January 2008 Why can t a man be more like a woman Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 94 1 168 82 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 94 1 168 PMID 18179326 Harris J R 2006 No two alike Human nature and human individuality WW Norton amp Company Jefferson T Herbst JH McCrae RR 1998 Associations between birth order and personality traits Evidence from self reports and observer ratings Journal of Research in Personality 32 4 498 509 doi 10 1006 jrpe 1998 2233 Damian RI Roberts BW October 2015 The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample of U S high school students Journal of Research in Personality 58 96 105 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2015 05 005 Thompson RL Brossart DF Carlozzi AF Miville ML September 2002 Five factor model Big Five personality traits and universal diverse orientation in counselor trainees The Journal of Psychology 136 5 561 72 doi 10 1080 00223980209605551 PMID 12431039 S2CID 22076221 Ostendorf F 1990 Sprache und Persoenlichkeitsstruktur Zur Validitaet des Funf Factoren Modells der Persoenlichkeit Regensburg Germany S Roderer Verlag page needed Trull TJ Geary DC October 1997 Comparison of the big five factor structure across samples of Chinese and American adults Journal of Personality Assessment 69 2 324 41 doi 10 1207 s15327752jpa6902 6 PMID 9392894 Lodhi PH Deo S Belhekar VM 2002 The Five Factor model of personality in Indian context measurement and correlates In McCrae RR Allik J eds The Five Factor model of personality across cultures New York Kluwer Academic Publisher pp 227 48 McCrae RR 2002 NEO PI R data from 36 cultures Further Intercultural comparisons In McCrae RR Allik J eds The Five Factor model of personality across cultures New York Kluwer Academic Publisher pp 105 25 a b Thompson ER 2008 Development and validation of an international English big five mini markers Personality and Individual Differences 45 6 542 48 doi 10 1016 j paid 2008 06 013 Cheung FM van de Vijver FJ Leong FT October 2011 Toward a new approach to the study of personality in culture The American Psychologist 66 7 593 603 doi 10 1037 a0022389 PMID 21261408 Archived from the original on 2013 05 18 Retrieved 2013 01 16 McCrae RR Terracciano A September 2005 Personality profiles of cultures aggregate personality traits PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 89 3 407 25 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 89 3 407 PMID 16248722 Hofstede Geert Bond Michael H 1984 Hofstede s Culture Dimensions An Independent Validation Using Rokeach s Value Survey Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 15 4 417 433 doi 10 1177 0022002184015004003 ISSN 0022 0221 S2CID 145651845 Mooradian Todd A Swan K Scott 2006 06 01 Personality and culture The case of national extraversion and word of mouth Journal of Business Research Special Section The 2005 La Londe Seminar 59 6 778 785 doi 10 1016 j jbusres 2006 01 015 ISSN 0148 2963 Barcelo J 2017 National Personality Traits and Regime Type A Cross National Study of 47 Countries Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 48 2 195 216 doi 10 1177 0022022116678324 S2CID 151607260 Szirmak Z De Raad B 1994 Taxonomy and structure of Hungarian personality traits European Journal of Personality 8 2 95 117 doi 10 1002 per 2410080203 S2CID 145275826 a b c De Fruyt F McCrae RR Szirmak Z Nagy J September 2004 The Five factor Personality Inventory as a measure of the Five factor Model Belgian American and Hungarian comparisons with the NEO PI R Assessment 11 3 207 15 doi 10 1177 1073191104265800 PMID 15358876 S2CID 29733250 Robins Wahlin TB Byrne GJ October 2011 Personality changes in Alzheimer s disease a systematic review International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 26 10 1019 29 doi 10 1002 gps 2655 PMID 21905097 S2CID 40949990 Widiger TA Costa PT Jr Five Factor model personality disorder research In Costa Paul T Jr Widiger Thomas A editors Personality disorders and the five factor model of personality 2nd Washington DC US American Psychological Association 2002 pp 59 87 2002 ISBN missing Mullins Sweatt SN Widiger TA 2006 The five factor model of personality disorder A translation across science and practice In Krueger R Tackett J eds Personality and psychopathology Building bridges New York Guilford pp 39 70 ISBN missing Clark