fbpx
Wikipedia

Bottlenose dolphin

Bottlenose dolphins are aquatic mammals in the genus Tursiops. They are common, cosmopolitan members of the family Delphinidae, the family of oceanic dolphins.[2] Molecular studies show the genus definitively contains two species: the common bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops aduncus).[3] Others, like the Burrunan dolphin (Tursiops (aduncus) australis), may be alternately considered their own species or be subspecies of T. aduncus.[4] Bottlenose dolphins inhabit warm and temperate seas worldwide, being found everywhere except for the Arctic and Antarctic Circle regions. Their name derives from the Latin tursio (dolphin) and truncatus for their characteristic truncated teeth.[5]

Bottlenose dolphin
Temporal range: Miocene–Recent 3.6–0 Ma
[1]
Bottlenose dolphin surfing in the wake of a boat
Size compared to an average human
Scientific classification
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Mammalia
Order: Artiodactyla
Infraorder: Cetacea
Family: Delphinidae
Subfamily: Delphininae
Genus: Tursiops
Gervais, 1855
Type species
Delphinus truncatus
Montagu, 1821
Species
Bottlenose dolphin range (in blue)

Numerous investigations of bottlenose dolphin intelligence have been conducted, examining mimicry, use of artificial language, object categorization, and self-recognition. They can use tools (sponging; using marine sponges to forage for food sources they normally could not access)[6] and transmit cultural knowledge from generation to generation, and their considerable intelligence has driven interaction with humans. Bottlenose dolphins gained popularity from aquarium shows and television programs such as Flipper. They have also been trained by militaries to locate sea mines or detect and mark enemy divers. In some areas, they cooperate with local fishermen by driving fish into their nets and eating the fish that escape. Some encounters with humans are harmful to the dolphins: people hunt them for food, and dolphins are killed inadvertently as a bycatch of tuna fishing and by getting caught in crab traps.

Bottlenose dolphins have the third largest encephalization levels of any mammal on Earth (humans have the largest, followed by Northern Right whale dolphins), sharing close ratios with those of humans and other cetaceans, while being twice as high of other great apes.[7] This more than likely contributes to their high intelligence and emotional intelligence.[8]

Taxonomy

Scientists have been long aware of the fact that the Tursiops dolphins might consist of more than one species, as there is extensive variation in color and morphology along its range. In the past, most studies used morphology to evaluate differences between and within species, but in the late 20th century, combining morphological and molecular genetics allowed much greater insight into this previously intractable problem.[9] Since the late 1990s and early 2000s, most researchers acknowledged the existence of two species:[10] the common bottlenose dolphin (T. truncatus), found in coastal and oceanic habitats of most tropical to temperate oceans, and the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin (T. aduncus), that lives in coastal waters around India, northern Australia, South China, the Red Sea, and the eastern coast of Africa.

In 2011, a third distinct species was described, the Burrunan dolphin (T. (aduncus) australis), found in the Port Phillip and Gippsland Lakes areas of Victoria, Australia, after research showed it was distinct from T. truncatus and T. aduncus, both in morphology[11] and genetics.[12] Also, evidence has been accumulating to validate the existence of a separate species, Lahille's bottlenose dolphin, T. gephyreus,[13][14][15][16] that occurs in coastal waters of Argentina, Uruguay and southern Brazil. The Society for Marine Mammalogy's Committee on Taxonomy,[17] presently recognizes only two species, T. truncatus and T. aduncus, and two subspecies: the Black Sea bottlenose dolphin (T. t. ponticus), that lives in the Black Sea, and Lahille's bottlenose dolphin (T. t. gephyreus). Other sources also accept the Pacific bottlenose dolphin (T. t. gillii or T. gillii),[18] that inhabits the Pacific, and has a black line from the eye to the forehead.[19][failed verification] The IUCN, on their Red List of endangered species, also recognises only two species of bottlenose dolphins.[20] The American Society of Mammalogists also recognizes only two species; while acknowledging the studies describing T. australis, it classifies it within T. aduncus.[4]

 
Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin, T. aduncus

Much of the discussion and doubts about its taxonomy is related to the existence of two ecotypes of bottlenose dolphins in many part of its distribution. The two ecotypes of the common bottlenose dolphin within the western North Atlantic[21] are represented by the shallower water or coastal ecotype and the more offshore ecotype.[21] Their ranges overlap, but they have been shown to be genetically distinct.[21] They are not currently described, however, as separate species or subspecies. In general, genetic variation between populations is significant, even among nearby populations.[22] As a result of this genetic variation, other distinct species currently considered to be populations of common bottlenose dolphin are possible.[22]

Some recent genetic evidence suggests the Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin belongs in the genus Stenella, since it is more like the Atlantic spotted dolphin (Stenella frontalis) than the common bottlenose dolphin.[23] However, more recent studies indicate that this is a consequence of reticulate evolution (such as past hybridization between Stenella and ancestral Tursiops) and incomplete lineage sorting, and thus support T. truncatus and T. aduncus belonging to the same genus.[24]

 
Wolphin Kawili'Kai at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii
 
The fossil species Tursiops osennae

Hybrids

Bottlenose dolphins have been known to hybridize with other dolphin species. Hybrids with Risso's dolphin occur both in the wild and in captivity.[25][26] The best known hybrid is the wholphin, a false killer whale-bottlenose dolphin hybrid. The wholphin is fertile, and two currently live at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii. The first was born in 1985 to a female bottlenose. Wholphins also exist in the wild.[27] In captivity, a bottlenose dolphin and a rough-toothed dolphin hybridized.[28] A common dolphin-bottlenose dolphin hybrid born in captivity lives at SeaWorld California.[29][30] Other hybrids live in captivity around the world and in the wild, such as a bottlenose dolphin-Atlantic spotted dolphin hybrid.[31]

Fossil species

Bottlenose dolphins appeared during the Miocene.[32] Known fossil species include Tursiops osennae (late Miocene to early Pliocene)[33] from the Piacenzian coastal mudstone, and Tursiops miocaenus (Miocene) from the Burdigalian marine sandstone, all in Italy.[1]

Description

The bottlenose dolphin weighs an average of 300 kg (660 pounds), but can range from 150 and 650 kg (330 and 1,430 lb).[34] It can reach a length of just over 4 meters (13 feet). Its color varies considerably, is usually dark gray on the back and lighter gray on the flanks, but it can be bluish-grey, brownish-grey, or even nearly black, and is often darker on the back from the rostrum to behind the dorsal fin. This is called countershading and is a form of camouflage. Older dolphins sometimes have a few spots.

Bottlenose dolphins can live for more than 40 years. Females typically live 5–10 years longer than males, with some females exceeding 60 years.[35][36][37] This extreme age is rare and less than 2% of all Bottlenose dolphins will live longer than 60 years.[38] Bottlenose dolphins can jump to a height of 6 metres (20 feet) in the air.[39]

Anatomy

Their elongated upper and lower jaws form what is called a rostrum, or snout, which gives the animal its common name.[40] The real, functional nose is the blowhole on top of its head; the nasal septum is visible when the blowhole is open.[35]

 
Bottlenose dolphin head, showing rostrum and blowhole

Bottlenose dolphins have 18 to 28 conical teeth on each side of each jaw.[35][40]

The flukes (lobes of the tail) and dorsal fin are formed of dense connective tissue and do not contain bone or muscle. The dorsal fin usually shows phenotypic variations that help discriminate among populations.[41] The animal propels itself by moving the flukes up and down. The pectoral flippers (at the sides of the body) are for steering; they contain bones homologous to the forelimbs of land mammals. A bottlenose dolphin discovered in Japan has two additional pectoral fins, or "hind legs", at the tail, about the size of a human's pair of hands. Scientists believe a mutation caused the ancient trait to reassert itself as a form of atavism.[42]

Physiology and senses

 
Dolphin and a paddler at Dalkey Island

In colder waters, they have more body fat and blood, and are more suited to deeper diving. Typically, 18%–20% of their body weight is blubber.[43] Most research in this area has been restricted to the North Atlantic Ocean.[44] Bottlenose dolphins typically swim at 5 to 11 km/h (1.4 to 3.1 m/s), but are capable of bursts of up to 29 to 35 km/h (8.1 to 9.7 m/s). The higher speeds can only be sustained for a short time.[45][46]

Senses

The dolphin's search for food is aided by a form of sonar known as echolocation: it locates objects by producing sounds and listening for the echoes. A broadband burst pulse of clicking sounds is emitted in a focused beam in front of the dolphin. When the clicking sounds hit an object in the water, like a fish or rock, they bounce off and come back to the dolphin as echoes. Echolocation tells the dolphins the shape, size, speed, distance, and location of the object.[39] To hear the returning echo, they have two small ear openings behind the eyes, but most sound waves are transmitted to the inner ear through the lower jaw. As the object of interest is approached, the echo becomes booming, and the dolphins adjust by decreasing the intensity of the emitted sounds. (This contrasts with bats and sonar, which reduce the sensitivity of the sound receptor.) The interclick interval also decreases as the animal nears the target. Evidently, the dolphin waits for each click's echo before clicking again. Echolocation details, such as signal strength, spectral qualities, and discrimination, are well understood by researchers.[47] Bottlenose dolphins are also able to extract shape information, suggesting they are able to form an "echoic image" or sound picture of their targets.[48]

Dolphins have sharp eyesight. The eyes are located at the sides of the head and have a tapetum lucidum, or reflecting membrane, at the back of the retina, which aids vision in dim light. Their horseshoe-shaped, double-slit pupils enable dolphins to have good vision both in air and underwater, despite the different indices of refraction of these media.[49] When under water, the eyeball's lens serves to focus light, whereas in the in-air environment, the typically bright light serves to contract the specialized pupil, resulting in sharpness from a smaller aperture (similar to a pinhole camera).[50]

By contrast, a bottlenose's sense of smell is poor,[51] because its blowhole, the analog to the nose, is closed when underwater and it opens only for breathing. It has no olfactory nerves or olfactory lobe in the brain.[51] Bottlenose dolphins are able to detect salty, sweet, bitter (quinine sulphate), and sour (citric acid) tastes, but this has not been well-studied.[51] Anecdotally, some individuals in captivity have been noted to have preferences for food fish types, although it is not clear if taste mediates this preference.[51]

In 2022, a study at the University of St Andrews in Scotland found that dolphins were able to identify their "friends" and family members by the taste of their urine in the water.[52][53]

Communication

Bottlenose dolphins communicate through burst pulsed sounds, whistles, and body language.[54] Examples of body language include leaping out of the water, snapping jaws, slapping the tail on the surface and butting heads.[55] Sounds and gestures help keep track of other dolphins in the group, and alert other dolphins to danger and nearby food. Lacking vocal cords, they produce sounds using six air sacs near their blow hole. Each animal has a uniquely identifying, frequency-modulated narrow-band signature vocalization (signature whistle).[56]

Researchers from the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute (BDRI), based in Sardinia (Italy) have now shown whistles and burst pulsed sounds are vital to the animals' social life and mirror their behaviors.[57]

The tonal whistle sounds (the most melodious ones) allow dolphins to stay in contact with each other (above all, mothers and offspring), and to coordinate hunting strategies.[58] The burst-pulsed sounds (which are more complex and varied than the whistles) are used "to avoid physical aggression in situations of high excitement", such as when they are competing for the same piece of food, for example. The dolphins emit these strident sounds when in the presence of other individuals moving towards the same prey. The "least dominant" one soon moves away to avoid confrontation.[57]

Other communication uses about 30 distinguishable sounds, and although famously proposed by John C. Lilly in the 1950s, no "dolphin language" has been found. However, Herman, Richards, and Wolz demonstrated comprehension of an artificial language by two bottlenose dolphins (named Akeakamai and Phoenix) in the period of skepticism toward animal language following Herbert Terrace's critique.[59]

Intelligence

 
Bottlenose dolphin responding to human hand gestures

Cognition

Cognitive abilities that have been investigated include concept formation, sensory skills, and mental representations. Such research has been ongoing since the 1970s. This includes: acoustic and behavioral mimicry,[60][61] comprehension of novel sequences in an artificial language,[62][63] memory,[64] monitoring of self behavior,[65] discrimination and matching,[64][66] comprehension of symbols for various body parts,[67] comprehension of pointing gestures and gaze (as made by dolphins or humans),[68][69] mirror self-recognition,[70][71] and numerical values.[72]

Tool use and culture

At least some wild bottlenose dolphins use tools. In Shark Bay, off Western Australia, dolphins place a marine sponge on their rostrum, presumably to protect it when searching for food on the sandy sea bottom.[73] This has only been observed in this bay (first in 1997), and is predominantly practiced by females. A 2005 study showed mothers most likely teach the behavior to their offspring, evincing culture (behavior learned from other species members).[74][75]

Mud plume feeding is a feeding technique performed by a small community of bottlenose dolphins over shallow seagrass beds (less than 1 m) in the Florida Keys in the United States. The behavior involves creation of a U-shaped plume of mud in the water column and then rushing through the plume to capture fish.[76]

Along the beaches and tidal marshes of South Carolina and Georgia in the United States, bottlenose dolphins cooperatively herd prey fish onto steep and sandy banks in a practice known as "strand feeding". Groups of between two and six dolphins are regularly observed creating a bow wave to force the fish out of the water. The dolphins follow the fish, stranding themselves briefly, to eat their prey before twisting their bodies back and forth in order to slide back into the water.[77] While initially documented in South Carolina and Georgia, strand feeding has also been observed in Louisiana, Texas, Baja California, Ecuador, and Australia.[78]

Some Mauritanian dolphins cooperate with human fishermen. The dolphins drive a school of fish towards the shore, where humans await with nets. In the confusion of casting nets, the dolphins catch a large number of fish as well. Intraspecies cooperative foraging has also been observed. These behaviors may also be transmitted via teaching. Controversially, Rendell and Whitehead have proposed a structure for the study of cetacean culture.[79][80] Similar cases have been observed in Laguna, Santa Catarina in Brazil since during 19th century as well.[81][82]

Near Adelaide, in South Australia, several bottlenose dolphins "tail-walk", whereby they elevate the upper part of their bodies vertically out of the water, and propel themselves along the surface with powerful tail movements. Tail-walking mostly arises via human training in dolphinaria. In the 1980s, a female from the local population was kept at a local dolphinarium for three weeks, and the scientist suggests she copied the tail-walking behavior from other dolphins. Two other wild adult female dolphins copied it from her,[83] and the behaviour has continued through generations until 2022.[84][85][86]

A study conducted by the University of Chicago showed that bottlenose dolphins can remember whistles of other dolphins they had lived with after 20 years of separation. Each dolphin has a unique whistle that functions like a name, allowing the marine mammals to keep close social bonds. The new research shows that dolphins have the longest memory yet known in any species other than humans.[87][84]

The bottlenose dolphins of John's Pass in Boca Ciega Bay, St. Petersburg, Florida, exhibit a rare form of self-decoration and social object use called grass-wearing. Self-decoration by wearing grass appears to be an attention-getting device rather than purely play and varies from a single blade to large clusters of grass. John's Pass dolphins self-decorate with grass primarily when they form new social groups or engage in procreative activities. Grass-wearing behavior among these dolphins is a local behavioral tradition that could constitute a cultural difference from other communities.[88]

Cortical neurons

Some researchers hypothesize that the number of nerve cells (neurons) in the cortex of the brain predicts intelligence in mammals.[89] A 2019 study estimated the number of neurons in the cerebral cortex of three common bottlenose dolphins and found numbers ranging from 11.7 to 15.2 billion neurons.[90] The human average being approximately 16 billion, this is likely within the range found in the human population.[91]

Life history

Bottlenose dolphins have a lifespan of 40–60 years. Females can outlive males and live for 60 years or more. Dolphins start to reproduce aged 5 to 15 years.[92]

Respiration and sleep

The bottlenose dolphin has a single blowhole located on the dorsal surface of the head consisting of a hole and a muscular flap. The flap is closed during muscle relaxation and opens during contraction.[93] Dolphins are voluntary breathers, who must deliberately surface and open their blowholes to get air. They can store almost twice as much oxygen in proportion to their body weight as a human can: the dolphin can store 36 milliliters (ml) of oxygen per kg of body weight, compared with 20 ml per kg for humans. This is an adaptation to diving.[94] The bottlenose dolphin typically rises to the surface to breathe through its blowhole two to three times per minute,[43] although it can remain submerged for up to 20 minutes.[95]

