fbpx
Wikipedia

Behavioral ecology

Behavioral ecology, also spelled behavioural ecology, is the study of the evolutionary basis for animal behavior due to ecological pressures. Behavioral ecology emerged from ethology after Niko Tinbergen outlined four questions to address when studying animal behaviors: What are the proximate causes, ontogeny, survival value, and phylogeny of a behavior?

Some examples of behavioural ecology
Penguins huddling in the Antarctic
Geese flying in aerodynamic V-formation
The bee waggle dance communicating information
An African elephant crossing a river
A frog with inflated vocal sac
A stotting gazelle
A male moor frog colored blue

If an organism has a trait that provides a selective advantage (i.e., has adaptive significance) in its environment, then natural selection favors it. Adaptive significance refers to the expression of a trait that affects fitness, measured by an individual's reproductive success. Adaptive traits are those that produce more copies of the individual's genes in future generations. Maladaptive traits are those that leave fewer. For example, if a bird that can call more loudly attracts more mates, then a loud call is an adaptive trait for that species because a louder bird mates more frequently than less loud birds—thus sending more loud-calling genes into future generations. Conversely, loud calling birds may attract the attention of predators more often, decreasing their presence in the gene pool.

Individuals are always in competition with others for limited resources, including food, territories, and mates. Conflict occurs between predators and prey, between rivals for mates, between siblings, mates, and even between parents and offspring.

Competing for resources

The value of a social behavior depends in part on the social behavior of an animal's neighbors. For example, the more likely a rival male is to back down from a threat, the more value a male gets out of making the threat. The more likely, however, that a rival will attack if threatened, the less useful it is to threaten other males. When a population exhibits a number of interacting social behaviors such as this, it can evolve a stable pattern of behaviors known as an evolutionarily stable strategy (or ESS). This term, derived from economic game theory, became prominent after John Maynard Smith (1982)[1] recognized the possible application of the concept of a Nash equilibrium to model the evolution of behavioral strategies.

Evolutionarily stable strategy

In short, evolutionary game theory asserts that only strategies that, when common in the population, cannot be "invaded" by any alternative (mutant) strategy is an ESS, and thus maintained in the population. In other words, at equilibrium every player should play the best strategic response to each other. When the game is two player and symmetric, each player should play the strategy that provides the response best for it.

Therefore, the ESS is considered the evolutionary end point subsequent to the interactions. As the fitness conveyed by a strategy is influenced by what other individuals are doing (the relative frequency of each strategy in the population), behavior can be governed not only by optimality but the frequencies of strategies adopted by others and are therefore frequency dependent (frequency dependence).

Behavioral evolution is therefore influenced by both the physical environment and interactions between other individuals.

An example of how changes in geography can make a strategy susceptible to alternative strategies is the parasitization of the African honey bee, A. m. scutellata.

Resource defense

The term economic defendability was first introduced by Jerram Brown in 1964. Economic defendability states that defense of a resource have costs, such as energy expenditure or risk of injury, as well as benefits of priority access to the resource. Territorial behavior arises when benefits are greater than the costs.[2]

Studies of the golden-winged sunbird have validated the concept of economic defendability. Comparing the energetic costs a sunbird expends in a day to the extra nectar gained by defending a territory, researchers showed that birds only became territorial when they were making a net energetic profit.[3] When resources are at low density, the gains from excluding others may not be sufficient to pay for the cost of territorial defense. In contrast, when resource availability is high, there may be so many intruders that the defender would have no time to make use of the resources made available by defense.

Sometimes the economics of resource competition favors shared defense. An example is the feeding territories of the white wagtail. The white wagtails feed on insects washed up by the river onto the bank, which acts as a renewing food supply. If any intruders harvested their territory then the prey would quickly become depleted, but sometimes territory owners tolerate a second bird, known as a satellite. The two sharers would then move out of phase with one another, resulting in decreased feeding rate but also increased defense, illustrating advantages of group living.[4]

Ideal free distribution

One of the major models used to predict the distribution of competing individuals amongst resource patches is the ideal free distribution model. Within this model, resource patches can be of variable quality, and there is no limit to the number of individuals that can occupy and extract resources from a particular patch. Competition within a particular patch means that the benefit each individual receives from exploiting a patch decreases logarithmically with increasing number of competitors sharing that resource patch. The model predicts that individuals will initially flock to higher-quality patches until the costs of crowding bring the benefits of exploiting them in line with the benefits of being the only individual on the lesser-quality resource patch. After this point has been reached, individuals will alternate between exploiting the higher-quality patches and the lower-quality patches in such a way that the average benefit for all individuals in both patches is the same. This model is ideal in that individuals have complete information about the quality of a resource patch and the number of individuals currently exploiting it, and free in that individuals are freely able to choose which resource patch to exploit.[5]

An experiment by Manfred Malinski in 1979 demonstrated that feeding behavior in three-spined sticklebacks follows an ideal free distribution. Six fish were placed in a tank, and food items were dropped into opposite ends of the tank at different rates. The rate of food deposition at one end was set at twice that of the other end, and the fish distributed themselves with four individuals at the faster-depositing end and two individuals at the slower-depositing end. In this way, the average feeding rate was the same for all of the fish in the tank.[6]

Mating strategies and tactics

As with any competition of resources, species across the animal kingdom may also engage in competitions for mating. If one considers mates or potentials mates as a resource, these sexual partners can be randomly distributed amongst resource pools within a given environment. Following the ideal free distribution model, suitors distribute themselves amongst the potential mates in an effort to maximize their chances or the number of potential matings. For all competitors, males of a species in most cases, there are variations in both the strategies and tactics used to obtain matings. Strategies generally refer to the genetically determined behaviors that can be described as conditional. Tactics refer to the subset of behaviors within a given genetic strategy. Thus it is not difficult for a great many variations in mating strategies to exist in a given environment or species.[7]

An experiment conducted by Anthony Arak, where playback of synthetic calls from male natterjack toads was used to manipulate behavior of the males in a chorus, the difference between strategies and tactics is clear. While small and immature, male natterjack toads adopted a satellite tactic to parasitize larger males. Though large males on average still retained greater reproductive success, smaller males were able to intercept matings. When the large males of the chorus were removed, smaller males adopted a calling behavior, no longer competing against the loud calls of larger males. When smaller males got larger and their calls more competitive, then they started calling and competing directly for mates.[8]

Sexual selection

Mate choice by resources

In many sexually reproducing species, such as mammals, birds, and amphibians, females are able to bear offspring for a certain time period, during which the males are free to mate with other available females, and therefore can father many more offspring to pass on their genes. The fundamental difference between male and female reproduction mechanisms determines the different strategies each sex employs to maximize their reproductive success. For males, their reproductive success is limited by access to females, while females are limited by their access to resources. In this sense, females can be much choosier than males because they have to bet on the resources provided by the males to ensure reproductive success.[9]

Resources usually include nest sites, food and protection. In some cases, the males provide all of them (e.g. sedge warblers).[10] The females dwell in their chosen males’ territories for access to these resources. The males gain ownership to the territories through male–male competition that often involves physical aggression. Only the largest and strongest males manage to defend the best quality nest sites. Females choose males by inspecting the quality of different territories or by looking at some male traits that can indicate the quality of resources.[9] One example of this is with the grayling butterfly (Hipparchia semele), where males engage in complex flight patterns to decide who defends a particular territory. The female grayling butterfly chooses a male based on the most optimal location for oviposition.[11] Sometimes, males leave after mating. The only resource that a male provides is a nuptial gift, such as protection or food, as seen in Drosophila subobscura.[12][13] The female can evaluate the quality of the protection or food provided by the male so as to decide whether to mate or not or how long she is willing to copulate.

Mate choice by genes

When males' only contribution to offspring is their sperm, females are particularly choosy. With this high level of female choice, sexual ornaments are seen in males, where the ornaments reflect the male's social status. Two hypotheses have been proposed to conceptualize the genetic benefits from female mate choice.[9]

First, the good genes hypothesis suggests that female choice is for higher genetic quality and that this preference is favored because it increases fitness of the offspring.[14] This includes Zahavi's handicap hypothesis and Hamilton and Zuk's host and parasite arms race. Zahavi's handicap hypothesis was proposed within the context of looking at elaborate male sexual displays. He suggested that females favor ornamented traits because they are handicaps and are indicators of the male's genetic quality. Since these ornamented traits are hazards, the male's survival must be indicative of his high genetic quality in other areas. In this way, the degree that a male expresses his sexual display indicates to the female his genetic quality.[9] Zuk and Hamilton proposed a hypothesis after observing disease as a powerful selective pressure on a rabbit population. They suggested that sexual displays were indicators of resistance of disease on a genetic level.[9]

Such 'choosiness' from the female individuals can be seen in wasp species too, especially among Polistes dominula wasps. The females tend to prefer males with smaller, more elliptically shaped spots than those with larger and more irregularly shaped spots. Those males would have reproductive superiority over males with irregular spots.

In marbled newts, females show preference to mates with larger crests. This however, is not considered a handicap as it does not negatively affect males' chances of survival. It is simply a trait females show preference for when choosing their mate as it is an indication of health and fitness. [15]

Fisher's hypothesis of runaway sexual selection suggests that female preference is genetically correlated with male traits and that the preference co-evolves with the evolution of that trait, thus the preference is under indirect selection.[14] Fisher suggests that female preference began because the trait indicated the male's quality. The female preference spread, so that the females’ offspring now benefited from the higher quality from specific trait but also greater attractiveness to mates. Eventually, the trait only represents attractiveness to mates, and no longer represents increased survival.[9]

An example of mate choice by genes is seen in the cichlid fish Tropheus moorii where males provide no parental care. An experiment found that a female T. moorii is more likely to choose a mate with the same color morph as her own.[16] In another experiment, females have been shown to share preferences for the same males when given two to choose from, meaning some males get to reproduce more often than others.[17]

Sensory bias

The sensory bias hypothesis states that the preference for a trait evolves in a non-mating context, and is then exploited by one sex to obtain more mating opportunities. The competitive sex evolves traits that exploit a pre-existing bias that the choosy sex already possesses. This mechanism is thought to explain remarkable trait differences in closely related species because it produces a divergence in signaling systems, which leads to reproductive isolation.[18]

Sensory bias has been demonstrated in guppies, freshwater fish from Trinidad and Tobago. In this mating system, female guppies prefer to mate with males with more orange body coloration. However, outside of a mating context, both sexes prefer animate orange objects, which suggests that preference originally evolved in another context, like foraging.[19] Orange fruits are a rare treat that fall into streams where the guppies live. The ability to find these fruits quickly is an adaptive quality that has evolved outside of a mating context. Sometime after the affinity for orange objects arose, male guppies exploited this preference by incorporating large orange spots to attract females.

Another example of sensory exploitation is in the water mite Neumania papillator, an ambush predator that hunts copepods (small crustaceans) passing by in the water column.[20] When hunting, N. papillator adopts a characteristic stance termed the 'net stance' - their first four legs are held out into the water column, with their four hind legs resting on aquatic vegetation; this allows them to detect vibrational stimuli produced by swimming prey and use this to orient towards and clutch at prey.[21] During courtship, males actively search for females[22] - if a male finds a female, he slowly circles around the female whilst trembling his first and second leg near her.[20][21] Male leg trembling causes females (who were in the 'net stance') to orient towards often clutch the male.[20] This did not damage the male or deter further courtship; the male then deposited spermatophores and began to vigorously fan and jerk his fourth pair of legs over the spermatophore, generating a current of water that passed over the spermatophores and towards the female.[20] Sperm packet uptake by the female would sometimes follow.[20] Heather Proctor hypothesised that the vibrations trembling male legs made were done to mimic the vibrations that females detect from swimming prey - this would trigger the female prey-detection responses causing females to orient and then clutch at males, mediating courtship.[20][23] If this was true and males were exploiting female predation responses, then hungry females should be more receptive to male trembling – Proctor found that unfed captive females did orient and clutch at males significantly more than fed captive females did, consistent with the sensory exploitation hypothesis.[20]

Other examples for the sensory bias mechanism include traits in auklets,[24] wolf spiders,[25] and manakins.[26] Further experimental work is required to reach a fuller understanding of the prevalence and mechanisms of sensory bias.[27]

Sexual conflict

Sexual conflict, in some form or another, may very well be inherent in the ways most animals reproduce.[28] Females invest more in offspring prior to mating, due to the differences in gametes in species that exhibit anisogamy, and often invest more in offspring after mating.[29] This unequal investment leads, on one hand, to intense competition between males for mates and, on the other hand, to females choosing among males for better access to resources and good genes. Because of differences in mating goals, males and females may have very different preferred outcomes to mating.

Sexual conflict occurs whenever the preferred outcome of mating is different for the male and female. This difference, in theory, should lead to each sex evolving adaptations that bias the outcome of reproduction towards its own interests. This sexual competition leads to sexually antagonistic coevolution between males and females, resulting in what has been described as an evolutionary arms race between males and females.[30][31]

Conflict over mating

Males’ reproductive successes are often limited by access to mates, whereas females’ reproductive successes are more often limited by access to resources. Thus, for a given sexual encounter, it benefits the male to mate, but benefits the female to be choosy and resist.[29] For example, male small tortoiseshell butterfly compete to gain the best territory to mate.[32] Another example of this conflict can be found in the Eastern carpenter bee, Xylocopa virginica. Males of this species are limited in reproduction primarily by access to mates, so they claim a territory and wait for a female to pass through. Big males are, therefore, more successful in mating because they claim territories near the female nesting sites that are more sought after. Smaller males, on the other hand, monopolize less competitive sites in foraging areas so that they may mate with reduced conflict.[33] Another example of this is Sepsis cynipsea, where males of the species mount females to guard them from other males and remain on the female, attempting to copulate, until the female either shakes them off or consents to mating.[34] Similarly the neriid fly Derocephalus angusticollis demonstrates mate guarding by using their long limbs to hold onto the female as well as push other males away during copulation.[35] Extreme manifestations of this conflict are seen throughout nature. For example, the male Panorpa scorpionflies attempt to force copulation. Male scorpionflies usually acquire mates by presenting them with edible nuptial gifts in the forms of salivary secretions or dead insects. However, some males attempt to force copulation by grabbing females with a specialized abdominal organ without offering a gift.[36] Forced copulation is costly to the female as she does not receive the food from the male and has to search for food herself (costing time and energy), while it is beneficial for the male as he does not need to find a nuptial gift.

In other cases, however, it pays for the female to gain more matings and her social mate to prevent these so as to guard paternity. For example, in many socially monogamous birds, males follow females closely during their fertile periods and attempt to chase away any other males to prevent extra-pair matings. The female may attempt to sneak off to achieve these extra matings. In species where males are incapable of constant guarding, the social male may frequently copulate with the female so as to swamp rival males’ sperm.[37]

 
Female red junglefowl in Thailand

Sexual conflict after mating has also been shown to occur in both males and females. Males employ a diverse array of tactics to increase their success in sperm competition. These can include removing other male's sperm from females, displacing other male's sperm by flushing out prior inseminations with large amounts of their own sperm, creating copulatory plugs in females’ reproductive tracts to prevent future matings with other males, spraying females with anti-aphrodisiacs to discourage other males from mating with the female, and producing sterile parasperm to protect fertile eusperm in the female's reproductive tract.[29] For example, the male spruce bud moth (Zeiraphera canadensis) secretes an accessory gland protein during mating that makes them unattractive to other males and thus prevents females from future copulation.[38] The Rocky Mountain parnassian also exhibits this type of sexual conflict when the male butterflies deposit a waxy genital plug onto the tip of the female's abdomen that physically prevents the female from mating again.[39] Males can also prevent future mating by transferring an anti-Aphrodiasic to the female during mating. This behavior is seen in butterfly species such as Heliconius melpomene, where males transfer a compound that causes the female to smell like a male butterfly and thus deter any future potential mates.[40] Furthermore, males may control the strategic allocation of sperm, producing more sperm when females are more promiscuous. All these methods are meant to ensure that females are more likely to produce offspring belonging to the males who uses the method.[29]

Females also control the outcomes of matings, and there exists the possibility that females choose sperm (cryptic female choice).[29] A dramatic example of this is the feral fowl Gallus gallus. In this species, females prefer to copulate with dominant males, but subordinate males can force matings. In these cases, the female is able to eject the subordinate male's sperm using cloacal contractions.[41]

Parental care and family conflicts

Parental care is the investment a parent puts into their offspring—which includes protecting and feeding the young, preparing burrows or nests, and providing eggs with yolk.[42] There is great variation in parental care in the animal kingdom. In some species, the parents may not care for their offspring at all, while in others the parents exhibit single-parental or even bi-parental care. As with other topics in behavioral ecology, interactions within a family involve conflicts. These conflicts can be broken down into three general types: sexual (male–female) conflict, parent–offspring conflict, and sibling conflict.

Types of parental care

There are many different patterns of parental care in the animal kingdom. The patterns can be explained by physiological constraints or ecological conditions, such as mating opportunities. In invertebrates, there is no parental care in most species because it is more favorable for parents to produce a large number of eggs whose fate is left to chance than to protect a few individual young. In other cases, parental care is indirect, manifested via actions taken before the offspring is produced, but nonetheless essential for their survival; for example, female Lasioglossum figueresi sweat bees excavate a nest, construct brood cells, and stock the cells with pollen and nectar before they lay their eggs, so when the larvae hatch they are sheltered and fed, but the females die without ever interacting with their brood.[43] In birds, biparental care is the most common, because reproductive success directly depends on the parents' ability to feed their chicks. Two parents can feed twice as many young, so it is more favorable for birds to have both parents delivering food. In mammals, female-only care is the most common. This is most likely because females are internally fertilized and so are holding the young inside for a prolonged period of gestation, which provides males with the opportunity to desert. Females also feed the young through lactation after birth, so males are not required for feeding. Male parental care is only observed in species where they contribute to feeding or carrying of the young, such as in marmosets.[44] In fish there is no parental care in 79% of bony fish.[45] In fish with parental care, it usually limited to selecting, preparing, and defending a nest, as seen in sockeye salmon, for example.[46] Also, parental care in fish, if any, is primarily done by males, as seen in gobies and redlip blennies.[47][42] The cichlid fish V. moorii exhibits biparental care.[48] In species with internal fertilization, the female is usually the one to take care of the young. In cases where fertilization is external the male becomes the main caretaker.

