fbpx
Wikipedia

Speech repetition

Speech repetition occurs when individuals speak the sounds that they have heard another person pronounce or say. In other words, it is the saying by one individual of the spoken vocalizations made by another individual. Speech repetition requires the person repeating the utterance to have the ability to map the sounds that they hear from the other person's oral pronunciation to similar places and manners of articulation in their own vocal tract.

Children copy with their own mouths the words spoken by the mouths of those around them. That enables them to learn the pronunciation of words not already in their vocabulary.

Such speech imitation often occurs independently of speech comprehension such as in speech shadowing in which people automatically say words heard in earphones, and the pathological condition of echolalia in which people reflexively repeat overheard words. That links to speech repetition of words being separate in the brain to speech perception. Speech repetition occurs in the dorsal speech processing stream, and speech perception occurs in the ventral speech processing stream. Repetitions are often incorporated unawares by that route into spontaneous novel sentences immediately or after delay after the storage in phonological memory.

In humans, the ability to map heard input vocalizations into motor output is highly developed because of the copying ability playing a critical role in children's rapid expansion of their spoken vocabulary. In older children and adults, that ability remains important, as it enables the continued learning of novel words and names and additional languages. That repetition is also necessary for the propagation of language from generation to generation. It has also been suggested that the phonetic units out of which speech is made have been selected upon by the process of vocabulary expansion and vocabulary transmissions because children prefer to copy words in terms of more easily-imitated elementary units.

Properties edit

Automatic edit

Vocal imitation happens quickly: words can be repeated within 250-300 milliseconds[1] both in normals (during speech shadowing)[2] and during echolalia. The imitation of speech syllables possibly happens even more quickly: people begin imitating the second phone in the syllable [ao] earlier than they can identify it (out of the set [ao], [aæ] and [ai]).[3] Indeed, "...simply executing a shift to [o] upon detection of a second vowel in [ao] takes very little longer than does interpreting and executing it as a shadowed response".[3] Neurobiologically this suggests "...that the early phases of speech analysis yield information which is directly convertible to information required for speech production".[3] Vocal repetition can be done immediately as in speech shadowing and echolalia. It can also be done after the pattern of pronunciation is stored in short-term memory or long-term memory. It automatically uses both auditory and where available visual information about how a word is produced.[4][5]

The automatic nature of speech repetition was noted by Carl Wernicke, the late nineteenth century neurologist, who observed that "The primary speech movements, enacted before the development of consciousness, are reflexive and mimicking in nature..".[6]

Independent of speech edit

Vocal imitiation arises in development before speech comprehension and also babbling: 18-week-old infants spontaneously copy vocal expressions provided the accompanying voice matches.[7] Imitation of vowels has been found as young as 12 weeks.[8] It is independent of native language, language skills, word comprehension and a speaker's intelligence. Many autistic and some mentally disabled people engage in the echolalia of overheard words (often their only vocal interaction with others) without understanding what they echo.[9][10][11][12] Reflex uncontrolled echoing of others words and sentences occurs in roughly half of those with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome.[13] The ability to repeat words without comprehension also occurs in mixed transcortical aphasia where it links to the sparing of the short-term phonological store.[14]

The ability to repeat and imitate speech sounds occurs separately to that of normal speech. Speech shadowing provides evidence of a 'privileged' input/output speech loop that is distinct to the other components of the speech system.[15] Neurocognitive research likewise finds evidence of a direct (nonlexical) link between phonological analysis input and motor programming output.[16][17][18]

Effector independent edit

Speech sounds can be imitatively mapped into vocal articulations in spite of vocal tract anatomy differences in size and shape due to gender, age and individual anatomical variability. Such variability is extensive making input output mapping of speech more complex than a simple mapping of vocal track movements. The shape of the mouth varies widely: dentists recognize three basic shapes of palate: trapezoid, ovoid, and triangular; six types of malocclusion between the two jaws; nine ways teeth relate to the dental arch and a wide range of maxillary and mandible deformities.[19] Vocal sound can also vary due to dental injury and dental caries. Other factors that do not impede the sensory motor mapping needed for vocal imitation are gross oral deformations such as hare-lips, cleft palates or amputations of the tongue tip, pipe smoking, pencil biting and teeth clinching (such as in ventriloquism). Paranasal sinuses vary between individuals 20-fold in volume, and differ in the presence and the degree of their asymmetry.[20][21]

Diverse linguistic vocalizations edit

Vocal imitation occurs potentially in regard to a diverse range of phonetic units and types of vocalization. The world's languages use consonantal phones that differ in thirteen imitable vocal tract place of articulations (from the lips to the glottis). These phones can potentially be pronounced with eleven types of imitable manner of articulations (nasal stops to lateral clicks). Speech can be copied in regard to its social accent, intonation, pitch and individuality (as with entertainment impersonators). Speech can be articulated in ways which diverge considerably in speed, timbre, pitch, loudness and emotion. Speech further exists in different forms such as song, verse, scream and whisper. Intelligible speech can be produced with pragmatic intonation and in regional dialects and foreign accents. These aspects are readily copied: people asked to repeat speech-like words imitate not only phones but also accurately other pronunciation aspects such as fundamental frequency,[22] schwa-syllable expression,[22] voice spectra and lip kinematics,[23] voice onset times,[24] and regional accent.[25]

Language acquisition edit

Vocabulary expansion edit

In 1874 Carl Wernicke proposed[26] that the ability to imitate speech plays a key role in language acquisition. This is now a widely researched issue in child development.[27][28][29][30][31] A study of 17,000 one and two word utterances made by six children between 18 months to 25 months found that, depending upon the particular infant, between 5% and 45% of their words might be mimicked.[27] These figures are minima since they concern only immediately heard words. Many words that may seem spontaneous are in fact delayed imitations heard days or weeks previously.[28] At 13 months children who imitate new words (but not ones they already know) show a greater increase in noun vocabulary at four months and non noun vocabulary at eight months.[29] A major predictor of vocabulary increase in both 20 months,[32] 24 months,[33] and older children between 4 and 8 years is their skill in repeating nonword phone sequences (a measure of mimicry and storage).[30][31] This is also the case with children with Down's syndrome .[34] The effect is larger than even age: in a study of 222 two-year-old children that had spoken vocabularies ranging between 3–601 words the ability to repeat nonwords accounted for 24% of the variance compared to 15% for age and 6% for gender (girls better than boys).[33]

