fbpx
Wikipedia

Child development

Child development involves the biological, psychological and emotional changes that occur in human beings between birth and the conclusion of adolescence.

A child using fingers to make a small, circular hole in the sand, 1997

Childhood is divided into three stages of life which include early childhood, middle childhood, and late childhood (preadolescence).[1] Early childhood typically ranges from infancy to the age of 6 years old. During this period, development is significant, as many of life's milestones happen during this time period such as first words, learning to crawl, and learning to walk. There is speculation that middle childhood/preadolescence or ages 6–12[1] are the most crucial years of a child's life. Adolescence is the stage of life that typically starts around the major onset of puberty, with markers such as menarche and spermarche, typically occurring at 12–13 years of age.[2] It has been defined as ages 10 to 19 by the World Health Organization.[3] In the course of development, the individual human progresses from dependency to increasing autonomy. It is a continuous process with a predictable sequence, yet has a unique course for every child. It does not always progress at the same rate and each stage is affected by the preceding developmental experiences. As genetic factors and events during prenatal life may strongly influence developmental changes, genetics and prenatal development usually form a part of the study of child development. Related terms include developmental psychology, referring to development from birth to death, and pediatrics, the branch of medicine relating to the care of children.

Developmental change may occur as a result of genetically controlled processes, known as maturation,[4] or environmental factors and learning, but most commonly involves an interaction between the two. Development may also occur as a result of human nature and of human ability to learn from the environment.

There are various definitions of the periods in a child's development, since each period is a continuum with individual differences regarding starting and ending. Some age-related development periods with defined intervals include: newborn (ages 0 – 2 months); infant (ages 3 – 11 months); toddler (ages 1 – 2 years); preschooler (ages 3 – 4 years); school-aged child (ages 5 – 12 years); teens (ages 13 – 17 years).[5]

Parents play a large role in a child's activities, socialization, and development; having multiple parents can add stability to a child's life and therefore encourage healthy development.[6] Another influential factor in children's development is the quality of their care. Child-care programs may be beneficial for childhood development such as learning capabilities and social skills.[7]

The optimal development of children is considered vital to society and it is important to understand the social, cognitive, emotional, and educational development of children. Increased research and interest in this field has resulted in new theories and strategies, especially with regard to practices that promote development within the school systems. Some theories seek to describe a sequence of states that compose child development.

Theories edit

Ecological systems edit

Also called "development in context" or "human ecology" theory, ecological systems theory was originally formulated by Urie Bronfenbrenner. It specifies four types of nested environmental systems, with bi-directional influences within and between the systems; they are the microsystem, mesosystem, exosystem, and macrosystem. Each system contains roles, norms, and rules that can powerfully shape development. Since its publication in 1979, Bronfenbrenner's major statement of this theory, The Ecology of Human Development,[8] has had widespread influence on the way psychologists and others approach the study of human beings and their environments. As a result of this influential conceptualization of development, these environments — from the family to economic and political structures — have come to be viewed as part of the life course from childhood through adulthood.[9]

Piaget edit

Jean Piaget was a Swiss scholar who began his studies in intellectual development in the 1920s. Interested in the ways animals adapt to their environments, his first scientific article was published when he was 10 years old, and he pursued a Ph.D. in zoology, where he became interested in epistemology.[10] Epistemology branches off from philosophy and deals with the origin of knowledge, which Piaget believed came from Psychology. After travelling to Paris, he began working on the first "standardized intelligence test" at Alfred Binet laboratories, which influenced his career greatly. During this intelligence testing he began developing a profound interest in the way children's intellectualism works. As a result, he developed his own laboratory, where he spent years recording children's intellectual growth and attempting to find out how children develop through various stages of thinking. This led Piaget to develop four important stages of cognitive development: sensorimotor stage (birth to age 2), preoperational stage (age 2 to 7), concrete-operational stage (ages 7 to 12), and formal-operational stage (ages 11 to 12, and thereafter).[10] Piaget concluded that adaption to an environment (behaviour) is managed through schemas and adaption occurs through assimilation and accommodation.[11]

Stages edit

Sensory Motor: (birth to about age 2)

In the first stage in Piaget's theory, infants have the following basic senses: vision, hearing, and motor skills. In this stage, knowledge of the world is limited but is constantly developing due to the child's experiences and interactions.[12] According to Piaget, when an infant reaches about 7–9 months of age they begin to develop what he called object permanence, meaning the child now has the ability to understand that objects keep existing even when they cannot be seen. An example of this would be hiding the child's favorite toy under a blanket, and although the child cannot physically see it they still know to look under the blanket.[13]

Preoperational: (begins about the time the child starts to talk, around age 2)

During this stage, young children begin analyzing their environment using mental symbols, including words and images; the child will begin to apply these in their everyday lives as they come across different objects, events, and situations.[10] However, Piaget's main focus on this stage, and the reason why he named it "preoperational," is that children at this point are not able to apply specific cognitive operations, such as mental math. In addition to symbolism, children start to engage in pretend play, pretending to be people they are not, for example teachers or superheroes; they sometimes use different props to make this pretend play more real.[10] Some weaknesses in this stage are that children who are about 3–4 years old often display what is called egocentrism, meaning the child is not able to see someone else's point of view, and they feel as if every other person is experiencing the same events and feelings that they are. However, at about 7, thought processes of children are no longer egocentric and are more intuitive, meaning they now think about the way something looks, though they do not yet use rational thinking.[10]

Concrete: (about first grade to early adolescence)

In this stage, children between the age of 7 and 11 use appropriate logic to develop cognitive operations and begin applying this new way of thinking to different events they encounter.[10] Children in this stage incorporate inductive reasoning, which involves drawing conclusions from other observations in order to make a generalization.[14] Unlike in the preoperational stage, children can now change and rearrange mental images and symbols to form a logical thought, an example of this is "reversibility," where the child now knows to reverse an action by doing the opposite.[10]

Formal operations: (around early adolescence to mid/late adolescence)

The final stage of Piaget's cognitive development defines a child as now having the ability to "think more rationally and systematically about abstract concepts and hypothetical events".[10] Some strengths during this time are that the child or adolescent begins forming their identity and begins understanding why people behave the way they behave. While some weaknesses include the child or adolescent developing some egocentric thoughts, including the imaginary audience and the personal fable.[10] An imaginary audience is when an adolescent feels that the world is just as concerned and judgemental of anything the adolescent does as they themselves are; an adolescent may feel as if they are "on stage" and everyone is a critic and they are the ones being critiqued.[10] A personal fable is when the adolescent feels that he or she is a unique person and everything they do is unique. They feel as if they are the only ones that have ever experienced what they are experiencing and that they are invincible and nothing bad will happen to them, bad things only happen to other people.[10]

Vygotsky edit

Vygotsky, a Russian theorist, proposed the sociocultural theory of child development. During the 1920s–1930s, while Piaget was developing his own theory, Vygotsky was an active scholar and at that time his theory was said to be "recent" because it was translated out of Russian and began influencing Western thinking.[10] He posited that children learn through hands-on experience, as Piaget suggested. However, unlike Piaget, he claimed that timely and sensitive intervention by adults when a child is on the edge of learning a new task (called the zone of proximal development) could help children learn new tasks. This technique, called "scaffolding," builds new knowledge onto the knowledge children already have to help the child learn.[15] An example of this might be when a parent "helps" an infant clap or roll their hands to the pat-a-cake rhyme, until they can clap and roll their hands themself.[16][17]

Vygotsky was strongly focused on the role of culture in determining the child's pattern of development.[15] He argued that "Every function in the child's cultural development appears twice: first, on the social level, and later, on the individual level; first, between people (interpsychological) and then inside the child (intrapsychological). This applies equally to voluntary attention, to logical memory, and to the formation of concepts. All the higher functions originate as actual relationships between individuals."[15]

Vygotsky felt that development was a process, and saw that during periods of crisis there was a qualitative transformation in the child's mental functioning.[18]

Attachment edit

Attachment theory, originating in the work of John Bowlby and developed by Mary Ainsworth, is a psychological, evolutionary and ethological theory that provides a descriptive and explanatory framework for understanding interpersonal relationships. Bowlby's observations led him to believe that close emotional bonds or "attachments" between an infant and their primary caregiver were an important requirement for forming "normal social and emotional development".[10]

Erik Erikson edit

Erikson, a follower of Freud, synthesized his theories with Freud's to create what is known as the "psychosocial" stages of human development. Spanning from birth to death, they focus on "tasks" at each stage that must be accomplished to successfully navigate life's challenges.[19]

Erikson's eight stages consist of the following:[20]

  • Trust vs. mistrust (infant)
  • Autonomy vs. shame (toddlerhood)
  • Initiative vs. guilt (preschooler)
  • Industry vs. inferiority (young adolescent)
  • Identity vs. role confusion (adolescent)
  • Intimacy vs. isolation (young adulthood)
  • Generativity vs. stagnation (middle adulthood)
  • Ego integrity vs. despair (old age)

Behavioral edit

John B. Watson's behaviorism theory forms the foundation of the behavioral model of development.[21] Watson explained human psychology through the process of classical conditioning, and he believed that all individual differences in behavior were due to different learning experiences.[22] He wrote extensively on child development and conducted research, such as the Little Albert experiment, which showed that a phobia could be created by classical conditioning. Watson was instrumental in the modification of William James' stream of consciousness approach to construct behavior theory.[23] He also helped bring a natural science perspective to child psychology by introducing objective research methods based on observable and measurable behavior.[23] Following Watson's lead, B.F. Skinner further extended this model to cover operant conditioning and verbal behavior.[24] Skinner used the operant chamber, or Skinner box, to observe the behavior of animals in a controlled situation and proved that behaviors are influenced by the environment. Furthermore, he used reinforcement and punishment to shape the desired behavior. Children's behavior can strongly depend on their psychological development.

Freud edit

Sigmund Freud divided development, from infancy onward, into five stages.[25] In accordance with his view that the sexual drive is a basic human motivation,[26] each stage centered around the gratification of the libido within a particular area, or erogenous zone, of the body.[27] He argued that as humans develop, they become fixated on different and specific objects throughout their stages of development.[28][29] Each stage contains conflict which requires resolution to enable the child to develop.[30]

Other edit

The use of dynamical systems theory as a framework for the consideration of development began in the early 1990s and has continued into the present.[31] This theory stresses nonlinear connections (e.g., between earlier and later social assertiveness) and the capacity of a system to reorganize as a phase shift that is stage-like in nature. Another useful concept for developmentalists is the attractor state, a condition (such as teething or stranger anxiety) that helps to determine apparently unrelated behaviors as well as related ones.[32] Dynamic systems theory has been applied extensively to the study of motor development; the theory also has strong associations with some of Bowlby's views about attachment systems. Dynamic systems theory also relates to the concept of the transactional process,[33] a mutually interactive process in which children and parents simultaneously influence each other, producing developmental change in both over time.[34]

The "core knowledge perspective" is an evolutionary theory in child development that proposes "infants begin life with innate, special-purpose knowledge systems referred to as core domains of thought".[35] These five domains are each crucial for survival, and prepare us to develop key aspects of early cognition, they are: physical, numerical, linguistic, psychological, and biological.[35]

Beginning of cognition edit

The most influential theories emphasize social interaction's essential contribution to child development from birth (e.g., the theories of Bronfenbrenner,[9] Piaget,[10] Vygotsky[15]). It means that organisms with simple reflexes begin to cognize the environment in collaboration with caregivers. However, different viewpoints on this issue - the binding problem[36] and the primary data entry problem[37][38] - challenge the ability of children in this stage of development to meaningfully interact with the environment.[39][40]

Recent advances in neuroscience and wisdom from physiology and physics studies reconsider the knowledge gap on how social interaction provides cognition in newborns and infants. Developmental psychologist Michael Tomasello contributed to knowledge about the origins of social cognition in children by developing the notion of Shared intentionality.[41] He posed ideas about unaware processes during social learning after birth to explain processes in shaping Intentionality.[41] Other researchers developed the notion, by observing this collaborative interaction in psychophysiological research.[42][43][44]

This concept has been expanded to the intrauterine period. Research professor in bioengineering at Liepaja University Igor Val Danilov developed the idea of Michael Tomasello by introducing a hypothesis of neurophysiological processes occurring during Shared intentionality.[37][39][45] It explains the onset of childhood development, describing this cooperative interaction at different levels of bio-system complexity, from interpersonal dynamics to neuronal interactions.[39] The Shared intentionality hypothesis argues that nervous system synchronization provides non-local neuronal coupling in a mother-child pair, contributing to the proper development of the child's nervous system from the embryo onward.[39] From the cognitive development perspective, this non-local neuronal coupling enables the mother to indicate the relevant sensory stimulus of an actual cognitive problem to the child, helping the child to grasp the perception of the object.[39] A growing body of evidence in neuroscience supports the Shared intentionality approach. Hyperscanning research studies show inter-brain coordinated activity under conditions without communication in pairs while subjects are solving a shared cognitive task[46][47][48][49][50][51] This increased inter-brain activity is observed in pairs, which differs from the result in the condition where subjects solve a similar task alone. The significance of this knowledge is that although Shared intentionality enables social cooperation to be achieved in the unaware condition (unconsciously), it constitutes society. While this social interaction modality facilitates child development, it also contributes to grasping social norms and shaping individual values in children.[52]

Continuity and discontinuity edit

Although the identification of developmental milestones is of interest to researchers and caregivers, many aspects of development are continuous and do not display noticeable milestones.[53] Continuous changes, like growth in stature, involve fairly gradual and predictable progress toward adult characteristics. When developmental change is discontinuous, however, researchers may identify not only milestones of development, but related age periods often called stages. These stages are periods of time, often associated with known age ranges, during which a behavior or physical characteristic is qualitatively different from what it is at other ages. When an age period is referred to as a stage, the term implies not only this qualitative difference, but also a predictable sequence of developmental events, such that each stage is preceded and followed by specific other periods associated with characteristic behavioral or physical qualities.[54]

Stages of development may overlap or be associated with specific other aspects of development, such as speech or movement. Even within a particular developmental area, transition into a stage may not mean that the previous stage is completely finished. For example, in Erikson's stages, he suggests that a lifetime is spent in reworking issues that were originally characteristic of a childhood stage.[55] Similarly, the theorist of cognitive development, Piaget, described situations in which children could solve one type of problem using mature thinking skills, but could not accomplish this for less familiar problems, a phenomenon he called horizontal decalage.[56]

Mechanisms edit

 
Girl playing in a playground

Although developmental change runs parallel with chronological age,[57] age itself cannot cause development.[57] The basic causes for developmental change are genetic and environmental factors.[58] Genetic factors are responsible for cellular changes like overall growth, changes in proportion of body and brain parts,[59] and the maturation of aspects of function such as vision and dietary needs.[57] Because genes can be "turned off" and "turned on",[57] the individual's initial genotype may change in function over time, giving rise to further developmental change. Environmental factors affecting development may include both diet and disease exposure, as well as social, emotional, and cognitive experiences.[57] However, examination of environmental factors also shows that children can survive a fairly broad range of environmental experiences.[56]

Rather than acting as independent mechanisms, genetic and environmental factors often interact to cause developmental change.[57] Some aspects of child development are notable for their plasticity, or the extent to which the direction of development is guided by environmental factors as well as initiated by genetic factors.[57] When an aspect of development is strongly affected by early experience, it is said to show a high degree of plasticity; when the genetic make-up is the primary cause of development, plasticity is said to be low.[60] Plasticity may involve guidance by endogenous factors like hormones as well as by exogenous factors like infection.[57]

 
Child playing with bubbles

One way the environment guides development is through experience-dependent plasticity, in which behavior is altered as a result of learning from the environment. Plasticity of this type can occur throughout the lifespan and involve many kinds of behavior, including some emotional reactions.[57] A second type of plasticity, experience-expectant plasticity, involves the strong effect of specific experiences during limited sensitive periods of development.[57] For example, the coordinated use of two eyes, and the experience of a single three-dimensional image rather than the two-dimensional images created by each eye, depends on experiences with vision during the second half of the first year of life.[57] Experience-expectant plasticity works to fine-tune aspects of development that cannot proceed to optimum outcomes as a result of genetic factors alone.[61][62]

In addition to plasticity, genetic-environmental correlations may function in several ways to determine the mature characteristics of the individual. Genetic-environmental correlations are circumstances in which genetic factors interact with the environment to make certain experiences more likely to occur.[57] In passive genetic-environmental correlation, a child is likely to experience a particular environment because his or her parents' genetic make-up makes them likely to choose or create such an environment.[57] In evocative genetic-environmental correlation, the child's genetically produced characteristics cause other people to respond in certain ways, providing a different environment than might occur for a genetically different child;[57] for instance, a child with Down syndrome may be protected more and challenged less than a child without Down syndrome.[57] Finally, an active genetic-environmental correlation is one in which the child chooses experiences that in turn have their effect,[57] for instance, a muscular, active child may choose after-school sports experiences that increase athletic skills, but may forgo music lessons. In all of these cases, it becomes difficult to know whether the child's characteristics were shaped by genetic factors, by experiences, or by a combination of the two.[63]

Asynchronous development edit

Asynchronous development occurs in cases when a child's cognitive, physical, and/or emotional development occur at different rates. This is common for gifted children when their cognitive development outpaces their physical and/or emotional maturity, such as when a child is academically advanced and skipping school grade levels yet still cries over childish matters and/or still looks their age. Asynchronous development presents challenges for schools, parents, siblings, peers, and the children themselves, such as making it hard for the child to fit in or frustrating adults who have become accustomed to the child's advancement in other areas.[64]

Research issues and methods edit

Research questions include:

  1. What develops? What relevant aspects of the individual change over a period of time?
  2. What are the rate and speed of development?
  3. What are the mechanisms of development – what aspects of experience and heredity cause developmental change?
  4. Are there typical individual differences in the relevant developmental changes?
  5. Are there population differences in this aspect of development (for example, differences in the development of boys and of girls)?

Empirical research that attempts to answer these questions may follow a number of patterns. Initially, observational research in naturalistic conditions may be needed to develop a narrative describing and defining an aspect of developmental change, such as changes in reflex reactions in the first year.[65] Observational research may be followed by correlational studies, which collect information about chronological age and some type of development, such as increasing vocabulary; such studies examine the characteristics of children at different ages.[66] Other methods may include longitudinal studies, in which a group of children is re-examined on a number of occasions as they get older; cross-sectional studies, where groups of children of different ages are tested once and compared with each other; or there may be a combination of these approaches. Some child development studies that examine the effects of experience or heredity by comparing characteristics of different groups of children cannot use a randomized design; while other studies use randomized designs to compare outcomes for groups of children who receive different interventions or educational treatments.[56]

Infant research methods edit

When conducting psychological research on infants and children, certain key aspects need to be considered.[67] These include that infants cannot talk, have a limited behavioral repertoire, cannot follow instructions, have a short attention span, and that, due to how rapidly infants develop, methods need to be updated for different ages and developmental stages.[67]

High-amplitude sucking technique (HAS) is a common way to explore infants' preferences, and is appropriate from birth to four months since it takes advantage of infants' sucking reflex.[68] When this is being measured, researchers will code a baseline sucking rate for each baby before exposing them to the item of interest. A common finding of HAS shows a relaxed, natural sucking rate when exposed to something the infant is familiar with, like their mother's voice, compared to an increased sucking rate around novel stimuli.[69]

The preferential-looking technique was a breakthrough made by Robert L. Fantz in 1961.[67] In his experiments, he would show the infants in his study two different stimuli. If an infant looks at one image longer than the other, there are two things that can be inferred: the infant can see that they are two different images and that the infant is showing preference to one image in some capacity. Depending on the experiment, infants may prefer to look at the novel and more interesting stimulus or they may look at the more comforting and familiar image.[70]

Eye tracking is a straightforward way of looking at infants' preferences. Using an eye tracking software, it is possible to see if infants understand commonly used nouns by tracking their eyes after they are cued with the target word.[71]

 
Typical pattern of habituation

Another unique way to study infants' cognition is through habituation, which is the process of repeatedly showing a stimulus to an infant until they give no response.[72] Then, when infants are presented with a novel stimulus, they show a response, which reveals patterns of cognition and perception.[72] Using this study method, many different cognitive and perceptual ideas can be studied. Looking time, a common measure of habituation, is studied by recording how long an infant looks at a stimulus before they are habituated to it. Then, researchers record if an infant becomes dishabituated to a novel stimulus. This method can be used to measure preferences infants, including preferences for colors,[73] and other discriminatory tasks, such as auditory discrimination between different musical excerpts.[74]

Another way of studying children is through brain imaging technology, such as Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI), electroencephalography (EEG). MRI can be used to track brain activity, growth, and connectivity in children,[75] and can track brain development from when a child is a fetus.[76] EEG can be used to diagnose seizures and encephalopathy, but the conceptual age of the infant must be considered when analyzing the results.[77]

Ethical considerations edit

Most of the ethical challenges that exist in studies with adults also exist in studying children, with some notable differences.[78][79] Namely informed consent, as while it is important that children consent to participate in research, they cannot give legal consent; parents must give informed consent for their children. Children can informally consent though, and their continued agreement should be reliably checked for by both verbal and nonverbal cues throughout their participation. Also, due to the inherent power structure in most research settings, researchers must consider study designs that protect children from feeling coerced.[78]

Milestones edit

Milestones are changes in specific physical and mental abilities (such as walking and understanding language) that mark the end of one developmental period and the beginning of another;[80] for stage theories, milestones indicate a stage transition. These milestones, and the chronological age at which they typically occur, have been established via study of when various developmental tasks are accomplished. However, there is considerable variation in when milestones are reached, even between children developing within the typical range. Some milestones are more variable than others; for example, receptive speech indicators do not show much variation among children with typical hearing, but expressive speech milestones can be quite variable.[81][failed verification]

A common concern in child development is delayed development of age-specific developmental milestones. Preventing, and intervening early, in developmental delays is a significant topic in the study of child development. Developmental delays are characterized by comparison with age variability of a milestone, not with respect to average age at achievement.[82]

Physical aspects of development edit

Physical growth edit

For North American, Indo-Iranian (India, Iran), and European ...
girls boys
  • Thelarche (breast development) 10.5y (8y–13y)
  • Pubarche (pubic hair) 11y (8.5y–13.5y)
  • Growth spurt 11.25y (10y–12.5y)
  • Menarche (first menstrual bleeding) 12.5y (10.5y–14.5y)
  • Wisdom tooth eruption 15.5y (14y-17y)
  • Adult height reached 15.5y (14y–17y)
  • Gonadarche (testicular enlargement) 11.5y (9.5y–13.5y)
  • Pubarche (pubic hair) 12y (10y–14y)
  • Growth spurt 13y (11y–15y)
  • Spermarche (first ejaculation) 13.5y (11.5y–15.5y)
  • Wisdom tooth eruption 17y (15y-19y)
  • Completion of growth 17y (15y–19y)

Physical growth in stature and weight occurs for 15–20 years following birth, as the individual changes from the average weight of 3.5 kg (7.7 lb) and length of 50 cm (20 in) at full term birth to their final adult size. As stature and weight increase, proportions also change, from the relatively large head and small torso and limbs of the neonate, to the adult's relatively small head and long torso and limbs.[83] In a book directed toward pediatricians it says a child's pattern of growth is in a head-to-toe direction, or cephalocaudal, and in an inward to outward pattern (center of the body to the peripheral) called proximodistal.[84]

Development from childhood to the end of puberty, from The Adolescent Period
 
Development of a boy from 10 to 17 years old
 
Development of a girl from 4 to 16 years old

Speed and pattern edit

The speed of physical growth is rapid in the months after birth, then slows, so birth weight is doubled in the first four months, tripled by 1 year, but not quadrupled until 2 years.[85] Growth then proceeds at a slow rate until a period of rapid growth occurs shortly before puberty (between about 9 and 15 years of age).[86] Growth is not uniform in rate and timing across all parts of the body. At birth, head size is already relatively near that of an adult, but the lower parts of the body are much smaller than adult size. Thus during development, the head grows relatively little, while the torso and limbs undergo a great deal of growth.[83]

Mechanisms of change edit

Genetic factors play a major role in determining the growth rate, particularly in the characteristic changes in proportions during early human development. However, genetic factors can produce maximum growth only if environmental conditions are adequate, as poor nutrition, frequent injury, or disease can reduce the individual's adult stature; though even the best environment cannot cause growth to a greater stature than is determined by heredity.[83]

Individual variation versus disease edit

Individual differences in height and weight during childhood can be considerable. Some of these differences are due to genetic or environmental factors, but individual differences in reproductive maturation strongly influence development at some points.[83] For individuals falling outside these typical variations, the American Association of Clinical Endocrinologists defines short stature as height more than 2 standard deviations below the mean for age and gender, which corresponds to the shortest 2.3% of individuals.[87] In contrast, failure to thrive is usually defined in terms of weight, and can be evaluated either by a low weight for the child's age, or by a low rate of weight gain.[88] A similar term, stunted growth, generally refers to reduced growth rate as a manifestation of malnutrition in early childhood.[89]

Motor skills edit

 
A child learning to walk

Physical abilities change through childhood from the largely reflexive (unlearned, involuntary) movement young infants to the highly skilled voluntary movements characteristic of later childhood and adolescence.

Definition edit

"Motor learning refers to the increasing spatial and temporal accuracy of movements with practice".[90]Motor skills can be divided into two categories: basic skills necessary for everyday life and recreational skills, including skills for employment or interest based skills.

Speed and pattern edit

The speed of motor development is rapid in early life, as many of the reflexes of the newborn alter or disappear within the first year, and slows later. Like physical growth, motor development shows predictable patterns of cephalocaudal (head to foot) and proximodistal (torso to extremities) development, with movements at the head and in the more central areas coming under control before those of the lower part of the body or the hands and feet.[91] Movement ability develops in stage-like sequences,[92] for example: locomotion at 6–8 months involves creeping on all fours, then proceeds to pulling to stand, "cruising" while holding on to an object, walking while holding an adult's hand, and finally walking independently.[92] By middle childhood and adolescence, new motor skills are acquired by instruction or observation rather than in a predictable sequence.[53] There are executive functions of the brain (working memory, timing measure of inhibition and switching) which are important to motor skills. Critiques to the order of Executive Functioning leads to Motor Skills, suggesting Motor Skills can support Executive Functioning in the brain.[incomprehensible]

Mechanisms edit

The mechanisms involved in motor development involve some genetic components that determine aspects of muscle and bone strength, as well as the physical size of body parts at a given age. The main areas of the brain involved in motor skills are the frontal cortex, parietal cortex and basal ganglia. The dorsolateral frontal cortex is responsible for strategic processing, the parietal cortex is important in controlling perceptual-motor integration and the basal ganglia and supplementary motor cortex are responsible for motor sequences.[citation needed]

According to a study showing the relationship between coordination and limb growth in infants, genetic components have a huge impact on motor development.[93] Intra-limb correlations, like the distance between hip and knee joints, were studied and proved to affect the way an infant will walk. There are also genetic factors like the tendency to use the left or right side of the body more (which allows for early prediction of the dominant hand early). Sample t-tests showed that, for female babies, there was a significant difference between the left and right sides at 18 weeks and that the right side was usually dominant.[93] Some factors are biological constraints that we cannot control, like male infants tending to have larger and longer arms, yet have an influence on measures like when an infant's reach. Overall, there are both sociological and genetic factors that influence motor development.[93]

Nutrition and exercise also determine strength, flexibility, and the ease and accuracy with which a body part can be moved.[53][94] It has also been shown that the frontal lobe develops posterio-anteriorally (from back to front), which is significant in motor development because the hind portion of the frontal lobe is known to control motor functions. This form of development (known as "Proportional Development") explains why motor functions typically develop relatively quickly during childhood, while logic, which is controlled by the middle and front portions of the frontal lobe, usually will not develop until late childhood or early adolescence.[95] Opportunities to carry out movements help establish the abilities to flex (move toward the trunk) and extend body parts, both capacities are necessary for good motor ability. Skilled voluntary movements such as passing objects from hand to hand develop as a result of practice and learning.[53] Mastery Climates are autonomy-supportive climates that a teacher can adopt to as a suggested successful learning environment for children to promote and reinforce motor skills by their own motivation. This promotes participation and active learning in children, which Piaget's theory of cognitive development says is extremely important in early childhood.[96]

Individual differences edit

Individual differences in motor ability are common and depend in part on the child's weight and build. Infants with smaller, slimmer, and more mature builds (proportionally) tend to belly crawl and crawl earlier than infants with larger builds.[93] Infants with more motor experience have been shown to belly crawl and crawl sooner. Not all infants belly crawl, however, those who skip this are not as proficient in their ability to crawl on their hands and knees.[97] After the infant period, individual differences are strongly affected by opportunities to practice, observe, and be instructed on specific movements. Atypical motor development such as persistent primitive reflexes beyond 4–6 months, or delayed walking may be an indication of developmental delays or conditions such as autism, cerebral palsy, or down syndrome.[53] Lower motor coordination results in difficulties with speed accuracy and with trade-off in complex tasks.

