fbpx
Wikipedia

Limbic system

The limbic system, also known as the paleomammalian cortex, is a set of brain structures located on both sides of the thalamus, immediately beneath the medial temporal lobe of the cerebrum primarily in the forebrain.[1]

Limbic system
Cross section of the human brain showing parts of the limbic system from below.
Traité d'Anatomie et de Physiologie (1786)
The limbic system largely consists of what was previously known as the limbic lobe.
Details
Identifiers
LatinSystema limbicum
MeSHD008032
NeuroNames2055
FMA242000
Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy
[edit on Wikidata]

Its various components support a variety of functions including emotion, behavior, long-term memory, and olfaction.[2]

The limbic system is involved in lower order emotional processing of input from sensory systems and consists of the amygdaloid nuclear complex (amygdala), mammillary bodies, stria medullaris, central gray and dorsal and ventral nuclei of Gudden.[3] This processed information is often relayed to a collection of structures from the telencephalon, diencephalon, and mesencephalon, including the prefrontal cortex, cingulate gyrus, limbic thalamus, hippocampus including the parahippocampal gyrus and subiculum, nucleus accumbens (limbic striatum), anterior hypothalamus, ventral tegmental area, midbrain raphe nuclei, habenular commissure, entorhinal cortex, and olfactory bulbs.[3][4][5]

Structure

 
Anatomical components of the limbic system

The limbic system was originally defined by Paul D. MacLean as a series of cortical structures surrounding the boundary between the cerebral hemispheres and the brainstem. The name "limbic" comes from the Latin word for the border, limbus, and these structures were known together as the limbic lobe.[6] Further studies began to associate these areas with emotional and motivational processes and linked them to subcortical components that were then grouped into the limbic system.[7]

Currently, it is not considered an isolated entity responsible for the neurological regulation of emotion, but rather one of the many parts of the brain that regulate visceral autonomic processes.[8] Therefore, the set of anatomical structures considered part of the limbic system is controversial. The following structures are, or have been considered, part of the limbic system:[9][10]

Function

The structures and interacting areas of the limbic system are involved in motivation, emotion, learning, and memory. The limbic system is where the subcortical structures meet the cerebral cortex.[1] The limbic system operates by influencing the endocrine system and the autonomic nervous system. It is highly interconnected with the nucleus accumbens, which plays a role in sexual arousal and the "high" derived from certain recreational drugs. These responses are heavily modulated by dopaminergic projections from the limbic system. In 1954, Olds and Milner found that rats with metal electrodes implanted into their nucleus accumbens, as well as their septal nuclei, repeatedly pressed a lever activating this region.[11]

The limbic system also interacts with the basal ganglia. The basal ganglia are a set of subcortical structures that direct intentional movements. The basal ganglia are located near the thalamus and hypothalamus. They receive input from the cerebral cortex, which sends outputs to the motor centers in the brain stem. A part of the basal ganglia called the striatum controls posture and movement. Recent studies indicate that if there is an inadequate supply of dopamine in the striatum, this can lead to the symptoms of Parkinson's disease.[1]

The limbic system is also tightly connected to the prefrontal cortex. Some scientists contend that this connection is related to the pleasure obtained from solving problems.[citation needed] To cure severe emotional disorders, this connection was sometimes surgically severed, a procedure of psychosurgery, called a prefrontal lobotomy (this is actually a misnomer). Patients having undergone this procedure often became passive and lacked all motivation.[12]

The limbic system is often incorrectly classified as a cerebral structure,[citation needed] but simply interacts heavily with the cerebral cortex. These interactions are closely linked to olfaction, emotions, drives, autonomic regulation, memory, and pathologically to encephalopathy, epilepsy, psychotic symptoms, cognitive defects.[13] The functional relevance of the limbic system has proven to serve many different functions such as affects/emotions, memory, sensory processing, time perception, attention, consciousness, instincts, autonomic/vegetative control, and actions/motor behavior. Some of the disorders associated with the limbic system and its interacting components are epilepsy and schizophrenia.[14]

Hippocampus

 
Location and basic anatomy of the hippocampus, as a coronal section

The hippocampus is involved with various processes relating to cognition and is one of the most well understood and heavily involved limbic interacting structure.

Spatial memory

The first and most widely researched area concerns memory, particularly spatial memory. Spatial memory was found to have many sub-regions in the hippocampus, such as the dentate gyrus (DG) in the dorsal hippocampus, the left hippocampus, and the parahippocampal region. The dorsal hippocampus was found to be an important component for the generation of new neurons, called adult-born granules (GC), in adolescence and adulthood.[15] These new neurons contribute to pattern separation in spatial memory, increasing the firing in cell networks, and overall causing stronger memory formations. This is thought to integrate spatial and episodic memories with the limbic system via a feedback loop that provides emotional context of a particular sensory input.[16]

While the dorsal hippocampus is involved in spatial memory formation, the left hippocampus is a participant in the recall of these spatial memories. Eichenbaum[17] and his team found, when studying the hippocampal lesions in rats, that the left hippocampus is "critical for effectively combining the 'what', 'when', and 'where' qualities of each experience to compose the retrieved memory". This makes the left hippocampus a key component in the retrieval of spatial memory. However, Spreng[18] found that the left hippocampus is a general concentrated region for binding together bits and pieces of memory composed not only by the hippocampus, but also by other areas of the brain to be recalled at a later time. Eichenbaum's research in 2007 also demonstrates that the parahippocampal area of the hippocampus is another specialized region for the retrieval of memories just like the left hippocampus.[citation needed]

Learning

The hippocampus, over the decades, has also been found to have a huge impact in learning. Curlik and Shors[19] examined the effects of neurogenesis in the hippocampus and its effects on learning. This researcher and his team employed many different types of mental and physical training on their subjects, and found that the hippocampus is highly responsive to these latter tasks. Thus, they discovered an upsurge of new neurons and neural circuits in the hippocampus as a result of the training, causing an overall improvement in the learning of the task. This neurogenesis contributes to the creation of adult-born granules cells (GC), cells also described by Eichenbaum[17] in his own research on neurogenesis and its contributions to learning. The creation of these cells exhibited "enhanced excitability" in the dentate gyrus (DG) of the dorsal hippocampus, impacting the hippocampus and its contribution to the learning process.[17]

