fbpx
Wikipedia

Conscientiousness

Conscientiousness is the personality trait of being careful, or diligent. Conscientiousness implies a desire to do a task well, and to take obligations to others seriously. Conscientious people tend to be efficient and organized as opposed to easy-going and disorderly. They exhibit a tendency to show self-discipline, act dutifully, and aim for achievement; they display planned rather than spontaneous behavior; and they are generally dependable. It is manifested in characteristic behaviors such as being neat, and systematic; also including such elements as carefulness, thoroughness, and deliberation (the tendency to think carefully before acting).[1]

Conscientiousness is one of the five traits of both the Five Factor Model and the HEXACO model of personality and is an aspect of what has traditionally been referred to as having character. Conscientious individuals are generally hard-working, and reliable. When taken to an extreme, they may also be "workaholics", perfectionists, and compulsive in their behavior.[2] People who score low on conscientiousness tend to be laid back, less goal-oriented, and less driven by success; they also are more likely to engage in antisocial and criminal behavior.[3]

Personality models

Conscientiousness is one of the five major dimensions in the Big Five model of personality (also called Five Factor Model or OCEAN), which consists of openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism. Two of many personality tests that assess these traits are Costa and McCrae's NEO PI-R[4] and Goldberg's NEO-IPIP.[5] According to these models, conscientiousness is considered to be a continuous dimension of personality, rather than a categorical 'type' of person.

In the NEO framework, Conscientiousness is seen as having six facets: Competence, Order, Dutifulness, Achievement Striving, Self-Discipline, and Deliberation. Other models suggest a smaller set of two "aspects": orderliness and industriousness form an intermediate level of organization, with orderliness associated with the desire to keep things organized and tidy and industriousness being more associated with productivity and work ethic.[6]

Other personality traits ((Low) extraversion, (high) agreeableness, (low) openness and (low) neuroticism) are linked to high conscientiousness along with impulse control. Behaviorally, low conscientiousness is associated with an inability to motivate one's self to perform tasks that the individual desires to accomplish.[4]

Conscientiousness also appears in other models of personality, such as Cloninger's Temperament and Character Inventory, in which it is related to both self-directedness and persistence.[7] It also includes the specific traits of rule consciousness and perfectionism in Cattell's 16 PF model. It is negatively associated with impulsive sensation-seeking in Zuckerman's alternative five model. Traits associated with conscientiousness are frequently assessed by self-report integrity tests given by various corporations to prospective employees.

Origin

Terms such as 'hard-working,' 'reliable,' and 'persevering' describe desirable aspects of character. Because it was once believed to be a moral evaluation, conscientiousness was overlooked as a real psychological attribute. The reality of individual differences in conscientiousness has now been clearly established by studies of cross-observer agreement. Peer and expert ratings confirm the self-reports that people make about their degrees of conscientiousness. Furthermore, both self-reports and observer ratings of conscientiousness predict real-life outcomes such as academic success.[citation needed]

During most of the 20th century, psychologists believed that personality traits could be divided into two categories: temperament and character. Temperament traits were thought to be biologically based, whereas character traits were thought to be learned either during childhood or throughout life. With the advent of the FFM (Five-Factor Model), behavior geneticists began systematic studies of the full range of personality traits, and it soon became clear that all five factors are substantially heritable. Identical twins showed very similar personality traits even when they had been separated at birth and raised apart, and this was equally true for both character traits and temperament traits. Parents and communities influence the ways in which conscientiousness is expressed, but they apparently do not influence its level.[8]

Measurement

A person's level of conscientiousness is generally assessed using self-report measures, although peer-reports and third-party observation can also be used. Self-report measures are either lexical[1] or based on statements.[9] Deciding which measure of either type to use in research is determined by an assessment of psychometric properties and the time and space constraints of the study being undertaken.

Lexical

Lexical measures use individual adjectives that reflect conscientiousness traits, such as efficient and systematic, and are very space and time efficient for research purposes. Goldberg (1992)[10] developed a 20-word measure as part of his 100-word Big Five markers. Saucier (1994)[11] developed a briefer 8-word measure as part of his 40-word mini-markers. Thompson (2008)[1] systematically revised these measures to develop the International English Mini-Markers which has superior validity and reliability in populations both within and outside North America. Internal consistency reliability of the International English Mini-Markers for the Conscientiousness measure for native English-speakers is reported as .90, that for non-native English-speakers is .86.[1]

Statement

Statement measures tend to comprise more words than lexical measures, so hence consume more research instrument space and more respondent time to complete. Respondents are asked the extent to which they, for example, often forget to put things back in their proper place, or are careful to avoid making mistakes.[9] Some statement-based measures of conscientiousness have similarly acceptable psychometric properties in North American populations to lexical measures, but their generally emic development makes them less suited to use in other populations.[12] For instance, statements in colloquial North American English like Often forget to put things back in their proper place or Am careful to avoid making mistakes can be hard for non-native English-speakers to understand, suggesting internationally validated measures might be more appropriate for research conducted with non-North Americans.

Behavior

Development

Currently,[when?] little is known about conscientiousness in young children because the self-report inventories typically used to assess it are not appropriate for that age group. There are likely individual differences in this factor at an early age. It is known, for example, that some children have attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, which is characterized in part by problems with concentration, organization, and persistence.[13] Longitudinal and cross-sectional studies suggest that conscientiousness is relatively low among adolescents but increases between 18 and 30 years of age. Research has also shown that conscientiousness generally increases with age from 21 to 60, though the rate of increase is slow.[14]

Individual differences are also strongly preserved, meaning that a careful, neat, and scrupulous 30-year-old is likely to become a careful, neat, and scrupulous 80-year-old.[8]

Daily life

People who score high on the trait of conscientiousness tend to be more organized and less cluttered in their homes and offices. For example, their books tend to be neatly shelved in alphabetical order, or categorized by topic, rather than scattered around the room. Their clothes tend to be folded and arranged in drawers or closets instead of lying on the floor. The presence of planners and to-do lists are also signs of conscientiousness. Their homes tend to have better lighting than the homes of people who score low on this trait.[15] In a 2009 study, ten behaviors strongly associated with conscientiousness were scientifically categorized (the number at the end of each behavior is a correlation coefficient; a negative number means conscientious people were less likely to manifest the behavior):[16]