Children with disabilities edit

Children with Down syndrome or developmental coordination disorder are late to reach major motor skills milestones like sucking, grasping, rolling, sitting up and walking, talking. Children with Down syndrome sometimes have heart problems, frequent ear infections, hypotonia, or undeveloped muscle mass. Children can also be diagnosed with a learning disability, which are disabilities in any of the areas related to language, reading, and mathematics, with basic reading skills being the most common learning disability. The definition of a learning disability focuses on the difference between a child's academic achievement and their apparent capacity to learn.[98]

Population differences edit

Regardless of the culture a baby is born into, they are born with a few core domains of knowledge which allow them to make sense of their environment and learn upon previous experience by using motor skills such as grasping or crawling. There are some population differences in motor development, with girls showing some advantages in small muscle usage, including articulation of sounds with lips and tongue. Ethnic differences in reflex movements of newborn infants have been reported, suggesting that some biological factor is at work. Cultural differences may encourage learning of motor skills like using the left hand only for sanitary purposes and the right hand for all other uses, producing a population difference.[99] Cultural factors are play a role in practiced voluntary movements, such as the use of the foot to dribble a soccer ball or the hand to dribble a basketball.[53]

Mental and emotional aspects of development edit

Cognitive/intellectual edit

Cognitive development is primarily concerned with ways in which young children acquire, develop, and use internal mental capabilities such as problem-solving, memory, and language.[100]

Mechanisms edit

Cognitive development has genetic and other biological mechanisms, as is seen in the many genetic causes of intellectual disability. Environmental factors including food and nutrition, the responsiveness of parents, love, daily experiences, and physical activity can influence early brain development of children.[101] However, although it is assumed that the brain causes cognition, it is not yet possible to measure specific brain changes and show the cognitive changes they cause. Developmental advances in cognition are also related to experience and learning, especially for higher-level abilities like abstraction, which depend to a considerable extent on formal education.[53]

Speed and pattern edit

The ability to learn temporal patterns in sequenced actions was investigated in elementary-school-age children. Temporal learning depends upon a process of integrating timing patterns with action sequences. Children ages 6–13 and young adults performed a serial response time task in which a response and a timing sequence were presented repeatedly in a phase-matched manner, allowing for integrative learning. The degree of integrative learning was measured as the slowing in performance that resulted when phase-shifting the sequences. Learning was similar for the children and adults on average but increased with age for the children. Executive function measured by Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) performance as well as a measure of response speed also improved with age. Finally, WCST performance and response speed predicted temporal learning. Taken together, the results indicate that temporal learning continues to develop in pre-adolescents and that maturing executive function or processing speed may play an important role in acquiring temporal patterns in sequenced actions and the development of this ability.[102][non-primary source needed]

Individual differences edit

There are typical individual differences in the ages at which specific cognitive abilities are achieved, but schooling for children in industrialized countries is based on the assumption that there are no large differences.[53] Delays in cognitive development are problematic for children in cultures that demand advanced cognitive skills for work and for independent living.[53] Everyday cognitive skills include problem-solving, reasoning, and abstract thinking among many others.[103] In the absence of these life skills, children may struggle to complete work in a timely manner or understand certain tasks they are asked to do. If a delay is noticed screenings can possibly find the source of the issue;[104] if there is no underlying issue it is important to help aid the child by reading with them, playing games with them, or reaching out to professionals that can help.[104]

Population differences edit

There are few population differences in cognitive development: boys and girls show some differences in their skills and preferences, but there is a great deal of overlap between them.[53] Some differences are seen in fluid reasoning and visual processing, as until about the age of four girls outperform boys in tests of these skills, but by about six or seven boys and girls score similarly. This is also true of IQ tests where girls tend to outscore boys, but again, as they age the gap lessens.[105] Differences in cognitive achievement between different ethnic groups appears to result from cultural or other environmental factors.[53]

Social-emotional edit

Factors edit

Newborn infants do not seem to experience fear or have preferences for contact with any specific people. In the first few months they only experience happiness, sadness, and anger.[106][107] A baby's first smile usually occurs between 6 and 10 weeks, as this usually occurs during social interactions it is called a "social smile".[108] By about 8–12 months, they go through a fairly rapid change and become fearful of perceived threats.[109] By around 6–36 months, infants begin to prefer familiar people and show anxiety and distress when separated from them, and when approached by strangers.[110]

Separation anxiety is a typical stage of development to an extent. Kicking, screaming, and throwing temper tantrums are normal symptoms of separation anxiety. The level of intensity of these symptoms can help determine whether or not a child has separation anxiety disorder, which is when a child constantly and intensely refuses to separate from the parent.[111]

The capacity for empathy and the understanding of social rules begin in the preschool period and continue to develop into adulthood. Middle childhood is characterized by friendships with age-mates, and adolescence by emotions connected with sexuality and the beginnings of romantic love. Anger seems most intense during the toddler and early preschool period, and during adolescence.[53]

Speed and pattern edit

Some aspects of social-emotional development, like empathy, develop gradually, but others, like fearfulness, seem to involve a rather sudden reorganization of the child's experiences of emotion.[53] Sexual and romantic emotions develop in connection with physical maturation.[53]

Mechanisms edit

Genetic factors appear to regulate some of the social-emotional developments that occur at predictable ages, such as fearfulness and attachment to familiar people. Experience plays a role in determining which people are familiar, which social rules are obeyed, and how anger is expressed.[53]

Parenting practices have been shown to predict children's emotional intelligence. The amount of time mothers spent with their children and the quality of their interactions are important in terms of children's trait emotional intelligence, not only because those times of joint activity reflect a more positive parenting, but because they are likely to promote modeling, reinforcement, shared attention, and social cooperation.[112]

Population differences edit

Population differences may occur in older children, if, for example, they have learned that it is appropriate for boys to express emotion or behave differently than girls, or if customs learned by children of one ethnic group are different than those learned by another.[113] Social and emotional differences between boys and girls of the same age may also be associated with the differences in the timing of puberty seen between the two sexes.[53]

Development of language and communication edit

Mechanisms edit

Language serves the purpose of communication to express oneself through a systematic and conventional use of sounds, signs, or written symbols.[114] There are four subcomponents a child must know to acquire language competence: phonology, lexicon, morphology and syntax, and pragmatics.[115] These subcomponents combine to form the components of language: sociolinguistics and literacy.[114] Currently, there is no single accepted theory of language acquisition but various explanations of language development have been given.

Components edit

The four components of language development include:

  • Phonology is concerned with the sounds of language.[116] It is the function, behavior, and organization of sounds as linguistic items.[117] Phonology considers what the sounds of language are and what the rules are for combining sounds. Phonological acquisition in children can be measured by frequency and accuracy of production of various vowels and consonants, the acquisition of phonemic contrasts and distinctive features, or by viewing development in regular stages and to characterizing systematic strategies they adopt.[118]
  • Lexicon is similar to vocabulary as they both describe the complex dictionary of words used in speech production and comprehension.[119] The lexicon of a language also includes that language's morphemes. Morphemes act as minimal meaning-bearing elements or building blocks of something in language that makes sense. For example, in the word "cat", the component "cat" makes sense as does "at", but "at" does not mean the same thing as "cat". In this example, "ca" does not mean anything.
  • Morphology is the study of words and how they are formed. Morphology is also the branch of linguistics that deals with words, their internal structure and how they are formed. It is also the mental system involved in word formation.[120]
  • Pragmatics is the study of the relationship between linguistic forms and speakers of the language,[121] it also incorporates how speech is used to serve different functions. Pragmatics can be defined as the ability to communicate one's feelings and desires to others.[122]

Children's development of language also includes semantics which is the attachment of meaning to words. This happens in three stages. First, each word means an entire sentence. For example, a young child may say "mama" but the child may mean "Here is Mama", "Where is Mama?", or "I see Mama." In the second stage, words have meaning but do not have complete definitions. This stage occurs around age two or three. Third, around age seven or eight, words have adult-like definitions and their meanings are more complete.[123]

A child learns the syntax of their language when they are able to join words together into sentences and understand multiple-word sentences said by other people. There appear to be six major stages in which a child's acquisition of syntax develops.[124] First, is the use of sentence-like words in which the child communicates using one word with additional vocal and bodily cues. This stage usually occurs between 12 and 18 months of age. Second, between 18 months to two years, there is the modification stage where children communicate concepts by modifying a topic word. The third stage, between two and three years old, involves the child using complete subject-predicate structures to communicate concepts. Fourth, children make changes on basic sentence structure that enables them to communicate more complex concepts. This stage occurs between the ages of two and a half years to four years. The fifth stage of categorization involves children aged three and a half to seven years refining their sentences with more purposeful word choice that reflects their complex system of categorizing word types. Finally, children use structures of language that involve more complicated syntactic relationships between the ages of five years old to ten years old.[124]

Sequential skills and milestones edit

Sequential skills in learning to talk[125]
Child Age in Months Language Skill
0–3 Vocal play: cry, coo, gurgle, grunt
3+ Babble: undifferentiated sounds
6–10 Babble: canonical/reduplicated syllables
9+ Imitation
8–18 First words
13–15 Expressive jargon, intonational sentences
13–19 10-word vocabulary
14–24 50-word vocabulary
13–27 Single-word stage and a few sentences; 2–3 word combinations; Articles: a/the; Plural: -s
23–24 Irregular past: went, modal and verb: can/will; 28 to 436-word vocabulary; 93–265 utterances per hour
25–27 Regular past: -ed; Auxiliary "be": -'m, -'s
23–26 Third-person singular: -s; 896 to 1,507-word vocabulary; 1,500 to 1,700 words per hour

Infants begin with cooing and soft vowel sounds. Shortly after birth, this system is developed as the infants begin to understand that their noises, or non-verbal communication, lead to a response from their caregiver.[126] This will then progress into babbling around 5 months of age, with infants first babbling consonant and vowel sounds together that may sound like "ma" or "da".[127] At around 8 months of age, babbling increases to include repetition of sounds, such as "ma-ma" and "da-da". Around this age infants also learn the forms for words and which sounds are more likely to follow other sounds.[127] At this stage, much of the child's communication is open to interpretation. For example, if a child says "bah" when they are in a toy room with their guardian, it is likely to be interpreted as "ball" because the toy is in sight. However, if you were to listen to the same 'word' on a recorded tape without knowing the context, one might not be able to figure out what the child was trying to say.[126]

A child's receptive language, the understanding of others' speech, has a gradual development beginning at about 6 months.[128] However, expressive language, the production of words, moves rapidly after its beginning at about a year of age, with a "vocabulary explosion" of rapid word acquisition occurring in the middle of the second year.[128] Grammatical rules and word combinations appear at about age two.[128] Between 20 and 28 months, children move from understanding the difference between high and low, hot and cold and begin to change "no" to "wait a minute", "not now" and "why". Eventually, they are able to add pronouns to words and combine them to form short sentences.[126] Mastery of vocabulary and grammar continue gradually through the preschool and school years, with adolescents having smaller vocabularies than adults and experiencing more difficulty with constructions such as the passive voice.[128]

By age 1, children are able to say 1–2 words, respond to their name, imitate familiar sounds and follow simple instructions.[127] Between 1–2 years old, the child uses 5–20 words, says 2-word sentences, expresses their wishes by saying words like "more" or "up", and understands the word "no".[127] Between 2 and 3 years of age, the child is able to refer to themself as "me", combine nouns and verbs, use short sentences, use some simple plurals, answer "where" questions, and has a vocabulary of about 450 words.[127] By age 4, children are able to use sentences of 4–5 words and have a vocabulary of about 1000 words.[127] Children between the ages of 4 and 5 years old are able to use past tense, have a vocabulary of about 1,500 words, and ask questions like "why?" and "who?".[127] By age 6, the child has a vocabulary of 2,600 words, is able to form sentences of 5–6 words and use a variety of different types of sentences.[127] By the age of 5 or 6 years old, the majority of children have mastered the basics of their native language.[127]

Infants, up to 15 month-olds, are initially unable to understand familiar words in their native language pronounced using an unfamiliar accent.[129] This means that a Canadian-English speaking infant cannot recognize familiar words pronounced with an Australian-English accent. This skill develops close to their second birthday.[129] However, this can be overcome when a highly familiar story is read in the new accent prior to the test, suggesting the essential functions of underlying spoken language is in place earlier than previously thought.[129]

Vocabulary typically grows from about 20 words at 18 months to around 200 words at 21 months. Starting around 18 months the child begins to combine words into two-word sentences, which the adult typically expands to clarify its meaning. By 24–27 months the child is producing three or four-word sentences using a logical, if not strictly correct, syntax.[128] The theory is that children apply a basic set of rules such as adding 's' for plurals or inventing simpler words out of words too complicated to repeat like "choskit" for chocolate biscuit.[128] Following this there is a rapid appearance of grammatical rules and ordering of sentences.[128] There is often an interest in rhyme, and imaginative play frequently includes conversations. Children's recorded monologues give insight into the development of the process of organizing information into meaningful units.[128]

By age three the child begins to use complex sentences, including relative clauses, although they are still perfecting various linguistic systems. By five years of age the child's use of language is very similar to that of an adult. From the age of about three children can indicate fantasy or make-believe linguistics, produce coherent personal stories and fictional narratives with beginnings and endings.[128] It is argued that children devise narrative as a way of understanding their own experience and as a medium for communicating their meaning to others.[128]

The ability to engage in extended discourse emerges over time from regular conversation with adults and peers. For this, a child needs to learn to combine their perspective with that of others and with outside events and learn to use linguistic indicators to show they are doing this. They also learn to adjust their language depending on who they are speaking to.[128] Typically by the age of about 9 a child can recount other narratives in addition to their own experiences, from the perspectives of the author, the characters in the story and their own views.[128]

Theories edit

Although the role of adult speech is important in facilitating the child's learning, there is considerable disagreement among theorists about the extent to which it influences children's early meanings and expressive words. Findings about the initial mapping of new words, the ability to decontextualize words, and refine meaning of words are diverse.[9] One hypothesis, known as the syntactic bootstrapping hypothesis, refers to the child's ability to infer meaning from cues by using grammatical information from the structure of sentences.[130]

Another theory is the multi-route model which argues that context-bound words and referential words follow different routes; the first being mapped onto event representations and the latter onto mental representations. In this model, parental input has a critical role but the children ultimately rely on cognitive processing to establish subsequent use of words.[131] However, naturalistic research on language development has indicated that preschoolers' vocabularies are strongly associated with the number of words said to them by adults.[132]

There is no single accepted theory of language acquisition. Instead, there are current theories that help to explain theories of language, theories of cognition, and theories of development. They include the generativist theory, social interactionist theory, usage-based theory (Tomasello), connectionist theory, and behaviorist theory (Skinner). Generativist theories say that universal grammar is innate and language experience activates that innate knowledge.[133] Social interactionist theories define language as a social phenomenon where children acquire language because they want to communicate with others; this theory is heavily based on social-cognitive abilities that drive the language acquisition process.[133] Usage-based theories define language as a set of formulas that emerge from the child's learning abilities in correlation with their social cognitive interpretation and their understanding of the speakers' intended meanings.[133] Connectionist theory is a pattern-learning procedure that defines language as a system composed of smaller subsystems or patterns of sound or meaning.[133] Behaviorist theories defined language as the establishment of positive reinforcement, but are now regarded as only being of historical interest.[133]

Communication edit

Communication can be defined as the exchange and negotiation of information between two or more individuals through verbal and nonverbal symbols, oral and written (or visual) modes, and the production and comprehension processes of communication.[134] According to First International Congress for the Study of Child Language, "the general hypothesis [is that] access to social interaction is a prerequisite to normal language acquisition".[135] Principles of conversation include two or more people focusing on one topic. All questions in a conversation should be answered, comments should be understood or acknowledged and any directions should, in theory, be followed. In the case of young children these conversations are expected to be basic or redundant. The role of a guardians during developing stages is to convey that conversation is meant to have a purpose, as well as teaching children to recognize the other speaker's emotions.[135]

Communicative language is both verbal and nonverbal, and to achieve communication competence, four components must be mastered. These components are: grammatical competence, including vocabulary knowledge, rules of word sentence formation, etc.; sociolinguistic competence, or the appropriate meanings and grammatical forms in different social contexts; discourse competence, which is having the knowledge required to combine forms and meanings; and strategic competence in the form of knowledge about verbal and nonverbal communication strategies.[134] The attainment of communicative competence is an essential part of actual communication.[136]

Language development is viewed as a motive to communication, and the communicative function of language in-turn provides the motive for language development. Jean Piaget uses the term "acted conversations" to explain a child's style of communication that relies more heavily on gestures and body movements than words.[124] Younger children depend on gestures for a direct statement of their message. As they begin to acquire more language, body movements take on a different role and begin to complement the verbal message.[124] These nonverbal bodily movements allow children to express their emotions before they can express them verbally. The child's nonverbal communication of how they are feeling is seen in babies 0 to 3 months who use wild, jerky movements of the body to show excitement or distress.[124] This develops to more rhythmic movements of the entire body at 3 to 5 months to demonstrate the child's anger or delight.[124] Between 9–12 months of age, children view themselves as joining the communicative world.[114]

Before 9–12 months, babies interact with objects and interact with people, but they do not interact with people about objects. This developmental change is the change from primary intersubjectivity (capacity to share oneself with others) to secondary intersubjectivity (capacity to share one's experience), which changes the infant from an unsociable to socially engaging creature.[114] Around 12 months of age the use of communicative gestures begins, including communicative pointing where an infant points to request something, or to point to provide information.[114] Another communicative gesture presents around the age of 10 and 11 months where infants start gaze-following, by looking where another person is looking.[114] This joint attention results in changes to their social cognitive skills between the ages of 9 and 15 months as their time is increasingly spent with others.[114] Children's use of non-verbal communicative gestures predicts future language development. The use of non-verbal communication in the form of gestures indicate the child's interest in communication development, and the meanings they choose to convey that are soon revealed through the verbalization of language.[114]

Language acquisition and development contribute to the verbal form of communication. Children originate with a linguistic system where the words they learn are the words used for functional meaning.[133] This instigation of speech has been termed pragmatic bootstrapping. According to this theory children view words as a means of social connection, in that words are used to connect the communicative intentions of the speaker to new words.[133] Hence, the competence of verbal communication through language is achieved by gains in syntax or grammar.

Another function of communicating through language is related to pragmatic development.[137] Pragmatic development includes the child's intentions of communication before they knows how to express these intentions, and throughout the first few years of life both language and communicative functions develop.[133]

When children acquire language and learn to use language for communicative functions (pragmatics), children also gain knowledge about how to participate in conversations and how to relay past experiences/events (discourse knowledge), as well as learning how to use language appropriately for their social situation or social group (sociolinguistic knowledge).[133]

Within the first two years of life, a child's language ability progresses and conversational skills, such as the mechanics of verbal interaction, develop. Mechanics of verbal interaction include taking turns, initiating topics, repairing miscommunication, and responding to lengthen or sustain the conversation.[133]

Conversation is asymmetrical when a child interacts with an adult because the adult is the one to create structure in the conversation, and to build upon the child's contributions. In accordance to the child's developing conversational skills, asymmetrical conversation between adult and child advance to an equal temperament of conversation. This shift in balance of conversation suggests a development of narrative discourse in communication.[133] Ordinarily, the development of communicative competence and the development of language are linked to one another.[133]

Causes of delays edit

Individual differences edit

Delays in language skills are the most frequent type of developmental delay. According to demographics 1 out of 6 children have a significant language delay;[138] speech/language delay is three to four times more common in boys than in girls.[139] Some children also display behavioral problems due to their frustration of not being able to express what they want or need.[140]

Simple speech delays are usually temporary. Most cases are solved on their own or with a little extra attention from the family. It is the parent's duty to encourage their baby to talk to them with gestures or sounds and for them to spend a great amount of time playing with, reading to, and communicating with their baby. In certain circumstances, parents will have to seek professional help, such as a speech therapist.[140]

It is important to take into consideration that sometimes delays can be a warning sign of more serious conditions that could include auditory processing disorders, hearing loss, developmental verbal dyspraxia, developmental delay in other areas, or an autism spectrum disorder (ASD).[140]

Environmental causes edit

There are many environmental causes that are linked to language delays, including situations where the child has their full attention on another skill, such as walking. The child may have a twin or a sibling close to their own age and may not be receiving the parent's full attention. Another possibility is that the child is in a daycare with too few adults to administer individual attention. Perhaps the most obvious environmental cause would be a child that suffers from psychosocial deprivation such as poverty, malnutrition, poor housing, neglect, inadequate linguistic stimulation, or emotional stress.[141]

Neurological causes edit

Language delay can be caused by a substantial number of underlying disorders, such as intellectual disability, which accounts for more than 50 percent of language delays. Language delay is usually more severe than other developmental delays in intellectually disabled children, and it is usually the first obvious symptom of intellectual disability. Intellectual disability causes global language delay, including delayed auditory comprehension and delayed use of gestures.[142]

Impaired hearing is another of the most common causes of language delay. A child who can not hear or process speech in a clear and consistent manner will have a language delay, and even the most minimum hearing impairment or auditory processing deficit can considerably affect language development. Generally the more the severe the impairment, the more serious the language delay.[143] Nonetheless, deaf children that are born to families who use sign language develop infant babble and use a fully expressive sign language at the same pace as hearing children.[144]

Developmental Dyslexia is a developmental reading disorder that occurs when the brain does not properly recognize and process the graphic symbols that represent the sounds of speech. Children with dyslexia may encounter problems in rhyming and separating the sounds that compose words, which is essential in learning to read as early reading skills rely heavily on word recognition. When using an alphabet writing system this involves in having the ability to separate out the sounds in words and be able to match them with letter and groups of letters. Difficulty connecting the sounds of language to the letters of words may result difficulty in understanding sentences. Confusion between similar letters, such as "b" and "d" can occur. In general the symptoms of dyslexia are: difficulty in determining the meaning of a simple sentence, learning to recognize written words, and difficulty in rhyming.

Autism and speech delay are usually correlated. Problems with verbal language is the most common sign of autism. Early diagnosis and treatment of autism can significantly help the child improve their speech skills. Autism is recognized as one of the five pervasive developmental disorders, distinguished by problems with language, speech, communication and social skills that present in early childhood. Some common types of language disorders are having limited to no verbal speech, echolalia or repeating words out of context, problems responding to verbal instruction and ignoring people who speak to them directly.[145]

Other aspects of development edit

Gender edit

Gender identity involves how a person perceives themselves as male, female, or a variation of the two. Children can identify themselves as belonging to a certain gender as early as two years old,[146] but how gender identity is developed is a topic of scientific debate. Several factors are involved in determining an individual's gender, including neonatal hormones, postnatal socialization, and genetic influences.[147] Some believe that gender is malleable until late childhood,[147] while others argue that gender is established early and gender-typed socialization patterns either reinforce or soften the individual's notion of gender.[148] Since most people identify as the gender that is typically associated to their genitalia, studying the impact of these factors is difficult.

Evidence suggests that neonatal androgens, male sex hormones produced in the womb during gestation, play an important role. Testosterone in the womb directly codes the brain for either male or female-typical development. This includes both the physical structure of the brain and the characteristics the person expresses because of it. People exposed to high levels of testosterone during gestation typically develop a male gender identity, while those not exposed to testosterone, or who lack the receptors necessary to interact with it, typically develop a female gender identity.[147][149]

An individual's genes are also thought to interact with the hormones during gestation and, in turn, affect gender identity, but the genes responsible for this and their effects have not been precisely documented and evidence is limited.[149] It is unknown whether socialization plays a part in determining gender identity postnatally. It is well documented that children actively seek out information on how to properly interact with others based on their gender,[148] but the extent to which these role models, which can include parents, friends, and TV characters, influence gender identity is less clear and no consensus has been reached.

Race edit

In addition to the course of development, previous literature has looked at how race, ethnicity, and socioeconomic status has affected child development. Some studies seem to speak to the importance of adult supervision of adolescent youth.[150] Literature suggested that African American child development was sometimes differentiated between cultural socialization and racial socialization.[citation needed] Further, a different study found that most immigrant youth choose majors focusing on the fields of science and math.[151]

Risk factors edit

Risk factors in child development include: malnutrition, maternal depression, and maternal substance use; though many more factors have been studied.[152][153][154]

Postnatal depression edit

Although there are a large number of studies regarding the effect of maternal depression and postnatal depression on various areas of infant development, they are yet to come to a consensus regarding the true effects. Numerous studies indicate impaired development, while many others find no effect of depression on development.[citation needed]

A study of 18-month-olds whose mothers had depressive symptoms while the children were 6 weeks and/or 6 months old found that maternal depression had no effect on the child's cognitive development. Furthermore, the study indicates that maternal depression combined with a poor home environment is more likely to have an effect on cognitive development than maternal depression alone. However, the authors conclude that it may be that short term depression has no effect, but long term depression could cause more serious problems.[155]

A longitudinal study spanning 7 years found no effect of maternal depression on cognitive development as a whole, however it found that boys are more susceptible to cognitive developmental issues when their mothers had depression.[153]

This trend is continued in a study of children up to 2 years old, which revealed a significant difference on cognitive development between genders, with girls having a higher score; however girls scored higher regardless of the mother's history of depression. Infants with chronically depressed mothers showed significantly lower scores on the motor and mental scales within the Bayley Scales of Infant Development,[156] contrasting with many older studies.[153][155]

A similar effect has been found at 11 years: male children of depressed mothers score an average of 19.4 points lower on an IQ test than peers with healthy mothers, while this difference is less pronounced in girls.[157] Three month olds with depressed mothers show significantly lower scores on the Griffiths Mental Development Scale, which covers a range of developmental areas including cognitive, motor and social development.[158] Furthermore, interactions between depressed mothers and their children may affect social and cognitive abilities in later life.[159]

Maternal depression has been shown to influence the mothers' interaction with her child.[160] When communicating with their child, depressed mothers fail to make changes to their vocal intonation, and tend to use unstructured vocal behaviours.[161] Furthermore, compared to when interacting with healthy mothers, infants interacting with depressed mothers show signs of stress, such as increased pulse and raised cortisol levels, and make more use of avoidance behaviours, for example looking away.[159] Mother-infant interaction at 2 months has been shown to affect the child's cognitive performance at 5 years.[162]

Studies have begun to show that other forms of psychopathology (mental illness) can independently influence infants' and toddlers' subsequent social-emotional development through effects on regulatory processes within the child-parent attachment.[163] Maternal interpersonal violence-related post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), for example, has been associated with subsequent dysregulation of emotion and aggression by ages 4–7 years.[164]

Maternal drug use edit

Cocaine edit

Research has provided conflicting evidence regarding the severity of effects on children's development posed by maternal substance use during and after pregnancy.[154] Children exposed to cocaine in utero weigh less than those not exposed at ages ranging from 6 to 30 months.[165] Additionally, studies indicate that the head circumference of children exposed to cocaine is lower than those that of children without cocaine exposure.[165][166] However, two more recent studies found no significant differences in either measure between those exposed to cocaine and those who were not.[167][168]

Maternal cocaine use may also affect the child's cognitive development, with exposed children achieving lower scores on measures of psychomotor and mental development.[169][170] Again though, there is conflicting evidence, and a number of studies indicate no effect of maternal cocaine use on a child's cognitive development.[171][172]

Continuing the trend, some studies found maternal cocaine use to impair motor development,[173][174] while others showed no effect of cocaine use on motor development.[165][168]

Other edit

The use of cocaine by pregnant women is not the only drug that can have a negative effect on the fetus. Tobacco, marijuana, and opiates can also affect an unborn child's cognitive and behavioral development.[175]

Smoking tobacco increases pregnancy complications including low birth weight, prematurity, placental abruption, and intrauterine death. After birth it can disturb maternal-infant interactions, reduce IQ, increase the risk of ADHD, and lead to tobacco use in the child.[175]

Prenatal marijuana exposure may have long-term emotional and behavioral consequences, as at ten-years-old children who had been exposed to the drug during pregnancy reported more depressive symptoms than unexposed peers. Some other effects include executive function impairment, reading difficulty, and delayed emotional regulation.[175]

An opiate drug, such as heroin, in utero decreases birth weight, birth length, and head circumference. Parental opiate exposure may impact the infant's central nervous system and autonomic nervous system, though the evidence is even more inconsistent than with parental cocaine exposure. There are also some unexpected negative consequences on a child, such as: less rhythmic swallowing, strabismus, and feelings of rejection.[175]

Malnutrition and Undernutrition edit

Poor nutrition early in life contributes to stunting, and by the age of two or three can be associated with cognitive deficits, poor school achievement, and, later in life, poor social relationships.[176] Malnutrition is a large problem in developing nations, and has an important effect on young children's weight and height. Children suffering malnutrition in Colombia weighed less than those living in upper class conditions at the age of 36 months (11.88 kg [26.2 lb] compared to 14 kg [31 lb]), and were shorter (85.3 cm [33.6 in] versus 94 cm [37 in]).[152]

Malnutrition during the first 1000 days of a child's life can cause irreversible physical and mental stunting.[177] Infections and parasites related to poor sanitation and hygiene can impact absorption of nutrients in the gut.[178] Adequate sanitation and hygiene (rather than just access to food) play a critical role in preventing undernutrition, malnutrition and stunting[179][180][181][178] and ensuring normal early childhood development.[182] Malnutrition has been indicated as a negative influence on childhood intelligence quotient (IQ).[183][184] Although it has also been suggested that this effect is nullified when parental IQ is considered, implying that this difference is genetic.[185]

Specific nutrients edit

The effect of low iron levels on cognitive development and IQ has yet to reach consensus.[186] Some evidence suggests that even well-nourished children with lower levels of iron and folate (although not at such a level to be considered deficient) have a lower IQ than those with higher levels of iron and folate.[187] Furthermore, anaemic children perform worse on cognitive measures than non-anaemic children.[188]

Other nutrients have been strongly implicated in brain development, including iodine and zinc.[189] Iodine is required for the formation of thyroid hormones necessary for brain development.[190] Iodine deficiency may reduce IQ by an average of 13.5 points compared to healthy individual.[191] Zinc deficiency has also been shown to slow childhood growth and development.[192][193] Zinc supplementation appears to be beneficial for growth in infants under six months old.[194]

Socioeconomic status edit

Socioeconomic status is measured primarily based on income, educational attainment and occupation.[195] Investigations into the role of socioeconomic factors on child development repeatedly show that continual poverty is more harmful on IQ,[196] and cognitive abilities[197] than short-term poverty.