Hippocampus damage

Damage related to the hippocampal region of the brain has reported vast effects on overall cognitive functioning, particularly memory such as spatial memory. As previously mentioned, spatial memory is a cognitive function greatly intertwined with the hippocampus. While damage to the hippocampus may be a result of a brain injury or other injuries of that sort, researchers particularly investigated the effects that high emotional arousal and certain types of drugs had on the recall ability in this specific memory type. In particular, in a study performed by Parkard,[20] rats were given the task of correctly making their way through a maze. In the first condition, rats were stressed by shock or restraint which caused a high emotional arousal. When completing the maze task, these rats had an impaired effect on their hippocampal-dependent memory when compared to the control group. Then, in a second condition, a group of rats were injected with anxiogenic drugs. Like the former these results reported similar outcomes, in that hippocampal-memory was also impaired. Studies such as these reinforce the impact that the hippocampus has on memory processing, in particular the recall function of spatial memory. Furthermore, impairment to the hippocampus can occur from prolonged exposure to stress hormones such as glucocorticoids (GCs), which target the hippocampus and cause disruption in explicit memory.[21]

In an attempt to curtail life-threatening epileptic seizures, 27-year-old Henry Gustav Molaison underwent bilateral removal of almost all of his hippocampus in 1953. Over the course of fifty years he participated in thousands of tests and research projects that provided specific information on exactly what he had lost. Semantic and episodic events faded within minutes, having never reached his long-term memory, yet emotions, unconnected from the details of causation, were often retained. Dr. Suzanne Corkin, who worked with him for 46 years until his death, described the contribution of this tragic "experiment" in her 2013 book.[22]

Amygdala

Episodic-autobiographical memory (EAM) networks

Another integrative part of the limbic system, the amygdala, which is the deepest part of the limbic system, is involved in many cognitive processes and is largely considered the most primordial and vital part of the limbic system. Like the hippocampus, processes in the amygdala seem to impact memory; however, it is not spatial memory as in the hippocampus but the semantic division of episodic-autobiographical memory (EAM) networks. Markowitsch's[23] amygdala research shows it encodes, stores, and retrieves EAM memories. To delve deeper into these types of processes by the amygdala, Markowitsch[23] and his team provided extensive evidence through investigations that the "amygdala's main function is to charge cues so that mnemonic events of a specific emotional significance can be successfully searched within the appropriate neural nets and re-activated." These cues for emotional events created by the amygdala encompass the EAM networks previously mentioned.

Attentional and emotional processes

Besides memory, the amygdala also seems to be an important brain region involved in attentional and emotional processes. First, to define attention in cognitive terms, attention is the ability to focus on some stimuli while ignoring others. Thus, the amygdala seems to be an important structure in this ability.

Foremost, however, this structure was historically thought to be linked to fear, allowing the individual to take action in response to that fear. However, as time has gone by, researchers such as Pessoa,[24] generalized this concept with help from evidence of EEG recordings, and concluded that the amygdala helps an organism to define a stimulus and therefore respond accordingly. However, when the amygdala was initially thought to be linked to fear, this gave way for research in the amygdala for emotional processes. Kheirbek[15] demonstrated research that the amygdala is involved in emotional processes, in particular the ventral hippocampus. He described the ventral hippocampus as having a role in neurogenesis and the creation of adult-born granule cells (GC). These cells not only were a crucial part of neurogenesis and the strengthening of spatial memory and learning in the hippocampus but also appear to be an essential component to the function of the amygdala. A deficit of these cells, as Pessoa (2009) predicted in his studies, would result in low emotional functioning, leading to high retention rate of mental diseases, such as anxiety disorders.[citation needed]

Social processing

Social processing, specifically the evaluation of faces in social processing, is an area of cognition specific to the amygdala. In a study done by Todorov,[25] fMRI tasks were performed with participants to evaluate whether the amygdala was involved in the general evaluation of faces. After the study, Todorov concluded from his fMRI results that the amygdala did indeed play a key role in the general evaluation of faces. However, in a study performed by researchers Koscik[26] and his team, the trait of trustworthiness was particularly examined in the evaluation of faces. Koscik and his team demonstrated that the amygdala was involved in evaluating the trustworthiness of an individual. They investigated how brain damage to the amygdala played a role in trustworthiness, and found that individuals with damaged amygdalas tended to confuse trust and betrayal, and thus placed trust in those having done them wrong. Furthermore, Rule,[27] along with his colleagues, expanded on the idea of the amygdala in its critique of trustworthiness in others by performing a study in 2009 in which he examined the amygdala's role in evaluating general first impressions and relating them to real-world outcomes. Their study involved first impressions of CEOs. Rule demonstrated that while the amygdala did play a role in the evaluation of trustworthiness, as observed by Koscik in his own research two years later in 2011, the amygdala also played a generalized role in the overall evaluation of first impression of faces. This latter conclusion, along with Todorov's study on the amygdala's role in general evaluations of faces and Koscik's research on trustworthiness and the amygdala, further solidified evidence that the amygdala plays a role in overall social processing.

Klüver–Bucy syndrome

Based on experiments done on monkeys, the destruction of the temporal cortex almost always led to damage of the amygdala. This damage done to the amygdala led the physiologists Kluver and Bucy to pinpoint major changes in the behavior of the monkeys. The monkeys demonstrated the following changes:

  1. The monkeys were not afraid of anything.
  2. The monkeys had extreme curiosity about everything.
  3. The monkeys forgot rapidly.
  4. The monkeys had a tendency to place everything in its mouth.
  5. The monkeys often had a sexual drive so strong that they attempted to copulate with immature animals, animals of the same sex, or even animals of a different species.

This set of behavioral change came to be known as the Klüver–Bucy syndrome.

Evolutionary claims

Paul D. MacLean, as part of his triune brain theory (which is now considered outdated [28][29]), hypothesized that the limbic system is older than other parts of the forebrain, and that it developed to manage circuitry attributed to the fight or flight first identified by Hans Selye[30] in his report of the General Adaptation Syndrome in 1936. It may be considered a part of survival adaptation in reptiles as well as mammals (including humans). MacLean postulated that the human brain has evolved three components, that evolved successively, with more recent components developing at the top/front. These components are, respectively:

  1. The archipallium or primitive ("reptilian") brain, comprising the structures of the brain stem – medulla, pons, cerebellum, mesencephalon, the oldest basal nuclei – the globus pallidus and the olfactory bulbs.
  2. The paleopallium or intermediate ("old mammalian") brain, comprising the structures of the limbic system.
  3. The neopallium, also known as the superior or rational ("new mammalian") brain, comprises almost the whole of the hemispheres (made up of a more recent type of cortex, called neocortex) and some subcortical neuronal groups. It corresponds to the brain of the superior mammals, thus including the primates and, as a consequence, the human species. Similar development of the neocortex in mammalian species unrelated to humans and primates has also occurred, for example in cetaceans and elephants; thus the designation of "superior mammals" is not an evolutionary one, as it has occurred independently in different species.[dubious ] The evolution of higher degrees of intelligence is an example of convergent evolution, and is also seen in non-mammals such as birds.[citation needed]

According to Maclean, each of the components, although connected with the others, retained "their peculiar types of intelligence, subjectivity, sense of time and space, memory, mobility and other less specific functions".