LA 2007 Assessment and diagnosis of personality disorder perennial issues and an emerging reconceptualization Annual Review of Psychology 58 227 57 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 57 102904 190200 PMID 16903806 Trofimova I Robbins TW May 2016 Temperament and arousal systems A new synthesis of differential psychology and functional neurochemistry Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 64 382 402 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2016 03 008 hdl 11375 26202 PMID 26969100 S2CID 13937324 Trofimova I Sulis W 2016 Benefits of Distinguishing between Physical and Social Verbal Aspects of Behavior An Example of Generalized Anxiety Frontiers in Psychology 7 338 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2016 00338 PMC 4789559 PMID 27014146 Trofimova I Christiansen J April 2016 Coupling of Temperament with Mental Illness in Four Age Groups Psychological Reports 118 2 387 412 doi 10 1177 0033294116639430 PMID 27154370 S2CID 24465522 Depue R Fu Y 2012 Neurobiology and neurochemistry of temperament in adults In Zentner M Shiner R eds Handbook of Temperament New York Guilford Publications pp 368 99 Bagby RM Sellbom M Costa PT Widiger TA April 2008 PredictingDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders IV personality disorders with the five factor model of personality and the personality psychopathology five Personality and Mental Health 2 2 55 69 doi 10 1002 pmh 33 The five factor model and personality disorder empirical literature A meta analytic review LM Saulsman AC Page Clinical Psychology Review 2004 Elsevier Science ISBN missing page needed Fehrman E Muhammad AK Mirkes EM Egan V Gorban AN 2015 The Five Factor Model of personality and evaluation of drug consumption risk arXiv 1506 06297 stat AP a b Kessler RC Chiu WT Demler O Merikangas KR Walters EE June 2005 Prevalence severity and comorbidity of 12 month DSM IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Archives of General Psychiatry 62 6 617 27 doi 10 1001 archpsyc 62 6 617 PMC 2847357 PMID 15939839 Compton WM Conway KP Stinson FS Colliver JD Grant BF June 2005 Prevalence correlates and comorbidity of DSM IV antisocial personality syndromes and alcohol and specific drug use disorders in the United States results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 66 6 677 85 doi 10 4088 jcp v66n0602 PMID 15960559 Hasin DS Goodwin RD Stinson FS Grant BF October 2005 Epidemiology of major depressive disorder results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcoholism and Related Conditions Archives of General Psychiatry 62 10 1097 106 doi 10 1001 archpsyc 62 10 1097 PMID 16203955 a b Fehrman Elaine Egan Vincent Gorban Alexander N Levesley Jeremy Mirkes Evgeny M Muhammad Awaz K 2019 Personality Traits and Drug Consumption A Story Told by Data Springer Cham arXiv 2001 06520 doi 10 1007 978 3 030 10442 9 ISBN 978 3 030 10441 2 S2CID 151160405 Khan AA Jacobson KC Gardner CO Prescott CA Kendler KS March 2005 Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders The British Journal of Psychiatry 186 3 190 96 doi 10 1192 bjp 186 3 190 PMID 15738498 Cuijpers P Smit F Penninx BW de Graaf R ten Have M Beekman AT October 2010 Economic costs of neuroticism a population based study Archives of General Psychiatry 67 10 1086 93 doi 10 1001 archgenpsychiatry 2010 130 PMID 20921124 a b c d e f g Kotov R Gamez W Schmidt F Watson D September 2010 Linking big personality traits to anxiety depressive and substance use disorders a meta analysis Psychological Bulletin 136 5 768 821 doi 10 1037 a0020327 PMID 20804236 Bogg T Roberts BW November 2004 Conscientiousness and health related behaviors a meta analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to mortality Psychological Bulletin 130 6 887 919 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 130 6 887 PMID 15535742 a b Roberts BW Kuncel NR Shiner R Caspi A Goldberg LR December 2007 The Power of Personality The Comparative Validity of Personality Traits Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Ability for Predicting Important Life Outcomes PDF Perspectives on Psychological Science 2 4 313 45 doi 10 1111 j 1745 6916 2007 00047 x PMC 4499872 PMID 26151971 a b c d Jeronimus BF Kotov R Riese H Ormel J October 2016 Neuroticism s prospective association with mental disorders halves after adjustment