fbpx
Wikipedia

Animal cognition

Animal cognition encompasses the mental capacities of non-human animals including insect cognition. The study of animal conditioning and learning used in this field was developed from comparative psychology. It has also been strongly influenced by research in ethology, behavioral ecology, and evolutionary psychology; the alternative name cognitive ethology is sometimes used. Many behaviors associated with the term animal intelligence are also subsumed within animal cognition.[1]

A crab-eating macaque using a stone tool to crack open a nut
Experiments like the string-pulling task performed here by a Carib grackle provide insights into animal cognition.

Researchers have examined animal cognition in mammals (especially primates, cetaceans, elephants, dogs, cats, pigs, horses,[2][3][4] cattle, raccoons and rodents), birds (including parrots, fowl, corvids and pigeons), reptiles (lizards, snakes, and turtles),[5] fish and invertebrates (including cephalopods, spiders and insects).[6]

Historical background edit

Earliest inferences edit

 
A monkey drinking Frooti from a juice box using its hands

The mind and behavior of non-human animals has captivated the human imagination for centuries. Many writers, such as Descartes, have speculated about the presence or absence of the animal mind.[7] These speculations led to many observations of animal behavior before modern science and testing were available. This ultimately resulted in the creation of multiple hypotheses about animal intelligence.

One of Aesop's Fables was The Crow and the Pitcher, in which a crow drops pebbles into a vessel of water until he is able to drink. This was a relatively accurate reflection of the capability of corvids to understand water displacement.[8] The Roman naturalist Pliny the Elder was the earliest to attest that said story reflects the behavior of real-life corvids.[9]

Aristotle, in his biology, hypothesized a causal chain where an animal's sense organs transmitted information to an organ capable of making decisions, and then to a motor organ. Despite Aristotle's cardiocentrism (mistaken belief that cognition occurred in the heart), this approached some modern understandings of information processing.[10]

Early inferences were not necessarily precise or accurate. Nonetheless, interest in animal mental abilities, and comparisons to humans, increased with early myrmecology, the study of ant behavior, as well as the classification of humans as primates beginning with Linnaeus.

Morgan's Canon edit

Coined by 19th-century British psychologist C. Lloyd Morgan, Morgan's Canon remains a fundamental precept of comparative (animal) psychology. In its developed form, it states that:[11]

In no case is an animal activity to be interpreted in terms of higher psychological processes if it can be fairly interpreted in terms of processes which stand lower in the scale of psychological evolution and development.

In other words, Morgan believed that anthropomorphic approaches to animal behavior were fallacious, and that people should only consider behaviour as, for example, rational, purposive or affectionate, if there is no other explanation in terms of the behaviours of more primitive life-forms to which we do not attribute those faculties.

From anecdote to laboratory edit

Speculation about animal intelligence gradually yielded to scientific study after Darwin placed humans and animals on a continuum, although Darwin's largely anecdotal approach to the cognition topic would not pass scientific muster later on.[12] This method would be expanded by his protégé George J. Romanes,[13] who played a key role in the defense of Darwinism and its refinement over the years. Still, Romanes is most famous for two major flaws in his work: his focus on anecdotal observations and entrenched anthropomorphism.[14] Unsatisfied with the previous approach, E. L. Thorndike brought animal behavior into the laboratory for objective scrutiny. Thorndike's careful observations of the escape of cats, dogs, and chicks from puzzle boxes led him to conclude that what appears to the naive human observer to be intelligent behavior may be strictly attributable to simple associations. According to Thorndike, using Morgan's Canon, the inference of animal reason, insight, or consciousness is unnecessary and misleading.[15] At about the same time, I. P. Pavlov began his seminal studies of conditioned reflexes in dogs. Pavlov quickly abandoned attempts to infer canine mental processes; such attempts, he said, led only to disagreement and confusion. He was, however, willing to propose unseen physiological processes that might explain his observations.[16]

The behavioristic half-century edit

The work of Thorndike, Pavlov and a little later of the outspoken behaviorist John B. Watson[17] set the direction of much research on animal behavior for more than half a century. During this time there was considerable progress in understanding simple associations; notably, around 1930 the differences between Thorndike's instrumental (or operant) conditioning and Pavlov's classical (or Pavlovian) conditioning were clarified, first by Miller and Kanorski, and then by B. F. Skinner.[18][19] Many experiments on conditioning followed; they generated some complex theories,[20] but they made little or no reference to intervening mental processes. Probably the most explicit dismissal of the idea that mental processes control behavior was the radical behaviorism of Skinner. This view seeks to explain behavior, including "private events" like mental images, solely by reference to the environmental contingencies impinging on the human or animal.[21]

Despite the predominantly behaviorist orientation of research before 1960, the rejection of mental processes in animals was not universal during those years. Influential exceptions included, for example, Wolfgang Köhler and his insightful chimpanzees[22] and Edward Tolman whose proposed cognitive map was a significant contribution to subsequent cognitive research in both humans and animals.[23]

The cognitive revolution edit

Beginning around 1960, a "cognitive revolution" in research on humans[24] gradually spurred a similar transformation of research with animals. Inference to processes not directly observable became acceptable and then commonplace. An important proponent of this shift in thinking was Donald O. Hebb, who argued that "mind" is simply a name for processes in the head that control complex behavior, and that it is both necessary and possible to infer those processes from behavior.[25] Animals came to be seen as "goal seeking agents that acquire, store, retrieve, and internally process information at many levels of cognitive complexity".[26]

Methods edit

The acceleration of research on animal cognition in the last 50 years or so has led to a rapid expansion in the variety of species studied and methods employed. The remarkable behavior of large-brained animals such as primates and cetacea have claimed special attention, but all sorts of animals large and small (birds, fish, ants, bees, and others) have been brought into the laboratory or observed in carefully controlled field studies. In the laboratory, animals push levers, pull strings, dig for food, swim in water mazes, or respond to images on computer screens to get information for discrimination, attention, memory, and categorization experiments.[27] Careful field studies explore memory for food caches, navigation by stars,[28] communication, tool use, identification of conspecifics, and many other matters. Studies often focus on the behavior of animals in their natural environments and discuss the putative function of the behavior for the propagation and survival of the species. These developments reflect an increased cross-fertilization from related fields such as ethology and behavioral ecology. Contributions from behavioral neuroscience are beginning to clarify the physiological substrate of some inferred mental process.

Some researchers have made effective use of a Piagetian methodology, taking tasks which human children are known to master at different stages of development and investigating which of them can be performed by particular species. Others have been inspired by concerns for animal welfare and the management of domestic species; for example, Temple Grandin has harnessed her unique expertise in animal welfare and the ethical treatment of farm livestock to highlight underlying similarities between humans and other animals.[29] From a methodological point of view, one of the main risks in this sort of work is anthropomorphism, the tendency to interpret an animal's behavior in terms of human feelings, thoughts, and motivations.[1]

Research questions edit

 
The common chimpanzee can use tools. This individual is using a stick to get food.

Human and non-human animal cognition have much in common, and this is reflected in the research summarized below; most of the headings found here might also appear in an article on human cognition. Of course, research in the two also differs in important respects. Notably, much research with humans either studies or involves language, and much research with animals is related directly or indirectly to behaviors important to survival in natural settings. Following are summaries of some of the major areas of research in animal cognition.

Perception edit

Animals process information from eyes, ears, and other sensory organs to perceive the environment. Perceptual processes have been studied in many species, with results that are often similar to those in humans. Equally interesting are those perceptual processes that differ from, or go beyond those found in humans, such as echolocation in bats and dolphins, motion detection by skin receptors in fish, and extraordinary visual acuity, motion sensitivity and ability to see ultraviolet light in some birds.[30]

Attention edit

Much of what is happening in the world at any moment is irrelevant to current behavior. Attention refers to mental processes that select relevant information, inhibit irrelevant information, and switch among these as the situation demands.[31] Often the selective process is tuned before relevant information appears; such expectation makes for rapid selection of key stimuli when they become available. A large body of research has explored the way attention and expectation affect the behavior of non-human animals, and much of this work suggests that attention operates in birds, mammals and reptiles in much the same way that it does in humans.[32]

Selective learning edit

Animals trained to discriminate between two stimuli, say black versus white, can be said to attend to the "brightness dimension", but this says little about whether this dimension is selected in preference to others. More enlightenment comes from experiments that allow the animal to choose from several alternatives. For example, several studies have shown that performance is better on, for example, a color discrimination (e.g. blue vs green) after the animal has learned another color discrimination (e.g. red vs orange) than it is after training on a different dimension such as an X shape versus an O shape. The reverse effect happens after training on forms. Thus, the earlier learning appears to affect which dimension, color or form, the animal will attend to.[33]

Other experiments have shown that after animals have learned to respond to one aspect of the environment responsiveness to other aspects is suppressed. In "blocking", for example, an animal is conditioned to respond to one stimulus ("A") by pairing that stimulus with reward or punishment. After the animal responds consistently to A, a second stimulus ("B") accompanies A on additional training trials. Later tests with the B stimulus alone elicit little response, suggesting that learning about B has been blocked by prior learning about A.[34] This result supports the hypothesis that stimuli are neglected if they fail to provide new information. Thus, in the experiment just cited, the animal failed to attend to B because B added no information to that supplied by A. If true, this interpretation is an important insight into attentional processing, but this conclusion remains uncertain because blocking and several related phenomena can be explained by models of conditioning that do not invoke attention.[35]

Divided attention edit

Attention is a limited resource and is not a none-or-all response: the more attention devoted to one aspect of the environment, the less is available for others.[36] A number of experiments have studied this in animals. In one experiment, a tone and a light are presented simultaneously to pigeons. The pigeons gain a reward only by choosing the correct combination of the two stimuli (e.g. a high frequency tone together with a yellow light). The birds perform well at this task, presumably by dividing attention between the two stimuli. When only one of the stimuli varies and the other is presented at its rewarded value, discrimination improves on the variable stimulus but discrimination on the alternative stimulus worsens.[37] These outcomes are consistent with the notion that attention is a limited resource that can be more or less focused among incoming stimuli.

Visual search and attentional priming edit

As noted above, the function of attention is to select information that is of special use to the animal. Visual search typically calls for this sort of selection, and search tasks have been used extensively in both humans and animals to determine the characteristics of attentional selection and the factors that control it.

Experimental research on visual search in animals was initially prompted by field observations published by Luc Tinbergen (1960).[38] Tinbergen observed that birds are selective when foraging for insects. For example, he found that birds tended to catch the same type of insect repeatedly even though several types were available. Tinbergen suggested that this prey selection was caused by an attentional bias that improved detection of one type of insect while suppressing detection of others. This "attentional priming" is commonly said to result from a pretrial activation of a mental representation of the attended object, which Tinbergen called a "searching image".

Tinbergen's field observations on priming have been supported by a number of experiments. For example, Pietrewicz and Kamil (1977, 1979)[39][40] presented blue jays with pictures of tree trunks upon which rested either a moth of species A, a moth of species B, or no moth at all. The birds were rewarded for pecks at a picture showing a moth. Crucially, the probability with which a particular species of moth was detected was higher after repeated trials with that species (e.g. A, A, A,...) than it was after a mixture of trials (e.g. A, B, B, A, B, A, A...). These results suggest again that sequential encounters with an object can establish an attentional predisposition to see the object.

Another way to produce attentional priming in search is to provide an advance signal that is associated with the target. For example, if a person hears a song sparrow he or she may be predisposed to detect a song sparrow in a shrub, or among other birds. A number of experiments have reproduced this effect in animal subjects.[41][42]

Still other experiments have explored nature of stimulus factors that affect the speed and accuracy of visual search. For example, the time taken to find a single target increases as the number of items in the visual field increases. This rise in reaction time is steep if the distracters are similar to the target, less steep if they are dissimilar, and may not occur if the distracters are very different from the target in form or color.[43]

Concepts and categories edit

Fundamental but difficult to define, the concept of "concept" was discussed for hundreds of years by philosophers before it became a focus of psychological study. Concepts enable humans and animals to organize the world into functional groups; the groups may be composed of perceptually similar objects or events, diverse things that have a common function, relationships such as same versus different, or relations among relations such as analogies.[44] Extensive discussions on these matters together with many references may be found in Shettleworth (2010)[1] Wasserman and Zentall (2006)[27] and in Zentall et al. (2008). The latter is freely available online.[45]

Methods edit

Most work on animal concepts has been done with visual stimuli, which can easily be constructed and presented in great variety, but auditory and other stimuli have been used as well.[46] Pigeons have been widely used, for they have excellent vision and are readily conditioned to respond to visual targets; other birds and a number of other animals have been studied as well.[1] In a typical experiment, a bird or other animal confronts a computer monitor on which a large number of pictures appear one by one, and the subject gets a reward for pecking or touching a picture of a category item and no reward for non-category items. Alternatively, a subject may be offered a choice between two or more pictures. Many experiments end with the presentation of items never seen before; successful sorting of these items shows that the animal has not simply learned many specific stimulus-response associations. A related method, sometimes used to study relational concepts, is matching-to-sample. In this task an animal sees one stimulus and then chooses between two or more alternatives, one of which is the same as the first; the animal is then rewarded for choosing the matching stimulus.[1][27][45]

Perceptual categories edit

Perceptual categorization is said to occur when a person or animal responds in a similar way to a range of stimuli that share common features. For example, a squirrel climbs a tree when it sees Rex, Shep, or Trixie, which suggests that it categorizes all three as something to avoid. This sorting of instances into groups is crucial to survival. Among other things, an animal must categorize if it is to apply learning about one object (e.g. Rex bit me) to new instances of that category (dogs may bite).[1][27][45]

Natural categories edit

Many animals readily classify objects by perceived differences in form or color. For example, bees or pigeons quickly learn to choose any red object and reject any green object if red leads to reward and green does not. Seemingly much more difficult is an animal's ability to categorize natural objects that vary a great deal in color and form even while belonging to the same group. In a classic study, Richard J. Herrnstein trained pigeons to respond to the presence or absence of human beings in photographs.[47] The birds readily learned to peck photos that contained partial or full views of humans and to avoid pecking photos with no human, despite great differences in the form, size, and color of both the humans displayed and in the non-human pictures. In follow-up studies, pigeons categorized other natural objects (e.g. trees) and after training they were able without reward to sort photos they had not seen before .[48][49] Similar work has been done with natural auditory categories, for example, bird songs.[50] Honeybees (Apis mellifera) are able to form concepts of "up" and "down".[51]

Functional or associative categories edit

Perceptually unrelated stimuli may come to be responded to as members of a class if they have a common use or lead to common consequences. An oft-cited study by Vaughan (1988) provides an example.[52] Vaughan divided a large set of unrelated pictures into two arbitrary sets, A and B. Pigeons got food for pecking at pictures in set A but not for pecks at pictures in set B. After they had learned this task fairly well, the outcome was reversed: items in set B led to food and items in set A did not. Then the outcome was reversed again, and then again, and so on. Vaughan found that after 20 or more reversals, associating a reward with a few pictures in one set caused the birds to respond to the other pictures in that set without further reward as if they were thinking "if these pictures in set A bring food, the others in set A must also bring food." That is, the birds now categorized the pictures in each set as functionally equivalent. Several other procedures have yielded similar results.[1][45]

Relational or abstract categories edit

When tested in a simple stimulus matching-to-sample task (described above) many animals readily learn specific item combinations, such as "touch red if the sample is red, touch green if the sample is green." But this does not demonstrate that they distinguish between "same" and "different" as general concepts. Better evidence is provided if, after training, an animal successfully makes a choice that matches a novel sample that it has never seen before. Monkeys and chimpanzees do learn to do this, as do pigeons if they are given a great deal of practice with many different stimuli. However, because the sample is presented first, successful matching might mean that the animal is simply choosing the most recently seen "familiar" item rather than the conceptually "same" item. A number of studies have attempted to distinguish these possibilities, with mixed results.[1][45]

Rule learning edit

The use of rules has sometimes been considered an ability restricted to humans, but a number of experiments have shown evidence of simple rule learning in primates[53] and also in other animals. Much of the evidence has come from studies of sequence learning in which the "rule" consists of the order in which a series of events occurs. Rule use is shown if the animal learns to discriminate different orders of events and transfers this discrimination to new events arranged in the same order. For example, Murphy et al. (2008)[54] trained rats to discriminate between visual sequences. For one group ABA and BAB were rewarded, where A="bright light" and B="dim light". Other stimulus triplets were not rewarded. The rats learned the visual sequence, although both bright and dim lights were equally associated with reward. More importantly, in a second experiment with auditory stimuli, rats responded correctly to sequences of novel stimuli that were arranged in the same order as those previously learned. Similar sequence learning has been demonstrated in birds and other animals as well.[55]

Memory edit

The categories that have been developed to analyze human memory (short term memory, long term memory, working memory) have been applied to the study of animal memory, and some of the phenomena characteristic of human short term memory (e.g. the serial position effect) have been detected in animals, particularly monkeys.[56] However most progress has been made in the analysis of spatial memory; some of this work has sought to clarify the physiological basis of spatial memory and the role of the hippocampus; other work has explored the spatial memory of scatter-hoarder animals such as Clark's nutcracker, certain jays, tits and certain squirrels, whose ecological niches require them to remember the locations of thousands of caches,[1][57] often following radical changes in the environment.

Memory has been widely investigated in foraging honeybees, Apis mellifera, which use both transient short-term working memory that is non-feeder specific and a feeder specific long-term reference memory.[58][59][60] Memory induced in a free-flying honeybee by a single learning trial lasts for days and, by three learning trials, for a lifetime.[61] Bombus terrestris audax workers vary in their effort investment towards memorising flower locations, with smaller workers less able to be selective and thus less interested in which flowers are richer sugar sources.[62][63] Meanwhile, bigger B. t. audax workers have more carrying capacity and thus more reason to memorise that information, and so they do.[62][63] Slugs, Limax flavus, have a short-term memory of approximately 1 min and long-term memory of 1 month.[64]

Methods edit

As in humans, research with animals distinguishes between "working" or "short-term" memory from "reference" or long-term memory. Tests of working memory evaluate memory for events that happened in the recent past, usually within the last few seconds or minutes. Tests of reference memory evaluate memory for regularities such as "pressing a lever brings food" or "children give me peanuts".

Habituation edit

This is one of the simplest tests for memory spanning a short time interval. The test compares an animal's response to a stimulus or event on one occasion to its response on a previous occasion. If the second response differs consistently from the first, the animal must have remembered something about the first, unless some other factor such as motivation, sensory sensitivity, or the test stimulus has changed.

