fbpx
Wikipedia

Linguistic homeland

In historical linguistics, the homeland or Urheimat (/ˈʊərhmɑːt/, from German ur- "original" and Heimat, home) of a proto-language is the region in which it was spoken before splitting into different daughter languages. A proto-language is the reconstructed or historically-attested parent language of a group of languages that are genetically related.

Depending on the age of the language family under consideration, its homeland may be known with near-certainty (in the case of historical or near-historical migrations) or it may be very uncertain (in the case of deep prehistory). Next to internal linguistic evidence, the reconstruction of a prehistoric homeland makes use of a variety of disciplines, including archaeology and archaeogenetics.

Methods edit

There are several methods to determine the homeland of a given language family. One method is based on the vocabulary that can be reconstructed for the proto-language. This vocabulary – especially terms for flora and fauna – can provide clues for the geographical and ecological environment in which the proto-language was spoken. An estimate for the time-depth of the proto-language is necessary in order to account for prehistorical changes in climate and the distribution of flora and fauna.[1][2]

Another method is based on the linguistic migration theory (first proposed by Edward Sapir), which states that the most likely candidate for the last homeland of a language family can be located in the area of its highest linguistic diversity.[3] This presupposes an established view about the internal subgrouping of the language family. Different assumptions about high-order subgrouping can thus lead to very divergent proposals for a linguistic homeland (e.g. Isidore Dyen's proposal for New Guinea as the center of dispersal of the Austronesian languages).[4] The linguistic migration theory has its limits because it only works when linguistic diversity evolves continuously without major disruptions. Its results can be distorted e.g. when this diversity is wiped out by more recent migrations.[5]

Limitations of the concept edit

The concept of a (single, identifiable) "homeland" of a given language family implies a purely genealogical view of the development of languages. This assumption is often reasonable and useful, but it is by no means a logical necessity, as languages are well known to be susceptible to areal change such as substrate or superstrate influence.

Time depth edit

Over a sufficient period of time, in the absence of evidence of intermediary steps in the process, it may be impossible to observe linkages between languages that have a shared Urheimat: given enough time, natural language change will obliterate any meaningful linguistic evidence of a common genetic source. This general concern is a manifestation of the larger issue of "time depth" in historical linguistics.[6]

For example, the languages of the New World are believed to be descended from a relatively "rapid" peopling of the Americas (relative to the duration of the Upper Paleolithic) within a few millennia (roughly between 20,000 and 15,000 years ago),[7] but their genetic relationship has become completely obscured over the more than ten millennia which have passed between their separation and their first written record in the early modern period. Similarly, the Australian Aboriginal languages are divided into some 28 families and isolates for which no genetic relationship can be shown.[8]

The Urheimaten reconstructed using the methods of comparative linguistics typically estimate separation times dating to the Neolithic or later. It is undisputed that fully developed languages were present throughout the Upper Paleolithic, and possibly into the deep Middle Paleolithic (see origin of language, behavioral modernity). These languages would have spread with the early human migrations of the first "peopling of the world", but they are no longer amenable to linguistic reconstruction. The Last Glacial Maximum (LGM) has imposed linguistic separation lasting several millennia on many Upper Paleolithic populations in Eurasia, as they were forced to retreat into "refugia" before the advancing ice sheets. After the end of the LGM, Mesolithic populations of the Holocene again became more mobile, and most of the prehistoric spread of the world's major linguistic families seem to reflect the expansion of population cores during the Mesolithic followed by the Neolithic Revolution.

The Nostratic theory is the best-known attempt to expand the deep prehistory of the main language families of Eurasia (excepting Sino-Tibetan and the languages of Southeast Asia) to the beginning of the Holocene. First proposed in the early 20th century, the Nostratic theory still receives serious consideration, but it is by no means generally accepted. The more recent and more speculative "Borean" hypothesis attempts to unite Nostratic with Dené–Caucasian and Austric, in a "mega-phylum" that would unite most languages of Eurasia, with a time depth going back to the Last Glacial Maximum.

The argument surrounding the "Proto-Human language", finally, is almost completely detached from linguistic reconstruction, instead surrounding questions of phonology and the origin of speech. Time depths involved in the deep prehistory of all the world's extant languages are of the order of at least 100,000 years.[9]

Language contact and creolization edit

The concept of an Urheimat only applies to populations speaking a proto-language defined by the tree model. This is not always the case.

For example, in places where language families meet, the relationship between a group that speaks a language and the Urheimat for that language is complicated by "processes of migration, language shift and group absorption are documented by linguists and ethnographers" in groups that are themselves "transient and plastic." Thus, in the contact area in western Ethiopia between languages belonging to the Nilo-Saharan and Afroasiatic families, the Nilo-Saharan-speaking Nyangatom and the Afroasiatic-speaking Daasanach have been observed to be closely related to each other but genetically distinct from neighboring Afroasiatic-speaking populations. This is a reflection of the fact that the Daasanach, like the Nyangatom, originally spoke a Nilo-Saharan language, with the ancestral Daasanach later adopting an Afroasiatic language around the 19th century.[10]

Creole languages are hybrids of languages that are sometimes unrelated. Similarities arise from the creole formation process, rather than from genetic descent.[11] For example, a creole language may lack significant inflectional morphology, lack tone on monosyllabic words, or lack semantically opaque word formation, even if these features are found in all of the parent languages of the languages from which the creole was formed.[12]

Isolates edit

Some languages are language isolates. That is to say, they have no well accepted language family connection, no nodes in a family tree, and therefore no known Urheimat. An example is the Basque language of Northern Spain and southwest France. Nevertheless, it is a scientific fact that all languages evolve. An unknown Urheimat may still be hypothesized, such as that for a Proto-Basque, and may be supported by archaeological and historical evidence.

Sometimes relatives are found for a language originally believed to be an isolate. An example is the Etruscan language, which, even though only partially understood, is believed to be related to the Rhaetic language and to the Lemnian language. A single family may be an isolate. In the case of the non-Austronesian indigenous languages of Papua New Guinea and the indigenous languages of Australia, there is no published linguistic hypothesis supported by any evidence that these languages have links to any other families. Nevertheless, an unknown Urheimat is implied. The entire Indo-European family itself is a language isolate: no further connections are known. This lack of information does not prevent some professional linguists from formulating additional hypothetical nodes (Nostratic) and additional homelands for the speakers.

Homelands of major language families edit

Western and central Eurasia edit

 
Map showing the present-day distribution of Indo-European languages in Eurasia (light green) and the likely Proto-Indo-European homeland (dark green).
Indo-European
The identification of the Proto-Indo-European homeland has been debated for centuries, but the steppe hypothesis is now widely accepted, placing it in the Pontic–Caspian steppe in the late 5th millennium BCE.[13] The leading alternative is the Anatolian hypothesis, proposing a homeland in Anatolia in the early 7th millennium BCE.[14]
Caucasian
The unrelated Kartvelian, Northwest Caucasian (Abkhaz-Adygean) and Northeast Caucasian (Nakh-Daghestanian) language families are presumed to be indigenous to the Caucasus.[15] There is extensive evidence for contact between the Caucasian languages, especially Proto-Kartvelian, and Proto-Indo-European, indicating that they were spoken in close proximity at least three to four thousand years ago.[16][17]
Dravidian
Although Dravidian languages are now concentrated in southern India, isolated pockets further north, placenames and substrate influences on Indo-Aryan languages indicate that they were once spoken more widely across the Indian subcontinent.[18] Reconstructed Proto-Dravidian terms for flora and fauna support the idea that Dravidian is indigenous to India.[19] Proponents of a migration from the northwest cite the location of Brahui, a hypothesized connection to the undeciphered Indus script, and claims of a link to Elamite.[20]
Turkic
Turkic languages are today spoken across an area stretching from northwest China to the edge of Europe, but Proto-Turkic lexical items about the climate, topography, flora, fauna and subsistence point to a homeland in the taiga-steppe zone of southern Siberia and Mongolia around the Altai-Sayan region.[21] Early contact with Mongolic languages also points to this area.[22] Genetic studies suggest that most of the expansion of the language family was due to language replacement rather than migration, but have identified shared elements originating from the South Siberia-Mongolia area.[23]
Uralic
Inherited tree names seem to indicate a Uralic homeland to the east of the Ural Mountains. The internal branching of the family suggests an area between the Ob River and Yenisey River.[24] Uralic speakers are not genetically distinguished from their neighbours, but do share a genetic component that is of Siberian origin.[25][26]