However, while the categorization into structures is reasonable, the recent studies of the limbic system of tetrapods, both living and extinct, have challenged several aspects of this hypothesis, notably the accuracy of the terms "reptilian" and "old mammalian". The common ancestors of reptiles and mammals had a well-developed limbic system in which the basic subdivisions and connections of the amygdalar nuclei were established.[31] Further, birds, which evolved from the dinosaurs, which in turn evolved separately but around the same time as the mammals, have a well-developed limbic system. While the anatomic structures of the limbic system are different in birds and mammals, there are functional equivalents.[citation needed]

History

Etymology and history

The term limbic comes from the Latin limbus, for "border" or "edge", or, particularly in medical terminology, a border of an anatomical component. Paul Broca coined the term based on its physical location in the brain, sandwiched between two functionally different components.

The limbic system is a term that was introduced in 1949 by the American physician and neuroscientist, Paul D. MacLean.[32][33] The French physician Paul Broca first called this part of the brain le grand lobe limbique in 1878.[6] He examined the differentiation between deeply recessed cortical tissue and underlying, subcortical nuclei.[34] However, most of its putative role in emotion was developed only in 1937 when the American physician James Papez described his anatomical model of emotion, the Papez circuit.[35]

The first evidence that the limbic system was responsible for the cortical representation of emotions was discovered in 1939, by Heinrich Kluver and Paul Bucy. Kluver and Bucy, after much research, demonstrated that the bilateral removal of the temporal lobes in monkeys created an extreme behavioral syndrome. After performing a temporal lobectomy, the monkeys showed a decrease in aggression. The animals revealed a reduced threshold to visual stimuli, and were thus unable to recognize objects that were once familiar.[36] MacLean expanded these ideas to include additional structures in a more dispersed "limbic system", more on the lines of the system described above.[33] MacLean developed the theory of the "triune brain" to explain its evolution and to try to reconcile rational human behavior with its more "primal" and "violent" side. He became interested in the brain's control of emotion and behavior. After initial studies of brain activity in epileptic patients, he turned to cats, monkeys, and other models, using electrodes to stimulate different parts of the brain in conscious animals recording their responses.[37]

In the 1950s, he began to trace individual behaviors like aggression and sexual arousal to their physiological sources. He postulated the limbic system as the brain's center of emotions, including the hippocampus and amygdala. Developing observations made by Papez, he hypothesized that the limbic system had evolved in early mammals to control fight-or-flight responses and react to both emotionally pleasurable and painful sensations. The concept is now broadly accepted in neuroscience.[citation needed][38] Additionally, MacLean said that the idea of the limbic system leads to a recognition that its presence "represents the history of the evolution of mammals and their distinctive family way of life."[citation needed]

In the 1960s, Dr. MacLean enlarged his theory to address the human brain's overall structure and divided its evolution into three parts, an idea that he termed the triune brain. In addition to identifying the limbic system, he hypothesized a supposedly more primitive brain called the R-complex, related to reptiles, which controls basic functions like muscle movement and breathing. According to him, the third part, the neocortex, controls speech and reasoning and is the most recent evolutionary arrival.[39] The concept of the limbic system has since been further expanded and developed by Walle Nauta, Lennart Heimer, and others.[citation needed]