Familial conflict

Familial conflict is a result of trade-offs as a function of lifetime parental investment. Parental investment was defined by Robert Trivers in 1972 as “any investment by the parent in an individual offspring that increases the offspring's chance of surviving at the cost of the parent’s ability to invest in other offspring”.[citation needed] Parental investment includes behaviors like guarding and feeding. Each parent has a limited amount of parental investment over the course of their lifetime. Investment trade-offs in offspring quality and quantity within a brood and trade offs between current and future broods leads to conflict over how much parental investment to provide and to whom parents should invest in. There are three major types of familial conflict: sexual, parent–offspring, and sibling–sibling conflict.[9]

Sexual conflict

 
Great tit

There is conflict among parents as to who should provide the care as well as how much care to provide. Each parent must decide whether or not to stay and care for their offspring, or to desert their offspring. This decision is best modeled by game theoretic approaches to evolutionarily stable strategies (ESS) where the best strategy for one parent depends on the strategy adopted by the other parent. Recent research has found response matching in parents who determine how much care to invest in their offspring. Studies found that parent great tits match their partner's increased care-giving efforts with increased provisioning rates of their own.[49] This cued parental response is a type of behavioral negotiation between parents that leads to stabilized compensation. Sexual conflicts can give rise to antagonistic co-evolution between the sexes to try to get the other sex to care more for offspring. For example, in the waltzing fly Prochyliza xanthostoma, ejaculate feeding maximizes female reproductive success and minimizes the female's chance of mating multiply.[50] Evidence suggests that the sperm evolved to prevent female waltzing flies from mating multiply in order to ensure the male's paternity.[50]

Parent–offspring conflict

 
Blackbird chicks in a nest

According to Robert Trivers's theory on relatedness,[citation needed] each offspring is related to itself by 1, but is only 0.5 related to their parents and siblings. Genetically, offspring are predisposed to behave in their own self-interest while parents are predisposed to behave equally to all their offspring, including both current and future ones. Offspring selfishly try to take more than their fair shares of parental investment, while parents try to spread out their parental investment equally amongst their present young and future young. There are many examples of parent–offspring conflict in nature. One manifestation of this is asynchronous hatching in birds. A behavioral ecology hypothesis is known as Lack's brood reduction hypothesis (named after David Lack).[citation needed] Lack's hypothesis posits an evolutionary and ecological explanation as to why birds lay a series of eggs with an asynchronous delay leading to nestlings of mixed age and weights. According to Lack, this brood behavior is an ecological insurance that allows the larger birds to survive in poor years and all birds to survive when food is plentiful.[51][52] We also see sex-ratio conflict between the queen and her workers in social hymenoptera. Because of haplodiploidy, the workers (offspring) prefer a 3:1 female to male sex allocation while the queen prefers a 1:1 sex ratio. Both the queen and the workers try to bias the sex ratio in their favor.[53] In some species, the workers gain control of the sex ratio, while in other species, like B. terrestris, the queen has a considerable amount of control over the colony sex ratio.[54] Lastly, there has been recent evidence regarding genomic imprinting that is a result of parent–offspring conflict. Paternal genes in offspring demand more maternal resources than maternal genes in the same offspring and vice versa. This has been show in imprinted genes like insulin-like growth factor-II.[55]

Parent–offspring conflict resolution

Parents need an honest signal from their offspring that indicates their level of hunger or need, so that the parents can distribute resources accordingly. Offspring want more than their fair share of resources, so they exaggerate their signals to wheedle more parental investment. However, this conflict is countered by the cost of excessive begging. Not only does excessive begging attract predators, but it also retards chick growth if begging goes unrewarded.[56] Thus, the cost of increased begging enforces offspring honesty.

Another resolution for parent–offspring conflict is that parental provisioning and offspring demand have actually coevolved, so that there is no obvious underlying conflict. Cross-fostering experiments in great tits (Parus major) have shown that offspring beg more when their biological mothers are more generous.[57] Therefore, it seems that the willingness to invest in offspring is co-adapted to offspring demand.

Sibling–sibling conflict

 
Galápagos fur seals

The lifetime parental investment is the fixed amount of parental resources available for all of a parent's young, and an offspring wants as much of it as possible. Siblings in a brood often compete for parental resources by trying to gain more than their fair share of what their parents can offer. Nature provides numerous examples in which sibling rivalry escalates to such an extreme that one sibling tries to kill off broodmates to maximize parental investment (See Siblicide). In the Galápagos fur seal, the second pup of a female is usually born when the first pup is still suckling. This competition for the mother's milk is especially fierce during periods of food shortage such as an El Niño year, and this usually results in the older pup directly attacking and killing the younger one.[58]

In some bird species, sibling rivalry is also abetted by the asynchronous hatching of eggs. In the blue-footed booby, for example, the first egg in a nest is hatched four days before the second one, resulting in the elder chick having a four-day head start in growth. When the elder chick falls 20-25% below its expected weight threshold, it attacks its younger sibling and drives it from the nest.[59]

Sibling relatedness in a brood also influences the level of sibling–sibling conflict. In a study on passerine birds, it was found that chicks begged more loudly in species with higher levels of extra-pair paternity.[60]

Brood parasitism

 
Adult reed warbler feeding a common cuckoo chick

Some animals deceive other species into providing all parental care. These brood parasites selfishly exploit their hosts' parents and host offspring. The common cuckoo is a well known example of a brood parasite. Female cuckoos lay a single egg in the nest of the host species and when the cuckoo chick hatches, it ejects all the host eggs and young. Other examples of brood parasites include honeyguides, cowbirds, and the large blue butterfly.[61][62][63] Brood parasite offspring have many strategies to induce their host parents to invest parental care. Studies show that the common cuckoo uses vocal mimicry to reproduce the sound of multiple hungry host young to solicit more food.[64] Other cuckoos use visual deception with their wings to exaggerate the begging display. False gapes from brood parasite offspring cause host parents to collect more food.[65] Another example of a brood parasite is Phengaris butterflies such as Phengaris rebeli and Phengaris arion, which differ from the cuckoo in that the butterflies do not oviposit directly in the nest of the host, an ant species Myrmica schencki.[66][67] Rather, the butterfly larvae release chemicals that deceive the ants into believing that they are ant larvae, causing the ants to bring the butterfly larvae back to their own nests to feed them.[66][67] Other examples of brood parasites are Polistes sulcifer, a paper wasp that has lost the ability to build its own nests so females lay their eggs in the nest of a host species, Polistes dominula, and rely on the host workers to take care of their brood,[68] as well as Bombus bohemicus, a bumblebee that relies on host workers of various other Bombus species.[69] Similarly, in Eulaema meriana, some Leucospidae wasps exploit the brood cells and nest for shelter and food from the bees.[70][71] Vespula austriaca is another wasp in which the females force the host workers to feed and take care of the brood.[72] In particular, Bombus hyperboreus, an Arctic bee species, is also classified as a brood parasite in that it attacks and enslaves other species within their subgenus, Alpinobombus to propagate their population.[73]

Mating systems

Various types of mating systems include monogamy, polygyny, polyandry, and promiscuity. Each is differentiated by the sexual behavior between mates, such as which males mate with certain females. An influential paper by Stephen Emlen and Lewis Oring (1977)[74] argued that two main factors of animal behavior influence the diversity of mating systems: the relative accessibility that each sex has to mates, and the parental desertion by either sex.

Mating systems with no male parental care

In a system that does not have male parental care, resource dispersion, predation, and the effects of social living primarily influence female dispersion, which in turn influences male dispersion. Since males' primary concern is female acquisition, the males either indirectly or directly compete for the females. In direct competition, the males are directly focused on the females.[75] Blue-headed wrasse demonstrate the behavior in which females follow resources—such as good nest sites—and males follow the females.[75][76] Conversely, species with males that exemplify indirectly competitive behavior tend towards the males’ anticipation of the resources desired by females and their subsequent effort to control or acquire these resources, which helps them to achieve success with females.[75] Grey-sided voles demonstrate indirect male competition for females. The males were experimentally observed to home in on the sites with the best food in anticipation of females settling in these areas.[75][77] Males of Euglossa imperialis, a non-social bee species, also demonstrate indirect competitive behavior by forming aggregations of territories, which can be considered leks, to defend fragrant-rich primary territories. The purpose of these aggregations is largely only facultative, since the more suitable fragrant-rich sites there are, the more habitable territories there are to inhabit, giving females of this species a large selection of males with whom to potentially mate.[78] Leks and choruses have also been deemed another behavior among the phenomena of male competition for females. Due to the resource-poor nature of the territories that lekking males often defend, it is difficult to categorize them as indirect competitors. For example, the ghost moth males display in leks to attract a female mate. Additionally, it is difficult to classify them as direct competitors seeing as they put a great deal of effort into their defense of their territories before females arrive, and upon female arrival they put for the great mating displays to attract the females to their individual sites. These observations make it difficult to determine whether female or resource dispersion primarily influences male aggregation, especially in lieu of the apparent difficulty that males may have defending resources and females in such densely populated areas.[75] Because the reason for male aggregation into leks is unclear, five hypotheses have been proposed. These postulates propose the following as reasons for male lekking: hotspot, predation reduction, increased female attraction, hotshot males, facilitation of female choice.[75][79] With all of the mating behaviors discussed, the primary factors influencing differences within and between species are ecology, social conflicts, and life history differences.[75]

In some other instances, neither direct nor indirect competition is seen. Instead, in species like the Edith's checkerspot butterfly, males' efforts are directed at acquisition of females and they exhibit indiscriminate mate location behavior, where, given the low cost of mistakes, they blindly attempt to mate both correctly with females and incorrectly with other objects.[80]

Mating systems with male parental care

Monogamy

Monogamy is the mating system in 90% of birds, possibly because each male and female has a greater number of offspring if they share in raising a brood.[81] In obligate monogamy, males feed females on the nest, or share in incubation and chick-feeding. In some species, males and females form lifelong pair bonds. Monogamy may also arise from limited opportunities for polygamy, due to strong competition among males for mates, females suffering from loss of male help, and female–female aggression.[82]

Polygyny

In birds, polygyny occurs when males indirectly monopolize females by controlling resources. In species where males normally do not contribute much to parental care, females suffer relatively little or not at all.[83] In other species, however, females suffer through the loss of male contribution, and the cost of having to share resources that the male controls, such as nest sites or food. In some cases, a polygynous male may control a high-quality territory so for the female, the benefits of polygyny may outweigh the costs.[84]

Polyandry threshold

There also seems to be a “polyandry threshold” where males may do better by agreeing to share a female instead of maintaining a monogamous mating system.[85] Situations that may lead to cooperation among males include when food is scarce, and when there is intense competition for territories or females. For example, male lions sometimes form coalitions to gain control of a pride of females. In some populations of Galapagos hawks, groups of males would cooperate to defend one breeding territory. The males would share matings with the female and share paternity with the offspring.[86]

Female desertion and sex role reversal

In birds, desertion often happens when food is abundant, so the remaining partner is better able to raise the young unaided. Desertion also occurs if there is a great chance of a parent to gain another mate, which depends on environmental and populational factors.[87] Some birds, such as the phalaropes, have reversed sex roles where the female is larger and more brightly colored, and compete for males to incubate their clutches.[88] In jacanas, the female is larger than the male and her territory could overlap the multiple territories of up to four males.[89] In the frog species P. bibronii, the female is fertilizes multiple nests, and the male is left to tend to each nest while the female moves on.

Social behaviors

Animals cooperate with each other to increase their own fitness.[90] These altruistic, and sometimes spiteful behaviors can be explained by Hamilton's rule, which states that rB-C > 0 where r= relatedness, B= benefits, and C= costs.[91]

Kin selection

Kin selection refers to evolutionary strategies where an individual acts to favor the reproductive success of relatives, or kin, even if the action incurs some cost to the organism's own survival and ability to procreate.[90] John Maynard Smith coined the term in 1964,[92] although the concept was referred to by Charles Darwin who cited that helping relatives would be favored by group selection. Mathematical descriptions of kin selection were initially offered by R. A. Fisher in 1930[93] and J. B. S. Haldane in 1932.[94] and 1955.[95] W. D. Hamilton popularized the concept later, including the mathematical treatment by George Price in 1963 and 1964.[96][97]

Kin selection predicts that individuals will harbor personal costs in favor of one or multiple individuals because this can maximize their genetic contribution to future generations. For example, an organism may be inclined to expend great time and energy in parental investment to rear offspring since this future generation may be better suited for propagating genes that are highly shared between the parent and offspring.[90] Ultimately, the initial actor performs apparent altruistic actions for kin to enhance its own reproductive fitness. In particular, organisms are hypothesized to act in favor of kin depending on their genetic relatedness.[96][97] So, individuals are inclined to act altruistically for siblings, grandparents, cousins, and other relatives, but to differing degrees.[90]

Inclusive fitness

Inclusive fitness describes the component of reproductive success in both a focal individual and their relatives.[90] Importantly, the measure embodies the sum of direct and indirect fitness and the change in their reproductive success based on the actor's behavior.[98] That is, the effect an individual's behaviors have on: being personally better-suited to reproduce offspring, and aiding descendant and non-descendant relatives in their reproductive efforts.[90] Natural selection is predicted to push individuals to behave in ways that maximize their inclusive fitness. Studying inclusive fitness is often done using predictions from Hamilton's rule.

Kin recognition

Genetic cues

One possible method of kin selection is based on genetic cues that can be recognized phenotypically.[9] Genetic recognition has been exemplified in a species that is usually not thought of as a social creature: amoebae. Social amoebae form fruiting bodies when starved for food. These amoebae preferentially formed slugs and fruiting bodies with members of their own lineage, which is clonally related.[99] The genetic cue comes from variable lag genes, which are involved in signaling and adhesion between cells.[100]

Kin can also be recognized a genetically determined odor, as studied in the primitively social sweat bee, Lasioglossum zephyrus. These bees can even recognize relatives they have never met and roughly determine relatedness.[101] The Brazilian stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata uses a distinct combination of chemical hydrocarbons to recognize and locate kin. Each chemical odor, emitted from the organism's epicuticles, is unique and varies according to age, sex, location, and hierarchical position.[102] Similarly, individuals of the stingless bee species Trigona fulviventris can distinguish kin from non-kin through recognition of a number of compounds, including hydrocarbons and fatty acids that are present in their wax and floral oils from plants used to construct their nests.[103] In the species, Osmia rufa, kin selection has also been associated with mating selection. Females, specifically, select males for mating with whom they are genetically more related to.[104]

Environmental cues

There are two simple rules that animals follow to determine who is kin. These rules can be exploited, but exist because they are generally successful.

The first rule is ‘treat anyone in my home as kin.’ This rule is readily seen in the reed warbler, a bird species that only focuses on chicks in their own nest. If its own kin is placed outside of the nest, a parent bird ignores that chick. This rule can sometimes lead to odd results, especially if there is a parasitic bird that lays eggs in the reed warbler nest. For example, an adult cuckoo may sneak its egg into the nest. Once the cuckoo hatches, the reed warbler parent feeds the invading bird like its own child. Even with the risk for exploitation, the rule generally proves successful.[9][105]

The second rule, named by Konrad Lorenz as ‘imprinting,’ states that those who you grow up with are kin. Several species exhibit this behavior, including, but not limited to the Belding's ground squirrel.[9] Experimentation with these squirrels showed that regardless of true genetic relatedness, those that were reared together rarely fought. Further research suggests that there is partially some genetic recognition going on as well, as siblings that were raised apart were less aggressive toward one another compared to non-relatives reared apart.[106]

Another way animals may recognize their kin include the interchange of unique signals. While song singing is often considered a sexual trait between males and females, male–male song singing also occurs. For example, male vinegar flies Zaprionus tuberculatus can recognize each other by song.[107]

Cooperation

Cooperation is broadly defined as behavior that provides a benefit to another individual that specifically evolved for that benefit. This excludes behavior that has not been expressly selected for to provide a benefit for another individual, because there are many commensal and parasitic relationships where the behavior one individual (which has evolved to benefit that individual and no others) is taken advantage of by other organisms. Stable cooperative behavior requires that it provide a benefit to both the actor and recipient, though the benefit to the actor can take many different forms.[9]

Within species

Within species cooperation occurs among members of the same species. Examples of intraspecific cooperation include cooperative breeding (such as in weeper capuchins) and cooperative foraging (such as in wolves). There are also forms of cooperative defense mechanisms, such as the "fighting swarm" behavior used by the stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria.[108] Much of this behavior occurs due to kin selection. Kin selection allows cooperative behavior to evolve where the actor receives no direct benefits from the cooperation.[9]

Cooperation (without kin selection) must evolve to provide benefits to both the actor and recipient of the behavior. This includes reciprocity, where the recipient of the cooperative behavior repays the actor at a later time. This may occur in vampire bats but it is uncommon in non-human animals.[109] Cooperation can occur willingly between individuals when both benefit directly as well. Cooperative breeding, where one individual cares for the offspring of another, occurs in several species, including wedge-capped capuchin monkeys.[110]

Cooperative behavior may also be enforced, where their failure to cooperate results in negative consequences. One of the best examples of this is worker policing, which occurs in social insect colonies.[111]

The cooperative pulling paradigm is a popular experimental design used to assess if and under which conditions animals cooperate. It involves two or more animals pulling rewards towards themselves via an apparatus they can not successfully operate alone.[112]

Between species

Cooperation can occur between members of different species. For interspecific cooperation to be evolutionarily stable, it must benefit individuals in both species. Examples include pistol shrimp and goby fish, nitrogen fixing microbes and legumes,[113] ants and aphids.[114] In ants and aphids, aphids secrete a sugary liquid called honeydew, which ants eat. The ants provide protection to the aphids against predators, and, in some instances, raise the aphid eggs and larvae inside the ant colony. This behavior is analogous to human domestication.[114] The genus of goby fish, Elacatinus also demonstrate cooperation by removing and feeding on ectoparasites of their clients.[115] The species of wasp Polybia rejecta and ants Azteca chartifex show a cooperative behavior protecting one another's nests from predators.

Market economics often govern the details of the cooperation: e.g. the amount exchanged between individual animals follow the rules of supply and demand.[116]

Spite

Hamilton's rule can also predict spiteful behaviors between non-relatives.[9] A spiteful behavior is one that is harmful to both the actor and to the recipient. Spiteful behavior is favored if the actor is less related to the recipient than to the average member of the population making r negative and if rB-C is still greater than zero. Spite can also be thought of as a type of altruism because harming a non-relative, by taking his resources for example, could also benefit a relative, by allowing him access to those resources. Furthermore, certain spiteful behaviors may provide harmful short term consequences to the actor but also give long term reproductive benefits.[117] Many behaviors that are commonly thought of as spiteful are actually better explained as being selfish, that is benefiting the actor and harming the recipient, and true spiteful behaviors are rare in the animal kingdom.

An example of spite is the sterile soldiers of the polyembryonic parasitoid wasp. A female wasp lays a male and a female egg in a caterpillar. The eggs divide asexually, creating many genetically identical male and female larvae. Sterile soldier wasps also develop and attack the relatively unrelated brother larvae so that the genetically identical sisters have more access to food.[9]

Another example is bacteria that release bacteriocins.[9] The bacteria that releases the bacteriocin may have to die to do so, but most of the harm is to unrelated individuals who are killed by the bacteriocin. This is because the ability to produce and release the bacteriocin is linked to an immunity to it. Therefore, close relatives to the releasing cell are less likely to die than non-relatives.

Altruism and conflict in social insects

 
Honeypot ant

Many insect species of the order Hymenoptera (bees, ants, wasps) are eusocial. Within the nests or hives of social insects, individuals engage in specialized tasks to ensure the survival of the colony. Dramatic examples of these specializations include changes in body morphology or unique behaviors, such as the engorged bodies of the honeypot ant Myrmecocystus mexicanus or the waggle dance of honey bees and a wasp species, Vespula vulgaris.