Eastern Eurasia edit

Japonic
Most scholars believe that Japonic was brought to northern Kyushu from the Korean Peninsula around 700 to 300 BCE by wet-rice farmers of the Yayoi culture, spreading from there throughout the Japanese Archipelago and somewhat later to the Ryukyu Islands.[27][28] There is fragmentary placename evidence that now-extinct Japonic languages were still spoken in central and southern parts of the Korean peninsula several centuries later.[29]
Koreanic
All modern Koreanic varieties are descended from the language of Unified Silla, which ruled the southern two-thirds of the Korean peninsula between the 7th and 10th centuries.[30][31] Evidence for the earlier linguistic history of the peninsula is extremely sparse.[32] The orthodox view among Korean social historians is that the Korean people migrated to the peninsula from the north, but no archaeological evidence of such a migration has been found.[33][34]
Sino-Tibetan
The reconstruction of Sino-Tibetan is much less developed than for other major families, so its higher-level structure and time depth remain unclear.[35] Proposed homelands and periods include: the upper and middle reaches of the Yellow River about 4–8 kya, associated with the hypothesis of a top-level branching between Chinese and the rest (most likely); southwestern Sichuan around 9 kya, associated with the hypothesis that Chinese and Tibetan form a subbranch; Northeast India (the area of maximal diversity) 9–10 kya.[36]
Hmong–Mien
The most likely homeland of the Hmong–Mien languages is in Southern China between the Yangtze and Mekong rivers, but speakers of these languages may have migrated from Central China as a result of the expansion of the Han Chinese.[37]
Kra–Dai
Most scholars locate the homeland of the Kra–Dai languages in Southern China, possibly coastal Fujian or Guangdong.[38]
Austroasiatic
Austroasiatic is widely held to be the oldest family in mainland Southeast Asia, with its current discontinuous distribution resulting from the later arrival of other families. The various branches share a great deal of vocabulary concerning rice cultivation, but few related to metals.[39] Identification of the homeland of the family has been hampered by the lack of progress on its branching. The main proposals are Northern India (favoured by those who assume an early branching of Munda), Southeast Asia (the area of maximal diversity; most likely) and southern China (based on claimed loanwords in Chinese).[40]
Austronesian
The homeland of the Austronesian languages is widely accepted by linguists to be Taiwan, since nine of its ten branches are found there, with all Austronesian languages found outside Taiwan belonging to the remaining Malayo-Polynesian branch.[41]

North America edit

Eskimo–Aleut
The Eskimo–Aleut languages originated in the region of the Bering Strait or Southwest Alaska.[42]
Na-Dené and Yeniseian
The Dené–Yeniseian hypothesis proposes that the Na-Dené languages of North America and the Yeniseian languages of Central Siberia share a common ancestor. Suggested homelands for this family include Central or West Asia,[43] Siberia,[44] or Beringia,[45] but there is currently not enough evidence to resolve the question.[46]
Algic
The Algic languages are distributed from the Pacific coast to the Atlantic coast of North America. It is suggested that Proto-Algic was spoken on the Columbia Plateau. From there, pre-Wiyot and pre-Yurok speakers moved southwest to the North Coast of California, while the pre-Proto-Algonquian speakers moved to the Great Plains, which was the center of dispersal of the Algonquian languages.[47][48]
Uto-Aztecan
Some authorities on the history of the Uto-Aztecan language family place the Proto-Uto-Aztecan homeland in the border region between the USA and Mexico, namely the upland regions of Arizona and New Mexico and the adjacent areas of the Mexican states of Sonora and Chihuahua, roughly corresponding to the Sonoran Desert. The proto-language would have been spoken by foragers, about 5,000 years ago. Hill (2001) proposes instead a homeland further south, making the assumed speakers of Proto-Uto-Aztecan maize cultivators in Mesoamerica, who were gradually pushed north, bringing maize cultivation with them, during the period of roughly 4,500 to 3,000 years ago, the geographic diffusion of speakers corresponding to the breakup of linguistic unity.[49]

South America edit

Tupian
Proto-Tupian, the reconstructed common ancestor of the Tupian languages of South America, was probably spoken in the region between the Guaporé and Aripuanã rivers, around 5,000 years ago.[50]

Africa and Middle East edit

Afroasiatic
There is no consensus on the location of the Afroasiatic homeland, though based on current evidence somewhere in the eastern Sahara or adjacent regions is considered most likely.[51] Proto-Afroasiatic is estimated to have begun to break up in the 8th millennium BCE.[51] Proto-Semitic is thought to have been spoken in the Near East between 4400 and 7400 BCE, with Akkadian representing its earliest known branch.[52]
Niger–Congo
The validity of the Niger–Congo languages has become controversial. It probably originated in West Africa — where the greatest diversity is found — soon after the start of the Holocene. Its expansion may have been associated with the expansion of Sahel agriculture in the African Neolithic period, following the desiccation of the Sahara in c. 3500 BCE.[53][54]
Mande
Valentin Vydrin concluded that "the Mande homeland at the second half of the 4th millennium BC was located in Southern Sahara, somewhere to the North of 16° or even 18° of Northern latitude and between 3° and 12° of Western longitude."[55] That is now Mauritania and/or southern Western Sahara.[56]
Nilo-Saharan
The validity of the Nilo-Saharan family remains controversial. Proponents of the family view the border area between Chad, Sudan, and the Central African Republic as a likely candidate for its homeland from around the start of the Holocene.[57]
Central-Sudanic
The original homeland of Central Sudanic speakers is likely somewhere in the Bahr el Ghazal region.[58]
Khoe-Kwadi
The homeland of Khoe-Kwadi was likely the middle Zambezi Valley over 2,000 years ago.[59]