Academic dispute

There is controversy over the use of the term limbic system, with scientists such as Joseph E. LeDoux and Edmund Rolls arguing that the term be considered obsolete and abandoned.[40][41] Originally, the limbic system was believed to be the emotional center of the brain, with cognition being the business of the neocortex. However, cognition depends on acquisition and retention of memories, in which the hippocampus, a primary limbic interacting structure, is involved: hippocampus damage causes severe cognitive (memory) deficits. More important, the "boundaries" of the limbic system have been repeatedly redefined because of advances in neuroscience.[40] Therefore, while it is true that limbic interacting structures are more closely related to emotion, the limbic system itself is best thought of as a component of a larger emotional processing plant. It is essentially responsible for sifting through and organizing lower order processing, and relaying sensory information to other brain areas for higher order emotional processing.[citation needed]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c Schacter, Daniel L. 2012. Psychology.sec. 3.20
  2. ^ Medline Plus Medical Encyclopedia
  3. ^ a b Morgane, PJ (Feb 2005). "A review of systems and networks of the limbic forebrain/limbic midbrain". Progress in Neurobiology. 75 (2): 143–60. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.01.001. PMID 15784304. S2CID 2612681.
  4. ^ Catani, M; Dell'Acqua, F; Thiebaut De Schotten, M (2013). "A revised limbic system model for memory, emotion and behaviour". Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 37 (8): 1724–37. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.07.001. PMID 23850593. S2CID 28044712.
  5. ^ Catani, M.; Dell’Acqua, F.; Vergani, F.; Malik, F.; Hodge, H.; Roy, P.; Valabregue, R.; de Schotten, M. T. (2011). "Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain". Cortex. 48 (2): 273–291. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2011.12.001. PMID 22209688. S2CID 14566150.
  6. ^ a b Broca, P (1878). "Anatomie comparee des circonvolutions cerebrales: Le grand lobe limbique et la scissure limbique dans la serie des mammifères". Revue d'Anthropologie. 1: 385–498.
  7. ^ Morgane PJ, Galler JR, Mokler DJ (2005). "A review of systems and networks of the limbic forebrain/limbic midbrain". Progress in Neurobiology. 75 (2): 143–60. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.01.001. PMID 15784304. S2CID 2612681.
  8. ^ Blessing WW (1997). "Inadequate frameworks for understanding bodily homeostasis". Trends in Neurosciences. 20 (6): 235–239. doi:10.1016/S0166-2236(96)01029-6. PMID 9185301. S2CID 41159244.
  9. ^ Swenson, Rand. "Chapter 9 - Limbic System". Retrieved 9 January 2015.:
  10. ^ Rajmohan V, Mohandas E (2007). "The limbic system". Indian Journal of Psychiatry. 49 (2): 132–139. doi:10.4103/0019-5545.33264. PMC 2917081. PMID 20711399.
  11. ^ Olds, J.; Milner, P. (1954). "Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain". J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 47 (6): 419–427. doi:10.1037/h0058775. PMID 13233369.
  12. ^ Terrier, Louis-Marie; Lévêque, Marc; Amelot, Aymeric (2019-12-01). "Brain Lobotomy: A Historical and Moral Dilemma with No Alternative?". World Neurosurgery. 132: 211–218. doi:10.1016/j.wneu.2019.08.254. ISSN 1878-8750. PMID 31518743. S2CID 202571631.
  13. ^ Adams, R.D.; Victor, M. (1985). Principles of Neurology (3rd ed.). New York: MacGraw-Hill. ISBN 9780070002968.
  14. ^ lversen, S.D. (1984). "Recent advances in the anatomy and chemistry of the limbic system". Psychophannacology of the Limbic System: 1–16.
  15. ^ a b Kheirbeck, M.A.; Hen, R. (2011). "Dorsal vs ventral hippocampal neurogenensis: Implications for cognition and mood". Neuropsychopharmacology. 36 (1): 373–374. doi:10.1038/npp.2010.148. PMC 3055508. PMID 21116266.
  16. ^ Jin, Jingji (Dec 15, 2015). "Prefrontal-Hippocampal Interactions in Memory and Emotion". Front Syst Neurosci. 9 (1): 170. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2015.00170. PMC 4678200. PMID 26696844.
  17. ^ a b c Eichenbaum, H. (2007). "Comparative cognition, hippocampal function, and recollection". Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews. 2 (1): 47–66. doi:10.3819/ccbr.2008.20003.
  18. ^ Spreng, R. N.; Mar, R. A. (2012). "I remember you: A role for memory in social cognition and the functional neuroanatomy of their interaction". Brain Research. 1428: 43–50. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.024. PMC 3085056. PMID 21172325.
  19. ^ CurlikShors, D.; Shors, T.J. (2012). "Training your brain: Do mental and physical (map) training enhance cognition through the process of neurogenesis in the hippocampus?". Neuropharmacology. 64 (1): 506–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuropharm.2012.07.027. PMC 3445739. PMID 22898496.
  20. ^ Parkard, M.G. (2009). "Anxiety, cognition, and habit: A multiple memory systems perspective". Brain Research. 1293: 121–128. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.03.029. PMID 19328775. S2CID 39710208.
  21. ^ Sapolsky, Robert M. (2003). "Stress and Plasticity in the Limbic System". Neurochemical Research. 28 (11): 1735–1742. doi:10.1023/A:1026021307833. ISSN 0364-3190. PMID 14584827. S2CID 12012982.
  22. ^ Corkin, Suzanne (2013). Permanent Present Tense. Basic Books. ISBN 978-0465031597.
  23. ^ a b Markowitsch, H.J.; Staniloiu, A (2011). "Amygdala in action: Relaying biological and social significance to autobiographical memory". Neuropsychologia. 49 (4): 718–733. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.10.007. PMID 20933525. S2CID 12632856.
  24. ^ Pessoa, L. (2010). "Emotion and cognition and the amygdale: From "what is it?" to "what's to be done?"". Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3416–3429. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.038. PMC 2949460. PMID 20619280.
  25. ^ Todorov, A.; Engell, A. D. (2008). "The role of the amygdala in implicit evaluation of emotionally neutral faces". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 3 (4): 303–312. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn033. PMC 2607057. PMID 19015082.
  26. ^ Koscik, T.R.; Tranel, D. (2011). "The human amygdala is necessary for developing and expressing normal interpersonal trust". Neuropsychologia. 49 (4): 602–611. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.09.023. PMC 3056169. PMID 20920512.
  27. ^ Rule, N.O.; Moran, J. M.; Freeman, J. B.; Whitfield-Gabrieli, S.; Gabrieli, J. D. E.; Ambady, N. (2011). "Face value: Amygdala response reflects the validity of first impressions" (PDF). NeuroImage. 54 (1): 734–741. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.007. hdl:1807/33192. PMID 20633663. S2CID 13253523.
  28. ^ Cesario, J., Johnson, D. J., & Eisthen, H. L. (2020). Your Brain Is Not an Onion With a Tiny Reptile Inside. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(3), 255–260. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721420917687
  29. ^ Steffen PR, Hedges D, Matheson R. The Brain Is Adaptive Not Triune: How the Brain Responds to Threat, Challenge, and Change. Front Psychiatry. 2022 Apr 1;13:802606. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.802606. PMID: 35432041; PMCID: PMC9010774.
  30. ^ Selye, Hans (1 January 1950). "The physiology and pathology of exposure to stress". APA PsycNET.
  31. ^ Bruce LL, Neary TJ (1995). "The limbic system of tetrapods: a comparative analysis of cortical and amygdalar populations". Brain Behav. Evol. 46 (4–5): 224–34. doi:10.1159/000113276. PMID 8564465.
  32. ^ MacLean, PD (1949). "Psychosomatic disease and the visceral brain; recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion". Psychosom Med. 11 (6): 338–53. doi:10.1097/00006842-194911000-00003. PMID 15410445. S2CID 12779897.
  33. ^ a b MacLean, P.D. (1952). "Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system (visceral brain)". Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology. 4 (4): 407–418. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(52)90073-4. PMID 12998590.
  34. ^ Binder, Marc D (2009). Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Springer. p. 2592.
  35. ^ Papez, JW. (1995). "A proposed mechanism of emotion. 1937". J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 7 (1): 103–12. doi:10.1176/jnp.7.1.103. PMID 7711480.
  36. ^ Kluver, H.; Bucy, P. C. (June 1937). "Psychic blindness" and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy". American Journal of Physiology. 119 (2): 254–284. Retrieved 15 February 2019.
  37. ^ Robert L, Isaacson (31 December 1992). "A fuzzy limbic system". Behavioural Brain Research. 52 (2): 129–131. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80222-0. PMID 1294191. S2CID 9512977.
  38. ^ Simpson, J. A. (November 1973). "The Limbic System". J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 39 (11): 1138. doi:10.1136/jnnp.39.11.1138-a. PMC 1083320.
  39. ^ Fulton, John (November 1953). "The Limbic System". Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine. 26 (2): 107–118. PMC 2599366. PMID 13123136.
  40. ^ a b LeDoux, Joseph E. (2000). "Emotion Circuits in the Brain". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 23: 155–184. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.155. PMID 10845062.
  41. ^ Rolls, Edmund T. (January 2015). "Limbic systems for emotion and for memory, but no single limbic system". Cortex. 62: 119–157. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.12.005. PMID 24439664. S2CID 16524733.