Academic and workplace performance

Conscientiousness is importantly related to successful academic performance in students and workplace performance among managers and workers.[17] Low levels of conscientiousness are strongly associated with procrastination.[18] A considerable amount of research indicates that conscientiousness has a moderate to large positive correlation with performance in the workplace,[19][20] and indeed that after general mental ability is taken into account, the other four of the Big Five personality traits do not aid in predicting career success.[21]: 169 [irrelevant citation]

Conscientious employees are generally more reliable, more motivated, and harder working. They also have lower rates of absenteeism and counterproductive work behaviors such as stealing and fighting with other employees.[22] Furthermore, conscientiousness is the personality trait that mostly correlates with performance across all categories of jobs.[23]Agreeableness and emotional stability may also be important, particularly in jobs that involve a significant amount of social interaction.[24] Of all manager/leader types, top executives show the lowest level of rule-following, a conscientious trait.[25] Conscientiousness is not always positively related to job performance, sometimes the opposite is true. Being too conscientious could lead to taking too much time to making urgent decisions and to working too attached to the rules and lack innovation.[26]

Subjective well-being

A 2008 meta-analysis found that conscientiousness has a positive relationship with subjective well-being, particularly satisfaction with life, so highly conscientious people tend to be happier with their lives than those who score low on this trait.[27] Although conscientiousness is generally seen as a positive trait to possess, recent[when?] research has suggested that in some situations it may be harmful for well-being. In a prospective study of 9570 individuals over four years, highly conscientious people suffered more than twice as much if they became unemployed.[28] The authors suggested this may be due to conscientious people making different attributions about why they became unemployed, or through experiencing stronger reactions following failure. This finding is consistent with perspectives which see no trait as inherently positive or negative, but rather the consequences of the trait being dependent on the situation and concomitant goals and motivations.[29]

Problematic life outcomes

Low conscientiousness has been linked to antisocial and criminal behaviors,[3] as well as unemployment, homelessness, and imprisonment.[22] Low conscientiousness and low agreeableness taken together are also associated with substance use disorders.[30] People low in conscientiousness have difficulty saving money and have different borrowing practices than conscientious people. High conscientiousness is associated with more careful planning of shopping trips and less impulse buying of unneeded items.[22] Conscientiousness has been found to be positively correlated with business and white-collar crime.[31]

Health and longevity

According to an 80-year old and ongoing[when?] study started in 1921 by psychologist Lewis Terman on over 1,500 gifted adolescent Californians, "The strongest predictor of long life was conscientiousness."[32][33] Specific behaviors associated with low conscientiousness may explain its influence on longevity. Nine different behaviors that are among the leading causes of mortality—alcohol use, disordered eating (including obesity), drug use, lack of exercise, risky sexual behavior, risky driving, tobacco use, suicide, and violence—are all predicted by low conscientiousness. Health behaviors are more strongly correlated with the conventionality rather than the impulse-control aspect of conscientiousness. Apparently, social norms influence many health-relevant behavior, such as healthy diet and exercise, not smoking and moderate drinking, and highly conscientious people adhere the most strongly to these norms.[22] Additionally, conscientiousness is positively related to health behaviors[34] such as regular visits to a doctor, checking smoke alarms, and adherence to medication regimens. Such behavior may better safeguard health and prevent disease.[22][better source needed]

Relationships

Relationship quality is positively associated with partners' level of conscientiousness, and highly conscientious people are less likely to get divorced. Conscientiousness is associated with lower rates of behavior associated with divorce, such as extramarital affairs, spousal abuse, and alcohol use disorder. Conscientious behaviors may have a direct influence on relationship quality, as people low in conscientiousness are less responsible, less responsive to their partners, are more condescending, and less likely to hold back offensive comments. On the other hand, more conscientious people are better at managing conflict and tend to provoke fewer disagreements, perhaps because they elicit less criticism due to their well-controlled and responsible behavior.[22][better source needed]

Intelligence

Conscientiousness significantly correlated negatively with abstract reasoning (−0.26) and verbal reasoning (−0.23).[35]

Large unselected studies have found null relationships.[36] The negative relationship sometimes found in selected samples such as universities may be artificially created by the selection, from students who are low on one factor and high on the other (negative relationship) using the strong one to compensate and pass the selection, while students who are low on both (positive relationship) are removed by the selection. [37]

A large study found that fluid intelligence was significantly negatively correlated with the order (−0.15), self-discipline (−0.08), and deliberation (−0.09) subfactors of conscientiousness (all correlations significant with p < 0.001).[38]

Political attitudes

Conscientiousness is related to conservative political attitudes.[39][40][41]

Because conscientiousness is positively related to job performance, [42][43] conservative (vs. liberal) service workers have been shown to earn higher ratings, evaluations, and tips. [44]

Although right-wing authoritarianism is one of the most powerful predictors of prejudice, a large scale meta-analysis found that conscientiousness itself is uncorrelated with general prejudice.[45][46] Rebellion against control is significantly negatively correlated with conscientiousness.[47]

Conscientiousness is associated with rule compliance, obedience and integrity.[48]

Creativity

The orderliness/dependability subfactors (order, dutifulness, and deliberation) of conscientiousness correlate negatively with creativity while the industriousness/achievement subfactors correlate positively.[49] Another study showed that people who score high on the order subfactor of conscientiousness show less innovative behavior.[50] Group conscientiousness has a negative effect on group performance during creative tasks.[51] Groups with only conscientious members have difficulty solving open-ended problems.[52]

Adaptability

A study from 2006 found that those scoring low on conscientiousness make better decisions after unanticipated changes in the context of a task. Specifically, the subfactors order, dutifulness, and deliberation negatively correlated with decision-making quality, but not competence, achievement striving, and self-discipline.[53]

Religiosity

General religiosity was mainly related to high Agreeableness and Conscientiousness of the Big Five traits.[54]

Geography

United States

Average levels of conscientiousness vary by state in the United States. People living in the central part, including the states of Kansas, Nebraska, Oklahoma, and Missouri, tend to have higher scores on average than people living in other regions. People in the southwestern states of New Mexico, Utah, and Arizona also have relatively high average scores on conscientiousness. Among the eastern states, Florida is the only one that scores in the top ten for this personality trait. The four states with the lowest scores on conscientiousness on average were, in descending order, Rhode Island, Hawaii, Maine, and Alaska.[55]