Delayed response edit

Delayed response tasks are often used to study short-term memory in animals. Introduced by Hunter (1913), a typical delayed response task presents an animal with a stimulus such as colored light, and after a short time interval the animal chooses among alternatives that match the stimulus, or are related to the stimulus in some other way. In Hunter's studies, for example, a light appeared briefly in one of three goal boxes and then later the animal chose among the boxes, finding food behind the one that had been lighted.[65] Most research has been done with some variation of the "delayed matching-to-sample" task. For example, in the initial study with this task, a pigeon was presented with a flickering or steady light. Then, a few seconds later, two pecking keys were illuminated, one with a steady light and one with a flickering light. The bird got food if it pecked the key that matched the original stimulus.[66]

A commonly-used variation of the matching-to-sample task requires the animal to use the initial stimulus to control a later choice between different stimuli. For example, if the initial stimulus is a black circle, the animal learns to choose "red" after the delay; if it is a black square, the correct choice is "green". Ingenious variations of this method have been used to explore many aspects of memory, including forgetting due to interference and memory for multiple items.[1]

Radial arm maze edit

The radial arm maze is used to test memory for spatial location and to determine the mental processes by which location is determined. In a radial maze test, an animal is placed on a small platform from which paths lead in various directions to goal boxes; the animal finds food in one or more goal boxes. Having found food in a box, the animal must return to the central platform. The maze may be used to test both reference and working memory. Suppose, for example, that over a number of sessions the same 4 arms of an 8-arm maze always lead to food. If in a later test session the animal goes to a box that has never been baited, this indicates a failure of reference memory. On the other hand, if the animal goes to a box that it has already emptied during the same test session, this indicates a failure of working memory. Various confounding factors, such as odor cues, are carefully controlled in such experiments.[67]

Water maze edit

The water maze is used to test an animal's memory for spatial location and to discover how an animal is able to determine locations. Typically the maze is a circular tank filled with water that has been made milky so that it is opaque. Located somewhere in the maze is a small platform placed just below the surface of the water. When placed in the tank, the animal swims around until it finds and climbs up on the platform. With practice, the animal finds the platform more and more quickly. Reference memory is assessed by removing the platform and observing the relative amount of time the animal spends swimming in the area where the platform had been located. Visual and other cues in and around the tank may be varied to assess the animal's reliance on landmarks and the geometric relations among them.[68]

Novel object recognition test edit

The novel object recognition (NOR) test is an animal behavior test that is primarily used to assess memory alterations in rodents. It is a simple behavioral test that is based on a rodents innate exploratory behavior. The test is divided into three phases: habituation, training/adaptation and test phase. During the habituation phase the animal is placed in an empty test arena. This is followed by the adaptation phase, where the animal is placed in the arena with two identical objects. In the third phase, the test phase, the animal is placed in the arena with one of the familiar objects from the previous phase and with one novel object. Based on the rodents innate curiosity, the animals that remember the familiar object will spend more time on investigating the novel object.[69]

Spatial cognition edit

Whether an animal ranges over a territory measured in square kilometers or square meters, its survival typically depends on its ability to do such things as find a food source and then return to its nest. Sometimes such a task can be performed rather simply, for example by following a chemical trail. Typically, however, the animal must somehow acquire and use information about locations, directions, and distances. The following paragraphs outline some of the ways that animals do this.[1][70]

  • Beacons Animals often learn what their nest or other goal looks like, and if it is within sight they may simply move toward it; it is said to serve as a "beacon".
  • Landmarks When an animal is unable to see its goal, it may learn the appearance of nearby objects and use these landmarks as guides. Researchers working with birds and bees have demonstrated this by moving prominent objects in the vicinity of nest sites, causing returning foragers to hunt for their nest in a new location.[1]
  • Dead reckoning, also known as "path integration", is the process of computing one's position by starting from a known location and keeping track of the distances and directions subsequently traveled. Classic experiments have shown that the desert ant keeps track of its position in this way as it wanders for many meters searching for food. Though it travels in a randomly twisted path, it heads straight home when it finds food. However, if the ant is picked up and released some meters to the east, for example, it heads for a location displaced by the same amount to the east of its home nest.
  • Cognitive maps Some animals appear to construct a cognitive map of their surroundings, meaning that they acquire and use information that enables them to compute how far and in what direction to go to get from one location to another. Such a map-like representation is thought to be used, for example, when an animal goes directly from one food source to another even though its previous experience has involved only travel between each source and home.[1][71] Research in this area[70] has also explored such topics as the use of geometric properties of the environment by rats and pigeons, and the ability of rats to represent a spatial pattern in either radial arm mazes or water mazes. Spatial cognition is used in visual search when an animal or human searches their environment for specific objects to focus on among other objects in the environment.[72]
  • Detour behaviour Some animals appear to have an advanced understanding of their spatial environment and will not take the most direct route if this confers an advantage to them. Some jumping spiders take an indirect route to prey rather than the most direct route, thereby indicating flexibility in behaviour and route planning, and possibly insight learning.[73]

Long-distance navigation; homing edit

Many animals travel hundreds or thousands of miles in seasonal migrations or returns to breeding grounds. They may be guided by the Sun, the stars, the polarization of light, magnetic cues, olfactory cues, winds, or a combination of these.[74] This extensive area of research is covered in the main article on Animal navigation.

It has been hypothesized that animals such as apes and wolves are good at spatial cognition because this skill is necessary for survival. Some researchers argue that this ability may have diminished somewhat in dogs because humans have provided necessities such as food and shelter during some 15,000 years of domestication.[75][76][77]

Timing edit

Time of day: circadian rhythms edit

The behavior of most animals is synchronized with the earth's daily light-dark cycle. Thus, many animals are active during the day, others are active at night, still others near dawn and dusk. Though one might think that these "circadian rhythms" are controlled simply by the presence or absence of light, nearly every animal that has been studied has been shown to have a "biological clock" that yields cycles of activity even when the animal is in constant illumination or darkness.[1] Circadian rhythms are so automatic and fundamental to living things – they occur even in plants[78] – that they are usually discussed separately from cognitive processes, and the reader is referred to the main article (Circadian rhythms) for further information.[79]

Interval timing edit

Survival often depends on an animal's ability to time intervals. For example, rufous hummingbirds feed on the nectar of flowers, and they often return to the same flower, but only after the flower has had enough time to replenish its supply of nectar. In one experiment hummingbirds fed on artificial flowers that quickly emptied of nectar but were refilled at some fixed time (e.g. twenty minutes) later. The birds learned to come back to the flowers at about the right time, learning the refill rates of up to eight separate flowers and remembering how long ago they had visited each one.[80]

The details of interval timing have been studied in a number of species. One of the most common methods is the "peak procedure". In a typical experiment, a rat in an operant chamber presses a lever for food. A light comes on, a lever-press brings a food pellet at a fixed later time, say 10 seconds, and then the light goes off. Timing is measured during occasional test trials on which no food is presented and the light stays on. On these test trials, the rat presses the lever more and more until about 10 sec and then, when no food comes, gradually stops pressing. The time at which the rat presses most on these test trials is taken to be its estimate of the payoff time.

Experiments using the peak procedure and other methods have shown that animals can time short intervals quite exactly, can time more than one event at once, and can integrate time with spatial and other cues. Such tests have also been used for quantitative tests of theories of animal timing, such as Gibbon's Scalar Expectancy Theory ("SET"),[81] Killeen's Behavioral Theory of Timing,[82] and Machado's Learning to Time model.[83] No one theory has yet gained unanimous agreement.[1]

Tool and weapon use edit

Although tool use was long assumed to be a uniquely human trait, there is now much evidence that many animals use tools, including mammals, birds, fish, cephalopods and insects. Discussions of tool use often involve a debate about what constitutes a "tool", and they often consider the relation of tool use to the animal's intelligence and brain size.

Mammals edit

Series of photographs showing a bonobo fishing for termites
 
A bonobo inserting a stick into a termite mound
 
The bonobo starts "fishing" for the termites.
 
The bonobo withdraws the stick and begins eating the termites.
 
The bonobo eats the termites extracted with the tool.

Tool use has been reported many times in both wild and captive primates, particularly the great apes. The use of tools by primates is varied and includes hunting (mammals, invertebrates, fish), collecting honey, processing food (nuts, fruits, vegetables and seeds), collecting water, weapons and shelter. Research in 2007 shows that chimpanzees in the Fongoli savannah sharpen sticks to use as spears when hunting, considered the first evidence of systematic use of weapons in a species other than humans.[84] Other mammals that spontaneously use tools in the wild or in captivity include elephants, bears, cetaceans, sea otters and mongooses.

Birds edit

Several species of birds have been observed to use tools in the wild, including warblers, parrots, Egyptian vultures, brown-headed nuthatches, gulls and owls. Some species, such as the woodpecker finch of the Galapagos Islands, use particular tools as an essential part of their foraging behavior. However, these behaviors are often quite inflexible and cannot be applied effectively in new situations. A great many species of birds build nests with a wide range of complexities, but although nest-building behaviour fulfills the criteria of some definitions of "tool-use", this is not the case with other definitions.

Several species of corvids have been trained to use tools in controlled experiments. One species examined extensively under laboratory conditions is the New Caledonian crow. One individual called "Betty" spontaneously made a wire tool to solve a novel problem. She was being tested to see whether she would select a wire hook rather than a straight wire to pull a little bucket of meat out of a well. Betty tried poking the straight wire at the meat. After a series of failures with this direct approach, she withdrew the wire and began directing it at the bottom of the well, which was secured to its base with duct tape. The wire soon became stuck, whereupon Betty pulled it sideways, bending it and unsticking it. She then inserted the hook into the well and extracted the meat. In all but one of 10 subsequent trials with only straight wire provided, she also made and used a hook in the same manner, but not before trying the straight wire first.[85][86]

Another bird that is highly studied for its intelligence is the African Gray Parrot. American animal behaviorist and psychologist Irene Pepperberg vindicated that African Grays possess cognitive abilities. Pepperberg used a bird named "Alex" in her trials and was able to prove that parrots could associate sound and meaning, demolishing long-held theories that birds were only capable of mimicking human voices. Studies by other researchers have determined that African Grays can use deductive reasoning to correctly choose between pairs of boxes containing food and boxes that are empty.[87] Until Pepperberg began this research in the 1970s, few scientists had studied intelligence in parrots, and few do today. Most inquiries have instead focused on monkeys, chimpanzees, gorillas, and dolphins, all of which are much more difficult to raise, feed, and handle.[88] By the late 1980s, Alex had learned the names of more than 50 different objects, five shapes, and seven colors. He'd also learned what "same" and "different" mean—a step so crucial in human intellectual development[89]

Fish edit

Several species of wrasses have been observed using rocks as anvils to crack bivalve (scallops, urchins and clams) shells. This behavior was first filmed[90] in an orange-dotted tuskfish (Choerodon anchorago) in 2009 by Giacomo Bernardi. The fish fans sand to unearth the bivalve, takes it into its mouth, swims several meters to a rock, which it then uses as an anvil by smashing the mollusc apart with sideward thrashes of the head. This behaviour has also been recorded in a blackspot tuskfish (Choerodon schoenleinii) on Australia's Great Barrier Reef, yellowhead wrasse (Halichoeres garnoti) in Florida and a six-bar wrasse (Thalassoma hardwicke) in an aquarium setting. These species are at opposite ends of the phylogenetic tree in this family, so this behaviour may be a deep-seated trait in all wrasses.[91]

Invertebrates edit

Cephalopods are capable of complex tasks, thus earning them the reputation of being among the smartest of invertebrates. For example, octopuses can open jars to get the contents inside and have remarkable ability to learn new skills from the moment they are born.[92] Some cephalopods are known to use coconut shells for protection or camouflage.[93] Cephalopod cognitive evolution is hypothesized to have been shaped primarily by predatory and foraging pressures, but a challenging mating context may also have played a role.[92]

Ants of the species Conomyrma bicolor pick up stones and other small objects with their mandibles and drop them down the vertical entrances of rival colonies, allowing workers to forage for food without competition.[94]

Reasoning and problem solving edit

It is clear that animals of quite a range of species are capable of solving problems that appear to require abstract reasoning;[95] Wolfgang Köhler's (1917) work with chimpanzees is a famous early example. He observed that chimpanzees did not use trial and error to solve problems such as retrieving bananas hung out of reach. Instead, they behaved in a manner that was "unwaveringly purposeful", spontaneously placing boxes so that they could climb to reach the fruit.[22] Modern research has identified similar behavior in animals usually thought of as much less intelligent, if appropriate pre-training is given.[96] Causal reasoning has also been observed in rooks and New Caledonian crows.[97][98]

It has been shown that Barbados bullfinches (Loxigilla barbadensis) from urbanized areas are better at innovative problem-solving tasks than bullfinches from rural environments, but that they did not differ in colour discrimination learning.[99]

Cognitive bias edit

 
Is the glass half empty or half full?

A cognitive bias refers to a systematic pattern of deviation from norm or rationality in judgment, whereby inferences about other individuals or situations may be drawn in an illogical fashion.

Cognitive bias is sometimes illustrated by using answers to the question "Is the glass half empty or half full?". Choosing "half empty" is supposed to indicate pessimism whereas choosing "half full" indicates optimism. To test this in animals, an individual is trained to anticipate that stimulus A, e.g. a 100 Hz tone, precedes a positive event, e.g. highly desired food is delivered when a lever is pressed by the animal. The same individual is trained to anticipate that stimulus B, e.g. a 900 Hz tone, precedes a negative event, e.g. bland food is delivered when the animal presses a lever. The animal is then tested by being given an intermediate stimulus C, e.g. a 500 Hz tone, and observing whether the animal presses the lever associated with the positive or negative reward. This has been suggested to indicate whether the animal is in a positive or negative mood.[100]

In a study that used this approach, rats that were playfully tickled responded differently than rats that were simply handled. The rats that had been tickled were more optimistic than the handled rats.[101] The authors suggested that they had demonstrated "...for the first time a link between the directly measured positive affective state and decision making under uncertainty in an animal model".

There is some evidence for cognitive bias in a number of species, including rats, dogs, rhesus macaques, sheep, chicks, starlings and honeybees.[102]

Language edit

The modeling of human language in animals is known as animal language research. In addition to the ape-language experiments mentioned above, there have also been more or less successful attempts to teach language or language-like behavior to some non-primate species, including parrots and great spotted woodpeckers. Arguing from his own results with the animal Nim Chimpsky and his analysis of others results, Herbert Terrace criticized the idea that chimps can produce new sentences.[103] Shortly thereafter Louis Herman published research on artificial language comprehension in the bottlenosed dolphin (Herman, Richards, & Wolz, 1984). Though this sort of research has been controversial, especially among cognitive linguists, many researchers agree that many animals can understand the meaning of individual words, and that some may understand simple sentences and syntactic variations, but there is little evidence that any animal can produce new strings of symbols that correspond to new sentences.[1]

Insight edit

Wolfgang Köhler is usually credited with introducing the concept of insight into experimental psychology.[86] Working with chimpanzees, Köhler came to dispute Edward Thorndike's theory that animals must solve problems gradually, by trial and error. He said that Thorndike's animals could only use trial and error because the situation precluded other forms of problem solving. He provided chimps with a relatively unstructured situation, and he observed sudden "ah-ha!" insightful changes of behavior, as, for example, when a chimp suddenly moved a box into position so that it could retrieve a banana.[104] More recently, Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) were shown to exhibit similar insightful problem solving. A male was observed moving a box to a position where it could be stood upon to reach food that had been deliberately hung out of reach.[105]

Numeracy edit

A variety of studies indicates that animals are able to use and communicate quantitative information, and that some can count in a rudimentary way. Some examples of this research follow.

In one study, rhesus monkeys viewed visual displays containing, for example, 1, 2, 3, or 4 items of different sorts. They were trained to respond to them in several ways involving numerical ordering, for example touching "1" first, "2" second and so on. When tested with displays containing items they had never seen before, they continued to respond to them in order. The authors conclude that monkeys can represent the numerosities 1 to 9 at least on an ordinal scale.[106]

Ants are able to use quantitative values and transmit this information.[107][108] For instance, ants of several species are able to estimate quite precisely numbers of encounters with members of other colonies on their feeding territories.[109][110] Numeracy has been described in the yellow mealworm beetle (Tenebrio molitor)[111] and the honeybee.[112]

Western lowland gorillas given the choice between two food trays demonstrated the ability to choose the tray with more food items at a rate higher than chance after training.[113] In a similar task, chimpanzees chose the option with the larger amount of food.[114] Salamanders given a choice between two displays with differing amounts of fruit flies, used as a food reward, reliably choose the display with more flies, as shown in a particular experiment.[115]

Other experiments have been conducted that show animals' abilities to differentiate between non-food quantities. American black bears demonstrated quantity differentiation abilities in a task with a computer screen. The bears were trained to touch a computer monitor with a paw or nose to choose a quantity of dots in one of two boxes on the screen. Each bear was trained with reinforcement to pick a larger or smaller amount. During training, the bears were rewarded with food for a correct response. All bears performed better than what random error predicted on the trials with static, non-moving dots, indicating that they could differentiate between the two quantities. The bears choosing correctly in congruent (number of dots coincided with area of the dots) and incongruent (number of dots did not coincide with area of the dots) trials suggests that they were indeed choosing between quantities that appeared on the screen, not just a larger or smaller retinal image, which would indicate they are only judging size.[116]

Bottlenose dolphins have shown the ability to choose an array with fewer dots compared to one with more dots. Experimenters set up two boards showing various numbers of dots in a poolside setup. The dolphins were initially trained to choose the board with the fewer number of dots. This was done by rewarding the dolphin when it chose the board with the fewer number of dots. In the experimental trials, two boards were set up, and the dolphin would emerge from the water and point to one board. The dolphins chose the arrays with fewer dots at a rate much larger than chance, indicating they can differentiate between quantities.[117] A particular grey parrot, after training, has shown the ability to differentiate between the numbers zero through six using vocalizations. After number and vocalization training, this was done by asking the parrot how many objects there were in a display. The parrot was able to identify the correct amount at a rate higher than chance.[118]Angelfish, when put in an unfamiliar environment will group together with conspecifics, an action named shoaling. Given the choice between two groups of differing size, the angelfish will choose the larger of the two groups. This can be seen with a discrimination ratio of 2:1 or greater, such that, as long as one group has at least twice the fish as another group, it will join the larger one.[119]

Monitor lizards have been shown to be capable of numeracy, and some species can distinguish among numbers up to six.[120]

Sapience edit

As the cognitive ability and intelligence in non-human animals cannot be measured with verbal scales, it has been measured using a variety of methods that involve such things as habit reversal, social learning, and responses to novelty. Principal component analysis and factor analytic studies have shown that a single factor of intelligence is responsible for 47% of the individual variance in cognitive ability measures in primates[121] and between 55% and 60% of the variance in mice.[122][123] These values are similar to the accepted variance in IQ explained by a similar single factor known as the general factor of intelligence in humans (40-50%).[124] However, results from a recent meta-analysis suggest that the average correlation between performance scores on various cognitive tasks is only 0.18.[125] Results from this study suggest that current evidence for general intelligence is weak in non-human animals.[125]

The general factor of intelligence, or g factor, is a psychometric construct that summarizes the correlations observed between an individual's scores on various measures of cognitive abilities. It has been suggested that g is related to evolutionary life histories and the evolution of intelligence[126] as well as to social learning and cultural intelligence.[127][128] Non-human models of g have been used in genetic[129] and neurological[130] research on intelligence to help understand the mechanisms behind variation in g.

Theory of mind edit

Theory of mind is the ability to attribute mental states, e.g. intents, desires, pretending, knowledge, to oneself and others and to understand that others have desires, intentions, and perspectives that are different from one's own.[131]

Some research with ravens provides an example of evidence for theory of mind in a non-human species. Ravens are members of the family Corvidae, which is widely regarded as having high cognitive abilities. These birds have been observed to hide their food when dominant ravens are visible and audible at the same time. Based on this observation, ravens were tested for their understanding of "seeing" as a mental state. In a first step, the birds protected their cache when dominants were visible but not when they could only be heard from an adjacent room. In the next step, they had access to a small peephole which allowed them to see into the adjacent room. With the peephole open, the ravens guarded their caches against discovery when they could hear dominants in the adjacent room, even when the dominant's sounds were playbacks of recordings.[132]

Consciousness edit

 
Mirror test with a baboon

The sense in which animals can be said to have self-consciousness or a self-concept has been hotly debated. The best known research technique in this area is the mirror test devised by Gordon G. Gallup, in which an animal's skin is marked in some way while it is asleep or sedated, and it is then allowed to see its reflection in a mirror; if the animal spontaneously directs grooming behavior towards the mark, that is taken as an indication that it is aware of itself.[133][134] Self-awareness, by this criterion, has been reported for chimpanzees[135][136] and also for other great apes,[137] the European magpie,[138] some cetaceans[139][140][141] and an Asian elephant,[142] but not for monkeys. The mirror test has been criticized by researchers because it is entirely focused on vision, the primary sense in humans, while other species rely more heavily on other senses such as the sense of smell in dogs.[143][144][145]

It has been suggested that metacognition in some animals provides some evidence for cognitive self-awareness.[146] The great apes, dolphins, and rhesus monkeys have demonstrated the ability to monitor their own mental states and use an "I don't know" response to avoid answering difficult questions. Unlike the mirror test, which reveals awareness of the condition of one's own body, this uncertainty monitoring is thought to reveal awareness of one's internal mental state. A 2007 study has provided some evidence for metacognition in rats,[147][148] although this interpretation has been questioned.[149][150] These species might also be aware of the strength of their memories.

Some researchers propose that animal calls and other vocal behaviors provide evidence of consciousness. This idea arose from research on children's crib talk by Weir (1962) and in investigations of early speech in children by Greenfield and others (1976). Some such research has been done with a macaw (see Arielle).