Dolphins can breathe while "half-asleep". During the sleeping cycle, one brain hemisphere remains active, while the other hemisphere shuts down. The active hemisphere handles surfacing and breathing behavior.[96] The daily sleeping cycle lasts for about 8 hours, in increments of minutes to hours. During the sleeping cycle, they remain near the surface, swimming slowly or "logging", and occasionally closing one eye.[95]

Reproduction

 
Mother and juvenile bottlenose dolphins head to the seafloor

Both sexes have genital slits on the underside of their bodies. The male can retract and conceal his penis through his slit.[97][98] The female's slit houses her vagina and anus. Females have two mammary slits, each housing one nipple, one on each side of the genital slit.[99] The ability to stow their reproductive organs (especially in males) allows for maximum hydrodynamics. The breeding season produces significant physiological changes in males. At that time, the testes enlarge, enabling them to hold more sperm. Large amounts of sperm allow a male to wash away the previous suitor's sperm, while leaving some of his own for fertilization. Also, sperm concentration markedly increases. Having less sperm for out-of-season social mating means it wastes less. This suggests sperm production is energetically expensive. Males have large testes in relation to their body size.[100]

During the breeding season, males compete for access to females. Such competition can take the form of fighting other males or of herding females to prevent access by other males.[101][102] In Shark Bay, male bottlenose dolphins have been observed working in pairs or larger groups to follow and/or restrict the movement of a female for weeks at a time, waiting for her to become sexually receptive.[101][103] These coalitions, also known as male reproductive alliances, will fight with other coalitions for control of females.[103] Humans and dolphins are the only species that share this type of "gang formation" habit as a form of cooperation.[104]

Mating occurs belly to belly.[102] Dolphins have been observed engaging in intercourse when the females are not in their estrous cycles and cannot produce young, suggesting they may mate for pleasure.[105][106] The gestation period averages 12 months.[22] Births can occur at any time of year, although peaks occur in warmer months.[2] The young are born in shallow water, sometimes assisted by a (possibly male) "midwife", and usually only a single calf is born.[107] Twins are possible, but rare. Newborn bottlenose dolphins are 0.8 to 1.4 m (2.6 to 4.6 ft) long and weigh 9 to 30 kg (20 to 66 lb), with Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphin infants being generally smaller than common bottlenose dolphin infants.[22] To accelerate nursing, the mother can eject milk from her mammary glands. The calf suckles for 18 months to up to 8 years,[108] and continues to closely associate with its mother for several years after weaning.[35] Females sexually mature at ages 5–13, males at ages 9–14.[2] Females reproduce every two to six years. Reproduction is moderately seasonal (September–January), peaking from October to December. Calf loss between August and December is followed by rapid conception (1–2 months), whereas conception is delayed (2–9 months) if calf loss occurs between January and July. Weaning ages ranged from 2.7 to 8.0 years, but 66.7% (42 calves) were weaned by their fourth birthday. Females tended to wean mid-pregnancy. Group size was unrelated to water depth or female reproductive success, but reproductive success was predicted by water depth. Shallow water may allow mothers and calves to detect and avoid predatory sharks. Alternatively, or additionally, prey density may be higher in shallow water compared to deep water.[22]

Georgetown University professor Janet Mann argues the strong personal behavior among male calves is about bond formation and benefits the species in an evolutionary context. She cites studies showing these dolphins as adults are inseparable, and that early bonds aid protection, as well as in locating females.[109]

Female bottlenose dolphins have to expend additional energy in carrying out parental care, e.g., infant-carrying behavior. Dolphins don't physically hold their infants but line up in an echelon position with infants swimming beside them. This position creates a change of water flow pattern from the infant which minimizes separation between the mother and infant, but also increases the mother's surface area and creates a drag for the swimmer. This also leaves less energy to use in swimming speed, foraging, and predator evasion.[110]

Social interaction

 
An adult female bottlenose dolphin with her young, Moray Firth, Scotland

Adult males live mostly alone or in groups of two to three, and join pods for short periods of time. Adult females and young dolphins normally live in groups of up to 15 animals.[22] Males give strong mutual support if other males help them, even if they are not friends.[111] However, they live in fission-fusion societies of varying group size, within which individuals change associations, often on a daily or hourly basis.[112][113] Group compositions are usually determined by sex, age, reproductive condition, familial relations and affiliation histories. In a dolphin community near Sarasota, Florida, the most common group types are adult females with their recent offspring, older subadults of both sexes, and adult males either alone or in bonded pairs.[114] Smaller groups can join to form larger groups of 100 or more, and occasionally exceed 1,000.[22][115] The social strategies of marine mammals such as bottlenose dolphins "provide interesting parallels" with the social strategies of elephants and chimpanzees.[116]: 519 

Bottlenose dolphins studied by Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute researchers off the island of Sardinia show random social behavior while feeding, and their social behavior does not depend on feeding activity.[117] In Sardinia, the presence of a floating marine fin-fish farm has been linked to a change in bottlenose dolphin distribution as a result of high fish density around the floating cages in the farming area.[118]

Ecology

Feeding

Fish is one of the main items in the dolphin diet. They also eat shrimps, squid, mollusks, and cuttlefish, and only swallow the soft parts. They eat 22 pounds of fish a day. When they encounter a shoal of fish, they work as a team to herd them towards the shore to maximize the harvest.[22] They also hunt alone, often targeting bottom-dwelling species. The bottlenose dolphin sometimes hits a fish with its fluke, sometimes knocking it out of the water, using a strategy called "fish whacking".[36][119] "Strand feeding" is an inherited feeding technique used by bottlenose dolphins near and around coastal regions of Georgia and South Carolina. When a pod finds a school of fish, they will circle the school and trap the fish in a mini whirlpool. Then, the dolphins will charge at the school and push their bodies up onto a mud-flat, forcing the fish on the mud-flat, as well. The dolphins then crawl around on their sides, consuming the fish they washed up on shore. This happens only during low tides.[120]

One type of feeding behavior seen in bottlenose dolphins is mud ring feeding.[121]

Bottlenose dolphins conflict with small-scale coastal commercial fisheries in some Mediterranean areas. Common bottlenose dolphins are probably attracted to fishing nets because they offer a concentrated food source.[122]

Relations with other species

 
A bottlenose dolphin attacks and kills a harbour porpoise at Chanonry Point, Scotland

Dolphins can exhibit altruistic behaviour toward other sea creatures. On Mahia Beach, New Zealand, on March 10, 2008,[123] two pygmy sperm whales, a female and calf, stranded on the beach. Rescuers attempted to refloat them four times. Shortly, a playful bottlenose dolphin known to local residents as Moko arrived and, after apparently vocalizing at the whales, led them 200 m (660 ft) along a sandbar to the open sea, saving them from imminent euthanasia.[124] In 2019 a female was observed caring for a juvenile melon-headed whale, the first reported instance of a bottlenose dolphin adopting a non-conspecific infant.[125]

The bottlenose dolphin can behave aggressively. Males fight for rank and access to females. During mating season, males compete vigorously with each other through displays of toughness and size, with a series of acts, such as head-butting. They display aggression towards sharks and smaller dolphin species. At least one population, off Scotland, has practiced infanticide, and also has attacked and killed harbour porpoises. University of Aberdeen researchers say the dolphins do not eat their victims, but are simply competing for food.[126] However, Dr. Read of Duke University, a porpoise expert researching similar cases of porpoise killings that had occurred in Virginia in 1996 and 1997, holds a different view. He states dolphins and porpoises feed on different types of fish, thus food competition is an unlikely cause of the killings.[127] Similar behaviour has been observed in Ireland.[128] In the first half of July 2014, four attacks with three porpoise fatalities were observed and caught on video by the Cardigan Bay Marine Wildlife Centre in the Cardigan Bay, Wales.[129]

The bottlenose dolphin sometimes forms mixed species groups with other species from the dolphin family, particularly larger species, such as the short-finned pilot whale, the false killer whale and Risso's dolphin.[101][130][131] They also interact with smaller species, such as the Atlantic spotted dolphin and the rough-toothed dolphin.[101][132] While interactions with smaller species are sometimes affiliative, they can also be hostile.[101]

Predators

Some large shark species, such as the tiger shark, the dusky shark, the great white shark and the bull shark, prey on the bottlenose dolphin, especially calves.[102][133][134][135][136] The bottlenose dolphin is capable of defending itself by charging the predator; dolphin 'mobbing' behavior of sharks can occasionally prove fatal for the shark.[133] Targeting a single adult dolphin can be dangerous for a shark of similar size. Killer whale populations in New Zealand and Peru have been observed preying on bottlenose dolphins, but this seems rare,[133] and other orcas may swim with dolphins. Swimming in pods allows dolphins to better defend themselves against predators. Bottlenose dolphins either use complex evasive strategies to outswim their predators, or mobbing techniques to batter the predator to death or force it to flee.[137]

Relation to humans

Interaction

 
At Notojima Aquarium, Japan

The species sometimes shows curiosity towards humans in or near water. Occasionally, they rescue injured divers by raising them to the surface. They also do this to help injured members of their own species.[133] In November 2004, a dramatic report of dolphin intervention came from New Zealand. Four lifeguards, swimming 100 m (330 ft) off the coast near Whangarei, were approached by a shark (reportedly a great white shark). Bottlenose dolphins herded the swimmers together and surrounded them for 40 minutes, preventing the shark from attacking, as they slowly swam to shore.[138]

In coastal regions, dolphins run the risk of colliding with boats. Researchers of the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute first quantified data about solitary bottlenose dolphin diving behavior in the presence and absence of boats. Dolphins responded more to tourist than fishing vessels. Driving behavior, speed, engine type and separation distance all affected dolphin safety.[139]

However, dolphins in these areas can also coexist with humans. For example, in the town of Laguna in south Brazil, a pod of bottlenose dolphins resides in the estuary, and some of its members cooperate with humans. These cooperating dolphins are individually recognized by the local fishermen, who name them. The fishermen typically stand up to their knees in the shallow waters or sit in canoes, waiting for the dolphins. Now and then, one or more dolphins appear, driving the fish towards the line of fishermen. One dolphin then displays a unique body movement outside the water, which serves as a signal to the fishermen to cast their nets (the entire sequence is shown here,[82] and a detailed description of the signal's characteristics is available here[140]). In this unique form of cooperation, the dolphins gain because the fish are disoriented and because the fish cannot escape to shallow water where the larger dolphins cannot swim. Likewise, studies show that fishermen casting their nets following the unique signal catch more fish than when fishing alone, without the help of the dolphins.[141] The dolphins were not trained for this behavior; the collaboration began before 1847. Similar cooperative fisheries also exist in Mauritania, Africa.[142]

Commercial 'dolphin encounter' enterprises and tours operate in many countries. The documentary film The Cove documents how dolphins are captured and sold to some of these enterprises (particularly in Asia) while the remaining pod is slaughtered. In addition to such endeavors, the individuals swim with and surface near surfers at the beach.[143] Bottlenose dolphins perform in many aquaria, generating controversy. Animal welfare activists and certain scientists have claimed that the dolphins do not have adequate space or receive adequate care or stimulation.[144] However, others, notably SeaWorld, counter by claiming that the dolphins are properly cared for, have much environmental stimulation and enjoy interacting with humans.[133][145]

Eight bottlenose dolphins that lived at the Marine Life Aquarium in Gulfport, Mississippi were swept away from their aquarium pool during Hurricane Katrina. They were later found in the Gulf of Mexico and returned to captivity.[146]

 
K-Dog, trained by the United States Navy to find mines and boobytraps underwater, leaping out of the water

The military of the United States and Russia train bottlenose dolphins as military dolphins for wartime tasks, such as locating sea mines and detecting enemy divers.[147][148] The U.S.'s program is the U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program, located in San Diego.[149]

Tião was a well-known solitary male bottlenose dolphin that was first spotted in the town of São Sebastião in Brazil around 1994 and frequently allowed humans to interact with him. The dolphin became infamous for killing a swimmer and injuring many others, which later earned him the nickname "Killer Dolphin".[150]

Cultural influence

The popular television show Flipper, created by Ivan Tors, portrayed a bottlenose dolphin[151] in a friendly relationship with two boys, Sandy and Bud. A seagoing "Lassie", Flipper understood English and was a hero: "Go tell Dad we're in trouble, Flipper! Hurry!" The show's theme song contains the lyric "no one you see / is smarter than he". The television show was based on a 1963 film, with a sequel, Flipper's New Adventure (1964), and was remade as a feature film in 1996, starring Elijah Wood and Paul Hogan, as well as a second TV series running from 1995 to 2000, starring Jessica Alba.[152]

Other television appearances by bottlenose dolphins include Wonder Woman, Highway to Heaven, Dolphin Cove, seaQuest DSV, and The Penguins of Madagascar, in which a dolphin, Doctor Blowhole, is a villain. In the HBO movie Zeus and Roxanne, a female bottlenose dolphin befriends a male dog, and in Secrets of the Bermuda Triangle (1996 Ian Toynton movie), a girl named Annie (played by Lisa Jakub) swims with dolphins. Human and dolphin interaction segments, shot on location in the Florida Keys with Dolphin Research Center, are featured on Sesame Street[citation needed] and on a Halloween episode of The Simpsons, Treehouse of Horror XI.[153]

Dolphin Tale, directed by Charles Martin Smith, starring Nathan Gamble, Ashley Judd, Harry Connick Jr., Morgan Freeman, Cozi Zuehlsdorff and Kris Kristofferson, is based on the real-life story of the dolphin Winter, who was rescued from a crab trap in December 2005 and lost her tail, but learned to swim with a prosthetic one.[154] Dolphin Tale 2, a sequel to the 2011 film, featured another rescued dolphin named Hope and an appearance by Bethany Hamilton. The sequel was released on September 12, 2014.[155] The NFL's Miami Dolphins uses the bottlenose dolphin as its mascot and team logo.[156] Factual descriptions of the dolphins date back into antiquity — the writings of Aristotle, Oppian and Pliny the Elder all mention the species.[2][157]

Threats

Millions of dolphins drown in fishing nets. Tuna fishing crews have been the most responsible for the largest number of deaths. In 1972, the U.S. government passed a law limiting the number of dolphins that could be killed yearly by tuna fishing crews. Dolphins in the United Kingdom have also been found to contain high levels of pollutants in their tissues. Heavy metals including mercury, PCB's and DDT are of great concern. These pollutants can cause harm in dolphins growth development, reproduction, and immunity. Since the mid-1990s, hundreds of dolphins have been trained to perform in shows presented by aquariums, zoos, and amusement parks. Scientists conduct various types of research to understand the dolphin's communication system.[158]

The man-made chemical perfluorooctanesulfonic acid (PFOS) may be compromising the immune system of bottlenose dolphins.[159] PFOS affects the immune system of male mice at a concentration of 91.5 ppb,[160] while PFOS has been reported in bottlenose dolphins in excess of 1 ppm.[161] High levels of metal contaminants have been measured in tissues in many areas of the globe. A recent study found high levels of cadmium and mercury in bottlenose dolphins from South Australia,[162] levels which were later found to be associated with kidney malformations, indicating possible health effects of high heavy metal concentrations in dolphins.[163]

Conservation

 
Bottlenose dolphin (at Hundred Islands National Park)

Bottlenose dolphins are not endangered. Their future is stable because of their abundance and adaptability. However, specific populations are threatened due to various environmental changes. The population in the Moray Firth in Scotland is estimated to consist of around 190 individuals, and are under threat from harassment, traumatic injury, water pollution and reduction in food availability.[164] Likewise, an isolated population in Doubtful Sound, New Zealand, is in decline due to calf loss coincident to an increase in warm freshwater discharge into the fiord.[165] Less local climate change, such as increasing water temperature may also play a role but has never been shown to be the case.[166] One of the largest coastal populations of bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay, Western Australia was forecast to be stable with little variation in mortality over time (Manlik et al. 2016).[167]

In United States waters, hunting and harassing of marine mammals is forbidden in almost all circumstances, from passage of the Marine Mammal Protection Act of 1972.[168]