Nonvocabulary expansion uses of imitation edit

Imitation provides the basis for making longer sentences than children could otherwise spontaneously make on their own.[35] Children analyze the linguistic rules, pronunciation patterns, and conversational pragmatics of speech by making monologues (often in crib talk) in which they repeat and manipulate in word play phrases and sentences previously overheard.[36] Many proto-conversations involve children (and parents) repeating what each other has said in order to sustain social and linguistic interaction. It has been suggested that the conversion of speech sound into motor responses helps aid the vocal "alignment of interactions" by "coordinating the rhythm and melody of their speech".[37] Repetition enables immigrant monolingual children to learn a second language by allowing them to take part in 'conversations'.[38] Imitation related processes aids the storage of overheard words by putting them into speech based short- and long-term memory.[39]

Language learning edit

The ability to repeat nonwords predicts the ability to learn second-language vocabulary.[40] A study found that adult polyglots performed better in short-term memory tasks such as repeating nonword vocalizations compared to nonpolyglots though both are otherwise similar in general intelligence, visuo-spatial short-term memory and paired-associate learning ability.[41] Language delay in contrast links to impairments in vocal imitation.[42]

Speech repetition and phones edit

Electrical brain stimulation research upon the human brain finds that 81% of areas that show disruption of phone identification are also those in which the imitating of oral movements is disrupted and vice versa;[43] Brain injuries in the speech areas show a 0.9 correlation between those causing impairments to the copying of oral movements and those impairing phone production and perception.[44]

Mechanism edit

Spoken words are sequences of motor movements organized around vocal tract gesture motor targets.[45] Vocalization due to this is copied in terms of the motor goals that organize it rather than the exact movements with which it is produced. These vocal motor goals are auditory. According to James Abbs[46] 'For speech motor actions, the individual articulatory movements would not appear to be controlled with regard to three- dimensional spatial targets, but rather with regard to their contribution to complex vocal tract goals such as resonance properties (e.g., shape, degree of constriction) and or aerodynamically significant variables'. Speech sounds also have duplicable higher-order characteristics such as rates and shape of modulations and rates and shape of frequency shifts.[47] Such complex auditory goals (which often link—though not always—to internal vocal gestures) are detectable from the speech sound which they create.

Neurology edit

Dorsal speech processing stream function edit

Two cortical processing streams exist: a ventral one which maps sound onto meaning, and a dorsal one, that maps sound onto motor representations. The dorsal stream projects from the posterior Sylvian fissure at the temporoparietal junction, onto frontal motor areas, and is not normally involved in speech perception.[48]Carl Wernicke identified a pathway between the left posterior superior temporal sulcus (a cerebral cortex region sometimes called the Wernicke's area) as a centre of the sound "images" of speech and its syllables that connected through the arcuate fasciculus with part of the inferior frontal gyrus (sometimes called the Broca's area) responsible for their articulation.[6] This pathway is now broadly identified as the dorsal speech pathway, one of the two pathways (together with the ventral pathway) that process speech.[49] The posterior superior temporal gyrus is specialized for the transient representation of the phonetic sequences used for vocal repetition.[50] Part of the auditory cortex also can represent aspects of speech such as its consonantal features.[51]

Mirror neurons edit

Mirror neurons have been identified that both process the perception and production of motor movements. This is done not in terms of their exact motor performance but an inference of the intended motor goals with which it is organized.[52] Mirror neurons that both perceive and produce the motor movements of speech have been identified.[53] Speech is mirrored constantly into its articulations since speakers cannot know in advance that a word is unfamiliar and in need of repetition—which is only learnt after the opportunity to map it into articulations has gone. Thus, speakers if they are to incorporate unfamiliar words into their spoken vocabulary must by default map all spoken input.[54]

Sign language edit

Words in sign languages, unlike those in spoken ones, are made not of sequential units but of spatial configurations of subword unit arrangements, the spatial analogue of the sonic-chronological morphemes of spoken language.[55] These words, like spoken ones, are learnt by imitation. Indeed, rare cases of compulsive sign-language echolalia exist in otherwise language-deficient deaf autistic individuals born into signing families.[55] At least some cortical areas neurobiologically active during both sign and vocal speech, such as the auditory cortex, are associated with the act of imitation.[56]

Nonhuman animals edit

Birds edit

Birds learn their songs from those made by other birds. In several examples, birds show highly developed repetition abilities: the Sri Lankan Greater racket-tailed drongo (Dicrurus paradiseus) copies the calls of predators and the alarm signals of other birds[57] Albert's lyrebird (Menura alberti) can accurately imitate the satin bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus),[58]

Research upon avian vocal motor neurons finds that they perceive their song as a series of articulatory gestures as in humans.[59] Birds that can imitate humans, such as the Indian hill myna (Gracula religiosa), imitate human speech by mimicking the various speech formants, created by changing the shape of the human vocal tract, with different vibration frequencies of its internal tympaniform membrane.[60] Indian hill mynahs also imitate such phonetic characteristics as voicing, fundamental frequencies, formant transitions, nasalization, and timing, through their vocal movements are made in a different way from those of the human vocal apparatus.[60]

Nonhuman mammals edit

Apes edit

Apes taught language show an ability to imitate language signs with chimpanzees such as Washoe who was able to learn with his arms a vocabulary of 250 American Sign Language gestures. However, such human trained apes show no ability to imitate human speech vocalizations.[67]

