fbpx
Wikipedia

Problem solving

Problem solving is the process of achieving a goal by overcoming obstacles, a frequent part of most activities. Problems in need of solutions range from simple personal tasks (e.g. how to turn on an appliance) to complex issues in business and technical fields. The former is an example of simple problem solving (SPS) addressing one issue, whereas the latter is complex problem solving (CPS) with multiple interrelated obstacles.[1] Another classification of problem-solving tasks is into well-defined problems with specific obstacles and goals, and ill-defined problems in which the current situation is troublesome but it is not clear what kind of resolution to aim for.[2] Similarly, one may distinguish formal or fact-based problems requiring psychometric intelligence, versus socio-emotional problems which depend on the changeable emotions of individuals or groups, such as tactful behavior, fashion, or gift choices.[3]

Solutions require sufficient resources and knowledge to attain the goal. Professionals such as lawyers, doctors, programmers, and consultants are largely problem solvers for issues that require technical skills and knowledge beyond general competence. Many businesses have found profitable markets by recognizing a problem and creating a solution: the more widespread and inconvenient the problem, the greater the opportunity to develop a scalable solution.

There are many specialized problem-solving techniques and methods in fields such as engineering, business, medicine, mathematics, computer science, philosophy, and social organization. The mental techniques to identify, analyze, and solve problems are studied in psychology and cognitive sciences. Also widely researched are the mental obstacles that prevent people from finding solutions; problem-solving impediments include confirmation bias, mental set, and functional fixedness.

Definition edit

The term problem solving has a slightly different meaning depending on the discipline. For instance, it is a mental process in psychology and a computerized process in computer science. There are two different types of problems: ill-defined and well-defined; different approaches are used for each. Well-defined problems have specific end goals and clearly expected solutions, while ill-defined problems do not. Well-defined problems allow for more initial planning than ill-defined problems.[2] Solving problems sometimes involves dealing with pragmatics (the way that context contributes to meaning) and semantics (the interpretation of the problem). The ability to understand what the end goal of the problem is, and what rules could be applied, represents the key to solving the problem. Sometimes a problem requires abstract thinking or coming up with a creative solution.

Problem solving has two major domains: mathematical problem solving and personal problem solving. Each concerns some difficulty or barrier that is encountered.[4]

Psychology edit

Problem solving in psychology refers to the process of finding solutions to problems encountered in life.[5] Solutions to these problems are usually situation- or context-specific. The process starts with problem finding and problem shaping, in which the problem is discovered and simplified. The next step is to generate possible solutions and evaluate them. Finally a solution is selected to be implemented and verified. Problems have an end goal to be reached; how you get there depends upon problem orientation (problem-solving coping style and skills) and systematic analysis.[6]

Mental health professionals study the human problem-solving processes using methods such as introspection, behaviorism, simulation, computer modeling, and experiment. Social psychologists look into the person-environment relationship aspect of the problem and independent and interdependent problem-solving methods.[7] Problem solving has been defined as a higher-order cognitive process and intellectual function that requires the modulation and control of more routine or fundamental skills.[8]

Empirical research shows many different strategies and factors influence everyday problem solving.[9] Rehabilitation psychologists studying people with frontal lobe injuries have found that deficits in emotional control and reasoning can be re-mediated with effective rehabilitation and could improve the capacity of injured persons to resolve everyday problems.[10] Interpersonal everyday problem solving is dependent upon personal motivational and contextual components. One such component is the emotional valence of "real-world" problems, which can either impede or aid problem-solving performance. Researchers have focused on the role of emotions in problem solving,[11] demonstrating that poor emotional control can disrupt focus on the target task, impede problem resolution, and lead to negative outcomes such as fatigue, depression, and inertia.[12] In conceptualization,[clarification needed]human problem solving consists of two related processes: problem orientation, and the motivational/attitudinal/affective approach to problematic situations and problem-solving skills. People's strategies cohere with their goals[13] and stem from the process of comparing oneself with others.

Cognitive sciences edit

Among the first experimental psychologists to study problem solving were the Gestaltists in Germany, such as Karl Duncker in The Psychology of Productive Thinking (1935).[14] Perhaps best known is the work of Allen Newell and Herbert A. Simon.[15]

Experiments in the 1960s and early 1970s asked participants to solve relatively simple, well-defined, but not previously seen laboratory tasks.[16][17] These simple problems, such as the Tower of Hanoi, admitted optimal solutions that could be found quickly, allowing researchers to observe the full problem-solving process. Researchers assumed that these model problems would elicit the characteristic cognitive processes by which more complex "real world" problems are solved.

An outstanding problem-solving technique found by this research is the principle of decomposition.[18]

Computer science edit

Much of computer science and artificial intelligence involves designing automated systems to solve a specified type of problem: to accept input data and calculate a correct or adequate response, reasonably quickly. Algorithms are recipes or instructions that direct such systems, written into computer programs.

Steps for designing such systems include problem determination, heuristics, root cause analysis, de-duplication, analysis, diagnosis, and repair. Analytic techniques include linear and nonlinear programming, queuing systems, and simulation.[19] A large, perennial obstacle is to find and fix errors in computer programs: debugging.

Logic edit

Formal logic concerns issues like validity, truth, inference, argumentation, and proof. In a problem-solving context, it can be used to formally represent a problem as a theorem to be proved, and to represent the knowledge needed to solve the problem as the premises to be used in a proof that the problem has a solution.

The use of computers to prove mathematical theorems using formal logic emerged as the field of automated theorem proving in the 1950s. It included the use of heuristic methods designed to simulate human problem solving, as in the Logic Theory Machine, developed by Allen Newell, Herbert A. Simon and J. C. Shaw, as well as algorithmic methods such as the resolution principle developed by John Alan Robinson.

In addition to its use for finding proofs of mathematical theorems, automated theorem-proving has also been used for program verification in computer science. In 1958, John McCarthy proposed the advice taker, to represent information in formal logic and to derive answers to questions using automated theorem-proving. An important step in this direction was made by Cordell Green in 1969, who used a resolution theorem prover for question-answering and for such other applications in artificial intelligence as robot planning.

The resolution theorem-prover used by Cordell Green bore little resemblance to human problem solving methods. In response to criticism of that approach from researchers at MIT, Robert Kowalski developed logic programming and SLD resolution,[20] which solves problems by problem decomposition. He has advocated logic for both computer and human problem solving[21] and computational logic to improve human thinking.[22]

Engineering edit

When products or processes fail, problem solving techniques can be used to develop corrective actions that can be taken to prevent further failures. Such techniques can also be applied to a product or process prior to an actual failure event—to predict, analyze, and mitigate a potential problem in advance. Techniques such as failure mode and effects analysis can proactively reduce the likelihood of problems.

In either the reactive or the proactive case, it is necessary to build a causal explanation through a process of diagnosis. In deriving an explanation of effects in terms of causes, abduction generates new ideas or hypotheses (asking "how?"); deduction evaluates and refines hypotheses based on other plausible premises (asking "why?"); and induction justifies a hypothesis with empirical data (asking "how much?").[23] The objective of abduction is to determine which hypothesis or proposition to test, not which one to adopt or assert.[24] In the Peircean logical system, the logic of abduction and deduction contribute to our conceptual understanding of a phenomenon, while the logic of induction adds quantitative details (empirical substantiation) to our conceptual knowledge.[25]

Forensic engineering is an important technique of failure analysis that involves tracing product defects and flaws. Corrective action can then be taken to prevent further failures.

