fbpx
Wikipedia

English auxiliary verbs

English auxiliary verbs are a small set of English verbs, which include the English modal auxiliary verbs and a few others.[1]: 19 [2]: 11–12  Although the auxiliary verbs of English are widely believed to lack inherent semantic meaning and instead to modify the meaning of the verbs they accompany, they are nowadays classed by linguists as auxiliary on the basis not of semantic but of grammatical properties: among these, that they invert with their subjects in interrogative main clauses (Has John arrived?) and are negated either by the simple addition of not (He has not arrived) or (with a very few exceptions) by negative inflection (He hasn't arrived).

History of the concept edit

When describing English, the adjective auxiliary was "formerly applied to any formative or subordinate elements of language, e.g. prefixes, prepositions."[3] As applied to verbs, its conception was originally rather vague and varied significantly.

Some historical examples edit

The first English grammar, Bref Grammar for English by William Bullokar, published in 1586, does not use the term "auxiliary" but says:

All other verbs are called verbs-neuters-un-perfect because they require the infinitive mood of another verb to express their signification of meaning perfectly: and be these, may, can, might or mought, could, would, should, must, ought, and sometimes, will, that being a mere sign of the future tense. [orthography standardized and modernized][4]: 353 

In volume 5 (1762) of Tristram Shandy, the narrator's father explains that "The verbs auxiliary we are concerned in here, . . . , are, am; was; have; had; do; did; make; made; suffer; shall; should; will; would; can; could; owe; ought; used; or is wont."[5]: 146–147 

Charles Wiseman's Complete English Grammar of 1764 notes that most verbs

cannot be conjugated through all their Moods and Tenses, without one of the following principal Verbs have and be. The first serves to conjugate the rest, by supplying the compound tenses of all Verbs both Regular and Irregular, whether Active, Passive, Neuter, or Impersonal, as may be seen in its own variation, &c.[a]

Along with have and be, it goes on to include do, may, can, shall, will as auxiliary verbs.[6]: 156–167 

W. C. Fowler's The English Language of 1857 says:

Auxiliary Verbs, or Helping Verbs, perform the same office in the conjugation of principal verbs which inflection does in the classical languages, though even in those languages the substantive verb is sometimes used as a helping verb. . . . I. The verbs that are always auxiliary to others are, May, can, shall, must; II. Those that are sometimes auxiliary and sometimes principal verbs are, Will, have, do, be, and let.[7]: 202 

The verbs that all the sources cited above agree are auxiliary verbs are the modal auxiliary verbs may, can, and shall; most also include be, do, and have.

Auxiliary verbs as heads edit

Modern grammars do not differ substantially over membership in the list of auxiliary verbs, though they have refined the concept and, following an idea first put forward by John Ross in 1969,[8] have tended to take the auxiliary verb not as subordinate to a "main verb" (a concept that pedagogical grammars perpetuate), but instead as the head of a verb phrase. Examples include The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language and Bas Aarts' Oxford Modern English Grammar.[9]: 104 [10]: 237–239  This is shown in the tree diagram below for the clause I can swim.

 

The clause has a subject noun phrase I and a head verb phrase (VP), headed by the auxiliary verb can. The VP also has a complement clause, which has a head VP, with the head verb swim.

Recent definitions edit

A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language (1985) says of "verbs in auxiliary function" that "In contrast to full [i.e. lexical] verbs, [these verbs] are capable of functioning as auxiliary or 'helping' verbs (cf 2.27f)", which seems to refer back to a table showing the "main verb" (sink in various inflected forms) following one to four auxiliary verbs (be and have, again in various inflected forms; and may and must). It is not obvious how this definition would exclude lexical verbs such as try (in tried sinking, tried to have sunk, tried being sunk, etc)[11]: 62, 120  – although they would certainly fail the book's own list of criteria for auxiliary verbs, as listed later.[11]: 121–127 

In his book English Auxiliaries: History and Structure (1993), Anthony R. Warner writes that the English auxiliary verbs "are rather sharply defined as a group by distinctive formal properties."[2]: 3 

The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language (2002) describes auxiliary verbs as "a small list of verbs with very specific syntactic properties", differing from "all the rest of the verbs in the dictionary, which we will call the lexical verbs . . . in inflectional morphology as well as syntax"[9]: 74  And later: "A general definition of auxiliary verb is that it denotes a closed class of verbs that are characteristically used as markers of tense, aspect, mood, and voice."[9]: 102  It too adds a list of criteria.[9]: 92–102 

Auxiliary verbs distinguished grammatically edit

The list of auxiliary verbs in Modern English, along with their inflected forms, is shown in the following table.

Contractions are only shown if their orthography is distinctive. There are also numerous unstressed versions that are typically, although not necessarily, written in the standard way.[12]: 242–248  For these, see a later section. Where there is a blank, the auxiliary verb lacks this form. (In some cases, a corresponding lexical verb may have the form. For example, although lexical verb need has a plain past tense form, auxiliary verb need does not.)

English auxiliary verb paradigm
Citation
form
Modal/
Non-modal
Plain Present tense Past tense Participles Confusible
lexical
homonym?[b]
Neutral Contr. Negative Neutral Contr. Negative Present Past
will Modal will 'll won't would 'd wouldn't none
may[c] may might mightn't none
can can can't, cannot could couldn't none
shall shall 'll shan't should shouldn't none
must must mustn't none
ought ought oughtn't exists[d]
need[e] need needn't exists
dare[e] dare daren't dared exists[f]
be Non-modal be am, is, are 'm, 's, 're %ain't,[g] isn't, aren't was, were wasn't, weren't being been exists[h]
do do[i] does, do 's[j] doesn't, don't did 'd[k], d' didn't exists
have have has, have 's, 've hasn't, haven't had 'd hadn't having exists
use[l] used usedn't exists
to to none

A major difference between the results, shown above, of defining auxiliary verbs syntactically and doing so based on a notion of "helping" is that the syntactic definition includes:

  • be even when used simply as a copular verb (I am hungry; It was a cat)
  • idioms using would (would rather, would sooner, would as soon) that take a finite clause complement (I'd rather you went)
  • have with no other verb (as in %Have you any change?): uses where it cannot be said to "help" any other verb.[9]: 103, 108, 112 

Archaic forms of be, do, and have edit

A study of 17th-century American English found the form be used for the 1st and 3rd person plural present;[16]: 192–193  was for the 3rd person plural preterite;[16]: 194  art and are for the 2nd person singular present; wast and wert for the 2nd person singular preterite;[16]: 193  and dost and hast (2nd person) and doth and hath (3rd person) for the singular present.[16]: 185–187 

The NICE criteria edit

One set of criteria for distinguishing between auxiliary and lexical verbs is F. R. Palmer's "NICE": "Basically the criteria are that the auxiliary verbs occur with negation, inversion, 'code', and emphatic affirmation while the [lexical] verbs do not."[1]: 15, 21 [m]

The NICE properties
Auxiliary verb Lexical verb
Negation I will not eat apples. 
I won't eat apples.
*I eat not apples.[g]
*I eatn't apples.
Inversion Has Lee eaten apples? *Eats Lee apples?
Code Can it devour 3 kg of meat? /
Yes it can.
Does it devour 3 kg of meat? /
*Yes it devours.
Emphatic affirmation You say we're not ready?
We ARE ready.
You say we didn't practise enough?
*We PRACTISED enough.

NICE: Negation edit

Clausal negation[n] most commonly employs an auxiliary verb, for example, We can't believe it'll rain today or I don't need an umbrella. As late as Middle English, lexical verbs could also participate in clausal negation, so a clause like Lee eats not apples would have been grammatical,[17]: vol 2, p 280  but this is no longer possible in Modern English, where lexical verbs require "do‑support".

(At first glance, the grammaticality of I hope/guess/suppose/think not may suggest that some lexical verbs too have no need for do‑support; but ungrammatical *I hope/guess/suppose/think not you are right shows that this is quite mistaken. Not in these examples does not negate a clause but is instead the negative equivalent of so, a pro-form for a negative proposition.[9]: 1536 )

Palmer writes that the "Negation" criterion is "whether [the verb] occurs with the negative particle not, or more strictly, whether it has a negative form",[1]: 21  the latter referring to negatively inflected won't, hasn't, haven't, etc. (As seen in the paradigm table above, in today's Standard English not every auxiliary verb has such a form.)

NICE: Inversion edit

Although English is a subject–verb–object language, an interrogative main clause is the most important among several constructions that put a verb before the subject. This is called subject–auxiliary inversion because only auxiliary verbs participate in such constructions: Can/should/must Lee eat apples?; Never have I enjoyed a quince. Again, in Middle English, lexical verbs were no different; but in Modern English *Eats Lee apples? and *Never enjoy I a quince are ungrammatical, and do‑support is again required: Does Lee eat apples?; Never do I enjoy quinces.

NICE: Code edit

F. R. Palmer attributes this term to J. R. Firth, writing:

There are sentences in English in which a full verb is later 'picked up' by an auxiliary. The position is very similar to that of a noun being 'picked up' by a pronoun. [. . .] If the initial sentence, which contains the main verb, is not heard, all the remainder is unintelligible; it is, in fact, truly in code. The following example is from Firth:

Do you think he will?
I don't know. He might.
I suppose he ought to, but perhaps he feels he can't.
Well, his brothers have. They perhaps think he needn't.
Perhaps eventually he may. I think he should, and I very much hope he will.[1]: 25 [18]: 104–105 

(What "picks up" is called an anaphor; what is picked up is called an antecedent.) Attempting to remove the complement(s) of a lexical verb normally has an ungrammatical result (Did you put it in the fridge? / *Yes, I put) or an inappropriate one (Did you eat the chicken? / #Yes, I ate[g]). However, if a number of conditions are met, the result may be acceptable.[9]: 1527–1529 

NICE: Emphatic affirmation edit

F. R. Palmer writes that "a characteristic of the auxiliaries is their use in emphatic affirmation with nuclear stress upon the auxiliary", as in You must see him. He concedes that "any verbal form may have nuclear stress"; thus We saw them; however, auxiliaries stressed in this way are used for "the denial of the negative", whereas lexical verbs again use do‑support.[1]: 25–26 

  • You say you heard them? / No, we SAW them.
  • You can't have seen them. / We DID see them.

NICE is widely cited (with "emphatic affirmation" usually simplified as "emphasis"): as examples, by A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language (1985),[11]: 121–124 [o] The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language (2002),[9]: 92–101  and the Oxford Modern English Grammar (2011).[10]: 68–69 

The NICER criteria edit

A revised set of criteria, NICER, owes much to NICE but does more than merely add a fifth criterion to it.

The NICER properties[19]: 3 
Auxiliary verb Lexical verb
(Finite) Negation Lee will not eat apples. *Lee eats not apples.
Auxiliary-initial constructions Has Lee eaten apples? *Eats Lee apples?
"Contraction" of not didn't, shouldn't, isn't *eatn't, *gon't, *maken't
(Post-auxiliary) Ellipsis Lee was eating and Kim was too. *Lee kept eating and Kim kept too.
Rebuttal A: We shouldn't eat apples.

B: We should SO.

A: We didn't try to eat apples.

B: *We tried SO.

In this section, a number of verbs – not limited to those in the paradigm table above – are checked against four of the five criteria of NICER. As would, might, could and should are sometimes understood as discrete verbs (and not merely as the preterite forms of will, may, can and shall), they are tested too.

NICER: Negation edit

Auxiliary verbs can be negated with not; lexical verbs require do-support: a less stringent version of "negation" as the first criterion of NICE.[19]: 10, 38–48 

We add not immediately after the verb, and obtain: She will/would/may/might/can/could/shall/should/must/need/dare not live there. Each of these has clausal negation, as we see by adding a positive tag and thereby creating a straightforward question: She can not live there, can she?; She need not live there, need she?; and so forth. (Compare She can live there, can she? and She needs to live there, does she?. In both of these a positive tag is added to a positive clause, for a result that is not a straightforward question. Context and tone of voice may suggest that the speaker is impressed or incredulous.)

Similarly for She ought/used not to live there; She is not a resident; She does not live there; and She has not lived there; and indeed for She wants to not go, awkward though this may sound.

This criterion does not require the same verb for the tag as in the anchor (the part of the sentence that precedes the tag). So the informal better works as well: She better not be late, had she? Irrespective of any tag, lexical do does not work (*You did not your homework), and neither does go (*He goes not to school). Putting not immediately after some other lexical verbs brings a grammatical result (He seems/intends not to live there), but one that does not work as expected with a positive tag – He seems/intends not to live there, does he? do not straightforwardly ask – showing that what not has negated is not the clause as a whole.

NICER: Auxiliary-initial constructions edit

The same as "inversion" as the second criterion of NICE.[19]: 8–9, 27–33 

Will/would/may/might/can/could/shall/should/must/need/dare I wear a mankini? – all of these can invert with the subject.

Likewise for Ought/used/have you to wear a suit?; Am I forced to wear a suit?; and Do I wear a suit?

This again accounts for all of the verbs in the paradigm table above, other than to. The construction requires a tensed form of the verb; to lacks one, and therefore this criterion does not apply to it.

Attempts to invert lexical verbs such as do (*Did you your homework?) or go (*Goes he to school?) bring ungrammatical results. Surprisingly, How goes it? is grammatical; but even a minor adjustment to it (*How went it?; *How goes your job?) brings an ungrammatical result, showing that it is merely a fixed formula.

NICER: "Contraction" of not edit

Most English auxiliary verbs have a negative inflected form with -n't,[9]: 1611 [19]: 10, 49–54 [20] commonly regarded as a contracted form of not. Available are won't, wouldn't, mightn't, can't, couldn't, shan't, shouldn't, mustn't, oughtn't, needn't, aren't, isn't, wasn't, weren't, don't, doesn't, didn't, haven't, hasn't, hadn't, and %usedn't.

No lexical verb has such a form (*She gon't to bars much these days; *She didn't her homework last week).

A small number of defective auxiliary verbs lack this inflection: %mayn't and *daredn't are now dated, and there is no universally accepted negative inflection of am: %amn't is dialectal, the acceptability of ain't depends on the variety of Standard English, and aren't is only used when it and I are inverted (Aren't I invited?, compare *I aren't tired).[9]: 1611–1612 

For do, must, used (/just/), and (depending on the variety of Standard English) can, the negative inflected form is spelt as expected but its pronunciation is anomalous (change of vowel in don't and perhaps can't; elision of /t/ within the root of mustn't and usedn't); for shan't and won't, both the pronunciation and the spelling are anomalous.

NICER: Post-auxiliary ellipsis edit

The same as "code" as the third criterion of NICE.[19]: 9–10, 33–38 

The possibility of ellipsis with will, may, might, can't, should, needn't and have (and indeed to) is illustrated in Firth's example of "code".