See also

References

  1. ^ a b "Fossilworks: Tursiops miocaenus". www.fossilworks.org.
  2. ^ a b c d Wells, R.; Scott, M. (2002). "Bottlenose Dolphins". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B.; Thewissen, J (eds.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 122–127. ISBN 978-0-12-551340-1.
  3. ^ "Common Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops Truncatus) - Dolphin Facts and Information". www.dolphins-world.com. Retrieved July 13, 2017.
  4. ^ a b "Explore the Database". www.mammaldiversity.org. Retrieved September 5, 2021.
  5. ^ Wells, R.S.; Scott, Michael (January 1, 2009). "Common bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. 6: 249–255. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-373553-9.00062-6.
  6. ^ Krützen, M.; Kreicker, S.; MacLeod, C.D.; Learmonth, J.; Kopps, A.M.; Walsham, P.; Allen, S.J. (2014). "Cultural transmission of tool use by Indo-Pacific bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.) provides access to a novel foraging niche". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 281 (1784): 1–9. doi:10.1098/rspb.2014.0374. PMC 4043097. PMID 24759862.
  7. ^ Osvaldo, Cairo (October 4, 2011). "External Measures of Cognition". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 5 (108): 108. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2011.00108. PMC 3207484. PMID 22065955. Table 1
  8. ^ Marino, Lori (2004). (PDF). International Society for Comparative Psychology. The International Society for Comparative Psychology (17): 1–16. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 10, 2011.
  9. ^ Natoli, Ada; Peddemors, Victor M.; Rus Hoelzel, A. (March 1, 2004). "Population structure and speciation in the genus Tursiops based on microsatellite and mitochondrial DNA analyses". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 17 (2): 363–375. doi:10.1046/j.1420-9101.2003.00672.x. ISSN 1420-9101. PMID 15009270. S2CID 19615055.
  10. ^ Rice, Dale W (1998). Marine mammals of the world: systematics and distribution (Special Publication). Society of Marine Mammalogy. ISBN 978-1-891276-03-3.
  11. ^ Charlton-Robb, Kate; Gershwin, Lisa-ann; Thompson, Ross; Austin, Jeremy; Owen, Kylie; McKechnie, Stephen (September 14, 2011). Fleischer, Robert C. (ed.). "A New Dolphin Species, the Burrunan Dolphin Tursiops australis sp. nov., Endemic to Southern Australian Coastal Waters". PLOS ONE. 6 (9): e24047. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...624047C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024047. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3173360. PMID 21935372.
  12. ^ Charlton-Robb, K.; Taylor, A. C.; McKechnie, S. W. (February 2015). "Population genetic structure of the Burrunan dolphin (Tursiops australis) in coastal waters of south-eastern Australia: conservation implications". Conservation Genetics. 16 (1): 195–207. doi:10.1007/s10592-014-0652-6. ISSN 1566-0621. S2CID 16178397.
  13. ^ Wickert, Janaína Carrion; von Eye, Sophie Maillard; Oliveira, Larissa Rosa; Moreno, Ignacio Benites (December 5, 2016). "Revalidation of Tursiops gephyreus Lahille, 1908 (Cetartiodactyla: Delphinidae) from the southwestern Atlantic Ocean". Journal of Mammalogy. 97 (6): 1728–1737. doi:10.1093/jmammal/gyw139. ISSN 0022-2372.
  14. ^ Hohl, Leandro S. L.; Sicuro, Fernando L.; Wickert, Janaína C.; Moreno, Ignacio B.; Rocha-Barbosa, Oscar; Barreto, André S. (April 6, 2020). "Skull morphology of bottlenose dolphins from different ocean populations with emphasis on South America". Journal of Morphology. 281 (6): 564–577. doi:10.1002/jmor.21121. PMID 32249999. S2CID 214808512.
  15. ^ Oliveira, Larissa Rosa de; Fraga, Lúcia D; Ott, Paulo H; Siciliano, Salvatore; Lopes, Fernando; Almeida, Raquel; Wickert, Janaína C; Milmann, Lucas; Danilewicz, Daniel; Emin-Lima, Neusa Renata; Meirelles, Ana Carolina (April 24, 2019). "Population structure, phylogeography, and genetic diversity of the common bottlenose dolphin in the tropical and subtropical southwestern Atlantic Ocean". Journal of Mammalogy. 100 (2): 564–577. doi:10.1093/jmammal/gyz065. hdl:10923/20559. ISSN 0022-2372.
  16. ^ Costa, Ana P. B.; Rosel, Patricia E.; Daura-Jorge, Fábio G.; Simões-Lopes, Paulo C. (October 2016). "Offshore and coastal common bottlenose dolphins of the western South Atlantic face-to-face: What the skull and the spine can tell us". Marine Mammal Science. 32 (4): 1433–1457. doi:10.1111/mms.12342.
  17. ^ "List of Marine Mammal Species and Subspecies". Society for Marine Mammalogy. 2020. Retrieved April 6, 2020.
  18. ^ Groves, C. P. (2005). Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. ISBN 0-801-88221-4. OCLC 62265494.
  19. ^ Mead, J. G.; Brownell, R. L. Jr. (2005). "Order Cetacea". In Wilson, D. E.; Reeder, D. M. (eds.). Mammal Species of the World: A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference (3rd ed.). Johns Hopkins University Press. pp. 723–743. ISBN 978-0-8018-8221-0. OCLC 62265494.
  20. ^ "IUCN Red List - Bottlenose Dolphin query". IUCN Red List. April 6, 2020. Retrieved April 6, 2020.
  21. ^ a b c "Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Western North Atlantic Offshore Stock" (PDF). Retrieved September 30, 2008.
  22. ^ a b c d e f g h Shirihai, H.; Jarrett, B. (2006). Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. pp. 155–161. ISBN 978-0-691-12757-6.
  23. ^ Leduc, R.; Perrin, W.; Dizon, E. (August 18, 1998). "Phylogenetic Relationships among the Delphinid Cetaceans Based on Full Cytochrome B Sequences". Marine Mammal Science. 15 (3): 619–648. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1999.tb00833.x.
  24. ^ Moura, Andre E.; Shreves, Kypher; Pilot, Małgorzata; Andrews, Kimberly R.; Moore, Daniel M.; Kishida, Takushi; Möller, Luciana; Natoli, Ada; Gaspari, Stefania; McGowen, Michael; Chen, Ing; Gray, Howard; Gore, Mauvis; Culloch, Ross M.; Kiani, Muhammad S.; Willson, Maia Sarrouf; Bulushi, Asma; Collins, Tim; Baldwin, Robert; Willson, Andrew; Minton, Gianna; Ponnampalam, Louisa; Hoelzel, A. Rus (May 1, 2020). "Phylogenomics of the genus Tursiops and closely related Delphininae reveals extensive reticulation among lineages and provides inference about eco-evolutionary drivers". Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution. 146: 106756. doi:10.1016/j.ympev.2020.106756. hdl:2164/16438. ISSN 1055-7903. PMID 32028032. S2CID 211048062.
  25. ^ Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A.A. Knopf. p. 422. ISBN 978-0-375-41141-0.
  26. ^ . American Cetacean Society. Archived from the original on May 17, 2008. Retrieved September 20, 2008.
  27. ^ Lee, Jaennette (April 15, 2005). "Whale-Dolphin Hybrid Has Baby 'Wholphin'". Associated Press. Retrieved September 20, 2008.
  28. ^ . Archived from the original on September 15, 2008. Retrieved September 20, 2008.
  29. ^ . Archived from the original on January 23, 2009. Retrieved September 20, 2008.
  30. ^ Zornetzer H.R.; Duffield D.A. (2003). "Captive-born bottlenose dolphin × common dolphin (Tursiops truncatus × Delphinus capensis) intergeneric hybrids". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 81 (10): 1755–1762. doi:10.1139/z03-150.
  31. ^ Herzing, D.; Moewe, K. & Brunnick, B. (2003). (PDF). Aquatic Mammals. 29 (3): 335–341. doi:10.1578/01675420360736505. Archived from the original (PDF) on June 14, 2007. Retrieved September 20, 2008.
  32. ^ "Evolution of Dolphins".
  33. ^ "Fossilworks: Tursiops osennae". www.fossilworks.org.
  34. ^ American Cetacean Society Fact Sheet – Bottlenose Dolphin 2008-07-25 at the Wayback Machine
  35. ^ a b c d Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). National Audubon Society Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A. A. Knopf. pp. 362–365. ISBN 978-0-375-41141-0.
  36. ^ a b "Office of Protected Resources – Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". noaa.gov. September 16, 2019.
  37. ^ team, facts.co (September 29, 2017). . Bottlenose dolphin facts. Archived from the original on August 13, 2018. Retrieved March 3, 2018.
  38. ^ "The Life Cycle of Bottlenose Dolphins". Sciencing. Retrieved March 3, 2018.
  39. ^ a b . March 26, 2014. Archived from the original on February 11, 2017. Retrieved September 29, 2016.
  40. ^ a b "Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus at MarineBio.org". marinebio.org. Archived from the original on July 13, 2012. Retrieved January 26, 2005.
  41. ^ Morteo, Eduardo; Rocha-Olivares, Axayácatl; Morteo, Rodrigo; Weller, David W. (June 13, 2017). "Phenotypic variation in dorsal fin morphology of coastal bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off Mexico". PeerJ. 5: e3415. doi:10.7717/peerj.3415. PMC 5472037. PMID 28626607.
  42. ^ Tabuchi, Hiroko (2006). "Dolphin reveals an extra set of 'legs'". NBC News. Associated Press.
  43. ^ a b . Busch Gardens. Archived from the original on May 17, 2008. Retrieved March 14, 2008.
  44. ^ Hersh, Sandra L.; Deborah A. Duffield (1990). "Distinction Between Northwest Atlantic Offshore and Coastal Bottlenose Dolphins Based on Hemoglobin Profile and Morphometry". In Stephen Leatherwood; Randall R. Reeves (eds.). The Bottlenose Dolphin. San Diego: Academic Press. pp. 129–139. ISBN 978-0-12-440280-5.
  45. ^ Goforth, Harold W. Jr. (1990) "Ergometry (Exercise Testing) of the Bottlenose Dolphin." In The Bottlenose Dolphin, edited by Stephen Leatherwood and Randall R. Reeves, pp. 559–574. San Diego: Academic Press, Inc., ISBN 0-12-440280-1.
  46. ^ Fish, F. E.; Hui, C. A. (1991). "Dolphin swimming – a review". Mammal Review. 21 (4): 181–195. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2907.1991.tb00292.x.
  47. ^ Au, Whitlow (1993). The Sonar of Dolphins. New York: Springer-Verlag. ISBN 978-3-540-97835-0.
  48. ^ Pack AA, Herman LM (1995). "Sensory integration in the bottlenosed dolphin: immediate recognition of complex shapes across the senses of echolocation and vision". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 98 (2 Pt 1): 722–33. Bibcode:1995ASAJ...98..722P. doi:10.1121/1.413566. PMID 7642811.
  49. ^ Herman, L. M.; Peacock, M. F.; Yunker, M. P.; Madsen, C. (1975). "Bottlenosed dolphin: Double-slit pupil yields equivalent aerial and underwater diurnal acuity". Science. 189 (4203): 650–652. Bibcode:1975Sci...189..650H. doi:10.1126/science.1162351. PMID 1162351.
  50. ^ M. Mass, Alla; Y. Supin, Alexander (2007). "Adaptive features of aquatic mammals' eyes". The Anatomical Record. 290 (6): 701–715. doi:10.1002/ar.20529. PMID 17516421. S2CID 39925190.
  51. ^ a b c d "Dolphin Characteristics". Western Illinois University. Retrieved August 31, 2008.
  52. ^ "Dolphins can recognise each other by taste of their urine, study finds". the Guardian. May 19, 2022. Retrieved May 29, 2022.
  53. ^ Bruck, Jason N.; Walmsley, Sam F.; Janik, Vincent M. (May 20, 2022). "Cross-modal perception of identity by sound and taste in bottlenose dolphins". Science Advances. 8 (20): eabm7684. Bibcode:2022SciA....8M7684B. doi:10.1126/sciadv.abm7684. ISSN 2375-2548. PMC 9116882. PMID 35584227.
  54. ^ . Archived from the original on April 20, 2013.
  55. ^ . National Geographic. Archived from the original on October 23, 2006. Retrieved November 3, 2006.
  56. ^ Janik VM, Slater PJ (1998). "Context-specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls". Animal Behaviour. 56 (4): 829–838. doi:10.1006/anbe.1998.0881. PMID 9790693. S2CID 32367435.
  57. ^ a b Díaz López, B.; Shirai, J.A.B. (2009). Mediterranean common bottlenose dolphin's repertoire and communication use. Dolphins: Anatomy, Behavior and Threats. pp. 129–148. ISBN 978-1-60876-849-3.
  58. ^ Díaz López (2010). "Whistle characteristics in free-ranging bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Mediterranean Sea: Influence of behaviour". Mammalian Biology. 76 (2): 180–189. doi:10.1016/j.mambio.2010.06.006.
  59. ^ Herman LM, Richards DG, Wolz JP (1984). "Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins". Cognition. 16 (2): 129–219. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(84)90003-9. PMID 6540652. S2CID 43237011.
  60. ^ Reiss D, McCowan B (1993). "Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): evidence for vocal learning". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 107 (3): 301–12. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.107.3.301. PMID 8375147.
  61. ^ . Archived from the original on May 11, 2008. Retrieved August 31, 2008.
  62. ^ Herman, L. (2002). "Language Learning". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B.; Thewissen, J (eds.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 685–689. ISBN 978-0-12-551340-1.
  63. ^ The Dolphin Institute. . dolphin-institute.org. Archived from the original on December 11, 2008.
  64. ^ a b Herman, L.; Pack, A.; Wood, A. (August 26, 2006). "Bottlenose Dolphins Can Generalize Rules and Develop Abstract Concepts". Marine Mammal Science. 10 (1): 70–80. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1994.tb00390.x.
  65. ^ . Archived from the original on May 9, 2008. Retrieved August 31, 2008.
  66. ^ Herman LM, Gordon JA (1974). "Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 21 (1): 19–26. doi:10.1901/jeab.1974.21-19. PMC 1333166. PMID 4204143.
  67. ^ . Archived from the original on May 11, 2008. Retrieved August 31, 2008.
  68. ^ Foer, Joshua (2015). "It's Time for Conservation: Breaking the communication barrier between dolphins and humans". National Geographic. Retrieved February 12, 2016.
  69. ^ . Archived from the original on May 9, 2008. Retrieved August 31, 2008.
  70. ^ "Intelligence and Humans". Retrieved August 11, 2008.
  71. ^ Marten, K.; Psarakos, S. (1995). . In Parker, S. T.; Mitchell, R.; Boccia, M (eds.). Self-awareness in Animals and Humans: Developmental Perspectives. Cambridge University Press. pp. 361–379. ISBN 978-0-521-44108-7. Archived from the original on October 13, 2008. Retrieved October 4, 2008.
  72. ^ Kilian, Annette; Yaman, Sevgi; von Fersen, Lorenzo; Güntürkün, Onur (2003). "A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity" (PDF). Learning & Behavior. 31 (2): 133–142. doi:10.3758/BF03195976. PMID 12882372. S2CID 12368215.
  73. ^ Smolker, R.A.; et al. (1997). "Sponge Carrying by Dolphins (Delphinidae, Tursiops sp.): A Foraging Specialization Involving Tool Use?" (PDF). Ethology. 103 (6): 454–465. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1997.tb00160.x. hdl:2027.42/71936.
  74. ^ Krutzen M, Mann J, Heithaus MR, Connor RC, Bejder L, Sherwin WB (2005). "Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (25): 8939–8943. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8939K. doi:10.1073/pnas.0500232102. PMC 1157020. PMID 15947077.
  75. ^ Krutzen, M.; Mann, J.; Heithaus, M. R.; Connor, R. C.; Bejder, L.; Sherwin, W. B. (2005). "Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (25): 8939–43. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8939K. doi:10.1073/pnas.0500232102. PMC 1157020. PMID 15947077.
  76. ^ Lewis, J.S.; Schroeder, W. (2003). "Mud plume feeding, a unique foraging behavior of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) in the Florida Keys". Gulf of Mexico Science. 21 (1): 92. doi:10.18785/goms.2101.09.
  77. ^ Feldman, Paula. "Dinner Is Served!". South Carolina Wildlife Magazine.
  78. ^ Jimenez, Pedro; Alava, Juan Jose (2015). "Strand-feeding by coastal bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Gulf of Guayaquil, Ecuador". Latin American Journal of Aquatic Mammals. 10 (1): 33–37. doi:10.5597/lajam00191.
  79. ^ Rendell, L.; Whitehead, H. (2001). "Culture in whales and dolphins". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (2): 309–382. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0100396X. PMID 11530544. S2CID 24052064.
  80. ^ Premack, D.; Hauser, M. (2001). (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (2): 350–351. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01513965. S2CID 143443845. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 6, 2009.
  81. ^ The Daily Telegraph (2006), "Brazil's sexiest secret", article retrieved January 24, 2016.
  82. ^ a b Dr. Moti Nissani (2007) Bottlenose Dolphins in Laguna Requesting a Throw Net (video). Supporting material for Dr. Nissani's presentation at the 2007 International Ethological Conference. Video retrieved January 24, 2016.
