fbpx
Wikipedia

Voice onset time

In phonetics, voice onset time (VOT) is a feature of the production of stop consonants. It is defined as the length of time that passes between the release of a stop consonant and the onset of voicing, the vibration of the vocal folds, or, according to other authors, periodicity. Some authors allow negative values to mark voicing that begins during the period of articulatory closure for the consonant and continues in the release, for those unaspirated voiced stops in which there is no voicing present at the instant of articulatory closure.

A graphical representation of the VOT of voiced, tenuis, and aspirated stops

History

The concept of voice onset time can be traced back as far as the 19th century, when Adjarian (1899: 119)[1] studied the Armenian stops, and characterized them by "the relation that exists between two moments: the one when the consonant bursts when the air is released out of the mouth, or explosion, and the one when the larynx starts vibrating". However, the concept became "popular" only in the 1960s, in a context described by Lin & Wang (2011: 514):[2] "At that time, there was an ongoing debate about which phonetic attribute would allow voiced and voiceless stops to be effectively distinguished. For instance, voicing, aspiration, and articulatory force were some of the attributes being studied regularly. In English, "voicing" can successfully separate /b, d, ɡ/ from /p, t, k/ when stops are at word-medial positions, but this is not always true for word-initial stops. Strictly speaking, word-initial voiced stops /b, d, ɡ/ are only partially voiced, and sometimes are even voiceless." The concept of VOT finally acquired its name in the famous study of Leigh Lisker and Arthur Abramson (Word, 1964), done while working together at Haskins Laboratories.[3]

Analytic problems

A number of problems arose in defining VOT in some languages, and there is a call for reconsidering whether this speech synthesis parameter should be used to replace articulatory or aerodynamic model parameters[clarification needed] which do not have these problems, and which have a stronger explanatory significance.[4] As in the discussion below, any explication of VOT variations will invariably lead back to such aerodynamic and articulatory concepts, and there is no reason presented why VOT adds to an analysis, other than that, as an acoustic parameter, it may sometimes be easier to measure than an aerodynamic parameter (pressure or airflow) or an articulatory parameter (closure interval or the duration, extent and timing of a vocal fold abductory gesture).

Types

 
Voice Onset Timing spectrograms for English "die" and "tie". The voiceless gap between release and voicing is highlighted in red. Here the phoneme /t/ has a VOT of 95 ms, and /d/ has one of 25 ms.

Three major phonation types of stops can be analyzed in terms of their voice onset time.

  • Simple unaspirated voiceless stops, sometimes called "tenuis" stops, have a voice onset time at or near zero, meaning that the voicing of a following sonorant (such as a vowel) begins at or near to when the stop is released. (An offset of 15 ms or less on [t] and 30 ms or less on [k] is inaudible, and counts as tenuis.)
  • Aspirated stops followed by a sonorant have a voice onset time greater than this amount, called a positive VOT. The length of the VOT in such cases is a practical measure of aspiration: The longer the VOT, the stronger the aspiration. In Navajo, for example, which is strongly aspirated, the aspiration (and therefore the VOT) lasts twice as long as it does in English: 160ms vs. 80ms for [kʰ], and 45ms for [k]. Some languages have weaker aspiration than English. For velar stops, tenuis [k] typically has a VOT of 20-30 ms, weakly aspirated [k] of some 50-60 ms, moderately aspirated [kʰ] averages 80–90 ms, and anything much over 100 ms would be considered strong aspiration. (Another phonation, breathy voice, is commonly called voiced aspiration; in order for the VOT measure to apply to it, VOT needs to be understood as the onset of modal voicing. Of course, an aspirated consonant will not always be followed by a voiced sound, in which case VOT cannot be used to measure it.)
  • Voiced stops have a voice onset time noticeably less than zero, a "negative VOT", meaning the vocal cords start vibrating before the stop is released. With a "fully voiced stop", the VOT coincides with the onset of the stop; with a "partially voiced stop", such as English [b, d, ɡ] in initial position, voicing begins sometime during the closure (occlusion) of the consonant.

Because neither aspiration nor voicing is absolute, with intermediate degrees of both, the relative terms fortis and lenis are often used to describe a binary opposition between a series of consonants with higher (more positive) VOT, defined as fortis, and a second series with lower (more negative) VOT, defined as lenis. Of course, being relative, what fortis and lenis mean in one language will not in general correspond to what they mean in another.

Voicing contrast applies to all types of consonants, but aspiration is generally only a feature of stops and affricates.