Children in families who experience persistent financial hardships and poverty have significantly impaired cognitive abilities compared to those in families who do not face these issues.[197] Poverty can also cause a number of other factors shown to effect child development, such as poor academic success, less family involvement, iron deficiency, infections, a lack of stimulation,[198] and malnutrition. Poverty also increases the risk of lead poisoning due to lead paint found on the walls of some houses;[199] child blood levels of lead increase as income decreases.[200] Income based poverty is associated with a 6–13 point reduction in IQ for those earning half of the poverty threshold compared to those earning twice the poverty threshold,[196] and children coming from households featuring continual or temporary poverty perform lower than children in middle-class families.[197]

Parental educational attainment is the most significant socioeconomic factor in predicting the child's cognitive abilities,[201] as those with a mother with a high IQ are likely to have higher IQs themselves.[185][202] Similarly, maternal occupation is associated with better cognitive achievement. Those whose mothers' job entails problem-solving are more likely to be given stimulating tasks and games, and are likely to achieve more advanced verbal competency.[203]

On the other hand, maternal employment is associated with slightly lower test scores, regardless of socioeconomic status. Counterintuitively, maternal employment results in more disadvantages the higher the higher socioeconomic status, as these children are being removed from a more enriching environment to be put in child care, though the quality of the child care must be considered. Low income children tend to be cared for by grandparents or extended family[204] and therefore form strong bonds with family. High income children tend to be cared for in a child care setting or in home care such as with a nanny. If the mother is highly educated, this can be a disadvantage to the child.[205]

Even with quality of care controlled for, studies still found that full-time work within the first year was correlated with negative effects on child development.[204] Children whose mothers work are also less likely to receive regular well-baby doctor visits and less likely to be breastfed,[206] which has been proven to improve developmental results. Effects are felt more strongly when women resume full-time work within the first year of the child's life.[207][208] These effects may be due in part to pre-existing differences between mothers who return to work and those who do not, such as differences in character or reason for returning to work.[209]

Low-income families are less likely to provide a stimulating home learning environment to their children due to time constraints and financial stress.[210] Compared to two-parent households, children with a single-parent households have greater economic vulnerability and less parental involvement, leading to worse social, behavioral, educational, or cognitive outcomes.[211]

A child's academic achievement is influenced by parents' educational attainment, parenting style, and parental investment in their child's cognitive and educational success. Higher-income families are able to afford learning opportunities both inside and outside the classroom.[212] Poverty-stricken children have fewer opportunities for stimulating recreational activities, often missing out on trips to libraries or museums, and are unable to access a tutor to help with problematic academic areas.[213]

A further factor in a child's educational attainment involves the school environment, more specifically teacher expectations and attitudes.[214] If teachers perceive low-SES children as being less academically able then they may provide them with less attention and reinforcement.[214] On the other hand, when schools make an effort to increase family and school involvement, children perform better on state tests.[215]

Parasites edit

Diarrhea caused by the parasitic disease Giardiasis is associated with lower IQ.[216] Parasitic worms (helminths) are associated with nutritional deficiencies known to be a risk to child development.[217] Intestinal parasitism is one of the most neglected tropical diseases in the developed world, and harboring of this parasite could have several health implications in children that negatively affect childhood development and morbidity. Prolonged exposure to faecally-transmitted infections, including environmental enteropathy, other intestinal infections, and parasites during early childhood can lead to irreversible stunting.[218] Reducing the prevalence of these parasites can be a benefit in child growth, development, and educational outcome.[219]

Toxin exposure edit

High levels of lead in the blood is associated with attention deficits,[220] while arsenic poisoning has a negative effect on both verbal IQ and on total intelligence quotient.[220] Manganese poisoning due to levels in drinking water is also associated with a reduced IQ of 6.2 points between the highest and lowest level of poisoning.[221]

Prenatal exposure to various pesticides including organophosphates,[222] and chlorpyrifos[223] has also been linked to reduced IQ score. Organophosphates have been specifically linked to poorer working memory, verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning and processing speed.[222]

Other edit

Intrauterine growth restriction is associated with learning deficits in childhood, and as such, is related to lower IQ.[224] Cognitive development can also be harmed by childhood exposure to violence and trauma, including spousal abuse between the parents and sexual abuse.[225][226]

Neglect edit

When a child is unable to meet their developmental goals because they have not been provided with the correct amount of care, stimulation or nutrition this situation is commonly referred to as child neglect. It is the most widespread form of child abuse, accounting for 78% of all child abuse cases in the United States in 2010 alone. Scientific studies show that child neglect can have lifelong consequences for children.[227]

Assessing and identifying edit

Assessing and identifying neglect pose a number of challenges for practitioners. Given that neglect is a dynamic between the child's development and levels of nurturance, the question in identifying neglect, becomes one of where do you start, with the child's development or with the levels of nurturance?[citation needed]

Development focused methods edit

Some professionals identify neglect by measuring the developmental levels of a child, as if those levels are normal, one can, by definition, conclude that a child is not being neglected. Measured areas of development can include weight, height, stamina, social and emotional responses, speech and motor development. As all these features go into making a medical assessment of whether a child is thriving, a professional looking to start an assessment of neglect might start with information collected by a doctor.[228]

Infants are often weighed and measured when seen by their pediatrician for well-baby check-ups. The physician initiates a more complete evaluation when the infant's development and functioning are found to be delayed. Then social work staff could consult medical notes to establish if the baby or child is failing to thrive, as a first step in a pathway towards identifying neglect. If developmental levels are below normal, the identification of neglect then requires the professional to establish if this can be put down to the level of nurturance experienced by the child. Developmental delays caused by genetic conditions or disease need to be discounted, as they do not have their basis in a lack of nurturance.[228]

Starting the assessment edit

Besides routine pediatrician visits, another way of starting the process of identifying neglect is to determine if the child is experiencing a level of nurturance lower than that considered necessary to support normal development,[229] which might be unique to the child's age, gender and other factors.[229] Exactly how to ascertain what a particular child needs, without referring back to their level of development, is not something theory and policy on neglect is clear about. Furthermore, determining whether a child is getting the requisite level of nurturance needs to take into account not just the intensity of the nurturance, but also the duration and frequency of the nurturance.[citation needed]

Children may experience varying and low levels of certain types of nurturance across a day and from time to time, however, the levels of nurturance should never cross thresholds of intensity, duration and frequency. For this reason, professionals must keep detailed histories of care provision, which demonstrate the duration of subnormal exposure to care, stimulation, and nutrition.[230]

Common guidance suggests professionals should focus on the levels of nurturance provided by the carers of the child, as neglect is understood as an issue of the parents' behaviour towards the child.[231] Some authors feel that establishing the failure of parents and caregivers to provide care is sufficient to conclude that neglect is occurring.[232][233] One definition is that, "a child experiences neglect when the adults who look after them fail to meet their needs", which clearly defines neglect as a matter of parental performance.

This raises the question about what level of nurturance a carer or parent needs to fall under to provoke developmental delay, and how one goes about measuring that accurately. This definition, which focuses on the stimulation provided by the carer, can be subject to critique. Neglect is about the child's development being adversely affected by the levels of nurturance, but the carers' provision of nurturance is not always a good indicator of the level of nurturance received by the child. Neglect may be occurring at school, outside of parental care. The child may be receiving nurturance from siblings or through a boarding school education, which compensates for the lack of nurturance provided by the parents.[citation needed]

Linking to stimulation edit

Neglect is a process whereby children experience developmental delay owing to experiencing insufficient levels of nurturance. In practice, this means that when starting an assessment of neglect by identifying developmental delay one needs to then check the levels of nurturance received by the child. While some guidance on identifying neglect urges practitioners to measure developmental levels, other guidance focuses on how developmental levels can be attributed to parental behaviour.[234] However, the narrow focus on parental behaviour can be criticised for unnecessarily ruling out the possible effect of institutionalised neglect, e.g. neglect at school.

If one starts by concluding that the levels of nurture received by the child are insufficient, one then needs to consider the developmental levels achieved by the child. Further challenges arise, however, as even when one has established developmental delay and exposure to low levels of nurture, one needs to rule out the possibility that the link between the two is coincidental. The developmental delay may be caused by a genetic disorder, disease or physical, sexual or emotional abuse. The developmental delay may be caused by a mixture of underexposure to nurture, abuse, genetics, and disease.[citation needed]

Measuring tools edit

The Graded Care Profile Tool is a practice tool which gives an objective measure of the quality of care in terms of a parent/carer's commitment. It was developed in the UK.[235]

The North Carolina Family Assessment Scale is a tool that can be used by a practitioner to explore whether neglect is taking place across a range of family functioning areas.[236]

Intervention programs edit

Early intervention programs and treatments include individual counselling, family and group counselling, social support services, behavioural skills training programs to eliminate problematic behaviour and teach parents appropriate parenting behaviour.[237]

Video interaction guidance is a video feedback intervention through which a "guider" helps a client to enhance communication within relationships. The client is guided to analyse and reflect on video clips of their own interactions.[238][239] Video Interaction Guidance has been used where concerns have been expressed over possible parental neglect in cases where the focus child is aged 2–12, and where the child is not the subject of a child protection plan.[240]

The SafeCare programme is a preventive programme working with parents of children under 6 years old who are at risk of significant harm through neglect. The programme is delivered in the home by trained practitioners, and is 18 to 20 sessions focused on 3 key areas: parent-infant/child interaction, home safety and child health.[241]