  83. ^ Black, Richard (2008). "Wild dolphins tail-walk on water". BBC News. Retrieved March 10, 2016.
  84. ^ a b Sutton, Malcolm (April 4, 2022). "Why are the dolphins dying?". ABC News. Australian Broadcasting Corporation. Retrieved April 4, 2022.
  85. ^ Bossley M, Steiner A, Brakes P, Shrimpton J, Foster C, Rendell L. (September 2018). "Tail walking in a bottlenose dolphin community: the rise and fall of an arbitrary cultural 'fad'". Biol. Lett. 14 (9). doi:10.1098/rsbl.2018.0314. PMC 6170752. PMID 30185606.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: uses authors parameter (link)
  86. ^ Pisani, Ashleigh (May 9, 2018). "The incredible tale of Billie the Port River dolphin — the world's first wild tailwalker". Adelaide Now. Portside Messenger.
  87. ^ "Dolphins Have Longest Memories in Animal Kingdom". August 6, 2013. Retrieved March 3, 2018.
  88. ^ Weaver, A., & Kuczaj, S. (2016). Neither toy nor tool: Grass-wearing behavior among free-ranging bottlenose dolphins in western Florida. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 29, 1-18, uclapsych_ijcp_31885. Retrieved from: http://escholarship.org/uc/item/893417x3
  89. ^ Dicke, U.; Roth G. (August–September 2008). "Intelligence Evolved". Scientific American Mind. pp. 75–77.
  90. ^ Ridgeway, Sam H. (December 16, 2019). "Higher neuron densities in the cerebral cortex and larger cerebellums may limit dive times of delphinids compared to deep-diving toothed whales". PLOS ONE. 14 (12): e0226206. Bibcode:2019PLoSO..1426206R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0226206. PMC 6914331. PMID 31841529.
  91. ^ Herculano-Houzel, Suzana (November 9, 2009). "The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain". Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 3 (12): 31. doi:10.3389/neuro.09.031.2009. PMC 6914331. PMID 31841529.
  92. ^ "Common Bottlenose Dolphin | NOAA Fisheries". December 29, 2021.
  93. ^ . Seaworld. Archived from the original on May 18, 2012. Retrieved March 14, 2008.
  94. ^ Kooyman, G. (2002). "Diving Physiology". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B.; Thewissen, J (eds.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 339–341. ISBN 978-0-12-551340-1.
  95. ^ a b . Mia Research Foundation. Archived from the original on March 18, 2008. Retrieved March 13, 2008.
  96. ^ "Do whales and dolphins sleep". howstuffworks. May 24, 2001. Retrieved March 13, 2008.
  97. ^ Robeck, Todd R., et al. "Reproductive biology of the bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) and the potential application of advanced reproductive technologies." Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine (1994): 321-336.
  98. ^ Atkinson, S. (2002). "Male Reproductive Systems". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B.; Thewissen, J (eds.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. p. 700. ISBN 978-0-12-551340-1.
  99. ^ Stewart, R. (2002). "Female reproductive systems". In Perrin, W.; Wursig, B.; Thewissen, J (eds.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. pp. 422–428. ISBN 978-0-12-551340-1.
  100. ^ Connor Richard C.; Peterson, Dawn M. (1994). The Lives of Whales and Dolphins. New York.: Henry Holt and Company. ISBN 978-0-8050-1950-6.
  101. ^ a b c d e Connor, R.; Wells, R.; Mann, J.; Read, A. (2000). "The Bottlenose Dolphin". In Mann, J.; Connor, R.; Tyack, P.; Whitehead, H (eds.). Cetacean Societies. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. p. 102. ISBN 978-0-226-50341-7.
  102. ^ a b c . Smithsonian National Zoo. Archived from the original on February 18, 2009.
  103. ^ a b Connor R. C. (1990) Alliances among male bottlenose dolphins and comparative analyses of mutualism. Ph.D. Dissertation. The University of Michigan, Michigan.
  104. ^ Guy, Jack (April 2020). "Male bottlenose dolphins form gangs to get a mate". CNN. Retrieved April 1, 2020.
  105. ^ Volker Sommer; Paul L. Vasey (2006). "Chapter 4". Homosexual Behaviour in Animals – an Evolutionary perspective.
  106. ^ Bruce Bagemihl (1999). Biological Exuberance – Animal Homosexuality and Natural Diversity.
  107. ^ Connor, R.; Wells, R.; Mann, J.; Read, A. (2000). "The Bottlenose Dolphin". In Mann, J.; Connor, R.; Tyack, P.; Whitehead, H (eds.). Cetacean Societies. Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press. p. 95. ISBN 978-0-226-50341-7.
  108. ^ Mann, J.; Connor, R. C.; Barre, L. M.; Heithaus, M. R. (2000). "Female reproductive success in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops sp.): Life history, habitat, provisioning, and group size effects". Behavioral Ecology. 11 (2): 210–219. doi:10.1093/beheco/11.2.210.
  109. ^ Mann, J. (2006). "Establishing Trust: Sociosexual behaviour and the development of male-male bonds among Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphin calves". In Vasey, P.; Sommer, V (eds.). Homosexual Behaviour in Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-86446-6.
  110. ^ Noren, S. R. (2008). "Infant Carrying Behaviour in Dolphins: Costly Parental Care in an Aquatic Environment". Functional Ecology. 22 (2): 284–288. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2007.01354.x. JSTOR 20142803. Retrieved November 19, 2022.
  111. ^ "Know your ally: Cooperative male dolphins can tell who's on their team". phys.org. Retrieved April 22, 2021.
  112. ^ Connor, Richards (2000). Cetacean Societies: Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-50341-7.
  113. ^ Díaz López, Bruno; Shirai J.A. (2007). "Marine aquaculture and bottlenose dolphins' (Tursiops truncatus) social structure". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 62 (6): 887–894. doi:10.1007/s00265-007-0512-1. S2CID 24197908.
  114. ^ Wells, R.S., M.D. Scott and A.B. Irvine. (1987) "The social structure of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins", pp. 247–305 in: Genoways, H. (ed.), Current Mammalogy, Vol. 1. New York: Plenum Press.
  115. ^ LUSSEAU, DAVID; WILSON, BEN; HAMMOND, PHILIP S.; GRELLIER, KATE; DURBAN, JOHN W.; PARSONS, KIM M.; BARTON, TIM R.; THOMPSON, PAUL M. (January 2006). "Quantifying the influence of sociality on population structure in bottlenose dolphins". Journal of Animal Ecology. 75 (1): 14–24. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2005.01013.x. ISSN 0021-8790. PMID 16903039.
  116. ^ Acevedo-Gutiérrez, Alejandro; William F. Perrin; Bernd G. Würsig; J. G. M. Thewissen (2008). "Group behavior". Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals (2 ed.). United States: Academic Press. pp. 511–520. ISBN 978-0-12-373553-9.
  117. ^ Díaz López, Bruno; Shirai J.B. (2008). "Marine aquaculture off Sardinia Island (Italy):ecosystem effects evaluated through a trophic mass-balance model". Ecological Modelling. 212 (3–4): 292–303. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2007.10.028.
  118. ^ Díaz López, Bruno; Shirai J.B. (2006). "Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) presence and incidental capture in a marine fish farm on the northeastern coast of Sardinia (Italy)". Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom. 87 (1): 113–117. doi:10.1017/S0025315407054215. S2CID 86115152.
  119. ^ "Comparative Mammalian Brain Collections – Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus)". Retrieved September 30, 2008.
  120. ^ Duffy-Echevarria, Erin E.; Connor, Richard C.; Aubin, David J. St. (January 1, 2008). "Observations of strand-feeding behavior by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in Bull Creek, South Carolina". Marine Mammal Science. 24 (1): 202–206. doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2007.00151.x.(subscription required)
  121. ^ Engleby, Laura K.; Powell, Jessica R. (2019). "Detailed observations and mechanisms of mud ring feeding by common bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus truncatus) in Florida Bay, Florida, U.S.A." Marine Mammal Science. 35 (3): 1162–1172. doi:10.1111/mms.12583. ISSN 1748-7692. S2CID 91392196.
  122. ^ Díaz López, Bruno (2006). "Interaction between bottlenose dolphins and fisheries off Sardinia". ICES Journal of Marine Science. 63 (5): 946–951. doi:10.1016/j.icesjms.2005.06.012.
  123. ^ . CNN. March 12, 2008. Archived from the original on May 12, 2008.
  124. ^ Brooks, David (March 12, 2008). . Herald Sun. Australia. Archived from the original on April 29, 2008.
  125. ^ Tennenhouse, Erica (July 30, 2019). "Dolphin mom adopts whale calf—a first". National Geographic. National Geographic Society. Retrieved July 31, 2019.
  126. ^ Read, Andrew (1999). Porpoises. Stillwater, MN, USA: Voyageur Press. ISBN 978-0-89658-420-4.
  127. ^ "Evidence Puts Dolphins in New Light, as Killers". Luna.pos.to. July 6, 1999. Retrieved June 12, 2012.
  128. ^ Ryan, Conor (2008). "Attack on Harbour Porpoise (Phocoena phocoena (L. 1758) by Bottle-nosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus (Montagu 1821)". Irish Naturalists' Journal. 29 (2): 130.
  129. ^ Dolphin attacks on porpoises baffle experts BBC News, July 15, 2014
  130. ^ Reeves, R.; Stewart, B.; Clapham, P.; Powell, J. (2002). Guide to Marine Mammals of the World. New York: A.A. Knopf. p. 434. ISBN 978-0-375-41141-0.
  131. ^ NOAA (2003). "Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): California/Oregon/Washington Offshore Stock" (PDF). nmfs.noaa.gov. Retrieved September 21, 2008.
  132. ^ Shirihai, H.; Jarrett, B. (2006). Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World. Princeton: Princeton Univ. Press. p. 166. ISBN 978-0-691-12757-6.
  133. ^ a b c d e "Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". Animal Corner. Retrieved September 16, 2008.
  134. ^ Heithaus, M.; Dill, L. (2002). "Food Availability and Tiger Shark Predation Risk Influence Bottlenose Dolphin Habitat Use". Ecology. 83 (2): 480–491. doi:10.1890/0012-9658(2002)083[0480:FAATSP]2.0.CO;2. S2CID 14944929.
  135. ^ Gannon, D.; Barros, N.; Nowacek, D.; Read, A.; Waples, D.; Wells, R. (2004). "Prey detection by bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus: an experimental test of the passive listening hypothesis". Animal Behaviour. 69 (3): 709–720. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.06.020. S2CID 53201926.
  136. ^ Eisfeld, S (October 2003). (PDF). MSc Thesis, School of Biological Sciences, University of Wales, Bangor. p. 42. Archived from the original (PDF) on October 2, 2008.
  137. ^ The Dolphin Institute. . dolphin-institute.org. Archived from the original on April 5, 2002. Retrieved March 6, 2016.
  138. ^ Thomson, Ainsley (November 25, 2004). "Dolphins saved us from shark, lifeguards say". The New Zealand Herald.
  139. ^ Diaz Lopez, B.; Shirai, J.A.; Prieto, A.; Fernández, P. (2008). (PDF). Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the UK. 88 (6): 1153–1157. doi:10.1017/S0025315408000921. S2CID 85607255. Archived from the original (PDF) on January 31, 2009.
  140. ^ Dr. Moti Nissani (2008). "Cooperative Dolphins of Laguna: Data on Nature of Signal (video and detailed description)". YouTube. Archived from the original on December 12, 2021.
  141. ^ Simões-Lopes, Paulo C. (1998). "Dolphin interactions with the mullet artisanal fishing on southern Brazil: a qualitative and quantitative approach". Revista Brasileira de Zoologia. 15 (3): 709–726. doi:10.1590/s0101-81751998000300016.
  142. ^ . Archived from the original on April 21, 2008. Retrieved August 11, 2008.
  143. ^ Australian Government. . Department of the Environment, Water, Heritage and the Arts. Archived from the original on June 29, 2007. Retrieved October 24, 2008.
  144. ^ Waples and Gales 2002. Evaluating and minimising social stress in the care of captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) http://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/zoo.10004/abstract
  145. ^ Alave, Kristine L. (September 12, 2007). . Archived from the original on January 17, 2008.
  146. ^ "Katrina dolphin rescue launched". BBC. September 15, 2005. Retrieved February 14, 2008.
  147. ^ "Dolphins Deployed as Undersea Agents in Iraq". National Geographic. Retrieved January 18, 2009.
  148. ^ Rehn, KW; Riggs, PK (2002). . U.S. Space and Naval Warfare Systems Center Technical Report. Document Number 3138. Archived from the original on May 30, 2011. Retrieved September 25, 2008.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  149. ^ . U.S. Navy. U.S. Navy Marine Mammal Program. Archived from the original on January 15, 2009.
  150. ^ Reuters (1994), Female-friendly dolphin kills male swimmer in Brazil, article retrieved October 26, 2006.
  151. ^ . Archived from the original on July 25, 2008. Retrieved August 31, 2008.
  152. ^ "Flipper (1995)". IMDb. October 1995. Retrieved November 3, 2006.
  153. ^ Scully, Mike (2009). The Simpsons The Complete Twelfth Season DVD commentary for the episode "Treehouse of Horror IX" (DVD). 20th Century Fox.
  154. ^ "Dolphin Tale". Rotten Tomatoes. Flixster. Retrieved January 9, 2016.
  155. ^ "Dolphin Tale 2". Rotten Tomatoes. Flixster. Retrieved January 9, 2016.
  156. ^ Sports Ecyclopedia. "Miami dolphins (1996-Present)". sportsecyclopedia.com. Retrieved March 6, 2016.
  157. ^ Constantine, R. (2002). "Folklore and Legends". In Perrin, W.; Würsig, B.; Thewissen, J (eds.). Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. Academic Press. p. 448. ISBN 978-0-12-551340-1.
  158. ^ "More than 80% of Indian Ocean dolphins may have been killed by commercial fishing, study finds". TheGuardian.com. March 2, 2020.
  159. ^ Betts KS (May 2007). . Environmental Health Perspectives. 115 (5): A250–A256. doi:10.1289/ehp.115-a250. PMC 1867999. PMID 17520044. Archived from the original on June 27, 2007.
  160. ^ Betts KS (2008). "Not immune to PFOS effects?". Environ. Health Perspect. 116 (7): A290. doi:10.1289/ehp.116-a290a. PMC 2453185. PMID 18629339.
  161. ^ Houde M; Martin JW; Letcher RJ; Solomon KR; Muir DC (2006). "Biological monitoring of polyfluoroalkyl substances: A review". Environmental Science & Technology. 40 (11): 3463–73. Bibcode:2006EnST...40.3463H. doi:10.1021/es052580b. PMID 16786681.
  162. ^ Lavery TJ, Butterfield N, Kemper CM, Reid RJ, Sanderson K (2008). "Metals and selenium in the liver and bone of three dolphin species from South Australia, 1988–2004". The Science of the Total Environment. 390 (1): 77–85. Bibcode:2008ScTEn.390...77L. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2007.09.016. PMID 18006044.
  163. ^ Lavery TJ, Kemper CM, Sanderson K, et al. (2009). "Heavy metal toxicity of kidney and bone tissues in South Australian adult bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus)" (PDF). Marine Environmental Research. 67 (1): 1–7. doi:10.1016/j.marenvres.2008.09.005. PMID 19012959.
  164. ^ Curran S, Wilson B, Thompson P (1996). . Scottish Natural Heritage Review. 56. Archived from the original on December 22, 2015. Retrieved August 27, 2013.
  165. ^ Currey RJ, Dawson SM, Slooten E, Schneider K, Lusseau D, Boisseau OJ, Haase P, Williams JA (November 25, 2008). "Survival rates for a declining population of bottlenose dolphins in Doubtful Sound, New Zealand: an information theoretic approach to assessing the role of human impacts". Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems. 19 (6): 658–670. doi:10.1002/aqc.1015.
  166. ^ Currey, Rohan; M. Dawson, Stephen; Slooten, Elisabeth; Schneider, Karsten; Lusseau, David; J. Boisseau, Oliver (2009). "Survival rates for a declining population of bottlenose dolphins in Doubtful Sound, New Zealand: An information theoretic approach to assessing the role of human impacts". Aquatic Conservation: Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems. 19 (6): 658–670. doi:10.1002/aqc.1015.
  167. ^ Manlik O.; McDonald J.A.; Mann J.; Raudino H.C.; Bejder L.; Krützen M.; Connor R.C.; Heithaus M.R.; Lacy R.C.; Sherwin W.B. (2016). "The relative importance of reproduction and survival for the conservation of two dolphin populations". Ecology and Evolution. 6 (11): 3496–3512. doi:10.1002/ece3.2130. PMC 5513288. PMID 28725349.
  168. ^ Marine Mammal Center. "The Marine Mammal Protection Act of 1972, Amended 1994". marinemammalcenter.org. Retrieved March 6, 2016.