In July, 2012 during the "Consciousness in Human and Nonhuman Animals" conference in Cambridge a group of scientists announced and signed a declaration with the following conclusions:

Convergent evidence indicates that non-human animals have the neuroanatomical, neurochemical, and neurophysiological substrates of conscious states along with the capacity to exhibit intentional behaviors. Consequently, the weight of evidence indicates that humans are not unique in possessing the neurological substrates that generate consciousness. Non-human animals, including all mammals and birds, and many other creatures, including octopuses, also possess these neurological substrates.[151]

Biological constraints edit

Instinctive drift can influence the interpretation of cognitive research. Instinctive drift is the tendency of an animal to revert to instinctive behaviors that can interfere with learned responses. The concept originated with Keller and Marian Breland when they taught a raccoon to put coins into a box. The raccoon drifted to its instinctive behavior of rubbing the coins with its paws, as it would do when foraging for food.[152]

Animal ability to process and respond to stimuli is correlated with brain size. Small-brain animals tend to show simple behaviors that are less dependent on learning than those of large-brained animals. Vertebrates, particularly mammals, have large brains and complex behavior that changes with experience. A formula called the encephalization quotient (EQ) expresses a relationship between brain and body size; it was developed by H.J. Jerison in the late 1960s.[153] When the encephalization quotient is plotted as a curve, an animal with an EQ above the curve is expected to show more cognitive ability than the average animal of its size, whereas an animal with an EQ below the curve is expected to have less. Various formulas been suggested, but the equation Ew(brain) = 0.12w(body)2/3 has been found to fit data from a sample of mammals.[154] The formula is suggestive at best, and should only be applied to non-mammals with extreme caution. For some of the other vertebrate classes, the power of 3/4 rather than 2/3 is sometimes used, and for many groups of invertebrates, the formula may not give meaningful results.

Experimental evidence against animal cognition edit

Several experiments cannot be readily reconciled with the belief that some animal species are intelligent, insightful, or possess a theory of mind.

Jean-Henri Fabre[155] (1823–1915), setting the stage for all subsequent experiments of this kind, argued that insects "obey their compelling instinct, without realizing what they do". For instance, to understand that she can grab her paralyzed prey by a leg instead of an antenna is utterly beyond the powers of a sand wasp. "Her actions are like a series of echoes each awakening the next in a settled order, which allows none to sound until the previous one has sounded." Fabre's numerous experiments led him, in turn, to the view that scientists often try to "exalt animals" instead of objectively studying them.

C. Lloyd Morgan's[156] (1852–1936) observations suggested to him that prima facie intelligent behavior in animals is often the result of either instincts or trial and error. For instance, most visitors watching Morgan's dog smoothly lifting a latch with the back of its head (and thereby opening a garden gate and escaping) were convinced that the dog's actions involved thinking. Morgan, however, carefully observed the dog's prior, random, purposeless actions and argued that they involved "continued trial and failure, until a happy effect is reached", rather than "methodical planning".

E. L. Thorndike[15] (1874–1949) placed hungry cats and dogs in enclosures "from which they could escape by some simple act, such as pulling at a loop of cord". Their behavior suggested to him that they did not "possess the power of rationality". Most books about animal behavior, Thorndike wrote, "do not give us a psychology, but rather a eulogy of animals".

Although Wolfgang Köhler's[157] experiments are often cited as providing support for the animal cognition hypothesis, his book is replete with counterexamples. For instance, he placed chimpanzees in a situation where they could only get bananas by removing a box. The chimpanzee, Köhler observed, "has special difficulty in solving such problems; he often draws into a situation the strangest and most distant tools, and adopts the most peculiar methods, rather than remove a simple obstacle which could be displaced with perfect ease".

Daniel J. Povinelli and Timothy Eddy[158] of the University of Louisiana showed that young chimpanzees, when given a choice between two food providers, were just as likely to beg food from a person who could see the begging gesture as from a person who could not, thereby raising the possibility that young chimpanzees do not understand that people see.

Moty Nissani[159] of Wayne State University trained Burmese logging elephants to lift a lid in order to retrieve food from a bucket. The lid was then placed on the ground alongside the bucket (where it no longer obstructed access to the food) while the treat was simultaneously placed inside the bucket. All elephants continued to toss the lid before retrieving the reward, thus suggesting that elephants do not grasp simple causal relationships.

Cognitive faculty by species edit

A traditionally common image is the scala naturae, the ladder of nature on which animals of different species occupy successively higher rungs, with humans typically at the top.[160][161] However, there is some disagreement with the use of such a hierarchy, with some critics saying it may be necessary to understand specific cognitive capacities as adaptations to differing ecological niches.[162] Some biologists argue that humans are not, in fact, the smartest animal, and that no animal can be characterized as the smartest, given that some animals have superior cognitive skills in certain areas.[163][164] This contrasts with evolutionary psychologists such as John Tooby, who assess, based on the large list of related unique characteristics that humans do possess, that humans evolved to fill a unique "cognitive niche" and can fairly be characterized as the smartest animal.[165]

Whether fairly or not, the performance of animals is often compared to that of humans on cognitive tasks. Our closest biological relatives, the great apes, tend to perform most like humans. Among the birds, corvids and parrots have typically been found to perform well on human-like tasks.[166] Some octopodes have also been shown to exhibit a number of higher-level skills such as tool use,[93] but the amount of research on cephalopod intelligence is still limited.[167] Baboons have been shown to be capable of recognizing words.[168][169][170]