Australia edit

Pama-Nyungan

The Gulf Plains, west of Queensland[60]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Blust (1984), p. 46.
  2. ^ Campbell (2013), p. 423.
  3. ^ Campbell (2013), pp. 423–424.
  4. ^ Blust (1984), p. 48.
  5. ^ Campbell (2013), pp. 430–432.
  6. ^ Renfrew, Colin; McMahon, April; Trask, Larry, eds. (1999). Time Depth in Historical Linguistics. ISBN 978-1-902937-06-9.[page needed]
  7. ^ O'Rourke, Dennis H.; Raff, Jennifer A. (2010), "The Human Genetic History of the Americas: The Final Frontier", Current Biology, 20 (4): R202–7, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.051, PMID 20178768, S2CID 14479088
  8. ^ Bowern, Claire; Atkinson, Quentin (2012). "Computational Phylogenetics and the Internal Structure of Pama-Nyungan". Language. 84 (4): 817–845. Kayser, Manfred (2010), "The Human Genetic History of Oceania: Near and Remote Views of Dispersal", Current Biology, 20 (4): R194–201, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.004, PMID 20178767, S2CID 7282462
  9. ^ Bengtson and Ruhlen (1994) offered a list of 27 "global etymologies". Bengtson, John D. and Merritt Ruhlen. 1994. "Global etymologies" 2007-09-28 at the Wayback Machine. In Ruhlen 1994a, pp. 277–336. This approach has been criticized as flawed by Campbell and Poser (2008) who used the same criteria employed by Bengtson and Ruhlen to identify "cognates" in Spanish known to be false. Campbell, Lyle, and William J. Poser. 2008. Language Classification: History and Method. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 370–372.
  10. ^ Poloni, Estella S.; Naciri, Yamama; Bucho, Rute; Niba, Régine; Kervaire, Barbara; Excoffier, Laurent; Langaney, André; Sanchez-Mazas, Alicia (November 2009). "Genetic Evidence for Complexity in Ethnic Differentiation and History in East Africa". Annals of Human Genetics. 73 (6): 582–600. doi:10.1111/j.1469-1809.2009.00541.x. PMID 19706029. S2CID 2488794.
  11. ^ McWhorter, John H. (December 1998). "Identifying the Creole Prototype: Vindicating a Typological Class". Language. 74 (4): 788–818. doi:10.2307/417003. JSTOR 417003.
  12. ^ McWhorter, John H. (1999), "The Afrogenesis Hypothesis of Plantation Creole Origin", in Huber, Magnus; Parkvall, Mikael (eds.), Spreading the Word: The Issue of Diffusion among the Atlantic Creoles, London: Westminster University Press, pp. 111–152
  13. ^ Anthony & Ringe 2015, pp. 208–209.
  14. ^ Anthony & Ringe 2015, p. 202.
  15. ^ Amiridze, Nino (27 May 2019). "Languages of the Caucasus and contact-induced language change". STUF – Language Typology and Universals. 72 (2): 185–192. doi:10.1515/stuf-2019-0007.
  16. ^ Anthony & Ringe 2015, p. 207.
  17. ^ Rayfield, Donald (2019). "The Emergence of the Kartvelians" (PDF). Edge of Empires: A History of Georgia. London: Reaktion Books. pp. 11–12. ISBN 9781789140590.
  18. ^ Krishnamurti (2003), pp. 5–6.
  19. ^ Krishnamurti (2003), p. 15.
  20. ^ Krishnamurti (2003), pp. 3, 5.
  21. ^ Golden, Peter B. (2011). "Ethnogenesis in the tribal zone: The Shaping of the Turks". Studies on the Peoples and Cultures of the Eurasian Steppes. Editura Academiei Române. pp. 17–63. ISBN 978-973-27-2152-0. pp. 35–37.
  22. ^ Janhunen, Juha (2003). "Ethnicity and language in prehistoric Northeast Asia". In Blench, Roger; Spriggs, Matthew (eds.). Archaeology and Language II: Archaeological Data and Linguistic Hypotheses. Routledge. pp. 195–208. ISBN 0-415-11761-5. p. 203.
  23. ^ Yunusbayev, Bayazit; Metspalu, Mait; Metspalu, Ene; Valeev, Albert; Litvinov, Sergei; Valiev, Ruslan; Akhmetova, Vita; Balanovska, Elena; Balanovsky, Oleg; Turdikulova, Shahlo; Dalimova, Dilbar; Nymadawa, Pagbajabyn; Bahmanimehr, Ardeshir; Sahakyan, Hovhannes; Tambets, Kristiina; Fedorova, Sardana; Barashkov, Nikolay; Khidiyatova, Irina; Mihailov, Evelin; Khusainova, Rita; Damba, Larisa; Derenko, Miroslava; Malyarchuk, Boris; Osipova, Ludmila; Voevoda, Mikhail; Yepiskoposyan, Levon; Kivisild, Toomas; Khusnutdinova, Elza; Villems, Richard (21 April 2015). "The Genetic Legacy of the Expansion of Turkic-Speaking Nomads across Eurasia". PLOS Genetics. 11 (4): e1005068. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1005068. PMC 4405460. PMID 25898006.
  24. ^ Janhunen, Juha (2009). "Proto-Uralic — what, where, and when?" (PDF). In Ylikoski, Jussi (ed.). The Quasquicentennial of the Finno-Ugrian Society. Suomalais-Ugrilaisen Seuran Toimituksia. Vol. 258. Helsinki: Finno-Ugrian Society. pp. 57–78. ISBN 978-952-5667-11-0. p. 71.
  25. ^ Tambets, Kristiina; Yunusbayev, Bayazit; Hudjashov, Georgi; Ilumäe, Anne-Mai; Rootsi, Siiri; Honkola, Terhi; Vesakoski, Outi; Atkinson, Quentin; Skoglund, Pontus; Kushniarevich, Alena; Litvinov, Sergey; Reidla, Maere; Metspalu, Ene; Saag, Lehti; Rantanen, Timo; Karmin, Monika; Parik, Jüri; Zhadanov, Sergey I.; Gubina, Marina; Damba, Larisa D.; Bermisheva, Marina; Reisberg, Tuuli; Dibirova, Khadizhat; Evseeva, Irina; Nelis, Mari; Klovins, Janis; Metspalu, Andres; Esko, Tõnu; Balanovsky, Oleg; Balanovska, Elena; Khusnutdinova, Elza K.; Osipova, Ludmila P.; Voevoda, Mikhail; Villems, Richard; Kivisild, Toomas; Metspalu, Mait (December 2018). "Genes reveal traces of common recent demographic history for most of the Uralic-speaking populations". Genome Biology. 19 (1): 139. doi:10.1186/s13059-018-1522-1. PMC 6151024. PMID 30241495.
  26. ^ Saag, Lehti; Laneman, Margot; Varul, Liivi; Malve, Martin; Valk, Heiki; Razzak, Maria A.; Shirobokov, Ivan G.; Khartanovich, Valeri I.; Mikhaylova, Elena R.; Kushniarevich, Alena; Scheib, Christiana Lyn; Solnik, Anu; Reisberg, Tuuli; Parik, Jüri; Saag, Lauri; Metspalu, Ene; Rootsi, Siiri; Montinaro, Francesco; Remm, Maido; Mägi, Reedik; D'Atanasio, Eugenia; Crema, Enrico Ryunosuke; Díez-del-Molino, David; Thomas, Mark G.; Kriiska, Aivar; Kivisild, Toomas; Villems, Richard; Lang, Valter; Metspalu, Mait; Tambets, Kristiina (May 2019). "The Arrival of Siberian Ancestry Connecting the Eastern Baltic to Uralic Speakers further East". Current Biology. 29 (10): 1701–1711.e16. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.04.026. PMC 6544527. PMID 31080083.
  27. ^ Serafim, Leon A. (2008). "The uses of Ryukyuan in understanding Japanese language history". In Frellesvig, Bjarke; Whitman, John (eds.). Proto-Japanese: Issues and Prospects. John Benjamins. pp. 79–99. ISBN 978-90-272-4809-1. p. 98.
  28. ^ Vovin, Alexander (2017). "Origins of the Japanese Language". Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.277. ISBN 978-0-19-938465-5.
  29. ^ Sohn (1999), pp. 35–36.
  30. ^ Sohn (1999), p. 40.
