fbpx
Wikipedia

Vocal learning

Vocal learning is the ability to modify acoustic and syntactic sounds, acquire new sounds via imitation, and produce vocalizations. "Vocalizations" in this case refers only to sounds generated by the vocal organ (mammalian larynx or avian syrinx) as opposed to by the lips, teeth, and tongue, which require substantially less motor control.[1] A rare trait, vocal learning is a critical substrate for spoken language and has only been detected in eight animal groups despite the wide array of vocalizing species; these include humans, bats, cetaceans, pinnipeds (seals and sea lions), elephants, and three distantly related bird groups including songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds. Vocal learning is distinct from auditory learning, or the ability to form memories of sounds heard, a relatively common trait which is present in all vertebrates tested. For example, dogs can be trained to understand the word "sit" even though the human word is not in its innate auditory repertoire (auditory learning). However, the dog cannot imitate and produce the word "sit" itself as vocal learners can.

Classification edit

 
Hypothetical distributions of two behavioral phenotypes: vocal learning and sensory (auditory) sequence learning. We hypothesize that the behavioral phenotypes of vocal learning and auditory learning are distributed along several categories.[original research?] (A) Vocal learning complexity phenotype and (B) auditory sequence learning phenotype. The left axis (blue) illustrates the hypothetical distribution of species along the behavioral phenotype dimensions. The right axis (black step functions) illustrates different types of transitions along the hypothesized vocal-learning (A) or auditory-learning (B) complexity dimensions. Whether the actual distributions are continuous functions (blue curves), will need to be tested, in relation to the alternatives that there are several categories with gradual transitions or step functions (black curves). Although auditory learning is a prerequisite for vocal learning and there can be a correlation between the two phenotypes (A–B), the two need not be interdependent. A theoretical Turing machine (Turing, 1968) is illustrated [G∗], which can outperform humans on memory for digitized auditory input but is not a vocal learner. From Petkov, CI; Jarvis ED (2012). "Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates". Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4:12.

Historically, species have been classified into the binary categories of vocal learner or vocal non-learner based on their ability to produce novel vocalizations or imitate other species, with evidence from social isolation, deafening studies, and cross-fostering experiments.[1] However, vocal learners exhibit a great deal of plasticity or variation between species, resulting in a spectrum of ability. The vocalizations of songbirds and whales have a syntactic-like organization similar to that of humans but are limited to Finite-State Grammars (FSGs), where they can generate strings of sequences with limited structural complexity.[2] Humans, on the other hand, show deeper hierarchical relationships, such as the nesting of phrases within others, and demonstrate compositional syntax, where changes in syntactic organization generate new meanings, both of which are beyond the capabilities of other vocal learning groups[3]

Vocal learning phenotype also differ within groups and closely related species will not display the same abilities. Within avian vocal learners, for example, zebra finch songs only contain strictly linear transitions that go through different syllables in a motif from beginning to end, yet mockingbird and nightingale songs show element repetition within a range of legal repetitions, non-adjacent relationships between distant song elements, and forward and backward branching in song element transitions.[4] Parrots are even more complex as they can imitate the speech of heterospecifics like humans and synchronize their movements to a rhythmic beat.[5]

Continuum hypothesis edit

Even further complicating the original binary classification is evidence from recent studies that suggests that there is greater variability in a non-learner's ability to modify vocalizations based on experience than previously thought. Findings in suboscine passerine birds, non-human primates, mice, and goats, has led to the proposal of the vocal learning continuum hypothesis by Erich Jarvis and Gustavo Arriaga. Based on the apparent variations seen in various studies, the continuum hypothesis reclassifies species into non-learner, limited vocal learner, moderate vocal learning, complex vocal learner and high vocal learner categories where higher tiers have fewer species. Under this system, previously identified non-human vocal learners like songbirds are considered complex learners while humans fall under the “high” category; non-human primates, mice, and goats, which are traditionally classified as non-learners, are considered limited vocal learners under this system.[1] Recent work, while generally acknowledging the usefulness of this richer view of vocal learning, has pointed out conceptual and empirical limitations of the vocal learning continuum hypothesis, suggesting more species and factors should be taken into account.[6][7]

Evidence of vocal learning in various species edit

Known vocal learners edit

Birds edit

The most extensively studied model organisms of vocal learning are found in birds, namely songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds. The degree of vocal learning in each specific species varies. While many parrots and certain songbirds like canaries can imitate and spontaneously combine learned sounds during all periods of their life, other songbirds and hummingbirds are limited to a certain songs learned during their critical period.

Bats edit

The first evidence for audio-vocal learning in a non-human mammal was produced by Karl-Heinz Esser in 1994. Hand-reared infant lesser spear-nosed bats (Phyllostomos discolor) were able to adapt their isolation calls to an external reference signal. Isolation calls in a control group that had no reference signal did not show the same adaptation.[8]

Further evidence for vocal learning in bats appeared in 1998 when Janette Wenrick Boughman studied female greater spear-nosed bats (Phyllostomus hastatus). These bats live in unrelated groups and use group contact calls that differ among social groups. Each social group has a single call, which differs in frequency and temporal characteristics. When individual bats were introduced to a new social group, the group call began to morph, taking on new frequency and temporal characteristics, and over time, calls of transfer and resident bats in the same group more closely resembled their new modified call than their old calls.[9]

Cetaceans edit

Whales edit

Male humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) sing as a form of sexual display while migrating to and from their breeding grounds. All males in a population produce the same song which can change over time, indicating vocal learning and cultural transmission, a characteristic shared by some bird populations. Songs become increasingly dissimilar over distance and populations in different oceans have dissimilar songs.

Whale songs recorded along the east coast of Australia in 1996 showed introduction of a novel song by two foreign whales who had migrated from the west Australian coast to the east Australian coast. In just two years, all members of the population had switched songs. This new song was nearly identical to ones sung by migrating humpback whales on the west Australian Coast, and the two new singers who introduced the song are hypothesized to have introduced the new "foreign" song to the population on the east Australian coast.[10]

Vocal learning has also been seen in killer whales (Orcinus orca). Two juvenile killer whales, separated from their natal pods, were seen mimicking cries of California sea lions (Zalophus californianus) that were near the region they lived in. The composition of the calls of these two juveniles were also different from their natal groups, reflecting more of the sea lion calls than that of the whales.[11]

Dolphins edit

Captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) can be trained to emit sounds through their blowhole in open air. Through training, these vocal emissions can be altered from natural patterns to resemble sounds like the human voice, measurable through the number of bursts of sound emitted by the dolphin. In 92% of exchanges between humans and dolphins, the number of bursts equaled ±1 of the number of syllables spoken by a human.[12] Another study used an underwater keyboard to demonstrate that dolphins are able to learn various whistles in order to do an activity or obtain an object. Complete mimicry occurred within ten attempts for these trained dolphins.[13] Other studies of dolphins have given even more evidence of spontaneous mimicry of species-specific whistles and other biological and computer-generated signals.[14]

Such vocal learning has also been identified in wild bottlenose dolphins. Bottlenose dolphins develop a distinct signature whistle in the first few months of life, which is used to identify and distinguish itself from other individuals. This individual distinctiveness could have been a driving force for evolution by providing higher species fitness since complex communication is largely correlated with increased intelligence. However, vocal identification is present in vocal non-learners as well. Therefore, it is unlikely that individual identification was a primary driving force for the evolution of vocal learning. Each signature whistle can be learned by other individuals for identification purposes and are used primarily when the dolphin in question is out of sight. Bottlenose dolphins use their learned whistles in matching interactions, which are likely to be used while addressing each other, signalling alliance membership to a third party, or preventing deception by an imitating dolphin.[15]

Mate attraction and territory defense have also been seen as possible contributors to vocal learning evolution. Studies on this topic point out that while both vocal learners and non-learners use vocalizations to attract mates or defend territories, there is one key difference: variability. Vocal learners can produce a more varied arrangement of vocalizations and frequencies, which studies show may be more preferred by females. For example, Caldwell[16] observed that male Atlantic bottlenose dolphins may initiate a challenge by facing another dolphin, opening its mouth, thereby exposing its teeth, or arching its back slightly and holding its head downward. This behavior is more along the lines of visual communication but still may or may not be accompanied by vocalizations such as burst-pulsed sounds. The burst-pulsed sounds, which are more complex and varied than the whistles, are often utilized to convey excitement, dominance or aggression such as when they are competing for the same piece of food.[17] The dolphins also produce these forceful sounds when in the presence of other individuals moving towards the same prey. On the sexual side, Caldwell saw that dolphins may solicit a sexual response from another by swimming in front of it, looking back, and rolling on its side to display the genital region.[18] These observations provide yet another example of visual communication where dolphins exhibit different postures and non-vocal behaviors to communicate with others that also may or may not be accompanied by vocalizations. Sexual selection for greater variability, and thus in turn vocal learning, may then be a major driving force for the evolution of vocal learning.

Seals edit

Captive harbor seals (Phoca vitulina) were recorded mimicking human words such as "hello", "Hoover" (the seal's own name) and producing other speech-like sounds. Most of the vocalizations occurred during the reproductive season.[19]

More evidence of vocal learning in seals occurs in southern elephant seals (Mirounga leonine). Young males imitate the vocal cries of successful older males during their breeding season. northern and southern elephant seals have a highly polygynous mating system with a vast disparity in mating success. In other words, few males guard huge harems of females, eliciting intense male-male competition. Antagonistic vocal cries play an important role in inter-male competitions and are hypothesized to demonstrate the resource-holding potential of the emitter. In both species, antagonistic vocal cries vary geographically and are structurally complex and individually distinct. Males displays unique calls, which can be identified by the specific arrangement of syllable and syllable parts.

Harem holders frequently vocalize to keep peripheral males away from females, and these vocalizations are the dominant component in a young juvenile's acoustic habitat. Successful vocalizations are heard by juveniles, who then imitate these calls as they get older in an attempt to obtain a harem for themselves. Novel vocal types expressed by dominant males spread quickly through populations of breeding elephant seals and are even imitated by juveniles in the same season.

Genetic analysis indicated that successful vocal patterns were not passed down hereditarily, indicating that this behavior is learned. Progeny of successful harem holders do not display their father's vocal calls and the call that makes one male successful often disappears entirely from the population.[20]

Elephants edit

Mlaika, a ten-year-old adolescent female African elephant, has been recorded imitating truck sounds coming from the Nairobi-Mombasa highway three miles away. Analysis of Mlaika's truck-like calls show that they are different from the normal calls of African elephants, and that her calls are a general model of truck sounds, not copies of the sounds of trucks recorded at the same time of the calls. In other words, Mlaika's truck calls are not imitations of the trucks that she hears, but rather, a generalized model she developed over time.

Other evidence of vocal learning in elephants occurred in a cross-fostering situation with a captive African elephant. At the Basel Zoo in Switzerland, Calimero, a male African elephant, was kept with two female Asian elephants. Recordings of his cries shows evidence of chirping noises, typically only produced by Asian elephants. The duration and frequency of these calls differs from recorded instances of chirping calls from other African elephants and more closely resembles the chirping calls of Asian elephants.[21]

Controversial or limited vocal learners edit

The following species are not formally considered vocal learners, but some evidence has suggested they may have limited abilities to modify their vocalizations. Further research is needed in these species to fully understand their learning abilities.

Non-human primates edit

Early research asserted that primate calls are fully formed at an early age in development, yet recently some studies have suggested these calls are modified later in life.[22] In 1989, Masataka and Fujita cross-fostered Japanese and rhesus monkeys in the same room and demonstrated that foraging calls were learned directly from their foster mothers, providing evidence of vocal learning.[23] However, when another independent group was unable to reproduce these results, Masataka and Fujita's findings were questioned.[22] Adding to the evidence against vocal learning in non-human primates is the suggestion that regional differences in calls maybe be attributed to genetic differences between populations and not vocal learning.[24]

Other studies argue that non-human primates do have some limited vocal learning ability, demonstrating that they can modify their vocalizations in a limited fashion through laryngeal control[24] and lip movements.[25][26] For example, chimpanzees in both captivity and in the wild have been recorded producing novel sounds to attract attention. By puckering their lips and making a vibrating sounds, they can make a "raspberry" call, which has been imitated by both naïve captive and wild individuals.[25] There is also evidence of an orangutan learning to whistle by copying a human, an ability previously unseen in the species.[26] A cross-fostering experiment with marmosets and macaques showed convergence in pitch and other acoustic features in their supposedly innate calls,[24] demonstrating the ability, albeit limited, for vocal learning.