Further reading

  • Berrow, S.D. (2009). (PDF). Irish Naturalists' Journal. 30 (1): 35–39. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 21, 2011.
  • Hale, P.T.; Barreto, A.S.; Ross, G.J.B (2000). "Comparative morphology and distribution of the aduncus and truncatus forms of bottlenose dolphin Tursiops in the Indian and Western Pacific Oceans" (PDF). Aquatic Mammals. 26 (2): 101–110. — Discusses distinguishing features between Bottlenose Dolphin species
  • Reiss D, Marino L (2001). "Mirror self-recognition in the bottlenose dolphin: a case of cognitive convergence". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 98 (10): 5937–5942. Bibcode:2001PNAS...98.5937R. doi:10.1073/pnas.101086398. PMC 33317. PMID 11331768.

External links

  • Voices in the Sea Sounds and Videos of the Bottlenose Dolphin April 20, 2013, at the Wayback Machine
  • Tropical Dolphin Research Foundation

bottlenose, dolphin, this, article, about, genus, bottlenose, dolphins, common, species, common, bottlenose, dolphin, aquatic, mammals, genus, tursiops, they, common, cosmopolitan, members, family, delphinidae, family, oceanic, dolphins, molecular, studies, sh. This article is about the genus of bottlenose dolphins For the common species see Common bottlenose dolphin Bottlenose dolphins are aquatic mammals in the genus Tursiops They are common cosmopolitan members of the family Delphinidae the family of oceanic dolphins 2 Molecular studies show the genus definitively contains two species the common bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus and the Indo Pacific bottlenose dolphin Tursiops aduncus 3 Others like the Burrunan dolphin Tursiops aduncus australis may be alternately considered their own species or be subspecies of T aduncus 4 Bottlenose dolphins inhabit warm and temperate seas worldwide being found everywhere except for the Arctic and Antarctic Circle regions Their name derives from the Latin tursio dolphin and truncatus for their characteristic truncated teeth 5 Bottlenose dolphinTemporal range Miocene Recent 3 6 0 Ma PreꞒ Ꞓ O S D C P T J K Pg N 1 Bottlenose dolphin surfing in the wake of a boatSize compared to an average humanScientific classificationKingdom AnimaliaPhylum ChordataClass MammaliaOrder ArtiodactylaInfraorder CetaceaFamily DelphinidaeSubfamily DelphininaeGenus TursiopsGervais 1855Type speciesDelphinus truncatusMontagu 1821SpeciesTursiops truncatus Tursiops aduncusBottlenose dolphin range in blue Numerous investigations of bottlenose dolphin intelligence have been conducted examining mimicry use of artificial language object categorization and self recognition They can use tools sponging using marine sponges to forage for food sources they normally could not access 6 and transmit cultural knowledge from generation to generation and their considerable intelligence has driven interaction with humans Bottlenose dolphins gained popularity from aquarium shows and television programs such as Flipper They have also been trained by militaries to locate sea mines or detect and mark enemy divers In some areas they cooperate with local fishermen by driving fish into their nets and eating the fish that escape Some encounters with humans are harmful to the dolphins people hunt them for food and dolphins are killed inadvertently as a bycatch of tuna fishing and by getting caught in crab traps Bottlenose dolphins have the third largest encephalization levels of any mammal on Earth humans have the largest followed by Northern Right whale dolphins sharing close ratios with those of humans and other cetaceans while being twice as high of other great apes 7 This more than likely contributes to their high intelligence and emotional intelligence 8 Contents 1 Taxonomy 1 1 Hybrids 1 2 Fossil species 2 Description 3 Anatomy 4 Physiology and senses 4 1 Senses 4 2 Communication 5 Intelligence 5 1 Cognition 5 2 Tool use and culture 5 3 Cortical neurons 6 Life history 6 1 Respiration and sleep 6 2 Reproduction 6 3 Social interaction 7 Ecology 7 1 Feeding 7 2 Relations with other species 7 3 Predators 8 Relation to humans 8 1 Interaction 8 2 Cultural influence 8 3 Threats 8 4 Conservation 9 See also 10 References 11 Further reading 12 External linksTaxonomy EditScientists have been long aware of the fact that the Tursiops dolphins might consist of more than one species as there is extensive variation in color and morphology along its range In the past most studies used morphology to evaluate differences between and within species but in the late 20th century combining morphological and molecular genetics allowed much greater insight into this previously intractable problem 9 Since the late 1990s and early 2000s most researchers acknowledged the existence of two species 10 the common bottlenose dolphin T truncatus found in coastal and oceanic habitats of most tropical to temperate oceans and the Indo Pacific bottlenose dolphin T aduncus that lives in coastal waters around India northern Australia South China the Red Sea and the eastern coast of Africa In 2011 a third distinct species was described the Burrunan dolphin T aduncus australis found in the Port Phillip and Gippsland Lakes areas of Victoria Australia after research showed it was distinct from T truncatus and T aduncus both in morphology 11 and genetics 12 Also evidence has been accumulating to validate the existence of a separate species Lahille s bottlenose dolphin T gephyreus 13 14 15 16 that occurs in coastal waters of Argentina Uruguay and southern Brazil The Society for Marine Mammalogy s Committee on Taxonomy 17 presently recognizes only two species T truncatus and T aduncus and two subspecies the Black Sea bottlenose dolphin T t ponticus that lives in the Black Sea and Lahille s bottlenose dolphin T t gephyreus Other sources also accept the Pacific bottlenose dolphin T t gillii or T gillii 18 that inhabits the Pacific and has a black line from the eye to the forehead 19 failed verification The IUCN on their Red List of endangered species also recognises only two species of bottlenose dolphins 20 The American Society of Mammalogists also recognizes only two species while acknowledging the studies describing T australis it classifies it within T aduncus 4 Indo Pacific bottlenose dolphin T aduncus Much of the discussion and doubts about its taxonomy is related to the existence of two ecotypes of bottlenose dolphins in many part of its distribution The two ecotypes of the common bottlenose dolphin within the western North Atlantic 21 are represented by the shallower water or coastal ecotype and the more offshore ecotype 21 Their ranges overlap but they have been shown to be genetically distinct 21 They are not currently described however as separate species or subspecies In general genetic variation between populations is significant even among nearby populations 22 As a result of this genetic variation other distinct species currently considered to be populations of common bottlenose dolphin are possible 22 Some recent genetic evidence suggests the Indo Pacific bottlenose dolphin belongs in the genus Stenella since it is more like the Atlantic spotted dolphin Stenella frontalis than the common bottlenose dolphin 23 However more recent studies indicate that this is a consequence of reticulate evolution such as past hybridization between Stenella and ancestral Tursiops and incomplete lineage sorting and thus support T truncatus and T aduncus belonging to the same genus 24 Wolphin Kawili Kai at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii The fossil species Tursiops osennae Hybrids Edit Bottlenose dolphins have been known to hybridize with other dolphin species Hybrids with Risso s dolphin occur both in the wild and in captivity 25 26 The best known hybrid is the wholphin a false killer whale bottlenose dolphin hybrid The wholphin is fertile and two currently live at the Sea Life Park in Hawaii The first was born in 1985 to a female bottlenose Wholphins also exist in the wild 27 In captivity a bottlenose dolphin and a rough toothed dolphin hybridized 28 A common dolphin bottlenose dolphin hybrid born in captivity lives at SeaWorld California 29 30 Other hybrids live in captivity around the world and in the wild such as a bottlenose dolphin Atlantic spotted dolphin hybrid 31 Fossil species Edit Bottlenose dolphins appeared during the Miocene 32 Known fossil species include Tursiops osennae late Miocene to early Pliocene 33 from the Piacenzian coastal mudstone and Tursiops miocaenus Miocene from the Burdigalian marine sandstone all in Italy 1 Description EditThe bottlenose dolphin weighs an average of 300 kg 660 pounds but can range from 150 and 650 kg 330 and 1 430 lb 34 It can reach a length of just over 4 meters 13 feet Its color varies considerably is usually dark gray on the back and lighter gray on the flanks but it can be bluish grey brownish grey or even nearly black and is often darker on the back from the rostrum to behind the dorsal fin This is called countershading and is a form of camouflage Older dolphins sometimes have a few spots Bottlenose dolphins can live for more than 40 years Females typically live 5 10 years longer than males with some females exceeding 60 years 35 36 37 This extreme age is rare and less than 2 of all Bottlenose dolphins will live longer than 60 years 38 Bottlenose dolphins can jump to a height of 6 metres 20 feet in the air 39 Anatomy EditSee also Dolphin Anatomy Their elongated upper and lower jaws form what is called a rostrum or snout which gives the animal its common name 40 The real functional nose is the blowhole on top of its head the nasal septum is visible when the blowhole is open 35 Bottlenose dolphin head showing rostrum and blowhole Bottlenose dolphins have 18 to 28 conical teeth on each side of each jaw 35 40 The flukes lobes of the tail and dorsal fin are formed of dense connective tissue and do not contain bone or muscle The dorsal fin usually shows phenotypic variations that help discriminate among populations 41 The animal propels itself by moving the flukes up and down The pectoral flippers at the sides of the body are for steering they contain bones homologous to the forelimbs of land mammals A bottlenose dolphin discovered in Japan has two additional pectoral fins or hind legs at the tail about the size of a human s pair of hands Scientists believe a mutation caused the ancient trait to reassert itself as a form of atavism 42 Physiology and senses Edit Dolphin and a paddler at Dalkey Island In colder waters they have more body fat and blood and are more suited to deeper diving Typically 18 20 of their body weight is blubber 43 Most research in this area has been restricted to the North Atlantic Ocean 44 Bottlenose dolphins typically swim at 5 to 11 km h 1 4 to 3 1 m s but are capable of bursts of up to 29 to 35 km h 8 1 to 9 7 m s The higher speeds can only be sustained for a short time 45 46 Senses Edit The dolphin s search for food is aided by a form of sonar known as echolocation it locates objects by producing sounds and listening for the echoes A broadband burst pulse of clicking sounds is emitted in a focused beam in front of the dolphin When the clicking sounds hit an object in the water like a fish or rock they bounce off and come back to the dolphin as echoes Echolocation tells the dolphins the shape size speed distance and location of the object 39 To hear the returning echo they have two small ear openings behind the eyes but most sound waves are transmitted to the inner ear through the lower jaw As the object of interest is approached the echo becomes booming and the dolphins adjust by decreasing the intensity of the emitted sounds This contrasts with bats and sonar which reduce the sensitivity of the sound receptor The interclick interval also decreases as the animal nears the target Evidently the dolphin waits for each click s echo before clicking again Echolocation details such as signal strength spectral qualities and discrimination are well understood by researchers 47 Bottlenose dolphins are also able to extract shape information suggesting they are able to form an echoic image or sound picture of their targets 48 Dolphins have sharp eyesight The eyes are located at the sides of the head and have a tapetum lucidum or reflecting membrane at the back of the retina which aids vision in dim light Their horseshoe shaped double slit pupils enable dolphins to have good vision both in air and underwater despite the different indices of refraction of these media 49 When under water the eyeball s lens serves to focus light whereas in the in air environment the typically bright light serves to contract the specialized pupil resulting in sharpness from a smaller aperture similar to a pinhole camera 50 By contrast a bottlenose s sense of smell is poor 51 because its blowhole the analog to the nose is closed when underwater and it opens only for breathing It has no olfactory nerves or olfactory lobe in the brain 51 Bottlenose dolphins are able to detect salty sweet bitter quinine sulphate and sour citric acid tastes but this has not been well studied 51 Anecdotally some individuals in captivity have been noted to have preferences for food fish types although it is not clear if taste mediates this preference 51 In 2022 a study at the University of St Andrews in Scotland found that dolphins were able to identify their friends and family members by the taste of their urine in the water 52 53 Communication Edit Bottlenose dolphins communicate through burst pulsed sounds whistles and body language 54 Examples of body language include leaping out of the water snapping jaws slapping the tail on the surface and butting heads 55 Sounds and gestures help keep track of other dolphins in the group and alert other dolphins to danger and nearby food Lacking vocal cords they produce sounds using six air sacs near their blow hole Each animal has a uniquely identifying frequency modulated narrow band signature vocalization signature whistle 56 Researchers from the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute BDRI based in Sardinia Italy have now shown whistles and burst pulsed sounds are vital to the animals social life and mirror their behaviors 57 The tonal whistle sounds the most melodious ones allow dolphins to stay in contact with each other above all mothers and offspring and to coordinate hunting strategies 58 The burst pulsed sounds which are more complex and varied than the whistles are used to avoid physical aggression in situations of high excitement such as when they are competing for the same piece of food for example The dolphins emit these strident sounds when in the presence of other individuals moving towards the same prey The least dominant one soon moves away to avoid confrontation 57 Other communication uses about 30 distinguishable sounds and although famously proposed by John C Lilly in the 1950s no dolphin language has been found However Herman Richards and Wolz demonstrated comprehension of an artificial language by two bottlenose dolphins named Akeakamai and Phoenix in the period of skepticism toward animal language following Herbert Terrace s critique 59 Intelligence EditSee also Cetacean intelligence Bottlenose dolphin responding to human hand gestures Cognition Edit Cognitive abilities that have been investigated include concept formation sensory skills and mental representations Such research has been ongoing since the 1970s This includes acoustic and behavioral mimicry 60 61 comprehension of novel sequences in an artificial language 62 63 memory 64 monitoring of self behavior 65 discrimination and matching 64 66 comprehension of symbols for various body parts 67 comprehension of pointing gestures and gaze as made by dolphins or humans 68 69 mirror self recognition 70 71 and numerical values 72 Tool use and culture Edit See also Dolphins Behavior At least some wild bottlenose dolphins use tools In Shark Bay off Western Australia dolphins place a marine sponge on their rostrum presumably to protect it when searching for food on the sandy sea bottom 73 This has only been observed in this bay first in 1997 and is predominantly practiced by females A 2005 study showed mothers most likely teach the behavior to their offspring evincing culture behavior learned from other species members 74 75 Mud plume feeding is a feeding technique performed by a small community of bottlenose dolphins over shallow seagrass beds less than 1 m in the Florida Keys in the United States The behavior involves creation of a U shaped plume of mud in the water column and then rushing through the plume to capture fish 76 Along the beaches and tidal marshes of South Carolina and Georgia in the United States bottlenose dolphins cooperatively herd prey fish onto steep and sandy banks in a practice known as strand feeding Groups of between two and six dolphins are regularly observed creating a bow wave to force the fish out of the water The dolphins follow the fish stranding themselves briefly to eat their prey before twisting their bodies back and forth in order to slide back into the water 77 While initially documented in South Carolina and Georgia strand feeding has also been observed in Louisiana Texas Baja California Ecuador and Australia 78 Some Mauritanian dolphins cooperate with human fishermen The dolphins drive a school of fish towards the shore where humans await with nets In the confusion of casting nets the dolphins catch a large number of fish as well Intraspecies cooperative foraging has also been observed These behaviors may also be transmitted via teaching Controversially Rendell and Whitehead have proposed a structure for the study of