  31. ^ Lee, Ki-Moon; Ramsey, S. Robert (2011). A History of the Korean Language. Cambridge University Press. p. 4. ISBN 978-1-139-49448-9.
  32. ^ Lee, Iksop; Ramsey, S. Robert (2000). The Korean Language. SUNY Press. p. 31. ISBN 978-0-7914-4831-1.
  33. ^ Yi, Seonbok (2014). "Korea: archaeology". In Bellwood, Peter (ed.). The Global Prehistory of Human Migration. Wiley. pp. 586–597. ISBN 978-1-118-97059-1. pp. 586–587.
  34. ^ Nelson, Sara M. (1995). "The Politics of Ethnicity in Prehistoric Korea". In Kohl, Philip L.; Fawcett, Clare (eds.). Nationalism, Politics and the Practice of Archaeology. Cambridge University Press. pp. 218–231. ISBN 978-0-521-55839-6. p. 230.
  35. ^ Handel, Zev (May 2008). "What is Sino-Tibetan? Snapshot of a Field and a Language Family in Flux: Sino-Tibetan: a Snapshot". Language and Linguistics Compass. 2 (3): 422–441. doi:10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00061.x.
  36. ^ Zhang, Menghan; Yan, Shi; Pan, Wuyun; Jin, Li (2019). "Phylogenetic evidence for Sino-Tibetan origin in northern China in the Late Neolithic". Nature. 569 (7754): 112–115. Bibcode:2019Natur.569..112Z. doi:10.1038/s41586-019-1153-z. PMID 31019300. S2CID 129946000. p. 112.
  37. ^ Blench, Roger (2008). "Stratification in the peopling of China: How far does the linguistic evidence match genetics and archaeology?". In Sanchez-Mazas, Alicia; Blench, Roger; Ross, Malcolm D; Peiros, Ilia; Lin, Marie (eds.). Past Human Migrations in East Asia. Routledge. doi:10.4324/9780203926789. ISBN 978-0-203-92678-9.
  38. ^ "Kra-dai and Austronesian: Notes on phonological correspondences and vocabulary distribution WEERA OSTAPIRAT". The Peopling of East Asia. 2005. pp. 135–159. doi:10.4324/9780203343685-20. ISBN 9780203343685.
  39. ^ Sidwell, Paul (2015). "Austroasiatic Classification". In Jenny, Mathias; Sidwell, Paul (eds.). The Handbook of the Austroasiatic Languages. Leiden: BRILL. pp. 144–220. ISBN 978-90-04-28295-7. p. 146.
  40. ^ Rau, Felix; Sidwell, Paul (12 September 2019). "The Munda Maritime Hypothesis". Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society. 12 (2): 35–57. hdl:10524/52454. Gale A651276154.
  41. ^ Blust, Robert (2013). The Austronesian Languages (revised ed.). Australian National University. p. 749. hdl:1885/10191. ISBN 978-1-922185-07-5.
  42. ^ Holton, Gary. "Language Relationships". Alaska Native Language Center. Retrieved 19 November 2020.
  43. ^ Ruhlen, Merritt (10 November 1998). "The origin of the Na-Dene". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 95 (23): 13994–13996. Bibcode:1998PNAS...9513994R. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.23.13994. PMC 25007. PMID 9811914.
  44. ^ Potter, Ben A. (2010). "Archaeological Patterning in Northeast Asia and Northwest North America: An Examination of the Dene-Yeniseian Hypothesis". Anthropological Papers of the University of Alaska. 5 (1–2): 138–167.
  45. ^ Sicoli, Mark A.; Holton, Gary (12 March 2014). "Linguistic Phylogenies Support Back-Migration from Beringia to Asia". PLOS ONE. 9 (3): e91722. Bibcode:2014PLoSO...991722S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0091722. PMC 3951421. PMID 24621925.
  46. ^ Yanovich, Igor (16 September 2020). "Phylogenetic linguistic evidence and the Dene-Yeniseian homeland". Diachronica. 37 (3): 410–446. doi:10.1075/dia.17038.yan. S2CID 209542004.
  47. ^ Bakker, Peter (2013). "Diachrony and typology in the history of Cree". In Folke Josephson; Ingmar Söhrman (eds.). Diachronic and typological perspectives on verbs. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 223–260.
  48. ^ Golla, Victor (2011). California Indian Languages. Berkeley: University of California Press. p. 256.
  49. ^ Hill, Jane H. (2001). "Proto-Uto-Aztecan: A Community of Cultivators in Central Mexico?". American Anthropologist. 103 (4): 913–934. doi:10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.913. JSTOR 684121.
  50. ^ Rodrigues, Aryon Dall'Igna; Cabral, Ana Suelly Arruda Câmara (2012). "Tupían". In Campbell, Lyle; Grondona, Verónica (eds.). The Indigenous Languages of South America: A Comprehensive Guide. Walter de Gruyter. pp. 495–574. ISBN 978-3-11-025803-5.
  51. ^ a b Porkhomovsky, Victor (2020). "Afro-Asiatic Overview". In Vossen, Rainer; Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of African Languages. Oxford University Press. pp. 269–274. ISBN 978-0-19-960989-5. p. 273.
  52. ^ Kitchen, Andrew; Ehret, Christopher; Assefa, Shiferaw; Mulligan, Connie J. (7 August 2009). "Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of Semitic languages identifies an Early Bronze Age origin of Semitic in the Near East". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 276 (1668): 2703–2710. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0408. PMC 2839953. PMID 19403539.
  53. ^ Manning, Katie; Timpson, Adrian (October 2014). "The demographic response to Holocene climate change in the Sahara". Quaternary Science Reviews. 101: 28–35. Bibcode:2014QSRv..101...28M. doi:10.1016/j.quascirev.2014.07.003.
  54. ^ Kopytoff, Igor (1989). The African Frontier: The Reproduction of Traditional African Societies. Indiana University Press. ISBN 978-0-253-20539-1. (cited after Igbo Language Roots and (Pre)-History 2019-07-17 at the Wayback Machine, A Mighty Tree, 2011).
  55. ^ Vydrin, Valentin. "On the Problem of the Proto-Mande Homeland" (PDF). Journal of Language Relationship.
  56. ^ Fields-Black, Edda L. (2015). "Rice and Rice Farmers in the Upper Guinea Coast and Environmental History". In Bray, Francesca; Coclanis, Peter A.; Fields-Black, Edda L.; Schäfer, Dagmar (eds.). Rice: Global Networks and New Histories. Cambridge University Press. p. 170. ISBN 9781107044395. from the original on 2022-04-14.
  57. ^ Dimmendaal, Gerrit J. (2020). "Nilo-Saharan and Its Limits". In Rainer Vossen; Gerrit J. Dimmendaal (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of African Languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 364–382. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199609895.013.15. ISBN 978-0-19-960989-5.
  58. ^ Andah, Bassey; Okpoko, Alex; Shaw, Thurstan; Sinclair, Paul (22 May 2014). The Archaeology of Africa: Food, Metals and Towns. Routledge. ISBN 9781134679492.
  59. ^ Newman, James L. (January 1995). The Peopling of Africa: A Geographic Interpretation. Yale University Press. ISBN 0300072805.
  60. ^ Bouckaert, Remco R.; Bowern, Claire; Atkinson, Quentin D. (April 2018). "The origin and expansion of Pama–Nyungan languages across Australia". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 2 (4): 741–749. doi:10.1038/s41559-018-0489-3.