Mice edit

Mice produce long sequences of vocalizations or "songs" that are used for both isolation calls in pups when cold or removed from nest and for courtship when males sense a female or detect pheromones in their urine. These ultrasonic vocalizations consist of discrete syllables and patterns, with species-specific differences. Males tend to use particular syllable types that can be used to differentiate individuals.[27]

There has been intense debate on whether these songs are innate or learned. In 2011, Kikusui et al. cross-fostered two strains of mice with distinct song phenotypes and discovered that strain-specific characteristics of each song persisted in the offspring, indicating that these vocalizations are innate.[28] However, a year later work by Arriaga et al. contradicted these results as their study found a motor cortex region active during singing, which projects directly to brainstem motor neurons and is also important for keeping songs stereotyped and on pitch. Vocal control by forebrain motor areas and direct cortical projections to vocal motor neurons are both features of vocal learning. Furthermore, male mice were shown to depend on auditory feedback to maintain some ultrasonic song features, and sub-strains with differences in their songs were able to match each other's pitch when cross-housed under competitive social conditions.[29][30]

In 2013, Mahrt et al. showed that genetically deafened mice produce calls of the same types, number, duration, frequency as normal hearing mice. This finding shows that mice do not require auditory experience to produce normal vocalizations, suggesting that mice are not vocal learners.[31]

With this conflicting evidence, it remains unclear whether mice are vocal non-learners or limited vocal learners.

Goats edit

When goats are placed in different social groups, they modify their calls to show more similarity to that of the group, which provides evidence they may be limited vocal learners according to Erich Jarvis' continuum hypothesis.[29]

Evolution edit

As vocal learning is such a rare trait that evolved in distant groups, there are many theories to explain the striking similarities between vocal learners, especially within avian vocal learners.

Adaptive advantage edit

There are several proposed hypotheses that explain the selection for vocal learning based on environment and behavior. These include:[32]

  • Individual identification: In most vocal-learning species, individuals have their own songs which serve as a unique signature to differentiate themselves from others in the population, which some suggest has driven selection of vocal learning. However, identification by voice, rather than by song or name, is present in vocal non-learners as well. Among vocal learners, only humans and maybe bottlenose dolphins actually use unique names. Therefore, it is unlikely that individual identification was a primary driving force for the evolution of vocal learning.
  • Semantic communication: Semantic vocal communication associates specific vocalizations with animate or inanimate objects to convey a factual message. This hypothesis asserts that vocal learning evolved to facilitate enhanced communication of these specific messages as opposed to affective communication, which conveys emotional content. For example, humans are able to shout "watch out for that car!" when another is in danger while crossing the street instead of just making a noise to indicate urgency, which is less effective at conveying the exact danger at hand. However, many vocal non-learners, including chickens and velvet monkeys, have been shown to use their innate calls to communicate semantic information such as ‘a food source’ or 'predator.' Further discrediting this hypothesis is the fact that vocal learning birds also use innate calls for this purpose and only rarely use their learned vocalizations for semantic communication (for example, the grey parrot can mimic human speech and the black-capped chickadee uses calls to indicate predator size). As learned vocalizations rarely convey semantic information, this hypothesis also does not fully explain the evolution of vocal learning.
  • Mate attraction and territory defense: While both vocal learners and non-learners use vocalizations to attract mates or defend territories, there is one key difference: variability. Vocal learners can produce more varied syntax and frequency modulation, which have been shown to be preferred by females in songbirds. For example, canaries use two voices to produce large frequency modulation variations called "sexy syllables" or "sexy songs", which are thought to stimulate estrogen production in females. When vocal non-learner females were presented with artificially increased frequency modulations in their innate vocalizations, more mating was stimulated. Sexual selection for greater variability, and thus in turn vocal learning, may then be a major driving force for the evolution of vocal learning.
  • Rapid adaptation to sound propagation in different environments: Vocal non-learners produce their sounds best in specific habitats, making them more susceptible to changes in the environment. For example, pigeons' low-frequency calls travel best near the ground, and so communication higher in the air is much less effective. In contrast, vocal learners can change voice characteristics to suit their current environment, which presumably allows for better group communication.

Predatory pressure edit

With the many possible advantages outlined above, it still remains unclear as to why vocal learning is so rare. One proposed explanation is that predatory pressure applies a strong selective force against vocal learning.[32] If mates prefer more variable vocalizations, predators may also be more strongly attracted to more variable vocalizations. As innate calls are typically constant, predators quickly habituate to these vocalizations and ignore them as background noise. In contrast, the variable vocalizations of vocal learners are less likely to be ignored, possibly increasing the predation rate among vocal learners. In this case, relaxed predation pressure or some mechanism to overcome increased predation must first develop to facilitate the evolution of vocal learning. Supporting this hypothesis is the fact that many mammalian vocal learners including humans, whales, and elephants have very few major predators. Similarly, several avian vocal learners have behaviors that are effective in avoiding predators, from the rapid flight and escape behavior of hummingbirds to predator mobbing in parrots and songbirds.

While little research has been done in this area, some studies have supported the predation hypothesis. One study showed that Bengalese finches bred in captivity for 250 years without predation or human selection for singing behavior show increased variability in syntax than their conspecifics in the wild. A similar experiment with captive zebra finches demonstrated the same result as captive birds had increased song variability, which was then preferred by females.[33] Although these studies are promising, more research is needed in this area to compare predation rates across vocal learners and non-learners.

Phylogeny edit

Birds edit

 
Avian phylogenetic tree and the complex-vocal learning phenotype. Shown is an avian phylogenetic tree (based on: Hackett et al., 2008). Identified in red text and ∗ are three groups of complex-vocal learning birds. Below the figure are summarized three alternative hypotheses on the evolutionary mechanisms of complex-vocal learning in birds. From Petkov, CI; Jarvis ED (2012). "Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates". Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4:12.

Modern birds supposedly evolved from a common ancestor around the Cretaceous-Paleogene boundary at the time of the extinction of dinosaurs, about 66 million years ago. Out of the thirty avian orders, only three evolved vocal learning and all have incredibly similar forebrain structures despite the fact that they are distantly related (for example, parrots and songbirds are as distantly related as humans and dolphins). Phylogenetic comparisons have suggested that vocal learning evolved among birds at least two or three independent times, in songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds. Depending on the interpretation of the trees, there were either three gains in all three lineages or two gains, in hummingbirds and the common ancestor of parrots and songbirds, with a loss in the suboscine songbirds. There are several hypotheses to explain this phenomenon:[1]

  • Independent convergent evolution: All three avian groups evolved vocal learning and similar neural pathways independently (not through a common ancestor). This suggests that there are strong epigenetic constraints imposed by the environment or morphological needs, and so this hypothesis predicts that groups that newly evolve vocal learning will also develop similar neural circuits.
  • Common ancestor: This alternative hypothesis suggests that vocal learning birds evolved the trait from a distant common ancestor, which was then lost four independent times in interrelated vocal non-learners. Possible causes include high survival costs of vocal learning (predation) or weak adaptive benefits that did not induce strong selection for the trait for organisms in other environments.
  • Rudimentary structures in non-learners: This alternative hypothesis states that avian non-learners actually do possess rudimentary or undeveloped brain structures necessary for song learning, which were enlarged in vocal learning species. Significantly, this concept challenges the current assumption that vocal nuclei are unique to vocal learners, suggesting that these structures are universal even in other groups such as mammals.
  • Motor theory: This hypothesis suggests that cerebral systems that control vocal learning in distantly related animals evolved as specializations of a pre-existing motor system inherited from a common ancestor. Thus in avian vocal learners, each of the three groups of vocal learning birds evolved cerebral vocal systems independently, but the systems were constrained by a previous genetically determined motor system inherited from the common ancestor that controls learned movement sequencing. Evidence for this hypothesis was provided by Feenders and colleagues in 2008 as they found that EGR1, an immediate early gene associated with increases in neuronal activity, was expressed in forebrain regions surrounding or directly adjacent to song nuclei when vocal learning birds performed non-vocal movement behaviors such as hopping and flying. In non-learners, comparable areas were activated, but without the adjacent presence of song nuclei.[34] EGR1 expression patterns were correlated with the amount of movement, just as its expression typically correlates with the amount of singing performed in vocal birds. These finding suggest that vocal learning brain regions developed from the same cell lineages that gave rise to the motor pathway, which then formed a direct projection onto the brainstem vocal motor neurons to provide greater control.[1]

Currently, it remains unclear as to which of these hypotheses is the most accurate.

Primates edit

 
Primate phylogenetic tree and complex-vocal learning vs. auditory sequence learning. Shown is a primate phylogenetic tree based on a combination of DNA sequence and fossil age data (Goodman et al., 1998; Page et al., 1999). Humans (Homo) are the only primates classified as “vocal learners.” However, non-human primates might be better at auditory sequence learning than their limited vocal-production learning capabilities would suggest. In blue text and (#) we highlight species for which there is some evidence of Artificial Grammar Learning capabilities for at least adjacent relationships between the elements in a sequence (tamarins: Fitch and Hauser, 2004), (macaques: Wilson et al., 2011). Presuming that the auditory capabilities of guenons and gibbons (or the symbolic learning of signs by apes) would mean that these animals are able to learn at least adjacent relationships in Artificial Grammars we can tentatively mark these species also in blue #. Note however, that for the species labeled in black text, future studies might show them to be capable of some limited-vocal learning or various levels of complexity in learning the structure of auditory sequences. Three not mutually exclusive hypotheses are illustrated for both complex-vocal learning and auditory sequence learning. From Petkov, CI; Jarvis ED (2012). "Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates". Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4:12.

In primates, only humans are known to be capable of complex vocal learning. Similar to the first hypothesis relating to birds, one explanation is that vocal learning evolved independently in humans. An alternative hypothesis suggests evolution from a primate common ancestor capable of vocal learning, with the trait subsequently being lost at least eight other times. Considering the most parsimonious analysis, it seems unlikely that the number of independent gains (one in humans) would be exceeded so greatly by the number of independent losses (at least eight), which supports the independent evolution hypothesis.[1]

Neurobiology edit

Neural pathways in avian vocal learners edit

As avian vocal learners are the most amenable to experimental manipulations, the vast majority of work to elucidate the neurobiological mechanisms of vocal learning has been conducted with zebra finches, with a few studies focusing on budgerigars and other species. Despite variation in vocal learning phenotype, the neural circuitry necessary for producing learned song is conserved in songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds. As opposed to their non-learner avian counterparts such as quail, doves, and pigeons, these avian vocal learners contain seven distinct cerebral song nuclei, or distinct brain areas associated with auditory learning and song production defined by their gene expression patterns. As current evidence suggests independent evolution of these structures, the names of each equivalent vocal nucleus are different per bird group, as shown in the table below.

Parallel Song Nuclei in Avian Vocal Learners
Songbirds Parrots Hummingbirds
HVC: a letter based name NLC: central nucleus of the lateral nidopallium VLN: vocal nucleus of the lateral nidopallium
RA: robust nucleus of the arcopallium AAC: central nucleus of the anterior arcopallium VA: vocal nucleus of the arcopallium
MAN: magnocellular nucleus of anterior nidopallium NAOc: oval nucleus of the anterior nidopallium complex
Area X: area X of the striatum MMSt: magnocellular nucleus of the anterior striatum
DLM: medial nucleus of dorsolateral thalamus DMM: magnocellular nucleus of the dorsomedial thalamus
MO: oval nucleus of the mesopallium MOc: oval nucleus of the mesopallium complex

Vocal nuclei are found in two separate brain pathways, which will be described in songbirds as most research has been conducted in this group, yet connections are similar in parrots[35] and hummingbirds.[36] Projections of the anterior vocal pathway in the hummingbird remain unclear and so are not listed in the table above.

The posterior vocal pathway (also known as vocal motor pathway), involved in the production of learned vocalizations, begins with projections from a nidopallial nucleus, the HVC in songbirds. The HVC then projects to the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA). The RA connects to the midbrain vocal center DM (dorsal medial nucleus of the midbrain) and the brainstem (nXIIts) vocal motor neurons that control the muscles of the syrinx, a direct projection similar to the projection from LMC to the nucleus ambiguus in humans[1][37] The HVC is considered the syntax generator while the RA modulates the acoustic structure of syllables. Vocal non-learners do possess the DM and twelfth motor neurons (nXIIts), but lack the connections to the arcopallium. As a result, they can produce vocalizations, but not learned vocalizations.