In many, but not all social insects, reproduction is monopolized by the queen of the colony. Due to the effects of a haplodiploid mating system, in which unfertilized eggs become male drones and fertilized eggs become worker females, average relatedness values between sister workers can be higher than those seen in humans or other eutherian mammals. This has led to the suggestion that kin selection may be a driving force in the evolution of eusociality, as individuals could provide cooperative care that establishes a favorable benefit to cost ratio (rB-c > 0).[118] However, not all social insects follow this rule. In the social wasp Polistes dominula, 35% of the nest mates are unrelated.[32][59] In many other species, unrelated individuals only help the queen when no other options are present. In this case, subordinates work for unrelated queens even when other options may be present. No other social insect submits to unrelated queens in this way. This seemingly unfavorable behavior parallels some vertebrate systems. It is thought that this unrelated assistance is evidence of altruism in P. dominula.[32]

 
Naked mole-rats

Cooperation in social organisms has numerous ecological factors that can determine the benefits and costs associated with this form of organization. One suggested benefit is a type of "life insurance" for individuals who participate in the care of the young. In this instance, individuals may have a greater likelihood of transmitting genes to the next generation when helping in a group compared to individual reproduction. Another suggested benefit is the possibility of "fortress defense", where soldier castes threaten or attack intruders, thus protecting related individuals inside the territory. Such behaviors are seen in the snapping shrimp Synalpheus regalis and gall-forming aphid Pemphigus spyrothecae.[119][120] A third ecological factor that is posited to promote eusociality is the distribution of resources: when food is sparse and concentrated in patches, eusociality is favored. Evidence supporting this third factor comes from studies of naked mole-rats and Damaraland mole-rats, which have communities containing a single pair of reproductive individuals.[121]

Conflicts in social insects

Although eusociality has been shown to offer many benefits to the colony, there is also potential for conflict. Examples include the sex-ratio conflict and worker policing seen in certain species of social Hymenoptera such as Dolichovespula media, Dolichovespula sylvestris, Dolichovespula norwegica[122] and Vespula vulgaris.[123][124] The queen and the worker wasps either indirectly kill the laying-workers' offspring by neglecting them or directly condemn them by cannibalizing and scavenging.[125]

The sex-ratio conflict arises from a relatedness asymmetry, which is caused by the haplodiploidy nature of Hymenoptera. For instance, workers are most related to each other because they share half of the genes from the queen and inherit all of the father's genes. Their total relatedness to each other would be 0.5+ (0.5 x 0.5) = 0.75. Thus, sisters are three-fourths related to each other. On the other hand, males arise from unfertilized larva, meaning they only inherit half of the queen's genes and none from the father. As a result, a female is related to her brother by 0.25, because 50% of her genes that come from her father have no chance of being shared with a brother. Her relatedness to her brother would therefore be 0.5 x 0.5=0.25.[9]: 382 

According to Trivers and Hare's population-level sex-investment ratio theory, the ratio of relatedness between sexes determines the sex investment ratios.[126] As a result, it has been observed that there is a tug-of-war between the queen and the workers, where the queen would prefer a 1:1 female to male ratio because she is equally related to her sons and daughters (r=0.5 in each case). However, the workers would prefer a 3:1 female to male ratio because they are 0.75 related to each other and only 0.25 related to their brothers.[9]: 382  Allozyme data of a colony may indicate who wins this conflict.[127]

Conflict can also arise between workers in colonies of social insects. In some species, worker females retain their ability to mate and lay eggs. The colony's queen is related to her sons by half of her genes and a quarter to the sons of her worker daughters. Workers, however, are related to their sons by half of their genes and to their brothers by a quarter. Thus, the queen and her worker daughters would compete for reproduction to maximize their own reproductive fitness. Worker reproduction is limited by other workers who are more related to the queen than their sisters, a situation occurring in many polyandrous hymenopteran species. Workers police the egg-laying females by engaging in oophagy or directed acts of aggression.[128][129]

The monogamy hypothesis

The monogamy hypothesis states that the presence of monogamy in insects is crucial for eusociality to occur. This is thought to be true because of Hamilton's rule that states that rB-C>0. By having a monogamous mating system, all of the offspring have high relatedness to each other. This means that it is equally beneficial to help out a sibling, as it is to help out an offspring. If there were many fathers the relatedness of the colony would be lowered.[9]: 371–375 

This monogamous mating system has been observed in insects such as termites, ants, bees and wasps.[9]: 371–375  In termites the queen commits to a single male when founding a nest. In ants, bees and wasps the queens have a functional equivalent to lifetime monogamy. The male can even die before the founding of the colony. The queen can store and use the sperm from a single male throughout their lifetime, sometimes up to 30 years.[9]: 371–375 

In an experiment looking at the mating of 267 hymenopteran species, the results were mapped onto a phylogeny. It was found that monogamy was the ancestral state in all the independent transitions to eusociality. This indicates that monogamy is the ancestral, likely to be crucial state for the development of eusociality. In species where queens mated with multiple mates, it was found that these were developed from lineages where sterile castes already evolved, so the multiple mating was secondary.[130] In these cases, multiple mating is likely to be advantageous for reasons other than those important at the origin of eusociality. Most likely reasons are that a diverse worker pool attained by multiple mating by the queen increases disease resistance and may facilitate a division of labor among workers[9]: 371–375 

Communication and signaling

Communication is varied at all scales of life, from interactions between microscopic organisms to those of large groups of people. Nevertheless, the signals used in communication abide by a fundamental property: they must be a quality of the receiver that can transfer information to a receiver that is capable of interpreting the signal and modifying its behavior accordingly. Signals are distinct from cues in that evolution has selected for signalling between both parties, whereas cues are merely informative to the observer and may not have originally been used for the intended purpose. The natural world is replete with examples of signals, from the luminescent flashes of light from fireflies, to chemical signaling in red harvester ants to prominent mating displays of birds such as the Guianan cock-of-the-rock, which gather in leks, the pheromones released by the corn earworm moth,[131] the dancing patterns of the blue-footed booby, or the alarm sound Synoeca cyanea make by rubbing their mandibles against their nest.[132] Yet other examples are the cases of the grizzled skipper and Spodoptera littoralis where pheromones are released as a sexual recognition mechanism that drives evolution.[133][134] In a type of mating signal, male orb-weaving spiders of the species Zygiella x-notata pluck the signal thread of a female's web with their forelegs. This performance conveys vibratory signals informing the female spider of the male's presence.[135]

The nature of communication poses evolutionary concerns, such as the potential for deceit or manipulation on the part of the sender. In this situation, the receiver must be able to anticipate the interests of the sender and act appropriately to a given signal. Should any side gain advantage in the short term, evolution would select against the signal or the response. The conflict of interests between the sender and the receiver results in an evolutionarily stable state only if both sides can derive an overall benefit.

Although the potential benefits of deceit could be great in terms of mating success, there are several possibilities for how dishonesty is controlled, which include indices, handicaps, and common interests. Indices are reliable indicators of a desirable quality, such as overall health, fertility, or fighting ability of the organism. Handicaps, as the term suggests, place a restrictive cost on the organisms that own them, and thus lower quality competitors experience a greater relative cost compared to their higher quality counterparts. In the common interest situation, it is beneficial to both sender and receiver to communicate honestly such that the benefit of the interaction is maximized.

Signals are often honest, but there are exceptions. Prime examples of dishonest signals include the luminescent lure of the anglerfish, which is used to attract prey, or the mimicry of non-poisonous butterfly species, like the Batesian mimic Papilio polyxenes of the poisonous model Battus philenor.[136] Although evolution should normally favor selection against the dishonest signal, in these cases it appears that the receiver would benefit more on average by accepting the signal.