Sources edit

  • Anthony, David W. (2007), The Horse, the Wheel and Language: How Bronze-Age Riders from the Eurasian Steppes Shaped the Modern World, Princeton University Press
  • Anthony, David; Ringe, Don (2015), "The Indo-European Homeland from Linguistic and Archaeological Perspectives", Annual Review of Linguistics, 1: 199–219, doi:10.1146/annurev-linguist-030514-124812
  • Bellwood, Peter (2007) [1997]. Prehistory of the Indo-Malaysian Archipelago: Revised Edition. ANU E Press. ISBN 978-1-921313-12-7.
  • Bellwood, Peter S. (2000), Presejarah Kepulauan Indo-Malaysia (Translation of Bellwood 2007)
  • Blust, Robert (1984). "The Austronesian Homeland: A Linguistic Perspective". Asian Perspectives. 26 (1): 45–67. JSTOR 42928105.
  • Campbell, Lyle (1997), American Indian Languages: The Historical Linguistics of Native America, Oxford University Press, ISBN 978-0-19-509427-5.
  • ——— (2013), Historical Linguistics: An Introduction (3rd ed.), Edinburgh University Press, ISBN 978-0-7486-4601-2.
  • Gray, Russell D.; Atkinson, Quentin D. (2003). "Language-tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo-European origin". Nature. 426 (6965): 435–439. Bibcode:2003Natur.426..435G. doi:10.1038/nature02029. PMID 14647380. S2CID 42340.
  • Haak, Wolfgang; Lazaridis, Iosif; Patterson, Nick; Rohland, Nadin; Mallick, Swapan; Llamas, Bastien; Brandt, Guido; Nordenfelt, Susanne; Harney, Eadaoin; Stewardson, Kristin; Fu, Qiaomei; Mittnik, Alissa; Bánffy, Eszter; Economou, Christos; Francken, Michael; Friederich, Susanne; Pena, Rafael Garrido; Hallgren, Fredrik; Khartanovich, Valery; Khokhlov, Aleksandr; Kunst, Michael; Kuznetsov, Pavel; Meller, Harald; Mochalov, Oleg; Moiseyev, Vayacheslav; Nicklisch, Nicole; Pichler, Sandra L.; Risch, Roberto; Rojo Guerra, Manuel A.; Roth, Christina; Szécsényi-Nagy, Anna; Wahl, Joachim; Meyer, Matthias; Krause, Johannes; Brown, Dorcas; Anthony, David; Cooper, Alan; Alt, Kurt Werner; Reich, David (11 June 2015). "Massive migration from the steppe was a source for Indo-European languages in Europe". Nature. 522 (7555): 207–211. arXiv:1502.02783. Bibcode:2015Natur.522..207H. doi:10.1038/nature14317. PMC 5048219. PMID 25731166.
  • Koerner, E. F. K. (2001). "Linguistics and Ideology in 19th and 20th Century Studies of Language". In Dirven, René; Hawkins, Bruce; Sandikcioglu, Esra (eds.). Language and Ideology: Volume 1: theoretical cognitive approaches. John Benjamins Publishing. pp. 253–276. ISBN 978-90-272-9954-3.
  • Krishnamurti, Bhadriraju (2003). The Dravidian Languages. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-77111-0.
  • Mallory, J.P. (1989), In Search of the Indo-Europeans: Language, Archaeology, and Myth, London: Thames & Hudson.
  • Mallory, James P. (1997), "The homelands of the Indo-Europeans", in Blench, Roger; Spriggs, Matthew (eds.), Archaeology and Language, vol. I: Theoretical and Methodological Orientations, London: Routledge, ISBN 978-0-415-11760-9.
  • Mallory, J.P.; Adams, D.Q. (2006), The Oxford introduction to Proto-Indo-European and the Proto-Indo-European world (Repr. ed.), Oxford [u.a.]: Oxford Univ. Press, ISBN 978-0-19-928791-8
  • Pereltsvaig, Asya; Lewis, Martin W. (2015), "Searching for Indo-European origins", The Indo-European Controversy, Cambridge University Press, ISBN 978-1-107-05453-0
  • Sohn, Ho-Min (1999), The Korean Language, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, ISBN 978-0-521-36123-1.