The anterior vocal pathway (also known as vocal learning pathway) is associated with learning, syntax, and social contexts, starting with projections from the magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium (MAN) to the striatal nucleus Area X. Area X then projects to the medial nucleus of dorsolateral thalamus (DLM), which ultimately projects back to MAN in a loop[38] The lateral part of MAN (LMAN) generates variability in song, while Area X is responsible for stereotypy, or the generation of low variability in syllable production and order after song crystallization.[32]

Despite the similarities in vocal learning neural circuits, there are some major connectivity differences between the posterior and anterior pathways among avian vocal learners. In songbirds, the posterior pathway communicates with the anterior pathway via projections from the HVC to Area X; the anterior pathway sends output to the posterior pathway via connections from LMAN to RA and medial MAN (MMAN) to HVC. Parrots, on the other hand, have projections from the ventral part of the AAC (AACv), the parallel of the songbird RA, to the NAOc, parallel of the songbird MAN, and the oval nucleus of the mesopallium (MO). The anterior pathway in parrots connects to the posterior pathway via NAOc projections to the NLC, parallel of the songbird HVC, and AAC. Thus, parrots do not send projections to the striatal nucleus of the anterior pathway from their posterior pathway as do songbirds. Another crucial difference is the location of the posterior vocal nuclei among species. Posterior nuclei are located in auditory regions for songbirds, laterally adjacent to auditory regions in hummingbirds, and are physically separate from auditory regions in parrots. Axons must therefore take different routes to connect nuclei in different vocal learning species. Exactly how these connectivity differences affect song production and/or vocal learning ability remains unclear.[32][39]

An auditory pathway that is used for auditory learning brings auditory information into the vocal pathway, but the auditory pathway is not unique to vocal learners. Ear hair cells project to cochlear ganglia neurons to auditory pontine nuclei to midbrain and thalamic nuclei and to primary and secondary pallial areas. A descending auditory feedback pathway exists projecting from the dorsal nidopallium to the intermediate arcopallium to shell regions around the thalamic and midbrain auditory nuclei. Remaining unclear is the source of auditory input into the vocal pathways described above. It is hypothesized that songs are processed in these areas in a hierarchical manner, with the primary pallial area responsible for acoustic features (field L2), the secondary pallial area (fields L1 and L3 as well as the caudal medial nidopallium or NCM) determining sequencing and discrimination, and the highest station, the caudal mesopallium (CM), modulating fine discrimination of sounds. Secondary pallial areas including the NCM and CM are also thought to be involved in auditory memory formation of songs used for vocal learning, but more evidence is needed to substantiate this hypothesis.[32]

Critical period edit

The development of the sensory modalities necessary for song learning occurs within a “critical period” of development that varies among avian vocal learners. Closed-ended learners such as the zebra finch and aphantochroa hummingbird can only learn during a limited time period and subsequently produce highly stereotyped or non-variable vocalizations consisting of a single, fixed song which they repeat their entire lives. In contrast, open-ended learners, including canaries and various parrot species, display significant plasticity and continue to learn new songs throughout the course of their lives.[40]

In the male zebra finch, vocal learning begins with a period of sensory acquisition or auditory learning where juveniles are exposed to the song of an adult male “tutor” at about posthatch day 30 to 60.[41] During this stage, juveniles listen and memorize the song pattern of their tutor and produce subsong, characterized by the production of highly variable syllables and syllable sequences. Subsong is thought to be analogous to babbling in human infants. Subsequently during the sensorimotor learning phase at posthatch day 35 to 90, juveniles practice the motor commands required for song production and use auditory feedback to alter vocalizations to match the song template. Songs during this period are plastic as specific syllables begin to emerge but are frequently in the wrong sequence, errors that are similar to phonological mistakes made by young children when learning a language. As the bird ages, its song becomes more stereotyped until at posthatch day 120 the song syllables and sequence are crystallized or fixed. At this point, the zebra finch can no longer learn new songs and thus sings this single song for the duration of its life.[42]

The neural mechanisms behind the closing of the critical period remain unclear, but early deprivation of juveniles from their adult tutors has been shown to extend the critical period of song acquisition[43] “Synapse selection” theories hypothesize that synaptic plasticity during the critical period is gradually reduced as dendritic spines are pruned through activity-dependent synaptic rearrangement[44] The pruning of dendritic spines in the LMAN song nucleus was delayed in isolated zebra finches with extended critical periods, suggesting that this form of synaptic reorganization may be important in closing the critical period.[45] However, other studies have shown that birds reared normally as well as isolated juveniles have similar levels of dendritic pruning despite an extended critical period in the latter group,[46] demonstrating that this theory does not completely explain critical period modulation.

Previous research has suggested that the length of the critical period may be linked to differential gene expression within song nuclei, thought to be caused by neurotransmitter binding of receptors during neural activation.[47] One key area is the LMAN song nucleus, part of the specialized cortical-basal-ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop in the anterior forebrain pathway, which is essential for vocal plasticity.[38] While inducing deafness in songbirds usually disrupts the sensory phase of learning and leads to production of highly abnormal song structures, lesioning of LMAN in zebra finches prevents this song deterioration,[48] leading to the earlier development of stable song. One of the neurotransmitter receptors shown to affect LMAN is the N- methyl-D-aspartate glutamate receptor (NMDAR), which is required for learning and activity-dependent gene regulation in the post-synaptic neuron. Infusions of the NMDAR antagonist APV (R-2-amino-5-phosphonopentanoate) into the LMAN song nucleus disrupts the critical period in the zebra finch.[49] NMDAR density and mRNA levels of the NR1 subunit also decrease in LMAN during early song development.[50] When the song becomes crystallized, expression of the NR2B subunit decreases in LMAN and NMDAR-mediated synaptic currents shorten.[51] It has been hypothesized that LMAN actively maintains RA microcircuitry in a state permissive for song plasticity and in a process of normal development it regulates HVC-RA synapses.

In humans edit

 
Vocalization subsystems in complex-vocal learners and in limited-vocal learners or vocal non-learners: Direct and indirect pathways. The different subsystems for vocalization and their interconnectivity are illustrated using different colors. (A) Schematic of a songbird brain showing some connectivity of the four major song nuclei (HVC, RA, AreaX, and LMAN). (B) Human brain schematic showing the different proposed vocal subsystems. The learned vocalization subsystem consists of a primary motor cortex pathway (blue arrow) and a cortico-striatal-thalamic loop for learning vocalizations (white). Also shown is the limbic vocal subsystem that is broadly conserved in primates for producing innate vocalizations (black), and the motoneurons that control laryngeal muscles (red). (C) Known connectivity of a brainstem vocal system (not all connections shown) showing absence of forebrain song nuclei in vocal non-learning birds. (D) Known connectivity of limited-vocal learning monkeys (based on data in squirrel monkeys and macaques) showing presence of forebrain regions for innate vocalization (ACC, OFC, and amygdala) and also of a ventral premotor area (Area 6vr) of currently poorly understood function that is indirectly connected to nucleus ambiguous. The LMC in humans is directly connected with motoneurons in the nucleus ambiguus, which orchestrate the production of learned vocalizations. Only the direct pathway through the mammalian basal ganglia (ASt, anterior striatum; GPi, globus palidus, internal) is shown as this is the one most similar to AreaX connectivity in songbirds. Modified figure based on (Jarvis, 2004; Jarvis et al., 2005). Abbreviations: ACC, anterior cingulate cortex; Am, nucleus ambiguus; Amyg, amygdala; AT, anterior thalamus; Av, nucleus avalanche; DLM, dorsolateral nucleus of the medial thalamus; DM, dorsal medial nucleus of the midbrain; HVC, high vocal center; LMAN, lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium; LMC, laryngeal motor cortex; OFC, orbito-frontal cortex; PAG, periaqueductal gray; RA, robust nucleus of the arcopallium; RF, reticular formation; vPFC, ventral prefrontal cortex; VLT, ventro-lateral division of thalamus; XIIts, bird twelfth nerve nucleus. From Petkov, CI; Jarvis ED (2012). "Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates". Front. Evol. Neurosci. 4:12.

Humans seem to have analogous anterior and posterior vocal pathways which are implicated in speech production and learning. Parallel to the avian posterior vocal pathway mentioned above is the motor cortico-brainstem pathway. Within this pathway, the face motor cortex projects to the nucleus ambiguous of the medulla, which then projects to the muscles of the larynx. Humans also have a vocal pathway that is analogous to the avian anterior pathway. This pathway is a cortico-basal ganglia-thalamic-cortico loop which begins at a strip of the premotor cortex, called the cortical strip, which is responsible for speech learning and syntax production. The cortical strip includes spans across five brain regions: the anterior insula, Broca's area, the anterior dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex, the anterior pre-supplementary motor area, and the anterior cingulate cortex. This cortical strip has projections to the anterior striatum which projects to the globus pallidus to the anterior dorsal thalamus back to the cortical strip. All of these regions are also involved in syntax and speech learning.[52]

Genetic applications to humans edit

In addition to the similarities in the neurobiological circuits necessary for vocalizations between animal vocal learners and humans, there are also a few genetic similarities. The most prominent of these genetic links are the FOXP1 and FOXP2 genes, which code for forkhead box (FOX) proteins P1 and P2, respectively. FOXP1 and FOXP2 are transcription factors which play a role in the development and maturation of the lungs, heart, and brain,[53][54] and are also highly expressed in brain regions of the vocal learning pathway, including the basal ganglia and the frontal cortex. In these regions (i.e. the basal ganglia and frontal cortex), FOXP1 and FOXP2 are thought to be essential for brain maturation and development of speech and language.[54]

Orthologues of FOXP2 are found in a number of vertebrates including mice and songbirds, and have been implicated in modulating plasticity of neural circuits. In fact, although mammals and birds are very distant relatives and diverged more than 300 million years ago, the FOXP2 gene in zebra finches and mice differs at only five amino acid positions, and differs between zebra finches and humans at only eight amino acid positions. In addition, researchers have found that patterns of expression of FOXP1 and FOXP2 are amazingly similar in the human fetal brain and the songbird.[54]

These similarities are especially interesting in the context of the aforementioned avian song circuit. FOXP2 is expressed in the avian Area X, and is especially highly expressed in the striatum during the critical period of song plasticity in songbirds. In humans, FOXP2 is highly expressed in the basal ganglia, frontal cortex, and insular cortex, all thought to be important nodes in the human vocal pathway. Thus, mutations in the FOXP2 gene are proposed to have detrimental effects on human speech and language, such as grammar, language processing, and impaired movement of the mouth, lips, and tongue,[55] as well as potential detrimental effects on song learning in songbirds. Indeed, FOXP2 was the first gene to be implicated in the cognition of speech and language in a family of individuals with a severe speech and language disorder.

Additionally, it has been suggested that due to the overlap of FOXP1 and FOXP2 expression in songbirds and humans, mutations in FOXP1 may also result in speech and language abnormalities seen in individuals with mutations in FOXP2.[53]

These genetic links have important implications for studying the origin of language because FOXP2 is so similar among vocal learners and humans, as well as important implications for understanding the etiology of certain speech and language disorders in humans.

Currently, no other genes have been linked as compellingly to vocal learning in animals or humans.