Triple P (Parenting Program) is a positive parenting program. It is a multilevel parenting and family support strategy. The idea behind it is that if parents are educated on proper parenting and given the appropriate resources, it could help decrease the number of child neglect cases.[242]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Collins WA, ed. (1984). "Introduction". Development During Middle Childhood: The Years From Six to Twelve. National Academies Press (US).
  2. ^ "Child Development: Young Teens (12-14 years old)". U.S. Centers for Diesase Control and Prevention (CDC). 23 September 2021.
  3. ^ "Adolescent health". World Health Organization.
  4. ^ Toga AW, Thompson PM, Sowell ER (March 2006). "Mapping brain maturation". Trends in Neurosciences. 29 (3): 148–159. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2006.01.007. PMC 3113697. PMID 16472876.
  5. ^ Kail RV (2011). Children and Their Development (6th Edition) (Mydevelopmentlab Series). Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-205-03494-9. OCLC 727047867.
  6. ^ (PDF). U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 June 2017.
  7. ^ Brundavani V, Murthy SR, Kurpad AV (May 2006). "Estimation of deep-abdominal-adipose-tissue (DAAT) accumulation from simple anthropometric measurements in Indian men and women". European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 60 (5): 658–666. doi:10.2307/1602366. JSTOR 1602366. PMID 16391572.
  8. ^ Bronfenbrenner U (1979). The ecology of human development: experiments by nature and design. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-22456-8. OCLC 4515541.
  9. ^ a b c Blades M, Smith PK, Cowie H (2011). Understanding Children's Development. Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-1-4051-7601-9. OCLC 620124946.
  10. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n Shaffer DR (2009). Social and personality development (6th ed.). Australia: Wadsworth. ISBN 978-0-495-60038-1.
  11. ^ Huitt W, Hummel J (July 2003). "Piaget's theory of cognitive development" (PDF). Educational Psychology Interactive. 3 (2): 1–5.
  12. ^ "Cognitive Development - stages, meaning, average, Definition, Description, Common problems". Encyclopedia of Children's Health.
  13. ^ Stangor C, Walinga J (2014-10-17). "7.2 Infancy and Childhood: Exploring and Learning". Introduction to Psychology (1st Canadian ed.). BCcampus.
  14. ^ "Deduction & Induction". www.socialresearchmethods.net. Retrieved 2017-01-24.
  15. ^ a b c d Cole M, Vygotskiĭ LS (1978) [Published originally in Russian in 1930]. Mind in society: the development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-57628-5. OCLC 3517053.
  16. ^ Kozulin A, Vygotsky LS (2012). Thought and Language. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-51771-3. OCLC 768728899.
  17. ^ Wertsch JV (1985). Culture, communication, and cognition: Vygotskian perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-33830-1. OCLC 715955821.
  18. ^ Carton AS, Vygotskiĭ LS, Rieber RW (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. 5, Child psychology. New York: Plenum Press. ISBN 978-0-306-45707-4. OCLC 312913751.
  19. ^ Boyd D, Wood SH, Wood EM (2010). Mastering the World of Psychology, Books a la Carte Edition (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, N.J: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-205-00505-5. OCLC 660087952.
  20. ^ Manning ML (September 1988). "Erikson's Psychosocial Theories Help Explain Early Adolescence". NASSP Bulletin. 72 (509): 95–100. doi:10.1177/019263658807250914. S2CID 144082864.
  21. ^ Watson JB (1926). "What the nursery has to say about instincts.". In Murchison E (ed.). Psychologies of 1925. Clark University, Worcester, MA, US: Clark University Press. pp. 1–35. doi:10.1037/11020-001.
  22. ^ Watson JB, Rayner R (February 1920). "Conditioned emotional reactions". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 3 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1037/h0069608. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-9171-B.
  23. ^ a b Hergenhahn BR, Henley T (2013-03-01). An Introduction to the History of Psychology. Cengage Learning. p. 398. ISBN 978-1-133-95809-3.
  24. ^ Skinner BF (August 1963). "Operant behavior". American Psychologist. 18 (8): 503–515. doi:10.1037/h0045185. ISSN 1935-990X. S2CID 51796099.
  25. ^ Frager R, Fadiman J (2005). Personality and personal growth (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, N.J.: Pearson Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-144451-5. OCLC 57168823.
  26. ^ Felluga D (2012-12-11). "Modules on Freud: On Psychosexual Development". Introductory Guide to Critical Theory. Purdue University. Archived from the original on 2012-12-11. Retrieved 2020-12-08.
  27. ^ "Freud and the Psychodynamic Perspective | Introduction to Psychology". courses.lumenlearning.com. Retrieved 2020-12-08.
  28. ^ Felluga D. "Definition: Fixation". Introduction to Psychoanalysis. Purdue University. Retrieved 2020-12-08.
  29. ^ Tyson P, Tyson RL (1990). Psychoanalytic theories of development : an integration. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ISBN 978-0-300-04578-9. OCLC 21197269.
  30. ^ Lemma A (2002). "Psychodynamic Therapy: The Freudian Approach". In Dryden W (ed.). Handbook of individual therapy. Thousand Oaks, Calif: Sage. ISBN 978-0-7619-6943-3. OCLC 50279223.
  31. ^ Aslin RN (1993). "Commentary : the strange attractiveness of dynamic systems to development". In Thelen E, Smith LC (eds.). A Dynamic systems approach to development applications. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-585-03867-4. OCLC 42854628.
  32. ^ Gleick J (1987). Chaos: Making a New Science. Penguin Books.
  33. ^ Sameroff A (1983). "Factors in predicting successful parenting.". In Hoekelman RA, Sasserath V (eds.). Minimizing high-risk parenting: a review of what is known and consideration of appropriate preventive intervention. Radnor, Pa: Johnson & Johnson Baby Products Company pediatric round table series. ISBN 978-0-931562-07-5. OCLC 8689673.
  34. ^ Thelen E (2005-04-15). "Dynamic Systems Theory and the Complexity of Change". Psychoanalytic Dialogues. 15 (2): 255–283. doi:10.1080/10481881509348831. ISSN 1048-1885. S2CID 1327081.
  35. ^ a b Berk LE (2009). Child development. Boston: Pearson Education/Allyn & Bacon. ISBN 978-0-205-61559-9. OCLC 232605723.
  36. ^ Treisman A (September 1999). "Solutions to the binding problem: progress through controversy and convergence". Neuron. 24 (1): 105–10, 111–25. doi:10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80826-0. PMID 10677031.
  37. ^ a b Val Danilov, Igor (2023-02-17). "Theoretical Grounds of Shared Intentionality for Neuroscience in Developing Bioengineering Systems". OBM Neurobiology. 7 (1): 156. doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2301156.
  38. ^ Val Danilov, Igor. (2022). Smartphone in Detecting Developmental Disability in Infancy: A Theoretical Approach to Shared Intentionality for Assessment Tool of Cognitive Decline and e-Learning. K. Arai (Ed.): Proceedings of the SAI 2022, LNNS 508, pp. 1–11, 2022. Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-031-10467-1_19
  39. ^ a b c d e Val Danilov I (2023). "Low-Frequency Oscillations for Nonlocal Neuronal Coupling in Shared Intentionality Before and After Birth: Toward the Origin of Perception". OBM Neurobiology. 7 (4): 1–17. doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2304192.
  40. ^ Val Danilov I, Mihailova S (March 2022). "A New Perspective on Assessing Cognition in Children through Estimating Shared Intentionality". Journal of Intelligence. 10 (2): 21. doi:10.3390/jintelligence10020021. PMC 9036231. PMID 35466234.
  41. ^ a b Tomasello M (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA: Harvard University Press.
  42. ^ Val Danilov I, Mihailova S (April 2023). "Empirical Evidence of Shared Intentionality: Towards Bioengineering Systems Development". OBM Neurobiology. 7 (2): 1–20. doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2302167.
  43. ^ McClung JS, Placì S, Bangerter A, Clément F, Bshary R (September 2017). "The language of cooperation: shared intentionality drives variation in helping as a function of group membership". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 284 (1863): 20171682. doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.1682. PMC 5627217. PMID 28931743.
  44. ^ Shteynberg G, Galinsky AD (November 2011). "Implicit coordination: Sharing goals with similar others intensifies goal pursuit". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 47 (6): 1291–1294. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.04.012..
  45. ^ Val Danilov I (2023). "Shared Intentionality Modulation at the Cell Level: Low-Frequency Oscillations for Temporal Coordination in Bioengineering Systems". OBM Neurobiology. 7 (4): 1–17. doi:10.21926/obm.neurobiol.2304185.
  46. ^ Liu J, Zhang R, Xie E, Lin Y, Chen D, Liu Y, et al. (August 2023). "Shared intentionality modulates interpersonal neural synchronization at the establishment of communication system". Communications Biology. 6 (1): 832. doi:10.1038/s42003-023-05197-z. PMC 10415255. PMID 37563301.
  47. ^ Painter DR, Kim JJ, Renton AI, Mattingley JB (June 2021). "Joint control of visually guided actions involves concordant increases in behavioural and neural coupling". Communications Biology. 4 (1): 816. doi:10.1038/s42003-021-02319-3. PMC 8242020. PMID 34188170.
  48. ^ Hu Y, Pan Y, Shi X, Cai Q, Li X, Cheng X (March 2018). "Inter-brain synchrony and cooperation context in interactive decision making". Biological Psychology. 133: 54–62. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.12.005. PMID 29292232. S2CID 46859640.
  49. ^ Fishburn FA, Murty VP, Hlutkowsky CO, MacGillivray CE, Bemis LM, Murphy ME, et al. (September 2018). "Putting our heads together: interpersonal neural synchronization as a biological mechanism for shared intentionality". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 13 (8): 841–849. doi:10.1093/scan/nsy060. PMC 6123517. PMID 30060130.
  50. ^ Szymanski C, Pesquita A, Brennan AA, Perdikis D, Enns JT, Brick TR, et al. (May 2017). "Teams on the same wavelength perform better: Inter-brain phase synchronization constitutes a neural substrate for social facilitation". NeuroImage. 152: 425–436. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.013. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002D-059A-1. PMID 28284802. S2CID 3807834.
  51. ^ Astolfi L, Toppi J, De Vico Fallani F, Vecchiato G, Salinari S, Mattia D, et al. (September 2010). "Neuroelectrical hyperscanning measures simultaneous brain activity in humans". Brain Topography. 23 (3): 243–256. doi:10.1007/s10548-010-0147-9. PMID 20480221. S2CID 3488268.
  52. ^ "David E. Rumelhart Prize". CognitiveScienceSociety.org. Cognitive Science Society. 2022. "2022 Recipient – Michael Tomasello" section. Retrieved 12 January 2024.
  53. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Patterson C (2008). Child development. Boston: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. ISBN 978-0-07-234795-1. OCLC 179102376.
  54. ^ Gross J (1987). Introducing Erik Erikson. Lanham, MD: University Press of America. ISBN 978-0-8191-5788-1.
  55. ^ Erikson EH (1968). Identity: youth, and crisis. New York: W.W. Norton. ISBN 978-0-393-31144-0. OCLC 750885788.
  56. ^ a b c Mercer J (1998). Infant Development: A Multidisciplinary Introduction. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. ISBN 978-0-534-33977-7.
  57. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Feldman RS (2011). Development across the life span (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-205-80591-4.
  58. ^ Plomin R, Asbury K (2005). "Nature and Nurture: Genetic and Environmental Influences on Behavior". Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 600: 86–98. doi:10.1177/0002716205277184. JSTOR 25046112. S2CID 20315503.
  59. ^ Tau GZ, Peterson BS (January 2010). "Normal development of brain circuits". Neuropsychopharmacology. 35 (1): 147–168. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.115. PMC 3055433. PMID 19794405.
  60. ^ Buchwald J (1987). "A comparison of plasticity in sensory and cognitive processing systems". In Gunzenhauser N (ed.). Infant Stimulation. Skillman NJ: Johnson & Johnson. ISBN 978-0-931562-15-0.
  61. ^ Greenough WT, Black JE, Wallace CS (June 1987). (PDF). Child Development. 58 (3): 539–559. doi:10.2307/1130197. JSTOR 1130197. PMID 3038480. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-08-10. Retrieved 2012-08-04.
  62. ^ Greenough W, Black J, Wallace C (1993). "Experience and brain development". In Johnson M (ed.). Brain Development and Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 319–322. ISBN 978-0-631-18222-1. OCLC 25874371.
  63. ^ Berk L (2005). Infants, Children, and Adolescents. Boston: Allyn & Bacons. ISBN 978-0205511389.
  64. ^ Rubenstein LD, Schelling N, Wilczynski SM, Hooks EN (October 2015). "Lived experiences of parents of gifted students with autism spectrum disorder: The struggle to find appropriate educational experiences". Gifted Child Quarterly. 59 (4): 283–298. doi:10.1177/0016986215592193. S2CID 146380252.
  65. ^ "Newborn Reflexes". HealthyChildren.org. Retrieved 2017-01-24.
  66. ^ Goodwin CJ (2009). Research in psychology: Methods and design. Wiley.
  67. ^ a b c Siegler RS, Gershoff ET, Saffran J, Eisenberg N, Leaper C (2020). How children develop (Sixth ed.). New York, New York: Worth Publishers, Macmillan Learning. ISBN 978-1-319-18456-8. OCLC 1137233012.
  68. ^ Byers-Heinlein K (2014). "High-Amplitude Sucking Procedure". Encyclopedia of Language Development. Thousand Oaks: SAGE Publications, Inc. pp. 263–264. doi:10.4135/9781483346441. ISBN 978-1-4522-5876-8. Retrieved 2022-05-08.
  69. ^ Butler SC, O'Sullivan LP, Shah BL, Berthier NE (November 2014). "Preference for infant-directed speech in preterm infants". Infant Behavior & Development. 37 (4): 505–511. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2014.06.007. PMID 25009957.
  70. ^ Matsuda YT, Okamoto Y, Ida M, Okanoya K, Myowa-Yamakoshi M (October 2012). "Infants prefer the faces of strangers or mothers to morphed faces: an uncanny valley between social novelty and familiarity". Biology Letters. 8 (5): 725–728. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2012.0346. PMC 3440980. PMID 22696289.
  71. ^ Bergelson E, Swingley D (February 2012). "At 6-9 months, human infants know the meanings of many common nouns". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 109 (9): 3253–3258. Bibcode:2012PNAS..109.3253B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113380109. JSTOR 41506937. PMC 3295309. PMID 22331874.
  72. ^ a b Daum MM (2017). Hopkins B, Geangu E, Linkenauger S (eds.). Cognitive development during infancy. Cambridge University Press. pp. 277–287.
  73. ^ Skelton AE, Franklin A (February 2020). "Infants look longer at colours that adults like when colours are highly saturated". Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 27 (1): 78–85. doi:10.3758/s13423-019-01688-5. PMC 7000485. PMID 31848908. S2CID 209407243.
  74. ^ Flom R, Pick AD (December 2012). "Dynamics of infant habituation: infants' discrimination of musical excerpts". Infant Behavior & Development. 35 (4): 697–704. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.022. PMID 22982268.
  75. ^ Thomason ME (2017). Hopkins B, Geangu E, Linkenauger S (eds.). Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press. pp. 121–128.
  76. ^ Giedd JN, Raznahan A, Alexander-Bloch A, Schmitt E, Gogtay N, Rapoport JL (January 2015). "Child psychiatry branch of the National Institute of Mental Health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development". Neuropsychopharmacology. 40 (1): 43–49. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.236. PMC 4262916. PMID 25195638.
  77. ^ Britton JW, Frey LC, Hopp JL, Korb P, Koubeissi MZ, Lievens WE, et al. (2016). St Louis EK, Frey LC (eds.). The Developmental EEG: Premature, Neonatal, Infant, and Children. American Epilepsy Society.
  78. ^ a b Aldridge J (2015). Participatory research: Working with vulnerable groups in research and practice. Bristol University Press. pp. 31–64.
  79. ^ Barbosa B, Brito PQ (2018). "Ethical Challenges in Researching with Children: An Application Adopting a Mixed Method Approach". Computer Supported Qualitative Research. Second International Symposium on Qualitative Research (ISQR 2017). Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing. Vol. 621. pp. 91–100. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-61121-1_8. ISBN 978-3-319-61120-4.
  80. ^ "Child Development". Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). February 15, 2017. Retrieved February 14, 2018.
  81. ^ "Speech and Language Developmental Milestones". NIDCD. 2015-08-18. Retrieved 2020-11-14.
  82. ^ Khan I, Leventhal BL (2023). "Developmental Delay". StatPearls. StatPearls Publishing. PMID 32965902. Retrieved 25 September 2023.
  83. ^ a b c d Tanner JM (1990). Fetus into Man. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0674306929.
  84. ^ CPNP, Kristine M. Ruggiero, PhD, MSN, RN; PA-C, Michael Ruggiero, MHS (2020-09-14). Fast Facts Handbook for Pediatric Primary Care: A Guide for Nurse Practitioners and Physician Assistants. Springer Publishing Company. ISBN 978-0-8261-5184-1.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  85. ^ Graber EG. "Physical Growth of Infants and Children - Children's Health Issues". Merck Manuals Consumer Version. Retrieved 2020-11-14.
  86. ^ "Puberty Definition, Stages, Duration, Signs for Boys & Girls". MedicineNet. Retrieved 2020-11-14.
  87. ^ . U.S. Food and Drug Administration. 2003-07-25. Archived from the original on February 14, 2005. Retrieved 2009-01-13.
  88. ^ Palumbo EJ. "Failure to Thrive: Miscellaneous Disorders in Infants and Children". Merck Manual Professional. Merck Sharp & Dohme Corp. Archived from the original on 2012-11-29. Retrieved 2010-03-23.
  89. ^ "Stunting in a nutshell". www.who.int. Retrieved 28 September 2023.
  90. ^ Willingham DB (1999). "The Neural Basis of Motor-Skill Learning". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 8 (6): 178–182. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00042. S2CID 143942008.
  91. ^ Lang D (August 1, 2022). Individual and Family Development, Health, and Well-being. Iowa State University Digital Press. doi:10.31274/isudp.2022.122.
  92. ^ a b Bayley N (2005). Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. Harcourt Assessment.
  93. ^ a b c d Piek JP, Gasson N, Barrett N, Case I (December 2002). "Limb and gender differences in the development of coordination in early infancy". Human Movement Science. 21 (5–6): 621–39. doi:10.1016/s0167-9457(02)00172-0. PMID 12620715.
  94. ^ Bell S (14 June 2011). . livestrong.com. Archived from the original on 28 June 2018.
  95. ^ Soska KC, Adolph KE, Johnson SP (January 2010). "Systems in development: motor skill acquisition facilitates three-dimensional object completion". Developmental Psychology. 46 (1): 129–138. doi:10.1037/a0014618. PMC 2805173. PMID 20053012.
  96. ^ Rudisill ME, Johnson JL (2018-07-24). "Mastery Motivational Climates in Early Childhood Physical Education: What Have We Learned over the Years?". Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance. 89 (6): 26–32. doi:10.1080/07303084.2018.1476940. ISSN 0730-3084. S2CID 150213725.
  97. ^ Adolph KE, Vereijken B, Denny MA (October 1998). "Learning to crawl". Child Development. 69 (5): 1299–312. doi:10.2307/1132267. JSTOR 1132267. PMID 9839417.
  98. ^ . futureofchildren.org. Princeton University. Archived from the original on 2016-06-04. Retrieved 2016-05-05.
  99. ^ Scharoun SM, Bryden PJ (2014-02-18). "Hand preference, performance abilities, and hand selection in children". Frontiers in Psychology. 5: 82. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00082. PMC 3927078. PMID 24600414.
  100. ^ "Cognitive Development Domain - Child Development (CA Dept of Education)". www.cde.ca.gov. Retrieved 2024-01-09.
  101. ^ Brotherson SE (April 2005). (PDF). Fargo, ND: NDSU Extension Service. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 November 2014.
  102. ^ Shin JC (June 2011). "The development of temporal coordination in children". Brain and Cognition. 76 (1): 106–114. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2011.02.011. PMID 21463915. S2CID 20708124.
  103. ^ "default - Stanford Medicine Children's Health". www.stanfordchildrens.org. Retrieved 2023-04-13.
  104. ^ a b "Developmental Delay in Children". Cleveland Clinic. Retrieved 2023-04-13.
  105. ^ Buczyłowska D, Ronniger P, Melzer J, Petermann F (May 2019). "Sex Similarities and Differences in Intelligence in Children Aged Two to Eight: Analysis of SON-R 2-8 Scores". Journal of Intelligence. 7 (2): 11. doi:10.3390/jintelligence7020011. PMC 6630280. PMID 31162390.
  106. ^ Gu J, Bourne PE (2009). Structural Bioinformatics (2nd ed.). Hoboken (N.J.): Wiley-Blackwell. ISBN 978-0-470-18105-8.
  107. ^ "Infant Emotion: Lifespan Development". Open Education Resource. Lumen Learning. Retrieved 3 October 2023.
  108. ^ Mendes DM, Seidl-de-Moura ML (November 2014). "Different kinds of infants' smiles in the first six months and contingency to maternal affective behavior". The Spanish Journal of Psychology. 17: E80. doi:10.1017/sjp.2014.86. PMID 26055068. S2CID 39470102.
  109. ^ "Emotional and Social Development: 8 to 12 Months". HealthyChildren.org. Retrieved 28 September 2023.
  110. ^ Brooker RJ, Buss KA, Lemery-Chalfant K, Aksan N, Davidson RJ, Goldsmith HH (November 2013). "The development of stranger fear in infancy and toddlerhood: normative development, individual differences, antecedents, and outcomes". Developmental Science. 16 (6): 864–878. doi:10.1111/desc.12058. PMC 4129944. PMID 24118713.
  111. ^ Robinson L (2012). . helpguide.org. Archived from the original on 6 November 2012.
  112. ^ Alegre A (2012). "The Relation Between the Time Mothers and Children Spent Together and the Children's Trait Emotional Intelligence". Child & Youth Care Forum. 41 (5): 493–508. doi:10.1007/s10566-012-9180-z. S2CID 144031333.
  113. ^ Vilhjalmsson R, Kristjansdottir G (January 2003). "Gender differences in physical activity in older children and adolescents: the central role of organized sport". Social Science & Medicine. 56 (2): 363–374. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00042-4. PMID 12473321. S2CID 30233756.
  114. ^ a b c d e f g h Hoff, E. (2014). Language Development. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning
  115. ^ . The Learning House, Inc. Archived from the original on 13 December 2012. Retrieved 5 March 2013.
  116. ^ de Villiers JG, de Villiers PA (1979). Language acquisition (3rd ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard Univ. Press. pp. 7–16. ISBN 978-0-674-50931-3.
  117. ^ Lass R (1984). Phonology : an introduction to basic concepts. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. p. 1. ISBN 978-0-521-28183-6.
  118. ^ de Villiers JG, de Villiers PA (1979). Language acquisition (3rd ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard Univ. Press. pp. 42–43. ISBN 978-0-674-50931-3.
  119. ^ Plag I, Braun M, Lappe S, Schramm M (2007). Introduction to English linguistics (1st ed.). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. p. 150. ISBN 978-3-11-018969-8.
  120. ^ Aronoff M, Fudeman K (2011). What is Morphology (2nd ed.). John Wiley & Sons.
  121. ^ Peterwagner R (2005). What is the matter with communicative competence? : an analysis to encourage teachers of English to assess the very basis of their teaching. Münster: Lit-Verl. p. 24. ISBN 978-3825884871.
  122. ^ de Villiers JG, de Villiers PA (1979). Language acquisition (3rd ed.). Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard Univ. Press. p. 229. ISBN 978-0-674-50931-3.
  123. ^ Hopper R, Naremore RC (1973). Children's Speech. New York, NY: Harper & Row.
  124. ^ a b c d e f Wood BS (1976). Children and communication: verbal and nonverbal language development. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
  125. ^ Hart B, Risley TR (1999). Developmental Change. The Social World of Children Learning to Talk. Baltimore, Md.: P.H. Brookes Pub. p. 271.
  126. ^ a b c Hart B (1999). The Social World of Children Learning to Talk. Baltimore, Md.: P.H. Brookes Pub. pp. 53–72. ISBN 978-1-55766-420-4.
  127. ^ a b c d e f g h i Woolfolk A, Perry NE (2012). Child and adolescent development. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. ISBN 978-1-292-04106-3.
  128. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Pal BK (2011). Education Psychology. Delhi: Prerna Parkashan.
  129. ^ a b c van Heugten M, Johnson EK (February 2014). "Learning to contend with accents in infancy: benefits of brief speaker exposure". Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 143 (1): 340–350. doi:10.1037/a0032192. PMID 23506084.
  130. ^ Gleitman LR (1990). "The structural sources of verb meaning". Language Acquisition. 1 (1): 3–55. doi:10.1207/s15327817la0101_2. S2CID 144713838.
  131. ^ Barrett M, Harris M, Chasin J (February 1991). "Early lexical development and maternal speech: a comparison of children's initial and subsequent uses of words". Journal of Child Language. 18 (1): 21–40. doi:10.1017/S0305000900013271. PMID 2010501. S2CID 35724493.
  132. ^ Hart B, Risley TR (1995). Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children. P.H. Brookes. ISBN 978-1-55766-197-5.[page needed]
  133. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Hoff E (2009). Language development. Wadsworth Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-0-495-50820-5.[page needed]
  134. ^ a b Richards JC, Schmidt WR, eds. (1986). Language and Communication. USA: Longman Group Limited.
  135. ^ a b Dale PS, Ingram D, Snow CE (1981). "Social Interaction and Language Acquisition.". Child language, an international perspective: selected papers from the First International Congress for the Study of Child Language. Baltimore, Md.: University Park Press. pp. 195–214. ISBN 978-0-8391-1608-0.
  136. ^ Burgoon KJ, Le Poire AB (1993). "Effects of Communication Expectancies, Actual Communication, and Expectancy Disconfirmation on Evaluations of Communicators and Their Communication Behavior". Human Communication Research. 20: 67–96. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1993.tb00316.x.
  137. ^ Ninio A, Snow EC (1996). Essays in Developmental Science. Boulder, CO, US: Westview Press.
  138. ^ Sansavini A, Favilla ME, Guasti MT, Marini A, Millepiedi S, Di Martino MV, et al. (May 2021). "Developmental Language Disorder: Early Predictors, Age for the Diagnosis, and Diagnostic Tools. A Scoping Review". Brain Sciences. 11 (5): 654. doi:10.3390/brainsci11050654. PMC 8156743. PMID 34067874.
  139. ^ Leung AK, Kao CP (June 1999). "Evaluation and management of the child with speech delay". American Family Physician. 59 (11): 3121–8, 3135. PMID 10392594.
  140. ^ a b c McLaughlin MR (May 2011). "Speech and language delay in children". American Family Physician. 83 (10): 1183–1188. PMID 21568252.
  141. ^ Sunderajan T, Kanhere SV (May 2019). "Speech and language delay in children: Prevalence and risk factors". Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care. 8 (5): 1642–1646. doi:10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_162_19. PMC 6559061. PMID 31198730.
  142. ^ Marrus N, Hall L (July 2017). "Intellectual Disability and Language Disorder". Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 26 (3): 539–554. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2017.03.001. PMC 5801738. PMID 28577608.
  143. ^ Lang-Roth R (2014-12-01). "Hearing impairment and language delay in infants: Diagnostics and genetics". GMS Current Topics in Otorhinolaryngology, Head and Neck Surgery. 13: Doc05. doi:10.3205/cto000108. PMC 4273166. PMID 25587365.
  144. ^ Caselli N, Pyers J, Lieberman AM (May 2021). "Deaf Children of Hearing Parents Have Age-Level Vocabulary Growth When Exposed to American Sign Language by 6 Months of Age". The Journal of Pediatrics. 232: 229–236. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2021.01.029. PMC 8085057. PMID 33482219.
  145. ^ Vogindroukas I, Stankova M, Chelas EN, Proedrou A (2022-10-14). "Language and Speech Characteristics in Autism". Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment. 18: 2367–2377. doi:10.2147/NDT.S331987. PMC 9578461. PMID 36268264.
  146. ^ Halim ML (2016-03-29). "Princesses and Superheroes: Social-Cognitive Influences on Early Gender Rigidity". Child Development Perspectives. 10 (3): 155–160. doi:10.1111/cdep.12176. ISSN 1750-8592.
  147. ^ a b c Steensma TD, Kreukels BP, de Vries AL, Cohen-Kettenis PT (July 2013). "Gender identity development in adolescence". Hormones and Behavior. 64 (2): 288–297. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.02.020. PMID 23998673. S2CID 23056757.
  148. ^ a b Maccoby EE (2000-12-02). "Perspectives on gender development". International Journal of Behavioral Development. 24 (4): 398–406. doi:10.1080/016502500750037946. ISSN 0165-0254. S2CID 143918530.
  149. ^ a b Roselli CE (July 2018). "Neurobiology of gender identity and sexual orientation". Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 30 (7): e12562. doi:10.1111/jne.12562. PMC 6677266. PMID 29211317.
  150. ^ Quintana SM, Aboud FE, Chao RK, Contreras-Grau J, Cross WE, Hudley C, et al. (2006). . Child Development. 77 (5): 1129–1141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00951.x. PMID 16999787. Archived from the original on 2022-03-08. Retrieved 2020-10-18.
  151. ^ Rangel MA, Shi Y (January 2019). "Early patterns of skill acquisition and immigrants' specialization in STEM careers". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 116 (2): 484–489. Bibcode:2019PNAS..116..484R. doi:10.1073/pnas.1812041116. PMC 6329961. PMID 30598440.
  152. ^ a b Mora JO, Herrera MG, Suescun J, de Navarro L, Wagner M (September 1981). "The effects of nutritional supplementation on physical growth of children at risk of malnutrition". The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 34 (9): 1885–1892. doi:10.1093/ajcn/34.9.1885. PMID 7282613.
  153. ^ a b c Kurstjens S, Wolke D (July 2001). "Effects of maternal depression on cognitive development of children over the first 7 years of life". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 42 (5): 623–636. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00758. PMID 11464967.
  154. ^ a b Frank DA, Augustyn M, Knight WG, Pell T, Zuckerman B (March 2001). "Growth, development, and behavior in early childhood following prenatal cocaine exposure: a systematic review". JAMA. 285 (12): 1613–1625. doi:10.1001/jama.285.12.1613. PMC 2504866. PMID 11268270.
  155. ^ a b Piteo AM, Yelland LN, Makrides M (August 2012). "Does maternal depression predict developmental outcome in 18 month old infants?". Early Human Development. 88 (8): 651–655. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2012.01.013. PMID 22361258.
  156. ^ Cornish AM, McMahon CA, Ungerer JA, Barnett B, Kowalenko N, Tennant C (2005). "Postnatal depression and infant cognitive and motor development in the second postnatal year: The impact of depression chronicity and infant gender". Infant Behavior and Development. 28 (4): 407–417. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.03.004.
  157. ^ Hay DF, Pawlby S, Sharp D, Asten P, Mills A, Kumar R (October 2001). "Intellectual problems shown by 11-year-old children whose mothers had postnatal depression". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 42 (7): 871–889. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00784. PMID 11693583.
  158. ^ Galler JR, Harrison RH, Ramsey F, Forde V, Butler SC (September 2000). "Maternal depressive symptoms affect infant cognitive development in Barbados". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 41 (6): 747–757. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00662. PMID 11039687.
  159. ^ a b Field T, Healy B, Goldstein S, Guthertz M (1990). "Behaviour-state matching and synchrony in mother-infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads". Developmental Psychology. 26 (1): 7–14. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.1.7.
  160. ^ Reck C, Hunt A, Fuchs T, Weiss R, Noon A, Moehler E, et al. (2004). "Interactive regulation of affect in postpartum depressed mothers and their infants: an overview". Psychopathology. 37 (6): 272–280. doi:10.1159/000081983. PMID 15539778. S2CID 21040790.
  161. ^ Bettes BA (August 1988). "Maternal depression and motherese: temporal and intonational features". Child Development. 59 (4): 1089–1096. doi:10.2307/1130275. JSTOR 1130275. PMID 3168616.
  162. ^ Murray L, Kempton C, Woolgar M, Hooper R (October 1993). "Depressed mothers' speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 34 (7): 1083–1101. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01775.x. PMID 8245134.
  163. ^ Schechter DS, Willheim E (July 2009). "Disturbances of attachment and parental psychopathology in early childhood". Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 18 (3): 665–686. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2009.03.001. PMC 2690512. PMID 19486844.
  164. ^ Schechter DS, Zygmunt A, Coates SW, Davies M, Trabka K, McCaw J, et al. (September 2007). "Caregiver traumatization adversely impacts young children's mental representations on the MacArthur Story Stem Battery". Attachment & Human Development. 9 (3): 187–205. doi:10.1080/14616730701453762. PMC 2078523. PMID 18007959.
  165. ^ a b c Hurt H, Brodsky NL, Betancourt L, Braitman LE, Malmud E, Giannetta J (February 1995). "Cocaine-exposed children: follow-up through 30 months". Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 16 (1): 29–35. doi:10.1097/00004703-199502000-00005. PMID 7730454. S2CID 241091.
  166. ^ Azuma SD, Chasnoff IJ (September 1993). "Outcome of children prenatally exposed to cocaine and other drugs: a path analysis of three-year data". Pediatrics. 92 (3): 396–402. PMID 7689727.
  167. ^ Richardson GA, Conroy ML, Day NL (1996). "Prenatal cocaine exposure: effects on the development of school-age children". Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 18 (6): 627–634. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(96)00121-3. PMID 8947939.
  168. ^ a b Kilbride H, Castor C, Hoffman E, Fuger KL (February 2000). "Thirty-six-month outcome of prenatal cocaine exposure for term or near-term infants: impact of early case management". Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 21 (1): 19–26. doi:10.1097/00004703-200002000-00004. PMID 10706345. S2CID 28960955.
  169. ^ Singer LT, Yamashita TS, Hawkins S, Cairns D, Baley J, Kliegman R (May 1994). "Increased incidence of intraventricular hemorrhage and developmental delay in cocaine-exposed, very low birth weight infants". The Journal of Pediatrics. 124 (5 Pt 1): 765–771. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(05)81372-1. PMC 4181569. PMID 7513757.
  170. ^ Chasnoff IJ, Griffith DR, Freier C, Murray J (February 1992). "Cocaine/polydrug use in pregnancy: two-year follow-up". Pediatrics. 89 (2): 284–289. doi:10.1542/peds.89.2.284. PMID 1370867. S2CID 245214695.
  171. ^ Coles CD, Bard KA, Platzman KA, Lynch ME (1999). "Attentional response at eight weeks in prenatally drug-exposed and preterm infants". Neurotoxicology and Teratology. 21 (5): 527–537. doi:10.1016/S0892-0362(99)00023-9. PMID 10492387.
  172. ^ Graham K, Feigenbaum A, Pastuszak A, Nulman I, Weksberg R, Einarson T, et al. (August 1992). "Pregnancy outcome and infant development following gestational cocaine use by social cocaine users in Toronto, Canada". Clinical and Investigative Medicine. 15 (4): 384–394. OCLC 121200190. PMID 1516296. INIST 5556914.
  173. ^ Fetters L, Tronick EZ (November 1996). "Neuromotor development of cocaine-exposed and control infants from birth through 15 months: poor and poorer performance". Pediatrics. 98 (5): 938–943. doi:10.1542/peds.98.5.938. PMID 8909489. S2CID 34639176.
  174. ^ Swanson MW, Streissguth AP, Sampson PD, Olson HC (October 1999). "Prenatal cocaine and neuromotor outcome at four months: effect of duration of exposure". Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. 20 (5): 325–334. doi:10.1097/00004703-199910000-00007. PMID 10533991.
  175. ^ a b c d Minnes S, Lang A, Singer L (July 2011). "Prenatal tobacco, marijuana, stimulant, and opiate exposure: outcomes and practice implications". Addiction Science & Clinical Practice. 6 (1): 57–70. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.290.287. PMC 3188826. PMID 22003423.
  176. ^ Chilton M, Chyatte M, Breaux J (October 2007). "The negative effects of poverty & food insecurity on child development". The Indian Journal of Medical Research. 126 (4): 262–272. PMID 18032801.
  177. ^ Dominguez EI (2017). BabyWASH and the 1,000 Days: A Practical Package for Stunting Reduction. London: Action Against Hunger.
  178. ^ a b Budge S, Parker AH, Hutchings PT, Garbutt C (April 2019). "Environmental enteric dysfunction and child stunting". Nutrition Reviews. 77 (4): 240–253. doi:10.1093/nutrit/nuy068. PMC 6394759. PMID 30753710.
  179. ^ Scoones I (2015). . Blog on Sanitation Learning Hub Website. Archived from the original on 2021-12-13.[unreliable source?]
  180. ^ Chambers R (2 May 2012). "Sanitation and hygiene: undernutrition's blind spot". Sanitation Learning Hub. Retrieved 2021-12-13.[unreliable source?]