Reverse engineering attempts to discover the original problem-solving logic used in developing a product by disassembling the product and developing a plausible pathway to creating and assembling its parts.[26]

Military science edit

In military science, problem solving is linked to the concept of "end-states", the conditions or situations which are the aims of the strategy.[27]: xiii, E-2  Ability to solve problems is important at any military rank, but is essential at the command and control level. It results from deep qualitative and quantitative understanding of possible scenarios. Effectiveness in this context is an evaluation of results: to what extent the end states were accomplished.[27]: IV-24  Planning is the process of determining how to effect those end states.[27]: IV-1 

Processes edit

Some models of problem solving involve identifying a goal and then a sequence of subgoals towards achieving this goal. Andersson, who introduced the ACT-R model of cognition, modelled this collection of goals and subgoals as a goal stack in which the mind contains a stack of goals and subgoals to be completed, and a single task being carried out at any time.[28]: 51 

Knowledge of how to solve one problem can be applied to another problem, in a process known as transfer.[28]: 56 

Problem-solving strategies edit

Problem-solving strategies are steps to overcoming the obstacles to achieving a goal. The iteration of such strategies over the course of solving a problem is the "problem-solving cycle".[29]

Common steps in this cycle include recognizing the problem, defining it, developing a strategy to fix it, organizing knowledge and resources available, monitoring progress, and evaluating the effectiveness of the solution. Once a solution is achieved, another problem usually arises, and the cycle starts again.

Insight is the sudden aha! solution to a problem, the birth of a new idea to simplify a complex situation. Solutions found through insight are often more incisive than those from step-by-step analysis. A quick solution process requires insight to select productive moves at different stages of the problem-solving cycle. Unlike Newell and Simon's formal definition of a move problem, there is no consensus definition of an insight problem.[30]

Some problem-solving strategies include:[31]

Abstraction
solving the problem in a tractable model system to gain insight into the real system
Analogy
adapting the solution to a previous problem which has similar features or mechanisms
Brainstorming
(especially among groups of people) suggesting a large number of solutions or ideas and combining and developing them until an optimum solution is found
Bypasses
transform the problem into another problem that is easier to solve, bypassing the barrier, then transform that solution back to a solution to the original problem.
Critical thinking
analysis of available evidence and arguments to form a judgement via rational, skeptical, and unbiased evaluation
Divide and conquer
breaking down a large, complex problem into smaller, solvable problems
Help-seeking
obtaining external assistance to deal with obstacles
Hypothesis testing
assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove (or, in some contexts, disprove) the assumption
Lateral thinking
approaching solutions indirectly and creatively
Means-ends analysis
choosing an action at each step to move closer to the goal
Morphological analysis
assessing the output and interactions of an entire system

Observation / Question edit

in the natural sciences is an act or instance of noticing or perceiving and the acquisition of information from a primary source. is an utterance which serves as a request for information.
Proof of impossibility
try to prove that the problem cannot be solved. The point where the proof fails will be the starting point for solving it
Reduction
transforming the problem into another problem for which solutions exist
Research
employing existing ideas or adapting existing solutions to similar problems
Root cause analysis
identifying the cause of a problem
Trial-and-error
testing possible solutions until the right one is found

Problem-solving methods edit

Common barriers edit

Common barriers to problem solving include mental constructs that impede an efficient search for solutions. Five of the most common identified by researchers are: confirmation bias, mental set, functional fixedness, unnecessary constraints, and irrelevant information.

Confirmation bias edit

Confirmation bias is an unintentional tendency to collect and use data which favors preconceived notions. Such notions may be incidental rather than motivated by important personal beliefs: the desire to be right may be sufficient motivation.[32]

Scientific and technical professionals also experience confirmation bias. One online experiment, for example, suggested that professionals within the field of psychological research are likely to view scientific studies that agree with their preconceived notions more favorably than clashing studies.[33] According to Raymond Nickerson, one can see the consequences of confirmation bias in real-life situations, which range in severity from inefficient government policies to genocide. Nickerson argued that those who killed people accused of witchcraft demonstrated confirmation bias with motivation.[citation needed] Researcher Michael Allen found evidence for confirmation bias with motivation in school children who worked to manipulate their science experiments to produce favorable results.[34]

However, confirmation bias does not necessarily require motivation. In 1960, Peter Cathcart Wason conducted an experiment in which participants first viewed three numbers and then created a hypothesis in the form of a rule that could have been used to create that triplet of numbers. When testing their hypotheses, participants tended to only create additional triplets of numbers that would confirm their hypotheses, and tended not to create triplets that would negate or disprove their hypotheses.[35]

Mental set edit

Mental set is the inclination to re-use a previously successful solution, rather than search for new and better solutions. It is a reliance on habit.

It was first articulated by Abraham S. Luchins in the 1940s with his well-known water jug experiments.[36] Participants were asked to fill one jug with a specific amount of water by using other jugs with different maximum capacities. After Luchins gave a set of jug problems that could all be solved by a single technique, he then introduced a problem that could be solved by the same technique, but also by a novel and simpler method. His participants tended to use the accustomed technique, oblivious of the simpler alternative.[37] This was again demonstrated in Norman Maier's 1931 experiment, which challenged participants to solve a problem by using a familiar tool (pliers) in an unconventional manner. Participants were often unable to view the object in a way that strayed from its typical use, a type of mental set known as functional fixedness (see the following section).

Rigidly clinging to a mental set is called fixation, which can deepen to an obsession or preoccupation with attempted strategies that are repeatedly unsuccessful.[38] In the late 1990s, researcher Jennifer Wiley found that professional expertise in a field can create a mental set, perhaps leading to fixation.[38]

Groupthink, in which each individual takes on the mindset of the rest of the group, can produce and exacerbate mental set.[39] Social pressure leads to everybody thinking the same thing and reaching the same conclusions.

Functional fixedness edit

Functional fixedness is the tendency to view an object as having only one function, and to be unable to conceive of any novel use, as in the Maier pliers experiment described above. Functional fixedness is a specific form of mental set, and is one of the most common forms of cognitive bias in daily life.

As an example, imagine a man wants to kill a bug in his house, but the only thing at hand is a can of air freshener. He may start searching for something to kill the bug instead of squashing it with the can, thinking only of its main function of deodorizing.

Tim German and Clark Barrett describe this barrier: "subjects become 'fixed' on the design function of the objects, and problem solving suffers relative to control conditions in which the object's function is not demonstrated."[40] Their research found that young children's limited knowledge of an object's intended function reduces this barrier[41] Research has also discovered functional fixedness in educational contexts, as an obstacle to understanding: "functional fixedness may be found in learning concepts as well as in solving chemistry problems."[42]

There are several hypotheses in regards to how functional fixedness relates to problem solving.[43] It may waste time, delaying or entirely preventing the correct use of a tool.