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g Petkov CI, Jarvis ED (2012). "Birds, primates, and spoken language origins: behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates". Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience. 4: 12. doi:10.3389/fnevo.2012.00012. PMC 3419981. PMID 22912615.
  2. ^ Okanoya K (June 2004). "The Bengalese finch: a window on the behavioral neurobiology of birdsong syntax". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1016 (1): 724–35. Bibcode:2004NYASA1016..724O. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.026. PMID 15313802. S2CID 21327383.
  3. ^ Hurford JR (2012). The Origins of Grammar: Language in the Light of Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  4. ^ Honda E, Okanoya K (1999). "Acoustical and syntactical com- parisons between songs of the white-backed Munia (Lonchura striata) and its domesticated strain, the Bengalese finch (Lonchura striata var. domestica)". Zool. Sci. 16 (2): 319–326. doi:10.2108/zsj.16.319. S2CID 85306560.
  5. ^ Schachner A, Brady TF, Pepperberg IM, Hauser MD (May 2009). "Spontaneous motor entrainment to music in multiple vocal mimicking species". Current Biology. 19 (10): 831–6. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.03.061. hdl:1721.1/96194. PMID 19409786. S2CID 6150861.
  6. ^ Lattenkamp, Ella Z.; Vernes, Sonja C. (2018-06-01). "Vocal learning: a language-relevant trait in need of a broad cross-species approach". Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 21: 209–215. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.04.007. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-237D-C. ISSN 2352-1546. S2CID 13809803.
  7. ^ Martins PT, Boeckx C (March 2020). "Vocal learning: Beyond the continuum". PLOS Biology. 18 (3): e3000672. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000672. PMC 7145199. PMID 32226012.
  8. ^ Esser KH (September 1994). "Audio-vocal learning in a non-human mammal: the lesser spear-nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor". NeuroReport. 5 (14): 1718–20. doi:10.1097/00001756-199409080-00007. PMID 7827315.
  9. ^ Boughman JW (February 1998). "Vocal learning by greater spear-nosed bats". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 265 (1392): 227–33. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0286. PMC 1688873. PMID 9493408.
  10. ^ Noad MJ, Cato DH, Bryden MM, Jenner MN, Jenner KC (November 2000). "Cultural revolution in whale songs". Nature. 408 (6812): 537. Bibcode:2000Natur.408..537N. doi:10.1038/35046199. PMID 11117730. S2CID 4398582.
  11. ^ Foote AD, Griffin RM, Howitt D, Larsson L, Miller PJ, Hoelzel AR (December 2006). "Killer whales are capable of vocal learning". Biology Letters. 2 (4): 509–12. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0525. PMC 1834009. PMID 17148275.
  12. ^ Lilly JC (January 1965). "Vocal Mimicry in Tursiops: Ability to Match Numbers and Durations of Human Vocal Bursts". Science. 147 (3655): 300–1. Bibcode:1965Sci...147..300L. doi:10.1126/science.147.3655.300. PMID 17788214. S2CID 2038221.
  13. ^ Reiss D, McCowan B (September 1993). "Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): evidence for vocal learning". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 107 (3): 301–12. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.107.3.301. PMID 8375147.
  14. ^ Herman LM, Richards DG, Wolz JP (March 1984). "Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins". Cognition. 16 (2): 129–219. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(84)90003-9. PMID 6540652. S2CID 43237011.
  15. ^ Janik VM (August 2000). "Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)". Science. 289 (5483): 1355–7. Bibcode:2000Sci...289.1355J. doi:10.1126/science.289.5483.1355. PMID 10958783. Comparative studies indicate that whistles vary in structure across populations and species (Steiner 1981; Ding et al. 1995; Rendell et al. 1999), with whistle divergence perhaps facilitating species recognition and speciation (Podos et al. 2002).
  16. ^ Caldwell DK (1977). How animals communicate.
  17. ^ Janik VM, Sayigh LS (June 2013). "Communication in bottlenose dolphins: 50 years of signature whistle research". Journal of Comparative Physiology A: Neuroethology, Sensory, Neural & Behavioral Physiology. 199 (6): 479–89. doi:10.1007/s00359-013-0817-7. PMID 23649908. S2CID 15378374.
  18. ^ Caldwell DK. How Animals Communicate. p. 794.
  19. ^ Ralls K, Fiorelli P, Gish S (1985). "Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbour seals, Phoca vitulina". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 63 (5): 1050–1056. doi:10.1139/z85-157.
  20. ^ Sanvito S, Galimberti F, Miller EH (2007). "Observational evidences of vocal learning in southern elephant seals: a longitudinal study". Ethology. 113 (2): 137–146. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01306.x.
  21. ^ Poole JH, Tyack PL, Stoeger-Horwath AS, Watwood S (March 2005). "Animal behaviour: elephants are capable of vocal learning". Nature. 434 (7032): 455–6. Bibcode:2005Natur.434..455P. doi:10.1038/434455a. PMID 15791244. S2CID 4369863.
  22. ^ a b Cheney DL, Owren MJ, Dieter JA, Seyfarth RM (January 1992). "'Food' calls produced by adult female rhesus (Macaca mulatta) and Japanese (M. fuscata) macaques, their normally-raised offspring, and offspring cross-fostered between species". Behaviour. 120 (3–4): 218–31. doi:10.1163/156853992X00615.
  23. ^ Masataka N, Fujita K (January 1989). "Vocal learning of Japanese and rhesus monkeys". Behaviour. 109 (3–4): 191–9. doi:10.1163/156853989X00222.
  24. ^ a b c Snowdon, CT. (2009). "Plasticity of communication in non-human primates," in Advances in the Study of Behavior, eds M. Naguib and V. M. Janik (Burlington, NJ: Academic Press), 239–276.
  25. ^ a b Hopkins WD, Taglialatela J, Leavens DA (February 2007). "Chimpanzees Differentially Produce Novel Vocalizations to Capture the Attention of a Human". Animal Behaviour. 73 (2): 281–286. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.08.004. PMC 1832264. PMID 17389908.
  26. ^ a b Wich SA, Swartz KB, Hardus ME, Lameira AR, Stromberg E, Shumaker RW (January 2009). "A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan". Primates; Journal of Primatology. 50 (1): 56–64. doi:10.1007/s10329-008-0117-y. PMID 19052691. S2CID 708682.
  27. ^ Holy TE, Guo Z (December 2005). "Ultrasonic songs of male mice". PLOS Biology. 3 (12): e386. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030386. PMC 1275525. PMID 16248680.
  28. ^ Kikusui T, Nakanishi K, Nakagawa R, Nagasawa M, Mogi K, Okanoya K (March 2011). "Cross fostering experiments suggest that mice songs are innate". PLOS ONE. 6 (3): e17721. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...617721K. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0017721. PMC 3052373. PMID 21408017.
  29. ^ a b Arriaga G, Zhou EP, Jarvis ED (2012). "Of mice, birds, and men: the mouse ultrasonic song system has some features similar to humans and song-learning birds". PLOS ONE. 7 (10): e46610. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...746610A. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046610. PMC 346858. PMID 23071596.
  30. ^ Arriaga G, Jarvis ED (January 2013). "Mouse vocal communication system: are ultrasounds learned or innate?". Brain and Language. 124 (1): 96–116. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.002. PMC 3886250. PMID 23295209.
  31. ^ Mahrt EJ, Perkel DJ, Tong L, Rubel EW, Portfors CV (March 2013). "Engineered deafness reveals that mouse courtship vocalizations do not require auditory experience". The Journal of Neuroscience. 33 (13): 5573–83. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5054-12.2013. PMC 3691057. PMID 23536072.
  32. ^ a b c d e Jarvis ED (2009). "Bird Song Systems: Evolution". Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. 2: 217–225. doi:10.1016/b978-008045046-9.00935-9. hdl:10161/11242. ISBN 9780080450469.
  33. ^ Zann RA (1996). The Zebra Finch: A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies. New York: Oxford University Press.
  34. ^ Feenders G, Liedvogel M, Rivas M, Zapka M, Horita H, Hara E, et al. (March 2008). "Molecular mapping of movement-associated areas in the avian brain: a motor theory for vocal learning origin". PLOS ONE. 3 (3): e1768. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.1768F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001768. PMC 2258151. PMID 18335043.
  35. ^ Jarvis ED, Mello CV (March 2000). "Molecular mapping of brain areas involved in parrot vocal communication". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 419 (1): 1–31. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000327)419:1<1::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-m. PMC 2538445. PMID 10717637.
  36. ^ Jarvis ED, Ribeiro S, da Silva ML, Ventura D, Vielliard J, Mello CV (August 2000). "Behaviourally driven gene expression reveals song nuclei in hummingbird brain". Nature. 406 (6796): 628–32. Bibcode:2000Natur.406..628J. doi:10.1038/35020570. PMC 2531203. PMID 10949303.
  37. ^ Jarvis ED (June 2004). "Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1016 (1): 749–77. Bibcode:2004NYASA1016..749J. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.038. PMC 2485240. PMID 15313804.
  38. ^ a b Schraff C, Nottebohm F (1991). "A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system: implications for vocal learning". J Neurosci. 11 (9): 2896–2913. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.11-09-02896.1991. PMC 6575264. PMID 1880555.
  39. ^ Wild JM (1997). "Neural pathways for the control of birdsong production". Journal of Neurobiology. 33 (5): 653–670. doi:10.1002/(sici)1097-4695(19971105)33:5<653::aid-neu11>3.0.co;2-a. PMID 9369465.
  40. ^ Brenowitz EA, Beecher MD (March 2005). "Song learning in birds: diversity and plasticity, opportunities and challenges". Trends in Neurosciences. 28 (3): 127–32. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.01.004. PMID 15749165. S2CID 14586913.
  41. ^ Eales LA (1985). "Song learning in zebra finches: some effects of song model availability on what is learnt and when". Anim Behav. 33 (4): 1293–1300. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(85)80189-5. S2CID 54229759.
  42. ^ Jones AE, Ten Cate C, Slater PJ (December 1996). "Early experience and plasticity of song in adult male Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 110 (4): 354–69. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.110.4.354.
  43. ^ Aamodt SM, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW (May 1995). "Early isolation from conspecific song does not affect the normal developmental decline of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor binding in an avian song nucleus". Journal of Neurobiology. 27 (1): 76–84. doi:10.1002/neu.480270108. PMID 7643077.
  44. ^ Bischof HJ, Geissler E, Rollenhagen A (2002). "Limitations of the sensitive period for sexual imprinting: Neuroanatomical and behavioral experiments in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata)". Behavioural Brain Research. 133 (2): 317–322. doi:10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00016-5. PMID 12110465. S2CID 28662586.
  45. ^ Wallhäusser-Franke E, Nixdorf-Bergweiler BE, DeVoogd TJ (July 1995). "Song isolation is associated with maintaining high spine frequencies on zebra finch 1MAN neurons". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 64 (1): 25–35. doi:10.1006/nlme.1995.1041. PMID 7582809. S2CID 19096150.
  46. ^ Heinrich JE, Nordeen KW, Nordeen EJ (March 2005). "Dissociation between extension of the sensitive period for avian vocal learning and dendritic spine loss in the song nucleus lMAN". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 83 (2): 143–50. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2004.11.002. PMID 15721798. S2CID 8953614.
  47. ^ Wada K, Sakaguchi H, Jarvis ED, Hagiwara M (August 2004). "Differential expression of glutamate receptors in avian neural pathways for learned vocalization". The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 476 (1): 44–64. doi:10.1002/cne.20201. PMC 2517240. PMID 15236466.
  48. ^ Brainard MS, Doupe AJ (2001). "Postlearning consolidation of birdsong: stabilizing effects of age and anterior forebrain lesions". J Neurosci. 21 (7): 2501–2517. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-07-02501.2001. PMC 6762407. PMID 11264324.
  49. ^ Basham ME, Nordeen EJ, Nordeen KW (November 1996). "Blockade of NMDA receptors in the anterior forebrain impairs sensory acquisition in the zebra finch (Poephila guttata)". Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 66 (3): 295–304. doi:10.1006/nlme.1996.0071. PMID 8946423. S2CID 9592089.
  50. ^ Heinrich JE, Singh TD, Sohrabji F, Nordeen KW, Nordeen EJ (May 2002). "Developmental and hormonal regulation of NR2A mRNA in forebrain regions controlling avian vocal learning". Journal of Neurobiology. 51 (2): 149–59. doi:10.1002/neu.10046. PMID 11932956.
  51. ^ Livingston FS, Mooney R (1997). "Development of intrinsic and synaptic properties in a forebrain nucleus essential to avian song learning". J Neurosci. 17 (23): 8997–9009. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-23-08997.1997. PMC 6573603. PMID 9364047.
  52. ^ Jarvis ED (December 2007). "Neural systems for vocal learning in birds and humans: a synopsis". Journal of Ornithology. 148 (1): 35–44. doi:10.1007/s10336-007-0243-0. PMC 2726745. PMID 19684872.
  53. ^ a b Pariani MJ, Spencer A, Graham JM, Rimoin DL (2009). "A 785kb deletion of 3p14.1p13, including the FOXP1 gene, associated with speech delay, contractures, hypertonia and blepharophimosis". European Journal of Medical Genetics. 52 (2–3): 123–7. doi:10.1016/j.ejmg.2009.03.012. PMC 2853231. PMID 19332160.
  54. ^ a b c Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M (February 2005). "FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language". Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 6 (2): 131–8. doi:10.1038/nrn1605. PMID 15685218. S2CID 2504002.
  55. ^ Takahashi K, Liu FC, Hirokawa K, Takahashi H (July 2003). "Expression of Foxp2, a gene involved in speech and language, in the developing and adult striatum". Journal of Neuroscience Research. 73 (1): 61–72. doi:10.1002/jnr.10638. PMID 12815709. S2CID 31971989.