See also edit

Footnotes edit

  1. ^ Indefrey, P.; Levelt, W. J. M. (2004). "The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components". Cognition. 92 (1–2): 101–144. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.475.251. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001. PMID 15037128. S2CID 12662702.
  2. ^ Marslen-Wilson, W. (1973). "Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies". Nature. 244 (5417): 522–523. Bibcode:1973Natur.244..522M. doi:10.1038/244522a0. PMID 4621131. S2CID 4220775.
  3. ^ a b c Porter Jr, R. J.; Lubker, J. F. (1980). "Rapid reproduction of vowel-vowel sequences: Evidence for a fast and direct acoustic-motoric linkage in speech". Journal of Speech and Hearing Research. 23 (3): 593–602. doi:10.1044/jshr.2303.593. PMID 7421161.
  4. ^ Gentilucci, M.; Cattaneo, L. (2005). "Automatic audiovisual integration in speech perception". Experimental Brain Research. 167 (1): 66–75. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-0008-z. PMID 16034571. S2CID 20166301.
  5. ^ "Acute hepatitis B virus infection in children and teachers, England and Wales 1985-90". Communicable Disease Report. 1 (17): 75–76. 1991. PMID 1669805.
  6. ^ a b Wernicke K. The aphasia symptom-complex. 1874. Breslau, Cohn and Weigert. Translated in: Eling P, editor. Reader in the history of aphasia. Vol. 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins; 1994. p. 69–89. ISBN 978-90-272-1893-3
  7. ^ Kuhl, P. K.; Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). "The bimodal perception of speech in infancy". Science. 218 (4577): 1138–1141. Bibcode:1982Sci...218.1138K. doi:10.1126/science.7146899. PMID 7146899.
  8. ^ Kuhl, P. K.; Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). "Infant vocalizations in response to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 100 (4 Pt 1): 2425–2438. Bibcode:1996ASAJ..100.2425K. doi:10.1121/1.417951. PMC 3651031. PMID 8865648.
  9. ^ Roberts, J. M. (1989). "Echolalia and comprehension in autistic children". Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 19 (2): 271–281. doi:10.1007/BF02211846. PMID 2745392. S2CID 6925526.
  10. ^ Schneider, DE (1938). "The clinical syndromes of echolalia, echopraxia, grasping and sucking". Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 88: 18–35, 200–216. doi:10.1097/00005053-193807000-00003. S2CID 143703500.
  11. ^ Schuler, A. L. (1979). "Echolalia: Issues and clinical applications". The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders. 44 (4): 411–34. doi:10.1044/jshd.4404.411. PMID 390245.
  12. ^ Stengel, E. (1947). "A Clinical and Psychological Study of Echo-Reactions". The British Journal of Psychiatry. 93 (392): 598–612. doi:10.1192/bjp.93.392.598. PMID 20273402.
  13. ^ Lees, A. J.; Robertson, M.; Trimble, M. R.; Murray, N. M. (1984). "A clinical study of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome in the United Kingdom". Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry. 47 (1): 1–8. doi:10.1136/jnnp.47.1.1. PMC 1027633. PMID 6582230.
  14. ^ Trojano, L.; Fragassi, N. A.; Postiglione, A.; Grossi, D. (1988). "Mixed transcortical aphasia. On relative sparing of phonological short-term store in a case". Neuropsychologia. 26 (4): 633–638. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(88)90120-0. PMID 2457182. S2CID 35115074.
  15. ^ McLeod P. Posner MI. (1984). Privileged loops from percept to act. In H. Bouma D. Bouwhuis, (Eds), Attention and performance X (pp. 55-66). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0-86377-005-0
  16. ^ Coslett, H. B.; Roeltgen, D. P.; Gonzalez Rothi, L.; Heilman, K. M. (1987). "Transcortical sensory aphasia: Evidence for subtypes". Brain and Language. 32 (2): 362–378. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(87)90133-7. PMID 3690258. S2CID 6079313.
  17. ^ McCarthy, R.; Warrington, E. K. (1984). "A two-route model of speech production. Evidence from aphasia". Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 107 (2): 463–485. doi:10.1093/brain/107.2.463. PMID 6722512.
  18. ^ McCarthy, R. A.; Warrington, E. K. (2001). "Repeating Without Semantics: Surface Dysphasia?". Neurocase. 7 (1): 77–87. doi:10.1093/neucas/7.1.77. PMID 11239078.
  19. ^ Bloomer HH. (1971). Speech defects associated with dental malocclusions and related abnormalities. In L. E. (Eds), Handbook of speech pathology and audiology (pp. 715-766), New York, Appleton Century. ISBN 978-0-13-381764-5
  20. ^ Williams RJ. (1967). You are extra-ordinary. New York, Random House. pp. 26-27. OCLC 156187572
  21. ^ Vocal traits also vary moreover when people get upper respiratory tract infections as the shape and size of sinus cavities is further changed with the swelling of mucous membranes.
  22. ^ a b Kappes, J.; Baumgaertner, A.; Peschke, C.; Ziegler, W. (2009). "Unintended imitation in nonword repetition". Brain and Language. 111 (3): 140–151. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.08.008. PMID 19811813. S2CID 2113790.
  23. ^ Gentilucci, M; Bernardis, P (2007). "Imitation during phoneme production". Neuropsychologia. 45 (3): 608–15. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.004. PMID 16698051. S2CID 40687020.
  24. ^ Shockley, K.; Sabadini, L.; Fowler, C. A. (2004). "Imitation in shadowing words". Perception & Psychophysics. 66 (3): 422–429. doi:10.3758/BF03194890. PMID 15283067.
  25. ^ Delvaux, V; Soquet, A (2007). "The influence of ambient speech on adult speech productions through unintentional imitation". Phonetica. 64 (2–3): 145–73. doi:10.1159/000107914. PMID 17914281. S2CID 22042824.
  26. ^ Wernicke K. (1874). The aphasia symptom-complex. Breslau, Cohn and Weigert. Translated in: Eling P, editor. (1994). p. 69–89.Reader in the history of aphasia. Vol. 4. Amsterdam: John Benjamins: "The major tasks of the child in speech acquisition is mimicry of the spoken word". p76
  27. ^ a b Bloom, L.; Hood, L.; Lightbown, P. (1974). "Imitation in language development: If, when, and why". Cognitive Psychology. 6 (3): 380–420. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(74)90018-8.
  28. ^ a b Miller GA. (1977). Spontaneous apprentices: Children and language. New York, Seabury Press. ISBN 978-0-8164-9330-2
  29. ^ a b Masur, EF (1995). "Infants' early verbal imitation and their later lexical development". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 41: 286–306. OCLC 89395784.
  30. ^ a b Gathercole, SE. Baddeley AD. (1989). . Journal of Memory and Language. 28 (2): 200–213. doi:10.1016/0749-596x(89)90044-2. Archived from the original on 2012-08-17. Retrieved 2009-12-19.
  31. ^ a b Gathercole, S. E. (2006). "Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship". Applied Psycholinguistics. 27 (4): 513–543. doi:10.1017/S0142716406060383. S2CID 145633911. PDF 2011-06-05 at the Wayback Machine
  32. ^ Hoff, E; Core, C; Bridges, K (2008). "Non-word repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20- to 24-month-olds". Journal of Child Language. 35 (4): 903–16. doi:10.1017/S0305000908008751. PMID 18838017. S2CID 18566002.
  33. ^ a b Stokes, S. F.; Klee, T (2009). "Factors that influence vocabulary development in two-year-old children". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 50 (4): 498–505. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01991.x. PMID 19017366.
  34. ^ Laws, G.; Gunn, D. (2004). "Phonological memory as a predictor of language comprehension in Down syndrome: A five-year follow-up study". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 45 (2): 326–337. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00224.x. PMID 14982246.