  181. ^ Cumming O, Cairncross S (May 2016). "Can water, sanitation and hygiene help eliminate stunting? Current evidence and policy implications". Maternal & Child Nutrition. 12 (Suppl 1): 91–105. doi:10.1111/mcn.12258. PMC 5084825. PMID 27187910.
  182. ^ Ngure FM, Reid BM, Humphrey JH, Mbuya MN, Pelto G, Stoltzfus RJ (January 2014). "Water, sanitation, and hygiene (WASH), environmental enteropathy, nutrition, and early child development: making the links". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1308 (1): 118–128. Bibcode:2014NYASA1308..118N. doi:10.1111/nyas.12330. PMID 24571214. S2CID 21280033.
  183. ^ Ivanovic DM, Leiva BP, Pérez HT, Olivares MG, Díaz NS, Urrutia MS, et al. (2004). "Head size and intelligence, learning, nutritional status and brain development. Head, IQ, learning, nutrition and brain". Neuropsychologia. 42 (8): 1118–1131. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.022. PMID 15093150. S2CID 2114185.
  184. ^ Liu J, Raine A, Venables PH, Dalais C, Mednick SA (June 2003). "Malnutrition at age 3 years and lower cognitive ability at age 11 years: independence from psychosocial adversity". Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 157 (6): 593–600. doi:10.1001/archpedi.157.6.593. PMC 3975917. PMID 12796242.
  185. ^ a b Webb KE, Horton NJ, Katz DL (April 2005). "Parental IQ and cognitive development of malnourished Indonesian children". European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 59 (4): 618–620. doi:10.1038/sj.ejcn.1602103. PMID 15688080.
  186. ^ Grantham-McGregor S, Ani C (February 2001). "A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children". The Journal of Nutrition. 131 (2S–2): 649S–666S, discussion 666S–668S. doi:10.1093/jn/131.2.649S. PMID 11160596.
  187. ^ Arija V, Esparó G, Fernández-Ballart J, Murphy MM, Biarnés E, Canals J (2006). "Nutritional status and performance in test of verbal and non-verbal intelligence in 6 year old children". Intelligence. 34 (2): 141–149. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2005.09.001.
  188. ^ Halterman JS, Kaczorowski JM, Aligne CA, Auinger P, Szilagyi PG (June 2001). "Iron deficiency and cognitive achievement among school-aged children and adolescents in the United States". Pediatrics. 107 (6): 1381–1386. doi:10.1542/peds.107.6.1381. PMID 11389261.
  189. ^ Bryan J, Osendarp S, Hughes D, Calvaresi E, Baghurst K, van Klinken JW (August 2004). "Nutrients for cognitive development in school-aged children". Nutrition Reviews. 62 (8): 295–306. doi:10.1111/j.1753-4887.2004.tb00055.x. PMID 15478684.
  190. ^ Reavley N. (1998). Vitamins, etc. Melbourne: Bookman Media Pty Ltd
  191. ^ Bleichrodt N, Born MP (1994). (PDF). In Stanbury JB (ed.). The Damaged Brain of Iodine Deficiency: Cognitive, Behavioral, Neuromotor, Educative Aspects. New York: Cognizant Communication Corporation. pp. 195–200. ISBN 978-1-882345-03-8. Archived from the original (PDF) on 30 January 2019.
  192. ^ Brown KH, Peerson JM, Rivera J, Allen LH (June 2002). "Effect of supplemental zinc on the growth and serum zinc concentrations of prepubertal children: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials". The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 75 (6): 1062–1071. doi:10.1093/ajcn/75.6.1062. PMID 12036814.
  193. ^ Bhutta ZA, Black RE, Brown KH, Gardner JM, Gore S, Hidayat A, et al. (December 1999). "Prevention of diarrhea and pneumonia by zinc supplementation in children in developing countries: pooled analysis of randomized controlled trials. Zinc Investigators' Collaborative Group". The Journal of Pediatrics. 135 (6): 689–697. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(99)70086-7. PMID 10586170.
  194. ^ Lassi ZS, Kurji J, Oliveira CS, Moin A, Bhutta ZA (April 2020). "Zinc supplementation for the promotion of growth and prevention of infections in infants less than six months of age". The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. 2020 (4): CD010205. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD010205.pub2. PMC 7140593. PMID 32266964.
  195. ^ . American Psychological Association. Archived from the original on 31 January 2019.
  196. ^ a b Smith JR, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov P (1997). "The consequences of living in poverty for young children's cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement.". In Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J (eds.). Consequences of growing up poor. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. ISBN 978-0-87154-143-7.[page needed]
  197. ^ a b c Schoon I, Jones E, Cheng H, Maughan B (August 2012). "Family hardship, family instability, and cognitive development". Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 66 (8): 716–722. doi:10.1136/jech.2010.121228. PMID 21507894. S2CID 24683428.
  198. ^ Bradley RH, Corwyn RF (2002). "Socioeconomic status and child development". Annual Review of Psychology. 53: 371–399. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233. PMID 11752490.
  199. ^ Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ (1997). (PDF). The Future of Children. 7 (2): 55–71. doi:10.2307/1602387. JSTOR 1602387. PMID 9299837. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-06-17. Retrieved 2012-12-06.
  200. ^ Brody DJ, Pirkle JL, Kramer RA, Flegal KM, Matte TD, Gunter EW, Paschal DC (July 1994). "Blood lead levels in the US population. Phase 1 of the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III, 1988 to 1991)". JAMA. 272 (4): 277–283. doi:10.1001/jama.272.4.277. PMID 8028140.
  201. ^ DeGarmo DS, Forgatch MS, Martinez CR (2003). "Parenting of divorced mothers as a link between social status and boys' academic outcomes: unpacking the effects of socioeconomic status". Child Development. 70 (5): 1231–1245. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00089. PMID 10546342.
  202. ^ Honzik MP (June 1957). "Developmental studies of parent-child resemblance in intelligence". Child Development. 28 (2): 215–228. doi:10.2307/1125882. JSTOR 1125882. PMID 13427072.
  203. ^ Ferguson MK (February 2022). "Caustic Ingestion-The Haves and the Have Nots". JAMA Surgery. 157 (2): 119. doi:10.2307/2786675. JSTOR 2786675. PMID 34878518.
  204. ^ a b Ruhm CJ (2004). "Parental Employment and Child Cognitive Development" (PDF). The Journal of Human Resources. 39 (1): 155–192. doi:10.2307/3559009. JSTOR 3559009.
  205. ^ Jackson M, Kiernan K, McLanahan S (November 2017). "Maternal Education, Changing Family Circumstances, and Children's Skill Development in the United States and UK". The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 674 (1): 59–84. doi:10.1177/0002716217729471. PMC 5857959. PMID 29563643.
  206. ^ Berger LM, Hill J, Waldfogel J (February 2005). "Maternity leave, early maternal employment and child health and development in the US". The Economic Journal. 115 (501): 29–47. doi:10.1111/j.0013-0133.2005.00971.x. S2CID 80026676.
  207. ^ Baum II CL (April 2003). "Does Early Maternal Employment Harm Child Development? An Analysis of the Potential Benefits of Leave Taking". Journal of Labor Economics. 21 (2): 409–448. doi:10.1086/345563. S2CID 222330144.
  208. ^ Hill JL, Waldfogel J, Brooks-Gunn J, Han WJ (November 2005). "Maternal employment and child development: a fresh look using newer methods". Developmental Psychology. 41 (6): 833–850. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.6.833. PMID 16351331.
  209. ^ Brooks-Gunn J, Han WJ, Waldfogel J (July 2002). "Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes in the first three years of life: the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development". Child Development. 73 (4): 1052–1072. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00457. PMID 12146733.
  210. ^ Bowman B, Ray A (2012). "Low-Income Families and Young Children's Development and School Success". In Falk B (ed.). Defending Childhood: Keeping the Promise of Early Education. Teachers College Press. pp. 63–85.
  211. ^ Solomon LD (2014). "Family Structure Trends and Child Well-Being". Building an Opportunity Society. Transaction Publishers. p. 58.
  212. ^ Solomon LD (2014). "Economic Immobility and Income and Wealth Inequalities: Realities and Casual Factors". Building an Opportunity Society. Transaction Publishers. p. 27.
  213. ^ Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, Coll CG (November 2001). "The home environments of children in the United States part I: variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status". Child Development. 72 (6): 1844–1867. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00382. PMID 11768149.
  214. ^ a b McLoyd VC (2013). "The impact of poverty and low socioeconomic status on the socioemotional functioning of African-American children and adolescents: Mediating effects". In Taylor RD, Wang MC (eds.). Social and Emotional Adjustment and Family Relations in Ethnic Minority Families. Routledge. pp. 7–34. ISBN 978-1-135-45262-9.
  215. ^ Bowman B, Ray A (2012). "Low-Income Families and Young Children's Development and School Success". In Falk B (ed.). Defending Childhood: Keeping the Promise of Early Education. Teachers College Press. pp. 63–85. ISBN 978-0-8077-5310-1.
  216. ^ Ajjampur SS, Koshy B, Venkataramani M, Sarkar R, Joseph AA, Jacob KS, et al. (2011). "Effect of cryptosporidial and giardial diarrhoea on social maturity, intelligence and physical growth in children in a semi-urban slum in south India". Annals of Tropical Paediatrics. 31 (3): 205–212. doi:10.1179/1465328111Y.0000000003. PMID 21781414. S2CID 4667351.
  217. ^ (PDF). World Health Organisation. 1987. Archived from the original (PDF) on 26 February 2015.
  218. ^ Chambers R, von Medeazza G (2014). . IDS Working Paper 450. Brighton: Institute of Development Studies. Archived from the original on 13 December 2021.
  219. ^ Opara KN, Udoidung NI, Opara DC, Okon OE, Edosomwan EU, Udoh AJ (2012). "The Impact of Intestinal Parasitic Infections on the Nutritional Status of Rural and Urban School-Aged Children in Nigeria". International Journal of MCH and AIDS. 1 (1): 73–82. doi:10.21106/ijma.8. PMC 4948163. PMID 27621960.
  220. ^ a b Calderón J, Navarro ME, Jimenez-Capdeville ME, Santos-Diaz MA, Golden A, Rodriguez-Leyva I, et al. (February 2001). "Exposure to arsenic and lead and neuropsychological development in Mexican children". Environmental Research. 85 (2): 69–76. Bibcode:2001ER.....85...69C. doi:10.1006/enrs.2000.4106. PMID 11161656.
  221. ^ Bouchard MF, Sauvé S, Barbeau B, Legrand M, Brodeur MÈ, Bouffard T, et al. (January 2011). "Intellectual impairment in school-age children exposed to manganese from drinking water". Environmental Health Perspectives. 119 (1): 138–143. doi:10.1289/ehp.1002321. PMC 3018493. PMID 20855239.
  222. ^ a b Bouchard MF, Chevrier J, Harley KG, Kogut K, Vedar M, Calderon N, et al. (August 2011). "Prenatal exposure to organophosphate pesticides and IQ in 7-year-old children". Environmental Health Perspectives. 119 (8): 1189–1195. doi:10.1289/ehp.1003185. PMC 3237357. PMID 21507776.
  223. ^ Rauh V, Arunajadai S, Horton M, Perera F, Hoepner L, Barr DB, Whyatt R (August 2011). "Seven-year neurodevelopmental scores and prenatal exposure to chlorpyrifos, a common agricultural pesticide". Environmental Health Perspectives. 119 (8): 1196–1201. doi:10.1289/ehp.1003160. PMC 3237355. PMID 21507777.
child, development, this, article, about, developmental, process, journal, child, development, journal, involves, biological, psychological, emotional, changes, that, occur, human, beings, between, birth, conclusion, adolescence, child, using, fingers, make, s. This article is about the developmental process For the journal see Child Development journal Child development involves the biological psychological and emotional changes that occur in human beings between birth and the conclusion of adolescence A child using fingers to make a small circular hole in the sand 1997Childhood is divided into three stages of life which include early childhood middle childhood and late childhood preadolescence 1 Early childhood typically ranges from infancy to the age of 6 years old During this period development is significant as many of life s milestones happen during this time period such as first words learning to crawl and learning to walk There is speculation that middle childhood preadolescence or ages 6 12 1 are the most crucial years of a child s life Adolescence is the stage of life that typically starts around the major onset of puberty with markers such as menarche and spermarche typically occurring at 12 13 years of age 2 It has been defined as ages 10 to 19 by the World Health Organization 3 In the course of development the individual human progresses from dependency to increasing autonomy It is a continuous process with a predictable sequence yet has a unique course for every child It does not always progress at the same rate and each stage is affected by the preceding developmental experiences As genetic factors and events during prenatal life may strongly influence developmental changes genetics and prenatal development usually form a part of the study of child development Related terms include developmental psychology referring to development from birth to death and pediatrics the branch of medicine relating to the care of children Developmental change may occur as a result of genetically controlled processes known as maturation 4 or environmental factors and learning but most commonly involves an interaction between the two Development may also occur as a result of human nature and of human ability to learn from the environment There are various definitions of the periods in a child s development since each period is a continuum with individual differences regarding starting and ending Some age related development periods with defined intervals include newborn ages 0 2 months infant ages 3 11 months toddler ages 1 2 years preschooler ages 3 4 years school aged child ages 5 12 years teens ages 13 17 years 5 Parents play a large role in a child s activities socialization and development having multiple parents can add stability to a child s life and therefore encourage healthy development 6 Another influential factor in children s development is the quality of their care Child care programs may be beneficial for childhood development such as learning capabilities and social skills 7 The optimal development of children is considered vital to society and it is important to understand the social cognitive emotional and educational development of children Increased research and interest in this field has resulted in new theories and strategies especially with regard to practices that promote development within the school systems Some theories seek to describe a sequence of states that compose child development Contents 1 Theories 1 1 Ecological systems 1 2 Piaget 1 2 1 Stages 1 3 Vygotsky 1 4 Attachment 1 5 Erik Erikson 1 6 Behavioral 1 7 Freud 1 8 Other 2 Beginning of cognition 3 Continuity and discontinuity 3 1 Mechanisms 3 2 Asynchronous development 4 Research issues and methods 4 1 Infant research methods 4 2 Ethical considerations 5 Milestones 6 Physical aspects of development 6 1 Physical growth 6 1 1 Speed and pattern 6 1 2 Mechanisms of change 6 1 3 Individual variation versus disease 6 2 Motor skills 6 2 1 Definition 6 2 2 Speed and pattern 6 2 3 Mechanisms 6 2 4 Individual differences 6 2 5 Children with disabilities 6 2 6 Population differences 7 Mental and emotional aspects of development 7 1 Cognitive intellectual 7 1 1 Mechanisms 7 1 2 Speed and pattern 7 1 3 Individual differences 7 1 4 Population differences 7 2 Social emotional 7 2 1 Factors 7 2 2 Speed and pattern 7 2 3 Mechanisms 7 2 4 Population differences 8 Development of language and communication 8 1 Mechanisms 8 2 Components 8 3 Sequential skills and milestones 8 4 Theories 8 5 Communication 8 6 Causes of delays 8 6 1 Individual differences 8 6 2 Environmental causes 8 6 3 Neurological causes 9 Other aspects of development 9 1 Gender 9 2 Race 10 Risk factors 10 1 Postnatal depression 10 2 Maternal drug use 10 2 1 Cocaine 10 2 2 Other 10 3 Malnutrition and Undernutrition 10 3 1 Specific nutrients 10 4 Socioeconomic status 10 5 Parasites 10 6 Toxin exposure 10 7 Other 11 Neglect 11 1 Assessing and identifying 11 1 1 Development focused methods 11 2 Starting the assessment 11 3 Linking to stimulation 11 4 Measuring tools 11 5 Intervention programs 12 See also 13 References 14 Further reading 15 External linksTheories editEcological systems edit Main article Ecological systems theory Also called development in context or human ecology theory ecological systems theory was originally formulated by Urie Bronfenbrenner It specifies four types of nested environmental systems with bi directional influences within and between the systems they are the microsystem mesosystem exosystem and macrosystem Each system contains roles norms and rules that can powerfully shape development Since its publication in 1979 Bronfenbrenner s major statement of this theory The Ecology of Human Development 8 has had widespread influence on the way psychologists and others approach the study of human beings and their environments As a result of this influential conceptualization of development these environments from the family to economic and political structures have come to be viewed as part of the life course from childhood through adulthood 9 Piaget edit Main articles Jean Piaget and Piaget s theory of cognitive development Jean Piaget was a Swiss scholar who began his studies in intellectual development in the 1920s Interested in the ways animals adapt to their environments his first scientific article was published when he was 10 years old and he pursued a Ph D in zoology where he became interested in epistemology 10 Epistemology branches off from philosophy and deals with the origin of knowledge which Piaget believed came from Psychology After travelling to Paris he began working on the first standardized intelligence test at Alfred Binet laboratories which influenced his career greatly During this intelligence testing he began developing a profound interest in the way children s intellectualism works As a result he developed his own laboratory where he spent years recording children s intellectual growth and attempting to find out how children develop through various stages of thinking This led Piaget to develop four important stages of cognitive development sensorimotor stage birth to age 2 preoperational stage age 2 to 7 concrete operational stage ages 7 to 12 and formal operational stage ages 11 to 12 and thereafter 10 Piaget concluded that adaption to an environment behaviour is managed through schemas and adaption occurs through assimilation and accommodation 11 Stages edit Main article Child development stages Sensory Motor birth to about age 2 In the first stage in Piaget s theory infants have the following basic senses vision hearing and motor skills In this stage knowledge of the world is limited but is constantly developing due to the child s experiences and interactions 12 According to Piaget when an infant reaches about 7 9 months of age they begin to develop what he called object permanence meaning the child now has the ability to understand that objects keep existing even when they cannot be seen An example of this would be hiding the child s favorite toy under a blanket and although the child cannot physically see it they still know to look under the blanket 13 Preoperational begins about the time the child starts to talk around age 2 During this stage young children begin analyzing their environment using mental symbols including words and images the child will begin to apply these in their everyday lives as they come across different objects events and situations 10 However Piaget s main focus on this stage and the reason why he named it preoperational is that children at this point are not able to apply specific cognitive operations such as mental math In addition to symbolism children start to engage in pretend play pretending to be people they are not for example teachers or superheroes they sometimes use different props to make this pretend play more real 10 Some weaknesses in this stage are that children who are about 3 4 years old often display what is called egocentrism meaning the child is not able to see someone else s point of view and they feel as if every other person is experiencing the same events and feelings that they are However at about 7 thought processes of children are no longer egocentric and are more intuitive meaning they now think about the way something looks though they do not yet use rational thinking 10 Concrete about first grade to early adolescence In this stage children between the age of 7 and 11 use appropriate logic to develop cognitive operations and begin applying this new way of thinking to different events they encounter 10 Children in this stage incorporate inductive reasoning which involves drawing conclusions from other observations in order to make a generalization 14 Unlike in the preoperational stage children can now change and rearrange mental images and symbols to form a logical thought an example of this is reversibility where the child now knows to reverse an action by doing the opposite 10 Formal operations around early adolescence to mid late adolescence The final stage of Piaget s cognitive development defines a child as now having the ability to think more rationally and systematically about abstract concepts and hypothetical events 10 Some strengths during this time are that the child or adolescent begins forming their identity and begins understanding why people behave the way they behave While some weaknesses include the child or adolescent developing some egocentric thoughts including the imaginary audience and the personal fable 10 An imaginary audience is when an adolescent feels that the world is just as concerned and judgemental of anything the adolescent does as they themselves are an adolescent may feel as if they are on stage and everyone is a critic and they are the ones being critiqued 10 A personal fable is when the adolescent feels that he or she is a unique person and everything they do is unique They feel as if they are the only ones that have ever experienced what they are experiencing and that they are invincible and nothing bad will happen to them bad things only happen to other people 10 Vygotsky edit Main articles Lev Vygotsky and Cultural historical psychology Vygotsky a Russian theorist proposed the sociocultural theory of child development During the 1920s 1930s while Piaget was developing his own theory Vygotsky was an active scholar and at that time his theory was said to be recent because it was translated out of Russian and began influencing Western thinking 10 He posited that children learn through hands on experience as Piaget suggested However unlike Piaget he claimed that timely and sensitive intervention by adults when a child is on the edge of learning a new task called the zone of proximal development could help children learn new tasks This technique called scaffolding builds new knowledge onto the knowledge children already have to help the child learn 15 An example of this might be when a parent helps an infant clap or roll their hands to the pat a cake rhyme until they can clap and roll their hands themself 16 17 Vygotsky was strongly focused on the role of culture in determining the child s pattern of development 15 He argued that Every function in the child s cultural development appears twice first on the social level and later on the individual level first between people interpsychological and then inside the child intrapsychological This applies equally to voluntary attention to logical memory and to the formation of concepts All the higher functions originate as actual relationships between individuals 15 Vygotsky felt that development was a process and saw that during periods of crisis there was a qualitative transformation in the child s mental functioning 18 Attachment edit Main article Attachment theory Attachment theory originating in the work of John Bowlby and developed by Mary Ainsworth is a psychological evolutionary and ethological theory that provides a descriptive and explanatory framework for understanding interpersonal relationships Bowlby s observations led him to believe that close emotional bonds or attachments between an infant and their primary caregiver were an important requirement for forming normal social and emotional development 10 Erik Erikson edit Main articles Erik Erikson and Psychosocial development Erikson a follower of Freud synthesized his theories with Freud s to create what is known as the psychosocial stages of human development Spanning from birth to death they focus on tasks at each stage that must be accomplished to successfully navigate life s challenges 19 Erikson s eight stages consist of the following 20 Trust vs mistrust infant Autonomy vs shame toddlerhood Initiative vs guilt preschooler Industry vs inferiority young adolescent Identity vs role confusion adolescent Intimacy vs isolation young adulthood Generativity vs stagnation middle adulthood Ego integrity vs despair old age Behavioral edit Main article Behavior analysis of child development John B Watson s behaviorism theory forms the foundation of the behavioral model of development 21 Watson explained human psychology through the process of classical conditioning and he believed that all individual differences in behavior were due to different learning experiences 22 He wrote extensively on child development and conducted research such as the Little Albert experiment which showed that a phobia could be created by classical conditioning Watson was instrumental in the modification of William James stream of consciousness approach to construct behavior theory 23 He also helped bring a natural science perspective to child psychology by introducing objective research methods based on observable and measurable behavior 23 Following Watson s lead B F Skinner further extended this model to cover operant conditioning and verbal behavior 24 Skinner used the operant chamber or Skinner box to observe the behavior of animals in a controlled situation and proved that behaviors are influenced by the environment Furthermore he used reinforcement and punishment to shape the desired behavior Children s behavior can strongly depend on their psychological development Freud edit Sigmund Freud divided development from infancy onward into five stages 25 In accordance with his view that the sexual drive is a basic human motivation 26 each stage centered around the gratification of the libido within a particular area or erogenous zone of the body 27 He argued that as humans develop they become fixated on different and specific objects throughout their stages of development 28 29 Each stage contains conflict which requires resolution to enable the child to develop 30 Other edit The use of dynamical systems theory as a framework for the consideration of development began in the early 1990s and has continued into the present 31 This theory stresses nonlinear connections e g between earlier and later social assertiveness and the capacity of a system to reorganize as a phase shift that is stage like in nature Another useful concept for developmentalists is the attractor state a condition such as teething or stranger anxiety that helps to determine apparently unrelated behaviors as well as related ones 32 Dynamic systems theory has been applied extensively to the study of motor development the theory also has strong associations with some of Bowlby s views about attachment systems Dynamic systems theory also relates to the concept of the transactional process 33 a mutually interactive process in which children and parents simultaneously influence each other producing developmental change in both over time 34 The core knowledge perspective is an evolutionary theory in child development that proposes infants begin life with innate special purpose knowledge systems referred to as core domains of thought 35 These five domains are each crucial for survival and prepare us to develop key aspects of early cognition they are physical numerical linguistic psychological and biological 35 Beginning of cognition editThe most influential theories emphasize social interaction s essential contribution to child development from birth e g the theories of Bronfenbrenner 9 Piaget 10 Vygotsky 15 It means that organisms with simple reflexes begin to cognize the environment in collaboration with caregivers However different viewpoints on this issue the binding problem 36 and the primary data entry problem 37 38 challenge the ability of children in this stage of development to meaningfully interact with the environment 39 40 Recent advances in neuroscience and wisdom from physiology and physics studies reconsider the knowledge gap on how social interaction provides cognition in newborns and infants Developmental psychologist Michael Tomasello contributed to knowledge about the origins of social cognition in children by developing the notion of Shared intentionality 41 He posed ideas about unaware processes during social learning after birth to explain processes in shaping Intentionality 41 Other researchers developed the notion by observing this collaborative interaction in psychophysiological research 42 43 44 This concept has been expanded to the intrauterine period Research professor in bioengineering at Liepaja University Igor Val Danilov developed the idea of Michael Tomasello by introducing a hypothesis of neurophysiological processes occurring during Shared intentionality 37 39 45 It explains the onset of childhood development describing this cooperative interaction at different levels of bio system complexity from interpersonal dynamics to neuronal interactions 39 The Shared intentionality hypothesis argues that nervous system synchronization provides non local neuronal coupling in a mother child pair contributing to the proper development of the child s nervous system from the embryo onward 39 From the cognitive development perspective this non local neuronal coupling enables the mother to indicate the relevant sensory stimulus of an actual cognitive problem to the child helping the child to grasp the perception of the object 39 A growing body of evidence in neuroscience supports the Shared intentionality approach Hyperscanning research studies show inter brain coordinated activity under conditions without communication in pairs while subjects are solving a shared cognitive task 46 47 48 49 50 51 This increased inter brain activity is observed in pairs which differs from the result in the condition where subjects solve a similar task alone The significance of this knowledge is that although Shared intentionality enables social cooperation to be achieved in the unaware condition unconsciously it constitutes society While this social interaction modality facilitates child development it also contributes to grasping social norms and shaping individual values in children 52 Continuity and discontinuity editAlthough the identification of developmental milestones is of interest to researchers and caregivers many aspects of development are continuous and do not display noticeable milestones 53 Continuous changes like growth in stature involve fairly gradual and predictable progress toward adult characteristics When developmental change is discontinuous however researchers may identify not only milestones of development but related age periods often called stages These stages are periods of time often associated with known age ranges during which a behavior or physical characteristic is qualitatively different from what it is at other ages When an age period is referred to as a stage the term implies not only this qualitative difference but also a predictable sequence of developmental events such that each stage is preceded and followed by specific other periods associated with characteristic behavioral or physical qualities 54 Stages of development may overlap or be associated with specific other aspects of development such as speech or movement Even within a particular developmental area transition into a stage may not mean that the previous stage is completely finished For example in Erikson s stages he suggests that a lifetime is spent in reworking issues that were originally characteristic of a childhood stage 55 Similarly the theorist of cognitive development Piaget described situations in which children could solve one type of problem using mature thinking skills but could not accomplish this for less familiar problems a phenomenon he called horizontal decalage 56 Mechanisms edit nbsp Girl playing in a playgroundSee also Nature versus nurture Although developmental change runs parallel with chronological age 57 age itself cannot cause development 57 The basic causes for developmental change are genetic and environmental factors 58 Genetic factors are responsible for cellular changes like overall growth changes in proportion of body and brain parts 59 and the maturation of aspects of function such as vision and dietary needs 57 Because genes can be turned off and turned on 57 the individual s initial genotype may change in function over time giving rise to further developmental change Environmental factors affecting development may include both diet and disease exposure as well as social emotional and cognitive experiences 57 However examination of environmental factors also shows that children can survive a fairly broad range of environmental experiences 56 Rather than acting as independent mechanisms genetic and environmental factors often interact to cause developmental change 57 Some aspects of child development are notable for their plasticity or the extent to which the direction of development is guided by environmental factors as well as initiated by genetic factors 57 When an aspect of development is strongly affected by early experience it is said to show a high degree of plasticity when the genetic make up is the primary cause of development plasticity is said to be low 60 Plasticity may involve guidance by endogenous factors like hormones as well as by exogenous factors like infection 57 nbsp Child playing with bubblesOne way the environment guides development is through experience dependent plasticity in which behavior is altered as a result of learning from the environment Plasticity of this type can occur throughout the lifespan and involve many kinds of behavior including some emotional reactions 57 A second type of plasticity experience expectant plasticity involves the strong effect of specific experiences during limited sensitive periods of development 57 For example the coordinated use of two eyes and the experience of a single three dimensional image rather than the two dimensional images created by each eye depends on experiences with vision during the second half of the first year of life 57 Experience expectant plasticity works to fine tune aspects of development that cannot proceed to optimum outcomes as a result of genetic factors alone 61 62 In addition to plasticity genetic environmental correlations may function in several ways to determine the mature characteristics of the individual Genetic environmental correlations are circumstances in which genetic factors interact with the environment to make certain experiences more likely to occur 57 In passive genetic environmental correlation a child is likely to experience a particular environment because his or her parents genetic make up makes them likely to choose or create such an environment 57 In evocative genetic environmental correlation the child s genetically produced characteristics cause other people to respond in certain ways providing a different environment than might occur for a genetically different child 57 for instance a child with Down syndrome may be protected more and challenged less than a child without Down syndrome 57 Finally an active genetic environmental correlation is one in which the child chooses experiences that in turn have their effect 57 for instance a muscular active child may choose after school sports experiences that increase athletic skills but may forgo music lessons In all of these cases it becomes difficult to know whether the child s characteristics were shaped by genetic factors by experiences or by a combination of the two 63 Asynchronous development edit Asynchronous development occurs in cases when a child s cognitive physical and or emotional development occur at different rates This is common for gifted children when their cognitive development outpaces their physical and or emotional maturity such as when a child is academically advanced and skipping school grade levels yet still cries over childish matters and or still looks their age Asynchronous development presents challenges for schools parents siblings peers and the children themselves such as making it hard for the child to fit in or frustrating adults who have become accustomed to the child s advancement in other areas 64 Research issues and methods editResearch questions include What develops What relevant aspects of the individual change over a period of time What are the rate and speed of development What are the mechanisms of development what aspects of experience and heredity cause developmental change Are there typical individual differences in the relevant developmental changes Are there population differences in this aspect of development for example differences in the