The average bird or mammal, both usually endotherms, have average brain-to-body ratios ten times larger than a typical ectotherm vertebrate. This has contributed to a common perception amongst researchers that mammals and birds share similar "advanced" cognitive characteristics as humans, while other vertebrates such as teleost fishes are more "primitive", which has led to them being understudied. Despite this, increasing evidence indicates that fish possess not just capabilities that cannot be explained through Pavlovian and operant conditioning alone, such as reversal learning, novel obstacle avoidance, and passing simultaneous two-choice tasks, but also even more complex capabilities such as navigational cognitive mapping,[171][172] inhibitory motor control,[173] and empathy enabled by oxytocin to sense fear in other fish.[174] Similarly in reptiles, a 2019 review of evidence indicates they can experience numerous emotions, such as pleasure and anxiety.[175]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Shettleworth SJ (2010). Cognition, Evolution and Behavior (2ND ed.). New York: Oxford Press.
  2. ^ Krueger K, Heinze J (July 2008). "Horse sense: social status of horses (Equus caballus) affects their likelihood of copying other horses' behavior" (PDF). Animal Cognition. 11 (3): 431–9. doi:10.1007/s10071-007-0133-0. PMID 18183432. S2CID 16621030.
  3. ^ Krueger K, Farmer K, Heinze J (May 2014). "The effects of age, rank and neophobia on social learning in horses" (PDF). Animal Cognition. 17 (3): 645–55. doi:10.1007/s10071-013-0696-x. PMID 24170136. S2CID 13326701.
  4. ^ Schuetz A, Farmer K, Krueger K (May 2017). "Social learning across species: horses (Equus caballus) learn from humans by observation" (PDF). Animal Cognition. 20 (3): 567–573. doi:10.1007/s10071-016-1060-8. PMID 27866286. S2CID 3957230.
  5. ^ "Reptiles known as 'living rocks' show surprising cognitive powers". Nature. 576 (7785): 10. 2019-11-29. Bibcode:2019Natur.576...10.. doi:10.1038/d41586-019-03655-5. S2CID 208613023.
  6. ^ Shettleworth SJ (2010). Cognition, Evolution and Behavior (2ND ed.). New York: Oxford Press.
  7. ^ Descartes R (1649). Passions of the Soul.
  8. ^ "Crows understand water displacement at the level of a small child: Show causal understanding of a 5- to 7-year-old child". ScienceDaily. Retrieved 2019-12-08.
  9. ^ Pliny the Elder (1855). The Natural History of Pliny. Vol. 2. H. G. Bohn. ISBN 9780598910769.
  10. ^ Corcilius K, Gregoric P (2013-01-01). "Aristotle's Model of Animal Motion". Phronesis. 58 (1): 52–97. doi:10.1163/15685284-12341242. S2CID 52242579.
  11. ^ Morgan CL (1903). An Introduction to Comparative Psychology (2 ed.). W. Scott, London. pp. 59.
  12. ^ Darwin C (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex.
  13. ^ Romanes JG (1883). Animal Intelligence.
  14. ^ Dewsbury D (1978). Comparative Animal Behavior. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Book Company.
  15. ^ a b Thorndike EL (1911). Animal Intelligence. New York: Macmillan.
  16. ^ Pavlov IP (1928). Lectures on conditioned reflexes.
  17. ^ Watson JB (1913). "Psychology as the Behaviorist Views it". Psychological Review. 20 (2): 158–177. doi:10.1037/h0074428. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-9182-7. S2CID 145372026.
  18. ^ Miller S, Konorski J (1928). "Sur une forme particulière des reflexes conditionels". Comptes Rendus des Séances de la Société de Biologie et de Ses Filiales. 99: 1155–1157.
  19. ^ Skinner BF (1932). The Behavior of Organisms.
  20. ^ Hull CL (1943). The Principles of Behavior.
  21. ^ Skinner BF (1976). About Behaviorism.
  22. ^ a b Köhler W (1917). The Mentality of Apes.
  23. ^ Tolman EC (1948). "Cognitive maps in rats and men". Psychological Review. 55 (4): 189–208. doi:10.1037/h0061626. PMID 18870876. S2CID 42496633.
  24. ^ Niesser U (1967). Cognitive Psychology.
  25. ^ Hebb DO (1958). A Textbook of Psychology. p. 3.
  26. ^ Menzel R, Fischer J (2010). Animal Thinking: Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition. p. 2.
  27. ^ a b c d Wasserman EA, Zentall TR, eds. (2006). Comparative cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelligence. USA: Oxford University Press. p. 8 ff.
  28. ^ Foster JJ, Smolka J, Nilsson DE, Dacke M (January 2018). "How animals follow the stars". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 285 (1871): 20172322. doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.2322. PMC 5805938. PMID 29367394.
  29. ^ Grandin T, Johnson C (January 2010). Animals Make Us Human: Creating the Best Life for Animals. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.
  30. ^ Stebbins WC, Berkley MA (1990). Comparative Perception, Vol. I, Basic Mechanisms; Vol. II, Complex Signals. New York: Wiley.
  31. ^ Smith EE, Kosslyn SM (2007). Cognitive Psychology: Mind and Brain. Pearson Prentice Hall.
  32. ^ Blough DS (2006). "Reaction-time explorations of visual attention, perception, and decision in pigeons.". In Wasserman EA, Zentall TR (eds.). Comparative Cognition: Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence'. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 89–105.
  33. ^ Mackintosh NJ (1983). Conditioning and Associative Learning. New York: Oxford University Press.
  34. ^ Kamin LJ (1969). "Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning". In Campbell BA, Church RM (eds.). Punishment and Aversive Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. pp. 279–296.
  35. ^ Mackintosh NJ (1994). Animal Learning and Cognition. San Diego: Academic Press.
  36. ^ Zentall TR (April 2005). "Selective and divided attention in animals". Behavioural Processes. 69 (1): 1–15. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2005.01.004. PMID 15795066. S2CID 24601938.
  37. ^ Blough DS (October 1969). "Attention shifts in a maintained discrimination". Science. 166 (3901): 125–6. Bibcode:1969Sci...166..125B. doi:10.1126/science.166.3901.125. PMID 5809588. S2CID 33256491.
  38. ^ Tinbergen L (1960). "The natural control of insects in pine woods: I. Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds". Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie. 13: 265–343. doi:10.1163/036551660X00053.
  39. ^ Pietrewicz AT, Kamil AC (February 1977). "Visual Detection of Cryptic Prey by Blue Jays (Cyanocitta cristata)". Science. 195 (4278): 580–2. Bibcode:1977Sci...195..580P. doi:10.1126/science.195.4278.580. PMID 17732294. S2CID 10858793.
  40. ^ Pietrewicz AT, Kamil AC (June 1979). "Search Image Formation in the Blue Jay (Cyanocitta cristata)". Science. 204 (4399): 1332–3. Bibcode:1979Sci...204.1332P. doi:10.1126/science.204.4399.1332. PMID 17813172. S2CID 14809014.
  41. ^ Blough PM (1989). "Attentional priming and visual search in pigeons". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition. 17 (4): 292–298. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.17.3.292. PMID 2794871.
  42. ^ Kamil AC, Bond AB (2006). "Selective attention, priming, and foraging behavior.". In Wasserman EA, Zentall TR (eds.). Comparative Cognition: Experimental Exploration of Animal Intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.
  43. ^ Blough DS, Blough PM (1990). "Reaction-time assessments of visual processes in pigeons.". In Berkley M, Stebbins W (eds.). Comparative perception. New York: Wiley. pp. 245–276.
  44. ^ Smith EE, Medin DL (1981). Categories and Concepts. Harvard Univ. Press.
  45. ^ a b c d e Zentall TR, Wasserman EA, Lazareva OF, Thompson RK, Rattermann MJ (2008). "Concept Learning in Animals". Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews. 3: 13–45. doi:10.3819/ccbr.2008.30002.
  46. ^ Dooling RJ, Okanoya K (1995). "Psychophysical methods for assessing perceptual categories.". In Klump GM, Dooling RJ, Fay RR, Stebbins WC (eds.). Methods in Comparative Psychoacoustics. Basel, Switzerland: Birkhäuser Verlag. pp. 307–318.
  47. ^ Herrnstein RJ, Loveland DH (October 1964). "Complex Visual Concept in the Pigeon". Science. 146 (3643): 549–51. Bibcode:1964Sci...146..549H. doi:10.1126/science.146.3643.549. PMID 14190250. S2CID 11940233.
  48. ^ Herrnstein RJ (1979). "Acquisition, Generalization, and Discrimination Reversal of a Natural Concept". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 5 (2): 116–129. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.5.2.116. PMID 528881.
  49. ^ Bhatt RS, Wasserman EA, Reynolds WF, Knauss KS (July 1988). "Conceptual behavior in pigeons: Categorization of both familiar and novel examples from four classes of natural and artificial stimuli". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 14 (3): 219–234. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.14.3.219.
  50. ^ Tu HW, Smith EW, Dooling RJ (November 2011). "Acoustic and perceptual categories of vocal elements in the warble song of budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 125 (4): 420–30. doi:10.1037/a0024396. PMC 4497543. PMID 22142040.
  51. ^ Avarguès-Weber A, Dyer AG, Giurfa M (March 2011). "Conceptualization of above and below relationships by an insect". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 278 (1707): 898–905. doi:10.1098/rspb.2010.1891. PMC 3049051. PMID 21068040.
  52. ^ Vaughan Jr W (1988). "Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 14: 36–42. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.14.1.36.
  53. ^ D'Amato MR, Colombo M (April 1988). "Representation of serial order in monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 14 (2): 131–9. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.14.2.131. PMID 3367099.
  54. ^ Murphy RA, Mondragón E, Murphy VA (March 2008). "Rule learning by rats" (PDF). Science. 319 (5871): 1849–51. Bibcode:2008Sci...319.1849M. doi:10.1126/science.1151564. PMID 18369151. S2CID 591112.
  55. ^ Kundey SM, Strandell B, Mathis H, Rowan JD (2010). "Learning of monotonic and nonmonotonic sequences in domesticated horses (Equus callabus") and chickens (Gallus domesticus")". Learning and Motivation. 14 (3): 213–223. doi:10.1016/j.lmot.2010.04.006.
  56. ^ Wright AA, Santiago HC, Sands SF, Kendrick DF, Cook RG (July 1985). "Memory processing of serial lists by pigeons, monkeys, and people". Science. 229 (4710): 287–9. Bibcode:1985Sci...229..287W. doi:10.1126/science.9304205. PMID 9304205.
  57. ^ Balda R, Kamil AC (1992). "Long-term spatial memory in Clark's nutcracker, Nucifraga columbiana". Animal Behaviour. 44 (4): 761–769. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80302-1. S2CID 54277040.
  58. ^ Greggers U, Menzel R (1993). "Memory dynamics and foraging strategies of honeybees". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 32: 17–29. doi:10.1007/BF00172219. S2CID 36624838.
  59. ^ Menzel R (1993). "Associative learning in honey-bees". Apidologie. 24 (3): 157–168. doi:10.1051/apido:19930301.
  60. ^ Wüstenberg D, Gerber B, Menzel R (August 1998). "Short communication: long- but not medium-term retention of olfactory memories in honeybees is impaired by actinomycin D and anisomycin". The European Journal of Neuroscience. 10 (8): 2742–5. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.1998.00319.x. PMID 9767405. S2CID 23691527.
  61. ^ Hammer M, Menzel R (March 1995). "Learning and memory in the honeybee". The Journal of Neuroscience. 15 (3 Pt 1): 1617–30. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-03-01617.1995. PMC 6578179. PMID 7891123.
  62. ^ a b University of Exeter (2020-12-28). "Big bumblebees learn locations of best flowers". Phys.org. Retrieved 2020-12-29.
  63. ^ a b Frasnelli, Elisa; Robert, Théo; Chow, Pizza Ka Yee; Scales, Ben; Gibson, Sam; Manning, Nicola; Philippides, Andrew O.; Collett, Thomas S.; Hempel de Ibarra, Natalie (2020). "Small and Large Bumblebees Invest Differently when Learning about Flowers". Current Biology. 31 (5). Elsevier BV: 1058–1064.e3. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2020.11.062. hdl:10871/124274. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 33373638.
  64. ^ Yamada A, Sekiguchi T, Suzuki H, Mizukami A (March 1992). "Behavioral analysis of internal memory states using cooling-induced retrograde amnesia in Limax flavus". The Journal of Neuroscience. 12 (3): 729–35. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.12-03-00729.1992. PMC 6576046. PMID 1545237.
  65. ^ Hunter WS (1913). The delayed reaction in animals and children. Behavior Monographs. Vol. 2.
  66. ^ Blough DS (April 1959). "Delayed matching in the pigeon". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2 (2): 151–60. doi:10.1901/jeab.1959.2-151. PMC 1403892. PMID 13801643.
  67. ^ Shettleworth SJ (2010). Cognition, Evolution, and Behavior (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-971781-1.
  68. ^ Vorhees CV, Williams MT (2006). "Morris water maze: procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory". Nature Protocols. 1 (2): 848–58. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006.116. PMC 2895266. PMID 17406317.
  69. ^ Antunes M, Biala G (May 2012). "The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications". Cognitive Processing. 13 (2): 93–110. doi:10.1007/s10339-011-0430-z. PMC 3332351. PMID 22160349.
  70. ^ a b Brown MF, Cook RG, eds. (2006). . Archived from the original on 2022-03-02. Retrieved 2020-09-29.
  71. ^ Lund N (2002). Animal cognition. Psychology Press. p. 4. ISBN 978-0-415-25298-0.
  72. ^ Treisman AM, Gelade G (January 1980). "A feature-integration theory of attention". Cognitive Psychology. 12 (1): 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. PMID 7351125. S2CID 353246.
  73. ^ Sherwin CM (2001). "Can invertebrates suffer? Or, how robust is argument-by-analogy?". Animal Welfare. 10 (supplement): S103–S118. doi:10.1017/S0962728600023551. S2CID 54126137.
  74. ^ Gauthreaux SA (1980). Animal Migration, Orientation, and Navigation. Academic Press.
  75. ^ Savolainen P, Zhang YP, Luo J, Lundeberg J, Leitner T (November 2002). "Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin of domestic dogs". Science. 298 (5598): 1610–3. Bibcode:2002Sci...298.1610S. doi:10.1126/science.1073906. PMID 12446907. S2CID 32583311.
  76. ^ Fiset S, Plourde V (May 2013). "Object permanence in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and gray wolves (Canis lupus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 127 (2): 115–27. doi:10.1037/a0030595. PMID 23106804.
  77. ^ Bräuer J, Kaminski J, Riedel J, Call J, Tomasello M (February 2006). "Making inferences about the location of hidden food: social dog, causal ape". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 120 (1): 38–47. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.120.1.38. PMID 16551163. S2CID 10162449.
  78. ^ Webb AR (2003). "The physiology of circadian rhythms in plants". New Phytologist. 160 (2): 281–303. doi:10.1046/j.1469-8137.2003.00895.x. PMID 33832173. S2CID 15688409.
  79. ^ Call JE, Burghardt GM, Pepperberg IM, Snowdon CT, Zentall TE, eds. (2017). "Chapter 23 : Timing in Animals". APA handbook of comparative psychology: Perception, learning, and cognition. Vol. 2. Washington D.C.: APA.
  80. ^ Henderson J, Hurly TA, Bateson M, Healy SD (March 2006). "Timing in free-living rufous hummingbirds, Selasphorus rufus". Current Biology. 16 (5): 512–5. Bibcode:2006CBio...16..512H. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.054. PMID 16527747.
  81. ^ Gibbon J (1977). "Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing". Psychological Review. 84 (3): 279–325. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.84.3.279.
  82. ^ Killeen PR (1991). "Behavior's time.". In Bower G (ed.). The psychology of learning and motivation. Vol. 27. New York: Academic Press. pp. 294–334.
  83. ^ Machado A, Pata P (2005). "Testing the Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET) and the Learning to Time model (LeT) in a double bisection task". Animal Learning & Behavior. 33 (1): 111–122. doi:10.3758/BF03196055. PMID 15971498. S2CID 16623835.
  84. ^ Roach J (February 22, 2007). . National Geographic News. Archived from the original on February 24, 2007. Retrieved June 12, 2010.
  85. ^ Hunt GR (1996). "Manufacture and use of hook-tools by New Caledonian crows". Nature. 379 (6562): 249–251. Bibcode:1996Natur.379..249H. doi:10.1038/379249a0. S2CID 4352835.
  86. ^ a b Shettleworth SJ (December 2012). "Do animals have insight, and what is insight anyway?". Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 66 (4): 217–26. doi:10.1037/a0030674. PMID 23231629.
  87. ^ Pallardy, R (May 28, 2020). "African gray parrot". Encyclopedia Britannica.
  88. ^ Caldwell, M (January 2000). "Polly Wanna PhD?". Discover. 21.
  89. ^ Partal, Y. "Animal intelligence: The Smartest Animal Species in the World". Zoo Portraits.
  90. ^ Com, Scienceblog (28 September 2011). "Video shows first tool use by a fish". ScienceBlog.com.
  91. ^ Bernardi G (2011). "The use of tools by wrasses (Labridae)". Coral Reefs. 31: 39. doi:10.1007/s00338-011-0823-6. S2CID 37924172.
  92. ^ a b Piero, Amodio (2020). "Bipedal locomotion in Octopus vulgaris: A complementary observation and some preliminary considerations". Ecology and Evolution. 11 (9): 3679–3684. Bibcode:2021EcoEv..11.3679A. doi:10.1002/ece3.7328. PMC 8093653. PMID 33976767.
  93. ^ a b Finn JK, Tregenza T, Norman MD (December 2009). "Defensive tool use in a coconut-carrying octopus". Current Biology. 19 (23): R1069-70. Bibcode:2009CBio...19R1069F. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.10.052. PMID 20064403. S2CID 26835945.
  94. ^ Möglich MH, Alpert GD (1979). "Stone dropping by Conomyrma bicolor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): A new technique of interference competition". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 2 (6): 105–113. doi:10.1007/bf00292556. JSTOR 4599265. S2CID 27266459.
  95. ^ For chimpanzees, see for example Premack D, Premack AJ (1983). The Mind of an Ape. New York: Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-30160-1. OCLC 152413818.
  96. ^ Pepperberg IM (1999). The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.
  97. ^ Tebbich S, Seed AM, Emery NJ, Clayton NS (April 2007). "Non-tool-using rooks, Corvus frugilegus, solve the trap-tube problem". Animal Cognition. 10 (2): 225–31. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0061-4. PMID 17171360. S2CID 13611664.
  98. ^ Taylor AH, Hunt GR, Medina FS, Gray RD (January 2009). "Do new caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning?". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 276 (1655): 247–54. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1107. PMC 2674354. PMID 18796393.
  99. ^ Audet JN, Ducatez S, Lefebvre L (2015). "The town bird and the country bird: problem solving and immunocompetence vary with urbanization". Behavioral Ecology. 27 (2): 637–644. doi:10.1093/beheco/arv201.
  100. ^ Harding EJ, Paul ES, Mendl M (January 2004). "Animal behaviour: cognitive bias and affective state". Nature. 427 (6972): 312. Bibcode:2004Natur.427..312H. doi:10.1038/427312a. PMID 14737158. S2CID 4411418.
  101. ^ Rygula R, Pluta H, Popik P (2012). "Laughing rats are optimistic". PLOS ONE. 7 (12): e51959. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...751959R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0051959. PMC 3530570. PMID 23300582.
  102. ^ Haselton MG, Nettle D, Andrews PW (2005). "The evolution of cognitive bias". In Buss DM (ed.). The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. Hoboken, NJ, US: John Wiley & Sons Inc. pp. 724–746.
  103. ^ Terrace HS, Petitto LA, Sanders RJ, Bever TG (November 1979). "Can an ape create a sentence?". Science. 206 (4421). New York, N.Y.: 891–902. Bibcode:1979Sci...206..891T. doi:10.1126/science.504995. PMID 504995.
  104. ^ Köhler W (1917). Mentality of Apes.
  105. ^ Foerder P, Galloway M, Barthel T, Moore DE, Reiss D (2011). Samuel A (ed.). "Insightful problem solving in an Asian elephant". PLOS ONE. 6 (8): e23251. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...623251F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023251. PMC 3158079. PMID 21876741.
  106. ^ Brannon EM, Terrace HS (January 2000). "Representation of the numerosities 1-9 by rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta)" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 26 (1): 31–49. doi:10.1037/0097-7403.26.1.31. PMID 10650542.
  107. ^ Reznikova Z, Ryabko B (2001). "A Study of Ants' Numerical Competence". Electronic Transactions on Artificial Intelligence. 5: 111–126.
  108. ^ Reznikova ZI (2007). Animal Intelligence: From Individual to Social Cognition. Cambridge University Press.
  109. ^ Reznikova ZI (1999). "Ethological mechanisms of population dynamic in species ant communities". Russian Journal of Ecology. 30 (3): 187–197.
  110. ^ Brown MJ, Gordon DM (2000). "How resources and encounters affect the distribution of foraging activity in a seed-harvesting ants". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 47 (3): 195–203. doi:10.1007/s002650050011. S2CID 15454830.
  111. ^ Carazo P, Font E, Forteza-Behrendt E, Desfilis E (May 2009). "Quantity discrimination in Tenebrio molitor: evidence of numerosity discrimination in an invertebrate?". Animal Cognition. 12 (3): 463–70. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0207-7. PMID 19118405. S2CID 14502342.
  112. ^ Dacke M, Srinivasan MV (October 2008). "Evidence for counting in insects". Animal Cognition. 11 (4): 683–9. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0159-y. PMID 18504627. S2CID 22273226.
  113. ^ Anderson US, Stoinski TS, Bloomsmith MA, Marr MJ, Smith AD, Maple TL (August 2005). "Relative numerousness judgment and summation in young and old Western lowland gorillas". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 119 (3): 285–95. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.3.285. PMID 16131257.
  114. ^ Boysen ST, Berntson GG, Mukobi KL (March 2001). "Size matters: impact of item size and quantity on array choice by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 115 (1): 106–10. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.1.106. PMID 11334213.
  115. ^ Uller C, Jaeger R, Guidry G, Martin C (June 2003). "Salamanders ( Plethodon cinereus) go for more: rudiments of number in an amphibian". Animal Cognition. 6 (2): 105–12. doi:10.1007/s10071-003-0167-x. PMID 12709845. S2CID 147018.
  116. ^ Vonk J, Beran MJ (July 2012). "Bears "Count" Too: Quantity Estimation and Comparison in Black Bears (Ursus Americanus)". Animal Behaviour. 84 (1): 231–238. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.001. PMC 3398692. PMID 22822244.
  117. ^ Jaakkola K, Fellner W, Erb L, Rodriguez M, Guarino E (August 2005). "Understanding of the concept of numerically "less" by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 119 (3): 296–303. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.3.296. PMID 16131258.
  118. ^ Pepperberg IM (October 2006). "Grey parrot numerical competence: a review". Animal Cognition. 9 (4): 377–91. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0034-7. PMID 16909236. S2CID 30689821.
  119. ^ Gómez-Laplaza LM, Gerlai R (January 2011). "Can angelfish (Pterophyllum scalare) count? Discrimination between different shoal sizes follows Weber's law". Animal Cognition. 14 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1007/s10071-010-0337-6. PMID 20607574. S2CID 26488837.
  120. ^ King D, Green B (1999). Goannas: The Biology of Varanid Lizards. University of New South Wales Press. p. 43. ISBN 0-86840-456-X.
  121. ^ Reader SM, Hager Y, Laland KN (April 2011). "The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 366 (1567): 1017–27. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0342. PMC 3049098. PMID 21357224.
  122. ^ Locurto C, Durkin E. "Problem-solving and individual differences in mice (Mus musculus) using water reinforcement". J Comp Psychol.
  123. ^ Locurto C, Scanlon C (1998). "Individual differences and a spatial learning factor in two strains of mice (Mus musculus)". J. Comp. Psychol. 112 (4): 344–352. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.112.4.344.
  124. ^ Kamphaus RW (2005). Clinical assessment of child and adolescent intelligence. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN 978-0-387-29149-9.
  125. ^ a b Poirier, Marc-Antoine; Kozlovsky, Dovid Y.; Morand-Ferron, Julie; Careau, Vincent (2020-12-09). "How general is cognitive ability in non-human animals? A meta-analytical and multi-level reanalysis approach". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 287 (1940): 20201853. doi:10.1098/rspb.2020.1853. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 7739923. PMID 33290683.
  126. ^ Rushton JP (2004). "Placing intelligence into an evolutionary framework or how g fits into the r–K matrix of life-history traits including longevity". Intelligence. 32 (4): 321–328. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2004.06.003.
  127. ^ van Schaik CP, Burkart JM (April 2011). "Social learning and evolution: the cultural intelligence hypothesis". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 366 (1567): 1008–16. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0304. PMC 3049085. PMID 21357223.
  128. ^ Herrmann E, Call J, Hernàndez-Lloreda MV, Hare B, Tomasello M (September 2007). "Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: the cultural intelligence hypothesis". Science. 317 (5843): 1360–6. Bibcode:2007Sci...317.1360H. doi:10.1126/science.1146282. PMID 17823346. S2CID 686663.
  129. ^ Plomin R (February 2001). "The genetics of g in human and mouse". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2 (2): 136–41. doi:10.1038/35053584. PMID 11252993. S2CID 205013267.
  130. ^ Anderson B (2000). "The g Factor in Non-Human Animals". In Bock GR, Goode JA, Webb K (eds.). The Nature of Intelligence. Novartis Foundation Symposia. Vol. 233. pp. 79–90, discussion 90–5. doi:10.1002/0470870850.ch5. ISBN 978-0-471-49434-8. PMID 11276911. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  131. ^ Premack D, Woodruff G (1978). "Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind?". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1 (4): 515–526. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00076512.
  132. ^ Bugnyar T, Reber SA, Buckner C (February 2016). "Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors". Nature Communications. 7: 10506. Bibcode:2016NatCo...710506B. doi:10.1038/ncomms10506. PMC 4740864. PMID 26835849.
  133. ^ Bischof-Köhler D (1991). "The development of empathy in infants". In Lamb ME, Keller H (eds.). Infant Development. Perspectives from German speaking countries. Routledge. pp. 245–273. ISBN 978-1-317-72827-6.
  134. ^ Prior H, Schwarz A, Güntürkün O (August 2008). "Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition". PLOS Biology. 6 (8): e202. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060202. PMC 2517622. PMID 18715117.
  135. ^ Gallop GG (January 1970). "Chimpanzees: self-recognition". Science. 167 (3914): 86–7. Bibcode:1970Sci...167...86G. doi:10.1126/science.167.3914.86. PMID 4982211. S2CID 145295899.
  136. ^ Walraven V, van Elsacker L, Verheyen R (1995). "Reactions of a group of pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus) to their mirror images: evidence of self-recognition". Primates. 36: 145–150. doi:10.1007/bf02381922. S2CID 38985498.
  137. ^ Patterson FG, Cohn RH (1994). "Self-recognition and self-awareness in lowland gorillas". In Parker ST, Mitchell R, Boccia M (eds.). Self-awareness in animals and humans: developmental perspectives. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 273–290.
  138. ^ Prior H, Schwarz A, Güntürkün O (August 2008). De Waal F (ed.). "Mirror-induced behavior in the magpie (Pica pica): evidence of self-recognition". PLOS Biology. 6 (8): e202. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060202. PMC 2517622. PMID 18715117.
  139. ^ Marten K, Psarakos S (1995). . In Parker ST, Mitchell R, Boccia M (eds.). Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge University Press. pp. 361–379. Archived from the original on 13 October 2008.
  140. ^ Reiss D, Marino L (May 2001). "Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98 (10): 5937–42. Bibcode:2001PNAS...98.5937R. doi:10.1073/pnas.101086398. PMC 33317. PMID 11331768.
  141. ^ Delfour F, Marten K (April 2001). "Mirror image processing in three marine mammal species: killer whales (Orcinus orca), false killer whales (Pseudorca crassidens) and California sea lions (Zalophus californianus)". Behavioural Processes. 53 (3): 181–190. doi:10.1016/s0376-6357(01)00134-6. PMID 11334706. S2CID 31124804.
  142. ^ Plotnik JM, de Waal FB, Reiss D (November 2006). "Self-recognition in an Asian elephant". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 103 (45): 17053–7. Bibcode:2006PNAS..10317053P. doi:10.1073/pnas.0608062103. PMC 1636577. PMID 17075063.
  143. ^ Lea SE (2010). "Concept learning in nonprimate mammals: In search of evidence". In Mareschal D, Quinn PC, Lea SE (eds.). The Making of Human Concepts. Oxford University Press. pp. 173–199. ISBN 978-0-19-954922-1.
  144. ^ Meng J (2012). "The Superior Human?". Documentary. Transcription on the official website. The Superior Human.
  145. ^ Gatti RC (2015). "Self-consciousness: beyond the looking-glass and what dogs found there". Ethology Ecology & Evolution. 28 (2): 232–240. doi:10.1080/03949370.2015.1102777. S2CID 217507938.
  146. ^ Couchman JJ, Coutinho MV, Beran MJ, Smith JD (November 2010). "Beyond stimulus cues and reinforcement signals: a new approach to animal metacognition" (PDF). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 124 (4): 356–68. doi:10.1037/a0020129. PMC 2991470. PMID 20836592.
  147. ^ "Rats Capable Of Reflecting On Mental Processes". ScienceDaily. 9 March 2007.
  148. ^ Foote AL, Crystal JD (March 2007). "Metacognition in the rat". Current Biology. 17 (6): 551–5. Bibcode:2007CBio...17..551F. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.01.061. PMC 1861845. PMID 17346969.
  149. ^ Smith JD, Beran MJ, Couchman JJ, Coutinho MV (August 2008). "The comparative study of metacognition: sharper paradigms, safer inferences". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 15 (4): 679–91. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.4.679. PMC 4607312. PMID 18792496.
  150. ^ Jozefowiez J, Staddon JE, Cerutti DT (2009). "Metacognition in animals: how do we know that they know?". Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews. 4: 29–39. doi:10.3819/ccbr.2009.40003.
  151. ^ "The Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness" (PDF). Retrieved 12 August 2012.
  152. ^ Breland K, Breland M (1961). "The misbehavior of organisms". American Psychologist. 16 (11): 681–684. doi:10.1037/h0040090. S2CID 51818837.
  153. ^ Brett-Surman MK, Holtz TR, Farlow JO, eds. (2012-06-27). The complete dinosaur. Illustrated by Bob Walters (2nd ed.). Bloomington, Ind.: Indiana University Press. pp. 191–208. ISBN 978-0-253-00849-7.
  154. ^ Moore J (1999). . University of California San Diego. Archived from the original on 2013-08-07. Retrieved 2014-08-18.
  155. ^ Fabre JH (1919). The Hunting Wasps. New York: Dodd, Mead and Company. ISBN 978-1587760280.
  156. ^ Morgan CL (1920). Animal Behaviour (Second ed.). London: Edward Arnold.
  157. ^ Köhler W (1925). Winter E (ed.). The mentality of apes, transl (2nd German ed.). London: Kegan, Trench. ISBN 978-0871401083. Original was Intelligenzprüfungen an Anthropoiden, Berlin 1917. 2nd German edition was titled Intelligenzprüfungen an Menschenaffen, Berlin: Springer 1921.
  158. ^ Povinelli DJ, Eddy TJ (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Vol. 61. pp. 1–189.
  159. ^ Nissani M (2005). "Do Asian elephants apply causal reasoning to tool use tasks? 31: 91–96". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 31: 91–96.
  160. ^ Campbell CB, Hodos W (September 1991). "The Scala naturae revisited: evolutionary scales and anagenesis in comparative psychology". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 105 (3): 211–21. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.105.3.211. PMID 1935002.
  161. ^ Gopnik A (May 2016). "How Animals Think; A new look at what humans can learn from nonhuman minds". The Atlantic. Retrieved 25 April 2016.
  162. ^ (see Shettleworth (1998), Reznikova 2007)).
  163. ^ "Humans are not smarter than animals - we just don't understand them". The Independent. 13 December 2013. Retrieved 9 November 2022.
  164. ^ Bräuer, Juliane; Hanus, Daniel; Pika, Simone; Gray, Russell; Uomini, Natalie (2 July 2020). "Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition: There Is Not "One Cognition"". Journal of Intelligence. 8 (3): 28. doi:10.3390/jintelligence8030028. PMC 7555673. PMID 32630788.
  165. ^ Pinker, Steven (11 May 2010). "The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107 (supplement_2): 8993–8999. doi:10.1073/pnas.0914630107. PMC 3024014. PMID 20445094.
  166. ^ Starr M (2017-12-31). "13 Surprisingly Weird Reasons Why Crows And Ravens Are The Best Birds, No Question". ScienceAlert. Retrieved 2020-04-04.
  167. ^ Mather JA, Kuba MJ (May 2013). "The cephalopod specialties: complex nervous systems, learning and cognition". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 91 (6): 431–449. doi:10.1139/cjz-2013-0009.
  168. ^ Haghighat L (12 April 2012). "Baboons can learn to recognize words; Monkeys' ability suggests that reading taps into general systems of pattern recognition". Nature. doi:10.1038/nature.2012.10432. S2CID 178872255.
  169. ^ Brown E (12 April 2012). "Baboons can recognize written words, study finds; The monkeys don't assign meaning to them, but learn what letter combinations are common to real words, the study authors say". Los Angeles Times.
  170. ^ Bower B (5 May 2012). "Baboons show their word skills; Reading may stem from a visual aptitude shared by all primates". ScienceNews.
  171. ^ Rodríguez, Fernando; Quintero, Blanca; Amores, Lucas; Madrid, David; Salas-Peña, Carmen; Salas, Cosme (August 11, 2021). "Spatial Cognition in Teleost Fish: Strategies and Mechanisms". Animals. 11 (8): 2271. doi:10.3390/ani11082271. ISSN 2076-2615. PMC 8388456. PMID 34438729.
  172. ^ Aellen, Mélisande; Burkart, Judith M.; Bshary, Redouan (March 14, 2022). "No evidence for general intelligence in a fish". Ethology. 128 (5): 424–436. Bibcode:2022Ethol.128..424A. doi:10.1111/eth.13275. ISSN 0179-1613.
  173. ^ Lucon-Xiccato, Tyrone; Gatto, Elia; Bisazza, Angelo (October 13, 2017). "Fish perform like mammals and birds in inhibitory motor control tasks". Scientific Reports. 7 (1): 13144. Bibcode:2017NatSR...713144L. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-13447-4. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 5640690. PMID 29030593.
  174. ^ Akinrinade, Ibukun; Kareklas, Kyriacos; Teles, Magda C.; Reis, Thais K.; Gliksberg, Michael; Petri, Giovanni; Levkowitz, Gil; Oliveira, Rui F. (March 23, 2023). "Evolutionarily conserved role of oxytocin in social fear contagion in zebrafish". Science. 379 (6638): 1232–1237. Bibcode:2023Sci...379.1232A. doi:10.1126/science.abq5158. ISSN 0036-8075.
  175. ^ Lambert, Helen; Carder, Gemma; D'Cruze, Neil (October 17, 2019). "Given the Cold Shoulder: A Review of the Scientific Literature for Evidence of Reptile Sentience". Animals: An Open Access Journal from MDPI. 9 (10): 821. doi:10.3390/ani9100821. ISSN 2076-2615. PMC 6827095. PMID 31627409.