Great Britain

A large scale survey of residents of Great Britain found that average levels of all the Big Five, including conscientiousness, vary across regional districts in England, Wales and Scotland. High levels of conscientiousness were found throughout much of Southern England, scattered areas of the Midlands, and most of the Scottish Highlands. Low levels of conscientiousness were observed in London, Wales, and parts of the North of England. Higher mean levels of regional conscientiousness were positively correlated with voting for the Conservative Party, and negatively correlated with voting for the Labour Party, in the 2005 and 2010 elections, and also correlated with a higher proportion of married residents, with higher life expectancy for men and women, fewer long-term health problems, and with lower rates of mortality from stroke, cancer, and heart disease. Higher regional conscientiousness was also correlated with lower median annual income in 2011.[56]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b c d Thompson, E.R. (October 2008). "Development and Validation of an International English Big-Five Mini-Markers". Personality and Individual Differences. 45 (6): 542–548. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.06.013.
  2. ^ Carter, Nathan L.; Guan, Li; Maples, Jessica L.; Williamson, Rachel L.; Miller, Joshua D. (2015). "The downsides of extreme conscientiousness for psychological wellbeing: The role of obsessive compulsive tendencies". Journal of Personality. 84 (4): 510–522. doi:10.1111/jopy.12177. PMID 25858019.
  3. ^ a b Ozer, D. J.; Benet-Martínez, V. (2006). "Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes". Annual Review of Psychology. 57: 401–421. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190127. PMID 16318601.
  4. ^ a b Costa, P. T. & McCrae, R. R. (1992). NEO personality Inventory professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  5. ^ "IPIP Home". ipip.ori.org.
  6. ^ DeYoung, C. G.; Quilty, L. C.; Peterson, J. B. (2007). "Between facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (5): 880–896. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.5.880. PMID 17983306. S2CID 8261816.
  7. ^ De Fruyt, F.; Van De Wiele, L.; Van Heeringen, C. (2000). "Cloninger's Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character and the Five-Factor Model of Personality". Personality and Individual Differences. 29 (3): 441–452. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00204-4.
  8. ^ a b McCrae, Robert. (2004). "Conscientiousness" Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology, Three-Volume Set. Academic Press. p. 470
  9. ^ a b Goldberg, L.R.; Johnson, JA; Eber, HW; et al. (2006). "The international personality item pool and the future of public-domain personality measures". Journal of Research in Personality. 40 (1): 84–96. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.007.
  10. ^ Goldberg, L.R. (1992). "The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure". Psychological Assessment. 4 (1): 26–42. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26.
  11. ^ Saucier, G (1994). "Mini-Markers – a brief version of Goldberg's unipolar big-five markers". Journal of Personality Assessment. 63 (3): 506–516. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa6303_8. PMID 7844738.
  12. ^ Piedmont, R.L.; Chae, J.H. (1997). "Cross-cultural generalizability of the five-factor model of personality – Development and validation of the NEO PI-R for Koreans". Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 28 (2): 131–155. doi:10.1177/0022022197282001. S2CID 145053137.
  13. ^ "Neurodevelopmental Disorders". neuronup.us. Retrieved 2021-03-09.
  14. ^ "What to do That Email List Everyone Says You Need | Web Hosting, VPS & Dedicated Server Provider I Hostao Web Hosting Solutions". 20 April 2021.
  15. ^ Gosling, S. (2008). Snoop: What your stuff says about you?. New York: Basic Books.
  16. ^ Hirsh, J.B.; DeYoung, C.G.; Peterson, J.B. (2009). "Metatraits of the Big Five differentially predict engagement and restraint of behavior". Journal of Personality. 77 (4): 1085–1101. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.607.3250. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00575.x. PMID 19558442. S2CID 8908663.
  17. ^ Higgins, D.M.; Peterson, J.B.; Lee, A.; Pihl, R.O. (2007). "Prefrontal cognitive ability, intelligence, Big Five personality and the prediction of advanced academic and workplace performance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 93 (2): 298–319. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.2.298. PMID 17645401.
  18. ^ Dewitt, S.; Schouwenburg, H. C. (2002). "Procrastination, temptations, and incentives: The struggle between the present and the future in procrastinators and the punctual". European Journal of Personality. 16 (6): 469–489. doi:10.1002/per.461. S2CID 7065818.
  19. ^ Judge, Timothy (1999). "The Big Five Personality Traits, General Mental Ability, And Career Success Across The Life Span" (PDF). Personnel Psychology. 52 (3): 621–652. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1999.tb00174.x. Retrieved 8 August 2019.
  20. ^ J. F. Salgado (February 1997). "The five factor model of personality and job performance in the European community". Journal of Applied Psychology. 82 (1): 30–43. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.1.30. PMID 9119797.
  21. ^ Schmidt, Frank L.; Hunter, John (2004). (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (1): 162–173. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.394.8878. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.1.162. PMID 14717634. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-10-28. Retrieved 2010-07-12.
  22. ^ a b c d e f Roberts, B.W.; Jackson, J.J.; Fayard, J.V.; Edmonds, G.; Meints, J (2009). "Chapter 25. Conscientiousness". In Mark R. Leary, & Rick H. Hoyle (ed.). Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior. New York/London: The Guildford Press. pp. 257–273. ISBN 978-1-59385-647-2.
  23. ^ "Which Personality Traits Are Most Important to Employers?". Association for Psychological Science - APS. Retrieved 2021-03-16.
  24. ^ M. K. Mount, M. R. Barrick and G. L. Stewart (1998). "Five-factor model of personality and Performance in jobs involving interpersonal interactions". Human Performance. 11 (2): 145–165. doi:10.1080/08959285.1998.9668029.
  25. ^ MacNab, Donald. "New research shows leaders are persuasive rule-breakers" (PDF). Psychometrics Canada.
  26. ^ Tett, Robert. "Is Conscientiousness ALWAYS Positively Related to Job Performance?". Wright State University.
  27. ^ Steel, Piers; Schmidt, Joseph; Shultz, Jonas (2008). (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 134 (1): 138–161. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.138. hdl:1880/47915. PMID 18193998. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2019-02-14. Retrieved 2012-07-11.
  28. ^ Boyce, Christopher J.; Wood, Alex M.; Brown, Gordon D.A. (2010). (PDF). Journal of Research in Personality. Elsevier BV. 44 (4): 535–539. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.05.001. ISSN 0092-6566. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-02-24.