Unnecessary constraints edit

Unnecessary constraints are arbitrary boundaries imposed unconsciously on the task at hand, which foreclose a productive avenue of solution. The solver may become fixated on only one type of solution, as if it were an inevitable requirement of the problem. Typically, this combines with mental set—clinging to a previously successful method.[44][page needed]

Visual problems can also produce mentally invented constraints.[45][page needed] A famous example is the dot problem: nine dots arranged in a three-by-three grid pattern must be connected by drawing four straight line segments, without lifting pen from paper or backtracking along a line. The subject typically assumes the pen must stay within the outer square of dots, but the solution requires lines continuing beyond this frame, and researchers have found a 0% solution rate within a brief allotted time.[46]

This problem has produced the expression "think outside the box".[47][page needed] Such problems are typically solved via a sudden insight which leaps over the mental barriers, often after long toil against them.[48] This can be difficult depending on how the subject has structured the problem in their mind, how they draw on past experiences, and how well they juggle this information in their working memory. In the example, envisioning the dots connected outside the framing square requires visualizing an unconventional arrangement, which is a strain on working memory.[47]

Irrelevant information edit

Irrelevant information is a specification or data presented in a problem that is unrelated to the solution.[44] If the solver assumes that all information presented needs to be used, this often derails the problem solving process, making relatively simple problems much harder.[49]

For example: "Fifteen percent of the people in Topeka have unlisted telephone numbers. You select 200 names at random from the Topeka phone book. How many of these people have unlisted phone numbers?"[47][page needed] The "obvious" answer is 15%, but in fact none of the unlisted people would be listed among the 200. This kind of "trick question" is often used in aptitude tests or cognitive evaluations.[50] Though not inherently difficult, they require independent thinking that is not necessarily common. Mathematical word problems often include irrelevant qualitative or numerical information as an extra challenge.

Avoiding barriers by changing problem representation edit

The disruption caused by the above cognitive biases can depend on how the information is represented:[50] visually, verbally, or mathematically. A classic example is the Buddhist monk problem:

A Buddhist monk begins at dawn one day walking up a mountain, reaches the top at sunset, meditates at the top for several days until one dawn when he begins to walk back to the foot of the mountain, which he reaches at sunset. Making no assumptions about his starting or stopping or about his pace during the trips, prove that there is a place on the path which he occupies at the same hour of the day on the two separate journeys.

The problem cannot be addressed in a verbal context, trying to describe the monk's progress on each day. It becomes much easier when the paragraph is represented mathematically by a function: one visualizes a graph whose horizontal axis is time of day, and whose vertical axis shows the monk's position (or altitude) on the path at each time. Superimposing the two journey curves, which traverse opposite diagonals of a rectangle, one sees they must cross each other somewhere. The visual representation by graphing has resolved the difficulty.

Similar strategies can often improve problem solving on tests.[44][51]

Other barriers for individuals edit

People who are engaged in problem solving tend to overlook subtractive changes, even those that are critical elements of efficient solutions.[example needed] This tendency to solve by first, only, or mostly creating or adding elements, rather than by subtracting elements or processes is shown to intensify with higher cognitive loads such as information overload.[52]

Dreaming: problem solving without waking consciousness edit

People can also solve problems while they are asleep. There are many reports of scientists and engineers who solved problems in their dreams. For example, Elias Howe, inventor of the sewing machine, figured out the structure of the bobbin from a dream.[53]

The chemist August Kekulé was considering how benzene arranged its six carbon and hydrogen atoms. Thinking about the problem, he dozed off, and dreamt of dancing atoms that fell into a snakelike pattern, which led him to discover the benzene ring. As Kekulé wrote in his diary,

One of the snakes seized hold of its own tail, and the form whirled mockingly before my eyes. As if by a flash of lightning I awoke; and this time also I spent the rest of the night in working out the consequences of the hypothesis.[54]

There also are empirical studies of how people can think consciously about a problem before going to sleep, and then solve the problem with a dream image. Dream researcher William C. Dement told his undergraduate class of 500 students that he wanted them to think about an infinite series, whose first elements were OTTFF, to see if they could deduce the principle behind it and to say what the next elements of the series would be.[55][page needed] He asked them to think about this problem every night for 15 minutes before going to sleep and to write down any dreams that they then had. They were instructed to think about the problem again for 15 minutes when they awakened in the morning.

The sequence OTTFF is the first letters of the numbers: one, two, three, four, five. The next five elements of the series are SSENT (six, seven, eight, nine, ten). Some of the students solved the puzzle by reflecting on their dreams. One example was a student who reported the following dream:[55][page needed]

I was standing in an art gallery, looking at the paintings on the wall. As I walked down the hall, I began to count the paintings: one, two, three, four, five. As I came to the sixth and seventh, the paintings had been ripped from their frames. I stared at the empty frames with a peculiar feeling that some mystery was about to be solved. Suddenly I realized that the sixth and seventh spaces were the solution to the problem!

With more than 500 undergraduate students, 87 dreams were judged to be related to the problems students were assigned (53 directly related and 34 indirectly related). Yet of the people who had dreams that apparently solved the problem, only seven were actually able to consciously know the solution. The rest (46 out of 53) thought they did not know the solution.

Mark Blechner conducted this experiment and obtained results similar to Dement's.[56][page needed] He found that while trying to solve the problem, people had dreams in which the solution appeared to be obvious from the dream, but it was rare for the dreamers to realize how their dreams had solved the puzzle. Coaxing or hints did not get them to realize it, although once they heard the solution, they recognized how their dream had solved it. For example, one person in that OTTFF experiment dreamed:[56][page needed]

There is a big clock. You can see the movement. The big hand of the clock was on the number six. You could see it move up, number by number, six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven, twelve. The dream focused on the small parts of the machinery. You could see the gears inside.

In the dream, the person counted out the next elements of the series—six, seven, eight, nine, ten, eleven, twelve—yet he did not realize that this was the solution of the problem. His sleeping mindbrain[jargon] solved the problem, but his waking mindbrain was not aware how.

Albert Einstein believed that much problem solving goes on unconsciously, and the person must then figure out and formulate consciously what the mindbrain[jargon] has already solved. He believed this was his process in formulating the theory of relativity: "The creator of the problem possesses the solution."[57] Einstein said that he did his problem solving without words, mostly in images. "The words or the language, as they are written or spoken, do not seem to play any role in my mechanism of thought. The psychical entities which seem to serve as elements in thought are certain signs and more or less clear images which can be 'voluntarily' reproduced and combined."[58]

Cognitive sciences: two schools edit

Problem-solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise.[59] For this reason, cognitive sciences findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to problem-solving situations outside the laboratory. This has led to a research emphasis on real-world problem solving, since the 1990s. This emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North America and Europe, however. Whereas North American research has typically concentrated on studying problem solving in separate, natural knowledge domains, much of the European research has focused on novel, complex problems, and has been performed with computerized scenarios.[60]

Europe edit

In Europe, two main approaches have surfaced, one initiated by Donald Broadbent[61] in the United Kingdom and the other one by Dietrich Dörner[62] in Germany. The two approaches share an emphasis on relatively complex, semantically rich, computerized laboratory tasks, constructed to resemble real-life problems. The approaches differ somewhat in their theoretical goals and methodology. The tradition initiated by Broadbent emphasizes the distinction between cognitive problem-solving processes that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness, and typically employs mathematically well-defined computerized systems. The tradition initiated by Dörner, on the other hand, has an interest in the interplay of the cognitive, motivational, and social components of problem solving, and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain up to 2,000 highly interconnected variables.[63]

North America edit

In North America, initiated by the work of Herbert A. Simon on "learning by doing" in semantically rich domains,[64] researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural knowledge domains—such as physics, writing, or chess playing—rather than attempt to extract a global theory of problem solving.[65] These researchers have focused on the development of problem solving within certain domains, that is on the development of expertise.[66]

Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include:

  • calculation[67]
  • computer skills[68]
  • game playing[69]
  • lawyers' reasoning[70]
  • managerial problem solving[71]
  • mathematical problem solving[72]
  • mechanical problem solving[73]
  • personal problem solving[74]
  • political decision making[75]
  • problem solving in electronics[76]
  • problem solving for innovations and inventions: TRIZ[77]
  • reading[78]
  • social problem solving[11]
  • writing[79]

Characteristics of complex problems edit

Complex problem solving (CPS) is distinguishable from simple problem solving (SPS). In SPS there is a singular and simple obstacle. In CPS there may be multiple simultaneous obstacles. For example, a surgeon at work has far more complex problems than an individual deciding what shoes to wear. As elucidated by Dietrich Dörner, and later expanded upon by Joachim Funke, complex problems have some typical characteristics, which include:[1]

Collective problem solving edit

People solve problems on many different levels—from the individual to the civilizational. Collective problem solving refers to problem solving performed collectively. Social issues and global issues can typically only be solved collectively.