Further reading edit

  • Bateson P (2017). Behaviour, Development and Evolution. Cambridge: Open Book Publishers. doi:10.11647/OBP.0097. ISBN 978-1-78374-248-6.
  • Brown MF, Cook RG, eds. (2006). . Archived from the original on 2022-03-02. Retrieved 2020-09-29.
  • Goodall J (1991). Through a window. London: Penguin.
  • Griffin, Donald R. (2001). Animal minds : beyond cognition to consciousness. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 9780226308654.
  • Hilgard ER (1958). Theories of learning (2nd ed.). London: Methuen.
  • Lurz RW (2009). Mindreading Animals: The Debate over What Animals Know about Other Minds. The MIT Press.
  • Narby, Jeremy (2005). Intelligence in nature : an inquiry into knowledge. New York: Jeremy P. Tarcher/Penguin. ISBN 1585424617.
  • Neisser U (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Romanes GJ (1886). Animal intelligence (4th ed.). London: Kegan Paul, Trench.
  • Shettleworth, Sara J. (2010). Cognition, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 9780195319842.
  • Skinner BF (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • de Waal F (2016). Are We Smart Enough to Know How Smart Animals Are?. W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 978-0393246186.

External links edit

animal, cognition, scientific, journal, animal, cognition, encompasses, mental, capacities, human, animals, including, insect, cognition, study, animal, conditioning, learning, used, this, field, developed, from, comparative, psychology, also, been, strongly, . For the scientific journal see Animal Cognition Animal cognition encompasses the mental capacities of non human animals including insect cognition The study of animal conditioning and learning used in this field was developed from comparative psychology It has also been strongly influenced by research in ethology behavioral ecology and evolutionary psychology the alternative name cognitive ethology is sometimes used Many behaviors associated with the term animal intelligence are also subsumed within animal cognition 1 A crab eating macaque using a stone tool to crack open a nut source source source source source source Experiments like the string pulling task performed here by a Carib grackle provide insights into animal cognition Researchers have examined animal cognition in mammals especially primates cetaceans elephants dogs cats pigs horses 2 3 4 cattle raccoons and rodents birds including parrots fowl corvids and pigeons reptiles lizards snakes and turtles 5 fish and invertebrates including cephalopods spiders and insects 6 Contents 1 Historical background 1 1 Earliest inferences 1 2 Morgan s Canon 1 3 From anecdote to laboratory 1 4 The behavioristic half century 1 5 The cognitive revolution 2 Methods 3 Research questions 3 1 Perception 3 2 Attention 3 2 1 Selective learning 3 2 2 Divided attention 3 2 3 Visual search and attentional priming 3 3 Concepts and categories 3 3 1 Methods 3 3 2 Perceptual categories 3 3 3 Natural categories 3 3 4 Functional or associative categories 3 3 5 Relational or abstract categories 3 3 6 Rule learning 3 4 Memory 3 4 1 Methods 3 4 1 1 Habituation 3 4 1 2 Delayed response 3 4 1 3 Radial arm maze 3 4 1 4 Water maze 3 4 1 5 Novel object recognition test 3 5 Spatial cognition 3 5 1 Long distance navigation homing 3 6 Timing 3 6 1 Time of day circadian rhythms 3 6 2 Interval timing 3 7 Tool and weapon use 3 7 1 Mammals 3 7 2 Birds 3 7 3 Fish 3 7 4 Invertebrates 3 8 Reasoning and problem solving 3 9 Cognitive bias 3 10 Language 3 11 Insight 3 12 Numeracy 3 13 Sapience 3 14 Theory of mind 3 15 Consciousness 4 Biological constraints 5 Experimental evidence against animal cognition 6 Cognitive faculty by species 7 See also 8 References 9 Further reading 10 External linksHistorical background editEarliest inferences edit nbsp A monkey drinking Frooti from a juice box using its handsThe mind and behavior of non human animals has captivated the human imagination for centuries Many writers such as Descartes have speculated about the presence or absence of the animal mind 7 These speculations led to many observations of animal behavior before modern science and testing were available This ultimately resulted in the creation of multiple hypotheses about animal intelligence One of Aesop s Fables was The Crow and the Pitcher in which a crow drops pebbles into a vessel of water until he is able to drink This was a relatively accurate reflection of the capability of corvids to understand water displacement 8 The Roman naturalist Pliny the Elder was the earliest to attest that said story reflects the behavior of real life corvids 9 Aristotle in his biology hypothesized a causal chain where an animal s sense organs transmitted information to an organ capable of making decisions and then to a motor organ Despite Aristotle s cardiocentrism mistaken belief that cognition occurred in the heart this approached some modern understandings of information processing 10 Early inferences were not necessarily precise or accurate Nonetheless interest in animal mental abilities and comparisons to humans increased with early myrmecology the study of ant behavior as well as the classification of humans as primates beginning with Linnaeus Morgan s Canon edit Main article Morgan s Canon Coined by 19th century British psychologist C Lloyd Morgan Morgan s Canon remains a fundamental precept of comparative animal psychology In its developed form it states that 11 In no case is an animal activity to be interpreted in terms of higher psychological processes if it can be fairly interpreted in terms of processes which stand lower in the scale of psychological evolution and development In other words Morgan believed that anthropomorphic approaches to animal behavior were fallacious and that people should only consider behaviour as for example rational purposive or affectionate if there is no other explanation in terms of the behaviours of more primitive life forms to which we do not attribute those faculties From anecdote to laboratory edit See also Comparative psychology Speculation about animal intelligence gradually yielded to scientific study after Darwin placed humans and animals on a continuum although Darwin s largely anecdotal approach to the cognition topic would not pass scientific muster later on 12 This method would be expanded by his protege George J Romanes 13 who played a key role in the defense of Darwinism and its refinement over the years Still Romanes is most famous for two major flaws in his work his focus on anecdotal observations and entrenched anthropomorphism 14 Unsatisfied with the previous approach E L Thorndike brought animal behavior into the laboratory for objective scrutiny Thorndike s careful observations of the escape of cats dogs and chicks from puzzle boxes led him to conclude that what appears to the naive human observer to be intelligent behavior may be strictly attributable to simple associations According to Thorndike using Morgan s Canon the inference of animal reason insight or consciousness is unnecessary and misleading 15 At about the same time I P Pavlov began his seminal studies of conditioned reflexes in dogs Pavlov quickly abandoned attempts to infer canine mental processes such attempts he said led only to disagreement and confusion He was however willing to propose unseen physiological processes that might explain his observations 16 The behavioristic half century edit The work of Thorndike Pavlov and a little later of the outspoken behaviorist John B Watson 17 set the direction of much research on animal behavior for more than half a century During this time there was considerable progress in understanding simple associations notably around 1930 the differences between Thorndike s instrumental or operant conditioning and Pavlov s classical or Pavlovian conditioning were clarified first by Miller and Kanorski and then by B F Skinner 18 19 Many experiments on conditioning followed they generated some complex theories 20 but they made little or no reference to intervening mental processes Probably the most explicit dismissal of the idea that mental processes control behavior was the radical behaviorism of Skinner This view seeks to explain behavior including private events like mental images solely by reference to the environmental contingencies impinging on the human or animal 21 Despite the predominantly behaviorist orientation of research before 1960 the rejection of mental processes in animals was not universal during those years Influential exceptions included for example Wolfgang Kohler and his insightful chimpanzees 22 and Edward Tolman whose proposed cognitive map was a significant contribution to subsequent cognitive research in both humans and animals 23 The cognitive revolution edit Main article Cognitive revolution Beginning around 1960 a cognitive revolution in research on humans 24 gradually spurred a similar transformation of research with animals Inference to processes not directly observable became acceptable and then commonplace An important proponent of this shift in thinking was Donald O Hebb who argued that mind is simply a name for processes in the head that control complex behavior and that it is both necessary and possible to infer those processes from behavior 25 Animals came to be seen as goal seeking agents that acquire store retrieve and internally process information at many levels of cognitive complexity 26 Methods editThe acceleration of research on animal cognition in the last 50 years or so has led to a rapid expansion in the variety of species studied and methods employed The remarkable behavior of large brained animals such as primates and cetacea have claimed special attention but all sorts of animals large and small birds fish ants bees and others have been brought into the laboratory or observed in carefully controlled field studies In the laboratory animals push levers pull strings dig for food swim in water mazes or respond to images on computer screens to get information for discrimination attention memory and categorization experiments 27 Careful field studies explore memory for food caches navigation by stars 28 communication tool use identification of conspecifics and many other matters Studies often focus on the behavior of animals in their natural environments and discuss the putative function of the behavior for the propagation and survival of the species These developments reflect an increased cross fertilization from related fields such as ethology and behavioral ecology Contributions from behavioral neuroscience are beginning to clarify the physiological substrate of some inferred mental process Some researchers have made effective use of a Piagetian methodology taking tasks which human children are known to master at different stages of development and investigating which of them can be performed by particular species Others have been inspired by concerns for animal welfare and the management of domestic species for example Temple Grandin has harnessed her unique expertise in animal welfare and the ethical treatment of farm livestock to highlight underlying similarities between humans and other animals 29 From a methodological point of view one of the main risks in this sort of work is anthropomorphism the tendency to interpret an animal s behavior in terms of human feelings thoughts and motivations 1 Research questions edit nbsp The common chimpanzee can use tools This individual is using a stick to get food Human and non human animal cognition have much in common and this is reflected in the research summarized below most of the headings found here might also appear in an article on human cognition Of course research in the two also differs in important respects Notably much research with humans either studies or involves language and much research with animals is related directly or indirectly to behaviors important to survival in natural settings Following are summaries of some of the major areas of research in animal cognition Perception edit Animals process information from eyes ears and other sensory organs to perceive the environment Perceptual processes have been studied in many species with results that are often similar to those in humans Equally interesting are those perceptual processes that differ from or go beyond those found in humans such as echolocation in bats and dolphins motion detection by skin receptors in fish and extraordinary visual acuity motion sensitivity and ability to see ultraviolet light in some birds 30 Attention edit Much of what is happening in the world at any moment is irrelevant to current behavior Attention refers to mental processes that select relevant information inhibit irrelevant information and switch among these as the situation demands 31 Often the selective process is tuned before relevant information appears such expectation makes for rapid selection of key stimuli when they become available A large body of research has explored the way attention and expectation affect the behavior of non human animals and much of this work suggests that attention operates in birds mammals and reptiles in much the same way that it does in humans 32 Selective learning edit Animals trained to discriminate between two stimuli say black versus white can be said to attend to the brightness dimension but this says little about whether this dimension is selected in preference to others More enlightenment comes from experiments that allow the animal to choose from several alternatives For example several studies have shown that performance is better on for example a color discrimination e g blue vs green after the animal has learned another color discrimination e g red vs orange than it is after training on a different dimension such as an X shape versus an O shape The reverse effect happens after training on forms Thus the earlier learning appears to affect which dimension color or form the animal will attend to 33 Other experiments have shown that after animals have learned to respond to one aspect of the environment responsiveness to other aspects is suppressed In blocking for example an animal is conditioned to respond to one stimulus A by pairing that stimulus with reward or punishment After the animal responds consistently to A a second stimulus B accompanies A on additional training trials Later tests with the B stimulus alone elicit little response suggesting that learning about B has been blocked by prior learning about A 34 This result supports the hypothesis that stimuli are neglected if they fail to provide new information Thus in the experiment just cited the animal failed to attend to B because B added no information to that supplied by A If true this interpretation is an important insight into attentional processing but this conclusion remains uncertain because blocking and several related phenomena can be explained by models of conditioning that do not invoke attention 35 Divided attention edit Attention is a limited resource and is not a none or all response the more attention devoted to one aspect of the environment the less is available for others 36 A number of experiments have studied this in animals In one experiment a tone and a light are presented simultaneously to pigeons The pigeons gain a reward only by choosing the correct combination of the two stimuli e g a high frequency tone together with a yellow light The birds perform well at this task presumably by dividing attention between the two stimuli When only one of the stimuli varies and the other is presented at its rewarded value discrimination improves on the variable stimulus but discrimination on the alternative stimulus worsens 37 These outcomes are consistent with the notion that attention is a limited resource that can be more or less focused among incoming stimuli Visual search and attentional priming edit As noted above the function of attention is to select information that is of special use to the animal Visual search typically calls for this sort of selection and search tasks have been used extensively in both humans and animals to determine the characteristics of attentional selection and the factors that control it Experimental research on visual search in animals was initially prompted by field observations published by Luc Tinbergen 1960 38 Tinbergen observed that birds are selective when foraging for insects For example he found that birds tended to catch the same type of insect repeatedly even though several types were available Tinbergen suggested that this prey selection was caused by an attentional bias that improved detection of one type of insect while suppressing detection of others This attentional priming is commonly said to result from a pretrial activation of a mental representation of the attended object which Tinbergen called a searching image Tinbergen s field observations on priming have been supported by a number of experiments For example Pietrewicz and Kamil 1977 1979 39 40 presented blue jays with pictures of tree trunks upon which rested either a moth of species A a moth of species B or no moth at all The birds were rewarded for pecks at a picture showing a moth Crucially the probability with which a particular species of moth was detected was higher after repeated trials with that species e g A A A than it was after a mixture of trials e g A B B A B A A These results suggest again that sequential encounters with an object can establish an attentional predisposition to see the object Another way to produce attentional priming in search is to provide an advance signal that is associated with the target For example if a person hears a song sparrow he or she may be predisposed to detect a song sparrow in a shrub or among other birds A number of experiments have reproduced this effect in animal subjects 41 42 Still other experiments have explored nature of stimulus factors that affect the speed and accuracy of visual search For example the time taken to find a single target increases as the number of items in the visual field increases This rise in reaction time is steep if the distracters are similar to the target less steep if they are dissimilar and may not occur if the distracters are very different from the target in form or color 43 Concepts and categories edit Fundamental but difficult to define the concept of concept was discussed for hundreds of years by philosophers before it became a focus of psychological study Concepts enable humans and animals to organize the world into functional groups the groups may be composed of perceptually similar objects or events diverse things that have a common function relationships such as same versus different or relations among relations such as analogies 44 Extensive discussions on these matters together with many references may be found in Shettleworth 2010 1 Wasserman and Zentall 2006 27 and in Zentall et al 2008 The latter is freely available online 45 Methods edit Most work on animal concepts has been done with visual stimuli which can easily be constructed and presented in great variety but auditory and other stimuli have been used as well 46 Pigeons have been widely used for they have excellent vision and are readily conditioned to respond to visual targets other birds and a number of other animals have been studied as well 1 In a typical experiment a bird or other animal confronts a computer monitor on which a large number of pictures appear one by one and the subject gets a reward for pecking or touching a picture of a category item and no reward for non category items Alternatively a subject may be offered a choice between two or more pictures Many experiments end with the presentation of items never seen before successful sorting of these items shows that the animal has not simply learned many specific stimulus response associations A related method sometimes used to study relational concepts is matching to sample In this task an animal sees one stimulus and then chooses between two or more alternatives one of which is the same as the first the animal is then rewarded for choosing the matching stimulus 1 27 45 Perceptual categories edit Perceptual categorization is said to occur when a person or animal responds in a similar way to a range of stimuli that share common features For example a squirrel climbs a tree when it sees Rex Shep or Trixie which suggests that it categorizes all three as something to avoid This sorting of instances into groups is crucial to survival Among other things an animal must categorize if it is to apply learning about one object e g Rex bit me to new instances of that category dogs may bite 1 27 45 Natural categories edit Many animals readily classify objects by perceived differences in form or color For example bees or pigeons quickly learn to choose any red object and reject any green object if red leads to reward and green does not Seemingly much more difficult is an animal s ability to categorize natural objects that vary a great deal in color and form even while belonging to the same group In a classic study Richard J Herrnstein trained pigeons to respond to the presence or absence of human beings in photographs 47 The birds readily learned to peck photos that contained partial or full views of humans and to avoid pecking photos with no human despite great differences in the form size and color of both the humans displayed and in the non human pictures In follow up studies pigeons categorized other natural objects e g trees and after training they were able without reward to sort photos they had not seen before 48 49 Similar work has been done with natural auditory categories for example bird songs 50 Honeybees Apis mellifera are able to form concepts of up and down 51 Functional or associative categories edit Perceptually unrelated stimuli may come to be responded to as members of a class if they have a common use or lead to common consequences An oft cited study by Vaughan 1988 provides an example 52 Vaughan divided a large set of unrelated pictures into two arbitrary sets A and B Pigeons got food for pecking at pictures in set A but not for pecks at pictures in set B After they had learned this task fairly well the outcome was reversed items in set B led to food and items in set A did not Then the outcome was reversed again and then again and so on Vaughan found that after 20 or more reversals associating a reward with a few pictures in one set caused the birds to respond to the other pictures in that set without further reward as if they were thinking if these pictures in set A bring food the others in set A must also bring food That is the birds now categorized the pictures in each set as functionally equivalent Several other procedures have yielded similar results 1 45 Relational or abstract categories edit When tested in a simple stimulus matching to sample task described above many animals readily learn specific item combinations such as touch red if the sample is red touch green if the sample is green But this does not demonstrate that they distinguish between same and different as general concepts Better evidence is provided if after training an animal successfully makes a choice that matches a novel sample that it has never seen before Monkeys and chimpanzees do learn to do this as do pigeons if they are given a great deal of practice with many different stimuli However because the sample is presented first successful matching might mean that the animal is simply choosing the most recently seen familiar item rather than the conceptually same item A number of studies have attempted to distinguish these possibilities with mixed results 1 45 Rule learning edit The use of rules has sometimes been considered an ability restricted to humans but a number of experiments have shown evidence of simple rule learning in primates 53 and also in other animals Much of the evidence has come from studies of sequence learning in which the rule consists of the order in which a