  29. ^ Wood, Alex M.; Tarrier, Nicholas (2010). (PDF). Clinical Psychology Review. Elsevier BV. 30 (7): 819–829. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.06.003. ISSN 0272-7358. PMID 20655136. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-17.
  30. ^ Walton, KE; Roberts, BW. (2004). "On the relationship between substance use and personality traits: abstainers are not maladjusted". J. Res. Personal. 38 (6): 515–35. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2004.01.002.
  31. ^ Blickle, G.; Schlegel, A. (2006). "Some Personality Correlates of Business White-Collar Crime". Applied Psychology. 55 (2): 220–233. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2006.00226.x.
  32. ^ "Psychological Predictors of Long Life: An 80-year study discovers traits that help people to live longer". Psychology Today. June 5, 2012.
  33. ^ "Howard S. Friedman and Leslie R. Martin, The Longevity Project: Surprising Discoveries for Health and Long Life from the Landmark Eight-Decade Study". Population and Development Review. 38 (1): 175–176. March 2012. doi:10.1111/j.1728-4457.2012.00481.x. ISSN 0098-7921.
  34. ^ Kern, Margaret L.; Friedman, Howard S. (2008–2009). "Do conscientious individuals live longer? A quantitative review". Health Psychology. 27 (5): 505–512. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.27.5.505. ISSN 1930-7810. PMID 18823176.
  35. ^ Moutafi, Joanna; Furnham, Adrian; Paltiel, Laurence (2004). (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 37 (5): 1013–1022. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.11.010. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2014-12-23.
  36. ^ Luciano, M., Wainwright, M. A., Wright, M. J., & Martin, N. G. (2006). The heritability of conscientiousness facets and their relationship to IQ and academic achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1189–1199.
  37. ^ Murray, A. L.; Johnson, W.; McGue, M.; Iacono, W. G. (2014). "How are conscientiousness and cognitive ability related to one another? A re-examination of the intelligence compensation hypothesis". Personality and Individual Differences. 70: 17–22. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2014.06.014.
  38. ^ Moutafi, Joanna; Furnham, Adrian; Crump, John (2006). "What facets of openness and conscientiousness predict fluid intelligence score?". Learning and Individual Differences. 16: 31–42. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2005.06.003.
  39. ^ Soto, C. J. (2019). How replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life outcomes? The life outcomes of personality replication project. Psychological Science, 30, 711–727.
  40. ^ Carney, D. R., Jost, J. T., Gosling, S. D., & Potter, J. (2008). The secret lives of liberals and conservatives: Personality profiles, interaction styles, and the things they leave behind. Political Psychology, 29, 807–840.
  41. ^ Sibley, Chris G.; Osborne, Danny; Duckitt, John (2012). "Personality and political orientation: Meta-analysis and test of a Threat-Constraint Model". Journal of Research in Personality. 46 (6): 664–677. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2012.08.002.
  42. ^ Brown, Tom J.; Mowen, John C.; Donavan, D. Todd; Licata, Jane W. (2002). "The Customer Orientation of Service Workers: Personality Trait Effects on Self-and Supervisor Performance Ratings". Journal of Marketing Research. SAGE Publications. 39 (1): 110–119. doi:10.1509/jmkr.39.1.110.18928. ISSN 0022-2437. S2CID 144095642.
  43. ^ Neal, Andrew; Yeo, Gillian; Koy, Annette; Xiao, Tania (2011-01-26). "Predicting the form and direction of work role performance from the Big 5 model of personality traits". Journal of Organizational Behavior. Wiley. 33 (2): 175–192. doi:10.1002/job.742. ISSN 0894-3796.
  44. ^ Davidson, Alexander; Theriault, Derek A. (2021-03-25). "How Consumer Experience Is Shaped by the Political Orientation of Service Providers". Journal of Consumer Psychology. Wiley. 31 (4): 792–800. doi:10.1002/jcpy.1233. ISSN 1057-7408. S2CID 233791649.
  45. ^ Sibley, Chris; Duckitt, John (2008). "Personality and Prejudice: A Meta-Analysis and Theoretical Review". Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 12 (3): 248–79. doi:10.1177/1088868308319226. PMID 18641385. S2CID 5156899.
  46. ^ Chen, Philip G.; Palmer, Carl L. (2017-08-04). "The Prejudiced Personality? Using the Big Five to Predict Susceptibility to Stereotyping Behavior". American Politics Research. SAGE Publications. 46 (2): 276–307. doi:10.1177/1532673x17719720. ISSN 1532-673X. S2CID 142142664.
  47. ^ Watson, David (2001). "Procrastination and the five-factor model: a facet level analysis". Personality and Individual Differences. 30: 149–158. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(00)00019-2.
  48. ^ Hogan, Robert; Johnson, John; Briggs, Stephen (1997). Handbook of Personality Psychology. Academic Press. p. 856. ISBN 978-0-12-134645-4.
  49. ^ Reiter-Palmon, Roni; Illies, Jody; Kobe-Cross, Lisa (2009). "Conscientiousness Is Not Always a Good Predictor of Performance: The Case of Creativity". The International Journal of Creativity & Problem Solving. 19: 27–45.
  50. ^ Bakx, Nina. . Universiteit van Amsterdam. Archived from the original on 2016-01-31. Retrieved 2015-04-12.
  51. ^ Robert, Christopher; Cheung, Yu Ha (April 2010). "An examination of the relationship between conscientiousness and group performance on a creative task". Journal of Research in Personality. 44 (2): 222–231. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2010.01.005.
  52. ^ Colarelli, Stephen (2003). No best way : an evolutionary perspective on human resource management. Westport, Conn. : Praeger. p. 159. ISBN 978-0-275-95739-1.
  53. ^ Lepine, Jeffrey; Colquitt, Jason; Erez, Amir (2000). "Adaptability to changing task context: effects of general cognitive ability, conscientiousness, and openness to experience". Personnel Psychology. 53 (3): 563–593. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.2000.tb00214.x.
  54. ^ Saroglou, Vassilis (2002). "Religion and the five-factors of personality: A meta-analytic review". Personality and Individual Differences. 32: 15–25. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00233-6.
  55. ^ Stephanie Simon (2008-09-23). "The United States of Mind. Researchers Identify Regional Personality Traits Across America". WSJ.com. Original research article: Peter J. Rentfrow, Samuel D. Gosling and Jeff Potter (2008). "A Theory of the Emergence, Persistence, and Expression of Geographic Variation in Psychological Characteristics". Perspectives on Psychological Science. 3 (5): 339–369. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00084.x. PMID 26158954. S2CID 17059908.
  56. ^ Rentfrow, Peter J.; Jokela, Markus; Lamb, Michael E. (March 24, 2015). "Regional Personality Differences in Great Britain". PLOS ONE. 10 (3): e0122245. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1022245R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0122245. PMC 4372610. PMID 25803819. S2CID 14827579.