See also

References

  1. ^ Maynard Smith, J. 1982. Evolution and the Theory of Games.
  2. ^ Brown, Jerram (June 1964). "The evolution of diversity in avian territorial systems". The Wilson Bulletin. 76 (2): 160–169. JSTOR 4159278.
  3. ^ Gill, Frank; Larry Wolf (1975). "Economics of feeding territoriality in the golden-winged sunbird". Ecology. 56 (2): 333–345. doi:10.2307/1934964. JSTOR 1934964.
  4. ^ Davies, N. B.; A. I. Houston (Feb 1981). "Owners and satellites: the economics of territory defence in the pied wagtail, Motacilla alba". Journal of Animal Ecology. 50 (1): 157–180. doi:10.2307/4038. JSTOR 4038.
  5. ^ Fretwell, Stephen D. (1972). Population in a Seasonal Environment. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
  6. ^ Milinski, Manfred (1979). "An Evolutionarily Stable Feeding Strategy in Sticklebacks". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 51 (1): 36–40. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1979.tb00669.x.
  7. ^ Dominey, Wallace (1984). "Alternative Mating Tactics and Evolutionarily Stable Strategies". American Zoology. 24 (2): 385–396. doi:10.1093/icb/24.2.385.
  8. ^ Arak, Anthony (1983). "Sexual selection by male–male competition in natterjack toad choruses". Nature. 306 (5940): 261–262. Bibcode:1983Natur.306..261A. doi:10.1038/306261a0. S2CID 4363873.
  9. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u Nicholas B. Davies; John R. Krebs; Stuart A. West (2012). An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 193–202. ISBN 978-1-4051-1416-5.
  10. ^ Buchanan, K.L.; Catchpole, C.K. (2000). "Song as an indicator of male parental effort in the sedge warbler". Proceedings of the Royal Society. B. 267 (1441): 321–326. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1003. PMC 1690544. PMID 10722211.
  11. ^ van Swaay, C.; Wynhoff, I.; Verovnik, R.; Wiemers, M.; López Munguira, M.; Maes, D.; Sasic, M.; Verstrael, T.; Warren, M.; Settele, J. (2010). "Hipparchia semele". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2010: e.T173254A6980554. doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2010-1.RLTS.T173254A6980554.en. Retrieved 13 November 2021.
  12. ^ Dussourd, D.E.; Harvis, C.A.; Meinwald, J.; Eisner, T. (1991). "Pheromonal advertisement of a nuptial gift by a male moth". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 88 (20): 9224–9227. Bibcode:1991PNAS...88.9224D. doi:10.1073/pnas.88.20.9224. PMC 52686. PMID 1924385.
  13. ^ Steele, RH (1986). "Courtship feeding in Drosophila subobscura. 2. Courtship feeding by males influences female mate choice". Animal Behaviour. 34: 1099–1108. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(86)80169-5. S2CID 53156432.
  14. ^ a b Ryan, Michael J.; Anne Keddy-Hector (March 1992). "Directional patterns of female mate choice and the role of sensory biases". The American Naturalist. 139: S4–S35. doi:10.1086/285303. JSTOR 2462426. S2CID 86383459.
  15. ^ Zuiderwijk, Annie; Sparreboom, Max (1986-01-01). "Territorial Behaviour in Crested Newt Triturus Cristatus and Marbled Newt T. Marmoratus (Amphibia, Urodela)". Bijdragen tot de Dierkunde. 56 (2): 205–213. doi:10.1163/26660644-05602002. ISSN 0067-8546.
  16. ^ Salzburger, Walter; Niederstätter, Harald; Brandstätter, Anita; Berger, Burkhard; Parson, Walther; Snoeks, Jos; Sturmbauer, Christian (2006). "Colour-assortative mating among populations of Tropheus moorii, a cichlid fish from Lake Tanganyika, East Africa". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 273 (1584): 257–66. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3321. PMC 1560039. PMID 16543167.
  17. ^ Steinwender, Bernd; Koblmüller, Stephan; Sefc, Kristina M. (2011). "Concordant female mate preferences in the cichlid fish Tropheus moorii". Hydrobiologia. 682 (1): 121–130. doi:10.1007/s10750-011-0766-5. PMC 3841713. PMID 24293682.
  18. ^ Boughman, J. W. (2002). "How sensory drive can promote speciation". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 17 (12): 571–577. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(02)02595-8.
  19. ^ Rodd, F. H.; Hughes, K. A.; Grether, G. F.; Baril, C. T. (2002). "A possible non-sexual origin of mate preference: are male guppies mimicking fruit?". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 269 (1490): 475–481. doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1891. PMC 1690917. PMID 11886639.
  20. ^ a b c d e f g Proctor, Heather C. (1991-10-01). "Courtship in the water mite Neumania papillator: males capitalize on female adaptations for predation". Animal Behaviour. 42 (4): 589–598. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80242-8. S2CID 53166756.
  21. ^ a b Proctor, Heather C. (1992-10-01). "Sensory exploitation and the evolution of male mating behaviour: a cladistic test using water mites (Acari: Parasitengona)". Animal Behaviour. 44 (4): 745–752. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80300-8. S2CID 54426553.
  22. ^ Proctor, H. C. (1992-01-01). "Effect of Food Deprivation on Mate Searching and Spermatophore Production in Male Water Mites (Acari: Unionicolidae)". Functional Ecology. 6 (6): 661–665. doi:10.2307/2389961. JSTOR 2389961.
  23. ^ Alcock, John (2013-07-01). Animal Behaviour: A Evolutionary Approach (10th ed.). Sinauer. pp. 70–72. ISBN 9780878939664.
  24. ^ Jones, I. L.; Hunter, F. M. (1998). "Heterospecific mating preferences for a feather ornament in least auklets". Behavioral Ecology. 9 (2): 187–192. doi:10.1093/beheco/9.2.187.
  25. ^ McClinktock, W. J.; Uetz, G. W. (1996). "Female choice and pre-existing bias: Visual cues during courtship in two Schizocosawolff spiders". Animal Behaviour. 52: 167–181. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0162. S2CID 24629559.
  26. ^ Prum, R. O. (1996). "Phylogenetic tests of alternative intersexual selection mechanisms: Trait macroevolution in a polygynous clade". The American Naturalist. 149 (4): 688–692. doi:10.1086/286014. JSTOR 2463543. S2CID 85039370.
  27. ^ Fuller, R. C.; Houle, D.; Travis, J. (2005). "Sensory bias as an explanation for the evolution of mate preferences". American Naturalist. 166 (4): 437–446. doi:10.1086/444443. PMID 16224700. S2CID 4849390.
  28. ^ Parker, G. A. (2006). "Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization: An overview". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 361 (1466): 235–259. doi:10.1098/rstb.2005.1785. PMC 1569603. PMID 16612884.
  29. ^ a b c d e Davies N, Krebs J, and West S. (2012). An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology, 4th Ed. Wiley-Blackwell; Oxford: pp. 209–220.[ISBN missing]
  30. ^ Parker, G. (1979). "Sexual selection and sexual conflict." In: Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects (eds. M.S. Blum and N.A. Blum). Academic Press, New York: pp. 123–166.[ISBN missing]
  31. ^ Chapman, T.; et al. (2003). "Sexual Selection". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 18: 41–47. doi:10.1016/s0169-5347(02)00004-6.
  32. ^ a b c Baker, R. R. (1972). "Territorial behaviour of the Nymphalid butterflies, Aglais urticae (L.) and Inachis io (L.)". Journal of Animal Ecology. 41 (2): 453–469. doi:10.2307/3480. JSTOR 3480.
  33. ^ Skandalis, Dimitri A.; Tattersall, Glenn J.; Prager, Sean; Richards, Miriam H. (2009). "Body Size and Shape of the Large Carpenter Bee, Xylocopa virginica (L.) (Hymenoptera: Apidae)". Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society. 82 (1): 30–42. doi:10.2317/JKES711.05.1. S2CID 73520512.
  34. ^ Blanckenhorn, W.U.; Mühlhäuser, C.; Morf, C.; Reusch, T.; Reuter, M. (2000). "Female choice, female reluctance to mate and sexual selection on body size in the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea". Ethology. 106 (7): 577–593. doi:10.1046/j.1439-0310.2000.00573.x.
  35. ^ Bonduriansky, Russell (2006). "Convergent evolution of sexual shape dimorphism in Diptera". Journal of Morphology. 267 (5): 602–611. doi:10.1002/jmor.10426. ISSN 1097-4687. PMID 16477603. S2CID 15548020.
  36. ^ Thornhill, R. (1980). "Rape in Panorpa scorpionflies and a general rape hypothesis". Animal Behaviour. 28: 52–59. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(80)80007-8. S2CID 53167007.
  37. ^ Birkhead, T. and Moller, A. (1992). Sperm Competition in Birds: Evolutionary Causes and Consequences. Academic Press, London.[ISBN missing][page needed]
  38. ^ Carroll, Allan L. (1994). "Interactions between body size and mating history influence the reproductive success of males of a tortricid moth, Zeiraphera canadensis". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 72 (12): 2124–2132. doi:10.1139/z94-284.
  39. ^ Shepard, Jon; Guppy, Crispin (2011). Butterflies of British Columbia: Including Western Alberta, Southern Yukon, the Alaska Panhandle, Washington, Northern Oregon, Northern Idaho, and Northwestern Montana. UBC Press. ISBN 9780774844376. Retrieved 13 November 2017.
  40. ^ Schulz, Stefan; Estrada, Catalina; Yildizhan, Selma; Boppré, Michael; Gilbert, Lawrence E. (2008-01-01). "An antiaphrodisiac in Heliconius melpomene butterflies". Journal of Chemical Ecology. 34 (1): 82–93. doi:10.1007/s10886-007-9393-z. ISSN 0098-0331. PMID 18080165. S2CID 22090974.
  41. ^ Pizzari, T.; Birkhead, T. (2000). "Female feral fowl eject sperm of subdominant males". Nature. 405 (6788): 787–789. Bibcode:2000Natur.405..787P. doi:10.1038/35015558. PMID 10866198. S2CID 4406792.
  42. ^ a b Clutton-Brock, T.H. (1991). The Evolution of Parental Care. Princeton NJ: Princeton University Press.
  43. ^ Wcislo, W. T.; Wille, A.; Orozco, E. (1993). "Nesting biology of tropical solitary and social sweat bees, Lasioglossum (Dialictus) figueresi Wcislo and L. (D.) aeneiventre (Friese) (Hymenoptera: Halictidae)". Insectes Sociaux. 40: 21–40. doi:10.1007/BF01338830. S2CID 6867760.
  44. ^ Daly, M. (1979). "Why Don't Male Mammals Lactate?". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 78 (3): 325–345. Bibcode:1979JThBi..78..325D. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(79)90334-5. PMID 513786.
  45. ^ Gross, M.R.; R.C Sargent (1985). "The evolution of male and female parentental care in fishes". American Zoologist. 25 (3): 807–822. doi:10.1093/icb/25.3.807.
  46. ^ Foote, Chris J; Brown, Gayle S; Hawryshyn, Craig W (1 January 2004). "Female colour and male choice in sockeye salmon: implications for the phenotypic convergence of anadromous and nonanadromous morphs". Animal Behaviour. 67 (1): 69–83. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.02.004. S2CID 53169458.
  47. ^ Svensson; Magnhagen, C. (Jul 1998). "Parental behavior in relation to the occurrence of sneaking in the common goby". Animal Behaviour. 56 (1): 175–179. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0769. PMID 9710475. S2CID 24806138.
  48. ^ Sturmbauer, Christian; Corinna Fuchs; Georg Harb; Elisabeth Damm; Nina Duftner; Michaela Maderbacher; Martin Koch; Stephan Koblmüller (2008). "Abundance, Distribution, and Territory Areas of Rock-dwelling Lake Tanganyika Cichlid Fish Species". Hydrobiologia. 615 (1): 57–68. doi:10.1007/s10750-008-9557-z. S2CID 23056207. Retrieved 30 September 2013.
  49. ^ Johnstone, R.A.; Hinde, C.A. (2006). "Negotiation over offspring care--how should parents respond to each other's efforts?". Behavioral Ecology. 17 (5): 818–827. doi:10.1093/beheco/arl009.
  50. ^ a b Bonduriansky, Russell; Wheeler, Jill; Rowe, Locke (2005-02-01). "Ejaculate feeding and female fitness in the sexually dimorphic fly Prochyliza xanthostoma (Diptera: Piophilidae)". Animal Behaviour. 69 (2): 489–497. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.03.018. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 16357692.
  51. ^ Amundsen, T.; Slagsvold, T. (1996). "Lack's Brood Reduction Hypothesis and Avian Hatching Asynchrony: What's Next?". Oikos. 76 (3): 613–620. doi:10.2307/3546359. JSTOR 3546359.
  52. ^ Pijanowski, B. C. (1992). "A Revision of Lack's Brood Reduction Hypothesis". The American Naturalist. 139 (6): 1270–1292. doi:10.1086/285386. S2CID 84884060.
  53. ^ Trivers, Robert L.; Willard, Dan E. (1976). "Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring". Science. 179 (191): 90–92. Bibcode:1973Sci...179...90T. doi:10.1126/science.179.4068.90. PMID 4682135. S2CID 29326420.
  54. ^ Bourke, A.F.G. & F.L.W. Ratnieks (2001). "Kin-selected conflict in the bumble-bee Bombus terrestris (Hymenoptera: Apidae)". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B. 268 (1465): 347–355. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1381. PMC 1088613. PMID 11270430.
  55. ^ Haig, D.; Graham, C. (1991). "Genomic imprinting and the strange case of the insulin-like growth factor-II receptor". Cell. 64 (6): 1045–1046. doi:10.1016/0092-8674(91)90256-x. PMID 1848481. S2CID 33682126.
  56. ^ Kilner, R. M. (2001). "A Growth Cost of Begging in Captive Canary Chicks". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 98 (20): 11394–11398. Bibcode:2001PNAS...9811394K. doi:10.1073/pnas.191221798. PMC 58740. PMID 11572988.
  57. ^ Kolliker, M.; Brinkhof, M.; Heeb, P.; Fitze, P.; Richner, H. (2000). "The Quantitative Genetic Basis of Offspring Solicitation and Parental Response in a Passerine Bird with Parental Care". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 267 (1457): 2127–2132. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1259. PMC 1690782. PMID 11416919.
  58. ^ Trillmitch, F.; Wolf, J.B.W. (2008). "Parent–offspring and sibling conflict in Galapagos fur seals and sea lions". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 62 (3): 363–375. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0423-1. S2CID 41834534.
  59. ^ a b Drummond, H.; Chavelas, C.G. (1989). "Food shortage influences sibling sggression in the Blue-footed Booby". Animal Behaviour. 37: 806–819. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(89)90065-1. S2CID 53165189.
  60. ^ Briskie, James V.; Naugler, Christopher T.; Leech, Susan M. (1994). "Begging intensity of nestling birds varies with sibling relatedness". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 258 (1351): 73–78. Bibcode:1994RSPSB.258...73B. doi:10.1098/rspb.1994.0144. S2CID 85105883.
  61. ^ Spottiswoode, C. N.; Stevens, M. (2010). "Visual modelling shows that avian host parents use multiple visual cues in rejecting parasitic eggs". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (19): 8672–8676. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107.8672S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0910486107. PMC 2889299. PMID 20421497.
  62. ^ Kliner, R.M.; Madden, Joah R.; Hauber, Mark E. (2004). "Brood parasitic cowbird nestlings use host young to procure resources". Science. 305 (5685): 877–879. Bibcode:2004Sci...305..877K. doi:10.1126/science.1098487. PMID 15297677. S2CID 30084518.
  63. ^ Thomas, J.A.; Settele, Josef (2004). "Butterfly mimics of ants". Nature. 432 (7015): 283–284. Bibcode:2004Natur.432..283T. doi:10.1038/432283a. PMID 15549080. S2CID 4379000.
  64. ^ Davies, N.B. (2011). "Cuckoo adaptations: trickery and tuning". Journal of Zoology. 281: 1–14. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2011.00810.x.
  65. ^ Tanaka, K. D.; Ueda, K. (2005). "Horsfield's hawk-cuckoo nestlings simulate multiple gapes for begging". Science. 308 (5722): 653. doi:10.1126/science.1109957. PMID 15860618. S2CID 42439025.
  66. ^ a b Akino, T; J. J. Knapp; J. A. Thomas; G. W. Elmes (1999). "Chemical mimicry and host specificity in the butterfly Maculinea rebeli, a social parasite of Myrmica ant colonies". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 266 (1427): 1419–1426. doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0796. PMC 1690087.
  67. ^ a b Thomas, Jeremy; Karsten Schönrogge; Simona Bonelli; Francesca Barbero; Emilio Balletto (2010). "Corruption of ant acoustical signals by mimetic social parasites". Communicative and Integrative Biology. 3 (2): 169–171. doi:10.4161/cib.3.2.10603. PMC 2889977. PMID 20585513.
  68. ^ Dapporto, L.; Cervo, R.; Sledge, M. F.; Turillazzi, S. (2004). "Rank integration in dominance hierarchies of host colonies by the paper wasp social parasite Polistes sulcifer (Hymenoptera, Vespidae)". Journal of Insect Physiology. 50 (2–3): 217–223. doi:10.1016/j.jinsphys.2003.11.012. PMID 15019524.
  69. ^ Kreuter, Kirsten; Bunk, Elfi (2011). "How the social parasitic bumblebee Bombus bohemicus sneaks into power of reproduction". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 66 (3): 475–486. doi:10.1007/s00265-011-1294-z. S2CID 7124725.
  70. ^ Cameron, Sydney A.; Ramírez, Santiago (2001-07-01). "Nest Architecture and Nesting Ecology of the Orchid Bee Eulaema meriana (Hymenoptera: Apinae: Euglossini)". Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society. 74 (3): 142–165. JSTOR 25086012.
  71. ^ Evans, Howard E. (1973). "Burrow sharing and nest transfer in the digger wasp Philanthus gibbosus (Fabricius)". Animal Behaviour. 21 (2): 302–308. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(73)80071-5.
  72. ^ Reed, H. C.; Akre, R. D.; Garnett, W. B. (1979). "A North American Host of the Yellowjacket Social Parasite Vespula austriaca (Panzer) (Hymenoptera: Vespidae)". Entomological News. 90 (2): 110–113.
  73. ^ Gjershaug, Jan Ove (2009). "The social parasite bumblebee Bombus hyperboreus Schönherr, 1809 usurp nest of Bombus balteatus Dahlbom, 1832 (Hymenoptera, Apidae) in Norway". Norwegian Journal of Entomology. 56 (1): 28–31.
  74. ^ Emlen, S. T.; Oring, L. W. (1977). "Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems". Science. 197 (4300): 214–223. Bibcode:1977Sci...197..215E. doi:10.1126/science.327542. PMID 327542. S2CID 16786432.
  75. ^ a b c d e f g Davies, N.B., Krebs, J.R. and West., S.A., (2012). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. 4th ed. John Wiley & Sons, pp. 254–263[ISBN missing]
  76. ^ Warner, R. R. (1987). "Female choice of sites versus mates in a coral reef fish Thalassoma bifasciatum". Animal Behaviour. 35 (5): 1470–1478. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(87)80019-2. S2CID 53177154.
  77. ^ Ims, R.A. (1987). "Responses in spatial organization and behavior to manipulations of the food resource in the vole Clethrionomys rufocanus". Journal of Animal Ecology. 56 (2): 585–596. doi:10.2307/5070. JSTOR 5070.
  78. ^ Kimsey, Lynn Siri (1980). "The behaviour of male orchid bees (Apidae, Hymenoptera, Insecta) and the question of leks". Animal Behaviour. 28 (4): 996–1004. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(80)80088-1. S2CID 53161684.
  79. ^ Bradbury, J. E. and Gibson, R. M. (1983) Leks and mate choice. In: Mate Choice (ed. P. Bateson). pp. 109–138. Cambridge University Press. Cambridge[ISBN missing]
  80. ^ Moore, Sandra D. (1987). "Male-Biased Mortality in the Butterfly Euphydryas editha: a Novel Cost of Mate Acquisition". The American Naturalist. 130 (2): 306–309. doi:10.1086/284711. S2CID 84989304.
  81. ^ Lack, D. (1968) Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds. Methuen, London.
  82. ^ Davies, N. B., Krebs, J. R and West, S. A., (2012). An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. p. 266. ISBN 978-1-4051-1416-5.
  83. ^ Lightbody, J.P.; Weatherhead, P.J. (1988). "Female settling patterns and polygyny: tests of a neutral-mate-choice hypothesis". American Naturalist. 132: 20–33. doi:10.1086/284835. S2CID 84147769.
  84. ^ Verner, J.; Wilson, M.F. (1966). "The influence of habitats on mating systems of North American passerine birds". Ecology. 47 (1): 143–147. doi:10.2307/1935753. JSTOR 1935753.
  85. ^ Gowaty, P.A. (1981). "An extension of the Orians-Verner-Willson model to account for mating systems besides polygyny". American Naturalist. 118 (6): 851–859. doi:10.1086/283875. S2CID 83991131.
  86. ^ Faaborg, J.; Parker, P.G.; DeLay, L.; et al. (1995). "Confirmation of cooperative polyandry in the Galapagos hawk Buteo galapagoensis". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 36 (2): 83–90. doi:10.1007/bf00170712. S2CID 3334592.
  87. ^ Beissinger, S. R.; Snyder, N. F. R. (1987). "Mate desertion in the snail kite". Animal Behaviour. 35 (2): 477–487. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(87)80273-7. S2CID 53192930.
  88. ^ (Reynolds)
  89. ^ Butchart, S. H. M.; Seddon, N.; Ekstrom, J. M. M. (1999b). "Yelling for sex: harem males compete for female access in bronze-winged jacanas". Animal Behaviour. 57 (3): 637–646. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0985. PMID 10196054. S2CID 24253395.
  90. ^ a b c d e f Davies, Nicholas B.; Krebs, John R.; West, Stuart A. (2012). An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology. West Sussex, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 307–333. ISBN 978-1-4051-1416-5.
  91. ^ Bergstrom, Theodore (Spring 2002). "Evolution of Social Behavior: Individual and Group Selection". The Journal of Economic Perspectives. 16 (2): 67–88. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.377.5059. doi:10.1257/0895330027265. JSTOR 2696497. S2CID 125841.
  92. ^ Smith, J. M. (1964). "Group Selection and Kin Selection". Nature. 201 (4924): 1145–1147. Bibcode:1964Natur.201.1145S. doi:10.1038/2011145a0. S2CID 4177102.
  93. ^ Fisher, R. A. (1930). The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  94. ^ Haldane, J.B.S. (1932). The Causes of Evolution. London: Longmans, Green & Co.
  95. ^ Haldane, J. B. S. (1955). "Population Genetics". New Biology. 18: 34–51.
  96. ^ a b Hamilton, W. D. (1963). "The evolution of altruistic behavior". American Naturalist. 97 (896): 354–356. doi:10.1086/497114. S2CID 84216415.
  97. ^ a b Hamilton, W. D. (1964). "The Genetical Evolution of Social Behavior". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 7 (1): 1–16. Bibcode:1964JThBi...7....1H. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4. PMID 5875341.
  98. ^ West, S.A.; Griffin, A.S.; Gardner, A. (2007b). "Social semantics: altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 20 (2): 415–432. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2006.01258.x. PMID 17305808. S2CID 1792464.
  99. ^ Mehdiabadi, N. J., C. N. Jack, T. T. Farnham et al. (2006). "Kin preference in a social microbe". Nature. 442 (7105): 881–882. Bibcode:2006Natur.442..881M. doi:10.1038/442881a. PMID 16929288. S2CID 4335666.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: uses authors parameter (link)
  100. ^ Benabentos, R., S. Hirose, R. Sucgang et al. (2009). "Polymorphic members of the lag-gene family mediate kin discrimination in Dictyostelium". Current Biology. 19 (7): 567–572. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.02.037. PMC 2694408. PMID 19285397.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: uses authors parameter (link)
  101. ^ Greenberg, Les (1988-07-01). "Kin recognition in the sweat bee, Lasioglossum zephyrum". Behavior Genetics. 18 (4): 425–438. doi:10.1007/BF01065512. ISSN 0001-8244. PMID 3190637. S2CID 44298800.
  102. ^ Nunes, T. M.; Turatti, I. C. C.; Mateus, S.; Nascimento, F. S.; Lopes, N. P.; Zucchi, R. (2009). "Cuticular hydrocarbons in the stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata (Hymenoptera, Apidae, Meliponini): Differences between colonies, castes and age". Genetics and Molecular Research. 8 (2): 589–595. doi:10.4238/vol8-2kerr012. PMID 19551647.
  103. ^ Buchwald, Robert; Breed, Michael D. (2005). "Nestmate recognition cues in a stingless bee, Trigona fulviventris". Animal Behaviour. 70 (6): 1331–1337. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.017. S2CID 53147658.
  104. ^ Seidelmann, Karsten (2006-09-01). "Open-cell parasitism shapes maternal investment patterns in the red mason bee Osmia rufa". Behavioral Ecology. 17 (5): 839–848. doi:10.1093/beheco/arl017. ISSN 1045-2249.
  105. ^ Davies, N. B. & M. de L. Brooke (1988). "Cuckoos versus reed warblers: Adaptations and counteradaptations". Animal Behaviour. 36 (1): 262–284. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(88)80269-0. S2CID 53191651.
  106. ^ Holmes, W.G & P.W. Sherman (1982). "The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels". American Zoologist. 22 (3): 491–517. doi:10.1093/icb/22.3.491.
  107. ^ Bennet-Clark, H. C.; Leroy, Y.; Tsacas, L. (1980-02-01). "Species and sex-specific songs and courtship behaviour in the genus Zaprionus (Diptera-Drosophilidae)". Animal Behaviour. 28 (1): 230–255. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(80)80027-3. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 53194769.
  108. ^ Gloag, R.; et al. (2008). "Nest defence in a stingless bee: What causes fighting swarms in Trigona carbonaria (Hymenoptera, Meliponini)?". Insectes Sociaux. 55 (4): 387–391. doi:10.1007/s00040-008-1018-1. S2CID 44720135.
  109. ^ Wilkinson, G.S. (1984). "Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat". Nature. 308 (5955): 181–184. Bibcode:1984Natur.308..181W. doi:10.1038/308181a0. S2CID 4354558.
  110. ^ O'Brien, Timothy G. & John G. Robinson (1991). "Allomaternal Care by Female Wedge-Capped Capuchin Monkeys: Effects of Age, Rank and Relatedness". Behaviour. 119 (1–2): 30–50. doi:10.1163/156853991X00355.
  111. ^ Ratnieks, Francis L. W.; Heikki Helanterä (October 2009). "The evolution of extreme altruism and inequality in insect societies". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 364 (1553): 3169–3179. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0129. PMC 2781879. PMID 19805425.
  112. ^ de Waal, Frans (2016). "Are We Smart Enough To Know How Smart Animals Are?" ISBN 978-1-78378-305-2, p. 276
  113. ^ Postgate, J (1998). Nitrogen Fixation, 3rd Edition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK.
  114. ^ a b Dawkins, Richard (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford University Press.
  115. ^ M.C. Soares; I.M. Côté; S.C. Cardoso & R.Bshary (August 2008). "The cleaning goby mutualism: a system without punishment, partner switching or tactile stimulation" (PDF). Journal of Zoology. 276 (3): 306–312. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2008.00489.x.
  116. ^ Crair, Ben (1 August 2017). "The Secret Economic Lives of Animals". Bloomberg News. Retrieved 1 August 2017.
  117. ^ Foster, Kevin; Tom Wenseleers; Francis L. W. Ratnieks (10 September 2001). "Spite: Hamilton's unproven theory" (PDF). Annales Zoologici Fennici: 229–238.
  118. ^ Strassmann, Joan E.; David C. Queller (May 2007). "Insect societies as divided organisms: The complexities of purpose and cross-purpose". PNAS. 104 (suppl. 1): 8619–8626. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.8619S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0701285104. PMC 1876438. PMID 17494750.
  119. ^ Duffy, Emmett J.; Cheryl L. Morrison; Kenneth S. Macdonald (April 2002). "Colony defense and behavioral differentiation in the eusocial shrimp Synalpheus regalis". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 51 (5): 488–495. doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0455-5. S2CID 25384748.
  120. ^ Foster, W.A. (December 1990). "Experimental evidence for effective and altruistic colony defence against natural predators by soldiers of the gall-forming aphid Pemphigus spyrothecae (Hemiptera: Pemphigidae)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 27 (6): 421–430. doi:10.1007/BF00164069. S2CID 37559224.
  121. ^ Jarvis, Jennifer U. M.; Nigel C. Bennett; Andrew C. Spinks (January 1998). "Food availability and foraging by wild colonies of Damaraland mole-rats (Cryptomys damarensis): implications for sociality". Oecologia. 113 (2): 290–298. Bibcode:1998Oecol.113..290J. doi:10.1007/s004420050380. PMID 28308209. S2CID 20472674.
  122. ^ Bonckaert, W.; Tofilski, A.; Nascimento, F.S.; Billen, J.; Ratnieks, F.L.W.; Wenseleers, T. (2001). "Co-occurrence of three types of egg policing in the Norwegian wasp Dolichovespsula wasp". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 65 (4): 633–640. doi:10.1007/s00265-010-1064-3. S2CID 2186614.
  123. ^ Wenseleers, Tom; Heikki Helanterä; Adam G. Hart; Francis L. W. Ratnieks (May 2004). "Worker reproduction and policing in insect societies: an ESS analysis". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 17 (5): 1035–1047. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00751.x. PMID 15312076.
  124. ^ Foster, Kevin R. (2001). "Colony kin structure and male production in Dolichovespula wasps". Molecular Ecology. 10 (4): 1003–1010. doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2001.01228.x. PMID 11348506. S2CID 12009153.
  125. ^ Vespula vulgaris#Defensive behaviors
  126. ^ Andrew F. G. Bourke (1999). "Sex allocation in a facultatively polygynous ant: between-population and between-colony variation". Behavioral Ecology. 10 (4): 409–421. doi:10.1093/beheco/10.4.409.
  127. ^ Jurgen Heinze; Lipski, Norbert; Schlehmeyer, Kathrin; Hōlldobler, Bert (1994). "Colony structure and reproduction in the ant, Leptothorax acervorum". Behavioral Ecology. 6 (4): 359–367. doi:10.1093/beheco/6.4.359.
  128. ^ Ratnieks, Francis L.W.; P. Kirk Visscher (December 1989). "Worker policing in the honeybee". Nature. 342 (6251): 796–797. Bibcode:1989Natur.342..796R. doi:10.1038/342796a0. S2CID 4366903.
  129. ^ Gobin, Bruno; J. Billen; C. Peeters (November 1999). "Policing behaviour towards virgin egg layers in a polygynous ponerine ant". Animal Behaviour. 58 (5): 1117–1122. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1245. PMID 10564615. S2CID 16428974.
  130. ^ Boomsma, J.J (21 August 2007). "Kin selection versus sexual selection: why the ends to not meet". Current Biology. 17 (16): R673–R683. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.033. PMID 17714661. S2CID 886746.
  131. ^ Raina, Ashok K.; Klun, Jerome A. (1984). "Brain factor control of sex pheromone production in the female corn earworm moth". Science. 225 (4661): 531–533. Bibcode:1984Sci...225..531R. doi:10.1126/science.225.4661.531. PMID 17750856. S2CID 40949867.
  132. ^ O'Donnell, Sean (1997). "Gaster-Flagging during Colony Defense in Neotropical Swarm-Founding Wasps (Hymenoptera: Vespidae, Epiponini)". Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society.
  133. ^ Hernández-Roldán, Juan L.; Bofill, Roger; Dapporto, Leonardo; Munguira, Miguel L.; Vila, Roger (2014-09-01). "Morphological and chemical analysis of male scent organs in the butterfly genus Pyrgus (Lepidoptera: Hesperiidae)". Organisms Diversity & Evolution. 14 (3): 269–278. doi:10.1007/s13127-014-0170-x. ISSN 1439-6092. S2CID 15709135.
  134. ^ Silverstein, Germund; Löfstedt, Christer; Rosén, Wen Qi (2005). "Circadian mating activity and effect of pheromone pre-exposure on pheromone response rhythms in the moth Spodoptera littoralis". Journal of Insect Physiology. 51 (3): 277–286. doi:10.1016/j.jinsphys.2004.11.013. PMID 15749110.
  135. ^ Tarsitano, Michael; Wolfgang, Kirchner (2001). "Vibrational courtship signals of Zygiella x-notata" (PDF). Bulletin of the British Arachnological Society. 12: 26–32.
  136. ^ Lederhouse, Robert C.; Silvio, G. Codella Jr (1989). "Intersexual Comparison of Mimetic Protection in the Black Swallowtail Butterfly, Papilio polyxenes: Experiments with Captive Blue Jay Predators". Evolution. 43 (2): 410–420. doi:10.2307/2409216. JSTOR 2409216. PMID 28568560.