vocal, learning, ability, modify, acoustic, syntactic, sounds, acquire, sounds, imitation, produce, vocalizations, vocalizations, this, case, refers, only, sounds, generated, vocal, organ, mammalian, larynx, avian, syrinx, opposed, lips, teeth, tongue, which, . Vocal learning is the ability to modify acoustic and syntactic sounds acquire new sounds via imitation and produce vocalizations Vocalizations in this case refers only to sounds generated by the vocal organ mammalian larynx or avian syrinx as opposed to by the lips teeth and tongue which require substantially less motor control 1 A rare trait vocal learning is a critical substrate for spoken language and has only been detected in eight animal groups despite the wide array of vocalizing species these include humans bats cetaceans pinnipeds seals and sea lions elephants and three distantly related bird groups including songbirds parrots and hummingbirds Vocal learning is distinct from auditory learning or the ability to form memories of sounds heard a relatively common trait which is present in all vertebrates tested For example dogs can be trained to understand the word sit even though the human word is not in its innate auditory repertoire auditory learning However the dog cannot imitate and produce the word sit itself as vocal learners can Contents 1 Classification 1 1 Continuum hypothesis 2 Evidence of vocal learning in various species 2 1 Known vocal learners 2 1 1 Birds 2 1 2 Bats 2 1 3 Cetaceans 2 1 3 1 Whales 2 1 3 2 Dolphins 2 1 4 Seals 2 1 5 Elephants 2 2 Controversial or limited vocal learners 2 2 1 Non human primates 2 2 2 Mice 2 2 3 Goats 3 Evolution 3 1 Adaptive advantage 3 2 Predatory pressure 3 3 Phylogeny 3 3 1 Birds 3 3 2 Primates 4 Neurobiology 4 1 Neural pathways in avian vocal learners 4 2 Critical period 4 3 In humans 5 Genetic applications to humans 6 See also 7 ReferencesClassification edit nbsp Hypothetical distributions of two behavioral phenotypes vocal learning and sensory auditory sequence learning We hypothesize that the behavioral phenotypes of vocal learning and auditory learning are distributed along several categories original research A Vocal learning complexity phenotype and B auditory sequence learning phenotype The left axis blue illustrates the hypothetical distribution of species along the behavioral phenotype dimensions The right axis black step functions illustrates different types of transitions along the hypothesized vocal learning A or auditory learning B complexity dimensions Whether the actual distributions are continuous functions blue curves will need to be tested in relation to the alternatives that there are several categories with gradual transitions or step functions black curves Although auditory learning is a prerequisite for vocal learning and there can be a correlation between the two phenotypes A B the two need not be interdependent A theoretical Turing machine Turing 1968 is illustrated G which can outperform humans on memory for digitized auditory input but is not a vocal learner From Petkov CI Jarvis ED 2012 Birds primates and spoken language origins behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates Front Evol Neurosci 4 12 Historically species have been classified into the binary categories of vocal learner or vocal non learner based on their ability to produce novel vocalizations or imitate other species with evidence from social isolation deafening studies and cross fostering experiments 1 However vocal learners exhibit a great deal of plasticity or variation between species resulting in a spectrum of ability The vocalizations of songbirds and whales have a syntactic like organization similar to that of humans but are limited to Finite State Grammars FSGs where they can generate strings of sequences with limited structural complexity 2 Humans on the other hand show deeper hierarchical relationships such as the nesting of phrases within others and demonstrate compositional syntax where changes in syntactic organization generate new meanings both of which are beyond the capabilities of other vocal learning groups 3 Vocal learning phenotype also differ within groups and closely related species will not display the same abilities Within avian vocal learners for example zebra finch songs only contain strictly linear transitions that go through different syllables in a motif from beginning to end yet mockingbird and nightingale songs show element repetition within a range of legal repetitions non adjacent relationships between distant song elements and forward and backward branching in song element transitions 4 Parrots are even more complex as they can imitate the speech of heterospecifics like humans and synchronize their movements to a rhythmic beat 5 Continuum hypothesis edit Even further complicating the original binary classification is evidence from recent studies that suggests that there is greater variability in a non learner s ability to modify vocalizations based on experience than previously thought Findings in suboscine passerine birds non human primates mice and goats has led to the proposal of the vocal learning continuum hypothesis by Erich Jarvis and Gustavo Arriaga Based on the apparent variations seen in various studies the continuum hypothesis reclassifies species into non learner limited vocal learner moderate vocal learning complex vocal learner and high vocal learner categories where higher tiers have fewer species Under this system previously identified non human vocal learners like songbirds are considered complex learners while humans fall under the high category non human primates mice and goats which are traditionally classified as non learners are considered limited vocal learners under this system 1 Recent work while generally acknowledging the usefulness of this richer view of vocal learning has pointed out conceptual and empirical limitations of the vocal learning continuum hypothesis suggesting more species and factors should be taken into account 6 7 Evidence of vocal learning in various species editKnown vocal learners edit Birds edit See also avian brain The most extensively studied model organisms of vocal learning are found in birds namely songbirds parrots and hummingbirds The degree of vocal learning in each specific species varies While many parrots and certain songbirds like canaries can imitate and spontaneously combine learned sounds during all periods of their life other songbirds and hummingbirds are limited to a certain songs learned during their critical period Bats edit The first evidence for audio vocal learning in a non human mammal was produced by Karl Heinz Esser in 1994 Hand reared infant lesser spear nosed bats Phyllostomos discolor were able to adapt their isolation calls to an external reference signal Isolation calls in a control group that had no reference signal did not show the same adaptation 8 Further evidence for vocal learning in bats appeared in 1998 when Janette Wenrick Boughman studied female greater spear nosed bats Phyllostomus hastatus These bats live in unrelated groups and use group contact calls that differ among social groups Each social group has a single call which differs in frequency and temporal characteristics When individual bats were introduced to a new social group the group call began to morph taking on new frequency and temporal characteristics and over time calls of transfer and resident bats in the same group more closely resembled their new modified call than their old calls 9 Cetaceans edit Whales edit Male humpback whales Megaptera novaeangliae sing as a form of sexual display while migrating to and from their breeding grounds All males in a population produce the same song which can change over time indicating vocal learning and cultural transmission a characteristic shared by some bird populations Songs become increasingly dissimilar over distance and populations in different oceans have dissimilar songs Whale songs recorded along the east coast of Australia in 1996 showed introduction of a novel song by two foreign whales who had migrated from the west Australian coast to the east Australian coast In just two years all members of the population had switched songs This new song was nearly identical to ones sung by migrating humpback whales on the west Australian Coast and the two new singers who introduced the song are hypothesized to have introduced the new foreign song to the population on the east Australian coast 10 Vocal learning has also been seen in killer whales Orcinus orca Two juvenile killer whales separated from their natal pods were seen mimicking cries of California sea lions Zalophus californianus that were near the region they lived in The composition of the calls of these two juveniles were also different from their natal groups reflecting more of the sea lion calls than that of the whales 11 Dolphins edit Captive bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus can be trained to emit sounds through their blowhole in open air Through training these vocal emissions can be altered from natural patterns to resemble sounds like the human voice measurable through the number of bursts of sound emitted by the dolphin In 92 of exchanges between humans and dolphins the number of bursts equaled 1 of the number of syllables spoken by a human 12 Another study used an underwater keyboard to demonstrate that dolphins are able to learn various whistles in order to do an activity or obtain an object Complete mimicry occurred within ten attempts for these trained dolphins 13 Other studies of dolphins have given even more evidence of spontaneous mimicry of species specific whistles and other biological and computer generated signals 14 Such vocal learning has also been identified in wild bottlenose dolphins Bottlenose dolphins develop a distinct signature whistle in the first few months of life which is used to identify and distinguish itself from other individuals This individual distinctiveness could have been a driving force for evolution by providing higher species fitness since complex communication is largely correlated with increased intelligence However vocal identification is present in vocal non learners as well Therefore it is unlikely that individual identification was a primary driving force for the evolution of vocal learning Each signature whistle can be learned by other individuals for identification purposes and are used primarily when the dolphin in question is out of sight Bottlenose dolphins use their learned whistles in matching interactions which are likely to be used while addressing each other signalling alliance membership to a third party or preventing deception by an imitating dolphin 15 Mate attraction and territory defense have also been seen as possible contributors to vocal learning evolution Studies on this topic point out that while both vocal learners and non learners use vocalizations to attract mates or defend territories there is one key difference variability Vocal learners can produce a more varied arrangement of vocalizations and frequencies which studies show may be more preferred by females For example Caldwell 16 observed that male Atlantic bottlenose dolphins may initiate a challenge by facing another dolphin opening its mouth thereby exposing its teeth or arching its back slightly and holding its head downward This behavior is more along the lines of visual communication but still may or may not be accompanied by vocalizations such as burst pulsed sounds The burst pulsed sounds which are more complex and varied than the whistles are often utilized to convey excitement dominance or aggression such as when they are competing for the same piece of food 17 The dolphins also produce these forceful sounds when in the presence of other individuals moving towards the same prey On the sexual side Caldwell saw that dolphins may solicit a sexual response from another by swimming in front of it looking back and rolling on its side to display the genital region 18 These observations provide yet another example of visual communication where dolphins exhibit different postures and non vocal behaviors to communicate with others that also may or may not be accompanied by vocalizations Sexual selection for greater variability and thus in turn vocal learning may then be a major driving force for the evolution of vocal learning Seals edit Captive harbor seals Phoca vitulina were recorded mimicking human words such as hello Hoover the seal s own name and producing other speech like sounds Most of the vocalizations occurred during the reproductive season 19 More evidence of vocal learning in seals occurs in southern elephant seals Mirounga leonine Young males imitate the vocal cries of successful older males during their breeding season northern and southern elephant seals have a highly polygynous mating system with a vast disparity in mating success In other words few males guard huge harems of females eliciting intense male male competition Antagonistic vocal cries play an important role in inter male competitions and are hypothesized to demonstrate the resource holding potential of the emitter In both species antagonistic vocal cries vary geographically and are structurally complex and individually distinct Males displays unique calls which can be identified by the specific arrangement of syllable and syllable parts Harem holders frequently vocalize to keep peripheral males away from females and these vocalizations are the dominant component in a young juvenile s acoustic habitat Successful vocalizations are heard by juveniles who then imitate these calls as they get older in an attempt to obtain a harem for themselves Novel vocal types expressed by dominant males spread quickly through populations of breeding elephant seals and are even imitated by juveniles in the same season Genetic analysis indicated that successful vocal patterns were not passed down hereditarily indicating that this behavior is learned Progeny of successful harem holders do not display their father s vocal calls and the call that makes one male successful often disappears entirely from the population 20 Elephants edit Mlaika a ten year old adolescent female African elephant has been recorded imitating truck sounds coming from the Nairobi Mombasa highway three miles away Analysis of Mlaika s truck like calls show that they are different from the normal calls of African elephants and that her calls are a general model of truck sounds not copies of the sounds of trucks recorded at the same time of the calls In other words Mlaika s truck calls are not imitations of the trucks that she hears but rather a generalized model she developed over time Other evidence of vocal learning in elephants occurred in a cross fostering situation