External links

limbic, system, limbic, system, also, known, paleomammalian, cortex, brain, structures, located, both, sides, thalamus, immediately, beneath, medial, temporal, lobe, cerebrum, primarily, forebrain, cross, section, human, brain, showing, parts, limbic, system, . The limbic system also known as the paleomammalian cortex is a set of brain structures located on both sides of the thalamus immediately beneath the medial temporal lobe of the cerebrum primarily in the forebrain 1 Limbic systemCross section of the human brain showing parts of the limbic system from below Traite d Anatomie et de Physiologie 1786 The limbic system largely consists of what was previously known as the limbic lobe DetailsIdentifiersLatinSystema limbicumMeSHD008032NeuroNames2055FMA242000Anatomical terms of neuroanatomy edit on Wikidata Its various components support a variety of functions including emotion behavior long term memory and olfaction 2 The limbic system is involved in lower order emotional processing of input from sensory systems and consists of the amygdaloid nuclear complex amygdala mammillary bodies stria medullaris central gray and dorsal and ventral nuclei of Gudden 3 This processed information is often relayed to a collection of structures from the telencephalon diencephalon and mesencephalon including the prefrontal cortex cingulate gyrus limbic thalamus hippocampus including the parahippocampal gyrus and subiculum nucleus accumbens limbic striatum anterior hypothalamus ventral tegmental area midbrain raphe nuclei habenular commissure entorhinal cortex and olfactory bulbs 3 4 5 Contents 1 Structure 2 Function 2 1 Hippocampus 2 1 1 Spatial memory 2 1 2 Learning 2 1 3 Hippocampus damage 2 2 Amygdala 2 2 1 Episodic autobiographical memory EAM networks 2 2 2 Attentional and emotional processes 2 2 3 Social processing 2 2 4 Kluver Bucy syndrome 3 Evolutionary claims 4 History 4 1 Etymology and history 4 2 Academic dispute 5 See also 6 References 7 External linksStructure Edit Anatomical components of the limbic system The limbic system was originally defined by Paul D MacLean as a series of cortical structures surrounding the boundary between the cerebral hemispheres and the brainstem The name limbic comes from the Latin word for the border limbus and these structures were known together as the limbic lobe 6 Further studies began to associate these areas with emotional and motivational processes and linked them to subcortical components that were then grouped into the limbic system 7 Currently it is not considered an isolated entity responsible for the neurological regulation of emotion but rather one of the many parts of the brain that regulate visceral autonomic processes 8 Therefore the set of anatomical structures considered part of the limbic system is controversial The following structures are or have been considered part of the limbic system 9 10 Cortical areas Limbic lobe Orbitofrontal cortex a region in the frontal lobe involved in the process of decision making Piriform cortex part of the olfactory system Entorhinal cortex related to memory and associative components Fornix a white matter structure connecting the hippocampus with other brain structures particularly the mammillary bodies and septal nuclei Subcortical areas Septal nuclei a set of structures that lie in front of the lamina terminalis considered a pleasure zone Hippocampus and associated structures play a central role in the consolidation of new memories Amygdala located deep within the temporal lobes and related with a number of emotional processes Nucleus accumbens involved in reward pleasure and addiction Diencephalic structures Hypothalamus a center for the limbic system connected with the frontal lobes septal nuclei and the brain stem reticular formation via the medial forebrain bundle with the hippocampus via the fornix and with the thalamus via the mammillothalamic fasciculus regulates many autonomic processes Mammillary bodies part of the hypothalamus that receives signals from the hippocampus via the fornix and projects them to the thalamus Anterior nuclei of thalamus receive input from the mammillary bodies and involved in memory processingFunction EditThe structures and interacting areas of the limbic system are involved in motivation emotion learning and memory The limbic system is where the subcortical structures meet the cerebral cortex 1 The limbic system operates by influencing the endocrine system and the autonomic nervous system It is highly interconnected with the nucleus accumbens which plays a role in sexual arousal and the high derived from certain recreational drugs These responses are heavily modulated by dopaminergic projections from the limbic system In 1954 Olds and Milner found that rats with metal electrodes implanted into their nucleus accumbens as well as their septal nuclei repeatedly pressed a lever activating this region 11 The limbic system also interacts with the basal ganglia The basal ganglia are a set of subcortical structures that direct intentional movements The basal ganglia are located near the thalamus and hypothalamus They receive input from the cerebral cortex which sends outputs to the motor centers in the brain stem A part of the basal ganglia called the striatum controls posture and movement Recent studies indicate that if there is an inadequate supply of dopamine in the striatum this can lead to the symptoms of Parkinson s disease 1 The limbic system is also tightly connected to the prefrontal cortex Some scientists contend that this connection is related to the pleasure obtained from solving problems citation needed To cure severe emotional disorders this connection was sometimes surgically severed a procedure of psychosurgery called a prefrontal lobotomy this is actually a misnomer Patients having undergone this procedure often became passive and lacked all motivation 12 The limbic system is often incorrectly classified as a cerebral structure citation needed but simply interacts heavily with the cerebral cortex These interactions are closely linked to olfaction emotions drives autonomic regulation memory and pathologically to encephalopathy epilepsy psychotic symptoms cognitive defects 13 The functional relevance of the limbic system has proven to serve many different functions such as affects emotions memory sensory processing time perception attention consciousness instincts autonomic vegetative control and actions motor behavior Some of the disorders associated with the limbic system and its interacting components are epilepsy and schizophrenia 14 Hippocampus Edit Location and basic anatomy of the hippocampus as a coronal section The hippocampus is involved with various processes relating to cognition and is one of the most well understood and heavily involved limbic interacting structure Spatial memory Edit The first and most widely researched area concerns memory particularly spatial memory Spatial memory was found to have many sub regions in the hippocampus such as the dentate gyrus DG in the dorsal hippocampus the left hippocampus and the parahippocampal region The dorsal hippocampus was found to be an important component for the generation of new neurons called adult born granules GC in adolescence and adulthood 15 These new neurons contribute to pattern separation in spatial memory increasing the firing in cell networks and overall causing stronger memory formations This is thought to integrate spatial and episodic memories with the limbic system via a feedback loop that provides emotional context of a particular sensory input 16 While the dorsal hippocampus is involved in spatial memory formation the left hippocampus is a participant in the recall of these spatial memories Eichenbaum 17 and his team found when studying the hippocampal