Further reading

  • Alcock, J. (2009). Animal Behavior: An Evolutionary Approach (9th edition). Sinauer Associates Inc. Sunderland, MA.[ISBN missing]
  • Bateson, P. (2017) Behaviour, Development and Evolution. Open Book Publishers, doi:10.11647/OBP.0097
  • Danchin, É., Girladeau, L.-A. and Cézilly, F. (2008). Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Perspective on Behaviour. Oxford University Press, Oxford.[ISBN missing]
  • Krebs, J.R. and Davies, N. An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology, ISBN 0-632-03546-3
  • Krebs, J.R. and Davies, N. Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach, ISBN 0-86542-731-3
  • Wajnberg, E., Bernstein E. and van Alphen, E. (2008). Behavioral Ecology of Insect Parasitoids – From Theoretical Approaches to Field Applications, Blackwell Publishing.[ISBN missing]

External links

  •   Media related to Behavioral ecology at Wikimedia Commons

behavioral, ecology, other, uses, disambiguation, also, spelled, behavioural, ecology, study, evolutionary, basis, animal, behavior, ecological, pressures, emerged, from, ethology, after, niko, tinbergen, outlined, four, questions, address, when, studying, ani. For other uses see Behavioral ecology disambiguation Behavioral ecology also spelled behavioural ecology is the study of the evolutionary basis for animal behavior due to ecological pressures Behavioral ecology emerged from ethology after Niko Tinbergen outlined four questions to address when studying animal behaviors What are the proximate causes ontogeny survival value and phylogeny of a behavior Some examples of behavioural ecologyPenguins huddling in the AntarcticGeese flying in aerodynamic V formationThe bee waggle dance communicating informationAn African elephant crossing a riverA frog with inflated vocal sacA stotting gazelleA male moor frog colored blue If an organism has a trait that provides a selective advantage i e has adaptive significance in its environment then natural selection favors it Adaptive significance refers to the expression of a trait that affects fitness measured by an individual s reproductive success Adaptive traits are those that produce more copies of the individual s genes in future generations Maladaptive traits are those that leave fewer For example if a bird that can call more loudly attracts more mates then a loud call is an adaptive trait for that species because a louder bird mates more frequently than less loud birds thus sending more loud calling genes into future generations Conversely loud calling birds may attract the attention of predators more often decreasing their presence in the gene pool Individuals are always in competition with others for limited resources including food territories and mates Conflict occurs between predators and prey between rivals for mates between siblings mates and even between parents and offspring Contents 1 Competing for resources 1 1 Evolutionarily stable strategy 1 2 Resource defense 1 3 Ideal free distribution 1 4 Mating strategies and tactics 2 Sexual selection 2 1 Mate choice by resources 2 2 Mate choice by genes 2 3 Sensory bias 3 Sexual conflict 3 1 Conflict over mating 4 Parental care and family conflicts 4 1 Types of parental care 4 2 Familial conflict 4 2 1 Sexual conflict 4 2 2 Parent offspring conflict 4 2 3 Parent offspring conflict resolution 4 2 4 Sibling sibling conflict 4 3 Brood parasitism 5 Mating systems 5 1 Mating systems with no male parental care 5 2 Mating systems with male parental care 5 2 1 Monogamy 5 2 2 Polygyny 5 2 3 Polyandry threshold 5 2 4 Female desertion and sex role reversal 6 Social behaviors 6 1 Kin selection 6 1 1 Inclusive fitness 6 2 Kin recognition 6 2 1 Genetic cues 6 2 2 Environmental cues 6 3 Cooperation 6 3 1 Within species 6 3 2 Between species 6 4 Spite 7 Altruism and conflict in social insects 7 1 Conflicts in social insects 7 2 The monogamy hypothesis 8 Communication and signaling 9 See also 10 References 11 Further reading 12 External linksCompeting for resources EditThe value of a social behavior depends in part on the social behavior of an animal s neighbors For example the more likely a rival male is to back down from a threat the more value a male gets out of making the threat The more likely however that a rival will attack if threatened the less useful it is to threaten other males When a population exhibits a number of interacting social behaviors such as this it can evolve a stable pattern of behaviors known as an evolutionarily stable strategy or ESS This term derived from economic game theory became prominent after John Maynard Smith 1982 1 recognized the possible application of the concept of a Nash equilibrium to model the evolution of behavioral strategies Evolutionarily stable strategy Edit In short evolutionary game theory asserts that only strategies that when common in the population cannot be invaded by any alternative mutant strategy is an ESS and thus maintained in the population In other words at equilibrium every player should play the best strategic response to each other When the game is two player and symmetric each player should play the strategy that provides the response best for it Therefore the ESS is considered the evolutionary end point subsequent to the interactions As the fitness conveyed by a strategy is influenced by what other individuals are doing the relative frequency of each strategy in the population behavior can be governed not only by optimality but the frequencies of strategies adopted by others and are therefore frequency dependent frequency dependence Behavioral evolution is therefore influenced by both the physical environment and interactions between other individuals An example of how changes in geography can make a strategy susceptible to alternative strategies is the parasitization of the African honey bee A m scutellata Resource defense Edit The term economic defendability was first introduced by Jerram Brown in 1964 Economic defendability states that defense of a resource have costs such as energy expenditure or risk of injury as well as benefits of priority access to the resource Territorial behavior arises when benefits are greater than the costs 2 Studies of the golden winged sunbird have validated the concept of economic defendability Comparing the energetic costs a sunbird expends in a day to the extra nectar gained by defending a territory researchers showed that birds only became territorial when they were making a net energetic profit 3 When resources are at low density the gains from excluding others may not be sufficient to pay for the cost of territorial defense In contrast when resource availability is high there may be so many intruders that the defender would have no time to make use of the resources made available by defense Sometimes the economics of resource competition favors shared defense An example is the feeding territories of the white wagtail The white wagtails feed on insects washed up by the river onto the bank which acts as a renewing food supply If any intruders harvested their territory then the prey would quickly become depleted but sometimes territory owners tolerate a second bird known as a satellite The two sharers would then move out of phase with one another resulting in decreased feeding rate but also increased defense illustrating advantages of group living 4 Ideal free distribution Edit Main article Ideal free distribution One of the major models used to predict the distribution of competing individuals amongst resource patches is the ideal free distribution model Within this model resource patches can be of variable quality and there is no limit to the number of individuals that can occupy and extract resources from a particular patch Competition within a particular patch means that the benefit each individual receives from exploiting a patch decreases logarithmically with increasing number of competitors sharing that resource patch The model predicts that individuals will initially flock to higher quality patches until the costs of crowding bring the benefits of exploiting them in line with the benefits of being the only individual on the lesser quality resource patch After this point has been reached individuals will alternate between exploiting the higher quality patches and the lower quality patches in such a way that the average benefit for all individuals in both patches is the same This model is ideal in that individuals have complete information about the quality of a resource patch and the number of individuals currently exploiting it and free in that individuals are freely able to choose which resource patch to exploit 5 An experiment by Manfred Malinski in 1979 demonstrated that feeding behavior in three spined sticklebacks follows an ideal free distribution Six fish were placed in a tank and food items were dropped into opposite ends of the tank at different rates The rate of food deposition at one end was set at twice that of the other end and the fish distributed themselves with four individuals at the faster depositing end and two individuals at the slower depositing end In this way the average feeding rate was the same for all of the fish in the tank 6 Mating strategies and tactics Edit As with any competition of resources species across the animal kingdom may also engage in competitions for mating If one considers mates or potentials mates as a resource these sexual partners can be randomly distributed amongst resource pools within a given environment Following the ideal free distribution model suitors distribute themselves amongst the potential mates in an effort to maximize their chances or the number of potential matings For all competitors males of a species in most cases there are variations in both the strategies and tactics used to obtain matings Strategies generally refer to the genetically determined behaviors that can be described as conditional Tactics refer to the subset of behaviors within a given genetic strategy Thus it is not difficult for a great many variations in mating strategies to exist in a given environment or species 7 An experiment conducted by Anthony Arak where playback of synthetic calls from male natterjack toads was used to manipulate behavior of the males in a chorus the difference between strategies and tactics is clear While small and immature male natterjack toads adopted a satellite tactic to parasitize larger males Though large males on average still retained greater reproductive success smaller males were able to intercept matings When the large males of the chorus were removed smaller males adopted a calling behavior no longer competing against the loud calls of larger males When smaller males got larger and their calls more competitive then they started calling and competing directly for mates 8 Sexual selection EditMate choice by resources Edit In many sexually reproducing species such as mammals birds and amphibians females are able to bear offspring for a certain time period during which the males are free to mate with other available females and therefore can father many more offspring to pass on their genes The fundamental difference between male and female reproduction mechanisms determines the different strategies each sex employs to maximize their reproductive success For males their reproductive success is limited by access to females while females are limited by their access to resources In this sense females can be much choosier than males because they have to bet on the resources provided by the males to ensure reproductive success 9 Resources usually include nest sites food and protection In some cases the males provide all of them e g sedge warblers 10 The females dwell in their chosen males territories for access to these resources The males gain ownership to the territories through male male competition that often involves physical aggression Only the largest and strongest males manage to defend the best quality nest sites Females choose males by inspecting the quality of different territories or by looking at some male traits that can indicate the quality of resources 9 One example of this is with the grayling butterfly Hipparchia semele where males engage in complex flight patterns to decide who defends a particular territory The female grayling butterfly chooses a male based on the most optimal location for oviposition 11 Sometimes males leave after mating The only resource that a male provides is a nuptial gift such as protection or food as seen in Drosophila subobscura 12 13 The female can evaluate the quality of the protection or food provided by the male so as to decide whether to mate or not or how long she is willing to copulate Mate choice by genes Edit When males only contribution to offspring is their sperm females are particularly choosy With this high level of female choice sexual ornaments are seen in males where the ornaments reflect the male s social status Two hypotheses have been proposed to conceptualize the genetic benefits from female mate choice 9 First the good genes hypothesis suggests that female choice is for higher genetic quality and that this preference is favored because it increases fitness of the offspring 14 This includes Zahavi s handicap hypothesis and Hamilton and Zuk s host and parasite arms race Zahavi s handicap hypothesis was proposed within the context of looking at elaborate male sexual displays He suggested that females favor ornamented traits because they are handicaps and are indicators of the male s genetic quality Since these ornamented traits are hazards the male s survival must be indicative of his high genetic quality in other areas In this way the degree that a male expresses his sexual display indicates to the female his genetic quality 9 Zuk and Hamilton proposed a hypothesis after observing disease as a powerful selective pressure on a rabbit population They suggested that sexual displays were indicators of resistance of disease on a genetic level 9 Such choosiness from the female individuals can be seen in wasp species too especially among Polistes dominula wasps The females tend to prefer males with smaller more elliptically shaped spots than those with larger and more irregularly shaped spots Those males would have reproductive superiority over males with irregular spots In marbled newts females show preference to mates with larger crests This however is not considered a handicap as it does not negatively affect males chances of survival It is simply a trait females show preference for when choosing their mate as it is an indication of health and fitness 15 Fisher s hypothesis of runaway sexual selection suggests that female preference is genetically correlated with male traits and that the preference co evolves with the evolution of that trait thus the preference is under indirect selection 14 Fisher suggests that female preference began because the trait indicated the male s quality The female preference spread so that the females offspring now benefited from the higher quality from specific trait but also greater attractiveness to mates Eventually the trait only represents attractiveness to mates and no longer represents increased survival 9 An example of mate choice by genes is seen in the cichlid fish Tropheus moorii where males provide no parental care An experiment found that a female T moorii is more likely to choose a mate with the same color morph as her own 16 In another experiment females have been shown to share preferences for the same males when given two to choose from meaning some males get to reproduce more often than others 17 Sensory bias Edit The sensory bias hypothesis states that the preference for a trait evolves in a non mating context and is then exploited by one sex to obtain more mating opportunities The competitive sex evolves traits that exploit a pre existing bias that the choosy sex already possesses This mechanism is thought to explain remarkable trait differences in closely related species because it produces a divergence in signaling systems which leads to reproductive isolation 18 Sensory bias has been demonstrated in guppies freshwater fish from Trinidad and Tobago In this mating system female guppies prefer to mate with males with more orange body coloration However outside of a mating context both sexes prefer animate orange objects which suggests that preference originally evolved in another context like foraging 19 Orange fruits are a rare treat that fall into streams where the guppies live The ability to find these fruits quickly is an adaptive quality that has evolved outside of a mating context Sometime after the affinity for orange objects arose male guppies exploited this preference by incorporating large orange spots to attract females Another example of sensory exploitation is in the water mite Neumania papillator an ambush predator that hunts copepods small crustaceans passing by in the water column 20 When hunting N papillator adopts a characteristic stance termed the net stance their first four legs are held out into the water column with their four hind legs resting on aquatic vegetation this allows them to detect vibrational stimuli produced by swimming prey and use this to orient towards and clutch at prey 21 During courtship males actively search for females 22 if a male finds a female he slowly circles around the female whilst trembling his first and second leg near her 20 21 Male leg trembling causes females who were in the net stance to orient towards often clutch the male 20 This did not damage the male or deter further courtship the male then deposited spermatophores and began to vigorously fan and jerk his fourth pair of legs over the spermatophore generating a current of water that passed over the spermatophores and towards the female 20 Sperm packet uptake by the female would sometimes follow 20 Heather Proctor hypothesised that the vibrations trembling male legs made were done to mimic the vibrations that females detect from swimming prey this would trigger the female prey detection responses causing females to orient and then clutch at males mediating courtship 20 23 If this was true and males were exploiting female predation responses then hungry females should be more receptive to male trembling Proctor found that unfed captive females did orient and clutch at males significantly more than fed captive females did consistent with the sensory exploitation hypothesis 20 Other examples for the sensory bias mechanism include traits in auklets 24 wolf spiders 25 and manakins 26 Further experimental work is required to reach a fuller understanding of the prevalence and mechanisms of sensory bias 27 Sexual conflict EditSexual conflict in some form or another may very well be inherent in the ways most animals reproduce 28 Females invest more in offspring prior to mating due to the differences in gametes in species that exhibit anisogamy and often invest more in offspring after mating 29 This unequal investment leads on one hand to intense competition between males for mates and on the other hand to females choosing among males for better access to resources and good genes Because of differences in mating goals males and females may have very different preferred outcomes to mating Sexual conflict occurs whenever the preferred outcome of mating is different for the male and female This difference in theory should lead to each sex evolving adaptations that bias the outcome of reproduction towards its own interests This sexual competition leads to sexually antagonistic coevolution between males and females resulting in what has been described as an evolutionary arms race between males and females 30 31 Conflict over mating Edit Male scorpionfly Males reproductive successes are often limited by access to mates whereas females reproductive successes are more often limited by access to resources Thus for a given sexual encounter it benefits the male to mate but benefits the female to be choosy and resist 29 For example male small tortoiseshell butterfly compete to gain the best territory to mate 32 Another example of this conflict can be found in the Eastern carpenter bee Xylocopa virginica Males of this species are limited in reproduction primarily by access to mates so they claim a territory and wait for a female to pass through Big males are therefore more successful in mating because they claim territories near the female nesting sites that are more sought after Smaller males on the other hand monopolize less competitive sites in foraging areas so that they may mate with reduced conflict 33 Another example of this is Sepsis cynipsea where males of the species mount females to guard them from other males and remain on the female attempting to copulate until the female either shakes them off or consents to mating 34 Similarly the neriid fly Derocephalus angusticollis demonstrates mate guarding by using their long limbs to hold onto the female as well as push other males away during copulation 35 Extreme manifestations of this conflict are seen throughout nature For example the male Panorpa scorpionflies attempt to force copulation Male scorpionflies usually acquire mates by presenting them with edible nuptial gifts in the forms of salivary secretions or dead insects However some males attempt to force copulation by grabbing females with a specialized abdominal organ without offering a gift 36 Forced copulation is costly to the female as she does not receive the food from the male and has to search for food herself costing time and energy while it is beneficial for the male as he does not need to find a nuptial gift In other cases however it pays for the female to gain more matings and her social mate to prevent these so as to guard paternity For example in many socially monogamous birds males follow females closely during their fertile periods and attempt to chase away any other males to prevent extra pair matings The female may attempt to sneak off to achieve these extra matings In species where males are incapable of constant guarding the social male may frequently copulate with the female so as to swamp rival males sperm 37 Female red junglefowl in Thailand Sexual conflict after mating has also been shown to occur in both males and females Males employ a diverse array of tactics to increase their success in sperm competition These can include removing other male s sperm from females displacing other male s sperm by flushing out prior inseminations with large amounts of their own sperm creating