with a captive African elephant At the Basel Zoo in Switzerland Calimero a male African elephant was kept with two female Asian elephants Recordings of his cries shows evidence of chirping noises typically only produced by Asian elephants The duration and frequency of these calls differs from recorded instances of chirping calls from other African elephants and more closely resembles the chirping calls of Asian elephants 21 Controversial or limited vocal learners edit The following species are not formally considered vocal learners but some evidence has suggested they may have limited abilities to modify their vocalizations Further research is needed in these species to fully understand their learning abilities Non human primates edit Early research asserted that primate calls are fully formed at an early age in development yet recently some studies have suggested these calls are modified later in life 22 In 1989 Masataka and Fujita cross fostered Japanese and rhesus monkeys in the same room and demonstrated that foraging calls were learned directly from their foster mothers providing evidence of vocal learning 23 However when another independent group was unable to reproduce these results Masataka and Fujita s findings were questioned 22 Adding to the evidence against vocal learning in non human primates is the suggestion that regional differences in calls maybe be attributed to genetic differences between populations and not vocal learning 24 Other studies argue that non human primates do have some limited vocal learning ability demonstrating that they can modify their vocalizations in a limited fashion through laryngeal control 24 and lip movements 25 26 For example chimpanzees in both captivity and in the wild have been recorded producing novel sounds to attract attention By puckering their lips and making a vibrating sounds they can make a raspberry call which has been imitated by both naive captive and wild individuals 25 There is also evidence of an orangutan learning to whistle by copying a human an ability previously unseen in the species 26 A cross fostering experiment with marmosets and macaques showed convergence in pitch and other acoustic features in their supposedly innate calls 24 demonstrating the ability albeit limited for vocal learning Mice edit Mice produce long sequences of vocalizations or songs that are used for both isolation calls in pups when cold or removed from nest and for courtship when males sense a female or detect pheromones in their urine These ultrasonic vocalizations consist of discrete syllables and patterns with species specific differences Males tend to use particular syllable types that can be used to differentiate individuals 27 There has been intense debate on whether these songs are innate or learned In 2011 Kikusui et al cross fostered two strains of mice with distinct song phenotypes and discovered that strain specific characteristics of each song persisted in the offspring indicating that these vocalizations are innate 28 However a year later work by Arriaga et al contradicted these results as their study found a motor cortex region active during singing which projects directly to brainstem motor neurons and is also important for keeping songs stereotyped and on pitch Vocal control by forebrain motor areas and direct cortical projections to vocal motor neurons are both features of vocal learning Furthermore male mice were shown to depend on auditory feedback to maintain some ultrasonic song features and sub strains with differences in their songs were able to match each other s pitch when cross housed under competitive social conditions 29 30 In 2013 Mahrt et al showed that genetically deafened mice produce calls of the same types number duration frequency as normal hearing mice This finding shows that mice do not require auditory experience to produce normal vocalizations suggesting that mice are not vocal learners 31 With this conflicting evidence it remains unclear whether mice are vocal non learners or limited vocal learners Goats edit When goats are placed in different social groups they modify their calls to show more similarity to that of the group which provides evidence they may be limited vocal learners according to Erich Jarvis continuum hypothesis 29 Evolution editAs vocal learning is such a rare trait that evolved in distant groups there are many theories to explain the striking similarities between vocal learners especially within avian vocal learners Adaptive advantage edit There are several proposed hypotheses that explain the selection for vocal learning based on environment and behavior These include 32 Individual identification In most vocal learning species individuals have their own songs which serve as a unique signature to differentiate themselves from others in the population which some suggest has driven selection of vocal learning However identification by voice rather than by song or name is present in vocal non learners as well Among vocal learners only humans and maybe bottlenose dolphins actually use unique names Therefore it is unlikely that individual identification was a primary driving force for the evolution of vocal learning Semantic communication Semantic vocal communication associates specific vocalizations with animate or inanimate objects to convey a factual message This hypothesis asserts that vocal learning evolved to facilitate enhanced communication of these specific messages as opposed to affective communication which conveys emotional content For example humans are able to shout watch out for that car when another is in danger while crossing the street instead of just making a noise to indicate urgency which is less effective at conveying the exact danger at hand However many vocal non learners including chickens and velvet monkeys have been shown to use their innate calls to communicate semantic information such as a food source or predator Further discrediting this hypothesis is the fact that vocal learning birds also use innate calls for this purpose and only rarely use their learned vocalizations for semantic communication for example the grey parrot can mimic human speech and the black capped chickadee uses calls to indicate predator size As learned vocalizations rarely convey semantic information this hypothesis also does not fully explain the evolution of vocal learning Mate attraction and territory defense While both vocal learners and non learners use vocalizations to attract mates or defend territories there is one key difference variability Vocal learners can produce more varied syntax and frequency modulation which have been shown to be preferred by females in songbirds For example canaries use two voices to produce large frequency modulation variations called sexy syllables or sexy songs which are thought to stimulate estrogen production in females When vocal non learner females were presented with artificially increased frequency modulations in their innate vocalizations more mating was stimulated Sexual selection for greater variability and thus in turn vocal learning may then be a major driving force for the evolution of vocal learning Rapid adaptation to sound propagation in different environments Vocal non learners produce their sounds best in specific habitats making them more susceptible to changes in the environment For example pigeons low frequency calls travel best near the ground and so communication higher in the air is much less effective In contrast vocal learners can change voice characteristics to suit their current environment which presumably allows for better group communication Predatory pressure edit With the many possible advantages outlined above it still remains unclear as to why vocal learning is so rare One proposed explanation is that predatory pressure applies a strong selective force against vocal learning 32 If mates prefer more variable vocalizations predators may also be more strongly attracted to more variable vocalizations As innate calls are typically constant predators quickly habituate to these vocalizations and ignore them as background noise In contrast the variable vocalizations of vocal learners are less likely to be ignored possibly increasing the predation rate among vocal learners In this case relaxed predation pressure or some mechanism to overcome increased predation must first develop to facilitate the evolution of vocal learning Supporting this hypothesis is the fact that many mammalian vocal learners including humans whales and elephants have very few major predators Similarly several avian vocal learners have behaviors that are effective in avoiding predators from the rapid flight and escape behavior of hummingbirds to predator mobbing in parrots and songbirds While little research has been done in this area some studies have supported the predation hypothesis One study showed that Bengalese finches bred in captivity for 250 years without predation or human selection for singing behavior show increased variability in syntax than their conspecifics in the wild A similar experiment with captive zebra finches demonstrated the same result as captive birds had increased song variability which was then preferred by females 33 Although these studies are promising more research is needed in this area to compare predation rates across vocal learners and non learners Phylogeny edit Birds edit nbsp Avian phylogenetic tree and the complex vocal learning phenotype Shown is an avian phylogenetic tree based on Hackett et al 2008 Identified in red text and are three groups of complex vocal learning birds Below the figure are summarized three alternative hypotheses on the evolutionary mechanisms of complex vocal learning in birds From Petkov CI Jarvis ED 2012 Birds primates and spoken language origins behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates Front Evol Neurosci 4 12 Modern birds supposedly evolved from a common ancestor around the Cretaceous Paleogene boundary at the time of the extinction of dinosaurs about 66 million years ago Out of the thirty avian orders only three evolved vocal learning and all have incredibly similar forebrain structures despite the fact that they are distantly related for example parrots and songbirds are as distantly related as humans and dolphins Phylogenetic comparisons have suggested that vocal learning evolved among birds at least two or three independent times in songbirds parrots and hummingbirds Depending on the interpretation of the trees there were either three gains in all three lineages or two gains in hummingbirds and the common ancestor of parrots and songbirds with a loss in the suboscine songbirds There are several hypotheses to explain this phenomenon 1 Independent convergent evolution All three avian groups evolved vocal learning and similar neural pathways independently not through a common ancestor This suggests that there are strong epigenetic constraints imposed by the environment or morphological needs and so this hypothesis predicts that groups that newly evolve vocal learning will also develop similar neural circuits Common ancestor This alternative hypothesis suggests that vocal learning birds evolved the trait from a distant common ancestor which was then lost four independent times in interrelated vocal non learners Possible causes include high survival costs of vocal learning predation or weak adaptive benefits that did not induce strong selection for the trait for organisms in other environments Rudimentary structures in non learners This alternative hypothesis states that avian non learners actually do possess rudimentary or undeveloped brain structures necessary for song learning which were enlarged in vocal learning species Significantly this concept challenges the current assumption that vocal nuclei are unique to vocal learners suggesting that these structures are universal even in other groups such as mammals Motor theory This hypothesis suggests that cerebral systems that control vocal learning in distantly related animals evolved as specializations of a pre existing motor system inherited from a common ancestor Thus in avian vocal learners each of the three groups of vocal learning birds evolved cerebral vocal systems independently but the systems were constrained by a previous genetically determined motor system inherited from the common ancestor that controls learned movement sequencing Evidence for this hypothesis was provided by Feenders and colleagues in 2008 as they found that EGR1 an immediate early gene associated with increases in neuronal activity was expressed in forebrain regions surrounding or directly adjacent to song nuclei when vocal learning birds performed non vocal movement behaviors such as hopping and flying In non learners comparable areas were activated but without the adjacent presence of song nuclei 34 EGR1 expression patterns were correlated with the amount of movement just as its expression typically correlates with the amount of singing performed in vocal birds These finding suggest that vocal learning brain regions developed from the same cell lineages that gave rise to the motor pathway which then formed a direct projection onto the brainstem vocal motor neurons to provide greater control 1 Currently it remains unclear as to which of these hypotheses is the most accurate Primates edit nbsp Primate phylogenetic tree and complex vocal learning vs auditory sequence learning Shown is a primate phylogenetic tree based on a combination of DNA sequence and fossil age data Goodman et al 1998 Page et al 1999 Humans Homo are the only primates classified as vocal learners However non human primates might be better at auditory sequence learning than their limited vocal production learning capabilities would suggest In blue text and we highlight species for which there is some evidence of Artificial Grammar Learning capabilities for at least adjacent relationships between the elements in a sequence tamarins Fitch and Hauser 2004 macaques Wilson et al 2011 Presuming that the auditory capabilities of guenons and gibbons or the symbolic learning of signs by apes would mean that these animals are able to learn at least adjacent relationships in Artificial Grammars we can tentatively mark these species also in blue Note however that for the species labeled in black text future studies might show them to be capable of some limited vocal learning or various levels of complexity in learning the structure of auditory sequences Three not mutually exclusive hypotheses are illustrated for both complex vocal learning and auditory sequence learning From Petkov CI Jarvis ED 2012 Birds primates and spoken language origins behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates Front Evol Neurosci 4 12 In primates