Transcription

Aspiration may be transcribed ⟨◌ʰ⟩, long (strong) aspiration ⟨◌ʰʰ⟩. Voicing is most commonly indicated by the choice of consonant letter. For one way of transcribing pre-voicing and other timing variants, see extensions to the IPA#Diacritics. Other systems include that of Laver (1994),[5] who distinguishes fully devoiced ⟨b̥a⟩ and ⟨ab̥⟩ from initial partial devoicing of the onset of a syllable by ⟨˳ba⟩ and from final partial devoicing of the coda of a syllable by ⟨ab˳⟩.

Examples in languages

Relative VOT distinctions in various languages[citation needed]
Voice Onset Time Examples
English Cantonese Tlingit Navajo Korean Japanese S. Japanese[clarification needed], Spanish, Russian Thai, Armenian
(fortis) Strong aspiration Yes Yes Yes
Moderate aspiration Yes Yes Yes
Mild aspiration Yes Yes Yes
Tenuis Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes
Partially voiced Yes
(lenis) Fully voiced Yes Yes Yes

Publications

  • Abramson, A., & Lisker, L. (1973). Voice timing perception in Spanish word-initial stops. Journal of Phonetics, 1, 1-8.
  • Abramson, A. S., & Whalen, D. H. (2017). Voice Onset Time (VOT) at 50: Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions. Journal of Phonetics, 63, 75–86.
  • Allen, J., Miller, J., & DeSteno, D. (2003). Individual talker differences in voice-onset-time. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113, 544–552.
  • Cho, T., & Ladefoged, P. (1999). Variation and universals in VOT: Evidence from 18 languages. Journal of Phonetics, 27, 207–229. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1999.0094
  • Cho, T., Whalen, D., & Docherty, G. (2019). Voice onset time and beyond: Exploring laryngeal contrast in 19 languages. Journal of Phonetics, 72, 52–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2018.11.002
  • Itoh, M., Sasanuma, S., Tatsumi, I. F., Murakami, S., Fukusako, Y., & Suzuki, T. (1982). Voice onset time characteristics in apraxia of speech. Brain and Language, 17, 193–210.
  • Kessinger, R. H., & Blumstein, S. E. (1997). Effects of speaking rate on voice-onset time in Thai, French, and English. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 143–168
  • Lisker, L., & Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Acoustical measurements. Word, 20(3), 384–422. DOI: 10.1080/00437956.1964.11659830.
  • Rubin, P. (2022). Arthur Abramson. In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics, April 20, 2022. doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.923
  • Scobbie, J. M. (2006). Flexibility in the face of incompatible English VOT systems. In L. Goldstein, D. H. Whalen, & C. T. Best (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 8: Varieties of phonological competence (pp. 367–392). Papers from 8th Conference on Laboratory Phonology, New Haven, CT. Phonology and Phonetics 4. Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Winn, M. B. (2020). Manipulation of voice onset time in speech stimuli: A tutorial and flexible Praat script. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147, 852-866.

References

  1. ^ ADJARIAN, H., Les explosives de l'ancien arménien étudiées dans les dialectes modernes, La Parole. Revue internationale de Rhinologie, Otologie, Laryngologie et Phonétique expérimentale, 119-127 (1899) "... la relation qui existe entre deux moments : celui où la consonne éclate par l'effet de l'expulsion de l'air hors de la bouche, ou explosion, et celui où le larynx entre en vibration."
  2. ^ LIN, C. & WANG, H., Automatic estimation of voice onset time for word-initial stops by applying random forest to onset detection, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 514-525 (2011)
  3. ^ "Lisker, L. and Abramson, A.S., A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustical measurements, Word Vol. 20, 384-422 (1964)" (PDF). Retrieved 2022-07-02.
  4. ^ ROTHENBERG, M. "Voice Onset Time vs. Articulatory Modeling for Stop Consonants", The Jan Gauffin Memorial Symposium, October 16, 2008. Royal Institute of Technology, Stockholm. (To be published in the proceedings)
  5. ^ Principles of Phonetics, p. 340

Sources

  • Taehong Cho and Peter Ladefoged, "Variations and universals in VOT: Evidence from 18 languages". Journal of Phonetics vol. 27. 207-229. 1999.
  • Angelika Braun, "VOT im 19. Jahrhundert oder "Die Wiederkehr des Gleichen"". Phonetica vol. 40. 323-327. 1983.