As for the other auxiliary verbs:

  • I'll attend if I must/dare/%ought.
  • If you're attending then I shall/am.
  • I'd be grateful if you would/could/did.
  • I'll go if you do.
  • I haven't swum much recently but I used/ought/want/hope to.

This is not possible with used (although it was in the past[p]), or with most lexical verbs: *I haven't swum much recently but I used/want/hope.

It does however work with a number of lexical verbs: I'd be grateful if you tried/started/stopped.

NICER: Rebuttal edit

When two people are arguing, one may use a stressed too or so immediately after the auxiliary verb to deny a statement made by the other. For example, having been told that he didn't do his homework, a child may reply I did too. (Or anyway, this is true for US English. For British English, indeed.[19]: 54n ) This kind of rebuttal is impossible with lexical verbs.[19]: 10, 54–57 

Additional criteria edit

Each of the two most compendious of postwar reference grammars of English offers a more detailed list of criteria for auxiliary verbs.

A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language has eight criteria. The first five of these approximate to the four of NICE, with the addition of cliticization as in It's raining or I've finished.[11]: 121–125  Slightly simplified, the sixth is that auxiliary verbs, unlike lexical verbs, "typically, but not necessarily" precede adverbs such as always, never, certainly and probably: He would always visit her (compare lexical verb visit in *He visited always her). The seventh is that "Quantifiers like all, both, and each which modify the subject of the clause may occur after the [verb] as an alternative, in many instances, to the predeterminer position"; thus either Both their children will attend or Their children will both attend (compare lexical verb attend in *Their children attended both).[q] The last is "Independence of subject", a claim that, compared with most lexical verbs, auxiliary verbs can be semantically independent of their subjects. This in turn is claimed to be manifested in three ways.[11]: 126–127  (The book provides four additional criteria for modal auxiliary verbs.[11]: 127–128 )

The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language adopts NICE and criteria approximating to the sixth and seventh of Comprehensive Grammar, although it dispenses with the eighth.[9]: 101–102  (It provides five additional criteria for modal auxiliary verbs.[9]: 106–107 )

Boundary fuzziness edit

Linguists who cite or propound clear grammatical criteria for auxiliary verbs then proceed to include among auxiliary verbs certain verbs that do not meet all these criteria. Having said that the English auxiliary verbs[r] "are rather sharply defined . . . by distinctive formal properties",[2]: 3  Anthony R. Warner points out that a class:

normally has some internal differentiation whereby a "nuclear" or "prototypical"[s] set of members shows more of the properties of the class than other less fully characterized members. A class may also not show sharply definable boundaries.[2]: 10 

He claims that what are the prototypical auxiliary verbs are the modal auxiliary verbs (other than ought, need, and dare) and that:

the presence of [be, do and have] in the category [auxiliary verb] is justified from a semantic point of view not so much by their possession of prototypical properties as by the fact that they are even more remote [than are the modal auxiliary verbs] from the [lexical verb] prototype, which denotes an action or event. . . .[2]: 19 

Infinitival to edit

Various linguists, notably Geoff Pullum, have suggested that the to of I want to go (not the preposition to as in I went to Rome) is a special case of an auxiliary verb with no tensed forms.[23][t] Rodney Huddleston argues against this position in The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language,[9]: 1183–1187  but Robert Levine counters these arguments.[25] In a book on the historical emergence and spread of infinitival to, Bettelou Los calls Pullum's arguments that it is an auxiliary verb "compelling".[26]

In terms of the NICER properties, examples like it's fine not to go show that to allows negation. Inversion, contraction of not, and rebuttal would only apply to tensed forms, and to is argued to have none. Although rebuttal is not possible, it does allow ellipsis: I don't want to.

(Had) better, (woul)d rather, and others edit

With their normal senses (as in You had better/best arrive early), had/'d better and had/'d best are not about the past. Indeed they do not seem to be usable for the past (*Yesterday I had better return home before the rain started); and they do not occur with other forms of have (*have/has better/best). The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language observes:

If we take [the had in had better] as a distinct lexeme, we will say that it has been reanalysed as a present tense form (like must and ought). . . . [In view of its syntactic behaviour], it undoubtedly should be included among the non-central members of the modal auxiliary class.[9]: 113 

Expressions ranging from had better to would rather have been argued to comprise "a family of morphosyntactic configurations with a moderate degree of formal and semantic homogeneity".[27]: 3  They would be:

  • Superlative modals: had best, 'd best
  • Comparative modals: had better, 'd better, better, would rather, 'd rather, had rather, should rather, would sooner, 'd sooner, had sooner, should sooner
  • Equative modals: would (just) as soon as, may (just) as well, might (just) as well

Among these, had better, 'd better, better occur the most commonly. They express either advice or a strong hope: a deontic and an optative sense respectively.[27]: 3–5 

Among these three forms, 'd better is the commonest in British English and plain better the commonest in American English.[27]: 11  However, the syntactic category of plain better when used in this or a similar way is not always clear: while it may have been reanalysed as an independent modal auxiliary verb – one with no preterite form and also no ability to invert (*Better I leave now?) – it can be an adverb instead of a verb.[27]: 21–23 

For more about would rather/sooner and would as soon, see Would rather, would sooner, and would as soon.

Contributions by auxiliaries to meaning and syntax edit

An auxiliary verb is traditionally understood as a verb that "helps" another verb by adding (only) grammatical information to it.[u] So understood, English auxiliaries include:

However, this understanding of auxiliaries has trouble with be (He wasn't asleep; Was he asleep?), have (%He hadn't any money), and would (Would you rather we left now?), each of which behaves syntactically like an auxiliary verb even when not accompanying another verb (or not merely doing so). Other approaches to defining auxiliary verbs are described below.

Be edit

Passive voice edit

Be, followed by the past participle of a lexical verb, realizes the passive voice: He was promoted.[9]: 1427ff  Its negative and interrogative versions (He wasn't promoted; Was he promoted?), lacking the need for do‑support, show that this is auxiliary be. (This simple test can be repeated for the other applications of be briefly described below.)

(However, the lexical verb get can also form a passive clause: He got promoted.[9]: 1429–1430  This is a long-established construction.[28]: 118 )

Followed by the present participle of a verb (whether lexical or auxiliary), be realizes the progressive aspect: He was promoting the film.[9]: 117, 119ff 

Either may be confused with the use of a participial adjective (that is, an adjective derived from and homonymous with a participle): He was excited; It was exciting.[v]

Other uses edit

What The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language terms quasi-modal be normally imparts a deontic meaning: that of He is never to come here again approximates to that of "He must never come here again".[9]: 113–114  In conditional contexts, was to (if both informal and with a singular subject) or were to imparts remoteness: If I were to jump out of the plane, . . . (compare with the open conditional If I jumped out of the plane, . . .).[9]: 151  In common with modal auxiliary verbs, quasi-modal be has no secondary form.[9]: 114 

What the same work terms motional be only occurs as been, when it follows the verb have in a perfect construction and is not followed by any verb: I've twice been to Minsk. Most of the NICE/NICER criteria are inapplicable, but sentences such as I don't need to go to the Grand People's Study House as I've already been show that it satisfies the "code" and "ellipsis" criteria of NICE and NICER respectively and thus is auxiliary rather than lexical be.[9]: 114 

What the Cambridge Grammar terms copular be links a subject, typically a noun phrase, and a predicative complement, typically a noun phrase, adjective phrase, or preposition phrase. Ascriptive copular be ascribes a property to the subject (The car was a wreck); specifying copular be identifies the subject (The woman in the green shoes is my aunt Louise) and can be reversed with a grammatical result (My aunt Louise is the woman in the green shoes). Be in an it‑cleft (It was my aunt Louise who wore the green shoes) is specifying.[9]: 266–267 

Auxiliary be also takes as complements a variety of words (able, about, bound, going and supposed among them) that in turn take as complements to‑infinitival subordinate clauses for results that are highly idiomatic (was about/supposed to depart, etc).[29]: 209 

In Early Modern English, perfect tenses could be formed with either have (as today) or be. The latter pattern persisted into the 19th century: a character in Pride and Prejudice says, But before I am run away with by my feelings on this subject, perhaps it will be advisable for me to state my reasons for marrying.[30]

Do edit

Do-support edit

The auxiliary verb do is primarily used for do‑support. This in turn is used for negation, interrogative main clauses, and more.

If a positive main clause is headed by an auxiliary verb, either the addition of not or (for most auxiliary verbs) a ‑n't inflection can negate. So They could reach home before dark becomes They couldn't reach home before dark. (This is the "negation" of NICE and NICER.) However, a lexical verb has to be supported by the verb do; so They reached home before dark becomes They didn't reach home before dark.[9]: 94–95 

If a declarative main clause is headed by an auxiliary verb, simple inversion of subject and verb will create a closed interrogative clause. So They could reach home before dark becomes Could they reach home before dark?. (This is the "inversion" of NICE and NICER.) However, a lexical verb requires do; so They reached home before dark becomes Did they reach home before dark? For an open interrogative clause, do has the same role: How far did you get?[9]: 95 

Although interrogative main clauses are by far the most obvious contexts for inversion using do‑support, there are others: While exclamative clauses usually lack subject–auxiliary inversion (What a foolish girl I was), it is a possibility (What a foolish girl was I[31]); the inverted alternative to How wonderful it tasted! would be How wonderful did it taste! A negative constituent that is not the subject can move to the front and trigger such inversion: None of the bottles did they leave unopened. A phrase with only can do the same: Only once did I win a medal. Ditto for phrases starting with so and such: So hard/Such a beating did Douglas give Tyson that Tyson lost. And in somewhat old-fashioned or formal writing, a miscellany of other constituents can be moved to the front with the same effect: Well do I remember, not so much the whipping, as the being shut up in a dark closet behind the study;[32]for years and years did they believe that France was on the brink of ruin.[33][9]: 95–96 

Negative imperative sentences require auxiliary do, even when there is another auxiliary verb. The declarative sentence They were goofing off is grammatical with the single auxiliary be; but the imperative sentence Don't be goofing off when the principal walks in adds don't. (Optionally, you may be added in front of or immediately after don't. A longer subject would normally come after: Don't any of you be goofing off. . . .)[9]: 928 

Emphatic polarity edit

Other than via a negative inflection (don't, doesn't), the verb do does not typically contribute any change in meaning, except when used to add emphasis to an accompanying verb. This is described as an emphatic construction,[11]: 133  as an emphatic version of the declarative clause,[10]: 74  as having emphatic polarity,[9]: 97–98  or is called the emphatic mood[according to whom?]: An example would be (i) I DO run five kilometres every morning (with intonational stress placed on do), compared to plain (ii) I run five kilometres every morning. It also differs from (iii) I RUN five kilometres every morning (with the stress on run): A context for (i), with its "emphasis on positive polarity", would be an allegation that the speaker didn't do so every morning; for (iii), with its "emphasis on lexical content", an allegation that the speaker merely walked. Do can be used for emphasis on negative polarity as well: He never DID remember my birthday.[9]: 98 

For emphatic positive polarity in imperatives, do is again added; thus standard Be quiet becomes emphatically positive Do be quiet.[9]: 929 

Have edit

Perfect tenses edit

Followed by the past participle of a verb (whether lexical or auxiliary), the auxiliary verb have realizes a perfect tense: Has she visited Qom?; Has she been to Qom?. In addition to its tensed forms (have/‑ve, has/‑s, had/‑d, haven't, hasn't, hadn't), it has a plain form (She could have arrived) and a present participle (I regret having lost it), but no past participle.[9]: 111 

("Perfect" is a syntactic term; in the context of English, "perfective" is a matter of semantic interpretation. Unlike, say, Slavic languages, which do have direct grammatical expression of perfectivity,[34]: 136  in English, a sentence using a perfect tense may or may not have a perfective interpretation.[35]: 57–58 )

The present perfect tense is illustrated by I've left it somewhere; the past perfect (also called the preterite perfect) tense by I'd left it somewhere.[9]: 140–141  A full description of their uses is necessarily complex: the discussion in The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language is long and intricate.[9]: 139–148 

The perfect is often considered as referring to an indefinite past: I've been to Oslo might raise the question of when, but is acceptable as is; by contrast, #I've been to Oslo in 2016, specifying the time, would be strange. A more careful analysis brings the continuative perfect, the experiential (or existential) perfect, the resultative perfect, and the perfect of recent past.[9]: 143 [w] The first, with an unspecified starting point and continuing uninterruptedly to the present, is illustrated by I've lived in Oslo since 2016; the experiential by Yes, I've watched a bullfight, and I never want to watch one again; the perfect of result by I've just watched a bullfight, and now I feel rather sick; and that of recent past by George Santos has just given a press conference (usable at the time of writing, but likely to become odder as time passes).[34]: 98–99 

Very simply, the present perfect refers to the past in a way that has some relevance to the present.[35]: 63–64  The perfect is also used in contexts that require both past reference and an untensed verb form (He seems to have left; Having left, he lit a cigarette).[35]: 65–66 

Corpus-based research has shown that American English saw a marked decrease from around 1800 until the mid-20th century in the use of the present perfect, and that British English followed this in the late 20th century.[37]

Other uses edit

When used to describe an event, have is exclusively a lexical verb (*Had you your teeth done?; Did you have your teeth done?; *Had you a nap?; Did you have a nap?). When used to describe a state, however, for many speakers (although for few Americans or younger people) there is also an auxiliary option: (he'd stop at a pub, settle up with a cheque because he hadn't any money on him;[38] Hasn't he any friends of his own?;[39] I'm afraid I haven't anything pithy to answer;[40] This hasn't anything directly to do with religion[41]).[9]: 111–112 [35]: 54  An alternative to auxiliary verb have in this sense is have got, although this is commoner among British speakers, and less formal[9]: 111–113  (Has he got old news for you;[42] It hasn’t got anything to do with the little green men and the blue orb;[43] What right had he got to get on this train without a ticket?;[44] Hasn't he got a toolbox?[45]).

With their meaning of obligation, have to, has to and had to – rarely if ever rendered as 've to, 's to and 'd to[46] – can use auxiliary have for inversion (if he wants to compel A. to do something to what Court has he to go?;[47] How much further has he to go?;[48] Now why has he to wait three weeks?[49]), although lexical have is commoner.

Use edit

Use /jus/ (rhyming with loose) satisfies only one of The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language's five criteria for modal as distinct from other auxiliary verbs. "It is also semantically quite distinct from the modal auxiliaries: the meaning it expresses is aspectual, not modal."[9]: 115 

Like ought, use is followed by a to-infinitival clause. Thus I used to go to college means that formerly the speaker habitually went to college, and normally implies that they no longer do so. Use is highly defective, existing only in preterite form. For some speakers of English as a first language (though very few Americans), it can follow auxiliary-verb syntax: some speakers of British English can form questions like Used he to come here? and negatives like He used not (rarely usedn't) to come here.[11]: 140  Far commoner, however, is treatment of used as the preterite of a lexical verb.