series of events occurs Rule use is shown if the animal learns to discriminate different orders of events and transfers this discrimination to new events arranged in the same order For example Murphy et al 2008 54 trained rats to discriminate between visual sequences For one group ABA and BAB were rewarded where A bright light and B dim light Other stimulus triplets were not rewarded The rats learned the visual sequence although both bright and dim lights were equally associated with reward More importantly in a second experiment with auditory stimuli rats responded correctly to sequences of novel stimuli that were arranged in the same order as those previously learned Similar sequence learning has been demonstrated in birds and other animals as well 55 Memory edit The categories that have been developed to analyze human memory short term memory long term memory working memory have been applied to the study of animal memory and some of the phenomena characteristic of human short term memory e g the serial position effect have been detected in animals particularly monkeys 56 However most progress has been made in the analysis of spatial memory some of this work has sought to clarify the physiological basis of spatial memory and the role of the hippocampus other work has explored the spatial memory of scatter hoarder animals such as Clark s nutcracker certain jays tits and certain squirrels whose ecological niches require them to remember the locations of thousands of caches 1 57 often following radical changes in the environment Memory has been widely investigated in foraging honeybees Apis mellifera which use both transient short term working memory that is non feeder specific and a feeder specific long term reference memory 58 59 60 Memory induced in a free flying honeybee by a single learning trial lasts for days and by three learning trials for a lifetime 61 Bombus terrestris audax workers vary in their effort investment towards memorising flower locations with smaller workers less able to be selective and thus less interested in which flowers are richer sugar sources 62 63 Meanwhile bigger B t audax workers have more carrying capacity and thus more reason to memorise that information and so they do 62 63 Slugs Limax flavus have a short term memory of approximately 1 min and long term memory of 1 month 64 Methods edit As in humans research with animals distinguishes between working or short term memory from reference or long term memory Tests of working memory evaluate memory for events that happened in the recent past usually within the last few seconds or minutes Tests of reference memory evaluate memory for regularities such as pressing a lever brings food or children give me peanuts Habituation edit Main article Habituation This is one of the simplest tests for memory spanning a short time interval The test compares an animal s response to a stimulus or event on one occasion to its response on a previous occasion If the second response differs consistently from the first the animal must have remembered something about the first unless some other factor such as motivation sensory sensitivity or the test stimulus has changed Delayed response edit Delayed response tasks are often used to study short term memory in animals Introduced by Hunter 1913 a typical delayed response task presents an animal with a stimulus such as colored light and after a short time interval the animal chooses among alternatives that match the stimulus or are related to the stimulus in some other way In Hunter s studies for example a light appeared briefly in one of three goal boxes and then later the animal chose among the boxes finding food behind the one that had been lighted 65 Most research has been done with some variation of the delayed matching to sample task For example in the initial study with this task a pigeon was presented with a flickering or steady light Then a few seconds later two pecking keys were illuminated one with a steady light and one with a flickering light The bird got food if it pecked the key that matched the original stimulus 66 A commonly used variation of the matching to sample task requires the animal to use the initial stimulus to control a later choice between different stimuli For example if the initial stimulus is a black circle the animal learns to choose red after the delay if it is a black square the correct choice is green Ingenious variations of this method have been used to explore many aspects of memory including forgetting due to interference and memory for multiple items 1 Radial arm maze edit Main article Radial arm maze The radial arm maze is used to test memory for spatial location and to determine the mental processes by which location is determined In a radial maze test an animal is placed on a small platform from which paths lead in various directions to goal boxes the animal finds food in one or more goal boxes Having found food in a box the animal must return to the central platform The maze may be used to test both reference and working memory Suppose for example that over a number of sessions the same 4 arms of an 8 arm maze always lead to food If in a later test session the animal goes to a box that has never been baited this indicates a failure of reference memory On the other hand if the animal goes to a box that it has already emptied during the same test session this indicates a failure of working memory Various confounding factors such as odor cues are carefully controlled in such experiments 67 Water maze edit Main article Morris water navigation task The water maze is used to test an animal s memory for spatial location and to discover how an animal is able to determine locations Typically the maze is a circular tank filled with water that has been made milky so that it is opaque Located somewhere in the maze is a small platform placed just below the surface of the water When placed in the tank the animal swims around until it finds and climbs up on the platform With practice the animal finds the platform more and more quickly Reference memory is assessed by removing the platform and observing the relative amount of time the animal spends swimming in the area where the platform had been located Visual and other cues in and around the tank may be varied to assess the animal s reliance on landmarks and the geometric relations among them 68 Novel object recognition test edit The novel object recognition NOR test is an animal behavior test that is primarily used to assess memory alterations in rodents It is a simple behavioral test that is based on a rodents innate exploratory behavior The test is divided into three phases habituation training adaptation and test phase During the habituation phase the animal is placed in an empty test arena This is followed by the adaptation phase where the animal is placed in the arena with two identical objects In the third phase the test phase the animal is placed in the arena with one of the familiar objects from the previous phase and with one novel object Based on the rodents innate curiosity the animals that remember the familiar object will spend more time on investigating the novel object 69 Spatial cognition edit Whether an animal ranges over a territory measured in square kilometers or square meters its survival typically depends on its ability to do such things as find a food source and then return to its nest Sometimes such a task can be performed rather simply for example by following a chemical trail Typically however the animal must somehow acquire and use information about locations directions and distances The following paragraphs outline some of the ways that animals do this 1 70 Beacons Animals often learn what their nest or other goal looks like and if it is within sight they may simply move toward it it is said to serve as a beacon Landmarks When an animal is unable to see its goal it may learn the appearance of nearby objects and use these landmarks as guides Researchers working with birds and bees have demonstrated this by moving prominent objects in the vicinity of nest sites causing returning foragers to hunt for their nest in a new location 1 Dead reckoning also known as path integration is the process of computing one s position by starting from a known location and keeping track of the distances and directions subsequently traveled Classic experiments have shown that the desert ant keeps track of its position in this way as it wanders for many meters searching for food Though it travels in a randomly twisted path it heads straight home when it finds food However if the ant is picked up and released some meters to the east for example it heads for a location displaced by the same amount to the east of its home nest Cognitive maps Some animals appear to construct a cognitive map of their surroundings meaning that they acquire and use information that enables them to compute how far and in what direction to go to get from one location to another Such a map like representation is thought to be used for example when an animal goes directly from one food source to another even though its previous experience has involved only travel between each source and home 1 71 Research in this area 70 has also explored such topics as the use of geometric properties of the environment by rats and pigeons and the ability of rats to represent a spatial pattern in either radial arm mazes or water mazes Spatial cognition is used in visual search when an animal or human searches their environment for specific objects to focus on among other objects in the environment 72 Detour behaviour Some animals appear to have an advanced understanding of their spatial environment and will not take the most direct route if this confers an advantage to them Some jumping spiders take an indirect route to prey rather than the most direct route thereby indicating flexibility in behaviour and route planning and possibly insight learning 73 Long distance navigation homing edit Main article Animal navigation Many animals travel hundreds or thousands of miles in seasonal migrations or returns to breeding grounds They may be guided by the Sun the stars the polarization of light magnetic cues olfactory cues winds or a combination of these 74 This extensive area of research is covered in the main article on Animal navigation It has been hypothesized that animals such as apes and wolves are good at spatial cognition because this skill is necessary for survival Some researchers argue that this ability may have diminished somewhat in dogs because humans have provided necessities such as food and shelter during some 15 000 years of domestication 75 76 77 Timing edit Further information Time perception Time of day circadian rhythms edit Main article Circadian rhythms The behavior of most animals is synchronized with the earth s daily light dark cycle Thus many animals are active during the day others are active at night still others near dawn and dusk Though one might think that these circadian rhythms are controlled simply by the presence or absence of light nearly every animal that has been studied has been shown to have a biological clock that yields cycles of activity even when the animal is in constant illumination or darkness 1 Circadian rhythms are so automatic and fundamental to living things they occur even in plants 78 that they are usually discussed separately from cognitive processes and the reader is referred to the main article Circadian rhythms for further information 79 Interval timing edit Survival often depends on an animal s ability to time intervals For example rufous hummingbirds feed on the nectar of flowers and they often return to the same flower but only after the flower has had enough time to replenish its supply of nectar In one experiment hummingbirds fed on artificial flowers that quickly emptied of nectar but were refilled at some fixed time e g twenty minutes later The birds learned to come back to the flowers at about the right time learning the refill rates of up to eight separate flowers and remembering how long ago they had visited each one 80 The details of interval timing have been studied in a number of species One of the most common methods is the peak procedure In a typical experiment a rat in an operant chamber presses a lever for food A light comes on a lever press brings a food pellet at a fixed later time say 10 seconds and then the light goes off Timing is measured during occasional test trials on which no food is presented and the light stays on On these test trials the rat presses the lever more and more until about 10 sec and then when no food comes gradually stops pressing The time at which the rat presses most on these test trials is taken to be its estimate of the payoff time Experiments using the peak procedure and other methods have shown that animals can time short intervals quite exactly can time more than one event at once and can integrate time with spatial and other cues Such tests have also been used for quantitative tests of theories of animal timing such as Gibbon s Scalar Expectancy Theory SET 81 Killeen s Behavioral Theory of Timing 82 and Machado s Learning to Time model 83 No one theory has yet gained unanimous agreement 1 Tool and weapon use edit Main article Tool use by animals Although tool use was long assumed to be a uniquely human trait there is now much evidence that many animals use tools including mammals birds fish cephalopods and insects Discussions of tool use often involve a debate about what constitutes a tool and they often consider the relation of tool use to the animal s intelligence and brain size Mammals edit Series of photographs showing a bonobo fishing for termites nbsp A bonobo inserting a stick into a termite mound nbsp The bonobo starts fishing for the termites nbsp The bonobo withdraws the stick and begins eating the termites nbsp The bonobo eats the termites extracted with the tool Tool use has been reported many times in both wild and captive primates particularly the great apes The use of tools by primates is varied and includes hunting mammals invertebrates fish collecting honey processing food nuts fruits vegetables and seeds collecting water weapons and shelter Research in 2007 shows that chimpanzees in the Fongoli savannah sharpen sticks to use as spears when hunting considered the first evidence of systematic use of weapons in a species other than humans 84 Other mammals that spontaneously use tools in the wild or in captivity include elephants bears cetaceans sea otters and mongooses Birds edit Main article Bird intelligence Several species of birds have been observed to use tools in the wild including warblers parrots Egyptian vultures brown headed nuthatches gulls and owls Some species such as the woodpecker finch of the Galapagos Islands use particular tools as an essential part of their foraging behavior However these behaviors are often quite inflexible and cannot be applied effectively in new situations A great many species of birds build nests with a wide range of complexities but although nest building behaviour fulfills the criteria of some definitions of tool use this is not the case with other definitions Several species of corvids have been trained to use tools in controlled experiments One species examined extensively under laboratory conditions is the New Caledonian crow One individual called Betty spontaneously made a wire tool to solve a novel problem She was being tested to see whether she would select a wire hook rather than a straight wire to pull a little bucket of meat out of a well Betty tried poking the straight wire at the meat After a series of failures with this direct approach she withdrew the wire and began directing it at the bottom of the well which was secured to its base with duct tape The wire soon became stuck whereupon Betty pulled it sideways bending it and unsticking it She then inserted the hook into the well and extracted the meat In all but one of 10 subsequent trials with only straight wire provided she also made and used a hook in the same manner but not before trying the straight wire first 85 86 Another bird that is highly studied for its intelligence is the African Gray Parrot American animal behaviorist and psychologist Irene Pepperberg vindicated that African Grays possess cognitive abilities Pepperberg used a bird named Alex in her trials and was able to prove that parrots could associate sound and meaning demolishing long held theories that birds were only capable of mimicking human voices Studies by other researchers have determined that African Grays can use deductive reasoning to correctly choose between pairs of boxes containing food and boxes that are empty 87 Until Pepperberg began this research in the 1970s few scientists had studied intelligence in parrots and few do today Most inquiries have instead focused on monkeys chimpanzees gorillas and dolphins all of which are much more difficult to raise feed and handle 88 By the late 1980s Alex had learned the names of more than 50 different objects five shapes and seven colors He d also learned what same and different mean a step so crucial in human intellectual development 89 Fish edit Main article Fish intelligence Several species of wrasses have been observed using rocks as anvils to crack bivalve scallops urchins and clams shells This behavior was first filmed 90 in an orange dotted tuskfish Choerodon anchorago in 2009 by Giacomo Bernardi The fish fans sand to unearth the bivalve takes it into its mouth swims several meters to a rock which it then uses as an anvil by smashing the mollusc apart with sideward thrashes of the head This behaviour has also been recorded in a blackspot tuskfish Choerodon schoenleinii on Australia s Great Barrier Reef yellowhead wrasse Halichoeres garnoti in Florida and a six bar wrasse Thalassoma hardwicke in an aquarium setting These species are at opposite ends of the phylogenetic tree in this family so this behaviour may be a deep seated trait in all wrasses 91 Invertebrates edit Main article Cephalopod intelligence Cephalopods are capable of complex tasks thus earning them the reputation of being among the smartest of invertebrates For example octopuses can open jars to get the contents inside and have remarkable ability to learn new skills from the moment they are born 92 Some cephalopods are known to use coconut shells for protection or camouflage 93 Cephalopod cognitive evolution is hypothesized to have been shaped primarily by predatory and foraging pressures but a challenging mating context may also have played a role 92 Ants of the species Conomyrma bicolor pick up stones and other small objects with their mandibles and drop them down the vertical entrances of rival colonies allowing workers to forage for food without competition 94 Reasoning and problem solving edit It is clear that animals of quite a range of species are capable of solving problems that appear to require abstract reasoning 95 Wolfgang Kohler s 1917 work with chimpanzees is a famous early example He observed that chimpanzees did not use trial and error to solve problems such as retrieving bananas hung out of reach Instead they behaved in a manner that was unwaveringly purposeful spontaneously placing boxes so that they could climb to reach the fruit 22 Modern research has identified similar behavior in animals usually thought of as much less intelligent if appropriate pre training is given 96 Causal reasoning has also been observed in rooks and New Caledonian crows 97 98 It has been shown that Barbados bullfinches Loxigilla barbadensis from urbanized areas are better at innovative problem solving tasks than bullfinches from rural environments but that they did not differ in colour discrimination learning 99 Cognitive bias edit Main article Cognitive bias nbsp Is the glass half empty or half full A cognitive bias refers to a systematic pattern of deviation from norm or rationality in judgment whereby inferences about other individuals or situations may be drawn in an illogical fashion Cognitive bias is sometimes illustrated by using answers to the question Is the glass half empty or half full Choosing half empty is supposed to indicate pessimism whereas choosing half full indicates optimism To test this in animals an individual is trained to anticipate that stimulus A e g a 100 Hz tone precedes a positive event e g highly desired food is delivered when a lever is pressed by the animal The same individual is trained to anticipate that stimulus B e g a 900 Hz tone precedes a negative event e g bland food is delivered when the animal presses a lever The animal is then tested by being given an intermediate stimulus C e g a 500 Hz tone and observing whether the animal presses the lever associated with the positive or negative reward This has been suggested to indicate whether the animal is in a positive or negative mood 100 In a study that used this approach rats that were playfully tickled responded differently than rats that were simply handled The rats that had been tickled were more optimistic than the handled rats 101 The authors suggested that they had demonstrated for the first time a link between the directly measured positive affective state and decision making under uncertainty in an animal model There is some evidence for cognitive bias in a number of species including rats dogs rhesus macaques sheep chicks starlings and honeybees 102 Language edit Main articles Animal language and Human animal communication Further information Talking animal The modeling of human language in animals is known as animal language research In addition to the ape language experiments mentioned above there have also been more or less successful attempts to teach language or language like behavior to some non primate species including parrots and great spotted woodpeckers Arguing from his own results with the animal Nim Chimpsky and his analysis of others results Herbert Terrace criticized the idea that chimps can produce new sentences 103 Shortly thereafter Louis Herman published research on artificial language comprehension in the bottlenosed dolphin Herman Richards amp Wolz 1984 Though this sort of research has been controversial especially among cognitive linguists many researchers agree that many animals can understand the meaning of individual words and that some may understand simple sentences and syntactic variations but there is little evidence that any animal can produce new strings of symbols that correspond to new sentences 1 Insight edit See also Reason Wolfgang Kohler is usually credited with introducing the concept of insight into experimental psychology 86 Working with chimpanzees Kohler came to dispute Edward Thorndike s theory that animals must solve problems gradually by trial and error He said that Thorndike s animals could only use trial and error because the situation precluded other forms of problem solving He provided chimps with a relatively unstructured situation and he observed sudden ah ha insightful changes of behavior as for example when a chimp suddenly moved a box into position so that it could retrieve a banana 104 More recently Asian elephants Elephas maximus were shown to exhibit similar insightful problem solving A male was observed moving a box to a position where it could be stood upon to reach food that had been deliberately hung out of reach 105 Numeracy edit Main article Number sense in animals A variety of studies indicates that animals are able to use and communicate quantitative information and that some can count in a rudimentary way Some examples of this research follow In one study rhesus monkeys viewed visual displays containing for example 1 2 3 or 4 items of different sorts They were trained to respond to them in several ways involving numerical ordering for example touching 1 first 2 second and so on When tested with displays containing items they had never seen before they continued to respond to them in