for baseline symptoms and psychiatric history but the adjusted association hardly decays with time a meta analysis on 59 longitudinal prospective studies with 443 313 participants Psychological Medicine 46 14 2883 906 doi 10 1017 S0033291716001653 PMID 27523506 S2CID 23548727 Livesley WJ 2001 Handbook of Personality Disorders New York The Guildford Press pp 84 104 ISBN 978 1 57230 629 5 OCLC 783011161 a b c d Ormel J Jeronimus BF Kotov R Riese H Bos EH Hankin B Rosmalen JG Oldehinkel AJ July 2013 Neuroticism and common mental disorders meaning and utility of a complex relationship Clinical Psychology Review 33 5 686 97 doi 10 1016 j cpr 2013 04 003 PMC 4382368 PMID 23702592 a b c d e f Millon T Krueger R Simonsen E 2011 Contemporary Directions in Psychopathology Scientific Foundations of the DSM IV and ICD 11 Guilford Press a b c d Krueger R Tackett L 2006 Personality and Psychopathology Guilford Press page needed ISBN missing a b Hudek Knezevic J Kardum I August 2009 Five factor personality dimensions and 3 health related personality constructs as predictors of health Croatian Medical Journal 50 4 394 402 doi 10 3325 cmj 2009 50 394 PMC 2728392 PMID 19673040 Jerram Kathryn L Coleman Peter G 1999 The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age British Journal of Health Psychology 4 2 181 92 doi 10 1348 135910799168560 ISSN 2044 8287 Jerram Kathryn 16 December 2010 The big five personality traits and reporting of health problems and health behaviour in old age British Journal of Health Psychology 4 2 181 192 doi 10 1348 135910799168560 via Wiley Iwasa H Masui Y Gondo Y Inagaki H Kawaai C Suzuki T May 2008 Personality and all cause mortality among older adults dwelling in a Japanese community a five year population based prospective cohort study The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 16 5 399 405 doi 10 1097 JGP 0b013e3181662ac9 PMID 18403571 Jokela M Hintsanen M Hakulinen C Batty GD Nabi H Singh Manoux A Kivimaki M April 2013 Association of personality with the development and persistence of obesity a meta analysis based on individual participant data Obesity Reviews 14 4 315 23 doi 10 1111 obr 12007 PMC 3717171 PMID 23176713 a b c d e f g h i Komarraju M Karau SJ Schmeck RR Avdic A September 2011 The Big Five personality traits learning styles and academic achievement Personality and Individual Differences 51 4 472 77 doi 10 1016 j paid 2011 04 019 Zeidner M Shani Zinovich I 11 October 2011 Do academically gifted and nongifted students differ on the Big Five and adaptive status Some recent data and conclusions Personality and Individual Differences 51 5 566 70 doi 10 1016 j paid 2011 05 007 a b Mutlu Tansu Balbag Zafer Cemrek Fatih 2010 01 01 The role of self esteem locus of control and big five personality traits in predicting hopelessness Procedia Social and Behavioral Sciences World Conference on Learning Teaching and Administration Papers 9 1788 1792 doi 10 1016 j sbspro 2010 12 401 ISSN 1877 0428 Singh AK 2012 Does trait predict psychological well being among students of professional courses Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology 38 2 234 41 a b c Klimstra TA Luyckx K Germeijs V Meeus WH Goossens L March 2012 Personality traits and educational identity formation in late adolescents longitudinal associations and academic progress PDF Journal of Youth and Adolescence 41 3 346 61 doi 10 1007 s10964 011 9734 7 PMID 22147120 S2CID 33747401 Pashler H McDaniel M Rohrer D Bjork R December 2008 Learning Styles Concepts and Evidence Psychological Science in the Public Interest 9 3 105 19 doi 10 1111 j 1539 6053 2009 01038 x PMID 26162104 Zhang L 6 September 2001 Measuring thinking styles in addition to measuring personality traits Personality and Individual Differences 33 3 445 58 doi 10 1016 s0191 8869 01 00166 0 Schmeck RR Ribich F Ramainah N 1997 Development of a Self Report inventory for assessing Individual Differences in Learning Processes Applied Psychological Measurement 1 3 413 31 doi 10 1177 014662167700100310 S2CID 143890188 a b c Jensen Mikael 2015 Personality Traits Learning and Academic Achievements Journal of Education and Learning 4 4 91 doi 10 5539 jel v4n4p91 De Feyter T Caers R Vigna C Berings D 22 March 2012 Unraveling the impact of the Big Five personality traits on academic performance The moderating and mediating effects of