The complexity of contemporary problems exceeds the cognitive capacity of any individual and requires different but complementary varieties of expertise and collective problem solving ability.[81]

Collective intelligence is shared or group intelligence that emerges from the collaboration, collective efforts, and competition of many individuals.

In collaborative problem solving people work together to solve real-world problems. Members of problem-solving groups share a common concern, a similar passion, and/or a commitment to their work. Members can ask questions, wonder, and try to understand common issues. They share expertise, experiences, tools, and methods.[82] Groups may be fluid based on need, may only occur temporarily to finish an assigned task, or may be more permanent depending on the nature of the problems.

For example, in the educational context, members of a group may all have input into the decision-making process and a role in the learning process. Members may be responsible for the thinking, teaching, and monitoring of all members in the group. Group work may be coordinated among members so that each member makes an equal contribution to the whole work. Members can identify and build on their individual strengths so that everyone can make a significant contribution to the task.[83] Collaborative group work has the ability to promote critical thinking skills, problem solving skills, social skills, and self-esteem. By using collaboration and communication, members often learn from one another and construct meaningful knowledge that often leads to better learning outcomes than individual work.[84]

Collaborative groups require joint intellectual efforts between the members and involve social interactions to solve problems together. The knowledge shared during these interactions is acquired during communication, negotiation, and production of materials.[85] Members actively seek information from others by asking questions. The capacity to use questions to acquire new information increases understanding and the ability to solve problems.[86]

In a 1962 research report, Douglas Engelbart linked collective intelligence to organizational effectiveness, and predicted that proactively "augmenting human intellect" would yield a multiplier effect in group problem solving: "Three people working together in this augmented mode [would] seem to be more than three times as effective in solving a complex problem as is one augmented person working alone".[87]

Henry Jenkins, a theorist of new media and media convergence, draws on the theory that collective intelligence can be attributed to media convergence and participatory culture.[88] He criticizes contemporary education for failing to incorporate online trends of collective problem solving into the classroom, stating "whereas a collective intelligence community encourages ownership of work as a group, schools grade individuals". Jenkins argues that interaction within a knowledge community builds vital skills for young people, and teamwork through collective intelligence communities contributes to the development of such skills.[89]

Collective impact is the commitment of a group of actors from different sectors to a common agenda for solving a specific social problem, using a structured form of collaboration.

After World War II the UN, the Bretton Woods organization, and the WTO were created. Collective problem solving on the international level crystallized around these three types of organization from the 1980s onward. As these global institutions remain state-like or state-centric it is unsurprising that they perpetuate state-like or state-centric approaches to collective problem solving rather than alternative ones.[90]

Crowdsourcing is a process of accumulating ideas, thoughts, or information from many independent participants, with aim of finding the best solution for a given challenge. Modern information technologies allow for many people to be involved and facilitate managing their suggestions in ways that provide good results.[91] The Internet allows for a new capacity of collective (including planetary-scale) problem solving.[92]

See also edit

  • Actuarial science – Statistics applied to risk in insurance and other financial products
  • Analytical skill – Crucial skill in all different fields of work and life
  • Creative problem-solving – mental process of searching for an original and previously unknown solution to a problem
  • Collective intelligence – Group intelligence that emerges from collective efforts
  • Community of practice
  • Coworking – Practice of independent contractors or scientists sharing office space without supervision
  • Crowdsolving – Sourcing services or funds from a group
  • Divergent thinking – A process of generating creative ideas
  • Grey problem – IT service problem where the causing technology is unknown or unconfirmed, making the problem solving difficult to allocate
  • Innovation – Practical implementation of improvements
  • Instrumentalism – Position in the philosophy of science
  • Problem statement – Description of an issue
  • Problem structuring methods
  • Shared intentionality – Description of the concept of shared intentionality
  • Structural fix – solving a problem or resolving a conflict by bringing about structural changes in underlying structures that provoked or sustained these problems
  • Subgoal labeling
  • Troubleshooting – Form of problem solving, often applied to repair failed products or processes
  • Wicked problem – Problem that is difficult or impossible to solve