development of boys and of girls Empirical research that attempts to answer these questions may follow a number of patterns Initially observational research in naturalistic conditions may be needed to develop a narrative describing and defining an aspect of developmental change such as changes in reflex reactions in the first year 65 Observational research may be followed by correlational studies which collect information about chronological age and some type of development such as increasing vocabulary such studies examine the characteristics of children at different ages 66 Other methods may include longitudinal studies in which a group of children is re examined on a number of occasions as they get older cross sectional studies where groups of children of different ages are tested once and compared with each other or there may be a combination of these approaches Some child development studies that examine the effects of experience or heredity by comparing characteristics of different groups of children cannot use a randomized design while other studies use randomized designs to compare outcomes for groups of children who receive different interventions or educational treatments 56 Infant research methods edit When conducting psychological research on infants and children certain key aspects need to be considered 67 These include that infants cannot talk have a limited behavioral repertoire cannot follow instructions have a short attention span and that due to how rapidly infants develop methods need to be updated for different ages and developmental stages 67 High amplitude sucking technique HAS is a common way to explore infants preferences and is appropriate from birth to four months since it takes advantage of infants sucking reflex 68 When this is being measured researchers will code a baseline sucking rate for each baby before exposing them to the item of interest A common finding of HAS shows a relaxed natural sucking rate when exposed to something the infant is familiar with like their mother s voice compared to an increased sucking rate around novel stimuli 69 The preferential looking technique was a breakthrough made by Robert L Fantz in 1961 67 In his experiments he would show the infants in his study two different stimuli If an infant looks at one image longer than the other there are two things that can be inferred the infant can see that they are two different images and that the infant is showing preference to one image in some capacity Depending on the experiment infants may prefer to look at the novel and more interesting stimulus or they may look at the more comforting and familiar image 70 Eye tracking is a straightforward way of looking at infants preferences Using an eye tracking software it is possible to see if infants understand commonly used nouns by tracking their eyes after they are cued with the target word 71 nbsp Typical pattern of habituationAnother unique way to study infants cognition is through habituation which is the process of repeatedly showing a stimulus to an infant until they give no response 72 Then when infants are presented with a novel stimulus they show a response which reveals patterns of cognition and perception 72 Using this study method many different cognitive and perceptual ideas can be studied Looking time a common measure of habituation is studied by recording how long an infant looks at a stimulus before they are habituated to it Then researchers record if an infant becomes dishabituated to a novel stimulus This method can be used to measure preferences infants including preferences for colors 73 and other discriminatory tasks such as auditory discrimination between different musical excerpts 74 Another way of studying children is through brain imaging technology such as Magnetic Resonance Imaging MRI electroencephalography EEG MRI can be used to track brain activity growth and connectivity in children 75 and can track brain development from when a child is a fetus 76 EEG can be used to diagnose seizures and encephalopathy but the conceptual age of the infant must be considered when analyzing the results 77 Ethical considerations edit Most of the ethical challenges that exist in studies with adults also exist in studying children with some notable differences 78 79 Namely informed consent as while it is important that children consent to participate in research they cannot give legal consent parents must give informed consent for their children Children can informally consent though and their continued agreement should be reliably checked for by both verbal and nonverbal cues throughout their participation Also due to the inherent power structure in most research settings researchers must consider study designs that protect children from feeling coerced 78 Milestones editSee also Child development stages Milestones are changes in specific physical and mental abilities such as walking and understanding language that mark the end of one developmental period and the beginning of another 80 for stage theories milestones indicate a stage transition These milestones and the chronological age at which they typically occur have been established via study of when various developmental tasks are accomplished However there is considerable variation in when milestones are reached even between children developing within the typical range Some milestones are more variable than others for example receptive speech indicators do not show much variation among children with typical hearing but expressive speech milestones can be quite variable 81 failed verification A common concern in child development is delayed development of age specific developmental milestones Preventing and intervening early in developmental delays is a significant topic in the study of child development Developmental delays are characterized by comparison with age variability of a milestone not with respect to average age at achievement 82 Physical aspects of development editPhysical growth edit For North American Indo Iranian India Iran and European girls boysThelarche breast development 10 5y 8y 13y Pubarche pubic hair 11y 8 5y 13 5y Growth spurt 11 25y 10y 12 5y Menarche first menstrual bleeding 12 5y 10 5y 14 5y Wisdom tooth eruption 15 5y 14y 17y Adult height reached 15 5y 14y 17y Gonadarche testicular enlargement 11 5y 9 5y 13 5y Pubarche pubic hair 12y 10y 14y Growth spurt 13y 11y 15y Spermarche first ejaculation 13 5y 11 5y 15 5y Wisdom tooth eruption 17y 15y 19y Completion of growth 17y 15y 19y Physical growth in stature and weight occurs for 15 20 years following birth as the individual changes from the average weight of 3 5 kg 7 7 lb and length of 50 cm 20 in at full term birth to their final adult size As stature and weight increase proportions also change from the relatively large head and small torso and limbs of the neonate to the adult s relatively small head and long torso and limbs 83 In a book directed toward pediatricians it says a child s pattern of growth is in a head to toe direction or cephalocaudal and in an inward to outward pattern center of the body to the peripheral called proximodistal 84 Development from childhood to the end of puberty from The Adolescent Period nbsp Development of a boy from 10 to 17 years old nbsp Development of a girl from 4 to 16 years old Speed and pattern edit The speed of physical growth is rapid in the months after birth then slows so birth weight is doubled in the first four months tripled by 1 year but not quadrupled until 2 years 85 Growth then proceeds at a slow rate until a period of rapid growth occurs shortly before puberty between about 9 and 15 years of age 86 Growth is not uniform in rate and timing across all parts of the body At birth head size is already relatively near that of an adult but the lower parts of the body are much smaller than adult size Thus during development the head grows relatively little while the torso and limbs undergo a great deal of growth 83 Mechanisms of change edit Genetic factors play a major role in determining the growth rate particularly in the characteristic changes in proportions during early human development However genetic factors can produce maximum growth only if environmental conditions are adequate as poor nutrition frequent injury or disease can reduce the individual s adult stature though even the best environment cannot cause growth to a greater stature than is determined by heredity 83 Individual variation versus disease edit Individual differences in height and weight during childhood can be considerable Some of these differences are due to genetic or environmental factors but individual differences in reproductive maturation strongly influence development at some points 83 For individuals falling outside these typical variations the American Association of Clinical Endocrinologists defines short stature as height more than 2 standard deviations below the mean for age and gender which corresponds to the shortest 2 3 of individuals 87 In contrast failure to thrive is usually defined in terms of weight and can be evaluated either by a low weight for the child s age or by a low rate of weight gain 88 A similar term stunted growth generally refers to reduced growth rate as a manifestation of malnutrition in early childhood 89 Motor skills edit nbsp A child learning to walkPhysical abilities change through childhood from the largely reflexive unlearned involuntary movement young infants to the highly skilled voluntary movements characteristic of later childhood and adolescence Definition edit Motor learning refers to the increasing spatial and temporal accuracy of movements with practice 90 Motor skills can be divided into two categories basic skills necessary for everyday life and recreational skills including skills for employment or interest based skills Speed and pattern edit The speed of motor development is rapid in early life as many of the reflexes of the newborn alter or disappear within the first year and slows later Like physical growth motor development shows predictable patterns of cephalocaudal head to foot and proximodistal torso to extremities development with movements at the head and in the more central areas coming under control before those of the lower part of the body or the hands and feet 91 Movement ability develops in stage like sequences 92 for example locomotion at 6 8 months involves creeping on all fours then proceeds to pulling to stand cruising while holding on to an object walking while holding an adult s hand and finally walking independently 92 By middle childhood and adolescence new motor skills are acquired by instruction or observation rather than in a predictable sequence 53 There are executive functions of the brain working memory timing measure of inhibition and switching which are important to motor skills Critiques to the order of Executive Functioning leads to Motor Skills suggesting Motor Skills can support Executive Functioning in the brain incomprehensible Mechanisms edit The mechanisms involved in motor development involve some genetic components that determine aspects of muscle and bone strength as well as the physical size of body parts at a given age The main areas of the brain involved in motor skills are the frontal cortex parietal cortex and basal ganglia The dorsolateral frontal cortex is responsible for strategic processing the parietal cortex is important in controlling perceptual motor integration and the basal ganglia and supplementary motor cortex are responsible for motor sequences citation needed According to a study showing the relationship between coordination and limb growth in infants genetic components have a huge impact on motor development 93 Intra limb correlations like the distance between hip and knee joints were studied and proved to affect the way an infant will walk There are also genetic factors like the tendency to use the left or right side of the body more which allows for early prediction of the dominant hand early Sample t tests showed that for female babies there was a significant difference between the left and right sides at 18 weeks and that the right side was usually dominant 93 Some factors are biological constraints that we cannot control like male infants tending to have larger and longer arms yet have an influence on measures like when an infant s reach Overall there are both sociological and genetic factors that influence motor development 93 Nutrition and exercise also determine strength flexibility and the ease and accuracy with which a body part can be moved 53 94 It has also been shown that the frontal lobe develops posterio anteriorally from back to front which is significant in motor development because the hind portion of the frontal lobe is known to control motor functions This form of development known as Proportional Development explains why motor functions typically develop relatively quickly during childhood while logic which is controlled by the middle and front portions of the frontal lobe usually will not develop until late childhood or early adolescence 95 Opportunities to carry out movements help establish the abilities to flex move toward the trunk and extend body parts both capacities are necessary for good motor ability Skilled voluntary movements such as passing objects from hand to hand develop as a result of practice and learning 53 Mastery Climates are autonomy supportive climates that a teacher can adopt to as a suggested successful learning environment for children to promote and reinforce motor skills by their own motivation This promotes participation and active learning in children which Piaget s theory of cognitive development says is extremely important in early childhood 96 Individual differences edit Individual differences in motor ability are common and depend in part on the child s weight and build Infants with smaller slimmer and more mature builds proportionally tend to belly crawl and crawl earlier than infants with larger builds 93 Infants with more motor experience have been shown to belly crawl and crawl sooner Not all infants belly crawl however those who skip this are not as proficient in their ability to crawl on their hands and knees 97 After the infant period individual differences are strongly affected by opportunities to practice observe and be instructed on specific movements Atypical motor development such as persistent primitive reflexes beyond 4 6 months or delayed walking may be an indication of developmental delays or conditions such as autism cerebral palsy or down syndrome 53 Lower motor coordination results in difficulties with speed accuracy and with trade off in complex tasks Children with disabilities edit Children with Down syndrome or developmental coordination disorder are late to reach major motor skills milestones like sucking grasping rolling sitting up and walking talking Children with Down syndrome sometimes have heart problems frequent ear infections hypotonia or undeveloped muscle mass Children can also be diagnosed with a learning disability which are disabilities in any of the areas related to language reading and mathematics with basic reading skills being the most common learning disability The definition of a learning disability focuses on the difference between a child s academic achievement and their apparent capacity to learn 98 Population differences edit Regardless of the culture a baby is born into they are born with a few core domains of knowledge which allow them to make sense of their environment and learn upon previous experience by using motor skills such as grasping or crawling There are some population differences in motor development with girls showing some advantages in small muscle usage including articulation of sounds with lips and tongue Ethnic differences in reflex movements of newborn infants have been reported suggesting that some biological factor is at work Cultural differences may encourage learning of motor skills like using the left hand only for sanitary purposes and the right hand for all other uses producing a population difference 99 Cultural factors are play a role in practiced voluntary movements such as the use of the foot to dribble a soccer ball or the hand to dribble a basketball 53 Mental and emotional aspects of development editCognitive intellectual edit Cognitive development is primarily concerned with ways in which young children acquire develop and use internal mental capabilities such as problem solving memory and language 100 Mechanisms edit Cognitive development has genetic and other biological mechanisms as is seen in the many genetic causes of intellectual disability Environmental factors including food and nutrition the responsiveness of parents love daily experiences and physical activity can influence early brain development of children 101 However although it is assumed that the brain causes cognition it is not yet possible to measure specific brain changes and show the cognitive changes they cause Developmental advances in cognition are also related to experience and learning especially for higher level abilities like abstraction which depend to a considerable extent on formal education 53 Speed and pattern edit The ability to learn temporal patterns in sequenced actions was investigated in elementary school age children Temporal learning depends upon a process of integrating timing patterns with action sequences Children ages 6 13 and young adults performed a serial response time task in which a response and a timing sequence were presented repeatedly in a phase matched manner allowing for integrative learning The degree of integrative learning was measured as the slowing in performance that resulted when phase shifting the sequences Learning was similar for the children and adults on average but increased with age for the children Executive function measured by Wisconsin Card Sorting Test WCST performance as well as a measure of response speed also improved with age Finally WCST performance and response speed predicted temporal learning Taken together the results indicate that temporal learning continues to develop in pre adolescents and that maturing executive function or processing speed may play an important role in acquiring temporal patterns in sequenced actions and the development of this ability 102 non primary source needed Individual differences edit There are typical individual differences in the ages at which specific cognitive abilities are achieved but schooling for children in industrialized countries is based on the assumption that there are no large differences 53 Delays in cognitive development are problematic for children in cultures that demand advanced cognitive skills for work and for independent living 53 Everyday cognitive skills include problem solving reasoning and abstract thinking among many others 103 In the absence of these life skills children may struggle to complete work in a timely manner or understand certain tasks they are asked to do If a delay is noticed screenings can possibly find the source of the issue 104 if there is no underlying issue it is important to help aid the child by reading with them playing games with them or reaching out to professionals that can help 104 Population differences edit There are few population differences in cognitive development boys and girls show some differences in their skills and preferences but there is a great deal of overlap between them 53 Some differences are seen in fluid reasoning and visual processing as until about the age of four girls outperform boys in tests of these skills but by about six or seven boys and girls score similarly This is also true of IQ tests where girls tend to outscore boys but again as they age the gap lessens 105 Differences in cognitive achievement between different ethnic groups appears to result from cultural or other environmental factors 53 Social emotional edit Main article Social emotional development Factors edit Newborn infants do not seem to experience fear or have preferences for contact with any specific people In the first few months they only experience happiness sadness and anger 106 107 A baby s first smile usually occurs between 6 and 10 weeks as this usually occurs during social interactions it is called a social smile 108 By about 8 12 months they go through a fairly rapid change and become fearful of perceived threats 109 By around 6 36 months infants begin to prefer familiar people and show anxiety and distress when separated from them and when approached by strangers 110 Separation anxiety is a typical stage of development to an extent Kicking screaming and throwing temper tantrums are normal symptoms of separation anxiety The level of intensity of these symptoms can help determine whether or not a child has separation anxiety disorder which is when a child constantly and intensely refuses to separate from the parent 111 The capacity for empathy and the understanding of social rules begin in the preschool period and continue to develop into adulthood Middle childhood is characterized by friendships with age mates and adolescence by emotions connected with sexuality and the beginnings of romantic love Anger seems most intense during the toddler and early preschool period and during adolescence 53 Speed and pattern edit Some aspects of social emotional development like empathy develop gradually but others like fearfulness seem to involve a rather sudden reorganization of the child s experiences of emotion 53 Sexual and romantic emotions develop in connection with physical maturation 53 Mechanisms edit Genetic factors appear to regulate some of the social emotional developments that occur at predictable ages such as fearfulness and attachment to familiar people Experience plays a role in determining which people are familiar which social rules are obeyed and how anger is expressed 53 Parenting practices have been shown to predict children s emotional intelligence The amount of time mothers spent with their children and the quality of their interactions are important in terms of children s trait emotional intelligence not only because those times of joint activity reflect a more positive parenting but because they are likely to promote modeling reinforcement shared attention and social cooperation 112 Population differences edit Population differences may occur in older children if for example they have learned that it is appropriate for boys to express emotion or behave differently than girls or if customs learned by children of one ethnic group are different than those learned by another 113 Social and emotional differences between boys and girls of the same age may also be associated with the differences in the timing of puberty seen between the two sexes 53 Development of language and communication editMain article Language development Mechanisms edit Language serves the purpose of communication to express oneself through a systematic and conventional use of sounds signs or written symbols 114 There are four subcomponents a child must know to acquire language competence phonology lexicon morphology and syntax and pragmatics 115 These subcomponents combine to form the components of language sociolinguistics and literacy 114 Currently there is no single accepted theory of language acquisition but various explanations of language development have been given Components edit The four components of language development include Phonology is concerned with the sounds of language 116 It is the function behavior and organization of sounds as linguistic items 117 Phonology considers what the sounds of language are and what the rules are for combining sounds Phonological acquisition in children can be measured by frequency and accuracy of production of various vowels and consonants the acquisition of phonemic contrasts and distinctive features or by viewing development in regular stages and to characterizing systematic strategies they adopt 118 Lexicon is similar to vocabulary as they both describe the complex dictionary of words used in speech production and comprehension 119 The lexicon of a language also includes that language s morphemes Morphemes act as minimal meaning bearing elements or building blocks of something in language that makes sense For example in the word cat the component cat makes sense as does at but at does not mean the same thing as cat In this example ca does not mean anything Morphology is the study of words and how they are formed Morphology is also the branch of linguistics that deals with words their internal structure and how they are formed It is also the mental system involved in word formation 120 Pragmatics is the study of the relationship between linguistic forms and speakers of the language 121 it also incorporates how speech is used to serve different functions Pragmatics can be defined as the ability to communicate one s feelings and desires to others 122 Children s development of language also includes semantics which is the attachment of meaning to words This happens in three stages First each word means an entire sentence For example a young child may say mama but the child may mean Here is Mama Where is Mama or I see Mama In the second stage words have meaning but do not have complete definitions This stage occurs around age two or three Third around age seven or eight words have adult like definitions and their meanings are more complete 123 A child learns the syntax of their language when they are able to join words together into sentences and understand multiple word sentences said by other people There appear to be six major stages in which a child s acquisition of syntax develops 124 First is the use of sentence like words in which the child communicates using one word with additional vocal and bodily cues This stage usually occurs between 12 and 18 months of age Second between 18 months to two years there is the modification stage where children communicate concepts by modifying a topic word The third stage between two and three years old involves the child using complete subject predicate structures to communicate concepts Fourth children make changes on basic sentence structure that enables them to communicate more complex concepts This stage occurs between the ages of two and a half years to four years The fifth stage of categorization involves children aged three and a half to seven years refining their sentences with more purposeful word choice that reflects their complex system of categorizing word types Finally children use structures of language that involve more complicated syntactic relationships between the ages of five years old to ten years old 124 Sequential skills and milestones edit Sequential skills in learning to talk 125 Child Age in Months Language Skill0 3 Vocal play cry coo gurgle grunt3 Babble undifferentiated sounds6 10 Babble canonical reduplicated syllables9 Imitation8 18 First words13 15 Expressive jargon intonational sentences13 19 10 word vocabulary14 24 50 word vocabulary13 27 Single word stage and a few sentences 2 3 word combinations Articles a the Plural s23 24 Irregular past went modal and verb can will 28 to 436 word vocabulary 93 265 utterances per hour25 27 Regular past ed Auxiliary be m s23 26 Third person singular s 896 to 1 507 word vocabulary 1 500 to 1 700 words per hourInfants begin with cooing and soft vowel sounds Shortly after birth this system is developed as the infants begin to understand that their noises or non verbal communication lead to a response from their caregiver 126 This will then progress into babbling around 5 months of age with infants first babbling consonant and vowel sounds together that may sound like ma or da 127 At around 8 months of age babbling increases to include repetition of sounds such as ma ma and da da Around this age infants also learn the forms for words and which sounds are more likely to follow other sounds 127 At this stage much of the child s communication is open to interpretation For example if a child says bah when they are in a toy room with their guardian it is likely to be interpreted as ball because the toy is in sight However if you were to listen to the same word on a recorded tape without knowing the context one might not be able to figure out what the child was trying to say 126 A child s receptive language the understanding of others speech has a gradual development beginning at about 6 months 128 However expressive language the production of words moves rapidly after its beginning at about a year of age with a vocabulary explosion of rapid word acquisition occurring in the middle of the second year 128 Grammatical rules and word combinations appear at about age two 128 Between 20 and 28 months children move from understanding the difference between high and low hot and cold and begin to change no to wait a minute not now and why Eventually they are able to add pronouns to words and combine them to form short sentences 126 Mastery of vocabulary and grammar continue gradually through the preschool and school years with adolescents having smaller vocabularies than adults and experiencing more difficulty with constructions such as the passive voice 128 By age 1 children are able to say 1 2 words respond to their name imitate familiar sounds and follow simple instructions 127 Between 1 2 years old the child uses 5 20 words says 2 word sentences expresses their wishes by saying words like more or up and understands the word no 127 Between 2 and 3 years of age the child is able to refer to themself as me combine nouns and verbs use short sentences use some simple plurals answer where questions and has a vocabulary of about 450 words 127 By age 4 children are able to use sentences of 4 5 words and have a vocabulary of about 1000 words 127 Children between the ages of 4 and 5 years old are able to use past tense have a vocabulary of about 1 500 words and ask questions like why and who 127 By age 6 the child has a vocabulary of 2 600 words is able to form sentences of 5 6 words and use a variety of different types of sentences 127 By the age of 5 or 6 years old the majority of children have mastered the basics of their native language 127 Infants up to 15 month olds are initially unable to understand familiar words in their native language pronounced using an unfamiliar accent 129 This means that a Canadian English speaking infant cannot recognize familiar words pronounced with an Australian English accent This skill develops close to their second birthday 129 However this can be overcome when a highly familiar story is read in the new accent prior to the test suggesting the essential functions of underlying spoken language is in place earlier than previously thought 129 Vocabulary typically grows from about 20 words at 18 months to around 200 words at 21 months Starting around 18 months the child begins to combine words into two word sentences which the adult typically expands to clarify its meaning By 24 27 months the child is producing three or four word sentences using a logical if not strictly correct syntax 128 The theory is that children apply a basic set of rules such as adding s for plurals or inventing simpler words out of words too complicated to repeat like choskit for chocolate biscuit 128 Following this there is a rapid appearance of grammatical rules and ordering of sentences 128 There is often an interest in rhyme and imaginative play frequently includes conversations Children s recorded monologues give insight into the development of the process of organizing information into meaningful units 128 By age three the child begins to use complex sentences including relative clauses although they are still perfecting various linguistic systems By five years of age the child s use of language is very similar to that of an adult From the age of about three children can indicate fantasy or make believe linguistics produce coherent personal stories and fictional narratives with beginnings and endings 128 It is argued that children devise narrative as a way of understanding their own experience and as a medium for communicating their meaning to others 128 The ability to engage in extended discourse emerges over time from regular conversation with adults and peers For this a child needs to learn to combine their perspective with that of others and with outside events and learn to use linguistic indicators to show they are doing this They also learn to adjust their language depending on who they are speaking to 128 Typically by the age of about 9 a child can recount other narratives in addition to their own experiences from the perspectives of the author the characters in the story and their own views 128 Theories edit Although the role of adult speech is important in facilitating the child s learning there is considerable disagreement among theorists about the extent to which it influences children s early meanings and expressive words Findings about the initial mapping of new words the ability to decontextualize words and refine meaning of words are diverse 9 One hypothesis known as the syntactic bootstrapping hypothesis refers to the child s ability to infer meaning from cues by using grammatical information from the structure of sentences 130 Another theory is the multi route model which argues that context bound words and referential words follow different routes the first being mapped onto event representations and the latter onto mental representations In this model parental input has a critical role but the children ultimately rely on cognitive processing to establish subsequent use of words 131 However naturalistic research on language development has indicated that preschoolers vocabularies are strongly associated with the number of words said to them by adults 132 There is no single accepted theory of language acquisition Instead there are current theories that help to explain theories of language theories of cognition and theories of development They include the generativist theory social interactionist theory usage based theory Tomasello connectionist theory and behaviorist theory Skinner Generativist theories say that universal grammar is innate and language experience activates that innate knowledge 133 Social interactionist theories define language as a social phenomenon where children acquire language because they want to communicate with others this theory is heavily based on social cognitive abilities that drive the language acquisition process 133 Usage based theories define language as a set of formulas that emerge from the child s learning abilities in correlation with their social cognitive interpretation and their understanding of the speakers intended meanings 133 Connectionist theory is a pattern learning procedure that defines language as a system composed of smaller subsystems or patterns of sound or meaning 133 Behaviorist theories defined language as the establishment of positive reinforcement but are now regarded as only being of historical interest 133 Communication edit Communication can be defined as the exchange and negotiation of information between two or more individuals through verbal and nonverbal symbols oral and written or visual modes and the production and comprehension processes of communication 134 According to First International Congress for the Study of Child Language the general hypothesis is that access to social interaction is a prerequisite to normal language acquisition 135 Principles of conversation include two or more people focusing on one topic All questions in a conversation should be answered comments should be understood or acknowledged and any directions should in theory be followed In the case of young children these conversations are expected to be basic or redundant The role of a guardians during developing stages is to convey that conversation is meant to have a purpose as well as teaching children to recognize the other speaker s emotions 135 Communicative language is both verbal and nonverbal and to achieve communication competence four components must be mastered These components are grammatical competence including vocabulary knowledge rules of word sentence formation etc sociolinguistic competence or the appropriate meanings and grammatical forms in different social contexts discourse competence which is having the knowledge required to combine forms and meanings and strategic competence in the form of knowledge about verbal and nonverbal communication strategies 134 The attainment of communicative competence is an essential part of actual communication 136 Language development is viewed as a motive to communication and the communicative function of language in turn provides the motive for language development Jean Piaget uses the term acted conversations to explain a child s style of communication that relies more heavily on gestures and body movements than words 124 Younger children depend on gestures for a direct statement of their message As they begin to acquire more language body movements take on a different role and begin to complement the verbal message 124 These nonverbal bodily movements allow children to express their emotions before they can express them verbally The child s nonverbal communication of how they are feeling is seen in babies 0 to 3 months who use wild jerky movements of the body to show excitement or distress 124 This develops to more rhythmic movements of the entire body at 3 to 5 months to demonstrate the child s anger or delight 124 Between 9 12 months of age children view themselves as joining the communicative world 114 