self efficacy and academic motivation Learning and Individual Differences 22 4 439 48 doi 10 1016 j lindif 2012 03 013 Vedel A 2014 The Big Five and tertiary academic performance A systematic review and meta analysis PDF Personality and Individual Differences 71 66 76 doi 10 1016 j paid 2014 07 011 Trapmann S Hell B Hirn J O W ve Schuler H 2007 Meta analysis of the relationship between the Big Five and academic success at university Zeitschrift fur Psychologie Journal of Psychology 215 2 132 51 Bartolic Zlomislic Bates A 1999 Investing in On line Learning Potential Benefits and Limitations Canadian Journal of Communication 24 3 doi 10 22230 CJC 1999V24N3A1111 Holland J L 1966 The Psychology of Vocational Choice A Theory of Personality Types and Model Environments Oxford Blaisdell Armitage Catherine Scientists are curious and passionate and ready to argue Retrieved 9 June 2021 Mount MK Barrick MR 1998 Five reasons why the big five article has been frequently cited Personnel Psychology 51 4 849 57 doi 10 1111 j 1744 6570 1998 tb00743 x Morgeson FP Campion MA Dipboye RL Hollenbeck JR Murphy K Schmitt N 2007 Reconsidering the use of personality tests in personnel selection contexts Personnel Psychology 60 3 683 729 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 493 5981 doi 10 1111 j 1744 6570 2007 00089 x Mischel W 1968 Personality and assessment London Wiley ISBN 978 0 8058 2330 1 Rosenthal R 1990 How are we doing in soft psychology American Psychologist 45 6 775 77 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 45 6 775 Hunter JE Schmidt FL Judiesch MK 1990 Individual differences in output variability as a function of job complexity Journal of Applied Psychology 75 28 42 doi 10 1037 0021 9010 75 1 28 S2CID 144507523 Fairweather J 2012 Personality nations and innovation Relationships between personality traits and national innovation scores Cross Cultural Research 46 3 30 doi 10 1177 1069397111409124 S2CID 144015495 Judge TA Bono JE Ilies R Gerhardt MW August 2002 Personality and leadership a qualitative and quantitative review The Journal of Applied Psychology 87 4 765 80 doi 10 1037 0021 9010 87 4 765 PMID 12184579 Mehta P 2012 Personality as a predictor of burnout among managers of manufacturing industries Journal of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology 32 321 28 Judge TA Livingston BA Hurst C February 2012 Do nice guys and gals really finish last The joint effects of sex and agreeableness on income Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 102 2 390 407 doi 10 1037 a0026021 PMID 22121889 a b Spurk D Abele AE 16 June 2010 Who Earns More and Why A Multiple Mediation Model from Personality to Salary Journal of Business and Psychology 26 87 103 doi 10 1007 s10869 010 9184 3 S2CID 144290202 Camps J Stouten J Euwema M February 2016 The relation between supervisors big five personality traits and employees experiences of abusive supervision Frontiers in Psychology 10 7 112 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2016 00112 PMC 4748047 PMID 26903919 Tepper BJ June 2007 Abusive supervision in work organizations Review synthesis and research agenda Journal of Management 33 3 261 89 doi 10 1177 0149206307300812 S2CID 143934380 McLean Dawson Bouaissa Mohsen Rainville Bruno Auger Ludovic 2019 12 04 Non Cognitive Skills How Much Do They Matter for Earnings in Canada American Journal of Management 19 4 doi 10 33423 ajm v19i4 2392 ISSN 2165 7998 a b c Neal A Yeo G Koy A Xiao T 26 January 2011 Predicting the Form and Direction of Work Role Performance From the Big 5 Model of Personality Traits Journal of Organizational Behavior 33 2 175 92 doi 10 1002 job 742 a b c d e Judge amp LePine Bright and Dark Sides Research Companion to the Dysfunctional Workplace 2007 a b c d Holland AS Roisman GI October 2008 Big five personality traits and relationship quality Self reported observational and physiological evidence PDF Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 25 5 811 29 doi 10 1177 0265407508096697 S2CID 28388979 Archived from the original PDF on 2 March 2013 Retrieved 12 April 2012 a b Gerber AS et al 2010 Personality and Political Attitudes Relationships across Issue Domains and Political Contexts The American Political Science Review 104 111 133 doi 10 1017 S0003055410000031 S2CID 6208090 Sweetser KD 2014 Partisan Personality The