copulatory plugs in females reproductive tracts to prevent future matings with other males spraying females with anti aphrodisiacs to discourage other males from mating with the female and producing sterile parasperm to protect fertile eusperm in the female s reproductive tract 29 For example the male spruce bud moth Zeiraphera canadensis secretes an accessory gland protein during mating that makes them unattractive to other males and thus prevents females from future copulation 38 The Rocky Mountain parnassian also exhibits this type of sexual conflict when the male butterflies deposit a waxy genital plug onto the tip of the female s abdomen that physically prevents the female from mating again 39 Males can also prevent future mating by transferring an anti Aphrodiasic to the female during mating This behavior is seen in butterfly species such as Heliconius melpomene where males transfer a compound that causes the female to smell like a male butterfly and thus deter any future potential mates 40 Furthermore males may control the strategic allocation of sperm producing more sperm when females are more promiscuous All these methods are meant to ensure that females are more likely to produce offspring belonging to the males who uses the method 29 Females also control the outcomes of matings and there exists the possibility that females choose sperm cryptic female choice 29 A dramatic example of this is the feral fowl Gallus gallus In this species females prefer to copulate with dominant males but subordinate males can force matings In these cases the female is able to eject the subordinate male s sperm using cloacal contractions 41 Parental care and family conflicts EditParental care is the investment a parent puts into their offspring which includes protecting and feeding the young preparing burrows or nests and providing eggs with yolk 42 There is great variation in parental care in the animal kingdom In some species the parents may not care for their offspring at all while in others the parents exhibit single parental or even bi parental care As with other topics in behavioral ecology interactions within a family involve conflicts These conflicts can be broken down into three general types sexual male female conflict parent offspring conflict and sibling conflict Types of parental care Edit There are many different patterns of parental care in the animal kingdom The patterns can be explained by physiological constraints or ecological conditions such as mating opportunities In invertebrates there is no parental care in most species because it is more favorable for parents to produce a large number of eggs whose fate is left to chance than to protect a few individual young In other cases parental care is indirect manifested via actions taken before the offspring is produced but nonetheless essential for their survival for example female Lasioglossum figueresi sweat bees excavate a nest construct brood cells and stock the cells with pollen and nectar before they lay their eggs so when the larvae hatch they are sheltered and fed but the females die without ever interacting with their brood 43 In birds biparental care is the most common because reproductive success directly depends on the parents ability to feed their chicks Two parents can feed twice as many young so it is more favorable for birds to have both parents delivering food In mammals female only care is the most common This is most likely because females are internally fertilized and so are holding the young inside for a prolonged period of gestation which provides males with the opportunity to desert Females also feed the young through lactation after birth so males are not required for feeding Male parental care is only observed in species where they contribute to feeding or carrying of the young such as in marmosets 44 In fish there is no parental care in 79 of bony fish 45 In fish with parental care it usually limited to selecting preparing and defending a nest as seen in sockeye salmon for example 46 Also parental care in fish if any is primarily done by males as seen in gobies and redlip blennies 47 42 The cichlid fish V moorii exhibits biparental care 48 In species with internal fertilization the female is usually the one to take care of the young In cases where fertilization is external the male becomes the main caretaker Familial conflict Edit Familial conflict is a result of trade offs as a function of lifetime parental investment Parental investment was defined by Robert Trivers in 1972 as any investment by the parent in an individual offspring that increases the offspring s chance of surviving at the cost of the parent s ability to invest in other offspring citation needed Parental investment includes behaviors like guarding and feeding Each parent has a limited amount of parental investment over the course of their lifetime Investment trade offs in offspring quality and quantity within a brood and trade offs between current and future broods leads to conflict over how much parental investment to provide and to whom parents should invest in There are three major types of familial conflict sexual parent offspring and sibling sibling conflict 9 Sexual conflict Edit Main article Sexual conflict Great tit There is conflict among parents as to who should provide the care as well as how much care to provide Each parent must decide whether or not to stay and care for their offspring or to desert their offspring This decision is best modeled by game theoretic approaches to evolutionarily stable strategies ESS where the best strategy for one parent depends on the strategy adopted by the other parent Recent research has found response matching in parents who determine how much care to invest in their offspring Studies found that parent great tits match their partner s increased care giving efforts with increased provisioning rates of their own 49 This cued parental response is a type of behavioral negotiation between parents that leads to stabilized compensation Sexual conflicts can give rise to antagonistic co evolution between the sexes to try to get the other sex to care more for offspring For example in the waltzing fly Prochyliza xanthostoma ejaculate feeding maximizes female reproductive success and minimizes the female s chance of mating multiply 50 Evidence suggests that the sperm evolved to prevent female waltzing flies from mating multiply in order to ensure the male s paternity 50 Parent offspring conflict Edit Blackbird chicks in a nest According to Robert Trivers s theory on relatedness citation needed each offspring is related to itself by 1 but is only 0 5 related to their parents and siblings Genetically offspring are predisposed to behave in their own self interest while parents are predisposed to behave equally to all their offspring including both current and future ones Offspring selfishly try to take more than their fair shares of parental investment while parents try to spread out their parental investment equally amongst their present young and future young There are many examples of parent offspring conflict in nature One manifestation of this is asynchronous hatching in birds A behavioral ecology hypothesis is known as Lack s brood reduction hypothesis named after David Lack citation needed Lack s hypothesis posits an evolutionary and ecological explanation as to why birds lay a series of eggs with an asynchronous delay leading to nestlings of mixed age and weights According to Lack this brood behavior is an ecological insurance that allows the larger birds to survive in poor years and all birds to survive when food is plentiful 51 52 We also see sex ratio conflict between the queen and her workers in social hymenoptera Because of haplodiploidy the workers offspring prefer a 3 1 female to male sex allocation while the queen prefers a 1 1 sex ratio Both the queen and the workers try to bias the sex ratio in their favor 53 In some species the workers gain control of the sex ratio while in other species like B terrestris the queen has a considerable amount of control over the colony sex ratio 54 Lastly there has been recent evidence regarding genomic imprinting that is a result of parent offspring conflict Paternal genes in offspring demand more maternal resources than maternal genes in the same offspring and vice versa This has been show in imprinted genes like insulin like growth factor II 55 Parent offspring conflict resolution Edit Parents need an honest signal from their offspring that indicates their level of hunger or need so that the parents can distribute resources accordingly Offspring want more than their fair share of resources so they exaggerate their signals to wheedle more parental investment However this conflict is countered by the cost of excessive begging Not only does excessive begging attract predators but it also retards chick growth if begging goes unrewarded 56 Thus the cost of increased begging enforces offspring honesty Another resolution for parent offspring conflict is that parental provisioning and offspring demand have actually coevolved so that there is no obvious underlying conflict Cross fostering experiments in great tits Parus major have shown that offspring beg more when their biological mothers are more generous 57 Therefore it seems that the willingness to invest in offspring is co adapted to offspring demand Sibling sibling conflict Edit Galapagos fur seals The lifetime parental investment is the fixed amount of parental resources available for all of a parent s young and an offspring wants as much of it as possible Siblings in a brood often compete for parental resources by trying to gain more than their fair share of what their parents can offer Nature provides numerous examples in which sibling rivalry escalates to such an extreme that one sibling tries to kill off broodmates to maximize parental investment See Siblicide In the Galapagos fur seal the second pup of a female is usually born when the first pup is still suckling This competition for the mother s milk is especially fierce during periods of food shortage such as an El Nino year and this usually results in the older pup directly attacking and killing the younger one 58 In some bird species sibling rivalry is also abetted by the asynchronous hatching of eggs In the blue footed booby for example the first egg in a nest is hatched four days before the second one resulting in the elder chick having a four day head start in growth When the elder chick falls 20 25 below its expected weight threshold it attacks its younger sibling and drives it from the nest 59 Sibling relatedness in a brood also influences the level of sibling sibling conflict In a study on passerine birds it was found that chicks begged more loudly in species with higher levels of extra pair paternity 60 Brood parasitism Edit Main article Brood parasite Adult reed warbler feeding a common cuckoo chick Some animals deceive other species into providing all parental care These brood parasites selfishly exploit their hosts parents and host offspring The common cuckoo is a well known example of a brood parasite Female cuckoos lay a single egg in the nest of the host species and when the cuckoo chick hatches it ejects all the host eggs and young Other examples of brood parasites include honeyguides cowbirds and the large blue butterfly 61 62 63 Brood parasite offspring have many strategies to induce their host parents to invest parental care Studies show that the common cuckoo uses vocal mimicry to reproduce the sound of multiple hungry host young to solicit more food 64 Other cuckoos use visual deception with their wings to exaggerate the begging display False gapes from brood parasite offspring cause host parents to collect more food 65 Another example of a brood parasite is Phengaris butterflies such as Phengaris rebeli and Phengaris arion which differ from the cuckoo in that the butterflies do not oviposit directly in the nest of the host an ant species Myrmica schencki 66 67 Rather the butterfly larvae release chemicals that deceive the ants into believing that they are ant larvae causing the ants to bring the butterfly larvae back to their own nests to feed them 66 67 Other examples of brood parasites are Polistes sulcifer a paper wasp that has lost the ability to build its own nests so females lay their eggs in the nest of a host species Polistes dominula and rely on the host workers to take care of their brood 68 as well as Bombus bohemicus a bumblebee that relies on host workers of various other Bombus species 69 Similarly in Eulaema meriana some Leucospidae wasps exploit the brood cells and nest for shelter and food from the bees 70 71 Vespula austriaca is another wasp in which the females force the host workers to feed and take care of the brood 72 In particular Bombus hyperboreus an Arctic bee species is also classified as a brood parasite in that it attacks and enslaves other species within their subgenus Alpinobombus to propagate their population 73 Mating systems EditMain article Mating systems Various types of mating systems include monogamy polygyny polyandry and promiscuity Each is differentiated by the sexual behavior between mates such as which males mate with certain females An influential paper by Stephen Emlen and Lewis Oring 1977 74 argued that two main factors of animal behavior influence the diversity of mating systems the relative accessibility that each sex has to mates and the parental desertion by either sex Mating systems with no male parental care Edit In a system that does not have male parental care resource dispersion predation and the effects of social living primarily influence female dispersion which in turn influences male dispersion Since males primary concern is female acquisition the males either indirectly or directly compete for the females In direct competition the males are directly focused on the females 75 Blue headed wrasse demonstrate the behavior in which females follow resources such as good nest sites and males follow the females 75 76 Conversely species with males that exemplify indirectly competitive behavior tend towards the males anticipation of the resources desired by females and their subsequent effort to control or acquire these resources which helps them to achieve success with females 75 Grey sided voles demonstrate indirect male competition for females The males were experimentally observed to home in on the sites with the best food in anticipation of females settling in these areas 75 77 Males of Euglossa imperialis a non social bee species also demonstrate indirect competitive behavior by forming aggregations of territories which can be considered leks to defend fragrant rich primary territories The purpose of these aggregations is largely only facultative since the more suitable fragrant rich sites there are the more habitable territories there are to inhabit giving females of this species a large selection of males with whom to potentially mate 78 Leks and choruses have also been deemed another behavior among the phenomena of male competition for females Due to the resource poor nature of the territories that lekking males often defend it is difficult to categorize them as indirect competitors For example the ghost moth males display in leks to attract a female mate Additionally it is difficult to classify them as direct competitors seeing as they put a great deal of effort into their defense of their territories before females arrive and upon female arrival they put for the great mating displays to attract the females to their individual sites These observations make it difficult to determine whether female or resource dispersion primarily influences male aggregation especially in lieu of the apparent difficulty that males may have defending resources and females in such densely populated areas 75 Because the reason for male aggregation into leks is unclear five hypotheses have been proposed These postulates propose the following as reasons for male lekking hotspot predation reduction increased female attraction hotshot males facilitation of female choice 75 79 With all of the mating behaviors discussed the primary factors influencing differences within and between species are ecology social conflicts and life history differences 75 In some other instances neither direct nor indirect competition is seen Instead in species like the Edith s checkerspot butterfly males efforts are directed at acquisition of females and they exhibit indiscriminate mate location behavior where given the low cost of mistakes they blindly attempt to mate both correctly with females and incorrectly with other objects 80 Mating systems with male parental care Edit Monogamy Edit Monogamy is the mating system in 90 of birds possibly because each male and female has a greater number of offspring if they share in raising a brood 81 In obligate monogamy males feed females on the nest or share in incubation and chick feeding In some species males and females form lifelong pair bonds Monogamy may also arise from limited opportunities for polygamy due to strong competition among males for mates females suffering from loss of male help and female female aggression 82 Polygyny Edit In birds polygyny occurs when males indirectly monopolize females by controlling resources In species where males normally do not contribute much to parental care females suffer relatively little or not at all 83 In other species however females suffer through the loss of male contribution and the cost of having to share resources that the male controls such as nest sites or food In some cases a polygynous male may control a high quality territory so for the female the benefits of polygyny may outweigh the costs 84 Polyandry threshold Edit There also seems to be a polyandry threshold where males may do better by agreeing to share a female instead of maintaining a monogamous mating system 85 Situations that may lead to cooperation among males include when food is scarce and when there is intense competition for territories or females For example male lions sometimes form coalitions to gain control of a pride of females In some populations of Galapagos hawks groups of males would cooperate to defend one breeding territory The males would share matings with the female and share paternity with the offspring 86 Female desertion and sex role reversal Edit In birds desertion often happens when food is abundant so the remaining partner is better able to raise the young unaided Desertion also occurs if there is a great chance of a parent to gain another mate which depends on environmental and populational factors 87 Some birds such as the phalaropes have reversed sex roles where the female is larger and more brightly colored and compete for males to incubate their clutches 88 In jacanas the female is larger than the male and her territory could overlap the multiple territories of up to four males 89 In the frog species P bibronii the female is fertilizes multiple nests and the male is left to tend to each nest while the female moves on Social behaviors EditAnimals cooperate with each other to increase their own fitness 90 These altruistic and sometimes spiteful behaviors can be explained by Hamilton s rule which states that rB C gt 0 where r relatedness B benefits and C costs 91 Kin selection Edit Main article Kin selection Kin selection refers to evolutionary strategies where an individual acts to favor the reproductive success of relatives or kin even if the action incurs some cost to the organism s own survival and ability to procreate 90 John Maynard Smith coined the term in 1964 92 although the concept was referred to by Charles Darwin who cited that helping relatives would be favored by group selection Mathematical descriptions of kin selection were initially offered by R A Fisher in 1930 93 and J B S Haldane in 1932 94 and 1955 95 W D Hamilton popularized the concept later including the mathematical treatment by George Price in 1963 and 1964 96 97 Kin selection predicts that individuals will harbor personal costs in favor of one or multiple individuals because this can maximize their genetic contribution to future generations For example an organism may be inclined to expend great time and energy in parental investment to rear offspring since this future generation may be better suited for propagating genes that are highly shared between the parent and offspring 90 Ultimately the initial actor performs apparent altruistic actions for kin to enhance its own reproductive fitness In particular organisms are hypothesized to act in favor of kin depending on their genetic relatedness 96 97 So individuals are inclined to act altruistically for siblings grandparents cousins and other relatives but to differing degrees 90 Inclusive fitness Edit Inclusive fitness describes the component of reproductive success in both a focal individual and their relatives 90 Importantly the measure embodies the sum of direct and indirect fitness and the change in their reproductive success based on the actor s behavior 98 That is the effect an individual s behaviors have on being personally better suited to reproduce offspring and aiding descendant and non descendant relatives in their reproductive efforts 90 Natural selection is predicted to push individuals to behave in ways that maximize their inclusive fitness Studying inclusive fitness is often done using predictions from Hamilton s rule Kin recognition Edit Main article Kin recognition Genetic cues Edit One possible method of kin selection is based on genetic cues that can be recognized phenotypically 9 Genetic recognition has been exemplified in a species that is usually not thought of as a social creature amoebae Social amoebae form fruiting bodies when starved for food These amoebae preferentially formed slugs and fruiting bodies with members of their own lineage which is clonally related 99 The genetic cue comes from variable lag genes which are involved in signaling and adhesion between cells 100 Kin can also be recognized a genetically determined odor as studied in the primitively social sweat bee Lasioglossum zephyrus These bees can even recognize relatives they have never met and roughly determine relatedness 101 The Brazilian stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata uses a distinct combination of chemical hydrocarbons to recognize and locate kin Each chemical odor emitted from the organism s epicuticles is unique and varies according to age sex location and