  35. ^ Speidel GE. Herreshoff MJ. (1989). Imitation and the construction of long utterances. In G. E. Speidel & K. E. Nelson, (Eds), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp. 181-197). New York, Springer-Verlag. ISBN 978-0-387-96885-8
  36. ^ Kuczaj SA. (1983). Crib speech and language practice. New York, Springer-Verlag. ISBN 978-0-387-90860-1
  37. ^ Scott, S. K.; McGettigan, C.; Eisner, F. (2009). "A little more conversation, a little less action — candidate roles for the motor cortex in speech perception". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 10 (4): 295–302. doi:10.1038/nrn2603. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0013-2999-F. PMC 4238059. PMID 19277052. p. 201
  38. ^ Fillmore LW. (1979). Individual differences in second language acquisition. In C. J. Fillmore, D. Kempler & W. S-Y. Wang, (Eds), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior (pp. 203-228). New York, Academic Press. OCLC 4983571
  39. ^ Gathercole, S. E. (1995). "Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords". Memory & Cognition. 23 (1): 83–94. doi:10.3758/BF03210559. PMID 7885268. S2CID 20774241.
  40. ^ Cheng, H (1996). "Nonword span as a unique predictor of second-language vocabulary learning". Developmental Psychology. 32 (5): 867–873. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.5.867.
  41. ^ Papagno, C.; Vallar, G. (1995). "Verbal short-term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology. 48 (1): 98–107. doi:10.1080/14640749508401378. PMID 7754088. S2CID 19242688.
  42. ^ Bishop, D. V.; North, T.; Donlan, C. (1996). "Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: Evidence from a twin study". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 37 (4): 391–403. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01420.x. PMID 8735439.
  43. ^ Ojemann, GA (1983). "Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 6 (2): 189–230. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00015491. S2CID 143189089.
  44. ^ Kimura, D.; Watson, N. (1989). "The relation between oral movement control and speech". Brain and Language. 37 (4): 565–590. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(89)90112-0. PMID 2479446. S2CID 39913744.
  45. ^ Shaffer LH. (1984). Motor programming in language production. In H. Bouma & D. G. Bouwhuis, (Eds), Attention and performance, X. pp. (17-41). London, Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0-86377-005-0
  46. ^ Abbs JH. (1986). Invariance and variability in speech production, A distinction between linguistic intent and its neuromotor implementation. In J. S. Perkell, & D. H. Klatt, (Eds), Invariance and variability in speech processes (pp. 202-219). Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0-89859-545-1
  47. ^ Porter RJ. (1987). What is the relation between speech production and speech perception? In: Allport A, MacKay D G, Prinz W G, Scheerer E, eds. Language Perception and Production. London: Academic Press,: 85-106. ISBN 978-0-12-052750-2
  48. ^ Hickok, G.; Poeppel, D. (2004). "Dorsal and ventral streams: A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language". Cognition. 92 (1–2): 67–99. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2003.10.011. PMID 15037127. S2CID 635860.
  49. ^ Okada, K.; Hickok, G. (2006). "Left posterior auditory-related cortices participate both in speech perception and speech production: Neural overlap revealed by fMRI". Brain and Language. 98 (1): 112–117. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2006.04.006. PMID 16716388. S2CID 1056984.
  50. ^ Wise, R. J.; Scott, S. K.; Blank, S. C.; Mummery, C. J.; Murphy, K.; Warburton, E. A. (2001). "Separate neural subsystems within 'Wernicke's area'". Brain: A Journal of Neurology. 124 (Pt 1): 83–95. doi:10.1093/brain/124.1.83. PMID 11133789.
  51. ^ Obleser, J.; Scott, S. K.; Eulitz, C. (2005). "Now You Hear It, Now You Don't: Transient Traces of Consonants and their Nonspeech Analogues in the Human Brain". Cerebral Cortex. 16 (8): 1069–1076. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj047. PMID 16207930.
  52. ^ Umiltà, M. A.; Kohler, E.; Gallese, V.; Fogassi, L.; Fadiga, L.; Keysers, C.; Rizzolatti, G. (2001). "I know what you are doing. A neurophysiological study". Neuron. 31 (1): 155–165. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00337-3. PMID 11498058.
  53. ^ Hickok, G. (2010). "The role of mirror neurons in speech and language processing". Brain and Language. 112 (1): 1–2. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2009.10.006. PMC 2813993. PMID 19948355.
  54. ^ Skoyles, J. R. (2010). "Mapping of heard speech into articulation information and speech acquisition". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 107 (18): E73. Bibcode:2010PNAS..107E..73S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1003007107. PMC 2889576. PMID 20427741.
  55. ^ a b Poizner H. Klima ES. Bellugi U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-66066-2
  56. ^ Nishimura, H.; Hashikawa, K.; Doi, K.; Iwaki, T.; Watanabe, Y.; Kusuoka, H.; Nishimura, T.; Kubo, T. (1999). "Sign language 'heard' in the auditory cortex". Nature. 397 (6715): 116. Bibcode:1999Natur.397..116N. doi:10.1038/16376. PMID 9923672. S2CID 4414422.
  57. ^ Goodale, E.; Kotagama, S. W. (2006). "Context-dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 273 (1588): 875–880. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3392. PMC 1560225. PMID 16618682.
  58. ^ Putland, D. A.; Nicholls, J. A.; Noad, M. J.; Goldizen, A. W. (2006). "Imitating the neighbours: Vocal dialect matching in a mimic-model system". Biology Letters. 2 (3): 367–370. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0502. PMC 1686190. PMID 17148405.
  59. ^ Williams, H.; Nottebohm, F. (1985). "Auditory responses in avian vocal motor neurons: A motor theory for song perception in birds". Science. 229 (4710): 279–282. Bibcode:1985Sci...229..279W. doi:10.1126/science.4012321. PMID 4012321. S2CID 19053313.
  60. ^ a b Klatt, D. H.; Stefanski, R. A. (1974). "How does a mynah bird imitate human speech?". The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 55 (4): 822–832. Bibcode:1974ASAJ...55..822K. doi:10.1121/1.1914607. PMID 4833078.
  61. ^ Reiss, D.; McCowan, B. (1993). "Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Evidence for vocal learning". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 107 (3): 301–312. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.107.3.301. PMID 8375147.
  62. ^ Foote, A. D.; Griffin, R. M.; Howitt, D.; Larsson, L.; Miller, P. J. O.; Hoelzel, A. (2006). "Killer whales are capable of vocal learning". Biology Letters. 2 (4): 509–512. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0525. PMC 1834009. PMID 17148275.
  63. ^ Ralls, K.; Fiorelli, P.; Gish, S. (1985). "Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals, Phoca vitulina". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 63 (5): 1050–1056. doi:10.1139/z85-157.
  64. ^ Poole, J. H.; Tyack, P. L.; Stoeger-Horwath, A. S.; Watwood, S. (2005). "Animal behaviour: Elephants are capable of vocal learning". Nature. 434 (7032): 455–456. Bibcode:2005Natur.434..455P. doi:10.1038/434455a. PMID 15791244. S2CID 4369863.
  65. ^ Esser, K. H. (1994). "Audio-vocal learning in a non-human mammal: The lesser spear-nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor". NeuroReport. 5 (14): 1718–1720. doi:10.1097/00001756-199409080-00007. PMID 7827315.
  66. ^ Wich, S. A.; Swartz, K. B.; Hardus, M. E.; Lameira, A. R.; Stromberg, E.; Shumaker, R. W. (2008). "A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan". Primates. 50 (1): 56–64. doi:10.1007/s10329-008-0117-y. PMID 19052691. S2CID 708682.
  67. ^ Hayes C. (1951). The ape in our house, Harper, New York. OCLC 1579444