conscientiousness, confused, with, conscience, conscientious, objector, consciousness, personality, trait, being, careful, diligent, implies, desire, task, well, take, obligations, others, seriously, conscientious, people, tend, efficient, organized, opposed, . Not to be confused with conscience conscientious objector or consciousness Conscientiousness is the personality trait of being careful or diligent Conscientiousness implies a desire to do a task well and to take obligations to others seriously Conscientious people tend to be efficient and organized as opposed to easy going and disorderly They exhibit a tendency to show self discipline act dutifully and aim for achievement they display planned rather than spontaneous behavior and they are generally dependable It is manifested in characteristic behaviors such as being neat and systematic also including such elements as carefulness thoroughness and deliberation the tendency to think carefully before acting 1 Conscientiousness is one of the five traits of both the Five Factor Model and the HEXACO model of personality and is an aspect of what has traditionally been referred to as having character Conscientious individuals are generally hard working and reliable When taken to an extreme they may also be workaholics perfectionists and compulsive in their behavior 2 People who score low on conscientiousness tend to be laid back less goal oriented and less driven by success they also are more likely to engage in antisocial and criminal behavior 3 Contents 1 Personality models 2 Origin 3 Measurement 3 1 Lexical 3 2 Statement 4 Behavior 4 1 Development 4 2 Daily life 4 3 Academic and workplace performance 4 4 Subjective well being 4 5 Problematic life outcomes 4 6 Health and longevity 4 7 Relationships 4 8 Intelligence 4 9 Political attitudes 4 10 Creativity 4 11 Adaptability 4 12 Religiosity 5 Geography 5 1 United States 5 2 Great Britain 6 See also 7 ReferencesPersonality models EditConscientiousness is one of the five major dimensions in the Big Five model of personality also called Five Factor Model or OCEAN which consists of openness to experience conscientiousness extraversion agreeableness and neuroticism Two of many personality tests that assess these traits are Costa and McCrae s NEO PI R 4 and Goldberg s NEO IPIP 5 According to these models conscientiousness is considered to be a continuous dimension of personality rather than a categorical type of person In the NEO framework Conscientiousness is seen as having six facets Competence Order Dutifulness Achievement Striving Self Discipline and Deliberation Other models suggest a smaller set of two aspects orderliness and industriousness form an intermediate level of organization with orderliness associated with the desire to keep things organized and tidy and industriousness being more associated with productivity and work ethic 6 Other personality traits Low extraversion high agreeableness low openness and low neuroticism are linked to high conscientiousness along with impulse control Behaviorally low conscientiousness is associated with an inability to motivate one s self to perform tasks that the individual desires to accomplish 4 Conscientiousness also appears in other models of personality such as Cloninger s Temperament and Character Inventory in which it is related to both self directedness and persistence 7 It also includes the specific traits of rule consciousness and perfectionism in Cattell s 16 PF model It is negatively associated with impulsive sensation seeking in Zuckerman s alternative five model Traits associated with conscientiousness are frequently assessed by self report integrity tests given by various corporations to prospective employees Origin EditTerms such as hard working reliable and persevering describe desirable aspects of character Because it was once believed to be a moral evaluation conscientiousness was overlooked as a real psychological attribute The reality of individual differences in conscientiousness has now been clearly established by studies of cross observer agreement Peer and expert ratings confirm the self reports that people make about their degrees of conscientiousness Furthermore both self reports and observer ratings of conscientiousness predict real life outcomes such as academic success citation needed During most of the 20th century psychologists believed that personality traits could be divided into two categories temperament and character Temperament traits were thought to be biologically based whereas character traits were thought to be learned either during childhood or throughout life With the advent of the FFM Five Factor Model behavior geneticists began systematic studies of the full range of personality traits and it soon became clear that all five factors are substantially heritable Identical twins showed very similar personality traits even when they had been separated at birth and raised apart and this was equally true for both character traits and temperament traits Parents and communities influence the ways in which conscientiousness is expressed but they apparently do not influence its level 8 Measurement EditA person s level of conscientiousness is generally assessed using self report measures although peer reports and third party observation can also be used Self report measures are either lexical 1 or based on statements 9 Deciding which measure of either type to use in research is determined by an assessment of psychometric properties and the time and space constraints of the study being undertaken Lexical Edit Lexical measures use individual adjectives that reflect conscientiousness traits such as efficient and systematic and are very space and time efficient for research purposes Goldberg 1992 10 developed a 20 word measure as part of his 100 word Big Five markers Saucier 1994 11 developed a briefer 8 word measure as part of his 40 word mini markers Thompson 2008 1 systematically revised these measures to develop the International English Mini Markers which has superior validity and reliability in populations both within and outside North America Internal consistency reliability of the International English Mini Markers for the Conscientiousness measure for native English speakers is reported as 90 that for non native English speakers is 86 1 Statement Edit Statement measures tend to comprise more words than lexical measures so hence consume more research instrument space and more respondent time to complete Respondents are asked the extent to which they for example often forget to put things back in their proper place or are careful to avoid making mistakes 9 Some statement based measures of conscientiousness have similarly acceptable psychometric properties in North American populations to lexical measures but their generally emic development makes them less suited to use in other populations 12 For instance statements in colloquial North American English like Often forget to put things back in their proper place or Am careful to avoid making mistakes can be hard for non native English speakers to understand suggesting internationally validated measures might be more appropriate for research conducted with non North Americans Behavior EditDevelopment Edit Currently when little is known about conscientiousness in young children because the self report inventories typically used to assess it are not appropriate for that age group There are likely individual differences in this factor at an early age It is known for example that some children