Whether auxiliary or lexical, used expresses past states or past habitual actions, usually with the implication that they no longer continue. After noting how constructions employing used (We used to play tennis every week), would (We would play tennis every week), and the preterite alone (We played tennis every week) often seem to be interchangeable, Robert I. Binnick teases them apart, concluding that used is an "anti-present-perfect": whereas the present perfect "includes the present in what is essentially a period of the past", the used construction "precisely excludes [it]"; and further that

The whole point of the used to construction is not to report a habit in the past but rather to contrast a past era with the present. . . . It's . . . essentially a present tense. . . . Like the present perfect, it is about a state of affairs, not a series of occurrences.[50]: 41, 43 

Use is far more commonly encountered as a lexical than as an auxiliary verb, particularly for younger or American speakers. This forms questions and negatives with did. The plain form use (sometimes spelt ⟨used⟩) of the lexical verb is seen in Did you use to play tennis?). Its preterite perfect had used is rare but attested. A simple declarative (I often used to play tennis) could be either auxiliary or lexical.

Use of the preterite used should not be confused with that of the participial adjective (i.e. the adjective etymologically derived from the participle), meaning "familiar with", as in I am used to this, We must get used to the cold. (As is common for adjectives and impossible for verbs, used here can be modified by very.) When the participial adjective is followed by to and a verb, the latter is a gerund-participle: I am used to going to college in the mornings.

Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that used not to, usedn't to (both auxiliary), and didn't use to (lexical) were then rare in both American and British English, other than used not to in British novels. Never used to is a commonly used alternative.[51]: 165  Modal auxiliary use is not used in interrogatives in conversation (Used you to . . . ?); and even the lexical version with do-support (Did you use to . . . ?) is rare.[51]: 218 

To edit

In the context for an argument that infinitival to is a subordinator, Rodney Huddleston points out that, just as for the subordinator that (I said (that) he could), there are contexts where to is optional, with no change in meaning.[x] His example is All I did was (to) ask a question; and from it he infers that to is meaningless.[9]: 1186 

Within an argument for categorizing to not as a subordinator but as an auxiliary verb, Robert D. Levine disagrees with the main thrust of Huddleston's argument, but not with the claim that to is meaningless – something that is also true of "dummy do" and copular be, both of them auxiliary verbs.[25]: 191–192  Its function is purely syntactic.

Modal auxiliary verbs edit

The modal auxiliary verbs contribute meaning chiefly via modality, although some of them (particularly will and sometimes shall) express future time reference. Their uses are detailed at English modal verbs, and tables summarizing their principal meaning contributions can be found in the articles Modal verb and Auxiliary verb.

For more details on the uses of auxiliaries to express aspect, mood and time reference, see English clause syntax.

Auxiliary verbs in sequence edit

Modal auxiliary verbs in sequence edit

As modal verbs only have tensed forms in Standard English, they would not be expected to appear in subordinate clauses, or in sequence (might be able to help them, but *might could help them). Yet what appear to be sequences of modal auxiliary verbs do occur: see "Double modals".

They can hardly be regarded as part of Standard English, and they are therefore ignored in the description below.

Other auxiliary verbs in sequence edit

There are constraints on the order within sequences of auxiliary verbs. As the modal auxiliary verbs and use only have tensed forms (or anyway only have these in Standard English), they can only go at the front. If we put aside the highly anomalous to, the order is then modal > perfect have > progressive be > passive be, and a lexical verb.

Patterns with two auxiliary verbs are exemplified by was being eaten, has been eaten, might be eaten, and might have eaten. Patterns with three include that exemplified by might have been eaten. Noting that "Structures containing two secondary forms of be (progressive and passive) [. . .] are avoided by some speakers, but they do occasionally occur", Huddleston and Pullum present will have been being taken as an example of a sequence with four.[9]: 104–106 

Unstressed and contracted forms edit

Contractions are a common feature of English, used frequently in ordinary speech. In written English, contractions are used in informal and sometimes in formal writing.[52] They usually involve the elision of a vowel – an apostrophe being inserted in its place in written English – possibly accompanied by other changes. Many of these contractions involve auxiliary verbs.

Certain contractions tend to be restricted to less formal speech and very informal writing, such as John'd or Mary'd for "John/Mary would". (Compare the personal pronoun forms I'd and you'd, much more likely to be encountered in relatively informal writing.) This applies in particular to constructions involving consecutive contractions, such as wouldn't've for "would not have".[citation needed]

Contractions in English are generally not mandatory, as they are in some other languages, although in speech uncontracted forms may seem overly formal. They are often used for emphasis: I AM ready! The uncontracted form of an auxiliary or copula must be used in elliptical sentences where its complement is omitted: Who's ready? / I am! (not *I'm!).

Some contractions lead to homophony, which sometimes causes errors in writing, such as confusing ‑'ve with of, as in "would of" for would have.[53]: 188 

Unstressed and contracted forms of individual verbs edit

The lists below derive from F. R. Palmer's The English Verb[12]: 242–248  and The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language.: 1613 

For the contracted forms of the modal auxiliary verbs, see English modal auxiliary verbs.

Unstressed and contracted forms of be edit

  • am /ˈæm//əm/, /m/ ('m)
  • is /ˈɪz//z/ or /s/ ('s)
  • are /ˈɑɹ//əɹ/, /ɹ/ ('re)
  • was /ˈwɒz//wəz/
  • were /ˈwəɹ//ɹ/ ('re)
  • be /ˈbi//bɪ/
  • been /ˈbin//bɪn/

In a non-rhotic dialect, clitic-final /ɹ/ is only realized as [ɹ] (or similar) when followed by a vowel (They're tired, no /ɹ/; They're angry, with /ɹ/).

For the contraction options for is, consider Bill's arriving /ˈbɪlz əˈɹaɪvɪŋ/ versus Janet's coming /ˈd͡ʒænɪts ˈkʌmɪŋ/.

Unstressed and contracted forms of do edit

  • does /ˈdʌz//dəz/, /z/ or /s/ ('s)
  • do /ˈdu//də/, /d/ (d' or 'd)

For the alternative nonsyllabic options for does, consider When's Bill leave? /ˈwɛnz ˈbɪl ˈliv/ versus What's Bill do? /ˈwɒts ˈbɪl ˈdu/.

The form 'd might appear in for example What'd he do?, spoken informally.

Uniquely among the forms for any of the auxiliary verbs, d' is a proclitic. It attaches to the front of the single word you (D'you follow me?).[9]: 1614 

Unstressed and contracted forms of have edit

  • have /ˈhæv//həv/, /əv/, /v/ ('ve), /ə/ (woulda, musta, etc)
  • has /ˈhæz//həz/, /əz/, /z/ or /s/ ('s)
  • had /ˈhæd//həd/, /əd/, /d/ ('d)

For the alternative nonsyllabic options for has, consider Bill's arrived /ˈbɪlz əˈɹaɪvd/ versus Janet's come /ˈd͡ʒænɪts ˈkʌm/.

Unstressed form of to edit

  • to /ˈtu//tə/

Double contractions edit

Being clitics, the contractions can replace their full equivalents in most (although not all) contexts: thus we see ‑ve not only in They've left but also in My friends've left (or even in My friends I hadn't seen in three years've left); not only in You should've been there but also in You shouldn't've been there, in which a contraction has clitized onto an auxiliary verb with negative inflection.

Double contractions are possible. Will have broken is grammatical, and thus His arm/helmet/glasses/rib/collarbone/nose [etc] 'll've broken are all grammatical too.

Negative inflected forms edit

Position of not/‑n't in a negative closed interrogative
After, or as an inflectional
suffix of, the auxiliary verb
After the subject
not *Would not you like another glass? Would you not like another glass?
-n't Wouldn't you like another glass? *Would you n't like another glass?

Contractions such as ‑d /(ə)d/ (from would) are clitics. By contrast, the ‑n't /(ə)nt/ of wouldn't is in reality a "contraction" only etymologically: wouldn't, isn't, haven't and so forth have long been inflected forms, and an auxiliary verb with negative inflection can behave differently from the combination of not and the same verb without the inflection:[9]: 91 

This article will continue to use "contraction" to include early instances of what at the time may not have become inflected forms.

During the early 17th century, not lost its requirement for stress, and subsequently came to be written as ‑n't, particularly in comedies and in the mouths of rustic characters or others speaking nonstandard dialects. In the 19th and 20th centuries, the use of ‑n't in writing spread beyond drama and fiction to personal letters, journalism, and descriptive texts.[54] An't, ben't, can't, don't, han't, shan't and won't were well established by the end of the 17th century; isn't, aren't, wasn't, weren't, didn't, doesn't, don't, hadn't, hasn't, haven't, can't, couldn't, daren't, mayn't (now obsolete or dialectal), mightn't, mustn't, needn't, shan't, shouldn't, won't and wouldn't by the end of the 18th; and oughtn't in the early 19th.[54]: 176, 189 

There were various other negative contractions that have not survived: as examples, Barron Brainerd cites A. C. Partridge as showing that from 1599 to 1632 Ben Jonson used i'not ("is not"), sha'not ("shall not"), wi'not ("will not"), wu'not and wou'not ("would not"), ha'not ("has/have not"), and do'not ("do not").[55][54]: 179–180 

Negative inflection of am edit

Standard English has no first-person singular form corresponding to the isn't of it isn't and isn't it? that is completely unproblematic. However, the following informal or dialectal options have been used:

Amn't edit

Otto Jespersen calls amn't "unpronounceable"[56]: 120  and Eric Partridge calls it "ugly",[57] but it is the standard inflected form in some varieties, mainly Hiberno-English (Irish English) and Scottish English.[20][58] In Hiberno-English the question form (amn't I?) is used more frequently than the declarative I amn't.[59] (The standard I'm not is available as an alternative to I amn't in both Scottish English and Hiberno-English.) An example appears in a poem by Oliver St. John Gogarty: If anyone thinks that I amn't divine, / He gets no free drinks when I'm making the wine. These lines are quoted in James Joyce's Ulysses,[60] which also contains other examples: Amn't I with you? Amn't I your girl? (spoken by Cissy Caffrey).[61]

Amnae edit

Amnae exists in Scots, and has been borrowed into Scottish English by many speakers. It is used in declarative sentences rather than questions.[59]

Ain't edit

Ain't is an inflected alternative to am not – and also to is not, was not, are not, were not, has not, and have not;[62]: 60–64  and in some dialects also do not, does not, did not, cannot (or can not), could not, will not, would not and should not. The usage of ain't is a perennial subject of controversy in English.[63] Geoffrey Nunberg has argued that ain't is used by Standard English speakers "to suggest that a fact is just obvious on the face of things".[64]

Aren't edit

Aren't is a very common means of filling the "amn't gap" in questions: Aren't I lucky to have you around? It was common by the early 20th century: Otto Jespersen writing in a book published in 1917 that:

Nowadays [/ɑːnt/] is frequently heard, especially in tag-questions: I'm a bad boy [/ɑːntaɪ/], but when authors want to write it, they are naturally induced to write aren't. . . . I find the spelling aren't I or arn't I pretty frequently in George Eliot . . . but only to represent vulgar or dialectal speech. In the younger generation of writers, however, it is also found as belonging to educated speakers. . . .[56]: 119 [y]

The style guides have disagreed on aren't: Eric Partridge considered the aren't in aren't I an "illogical and illiterate" spelling of "the phonetically natural and the philologically logical" a'n't;[57] H. W. Fowler (as revised by Ernest Gowers) wrote that aren't I? was "colloquially respectable and almost universal".[65] In 1979, however, it was described as "almost universal" among speakers of Standard English.[66] As an alternative to am not, aren't developed from one pronunciation of an't (which itself developed in part from amn't). In non-rhotic dialects, aren't and an't are homophones, and the spelling ⟨aren't I⟩ began to replace ⟨an't I⟩ in the early 20th century,[62]: 115–116  although examples of ⟨aren't I⟩ (or ⟨arn't I⟩) for am I not appear in the first half of the 19th century, as in arn't I listening; and isn't it only the breeze that's blowing the sheets and halliards about? from 1827.[67]

An't edit

An't (sometimes a'n't) arose from "am not" (via "amn't") and "are not" almost simultaneously. "An't" first appears in print in the work of English Restoration playwrights. In 1695 "an't" was used for "am not", and as early as 1696 "an't" was used to mean "are not". "An't" for "is not" may have developed independently from its use for "am not" and "are not". "Isn't" was sometimes written as "in't" or "en't", which could have changed into "an't". "An't" for "is not" may also have filled a gap in the paradigm for the verb be. From 1749, an't with a long "a" sound began to be written as ain't. By this time, an't was already being used for "am not", "are not", and "is not". An't and ain't coexisted as written forms well into the 19th century.

Bain't edit

Bain't, apparently from "be not", is found in a number of works employing eye dialect, including J. Sheridan Le Fanu's Uncle Silas.[68] It is also found in a ballad written in Newfoundland dialect.[69]

Other negative inflections of have edit

Han't or ha'n't, an early contraction for has not and have not, developed from the elision of the s of has not and the v of have not. Han't also appeared in the work of English Restoration playwrights. Much like an't, han't was sometimes pronounced with a long a, yielding hain't. With H-dropping, the h of han't or hain't gradually disappeared in most dialects, and became ain't. Ain't as a contraction for has not/have not appeared in print as early as 1819. As with an't, hain't and ain't were found together late into the nineteenth century.

Hain't, in addition to being an antecedent of ain’t, is a contraction of has not and have not in some dialects of English, such as Appalachian English. It is reminiscent of hae (have) in Lowland Scots. In dialects that retain the distinction between hain't and ain't, hain't is used for contractions of to have not and ain't for contractions of to be not.[70] In other dialects, hain't is used either in place of, or interchangeably with ain't. Hain't is seen for example in Chapter 33 of Mark Twain's Adventures of Huckleberry Finn: I hain't come back—I hain't been GONE.

Other negative inflections of do edit

Don't is the Standard English negative inflected form of do. However, in nonstandard English it may also be used for third person singular: Emma? She don't live here anymore.