Psychological Differences Between Democrats and Republicans and Independents Somewhere in Between American Behavioral Scientist 58 9 1183 94 doi 10 1177 0002764213506215 S2CID 145674720 Fatke M 2017 Personality Traits and Political Ideology A First Global Assessment Political Psychology 38 5 881 99 doi 10 1111 pops 12347 a b Bakker BN et al 2015 Personality Traits and Party Identification over Time European Journal of Political Research 54 2 197 215 doi 10 1111 1475 6765 12070 a b Gerber AS et al 2012 Personality and the Strength and Direction of Partisan Identification Political Behavior 34 4 653 688 doi 10 1007 s11109 011 9178 5 S2CID 144317734 Lowe Konstantin Felix Is Politics Downstream from Personality The Five Factor Model s Effect on Political Orientation in Sweden 2019 http lup lub lu se student papers record 8992021 Thesis Roberts p 338 Saroglou Vassilis 2002 Religion and the five factors of personality A meta analytic review Personality and Individual Differences 32 15 25 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 00 00233 6 IPIP Home ipip ori org Retrieved 2017 07 01 Gosling SD Rentfrow PJ Swann WB 2003 A very brief measure of the Big Five personality domains Journal of Research in Personality 37 6 504 28 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 1013 6925 doi 10 1016 S0092 6566 03 00046 1 ISSN 0092 6566 a b Goldberg LR 1992 The development of markers for the Big five factor structure Psychological Assessment 4 1 26 42 doi 10 1037 1040 3590 4 1 26 a b Donaldson SI Grant Vallone EJ 2002 Understanding self report bias in organizational behavior research Journal of Business and Psychology 17 2 245 60 doi 10 1023 A 1019637632584 JSTOR 25092818 S2CID 10464760 a b Hirsh JB Peterson JB October 2008 Predicting creativity and academic success with a Fake Proof measure of the Big Five Journal of Research in Personality 42 5 1323 33 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2008 04 006 S2CID 18849547 Gosling SD Rentfrow PJ Swann WB 2003 A very brief measure of the Big Five personality domains Journal of Research in Personality 37 6 504 28 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 1013 6925 doi 10 1016 S0092 6566 03 00046 1 Crede M Harms P Niehorster S Gaye Valentine A April 2012 An evaluation of the consequences of using short measures of the Big Five personality traits Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 102 4 874 88 doi 10 1037 a0027403 PMID 22352328 Goldberg LR December 1990 An alternative description of personality the big five factor structure Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59 6 1216 29 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 59 6 1216 PMID 2283588 McFarland Lynn A Ryan Ann Marie 2000 Variance in faking across noncognitive measures Journal of Applied Psychology 85 5 812 21 doi 10 1037 0021 9010 85 5 812 ISSN 1939 1854 PMID 11055152 Big Five Personality Tests traits and background Personality and Aptitude Career Tests Retrieved 2017 07 01 Schwartz HA Eichstaedt JC Kern ML Dziurzynski L Ramones SM Agrawal M Shah A Kosinski M Stillwell D Seligman ME Ungar LH 2013 Personality gender and age in the language of social media the open vocabulary approach PLOS ONE 8 9 e73791 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 873791S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0073791 PMC 3783449 PMID 24086296 Block J March 1995 A contrarian view of the five factor approach to personality description Psychological Bulletin 117 2 187 215 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 117 2 187 PMID 7724687 Eysenck HJ 1991 Dimensions of personality 16 5 3 Personality and Individual Differences 12 8 773 90 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 91 90144 z Eysenck HJ 1992 Four ways five factors are not basic Personality and Individual Differences 13 6 667 73 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 92 90237 j Cattell RB May 1995 The fallacy of five factors in the personality sphere The Psychologist 207 08 a b c Trofimova I 2014 Observer bias an interaction of temperament traits with biases in the semantic perception of lexical material PLOS ONE 9 1 e85677 Bibcode 2014PLoSO 985677T doi 10 1371 journal pone 0085677 PMC 3903487 PMID 24475048 Text was copied from this source which is available under a Creative Commons Attribution 4 0 International License a b Paunonen