Before 9 12 months babies interact with objects and interact with people but they do not interact with people about objects This developmental change is the change from primary intersubjectivity capacity to share oneself with others to secondary intersubjectivity capacity to share one s experience which changes the infant from an unsociable to socially engaging creature 114 Around 12 months of age the use of communicative gestures begins including communicative pointing where an infant points to request something or to point to provide information 114 Another communicative gesture presents around the age of 10 and 11 months where infants start gaze following by looking where another person is looking 114 This joint attention results in changes to their social cognitive skills between the ages of 9 and 15 months as their time is increasingly spent with others 114 Children s use of non verbal communicative gestures predicts future language development The use of non verbal communication in the form of gestures indicate the child s interest in communication development and the meanings they choose to convey that are soon revealed through the verbalization of language 114 Language acquisition and development contribute to the verbal form of communication Children originate with a linguistic system where the words they learn are the words used for functional meaning 133 This instigation of speech has been termed pragmatic bootstrapping According to this theory children view words as a means of social connection in that words are used to connect the communicative intentions of the speaker to new words 133 Hence the competence of verbal communication through language is achieved by gains in syntax or grammar Another function of communicating through language is related to pragmatic development 137 Pragmatic development includes the child s intentions of communication before they knows how to express these intentions and throughout the first few years of life both language and communicative functions develop 133 When children acquire language and learn to use language for communicative functions pragmatics children also gain knowledge about how to participate in conversations and how to relay past experiences events discourse knowledge as well as learning how to use language appropriately for their social situation or social group sociolinguistic knowledge 133 Within the first two years of life a child s language ability progresses and conversational skills such as the mechanics of verbal interaction develop Mechanics of verbal interaction include taking turns initiating topics repairing miscommunication and responding to lengthen or sustain the conversation 133 Conversation is asymmetrical when a child interacts with an adult because the adult is the one to create structure in the conversation and to build upon the child s contributions In accordance to the child s developing conversational skills asymmetrical conversation between adult and child advance to an equal temperament of conversation This shift in balance of conversation suggests a development of narrative discourse in communication 133 Ordinarily the development of communicative competence and the development of language are linked to one another 133 Causes of delays edit This section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed July 2023 Learn how and when to remove this template message Individual differences edit Delays in language skills are the most frequent type of developmental delay According to demographics 1 out of 6 children have a significant language delay 138 speech language delay is three to four times more common in boys than in girls 139 Some children also display behavioral problems due to their frustration of not being able to express what they want or need 140 Simple speech delays are usually temporary Most cases are solved on their own or with a little extra attention from the family It is the parent s duty to encourage their baby to talk to them with gestures or sounds and for them to spend a great amount of time playing with reading to and communicating with their baby In certain circumstances parents will have to seek professional help such as a speech therapist 140 It is important to take into consideration that sometimes delays can be a warning sign of more serious conditions that could include auditory processing disorders hearing loss developmental verbal dyspraxia developmental delay in other areas or an autism spectrum disorder ASD 140 Environmental causes edit There are many environmental causes that are linked to language delays including situations where the child has their full attention on another skill such as walking The child may have a twin or a sibling close to their own age and may not be receiving the parent s full attention Another possibility is that the child is in a daycare with too few adults to administer individual attention Perhaps the most obvious environmental cause would be a child that suffers from psychosocial deprivation such as poverty malnutrition poor housing neglect inadequate linguistic stimulation or emotional stress 141 Neurological causes edit Language delay can be caused by a substantial number of underlying disorders such as intellectual disability which accounts for more than 50 percent of language delays Language delay is usually more severe than other developmental delays in intellectually disabled children and it is usually the first obvious symptom of intellectual disability Intellectual disability causes global language delay including delayed auditory comprehension and delayed use of gestures 142 Impaired hearing is another of the most common causes of language delay A child who can not hear or process speech in a clear and consistent manner will have a language delay and even the most minimum hearing impairment or auditory processing deficit can considerably affect language development Generally the more the severe the impairment the more serious the language delay 143 Nonetheless deaf children that are born to families who use sign language develop infant babble and use a fully expressive sign language at the same pace as hearing children 144 Developmental Dyslexia is a developmental reading disorder that occurs when the brain does not properly recognize and process the graphic symbols that represent the sounds of speech Children with dyslexia may encounter problems in rhyming and separating the sounds that compose words which is essential in learning to read as early reading skills rely heavily on word recognition When using an alphabet writing system this involves in having the ability to separate out the sounds in words and be able to match them with letter and groups of letters Difficulty connecting the sounds of language to the letters of words may result difficulty in understanding sentences Confusion between similar letters such as b and d can occur In general the symptoms of dyslexia are difficulty in determining the meaning of a simple sentence learning to recognize written words and difficulty in rhyming Autism and speech delay are usually correlated Problems with verbal language is the most common sign of autism Early diagnosis and treatment of autism can significantly help the child improve their speech skills Autism is recognized as one of the five pervasive developmental disorders distinguished by problems with language speech communication and social skills that present in early childhood Some common types of language disorders are having limited to no verbal speech echolalia or repeating words out of context problems responding to verbal instruction and ignoring people who speak to them directly 145 Other aspects of development editGender edit Main article Gender identity Gender identity involves how a person perceives themselves as male female or a variation of the two Children can identify themselves as belonging to a certain gender as early as two years old 146 but how gender identity is developed is a topic of scientific debate Several factors are involved in determining an individual s gender including neonatal hormones postnatal socialization and genetic influences 147 Some believe that gender is malleable until late childhood 147 while others argue that gender is established early and gender typed socialization patterns either reinforce or soften the individual s notion of gender 148 Since most people identify as the gender that is typically associated to their genitalia studying the impact of these factors is difficult Evidence suggests that neonatal androgens male sex hormones produced in the womb during gestation play an important role Testosterone in the womb directly codes the brain for either male or female typical development This includes both the physical structure of the brain and the characteristics the person expresses because of it People exposed to high levels of testosterone during gestation typically develop a male gender identity while those not exposed to testosterone or who lack the receptors necessary to interact with it typically develop a female gender identity 147 149 An individual s genes are also thought to interact with the hormones during gestation and in turn affect gender identity but the genes responsible for this and their effects have not been precisely documented and evidence is limited 149 It is unknown whether socialization plays a part in determining gender identity postnatally It is well documented that children actively seek out information on how to properly interact with others based on their gender 148 but the extent to which these role models which can include parents friends and TV characters influence gender identity is less clear and no consensus has been reached Race edit In addition to the course of development previous literature has looked at how race ethnicity and socioeconomic status has affected child development Some studies seem to speak to the importance of adult supervision of adolescent youth 150 Literature suggested that African American child development was sometimes differentiated between cultural socialization and racial socialization citation needed Further a different study found that most immigrant youth choose majors focusing on the fields of science and math 151 Risk factors editRisk factors in child development include malnutrition maternal depression and maternal substance use though many more factors have been studied 152 153 154 Postnatal depression edit Main article Postpartum depression Although there are a large number of studies regarding the effect of maternal depression and postnatal depression on various areas of infant development they are yet to come to a consensus regarding the true effects Numerous studies indicate impaired development while many others find no effect of depression on development citation needed A study of 18 month olds whose mothers had depressive symptoms while the children were 6 weeks and or 6 months old found that maternal depression had no effect on the child s cognitive development Furthermore the study indicates that maternal depression combined with a poor home environment is more likely to have an effect on cognitive development than maternal depression alone However the authors conclude that it may be that short term depression has no effect but long term depression could cause more serious problems 155 A longitudinal study spanning 7 years found no effect of maternal depression on cognitive development as a whole however it found that boys are more susceptible to cognitive developmental issues when their mothers had depression 153 This trend is continued in a study of children up to 2 years old which revealed a significant difference on cognitive development between genders with girls having a higher score however girls scored higher regardless of the mother s history of depression Infants with chronically depressed mothers showed significantly lower scores on the motor and mental scales within the Bayley Scales of Infant Development 156 contrasting with many older studies 153 155 A similar effect has been found at 11 years male children of depressed mothers score an average of 19 4 points lower on an IQ test than peers with healthy mothers while this difference is less pronounced in girls 157 Three month olds with depressed mothers show significantly lower scores on the Griffiths Mental Development Scale which covers a range of developmental areas including cognitive motor and social development 158 Furthermore interactions between depressed mothers and their children may affect social and cognitive abilities in later life 159 Maternal depression has been shown to influence the mothers interaction with her child 160 When communicating with their child depressed mothers fail to make changes to their vocal intonation and tend to use unstructured vocal behaviours 161 Furthermore compared to when interacting with healthy mothers infants interacting with depressed mothers show signs of stress such as increased pulse and raised cortisol levels and make more use of avoidance behaviours for example looking away 159 Mother infant interaction at 2 months has been shown to affect the child s cognitive performance at 5 years 162 Studies have begun to show that other forms of psychopathology mental illness can independently influence infants and toddlers subsequent social emotional development through effects on regulatory processes within the child parent attachment 163 Maternal interpersonal violence related post traumatic stress disorder PTSD for example has been associated with subsequent dysregulation of emotion and aggression by ages 4 7 years 164 Maternal drug use edit Cocaine edit Main article Prenatal cocaine exposure Research has provided conflicting evidence regarding the severity of effects on children s development posed by maternal substance use during and after pregnancy 154 Children exposed to cocaine in utero weigh less than those not exposed at ages ranging from 6 to 30 months 165 Additionally studies indicate that the head circumference of children exposed to cocaine is lower than those that of children without cocaine exposure 165 166 However two more recent studies found no significant differences in either measure between those exposed to cocaine and those who were not 167 168 Maternal cocaine use may also affect the child s cognitive development with exposed children achieving lower scores on measures of psychomotor and mental development 169 170 Again though there is conflicting evidence and a number of studies indicate no effect of maternal cocaine use on a child s cognitive development 171 172 Continuing the trend some studies found maternal cocaine use to impair motor development 173 174 while others showed no effect of cocaine use on motor development 165 168 Other edit The use of cocaine by pregnant women is not the only drug that can have a negative effect on the fetus Tobacco marijuana and opiates can also affect an unborn child s cognitive and behavioral development 175 Smoking tobacco increases pregnancy complications including low birth weight prematurity placental abruption and intrauterine death After birth it can disturb maternal infant interactions reduce IQ increase the risk of ADHD and lead to tobacco use in the child 175 Prenatal marijuana exposure may have long term emotional and behavioral consequences as at ten years old children who had been exposed to the drug during pregnancy reported more depressive symptoms than unexposed peers Some other effects include executive function impairment reading difficulty and delayed emotional regulation 175 An opiate drug such as heroin in utero decreases birth weight birth length and head circumference Parental opiate exposure may impact the infant s central nervous system and autonomic nervous system though the evidence is even more inconsistent than with parental cocaine exposure There are also some unexpected negative consequences on a child such as less rhythmic swallowing strabismus and feelings of rejection 175 Malnutrition and Undernutrition edit Poor nutrition early in life contributes to stunting and by the age of two or three can be associated with cognitive deficits poor school achievement and later in life poor social relationships 176 Malnutrition is a large problem in developing nations and has an important effect on young children s weight and height Children suffering malnutrition in Colombia weighed less than those living in upper class conditions at the age of 36 months 11 88 kg 26 2 lb compared to 14 kg 31 lb and were shorter 85 3 cm 33 6 in versus 94 cm 37 in 152 Malnutrition during the first 1000 days of a child s life can cause irreversible physical and mental stunting 177 Infections and parasites related to poor sanitation and hygiene can impact absorption of nutrients in the gut 178 Adequate sanitation and hygiene rather than just access to food play a critical role in preventing undernutrition malnutrition and stunting 179 180 181 178 and ensuring normal early childhood development 182 Malnutrition has been indicated as a negative influence on childhood intelligence quotient IQ 183 184 Although it has also been suggested that this effect is nullified when parental IQ is considered implying that this difference is genetic 185 Specific nutrients edit The effect of low iron levels on cognitive development and IQ has yet to reach consensus 186 Some evidence suggests that even well nourished children with lower levels of iron and folate although not at such a level to be considered deficient have a lower IQ than those with higher levels of iron and folate 187 Furthermore anaemic children perform worse on cognitive measures than non anaemic children 188 Other nutrients have been strongly implicated in brain development including iodine and zinc 189 Iodine is required for the formation of thyroid hormones necessary for brain development 190 Iodine deficiency may reduce IQ by an average of 13 5 points compared to healthy individual 191 Zinc deficiency has also been shown to slow childhood growth and development 192 193 Zinc supplementation appears to be beneficial for growth in infants under six months old 194 Socioeconomic status edit Socioeconomic status is measured primarily based on income educational attainment and occupation 195 Investigations into the role of socioeconomic factors on child development repeatedly show that continual poverty is more harmful on IQ 196 and cognitive abilities 197 than short term poverty Children in families who experience persistent financial hardships and poverty have significantly impaired cognitive abilities compared to those in families who do not face these issues 197 Poverty can also cause a number of other factors shown to effect child development such as poor academic success less family involvement iron deficiency infections a lack of stimulation 198 and malnutrition Poverty also increases the risk of lead poisoning due to lead paint found on the walls of some houses 199 child blood levels of lead increase as income decreases 200 Income based poverty is associated with a 6 13 point reduction in IQ for those earning half of the poverty threshold compared to those earning twice the poverty threshold 196 and children coming from households featuring continual or temporary poverty perform lower than children in middle class families 197 Parental educational attainment is the most significant socioeconomic factor in predicting the child s cognitive abilities 201 as those with a mother with a high IQ are likely to have higher IQs themselves 185 202 Similarly maternal occupation is associated with better cognitive achievement Those whose mothers job entails problem solving are more likely to be given stimulating tasks and games and are likely to achieve more advanced verbal competency 203 On the other hand maternal employment is associated with slightly lower test scores regardless of socioeconomic status Counterintuitively maternal employment results in more disadvantages the higher the higher socioeconomic status as these children are being removed from a more enriching environment to be put in child care though the quality of the child care must be considered Low income children tend to be cared for by grandparents or extended family 204 and therefore form strong bonds with family High income children tend to be cared for in a child care setting or in home care such as with a nanny If the mother is highly educated this can be a disadvantage to the child 205 Even with quality of care controlled for studies still found that full time work within the first year was correlated with negative effects on child development 204 Children whose mothers work are also less likely to receive regular well baby doctor visits and less likely to be breastfed 206 which has been proven to improve developmental results Effects are felt more strongly when women resume full time work within the first year of the child s life 207 208 These effects may be due in part to pre existing differences between mothers who return to work and those who do not such as differences in character or reason for returning to work 209 Low income families are less likely to provide a stimulating home learning environment to their children due to time constraints and financial stress 210 Compared to two parent households children with a single parent households have greater economic vulnerability and less parental involvement leading to worse social behavioral educational or cognitive outcomes 211 A child s academic achievement is influenced by parents educational attainment parenting style and parental investment in their child s cognitive and educational success Higher income families are able to afford learning opportunities both inside and outside the classroom 212 Poverty stricken children have fewer opportunities for stimulating recreational activities often missing out on trips to libraries or museums and are unable to access a tutor to help with problematic academic areas 213 A further factor in a child s educational attainment involves the school environment more specifically teacher expectations and attitudes 214 If teachers perceive low SES children as being less academically able then they may provide them with less attention and reinforcement 214 On the other hand when schools make an effort to increase family and school involvement children perform better on state tests 215 Parasites edit Diarrhea caused by the parasitic disease Giardiasis is associated with lower IQ 216 Parasitic worms helminths are associated with nutritional deficiencies known to be a risk to child development 217 Intestinal parasitism is one of the most neglected tropical diseases in the developed world and harboring of this parasite could have several health implications in children that negatively affect childhood development and morbidity Prolonged exposure to faecally transmitted infections including environmental enteropathy other intestinal infections and parasites during early childhood can lead to irreversible stunting 218 Reducing the prevalence of these parasites can be a benefit in child growth development and educational outcome 219 Toxin exposure edit High levels of lead in the blood is associated with attention deficits 220 while arsenic poisoning has a negative effect on both verbal IQ and on total intelligence quotient 220 Manganese poisoning due to levels in drinking water is also associated with a reduced IQ of 6 2 points between the highest and lowest level of poisoning 221 Prenatal exposure to various pesticides including organophosphates 222 and chlorpyrifos 223 has also been linked to reduced IQ score Organophosphates have been specifically linked to poorer working memory verbal comprehension perceptual reasoning and processing speed 222 Other edit Intrauterine growth restriction is associated with learning deficits in childhood and as such is related to lower IQ 224 Cognitive development can also be harmed by childhood exposure to violence and trauma including spousal abuse between the parents and sexual abuse 225 226 Neglect editThis section may primarily relate to a different subject or place undue weight on a particular aspect rather than the subject as a whole Specifically see talk page Please help by spinning off or relocating any relevant information and removing excessive detail that may be against Wikipedia s inclusion policy July 2023 When a child is unable to meet their developmental goals because they have not been provided with the correct amount of care stimulation or nutrition this situation is commonly referred to as child neglect It is the most widespread form of child abuse accounting for 78 of all child abuse cases in the United States in 2010 alone Scientific studies show that child neglect can have lifelong consequences for children 227 Assessing and identifying edit Assessing and identifying neglect pose a number of challenges for practitioners Given that neglect is a dynamic between the child s development and levels of nurturance the question in identifying neglect becomes one of where do you start with the child s development or with the levels of nurturance citation needed Development focused methods edit Some professionals identify neglect by measuring the developmental levels of a child as if those levels are normal one can by definition conclude that a child is not being neglected Measured areas of development can include weight height stamina social and emotional responses speech and motor development As all these features go into making a medical assessment of whether a child is thriving a professional looking to start an assessment of neglect might start with information collected by a doctor 228 Infants are often weighed and measured when seen by their pediatrician for well baby check ups The physician initiates a more complete evaluation when the infant s development and functioning are found to be delayed Then social work staff could consult medical notes to establish if the baby or child is failing to thrive as a first step in a pathway towards identifying neglect If developmental levels are below normal the identification of neglect then requires the professional to establish if this can be put down to the level of nurturance experienced by the child Developmental delays caused by genetic conditions or disease need to be discounted as they do not have their basis in a lack of nurturance 228 Starting the assessment edit Besides routine pediatrician visits another way of starting the process of identifying neglect is to determine if the child is experiencing a level of nurturance lower than that considered necessary to support normal development 229 which might be unique to the child s age gender and other factors 229 Exactly how to ascertain what a particular child needs without referring back to their level of development is not something theory and policy on neglect is clear about Furthermore determining whether a child is getting the requisite level of nurturance needs to take into account not just the intensity of the nurturance but also the duration and frequency of the nurturance citation needed Children may experience varying and low levels of certain types of nurturance across a day and from time to time however the levels of nurturance should never cross thresholds of intensity duration and frequency For this reason professionals must keep detailed histories of care provision which demonstrate the duration of subnormal exposure to care stimulation and nutrition 230 Common guidance suggests professionals should focus on the levels of nurturance provided by the carers of the child as neglect is understood as an issue of the parents behaviour towards the child 231 Some authors feel that establishing the failure of parents and caregivers to provide care is sufficient to conclude that neglect is occurring 232 233 One definition is that a child experiences neglect when the adults who look after them fail to meet their needs which clearly defines neglect as a matter of parental performance This raises the question about what level of nurturance a carer or parent needs to fall under to provoke developmental delay and how one goes about measuring that accurately This definition which focuses on the stimulation provided by the carer can be subject to critique Neglect is about the child s development being adversely affected by the levels of nurturance but the carers provision of nurturance is not always a good indicator of the level of nurturance received by the child Neglect may be occurring at school outside of parental care The child may be receiving nurturance from siblings or through a boarding school education which compensates for the lack of nurturance provided by the parents citation needed Linking to stimulation edit Neglect is a process whereby children experience developmental delay owing to experiencing insufficient levels of nurturance In practice this means that when starting an assessment of neglect by identifying developmental delay one needs to then check the levels of nurturance received by the child While some guidance on identifying neglect urges practitioners to measure developmental levels other guidance focuses on how developmental levels can be attributed to parental behaviour 234 However the narrow focus on parental behaviour can be criticised for unnecessarily ruling out the possible effect of institutionalised neglect e g neglect at school If one starts by concluding that the levels of nurture received by the child are insufficient one then needs to consider the developmental levels achieved by the child Further challenges arise however as even when one has established developmental delay and exposure to low levels of nurture one needs to rule out the possibility that the link between the two is coincidental The developmental delay may be caused by a genetic disorder disease or physical sexual or emotional abuse The developmental delay may be caused by a mixture of underexposure to nurture abuse genetics and disease citation needed Measuring tools edit The Graded Care Profile Tool is a practice tool which gives an objective measure of the quality of care in terms of a parent carer s commitment It was developed in the UK 235 The North Carolina Family Assessment Scale is a tool that can be used by a practitioner to explore whether neglect is taking place across a range of family functioning areas 236 Intervention programs edit Early intervention programs and treatments include individual counselling family and group counselling social support services behavioural skills training programs to eliminate problematic behaviour and teach parents appropriate parenting behaviour 237 Video interaction guidance is a video feedback intervention through which a guider helps a client to enhance communication within relationships The client is guided to analyse and reflect on video clips of their own interactions 238 239 Video Interaction Guidance has been used where concerns have been expressed over possible parental neglect in cases where the focus child is aged 2 12 and where the child is not the subject of a child protection plan 240 The SafeCare programme is a preventive programme working with parents of children under 6 years old who are at risk of significant harm through neglect The programme is delivered in the home by trained practitioners and is 18 to 20 sessions focused on 3 key areas parent infant child interaction home safety and child health 241 Triple P Parenting Program is a positive parenting program It is a multilevel parenting and family support strategy The idea behind it is that if parents are educated on proper parenting and given the appropriate resources it could help decrease the number of child neglect cases 242 See also edit nbsp Psychology portal nbsp Biology portalAttachment theory Birth order Child development stages Child life specialist Child prodigy Clinical social work Critical period Developmental psychology Developmental psychobiology Developmental psychopathology Early childhood education Evolutionary developmental psychology Pedagogy Play Psychoanalytic infant observation Child development in Africa Child development in IndiaReferences edit a b Collins WA ed 1984 Introduction Development During Middle Childhood The Years From Six to Twelve National Academies Press US Child Development Young Teens 12 14 years old U S Centers for Diesase Control and Prevention CDC 23 September 2021 Adolescent health World Health Organization Toga AW Thompson PM Sowell ER March 2006 Mapping brain maturation Trends in Neurosciences 29 3 148 159 doi 10 1016 j tins 2006 01 007 PMC 3113697 PMID 16472876 Kail RV 2011 Children and Their Development 6th Edition Mydevelopmentlab Series Englewood Cliffs N J Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0 205 03494 9 OCLC 727047867 Parents Can Play Vital Role in Encouraging Children s Active Healthy Lifestyles PDF U S Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Archived from the original PDF on 20 June 2017 Brundavani V Murthy SR Kurpad AV May 2006 Estimation of deep abdominal adipose tissue DAAT accumulation from simple anthropometric measurements in Indian men and women European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 60 5 658 666 doi 10 2307 1602366 JSTOR 1602366 PMID 16391572 Bronfenbrenner U 1979 The ecology of human development experiments by nature and design Cambridge Harvard University Press ISBN 978 0 674 22456 8 OCLC 4515541 a b c Blades M Smith PK Cowie H 2011 Understanding Children s Development Wiley Blackwell ISBN 978 1 4051 7601 9 OCLC 620124946 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n Shaffer DR 2009 Social and personality development 6th ed Australia Wadsworth ISBN 978 0 495 60038 1 Huitt W Hummel J July 2003 Piaget s theory of cognitive development PDF Educational Psychology Interactive 3 2 1 5 Cognitive Development stages meaning average Definition Description Common problems Encyclopedia of Children s Health Stangor C Walinga J 2014 10 17 7 2 Infancy and Childhood Exploring and Learning Introduction to Psychology 1st Canadian ed BCcampus Deduction amp Induction www socialresearchmethods net Retrieved 2017 01 24 a b c d Cole M Vygotskiĭ LS 1978 Published originally in Russian in 1930 Mind in society the development of higher psychological processes Cambridge Harvard University Press ISBN 978 0 674 57628 5 OCLC 3517053 Kozulin A Vygotsky LS 2012 Thought and Language Cambridge Mass The MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 51771 3 OCLC 768728899 Wertsch JV 1985 Culture communication and cognition Vygotskian perspectives Cambridge UK Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 33830 1 OCLC 715955821 Carton AS Vygotskiĭ LS Rieber RW 1987 The collected works of L S Vygotsky 5 Child psychology New York Plenum Press ISBN 978 0 306 45707 4 OCLC 312913751 Boyd D Wood SH Wood EM 2010 Mastering the World of Psychology Books a la Carte Edition 4th ed Englewood Cliffs N J Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0 205 00505 5 OCLC 660087952 Manning ML September 1988 Erikson s Psychosocial Theories Help Explain Early Adolescence NASSP Bulletin 72 509 95 100 doi 10 1177 019263658807250914 S2CID 144082864 Watson JB 1926 What the nursery has to say about instincts In Murchison E ed Psychologies of 1925 Clark University Worcester MA US Clark University Press pp 1 35 doi 10 1037 11020 001 Watson JB Rayner R February 1920 Conditioned emotional reactions Journal of Experimental Psychology 3 1 1 14 doi 10 1037 h0069608 hdl 21 11116 0000 0001 9171 B a b Hergenhahn BR Henley T 2013 03 01 An Introduction to the History of Psychology Cengage Learning p 398 ISBN 978 1 133 95809 3 Skinner BF August 1963 Operant behavior American Psychologist 18 8 503 515 doi 10 1037 h0045185 ISSN 1935 990X S2CID 51796099 Frager R Fadiman J 2005 Personality and personal growth 6th ed Upper Saddle River N J Pearson Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0 13 144451 5 OCLC 57168823 Felluga D 2012 12 11 Modules on Freud On Psychosexual Development Introductory Guide to Critical Theory Purdue University Archived from the original on 2012 12 11 Retrieved 2020 12 08 Freud and the Psychodynamic Perspective Introduction to Psychology courses lumenlearning com Retrieved 2020 12 08 Felluga D Definition Fixation Introduction to Psychoanalysis Purdue University Retrieved 2020 12 08 Tyson P Tyson RL 1990 Psychoanalytic theories of development an integration New Haven CT Yale University Press ISBN 978 0 300 04578 9 OCLC 21197269 Lemma A 2002 Psychodynamic Therapy The Freudian Approach In Dryden W ed Handbook of individual therapy Thousand Oaks Calif Sage ISBN 978 0 7619 6943 3 OCLC 50279223 Aslin RN 1993 Commentary the strange attractiveness of dynamic systems to development In Thelen E Smith LC eds A Dynamic systems approach to development applications Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 585 03867 4 OCLC 42854628 Gleick J 1987 Chaos Making a New Science Penguin Books Sameroff A 1983 Factors in predicting successful parenting In Hoekelman RA Sasserath V eds Minimizing high risk parenting a review of what is known and consideration of appropriate preventive intervention Radnor Pa Johnson amp Johnson Baby Products Company pediatric round table series ISBN 978 0 931562 07 5 OCLC 8689673 Thelen E 2005 04 15 Dynamic Systems Theory and the Complexity of Change Psychoanalytic Dialogues 15 2 255 283 doi 10 1080 10481881509348831 ISSN 1048 1885 S2CID 1327081 a b Berk LE 2009 Child development Boston Pearson Education Allyn amp Bacon ISBN 978 0 205 61559 9 OCLC 232605723 Treisman A September 