lesions in rats that the left hippocampus is critical for effectively combining the what when and where qualities of each experience to compose the retrieved memory This makes the left hippocampus a key component in the retrieval of spatial memory However Spreng 18 found that the left hippocampus is a general concentrated region for binding together bits and pieces of memory composed not only by the hippocampus but also by other areas of the brain to be recalled at a later time Eichenbaum s research in 2007 also demonstrates that the parahippocampal area of the hippocampus is another specialized region for the retrieval of memories just like the left hippocampus citation needed Learning Edit The hippocampus over the decades has also been found to have a huge impact in learning Curlik and Shors 19 examined the effects of neurogenesis in the hippocampus and its effects on learning This researcher and his team employed many different types of mental and physical training on their subjects and found that the hippocampus is highly responsive to these latter tasks Thus they discovered an upsurge of new neurons and neural circuits in the hippocampus as a result of the training causing an overall improvement in the learning of the task This neurogenesis contributes to the creation of adult born granules cells GC cells also described by Eichenbaum 17 in his own research on neurogenesis and its contributions to learning The creation of these cells exhibited enhanced excitability in the dentate gyrus DG of the dorsal hippocampus impacting the hippocampus and its contribution to the learning process 17 Hippocampus damage Edit Damage related to the hippocampal region of the brain has reported vast effects on overall cognitive functioning particularly memory such as spatial memory As previously mentioned spatial memory is a cognitive function greatly intertwined with the hippocampus While damage to the hippocampus may be a result of a brain injury or other injuries of that sort researchers particularly investigated the effects that high emotional arousal and certain types of drugs had on the recall ability in this specific memory type In particular in a study performed by Parkard 20 rats were given the task of correctly making their way through a maze In the first condition rats were stressed by shock or restraint which caused a high emotional arousal When completing the maze task these rats had an impaired effect on their hippocampal dependent memory when compared to the control group Then in a second condition a group of rats were injected with anxiogenic drugs Like the former these results reported similar outcomes in that hippocampal memory was also impaired Studies such as these reinforce the impact that the hippocampus has on memory processing in particular the recall function of spatial memory Furthermore impairment to the hippocampus can occur from prolonged exposure to stress hormones such as glucocorticoids GCs which target the hippocampus and cause disruption in explicit memory 21 In an attempt to curtail life threatening epileptic seizures 27 year old Henry Gustav Molaison underwent bilateral removal of almost all of his hippocampus in 1953 Over the course of fifty years he participated in thousands of tests and research projects that provided specific information on exactly what he had lost Semantic and episodic events faded within minutes having never reached his long term memory yet emotions unconnected from the details of causation were often retained Dr Suzanne Corkin who worked with him for 46 years until his death described the contribution of this tragic experiment in her 2013 book 22 Amygdala Edit Main article Amygdala Episodic autobiographical memory EAM networks Edit Another integrative part of the limbic system the amygdala which is the deepest part of the limbic system is involved in many cognitive processes and is largely considered the most primordial and vital part of the limbic system Like the hippocampus processes in the amygdala seem to impact memory however it is not spatial memory as in the hippocampus but the semantic division of episodic autobiographical memory EAM networks Markowitsch s 23 amygdala research shows it encodes stores and retrieves EAM memories To delve deeper into these types of processes by the amygdala Markowitsch 23 and his team provided extensive evidence through investigations that the amygdala s main function is to charge cues so that mnemonic events of a specific emotional significance can be successfully searched within the appropriate neural nets and re activated These cues for emotional events created by the amygdala encompass the EAM networks previously mentioned Attentional and emotional processes Edit Besides memory the amygdala also seems to be an important brain region involved in attentional and emotional processes First to define attention in cognitive terms attention is the ability to focus on some stimuli while ignoring others Thus the amygdala seems to be an important structure in this ability Foremost however this structure was historically thought to be linked to fear allowing the individual to take action in response to that fear However as time has gone by researchers such as Pessoa 24 generalized this concept with help from evidence of EEG recordings and concluded that the amygdala helps an organism to define a stimulus and therefore respond accordingly However when the amygdala was initially thought to be linked to fear this gave way for research in the amygdala for emotional processes Kheirbek 15 demonstrated research that the amygdala is involved in emotional processes in particular the ventral hippocampus He described the ventral hippocampus as having a role in neurogenesis and the creation of adult born granule cells GC These cells not only were a crucial part of neurogenesis and the strengthening of spatial memory and learning in the hippocampus but also appear to be an essential component to the function of the amygdala A deficit of these cells as Pessoa 2009 predicted in his studies would result in low emotional functioning leading to high retention rate of mental diseases such as anxiety disorders citation needed Social processing Edit Social processing specifically the evaluation of faces in social processing is an area of cognition specific to the amygdala In a study done by Todorov 25 fMRI tasks were performed with participants to evaluate whether the amygdala was involved in the general evaluation of faces After the study Todorov concluded from his fMRI results that the amygdala did indeed play a key role in the general evaluation of faces However in a study performed by researchers Koscik 26 and his team the trait of trustworthiness was particularly examined in the evaluation of faces Koscik and his team demonstrated that the amygdala was involved in evaluating the trustworthiness of an individual They investigated how brain damage to the amygdala played a role in trustworthiness and found that individuals with damaged amygdalas tended to confuse trust and betrayal and thus placed trust in those having done them wrong Furthermore Rule 27 along with his colleagues expanded on the idea of the amygdala in its critique of trustworthiness in others by performing a study in 2009 in which he examined the amygdala s role in evaluating general first impressions and relating them to real world outcomes Their study involved first impressions of CEOs Rule demonstrated that while the amygdala did play a role in the evaluation of trustworthiness as observed by Koscik in his own research two years later in 2011 the amygdala also played a generalized role in the overall evaluation of first impression of faces This latter conclusion along with Todorov s study on the amygdala s role in general evaluations