speech, repetition, occurs, when, individuals, speak, sounds, that, they, have, heard, another, person, pronounce, other, words, saying, individual, spoken, vocalizations, made, another, individual, requires, person, repeating, utterance, have, ability, sounds. Speech repetition occurs when individuals speak the sounds that they have heard another person pronounce or say In other words it is the saying by one individual of the spoken vocalizations made by another individual Speech repetition requires the person repeating the utterance to have the ability to map the sounds that they hear from the other person s oral pronunciation to similar places and manners of articulation in their own vocal tract Children copy with their own mouths the words spoken by the mouths of those around them That enables them to learn the pronunciation of words not already in their vocabulary Such speech imitation often occurs independently of speech comprehension such as in speech shadowing in which people automatically say words heard in earphones and the pathological condition of echolalia in which people reflexively repeat overheard words That links to speech repetition of words being separate in the brain to speech perception Speech repetition occurs in the dorsal speech processing stream and speech perception occurs in the ventral speech processing stream Repetitions are often incorporated unawares by that route into spontaneous novel sentences immediately or after delay after the storage in phonological memory In humans the ability to map heard input vocalizations into motor output is highly developed because of the copying ability playing a critical role in children s rapid expansion of their spoken vocabulary In older children and adults that ability remains important as it enables the continued learning of novel words and names and additional languages That repetition is also necessary for the propagation of language from generation to generation It has also been suggested that the phonetic units out of which speech is made have been selected upon by the process of vocabulary expansion and vocabulary transmissions because children prefer to copy words in terms of more easily imitated elementary units Contents 1 Properties 1 1 Automatic 1 2 Independent of speech 1 3 Effector independent 1 4 Diverse linguistic vocalizations 2 Language acquisition 2 1 Vocabulary expansion 2 2 Nonvocabulary expansion uses of imitation 2 3 Language learning 3 Speech repetition and phones 4 Mechanism 5 Neurology 5 1 Dorsal speech processing stream function 5 2 Mirror neurons 6 Sign language 7 Nonhuman animals 7 1 Birds 7 2 Nonhuman mammals 7 3 Apes 8 See also 9 FootnotesProperties editAutomatic edit Vocal imitation happens quickly words can be repeated within 250 300 milliseconds 1 both in normals during speech shadowing 2 and during echolalia The imitation of speech syllables possibly happens even more quickly people begin imitating the second phone in the syllable ao earlier than they can identify it out of the set ao aae and ai 3 Indeed simply executing a shift to o upon detection of a second vowel in ao takes very little longer than does interpreting and executing it as a shadowed response 3 Neurobiologically this suggests that the early phases of speech analysis yield information which is directly convertible to information required for speech production 3 Vocal repetition can be done immediately as in speech shadowing and echolalia It can also be done after the pattern of pronunciation is stored in short term memory or long term memory It automatically uses both auditory and where available visual information about how a word is produced 4 5 The automatic nature of speech repetition was noted by Carl Wernicke the late nineteenth century neurologist who observed that The primary speech movements enacted before the development of consciousness are reflexive and mimicking in nature 6 Independent of speech edit Vocal imitiation arises in development before speech comprehension and also babbling 18 week old infants spontaneously copy vocal expressions provided the accompanying voice matches 7 Imitation of vowels has been found as young as 12 weeks 8 It is independent of native language language skills word comprehension and a speaker s intelligence Many autistic and some mentally disabled people engage in the echolalia of overheard words often their only vocal interaction with others without understanding what they echo 9 10 11 12 Reflex uncontrolled echoing of others words and sentences occurs in roughly half of those with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome 13 The ability to repeat words without comprehension also occurs in mixed transcortical aphasia where it links to the sparing of the short term phonological store 14 The ability to repeat and imitate speech sounds occurs separately to that of normal speech Speech shadowing provides evidence of a privileged input output speech loop that is distinct to the other components of the speech system 15 Neurocognitive research likewise finds evidence of a direct nonlexical link between phonological analysis input and motor programming output 16 17 18 Effector independent edit Speech sounds can be imitatively mapped into vocal articulations in spite of vocal tract anatomy differences in size and shape due to gender age and individual anatomical variability Such variability is extensive making input output mapping of speech more complex than a simple mapping of vocal track movements The shape of the mouth varies widely dentists recognize three basic shapes of palate trapezoid ovoid and triangular six types of malocclusion between the two jaws nine ways teeth relate to the dental arch and a wide range of maxillary and mandible deformities 19 Vocal sound can also vary due to dental injury and dental caries Other factors that do not impede the sensory motor mapping needed for vocal imitation are gross oral deformations such as hare lips cleft palates or amputations of the tongue tip pipe smoking pencil biting and teeth clinching such as in ventriloquism Paranasal sinuses vary between individuals 20 fold in volume and differ in the presence and the degree of their asymmetry 20 21 Diverse linguistic vocalizations edit Vocal imitation occurs potentially in regard to a diverse range of phonetic units and types of vocalization The world s languages use consonantal phones that differ in thirteen imitable vocal tract place of articulations from the lips to the glottis These phones can potentially be pronounced with eleven types of imitable manner of articulations nasal stops to lateral clicks Speech can be copied in regard to its social accent intonation pitch and individuality as with entertainment impersonators Speech can be articulated in ways which diverge considerably in speed timbre pitch loudness and emotion Speech further exists in different forms such as song verse scream and whisper Intelligible speech can be produced with pragmatic intonation and in regional dialects and foreign accents These aspects are readily copied people asked to repeat speech like words imitate not only phones but also accurately other pronunciation aspects such as fundamental frequency 22 schwa syllable expression 22 voice spectra and lip kinematics 23 voice onset times 24 and regional accent 25 Language acquisition editVocabulary expansion edit In 1874 Carl Wernicke proposed 26 that the ability to imitate speech plays a key role in language acquisition