1999 Solutions to the binding problem progress through controversy and convergence Neuron 24 1 105 10 111 25 doi 10 1016 s0896 6273 00 80826 0 PMID 10677031 a b Val Danilov Igor 2023 02 17 Theoretical Grounds of Shared Intentionality for Neuroscience in Developing Bioengineering Systems OBM Neurobiology 7 1 156 doi 10 21926 obm neurobiol 2301156 Val Danilov Igor 2022 Smartphone in Detecting Developmental Disability in Infancy A Theoretical Approach to Shared Intentionality for Assessment Tool of Cognitive Decline and e Learning K Arai Ed Proceedings of the SAI 2022 LNNS 508 pp 1 11 2022 Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2022 DOI https doi org 10 1007 978 3 031 10467 1 19 a b c d e Val Danilov I 2023 Low Frequency Oscillations for Nonlocal Neuronal Coupling in Shared Intentionality Before and After Birth Toward the Origin of Perception OBM Neurobiology 7 4 1 17 doi 10 21926 obm neurobiol 2304192 Val Danilov I Mihailova S March 2022 A New Perspective on Assessing Cognition in Children through Estimating Shared Intentionality Journal of Intelligence 10 2 21 doi 10 3390 jintelligence10020021 PMC 9036231 PMID 35466234 a b Tomasello M 2019 Becoming human A theory of ontogeny Cambridge Massachusetts USA Harvard University Press Val Danilov I Mihailova S April 2023 Empirical Evidence of Shared Intentionality Towards Bioengineering Systems Development OBM Neurobiology 7 2 1 20 doi 10 21926 obm neurobiol 2302167 McClung JS Placi S Bangerter A Clement F Bshary R September 2017 The language of cooperation shared intentionality drives variation in helping as a function of group membership Proceedings Biological Sciences 284 1863 20171682 doi 10 1098 rspb 2017 1682 PMC 5627217 PMID 28931743 Shteynberg G Galinsky AD November 2011 Implicit coordination Sharing goals with similar others intensifies goal pursuit Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 47 6 1291 1294 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2011 04 012 Val Danilov I 2023 Shared Intentionality Modulation at the Cell Level Low Frequency Oscillations for Temporal Coordination in Bioengineering Systems OBM Neurobiology 7 4 1 17 doi 10 21926 obm neurobiol 2304185 Liu J Zhang R Xie E Lin Y Chen D Liu Y et al August 2023 Shared intentionality modulates interpersonal neural synchronization at the establishment of communication system Communications Biology 6 1 832 doi 10 1038 s42003 023 05197 z PMC 10415255 PMID 37563301 Painter DR Kim JJ Renton AI Mattingley JB June 2021 Joint control of visually guided actions involves concordant increases in behavioural and neural coupling Communications Biology 4 1 816 doi 10 1038 s42003 021 02319 3 PMC 8242020 PMID 34188170 Hu Y Pan Y Shi X Cai Q Li X Cheng X March 2018 Inter brain synchrony and cooperation context in interactive decision making Biological Psychology 133 54 62 doi 10 1016 j biopsycho 2017 12 005 PMID 29292232 S2CID 46859640 Fishburn FA Murty VP Hlutkowsky CO MacGillivray CE Bemis LM Murphy ME et al September 2018 Putting our heads together interpersonal neural synchronization as a biological mechanism for shared intentionality Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 13 8 841 849 doi 10 1093 scan nsy060 PMC 6123517 PMID 30060130 Szymanski C Pesquita A Brennan AA Perdikis D Enns JT Brick TR et al May 2017 Teams on the same wavelength perform better Inter brain phase synchronization constitutes a neural substrate for social facilitation NeuroImage 152 425 436 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2017 03 013 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002D 059A 1 PMID 28284802 S2CID 3807834 Astolfi L Toppi J De Vico Fallani F Vecchiato G Salinari S Mattia D et al September 2010 Neuroelectrical hyperscanning measures simultaneous brain activity in humans Brain Topography 23 3 243 256 doi 10 1007 s10548 010 0147 9 PMID 20480221 S2CID 3488268 David E Rumelhart Prize CognitiveScienceSociety org Cognitive Science Society 2022 2022 Recipient Michael Tomasello section Retrieved 12 January 2024 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Patterson C 2008 Child development Boston McGraw Hill Higher Education ISBN 978 0 07 234795 1 OCLC 179102376 Gross J 1987 Introducing Erik Erikson Lanham MD University Press of America ISBN 978 0 8191 5788 1 Erikson EH 1968 Identity youth and crisis New York W W Norton ISBN 978 0 393 31144 0 OCLC 750885788 a b c Mercer J 1998 Infant Development A Multidisciplinary Introduction Pacific Grove CA Brooks Cole ISBN 978 0 534 33977 7 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Feldman RS 2011 Development across the life span 6th ed Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0 205 80591 4 Plomin R Asbury K 2005 Nature and Nurture Genetic and Environmental Influences on Behavior Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 600 86 98 doi 10 1177 0002716205277184 JSTOR 25046112 S2CID 20315503 Tau GZ Peterson BS January 2010 Normal development of brain circuits Neuropsychopharmacology 35 1 147 168 doi 10 1038 npp 2009 115 PMC 3055433 PMID 19794405 Buchwald J 1987 A comparison of plasticity in sensory and cognitive processing systems In Gunzenhauser N ed Infant Stimulation Skillman NJ Johnson amp Johnson ISBN 978 0 931562 15 0 Greenough WT Black JE Wallace CS June 1987 Experience and brain development PDF Child Development 58 3 539 559 doi 10 2307 1130197 JSTOR 1130197 PMID 3038480 Archived from the original PDF on 2017 08 10 Retrieved 2012 08 04 Greenough W Black J Wallace C 1993 Experience and brain development In Johnson M ed Brain Development and Cognition Oxford Blackwell pp 319 322 ISBN 978 0 631 18222 1 OCLC 25874371 Berk L 2005 Infants Children and Adolescents Boston Allyn amp Bacons ISBN 978 0205511389 Rubenstein LD Schelling N Wilczynski SM Hooks EN October 2015 Lived experiences of parents of gifted students with autism spectrum disorder The struggle to find appropriate educational experiences Gifted Child Quarterly 59 4 283 298 doi 10 1177 0016986215592193 S2CID 146380252 Newborn Reflexes HealthyChildren org Retrieved 2017 01 24 Goodwin CJ 2009 Research in psychology Methods and design Wiley a b c Siegler RS Gershoff ET Saffran J Eisenberg N Leaper C 2020 How children develop Sixth ed New York New York Worth Publishers Macmillan Learning ISBN 978 1 319 18456 8 OCLC 1137233012 Byers Heinlein K 2014 High Amplitude Sucking Procedure Encyclopedia of Language Development Thousand Oaks SAGE Publications Inc pp 263 264 doi 10 4135 9781483346441 ISBN 978 1 4522 5876 8 Retrieved 2022 05 08 Butler SC O Sullivan LP Shah BL Berthier NE November 2014 Preference for infant directed speech in preterm infants Infant Behavior amp Development 37 4 505 511 doi 10 1016 j infbeh 2014 06 007 PMID 25009957 Matsuda YT Okamoto Y Ida M Okanoya K Myowa Yamakoshi M October 2012 Infants prefer the faces of strangers or mothers to morphed faces an uncanny valley between social novelty and familiarity Biology Letters 8 5 725 728 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2012 0346 PMC 3440980 PMID 22696289 Bergelson E Swingley D February 2012 At 6 9 months human infants know the meanings of many common nouns Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109 9 3253 3258 Bibcode 2012PNAS 109 3253B doi 10 1073 pnas 1113380109 JSTOR 41506937 PMC 3295309 PMID 22331874 a b Daum MM 2017 Hopkins B Geangu E Linkenauger S eds Cognitive development during infancy Cambridge University Press pp 277 287 Skelton AE Franklin A February 2020 Infants look longer at colours that adults like when colours are highly saturated Psychonomic Bulletin amp Review 27 1 78 85 doi 10 3758 s13423 019 01688 5 PMC 7000485 PMID 31848908 S2CID 209407243 Flom R Pick AD December 2012 Dynamics of infant habituation infants discrimination of musical excerpts Infant Behavior amp Development 35 4 697 704 doi 10 1016 j infbeh 2012 07 022 PMID 22982268 Thomason ME 2017 Hopkins B Geangu E Linkenauger S eds Magnetic resonance imaging MRI 2nd ed Cambridge University Press pp 121 128 Giedd JN Raznahan A Alexander Bloch A Schmitt E Gogtay N Rapoport JL January 2015 Child psychiatry branch of the National Institute of Mental Health longitudinal structural magnetic resonance imaging study of human brain development Neuropsychopharmacology 40 1 43 49 doi 10 1038 npp 2014 236 PMC 4262916 PMID 25195638 Britton JW Frey LC Hopp JL Korb P Koubeissi MZ Lievens WE et al 2016 St Louis EK Frey LC eds The Developmental EEG Premature Neonatal Infant and Children American Epilepsy Society a b Aldridge J 2015 Participatory research Working with vulnerable groups in research and practice Bristol University Press pp 31 64 Barbosa B Brito PQ 2018 Ethical Challenges in Researching with Children An Application Adopting a Mixed Method Approach Computer Supported Qualitative Research Second International Symposium on Qualitative Research ISQR 2017 Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing Vol 621 pp 91 100 doi 10 1007 978 3 319 61121 1 8 ISBN 978 3 319 61120 4 Child Development Centers for Disease Control and Prevention CDC February 15 2017 Retrieved February 14 2018 Speech and Language Developmental Milestones NIDCD 2015 08 18 Retrieved 2020 11 14 Khan I Leventhal BL 2023 Developmental Delay StatPearls StatPearls Publishing PMID 32965902 Retrieved 25 September 2023 a b c d Tanner JM 1990 Fetus into Man Cambridge MA Harvard University Press ISBN 978 0674306929 CPNP Kristine M Ruggiero PhD MSN RN PA C Michael Ruggiero MHS 2020 09 14 Fast Facts Handbook for Pediatric Primary Care A Guide for Nurse Practitioners and Physician Assistants Springer Publishing Company ISBN 978 0 8261 5184 1 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Graber EG Physical Growth of Infants and Children Children s Health Issues Merck Manuals Consumer Version Retrieved 2020 11 14 Puberty Definition Stages Duration Signs for Boys amp Girls MedicineNet Retrieved 2020 11 14 FDA Approves Humatrope for Short Stature U S Food and Drug Administration 2003 07 25 Archived from the original on February 14 2005 Retrieved 2009 01 13 Palumbo EJ Failure to Thrive Miscellaneous Disorders in Infants and Children Merck Manual Professional Merck Sharp amp Dohme Corp Archived from the original on 2012 11 29 Retrieved 2010 03 23 Stunting in a nutshell www who int Retrieved 28 September 2023 Willingham DB 1999 The Neural Basis of Motor Skill Learning Current Directions in Psychological Science 8 6 178 182 doi 10 1111 1467 8721 00042 S2CID 143942008 Lang D August 1 2022 Individual and Family Development Health and Well being Iowa State University Digital Press doi 10 31274 isudp 2022 122 a b Bayley N 2005 Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development Harcourt Assessment a b c d Piek JP Gasson N Barrett N Case I December 2002 Limb and gender differences in the development of coordination in early infancy Human Movement Science 21 5 6 621 39 doi 10 1016 s0167 9457 02 00172 0 PMID 12620715 Bell S 14 June 2011 Flexibility diets livestrong com Archived from the original on 28 June 2018 Soska KC Adolph KE Johnson SP January 2010 Systems in development motor skill acquisition facilitates three dimensional object completion Developmental Psychology 46 1 129 138 doi 10 1037 a0014618 PMC 2805173 PMID 20053012 Rudisill ME Johnson JL 2018 07 24 Mastery Motivational Climates in Early Childhood Physical Education What Have We Learned over the Years Journal of Physical Education Recreation amp Dance 89 6 26 32 doi 10 1080 07303084 2018 1476940 ISSN 0730 3084 S2CID 150213725 Adolph KE Vereijken B Denny MA October 1998 Learning to crawl Child Development 69 5 1299 312 doi 10 2307 1132267 JSTOR 1132267 PMID 9839417 The Future of Children futureofchildren org Princeton University Archived from the original on 2016 06 04 Retrieved 2016 05 05 Scharoun SM Bryden PJ 2014 02 18 Hand preference performance abilities and hand selection in children Frontiers in Psychology 5 82 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2014 00082 PMC 3927078 PMID 24600414 Cognitive Development Domain Child Development CA Dept of Education www cde ca gov Retrieved 2024 01 09 Brotherson SE April 2005 Understanding Brain Development in Young Children PDF Fargo ND NDSU Extension Service Archived from the original PDF on 14 November 2014 Shin JC June 2011 The development of temporal coordination in children Brain and Cognition 76 1 106 114 doi 10 1016 j bandc 2011 02 011 PMID 21463915 S2CID 20708124 default Stanford Medicine Children s Health www stanfordchildrens org Retrieved 2023 04 13 a b Developmental Delay in Children Cleveland Clinic Retrieved 2023 04 13 Buczylowska D Ronniger P Melzer J Petermann F May 2019 Sex Similarities and Differences in Intelligence in Children Aged Two to Eight Analysis of SON R 2 8 Scores Journal of Intelligence 7 2 11 doi 10 3390 jintelligence7020011 PMC 6630280 PMID 31162390 Gu J Bourne PE 2009 Structural Bioinformatics 2nd ed Hoboken N J Wiley Blackwell ISBN 978 0 470 18105 8 Infant Emotion Lifespan Development Open Education Resource Lumen Learning Retrieved 3 October 2023 Mendes DM Seidl de Moura ML November 2014 Different kinds of infants smiles in the first six months and contingency to maternal affective behavior The Spanish Journal of Psychology 17 E80 doi 10 1017 sjp 2014 86 PMID 26055068 S2CID 39470102 Emotional and Social Development 8 to 12 Months HealthyChildren org Retrieved 28 September 2023 Brooker RJ Buss KA Lemery Chalfant K Aksan N Davidson RJ Goldsmith HH November 2013 The development of stranger fear in infancy and toddlerhood normative development individual differences antecedents and outcomes Developmental Science 16 6 864 878 doi 10 1111 desc 12058 PMC 4129944 PMID 24118713 Robinson L 2012 Separation Anxiety in Children helpguide org Archived from the original on 6 November 2012 Alegre A 2012 The Relation Between the Time Mothers and Children Spent Together and the Children s Trait Emotional Intelligence Child amp Youth Care Forum 41 5 493 508 doi 10 1007 s10566 012 9180 z S2CID 144031333 Vilhjalmsson R Kristjansdottir G January 2003 Gender differences in physical activity in older children and adolescents the central role of organized sport Social Science amp Medicine 56 2 363 374 doi 10 1016 S0277 9536 02 00042 4 PMID 12473321 S2CID 30233756 a b c d e f g h Hoff E 2014 Language Development Belmont CA Wadsworth Cengage Learning The Four Language Systems The Learning House Inc Archived from the original on 13 December 2012 Retrieved 5 March 2013 de Villiers JG de Villiers PA 1979 Language acquisition 3rd ed Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard Univ Press pp 7 16 ISBN 978 0 674 50931 3 Lass R 1984 Phonology an introduction to basic concepts Cambridge Cambridge University Press p 1 ISBN 978 0 521 28183 6 de Villiers JG de Villiers PA 1979 Language acquisition 3rd ed Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard Univ Press pp 42 43 ISBN 978 0 674 50931 3 Plag I Braun M Lappe S Schramm M 2007 Introduction to English linguistics 1st ed Berlin Mouton de Gruyter p 150 ISBN 978 3 11 018969 8 Aronoff M Fudeman K 2011 What is Morphology 2nd ed John Wiley amp Sons Peterwagner R 2005 What is the matter with communicative competence an analysis to encourage teachers of English to assess the very basis of their teaching Munster Lit Verl p 24 ISBN 978 3825884871 de Villiers JG de Villiers PA 1979 Language acquisition 3rd ed Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard Univ Press p 229 ISBN 978 0 674 50931 3 Hopper R Naremore RC 1973 Children s Speech New York NY Harper amp Row a b c d e f Wood BS 1976 Children and communication verbal and nonverbal language development Englewood Cliffs N J Prentice Hall Hart B Risley TR 1999 Developmental Change The Social World of Children Learning to Talk Baltimore Md P H Brookes Pub p 271 a b c Hart B 1999 The Social World of Children Learning to Talk Baltimore Md P H Brookes Pub pp 53 72 ISBN 978 1 55766 420 4 a b c d e f g h i Woolfolk A Perry NE 2012 Child and adolescent development Upper Saddle River NJ Pearson Education ISBN 978 1 292 04106 3 a b c d e f g h i j k l Pal BK 2011 Education Psychology Delhi Prerna Parkashan a b c van Heugten M Johnson EK February 2014 Learning to contend with accents in infancy benefits of brief speaker exposure Journal of Experimental Psychology General 143 1 340 350 doi 10 1037 a0032192 PMID 23506084 Gleitman LR 1990 The structural sources of verb meaning Language Acquisition 1 1 3 55 doi 10 1207 s15327817la0101 2 S2CID 144713838 Barrett M Harris M Chasin J February 1991 Early lexical development and maternal speech a comparison of children s initial and subsequent uses of words Journal of Child Language 18 1 21 40 doi 10 1017 S0305000900013271 PMID 2010501 S2CID 35724493 Hart B Risley TR 1995 Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children P H Brookes ISBN 978 1 55766 197 5 page needed a b c d e f g h i j k l Hoff E 2009 Language development Wadsworth Cengage Learning ISBN 978 0 495 50820 5 page needed a b Richards JC Schmidt WR eds 1986 Language and Communication USA Longman Group Limited a b Dale PS Ingram D Snow CE 1981 Social Interaction and Language Acquisition Child language an international perspective selected papers from the First International Congress for the Study of Child Language Baltimore Md University Park Press pp 195 214 ISBN 978 0 8391 1608 0 Burgoon KJ Le Poire AB 1993 Effects of Communication Expectancies Actual Communication and Expectancy Disconfirmation on Evaluations of Communicators and Their Communication Behavior Human Communication Research 20 67 96 doi 10 1111 j 1468 2958 1993 tb00316 x Ninio A Snow EC 1996 Essays in Developmental Science Boulder CO US Westview Press Sansavini A Favilla ME Guasti MT Marini A Millepiedi S Di Martino MV et al May 2021 Developmental Language Disorder Early Predictors Age for the Diagnosis and Diagnostic Tools A Scoping Review Brain Sciences 11 5 654 doi 10 3390 brainsci11050654 PMC 8156743 PMID 34067874 Leung AK Kao CP June 1999 Evaluation and management of the child with speech delay American Family Physician 59 11 3121 8 3135 PMID 10392594 a b c McLaughlin MR May 2011 Speech and language delay in children American Family Physician 83 10 1183 1188 PMID 21568252 Sunderajan T Kanhere SV May 2019 Speech and language delay in children Prevalence and risk factors Journal of Family Medicine and Primary Care 8 5 1642 1646 doi 10 4103 jfmpc jfmpc 162 19 PMC 6559061 PMID 31198730 Marrus N Hall L July 2017 Intellectual Disability and Language Disorder Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 26 3 539 554 doi 10 1016 j chc 2017 03 001 PMC 5801738 PMID 28577608 Lang Roth R 2014 12 01 Hearing impairment and language delay in infants Diagnostics and genetics GMS Current Topics in Otorhinolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery 13 Doc05 doi 10 3205 cto000108 PMC 4273166 PMID 25587365 Caselli N Pyers J Lieberman AM May 2021 Deaf Children of Hearing Parents Have Age Level Vocabulary Growth When Exposed to American Sign Language by 6 Months of Age The Journal of Pediatrics 232 229 236 doi 10 1016 j jpeds 2021 01 029 PMC 8085057 PMID 33482219 Vogindroukas I Stankova M Chelas EN Proedrou A 2022 10 14 Language and Speech Characteristics in Autism Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 18 2367 2377 doi 10 2147 NDT S331987 PMC 9578461 PMID 36268264 Halim ML 2016 03 29 Princesses and Superheroes Social Cognitive Influences on Early Gender Rigidity Child Development Perspectives 10 3 155 160 doi 10 1111 cdep 12176 ISSN 1750 8592 a b c Steensma TD Kreukels BP de Vries AL Cohen Kettenis PT July 2013 Gender identity development in adolescence Hormones and Behavior 64 2 288 297 doi 10 1016 j yhbeh 2013 02 020 PMID 23998673 S2CID 23056757 a b Maccoby EE 2000 12 02 Perspectives on gender development International Journal of Behavioral Development 24 4 398 406 doi 10 1080 016502500750037946 ISSN 0165 0254 S2CID 143918530 a b Roselli CE July 2018 Neurobiology of gender identity and sexual orientation Journal of Neuroendocrinology 30 7 e12562 doi 10 1111 jne 12562 PMC 6677266 PMID 29211317 Quintana SM Aboud FE Chao RK Contreras Grau J Cross WE Hudley C et al 2006 Race ethnicity and culture in child development contemporary research and future directions Child Development 77 5 1129 1141 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2006 00951 x PMID 16999787 Archived from the original on 2022 03 08 Retrieved 2020 10 18 Rangel MA Shi Y January 2019 Early patterns of skill acquisition and immigrants specialization in STEM careers Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 116 2 484 489 Bibcode 2019PNAS 116 484R doi 10 1073 pnas 1812041116 PMC 6329961 PMID 30598440 a b Mora JO Herrera MG Suescun J de Navarro L Wagner M September 1981 The effects of nutritional supplementation on physical growth of children at risk of malnutrition The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 34 9 1885 1892 doi 10 1093 ajcn 34 9 1885 PMID 7282613 a b c Kurstjens S Wolke D July 2001 Effects of maternal depression on cognitive development of children over the first 7 years of life Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 42 5 623 636 doi 10 1111 1469 7610 00758 PMID 11464967 a b Frank DA Augustyn M Knight WG Pell T Zuckerman B March 2001 Growth development and behavior in early childhood following prenatal cocaine exposure a systematic review JAMA 285 12 1613 1625 doi 10 1001 jama 285 12 1613 PMC 2504866 PMID 11268270 a b Piteo AM Yelland LN Makrides M August 2012 Does maternal depression predict developmental outcome in 18 month old infants Early Human Development 88 8 651 655 doi 10 1016 j earlhumdev 2012 01 013 PMID 22361258 Cornish AM McMahon CA Ungerer JA Barnett B Kowalenko N Tennant C 2005 Postnatal depression and infant cognitive and motor development in the second postnatal year The impact of depression chronicity and infant gender Infant Behavior and Development 28 4 407 417 doi 10 1016 j infbeh 2005 03 004 Hay DF Pawlby S Sharp D Asten P Mills A Kumar R October 2001 Intellectual problems shown by 11 year old children whose mothers had postnatal depression Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 42 7 871 889 doi 10 1111 1469 7610 00784 PMID 11693583 Galler JR Harrison RH Ramsey F Forde V Butler SC September 2000 Maternal depressive symptoms affect infant cognitive development in Barbados Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 41 6 747 757 doi 10 1111 1469 7610 00662 PMID 11039687 a b Field T Healy B Goldstein S Guthertz M 1990 Behaviour state matching and synchrony in mother infant interactions of nondepressed versus depressed dyads Developmental Psychology 26 1 7 14 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 26 1 7 Reck C Hunt A Fuchs T Weiss R Noon A Moehler E et al 2004 Interactive regulation of affect in postpartum depressed mothers and their infants an overview Psychopathology 37 6 272 280 doi 10 1159 000081983 PMID 15539778 S2CID 21040790 Bettes BA August 1988 Maternal depression and motherese temporal and intonational features Child Development 59 4 1089 1096 doi 10 2307 1130275 JSTOR 1130275 PMID 3168616 Murray L Kempton C Woolgar M Hooper R October 1993 Depressed mothers speech to their infants and its relation to infant gender and cognitive development Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 34 7 1083 1101 doi 10 1111 j 1469 7610 1993 tb01775 x PMID 8245134 Schechter DS Willheim E July 2009 Disturbances of attachment and parental psychopathology in early childhood Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 18 3 665 686 doi 10 1016 j chc 2009 03 001 PMC 2690512 PMID 19486844 Schechter DS Zygmunt A Coates SW Davies M Trabka K McCaw J et al September 2007 Caregiver traumatization adversely impacts young children s mental representations on the MacArthur Story Stem Battery Attachment amp Human Development 9 3 187 205 doi 10 1080 14616730701453762 PMC 2078523 PMID 18007959 a b c Hurt H Brodsky NL Betancourt L Braitman LE Malmud E Giannetta J February 1995 Cocaine exposed children follow up through 30 months Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 16 1 29 35 doi 10 1097 00004703 199502000 00005 PMID 7730454 S2CID 241091 Azuma SD Chasnoff IJ September 1993 Outcome of children prenatally exposed to cocaine and other drugs a path analysis of three year data Pediatrics 92 3 396 402 PMID 7689727 Richardson GA Conroy ML Day NL 1996 Prenatal cocaine exposure effects on the development of school age children Neurotoxicology and Teratology 18 6 627 634 doi 10 1016 S0892 0362 96 00121 3 PMID 8947939 a b Kilbride H Castor C Hoffman E Fuger KL February 2000 Thirty six month outcome of prenatal cocaine exposure for term or near term infants impact of early case management Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 21 1 19 26 doi 10 1097 00004703 200002000 00004 PMID 10706345 S2CID 28960955 Singer LT Yamashita TS Hawkins S Cairns D Baley J Kliegman R May 1994 Increased incidence of intraventricular hemorrhage and developmental delay in cocaine exposed very low birth weight infants The Journal of Pediatrics 124 5 Pt 1 765 771 doi 10 1016 S0022 3476 05 81372 1 PMC 4181569 PMID 7513757 Chasnoff IJ Griffith DR Freier C Murray J February 1992 Cocaine polydrug use in pregnancy two year follow up Pediatrics 89 2 284 289 doi 10 1542 peds 89 2 284 PMID 1370867 S2CID 245214695 Coles CD Bard KA Platzman KA Lynch ME 1999 Attentional response at eight weeks in prenatally drug exposed and preterm infants Neurotoxicology and Teratology 21 5 527 537 doi 10 1016 S0892 0362 99 00023 9 PMID 10492387 Graham K Feigenbaum A Pastuszak A Nulman I Weksberg R Einarson T et al August 1992 Pregnancy outcome and infant development following gestational cocaine use by social cocaine users in Toronto Canada Clinical and Investigative Medicine 15 4 384 394 OCLC 121200190 PMID 1516296 INIST 5556914 Fetters L Tronick EZ November 1996 Neuromotor development of cocaine exposed and control infants from birth through 15 months poor and poorer performance Pediatrics 98 5 938 943 doi 10 1542 peds 98 5 938 PMID 8909489 S2CID 34639176 Swanson MW Streissguth AP Sampson PD Olson HC October 1999 Prenatal cocaine and neuromotor outcome at four months effect of duration of exposure Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics 20 5 325 334 doi 10 1097 00004703 199910000 00007 PMID 10533991 a b c d Minnes S Lang A Singer L July 2011 Prenatal tobacco marijuana stimulant and opiate exposure outcomes and practice implications Addiction Science amp Clinical Practice 6 1 57 70 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 290 287 PMC 3188826 PMID 22003423 Chilton M Chyatte M Breaux J October 2007 The negative effects of poverty amp food insecurity on child development The Indian Journal of Medical Research 126 4 262 272 PMID 18032801 Dominguez EI 2017 BabyWASH and the 1 000 Days A Practical Package for Stunting Reduction London Action Against Hunger a b Budge S Parker AH Hutchings PT Garbutt C April 2019 Environmental enteric dysfunction and child stunting Nutrition Reviews 77 4 240 253 doi 10 1093 nutrit nuy068 PMC 6394759 PMID 30753710 Scoones I 2015 Nutrition Puzzles The Shit Factor Blog on Sanitation Learning Hub Website Archived from the original on 2021 12 13 unreliable source Chambers R 2 May 2012 Sanitation and hygiene undernutrition s blind spot Sanitation Learning Hub Retrieved 2021 12 13 unreliable source Cumming O Cairncross S May 2016 Can water sanitation and hygiene help eliminate stunting Current evidence and policy implications Maternal amp Child Nutrition 12 Suppl 1 91 105 doi 10 1111 mcn 12258 PMC 5084825 PMID 27187910 Ngure FM Reid BM Humphrey JH Mbuya MN Pelto G Stoltzfus RJ January 2014 Water sanitation and hygiene WASH environmental enteropathy nutrition and early child development making the links Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1308 1 118 128 Bibcode 2014NYASA1308 118N doi 10 1111 nyas 12330 PMID 24571214 S2CID 21280033 Ivanovic DM Leiva BP Perez HT Olivares MG Diaz NS Urrutia MS et al 2004 Head size and intelligence learning nutritional status and brain development Head IQ learning nutrition and brain Neuropsychologia 42 8 1118 1131 doi 10 1016 j neuropsychologia 2003 11 022 PMID 15093150 S2CID 2114185 Liu J Raine A Venables PH Dalais C Mednick SA June 2003 Malnutrition at age 3 years and lower cognitive ability at age 11 years independence from psychosocial adversity Archives of Pediatrics amp Adolescent Medicine 157 6 593 600 doi 10 1001 archpedi 157 6 593 PMC 3975917 PMID 12796242 a b Webb KE Horton NJ Katz DL April 2005 Parental IQ and cognitive development of malnourished Indonesian children European Journal of Clinical Nutrition 59 4 618 620 doi 10 1038 sj ejcn 1602103 PMID 15688080 Grantham McGregor S Ani C February 2001 A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children The Journal of Nutrition 131 2S 2 649S 666S discussion 666S 668S doi 10 1093 jn 131 2 649S PMID 11160596 Arija V Esparo G Fernandez Ballart J Murphy MM Biarnes E Canals J 2006 Nutritional status and performance in test of verbal and non verbal intelligence in 6 year old children Intelligence 34 2 141 149 doi 10 1016 j intell 2005 09 001 Halterman JS Kaczorowski JM Aligne CA Auinger P Szilagyi PG June 2001 Iron deficiency and cognitive achievement among school aged children and adolescents in the United States Pediatrics 107 6 1381 1386 doi 10 1542 peds 107 6 1381 PMID 11389261 Bryan J Osendarp S Hughes D Calvaresi E Baghurst K van Klinken JW August 2004 Nutrients for cognitive development in school aged children Nutrition Reviews 62 8 295 306 doi 10 1111 j 1753 4887 2004 tb00055 x PMID 15478684 Reavley N 1998 Vitamins etc Melbourne Bookman Media Pty Ltd Bleichrodt N Born MP 1994 Chapter 19 A meta analysis of research on iodine and its relationship to cognitive development PDF In Stanbury JB ed The Damaged Brain of Iodine Deficiency Cognitive Behavioral Neuromotor Educative Aspects New York Cognizant Communication Corporation pp 195 200 ISBN 978 1 882345 03 8 Archived from the original PDF on 30 January 2019 Brown KH Peerson JM Rivera J Allen LH June 2002 Effect of supplemental zinc on the growth and serum zinc concentrations of prepubertal children a meta analysis of randomized controlled trials The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition 75 6 1062 1071 doi 10 1093 ajcn 75 6 1062 PMID 12036814 Bhutta ZA Black RE Brown KH Gardner JM Gore S Hidayat A et al December 1999 Prevention of diarrhea and pneumonia by zinc supplementation in children in developing countries pooled analysis of randomized controlled trials Zinc Investigators Collaborative Group The Journal of Pediatrics 135 6 689 697 doi 10 1016 S0022 3476 99 70086 7 PMID 10586170 Lassi ZS Kurji J Oliveira CS Moin A Bhutta ZA April 2020 Zinc supplementation for the promotion of growth and prevention of infections in infants less than six months of age The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2020 4 CD010205 doi 10 1002 14651858 CD010205 pub2 PMC 7140593 PMID 32266964 Socioeconomic Status American Psychological Association Archived from the original on 31 January 2019 a b Smith JR Brooks Gunn J Klebanov P 1997 The consequences of living in poverty for young children s cognitive and verbal ability and early school achievement In Duncan GJ Brooks Gunn J eds Consequences of growing up poor New York Russell Sage Foundation ISBN 978 0 87154 143 7 page needed a b c Schoon I Jones E Cheng H Maughan B August 2012 Family hardship family instability and cognitive development Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health 66 8 716 722 doi 10 1136 jech 2010 121228 PMID 21507894 S2CID 24683428 Bradley RH Corwyn RF 2002 Socioeconomic status and child development Annual Review of Psychology 53 371 399 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 53 100901 135233 PMID 11752490 Brooks Gunn J Duncan GJ 1997 The effects of poverty on children PDF The Future of Children 7 2 55 71 doi 10 2307 1602387 JSTOR 1602387 PMID 9299837 Archived from the original PDF on 2017 06 17 Retrieved 2012 12 06 Brody DJ Pirkle JL Kramer RA Flegal KM Matte TD Gunter EW Paschal DC July 1994 Blood lead levels in the US population Phase 1 of the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey NHANES III 1988 to 1991 JAMA 272 4 277 283 doi 10 1001 jama 272 4 277 PMID 8028140 DeGarmo DS Forgatch MS Martinez CR 2003 Parenting of divorced mothers as a link between social status and boys academic outcomes unpacking the effects of socioeconomic status Child Development 70 5 1231 1245 doi 10 1111 1467 8624 00089 PMID 10546342 Honzik MP June 1957 Developmental studies of parent child resemblance in intelligence Child Development 28 2 215 228 doi 10 2307 1125882 JSTOR 1125882 PMID 13427072 Ferguson MK February 2022 Caustic Ingestion The Haves and the Have Nots JAMA Surgery 157 2 119 doi 10 2307 2786675 JSTOR 2786675 PMID 34878518 a b Ruhm CJ 2004 Parental Employment and Child Cognitive Development PDF The Journal of Human Resources 39 1 155 192 doi 10 2307 3559009 JSTOR 3559009 Jackson M Kiernan K McLanahan S November 2017 Maternal Education Changing Family Circumstances and Children s Skill Development in the United States and UK The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 674 1 59 84 doi 10 1177 0002716217729471 PMC 5857959 PMID 29563643 Berger LM Hill J Waldfogel J February 2005 Maternity leave early maternal employment and child health and development in the US The Economic Journal 115 501 29 47 doi 10 1111 j 0013 0133 2005 00971 x S2CID 80026676 Baum II CL April 2003 Does Early Maternal Employment Harm Child Development An Analysis of the Potential Benefits of Leave Taking Journal of Labor Economics 21 2 409 448 doi 10 1086 345563 S2CID 222330144 Hill JL Waldfogel J Brooks Gunn J Han WJ November 2005 Maternal employment and child development a fresh look using newer methods Developmental Psychology 41 6 833 850 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 41 6 833 PMID 16351331 Brooks Gunn J Han WJ Waldfogel J July 2002 Maternal employment and child cognitive outcomes in the first three years of life the NICHD Study of Early Child Care National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Child Development 73 4 1052 1072 doi 10 1111 1467 8624 00457 PMID 12146733 Bowman B Ray A 2012 Low Income Families and Young Children s Development and School Success In Falk B ed Defending Childhood Keeping the Promise of Early Education Teachers College Press pp 63 85 Solomon LD 2014 Family Structure Trends and Child Well Being Building an Opportunity Society Transaction Publishers p 58 Solomon LD 2014 Economic Immobility and Income and Wealth Inequalities Realities and Casual Factors Building an Opportunity Society Transaction Publishers p 27 Bradley RH Corwyn RF McAdoo HP Coll CG November 2001 The home environments of children in the United States part I variations by age ethnicity and poverty status Child Development 72 6 1844 1867 doi 10 1111 1467 8624 t01 1 00382 PMID 11768149 a b McLoyd VC 2013 The impact of poverty and low socioeconomic status on the socioemotional functioning of African American children and adolescents Mediating effects In Taylor RD Wang MC eds Social and Emotional Adjustment and Family Relations in Ethnic Minority Families Routledge pp 7 34 ISBN 978 1 135 45262 9 Bowman B Ray A 2012 Low Income Families and Young Children s Development and School Success In Falk B ed Defending Childhood Keeping the Promise of Early Education Teachers College Press pp 63 85 ISBN 978 0 8077 5310 1 Ajjampur SS Koshy B Venkataramani M Sarkar R Joseph AA Jacob KS et al 2011 Effect of cryptosporidial and giardial diarrhoea on social maturity intelligence and physical growth in children in a semi urban slum in south India Annals of Tropical Paediatrics 31 3 205 212 doi 10 1179 1465328111Y 0000000003 PMID 21781414 S2CID 4667351 Prevention and control of intestinal parasitic infections PDF World Health Organisation 1987 Archived from the original PDF on 26 February 2015 Chambers R von Medeazza G 2014 Reframing Undernutrition Faecally Transmitted Infections and the 5 As IDS Working Paper 450 Brighton Institute of Development Studies Archived from the original on 13 December 2021 Opara KN Udoidung NI Opara DC Okon OE Edosomwan EU Udoh AJ 2012 The Impact of Intestinal Parasitic Infections on the Nutritional Status of Rural and Urban School Aged Children in Nigeria International Journal of MCH and AIDS 1 1 73 82 doi 10 21106 ijma 8 PMC 4948163 PMID 27621960 a b Calderon J Navarro ME Jimenez Capdeville ME Santos Diaz MA Golden A Rodriguez Leyva I et al February 2001 Exposure to arsenic and lead and neuropsychological development in Mexican children Environmental Research 85 2 69 76 Bibcode 2001ER 85 69C doi 10 1006 enrs 2000 4106 PMID 11161656 Bouchard MF Sauve S Barbeau B Legrand M Brodeur ME Bouffard T et al January 2011 Intellectual impairment in school age children exposed to manganese from drinking water Environmental Health Perspectives 119 1 138 143 doi 10 1289 ehp 1002321 PMC 3018493 PMID 20855239 a b Bouchard MF Chevrier J Harley KG Kogut K Vedar M Calderon N et al August 2011 Prenatal exposure to organophosphate pesticides and IQ in 7 year old children Environmental Health Perspectives 119 8 1189 1195 doi 10 1289 ehp 1003185 PMC 3237357 PMID 21507776 Rauh V Arunajadai S Horton M Perera F Hoepner L Barr DB Whyatt R August 2011 Seven year neurodevelopmental scores and prenatal exposure to chlorpyrifos a common agricultural pesticide Environmental Health Perspectives 119 8 1196 1201 doi 10 1289 ehp 1003160 PMC 3237355 PMID 21507777 span, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.