Notes edit

  1. ^ Wiseman uses "regular", "irregular", "active" and "passive" with the meanings they still have in relation to verbs. A "neuter verb", he writes: "Comes from the Latin neuter, neither, because it is neither Active nor Passive, it denotes the existence of a person or thing, making a complete sense of itself, and requires no noun or other word to be joined with it, as, I sleep, we run, she cries, &c." An "impersonal verb", he writes, is one: "Having only the third person singular and plural applied to things both animate and inanimate, as, it freezes, it is said or they say, they grow, &c."[6]: 155 
  2. ^ More precisely: Does there exist a lexical verb with the same spelling and pronunciation that is synonymous or could be said to have an auxiliary (or copular) function? (Ignored here is any lexical verb – will meaning "exert one's will in an attempt to compel", can meaning "insert into cans", etc – that is unlikely to be mistaken for the auxiliary verb.)
  3. ^ "[T]here is evidence that for some speakers [of Standard English] may and might have diverged to the extent that they are no longer inflectional forms of a single lexeme, but belong to distinct lexemes, may and might, each of which – like must – lacks a preterite. . . ."[9]: 109 
  4. ^ The use of ought as a lexical verb as in They didn’t ought to go is generally thought of as restricted to non-standard dialects[11]: 140  but has been described as also sometimes found in informal standard usage.[13] "Lexical ought with the dummy operator do has been condemned in British usage handbooks. . . . What this censure suggests is that lexical ought with periphrastic do is a well-established usage in colloquial [British English]."[14]: 502 
  5. ^ a b An NPI, rare for speakers of Standard American English.[9]: 109 
  6. ^ The distinction between auxiliary and lexical is blurred: "lexical dare commonly occurs in non-affirmative contexts without to": She wouldn't dare ask her father; and it also "can be stranded", as in She ought to have asked for a raise, but she didn't dare.[9]: 110 
  7. ^ a b c This article uses an asterisk ⟨*⟩ to indicate ungrammatical examples; a percent sign ⟨%⟩ to indicate grammaticality for some speakers of English but not others; and a number sign ⟨#⟩ to indicate semantic or pragmatic oddity despite grammaticality.
  8. ^ Almost exclusively an NPI, found in the contexts exemplified by If you don't be quiet I'll ground you and Why don't you be quiet?[9]: 114 
  9. ^ Used for the imperative, as in Do be quiet. Uniquely among English auxiliary verbs, untensed do also has a negative inflected form: don't, as in Don't be noisy.[9]: 91–92 
  10. ^ In questions like What's he do?, rarely written.
  11. ^ In questions like What'd he do?, rarely written.
  12. ^ Pronounced /jus/ (rhyming with "loose"). Auxiliary verb form used should be distinguished from the homonymous adjective used, as in I've got (very) used to it.[15]: 85  (The homographic verb use /juz/, rhyming with "lose", is lexical only.) "For many speakers [of Standard English], especially younger ones", use /jus/ is exclusively a lexical verb.[9]: 115 
  13. ^ Rather than "lexical verb", Palmer uses the term "full verb".
  14. ^ Clausal negation can be tested by adding a straightforward interrogative tag (one simply asking for confirmation, not expressing incredulity, admiration, etc). A negative clause calls for a positive interrogative tag (and a positive clause calls for a negative one); thus the positive Do you? within You don't eat beef, do you? shows that You don't eat beef is negative. Despite lacking an auxiliary verb, You never/seldom/rarely eat beef is also negative.
  15. ^ The authors use NICE but do not name it.
  16. ^ Otto Jespersen points to an example by Leigh Hunt: "I did not stammer half so badly as I used".[21]: 222 [22]: 15 
  17. ^ If the context for Their children attended both is a discussion of two weddings, then it is of course grammatical. But if the intended meaning is instead "Both their children attended", it is not.
  18. ^ Warner's distinction is not between "auxiliary verbs" and "lexical verbs", but instead between "auxiliaries" and "verbs". His terminology has been adjusted for this article. The issue is not just one of naming; however, an account of the argument over whether what this article calls auxiliary verbs comprise a subcategory of "verbs" (as assumed in this article) or instead comprise an independent category (as Warner holds) is beyond the scope of this article.
  19. ^ Later in the book, Warner makes an appeal to prototype theory.[2]: 103–108 
  20. ^ Pullum credits unpublished insights of Paul Postal and Richard Hudson, and published work by Robert Fiengo.[24]
  21. ^ The Oxford English Dictionary, 2nd edition, 1989, defines an auxiliary verb as "a verb used to form the tenses, moods, voices, etc. of other verbs".
  22. ^ Very simply, if the word ending -ing can take an object; it is a verb; if it can be modified by either too in the sense of "excessively" or very, it is an adjective. (NB if it cannot take an object, it is not necessarily an adjective; if it cannot be so modified by too or very, it is not necessarily a verb. There are also participial prepositions and other complications.)[9]: 540–541 
  23. ^ The classification and terms are derived from Bernard Comrie,[36] although slightly revised.
  24. ^ This appears within chapter 14, "Non-finite and verbless clauses", attributed to Huddleston alone.
  25. ^ Jespersen provides lists of where examples may be found in the works of Eliot, Bennett, Benson, Galsworthy, Wells, Wilde and others. Rather than "/ɑːnt/" and "/ɑːntaɪ/?", Jespersen writes "[a·nt]" and "[a·nt ai?]" respectively.

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e Palmer, F. R. (1965). A Linguistic Study of the English Verb. London: Longmans, Green. OCLC 1666825.
  2. ^ a b c d e f Warner, Anthony R. (1993). English Auxiliaries: Structure and History. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 66. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-30284-5.
  3. ^ Oxford English Dictionary entry for auxiliary A1c.
  4. ^ Bullokar, William (1906). "Bref Grammar for English". In Plessow, Max (ed.). Geschichte der Fabeldichtung in England bis zu John Gay (1726). Nebst Neudruck von Bullokars "Fables of Aesop" 1585, "Booke at large" 1580, "Bref Grammar for English" 1586, und "Pamphlet for Grammar" 1586. Berlin: Mayer & Müller. pp. 339–385 – via Internet Archive.
  5. ^ Sterne, Laurence. The Life and Opinions of Tristram Shandy, Gentleman. Vol. 5. London – via Laurence Sterne in Cyberspace (Gifu University).
  6. ^ a b Wiseman, Charles (1764). A Complete English Grammar on a New Plan: For the Use of Foreigners, and Such Natives as Would Acquire a Scientifical Knowledge of Their Own Tongue . . . London: W. Nicol – via Google Books.
  7. ^ Fowler, William Chauncey (1857). The English Language, in Its Elements and Forms: With a History of Its Origin and Development. London: William Kent & Co. – via Google Books.
  8. ^ Ross, John Robert (1969). "Auxiliaries as main verbs" (PDF). Studies in Philosophical Linguistics. 1. Evanston, Illinois: Great Expectations. ISSN 0586-8882 – via Haj Ross's Papers on Syntax, Poetics, and Selected Short Subjects.
  9. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad ae af ag ah ai aj ak al am an ao ap aq ar as at au Huddleston, Rodney; Pullum, Geoffrey K. (2002). The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-43146-0.
  10. ^ a b c Aarts, Bas (2011). Oxford Modern English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-953319-0.
  11. ^ a b c d e f g h i Quirk, Randolph; Greenbaum, Sidney; Leech, Geoffrey; Svartvik, Jan (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language. London: Longman. ISBN 0-582-51734-6.
  12. ^ a b Palmer, F. R. (1988). The English Verb (2nd ed.). London: Longman. ISBN 0-582-01470-0.
  13. ^ Greenbaum, Sidney (1996). The Oxford English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 155. ISBN 978-0-19-861250-6.
  14. ^ Lee, Jackie F. K.; Collins, Peter (2004). "On the usage of have, dare, need, ought and used to in Australian English and Hong Kong English". World Englishes. 23 (4): 501–513. doi:10.1111/j.0083-2919.2004.00374.x.
  15. ^ Zandvoort, R. W. (1975). A Handbook of English Grammar (7th ed.). London: Longman. ISBN 0-582-55339-3.
  16. ^ a b c d Abbott, O. L. (October 1958). "Verbal endings in seventeenth-century American English". American Speech. 33 (3): 185–194. doi:10.2307/453204. JSTOR 453204.
  17. ^ Hogg, Richard M.; Blake, N. F.; Lass, Roger; Romaine, Suzanne; Burchfield, R. W.; Algeo, John (1992–2001). The Cambridge History of the English Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-26474-X. OCLC 23356833.
  18. ^ Firth, J. R. (1968) [May 1956]. "Descriptive linguistics and the study of English". In Palmer, F. R. (ed.). Selected Papers of J. R. Firth, 1952–59. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. pp. 96–113. OCLC 173639 – via Internet Archive.
  19. ^ a b c d e f g Sag, Ivan A.; Chaves, Rui P.; Abeillé, Anne; Estigarribia, Bruno; Flickinger, Dan; Kay, Paul; Michaelis, Laura A.; Müller, Stefan; Pullum, Geoffrey K.; Van Eynde, Frank; Wasow, Thomas (1 February 2020). "Lessons from the English auxiliary system". Journal of Linguistics. 56 (1): 87–155. doi:10.1017/S002222671800052X. hdl:20.500.11820/2a4f1c47-b2e1-4908-9d82-b02aa240befd. ISSN 0022-2267. S2CID 150096608.
  20. ^ a b Zwicky, Arnold M.; Pullum, Geoffrey K. (September 1983). "Cliticization vs. inflection: English n't". Language. 59 (3): 502–513. doi:10.2307/413900. ISSN 0097-8507. JSTOR 413900.
  21. ^ Hunt, Leigh (1891) [1828]. "An Account of Christ-Hospital". In Johnson, Reginald Brimley (ed.). Essays of Leigh Hunt (PDF). London: J. M. Dent. pp. 211–223 – via Internet Archive.
  22. ^ Jespersen, Otto (1961) [1931]. A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles. Vol. 4: Syntax (3, Time and Tense). London: George Allen & Unwin – via Internet Archive.
  23. ^ Pullum, Geoffrey K. (1982). "Syncategorematicity and English infinitival to". Glossa. 16: 181–215.
  24. ^ Fiengo, Robert (1980). Surface Structure: The Interface of Autonomous Components. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-85725-4.
  25. ^ a b Levine, Robert D. (2012). "Auxiliaries: To's company". Journal of Linguistics. 48 (1): 187–203. doi:10.1017/S002222671100034X. ISSN 0022-2267.
  26. ^ Los, Bettelou (2005). The Rise of the To-Infinitive. Oxford University Press. p. 208. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199274765.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-19-927476-5.
  27. ^ a b c d Van der Auwera, Johan; Noël, Dirk; Van linden, An (2013). "Had better, 'd better and better: Diachronic and transatlantic variation" (PDF). In Marín-Arrese, Juana I.; Carretero, Marta; Arús, Jorge; Van der Auwera, Johan (eds.). English modality: Core, periphery and evidentiality. Topics in English Linguistics 81. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. pp. 119–154. ISBN 9783110286328. Retrieved 29 November 2023 – via University of Liège.
  28. ^ Berk, Lynn M. (1999). English Syntax: From Word to Discourse. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-512353-1.
  29. ^ Depraetere, Ilse; Reed, Susan (2020). "Mood and modality in English". In Aarts, Bas; McMahon, April; Hinrichs, Lars (eds.). The Handbook of English Linguistics (PDF) (2nd ed.). Newark, New Jersey: Wiley. pp. 207–227. doi:10.1002/9781119540618.ch12. ISBN 9781119540564.
  30. ^ van Gelderen, Elly (2006). A History of the English Language. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. p. 215. ISBN 90-272-3236-9.
  31. ^ Mitchell, Joni (1968). "The Gift of the Magi". Joni Mitchell.
  32. ^ Groome, Francis Hindes (1895). Two Suffolk Friends. Edinburgh: William Blackwood. p. 10 – via Project Gutenberg.
  33. ^ Southey, Robert (1814). Letters from England: By Don Manuel Alvarez Espriella. Vol. 3 (3rd ed.). London: Longman, Hurst, Rees, Orme, and Brown – via Project Gutenberg.
  34. ^ a b Binnick, Robert I. (1991). Time and the Verb: A Guide to Tense and Aspect. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-506206-X.
  35. ^ a b c d Huddleston, Rodney; Pullum, Geoffrey K.; Reynolds, Brett (2022). A Student's Introduction to English Grammar (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-1-009-08574-8.
  36. ^ Comrie, Bernard (1976). Aspect. Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 56–61. ISBN 978-0-521-29045-6 – via Internet Archive.
  37. ^ Elsness, Johan (2009). "12. The present perfect and the preterite". In Rohdenburg, Günter; Schlüter, Julia (eds.). One Language, Two Grammars? Differences between British and American English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 228–245. ISBN 978-0-521-87219-5.
  38. ^ Harper, Nick (9 January 2004). "Interview: Jack Charlton". The Guardian. Retrieved 29 November 2023.
  39. ^ "No 2,024: Flipper the dolphin". The Guardian. 12 April 2002. Retrieved 29 November 2023.
  40. ^ Amis, Martin (31 December 1999). "What we'll be doing tonight". The Guardian. Retrieved 29 November 2023.
  41. ^ Brown, Andrew (23 December 2009). "Author! Author!". The Guardian. Retrieved 29 November 2023.
  42. ^ McCrum, Robert (20 April 2008). "Has he got old news for you". The Guardian. Retrieved 29 November 2023.)
  43. ^ Luscombe, Richard (13 October 2021). "William Shatner in tears after historic space flight: 'I'm so filled with emotion'". The Guardian. Retrieved 29 November 2023.)
  44. ^ Hoggart, Simon (25 November 2000). "No charm and plenty of offence is the railway response to a state of chaos". The Guardian. Retrieved 29 November 2023.)
  45. ^ Coren, Victoria (22 August 2010). "A Swann song for real men". The Guardian. Retrieved 29 November 2023.)
  46. ^ Kukucz, Marta (2009). Characteristics of English Modal Verbs (PDF) (PhD thesis). Palacký University Olomouc. p. 18.
  47. ^ Lord Dynevor (20 July 1926). "Commons Amendment". Parliamentary Debates (Hansard). Vol. 65. Parliament of the United Kingdom: Lords.
  48. ^ "12+ Entrance examination: Solihull: Sample paper: Mathematics". Solihull Preparatory School. Retrieved 29 November 2023.
  49. ^ "How does it feel for me?" (PDF). Healthwatch Leeds. NHS Leeds Clinical Commissioning Group. January 2020. Retrieved 29 November 2023.
  50. ^ Binnick, Robert I. (2006). "Used to and habitual aspect in English". Style. 40: 33–45. JSTOR 10.5325/style.40.1-2.33.
  51. ^ a b Biber, Douglas; Johansson, Stig; Leech, Geoffrey; Conrad, Susan; Finegan, Edward (1999). Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English. Harlow, Essex: Pearson Education. ISBN 0-582-23725-4 – via Internet Archive.
  52. ^ Castillo González, María del Pilar (2007). Uncontracted Negatives and Negative Contractions in Contemporary English: A Corpus-Based Study (PDF) (Ph.D. thesis). University of Santiago de Compostela. pp. 23–28.
  53. ^ Disterheft, Dorothy (1990). "The role of adaptive rules in language change". Diachronica. 7 (2): 181–198. doi:10.1075/dia.7.2.03dis.
  54. ^ a b c Brainerd, Barron (October 1989). "The contractions of not: A historical note". Journal of English Linguistics. 22 (2): 176–196. doi:10.1177/007542428902200202. from the original on 3 December 2023 – via Pennsylvania State University. (It appears that October 1989 was the scheduled time of publication but that actual publication only occurred in 1993.)
  55. ^ Partridge, A. C. (1953). The Accidence of Ben Jonson's Plays, Masques, and Entertainments; with an Appendix of Comparable Uses in Shakespeare. Cambridge: Bowes & Bowes. pp. 251–258. OCLC 924625.
  56. ^ a b Jespersen, Otto (1917). Negation in English and Other Languages. Det Kgl. Danske Videnskabernes Selskab: Historisk-filologiske Meddelelser I, 5. Copenhagen: Andr. Fred. Høst og søn. OCLC 457568567 – via Internet Archive.
  57. ^ a b Partridge, Eric (1948). "a'n't". Usage and Abusage: A Guide to Good English (4th ed.). London: Hamish Hamilton. OCLC 465857982 – via Internet Archive.
  58. ^ Rissanen, Matti (1999). "Isn't it? or is it not? On the order of postverbal subject and negative particle in the history of English". In Ingrid Tieken-Boon van Ostade; Gunnel Tottie; Wim van der Wurff (eds.). Negation in the History of English. Walter de Gruyter. pp. 189–206. ISBN 3-11-016198-2.
  59. ^ a b Bresnan, Joan (2002). "The Lexicon in Optimality Theory". In Paolo Merla; Suzanne Stevenson (eds.). The Lexical Basis of Sentence Processing: Formal, Computational and Experimental Issues. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 39–58. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.56.4001. ISBN 1-58811-156-3.
  60. ^ Joyce, James. "Chapter 1". Ulysses. Archived from the original on 22 October 2012.
  61. ^ Joyce, James. "Chapter 15". Ulysses. Archived from the original on 29 November 2012.
  62. ^ a b Gilman, E. Ward, ed. (1994). Merriam Webster's Dictionary of English Usage (2nd ed.). Springfield, Massachusetts: Merriam-Webster. ISBN 0-87779-132-5 – via Internet Archive.
  63. ^ Skinner, David (2012). The Story of Ain't: America, Its Language, and the Most Controversial Dictionary Ever Published. New York: HarperCollins. ISBN 978-0-06-202746-7.
  64. ^ Nunberg, Geoffrey. "Ain't Misbehavior". Geoffrey Nunberg. Retrieved 12 December 2023.
  65. ^ Fowler, H. W. (1965). "Be". In Gowers, Ernest (ed.). Fowler's Modern English Usage (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. OCLC 779416.
  66. ^ Jørgensen, Erik (1979). "'Aren't I?' and alternative patterns in modern English". English Studies. 60: 35–41. doi:10.1080/00138387908597940.
  67. ^ Griffin, Gerald (1827). "Holland-Tide"; or, Munster Popular Tales. London: W. Simpkin and R. Marshall. p. 281 – via Google Books.
  68. ^ J. Sheridan Le Fanu, Uncle Silas, ch. .
  69. ^ "The Outharbour Planter" by Maurice A. Devine (1859–1915) of Kings Cove, Bonavista Bay, NL: "The times bain't what they used to be, 'bout fifty ye'rs or so ago", as published in Old-Time Songs And Poetry Of Newfoundland: Songs Of The People From The Days Of Our Forefathers (first edition, p. 9, 1927; described ).
  70. ^ Malmstrom, Jean (1960). "Ain't again". The English Journal. 49: 204–205. doi:10.2307/809195. JSTOR 809195.