Notes edit

  1. ^ a b Frensch, Peter A.; Funke, Joachim, eds. (2014-04-04). Complex Problem Solving. doi:10.4324/9781315806723. ISBN 978-1-315-80672-3.
  2. ^ a b Schacter, D.L.; Gilbert, D.T.; Wegner, D.M. (2011). Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. p. 376.
  3. ^ Blanchard-Fields, F. (2007). "Everyday problem solving and emotion: An adult developmental perspective". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (1): 26–31. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00469.x. S2CID 145645352.
  4. ^ Zimmermann, Bernd (2004). On mathematical problem-solving processes and history of mathematics. ICME 10. Copenhagen.
  5. ^ Granvold, Donald K. (1997). "Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy with Adults". In Brandell, Jerrold R. (ed.). Theory and Practice in Clinical Social Work. Simon and Schuster. pp. 189. ISBN 978-0-684-82765-0.
  6. ^ Robertson, S. Ian (2001). "Introduction to the study of problem solving". Problem Solving. Psychology Press. ISBN 0-415-20300-7.
  7. ^ Rubin, M.; Watt, S. E.; Ramelli, M. (2012). "Immigrants' social integration as a function of approach-avoidance orientation and problem-solving style". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 36 (4): 498–505. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.12.009. hdl:1959.13/931119.
  8. ^ Goldstein F. C.; Levin H. S. (1987). "Disorders of reasoning and problem-solving ability". In M. Meier; A. Benton; L. Diller (eds.). Neuropsychological rehabilitation. London: Taylor & Francis Group.
  9. ^
    • Vallacher, Robin; M. Wegner, Daniel (2012). "Action Identification Theory". Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology. pp. 327–348. doi:10.4135/9781446249215.n17. ISBN 978-0-85702-960-7.
    • Margrett, J. A; Marsiske, M (2002). "Gender differences in older adults' everyday cognitive collaboration". International Journal of Behavioral Development. 26 (1): 45–59. doi:10.1080/01650250143000319. PMC 2909137. PMID 20657668.
    • Antonucci, T. C; Ajrouch, K. J; Birditt, K. S (2013). "The Convoy Model: Explaining Social Relations From a Multidisciplinary Perspective". The Gerontologist. 54 (1): 82–92. doi:10.1093/geront/gnt118. PMC 3894851. PMID 24142914.
  10. ^ Rath, Joseph F.; Simon, Dvorah; Langenbahn, Donna M.; Sherr, Rose Lynn; Diller, Leonard (2003). "Group treatment of problem-solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury: A randomised outcome study". Neuropsychological Rehabilitation. 13 (4): 461–488. doi:10.1080/09602010343000039. S2CID 143165070.
  11. ^ a b
    • D'Zurilla, T. J.; Goldfried, M. R. (1971). "Problem solving and behavior modification". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 78 (1): 107–126. doi:10.1037/h0031360. PMID 4938262.
    • D'Zurilla, T. J.; Nezu, A. M. (1982). "Social problem solving in adults". In P. C. Kendall (ed.). Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy. Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press. pp. 201–274.
  12. ^ Rath, J. F.; Langenbahn, D. M.; Simon, D; Sherr, R. L.; Fletcher, J.; Diller, L. (2004). "The construct of problem solving in higher level neuropsychological assessment and rehabilitation*1". Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 19 (5): 613–635. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2003.08.006. PMID 15271407.
  13. ^ Hoppmann, Christiane A.; Blanchard-Fields, Fredda (2010). "Goals and everyday problem solving: Manipulating goal preferences in young and older adults". Developmental Psychology. 46 (6): 1433–1443. doi:10.1037/a0020676. PMID 20873926.
  14. ^ Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.
  15. ^ Newell, Allen; Simon, Herbert A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.
  16. ^ For example:
    • X-ray problem, by Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.
    • Disk problem, later known as Tower of Hanoi, by Ewert, P. H.; Lambert, J. F. (1932). "Part II: The Effect of Verbal Instructions upon the Formation of a Concept". The Journal of General Psychology. 6 (2). Informa UK Limited: 400–413. doi:10.1080/00221309.1932.9711880. ISSN 0022-1309.
  17. ^ Mayer, R. E. (1992). Thinking, problem solving, cognition (Second ed.). New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
  18. ^ Armstrong, J. Scott; Denniston, William B. Jr.; Gordon, Matt M. (1975). (PDF). Organizational Behavior and Human Performance. 14 (2): 257–263. doi:10.1016/0030-5073(75)90028-8. S2CID 122659209. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2010-06-20.
  19. ^ Malakooti, Behnam (2013). Operations and Production Systems with Multiple Objectives. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-118-58537-5.
  20. ^ Kowalski, Robert (1974). "Predicate Logic as a Programming Language" (PDF). Information Processing. 74.
  21. ^ Kowalski, Robert (1979). Logic for Problem Solving (PDF). Artificial Intelligence Series. Vol. 7. Elsevier Science Publishing. ISBN 0-444-00368-1.
  22. ^ Kowalski, Robert (2011). Computational Logic and Human Thinking: How to be Artificially Intelligent (PDF). Cambridge University Press.
  23. ^ Staat, Wim (1993). "On abduction, deduction, induction and the categories". Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society. 29 (2): 225–237.
  24. ^ Sullivan, Patrick F. (1991). "On Falsificationist Interpretations of Peirce". Transactions of the Charles S. Peirce Society. 27 (2): 197–219.
  25. ^ Ho, Yu Chong (1994). Abduction? Deduction? Induction? Is There a Logic of Exploratory Data Analysis? (PDF). Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association. New Orleans, La.
  26. ^ . Litemind. 2008-11-04. Archived from the original on 2017-06-21. Retrieved 2017-06-11.
  27. ^ a b c (PDF). United States Joint Forces Command, Joint Warfighting Center, Suffolk, Va. 27 October 2009. Archived from the original (PDF) on April 29, 2011. Retrieved 10 October 2016.
  28. ^ a b Robertson, S. Ian (2017). Problem solving: perspectives from cognition and neuroscience (2nd ed.). London: Taylor & Francis. ISBN 978-1-317-49601-4. OCLC 962750529.
  29. ^ Bransford, J. D.; Stein, B. S (1993). The ideal problem solver: A guide for improving thinking, learning, and creativity (2nd ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman.
  30. ^
    • Ash, Ivan K.; Jee, Benjamin D.; Wiley, Jennifer (2012). "Investigating Insight as Sudden Learning". The Journal of Problem Solving. 4 (2). doi:10.7771/1932-6246.1123. ISSN 1932-6246.
    • Chronicle, Edward P.; MacGregor, James N.; Ormerod, Thomas C. (2004). "What Makes an Insight Problem? The Roles of Heuristics, Goal Conception, and Solution Recoding in Knowledge-Lean Problems" (PDF). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 30 (1): 14–27. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.30.1.14. ISSN 1939-1285. PMID 14736293. S2CID 15631498.
    • Chu, Yun; MacGregor, James N. (2011). "Human Performance on Insight Problem Solving: A Review". The Journal of Problem Solving. 3 (2). doi:10.7771/1932-6246.1094. ISSN 1932-6246.
  31. ^ Wang, Y.; Chiew, V. (2010). "On the cognitive process of human problem solving" (PDF). Cognitive Systems Research. 11 (1). Elsevier BV: 81–92. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2008.08.003. ISSN 1389-0417. S2CID 16238486.
  32. ^ Nickerson, Raymond S. (1998). "Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises". Review of General Psychology. 2 (2): 176. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175. S2CID 8508954.