have attention deficit hyperactivity disorder which is characterized in part by problems with concentration organization and persistence 13 Longitudinal and cross sectional studies suggest that conscientiousness is relatively low among adolescents but increases between 18 and 30 years of age Research has also shown that conscientiousness generally increases with age from 21 to 60 though the rate of increase is slow 14 Individual differences are also strongly preserved meaning that a careful neat and scrupulous 30 year old is likely to become a careful neat and scrupulous 80 year old 8 Daily life Edit People who score high on the trait of conscientiousness tend to be more organized and less cluttered in their homes and offices For example their books tend to be neatly shelved in alphabetical order or categorized by topic rather than scattered around the room Their clothes tend to be folded and arranged in drawers or closets instead of lying on the floor The presence of planners and to do lists are also signs of conscientiousness Their homes tend to have better lighting than the homes of people who score low on this trait 15 In a 2009 study ten behaviors strongly associated with conscientiousness were scientifically categorized the number at the end of each behavior is a correlation coefficient a negative number means conscientious people were less likely to manifest the behavior 16 Academic and workplace performance Edit Conscientiousness is importantly related to successful academic performance in students and workplace performance among managers and workers 17 Low levels of conscientiousness are strongly associated with procrastination 18 A considerable amount of research indicates that conscientiousness has a moderate to large positive correlation with performance in the workplace 19 20 and indeed that after general mental ability is taken into account the other four of the Big Five personality traits do not aid in predicting career success 21 169 irrelevant citation Conscientious employees are generally more reliable more motivated and harder working They also have lower rates of absenteeism and counterproductive work behaviors such as stealing and fighting with other employees 22 Furthermore conscientiousness is the personality trait that mostly correlates with performance across all categories of jobs 23 Agreeableness and emotional stability may also be important particularly in jobs that involve a significant amount of social interaction 24 Of all manager leader types top executives show the lowest level of rule following a conscientious trait 25 Conscientiousness is not always positively related to job performance sometimes the opposite is true Being too conscientious could lead to taking too much time to making urgent decisions and to working too attached to the rules and lack innovation 26 Subjective well being Edit Main article Subjective well being A 2008 meta analysis found that conscientiousness has a positive relationship with subjective well being particularly satisfaction with life so highly conscientious people tend to be happier with their lives than those who score low on this trait 27 Although conscientiousness is generally seen as a positive trait to possess recent when research has suggested that in some situations it may be harmful for well being In a prospective study of 9570 individuals over four years highly conscientious people suffered more than twice as much if they became unemployed 28 The authors suggested this may be due to conscientious people making different attributions about why they became unemployed or through experiencing stronger reactions following failure This finding is consistent with perspectives which see no trait as inherently positive or negative but rather the consequences of the trait being dependent on the situation and concomitant goals and motivations 29 Problematic life outcomes Edit Low conscientiousness has been linked to antisocial and criminal behaviors 3 as well as unemployment homelessness and imprisonment 22 Low conscientiousness and low agreeableness taken together are also associated with substance use disorders 30 People low in conscientiousness have difficulty saving money and have different borrowing practices than conscientious people High conscientiousness is associated with more careful planning of shopping trips and less impulse buying of unneeded items 22 Conscientiousness has been found to be positively correlated with business and white collar crime 31 Health and longevity Edit According to an 80 year old and ongoing when study started in 1921 by psychologist Lewis Terman on over 1 500 gifted adolescent Californians The strongest predictor of long life was conscientiousness 32 33 Specific behaviors associated with low conscientiousness may explain its influence on longevity Nine different behaviors that are among the leading causes of mortality alcohol use disordered eating including obesity drug use lack of exercise risky sexual behavior risky driving tobacco use suicide and violence are all predicted by low conscientiousness Health behaviors are more strongly correlated with the conventionality rather than the impulse control aspect of conscientiousness Apparently social norms influence many health relevant behavior such as healthy diet and exercise not smoking and moderate drinking and highly conscientious people adhere the most strongly to these norms 22 Additionally conscientiousness is positively related to health behaviors 34 such as regular visits to a doctor checking smoke alarms and adherence to medication regimens Such behavior may better safeguard health and prevent disease 22 better source needed Relationships Edit Relationship quality is positively associated with partners level of conscientiousness and highly conscientious people are less likely to get divorced Conscientiousness is associated with lower rates of behavior associated with divorce such as extramarital affairs spousal abuse and alcohol use disorder Conscientious behaviors may have a direct influence on relationship quality as people low in conscientiousness are less responsible less responsive to their partners are more condescending and less likely to hold back offensive comments On the other hand more conscientious people are better at managing conflict and tend to provoke fewer disagreements perhaps because they elicit less criticism due to their well controlled and responsible behavior 22 better source needed Intelligence Edit Conscientiousness significantly correlated negatively with abstract reasoning 0 26 and verbal reasoning 0 23 35 Large unselected studies have found null relationships 36 The negative relationship sometimes found in selected samples such as universities may be artificially created by the selection from students who are low on one factor and high on the other negative relationship using the strong one to compensate and pass the selection while students who are low on both positive relationship are removed by the selection 37 A large study found that fluid intelligence was significantly negatively correlated with the order 0 15 self discipline 0 08 and deliberation 0 09 subfactors of conscientiousness all correlations significant with p lt 0 001 38 Political attitudes Edit Conscientiousness is related to conservative political attitudes 39 40 41 Because conscientiousness is positively related to job performance 42 43 conservative vs liberal service workers have been shown to earn higher ratings evaluations and tips 44 Although right wing