linguistic, homeland, historical, linguistics, homeland, urheimat, ʊər, ɑː, from, german, original, heimat, home, proto, language, region, which, spoken, before, splitting, into, different, daughter, languages, proto, language, reconstructed, historically, att. In historical linguistics the homeland or Urheimat ˈ ʊer h aɪ m ɑː t from German ur original and Heimat home of a proto language is the region in which it was spoken before splitting into different daughter languages A proto language is the reconstructed or historically attested parent language of a group of languages that are genetically related Depending on the age of the language family under consideration its homeland may be known with near certainty in the case of historical or near historical migrations or it may be very uncertain in the case of deep prehistory Next to internal linguistic evidence the reconstruction of a prehistoric homeland makes use of a variety of disciplines including archaeology and archaeogenetics Contents 1 Methods 2 Limitations of the concept 2 1 Time depth 2 2 Language contact and creolization 2 3 Isolates 3 Homelands of major language families 3 1 Western and central Eurasia 3 2 Eastern Eurasia 3 3 North America 3 4 South America 3 5 Africa and Middle East 3 6 Australia 4 See also 5 References 6 SourcesMethods editThere are several methods to determine the homeland of a given language family One method is based on the vocabulary that can be reconstructed for the proto language This vocabulary especially terms for flora and fauna can provide clues for the geographical and ecological environment in which the proto language was spoken An estimate for the time depth of the proto language is necessary in order to account for prehistorical changes in climate and the distribution of flora and fauna 1 2 Another method is based on the linguistic migration theory first proposed by Edward Sapir which states that the most likely candidate for the last homeland of a language family can be located in the area of its highest linguistic diversity 3 This presupposes an established view about the internal subgrouping of the language family Different assumptions about high order subgrouping can thus lead to very divergent proposals for a linguistic homeland e g Isidore Dyen s proposal for New Guinea as the center of dispersal of the Austronesian languages 4 The linguistic migration theory has its limits because it only works when linguistic diversity evolves continuously without major disruptions Its results can be distorted e g when this diversity is wiped out by more recent migrations 5 Limitations of the concept editThis section possibly contains original research Please improve it by verifying the claims made and adding inline citations Statements consisting only of original research should be removed September 2021 Learn how and when to remove this template message This section may contain material not related to the topic of the article and should be moved to Tree model instead Please help improve this section or discuss this issue on the talk page March 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message Further information Language change The concept of a single identifiable homeland of a given language family implies a purely genealogical view of the development of languages This assumption is often reasonable and useful but it is by no means a logical necessity as languages are well known to be susceptible to areal change such as substrate or superstrate influence Time depth edit Further information Behavioral modernity Origin of language Origin of speech Proto Human language Borean languages Nostratic languages and Dene Caucasian languages Over a sufficient period of time in the absence of evidence of intermediary steps in the process it may be impossible to observe linkages between languages that have a shared Urheimat given enough time natural language change will obliterate any meaningful linguistic evidence of a common genetic source This general concern is a manifestation of the larger issue of time depth in historical linguistics 6 For example the languages of the New World are believed to be descended from a relatively rapid peopling of the Americas relative to the duration of the Upper Paleolithic within a few millennia roughly between 20 000 and 15 000 years ago 7 but their genetic relationship has become completely obscured over the more than ten millennia which have passed between their separation and their first written record in the early modern period Similarly the Australian Aboriginal languages are divided into some 28 families and isolates for which no genetic relationship can be shown 8 The Urheimaten reconstructed using the methods of comparative linguistics typically estimate separation times dating to the Neolithic or later It is undisputed that fully developed languages were present throughout the Upper Paleolithic and possibly into the deep Middle Paleolithic see origin of language behavioral modernity These languages would have spread with the early human migrations of the first peopling of the world but they are no longer amenable to linguistic reconstruction The Last Glacial Maximum LGM has imposed linguistic separation lasting several millennia on many Upper Paleolithic populations in Eurasia as they were forced to retreat into refugia before the advancing ice sheets After the end of the LGM Mesolithic populations of the Holocene again became more mobile and most of the prehistoric spread of the world s major linguistic families seem to reflect the expansion of population cores during the Mesolithic followed by the Neolithic Revolution The Nostratic theory is the best known attempt to expand the deep prehistory of the main language families of Eurasia excepting Sino Tibetan and the languages of Southeast Asia to the beginning of the Holocene First proposed in the early 20th century the Nostratic theory still receives serious consideration but it is by no means generally accepted The more recent and more speculative Borean hypothesis attempts to unite Nostratic with Dene Caucasian and Austric in a mega phylum that would unite most languages of Eurasia with a time depth going back to the Last Glacial Maximum The argument surrounding the Proto Human language finally is almost completely detached from linguistic reconstruction instead surrounding questions of phonology and the origin of speech Time depths involved in the deep prehistory of all the world s extant languages are of the order of at least 100 000 years 9 Language contact and creolization edit The concept of an Urheimat only applies to populations speaking a proto language defined by the tree model This is not always the case For example in places where language families meet the relationship between a group that speaks a language and the Urheimat for that language is complicated by processes of migration language shift and group absorption are documented by linguists and ethnographers in groups that are themselves transient and plastic Thus in the contact area in western Ethiopia between languages belonging to the Nilo Saharan and Afroasiatic families the Nilo Saharan speaking Nyangatom and the Afroasiatic speaking Daasanach have been observed to be closely related to each other but genetically distinct from neighboring Afroasiatic speaking populations This is a reflection of the fact that the Daasanach like the Nyangatom originally spoke a Nilo Saharan language with the ancestral Daasanach later adopting an Afroasiatic language around the 19th century 10 Creole languages are hybrids of languages that are sometimes unrelated Similarities arise from the creole formation process rather than from genetic descent 11 For example a creole language may lack significant inflectional morphology lack tone on monosyllabic words or lack semantically opaque word formation even if these features are found in all of the parent languages of the languages from which the creole was formed 12 Isolates edit Some languages are language isolates That is to say they have no well accepted language family connection no nodes in a family tree and therefore no known Urheimat An example is the Basque language of Northern Spain and southwest France Nevertheless it is a scientific fact that all languages evolve An unknown Urheimat may still be hypothesized such as that for a Proto Basque and may be supported by archaeological and historical evidence Sometimes relatives are found for a language originally believed to be an isolate An example is the Etruscan language which even though only partially understood is believed to be related to the Rhaetic language and to the Lemnian language A single family may be an isolate In the case of the non Austronesian indigenous languages of Papua New Guinea and the indigenous languages of Australia there is no published linguistic hypothesis supported by any evidence that these languages have links to any other families Nevertheless an unknown Urheimat is implied The entire Indo European family itself is a language isolate no further connections are known This lack of information does not prevent some professional linguists from formulating additional hypothetical nodes Nostratic and additional homelands for the speakers Homelands of major language families editWestern and central Eurasia edit nbsp Map showing the present day distribution of Indo European languages in Eurasia light green and the likely Proto Indo European homeland dark green Indo European The identification of the Proto Indo European homeland has been debated for centuries but the steppe hypothesis is now widely accepted placing it in the Pontic Caspian steppe in the late 5th millennium BCE 13 The leading alternative is the Anatolian hypothesis proposing a homeland in Anatolia in the early 7th millennium BCE 14 Caucasian The unrelated Kartvelian Northwest Caucasian Abkhaz Adygean and Northeast Caucasian Nakh Daghestanian language families are presumed to be indigenous to the Caucasus 15 There is extensive evidence for contact between the Caucasian languages especially Proto Kartvelian and Proto Indo European indicating that they were spoken in close proximity at least three to four thousand years ago 16 17 Dravidian Although Dravidian languages are now concentrated in southern India isolated pockets further north placenames and substrate influences on Indo Aryan languages indicate that they were once spoken more widely across the Indian subcontinent 18 Reconstructed Proto Dravidian terms for flora and fauna support the idea that Dravidian is indigenous to India 19 Proponents of a migration from the northwest cite the location of Brahui a hypothesized connection to the undeciphered Indus script and claims of a link to Elamite 20 Turkic Turkic languages are today spoken