english, auxiliary, verbs, small, english, verbs, which, include, english, modal, auxiliary, verbs, others, although, auxiliary, verbs, english, widely, believed, lack, inherent, semantic, meaning, instead, modify, meaning, verbs, they, accompany, they, nowada. English auxiliary verbs are a small set of English verbs which include the English modal auxiliary verbs and a few others 1 19 2 11 12 Although the auxiliary verbs of English are widely believed to lack inherent semantic meaning and instead to modify the meaning of the verbs they accompany they are nowadays classed by linguists as auxiliary on the basis not of semantic but of grammatical properties among these that they invert with their subjects in interrogative main clauses Has John arrived and are negated either by the simple addition of not He has not arrived or with a very few exceptions by negative inflection He hasn t arrived Contents 1 History of the concept 1 1 Some historical examples 1 2 Auxiliary verbs as heads 1 3 Recent definitions 2 Auxiliary verbs distinguished grammatically 2 1 Archaic forms of be do and have 2 2 The NICE criteria 2 2 1 NICE Negation 2 2 2 NICE Inversion 2 2 3 NICE Code 2 2 4 NICE Emphatic affirmation 2 3 The NICER criteria 2 3 1 NICER Negation 2 3 2 NICER Auxiliary initial constructions 2 3 3 NICER Contraction of not 2 3 4 NICER Post auxiliary ellipsis 2 3 5 NICER Rebuttal 2 4 Additional criteria 2 5 Boundary fuzziness 2 6 Infinitival to 2 7 Had better woul d rather and others 3 Contributions by auxiliaries to meaning and syntax 3 1 Be 3 1 1 Passive voice 3 1 2 Other uses 3 2 Do 3 2 1 Do support 3 2 2 Emphatic polarity 3 3 Have 3 3 1 Perfect tenses 3 3 2 Other uses 3 4 Use 3 5 To 3 6 Modal auxiliary verbs 4 Auxiliary verbs in sequence 4 1 Modal auxiliary verbs in sequence 4 2 Other auxiliary verbs in sequence 5 Unstressed and contracted forms 5 1 Unstressed and contracted forms of individual verbs 5 1 1 Unstressed and contracted forms of be 5 1 2 Unstressed and contracted forms of do 5 1 3 Unstressed and contracted forms of have 5 1 4 Unstressed form of to 5 2 Double contractions 6 Negative inflected forms 6 1 Negative inflection of am 6 1 1 Amn t 6 1 2 Amnae 6 1 3 Ain t 6 1 4 Aren t 6 1 5 An t 6 1 6 Bain t 6 2 Other negative inflections of have 6 3 Other negative inflections of do 7 Notes 8 ReferencesHistory of the concept editWhen describing English the adjective auxiliary was formerly applied to any formative or subordinate elements of language e g prefixes prepositions 3 As applied to verbs its conception was originally rather vague and varied significantly Some historical examples edit The first English grammar Bref Grammar for English by William Bullokar published in 1586 does not use the term auxiliary but says All other verbs are called verbs neuters un perfect because they require the infinitive mood of another verb to express their signification of meaning perfectly and be these may can might or mought could would should must ought and sometimes will that being a mere sign of the future tense orthography standardized and modernized 4 353 In volume 5 1762 of Tristram Shandy the narrator s father explains that The verbs auxiliary we are concerned in here are am was have had do did make made suffer shall should will would can could owe ought used or is wont 5 146 147 Charles Wiseman s Complete English Grammar of 1764 notes that most verbs cannot be conjugated through all their Moods and Tenses without one of the following principal Verbs have and be The first serves to conjugate the rest by supplying the compound tenses of all Verbs both Regular and Irregular whether Active Passive Neuter or Impersonal as may be seen in its own variation amp c a Along with have and be it goes on to include do may can shall will as auxiliary verbs 6 156 167 W C Fowler s The English Language of 1857 says Auxiliary Verbs or Helping Verbs perform the same office in the conjugation of principal verbs which inflection does in the classical languages though even in those languages the substantive verb is sometimes used as a helping verb I The verbs that are always auxiliary to others are May can shall must II Those that are sometimes auxiliary and sometimes principal verbs are Will have do be and let 7 202 The verbs that all the sources cited above agree are auxiliary verbs are the modal auxiliary verbs may can and shall most also include be do and have Auxiliary verbs as heads edit Modern grammars do not differ substantially over membership in the list of auxiliary verbs though they have refined the concept and following an idea first put forward by John Ross in 1969 8 have tended to take the auxiliary verb not as subordinate to a main verb a concept that pedagogical grammars perpetuate but instead as the head of a verb phrase Examples include The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language and Bas Aarts Oxford Modern English Grammar 9 104 10 237 239 This is shown in the tree diagram below for the clause I can swim nbsp The clause has a subject noun phrase I and a head verb phrase VP headed by the auxiliary verb can The VP also has a complement clause which has a head VP with the head verb swim Recent definitions edit A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language 1985 says of verbs in auxiliary function that In contrast to full i e lexical verbs these verbs are capable of functioning as auxiliary or helping verbs cf 2 27f which seems to refer back to a table showing the main verb sink in various inflected forms following one to four auxiliary verbs be and have again in various inflected forms and may and must It is not obvious how this definition would exclude lexical verbs such as try in tried sinking tried to have sunk tried being sunk etc 11 62 120 although they would certainly fail the book s own list of criteria for auxiliary verbs as listed later 11 121 127 In his book English Auxiliaries History and Structure 1993 Anthony R Warner writes that the English auxiliary verbs are rather sharply defined as a group by distinctive formal properties 2 3 The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language 2002 describes auxiliary verbs as a small list of verbs with very specific syntactic properties differing from all the rest of the verbs in the dictionary which we will call the lexical verbs in inflectional morphology as well as syntax 9 74 And later A general definition of auxiliary verb is that it denotes a closed class of verbs that are characteristically used as markers of tense aspect mood and voice 9 102 It too adds a list of criteria 9 92 102 Auxiliary verbs distinguished grammatically editThe list of auxiliary verbs in Modern English along with their inflected forms is shown in the following table Contractions are only shown if their orthography is distinctive There are also numerous unstressed versions that are typically although not necessarily written in the standard way 12 242 248 For these see a later section Where there is a blank the auxiliary verb lacks this form In some cases a corresponding lexical verb may have the form For example although lexical verb need has a plain past tense form auxiliary verb need does not English auxiliary verb paradigm Citationform Modal Non modal Plain Present tense Past tense Participles Confusiblelexicalhomonym b Neutral Contr Negative Neutral Contr Negative Present Pastwill Modal will ll won t would d wouldn t nonemay c may might mightn t nonecan can can t cannot could couldn t noneshall shall ll shan t should shouldn t nonemust must mustn t noneought ought oughtn t exists d need e need needn t existsdare e dare daren t dared exists f be Non modal be am is are m s re ain t g isn t aren t was were wasn t weren t being been exists h do do i does do s j doesn t don t did d k d didn t existshave have has have s ve hasn t haven t had d hadn t having existsuse l used usedn t existsto to noneA major difference between the results shown above of defining auxiliary verbs syntactically and doing so based on a notion of helping is that the syntactic definition includes be even when used simply as a copular verb I am hungry It was a cat idioms using would would rather would sooner would as soon that take a finite clause complement I d rather you went have with no other verb as in Have you any change uses where it cannot be said to help any other verb 9 103 108 112 Archaic forms of be do and have edit A study of 17th century American English found the form be used for the 1st and 3rd person plural present 16 192 193 was for the 3rd person plural preterite 16 194 art and are for the 2nd person singular present wast and wert for the 2nd person singular preterite 16 193 and dost and hast 2nd person and doth and hath 3rd person for the singular present 16 185 187 The NICE criteria edit One set of criteria for distinguishing between auxiliary and lexical verbs is F R Palmer s NICE Basically the criteria are that the auxiliary verbs occur with negation inversion code and emphatic affirmation while the lexical verbs do not 1 15 21 m The NICE properties Auxiliary verb Lexical verbNegation I will not eat apples I won t eat apples I eat not apples g I eatn t apples Inversion Has Lee eaten apples Eats Lee apples Code Can it devour 3 kg of meat Yes it can Does it devour 3 kg of meat Yes it devours Emphatic affirmation You say we re not ready We ARE ready You say we didn t practise enough We PRACTISED enough NICE Negation edit Clausal negation n most commonly employs an auxiliary verb for example We can t believe it ll rain today or I don t need an umbrella As late as Middle English lexical verbs could also participate in clausal negation so a clause like Lee eats not apples would have been grammatical 17 vol 2 p 280 but this is no longer possible in Modern English where lexical verbs require do support At first glance the grammaticality of I hope guess suppose think not may suggest that some lexical verbs too have no need for do support but ungrammatical I hope guess suppose think not you are right shows that this is quite mistaken Not in these examples does not negate a clause but is instead the negative equivalent of so a pro form for a negative proposition 9 1536 Palmer writes that the Negation criterion is whether the verb occurs with the negative particle not or more strictly whether it has a negative form 1 21 the latter referring to negatively inflected won t hasn t haven t etc As seen in the paradigm table above in today s Standard English not every auxiliary verb has such a form NICE Inversion edit Although English is a subject verb object language an interrogative main clause is the most important among several constructions that put a verb before the subject This is called subject auxiliary inversion because only auxiliary verbs participate in such constructions Can should must Lee eat apples Never have I enjoyed a quince Again in Middle English lexical verbs were no different but in Modern English Eats Lee apples and Never enjoy I a quince are ungrammatical and do support is again required Does Lee eat apples Never do I enjoy quinces NICE Code edit F R Palmer attributes this term to J R Firth writing There are sentences in English in which a full verb is later picked up by an auxiliary The position is very similar to that of a noun being picked up by a pronoun If the initial sentence which contains the main verb is not heard all the remainder is unintelligible it is in fact truly in code The following example is from Firth Do you think he will I don t know He might I suppose he ought to but perhaps he feels he can t Well his brothers have They perhaps think he needn t Perhaps eventually he may I think he should and I very much hope he will 1 25 18 104 105 What picks up is called an anaphor what is picked up is called an antecedent Attempting to remove the complement s of a lexical verb normally has an ungrammatical result Did you put it in the fridge Yes I put or an inappropriate one Did you eat the chicken Yes I ate g However if a number of conditions are met the result may be acceptable 9 1527 1529 NICE Emphatic affirmation edit F R Palmer writes that a characteristic of the auxiliaries is their use in emphatic affirmation with nuclear stress upon the auxiliary as in You must see him He concedes that any verbal form may have nuclear stress thus We saw them however auxiliaries stressed in this way are used for the denial of the negative whereas lexical verbs again use do support 1 25 26 You say you heard them No we SAW them You can t have seen them We DID see them NICE is widely cited with emphatic affirmation usually simplified as emphasis as examples by A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language 1985 11 121 124 o The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language 2002 9 92 101 and the Oxford Modern English Grammar 2011 10 68 69 The NICER criteria edit A revised set of criteria NICER owes much to NICE but does more than merely add a fifth criterion to it The NICER properties 19 3 Auxiliary verb Lexical verb Finite Negation Lee will not eat apples Lee eats not apples Auxiliary initial constructions Has Lee eaten apples Eats Lee apples Contraction of not didn t shouldn t isn t eatn t gon t maken t Post auxiliary Ellipsis Lee was eating and Kim was too Lee kept eating and Kim kept too Rebuttal A We shouldn t eat apples B We should SO A We didn t try to eat apples B We tried SO In this section a number of verbs not limited to those in the paradigm table above are checked against four of the five criteria of NICER As would might could and should are sometimes understood as discrete verbs and not merely as the preterite forms of will may can and shall they are tested too NICER Negation edit Auxiliary verbs can be negated with not lexical verbs require do support a less stringent version of negation as the first criterion of NICE 19 10 38 48 We add not immediately after the verb and obtain She will would may might can could shall should must need dare not live there Each of these has clausal negation as we see by adding a positive tag and thereby creating a straightforward question She can not live there can she She need not live there need she and so forth Compare She can live there can she and She needs to live there does she In both of these a positive tag is added to a positive clause for a result that is not a straightforward question Context and tone of voice may suggest that the speaker is impressed or incredulous Similarly for She ought used not to live there She is not a resident She does not live there and She has not lived there and indeed for She wants to not go awkward though this may sound This criterion does not require the same verb for the tag as in the anchor the part of the sentence that precedes the tag So the informal better works as well She better not be late had she Irrespective of any tag lexical do does not work You did not your homework and neither does go He goes not to school Putting not immediately after some other lexical verbs brings a grammatical result He seems intends not to live there but one that does not work as expected with a positive tag He seems intends not to live there does he do not straightforwardly ask showing that what not has negated is not the clause as a whole NICER Auxiliary initial constructions edit The same as inversion as the second criterion of NICE 19 8 9 27 33 Will would may might can could shall should must need dare I wear a mankini all of these can invert with the subject Likewise for Ought used have you to wear a suit Am I forced to wear a suit and Do I wear a suit This again accounts for all of the verbs in the paradigm table above other than to The construction requires a tensed form of the verb to lacks one and therefore this criterion does not apply to it Attempts to invert lexical verbs such as do Did you your homework or go Goes he to school bring ungrammatical results Surprisingly How goes it is grammatical but even a minor adjustment to it How went it How goes your job brings an ungrammatical result showing that it is merely a fixed formula NICER Contraction of not edit Most English auxiliary verbs have a negative inflected form with n t 9 1611 19 10 49 54 20 commonly regarded as a contracted form of not Available are won t wouldn t mightn t can t couldn t shan t shouldn t mustn t oughtn t needn t aren t isn t wasn t weren t don t doesn t didn t haven t hasn t hadn t and usedn t No lexical verb has such a form She gon t to bars much these days She didn t her homework last week A small number of defective auxiliary verbs lack this inflection mayn t and