of faces and Koscik s research on trustworthiness and the amygdala further solidified evidence that the amygdala plays a role in overall social processing Kluver Bucy syndrome Edit Main article Kluver Bucy syndrome Based on experiments done on monkeys the destruction of the temporal cortex almost always led to damage of the amygdala This damage done to the amygdala led the physiologists Kluver and Bucy to pinpoint major changes in the behavior of the monkeys The monkeys demonstrated the following changes The monkeys were not afraid of anything The monkeys had extreme curiosity about everything The monkeys forgot rapidly The monkeys had a tendency to place everything in its mouth The monkeys often had a sexual drive so strong that they attempted to copulate with immature animals animals of the same sex or even animals of a different species This set of behavioral change came to be known as the Kluver Bucy syndrome Evolutionary claims EditPaul D MacLean as part of his triune brain theory which is now considered outdated 28 29 hypothesized that the limbic system is older than other parts of the forebrain and that it developed to manage circuitry attributed to the fight or flight first identified by Hans Selye 30 in his report of the General Adaptation Syndrome in 1936 It may be considered a part of survival adaptation in reptiles as well as mammals including humans MacLean postulated that the human brain has evolved three components that evolved successively with more recent components developing at the top front These components are respectively The archipallium or primitive reptilian brain comprising the structures of the brain stem medulla pons cerebellum mesencephalon the oldest basal nuclei the globus pallidus and the olfactory bulbs The paleopallium or intermediate old mammalian brain comprising the structures of the limbic system The neopallium also known as the superior or rational new mammalian brain comprises almost the whole of the hemispheres made up of a more recent type of cortex called neocortex and some subcortical neuronal groups It corresponds to the brain of the superior mammals thus including the primates and as a consequence the human species Similar development of the neocortex in mammalian species unrelated to humans and primates has also occurred for example in cetaceans and elephants thus the designation of superior mammals is not an evolutionary one as it has occurred independently in different species dubious discuss The evolution of higher degrees of intelligence is an example of convergent evolution and is also seen in non mammals such as birds citation needed According to Maclean each of the components although connected with the others retained their peculiar types of intelligence subjectivity sense of time and space memory mobility and other less specific functions However while the categorization into structures is reasonable the recent studies of the limbic system of tetrapods both living and extinct have challenged several aspects of this hypothesis notably the accuracy of the terms reptilian and old mammalian The common ancestors of reptiles and mammals had a well developed limbic system in which the basic subdivisions and connections of the amygdalar nuclei were established 31 Further birds which evolved from the dinosaurs which in turn evolved separately but around the same time as the mammals have a well developed limbic system While the anatomic structures of the limbic system are different in birds and mammals there are functional equivalents citation needed History EditEtymology and history Edit The term limbic comes from the Latin limbus for border or edge or particularly in medical terminology a border of an anatomical component Paul Broca coined the term based on its physical location in the brain sandwiched between two functionally different components The limbic system is a term that was introduced in 1949 by the American physician and neuroscientist Paul D MacLean 32 33 The French physician Paul Broca first called this part of the brain le grand lobe limbique in 1878 6 He examined the differentiation between deeply recessed cortical tissue and underlying subcortical nuclei 34 However most of its putative role in emotion was developed only in 1937 when the American physician James Papez described his anatomical model of emotion the Papez circuit 35 The first evidence that the limbic system was responsible for the cortical representation of emotions was discovered in 1939 by Heinrich Kluver and Paul Bucy Kluver and Bucy after much research demonstrated that the bilateral removal of the temporal lobes in monkeys created an extreme behavioral syndrome After performing a temporal lobectomy the monkeys showed a decrease in aggression The animals revealed a reduced threshold to visual stimuli and were thus unable to recognize objects that were once familiar 36 MacLean expanded these ideas to include additional structures in a more dispersed limbic system more on the lines of the system described above 33 MacLean developed the theory of the triune brain to explain its evolution and to try to reconcile rational human behavior with its more primal and violent side He became interested in the brain s control of emotion and behavior After initial studies of brain activity in epileptic patients he turned to cats monkeys and other models using electrodes to stimulate different parts of the brain in conscious animals recording their responses 37 In the 1950s he began to trace individual behaviors like aggression and sexual arousal to their physiological sources He postulated the limbic system as the brain s center of emotions including the hippocampus and amygdala Developing observations made by Papez he hypothesized that the limbic system had evolved in early mammals to control fight or flight responses and react to both emotionally pleasurable and painful sensations The concept is now broadly accepted in neuroscience citation needed 38 Additionally MacLean said that the idea of the limbic system leads to a recognition that its presence represents the history of the evolution of mammals and their distinctive family way of life citation needed In the 1960s Dr MacLean enlarged his theory to address the human brain s overall structure and divided its evolution into three parts an idea that he termed the triune brain In addition to identifying the limbic system he hypothesized a supposedly more primitive brain called the R complex related to reptiles which controls basic functions like muscle movement and breathing According to him the third part the neocortex controls speech and reasoning and is the most recent evolutionary arrival 39 The concept of the limbic system has since been further expanded and developed by Walle Nauta Lennart Heimer and others citation needed Academic dispute Edit There is controversy over the use of the term limbic system with scientists such as Joseph E LeDoux and Edmund Rolls arguing that the term be considered obsolete and abandoned 40 41 Originally the limbic system was believed to be the emotional center of the brain with cognition being the business of the neocortex However cognition depends on acquisition and retention of memories in which the hippocampus a primary limbic interacting structure is involved hippocampus damage causes severe cognitive memory deficits More important the boundaries of the limbic system have been repeatedly redefined because of advances in neuroscience 40 Therefore while it is true that limbic interacting structures are more closely related to emotion the limbic system itself is best thought of as a component of a larger emotional processing plant It is essentially responsible for sifting through and organizing lower order processing and relaying sensory information to other