across an area stretching from northwest China to the edge of Europe but Proto Turkic lexical items about the climate topography flora fauna and subsistence point to a homeland in the taiga steppe zone of southern Siberia and Mongolia around the Altai Sayan region 21 Early contact with Mongolic languages also points to this area 22 Genetic studies suggest that most of the expansion of the language family was due to language replacement rather than migration but have identified shared elements originating from the South Siberia Mongolia area 23 Uralic Inherited tree names seem to indicate a Uralic homeland to the east of the Ural Mountains The internal branching of the family suggests an area between the Ob River and Yenisey River 24 Uralic speakers are not genetically distinguished from their neighbours but do share a genetic component that is of Siberian origin 25 26 Eastern Eurasia edit Japonic Most scholars believe that Japonic was brought to northern Kyushu from the Korean Peninsula around 700 to 300 BCE by wet rice farmers of the Yayoi culture spreading from there throughout the Japanese Archipelago and somewhat later to the Ryukyu Islands 27 28 There is fragmentary placename evidence that now extinct Japonic languages were still spoken in central and southern parts of the Korean peninsula several centuries later 29 Koreanic All modern Koreanic varieties are descended from the language of Unified Silla which ruled the southern two thirds of the Korean peninsula between the 7th and 10th centuries 30 31 Evidence for the earlier linguistic history of the peninsula is extremely sparse 32 The orthodox view among Korean social historians is that the Korean people migrated to the peninsula from the north but no archaeological evidence of such a migration has been found 33 34 Sino Tibetan The reconstruction of Sino Tibetan is much less developed than for other major families so its higher level structure and time depth remain unclear 35 Proposed homelands and periods include the upper and middle reaches of the Yellow River about 4 8 kya associated with the hypothesis of a top level branching between Chinese and the rest most likely southwestern Sichuan around 9 kya associated with the hypothesis that Chinese and Tibetan form a subbranch Northeast India the area of maximal diversity 9 10 kya 36 Hmong Mien The most likely homeland of the Hmong Mien languages is in Southern China between the Yangtze and Mekong rivers but speakers of these languages may have migrated from Central China as a result of the expansion of the Han Chinese 37 Kra Dai Most scholars locate the homeland of the Kra Dai languages in Southern China possibly coastal Fujian or Guangdong 38 Austroasiatic Austroasiatic is widely held to be the oldest family in mainland Southeast Asia with its current discontinuous distribution resulting from the later arrival of other families The various branches share a great deal of vocabulary concerning rice cultivation but few related to metals 39 Identification of the homeland of the family has been hampered by the lack of progress on its branching The main proposals are Northern India favoured by those who assume an early branching of Munda Southeast Asia the area of maximal diversity most likely and southern China based on claimed loanwords in Chinese 40 Austronesian The homeland of the Austronesian languages is widely accepted by linguists to be Taiwan since nine of its ten branches are found there with all Austronesian languages found outside Taiwan belonging to the remaining Malayo Polynesian branch 41 North America edit Eskimo Aleut The Eskimo Aleut languages originated in the region of the Bering Strait or Southwest Alaska 42 Na Dene and Yeniseian The Dene Yeniseian hypothesis proposes that the Na Dene languages of North America and the Yeniseian languages of Central Siberia share a common ancestor Suggested homelands for this family include Central or West Asia 43 Siberia 44 or Beringia 45 but there is currently not enough evidence to resolve the question 46 Algic The Algic languages are distributed from the Pacific coast to the Atlantic coast of North America It is suggested that Proto Algic was spoken on the Columbia Plateau From there pre Wiyot and pre Yurok speakers moved southwest to the North Coast of California while the pre Proto Algonquian speakers moved to the Great Plains which was the center of dispersal of the Algonquian languages 47 48 Uto Aztecan Some authorities on the history of the Uto Aztecan language family place the Proto Uto Aztecan homeland in the border region between the USA and Mexico namely the upland regions of Arizona and New Mexico and the adjacent areas of the Mexican states of Sonora and Chihuahua roughly corresponding to the Sonoran Desert The proto language would have been spoken by foragers about 5 000 years ago Hill 2001 proposes instead a homeland further south making the assumed speakers of Proto Uto Aztecan maize cultivators in Mesoamerica who were gradually pushed north bringing maize cultivation with them during the period of roughly 4 500 to 3 000 years ago the geographic diffusion of speakers corresponding to the breakup of linguistic unity 49 South America edit Tupian Proto Tupian the reconstructed common ancestor of the Tupian languages of South America was probably spoken in the region between the Guapore and Aripuana rivers around 5 000 years ago 50 Africa and Middle East edit Afroasiatic There is no consensus on the location of the Afroasiatic homeland though based on current evidence somewhere in the eastern Sahara or adjacent regions is considered most likely 51 Proto Afroasiatic is estimated to have begun to break up in the 8th millennium BCE 51 Proto Semitic is thought to have been spoken in the Near East between 4400 and 7400 BCE with Akkadian representing its earliest known branch 52 Niger Congo The validity of the Niger Congo languages has become controversial It probably originated in West Africa where the greatest diversity is found soon after the start of the Holocene Its expansion may have been associated with the expansion of Sahel agriculture in the African Neolithic period following the desiccation of the Sahara in c 3500 BCE 53 54 Mande Valentin Vydrin concluded that the Mande homeland at the second half of the 4th millennium BC was located in Southern Sahara somewhere to the North of 16 or even 18 of Northern latitude and between 3 and 12 of Western longitude 55 That is now Mauritania and or southern Western Sahara 56 Nilo Saharan The validity of the Nilo Saharan family remains controversial Proponents of the family view the border area between Chad Sudan and the Central African Republic as a likely candidate for its homeland from around the start of the Holocene 57 Central Sudanic The original homeland of Central Sudanic speakers is likely somewhere in the Bahr el Ghazal region 58 Khoe Kwadi The homeland of Khoe Kwadi was likely the middle Zambezi Valley over 2 000 years ago 59 Australia edit Pama NyunganThe Gulf Plains west of Queensland 60 See also editGenetic relationship linguistics Nationalist historiography SprachbundReferences edit Blust 1984 p 46 Campbell 2013 p 423 Campbell 2013 pp 423 424 Blust 1984 p 48 Campbell 2013 pp 430 432 Renfrew Colin McMahon April Trask Larry eds 1999 Time Depth in Historical Linguistics ISBN 978 1 902937 06 9 page needed O Rourke Dennis H Raff Jennifer A 2010 The Human Genetic History of the Americas The Final Frontier Current Biology 20 4 R202 7 doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 11 051 PMID 20178768 S2CID 14479088 Bowern Claire Atkinson Quentin 2012 Computational Phylogenetics and the Internal Structure of Pama Nyungan Language 84 4 817 845 Kayser Manfred 2010 The Human Genetic History of Oceania Near and Remote Views of Dispersal Current Biology 20 4 R194 201 doi 10 1016 j cub 2009 12 004 PMID 20178767 S2CID 7282462 Bengtson and Ruhlen 1994 offered a list of 27 global etymologies Bengtson John D and Merritt Ruhlen 1994 Global etymologies Archived 2007 09 28 at the Wayback Machine In Ruhlen 1994a pp 277 336 This approach has been criticized as flawed by Campbell and Poser 2008 who used the same criteria employed by Bengtson and Ruhlen to identify cognates in Spanish known to be false Campbell Lyle and William J Poser 2008 Language Classification History and Method Cambridge Cambridge University Press 370 372 Poloni Estella S Naciri Yamama Bucho Rute Niba Regine Kervaire Barbara Excoffier Laurent Langaney Andre Sanchez Mazas Alicia November 2009 Genetic Evidence for Complexity in Ethnic Differentiation and History in East Africa Annals of Human Genetics 73 6 582 600 doi 10 1111 j 1469 1809 2009 00541 x PMID 19706029 S2CID 2488794 McWhorter John H December 1998 Identifying the Creole Prototype Vindicating a Typological Class Language 74 4 788 818 doi 10 2307 417003 JSTOR 417003 McWhorter John H 1999 The Afrogenesis Hypothesis of Plantation Creole Origin in Huber Magnus Parkvall Mikael eds Spreading the Word The Issue of Diffusion among the Atlantic Creoles London Westminster University Press pp 111 152 Anthony amp Ringe 2015 pp 208 209 Anthony amp Ringe 2015 p 202 Amiridze Nino 27 May 2019 Languages of the Caucasus and contact induced language change STUF Language Typology and Universals 72 2 185 192 doi 10 1515 stuf 2019 0007 Anthony amp Ringe 2015 p 207 Rayfield Donald 2019 The Emergence of the Kartvelians PDF Edge of Empires A History of Georgia London Reaktion Books pp 11 12 ISBN 9781789140590 Krishnamurti 2003 pp 5 6 Krishnamurti 2003 p 15 Krishnamurti 2003 pp 3 5 Golden Peter B 2011 Ethnogenesis in the tribal zone The Shaping of the Turks Studies on the Peoples and Cultures of the Eurasian Steppes Editura Academiei Romane pp 17 63 ISBN 978 973 27 2152 0 pp 35 37 Janhunen Juha 2003 Ethnicity and language in prehistoric Northeast Asia In Blench Roger Spriggs Matthew eds Archaeology and Language II Archaeological Data and Linguistic Hypotheses Routledge pp 195 208 ISBN 0 415 11761 5 p 203 Yunusbayev Bayazit Metspalu Mait Metspalu Ene Valeev Albert Litvinov Sergei Valiev Ruslan Akhmetova Vita Balanovska Elena Balanovsky Oleg Turdikulova Shahlo Dalimova Dilbar Nymadawa Pagbajabyn Bahmanimehr Ardeshir Sahakyan Hovhannes Tambets Kristiina Fedorova Sardana Barashkov Nikolay Khidiyatova Irina Mihailov Evelin Khusainova Rita Damba Larisa Derenko Miroslava Malyarchuk Boris Osipova Ludmila Voevoda Mikhail Yepiskoposyan Levon Kivisild Toomas Khusnutdinova Elza Villems Richard 21 April 2015 The Genetic Legacy of the Expansion of Turkic Speaking Nomads across Eurasia PLOS Genetics 11 4 e1005068 doi 10 1371 journal pgen 1005068 PMC 4405460 PMID 25898006 Janhunen Juha 2009 Proto Uralic what where and when PDF In Ylikoski Jussi ed The Quasquicentennial of the Finno Ugrian Society Suomalais Ugrilaisen Seuran