cetacean culture 79 80 Similar cases have been observed in Laguna Santa Catarina in Brazil since during 19th century as well 81 82 Near Adelaide in South Australia several bottlenose dolphins tail walk whereby they elevate the upper part of their bodies vertically out of the water and propel themselves along the surface with powerful tail movements Tail walking mostly arises via human training in dolphinaria In the 1980s a female from the local population was kept at a local dolphinarium for three weeks and the scientist suggests she copied the tail walking behavior from other dolphins Two other wild adult female dolphins copied it from her 83 and the behaviour has continued through generations until 2022 84 85 86 A study conducted by the University of Chicago showed that bottlenose dolphins can remember whistles of other dolphins they had lived with after 20 years of separation Each dolphin has a unique whistle that functions like a name allowing the marine mammals to keep close social bonds The new research shows that dolphins have the longest memory yet known in any species other than humans 87 84 The bottlenose dolphins of John s Pass in Boca Ciega Bay St Petersburg Florida exhibit a rare form of self decoration and social object use called grass wearing Self decoration by wearing grass appears to be an attention getting device rather than purely play and varies from a single blade to large clusters of grass John s Pass dolphins self decorate with grass primarily when they form new social groups or engage in procreative activities Grass wearing behavior among these dolphins is a local behavioral tradition that could constitute a cultural difference from other communities 88 Cortical neurons Edit Some researchers hypothesize that the number of nerve cells neurons in the cortex of the brain predicts intelligence in mammals 89 A 2019 study estimated the number of neurons in the cerebral cortex of three common bottlenose dolphins and found numbers ranging from 11 7 to 15 2 billion neurons 90 The human average being approximately 16 billion this is likely within the range found in the human population 91 Life history EditBottlenose dolphins have a lifespan of 40 60 years Females can outlive males and live for 60 years or more Dolphins start to reproduce aged 5 to 15 years 92 Respiration and sleep Edit The bottlenose dolphin has a single blowhole located on the dorsal surface of the head consisting of a hole and a muscular flap The flap is closed during muscle relaxation and opens during contraction 93 Dolphins are voluntary breathers who must deliberately surface and open their blowholes to get air They can store almost twice as much oxygen in proportion to their body weight as a human can the dolphin can store 36 milliliters ml of oxygen per kg of body weight compared with 20 ml per kg for humans This is an adaptation to diving 94 The bottlenose dolphin typically rises to the surface to breathe through its blowhole two to three times per minute 43 although it can remain submerged for up to 20 minutes 95 Dolphins can breathe while half asleep During the sleeping cycle one brain hemisphere remains active while the other hemisphere shuts down The active hemisphere handles surfacing and breathing behavior 96 The daily sleeping cycle lasts for about 8 hours in increments of minutes to hours During the sleeping cycle they remain near the surface swimming slowly or logging and occasionally closing one eye 95 Reproduction Edit See also Animal sexual behaviour Bottlenose dolphins and Dolphin Reproduction and sexuality Mother and juvenile bottlenose dolphins head to the seafloor Both sexes have genital slits on the underside of their bodies The male can retract and conceal his penis through his slit 97 98 The female s slit houses her vagina and anus Females have two mammary slits each housing one nipple one on each side of the genital slit 99 The ability to stow their reproductive organs especially in males allows for maximum hydrodynamics The breeding season produces significant physiological changes in males At that time the testes enlarge enabling them to hold more sperm Large amounts of sperm allow a male to wash away the previous suitor s sperm while leaving some of his own for fertilization Also sperm concentration markedly increases Having less sperm for out of season social mating means it wastes less This suggests sperm production is energetically expensive Males have large testes in relation to their body size 100 During the breeding season males compete for access to females Such competition can take the form of fighting other males or of herding females to prevent access by other males 101 102 In Shark Bay male bottlenose dolphins have been observed working in pairs or larger groups to follow and or restrict the movement of a female for weeks at a time waiting for her to become sexually receptive 101 103 These coalitions also known as male reproductive alliances will fight with other coalitions for control of females 103 Humans and dolphins are the only species that share this type of gang formation habit as a form of cooperation 104 Mating occurs belly to belly 102 Dolphins have been observed engaging in intercourse when the females are not in their estrous cycles and cannot produce young suggesting they may mate for pleasure 105 106 The gestation period averages 12 months 22 Births can occur at any time of year although peaks occur in warmer months 2 The young are born in shallow water sometimes assisted by a possibly male midwife and usually only a single calf is born 107 Twins are possible but rare Newborn bottlenose dolphins are 0 8 to 1 4 m 2 6 to 4 6 ft long and weigh 9 to 30 kg 20 to 66 lb with Indo Pacific bottlenose dolphin infants being generally smaller than common bottlenose dolphin infants 22 To accelerate nursing the mother can eject milk from her mammary glands The calf suckles for 18 months to up to 8 years 108 and continues to closely associate with its mother for several years after weaning 35 Females sexually mature at ages 5 13 males at ages 9 14 2 Females reproduce every two to six years Reproduction is moderately seasonal September January peaking from October to December Calf loss between August and December is followed by rapid conception 1 2 months whereas conception is delayed 2 9 months if calf loss occurs between January and July Weaning ages ranged from 2 7 to 8 0 years but 66 7 42 calves were weaned by their fourth birthday Females tended to wean mid pregnancy Group size was unrelated to water depth or female reproductive success but reproductive success was predicted by water depth Shallow water may allow mothers and calves to detect and avoid predatory sharks Alternatively or additionally prey density may be higher in shallow water compared to deep water 22 Georgetown University professor Janet Mann argues the strong personal behavior among male calves is about bond formation and benefits the species in an evolutionary context She cites studies showing these dolphins as adults are inseparable and that early bonds aid protection as well as in locating females 109 Female bottlenose dolphins have to expend additional energy in carrying out parental care e g infant carrying behavior Dolphins don t physically hold their infants but line up in an echelon position with infants swimming beside them This position creates a change of water flow pattern from the infant which minimizes separation between the mother and infant but also increases the mother s surface area and creates a drag for the swimmer This also leaves less energy to use in swimming speed foraging and predator evasion 110 Social interaction Edit An adult female bottlenose dolphin with her young Moray Firth Scotland Adult males live mostly alone or in groups of two to three and join pods for short periods of time Adult females and young dolphins normally live in groups of up to 15 animals 22 Males give strong mutual support if other males help them even if they are not friends 111 However they live in fission fusion societies of varying group size within which individuals change associations often on a daily or hourly basis 112 113 Group compositions are usually determined by sex age reproductive condition familial relations and affiliation histories In a dolphin community near Sarasota Florida the most common group types are adult females with their recent offspring older subadults of both sexes and adult males either alone or in bonded pairs 114 Smaller groups can join to form larger groups of 100 or more and occasionally exceed 1 000 22 115 The social strategies of marine mammals such as bottlenose dolphins provide interesting parallels with the social strategies of elephants and chimpanzees 116 519 Bottlenose dolphins studied by Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute researchers off the island of Sardinia show random social behavior while feeding and their social behavior does not depend on feeding activity 117 In Sardinia the presence of a floating marine fin fish farm has been linked to a change in bottlenose dolphin distribution as a result of high fish density around the floating cages in the farming area 118 Ecology EditFeeding Edit Fish is one of the main items in the dolphin diet They also eat shrimps squid mollusks and cuttlefish and only swallow the soft parts They eat 22 pounds of fish a day When they encounter a shoal of fish they work as a team to herd them towards the shore to maximize the harvest 22 They also hunt alone often targeting bottom dwelling species The bottlenose dolphin sometimes hits a fish with its fluke sometimes knocking it out of the water using a strategy called fish whacking 36 119 Strand feeding is an inherited feeding technique used by bottlenose dolphins near and around coastal regions of Georgia and South Carolina When a pod finds a school of fish they will circle the school and trap the fish in a mini whirlpool Then the dolphins will charge at the school and push their bodies up onto a mud flat forcing the fish on the mud flat as well The dolphins then crawl around on their sides consuming the fish they washed up on shore This happens only during low tides 120 One type of feeding behavior seen in bottlenose dolphins is mud ring feeding 121 Bottlenose dolphins conflict with small scale coastal commercial fisheries in some Mediterranean areas Common bottlenose dolphins are probably attracted to fishing nets because they offer a concentrated food source 122 Relations with other species Edit A bottlenose dolphin attacks and kills a harbour porpoise at Chanonry Point Scotland Dolphins can exhibit altruistic behaviour toward other sea creatures On Mahia Beach New Zealand on March 10 2008 123 two pygmy sperm whales a female and calf stranded on the beach Rescuers attempted to refloat them four times Shortly a playful bottlenose dolphin known to local residents as Moko arrived and after apparently vocalizing at the whales led them 200 m 660 ft along a sandbar to the open sea saving them from imminent euthanasia 124 In 2019 a female was observed caring for a juvenile melon headed whale the first reported instance of a bottlenose dolphin adopting a non conspecific infant 125 The bottlenose dolphin can behave aggressively Males fight for rank and access to females During mating season males compete vigorously with each other through displays of toughness and size with a series of acts such as head butting They display aggression towards sharks and smaller dolphin species At least one population off Scotland has practiced infanticide and also has attacked and killed harbour porpoises University of Aberdeen researchers say the dolphins do not eat their victims but are simply competing for food 126 However Dr Read of Duke University a porpoise expert researching similar cases of porpoise killings that had occurred in Virginia in 1996 and 1997 holds a different view He states dolphins and porpoises feed on different types of fish thus food competition is an unlikely cause of the killings 127 Similar behaviour has been observed in Ireland 128 In the first half of July 2014 four attacks with three porpoise fatalities were observed and caught on video by the Cardigan Bay Marine Wildlife Centre in the Cardigan Bay Wales 129 The bottlenose dolphin sometimes forms mixed species groups with other species from the dolphin family particularly larger species such as the short finned pilot whale the false killer whale and Risso s dolphin 101 130 131 They also interact with smaller species such as the Atlantic spotted dolphin and the rough toothed dolphin 101 132 While interactions with smaller species are sometimes affiliative they can also be hostile 101 Predators Edit Some large shark species such as the tiger shark the dusky shark the great white shark and the bull shark prey on the bottlenose dolphin especially calves 102 133 134 135 136 The bottlenose dolphin is capable of defending itself by charging the predator dolphin mobbing behavior of sharks can occasionally prove fatal for the shark 133 Targeting a single adult dolphin can be dangerous for a shark of similar size Killer whale populations in New Zealand and Peru have been observed preying on bottlenose dolphins but this seems rare 133 and other orcas may swim with dolphins Swimming in pods allows dolphins to better defend themselves against predators Bottlenose dolphins either use complex evasive strategies to outswim their predators or mobbing techniques to batter the predator to death or force it to flee 137 Relation to humans EditInteraction Edit At Notojima Aquarium Japan The species sometimes shows curiosity towards humans in or near water Occasionally they rescue injured divers by raising them to the surface They also do this to help injured members of their own species 133 In November 2004 a dramatic report of dolphin intervention came from New Zealand Four lifeguards swimming 100 m 330 ft off the coast near Whangarei were approached by a shark reportedly a great white shark Bottlenose dolphins herded the swimmers together and surrounded them for 40 minutes preventing the shark from attacking as they slowly swam to shore 138 In coastal regions dolphins run the risk of colliding with boats Researchers of the Bottlenose Dolphin Research Institute first quantified data about solitary bottlenose dolphin diving behavior in the presence and absence of boats Dolphins responded more to tourist than fishing vessels Driving behavior speed engine type and separation distance all affected dolphin safety 139 However dolphins in these areas can also coexist with humans For example in the town of Laguna in south Brazil a pod of bottlenose dolphins resides in the estuary and some of its members cooperate with humans These cooperating dolphins are individually recognized by the local fishermen who name them The fishermen typically stand up to their knees in the shallow waters or sit in canoes waiting for the dolphins Now and then one or more dolphins appear driving the fish towards the line of fishermen One dolphin then displays a unique body movement outside the water which serves as a signal to the fishermen to cast their nets the entire sequence is shown here 82 and a detailed description of the signal s characteristics is available here 140 In this unique form of cooperation the dolphins gain because the fish are disoriented and because the fish cannot escape to shallow water where the larger dolphins cannot swim Likewise studies show that fishermen casting their nets following the unique signal catch more fish than when fishing alone without the help of the dolphins 141 The dolphins were not trained for this behavior the collaboration began before 1847 Similar cooperative fisheries also exist in Mauritania Africa 142 Commercial dolphin encounter enterprises and tours operate in many countries The documentary film The Cove documents how dolphins are captured and sold to some of these enterprises particularly in Asia while the remaining pod is slaughtered In addition to such endeavors the individuals swim with and surface near surfers at the beach 143 Bottlenose dolphins perform in many aquaria generating controversy Animal welfare activists and certain scientists have claimed that the dolphins do not have adequate space or receive adequate care or stimulation 144 However others notably SeaWorld counter by claiming that the dolphins are properly cared for have much environmental stimulation and enjoy interacting with humans 133 145 Eight bottlenose dolphins that lived at the Marine Life Aquarium in Gulfport Mississippi were swept away from their aquarium pool during Hurricane Katrina They were later found in the Gulf of Mexico and returned to captivity 146 K Dog trained by the United States Navy to find mines and boobytraps underwater leaping out of the water The military of the United States and Russia train bottlenose dolphins as military dolphins for wartime tasks such as locating sea mines and detecting enemy divers 147 148 The U S s program is the U S Navy Marine Mammal Program located in San Diego 149 Tiao was a well known solitary male bottlenose dolphin that was first spotted in the town of Sao Sebastiao in Brazil around 1994 and frequently allowed humans to interact with him The dolphin became infamous for killing a swimmer and injuring many others which later earned him the nickname Killer Dolphin 150 Cultural influence Edit The popular television show Flipper created by Ivan Tors portrayed a bottlenose dolphin 151 in a friendly relationship with two boys Sandy and Bud A seagoing Lassie Flipper understood English and was a hero Go tell Dad we re in trouble Flipper Hurry The show s theme