brain areas for higher order emotional processing citation needed See also Edit Wikimedia Commons has media related to Limbic system This article uses anatomical terminology Amygdala Hijack Hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis HPA axis Emotional memory Fundamentals of Neuroscience at Wikiversity Paralimbic cortex Triune brainReferences Edit a b c Schacter Daniel L 2012 Psychology sec 3 20 Medline Plus Medical Encyclopedia a b Morgane PJ Feb 2005 A review of systems and networks of the limbic forebrain limbic midbrain Progress in Neurobiology 75 2 143 60 doi 10 1016 j pneurobio 2005 01 001 PMID 15784304 S2CID 2612681 Catani M Dell Acqua F Thiebaut De Schotten M 2013 A revised limbic system model for memory emotion and behaviour Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 37 8 1724 37 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2013 07 001 PMID 23850593 S2CID 28044712 Catani M Dell Acqua F Vergani F Malik F Hodge H Roy P Valabregue R de Schotten M T 2011 Short frontal lobe connections of the human brain Cortex 48 2 273 291 doi 10 1016 j cortex 2011 12 001 PMID 22209688 S2CID 14566150 a b Broca P 1878 Anatomie comparee des circonvolutions cerebrales Le grand lobe limbique et la scissure limbique dans la serie des mammiferes Revue d Anthropologie 1 385 498 Morgane PJ Galler JR Mokler DJ 2005 A review of systems and networks of the limbic forebrain limbic midbrain Progress in Neurobiology 75 2 143 60 doi 10 1016 j pneurobio 2005 01 001 PMID 15784304 S2CID 2612681 Blessing WW 1997 Inadequate frameworks for understanding bodily homeostasis Trends in Neurosciences 20 6 235 239 doi 10 1016 S0166 2236 96 01029 6 PMID 9185301 S2CID 41159244 Swenson Rand Chapter 9 Limbic System Retrieved 9 January 2015 Rajmohan V Mohandas E 2007 The limbic system Indian Journal of Psychiatry 49 2 132 139 doi 10 4103 0019 5545 33264 PMC 2917081 PMID 20711399 Olds J Milner P 1954 Positive reinforcement produced by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of rat brain J Comp Physiol Psychol 47 6 419 427 doi 10 1037 h0058775 PMID 13233369 Terrier Louis Marie Leveque Marc Amelot Aymeric 2019 12 01 Brain Lobotomy A Historical and Moral Dilemma with No Alternative World Neurosurgery 132 211 218 doi 10 1016 j wneu 2019 08 254 ISSN 1878 8750 PMID 31518743 S2CID 202571631 Adams R D Victor M 1985 Principles of Neurology 3rd ed New York MacGraw Hill ISBN 9780070002968 lversen S D 1984 Recent advances in the anatomy and chemistry of the limbic system Psychophannacology of the Limbic System 1 16 a b Kheirbeck M A Hen R 2011 Dorsal vs ventral hippocampal neurogenensis Implications for cognition and mood Neuropsychopharmacology 36 1 373 374 doi 10 1038 npp 2010 148 PMC 3055508 PMID 21116266 Jin Jingji Dec 15 2015 Prefrontal Hippocampal Interactions in Memory and Emotion Front Syst Neurosci 9 1 170 doi 10 3389 fnsys 2015 00170 PMC 4678200 PMID 26696844 a b c Eichenbaum H 2007 Comparative cognition hippocampal function and recollection Comparative Cognition amp Behavior Reviews 2 1 47 66 doi 10 3819 ccbr 2008 20003 Spreng R N Mar R A 2012 I remember you A role for memory in social cognition and the functional neuroanatomy of their interaction Brain Research 1428 43 50 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2010 12 024 PMC 3085056 PMID 21172325 CurlikShors D Shors T J 2012 Training your brain Do mental and physical map training enhance cognition through the process of neurogenesis in the hippocampus Neuropharmacology 64 1 506 14 doi 10 1016 j neuropharm 2012 07 027 PMC 3445739 PMID 22898496 Parkard M G 2009 Anxiety cognition and habit A multiple memory systems perspective Brain Research 1293 121 128 doi 10 1016 j brainres 2009 03 029 PMID 19328775 S2CID 39710208 Sapolsky Robert M 2003 Stress and Plasticity in the Limbic System Neurochemical Research 28 11 1735 1742 doi 10 1023 A 1026021307833 ISSN 0364 3190 PMID 14584827 S2CID 12012982 Corkin Suzanne 2013 Permanent Present Tense Basic Books ISBN 978 0465031597 a b Markowitsch H J Staniloiu A 2011 Amygdala in action Relaying biological and social significance to autobiographical memory Neuropsychologia 49 4 718 733 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2010 10 007 PMID 20933525 S2CID 12632856 Pessoa L 2010 Emotion and cognition and the amygdale From what is it to what s to be done Neuropsychologia 48 12 3416 3429 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2010 06 038 PMC 2949460 PMID 20619280 Todorov A Engell A D 2008 The role of the amygdala in implicit evaluation of emotionally neutral faces Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 3 4 303 312 doi 10 1093 scan nsn033 PMC 2607057 PMID 19015082 Koscik T R Tranel D 2011 The human amygdala is necessary for developing and expressing normal interpersonal trust Neuropsychologia 49 4 602 611 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2010 09 023 PMC 3056169 PMID 20920512 Rule N O Moran J M Freeman J B Whitfield Gabrieli S Gabrieli J D E Ambady N 2011 Face value Amygdala response reflects the validity of first impressions PDF NeuroImage 54 1 734 741 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2010 07 007 hdl 1807 33192 PMID 20633663 S2CID 13253523 Cesario J Johnson D J amp Eisthen H L 2020 Your Brain Is Not an Onion With a Tiny Reptile Inside Current Directions in Psychological Science 29 3 255 260 https doi org 10 1177 0963721420917687 Steffen PR Hedges D Matheson R The Brain Is Adaptive Not Triune How the Brain Responds to Threat Challenge and Change Front Psychiatry 2022 Apr 1 13 802606 doi 10 3389 fpsyt 2022 802606 PMID 35432041 PMCID PMC9010774 Selye Hans 1 January 1950 The physiology and pathology of exposure to stress APA PsycNET Bruce LL Neary TJ 1995 The limbic system of tetrapods a comparative analysis of cortical and amygdalar populations Brain Behav Evol 46 4 5 224 34 doi 10 1159 000113276 PMID 8564465 MacLean PD 1949 Psychosomatic disease and the visceral brain recent developments bearing on the Papez theory of emotion Psychosom Med 11 6 338 53 doi 10 1097 00006842 194911000 00003 PMID 15410445 S2CID 12779897 a b MacLean P D 1952 Some psychiatric implications of physiological studies on frontotemporal portion of limbic system visceral brain Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 4 4 407 418 doi 10 1016 0013 4694 52 90073 4 PMID 12998590 Binder Marc D 2009 Encyclopedia of Neuroscience Springer p 2592 Papez JW 1995 A proposed mechanism of emotion 1937 J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 7 1 103 12 doi 10 1176 jnp 7 1 103 PMID 7711480 Kluver H Bucy P C June 1937 Psychic blindness and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy American Journal of Physiology 119 2 254 284 Retrieved 15 February 2019 Robert L Isaacson 31 December 1992 A fuzzy limbic system Behavioural Brain Research 52 2 129 131 doi 10 1016 S0166 4328 05 80222 0 PMID 1294191 S2CID 9512977 Simpson J A November 1973 The Limbic System J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 39 11 1138 doi 10 1136 jnnp 39 11 1138 a PMC 1083320 Fulton John November 1953 The Limbic System Yale Journal of Biology and Medicine 26 2 107 118 PMC 2599366 PMID 13123136 a b LeDoux Joseph E 2000 Emotion Circuits in the Brain Annual Review of Neuroscience 23 155 184 doi 10 1146 annurev neuro 23 1 155 PMID 10845062 Rolls Edmund T January 2015 Limbic systems for emotion and for memory but no single limbic system Cortex 62 119 157 doi 10 1016 j cortex 2013 12 005 PMID 24439664 S2CID 16524733 External links Edit Media related to Limbic system at Wikimedia Commons http biology about com od anatomy a aa042205a htm https qbi uq edu au brain brain anatomy limbic system Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Limbic system amp oldid 1131718185, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.