daredn t are now dated and there is no universally accepted negative inflection of am amn t is dialectal the acceptability of ain t depends on the variety of Standard English and aren t is only used when it and I are inverted Aren t I invited compare I aren t tired 9 1611 1612 For do must used just and depending on the variety of Standard English can the negative inflected form is spelt as expected but its pronunciation is anomalous change of vowel in don t and perhaps can t elision of t within the root of mustn t and usedn t for shan t and won t both the pronunciation and the spelling are anomalous NICER Post auxiliary ellipsis edit The same as code as the third criterion of NICE 19 9 10 33 38 The possibility of ellipsis with will may might can t should needn t and have and indeed to is illustrated in Firth s example of code As for the other auxiliary verbs I ll attend if I must dare ought If you re attending then I shall am I d be grateful if you would could did I ll go if you do I haven t swum much recently but I used ought want hope to This is not possible with used although it was in the past p or with most lexical verbs I haven t swum much recently but I used want hope It does however work with a number of lexical verbs I d be grateful if you tried started stopped NICER Rebuttal edit When two people are arguing one may use a stressed too or so immediately after the auxiliary verb to deny a statement made by the other For example having been told that he didn t do his homework a child may reply I did too Or anyway this is true for US English For British English indeed 19 54n This kind of rebuttal is impossible with lexical verbs 19 10 54 57 Additional criteria edit Each of the two most compendious of postwar reference grammars of English offers a more detailed list of criteria for auxiliary verbs A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language has eight criteria The first five of these approximate to the four of NICE with the addition of cliticization as in It s raining or I ve finished 11 121 125 Slightly simplified the sixth is that auxiliary verbs unlike lexical verbs typically but not necessarily precede adverbs such as always never certainly and probably He would always visit her compare lexical verb visit in He visited always her The seventh is that Quantifiers like all both and each which modify the subject of the clause may occur after the verb as an alternative in many instances to the predeterminer position thus either Both their children will attend or Their children will both attend compare lexical verb attend in Their children attended both q The last is Independence of subject a claim that compared with most lexical verbs auxiliary verbs can be semantically independent of their subjects This in turn is claimed to be manifested in three ways 11 126 127 The book provides four additional criteria for modal auxiliary verbs 11 127 128 The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language adopts NICE and criteria approximating to the sixth and seventh of Comprehensive Grammar although it dispenses with the eighth 9 101 102 It provides five additional criteria for modal auxiliary verbs 9 106 107 Boundary fuzziness edit Linguists who cite or propound clear grammatical criteria for auxiliary verbs then proceed to include among auxiliary verbs certain verbs that do not meet all these criteria Having said that the English auxiliary verbs r are rather sharply defined by distinctive formal properties 2 3 Anthony R Warner points out that a class normally has some internal differentiation whereby a nuclear or prototypical s set of members shows more of the properties of the class than other less fully characterized members A class may also not show sharply definable boundaries 2 10 He claims that what are the prototypical auxiliary verbs are the modal auxiliary verbs other than ought need and dare and that the presence of be do and have in the category auxiliary verb is justified from a semantic point of view not so much by their possession of prototypical properties as by the fact that they are even more remote than are the modal auxiliary verbs from the lexical verb prototype which denotes an action or event 2 19 Infinitival to edit Various linguists notably Geoff Pullum have suggested that the to of I want to go not the preposition to as in I went to Rome is a special case of an auxiliary verb with no tensed forms 23 t Rodney Huddleston argues against this position in The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language 9 1183 1187 but Robert Levine counters these arguments 25 In a book on the historical emergence and spread of infinitival to Bettelou Los calls Pullum s arguments that it is an auxiliary verb compelling 26 In terms of the NICER properties examples like it s fine not to go show that to allows negation Inversion contraction of not and rebuttal would only apply to tensed forms and to is argued to have none Although rebuttal is not possible it does allow ellipsis I don t want to Had better woul d rather and others edit With their normal senses as in You had better best arrive early had d better and had d best are not about the past Indeed they do not seem to be usable for the past Yesterday I had better return home before the rain started and they do not occur with other forms of have have has better best The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language observes If we take the had in had better as a distinct lexeme we will say that it has been reanalysed as a present tense form like must and ought In view of its syntactic behaviour it undoubtedly should be included among the non central members of the modal auxiliary class 9 113 Expressions ranging from had better to would rather have been argued to comprise a family of morphosyntactic configurations with a moderate degree of formal and semantic homogeneity 27 3 They would be Superlative modals had best d best Comparative modals had better d better better would rather d rather had rather should rather would sooner d sooner had sooner should sooner Equative modals would just as soon as may just as well might just as wellAmong these had better d better better occur the most commonly They express either advice or a strong hope a deontic and an optative sense respectively 27 3 5 Among these three forms d better is the commonest in British English and plain better the commonest in American English 27 11 However the syntactic category of plain better when used in this or a similar way is not always clear while it may have been reanalysed as an independent modal auxiliary verb one with no preterite form and also no ability to invert Better I leave now it can be an adverb instead of a verb 27 21 23 For more about would rather sooner and would as soon see Would rather would sooner and would as soon Contributions by auxiliaries to meaning and syntax editAn auxiliary verb is traditionally understood as a verb that helps another verb by adding only grammatical information to it u So understood English auxiliaries include Do when used to form questions Do you want tea to negate I don t want coffee or to emphasize I do want tea see do support Have when used to express perfect aspect He had given his all Be when used to express progressive aspect They were singing or passive voice It was destroyed The modal auxiliary verbs used with a variety of meanings principally relating to modality He can do it now However this understanding of auxiliaries has trouble with be He wasn t asleep Was he asleep have He hadn t any money and would Would you rather we left now each of which behaves syntactically like an auxiliary verb even when not accompanying another verb or not merely doing so Other approaches to defining auxiliary verbs are described below Be edit Passive voice edit Be followed by the past participle of a lexical verb realizes the passive voice He was promoted 9 1427ff Its negative and interrogative versions He wasn t promoted Was he promoted lacking the need for do support show that this is auxiliary be This simple test can be repeated for the other applications of be briefly described below However the lexical verb get can also form a passive clause He got promoted 9 1429 1430 This is a long established construction 28 118 Followed by the present participle of a verb whether lexical or auxiliary be realizes the progressive aspect He was promoting the film 9 117 119ff Either may be confused with the use of a participial adjective that is an adjective derived from and homonymous with a participle He was excited It was exciting v Other uses edit What The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language terms quasi modal be normally imparts a deontic meaning that of He is never to come here again approximates to that of He must never come here again 9 113 114 In conditional contexts was to if both informal and with a singular subject or were to imparts remoteness If I were to jump out of the plane compare with the open conditional If I jumped out of the plane 9 151 In common with modal auxiliary verbs quasi modal be has no secondary form 9 114 What the same work terms motional be only occurs as been when it follows the verb have in a perfect construction and is not followed by any verb I ve twice been to Minsk Most of the NICE NICER criteria are inapplicable but sentences such as I don t need to go to the Grand People s Study House as I ve already been show that it satisfies the code and ellipsis criteria of NICE and NICER respectively and thus is auxiliary rather than lexical be 9 114 What the Cambridge Grammar terms copular be links a subject typically a noun phrase and a predicative complement typically a noun phrase adjective phrase or preposition phrase Ascriptive copular be ascribes a property to the subject The car was a wreck specifying copular be identifies the subject The woman in the green shoes is my aunt Louise and can be reversed with a grammatical result My aunt Louise is the woman in the green shoes Be in an it cleft It was my aunt Louise who wore the green shoes is specifying 9 266 267 Auxiliary be also takes as complements a variety of words able about bound going and supposed among them that in turn take as complements to infinitival subordinate clauses for results that are highly idiomatic was about supposed to depart etc 29 209 In Early Modern English perfect tenses could be formed with either have as today or be The latter pattern persisted into the 19th century a character in Pride and Prejudice says But before I am run away with by my feelings on this subject perhaps it will be advisable for me to state my reasons for marrying 30 Do edit Do support edit The auxiliary verb do is primarily used for do support This in turn is used for negation interrogative main clauses and more If a positive main clause is headed by an auxiliary verb either the addition of not or for most auxiliary verbs a n t inflection can negate So They could reach home before dark becomes They couldn t reach home before dark This is the negation of NICE and NICER However a lexical verb has to be supported by the verb do so They reached home before dark becomes They didn t reach home before dark 9 94 95 If a declarative main clause is headed by an auxiliary verb simple inversion of subject and verb will create a closed interrogative clause So They could reach home before dark becomes Could they reach home before dark This is the inversion of NICE and NICER However a lexical verb requires do so They reached home before dark becomes Did they reach home before dark For an open interrogative clause do has the same role How far did you get 9 95 Although interrogative main clauses are by far the most obvious contexts for inversion using do support there are others While exclamative clauses usually lack subject auxiliary inversion What a foolish girl I was it is a possibility What a foolish girl was I 31 the inverted alternative to How wonderful it tasted would be How wonderful did it taste A negative constituent that is not the subject can move to the front and trigger such inversion None of the bottles did they leave unopened A phrase with only can do the same Only once did I win a medal Ditto for phrases starting with so and such So hard Such a beating did Douglas give Tyson that Tyson lost And in somewhat old fashioned or formal writing a miscellany of other constituents can be moved to the front with the same effect Well do I remember not so much the whipping as the being shut up in a dark closet behind the study 32 for years and years did they believe that France was on the brink of ruin 33 9 95 96 Negative imperative sentences require auxiliary do even when there is another auxiliary verb The declarative sentence They were goofing off is grammatical with the single auxiliary be but the imperative sentence Don t be goofing off when the principal walks in adds don t Optionally you may be added in front of or immediately after don t A longer subject would normally come after Don t any of you be goofing off 9 928 Emphatic polarity edit Other than via a negative inflection don t doesn t the verb do does not typically contribute any change in meaning except when used to add emphasis to an accompanying verb This is described as an emphatic construction 11 133 as an emphatic version of the declarative clause 10 74 as having emphatic polarity 9 97 98 or is called the emphatic mood according to whom An example would be i I DO run five kilometres every morning with intonational stress placed on do compared to plain ii I run five kilometres every morning It also differs from iii I RUN five kilometres every morning with the stress on run A context for i with its emphasis on positive polarity would be an allegation that the speaker didn t do so every morning for iii with its emphasis on lexical content an allegation that the speaker merely walked Do can be used for emphasis on negative polarity as well He never DID remember my birthday 9 98 For emphatic positive polarity in imperatives do is again added thus standard Be quiet becomes emphatically positive Do be quiet 9 929 Have edit Perfect tenses edit Followed by the past participle of a verb whether lexical or auxiliary the auxiliary verb have realizes a perfect tense Has she visited Qom Has she been to Qom In addition to its tensed forms have ve has s had d haven t hasn t hadn t it has a plain form She could have arrived and a present participle I regret having lost it but no past participle 9 111 Perfect is a syntactic term in the context of English perfective is a matter of semantic interpretation Unlike say Slavic languages which do have direct grammatical expression of perfectivity 34 136 in English a sentence using a perfect tense may or may not have a perfective interpretation 35 57 58 The present perfect tense is illustrated by I ve left it somewhere the past perfect also called the preterite perfect tense by I d left it somewhere 9 140 141 A full description of their uses is necessarily complex the discussion in The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language is long and intricate 9 139 148 The perfect is often considered as referring to an indefinite past I ve been to Oslo might raise the question of when but is acceptable as is by contrast I ve been to Oslo in 2016 specifying the time would be strange A more careful analysis brings the continuative perfect the experiential or existential perfect the resultative perfect and the perfect of recent past 9 143 w The first with an unspecified starting point and continuing uninterruptedly to the present is illustrated by I ve lived in Oslo since 2016 the experiential by Yes I ve watched a bullfight and I never want to watch one again the perfect of result by I ve just watched a bullfight and now I feel rather sick and that of recent past by George Santos has just given a press conference usable at the time of writing but likely to become odder as time passes 34 98 99 Very simply the present perfect refers to the past in a way that has some relevance to the present 35 63 64 The perfect is also used in contexts that require both past reference and an untensed verb form He seems to have left Having left he lit a cigarette 35 65 66 Corpus based research has shown that American English saw a marked decrease from around 1800 until the mid 20th century in the use of the present perfect and that British English followed this in the late 20th century 37 Other uses edit When used to describe an event have is exclusively a lexical verb Had you your teeth done Did you have your teeth done Had you