External links

  • Abramson-Lisker VOT Stimuli. An interactive demo of VOT stimuli created by Arthur Abramson and Leigh Lisker
  • Buy a pie for the spy A description of the mechanism of voiced, tenuis (voiceless unaspirated), and (voiceless) aspirated stops in relation to voice onset time

voice, onset, time, phonetics, voice, onset, time, feature, production, stop, consonants, defined, length, time, that, passes, between, release, stop, consonant, onset, voicing, vibration, vocal, folds, according, other, authors, periodicity, some, authors, al. In phonetics voice onset time VOT is a feature of the production of stop consonants It is defined as the length of time that passes between the release of a stop consonant and the onset of voicing the vibration of the vocal folds or according to other authors periodicity Some authors allow negative values to mark voicing that begins during the period of articulatory closure for the consonant and continues in the release for those unaspirated voiced stops in which there is no voicing present at the instant of articulatory closure A graphical representation of the VOT of voiced tenuis and aspirated stops This article contains phonetic transcriptions in the International Phonetic Alphabet IPA For an introductory guide on IPA symbols see Help IPA For the distinction between and see IPA Brackets and transcription delimiters Contents 1 History 2 Analytic problems 3 Types 4 Transcription 5 Examples in languages 6 Publications 7 References 8 Sources 9 External linksHistory EditThe concept of voice onset time can be traced back as far as the 19th century when Adjarian 1899 119 1 studied the Armenian stops and characterized them by the relation that exists between two moments the one when the consonant bursts when the air is released out of the mouth or explosion and the one when the larynx starts vibrating However the concept became popular only in the 1960s in a context described by Lin amp Wang 2011 514 2 At that time there was an ongoing debate about which phonetic attribute would allow voiced and voiceless stops to be effectively distinguished For instance voicing aspiration and articulatory force were some of the attributes being studied regularly In English voicing can successfully separate b d ɡ from p t k when stops are at word medial positions but this is not always true for word initial stops Strictly speaking word initial voiced stops b d ɡ are only partially voiced and sometimes are even voiceless The concept of VOT finally acquired its name in the famous study of Leigh Lisker and Arthur Abramson Word 1964 done while working together at Haskins Laboratories 3 Analytic problems EditA number of problems arose in defining VOT in some languages and there is a call for reconsidering whether this speech synthesis parameter should be used to replace articulatory or aerodynamic model parameters clarification needed which do not have these problems and which have a stronger explanatory significance 4 As in the discussion below any explication of VOT variations will invariably lead back to such aerodynamic and articulatory concepts and there is no reason presented why VOT adds to an analysis other than that as an acoustic parameter it may sometimes be easier to measure than an aerodynamic parameter pressure or airflow or an articulatory parameter closure interval or the duration extent and timing of a vocal fold abductory gesture Types Edit Voice Onset Timing spectrograms for English die and tie The voiceless gap between release and voicing is highlighted in red Here the phoneme t has a VOT of 95 ms and d has one of 25 ms Three major phonation types of stops can be analyzed in terms of their voice onset time Simple unaspirated voiceless stops sometimes called tenuis stops have a voice onset time at or near zero meaning that the voicing of a following sonorant such as a vowel begins at or near to when the stop is released An offset of 15 ms or less on t and 30 ms or less on k is inaudible and counts as tenuis Aspirated stops followed by a sonorant have a voice onset time greater than this amount called a positive VOT The length of the VOT in such cases is a practical measure of aspiration The longer the VOT the stronger the aspiration In Navajo for example which is strongly aspirated the aspiration and therefore the VOT lasts twice as long as it does in English 160ms vs 80ms for kʰ and 45ms for k Some languages have weaker aspiration than English For velar stops tenuis k typically has a VOT of 20 30 ms weakly aspirated k of some 50 60 ms moderately aspirated kʰ averages 80 90 ms and anything much over 100 ms would be considered strong aspiration Another phonation breathy voice is commonly called voiced aspiration in order for the VOT measure to apply to it VOT needs to be understood as the onset of modal voicing Of course an aspirated consonant will not always be followed by a voiced sound in which case VOT cannot be used to measure it Voiced stops have a voice onset time noticeably less than zero a negative VOT meaning the vocal cords start vibrating before the stop is released With a