hierarchical position 102 Similarly individuals of the stingless bee species Trigona fulviventris can distinguish kin from non kin through recognition of a number of compounds including hydrocarbons and fatty acids that are present in their wax and floral oils from plants used to construct their nests 103 In the species Osmia rufa kin selection has also been associated with mating selection Females specifically select males for mating with whom they are genetically more related to 104 Environmental cues Edit There are two simple rules that animals follow to determine who is kin These rules can be exploited but exist because they are generally successful The first rule is treat anyone in my home as kin This rule is readily seen in the reed warbler a bird species that only focuses on chicks in their own nest If its own kin is placed outside of the nest a parent bird ignores that chick This rule can sometimes lead to odd results especially if there is a parasitic bird that lays eggs in the reed warbler nest For example an adult cuckoo may sneak its egg into the nest Once the cuckoo hatches the reed warbler parent feeds the invading bird like its own child Even with the risk for exploitation the rule generally proves successful 9 105 The second rule named by Konrad Lorenz as imprinting states that those who you grow up with are kin Several species exhibit this behavior including but not limited to the Belding s ground squirrel 9 Experimentation with these squirrels showed that regardless of true genetic relatedness those that were reared together rarely fought Further research suggests that there is partially some genetic recognition going on as well as siblings that were raised apart were less aggressive toward one another compared to non relatives reared apart 106 Another way animals may recognize their kin include the interchange of unique signals While song singing is often considered a sexual trait between males and females male male song singing also occurs For example male vinegar flies Zaprionus tuberculatus can recognize each other by song 107 Cooperation Edit Cooperation is broadly defined as behavior that provides a benefit to another individual that specifically evolved for that benefit This excludes behavior that has not been expressly selected for to provide a benefit for another individual because there are many commensal and parasitic relationships where the behavior one individual which has evolved to benefit that individual and no others is taken advantage of by other organisms Stable cooperative behavior requires that it provide a benefit to both the actor and recipient though the benefit to the actor can take many different forms 9 Within species Edit Within species cooperation occurs among members of the same species Examples of intraspecific cooperation include cooperative breeding such as in weeper capuchins and cooperative foraging such as in wolves There are also forms of cooperative defense mechanisms such as the fighting swarm behavior used by the stingless bee Tetragonula carbonaria 108 Much of this behavior occurs due to kin selection Kin selection allows cooperative behavior to evolve where the actor receives no direct benefits from the cooperation 9 Cooperation without kin selection must evolve to provide benefits to both the actor and recipient of the behavior This includes reciprocity where the recipient of the cooperative behavior repays the actor at a later time This may occur in vampire bats but it is uncommon in non human animals 109 Cooperation can occur willingly between individuals when both benefit directly as well Cooperative breeding where one individual cares for the offspring of another occurs in several species including wedge capped capuchin monkeys 110 Cooperative behavior may also be enforced where their failure to cooperate results in negative consequences One of the best examples of this is worker policing which occurs in social insect colonies 111 The cooperative pulling paradigm is a popular experimental design used to assess if and under which conditions animals cooperate It involves two or more animals pulling rewards towards themselves via an apparatus they can not successfully operate alone 112 Between species Edit Cooperation can occur between members of different species For interspecific cooperation to be evolutionarily stable it must benefit individuals in both species Examples include pistol shrimp and goby fish nitrogen fixing microbes and legumes 113 ants and aphids 114 In ants and aphids aphids secrete a sugary liquid called honeydew which ants eat The ants provide protection to the aphids against predators and in some instances raise the aphid eggs and larvae inside the ant colony This behavior is analogous to human domestication 114 The genus of goby fish Elacatinus also demonstrate cooperation by removing and feeding on ectoparasites of their clients 115 The species of wasp Polybia rejecta and ants Azteca chartifex show a cooperative behavior protecting one another s nests from predators Market economics often govern the details of the cooperation e g the amount exchanged between individual animals follow the rules of supply and demand 116 Spite Edit Hamilton s rule can also predict spiteful behaviors between non relatives 9 A spiteful behavior is one that is harmful to both the actor and to the recipient Spiteful behavior is favored if the actor is less related to the recipient than to the average member of the population making r negative and if rB C is still greater than zero Spite can also be thought of as a type of altruism because harming a non relative by taking his resources for example could also benefit a relative by allowing him access to those resources Furthermore certain spiteful behaviors may provide harmful short term consequences to the actor but also give long term reproductive benefits 117 Many behaviors that are commonly thought of as spiteful are actually better explained as being selfish that is benefiting the actor and harming the recipient and true spiteful behaviors are rare in the animal kingdom An example of spite is the sterile soldiers of the polyembryonic parasitoid wasp A female wasp lays a male and a female egg in a caterpillar The eggs divide asexually creating many genetically identical male and female larvae Sterile soldier wasps also develop and attack the relatively unrelated brother larvae so that the genetically identical sisters have more access to food 9 Another example is bacteria that release bacteriocins 9 The bacteria that releases the bacteriocin may have to die to do so but most of the harm is to unrelated individuals who are killed by the bacteriocin This is because the ability to produce and release the bacteriocin is linked to an immunity to it Therefore close relatives to the releasing cell are less likely to die than non relatives Altruism and conflict in social insects Edit Honeypot ant Many insect species of the order Hymenoptera bees ants wasps are eusocial Within the nests or hives of social insects individuals engage in specialized tasks to ensure the survival of the colony Dramatic examples of these specializations include changes in body morphology or unique behaviors such as the engorged bodies of the honeypot ant Myrmecocystus mexicanus or the waggle dance of honey bees and a wasp species Vespula vulgaris In many but not all social insects reproduction is monopolized by the queen of the colony Due to the effects of a haplodiploid mating system in which unfertilized eggs become male drones and fertilized eggs become worker females average relatedness values between sister workers can be higher than those seen in humans or other eutherian mammals This has led to the suggestion that kin selection may be a driving force in the evolution of eusociality as individuals could provide cooperative care that establishes a favorable benefit to cost ratio rB c gt 0 118 However not all social insects follow this rule In the social wasp Polistes dominula 35 of the nest mates are unrelated 32 59 In many other species unrelated individuals only help the queen when no other options are present In this case subordinates work for unrelated queens even when other options may be present No other social insect submits to unrelated queens in this way This seemingly unfavorable behavior parallels some vertebrate systems It is thought that this unrelated assistance is evidence of altruism in P dominula 32 Naked mole rats Cooperation in social organisms has numerous ecological factors that can determine the benefits and costs associated with this form of organization One suggested benefit is a type of life insurance for individuals who participate in the care of the young In this instance individuals may have a greater likelihood of transmitting genes to the next generation when helping in a group compared to individual reproduction Another suggested benefit is the possibility of fortress defense where soldier castes threaten or attack intruders thus protecting related individuals inside the territory Such behaviors are seen in the snapping shrimp Synalpheus regalis and gall forming aphid Pemphigus spyrothecae 119 120 A third ecological factor that is posited to promote eusociality is the distribution of resources when food is sparse and concentrated in patches eusociality is favored Evidence supporting this third factor comes from studies of naked mole rats and Damaraland mole rats which have communities containing a single pair of reproductive individuals 121 Conflicts in social insects Edit Although eusociality has been shown to offer many benefits to the colony there is also potential for conflict Examples include the sex ratio conflict and worker policing seen in certain species of social Hymenoptera such as Dolichovespula media Dolichovespula sylvestris Dolichovespula norwegica 122 and Vespula vulgaris 123 124 The queen and the worker wasps either indirectly kill the laying workers offspring by neglecting them or directly condemn them by cannibalizing and scavenging 125 The sex ratio conflict arises from a relatedness asymmetry which is caused by the haplodiploidy nature of Hymenoptera For instance workers are most related to each other because they share half of the genes from the queen and inherit all of the father s genes Their total relatedness to each other would be 0 5 0 5 x 0 5 0 75 Thus sisters are three fourths related to each other On the other hand males arise from unfertilized larva meaning they only inherit half of the queen s genes and none from the father As a result a female is related to her brother by 0 25 because 50 of her genes that come from her father have no chance of being shared with a brother Her relatedness to her brother would therefore be 0 5 x 0 5 0 25 9 382 According to Trivers and Hare s population level sex investment ratio theory the ratio of relatedness between sexes determines the sex investment ratios 126 As a result it has been observed that there is a tug of war between the queen and the workers where the queen would prefer a 1 1 female to male ratio because she is equally related to her sons and daughters r 0 5 in each case However the workers would prefer a 3 1 female to male ratio because they are 0 75 related to each other and only 0 25 related to their brothers 9 382 Allozyme data of a colony may indicate who wins this conflict 127 Conflict can also arise between workers in colonies of social insects In some species worker females retain their ability to mate and lay eggs The colony s queen is related to her sons by half of her genes and a quarter to the sons of her worker daughters Workers however are related to their sons by half of their genes and to their brothers by a quarter Thus the queen and her worker daughters would compete for reproduction to maximize their own reproductive fitness Worker reproduction is limited by other workers who are more related to the queen than their sisters a situation occurring in many polyandrous hymenopteran species Workers police the egg laying females by engaging in oophagy or directed acts of aggression 128 129 The monogamy hypothesis Edit The monogamy hypothesis states that the presence of monogamy in insects is crucial for eusociality to occur This is thought to be true because of Hamilton s rule that states that rB C gt 0 By having a monogamous mating system all of the offspring have high relatedness to each other This means that it is equally beneficial to help out a sibling as it is to help out an offspring If there were many fathers the relatedness of the colony would be lowered 9 371 375 This monogamous mating system has been observed in insects such as termites ants bees and wasps 9 371 375 In termites the queen commits to a single male when founding a nest In ants bees and wasps the queens have a functional equivalent to lifetime monogamy The male can even die before the founding of the colony The queen can store and use the sperm from a single male throughout their lifetime sometimes up to 30 years 9 371 375 In an experiment looking at the mating of 267 hymenopteran species the results were mapped onto a phylogeny It was found that monogamy was the ancestral state in all the independent transitions to eusociality This indicates that monogamy is the ancestral likely to be crucial state for the development of eusociality In species where queens mated with multiple mates it was found that these were developed from lineages where sterile castes already evolved so the multiple mating was secondary 130 In these cases multiple mating is likely to be advantageous for reasons other than those important at the origin of eusociality Most likely reasons are that a diverse worker pool attained by multiple mating by the queen increases disease resistance and may facilitate a division of labor among workers 9 371 375 Communication and signaling EditMain article Animal communication Communication is varied at all scales of life from interactions between microscopic organisms to those of large groups of people Nevertheless the signals used in communication abide by a fundamental property they must be a quality of the receiver that can transfer information to a receiver that is capable of interpreting the signal and modifying its behavior accordingly Signals are distinct from cues in that evolution has selected for signalling between both parties whereas cues are merely informative to the observer and may not have originally been used for the intended purpose The natural world is replete with examples of signals from the luminescent flashes of light from fireflies to chemical signaling in red harvester ants to prominent mating displays of birds such as the Guianan cock of the rock which gather in leks the pheromones released by the corn earworm moth 131 the dancing patterns of the blue footed booby or the alarm sound Synoeca cyanea make by rubbing their mandibles against their nest 132 Yet other examples are the cases of the grizzled skipper and Spodoptera littoralis where pheromones are released as a sexual recognition mechanism that drives evolution 133 134 In a type of mating signal male orb weaving spiders of the species Zygiella x notata pluck the signal thread of a female s web with their forelegs This performance conveys vibratory signals informing the female spider of the male s presence 135 The nature of communication poses evolutionary concerns such as the potential for deceit or manipulation on the part of the sender In this situation the receiver must be able to anticipate the interests of the sender and act appropriately to a given signal Should any side gain advantage in the short term evolution would select against the signal or the response The conflict of interests between the sender and the receiver results in an evolutionarily stable state only if both sides can derive an overall benefit Although the potential benefits of deceit could be great in terms of mating success there are several possibilities for how dishonesty is controlled which include indices handicaps and common interests Indices are reliable indicators of a desirable quality such as overall health fertility or fighting ability of the organism Handicaps as the term suggests place a restrictive cost on the organisms that own them and thus lower quality competitors experience a greater relative cost compared to their higher quality counterparts In the common interest situation it is beneficial to both sender and receiver to communicate honestly such that the benefit of the interaction is maximized Signals are often honest but there are exceptions Prime examples of dishonest signals include the luminescent lure of the anglerfish which is used to attract prey or the mimicry of non poisonous butterfly species like the Batesian mimic Papilio polyxenes of the poisonous model Battus philenor 136 Although evolution should normally favor selection against the dishonest signal in these cases it appears that the receiver would benefit more on average by accepting the signal See also EditAutonomous foraging Behavioral plasticity Evolutionary models of food sharing Gene centered view of evolution Human behavioral ecology Life history theory Marginal value theorem Optimization Mating effort Parental effort Phylogenetic comparative methods Selection Balancing selection Directional selection Disruptive selection Stabilizing selection r K selection theory Somatic effortReferences Edit Maynard Smith J 1982 Evolution and the Theory of Games Brown Jerram June 1964 The evolution of diversity in avian territorial systems The Wilson Bulletin 76 2 160 169 JSTOR 4159278 Gill Frank Larry Wolf 1975 Economics of feeding territoriality in the golden winged sunbird Ecology 56 2 333 345 doi 10 2307 1934964 JSTOR 1934964 Davies N B A I Houston Feb 1981 Owners and satellites the economics of territory defence in the pied wagtail Motacilla alba Journal of Animal Ecology 50 1 157 180 doi 10 2307 4038 JSTOR 4038 Fretwell Stephen D 1972 Population in a Seasonal Environment Princeton NJ Princeton University Press Milinski Manfred 1979 An Evolutionarily Stable Feeding Strategy in Sticklebacks Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 51 1 36 40 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 1979 tb00669 x Dominey Wallace 1984 Alternative Mating Tactics and Evolutionarily Stable Strategies American Zoology 24 2 385 396 doi 10 1093 icb 24 2 385 Arak Anthony 1983 Sexual selection by male male competition in natterjack toad choruses Nature 306 5940 261 262 Bibcode 1983Natur 306 261A doi 10 1038 306261a0 S2CID 4363873 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u Nicholas B Davies John R Krebs Stuart A West 2012 An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology West Sussex UK Wiley Blackwell pp 193 202 ISBN 978 1 4051 1416 5 Buchanan K L Catchpole C K 2000 Song as an indicator of male parental effort in the sedge warbler Proceedings of the Royal Society B 267 1441 321 326 doi 10 1098 rspb 2000 1003 PMC 1690544 PMID 10722211 van Swaay C Wynhoff I Verovnik R Wiemers M Lopez Munguira M Maes D Sasic M Verstrael T Warren M Settele J 2010 Hipparchia semele IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2010 e T173254A6980554 doi 10 2305 IUCN UK 2010 1 RLTS T173254A6980554 en Retrieved 13 November 2021 Dussourd D E Harvis C A Meinwald J Eisner T 1991 Pheromonal advertisement of a nuptial gift by a male moth Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 88 20 9224 9227 Bibcode 1991PNAS 88 9224D doi 10 1073 pnas 88 20 9224 PMC 52686 PMID 1924385 Steele RH 1986 Courtship feeding in Drosophila subobscura 2 Courtship feeding by males influences female mate choice Animal Behaviour 34 1099 1108 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 86 80169 5 S2CID 53156432 a b Ryan Michael J Anne Keddy Hector March 1992 Directional patterns of female mate choice and the role of sensory biases The American Naturalist 139 S4 S35 doi 10 1086 285303 JSTOR 2462426 S2CID 86383459 Zuiderwijk Annie Sparreboom Max 1986 01 01 Territorial Behaviour in Crested Newt Triturus Cristatus and Marbled Newt T Marmoratus Amphibia Urodela Bijdragen tot de Dierkunde 56 2 205 213 doi 10 1163 26660644 05602002 ISSN 0067 8546 Salzburger Walter Niederstatter Harald Brandstatter Anita Berger Burkhard Parson Walther Snoeks Jos Sturmbauer Christian 2006 Colour assortative mating among populations of Tropheus moorii a cichlid fish from Lake Tanganyika East Africa Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 273 1584 257 66 doi 10 1098 rspb 2005 3321 PMC 1560039 PMID 16543167 Steinwender Bernd Koblmuller Stephan Sefc Kristina M 2011 Concordant female mate preferences in the cichlid fish Tropheus moorii Hydrobiologia 682 1 121 130 doi 10 1007 s10750 011 0766 5 PMC 3841713 PMID 24293682 Boughman J W 2002 How sensory drive can promote speciation Trends in Ecology and Evolution 17 12 571 577 doi 10 1016 S0169 5347 02 02595 8 Rodd F H Hughes K A Grether G F Baril C T 2002 A possible non sexual origin of mate preference are male guppies mimicking fruit Proceedings of the Royal Society B 269 1490 475 481 doi 10 1098 rspb 2001 1891 PMC 1690917 PMID 11886639 a b c d e f g Proctor Heather C 1991 10 01 Courtship in the water mite Neumania papillator males capitalize on female adaptations for predation Animal Behaviour 42 4 589 598 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 05 80242 8 S2CID 53166756 a b Proctor Heather C 1992 10 01 Sensory exploitation and the evolution of male mating behaviour a cladistic test using water mites Acari Parasitengona Animal Behaviour 44 4 745 752 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 05 80300 8 S2CID 54426553 Proctor H C 1992 01 01 Effect of Food Deprivation on Mate Searching and Spermatophore Production in Male Water Mites Acari Unionicolidae Functional Ecology 6 6 661 665 doi 10 2307 2389961 JSTOR 2389961 Alcock John 2013 07 01 Animal Behaviour A Evolutionary Approach 10th ed Sinauer pp 70 72 ISBN 9780878939664 Jones I L Hunter F M 1998 Heterospecific mating preferences for a feather ornament in least auklets Behavioral Ecology 9 2 187 192 doi 10 1093 beheco 9 2 187 McClinktock W J Uetz G W 1996 Female choice and pre existing bias Visual cues during courtship in two Schizocosawolff spiders Animal Behaviour 52 167 181 doi 10 1006 anbe 1996 0162 S2CID 24629559 Prum R O 1996 Phylogenetic tests of alternative intersexual selection mechanisms Trait macroevolution in a polygynous clade The American Naturalist 149 4 688 692 doi 10 1086 286014 JSTOR 2463543 S2CID 85039370 Fuller R C Houle D Travis