only humans are known to be capable of complex vocal learning Similar to the first hypothesis relating to birds one explanation is that vocal learning evolved independently in humans An alternative hypothesis suggests evolution from a primate common ancestor capable of vocal learning with the trait subsequently being lost at least eight other times Considering the most parsimonious analysis it seems unlikely that the number of independent gains one in humans would be exceeded so greatly by the number of independent losses at least eight which supports the independent evolution hypothesis 1 Neurobiology editNeural pathways in avian vocal learners edit As avian vocal learners are the most amenable to experimental manipulations the vast majority of work to elucidate the neurobiological mechanisms of vocal learning has been conducted with zebra finches with a few studies focusing on budgerigars and other species Despite variation in vocal learning phenotype the neural circuitry necessary for producing learned song is conserved in songbirds parrots and hummingbirds As opposed to their non learner avian counterparts such as quail doves and pigeons these avian vocal learners contain seven distinct cerebral song nuclei or distinct brain areas associated with auditory learning and song production defined by their gene expression patterns As current evidence suggests independent evolution of these structures the names of each equivalent vocal nucleus are different per bird group as shown in the table below Parallel Song Nuclei in Avian Vocal Learners Songbirds Parrots Hummingbirds HVC a letter based name NLC central nucleus of the lateral nidopallium VLN vocal nucleus of the lateral nidopallium RA robust nucleus of the arcopallium AAC central nucleus of the anterior arcopallium VA vocal nucleus of the arcopallium MAN magnocellular nucleus of anterior nidopallium NAOc oval nucleus of the anterior nidopallium complex Area X area X of the striatum MMSt magnocellular nucleus of the anterior striatum DLM medial nucleus of dorsolateral thalamus DMM magnocellular nucleus of the dorsomedial thalamus MO oval nucleus of the mesopallium MOc oval nucleus of the mesopallium complex Vocal nuclei are found in two separate brain pathways which will be described in songbirds as most research has been conducted in this group yet connections are similar in parrots 35 and hummingbirds 36 Projections of the anterior vocal pathway in the hummingbird remain unclear and so are not listed in the table above The posterior vocal pathway also known as vocal motor pathway involved in the production of learned vocalizations begins with projections from a nidopallial nucleus the HVC in songbirds The HVC then projects to the robust nucleus of the arcopallium RA The RA connects to the midbrain vocal center DM dorsal medial nucleus of the midbrain and the brainstem nXIIts vocal motor neurons that control the muscles of the syrinx a direct projection similar to the projection from LMC to the nucleus ambiguus in humans 1 37 The HVC is considered the syntax generator while the RA modulates the acoustic structure of syllables Vocal non learners do possess the DM and twelfth motor neurons nXIIts but lack the connections to the arcopallium As a result they can produce vocalizations but not learned vocalizations The anterior vocal pathway also known as vocal learning pathway is associated with learning syntax and social contexts starting with projections from the magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium MAN to the striatal nucleus Area X Area X then projects to the medial nucleus of dorsolateral thalamus DLM which ultimately projects back to MAN in a loop 38 The lateral part of MAN LMAN generates variability in song while Area X is responsible for stereotypy or the generation of low variability in syllable production and order after song crystallization 32 Despite the similarities in vocal learning neural circuits there are some major connectivity differences between the posterior and anterior pathways among avian vocal learners In songbirds the posterior pathway communicates with the anterior pathway via projections from the HVC to Area X the anterior pathway sends output to the posterior pathway via connections from LMAN to RA and medial MAN MMAN to HVC Parrots on the other hand have projections from the ventral part of the AAC AACv the parallel of the songbird RA to the NAOc parallel of the songbird MAN and the oval nucleus of the mesopallium MO The anterior pathway in parrots connects to the posterior pathway via NAOc projections to the NLC parallel of the songbird HVC and AAC Thus parrots do not send projections to the striatal nucleus of the anterior pathway from their posterior pathway as do songbirds Another crucial difference is the location of the posterior vocal nuclei among species Posterior nuclei are located in auditory regions for songbirds laterally adjacent to auditory regions in hummingbirds and are physically separate from auditory regions in parrots Axons must therefore take different routes to connect nuclei in different vocal learning species Exactly how these connectivity differences affect song production and or vocal learning ability remains unclear 32 39 An auditory pathway that is used for auditory learning brings auditory information into the vocal pathway but the auditory pathway is not unique to vocal learners Ear hair cells project to cochlear ganglia neurons to auditory pontine nuclei to midbrain and thalamic nuclei and to primary and secondary pallial areas A descending auditory feedback pathway exists projecting from the dorsal nidopallium to the intermediate arcopallium to shell regions around the thalamic and midbrain auditory nuclei Remaining unclear is the source of auditory input into the vocal pathways described above It is hypothesized that songs are processed in these areas in a hierarchical manner with the primary pallial area responsible for acoustic features field L2 the secondary pallial area fields L1 and L3 as well as the caudal medial nidopallium or NCM determining sequencing and discrimination and the highest station the caudal mesopallium CM modulating fine discrimination of sounds Secondary pallial areas including the NCM and CM are also thought to be involved in auditory memory formation of songs used for vocal learning but more evidence is needed to substantiate this hypothesis 32 Critical period edit The development of the sensory modalities necessary for song learning occurs within a critical period of development that varies among avian vocal learners Closed ended learners such as the zebra finch and aphantochroa hummingbird can only learn during a limited time period and subsequently produce highly stereotyped or non variable vocalizations consisting of a single fixed song which they repeat their entire lives In contrast open ended learners including canaries and various parrot species display significant plasticity and continue to learn new songs throughout the course of their lives 40 In the male zebra finch vocal learning begins with a period of sensory acquisition or auditory learning where juveniles are exposed to the song of an adult male tutor at about posthatch day 30 to 60 41 During this stage juveniles listen and memorize the song pattern of their tutor and produce subsong characterized by the production of highly variable syllables and syllable sequences Subsong is thought to be analogous to babbling in human infants Subsequently during the sensorimotor learning phase at posthatch day 35 to 90 juveniles practice the motor commands required for song production and use auditory feedback to alter vocalizations to match the song template Songs during this period are plastic as specific syllables begin to emerge but are frequently in the wrong sequence errors that are similar to phonological mistakes made by young children when learning a language As the bird ages its song becomes more stereotyped until at posthatch day 120 the song syllables and sequence are crystallized or fixed At this point the zebra finch can no longer learn new songs and thus sings this single song for the duration of its life 42 The neural mechanisms behind the closing of the critical period remain unclear but early deprivation of juveniles from their adult tutors has been shown to extend the critical period of song acquisition 43 Synapse selection theories hypothesize that synaptic plasticity during the critical period is gradually reduced as dendritic spines are pruned through activity dependent synaptic rearrangement 44 The pruning of dendritic spines in the LMAN song nucleus was delayed in isolated zebra finches with extended critical periods suggesting that this form of synaptic reorganization may be important in closing the critical period 45 However other studies have shown that birds reared normally as well as isolated juveniles have similar levels of dendritic pruning despite an extended critical period in the latter group 46 demonstrating that this theory does not completely explain critical period modulation Previous research has suggested that the length of the critical period may be linked to differential gene expression within song nuclei thought to be caused by neurotransmitter binding of receptors during neural activation 47 One key area is the LMAN song nucleus part of the specialized cortical basal ganglia thalamo cortical loop in the anterior forebrain pathway which is essential for vocal plasticity 38 While inducing deafness in songbirds usually disrupts the sensory phase of learning and leads to production of highly abnormal song structures lesioning of LMAN in zebra finches prevents this song deterioration 48 leading to the earlier development of stable song One of the neurotransmitter receptors shown to affect LMAN is the N methyl D aspartate glutamate receptor NMDAR which is required for learning and activity dependent gene regulation in the post synaptic neuron Infusions of the NMDAR antagonist APV R 2 amino 5 phosphonopentanoate into the LMAN song nucleus disrupts the critical period in the zebra finch 49 NMDAR density and mRNA levels of the NR1 subunit also decrease in LMAN during early song development 50 When the song becomes crystallized expression of the NR2B subunit decreases in LMAN and NMDAR mediated synaptic currents shorten 51 It has been hypothesized that LMAN actively maintains RA microcircuitry in a state permissive for song plasticity and in a process of normal development it regulates HVC RA synapses In humans edit nbsp Vocalization subsystems in complex vocal learners and in limited vocal learners or vocal non learners Direct and indirect pathways The different subsystems for vocalization and their interconnectivity are illustrated using different colors A Schematic of a songbird brain showing some connectivity of the four major song nuclei HVC RA AreaX and LMAN B Human brain schematic showing the different proposed vocal subsystems The learned vocalization subsystem consists of a primary motor cortex pathway blue arrow and a cortico striatal thalamic loop for learning vocalizations white Also shown is the limbic vocal subsystem that is broadly conserved in primates for producing innate vocalizations black and the motoneurons that control laryngeal muscles red C Known connectivity of a brainstem vocal system not all connections shown showing absence of forebrain song nuclei in vocal non learning birds D Known connectivity of limited vocal learning monkeys based on data in squirrel monkeys and macaques showing presence of forebrain regions for innate vocalization ACC OFC and amygdala and also of a ventral premotor area Area 6vr of currently poorly understood function that is indirectly connected to nucleus ambiguous The LMC in humans is directly connected with motoneurons in the nucleus ambiguus which orchestrate the production of learned vocalizations Only the direct pathway through the mammalian basal ganglia ASt anterior striatum GPi globus palidus internal is shown as this is the one most similar to AreaX connectivity in songbirds Modified figure based on Jarvis 2004 Jarvis et al 2005 Abbreviations ACC anterior cingulate cortex Am nucleus ambiguus Amyg amygdala AT anterior thalamus Av nucleus avalanche DLM dorsolateral nucleus of the medial thalamus DM dorsal medial nucleus of the midbrain HVC high vocal center LMAN lateral magnocellular nucleus of the anterior nidopallium LMC laryngeal motor cortex OFC orbito frontal cortex PAG periaqueductal gray RA robust nucleus of the arcopallium RF reticular formation vPFC ventral prefrontal cortex VLT ventro lateral division of thalamus XIIts bird twelfth nerve nucleus From Petkov CI Jarvis ED 2012 Birds primates and spoken language origins behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates Front Evol Neurosci 4 12 Humans seem to have analogous anterior and posterior vocal pathways which are implicated in speech production and learning Parallel to the avian posterior vocal pathway mentioned above is the motor cortico brainstem pathway Within this pathway the face motor cortex projects to the nucleus ambiguous of the medulla which then projects to the muscles of the larynx Humans also have a vocal pathway that is analogous to the avian anterior pathway This pathway is a cortico basal ganglia thalamic cortico loop which begins at a strip of the premotor cortex called the cortical strip which is responsible for speech learning and syntax production The cortical strip includes spans across five brain regions the anterior insula Broca s area the anterior dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex the anterior pre supplementary motor area and the anterior cingulate cortex This cortical strip has projections to the anterior striatum which projects to the globus pallidus to the anterior dorsal thalamus back to the cortical strip All of these regions are also involved in syntax and speech learning 52 Genetic applications to humans editIn addition to the similarities in the neurobiological circuits necessary for vocalizations between animal vocal learners and humans there are also a few genetic similarities The most prominent of these genetic links are the FOXP1 and FOXP2 genes which code for forkhead box FOX proteins P1 and P2 respectively FOXP1 and FOXP2 are transcription factors which play a role in the development and maturation of the lungs heart and brain 53 54 and are also highly expressed in brain regions of the vocal learning pathway including the basal ganglia and the frontal cortex In these regions i e the basal ganglia and frontal cortex FOXP1 and FOXP2 are thought to be essential for brain maturation and development of speech and language 54 Orthologues of FOXP2 are found in a number of vertebrates including mice and songbirds