authoritarianism is one of the most powerful predictors of prejudice a large scale meta analysis found that conscientiousness itself is uncorrelated with general prejudice 45 46 Rebellion against control is significantly negatively correlated with conscientiousness 47 Conscientiousness is associated with rule compliance obedience and integrity 48 Creativity Edit The orderliness dependability subfactors order dutifulness and deliberation of conscientiousness correlate negatively with creativity while the industriousness achievement subfactors correlate positively 49 Another study showed that people who score high on the order subfactor of conscientiousness show less innovative behavior 50 Group conscientiousness has a negative effect on group performance during creative tasks 51 Groups with only conscientious members have difficulty solving open ended problems 52 Adaptability Edit A study from 2006 found that those scoring low on conscientiousness make better decisions after unanticipated changes in the context of a task Specifically the subfactors order dutifulness and deliberation negatively correlated with decision making quality but not competence achievement striving and self discipline 53 Religiosity Edit General religiosity was mainly related to high Agreeableness and Conscientiousness of the Big Five traits 54 Geography EditUnited States Edit Average levels of conscientiousness vary by state in the United States People living in the central part including the states of Kansas Nebraska Oklahoma and Missouri tend to have higher scores on average than people living in other regions People in the southwestern states of New Mexico Utah and Arizona also have relatively high average scores on conscientiousness Among the eastern states Florida is the only one that scores in the top ten for this personality trait The four states with the lowest scores on conscientiousness on average were in descending order Rhode Island Hawaii Maine and Alaska 55 Great Britain Edit A large scale survey of residents of Great Britain found that average levels of all the Big Five including conscientiousness vary across regional districts in England Wales and Scotland High levels of conscientiousness were found throughout much of Southern England scattered areas of the Midlands and most of the Scottish Highlands Low levels of conscientiousness were observed in London Wales and parts of the North of England Higher mean levels of regional conscientiousness were positively correlated with voting for the Conservative Party and negatively correlated with voting for the Labour Party in the 2005 and 2010 elections and also correlated with a higher proportion of married residents with higher life expectancy for men and women fewer long term health problems and with lower rates of mortality from stroke cancer and heart disease Higher regional conscientiousness was also correlated with lower median annual income in 2011 56 See also EditPersonality psychologyReferences Edit a b c d Thompson E R October 2008 Development and Validation of an International English Big Five Mini Markers Personality and Individual Differences 45 6 542 548 doi 10 1016 j paid 2008 06 013 Carter Nathan L Guan Li Maples Jessica L Williamson Rachel L Miller Joshua D 2015 The downsides of extreme conscientiousness for psychological wellbeing The role of obsessive compulsive tendencies Journal of Personality 84 4 510 522 doi 10 1111 jopy 12177 PMID 25858019 a b Ozer D J Benet Martinez V 2006 Personality and the prediction of consequential outcomes Annual Review of Psychology 57 401 421 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 57 102904 190127 PMID 16318601 a b Costa P T amp McCrae R R 1992 NEO personality Inventory professional manual Odessa FL Psychological Assessment Resources IPIP Home ipip ori org DeYoung C G Quilty L C Peterson J B 2007 Between facets and domains 10 aspects of the Big Five Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93 5 880 896 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 93 5 880 PMID 17983306 S2CID 8261816 De Fruyt F Van De Wiele L Van Heeringen C 2000 Cloninger s Psychobiological Model of Temperament and Character and the Five Factor Model of Personality Personality and Individual Differences 29 3 441 452 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 99 00204 4 a b McCrae Robert 2004 Conscientiousness Encyclopedia of Applied Psychology Three Volume Set Academic Press p 470 a b Goldberg L R Johnson JA Eber HW et al 2006 The international personality item pool and the future of public domain personality measures Journal of Research in Personality 40 1 84 96 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2005 08 007 Goldberg L R 1992 The development of markers for the Big Five factor structure Psychological Assessment 4 1 26 42 doi 10 1037 1040 3590 4 1 26 Saucier G 1994 Mini Markers a brief version of Goldberg s unipolar big five markers Journal of Personality Assessment 63 3 506 516 doi 10 1207 s15327752jpa6303 8 PMID 7844738 Piedmont R L Chae J H 1997 Cross cultural generalizability of the five factor model of personality Development and validation of the NEO PI R for Koreans Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology 28 2 131 155 doi 10 1177 0022022197282001 S2CID 145053137 Neurodevelopmental Disorders neuronup us Retrieved 2021 03 09 What to do That Email List Everyone Says You Need Web Hosting VPS amp Dedicated Server Provider I Hostao Web Hosting Solutions 20 April 2021 Gosling S 2008 Snoop What your stuff says about you New York Basic Books Hirsh J B DeYoung C G Peterson J B 2009 Metatraits of the Big Five differentially predict engagement and restraint of behavior Journal of Personality 77 4 1085 1101 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 607 3250 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 2009 00575 x PMID 19558442 S2CID 8908663 Higgins D M Peterson J B Lee A Pihl R O 2007 Prefrontal cognitive ability intelligence Big Five personality and the prediction of advanced academic and workplace performance Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 93 2 298 319 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 93 2 298 PMID 17645401 Dewitt S Schouwenburg H C 2002 Procrastination temptations and incentives The struggle between the present and the future in procrastinators and the punctual European Journal of Personality 16 6 469 489 doi 10 1002 per 461 S2CID 7065818 Judge Timothy 1999 The Big Five Personality Traits General Mental Ability And Career Success Across The Life Span PDF Personnel Psychology 52 3 621 652 doi 10 1111 j 1744 6570 1999 tb00174 x Retrieved 8 August 2019 J F Salgado February 1997 The five factor model of personality and job performance in the European community Journal of Applied Psychology 82 1 30 43 doi 10 1037 0021 9010 82 1 30 PMID 9119797 Schmidt Frank L Hunter John 2004 General Mental Ability in the World of Work Occupational Attainment and Job Performance PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86 1 162 173 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 394 8878 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 86 1 162 PMID 14717634 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 10 28 Retrieved 2010 07 12 a b c d e f Roberts B W Jackson J J Fayard J V Edmonds G Meints J 2009 Chapter 25 Conscientiousness In Mark R Leary amp Rick H Hoyle ed Handbook of Individual Differences in Social Behavior New York London The Guildford Press pp 257 273 ISBN 978 1 59385 647 2 Which Personality Traits