SV Jackson DN 2000 What Is Beyond the Big Five Plenty PDF Journal of Personality 68 October 2000 821 35 doi 10 1111 1467 6494 00117 PMID 11001150 Archived from the original PDF on 2019 02 14 Retrieved 2012 01 15 a b Boyle GJ 2008 Critique of Five Factor Model FFM In Boyle GJ Matthews G Saklofske DH eds The Sage handbook of personality theory and assessment Vol 1 Personality theories and models Los Angeles CA Sage ISBN 978 1 4129 4651 3 Costa PT McCrae RR 1995 Solid ground in the wetlands of personality A reply to Block Psychological Bulletin 117 2 216 20 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 117 2 216 PMID 7724688 Block J 1995b Going beyond the five factors given Rejoinder to Costa and McCrae and Goldberg and Saucier Psychological Bulletin 117 2 226 29 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 117 2 226 Block J 2001 Millennial contrarianism Journal of Research in Personality 35 98 107 doi 10 1006 jrpe 2000 2293 S2CID 40747837 Block J 2010 The Five Factor framing of personality and beyond Some ruminations Psychological Inquiry 21 2 25 doi 10 1080 10478401003596626 S2CID 26355524 Cattell RB Boyle GJ Chant D 2002 The enriched behavioral prediction equation and its impact on structured learning and the dynamic calculus Psychological Review Submitted manuscript 109 1 202 05 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 109 1 202 PMID 11863038 Archived from the original on 2015 01 23 Retrieved 2018 10 25 Schacter DL Gilbert DT Wegner DM 2011 Psychology 2nd ed Worth pp 474 75 ISBN 978 1 4292 3719 2 Piekkola B 2011 Traits across cultures A neo Allportian perspective Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 31 2 24 doi 10 1037 a0022478 Paunonen SV Haddock G Forsterling F Keinonen M 2003 Broad versus Narrow Personality Measures and the Prediction of Behaviour Across Cultures European Journal of Personality 17 6 413 33 doi 10 1002 per 496 S2CID 143671349 McAdams DP 1995 What do we know when we know a person Journal of Personality 63 3 365 96 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 527 6832 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 1995 tb00500 x Saucier Gerard Srivastava Sanjay 2015 What makes a good structural model of personality Evaluating the big five and alternatives APA handbook of personality and social psychology Volume 4 Personality processes and individual differences Washington American Psychological Association pp 283 305 doi 10 1037 14343 013 ISBN 978 1 4338 1704 5 retrieved 2021 01 03 Musek J 2007 A general factor of personality Evidence for the Big One in the five factor model Journal of Research in Personality 41 6 1213 33 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2007 02 003 Van der Linden D te Nijenhuis J Bakker AB 2010 The General Factor of Personality A meta analysis of Big Five intercorrelations and a criterion related validity study PDF Journal of Research in Personality 44 3 315 27 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2010 03 003 Archived from the original PDF on 2012 07 11 Retrieved 2012 06 17 a b Eysenck HJ 1992 Four ways five factors are not basic PDF Personality and Individual Differences 13 8 667 73 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 92 90237 j Archived from the original PDF on 2012 11 07 Retrieved 2012 06 17 Costa PT McCrae RR 1992 Reply to Eysenck Personality and Individual Differences 13 8 861 65 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 92 90002 7 Gurven M von Rueden C Massenkoff M Kaplan H Lero Vie M 2013 APA PsycNet Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 104 2 354 370 doi 10 1037 a0030841 PMC 4104167 PMID 23245291 Block J 2010 The five factor framing of personality and beyond Some ruminations Psychological Inquiry 21 1 2 25 doi 10 1080 10478401003596626 S2CID 26355524 Ashton MC Lee K Goldberg LR November 2004 A hierarchical analysis of 1 710 English personality descriptive adjectives Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87 5 707 21 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 87 5 707 PMID 15535781 Ashton MC Lee K de Vries RE May 2014 The HEXACO Honesty Humility Agreeableness and Emotionality factors a review of research and theory Personality and Social Psychology Review 18 2 139 52 doi 10 1177 1088868314523838 PMID 24577101 S2CID 38312803 span, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.