song contains the lyric no one you see is smarter than he The television show was based on a 1963 film with a sequel Flipper s New Adventure 1964 and was remade as a feature film in 1996 starring Elijah Wood and Paul Hogan as well as a second TV series running from 1995 to 2000 starring Jessica Alba 152 Other television appearances by bottlenose dolphins include Wonder Woman Highway to Heaven Dolphin Cove seaQuest DSV and The Penguins of Madagascar in which a dolphin Doctor Blowhole is a villain In the HBO movie Zeus and Roxanne a female bottlenose dolphin befriends a male dog and in Secrets of the Bermuda Triangle 1996 Ian Toynton movie a girl named Annie played by Lisa Jakub swims with dolphins Human and dolphin interaction segments shot on location in the Florida Keys with Dolphin Research Center are featured on Sesame Street citation needed and on a Halloween episode of The Simpsons Treehouse of Horror XI 153 Dolphin Tale directed by Charles Martin Smith starring Nathan Gamble Ashley Judd Harry Connick Jr Morgan Freeman Cozi Zuehlsdorff and Kris Kristofferson is based on the real life story of the dolphin Winter who was rescued from a crab trap in December 2005 and lost her tail but learned to swim with a prosthetic one 154 Dolphin Tale 2 a sequel to the 2011 film featured another rescued dolphin named Hope and an appearance by Bethany Hamilton The sequel was released on September 12 2014 155 The NFL s Miami Dolphins uses the bottlenose dolphin as its mascot and team logo 156 Factual descriptions of the dolphins date back into antiquity the writings of Aristotle Oppian and Pliny the Elder all mention the species 2 157 Threats Edit Millions of dolphins drown in fishing nets Tuna fishing crews have been the most responsible for the largest number of deaths In 1972 the U S government passed a law limiting the number of dolphins that could be killed yearly by tuna fishing crews Dolphins in the United Kingdom have also been found to contain high levels of pollutants in their tissues Heavy metals including mercury PCB s and DDT are of great concern These pollutants can cause harm in dolphins growth development reproduction and immunity Since the mid 1990s hundreds of dolphins have been trained to perform in shows presented by aquariums zoos and amusement parks Scientists conduct various types of research to understand the dolphin s communication system 158 The man made chemical perfluorooctanesulfonic acid PFOS may be compromising the immune system of bottlenose dolphins 159 PFOS affects the immune system of male mice at a concentration of 91 5 ppb 160 while PFOS has been reported in bottlenose dolphins in excess of 1 ppm 161 High levels of metal contaminants have been measured in tissues in many areas of the globe A recent study found high levels of cadmium and mercury in bottlenose dolphins from South Australia 162 levels which were later found to be associated with kidney malformations indicating possible health effects of high heavy metal concentrations in dolphins 163 Conservation Edit Bottlenose dolphin at Hundred Islands National Park Bottlenose dolphins are not endangered Their future is stable because of their abundance and adaptability However specific populations are threatened due to various environmental changes The population in the Moray Firth in Scotland is estimated to consist of around 190 individuals and are under threat from harassment traumatic injury water pollution and reduction in food availability 164 Likewise an isolated population in Doubtful Sound New Zealand is in decline due to calf loss coincident to an increase in warm freshwater discharge into the fiord 165 Less local climate change such as increasing water temperature may also play a role but has never been shown to be the case 166 One of the largest coastal populations of bottlenose dolphins in Shark Bay Western Australia was forecast to be stable with little variation in mortality over time Manlik et al 2016 167 In United States waters hunting and harassing of marine mammals is forbidden in almost all circumstances from passage of the Marine Mammal Protection Act of 1972 168 See also EditUnihemispheric slow wave sleep Audiograms in mammals Cetacean intelligence Dolphinarium Common bottlenose dolphinReferences Edit a b Fossilworks Tursiops miocaenus www fossilworks org a b c d Wells R Scott M 2002 Bottlenose Dolphins In Perrin W Wursig B Thewissen J eds Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals Academic Press pp 122 127 ISBN 978 0 12 551340 1 Common Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops Truncatus Dolphin Facts and Information www dolphins world com Retrieved July 13 2017 a b Explore the Database www mammaldiversity org Retrieved September 5 2021 Wells R S Scott Michael January 1 2009 Common bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals 6 249 255 doi 10 1016 B978 0 12 373553 9 00062 6 Krutzen M Kreicker S MacLeod C D Learmonth J Kopps A M Walsham P Allen S J 2014 Cultural transmission of tool use by Indo Pacific bottlenose dolphins Tursiops sp provides access to a novel foraging niche Proceedings of the Royal Society B 281 1784 1 9 doi 10 1098 rspb 2014 0374 PMC 4043097 PMID 24759862 Osvaldo Cairo October 4 2011 External Measures of Cognition Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5 108 108 doi 10 3389 fnhum 2011 00108 PMC 3207484 PMID 22065955 Table 1 Marino Lori 2004 Cetacean Brain Evolution Multiplication Generates Complexity PDF International Society for Comparative Psychology The International Society for Comparative Psychology 17 1 16 Archived from the original PDF on March 10 2011 Natoli Ada Peddemors Victor M Rus Hoelzel A March 1 2004 Population structure and speciation in the genus Tursiops based on microsatellite and mitochondrial DNA analyses Journal of Evolutionary Biology 17 2 363 375 doi 10 1046 j 1420 9101 2003 00672 x ISSN 1420 9101 PMID 15009270 S2CID 19615055 Rice Dale W 1998 Marine mammals of the world systematics and distribution Special Publication Society of Marine Mammalogy ISBN 978 1 891276 03 3 Charlton Robb Kate Gershwin Lisa ann Thompson Ross Austin Jeremy Owen Kylie McKechnie Stephen September 14 2011 Fleischer Robert C ed A New Dolphin Species the Burrunan Dolphin Tursiops australis sp nov Endemic to Southern Australian Coastal Waters PLOS ONE 6 9 e24047 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 624047C doi 10 1371 journal pone 0024047 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3173360 PMID 21935372 Charlton Robb K Taylor A C McKechnie S W February 2015 Population genetic structure of the Burrunan dolphin Tursiops australis in coastal waters of south eastern Australia conservation implications Conservation Genetics 16 1 195 207 doi 10 1007 s10592 014 0652 6 ISSN 1566 0621 S2CID 16178397 Wickert Janaina Carrion von Eye Sophie Maillard Oliveira Larissa Rosa Moreno Ignacio Benites December 5 2016 Revalidation of Tursiops gephyreus Lahille 1908 Cetartiodactyla Delphinidae from the southwestern Atlantic Ocean Journal of Mammalogy 97 6 1728 1737 doi 10 1093 jmammal gyw139 ISSN 0022 2372 Hohl Leandro S L Sicuro Fernando L Wickert Janaina C Moreno Ignacio B Rocha Barbosa Oscar Barreto Andre S April 6 2020 Skull morphology of bottlenose dolphins from different ocean populations with emphasis on South America Journal of Morphology 281 6 564 577 doi 10 1002 jmor 21121 PMID 32249999 S2CID 214808512 Oliveira Larissa Rosa de Fraga Lucia D Ott Paulo H Siciliano Salvatore Lopes Fernando Almeida Raquel Wickert Janaina C Milmann Lucas Danilewicz Daniel Emin Lima Neusa Renata Meirelles Ana Carolina April 24 2019 Population structure phylogeography and genetic diversity of the common bottlenose dolphin in the tropical and subtropical southwestern Atlantic Ocean Journal of Mammalogy 100 2 564 577 doi 10 1093 jmammal gyz065 hdl 10923 20559 ISSN 0022 2372 Costa Ana P B Rosel Patricia E Daura Jorge Fabio G Simoes Lopes Paulo C October 2016 Offshore and coastal common bottlenose dolphins of the western South Atlantic face to face What the skull and the spine can tell us Marine Mammal Science 32 4 1433 1457 doi 10 1111 mms 12342 List of Marine Mammal Species and Subspecies Society for Marine Mammalogy 2020 Retrieved April 6 2020 Groves C P 2005 Wilson D E Reeder D M eds Mammal Species of the World A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference 3rd ed Baltimore Johns Hopkins University Press ISBN 0 801 88221 4 OCLC 62265494 Mead J G Brownell R L Jr 2005 Order Cetacea In Wilson D E Reeder D M eds Mammal Species of the World A Taxonomic and Geographic Reference 3rd ed Johns Hopkins University Press pp 723 743 ISBN 978 0 8018 8221 0 OCLC 62265494 IUCN Red List Bottlenose Dolphin query IUCN Red List April 6 2020 Retrieved April 6 2020 a b c Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus Western North Atlantic Offshore Stock PDF Retrieved September 30 2008 a b c d e f g h Shirihai H Jarrett B 2006 Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World Princeton Princeton Univ Press pp 155 161 ISBN 978 0 691 12757 6 Leduc R Perrin W Dizon E August 18 1998 Phylogenetic Relationships among the Delphinid Cetaceans Based on Full Cytochrome B Sequences Marine Mammal Science 15 3 619 648 doi 10 1111 j 1748 7692 1999 tb00833 x Moura Andre E Shreves Kypher Pilot Malgorzata Andrews Kimberly R Moore Daniel M Kishida Takushi Moller Luciana Natoli Ada Gaspari Stefania McGowen Michael Chen Ing Gray Howard Gore Mauvis Culloch Ross M Kiani Muhammad S Willson Maia Sarrouf Bulushi Asma Collins Tim Baldwin Robert Willson Andrew Minton Gianna Ponnampalam Louisa Hoelzel A Rus May 1 2020 Phylogenomics of the genus Tursiops and closely related Delphininae reveals extensive reticulation among lineages and provides inference about eco evolutionary drivers Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 146 106756 doi 10 1016 j ympev 2020 106756 hdl 2164 16438 ISSN 1055 7903 PMID 32028032 S2CID 211048062 Reeves R Stewart B Clapham P Powell J 2002 Guide to Marine Mammals of the World New York A A Knopf p 422 ISBN 978 0 375 41141 0 Risso s Dolphin American Cetacean Society Archived from the original on May 17 2008 Retrieved September 20 2008 Lee Jaennette April 15 2005 Whale Dolphin Hybrid Has Baby Wholphin Associated Press Retrieved September 20 2008 The Mammals of Texas Rough toothed Dolphin Archived from the original on September 15 2008 Retrieved September 20 2008 Robin s Island Database about captive Dolphins and Whales Archived from the original on January 23 2009 Retrieved September 20 2008 Zornetzer H R Duffield D A 2003 Captive born bottlenose dolphin common dolphin Tursiops truncatus Delphinus capensis intergeneric hybrids Canadian Journal of Zoology 81 10 1755 1762 doi 10 1139 z03 150 Herzing D Moewe K amp Brunnick B 2003 Interspecies interactions between Atlantic spotted dolphins Stenella frontalis and bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus on Great Bahama Bank Bahamas PDF Aquatic Mammals 29 3 335 341 doi 10 1578 01675420360736505 Archived from the original PDF on June 14 2007 Retrieved September 20 2008 Evolution of Dolphins Fossilworks Tursiops osennae www fossilworks org American Cetacean Society Fact Sheet Bottlenose Dolphin Archived 2008 07 25 at the Wayback Machine a b c d Reeves R Stewart B Clapham P Powell J 2002 National Audubon Society Guide to Marine Mammals of the World New York A A Knopf pp 362 365 ISBN 978 0 375 41141 0 a b Office of Protected Resources Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus noaa gov September 16 2019 team facts co September 29 2017 Bottlenose dolphin facts Bottlenose dolphin facts Archived from the original on August 13 2018 Retrieved March 3 2018 The Life Cycle of Bottlenose Dolphins Sciencing Retrieved March 3 2018 a b Bottlenose Dolphin March 26 2014 Archived from the original on February 11 2017 Retrieved September 29 2016 a b Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus at MarineBio org marinebio org Archived from the original on July 13 2012 Retrieved January 26 2005 Morteo Eduardo Rocha Olivares Axayacatl Morteo Rodrigo Weller David W June 13 2017 Phenotypic variation in dorsal fin morphology of coastal bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus off Mexico PeerJ 5 e3415 doi 10 7717 peerj 3415 PMC 5472037 PMID 28626607 Tabuchi Hiroko 2006 Dolphin reveals an extra set of legs NBC News Associated Press a b Adaptations for an aquatic environment Busch Gardens Archived from the original on May 17 2008 Retrieved March 14 2008 Hersh Sandra L Deborah A Duffield 1990 Distinction Between Northwest Atlantic Offshore and Coastal Bottlenose Dolphins Based on Hemoglobin Profile and Morphometry In Stephen Leatherwood Randall R Reeves eds The Bottlenose Dolphin San Diego Academic Press pp 129 139 ISBN 978 0 12 440280 5 Goforth Harold W Jr 1990 Ergometry Exercise Testing of the Bottlenose Dolphin In The Bottlenose Dolphin edited by Stephen Leatherwood and Randall R Reeves pp 559 574 San Diego Academic Press Inc ISBN 0 12 440280 1 Fish F E Hui C A 1991 Dolphin swimming a review Mammal Review 21 4 181 195 doi 10 1111 j 1365 2907 1991 tb00292 x Au Whitlow 1993 The Sonar of Dolphins New York Springer Verlag ISBN 978 3 540 97835 0 Pack AA Herman LM 1995 Sensory integration in the bottlenosed dolphin immediate recognition of complex shapes across the senses of echolocation and vision The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 98 2 Pt 1 722 33 Bibcode 1995ASAJ 98 722P doi 10 1121 1 413566 PMID 7642811 Herman L M Peacock M F Yunker M P Madsen C 1975 Bottlenosed dolphin Double slit pupil yields equivalent aerial and underwater diurnal acuity Science 189 4203 650 652 Bibcode 1975Sci 189 650H doi 10 1126 science 1162351 PMID 1162351 M Mass Alla Y Supin Alexander 2007 Adaptive features of aquatic mammals eyes The Anatomical Record 290 6 701 715 doi 10 1002 ar 20529 PMID 17516421 S2CID 39925190 a b c d Dolphin Characteristics Western Illinois University Retrieved August 31 2008 Dolphins can recognise each other by taste of their urine study finds the Guardian May 19 2022 Retrieved May 29 2022 Bruck Jason N Walmsley Sam F Janik Vincent M May 20 2022 Cross modal perception of identity by sound and taste in bottlenose dolphins Science Advances 8 20 eabm7684 Bibcode 2022SciA 8M7684B doi 10 1126 sciadv abm7684 ISSN 2375 2548 PMC 9116882 PMID 35584227 Bottlenose Dolphin Dolphins Voices in the Sea Archived from the original on April 20 2013 Bottlenose Dolphins Animal information pictures map National Geographic Archived from the original on October 23 2006 Retrieved November 3 2006 Janik VM Slater PJ 1998 Context specific use suggests that bottlenose dolphin signature whistles are cohesion calls Animal Behaviour 56 4 829 838 doi 10 1006 anbe 1998 0881 PMID 9790693 S2CID 32367435 a b Diaz Lopez B Shirai J A B 2009 Mediterranean common bottlenose dolphin s repertoire and communication use Dolphins Anatomy Behavior and Threats pp 129 148 ISBN 978 1 60876 849 3 Diaz Lopez 2010 Whistle characteristics in free ranging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus in the Mediterranean Sea Influence of behaviour Mammalian Biology 76 2 180 189 doi 10 1016 j mambio 2010 06 006 Herman LM Richards DG Wolz JP 1984 Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins Cognition 16 2 129 219 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 84 90003 9 PMID 6540652 S2CID 43237011 Reiss D McCowan B 1993 Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus evidence for vocal learning Journal of Comparative Psychology 107 3 301 12 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 107 3 301 PMID 8375147 The Dolphin Institute Behavioral Mimicry Archived from the original on May 11 2008 Retrieved August 31 2008 Herman L 2002 Language Learning In Perrin W Wursig B Thewissen J eds Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals Academic Press pp 685 689 ISBN 978 0 12 551340 1 The Dolphin Institute The Dolphin Institute Understanding Language dolphin institute org Archived from the original on December 11 2008 a b Herman L Pack A Wood A August 26 2006 Bottlenose Dolphins Can Generalize Rules and Develop Abstract Concepts Marine Mammal Science 10 1 70 80 doi 10 1111 j 1748 7692 1994 tb00390 x The Dolphin Institute Awareness of One s Own Recent Behaviors Archived from the original on May 9 2008 Retrieved August 31 2008 Herman LM Gordon JA 1974 Auditory delayed matching in the bottlenose dolphin Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 21 1 19 26 doi 10 1901 jeab 1974 21 19 PMC 1333166 PMID 4204143 The Dolphin Institute Awareness of One s Own Body Parts Archived from the original on May 11 2008 Retrieved August 31 2008 Foer Joshua 2015 It s Time for Conservation Breaking the communication barrier between dolphins and humans National Geographic Retrieved February 12 2016 The Dolphin Institute Pointing Gestures Archived from the original on May 9 2008 Retrieved August 31 2008 Intelligence and Humans Retrieved August 11 2008 Marten K Psarakos S 1995 Evidence of self awareness in the bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus In Parker S T Mitchell R Boccia M eds Self awareness in Animals and Humans Developmental Perspectives Cambridge University Press pp 361 379 ISBN 978 0 521 44108 7 Archived from the original on October 13 2008 Retrieved October 4 2008 Kilian Annette Yaman Sevgi von Fersen Lorenzo Gunturkun Onur 2003 A bottlenose dolphin discriminates visual stimuli differing in numerosity PDF Learning amp Behavior 31 2 133 142 doi 10 3758 BF03195976 PMID 12882372 S2CID 12368215 Smolker R A et al 1997 Sponge Carrying by Dolphins Delphinidae Tursiops sp A Foraging Specialization Involving Tool Use PDF