This is now a widely researched issue in child development 27 28 29 30 31 A study of 17 000 one and two word utterances made by six children between 18 months to 25 months found that depending upon the particular infant between 5 and 45 of their words might be mimicked 27 These figures are minima since they concern only immediately heard words Many words that may seem spontaneous are in fact delayed imitations heard days or weeks previously 28 At 13 months children who imitate new words but not ones they already know show a greater increase in noun vocabulary at four months and non noun vocabulary at eight months 29 A major predictor of vocabulary increase in both 20 months 32 24 months 33 and older children between 4 and 8 years is their skill in repeating nonword phone sequences a measure of mimicry and storage 30 31 This is also the case with children with Down s syndrome 34 The effect is larger than even age in a study of 222 two year old children that had spoken vocabularies ranging between 3 601 words the ability to repeat nonwords accounted for 24 of the variance compared to 15 for age and 6 for gender girls better than boys 33 Nonvocabulary expansion uses of imitation edit Imitation provides the basis for making longer sentences than children could otherwise spontaneously make on their own 35 Children analyze the linguistic rules pronunciation patterns and conversational pragmatics of speech by making monologues often in crib talk in which they repeat and manipulate in word play phrases and sentences previously overheard 36 Many proto conversations involve children and parents repeating what each other has said in order to sustain social and linguistic interaction It has been suggested that the conversion of speech sound into motor responses helps aid the vocal alignment of interactions by coordinating the rhythm and melody of their speech 37 Repetition enables immigrant monolingual children to learn a second language by allowing them to take part in conversations 38 Imitation related processes aids the storage of overheard words by putting them into speech based short and long term memory 39 Language learning edit The ability to repeat nonwords predicts the ability to learn second language vocabulary 40 A study found that adult polyglots performed better in short term memory tasks such as repeating nonword vocalizations compared to nonpolyglots though both are otherwise similar in general intelligence visuo spatial short term memory and paired associate learning ability 41 Language delay in contrast links to impairments in vocal imitation 42 Speech repetition and phones editElectrical brain stimulation research upon the human brain finds that 81 of areas that show disruption of phone identification are also those in which the imitating of oral movements is disrupted and vice versa 43 Brain injuries in the speech areas show a 0 9 correlation between those causing impairments to the copying of oral movements and those impairing phone production and perception 44 Mechanism editSpoken words are sequences of motor movements organized around vocal tract gesture motor targets 45 Vocalization due to this is copied in terms of the motor goals that organize it rather than the exact movements with which it is produced These vocal motor goals are auditory According to James Abbs 46 For speech motor actions the individual articulatory movements would not appear to be controlled with regard to three dimensional spatial targets but rather with regard to their contribution to complex vocal tract goals such as resonance properties e g shape degree of constriction and or aerodynamically significant variables Speech sounds also have duplicable higher order characteristics such as rates and shape of modulations and rates and shape of frequency shifts 47 Such complex auditory goals which often link though not always to internal vocal gestures are detectable from the speech sound which they create Neurology editDorsal speech processing stream function edit Two cortical processing streams exist a ventral one which maps sound onto meaning and a dorsal one that maps sound onto motor representations The dorsal stream projects from the posterior Sylvian fissure at the temporoparietal junction onto frontal motor areas and is not normally involved in speech perception 48 Carl Wernicke identified a pathway between the left posterior superior temporal sulcus a cerebral cortex region sometimes called the Wernicke s area as a centre of the sound images of speech and its syllables that connected through the arcuate fasciculus with part of the inferior frontal gyrus sometimes called the Broca s area responsible for their articulation 6 This pathway is now broadly identified as the dorsal speech pathway one of the two pathways together with the ventral pathway that process speech 49 The posterior superior temporal gyrus is specialized for the transient representation of the phonetic sequences used for vocal repetition 50 Part of the auditory cortex also can represent aspects of speech such as its consonantal features 51 Mirror neurons edit Mirror neurons have been identified that both process the perception and production of motor movements This is done not in terms of their exact motor performance but an inference of the intended motor goals with which it is organized 52 Mirror neurons that both perceive and produce the motor movements of speech have been identified 53 Speech is mirrored constantly into its articulations since speakers cannot know in advance that a word is unfamiliar and in need of repetition which is only learnt after the opportunity to map it into articulations has gone Thus speakers if they are to incorporate unfamiliar words into their spoken vocabulary must by default map all spoken input 54 Sign language editWords in sign languages unlike those in spoken ones are made not of sequential units but of spatial configurations of subword unit arrangements the spatial analogue of the sonic chronological morphemes of spoken language 55 These words like spoken ones are learnt by imitation Indeed rare cases of compulsive sign language echolalia exist in otherwise language deficient deaf autistic individuals born into signing families 55 At least some cortical areas neurobiologically active during both sign and vocal speech such as the auditory cortex are associated with the act of imitation 56 Nonhuman animals editBirds edit Birds learn their songs from those made by other birds In several examples birds show highly developed repetition abilities the Sri Lankan Greater racket tailed drongo Dicrurus paradiseus copies the calls of predators and the alarm signals of other birds 57 Albert s lyrebird Menura alberti can accurately imitate the satin bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus violaceus 58 Research upon avian vocal motor neurons finds that they perceive their song as a series of articulatory gestures as in humans 59 Birds that can imitate humans such as the Indian hill myna Gracula religiosa imitate human speech by mimicking the various speech formants created by changing the shape of the human vocal tract with different vibration frequencies of its internal tympaniform membrane 60 Indian hill mynahs also imitate such phonetic characteristics as voicing fundamental frequencies formant transitions nasalization and timing through their vocal movements are made in a different way from those of the human vocal apparatus 60 Nonhuman mammals edit Bottlenose