a nap Did you have a nap When used to describe a state however for many speakers although for few Americans or younger people there is also an auxiliary option he d stop at a pub settle up with a cheque because he hadn t any money on him 38 Hasn t he any friends of his own 39 I m afraid I haven t anything pithy to answer 40 This hasn t anything directly to do with religion 41 9 111 112 35 54 An alternative to auxiliary verb have in this sense is have got although this is commoner among British speakers and less formal 9 111 113 Has he got old news for you 42 It hasn t got anything to do with the little green men and the blue orb 43 What right had he got to get on this train without a ticket 44 Hasn t he got a toolbox 45 With their meaning of obligation have to has to and had to rarely if ever rendered as ve to s to and d to 46 can use auxiliary have for inversion if he wants to compel A to do something to what Court has he to go 47 How much further has he to go 48 Now why has he to wait three weeks 49 although lexical have is commoner Use edit Use jus rhyming with loose satisfies only one of The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language s five criteria for modal as distinct from other auxiliary verbs It is also semantically quite distinct from the modal auxiliaries the meaning it expresses is aspectual not modal 9 115 Like ought use is followed by a to infinitival clause Thus I used to go to college means that formerly the speaker habitually went to college and normally implies that they no longer do so Use is highly defective existing only in preterite form For some speakers of English as a first language though very few Americans it can follow auxiliary verb syntax some speakers of British English can form questions like Used he to come here and negatives like He used not rarely usedn t to come here 11 140 Far commoner however is treatment of used as the preterite of a lexical verb Whether auxiliary or lexical used expresses past states or past habitual actions usually with the implication that they no longer continue After noting how constructions employing used We used to play tennis every week would We would play tennis every week and the preterite alone We played tennis every week often seem to be interchangeable Robert I Binnick teases them apart concluding that used is an anti present perfect whereas the present perfect includes the present in what is essentially a period of the past the used construction precisely excludes it and further that The whole point of the used to construction is not to report a habit in the past but rather to contrast a past era with the present It s essentially a present tense Like the present perfect it is about a state of affairs not a series of occurrences 50 41 43 Use is far more commonly encountered as a lexical than as an auxiliary verb particularly for younger or American speakers This forms questions and negatives with did The plain form use sometimes spelt used of the lexical verb is seen in Did you use to play tennis Its preterite perfect had used is rare but attested A simple declarative I often used to play tennis could be either auxiliary or lexical Use of the preterite used should not be confused with that of the participial adjective i e the adjective etymologically derived from the participle meaning familiar with as in I am used to this We must get used to the cold As is common for adjectives and impossible for verbs used here can be modified by very When the participial adjective is followed by to and a verb the latter is a gerund participle I am used to going to college in the mornings Data from a corpus of American and British spoken and written English of the 1980s and 1990s show that used not to usedn t to both auxiliary and didn t use to lexical were then rare in both American and British English other than used not to in British novels Never used to is a commonly used alternative 51 165 Modal auxiliary use is not used in interrogatives in conversation Used you to and even the lexical version with do support Did you use to is rare 51 218 To edit In the context for an argument that infinitival to is a subordinator Rodney Huddleston points out that just as for the subordinator that I said that he could there are contexts where to is optional with no change in meaning x His example is All I did was to ask a question and from it he infers that to is meaningless 9 1186 Within an argument for categorizing to not as a subordinator but as an auxiliary verb Robert D Levine disagrees with the main thrust of Huddleston s argument but not with the claim that to is meaningless something that is also true of dummy do and copular be both of them auxiliary verbs 25 191 192 Its function is purely syntactic Modal auxiliary verbs edit The modal auxiliary verbs contribute meaning chiefly via modality although some of them particularly will and sometimes shall express future time reference Their uses are detailed at English modal verbs and tables summarizing their principal meaning contributions can be found in the articles Modal verb and Auxiliary verb For more details on the uses of auxiliaries to express aspect mood and time reference see English clause syntax Auxiliary verbs in sequence editModal auxiliary verbs in sequence edit As modal verbs only have tensed forms in Standard English they would not be expected to appear in subordinate clauses or in sequence might be able to help them but might could help them Yet what appear to be sequences of modal auxiliary verbs do occur see Double modals They can hardly be regarded as part of Standard English and they are therefore ignored in the description below Other auxiliary verbs in sequence edit There are constraints on the order within sequences of auxiliary verbs As the modal auxiliary verbs and use only have tensed forms or anyway only have these in Standard English they can only go at the front If we put aside the highly anomalous to the order is then modal gt perfect have gt progressive be gt passive be and a lexical verb Patterns with two auxiliary verbs are exemplified by was being eaten has been eaten might be eaten and might have eaten Patterns with three include that exemplified by might have been eaten Noting that Structures containing two secondary forms of be progressive and passive are avoided by some speakers but they do occasionally occur Huddleston and Pullum present will have been being taken as an example of a sequence with four 9 104 106 Unstressed and contracted forms edit It s redirects here For other uses see It s disambiguation Contractions are a common feature of English used frequently in ordinary speech In written English contractions are used in informal and sometimes in formal writing 52 They usually involve the elision of a vowel an apostrophe being inserted in its place in written English possibly accompanied by other changes Many of these contractions involve auxiliary verbs Certain contractions tend to be restricted to less formal speech and very informal writing such as John d or Mary d for John Mary would Compare the personal pronoun forms I d and you d much more likely to be encountered in relatively informal writing This applies in particular to constructions involving consecutive contractions such as wouldn t ve for would not have citation needed Contractions in English are generally not mandatory as they are in some other languages although in speech uncontracted forms may seem overly formal They are often used for emphasis I AM ready The uncontracted form of an auxiliary or copula must be used in elliptical sentences where its complement is omitted Who s ready I am not I m Some contractions lead to homophony which sometimes causes errors in writing such as confusing ve with of as in would of for would have 53 188 Unstressed and contracted forms of individual verbs edit The lists below derive from F R Palmer s The English Verb 12 242 248 and The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language 1613 For the contracted forms of the modal auxiliary verbs see English modal auxiliary verbs Unstressed and contracted forms of be edit am ˈaem em m m is ˈɪz z or s s are ˈɑɹ eɹ ɹ re was ˈwɒz wez were ˈweɹ ɹ re be ˈbi bɪ been ˈbin bɪn In a non rhotic dialect clitic final ɹ is only realized as ɹ or similar when followed by a vowel They re tired no ɹ They re angry with ɹ For the contraction options for is consider Bill s arriving ˈbɪlz eˈɹaɪvɪŋ versus Janet s coming ˈd ʒaenɪts ˈkʌmɪŋ Unstressed and contracted forms of do edit does ˈdʌz dez z or s s do ˈdu de d d or d For the alternative nonsyllabic options for does consider When s Bill leave ˈwɛnz ˈbɪl ˈliv versus What s Bill do ˈwɒts ˈbɪl ˈdu The form d might appear in for example What d he do spoken informally Uniquely among the forms for any of the auxiliary verbs d is a proclitic It attaches to the front of the single word you D you follow me 9 1614 Unstressed and contracted forms of have edit have ˈhaev hev ev v ve e woulda musta etc has ˈhaez hez ez z or s s had ˈhaed hed ed d d For the alternative nonsyllabic options for has consider Bill s arrived ˈbɪlz eˈɹaɪvd versus Janet s come ˈd ʒaenɪts ˈkʌm Unstressed form of to edit to ˈtu te Double contractions edit Being clitics the contractions can replace their full equivalents in most although not all contexts thus we see ve not only in They ve left but also in My friends ve left or even in My friends I hadn t seen in three years ve left not only in You should ve been there but also in You shouldn t ve been there in which a contraction has clitized onto an auxiliary verb with negative inflection Double contractions are possible Will have broken is grammatical and thus His arm helmet glasses rib collarbone nose etc ll ve broken are all grammatical too Negative inflected forms editPosition of not n t in a negative closed interrogative After or as an inflectional suffix of the auxiliary verb After the subjectnot Would not you like another glass Would you not like another glass n t Wouldn t you like another glass Would you n t like another glass Contractions such as d e d from would are clitics By contrast the n t e nt of wouldn t is in reality a contraction only etymologically wouldn t isn t haven t and so forth have long been inflected forms and an auxiliary verb with negative inflection can behave differently from the combination of not and the same verb without the inflection 9 91 This article will continue to use contraction to include early instances of what at the time may not have become inflected forms During the early 17th century not lost its requirement for stress and subsequently came to be written as n t particularly in comedies and in the mouths of rustic characters or others speaking nonstandard dialects In the 19th and 20th centuries the use of n t in writing spread beyond drama and fiction to personal letters journalism and descriptive texts 54 An t ben t can t don t han t shan t and won t were well established by the end of the 17th century isn t aren t wasn t weren t didn t doesn t don t hadn t hasn t haven t can t couldn t daren t mayn t now obsolete or dialectal mightn t mustn t needn t shan t shouldn t won t and wouldn t by the end of the 18th and oughtn t in the early 19th 54 176 189 There were various other negative contractions that have not survived as examples Barron Brainerd cites A C Partridge as showing that from 1599 to 1632 Ben Jonson used i not is not sha not shall not wi not will not wu not and wou not would not ha not has have not and do not do not 55 54 179 180 Negative inflection of am edit Standard English has no first person singular form corresponding to the isn t of it isn t and isn t it that is completely unproblematic However the following informal or dialectal options have been used Amn t edit Otto Jespersen calls amn t unpronounceable 56 120 and Eric Partridge calls it ugly 57 but it is the standard inflected form in some varieties mainly Hiberno English Irish English and Scottish English 20 58 In Hiberno English the question form amn t I is used more frequently than the declarative I amn t 59 The standard I m not is available as an alternative to I amn t in both Scottish English and Hiberno English An example appears in a poem by Oliver St John Gogarty If anyone thinks that I amn t divine He gets no free drinks when I m making the wine These lines are quoted in James Joyce s Ulysses 60 which also contains other examples Amn t I with you Amn t I your girl spoken by Cissy Caffrey 61 Amnae edit Amnae exists in Scots and has been borrowed into Scottish English by many speakers It is used in declarative sentences rather than questions 59 Ain t edit Further information Ain t Ain t is an inflected alternative to am not and also to is not was not are not were not has not and have not 62 60 64 and in some dialects also do not does not did not cannot or can not could not will not would not and should not The usage of ain t is a perennial subject of controversy in English 63 Geoffrey Nunberg has argued that ain t is used by Standard English speakers to suggest that a fact is just obvious on the face of things 64 Aren t edit Aren t is a very common means of filling the amn t gap in questions Aren t I lucky to have you around It was common by the early 20th century Otto Jespersen writing in a book published in 1917 that Nowadays ɑːnt is frequently heard especially in tag questions I m a bad boy ɑːntaɪ but when authors want to write it they are naturally induced to write aren t I find the spelling aren t I or arn t I pretty frequently in George Eliot but only to represent vulgar or dialectal speech In the younger generation of writers however it is also found as belonging to educated speakers 56 119 y The style guides have disagreed on aren t Eric Partridge considered the aren t in aren t I an illogical and illiterate spelling of the phonetically natural and the philologically logical a n t 57 H W Fowler as revised by Ernest Gowers wrote that aren t I was colloquially respectable and almost universal 65 In 1979 however it was described as almost universal among speakers of Standard English 66 As an alternative to am not aren t developed from one pronunciation of an t which itself developed in part from amn t In non rhotic dialects aren t and an t are homophones and the spelling aren t I began to replace an t I in the early 20th century 62 115 116 although examples of aren t I or arn t I for am I not appear in the first half of the 19th century as in arn t I listening and isn t it only the breeze that s blowing the sheets and halliards about from 1827 67 An t edit An t sometimes a n t arose from am not via amn t and are not almost simultaneously An t first appears in print in the work of English Restoration playwrights In 1695 an t was used for am not and as early as 1696 an t was used to mean are not An t for is not may have developed independently from its use for am not and are not Isn t was sometimes written as in t or en t which could have changed into an t An t for is not may also have filled a gap in the paradigm for the verb be From 1749 an t with a long a sound began to be written as ain t By this time an t was already being used for am not are not and is not An t and ain t coexisted as written forms well into the 19th century Bain t edit Bain t apparently from be not is found in a number of works employing eye dialect including J Sheridan Le Fanu s Uncle Silas 68 It is also found in a ballad written in Newfoundland dialect 69 Other negative inflections of have edit Han t or ha n t an early contraction for has not and have not developed from the elision of the s of has not and the v of have not Han t also appeared in the work of English Restoration playwrights Much like an t han t was sometimes pronounced with a long a yielding hain t With H dropping the h of han t or hain t gradually disappeared in most dialects and became ain t Ain t as a contraction for has not have not appeared in print as early as 1819 As with an t hain t and ain t were found together late into the nineteenth century Hain t in addition to being an antecedent of ain t is a contraction of has not and have not in some dialects of English such as Appalachian English It is reminiscent of hae have in Lowland Scots In dialects that retain the distinction between hain t and ain t hain t is used for contractions of to have not and ain t for contractions of to be not 70 In other dialects hain t is used either in place of or interchangeably with ain t Hain t is seen for example in Chapter 33 of Mark Twain s Adventures of Huckleberry Finn I hain t come back I