Are Most Important to Employers Association for Psychological Science APS Retrieved 2021 03 16 M K Mount M R Barrick and G L Stewart 1998 Five factor model of personality and Performance in jobs involving interpersonal interactions Human Performance 11 2 145 165 doi 10 1080 08959285 1998 9668029 MacNab Donald New research shows leaders are persuasive rule breakers PDF Psychometrics Canada Tett Robert Is Conscientiousness ALWAYS Positively Related to Job Performance Wright State University Steel Piers Schmidt Joseph Shultz Jonas 2008 Refining the relationship between personality and Subjective well being PDF Psychological Bulletin 134 1 138 161 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 134 1 138 hdl 1880 47915 PMID 18193998 Archived from the original PDF on 2019 02 14 Retrieved 2012 07 11 Boyce Christopher J Wood Alex M Brown Gordon D A 2010 The dark side of conscientiousness Conscientious people experience greater drops in life satisfaction following unemployment PDF Journal of Research in Personality Elsevier BV 44 4 535 539 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2010 05 001 ISSN 0092 6566 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 02 24 Wood Alex M Tarrier Nicholas 2010 Positive Clinical Psychology A new vision and strategy for integrated research and practice PDF Clinical Psychology Review Elsevier BV 30 7 819 829 doi 10 1016 j cpr 2010 06 003 ISSN 0272 7358 PMID 20655136 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 07 17 Walton KE Roberts BW 2004 On the relationship between substance use and personality traits abstainers are not maladjusted J Res Personal 38 6 515 35 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2004 01 002 Blickle G Schlegel A 2006 Some Personality Correlates of Business White Collar Crime Applied Psychology 55 2 220 233 doi 10 1111 j 1464 0597 2006 00226 x Psychological Predictors of Long Life An 80 year study discovers traits that help people to live longer Psychology Today June 5 2012 Howard S Friedman and Leslie R Martin The Longevity Project Surprising Discoveries for Health and Long Life from the Landmark Eight Decade Study Population and Development Review 38 1 175 176 March 2012 doi 10 1111 j 1728 4457 2012 00481 x ISSN 0098 7921 Kern Margaret L Friedman Howard S 2008 2009 Do conscientious individuals live longer A quantitative review Health Psychology 27 5 505 512 doi 10 1037 0278 6133 27 5 505 ISSN 1930 7810 PMID 18823176 Moutafi Joanna Furnham Adrian Paltiel Laurence 2004 Why is Conscientiousness negatively correlated with intelligence PDF Personality and Individual Differences 37 5 1013 1022 doi 10 1016 j paid 2003 11 010 Archived from the original PDF on 2014 12 23 Luciano M Wainwright M A Wright M J amp Martin N G 2006 The heritability of conscientiousness facets and their relationship to IQ and academic achievement Personality and Individual Differences 40 1189 1199 Murray A L Johnson W McGue M Iacono W G 2014 How are conscientiousness and cognitive ability related to one another A re examination of the intelligence compensation hypothesis Personality and Individual Differences 70 17 22 doi 10 1016 j paid 2014 06 014 Moutafi Joanna Furnham Adrian Crump John 2006 What facets of openness and conscientiousness predict fluid intelligence score Learning and Individual Differences 16 31 42 doi 10 1016 j lindif 2005 06 003 Soto C J 2019 How replicable are links between personality traits and consequential life outcomes The life outcomes of personality replication project Psychological Science 30 711 727 Carney D R Jost J T Gosling S D amp Potter J 2008 The secret lives of liberals and conservatives Personality profiles interaction styles and the things they leave behind Political Psychology 29 807 840 Sibley Chris G Osborne Danny Duckitt John 2012 Personality and political orientation Meta analysis and test of a Threat Constraint Model Journal of Research in Personality 46 6 664 677 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2012 08 002 Brown Tom J Mowen John C Donavan D Todd Licata Jane W 2002 The Customer Orientation of Service Workers Personality Trait Effects on Self and Supervisor Performance Ratings Journal of Marketing Research SAGE Publications 39 1 110 119 doi 10 1509 jmkr 39 1 110 18928 ISSN 0022 2437 S2CID 144095642 Neal Andrew Yeo Gillian Koy Annette Xiao Tania 2011 01 26 Predicting the form and direction of work role performance from the Big 5 model of personality traits Journal of Organizational Behavior Wiley 33 2 175 192 doi 10 1002 job 742 ISSN 0894 3796 Davidson Alexander Theriault Derek A 2021 03 25 How Consumer Experience Is Shaped by the Political Orientation of Service Providers Journal of Consumer Psychology Wiley 31 4 792 800 doi 10 1002 jcpy 1233 ISSN 1057 7408 S2CID 233791649 Sibley Chris Duckitt John 2008 Personality and Prejudice A Meta Analysis and Theoretical Review Pers Soc Psychol Rev 12 3 248 79 doi 10 1177 1088868308319226 PMID 18641385 S2CID 5156899 Chen Philip G Palmer Carl L 2017 08 04 The Prejudiced Personality Using the Big Five to Predict Susceptibility to Stereotyping Behavior American Politics Research SAGE Publications 46 2 276 307 doi 10 1177 1532673x17719720 ISSN 1532 673X S2CID 142142664 Watson David 2001 Procrastination and the five factor model a facet level analysis Personality and Individual Differences 30 149 158 doi 10 1016 s0191 8869 00 00019 2 Hogan Robert Johnson John Briggs Stephen 1997 Handbook of Personality Psychology Academic Press p 856 ISBN 978 0 12 134645 4 Reiter Palmon Roni Illies Jody Kobe Cross Lisa 2009 Conscientiousness Is Not Always a Good Predictor of Performance The Case of Creativity The International Journal of Creativity amp Problem Solving 19 27 45 Bakx Nina Which personality traits do innovative people possess Universiteit van Amsterdam Archived from the original on 2016 01 31 Retrieved 2015 04 12 Robert Christopher Cheung Yu Ha April 2010 An examination of the relationship between conscientiousness and group performance on a creative task Journal of Research in Personality 44 2 222 231 doi 10 1016 j jrp 2010 01 005 Colarelli Stephen 2003 No best way an evolutionary perspective on human resource management Westport Conn Praeger p 159 ISBN 978 0 275 95739 1 Lepine Jeffrey Colquitt Jason Erez Amir 2000 Adaptability to changing task context effects of general cognitive ability conscientiousness and openness to experience Personnel Psychology 53 3 563 593 doi 10 1111 j 1744 6570 2000 tb00214 x Saroglou Vassilis 2002 Religion and the five factors of personality A meta analytic review Personality and Individual Differences 32 15 25 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 00 00233 6 Stephanie Simon 2008 09 23 The United States of Mind Researchers Identify Regional Personality Traits Across America WSJ com Original research article Peter J Rentfrow Samuel D Gosling and Jeff Potter 2008 A Theory of the Emergence Persistence and Expression of Geographic Variation in Psychological Characteristics Perspectives on Psychological Science 3 5 339 369 doi 10 1111 j 1745 6924 2008 00084 x PMID 26158954 S2CID 17059908 Rentfrow Peter J Jokela Markus Lamb Michael E March 24 2015 Regional Personality Differences in Great Britain PLOS ONE 10 3 e0122245 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1022245R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0122245 PMC 4372610 PMID 25803819 S2CID 14827579 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Conscientiousness amp oldid 1125897578, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.