dolphins can show spontaneous vocal mimicry of computer generated whistles 61 Killer whales can mimic the barks of California sea lions 62 Harbor seals can mimic in a speech like manner one or more English words and phrases 63 Elephants can imitate trunk sounds 64 Lesser spear nosed bat can learn their call structure from artificial playback 65 An orangutan has spontaneously copied the whistles of humans 66 Apes edit Apes taught language show an ability to imitate language signs with chimpanzees such as Washoe who was able to learn with his arms a vocabulary of 250 American Sign Language gestures However such human trained apes show no ability to imitate human speech vocalizations 67 See also editAlan Baddeley Auditory processing disorder Baddeley s model of working memory Conduction aphasia Developmental verbal dyspraxia Echoic memory Echolalia Language development Language acquisition Language based learning disability Mirror neurons Mirroring psychology Motor cognition Motor theory of speech perception Origin of language Passive speakers Phonological development Pronunciation Second language acquisition Short term memory Speech perception Thematic coherence Transcortical motor aphasia Transcortical sensory aphasia Vocabulary growth Vocal learningFootnotes edit Indefrey P Levelt W J M 2004 The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components Cognition 92 1 2 101 144 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 475 251 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2002 06 001 PMID 15037128 S2CID 12662702 Marslen Wilson W 1973 Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies Nature 244 5417 522 523 Bibcode 1973Natur 244 522M doi 10 1038 244522a0 PMID 4621131 S2CID 4220775 a b c Porter Jr R J Lubker J F 1980 Rapid reproduction of vowel vowel sequences Evidence for a fast and direct acoustic motoric linkage in speech Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 23 3 593 602 doi 10 1044 jshr 2303 593 PMID 7421161 Gentilucci M Cattaneo L 2005 Automatic audiovisual integration in speech perception Experimental Brain Research 167 1 66 75 doi 10 1007 s00221 005 0008 z PMID 16034571 S2CID 20166301 Acute hepatitis B virus infection in children and teachers England and Wales 1985 90 Communicable Disease Report 1 17 75 76 1991 PMID 1669805 a b Wernicke K The aphasia symptom complex 1874 Breslau Cohn and Weigert Translated in Eling P editor Reader in the history of aphasia Vol 4 Amsterdam John Benjamins 1994 p 69 89 ISBN 978 90 272 1893 3 Kuhl P K Meltzoff A N 1982 The bimodal perception of speech in infancy Science 218 4577 1138 1141 Bibcode 1982Sci 218 1138K doi 10 1126 science 7146899 PMID 7146899 Kuhl P K Meltzoff A N 1996 Infant vocalizations in response to speech Vocal imitation and developmental change The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100 4 Pt 1 2425 2438 Bibcode 1996ASAJ 100 2425K doi 10 1121 1 417951 PMC 3651031 PMID 8865648 Roberts J M 1989 Echolalia and comprehension in autistic children Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 19 2 271 281 doi 10 1007 BF02211846 PMID 2745392 S2CID 6925526 Schneider DE 1938 The clinical syndromes of echolalia echopraxia grasping and sucking Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 88 18 35 200 216 doi 10 1097 00005053 193807000 00003 S2CID 143703500 Schuler A L 1979 Echolalia Issues and clinical applications The Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 44 4 411 34 doi 10 1044 jshd 4404 411 PMID 390245 Stengel E 1947 A Clinical and Psychological Study of Echo Reactions The British Journal of Psychiatry 93 392 598 612 doi 10 1192 bjp 93 392 598 PMID 20273402 Lees A J Robertson M Trimble M R Murray N M 1984 A clinical study of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome in the United Kingdom Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 47 1 1 8 doi 10 1136 jnnp 47 1 1 PMC 1027633 PMID 6582230 Trojano L Fragassi N A Postiglione A Grossi D 1988 Mixed transcortical aphasia On relative sparing of phonological short term store in a case Neuropsychologia 26 4 633 638 doi 10 1016 0028 3932 88 90120 0 PMID 2457182 S2CID 35115074 McLeod P Posner MI 1984 Privileged loops from percept to act In H Bouma D Bouwhuis Eds Attention and performance X pp 55 66 Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum ISBN 978 0 86377 005 0 Coslett H B Roeltgen D P Gonzalez Rothi L Heilman K M 1987 Transcortical sensory aphasia Evidence for subtypes Brain and Language 32 2 362 378 doi 10 1016 0093 934X 87 90133 7 PMID 3690258 S2CID 6079313 McCarthy R Warrington E K 1984 A two route model of speech production Evidence from aphasia Brain A Journal of Neurology 107 2 463 485 doi 10 1093 brain 107 2 463 PMID 6722512 McCarthy R A Warrington E K 2001 Repeating Without Semantics Surface Dysphasia Neurocase 7 1 77 87 doi 10 1093 neucas 7 1 77 PMID 11239078 Bloomer HH 1971 Speech defects associated with dental malocclusions and related abnormalities In L E Eds Handbook of speech pathology and audiology pp 715 766 New York Appleton Century ISBN 978 0 13 381764 5 Williams RJ 1967 You are extra ordinary New York Random House pp 26 27 OCLC 156187572 Vocal traits also vary moreover when people get upper respiratory tract infections as the shape and size of sinus cavities is further changed with the swelling of mucous membranes a b Kappes J Baumgaertner A Peschke C Ziegler W 2009 Unintended imitation in nonword repetition Brain and Language 111 3 140 151 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2009 08 008 PMID 19811813 S2CID 2113790 Gentilucci M Bernardis P 2007 Imitation during phoneme production Neuropsychologia 45 3 608 15 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2006 04 004 PMID 16698051 S2CID 40687020 Shockley K Sabadini L Fowler C A 2004 Imitation in shadowing words Perception amp Psychophysics 66 3 422 429 doi 10 3758 BF03194890 PMID 15283067 Delvaux V Soquet A 2007 The influence of ambient speech on adult speech productions through unintentional imitation Phonetica 64 2 3 145 73 doi 10 1159 000107914 PMID 17914281 S2CID 22042824 Wernicke K 1874 The aphasia symptom complex Breslau Cohn and Weigert Translated in Eling P editor 1994 p 69 89 Reader in the history of aphasia Vol 4 Amsterdam John Benjamins The major tasks of the child in speech acquisition is mimicry of the spoken word p76 a b Bloom L Hood L Lightbown P 1974 Imitation in language development If when and why Cognitive Psychology 6 3 380 420 doi 10 1016 0010 0285 74 90018 8 a b Miller GA 1977 Spontaneous apprentices Children and language New York Seabury Press ISBN 978 0 8164 9330 2 a b Masur EF 1995 Infants early verbal imitation and their later lexical development Merrill Palmer Quarterly 41 286 306 OCLC 89395784 a b Gathercole SE Baddeley AD 1989 Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children A longitudinal study Journal of Memory and Language 28 2 200 213 doi 10 1016 0749 596x 89 90044 2 Archived from the original on 2012 08 17 Retrieved 2009 12 19 a b Gathercole S E 2006 Nonword repetition and word learning The nature of the relationship Applied Psycholinguistics 27 4 513 543 doi 10 1017 S0142716406060383 S2CID 145633911 PDF Archived 2011 06 05 at the Wayback Machine Hoff E Core C Bridges K 2008 Non word repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in 20 to 24 month olds Journal of Child Language 35 4 903 16 doi 10 1017 S0305000908008751 PMID 18838017 S2CID 18566002 a b Stokes S F Klee T 2009 Factors that influence