hain t been GONE Other negative inflections of do edit Don t is the Standard English negative inflected form of do However in nonstandard English it may also be used for third person singular Emma She don t live here anymore Notes edit Wiseman uses regular irregular active and passive with the meanings they still have in relation to verbs A neuter verb he writes Comes from the Latin neuter neither because it is neither Active nor Passive it denotes the existence of a person or thing making a complete sense of itself and requires no noun or other word to be joined with it as I sleep we run she cries amp c An impersonal verb he writes is one Having only the third person singular and plural applied to things both animate and inanimate as it freezes it is said or they say they grow amp c 6 155 More precisely Does there exist a lexical verb with the same spelling and pronunciation that is synonymous or could be said to have an auxiliary or copular function Ignored here is any lexical verb will meaning exert one s will in an attempt to compel can meaning insert into cans etc that is unlikely to be mistaken for the auxiliary verb T here is evidence that for some speakers of Standard English may and might have diverged to the extent that they are no longer inflectional forms of a single lexeme but belong to distinct lexemes may and might each of which like must lacks a preterite 9 109 The use of ought as a lexical verb as in They didn t ought to go is generally thought of as restricted to non standard dialects 11 140 but has been described as also sometimes found in informal standard usage 13 Lexical ought with the dummy operator do has been condemned in British usage handbooks What this censure suggests is that lexical ought with periphrastic do is a well established usage in colloquial British English 14 502 a b An NPI rare for speakers of Standard American English 9 109 The distinction between auxiliary and lexical is blurred lexical dare commonly occurs in non affirmative contexts without to She wouldn t dare ask her father and it also can be stranded as in She ought to have asked for a raise but she didn t dare 9 110 a b c This article uses an asterisk to indicate ungrammatical examples a percent sign to indicate grammaticality for some speakers of English but not others and a number sign to indicate semantic or pragmatic oddity despite grammaticality Almost exclusively an NPI found in the contexts exemplified by If you don t be quiet I ll ground you and Why don t you be quiet 9 114 Used for the imperative as in Do be quiet Uniquely among English auxiliary verbs untensed do also has a negative inflected form don t as in Don t be noisy 9 91 92 In questions like What s he do rarely written In questions like What d he do rarely written Pronounced jus rhyming with loose Auxiliary verb form used should be distinguished from the homonymous adjective used as in I ve got very used to it 15 85 The homographic verb use juz rhyming with lose is lexical only For many speakers of Standard English especially younger ones use jus is exclusively a lexical verb 9 115 Rather than lexical verb Palmer uses the term full verb Clausal negation can be tested by adding a straightforward interrogative tag one simply asking for confirmation not expressing incredulity admiration etc A negative clause calls for a positive interrogative tag and a positive clause calls for a negative one thus the positive Do you within You don t eat beef do you shows that You don t eat beef is negative Despite lacking an auxiliary verb You never seldom rarely eat beef is also negative The authors use NICE but do not name it Otto Jespersen points to an example by Leigh Hunt I did not stammer half so badly as I used 21 222 22 15 If the context for Their children attended both is a discussion of two weddings then it is of course grammatical But if the intended meaning is instead Both their children attended it is not Warner s distinction is not between auxiliary verbs and lexical verbs but instead between auxiliaries and verbs His terminology has been adjusted for this article The issue is not just one of naming however an account of the argument over whether what this article calls auxiliary verbs comprise a subcategory of verbs as assumed in this article or instead comprise an independent category as Warner holds is beyond the scope of this article Later in the book Warner makes an appeal to prototype theory 2 103 108 Pullum credits unpublished insights of Paul Postal and Richard Hudson and published work by Robert Fiengo 24 The Oxford English Dictionary 2nd edition 1989 defines an auxiliary verb as a verb used to form the tenses moods voices etc of other verbs Very simply if the word ending ing can take an object it is a verb if it can be modified by either too in the sense of excessively or very it is an adjective NB if it cannot take an object it is not necessarily an adjective if it cannot be so modified by too or very it is not necessarily a verb There are also participial prepositions and other complications 9 540 541 The classification and terms are derived from Bernard Comrie 36 although slightly revised This appears within chapter 14 Non finite and verbless clauses attributed to Huddleston alone Jespersen provides lists of where examples may be found in the works of Eliot Bennett Benson Galsworthy Wells Wilde and others Rather than ɑːnt and ɑːntaɪ Jespersen writes a nt and a nt ai respectively References edit a b c d e Palmer F R 1965 A Linguistic Study of the English Verb London Longmans Green OCLC 1666825 a b c d e f Warner Anthony R 1993 English Auxiliaries Structure and History Cambridge Studies in Linguistics 66 Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 30284 5 Oxford English Dictionary entry for auxiliary A1c Bullokar William 1906 Bref Grammar for English In Plessow Max ed Geschichte der Fabeldichtung in England bis zu John Gay 1726 Nebst Neudruck von Bullokars Fables of Aesop 1585 Booke at large 1580 Bref Grammar for English 1586 und Pamphlet for Grammar 1586 Berlin Mayer amp Muller pp 339 385 via Internet Archive Sterne Laurence The Life and Opinions of Tristram Shandy Gentleman Vol 5 London via Laurence Sterne in Cyberspace Gifu University a b Wiseman Charles 1764 A Complete English Grammar on a New Plan For the Use of Foreigners and Such Natives as Would Acquire a Scientifical Knowledge of Their Own Tongue London W Nicol via Google Books Fowler William Chauncey 1857 The English Language in Its Elements and Forms With a History of Its Origin and Development London William Kent amp Co via Google Books Ross John Robert 1969 Auxiliaries as main verbs PDF Studies in Philosophical Linguistics 1 Evanston Illinois Great Expectations ISSN 0586 8882 via Haj Ross s Papers on Syntax Poetics and Selected Short Subjects a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w x y z aa ab ac ad ae af ag ah ai aj ak al am an ao ap aq ar as at au Huddleston Rodney Pullum Geoffrey K 2002 The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 43146 0 a b c Aarts Bas 2011 Oxford Modern English Grammar Oxford Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 953319 0 a b c d e f g h i Quirk Randolph Greenbaum Sidney Leech Geoffrey Svartvik Jan 1985 A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language London Longman ISBN 0 582 51734 6 a b Palmer F R 1988 The English Verb 2nd ed London Longman ISBN 0 582 01470 0 Greenbaum Sidney 1996 The Oxford English Grammar Oxford Oxford University Press p 155 ISBN 978 0 19 861250 6 Lee Jackie F K Collins Peter 2004 On the usage of have dare need ought and used to in Australian English and Hong Kong English World Englishes 23 4 501 513 doi 10 1111 j 0083 2919 2004 00374 x Zandvoort R W 1975 A Handbook of English Grammar 7th ed London Longman ISBN 0 582 55339 3 a b c d Abbott O L October 1958 Verbal endings in seventeenth century American English American Speech 33 3 185 194 doi 10 2307 453204 JSTOR 453204 Hogg Richard M Blake N F Lass Roger Romaine Suzanne Burchfield R W Algeo John 1992 2001 The Cambridge History of the English Language Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 26474 X OCLC 23356833 Firth J R 1968 May 1956 Descriptive linguistics and the study of English In Palmer F R ed Selected Papers of J R Firth 1952 59 Bloomington Indiana University Press pp 96 113 OCLC 173639 via Internet Archive a b c d e f g Sag Ivan A Chaves Rui P Abeille Anne Estigarribia Bruno Flickinger Dan Kay Paul Michaelis Laura A Muller Stefan Pullum Geoffrey K Van Eynde Frank Wasow Thomas 1 February 2020 Lessons from the English auxiliary system Journal of Linguistics 56 1 87 155 doi 10 1017 S002222671800052X hdl 20 500 11820 2a4f1c47 b2e1 4908 9d82 b02aa240befd ISSN 0022 2267 S2CID 150096608 a b Zwicky Arnold M Pullum Geoffrey K September 1983 Cliticization vs inflection English n t Language 59 3 502 513 doi 10 2307 413900 ISSN 0097 8507 JSTOR 413900 Hunt Leigh 1891 1828 An Account of Christ Hospital In Johnson Reginald Brimley ed Essays of Leigh Hunt PDF London J M Dent pp 211 223 via Internet Archive Jespersen Otto 1961 1931 A Modern English Grammar on Historical Principles Vol 4 Syntax 3 Time and Tense London George Allen amp Unwin via Internet Archive Pullum Geoffrey K 1982 Syncategorematicity and English infinitival to Glossa 16 181 215 Fiengo Robert 1980 Surface Structure The Interface of Autonomous Components Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard University Press ISBN 978 0 674 85725 4 a b Levine Robert D 2012 Auxiliaries To s company Journal of Linguistics 48 1 187 203 doi 10 1017 S002222671100034X ISSN 0022 2267 Los Bettelou 2005 The Rise of theTo Infinitive Oxford University Press p 208 doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199274765 001 0001 ISBN 978 0 19 927476 5 a b c d Van der Auwera Johan Noel Dirk Van linden An 2013 Had better d better and better Diachronic and transatlantic variation PDF In Marin Arrese Juana I Carretero Marta Arus Jorge Van der Auwera Johan eds English modality Core periphery and evidentiality Topics in English Linguistics 81 Berlin De Gruyter Mouton pp 119 154 ISBN 9783110286328 Retrieved 29 November 2023 via University of Liege Berk Lynn M 1999 English Syntax From Word to Discourse New York Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 512353 1 Depraetere Ilse Reed Susan 2020 Mood and modality in English In Aarts Bas McMahon April Hinrichs Lars eds The Handbook of English Linguistics PDF 2nd ed Newark New Jersey Wiley pp 207 227 doi 10 1002 9781119540618 ch12 ISBN 9781119540564 van Gelderen Elly 2006 A History of the English Language Amsterdam John Benjamins p 215 ISBN 90 272 3236 9 Mitchell Joni 1968 The Gift of the Magi Joni Mitchell Groome Francis Hindes 1895 Two Suffolk Friends Edinburgh William Blackwood p 10 via Project Gutenberg Southey Robert 1814 Letters from England By Don Manuel Alvarez Espriella Vol 3 3rd ed London Longman Hurst Rees Orme and Brown via Project Gutenberg a b Binnick Robert I 1991 Time and the Verb A Guide to Tense and Aspect New York Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 506206 X a b c d Huddleston Rodney Pullum Geoffrey K Reynolds Brett 2022 A Student s Introduction to English Grammar 2nd ed Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 1 009 08574 8 Comrie Bernard 1976 Aspect Cambridge Textbooks in Linguistics Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 56 61 ISBN 978 0 521 29045 6 via Internet Archive Elsness Johan 2009 12 The present perfect and the preterite In Rohdenburg Gunter Schluter Julia eds One Language Two Grammars Differences between British and American English Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 228 245 ISBN 978 0 521 87219 5 Harper Nick 9 January 2004 Interview Jack Charlton The Guardian Retrieved 29 November 2023 No 2 024 Flipper the dolphin The Guardian 12 April 2002 Retrieved 29 November 2023 Amis Martin 31 December 1999 What we ll be doing tonight The Guardian Retrieved 29 November 2023 Brown Andrew 23 December 2009 Author Author The Guardian Retrieved 29 November 2023 McCrum Robert 20 April 2008 Has he got old news for you The Guardian Retrieved 29 November 2023 Luscombe Richard 13 October 2021 William Shatner in tears after historic space flight I m so filled with emotion The Guardian Retrieved 29 November 2023 Hoggart Simon 25 November 2000 No charm and plenty of offence is the railway response to a state of chaos The Guardian Retrieved 29 November 2023 Coren Victoria 22 August 2010 A Swann song for real men The Guardian Retrieved 29 November 2023 Kukucz Marta 2009 Characteristics of English Modal Verbs PDF PhD thesis Palacky University Olomouc p 18 Lord Dynevor 20 July 1926 Commons Amendment Parliamentary Debates Hansard Vol 65 Parliament of the United Kingdom Lords 12 Entrance examination Solihull Sample paper Mathematics Solihull Preparatory School Retrieved 29 November 2023 How does it feel for me PDF Healthwatch Leeds NHS Leeds Clinical Commissioning Group January 2020 Retrieved 29 November 2023 Binnick Robert I 2006 Used to and habitual aspect in English Style 40 33 45 JSTOR 10 5325 style 40 1 2 33 a b Biber Douglas Johansson Stig Leech Geoffrey Conrad Susan Finegan Edward 1999 Longman Grammar of Spoken and Written English Harlow Essex Pearson Education ISBN 0 582 23725 4 via Internet Archive Castillo Gonzalez Maria del Pilar 2007 Uncontracted Negatives and Negative Contractions in Contemporary English A Corpus Based Study PDF Ph D thesis University of Santiago de Compostela pp 23 28 Disterheft Dorothy 1990 The role of adaptive rules in language change Diachronica 7 2 181 198 doi 10 1075 dia 7 2 03dis a b c Brainerd Barron October 1989 The contractions of not A historical note Journal of English Linguistics 22 2 176 196 doi 10 1177 007542428902200202 Archived from the original on 3 December 2023 via Pennsylvania State University It appears that October 1989 was the scheduled time of publication but that actual publication only occurred in 1993 Partridge A C 1953 The Accidence of Ben Jonson s Plays Masques and Entertainments with an Appendix of Comparable Uses in Shakespeare Cambridge Bowes amp Bowes pp 251 258 OCLC 924625 a b Jespersen Otto 1917 Negation in English and Other Languages Det Kgl Danske Videnskabernes Selskab Historisk filologiske Meddelelser I 5 Copenhagen Andr Fred Host og son OCLC 457568567 via Internet Archive a b Partridge Eric 1948 a n t Usage and Abusage A Guide to Good English 4th ed London Hamish Hamilton OCLC 465857982 via Internet Archive Rissanen Matti 1999 Isn t it or is it not On the order of postverbal subject and negative particle in the history of English In Ingrid Tieken Boon van Ostade Gunnel Tottie Wim van der Wurff eds Negation in the History of English Walter de Gruyter pp 189 206 ISBN 3 11 016198 2 a b Bresnan Joan 2002 The Lexicon in Optimality Theory In Paolo Merla Suzanne Stevenson eds The Lexical Basis of Sentence Processing Formal Computational and Experimental Issues Amsterdam John Benjamins pp 39 58 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 56 4001 ISBN 1 58811 156 3 Joyce James Chapter 1 Ulysses Archived from the original on 22 October 2012 Joyce James Chapter 15 Ulysses Archived from the original on 29 November 2012 a b Gilman E Ward ed 1994 Merriam Webster s Dictionary of English Usage 2nd ed Springfield Massachusetts Merriam Webster ISBN 0 87779 132 5 via Internet Archive Skinner David 2012 The Story of Ain t America Its Language and the Most Controversial Dictionary Ever Published New York HarperCollins ISBN 978 0 06 202746 7 Nunberg Geoffrey Ain t Misbehavior Geoffrey Nunberg Retrieved 12 December 2023 Fowler H W 1965 Be In Gowers Ernest ed Fowler s Modern English Usage 2nd ed Oxford Oxford University Press OCLC 779416 Jorgensen Erik 1979 Aren t I and alternative patterns in modern English English Studies 60 35 41 doi 10 1080 00138387908597940 Griffin Gerald 1827 Holland Tide or Munster Popular Tales London W Simpkin and R Marshall p 281 via Google Books J Sheridan Le Fanu Uncle Silas ch 53 The Outharbour Planter by Maurice A Devine 1859 1915 of Kings Cove Bonavista Bay NL The times bain t what they used to be bout fifty ye rs or so ago as published in Old Time Songs And Poetry Of Newfoundland Songs Of The People From The Days Of Our Forefathers first edition p 9 1927 described here Malmstrom Jean 1960 Ain t again The English Journal 49 204 205 doi 10 2307 809195 JSTOR 809195 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title English auxiliary verbs amp oldid 1214699332 Unstressed and contracted forms, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.