order The authors conclude that monkeys can represent the numerosities 1 to 9 at least on an ordinal scale 106 Ants are able to use quantitative values and transmit this information 107 108 For instance ants of several species are able to estimate quite precisely numbers of encounters with members of other colonies on their feeding territories 109 110 Numeracy has been described in the yellow mealworm beetle Tenebrio molitor 111 and the honeybee 112 Western lowland gorillas given the choice between two food trays demonstrated the ability to choose the tray with more food items at a rate higher than chance after training 113 In a similar task chimpanzees chose the option with the larger amount of food 114 Salamanders given a choice between two displays with differing amounts of fruit flies used as a food reward reliably choose the display with more flies as shown in a particular experiment 115 Other experiments have been conducted that show animals abilities to differentiate between non food quantities American black bears demonstrated quantity differentiation abilities in a task with a computer screen The bears were trained to touch a computer monitor with a paw or nose to choose a quantity of dots in one of two boxes on the screen Each bear was trained with reinforcement to pick a larger or smaller amount During training the bears were rewarded with food for a correct response All bears performed better than what random error predicted on the trials with static non moving dots indicating that they could differentiate between the two quantities The bears choosing correctly in congruent number of dots coincided with area of the dots and incongruent number of dots did not coincide with area of the dots trials suggests that they were indeed choosing between quantities that appeared on the screen not just a larger or smaller retinal image which would indicate they are only judging size 116 Bottlenose dolphins have shown the ability to choose an array with fewer dots compared to one with more dots Experimenters set up two boards showing various numbers of dots in a poolside setup The dolphins were initially trained to choose the board with the fewer number of dots This was done by rewarding the dolphin when it chose the board with the fewer number of dots In the experimental trials two boards were set up and the dolphin would emerge from the water and point to one board The dolphins chose the arrays with fewer dots at a rate much larger than chance indicating they can differentiate between quantities 117 A particular grey parrot after training has shown the ability to differentiate between the numbers zero through six using vocalizations After number and vocalization training this was done by asking the parrot how many objects there were in a display The parrot was able to identify the correct amount at a rate higher than chance 118 Angelfish when put in an unfamiliar environment will group together with conspecifics an action named shoaling Given the choice between two groups of differing size the angelfish will choose the larger of the two groups This can be seen with a discrimination ratio of 2 1 or greater such that as long as one group has at least twice the fish as another group it will join the larger one 119 Monitor lizards have been shown to be capable of numeracy and some species can distinguish among numbers up to six 120 Sapience edit Main article g factor in non humans As the cognitive ability and intelligence in non human animals cannot be measured with verbal scales it has been measured using a variety of methods that involve such things as habit reversal social learning and responses to novelty Principal component analysis and factor analytic studies have shown that a single factor of intelligence is responsible for 47 of the individual variance in cognitive ability measures in primates 121 and between 55 and 60 of the variance in mice 122 123 These values are similar to the accepted variance in IQ explained by a similar single factor known as the general factor of intelligence in humans 40 50 124 However results from a recent meta analysis suggest that the average correlation between performance scores on various cognitive tasks is only 0 18 125 Results from this study suggest that current evidence for general intelligence is weak in non human animals 125 The general factor of intelligence or g factor is a psychometric construct that summarizes the correlations observed between an individual s scores on various measures of cognitive abilities It has been suggested that g is related to evolutionary life histories and the evolution of intelligence 126 as well as to social learning and cultural intelligence 127 128 Non human models of g have been used in genetic 129 and neurological 130 research on intelligence to help understand the mechanisms behind variation in g Theory of mind edit Main article Theory of mind in animals Theory of mind is the ability to attribute mental states e g intents desires pretending knowledge to oneself and others and to understand that others have desires intentions and perspectives that are different from one s own 131 Some research with ravens provides an example of evidence for theory of mind in a non human species Ravens are members of the family Corvidae which is widely regarded as having high cognitive abilities These birds have been observed to hide their food when dominant ravens are visible and audible at the same time Based on this observation ravens were tested for their understanding of seeing as a mental state In a first step the birds protected their cache when dominants were visible but not when they could only be heard from an adjacent room In the next step they had access to a small peephole which allowed them to see into the adjacent room With the peephole open the ravens guarded their caches against discovery when they could hear dominants in the adjacent room even when the dominant s sounds were playbacks of recordings 132 Consciousness edit Main article Animal consciousness nbsp Mirror test with a baboonThe sense in which animals can be said to have self consciousness or a self concept has been hotly debated The best known research technique in this area is the mirror test devised by Gordon G Gallup in which an animal s skin is marked in some way while it is asleep or sedated and it is then allowed to see its reflection in a mirror if the animal spontaneously directs grooming behavior towards the mark that is taken as an indication that it is aware of itself 133 134 Self awareness by this criterion has been reported for chimpanzees 135 136 and also for other great apes 137 the European magpie 138 some cetaceans 139 140 141 and an Asian elephant 142 but not for monkeys The mirror test has been criticized by researchers because it is entirely focused on vision the primary sense in humans while other species rely more heavily on other senses such as the sense of smell in dogs 143 144 145 It has been suggested that metacognition in some animals provides some evidence for cognitive self awareness 146 The great apes dolphins and rhesus monkeys have demonstrated the ability to monitor their own mental states and use an I don t know response to avoid answering difficult questions Unlike the mirror test which reveals awareness of the condition of one s own body this uncertainty monitoring is thought to reveal awareness of one s internal mental state A 2007 study has provided some evidence for metacognition in rats 147 148 although this interpretation has been questioned 149 150 These species might also be aware of the strength of their memories Some researchers propose that animal calls and other vocal behaviors provide evidence of consciousness This idea arose from research on children s crib talk by Weir 1962 and in investigations of early speech in children by Greenfield and others 1976 Some such research has been done with a macaw see Arielle In July 2012 during the Consciousness in Human and Nonhuman Animals conference in Cambridge a group of scientists announced and signed a declaration with the following conclusions Convergent evidence indicates that non human animals have the neuroanatomical neurochemical and neurophysiological substrates of conscious states along with the capacity to exhibit intentional behaviors Consequently the weight of evidence indicates that humans are not unique in possessing the neurological substrates that generate consciousness Non human animals including all mammals and birds and many other creatures including octopuses also possess these neurological substrates 151 Biological constraints editInstinctive drift can influence the interpretation of cognitive research Instinctive drift is the tendency of an animal to revert to instinctive behaviors that can interfere with learned responses The concept originated with Keller and Marian Breland when they taught a raccoon to put coins into a box The raccoon drifted to its instinctive behavior of rubbing the coins with its paws as it would do when foraging for food 152 Animal ability to process and respond to stimuli is correlated with brain size Small brain animals tend to show simple behaviors that are less dependent on learning than those of large brained animals Vertebrates particularly mammals have large brains and complex behavior that changes with experience A formula called the encephalization quotient EQ expresses a relationship between brain and body size it was developed by H J Jerison in the late 1960s 153 When the encephalization quotient is plotted as a curve an animal with an EQ above the curve is expected to show more cognitive ability than the average animal of its size whereas an animal with an EQ below the curve is expected to have less Various formulas been suggested but the equation Ew brain 0 12w body 2 3 has been found to fit data from a sample of mammals 154 The formula is suggestive at best and should only be applied to non mammals with extreme caution For some of the other vertebrate classes the power of 3 4 rather than 2 3 is sometimes used and for many groups of invertebrates the formula may not give meaningful results Experimental evidence against animal cognition editSeveral experiments cannot be readily reconciled with the belief that some animal species are intelligent insightful or possess a theory of mind Jean Henri Fabre 155 1823 1915 setting the stage for all subsequent experiments of this kind argued that insects obey their compelling instinct without realizing what they do For instance to understand that she can grab her paralyzed prey by a leg instead of an antenna is utterly beyond the powers of a sand wasp Her actions are like a series of echoes each awakening the next in a settled order which allows none to sound until the previous one has sounded Fabre s numerous experiments led him in turn to the view that scientists often try to exalt animals instead of objectively studying them C Lloyd Morgan s 156 1852 1936 observations suggested to him that prima facie intelligent behavior in animals is often the result of either instincts or trial and error For instance most visitors watching Morgan s dog smoothly lifting a latch with the back of its head and thereby opening a garden gate and escaping were convinced that the dog s actions involved thinking Morgan however carefully observed the dog s prior random purposeless actions and argued that they involved continued trial and failure until a happy effect is reached rather than methodical planning E L Thorndike 15 1874 1949 placed hungry cats and dogs in enclosures from which they could escape by some simple act such as pulling at a loop of cord Their behavior suggested to him that they did not possess the power of rationality Most books about animal behavior Thorndike wrote do not give us a psychology but rather a eulogy of animals Although Wolfgang Kohler s 157 experiments are often cited as providing support for the animal cognition hypothesis his book is replete with counterexamples For instance he placed chimpanzees in a situation where they could only get bananas by removing a box The chimpanzee Kohler observed has special difficulty in solving such problems he often draws into a situation the strangest and most distant tools and adopts the most peculiar methods rather than remove a simple obstacle which could be displaced with perfect ease Daniel J Povinelli and Timothy Eddy 158 of the University of Louisiana showed that young chimpanzees when given a choice between two food providers were just as likely to beg food from a person who could see the begging gesture as from a person who could not thereby raising the possibility that young chimpanzees do not understand that people see Moty Nissani 159 of Wayne State University trained Burmese logging elephants to lift a lid in order to retrieve food from a bucket The lid was then placed on the ground alongside the bucket where it no longer obstructed access to the food while the treat was simultaneously placed inside the bucket All elephants continued to toss the lid before retrieving the reward thus suggesting that elephants do not grasp simple causal relationships Cognitive faculty by species editA traditionally common image is the scala naturae the ladder of nature on which animals of different species occupy successively higher rungs with humans typically at the top 160 161 However there is some disagreement with the use of such a hierarchy with some critics saying it may be necessary to understand specific cognitive capacities as adaptations to differing ecological niches 162 Some biologists argue that humans are not in fact the smartest animal and that no animal can be characterized as the smartest given that some animals have superior cognitive skills in certain areas 163 164 This contrasts with evolutionary psychologists such as John Tooby who assess based on the large list of related unique characteristics that humans do possess that humans evolved to fill a unique cognitive niche and can fairly be characterized as the smartest animal 165 Whether fairly or not the performance of animals is often compared to that of humans on cognitive tasks Our closest biological relatives the great apes tend to perform most like humans Among the birds corvids and parrots have typically been found to perform well on human like tasks 166 Some octopodes have also been shown to exhibit a number of higher level skills such as tool use 93 but the amount of research on cephalopod intelligence is still limited 167 Baboons have been shown to be capable of recognizing words 168 169 170 The average bird or mammal both usually endotherms have average brain to body ratios ten times larger than a typical ectotherm vertebrate This has contributed to a common perception amongst researchers that mammals and birds share similar advanced cognitive characteristics as humans while other vertebrates such as teleost fishes are more primitive which has led to them being understudied Despite this increasing evidence indicates that fish possess not just capabilities that cannot be explained through Pavlovian and operant conditioning alone such as reversal learning novel obstacle avoidance and passing simultaneous two choice tasks but also even more complex capabilities such as navigational cognitive mapping 171 172 inhibitory motor control 173 and empathy enabled by oxytocin to sense fear in other fish 174 Similarly in reptiles a 2019 review of evidence indicates they can experience numerous emotions such as pleasure and anxiety 175 See also editAnthropomorphism Biosemiotics Cognitive psychology Deception in animals Embodied cognition Evolution of cognition Human animal communication Number sense in animals Pain in invertebrates Cognitive abilities Plant cognition Tool use by animals Uplift science fiction Zoopharmacognosy Zoosemiotics Bird intelligence Cetacean intelligence Dog intelligence Fish intelligenceReferences edit a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Shettleworth SJ 2010 Cognition Evolution and Behavior 2ND ed New York Oxford Press Krueger K Heinze J July 2008 Horse sense social status of horses Equus caballus affects their likelihood of copying other horses behavior PDF Animal Cognition 11 3 431 9 doi 10 1007 s10071 007 0133 0 PMID 18183432 S2CID 16621030 Krueger K Farmer K Heinze J May 2014 The effects of age rank and neophobia on social learning in horses PDF Animal Cognition 17 3 645 55 doi 10 1007 s10071 013 0696 x PMID 24170136 S2CID 13326701 Schuetz A Farmer K Krueger K May 2017 Social learning across species horses Equus caballus learn from humans by observation PDF Animal Cognition 20 3 567 573 doi 10 1007 s10071 016 1060 8 PMID 27866286 S2CID 3957230 Reptiles known as living rocks show surprising cognitive powers Nature 576 7785 10 2019 11 29 Bibcode 2019Natur 576 10 doi 10 1038 d41586 019 03655 5 S2CID 208613023 Shettleworth SJ 2010 Cognition Evolution and Behavior 2ND ed New York Oxford Press Descartes R 1649 Passions of the Soul Crows understand water displacement at the level of a small child Show causal understanding of a 5 to 7 year old child ScienceDaily Retrieved 2019 12 08 Pliny the Elder 1855 The Natural History of Pliny Vol 2 H G Bohn ISBN 9780598910769 Corcilius K Gregoric P 2013 01 01 Aristotle s Model of Animal Motion Phronesis 58 1 52 97 doi 10 1163 15685284 12341242 S2CID 52242579 Morgan CL 1903 An Introduction to Comparative Psychology 2 ed W Scott London pp 59 Darwin C 1871 The descent of man and selection in relation to sex Romanes JG 1883 Animal Intelligence Dewsbury D 1978 Comparative Animal Behavior New York NY McGraw Hill Book Company a b Thorndike EL 1911 Animal Intelligence New York Macmillan Pavlov IP 1928 Lectures on conditioned reflexes Watson JB 1913 Psychology as the Behaviorist Views it Psychological Review 20 2 158 177 doi 10 1037 h0074428 hdl 21 11116 0000 0001 9182 7 S2CID 145372026 Miller S Konorski J 1928 Sur une forme particuliere des reflexes conditionels Comptes Rendus des Seances de la Societe de Biologie et de Ses Filiales 99 1155 1157 Skinner BF 1932 The Behavior of Organisms Hull CL 1943 The Principles of Behavior Skinner BF 1976 About Behaviorism a b Kohler W 1917 The Mentality of Apes Tolman EC 1948 Cognitive maps in rats and men Psychological Review 55 4 189 208 doi 10 1037 h0061626 PMID 18870876 S2CID 42496633 Niesser U 1967 Cognitive Psychology Hebb DO 1958 A Textbook of Psychology p 3 Menzel R Fischer J 2010 Animal Thinking Contemporary Issues in Comparative Cognition p 2 a b c d Wasserman EA Zentall TR eds 2006 Comparative cognition Experimental explorations of animal intelligence USA Oxford University Press p 8 ff Foster JJ Smolka J Nilsson DE Dacke M January 2018 How animals follow the stars Proceedings Biological Sciences 285 1871 20172322 doi 10 1098 rspb 2017 2322 PMC 5805938 PMID 29367394 Grandin T Johnson C January 2010 Animals Make Us Human Creating the Best Life for Animals Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Stebbins WC Berkley MA 1990 Comparative Perception Vol I Basic Mechanisms Vol II Complex Signals New York Wiley Smith EE Kosslyn SM 2007 Cognitive Psychology Mind and Brain Pearson Prentice Hall Blough DS 2006 Reaction time explorations of visual attention perception and decision in pigeons In Wasserman EA Zentall TR eds Comparative Cognition Experimental Explorations of Animal Intelligence New York Oxford University Press pp 89 105 Mackintosh NJ 1983 Conditioning and Associative Learning New York Oxford University Press Kamin LJ 1969 Predictability surprise attention and conditioning In Campbell BA Church RM eds Punishment and Aversive Behavior New York Appleton Century Crofts pp 279 296 Mackintosh NJ 1994 Animal Learning and Cognition San Diego Academic Press Zentall TR April 2005 Selective and divided attention in animals Behavioural Processes 69 1 1 15 doi 10 1016 j beproc 2005 01 004 PMID 15795066 S2CID 24601938 Blough DS October 1969 Attention shifts in a maintained discrimination Science 166 3901 125 6 Bibcode 1969Sci 166 125B doi 10 1126 science 166 3901 125 PMID 5809588 S2CID 33256491 Tinbergen L 1960 The natural control of insects in pine woods I Factors influencing the intensity of predation by songbirds Archives Neerlandaises de Zoologie 13 265 343 doi 10 1163 036551660X00053 Pietrewicz AT Kamil AC February 1977 Visual Detection of Cryptic Prey by Blue Jays Cyanocitta cristata Science 195 4278 580 2 Bibcode 1977Sci 195 580P doi 10 1126 science 195 4278 580 PMID 17732294 S2CID 10858793 Pietrewicz AT Kamil AC June 1979 Search Image Formation in the Blue Jay Cyanocitta cristata Science 204 4399 1332 3 Bibcode 1979Sci 204 1332P doi 10 1126 science 204 4399 1332 PMID 17813172 S2CID 14809014 Blough PM 1989 Attentional priming and visual search in pigeons Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Learning and Cognition 17 4 292 298 doi 10 1037 0097 7403 17 3 292 PMID 2794871 Kamil AC Bond AB 2006 Selective attention priming and foraging behavior In Wasserman EA Zentall TR eds Comparative Cognition Experimental Exploration of Animal Intelligence New York Oxford University Press Blough DS Blough PM 1990 Reaction time assessments of visual processes in pigeons In Berkley M Stebbins W eds Comparative perception New York Wiley pp 245 276 Smith EE Medin DL 1981 Categories and Concepts Harvard Univ Press a b c d e Zentall TR Wasserman EA Lazareva OF Thompson RK Rattermann MJ 2008 Concept Learning in Animals Comparative Cognition amp Behavior Reviews 3 13 45 doi 10 3819 ccbr 2008 30002 Dooling RJ Okanoya K 1995 Psychophysical methods for assessing perceptual categories In Klump GM Dooling RJ Fay RR Stebbins WC eds Methods in Comparative Psychoacoustics Basel Switzerland Birkhauser Verlag pp 307 318 Herrnstein RJ Loveland DH October 1964 Complex Visual Concept in the Pigeon Science 146 3643 549 51 Bibcode 1964Sci 146 549H doi 10 1126 science 146 3643 549 PMID 14190250 S2CID 11940233 Herrnstein RJ 1979 Acquisition Generalization and Discrimination Reversal of a Natural Concept Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 5 2 116 129 doi 10 1037 0097 7403 5 2 116 PMID 528881 Bhatt RS Wasserman EA Reynolds WF Knauss KS July 1988 Conceptual behavior in pigeons Categorization of both familiar and novel examples from four classes of natural and artificial stimuli Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 14 3 219 234 doi 10 1037 0097 7403 14 3 219 Tu HW Smith EW Dooling RJ November 2011 Acoustic and perceptual categories of vocal elements in the warble song of budgerigars Melopsittacus undulatus Journal of Comparative Psychology 125 4 420 30 doi 10 1037 a0024396 PMC 4497543 PMID 22142040 Avargues Weber A Dyer AG Giurfa M March 2011 Conceptualization of above and below relationships by an insect Proceedings Biological Sciences 278 1707 898 905 doi 10 1098 rspb 2010 1891 PMC 3049051 PMID 21068040 Vaughan Jr W 1988 Formation of equivalence sets in pigeons Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 14 36 42 doi 10 1037 0097 7403 14 1 36 D Amato MR Colombo M April 1988 Representation of serial order in monkeys Cebus apella Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 14 2 131 9 doi 10 1037 0097 7403 14 2 131 PMID 3367099 Murphy RA Mondragon E Murphy VA March 2008 Rule learning by rats PDF Science 319 5871 1849 51 Bibcode 2008Sci 319 1849M doi 10 1126 science 1151564 PMID 18369151 S2CID 591112 Kundey SM Strandell B Mathis H Rowan JD 2010 Learning of monotonic and nonmonotonic sequences in domesticated horses Equus callabus and chickens Gallus domesticus Learning and Motivation 14 3 213 223 doi 10 1016 j lmot 2010 04 006 Wright AA Santiago HC Sands SF Kendrick DF Cook RG July 1985 Memory processing of serial lists by pigeons monkeys and people Science 229 4710 287 9 Bibcode 1985Sci 229 287W doi 10 1126 science 9304205 PMID 9304205 Balda R Kamil AC 1992 Long term spatial memory in Clark s nutcracker Nucifraga columbiana Animal Behaviour 44 4 761 769 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 05 80302 1 