Toimituksia Vol 258 Helsinki Finno Ugrian Society pp 57 78 ISBN 978 952 5667 11 0 p 71 Tambets Kristiina Yunusbayev Bayazit Hudjashov Georgi Ilumae Anne Mai Rootsi Siiri Honkola Terhi Vesakoski Outi Atkinson Quentin Skoglund Pontus Kushniarevich Alena Litvinov Sergey Reidla Maere Metspalu Ene Saag Lehti Rantanen Timo Karmin Monika Parik Juri Zhadanov Sergey I Gubina Marina Damba Larisa D Bermisheva Marina Reisberg Tuuli Dibirova Khadizhat Evseeva Irina Nelis Mari Klovins Janis Metspalu Andres Esko Tonu Balanovsky Oleg Balanovska Elena Khusnutdinova Elza K Osipova Ludmila P Voevoda Mikhail Villems Richard Kivisild Toomas Metspalu Mait December 2018 Genes reveal traces of common recent demographic history for most of the Uralic speaking populations Genome Biology 19 1 139 doi 10 1186 s13059 018 1522 1 PMC 6151024 PMID 30241495 Saag Lehti Laneman Margot Varul Liivi Malve Martin Valk Heiki Razzak Maria A Shirobokov Ivan G Khartanovich Valeri I Mikhaylova Elena R Kushniarevich Alena Scheib Christiana Lyn Solnik Anu Reisberg Tuuli Parik Juri Saag Lauri Metspalu Ene Rootsi Siiri Montinaro Francesco Remm Maido Magi Reedik D Atanasio Eugenia Crema Enrico Ryunosuke Diez del Molino David Thomas Mark G Kriiska Aivar Kivisild Toomas Villems Richard Lang Valter Metspalu Mait Tambets Kristiina May 2019 The Arrival of Siberian Ancestry Connecting the Eastern Baltic to Uralic Speakers further East Current Biology 29 10 1701 1711 e16 doi 10 1016 j cub 2019 04 026 PMC 6544527 PMID 31080083 Serafim Leon A 2008 The uses of Ryukyuan in understanding Japanese language history In Frellesvig Bjarke Whitman John eds Proto Japanese Issues and Prospects John Benjamins pp 79 99 ISBN 978 90 272 4809 1 p 98 Vovin Alexander 2017 Origins of the Japanese Language Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 acrefore 9780199384655 013 277 ISBN 978 0 19 938465 5 Sohn 1999 pp 35 36 Sohn 1999 p 40 Lee Ki Moon Ramsey S Robert 2011 A History of the Korean Language Cambridge University Press p 4 ISBN 978 1 139 49448 9 Lee Iksop Ramsey S Robert 2000 The Korean Language SUNY Press p 31 ISBN 978 0 7914 4831 1 Yi Seonbok 2014 Korea archaeology In Bellwood Peter ed The Global Prehistory of Human Migration Wiley pp 586 597 ISBN 978 1 118 97059 1 pp 586 587 Nelson Sara M 1995 The Politics of Ethnicity in Prehistoric Korea In Kohl Philip L Fawcett Clare eds Nationalism Politics and the Practice of Archaeology Cambridge University Press pp 218 231 ISBN 978 0 521 55839 6 p 230 Handel Zev May 2008 What is Sino Tibetan Snapshot of a Field and a Language Family in Flux Sino Tibetan a Snapshot Language and Linguistics Compass 2 3 422 441 doi 10 1111 j 1749 818X 2008 00061 x Zhang Menghan Yan Shi Pan Wuyun Jin Li 2019 Phylogenetic evidence for Sino Tibetan origin in northern China in the Late Neolithic Nature 569 7754 112 115 Bibcode 2019Natur 569 112Z doi 10 1038 s41586 019 1153 z PMID 31019300 S2CID 129946000 p 112 Blench Roger 2008 Stratification in the peopling of China How far does the linguistic evidence match genetics and archaeology In Sanchez Mazas Alicia Blench Roger Ross Malcolm D Peiros Ilia Lin Marie eds Past Human Migrations in East Asia Routledge doi 10 4324 9780203926789 ISBN 978 0 203 92678 9 Kra dai and Austronesian Notes on phonological correspondences and vocabulary distribution WEERA OSTAPIRAT The Peopling of East Asia 2005 pp 135 159 doi 10 4324 9780203343685 20 ISBN 9780203343685 Sidwell Paul 2015 Austroasiatic Classification In Jenny Mathias Sidwell Paul eds The Handbook of the Austroasiatic Languages Leiden BRILL pp 144 220 ISBN 978 90 04 28295 7 p 146 Rau Felix Sidwell Paul 12 September 2019 The Munda Maritime Hypothesis Journal of the Southeast Asian Linguistics Society 12 2 35 57 hdl 10524 52454 Gale A651276154 Blust Robert 2013 The Austronesian Languages revised ed Australian National University p 749 hdl 1885 10191 ISBN 978 1 922185 07 5 Holton Gary Language Relationships Alaska Native Language Center Retrieved 19 November 2020 Ruhlen Merritt 10 November 1998 The origin of the Na Dene Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 95 23 13994 13996 Bibcode 1998PNAS 9513994R doi 10 1073 pnas 95 23 13994 PMC 25007 PMID 9811914 Potter Ben A 2010 Archaeological Patterning in Northeast Asia and Northwest North America An Examination of the Dene Yeniseian Hypothesis Anthropological Papers of the University of Alaska 5 1 2 138 167 Sicoli Mark A Holton Gary 12 March 2014 Linguistic Phylogenies Support Back Migration from Beringia to Asia PLOS ONE 9 3 e91722 Bibcode 2014PLoSO 991722S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0091722 PMC 3951421 PMID 24621925 Yanovich Igor 16 September 2020 Phylogenetic linguistic evidence and the Dene Yeniseian homeland Diachronica 37 3 410 446 doi 10 1075 dia 17038 yan S2CID 209542004 Bakker Peter 2013 Diachrony and typology in the history of Cree In Folke Josephson Ingmar Sohrman eds Diachronic and typological perspectives on verbs Amsterdam John Benjamins pp 223 260 Golla Victor 2011 California Indian Languages Berkeley University of California Press p 256 Hill Jane H 2001 Proto Uto Aztecan A Community of Cultivators in Central Mexico American Anthropologist 103 4 913 934 doi 10 1525 aa 2001 103 4 913 JSTOR 684121 Rodrigues Aryon Dall Igna Cabral Ana Suelly Arruda Camara 2012 Tupian In Campbell Lyle Grondona Veronica eds The Indigenous Languages of South America A Comprehensive Guide Walter de Gruyter pp 495 574 ISBN 978 3 11 025803 5 a b Porkhomovsky Victor 2020 Afro Asiatic Overview In Vossen Rainer Dimmendaal Gerrit J eds The Oxford Handbook of African Languages Oxford University Press pp 269 274 ISBN 978 0 19 960989 5 p 273 Kitchen Andrew Ehret Christopher Assefa Shiferaw Mulligan Connie J 7 August 2009 Bayesian phylogenetic analysis of Semitic languages identifies an Early Bronze Age origin of Semitic in the Near East Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 276 1668 2703 2710 doi 10 1098 rspb 2009 0408 PMC 2839953 PMID 19403539 Manning Katie Timpson Adrian October 2014 The demographic response to Holocene climate change in the Sahara Quaternary Science Reviews 101 28 35 Bibcode 2014QSRv 101 28M doi 10 1016 j quascirev 2014 07 003 Kopytoff Igor 1989 The African Frontier The Reproduction of Traditional African Societies Indiana University Press ISBN 978 0 253 20539 1 cited after Igbo Language Roots and Pre History Archived 2019 07 17 at the Wayback Machine A Mighty Tree 2011 Vydrin Valentin On the Problem of the Proto Mande Homeland PDF Journal of Language Relationship Fields Black Edda L 2015 Rice and Rice Farmers in the Upper Guinea Coast and Environmental History In Bray Francesca Coclanis Peter A Fields Black Edda L Schafer Dagmar eds Rice Global Networks and New Histories Cambridge University Press p 170 ISBN 9781107044395 Archived from the original on 2022 04 14 Dimmendaal Gerrit J 2020 Nilo Saharan and Its Limits In Rainer Vossen Gerrit J Dimmendaal eds The Oxford Handbook of African Languages Oxford Oxford University Press pp 364 382 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780199609895 013 15 ISBN 978 0 19 960989 5 Andah Bassey Okpoko Alex Shaw Thurstan Sinclair Paul 22 May 2014 The Archaeology of Africa Food Metals and Towns Routledge ISBN 9781134679492 Newman James L January 1995 The Peopling of Africa A Geographic Interpretation Yale University Press ISBN 0300072805 Bouckaert Remco R Bowern Claire Atkinson Quentin D April 2018 The origin and expansion of Pama Nyungan languages across Australia Nature Ecology amp Evolution 2 4 741 749 doi 10 1038 s41559 018 0489 3 Sources editAnthony David W 2007 The Horse the Wheel and Language How Bronze Age Riders from the Eurasian Steppes Shaped the Modern World Princeton University Press Anthony David Ringe Don 2015 The Indo European Homeland from Linguistic and Archaeological Perspectives Annual Review of Linguistics 1 199 219 doi 10 1146 annurev linguist 030514 124812 Bellwood Peter 2007 1997 Prehistory of the Indo Malaysian Archipelago Revised Edition ANU E Press ISBN 978 1 921313 12 7 Bellwood Peter S 2000 Presejarah Kepulauan Indo Malaysia Translation of Bellwood 2007 Blust Robert 1984 The Austronesian Homeland A Linguistic Perspective Asian Perspectives 26 1 45 67 JSTOR 42928105 Campbell Lyle 1997 American Indian Languages The Historical Linguistics of Native America Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 509427 5 2013 Historical Linguistics An Introduction 3rd ed Edinburgh University Press ISBN 978 0 7486 4601 2 Gray Russell D Atkinson Quentin D 2003 Language tree divergence times support the Anatolian theory of Indo European origin Nature 426 6965 435 439 Bibcode 2003Natur 426 435G doi 10 1038 nature02029 PMID 14647380 S2CID 42340 Haak Wolfgang Lazaridis Iosif Patterson Nick Rohland Nadin Mallick Swapan Llamas Bastien Brandt Guido Nordenfelt Susanne Harney Eadaoin Stewardson Kristin Fu Qiaomei Mittnik Alissa Banffy Eszter Economou Christos Francken Michael Friederich Susanne Pena Rafael Garrido Hallgren Fredrik Khartanovich Valery Khokhlov Aleksandr Kunst Michael Kuznetsov Pavel Meller Harald Mochalov Oleg Moiseyev Vayacheslav Nicklisch Nicole Pichler Sandra L Risch Roberto Rojo Guerra Manuel A Roth Christina Szecsenyi Nagy Anna Wahl Joachim Meyer Matthias Krause Johannes Brown Dorcas Anthony David Cooper Alan Alt Kurt Werner Reich David 11 June 2015 Massive migration from the steppe was a source for Indo European languages in Europe Nature 522 7555 207 211 arXiv 1502 02783 Bibcode 2015Natur 522 207H doi 10 1038 nature14317 PMC 5048219 PMID 25731166 Koerner E F K 2001 Linguistics and Ideology in 19th and 20th Century Studies of Language In Dirven Rene Hawkins Bruce Sandikcioglu Esra eds Language and Ideology Volume 1 theoretical cognitive approaches John Benjamins Publishing pp 253 276 ISBN 978 90 272 9954 3 Krishnamurti Bhadriraju 2003 The Dravidian Languages Cambridge University Press ISBN 0 521 77111 0 Mallory J P 1989 In Search of the Indo Europeans Language Archaeology and Myth London Thames amp Hudson Mallory James P 1997 The homelands of the Indo Europeans in Blench Roger Spriggs Matthew eds Archaeology and Language vol I Theoretical and Methodological Orientations London Routledge ISBN 978 0 415 11760 9 Mallory J P Adams D Q 2006 The Oxford introduction to Proto Indo European and the Proto Indo European world Repr ed Oxford u a Oxford Univ Press ISBN 978 0 19 928791 8 Pereltsvaig Asya Lewis Martin W 2015 Searching for Indo European origins The Indo European Controversy Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 1 107 05453 0 Sohn Ho Min 1999 The Korean Language Cambridge Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 36123 1 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Linguistic homeland amp oldid 1194454938, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.