vocabulary development in two year old children Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 50 4 498 505 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 2008 01991 x PMID 19017366 Laws G Gunn D 2004 Phonological memory as a predictor of language comprehension in Down syndrome A five year follow up study Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 45 2 326 337 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 2004 00224 x PMID 14982246 Speidel GE Herreshoff MJ 1989 Imitation and the construction of long utterances In G E Speidel amp K E Nelson Eds The many faces of imitation in language learning pp 181 197 New York Springer Verlag ISBN 978 0 387 96885 8 Kuczaj SA 1983 Crib speech and language practice New York Springer Verlag ISBN 978 0 387 90860 1 Scott S K McGettigan C Eisner F 2009 A little more conversation a little less action candidate roles for the motor cortex in speech perception Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10 4 295 302 doi 10 1038 nrn2603 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 0013 2999 F PMC 4238059 PMID 19277052 p 201 Fillmore LW 1979 Individual differences in second language acquisition In C J Fillmore D Kempler amp W S Y Wang Eds Individual differences in language ability and language behavior pp 203 228 New York Academic Press OCLC 4983571 Gathercole S E 1995 Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long term knowledge It all depends on the nonwords Memory amp Cognition 23 1 83 94 doi 10 3758 BF03210559 PMID 7885268 S2CID 20774241 Cheng H 1996 Nonword span as a unique predictor of second language vocabulary learning Developmental Psychology 32 5 867 873 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 32 5 867 Papagno C Vallar G 1995 Verbal short term memory and vocabulary learning in polyglots The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A Human Experimental Psychology 48 1 98 107 doi 10 1080 14640749508401378 PMID 7754088 S2CID 19242688 Bishop D V North T Donlan C 1996 Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment Evidence from a twin study Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 37 4 391 403 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 1996 tb01420 x PMID 8735439 Ojemann GA 1983 Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6 2 189 230 doi 10 1017 s0140525x00015491 S2CID 143189089 Kimura D Watson N 1989 The relation between oral movement control and speech Brain and Language 37 4 565 590 doi 10 1016 0093 934X 89 90112 0 PMID 2479446 S2CID 39913744 Shaffer LH 1984 Motor programming in language production In H Bouma amp D G Bouwhuis Eds Attention and performance X pp 17 41 London Erlbaum ISBN 978 0 86377 005 0 Abbs JH 1986 Invariance and variability in speech production A distinction between linguistic intent and its neuromotor implementation In J S Perkell amp D H Klatt Eds Invariance and variability in speech processes pp 202 219 Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum ISBN 978 0 89859 545 1 Porter RJ 1987 What is the relation between speech production and speech perception In Allport A MacKay D G Prinz W G Scheerer E eds Language Perception and Production London Academic Press 85 106 ISBN 978 0 12 052750 2 Hickok G Poeppel D 2004 Dorsal and ventral streams A framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language Cognition 92 1 2 67 99 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2003 10 011 PMID 15037127 S2CID 635860 Okada K Hickok G 2006 Left posterior auditory related cortices participate both in speech perception and speech production Neural overlap revealed by fMRI Brain and Language 98 1 112 117 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2006 04 006 PMID 16716388 S2CID 1056984 Wise R J Scott S K Blank S C Mummery C J Murphy K Warburton E A 2001 Separate neural subsystems within Wernicke s area Brain A Journal of Neurology 124 Pt 1 83 95 doi 10 1093 brain 124 1 83 PMID 11133789 Obleser J Scott S K Eulitz C 2005 Now You Hear It Now You Don t Transient Traces of Consonants and their Nonspeech Analogues in the Human Brain Cerebral Cortex 16 8 1069 1076 doi 10 1093 cercor bhj047 PMID 16207930 Umilta M A Kohler E Gallese V Fogassi L Fadiga L Keysers C Rizzolatti G 2001 I know what you are doing A neurophysiological study Neuron 31 1 155 165 doi 10 1016 s0896 6273 01 00337 3 PMID 11498058 Hickok G 2010 The role of mirror neurons in speech and language processing Brain and Language 112 1 1 2 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2009 10 006 PMC 2813993 PMID 19948355 Skoyles J R 2010 Mapping of heard speech into articulation information and speech acquisition Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 107 18 E73 Bibcode 2010PNAS 107E 73S doi 10 1073 pnas 1003007107 PMC 2889576 PMID 20427741 a b Poizner H Klima ES Bellugi U 1987 What the hands reveal about the brain MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 66066 2 Nishimura H Hashikawa K Doi K Iwaki T Watanabe Y Kusuoka H Nishimura T Kubo T 1999 Sign language heard in the auditory cortex Nature 397 6715 116 Bibcode 1999Natur 397 116N doi 10 1038 16376 PMID 9923672 S2CID 4414422 Goodale E Kotagama S W 2006 Context dependent vocal mimicry in a passerine bird Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 273 1588 875 880 doi 10 1098 rspb 2005 3392 PMC 1560225 PMID 16618682 Putland D A Nicholls J A Noad M J Goldizen A W 2006 Imitating the neighbours Vocal dialect matching in a mimic model system Biology Letters 2 3 367 370 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2006 0502 PMC 1686190 PMID 17148405 Williams H Nottebohm F 1985 Auditory responses in avian vocal motor neurons A motor theory for song perception in birds Science 229 4710 279 282 Bibcode 1985Sci 229 279W doi 10 1126 science 4012321 PMID 4012321 S2CID 19053313 a b Klatt D H Stefanski R A 1974 How does a mynah bird imitate human speech The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 55 4 822 832 Bibcode 1974ASAJ 55 822K doi 10 1121 1 1914607 PMID 4833078 Reiss D McCowan B 1993 Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus Evidence for vocal learning Journal of Comparative Psychology 107 3 301 312 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 107 3 301 PMID 8375147 Foote A D Griffin R M Howitt D Larsson L Miller P J O Hoelzel A 2006 Killer whales are capable of vocal learning Biology Letters 2 4 509 512 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2006 0525 PMC 1834009 PMID 17148275 Ralls K Fiorelli P Gish S 1985 Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbor seals Phoca vitulina Canadian Journal of Zoology 63 5 1050 1056 doi 10 1139 z85 157 Poole J H Tyack P L Stoeger Horwath A S Watwood S 2005 Animal behaviour Elephants are capable of vocal learning Nature 434 7032 455 456 Bibcode 2005Natur 434 455P doi 10 1038 434455a PMID 15791244 S2CID 4369863 Esser K H 1994 Audio vocal learning in a non human mammal The lesser spear nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor NeuroReport 5 14 1718 1720 doi 10 1097 00001756 199409080 00007 PMID 7827315 Wich S A Swartz K B Hardus M E Lameira A R Stromberg E Shumaker R W 2008 A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan Primates 50 1 56 64 doi 10 1007 s10329 008 0117 y PMID 19052691 S2CID 708682 Hayes C 1951 The ape in our house Harper New York OCLC 1579444 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Speech repetition amp oldid 1181907810, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.