and have been implicated in modulating plasticity of neural circuits In fact although mammals and birds are very distant relatives and diverged more than 300 million years ago the FOXP2 gene in zebra finches and mice differs at only five amino acid positions and differs between zebra finches and humans at only eight amino acid positions In addition researchers have found that patterns of expression of FOXP1 and FOXP2 are amazingly similar in the human fetal brain and the songbird 54 These similarities are especially interesting in the context of the aforementioned avian song circuit FOXP2 is expressed in the avian Area X and is especially highly expressed in the striatum during the critical period of song plasticity in songbirds In humans FOXP2 is highly expressed in the basal ganglia frontal cortex and insular cortex all thought to be important nodes in the human vocal pathway Thus mutations in the FOXP2 gene are proposed to have detrimental effects on human speech and language such as grammar language processing and impaired movement of the mouth lips and tongue 55 as well as potential detrimental effects on song learning in songbirds Indeed FOXP2 was the first gene to be implicated in the cognition of speech and language in a family of individuals with a severe speech and language disorder Additionally it has been suggested that due to the overlap of FOXP1 and FOXP2 expression in songbirds and humans mutations in FOXP1 may also result in speech and language abnormalities seen in individuals with mutations in FOXP2 53 These genetic links have important implications for studying the origin of language because FOXP2 is so similar among vocal learners and humans as well as important implications for understanding the etiology of certain speech and language disorders in humans Currently no other genes have been linked as compellingly to vocal learning in animals or humans See also editAnimal cognition Bioacoustics Biolinguistics Biomusic Birdsong Entrainment Evolution of language Evolution of music Evolutionary linguistics Evolutionary psychology Human voice Phonation Social learning Speech repetition Talking animal Whale songReferences edit a b c d e f g Petkov CI Jarvis ED 2012 Birds primates and spoken language origins behavioral phenotypes and neurobiological substrates Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience 4 12 doi 10 3389 fnevo 2012 00012 PMC 3419981 PMID 22912615 Okanoya K June 2004 The Bengalese finch a window on the behavioral neurobiology of birdsong syntax Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1016 1 724 35 Bibcode 2004NYASA1016 724O doi 10 1196 annals 1298 026 PMID 15313802 S2CID 21327383 Hurford JR 2012 The Origins of Grammar Language in the Light of Evolution Oxford Oxford University Press Honda E Okanoya K 1999 Acoustical and syntactical com parisons between songs of the white backed Munia Lonchura striata and its domesticated strain the Bengalese finch Lonchura striata var domestica Zool Sci 16 2 319 326 doi 10 2108 zsj 16 319 S2CID 85306560 Schachner A Brady TF Pepperberg IM Hauser MD May 2009 Spontaneous motor entrainment to music in multiple vocal mimicking species Current Biology 19 10 831 6 doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 03 061 hdl 1721 1 96194 PMID 19409786 S2CID 6150861 Lattenkamp Ella Z Vernes Sonja C 2018 06 01 Vocal learning a language relevant trait in need of a broad cross species approach Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences 21 209 215 doi 10 1016 j cobeha 2018 04 007 hdl 21 11116 0000 0001 237D C ISSN 2352 1546 S2CID 13809803 Martins PT Boeckx C March 2020 Vocal learning Beyond the continuum PLOS Biology 18 3 e3000672 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 3000672 PMC 7145199 PMID 32226012 Esser KH September 1994 Audio vocal learning in a non human mammal the lesser spear nosed bat Phyllostomus discolor NeuroReport 5 14 1718 20 doi 10 1097 00001756 199409080 00007 PMID 7827315 Boughman JW February 1998 Vocal learning by greater spear nosed bats Proceedings Biological Sciences 265 1392 227 33 doi 10 1098 rspb 1998 0286 PMC 1688873 PMID 9493408 Noad MJ Cato DH Bryden MM Jenner MN Jenner KC November 2000 Cultural revolution in whale songs Nature 408 6812 537 Bibcode 2000Natur 408 537N doi 10 1038 35046199 PMID 11117730 S2CID 4398582 Foote AD Griffin RM Howitt D Larsson L Miller PJ Hoelzel AR December 2006 Killer whales are capable of vocal learning Biology Letters 2 4 509 12 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2006 0525 PMC 1834009 PMID 17148275 Lilly JC January 1965 Vocal Mimicry in Tursiops Ability to Match Numbers and Durations of Human Vocal Bursts Science 147 3655 300 1 Bibcode 1965Sci 147 300L doi 10 1126 science 147 3655 300 PMID 17788214 S2CID 2038221 Reiss D McCowan B September 1993 Spontaneous vocal mimicry and production by bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus evidence for vocal learning Journal of Comparative Psychology 107 3 301 12 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 107 3 301 PMID 8375147 Herman LM Richards DG Wolz JP March 1984 Comprehension of sentences by bottlenosed dolphins Cognition 16 2 129 219 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 84 90003 9 PMID 6540652 S2CID 43237011 Janik VM August 2000 Whistle matching in wild bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus Science 289 5483 1355 7 Bibcode 2000Sci 289 1355J doi 10 1126 science 289 5483 1355 PMID 10958783 Comparative studies indicate that whistles vary in structure across populations and species Steiner 1981 Ding et al 1995 Rendell et al 1999 with whistle divergence perhaps facilitating species recognition and speciation Podos et al 2002 Caldwell DK 1977 How animals communicate Janik VM Sayigh LS June 2013 Communication in bottlenose dolphins 50 years of signature whistle research Journal of Comparative Physiology A Neuroethology Sensory Neural amp Behavioral Physiology 199 6 479 89 doi 10 1007 s00359 013 0817 7 PMID 23649908 S2CID 15378374 Caldwell DK How Animals Communicate p 794 Ralls K Fiorelli P Gish S 1985 Vocalizations and vocal mimicry in captive harbour seals Phoca vitulina Canadian Journal of Zoology 63 5 1050 1056 doi 10 1139 z85 157 Sanvito S Galimberti F Miller EH 2007 Observational evidences of vocal learning in southern elephant seals a longitudinal study Ethology 113 2 137 146 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 2006 01306 x Poole JH Tyack PL Stoeger Horwath AS Watwood S March 2005 Animal behaviour elephants are capable of vocal learning Nature 434 7032 455 6 Bibcode 2005Natur 434 455P doi 10 1038 434455a PMID 15791244 S2CID 4369863 a b Cheney DL Owren MJ Dieter JA Seyfarth RM January 1992 Food calls produced by adult female rhesus Macaca mulatta and Japanese M fuscata macaques their normally raised offspring and offspring cross fostered between species Behaviour 120 3 4 218 31 doi 10 1163 156853992X00615 Masataka N Fujita K January 1989 Vocal learning of Japanese and rhesus monkeys Behaviour 109 3 4 191 9 doi 10 1163 156853989X00222 a b c Snowdon CT 2009 Plasticity of communication in non human primates in Advances in the Study of Behavior eds M Naguib and V M Janik Burlington NJ Academic Press 239 276 a b Hopkins WD Taglialatela J Leavens DA February 2007 Chimpanzees Differentially Produce Novel Vocalizations to Capture the Attention of a Human Animal Behaviour 73 2 281 286 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2006 08 004 PMC 1832264 PMID 17389908 a b Wich SA Swartz KB Hardus ME Lameira AR Stromberg E Shumaker RW January 2009 A case of spontaneous acquisition of a human sound by an orangutan Primates Journal of Primatology 50 1 56 64 doi 10 1007 s10329 008 0117 y PMID 19052691 S2CID 708682 Holy TE Guo Z December 2005 Ultrasonic songs of male mice PLOS Biology 3 12 e386 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0030386 PMC 1275525 PMID 16248680 Kikusui T Nakanishi K Nakagawa R Nagasawa M Mogi K Okanoya K March 2011 Cross fostering experiments suggest that mice songs are innate PLOS ONE 6 3 e17721 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 617721K doi 10 1371 journal pone 0017721 PMC 3052373 PMID 21408017 a b Arriaga G Zhou EP Jarvis ED 2012 Of mice birds and men the mouse ultrasonic song system has some features similar to humans and song learning birds PLOS ONE 7 10 e46610 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 746610A doi 10 1371 journal pone 0046610 PMC 346858 PMID 23071596 Arriaga G Jarvis ED January 2013 Mouse vocal communication system are ultrasounds learned or innate Brain and Language 124 1 96 116 doi 10 1016 j bandl 2012 10 002 PMC 3886250 PMID 23295209 Mahrt EJ Perkel DJ Tong L Rubel EW Portfors CV March 2013 Engineered deafness reveals that mouse courtship vocalizations do not require auditory experience The Journal of Neuroscience 33 13 5573 83 doi 10 1523 jneurosci 5054 12 2013 PMC 3691057 PMID 23536072 a b c d e Jarvis ED 2009 Bird Song Systems Evolution Encyclopedia of Neuroscience 2 217 225 doi 10 1016 b978 008045046 9 00935 9 hdl 10161 11242 ISBN 9780080450469 Zann RA 1996 The Zebra Finch A Synthesis of Field and Laboratory Studies New York Oxford University Press Feenders G Liedvogel M Rivas M Zapka M Horita H Hara E et al March 2008 Molecular mapping of movement associated areas in the avian brain a motor theory for vocal learning origin PLOS ONE 3 3 e1768 Bibcode 2008PLoSO 3 1768F doi 10 1371 journal pone 0001768 PMC 2258151 PMID 18335043 Jarvis ED Mello CV March 2000 Molecular mapping of brain areas involved in parrot vocal communication The Journal of Comparative Neurology 419 1 1 31 doi 10 1002 sici 1096 9861 20000327 419 1 lt 1 aid cne1 gt 3 0 co 2 m PMC 2538445 PMID 10717637 Jarvis ED Ribeiro S da Silva ML Ventura D Vielliard J Mello CV August 2000 Behaviourally driven gene expression reveals song nuclei in hummingbird brain Nature 406 6796 628 32 Bibcode 2000Natur 406 628J doi 10 1038 35020570 PMC 2531203 PMID 10949303 Jarvis ED June 2004 Learned birdsong and the neurobiology of human language Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1016 1 749 77 Bibcode 2004NYASA1016 749J doi 10 1196 annals 1298 038 PMC 2485240 PMID 15313804 a b Schraff C Nottebohm F 1991 A comparative study of the behavioral deficits following lesions of various parts of the zebra finch song system implications for vocal learning J Neurosci 11 9 2896 2913 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 11 09 02896 1991 PMC 6575264 PMID 1880555 Wild JM 1997 Neural pathways for the control of birdsong production Journal of Neurobiology 33 5 653 670 doi 10 1002 sici 1097 4695 19971105 33 5 lt 653 aid neu11 gt 3 0 co 2 a PMID 9369465 Brenowitz EA Beecher MD March 2005 Song learning in birds diversity and plasticity opportunities and challenges Trends in Neurosciences 28 3 127 32 doi 10 1016 j tins 2005 01 004 PMID 15749165 S2CID 14586913 Eales LA 1985 Song learning in zebra finches some effects of song model availability on what is learnt and when Anim Behav 33 4 1293 1300 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 85 80189 5 S2CID 54229759 Jones AE Ten Cate C Slater PJ December 1996 Early experience and plasticity of song in adult male Zebra Finches Taeniopygia guttata Journal of Comparative Psychology 110 4 354 69 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 110 4 354 Aamodt SM Nordeen EJ Nordeen KW May 1995 Early isolation from conspecific song does not affect the normal developmental decline of N methyl D aspartate receptor binding in an avian song nucleus Journal of Neurobiology 27 1 76 84 doi 10 1002 neu 480270108 PMID 7643077 Bischof HJ Geissler E Rollenhagen A 2002 Limitations of the sensitive period for sexual imprinting Neuroanatomical and behavioral experiments in the zebra finch Taeniopygia guttata Behavioural Brain Research 133 2 317 322 doi 10 1016 s0166 4328 02 00016 5 PMID 12110465 S2CID 28662586 Wallhausser Franke E Nixdorf Bergweiler BE DeVoogd TJ July 1995 Song isolation is associated with maintaining high spine frequencies on zebra finch 1MAN neurons Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 64 1 25 35 doi 10 1006 nlme 1995 1041 PMID 7582809 S2CID 19096150 Heinrich JE Nordeen KW Nordeen EJ March 2005 Dissociation between extension of the sensitive period for avian vocal learning and dendritic spine loss in the song nucleus lMAN Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 83 2 143 50 doi 10 1016 j nlm 2004 11 002 PMID 15721798 S2CID 8953614 Wada K Sakaguchi H Jarvis ED Hagiwara M August 2004 Differential expression of glutamate receptors in avian neural pathways for learned vocalization The Journal of Comparative Neurology 476 1 44 64 doi 10 1002 cne 20201 PMC 2517240 PMID 15236466 Brainard MS Doupe AJ 2001 Postlearning consolidation of birdsong stabilizing effects of age and anterior forebrain lesions J Neurosci 21 7 2501 2517 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 21 07 02501 2001 PMC 6762407 PMID 11264324 Basham ME Nordeen EJ Nordeen KW November 1996 Blockade of NMDA receptors in the anterior forebrain impairs sensory acquisition in the zebra finch Poephila guttata Neurobiology of Learning and Memory 66 3 295 304 doi 10 1006 nlme 1996 0071 PMID 8946423 S2CID 9592089 Heinrich JE Singh TD Sohrabji F Nordeen KW Nordeen EJ May 2002 Developmental and hormonal regulation of NR2A mRNA in forebrain regions controlling avian vocal learning Journal of Neurobiology 51 2 149 59 doi 10 1002 neu 10046 PMID 11932956 Livingston FS Mooney R 1997 Development of intrinsic and synaptic properties in a forebrain nucleus essential to avian song learning J Neurosci 17 23 8997 9009 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 17 23 08997 1997 PMC 6573603 PMID 9364047 Jarvis ED December 2007 Neural systems for vocal learning in birds and humans a synopsis Journal of Ornithology 148 1 35 44 doi 10 1007 s10336 007 0243 0 PMC 2726745 PMID 19684872 a b Pariani MJ Spencer A Graham JM Rimoin DL 2009 A 785kb deletion of 3p14 1p13 including the FOXP1 gene associated with speech delay contractures hypertonia and blepharophimosis European Journal of Medical Genetics 52 2 3 123 7 doi 10 1016 j ejmg 2009 03 012 PMC 2853231 PMID 19332160 a b c Vargha Khadem F Gadian DG Copp A Mishkin M February 2005 FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language Nature Reviews Neuroscience 6 2 131 8 doi 10 1038 nrn1605 PMID 15685218 S2CID 2504002 Takahashi K Liu FC Hirokawa K Takahashi H July 2003 Expression of Foxp2 a gene involved in speech and language in the developing and adult striatum Journal of Neuroscience Research 73 1 61 72 doi 10 1002 jnr 10638 PMID 12815709 S2CID 31971989 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Vocal learning amp oldid 1204666531, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.