  33. ^ Hergovich, Andreas; Schott, Reinhard; Burger, Christoph (2010). "Biased Evaluation of Abstracts Depending on Topic and Conclusion: Further Evidence of a Confirmation Bias Within Scientific Psychology". Current Psychology. 29 (3). Springer Science and Business Media LLC: 188–209. doi:10.1007/s12144-010-9087-5. ISSN 1046-1310. S2CID 145497196.
  34. ^ Allen, Michael (2011). "Theory-led confirmation bias and experimental persona". Research in Science & Technological Education. 29 (1). Informa UK Limited: 107–127. Bibcode:2011RSTEd..29..107A. doi:10.1080/02635143.2010.539973. ISSN 0263-5143. S2CID 145706148.
  35. ^ Wason, P. C. (1960). "On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 12 (3): 129–140. doi:10.1080/17470216008416717. S2CID 19237642.
  36. ^ Luchins, Abraham S. (1942). "Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of Einstellung". Psychological Monographs. 54 (248): i-95. doi:10.1037/h0093502.
  37. ^ Öllinger, Michael; Jones, Gary; Knoblich, Günther (2008). "Investigating the Effect of Mental Set on Insight Problem Solving" (PDF). Experimental Psychology. 55 (4). Hogrefe Publishing Group: 269–282. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.55.4.269. ISSN 1618-3169. PMID 18683624.
  38. ^ a b Wiley, Jennifer (1998). "Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving". Memory & Cognition. 24 (4): 716–730. doi:10.3758/bf03211392. PMID 9701964.
  39. ^ Cottam, Martha L.; Dietz-Uhler, Beth; Mastors, Elena; Preston, Thomas (2010). Introduction to Political Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology Press.
  40. ^ German, Tim P.; Barrett, H. Clark (2005). "Functional Fixedness in a Technologically Sparse Culture". Psychological Science. 16 (1). SAGE Publications: 1–5. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00771.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 15660843. S2CID 1833823.
  41. ^ German, Tim P.; Defeyter, Margaret A. (2000). "Immunity to functional fixedness in young children". Psychonomic Bulletin and Review. 7 (4): 707–712. doi:10.3758/BF03213010. PMID 11206213.
  42. ^ Furio, C.; Calatayud, M. L.; Baracenas, S.; Padilla, O. (2000). "Functional fixedness and functional reduction as common sense reasonings in chemical equilibrium and in geometry and polarity of molecules". Science Education. 84 (5): 545–565. doi:10.1002/1098-237X(200009)84:5<545::AID-SCE1>3.0.CO;2-1.
  43. ^ Adamson, Robert E (1952). "Functional fixedness as related to problem solving: A repetition of three experiments". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 44 (4): 288–291. doi:10.1037/h0062487. PMID 13000071.
  44. ^ a b c Kellogg, R. T. (2003). Cognitive psychology (2nd ed.). California: Sage Publications, Inc.
  45. ^ Meloy, J. R. (1998). The Psychology of Stalking, Clinical and Forensic Perspectives (2nd ed.). London, England: Academic Press.
  46. ^ MacGregor, J.N.; Ormerod, T.C.; Chronicle, E.P. (2001). "Information-processing and insight: A process model of performance on the nine-dot and related problems". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 27 (1): 176–201. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.27.1.176. PMID 11204097.
  47. ^ a b c Weiten, Wayne (2011). Psychology: themes and variations (8th ed.). California: Wadsworth.
  48. ^ Novick, L. R.; Bassok, M. (2005). "Problem solving". In Holyoak, K. J.; Morrison, R. G. (eds.). Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning. New York, N.Y.: Cambridge University Press. pp. 321–349.
  49. ^ Walinga, Jennifer (2010). "From walls to windows: Using barriers as pathways to insightful solutions". The Journal of Creative Behavior. 44 (3): 143–167. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2010.tb01331.x.
  50. ^ a b Walinga, Jennifer; Cunningham, J. Barton; MacGregor, James N. (2011). "Training insight problem solving through focus on barriers and assumptions". The Journal of Creative Behavior. 45: 47–58. doi:10.1002/j.2162-6057.2011.tb01084.x.
  51. ^ Vlamings, Petra H. J. M.; Hare, Brian; Call, Joseph (2009). "Reaching around barriers: The performance of great apes and 3–5-year-old children". Animal Cognition. 13 (2): 273–285. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0265-5. PMC 2822225. PMID 19653018.
  52. ^
    • Gupta, Sujata (7 April 2021). "People add by default even when subtraction makes more sense". Science News. Retrieved 10 May 2021.
    • Adams, Gabrielle S.; Converse, Benjamin A.; Hales, Andrew H.; Klotz, Leidy E. (April 2021). "People systematically overlook subtractive changes". Nature. 592 (7853): 258–261. Bibcode:2021Natur.592..258A. doi:10.1038/s41586-021-03380-y. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 33828317. S2CID 233185662. Retrieved 10 May 2021.
  53. ^ Kaempffert, Waldemar B. (1924). A Popular History of American Invention. Vol. 2. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons. p. 385.
  54. ^
    • Kekulé, August (1890). "Benzolfest-Rede". Berichte der Deutschen Chemischen Gesellschaft. 23: 1302–1311.
    • Benfey, O. (1958). "Kekulé and the birth of the structural theory of organic chemistry in 1858". Journal of Chemical Education. 35 (1): 21–23. Bibcode:1958JChEd..35...21B. doi:10.1021/ed035p21.
  55. ^ a b Dement, W.C. (1972). Some Must Watch While Some Just Sleep. New York: Freeman.
  56. ^ a b Blechner, Mark J. (2018). The Mindbrain and Dreams: An Exploration of Dreaming, Thinking, and Artistic Creation. New York: Routledge.
  57. ^ Fromm, Erika O. (1998). "Lost and found half a century later: Letters by Freud and Einstein". American Psychologist. 53 (11): 1195–1198. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.53.11.1195.
  58. ^ Einstein, Albert (1954). "A Mathematician's Mind". Ideas and Opinions. New York: Bonanza Books. p. 25.
  59. ^ Sternberg, R. J. (1995). "Conceptions of expertise in complex problem solving: A comparison of alternative conceptions". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 295–321.
  60. ^ Funke, J. (1991). "Solving complex problems: Human identification and control of complex systems". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 185–222. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  61. ^
    • Broadbent, Donald E. (1977). "Levels, hierarchies, and the locus of control". Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 29 (2): 181–201. doi:10.1080/14640747708400596. S2CID 144328372.
    • Berry, Dianne C.; Broadbent, Donald E. (1995). "Implicit learning in the control of complex systems: A reconsideration of some of the earlier claims". In Frensch, P.A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 131–150.
  62. ^
    • Dörner, Dietrich (1975). "Wie Menschen eine Welt verbessern wollten" [How people wanted to improve the world]. Bild der Wissenschaft (in German). 12: 48–53.
    • Dörner, Dietrich (1985). "Verhalten, Denken und Emotionen" [Behavior, thinking, and emotions]. In Eckensberger, L. H.; Lantermann, E. D. (eds.). Emotion und Reflexivität (in German). München, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg. pp. 157–181.
    • Dörner, Dietrich; Wearing, Alex J. (1995). "Complex problem solving: Toward a (computer-simulated) theory". In Frensch, P.A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 65–99.
  63. ^
    • Buchner, A. (1995). "Theories of complex problem solving". In Frensch, P.A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 27–63.