J 2005 Sensory bias as an explanation for the evolution of mate preferences American Naturalist 166 4 437 446 doi 10 1086 444443 PMID 16224700 S2CID 4849390 Parker G A 2006 Sexual conflict over mating and fertilization An overview Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 361 1466 235 259 doi 10 1098 rstb 2005 1785 PMC 1569603 PMID 16612884 a b c d e Davies N Krebs J and West S 2012 An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology 4th Ed Wiley Blackwell Oxford pp 209 220 ISBN missing Parker G 1979 Sexual selection and sexual conflict In Sexual Selection and Reproductive Competition in Insects eds M S Blum and N A Blum Academic Press New York pp 123 166 ISBN missing Chapman T et al 2003 Sexual Selection Trends in Ecology and Evolution 18 41 47 doi 10 1016 s0169 5347 02 00004 6 a b c Baker R R 1972 Territorial behaviour of the Nymphalid butterflies Aglais urticae L and Inachis io L Journal of Animal Ecology 41 2 453 469 doi 10 2307 3480 JSTOR 3480 Skandalis Dimitri A Tattersall Glenn J Prager Sean Richards Miriam H 2009 Body Size and Shape of the Large Carpenter Bee Xylocopa virginica L Hymenoptera Apidae Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 82 1 30 42 doi 10 2317 JKES711 05 1 S2CID 73520512 Blanckenhorn W U Muhlhauser C Morf C Reusch T Reuter M 2000 Female choice female reluctance to mate and sexual selection on body size in the dung fly Sepsis cynipsea Ethology 106 7 577 593 doi 10 1046 j 1439 0310 2000 00573 x Bonduriansky Russell 2006 Convergent evolution of sexual shape dimorphism in Diptera Journal of Morphology 267 5 602 611 doi 10 1002 jmor 10426 ISSN 1097 4687 PMID 16477603 S2CID 15548020 Thornhill R 1980 Rape in Panorpa scorpionflies and a general rape hypothesis Animal Behaviour 28 52 59 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 80 80007 8 S2CID 53167007 Birkhead T and Moller A 1992 Sperm Competition in Birds Evolutionary Causes and Consequences Academic Press London ISBN missing page needed Carroll Allan L 1994 Interactions between body size and mating history influence the reproductive success of males of a tortricid moth Zeiraphera canadensis Canadian Journal of Zoology 72 12 2124 2132 doi 10 1139 z94 284 Shepard Jon Guppy Crispin 2011 Butterflies of British Columbia Including Western Alberta Southern Yukon the Alaska Panhandle Washington Northern Oregon Northern Idaho and Northwestern Montana UBC Press ISBN 9780774844376 Retrieved 13 November 2017 Schulz Stefan Estrada Catalina Yildizhan Selma Boppre Michael Gilbert Lawrence E 2008 01 01 An antiaphrodisiac in Heliconius melpomene butterflies Journal of Chemical Ecology 34 1 82 93 doi 10 1007 s10886 007 9393 z ISSN 0098 0331 PMID 18080165 S2CID 22090974 Pizzari T Birkhead T 2000 Female feral fowl eject sperm of subdominant males Nature 405 6788 787 789 Bibcode 2000Natur 405 787P doi 10 1038 35015558 PMID 10866198 S2CID 4406792 a b Clutton Brock T H 1991 The Evolution of Parental Care Princeton NJ Princeton University Press Wcislo W T Wille A Orozco E 1993 Nesting biology of tropical solitary and social sweat bees Lasioglossum Dialictus figueresi Wcislo and L D aeneiventre Friese Hymenoptera Halictidae Insectes Sociaux 40 21 40 doi 10 1007 BF01338830 S2CID 6867760 Daly M 1979 Why Don t Male Mammals Lactate Journal of Theoretical Biology 78 3 325 345 Bibcode 1979JThBi 78 325D doi 10 1016 0022 5193 79 90334 5 PMID 513786 Gross M R R C Sargent 1985 The evolution of male and female parentental care in fishes American Zoologist 25 3 807 822 doi 10 1093 icb 25 3 807 Foote Chris J Brown Gayle S Hawryshyn Craig W 1 January 2004 Female colour and male choice in sockeye salmon implications for the phenotypic convergence of anadromous and nonanadromous morphs Animal Behaviour 67 1 69 83 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2003 02 004 S2CID 53169458 Svensson Magnhagen C Jul 1998 Parental behavior in relation to the occurrence of sneaking in the common goby Animal Behaviour 56 1 175 179 doi 10 1006 anbe 1998 0769 PMID 9710475 S2CID 24806138 Sturmbauer Christian Corinna Fuchs Georg Harb Elisabeth Damm Nina Duftner Michaela Maderbacher Martin Koch Stephan Koblmuller 2008 Abundance Distribution and Territory Areas of Rock dwelling Lake Tanganyika Cichlid Fish Species Hydrobiologia 615 1 57 68 doi 10 1007 s10750 008 9557 z S2CID 23056207 Retrieved 30 September 2013 Johnstone R A Hinde C A 2006 Negotiation over offspring care how should parents respond to each other s efforts Behavioral Ecology 17 5 818 827 doi 10 1093 beheco arl009 a b Bonduriansky Russell Wheeler Jill Rowe Locke 2005 02 01 Ejaculate feeding and female fitness in the sexually dimorphic fly Prochyliza xanthostoma Diptera Piophilidae Animal Behaviour 69 2 489 497 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2004 03 018 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 16357692 Amundsen T Slagsvold T 1996 Lack s Brood Reduction Hypothesis and Avian Hatching Asynchrony What s Next Oikos 76 3 613 620 doi 10 2307 3546359 JSTOR 3546359 Pijanowski B C 1992 A Revision of Lack s Brood Reduction Hypothesis The American Naturalist 139 6 1270 1292 doi 10 1086 285386 S2CID 84884060 Trivers Robert L Willard Dan E 1976 Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring Science 179 191 90 92 Bibcode 1973Sci 179 90T doi 10 1126 science 179 4068 90 PMID 4682135 S2CID 29326420 Bourke A F G amp F L W Ratnieks 2001 Kin selected conflict in the bumble bee Bombus terrestris Hymenoptera Apidae Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 268 1465 347 355 doi 10 1098 rspb 2000 1381 PMC 1088613 PMID 11270430 Haig D Graham C 1991 Genomic imprinting and the strange case of the insulin like growth factor II receptor Cell 64 6 1045 1046 doi 10 1016 0092 8674 91 90256 x PMID 1848481 S2CID 33682126 Kilner R M 2001 A Growth Cost of Begging in Captive Canary Chicks Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 98 20 11394 11398 Bibcode 2001PNAS 9811394K doi 10 1073 pnas 191221798 PMC 58740 PMID 11572988 Kolliker M Brinkhof M Heeb P Fitze P Richner H 2000 The Quantitative Genetic Basis of Offspring Solicitation and Parental Response in a Passerine Bird with Parental Care Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 267 1457 2127 2132 doi 10 1098 rspb 2000 1259 PMC 1690782 PMID 11416919 Trillmitch F Wolf J B W 2008 Parent offspring and sibling conflict in Galapagos fur seals and sea lions Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62 3 363 375 doi 10 1007 s00265 007 0423 1 S2CID 41834534 a b Drummond H Chavelas C G 1989 Food shortage influences sibling sggression in the Blue footed Booby Animal Behaviour 37 806 819 doi 10 1016 0003 3472 89 90065 1 S2CID 53165189 Briskie James V Naugler Christopher T Leech Susan M 1994 Begging intensity of nestling birds varies with sibling relatedness Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 258 1351 73 78 Bibcode 1994RSPSB 258 73B doi 10 1098 rspb 1994 0144 S2CID 85105883 Spottiswoode C N Stevens M 2010 Visual modelling shows that avian host parents use multiple visual cues in rejecting parasitic eggs Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107 19 8672 8676 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107 8672S doi 10 1073 pnas 0910486107 PMC 2889299 PMID 20421497 Kliner R M Madden Joah R Hauber Mark E 2004 Brood parasitic cowbird nestlings use host young to procure resources Science 305 5685 877 879 Bibcode 2004Sci 305 877K doi 10 1126 science 1098487 PMID 15297677 S2CID 30084518 Thomas J A Settele Josef 2004 Butterfly mimics of ants Nature 432 7015 283 284 Bibcode 2004Natur 432 283T doi 10 1038 432283a PMID 15549080 S2CID 4379000 Davies N B 2011 Cuckoo adaptations trickery and tuning Journal of Zoology 281 1 14 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7998 2011 00810 x Tanaka K D Ueda K 2005 Horsfield s hawk cuckoo nestlings simulate multiple gapes for begging Science 308 5722 653 doi 10 1126 science 1109957 PMID 15860618 S2CID 42439025 a b Akino T J J Knapp J A Thomas G W Elmes 1999 Chemical mimicry and host specificity in the butterfly Maculinea rebeli a social parasite of Myrmica ant colonies Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 266 1427 1419 1426 doi 10 1098 rspb 1999 0796 PMC 1690087 a b Thomas Jeremy Karsten Schonrogge Simona Bonelli Francesca Barbero Emilio Balletto 2010 Corruption of ant acoustical signals by mimetic social parasites Communicative and Integrative Biology 3 2 169 171 doi 10 4161 cib 3 2 10603 PMC 2889977 PMID 20585513 Dapporto L Cervo R Sledge M F Turillazzi S 2004 Rank integration in dominance hierarchies of host colonies by the paper wasp social parasite Polistes sulcifer Hymenoptera Vespidae Journal of Insect Physiology 50 2 3 217 223 doi 10 1016 j jinsphys 2003 11 012 PMID 15019524 Kreuter Kirsten Bunk Elfi 2011 How the social parasitic bumblebee Bombus bohemicus sneaks into power of reproduction Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 66 3 475 486 doi 10 1007 s00265 011 1294 z S2CID 7124725 Cameron Sydney A Ramirez Santiago 2001 07 01 Nest Architecture and Nesting Ecology of the Orchid Bee Eulaema meriana Hymenoptera Apinae Euglossini Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society 74 3 142 165 JSTOR 25086012 Evans Howard E 1973 Burrow sharing and nest transfer in the digger wasp Philanthus gibbosus Fabricius Animal Behaviour 21 2 302 308 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 73 80071 5 Reed H C Akre R D Garnett W B 1979 A North American Host of the Yellowjacket Social Parasite Vespula austriaca Panzer Hymenoptera Vespidae Entomological News 90 2 110 113 Gjershaug Jan Ove 2009 The social parasite bumblebee Bombus hyperboreus Schonherr 1809 usurp nest of Bombus balteatus Dahlbom 1832 Hymenoptera Apidae in Norway Norwegian Journal of Entomology 56 1 28 31 Emlen S T Oring L W 1977 Ecology sexual selection and the evolution of mating systems Science 197 4300 214 223 Bibcode 1977Sci 197 215E doi 10 1126 science 327542 PMID 327542 S2CID 16786432 a b c d e f g Davies N B Krebs J R and West S A 2012 An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology 4th ed John Wiley amp Sons pp 254 263 ISBN missing Warner R R 1987 Female choice of sites versus mates in a coral reef fish Thalassoma bifasciatum Animal Behaviour 35 5 1470 1478 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 87 80019 2 S2CID 53177154 Ims R A 1987 Responses in spatial organization and behavior to manipulations of the food resource in the vole Clethrionomys rufocanus Journal of Animal Ecology 56 2 585 596 doi 10 2307 5070 JSTOR 5070 Kimsey Lynn Siri 1980 The behaviour of male orchid bees Apidae Hymenoptera Insecta and the question of leks Animal Behaviour 28 4 996 1004 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 80 80088 1 S2CID 53161684 Bradbury J E and Gibson R M 1983 Leks and mate choice In Mate Choice ed P Bateson pp 109 138 Cambridge University Press Cambridge ISBN missing Moore Sandra D 1987 Male Biased Mortality in the Butterfly Euphydryas editha a Novel Cost of Mate Acquisition The American Naturalist 130 2 306 309 doi 10 1086 284711 S2CID 84989304 Lack D 1968 Ecological Adaptations for Breeding in Birds Methuen London Davies N B Krebs J R and West S A 2012 An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology West Sussex UK Wiley Blackwell p 266 ISBN 978 1 4051 1416 5 Lightbody J P Weatherhead P J 1988 Female settling patterns and polygyny tests of a neutral mate choice hypothesis American Naturalist 132 20 33 doi 10 1086 284835 S2CID 84147769 Verner J Wilson M F 1966 The influence of habitats on mating systems of North American passerine birds Ecology 47 1 143 147 doi 10 2307 1935753 JSTOR 1935753 Gowaty P A 1981 An extension of the Orians Verner Willson model to account for mating systems besides polygyny American Naturalist 118 6 851 859 doi 10 1086 283875 S2CID 83991131 Faaborg J Parker P G DeLay L et al 1995 Confirmation of cooperative polyandry in the Galapagos hawk Buteo galapagoensis Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 36 2 83 90 doi 10 1007 bf00170712 S2CID 3334592 Beissinger S R Snyder N F R 1987 Mate desertion in the snail kite Animal Behaviour 35 2 477 487 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 87 80273 7 S2CID 53192930 Reynolds Butchart S H M Seddon N Ekstrom J M M 1999b Yelling for sex harem males compete for female access in bronze winged jacanas Animal Behaviour 57 3 637 646 doi 10 1006 anbe 1998 0985 PMID 10196054 S2CID 24253395 a b c d e f Davies Nicholas B Krebs John R West Stuart A 2012 An Introduction to Behavioral Ecology West Sussex UK Wiley Blackwell pp 307 333 ISBN 978 1 4051 1416 5 Bergstrom Theodore Spring 2002 Evolution of Social Behavior Individual and Group Selection The Journal of Economic Perspectives 16 2 67 88 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 377 5059 doi 10 1257 0895330027265 JSTOR 2696497 S2CID 125841 Smith J M 1964 Group Selection and Kin Selection Nature 201 4924 1145 1147 Bibcode 1964Natur 201 1145S doi 10 1038 2011145a0 S2CID 4177102 Fisher R A 1930 The Genetical Theory of Natural Selection Oxford Clarendon Press Haldane J B S 1932 The Causes of Evolution London Longmans Green amp Co Haldane J B S 1955 Population Genetics New Biology 18 34 51 a b Hamilton W D 1963 The evolution of altruistic behavior American Naturalist 97 896 354 356 doi 10 1086 497114 S2CID 84216415 a b Hamilton W D 1964 The Genetical Evolution of Social Behavior Journal of Theoretical Biology 7 1 1 16 Bibcode 1964JThBi 7 1H doi 10 1016 0022 5193 64 90038 4 PMID 5875341 West S A Griffin A S Gardner A 2007b Social semantics altruism cooperation mutualism strong reciprocity and group selection Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20 2 415 432 doi 10 1111 j 1420 9101 2006 01258 x PMID 17305808 S2CID 1792464 Mehdiabadi N J C N Jack T T Farnham et al 2006 Kin preference in a social microbe Nature 442 7105 881 882 Bibcode 2006Natur 442 881M doi 10 1038 442881a PMID 16929288 S2CID 4335666 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint uses authors parameter link Benabentos R S Hirose R Sucgang et al 2009 Polymorphic members of the lag gene family mediate kin discrimination in Dictyostelium Current Biology 19 7 567 572 doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 02 037 PMC 2694408 PMID 19285397 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint uses authors parameter link Greenberg Les 1988 07 01 Kin recognition in the sweat bee Lasioglossum zephyrum Behavior Genetics 18 4 425 438 doi 10 1007 BF01065512 ISSN 0001 8244 PMID 3190637 S2CID 44298800 Nunes T M Turatti I C C Mateus S Nascimento F S Lopes N P Zucchi R 2009 Cuticular hydrocarbons in the stingless bee Schwarziana quadripunctata Hymenoptera Apidae Meliponini Differences between colonies castes and age Genetics and Molecular Research 8 2 589 595 doi 10 4238 vol8 2kerr012 PMID 19551647 Buchwald Robert Breed Michael D 2005 Nestmate recognition cues in a stingless bee Trigona fulviventris Animal Behaviour 70 6 1331 1337 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2005 03 017 S2CID 53147658 Seidelmann Karsten 2006 09 01 Open cell parasitism shapes maternal investment patterns in the red mason bee Osmia rufa Behavioral Ecology 17 5 839 848 doi 10 1093 beheco arl017 ISSN 1045 2249 Davies N B amp M de L Brooke 1988 Cuckoos versus reed warblers Adaptations and counteradaptations Animal Behaviour 36 1 262 284 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 88 80269 0 S2CID 53191651 Holmes W G amp P W Sherman 1982 The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels American Zoologist 22 3 491 517 doi 10 1093 icb 22 3 491 Bennet Clark H C Leroy Y Tsacas L 1980 02 01 Species and sex specific songs and courtship behaviour in the genus Zaprionus Diptera Drosophilidae Animal Behaviour 28 1 230 255 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 80 80027 3 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 53194769 Gloag R et al 2008 Nest defence in a stingless bee What causes fighting swarms in Trigona carbonaria Hymenoptera Meliponini Insectes Sociaux 55 4 387 391 doi 10 1007 s00040 008 1018 1 S2CID 44720135 Wilkinson G S 1984 Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat Nature 308 5955 181 184 Bibcode 1984Natur 308 181W doi 10 1038 308181a0 S2CID 4354558 O Brien Timothy G amp John G Robinson 1991 Allomaternal Care by Female Wedge Capped Capuchin Monkeys Effects of Age Rank and Relatedness Behaviour 119 1 2 30 50 doi 10 1163 156853991X00355 Ratnieks Francis L W Heikki Helantera October 2009 The evolution of extreme altruism and inequality in insect societies Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364 1553 3169 3179 doi 10 1098 rstb 2009 0129 PMC 2781879 PMID 19805425 de Waal Frans 2016 Are We Smart Enough To Know How Smart Animals Are ISBN 978 1 78378 305 2 p 276 Postgate J 1998 Nitrogen Fixation 3rd Edition Cambridge University Press Cambridge UK a b Dawkins Richard 1976 The Selfish Gene Oxford University Press M C Soares I M Cote S C Cardoso amp R Bshary August 2008 The cleaning goby mutualism a system without punishment partner switching or tactile stimulation PDF Journal of Zoology 276 3 306 312 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7998 2008 00489 x Crair Ben 1 August 2017 The Secret Economic Lives of Animals Bloomberg News Retrieved 1 August 2017 Foster Kevin Tom Wenseleers Francis L W Ratnieks 10 September 2001 Spite Hamilton s unproven theory PDF Annales Zoologici Fennici 229 238 Strassmann Joan E David C Queller May 2007 Insect societies as divided organisms The complexities of purpose and cross purpose PNAS 104 suppl 1 8619 8626 Bibcode 2007PNAS 104 8619S doi 10 1073 pnas 0701285104 PMC 1876438 PMID 17494750 Duffy Emmett J Cheryl L Morrison Kenneth S Macdonald April 2002 Colony defense and behavioral differentiation in the eusocial shrimp Synalpheus regalis Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 51 5 488 495 doi 10 1007 s00265 002 0455 5 S2CID 25384748 Foster W A December 1990 Experimental evidence for effective and altruistic colony defence against natural predators by soldiers of the gall forming aphid Pemphigus spyrothecae Hemiptera Pemphigidae Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 27 6 421 430 doi 10 1007 BF00164069 S2CID 37559224 Jarvis Jennifer U M Nigel C Bennett Andrew C Spinks January 1998 Food availability and foraging by wild colonies of Damaraland mole rats Cryptomys damarensis implications for sociality Oecologia 113 2 290 298 Bibcode 1998Oecol 113 290J doi 10 1007 s004420050380 PMID 28308209 S2CID 20472674 Bonckaert W Tofilski A Nascimento F S Billen J Ratnieks F L W Wenseleers T 2001 Co occurrence of three types of egg policing in the Norwegian wasp Dolichovespsula wasp Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65 4 633 640 doi 10 1007 s00265 010 1064 3 S2CID 2186614 Wenseleers Tom Heikki Helantera Adam G Hart Francis L W Ratnieks May 2004 Worker reproduction and policing in insect societies an ESS analysis Journal of Evolutionary Biology 17 5 1035 1047 doi 10 1111 j 1420 9101 2004 00751 x PMID 15312076 Foster Kevin R 2001 Colony kin structure and male production in Dolichovespula wasps Molecular Ecology 10 4 1003 1010 doi 10 1046 j 1365 294X 2001 01228 x PMID 11348506 S2CID 12009153 Vespula vulgaris Defensive behaviors Andrew F G Bourke 1999 Sex allocation in a facultatively polygynous ant between population and between colony variation Behavioral Ecology 10 4 409 421 doi 10 1093 beheco 10 4 409 Jurgen Heinze Lipski Norbert Schlehmeyer Kathrin Hōlldobler Bert 1994 Colony structure and reproduction in the ant Leptothorax acervorum Behavioral Ecology 6 4 359 367 doi 10 1093 beheco 6 4 359 Ratnieks Francis L W P Kirk Visscher December 1989 Worker policing in the honeybee Nature 342 6251 796 797 Bibcode 1989Natur 342 796R doi 10 1038 342796a0 S2CID 4366903 Gobin Bruno J Billen C Peeters November 1999 Policing behaviour towards virgin egg layers in a polygynous ponerine ant Animal Behaviour 58 5 1117 1122 doi 10 1006 anbe 1999 1245 PMID 10564615 S2CID 16428974 Boomsma J J 21 August 2007 Kin selection versus sexual selection why the ends to not meet Current Biology 17 16 R673 R683 doi 10 1016 j cub 2007 06 033 PMID 17714661 S2CID 886746 Raina Ashok K Klun Jerome A 1984 Brain factor control of sex pheromone production in the female corn earworm moth Science 225 4661 531 533 Bibcode 1984Sci 225 531R doi 10 1126 science 225 4661 531 PMID 17750856 S2CID 40949867 O Donnell Sean 1997 Gaster Flagging during Colony Defense in Neotropical Swarm Founding Wasps Hymenoptera Vespidae Epiponini Journal of the Kansas Entomological Society Hernandez Roldan Juan L Bofill Roger Dapporto Leonardo Munguira Miguel L Vila Roger 2014 09 01 Morphological and chemical analysis of male scent organs in the butterfly genus Pyrgus Lepidoptera Hesperiidae Organisms Diversity amp Evolution 14 3 269 278 doi 10 1007 s13127 014 0170 x ISSN 1439 6092 S2CID 15709135 Silverstein Germund Lofstedt Christer Rosen Wen Qi 2005 Circadian mating activity and effect of pheromone pre exposure on pheromone response rhythms in the moth Spodoptera littoralis Journal of Insect Physiology 51 3 277 286 doi 10 1016 j jinsphys 2004 11 013 PMID 15749110 Tarsitano Michael Wolfgang Kirchner 2001 Vibrational courtship signals of Zygiella x notata PDF Bulletin of the British Arachnological Society 12 26 32 Lederhouse Robert C Silvio G Codella Jr 1989 Intersexual Comparison of Mimetic Protection in the Black Swallowtail Butterfly Papilio polyxenes Experiments with Captive Blue Jay Predators Evolution 43 2 410 420 doi 10 2307 2409216 JSTOR 2409216 PMID 28568560 Further reading EditAlcock J 2009 Animal Behavior An Evolutionary Approach 9th edition Sinauer Associates Inc Sunderland MA ISBN missing Bateson P 2017 Behaviour Development and Evolution Open Book Publishers doi 10 11647 OBP 0097 Danchin E Girladeau L A and Cezilly F 2008 Behavioural Ecology An Evolutionary Perspective on Behaviour Oxford University Press Oxford ISBN missing Krebs J R and Davies N An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology ISBN 0 632 03546 3 Krebs J R and Davies N Behavioural Ecology An Evolutionary Approach ISBN 0 86542 731 3 Wajnberg E Bernstein E and van Alphen E 2008 Behavioral Ecology of Insect Parasitoids From Theoretical Approaches to Field Applications Blackwell Publishing ISBN missing External links Edit Media related to Behavioral ecology at Wikimedia Commons Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Behavioral ecology amp oldid 1125586754, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.