S2CID 54277040 Greggers U Menzel R 1993 Memory dynamics and foraging strategies of honeybees Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 32 17 29 doi 10 1007 BF00172219 S2CID 36624838 Menzel R 1993 Associative learning in honey bees Apidologie 24 3 157 168 doi 10 1051 apido 19930301 Wustenberg D Gerber B Menzel R August 1998 Short communication long but not medium term retention of olfactory memories in honeybees is impaired by actinomycin D and anisomycin The European Journal of Neuroscience 10 8 2742 5 doi 10 1046 j 1460 9568 1998 00319 x PMID 9767405 S2CID 23691527 Hammer M Menzel R March 1995 Learning and memory in the honeybee The Journal of Neuroscience 15 3 Pt 1 1617 30 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 15 03 01617 1995 PMC 6578179 PMID 7891123 a b University of Exeter 2020 12 28 Big bumblebees learn locations of best flowers Phys org Retrieved 2020 12 29 a b Frasnelli Elisa Robert Theo Chow Pizza Ka Yee Scales Ben Gibson Sam Manning Nicola Philippides Andrew O Collett Thomas S Hempel de Ibarra Natalie 2020 Small and Large Bumblebees Invest Differently when Learning about Flowers Current Biology 31 5 Elsevier BV 1058 1064 e3 doi 10 1016 j cub 2020 11 062 hdl 10871 124274 ISSN 0960 9822 PMID 33373638 Yamada A Sekiguchi T Suzuki H Mizukami A March 1992 Behavioral analysis of internal memory states using cooling induced retrograde amnesia in Limax flavus The Journal of Neuroscience 12 3 729 35 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 12 03 00729 1992 PMC 6576046 PMID 1545237 Hunter WS 1913 The delayed reaction in animals and children Behavior Monographs Vol 2 Blough DS April 1959 Delayed matching in the pigeon Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 2 2 151 60 doi 10 1901 jeab 1959 2 151 PMC 1403892 PMID 13801643 Shettleworth SJ 2010 Cognition Evolution and Behavior 2nd ed New York Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 971781 1 Vorhees CV Williams MT 2006 Morris water maze procedures for assessing spatial and related forms of learning and memory Nature Protocols 1 2 848 58 doi 10 1038 nprot 2006 116 PMC 2895266 PMID 17406317 Antunes M Biala G May 2012 The novel object recognition memory neurobiology test procedure and its modifications Cognitive Processing 13 2 93 110 doi 10 1007 s10339 011 0430 z PMC 3332351 PMID 22160349 a b Brown MF Cook RG eds 2006 Animal Spatial Cognition Comparative Neural and Computational Approaches On line Archived from the original on 2022 03 02 Retrieved 2020 09 29 Lund N 2002 Animal cognition Psychology Press p 4 ISBN 978 0 415 25298 0 Treisman AM Gelade G January 1980 A feature integration theory of attention Cognitive Psychology 12 1 97 136 doi 10 1016 0010 0285 80 90005 5 PMID 7351125 S2CID 353246 Sherwin CM 2001 Can invertebrates suffer Or how robust is argument by analogy Animal Welfare 10 supplement S103 S118 doi 10 1017 S0962728600023551 S2CID 54126137 Gauthreaux SA 1980 Animal Migration Orientation and Navigation Academic Press Savolainen P Zhang YP Luo J Lundeberg J Leitner T November 2002 Genetic evidence for an East Asian origin of domestic dogs Science 298 5598 1610 3 Bibcode 2002Sci 298 1610S doi 10 1126 science 1073906 PMID 12446907 S2CID 32583311 Fiset S Plourde V May 2013 Object permanence in domestic dogs Canis lupus familiaris and gray wolves Canis lupus Journal of Comparative Psychology 127 2 115 27 doi 10 1037 a0030595 PMID 23106804 Brauer J Kaminski J Riedel J Call J Tomasello M February 2006 Making inferences about the location of hidden food social dog causal ape Journal of Comparative Psychology 120 1 38 47 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 120 1 38 PMID 16551163 S2CID 10162449 Webb AR 2003 The physiology of circadian rhythms in plants New Phytologist 160 2 281 303 doi 10 1046 j 1469 8137 2003 00895 x PMID 33832173 S2CID 15688409 Call JE Burghardt GM Pepperberg IM Snowdon CT Zentall TE eds 2017 Chapter 23 Timing in Animals APA handbook of comparative psychology Perception learning and cognition Vol 2 Washington D C APA Henderson J Hurly TA Bateson M Healy SD March 2006 Timing in free living rufous hummingbirds Selasphorus rufus Current Biology 16 5 512 5 Bibcode 2006CBio 16 512H doi 10 1016 j cub 2006 01 054 PMID 16527747 Gibbon J 1977 Scalar expectancy theory and Weber s law in animal timing Psychological Review 84 3 279 325 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 84 3 279 Killeen PR 1991 Behavior s time In Bower G ed The psychology of learning and motivation Vol 27 New York Academic Press pp 294 334 Machado A Pata P 2005 Testing the Scalar Expectancy Theory SET and the Learning to Time model LeT in a double bisection task Animal Learning amp Behavior 33 1 111 122 doi 10 3758 BF03196055 PMID 15971498 S2CID 16623835 Roach J February 22 2007 Chimps Use Spears to Hunt Mammals Study Says National Geographic News Archived from the original on February 24 2007 Retrieved June 12 2010 Hunt GR 1996 Manufacture and use of hook tools by New Caledonian crows Nature 379 6562 249 251 Bibcode 1996Natur 379 249H doi 10 1038 379249a0 S2CID 4352835 a b Shettleworth SJ December 2012 Do animals have insight and what is insight anyway Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 66 4 217 26 doi 10 1037 a0030674 PMID 23231629 Pallardy R May 28 2020 African gray parrot Encyclopedia Britannica Caldwell M January 2000 Polly Wanna PhD Discover 21 Partal Y Animal intelligence The Smartest Animal Species in the World Zoo Portraits Com Scienceblog 28 September 2011 Video shows first tool use by a fish ScienceBlog com Bernardi G 2011 The use of tools by wrasses Labridae Coral Reefs 31 39 doi 10 1007 s00338 011 0823 6 S2CID 37924172 a b Piero Amodio 2020 Bipedal locomotion in Octopus vulgaris A complementary observation and some preliminary considerations Ecology and Evolution 11 9 3679 3684 Bibcode 2021EcoEv 11 3679A doi 10 1002 ece3 7328 PMC 8093653 PMID 33976767 a b Finn JK Tregenza T Norman MD December 2009 Defensive tool use in a coconut carrying octopus Current Biology 19 23 R1069 70 Bibcode 2009CBio 19R1069F doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 10 052 PMID 20064403 S2CID 26835945 Moglich MH Alpert GD 1979 Stone dropping by Conomyrma bicolor Hymenoptera Formicidae A new technique of interference competition Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 2 6 105 113 doi 10 1007 bf00292556 JSTOR 4599265 S2CID 27266459 For chimpanzees see for example Premack D Premack AJ 1983 The Mind of an Ape New York Norton ISBN 978 0 393 30160 1 OCLC 152413818 Pepperberg IM 1999 The Alex Studies Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots Cambridge MA Harvard University Press Tebbich S Seed AM Emery NJ Clayton NS April 2007 Non tool using rooks Corvus frugilegus solve the trap tube problem Animal Cognition 10 2 225 31 doi 10 1007 s10071 006 0061 4 PMID 17171360 S2CID 13611664 Taylor AH Hunt GR Medina FS Gray RD January 2009 Do new caledonian crows solve physical problems through causal reasoning Proceedings Biological Sciences 276 1655 247 54 doi 10 1098 rspb 2008 1107 PMC 2674354 PMID 18796393 Audet JN Ducatez S Lefebvre L 2015 The town bird and the country bird problem solving and immunocompetence vary with urbanization Behavioral Ecology 27 2 637 644 doi 10 1093 beheco arv201 Harding EJ Paul ES Mendl M January 2004 Animal behaviour cognitive bias and affective state Nature 427 6972 312 Bibcode 2004Natur 427 312H doi 10 1038 427312a PMID 14737158 S2CID 4411418 Rygula R Pluta H Popik P 2012 Laughing rats are optimistic PLOS ONE 7 12 e51959 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 751959R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0051959 PMC 3530570 PMID 23300582 Haselton MG Nettle D Andrews PW 2005 The evolution of cognitive bias In Buss DM ed The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology Hoboken NJ US John Wiley amp Sons Inc pp 724 746 Terrace HS Petitto LA Sanders RJ Bever TG November 1979 Can an ape create a sentence Science 206 4421 New York N Y 891 902 Bibcode 1979Sci 206 891T doi 10 1126 science 504995 PMID 504995 Kohler W 1917 Mentality of Apes Foerder P Galloway M Barthel T Moore DE Reiss D 2011 Samuel A ed Insightful problem solving in an Asian elephant PLOS ONE 6 8 e23251 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 623251F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0023251 PMC 3158079 PMID 21876741 Brannon EM Terrace HS January 2000 Representation of the numerosities 1 9 by rhesus macaques Macaca mulatta PDF Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 26 1 31 49 doi 10 1037 0097 7403 26 1 31 PMID 10650542 Reznikova Z Ryabko B 2001 A Study of Ants Numerical Competence Electronic Transactions on Artificial Intelligence 5 111 126 Reznikova ZI 2007 Animal Intelligence From Individual to Social Cognition Cambridge University Press Reznikova ZI 1999 Ethological mechanisms of population dynamic in species ant communities Russian Journal of Ecology 30 3 187 197 Brown MJ Gordon DM 2000 How resources and encounters affect the distribution of foraging activity in a seed harvesting ants Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 47 3 195 203 doi 10 1007 s002650050011 S2CID 15454830 Carazo P Font E Forteza Behrendt E Desfilis E May 2009 Quantity discrimination in Tenebrio molitor evidence of numerosity discrimination in an invertebrate Animal Cognition 12 3 463 70 doi 10 1007 s10071 008 0207 7 PMID 19118405 S2CID 14502342 Dacke M Srinivasan MV October 2008 Evidence for counting in insects Animal Cognition 11 4 683 9 doi 10 1007 s10071 008 0159 y PMID 18504627 S2CID 22273226 Anderson US Stoinski TS Bloomsmith MA Marr MJ Smith AD Maple TL August 2005 Relative numerousness judgment and summation in young and old Western lowland gorillas Journal of Comparative Psychology 119 3 285 95 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 119 3 285 PMID 16131257 Boysen ST Berntson GG Mukobi KL March 2001 Size matters impact of item size and quantity on array choice by chimpanzees Pan troglodytes Journal of Comparative Psychology 115 1 106 10 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 115 1 106 PMID 11334213 Uller C Jaeger R Guidry G Martin C June 2003 Salamanders Plethodon cinereus go for more rudiments of number in an amphibian Animal Cognition 6 2 105 12 doi 10 1007 s10071 003 0167 x PMID 12709845 S2CID 147018 Vonk J Beran MJ July 2012 Bears Count Too Quantity Estimation and Comparison in Black Bears Ursus Americanus Animal Behaviour 84 1 231 238 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2012 05 001 PMC 3398692 PMID 22822244 Jaakkola K Fellner W Erb L Rodriguez M Guarino E August 2005 Understanding of the concept of numerically less by bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus Journal of Comparative Psychology 119 3 296 303 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 119 3 296 PMID 16131258 Pepperberg IM October 2006 Grey parrot numerical competence a review Animal Cognition 9 4 377 91 doi 10 1007 s10071 006 0034 7 PMID 16909236 S2CID 30689821 Gomez Laplaza LM Gerlai R January 2011 Can angelfish Pterophyllum scalare count Discrimination between different shoal sizes follows Weber s law Animal Cognition 14 1 1 9 doi 10 1007 s10071 010 0337 6 PMID 20607574 S2CID 26488837 King D Green B 1999 Goannas The Biology of Varanid Lizards University of New South Wales Press p 43 ISBN 0 86840 456 X Reader SM Hager Y Laland KN April 2011 The evolution of primate general and cultural intelligence Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 366 1567 1017 27 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0342 PMC 3049098 PMID 21357224 Locurto C Durkin E Problem solving and individual differences in mice Mus musculus using water reinforcement J Comp Psychol Locurto C Scanlon C 1998 Individual differences and a spatial learning factor in two strains of mice Mus musculus J Comp Psychol 112 4 344 352 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 112 4 344 Kamphaus RW 2005 Clinical assessment of child and adolescent intelligence Springer Science amp Business Media ISBN 978 0 387 29149 9 a b Poirier Marc Antoine Kozlovsky Dovid Y Morand Ferron Julie Careau Vincent 2020 12 09 How general is cognitive ability in non human animals A meta analytical and multi level reanalysis approach Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 287 1940 20201853 doi 10 1098 rspb 2020 1853 ISSN 0962 8452 PMC 7739923 PMID 33290683 Rushton JP 2004 Placing intelligence into an evolutionary framework or how g fits into the r K matrix of life history traits including longevity Intelligence 32 4 321 328 doi 10 1016 j intell 2004 06 003 van Schaik CP Burkart JM April 2011 Social learning and evolution the cultural intelligence hypothesis Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 366 1567 1008 16 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0304 PMC 3049085 PMID 21357223 Herrmann E Call J Hernandez Lloreda MV Hare B Tomasello M September 2007 Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition the cultural intelligence hypothesis Science 317 5843 1360 6 Bibcode 2007Sci 317 1360H doi 10 1126 science 1146282 PMID 17823346 S2CID 686663 Plomin R February 2001 The genetics of g in human and mouse Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2 2 136 41 doi 10 1038 35053584 PMID 11252993 S2CID 205013267 Anderson B 2000 The g Factor in Non Human Animals In Bock GR Goode JA Webb K eds The Nature of Intelligence Novartis Foundation Symposia Vol 233 pp 79 90 discussion 90 5 doi 10 1002 0470870850 ch5 ISBN 978 0 471 49434 8 PMID 11276911 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a journal ignored help Premack D Woodruff G 1978 Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1 4 515 526 doi 10 1017 S0140525X00076512 Bugnyar T Reber SA Buckner C February 2016 Ravens attribute visual access to unseen competitors Nature Communications 7 10506 Bibcode 2016NatCo 710506B doi 10 1038 ncomms10506 PMC 4740864 PMID 26835849 Bischof Kohler D 1991 The development of empathy in infants In Lamb ME Keller H eds Infant Development Perspectives from German speaking countries Routledge pp 245 273 ISBN 978 1 317 72827 6 Prior H Schwarz A Gunturkun O August 2008 Mirror induced behavior in the magpie Pica pica evidence of self recognition PLOS Biology 6 8 e202 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0060202 PMC 2517622 PMID 18715117 Gallop GG January 1970 Chimpanzees self recognition Science 167 3914 86 7 Bibcode 1970Sci 167 86G doi 10 1126 science 167 3914 86 PMID 4982211 S2CID 145295899 Walraven V van Elsacker L Verheyen R 1995 Reactions of a group of pygmy chimpanzees Pan paniscus to their mirror images evidence of self recognition Primates 36 145 150 doi 10 1007 bf02381922 S2CID 38985498 Patterson FG Cohn RH 1994 Self recognition and self awareness in lowland gorillas In Parker ST Mitchell R Boccia M eds Self awareness in animals and humans developmental perspectives New York Cambridge University Press pp 273 290 Prior H Schwarz A Gunturkun O August 2008 De Waal F ed Mirror induced behavior in the magpie Pica pica evidence of self recognition PLOS Biology 6 8 e202 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0060202 PMC 2517622 PMID 18715117 Marten K Psarakos S 1995 Evidence of self awareness in the bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus In Parker ST Mitchell R Boccia M eds Self awareness in Animals and Humans Developmental Perspectives Cambridge University Press pp 361 379 Archived from the original on 13 October 2008 Reiss D Marino L May 2001 Mirror self recognition in the bottlenose dolphin a case of cognitive convergence Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 10 5937 42 Bibcode 2001PNAS 98 5937R doi 10 1073 pnas 101086398 PMC 33317 PMID 11331768 Delfour F Marten K April 2001 Mirror image processing in three marine mammal species killer whales Orcinus orca false killer whales Pseudorca crassidens and California sea lions Zalophus californianus Behavioural Processes 53 3 181 190 doi 10 1016 s0376 6357 01 00134 6 PMID 11334706 S2CID 31124804 Plotnik JM de Waal FB Reiss D November 2006 Self recognition in an Asian elephant Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103 45 17053 7 Bibcode 2006PNAS 10317053P doi 10 1073 pnas 0608062103 PMC 1636577 PMID 17075063 Lea SE 2010 Concept learning in nonprimate mammals In search of evidence In Mareschal D Quinn PC Lea SE eds The Making of Human Concepts Oxford University Press pp 173 199 ISBN 978 0 19 954922 1 Meng J 2012 The Superior Human Documentary Transcription on the official website The Superior Human Gatti RC 2015 Self consciousness beyond the looking glass and what dogs found there Ethology Ecology amp Evolution 28 2 232 240 doi 10 1080 03949370 2015 1102777 S2CID 217507938 Couchman JJ Coutinho MV Beran MJ Smith JD November 2010 Beyond stimulus cues and reinforcement signals a new approach to animal metacognition PDF Journal of Comparative Psychology 124 4 356 68 doi 10 1037 a0020129 PMC 2991470 PMID 20836592 Rats Capable Of Reflecting On Mental Processes ScienceDaily 9 March 2007 Foote AL Crystal JD March 2007 Metacognition in the rat Current Biology 17 6 551 5 Bibcode 2007CBio 17 551F doi 10 1016 j cub 2007 01 061 PMC 1861845 PMID 17346969 Smith JD Beran MJ Couchman JJ Coutinho MV August 2008 The comparative study of metacognition sharper paradigms safer inferences Psychonomic Bulletin amp Review 15 4 679 91 doi 10 3758 PBR 15 4 679 PMC 4607312 PMID 18792496 Jozefowiez J Staddon JE Cerutti DT 2009 Metacognition in animals how do we know that they know Comparative Cognition amp Behavior Reviews 4 29 39 doi 10 3819 ccbr 2009 40003 The Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness PDF Retrieved 12 August 2012 Breland K Breland M 1961 The misbehavior of organisms American Psychologist 16 11 681 684 doi 10 1037 h0040090 S2CID 51818837 Brett Surman MK Holtz TR Farlow JO eds 2012 06 27 The complete dinosaur Illustrated by Bob Walters 2nd ed Bloomington Ind Indiana University Press pp 191 208 ISBN 978 0 253 00849 7 Moore J 1999 Allometry University of California San Diego Archived from the original on 2013 08 07 Retrieved 2014 08 18 Fabre JH 1919 The Hunting Wasps New York Dodd Mead and Company ISBN 978 1587760280 Morgan CL 1920 Animal Behaviour Second ed London Edward Arnold Kohler W 1925 Winter E ed The mentality of apes transl 2nd German ed London Kegan Trench ISBN 978 0871401083 Original was Intelligenzprufungen an Anthropoiden Berlin 1917 2nd German edition was titled Intelligenzprufungen an Menschenaffen Berlin Springer 1921 Povinelli DJ Eddy TJ 1996 What young chimpanzees know about seeing Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development Vol 61 pp 1 189 Nissani M 2005 Do Asian elephants apply causal reasoning to tool use tasks 31 91 96 Journal of Experimental Psychology Animal Behavior Processes 31 91 96 Campbell CB Hodos W September 1991 The Scala naturae revisited evolutionary scales and anagenesis in comparative psychology Journal of Comparative Psychology 105 3 211 21 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 105 3 211 PMID 1935002 Gopnik A May 2016 How Animals Think A new look at what humans can learn from nonhuman minds The Atlantic Retrieved 25 April 2016 see Shettleworth 1998 Reznikova 2007 Humans are not smarter than animals we just don t understand them The Independent 13 December 2013 Retrieved 9 November 2022 Brauer Juliane Hanus Daniel Pika Simone Gray Russell Uomini Natalie 2 July 2020 Old and New Approaches to Animal Cognition There Is Not One Cognition Journal of Intelligence 8 3 28 doi 10 3390 jintelligence8030028 PMC 7555673 PMID 32630788 Pinker Steven 11 May 2010 The cognitive niche Coevolution of intelligence sociality and language Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 supplement 2 8993 8999 doi 10 1073 pnas 0914630107 PMC 3024014 PMID 20445094 Starr M 2017 12 31 13 Surprisingly Weird Reasons Why Crows And Ravens Are The Best Birds No Question ScienceAlert Retrieved 2020 04 04 Mather JA Kuba MJ May 2013 The cephalopod specialties complex nervous systems learning and cognition Canadian Journal of Zoology 91 6 431 449 doi 10 1139 cjz 2013 0009 Haghighat L 12 April 2012 Baboons can learn to recognize words Monkeys ability suggests that reading taps into general systems of pattern recognition Nature doi 10 1038 nature 2012 10432 S2CID 178872255 Brown E 12 April 2012 Baboons can recognize written words study finds The monkeys don t assign meaning to them but learn what letter combinations are common to real words the study authors say Los Angeles Times Bower B 5 May 2012 Baboons show their word skills Reading may stem from a visual aptitude shared by all primates ScienceNews Rodriguez Fernando Quintero Blanca Amores Lucas Madrid David Salas Pena Carmen Salas Cosme August 11 2021 Spatial Cognition in Teleost Fish Strategies and Mechanisms Animals 11 8 2271 doi 10 3390 ani11082271 ISSN 2076 2615 PMC 8388456 PMID 34438729 Aellen Melisande Burkart Judith M Bshary Redouan March 14 2022 No evidence for general intelligence in a fish Ethology 128 5 424 436 Bibcode 2022Ethol 128 424A doi 10 1111 eth 13275 ISSN 0179 1613 Lucon Xiccato Tyrone Gatto Elia Bisazza Angelo October 13 2017 Fish perform like mammals and birds in inhibitory motor control tasks Scientific Reports 7 1 13144 Bibcode 2017NatSR 713144L doi 10 1038 s41598 017 13447 4 ISSN 2045 2322 PMC 5640690 PMID 29030593 Akinrinade Ibukun Kareklas Kyriacos Teles Magda C Reis Thais K Gliksberg Michael Petri Giovanni Levkowitz Gil Oliveira Rui F March 23 2023 Evolutionarily conserved role of oxytocin in social fear contagion in zebrafish Science 379 6638 1232 1237 Bibcode 2023Sci 379 1232A doi 10 1126 science abq5158 ISSN 0036 8075 Lambert Helen Carder Gemma D Cruze Neil October 17 2019 Given the Cold Shoulder A Review of the Scientific Literature for Evidence of Reptile Sentience Animals An Open Access Journal from MDPI 9 10 821 doi 10 3390 ani9100821 ISSN 2076 2615 PMC 6827095 PMID 31627409 Further reading editBateson P 2017 Behaviour Development and Evolution Cambridge Open Book Publishers doi 10 11647 OBP 0097 ISBN 978 1 78374 248 6 Brown MF Cook RG eds 2006 Animal Spatial Cognition Comparative Neural and Computational Approaches On line Archived from the original on 2022 03 02 Retrieved 2020 09 29 Goodall J 1991 Through a window London Penguin Griffin Donald R 2001 Animal minds beyond cognition to consciousness University of Chicago Press ISBN 9780226308654 Hilgard ER 1958 Theories of learning 2nd ed London Methuen Lurz RW 2009 Mindreading Animals The Debate over What Animals Know about Other Minds The MIT Press Narby Jeremy 2005 Intelligence in nature an inquiry into knowledge New York Jeremy P Tarcher Penguin ISBN 1585424617 Neisser U 1967 Cognitive psychology New York Appleton Century Crofts Romanes GJ 1886 Animal intelligence 4th ed London Kegan Paul Trench Shettleworth Sara J 2010 Cognition evolution and behavior 2nd ed Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 9780195319842 Skinner BF 1969 Contingencies of reinforcement a theoretical analysis New York Appleton Century Crofts de Waal F 2016 Are We Smart Enough to Know How Smart Animals Are W W Norton amp Company ISBN 978 0393246186 External links edit nbsp Wikiquote has quotations related to Animal cognition nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Animal cognition Allen C Animal Consciousness In Zalta EN ed Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Andrews K Animal Cognition In Zalta EN ed Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Fox D 14 June 2011 The limits of intelligence Scientific American 305 1 36 43 Bibcode 2011SciAm 305f 36F doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0711 36 PMID 21717956 Kamil A Bond A Center for Avian Cognition University of Nebraska Animal Cognition Network Archived from the original on 2008 05 09 Animal Minds Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Animal cognition amp oldid 1216203989, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.