    • Dörner, D.; Kreuzig, H. W.; Reither, F.; Stäudel, T., eds. (1983). Lohhausen. Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexität [Lohhausen. On dealing with uncertainty and complexity] (in German). Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
    • Ringelband, O. J.; Misiak, C.; Kluwe, R. H. (1990). "Mental models and strategies in the control of a complex system". In Ackermann, D.; Tauber, M. J. (eds.). Mental models and human-computer interaction. Vol. 1. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. pp. 151–164.
  64. ^
    • Anzai, K.; Simon, H. A. (1979). "The theory of learning by doing". Psychological Review. 86 (2): 124–140. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.86.2.124. PMID 493441.
    • Bhaskar, R.; Simon, Herbert A. (1977). "Problem Solving in Semantically Rich Domains: An Example from Engineering Thermodynamics". Cognitive Science. 1 (2). Wiley: 193–215. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0102_3. ISSN 0364-0213.
  65. ^ e.g., Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A., eds. (1991). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  66. ^
    • Chase, W. G.; Simon, H. A. (1973). "Perception in chess". Cognitive Psychology. 4: 55–81. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(73)90004-2.
    • Chi, M. T. H.; Feltovich, P. J.; Glaser, R. (1981). "Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices". Cognitive Science. 5 (2): 121–152. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0502_2.
    • Anderson, J. R.; Boyle, C. B.; Reiser, B. J. (1985). "Intelligent tutoring systems". Science. 228 (4698): 456–462. Bibcode:1985Sci...228..456A. doi:10.1126/science.228.4698.456. PMID 17746875. S2CID 62403455.
  67. ^ Sokol, S. M.; McCloskey, M. (1991). "Cognitive mechanisms in calculation". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 85–116. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  68. ^ Kay, D. S. (1991). "Computer interaction: Debugging the problems". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 317–340. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  69. ^ Frensch, P. A.; Sternberg, R. J. (1991). "Skill-related differences in game playing". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J .: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 343–381. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  70. ^ Amsel, E.; Langer, R.; Loutzenhiser, L. (1991). "Do lawyers reason differently from psychologists? A comparative design for studying expertise". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 223–250. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  71. ^ Wagner, R. K. (1991). "Managerial problem solving". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 159–183. PsycNET: 1991-98396-005.
  72. ^
    • Pólya, George (1945). How to Solve It. Princeton University Press.
    • Schoenfeld, A. H. (1985). Mathematical Problem Solving. Orlando, Fla.: Academic Press. ISBN 978-1-4832-9548-0.
  73. ^ Hegarty, M. (1991). "Knowledge and processes in mechanical problem solving". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 253–285. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  74. ^ Heppner, P. P.; Krauskopf, C. J. (1987). "An information-processing approach to personal problem solving". The Counseling Psychologist. 15 (3): 371–447. doi:10.1177/0011000087153001. S2CID 146180007.
  75. ^ Voss, J. F.; Wolfe, C. R.; Lawrence, J. A.; Engle, R. A. (1991). "From representation to decision: An analysis of problem solving in international relations". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 119–158. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443. PsycNET: 1991-98396-004.
  76. ^ Lesgold, A.; Lajoie, S. (1991). "Complex problem solving in electronics". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 287–316. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  77. ^ Altshuller, Genrich (1994). And Suddenly the Inventor Appeared. Translated by Lev Shulyak. Worcester, Mass.: Technical Innovation Center. ISBN 978-0-9640740-1-9.
  78. ^ Stanovich, K. E.; Cunningham, A. E. (1991). "Reading as constrained reasoning". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 3–60. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  79. ^ Bryson, M.; Bereiter, C.; Scardamalia, M.; Joram, E. (1991). "Going beyond the problem as given: Problem solving in expert and novice writers". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 61–84. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  80. ^ Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A., eds. (1991). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-8058-0650-4. OCLC 23254443.
  81. ^ Hung, Woei (2013). "Team-based complex problem solving: a collective cognition perspective". Educational Technology Research and Development. 61 (3): 365–384. doi:10.1007/s11423-013-9296-3. S2CID 62663840.
  82. ^ Jewett, Pamela; MacPhee, Deborah (2012). "Adding Collaborative Peer Coaching to Our Teaching Identities". The Reading Teacher. 66 (2): 105–110. doi:10.1002/TRTR.01089.
  83. ^ Wang, Qiyun (2009). "Design and Evaluation of a Collaborative Learning Environment". Computers and Education. 53 (4): 1138–1146. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2009.05.023.
  84. ^ Wang, Qiyan (2010). "Using online shared workspaces to support group collaborative learning". Computers and Education. 55 (3): 1270–1276. doi:10.1016/j.compedu.2010.05.023.
  85. ^ Kai-Wai Chu, Samuel; Kennedy, David M. (2011). "Using Online Collaborative tools for groups to Co-Construct Knowledge". Online Information Review. 35 (4): 581–597. doi:10.1108/14684521111161945. ISSN 1468-4527. S2CID 206388086.
  86. ^ Legare, Cristine; Mills, Candice; Souza, Andre; Plummer, Leigh; Yasskin, Rebecca (2013). "The use of questions as problem-solving strategies during early childhood". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 114 (1): 63–7. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.002. PMID 23044374.
  87. ^ Engelbart, Douglas (1962). "Team Cooperation". Augmenting Human Intellect: A Conceptual Framework. Vol. AFOSR-3223. Stanford Research Institute.
  88. ^ Flew, Terry (2008). New Media: an introduction. Melbourne: Oxford University Press.
  89. ^ Henry, Jenkins. (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on April 26, 2018. Retrieved December 11, 2016.
  90. ^ Finger, Matthias (2008-03-27). "Which governance for sustainable development? An organizational and institutional perspective". In Park, Jacob; Conca, Ken; Finger, Matthias (eds.). The Crisis of Global Environmental Governance: Towards a New Political Economy of Sustainability. Routledge. p. 48. ISBN 978-1-134-05982-9.
  91. ^
    • Guazzini, Andrea; Vilone, Daniele; Donati, Camillo; Nardi, Annalisa; Levnajić, Zoran (10 November 2015). "Modeling crowdsourcing as collective problem solving". Scientific Reports. 5: 16557. arXiv:1506.09155. Bibcode:2015NatSR...516557G. doi:10.1038/srep16557. PMC 4639727. PMID 26552943.
    • Boroomand, A.; Smaldino, P.E. (2021). "Hard Work, Risk-Taking, and Diversity in a Model of Collective Problem Solving". Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation. 24 (4). doi:10.18564/jasss.4704. S2CID 240483312.
  92. ^ Stefanovitch, Nicolas; Alshamsi, Aamena; Cebrian, Manuel; Rahwan, Iyad (30 September 2014). "Error and attack tolerance of collective problem solving: The DARPA Shredder Challenge". EPJ Data Science. 3 (1). doi:10.1140/epjds/s13688-014-0013-1. hdl:21.11116/0000-0002-D39F-D.