fully voiced stop the VOT coincides with the onset of the stop with a partially voiced stop such as English b d ɡ in initial position voicing begins sometime during the closure occlusion of the consonant Because neither aspiration nor voicing is absolute with intermediate degrees of both the relative terms fortis and lenis are often used to describe a binary opposition between a series of consonants with higher more positive VOT defined as fortis and a second series with lower more negative VOT defined as lenis Of course being relative what fortis and lenis mean in one language will not in general correspond to what they mean in another Voicing contrast applies to all types of consonants but aspiration is generally only a feature of stops and affricates Transcription EditAspiration may be transcribed ʰ long strong aspiration ʰʰ Voicing is most commonly indicated by the choice of consonant letter For one way of transcribing pre voicing and other timing variants see extensions to the IPA Diacritics Other systems include that of Laver 1994 5 who distinguishes fully devoiced b a and ab from initial partial devoicing of the onset of a syllable by ba and from final partial devoicing of the coda of a syllable by ab Examples in languages EditRelative VOT distinctions in various languages citation needed Voice Onset Time ExamplesEnglish Cantonese Tlingit Navajo Korean Japanese S Japanese clarification needed Spanish Russian Thai Armenian fortis Strong aspiration Yes Yes Yes Moderate aspiration Yes Yes YesMild aspiration Yes Yes YesTenuis Yes Yes Yes Yes Yes Partially voiced Yes lenis Fully voiced Yes Yes YesPublications EditAbramson A amp Lisker L 1973 Voice timing perception in Spanish word initial stops Journal of Phonetics 1 1 8 Abramson A S amp Whalen D H 2017 Voice Onset Time VOT at 50 Theoretical and practical issues in measuring voicing distinctions Journal of Phonetics 63 75 86 Allen J Miller J amp DeSteno D 2003 Individual talker differences in voice onset time Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 113 544 552 Cho T amp Ladefoged P 1999 Variation and universals in VOT Evidence from 18 languages Journal of Phonetics 27 207 229 https doi org 10 1006 jpho 1999 0094 Cho T Whalen D amp Docherty G 2019 Voice onset time and beyond Exploring laryngeal contrast in 19 languages Journal of Phonetics 72 52 65 https doi org 10 1016 j wocn 2018 11 002 Itoh M Sasanuma S Tatsumi I F Murakami S Fukusako Y amp Suzuki T 1982 Voice onset time characteristics in apraxia of speech Brain and Language 17 193 210 Kessinger R H amp Blumstein S E 1997 Effects of speaking rate on voice onset time in Thai French and English Journal of Phonetics 25 143 168 Lisker L amp Abramson A S 1964 A cross language study of voicing in initial stops Acoustical measurements Word 20 3 384 422 DOI 10 1080 00437956 1964 11659830 Rubin P 2022 Arthur Abramson In Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics April 20 2022 doi https doi org 10 1093 acrefore 9780199384655 013 923 Scobbie J M 2006 Flexibility in the face of incompatible English VOT systems In L Goldstein D H Whalen amp C T Best Eds Laboratory phonology 8 Varieties of phonological competence pp 367 392 Papers from 8th Conference on Laboratory Phonology New Haven CT Phonology and Phonetics 4 Berlin Germany Mouton de Gruyter Winn M B 2020 Manipulation of voice onset time in speech stimuli A tutorial and flexible Praat script Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 147 852 866 References Edit ADJARIAN H Les explosives de l ancien armenien etudiees dans les dialectes modernes La Parole Revue internationale de Rhinologie Otologie Laryngologie et Phonetique experimentale 119 127 1899 la relation qui existe entre deux moments celui ou la consonne eclate par l effet de l expulsion de l air hors de la bouche ou explosion et celui ou le larynx entre en vibration LIN C amp WANG H Automatic estimation of voice onset time for word initial stops by applying random forest to onset detection The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 514 525 2011 Lisker L and Abramson A S A cross language study of voicing in initial stops acoustical measurements Word Vol 20 384 422 1964 PDF Retrieved 2022 07 02 ROTHENBERG M Voice Onset Time vs Articulatory Modeling for Stop Consonants The Jan Gauffin Memorial Symposium October 16 2008 Royal Institute of Technology Stockholm To be published in the proceedings Principles of Phonetics p 340Sources EditTaehong Cho and Peter Ladefoged Variations and universals in VOT Evidence from 18 languages Journal of Phonetics vol 27 207 229 1999 Angelika Braun VOT im 19 Jahrhundert oder Die Wiederkehr des Gleichen Phonetica vol 40 323 327 1983 External links EditAbramson Lisker VOT Stimuli An interactive demo of VOT stimuli created by Arthur Abramson and Leigh Lisker Buy a pie for the spy A description of the mechanism of voiced tenuis voiceless unaspirated and voiceless aspirated stops in relation to voice onset time Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Voice onset time amp oldid 1119878956, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.