Ethology 103 6 454 465 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 1997 tb00160 x hdl 2027 42 71936 Krutzen M Mann J Heithaus MR Connor RC Bejder L Sherwin WB 2005 Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 25 8939 8943 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 8939K doi 10 1073 pnas 0500232102 PMC 1157020 PMID 15947077 Krutzen M Mann J Heithaus M R Connor R C Bejder L Sherwin W B 2005 Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 25 8939 43 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 8939K doi 10 1073 pnas 0500232102 PMC 1157020 PMID 15947077 Lewis J S Schroeder W 2003 Mud plume feeding a unique foraging behavior of the bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus in the Florida Keys Gulf of Mexico Science 21 1 92 doi 10 18785 goms 2101 09 Feldman Paula Dinner Is Served South Carolina Wildlife Magazine Jimenez Pedro Alava Juan Jose 2015 Strand feeding by coastal bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus in the Gulf of Guayaquil Ecuador Latin American Journal of Aquatic Mammals 10 1 33 37 doi 10 5597 lajam00191 Rendell L Whitehead H 2001 Culture in whales and dolphins Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 2 309 382 doi 10 1017 S0140525X0100396X PMID 11530544 S2CID 24052064 Premack D Hauser M 2001 A whale of a tale Calling it culture doesn t help PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 2 350 351 doi 10 1017 S0140525X01513965 S2CID 143443845 Archived from the original PDF on March 6 2009 The Daily Telegraph 2006 Brazil s sexiest secret article retrieved January 24 2016 a b Dr Moti Nissani 2007 Bottlenose Dolphins in Laguna Requesting a Throw Net video Supporting material for Dr Nissani s presentation at the 2007 International Ethological Conference Video retrieved January 24 2016 Black Richard 2008 Wild dolphins tail walk on water BBC News Retrieved March 10 2016 a b Sutton Malcolm April 4 2022 Why are the dolphins dying ABC News Australian Broadcasting Corporation Retrieved April 4 2022 Bossley M Steiner A Brakes P Shrimpton J Foster C Rendell L September 2018 Tail walking in a bottlenose dolphin community the rise and fall of an arbitrary cultural fad Biol Lett 14 9 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2018 0314 PMC 6170752 PMID 30185606 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint uses authors parameter link Pisani Ashleigh May 9 2018 The incredible tale of Billie the Port River dolphin the world s first wild tailwalker Adelaide Now Portside Messenger Dolphins Have Longest Memories in Animal Kingdom August 6 2013 Retrieved March 3 2018 Weaver A amp Kuczaj S 2016 Neither toy nor tool Grass wearing behavior among free ranging bottlenose dolphins in western Florida International Journal of Comparative Psychology 29 1 18 uclapsych ijcp 31885 Retrieved from http escholarship org uc item 893417x3 Dicke U Roth G August September 2008 Intelligence Evolved Scientific American Mind pp 75 77 Ridgeway Sam H December 16 2019 Higher neuron densities in the cerebral cortex and larger cerebellums may limit dive times of delphinids compared to deep diving toothed whales PLOS ONE 14 12 e0226206 Bibcode 2019PLoSO 1426206R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0226206 PMC 6914331 PMID 31841529 Herculano Houzel Suzana November 9 2009 The human brain in numbers a linearly scaled up primate brain Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 3 12 31 doi 10 3389 neuro 09 031 2009 PMC 6914331 PMID 31841529 Common Bottlenose Dolphin NOAA Fisheries December 29 2021 Bottlenose dolphins Seaworld Archived from the original on May 18 2012 Retrieved March 14 2008 Kooyman G 2002 Diving Physiology In Perrin W Wursig B Thewissen J eds Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals Academic Press pp 339 341 ISBN 978 0 12 551340 1 a b Dolphin Q amp A Mia Research Foundation Archived from the original on March 18 2008 Retrieved March 13 2008 Do whales and dolphins sleep howstuffworks May 24 2001 Retrieved March 13 2008 Robeck Todd R et al Reproductive biology of the bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus and the potential application of advanced reproductive technologies Journal of Zoo and Wildlife Medicine 1994 321 336 Atkinson S 2002 Male Reproductive Systems In Perrin W Wursig B Thewissen J eds Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals Academic Press p 700 ISBN 978 0 12 551340 1 Stewart R 2002 Female reproductive systems In Perrin W Wursig B Thewissen J eds Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals Academic Press pp 422 428 ISBN 978 0 12 551340 1 Connor Richard C Peterson Dawn M 1994 The Lives of Whales and Dolphins New York Henry Holt and Company ISBN 978 0 8050 1950 6 a b c d e Connor R Wells R Mann J Read A 2000 The Bottlenose Dolphin In Mann J Connor R Tyack P Whitehead H eds Cetacean Societies Chicago Univ of Chicago Press p 102 ISBN 978 0 226 50341 7 a b c Bottlenose Dolphins of Sarasota Bay Smithsonian National Zoo Archived from the original on February 18 2009 a b Connor R C 1990 Alliances among male bottlenose dolphins and comparative analyses of mutualism Ph D Dissertation The University of Michigan Michigan Guy Jack April 2020 Male bottlenose dolphins form gangs to get a mate CNN Retrieved April 1 2020 Volker Sommer Paul L Vasey 2006 Chapter 4 Homosexual Behaviour in Animals an Evolutionary perspective Bruce Bagemihl 1999 Biological Exuberance Animal Homosexuality and Natural Diversity Connor R Wells R Mann J Read A 2000 The Bottlenose Dolphin In Mann J Connor R Tyack P Whitehead H eds Cetacean Societies Chicago Univ of Chicago Press p 95 ISBN 978 0 226 50341 7 Mann J Connor R C Barre L M Heithaus M R 2000 Female reproductive success in wild bottlenose dolphins Tursiops sp Life history habitat provisioning and group size effects Behavioral Ecology 11 2 210 219 doi 10 1093 beheco 11 2 210 Mann J 2006 Establishing Trust Sociosexual behaviour and the development of male male bonds among Indian Ocean bottlenose dolphin calves In Vasey P Sommer V eds Homosexual Behaviour in Animals An Evolutionary Perspective Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 86446 6 Noren S R 2008 Infant Carrying Behaviour in Dolphins Costly Parental Care in an Aquatic Environment Functional Ecology 22 2 284 288 doi 10 1111 j 1365 2435 2007 01354 x JSTOR 20142803 Retrieved November 19 2022 Know your ally Cooperative male dolphins can tell who s on their team phys org Retrieved April 22 2021 Connor Richards 2000 Cetacean Societies Field Studies of Dolphins and Whales Chicago University of Chicago Press ISBN 978 0 226 50341 7 Diaz Lopez Bruno Shirai J A 2007 Marine aquaculture and bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus social structure Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62 6 887 894 doi 10 1007 s00265 007 0512 1 S2CID 24197908 Wells R S M D Scott and A B Irvine 1987 The social structure of free ranging bottlenose dolphins pp 247 305 in Genoways H ed Current Mammalogy Vol 1 New York Plenum Press LUSSEAU DAVID WILSON BEN HAMMOND PHILIP S GRELLIER KATE DURBAN JOHN W PARSONS KIM M BARTON TIM R THOMPSON PAUL M January 2006 Quantifying the influence of sociality on population structure in bottlenose dolphins Journal of Animal Ecology 75 1 14 24 doi 10 1111 j 1365 2656 2005 01013 x ISSN 0021 8790 PMID 16903039 Acevedo Gutierrez Alejandro William F Perrin Bernd G Wursig J G M Thewissen 2008 Group behavior Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals 2 ed United States Academic Press pp 511 520 ISBN 978 0 12 373553 9 Diaz Lopez Bruno Shirai J B 2008 Marine aquaculture off Sardinia Island Italy ecosystem effects evaluated through a trophic mass balance model Ecological Modelling 212 3 4 292 303 doi 10 1016 j ecolmodel 2007 10 028 Diaz Lopez Bruno Shirai J B 2006 Bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus presence and incidental capture in a marine fish farm on the northeastern coast of Sardinia Italy Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the United Kingdom 87 1 113 117 doi 10 1017 S0025315407054215 S2CID 86115152 Comparative Mammalian Brain Collections Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus Retrieved September 30 2008 Duffy Echevarria Erin E Connor Richard C Aubin David J St January 1 2008 Observations of strand feeding behavior by bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus in Bull Creek South Carolina Marine Mammal Science 24 1 202 206 doi 10 1111 j 1748 7692 2007 00151 x subscription required Engleby Laura K Powell Jessica R 2019 Detailed observations and mechanisms of mud ring feeding by common bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus truncatus in Florida Bay Florida U S A Marine Mammal Science 35 3 1162 1172 doi 10 1111 mms 12583 ISSN 1748 7692 S2CID 91392196 Diaz Lopez Bruno 2006 Interaction between bottlenose dolphins and fisheries off Sardinia ICES Journal of Marine Science 63 5 946 951 doi 10 1016 j icesjms 2005 06 012 Dolphin rescues stranded whales CNN March 12 2008 Archived from the original on May 12 2008 Brooks David March 12 2008 Dolphin rescues beached whales Herald Sun Australia Archived from the original on April 29 2008 Tennenhouse Erica July 30 2019 Dolphin mom adopts whale calf a first National Geographic National Geographic Society Retrieved July 31 2019 Read Andrew 1999 Porpoises Stillwater MN USA Voyageur Press ISBN 978 0 89658 420 4 Evidence Puts Dolphins in New Light as Killers Luna pos to July 6 1999 Retrieved June 12 2012 Ryan Conor 2008 Attack on Harbour Porpoise Phocoena phocoena L 1758 by Bottle nosed Dolphins Tursiops truncatus Montagu 1821 Irish Naturalists Journal 29 2 130 Dolphin attacks on porpoises baffle experts BBC News July 15 2014 Reeves R Stewart B Clapham P Powell J 2002 Guide to Marine Mammals of the World New York A A Knopf p 434 ISBN 978 0 375 41141 0 NOAA 2003 Bottlenose Dolphin Tursiops truncatus California Oregon Washington Offshore Stock PDF nmfs noaa gov Retrieved September 21 2008 Shirihai H Jarrett B 2006 Whales Dolphins and Other Marine Mammals of the World Princeton Princeton Univ Press p 166 ISBN 978 0 691 12757 6 a b c d e Bottlenose Dolphins Tursiops truncatus Animal Corner Retrieved September 16 2008 Heithaus M Dill L 2002 Food Availability and Tiger Shark Predation Risk Influence Bottlenose Dolphin Habitat Use Ecology 83 2 480 491 doi 10 1890 0012 9658 2002 083 0480 FAATSP 2 0 CO 2 S2CID 14944929 Gannon D Barros N Nowacek D Read A Waples D Wells R 2004 Prey detection by bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus an experimental test of the passive listening hypothesis Animal Behaviour 69 3 709 720 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2004 06 020 S2CID 53201926 Eisfeld S October 2003 The social affiliation and group composition of bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus in the outer southern Moray Firth NE Scotland PDF MSc Thesis School of Biological Sciences University of Wales Bangor p 42 Archived from the original PDF on October 2 2008 The Dolphin Institute Bottlenose Dolphin Natural History and Ecology dolphin institute org Archived from the original on April 5 2002 Retrieved March 6 2016 Thomson Ainsley November 25 2004 Dolphins saved us from shark lifeguards say The New Zealand Herald Diaz Lopez B Shirai J A Prieto A Fernandez P 2008 Diving activity of a solitary wild free ranging bottlenose dolphin Tursiops truncatus PDF Journal of the Marine Biological Association of the UK 88 6 1153 1157 doi 10 1017 S0025315408000921 S2CID 85607255 Archived from the original PDF on January 31 2009 Dr Moti Nissani 2008 Cooperative Dolphins of Laguna Data on Nature of Signal video and detailed description YouTube Archived from the original on December 12 2021 Simoes Lopes Paulo C 1998 Dolphin interactions with the mullet artisanal fishing on southern Brazil a qualitative and quantitative approach Revista Brasileira de Zoologia 15 3 709 726 doi 10 1590 s0101 81751998000300016 Bottlenose Dolphin Archived from the original on April 21 2008 Retrieved August 11 2008 Australian Government Dolphins and porspoises Department of the Environment Water Heritage and the Arts Archived from the original on June 29 2007 Retrieved October 24 2008 Waples and Gales 2002 Evaluating and minimising social stress in the care of captive bottlenose dolphins Tursiops aduncus http onlinelibrary wiley com doi 10 1002 zoo 10004 abstract Alave Kristine L September 12 2007 Animal rights groups rap Pasig dolphin show Archived from the original on January 17 2008 Katrina dolphin rescue launched BBC September 15 2005 Retrieved February 14 2008 Dolphins Deployed as Undersea Agents in Iraq National Geographic Retrieved January 18 2009 Rehn KW Riggs PK 2002 Non Lethal Swimmer Neutralization Study U S Space and Naval Warfare Systems Center Technical Report Document Number 3138 Archived from the original on May 30 2011 Retrieved September 25 2008 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint unfit URL link U S Navy Marine Mammal Program Web Site U S Navy U S Navy Marine Mammal Program Archived from the original on January 15 2009 Reuters 1994 Female friendly dolphin kills male swimmer in Brazil article retrieved October 26 2006 American Cetacean Society Bottlenose Dolphin Archived from the original on July 25 2008 Retrieved August 31 2008 Flipper 1995 IMDb October 1995 Retrieved November 3 2006 Scully Mike 2009 The Simpsons The Complete Twelfth Season DVD commentary for the episode Treehouse of Horror IX DVD 20th Century Fox Dolphin Tale Rotten Tomatoes Flixster Retrieved January 9 2016 Dolphin Tale 2 Rotten Tomatoes Flixster Retrieved January 9 2016 Sports Ecyclopedia Miami dolphins 1996 Present sportsecyclopedia com Retrieved March 6 2016 Constantine R 2002 Folklore and Legends In Perrin W Wursig B Thewissen J eds Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals Academic Press p 448 ISBN 978 0 12 551340 1 More than 80 of Indian Ocean dolphins may have been killed by commercial fishing study finds TheGuardian com March 2 2020 Betts KS May 2007 Perfluoroalkyl Acids What Is the Evidence Telling Us Environmental Health Perspectives 115 5 A250 A256 doi 10 1289 ehp 115 a250 PMC 1867999 PMID 17520044 Archived from the original on June 27 2007 Betts KS 2008 Not immune to PFOS effects Environ Health Perspect 116 7 A290 doi 10 1289 ehp 116 a290a PMC 2453185 PMID 18629339 Houde M Martin JW Letcher RJ Solomon KR Muir DC 2006 Biological monitoring of polyfluoroalkyl substances A review Environmental Science amp Technology 40 11 3463 73 Bibcode 2006EnST 40 3463H doi 10 1021 es052580b PMID 16786681 Lavery TJ Butterfield N Kemper CM Reid RJ Sanderson K 2008 Metals and selenium in the liver and bone of three dolphin species from South Australia 1988 2004 The Science of the Total Environment 390 1 77 85 Bibcode 2008ScTEn 390 77L doi 10 1016 j scitotenv 2007 09 016 PMID 18006044 Lavery TJ Kemper CM Sanderson K et al 2009 Heavy metal toxicity of kidney and bone tissues in South Australian adult bottlenose dolphins Tursiops aduncus PDF Marine Environmental Research 67 1 1 7 doi 10 1016 j marenvres 2008 09 005 PMID 19012959 Curran S Wilson B Thompson P 1996 Recommendations for the sustainable management of the bottlenose dolphin population in the Moray Firth Scottish Natural Heritage Review 56 Archived from the original on December 22 2015 Retrieved August 27 2013 Currey RJ Dawson SM Slooten E Schneider K Lusseau D Boisseau OJ Haase P Williams JA November 25 2008 Survival rates for a declining population of bottlenose dolphins in Doubtful Sound New Zealand an information theoretic approach to assessing the role of human impacts Aquatic Conservation Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems 19 6 658 670 doi 10 1002 aqc 1015 Currey Rohan M Dawson Stephen Slooten Elisabeth Schneider Karsten Lusseau David J Boisseau Oliver 2009 Survival rates for a declining population of bottlenose dolphins in Doubtful Sound New Zealand An information theoretic approach to assessing the role of human impacts Aquatic Conservation Marine and Freshwater Ecosystems 19 6 658 670 doi 10 1002 aqc 1015 Manlik O McDonald J A Mann J Raudino H C Bejder L Krutzen M Connor R C Heithaus M R Lacy R C Sherwin W B 2016 The relative importance of reproduction and survival for the conservation of two dolphin populations Ecology and Evolution 6 11 3496 3512 doi 10 1002 ece3 2130 PMC 5513288 PMID 28725349 Marine Mammal Center The Marine Mammal Protection Act of 1972 Amended 1994 marinemammalcenter org Retrieved March 6 2016 Further reading EditBerrow S D 2009 Winter distribution of Bottle nosed Dolphins Tursiops truncatus Montagu in the inner Shannon Estuary PDF Irish Naturalists Journal 30 1 35 39 Archived from the original PDF on July 21 2011 Hale P T Barreto A S Ross G J B 2000 Comparative morphology and distribution of the aduncus and truncatus forms of bottlenose dolphin Tursiops in the Indian and Western Pacific Oceans PDF Aquatic Mammals 26 2 101 110 Discusses distinguishing features between Bottlenose Dolphin species Reiss D Marino L 2001 Mirror self recognition in the bottlenose dolphin a case of cognitive convergence Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 98 10 5937 5942 Bibcode 2001PNAS 98 5937R doi 10 1073 pnas 101086398 PMC 33317 PMID 11331768 External links Edit Wikispecies has information related to Tursiops Wikimedia Commons has media related to Tursiops Voices in the Sea Sounds and Videos of the Bottlenose Dolphin Archived April 20 2013 at the Wayback Machine Tropical Dolphin Research Foundation Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Bottlenose dolphin amp oldid 1136473618, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.