Further reading edit

  • Beckmann, Jens F.; Guthke, Jürgen (1995). "Complex problem solving, intelligence, and learning ability". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 177–200.
  • Brehmer, Berndt (1995). "Feedback delays in dynamic decision making". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 103–130.
  • Brehmer, Berndt; Dörner, D. (1993). "Experiments with computer-simulated microworlds: Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study". Computers in Human Behavior. 9 (2–3): 171–184. doi:10.1016/0747-5632(93)90005-D.
  • Dörner, D. (1992). "Über die Philosophie der Verwendung von Mikrowelten oder 'Computerszenarios' in der psychologischen Forschung" [On the proper use of microworlds or "computer scenarios" in psychological research]. In Gundlach, H. (ed.). Psychologische Forschung und Methode: Das Versprechen des Experiments. Festschrift für Werner Traxel (in German). Passau, Germany: Passavia-Universitäts-Verlag. pp. 53–87.
  • Eyferth, K.; Schömann, M.; Widowski, D. (1986). "Der Umgang von Psychologen mit Komplexität" [On how psychologists deal with complexity]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 5: 11–26.
  • Funke, Joachim (1993). "Microworlds based on linear equation systems: A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results" (PDF). In Strube, G.; Wender, K.-F. (eds.). The cognitive psychology of knowledge. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. pp. 313–330.
  • Funke, Joachim (1995). "Experimental research on complex problem solving" (PDF). In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 243–268.
  • Funke, U. (1995). "Complex problem solving in personnel selection and training". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 219–240.
  • Groner, M.; Groner, R.; Bischof, W. F. (1983). "Approaches to heuristics: A historical review". In Groner, R.; Groner, M.; Bischof, W. F. (eds.). Methods of heuristics. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 1–18.
  • Hayes, J. (1980). The complete problem solver. Philadelphia: The Franklin Institute Press.
  • Huber, O. (1995). "Complex problem solving as multistage decision making". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 151–173.
  • Hübner, Ronald (1989). "Methoden zur Analyse und Konstruktion von Aufgaben zur kognitiven Steuerung dynamischer Systeme" [Methods for the analysis and construction of dynamic system control tasks] (PDF). Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie (in German). 36: 221–238.
  • Hunt, Earl (1991). "Some comments on the study of complexity". In Sternberg, R. J.; Frensch, P. A. (eds.). Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 383–395. ISBN 978-1-317-78386-2.
  • Hussy, W. (1985). "Komplexes Problemlösen—Eine Sackgasse?" [Complex problem solving—a dead end?]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie (in German). 32: 55–77.
  • Kluwe, R. H. (1993). "Chapter 19 Knowledge and Performance in Complex Problem Solving". The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge. Advances in Psychology. Vol. 101. pp. 401–423. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62668-0. ISBN 978-0-444-89942-2.
  • Kluwe, R. H. (1995). "Single case studies and models of complex problem solving". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 269–291.
  • Kolb, S.; Petzing, F.; Stumpf, S. (1992). "Komplexes Problemlösen: Bestimmung der Problemlösegüte von Probanden mittels Verfahren des Operations Research—ein interdisziplinärer Ansatz" [Complex problem solving: determining the quality of human problem solving by operations research tools—an interdisciplinary approach]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 11: 115–128.
  • Krems, Josef F. (1995). "Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving". In Frensch, P. A.; Funke, J. (eds.). Complex problem solving: The European Perspective. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 201–218.
  • Melzak, Z. (1983). Bypasses: A Simple Approach to Complexity. London, UK: Wiley.
  • Müller, H. (1993). Komplexes Problemlösen: Reliabilität und Wissen [Complex problem solving: Reliability and knowledge] (in German). Bonn, Germany: Holos.
  • Paradies, M.W.; Unger, L. W. (2000). TapRooT—The System for Root Cause Analysis, Problem Investigation, and Proactive Improvement. Knoxville, Tenn.: System Improvements.
  • Putz-Osterloh, Wiebke (1993). "Chapter 15 Strategies for Knowledge Acquisition and Transfer of Knowledge in Dynamic Tasks". The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge. Advances in Psychology. Vol. 101. pp. 331–350. doi:10.1016/S0166-4115(08)62664-3. ISBN 978-0-444-89942-2.
  • Riefer, David M.; Batchelder, William H. (1988). (PDF). Psychological Review. 95 (3): 318–339. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.95.3.318. S2CID 14994393. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-11-25.
  • Schaub, H. (1993). Modellierung der Handlungsorganisation (in German). Bern, Switzerland: Hans Huber.
  • Strauß, B. (1993). Konfundierungen beim Komplexen Problemlösen. Zum Einfluß des Anteils der richtigen Lösungen (ArL) auf das Problemlöseverhalten in komplexen Situationen [Confoundations in complex problem solving. On the influence of the degree of correct solutions on problem solving in complex situations] (in German). Bonn, Germany: Holos.
  • Strohschneider, S. (1991). "Kein System von Systemen! Kommentar zu dem Aufsatz 'Systemmerkmale als Determinanten des Umgangs mit dynamischen Systemen' von Joachim Funke" [No system of systems! Reply to the paper 'System features as determinants of behavior in dynamic task environments' by Joachim Funke]. Sprache & Kognition (in German). 10: 109–113.
  • Tonelli, Marcello (2011). Unstructured Processes of Strategic Decision-Making. Saarbrücken, Germany: Lambert Academic Publishing. ISBN 978-3-8465-5598-9.
  • Van Lehn, Kurt (1989). "Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition". In Posner, M. I. (ed.). Foundations of cognitive science (PDF). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. 527–579.
  • Wisconsin Educational Media Association (1993), Information literacy: A position paper on information problem-solving, WEMA Publications, vol. ED 376 817, Madison, Wis.{{citation}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link) (Portions adapted from Michigan State Board of Education's Position Paper on Information Processing Skills, 1992.)

External links edit

  •   Learning materials related to Solving Problems at Wikiversity

problem, solving, problem, redirects, here, other, uses, problem, disambiguation, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, this, article, adding, citations, reliable, sources, unsourced, material, challenged, removed, f. Problem redirects here For other uses see Problem disambiguation This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Problem solving news newspapers books scholar JSTOR September 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message Problem solving is the process of achieving a goal by overcoming obstacles a frequent part of most activities Problems in need of solutions range from simple personal tasks e g how to turn on an appliance to complex issues in business and technical fields The former is an example of simple problem solving SPS addressing one issue whereas the latter is complex problem solving CPS with multiple interrelated obstacles 1 Another classification of problem solving tasks is into well defined problems with specific obstacles and goals and ill defined problems in which the current situation is troublesome but it is not clear what kind of resolution to aim for 2 Similarly one may distinguish formal or fact based problems requiring psychometric intelligence versus socio emotional problems which depend on the changeable emotions of individuals or groups such as tactful behavior fashion or gift choices 3 Solutions require sufficient resources and knowledge to attain the goal Professionals such as lawyers doctors programmers and consultants are largely problem solvers for issues that require technical skills and knowledge beyond general competence Many businesses have found profitable markets by recognizing a problem and creating a solution the more widespread and inconvenient the problem the greater the opportunity to develop a scalable solution There are many specialized problem solving techniques and methods in fields such as engineering business medicine mathematics computer science philosophy and social organization The mental techniques to identify analyze and solve problems are studied in psychology and cognitive sciences Also widely researched are the mental obstacles that prevent people from finding solutions problem solving impediments include confirmation bias mental set and functional fixedness Contents 1 Definition 1 1 Psychology 1 2 Cognitive sciences 1 3 Computer science 1 4 Logic 1 5 Engineering 1 6 Military science 2 Processes 3 Problem solving strategies 3 1 Observation Question 4 Problem solving methods 5 Common barriers 5 1 Confirmation bias 5 2 Mental set 5 3 Functional fixedness 5 4 Unnecessary constraints 5 5 Irrelevant information 5 6 Avoiding barriers by changing problem representation 5 7 Other barriers for individuals 6 Dreaming problem solving without waking consciousness 7 Cognitive sciences two schools 7 1 Europe 7 2 North America 8 Characteristics of complex problems 9 Collective problem solving 10 See also 11 Notes 12 Further reading 13 External linksDefinition editThe term problem solving has a slightly different meaning depending on the discipline For instance it is a mental process in psychology and a computerized process in computer science There are two different types of problems ill defined and well defined different approaches are used for each Well defined problems have specific end goals and clearly expected solutions while ill defined problems do not Well defined problems allow for more initial planning than ill defined problems 2 Solving problems sometimes involves dealing with pragmatics the way that context contributes to meaning and semantics the interpretation of the problem The ability to understand what the end goal of the problem is and what rules could be applied represents the key to solving the problem Sometimes a problem requires abstract thinking or coming up with a creative solution Problem solving has two major domains mathematical problem solving and personal problem solving Each concerns some difficulty or barrier that is encountered 4 Psychology edit Problem solving in psychology refers to the process of finding solutions to problems encountered in life 5 Solutions to these problems are usually situation or context specific The process starts with problem finding and problem shaping in which the problem is discovered and simplified The next step is to generate possible solutions and evaluate them Finally a solution is selected to be implemented and verified Problems have an end goal to be reached how you get there depends upon problem orientation problem solving coping style and skills and systematic analysis 6 Mental health professionals study the human problem solving processes using methods such as introspection behaviorism simulation computer modeling and experiment Social psychologists look into the person environment relationship aspect of the problem and independent and interdependent problem solving methods 7 Problem solving has been defined as a higher order cognitive process and intellectual function that requires the modulation and control of more routine or fundamental skills 8 Empirical research shows many different strategies and factors influence everyday problem solving 9 Rehabilitation psychologists studying people with frontal lobe injuries have found that deficits in emotional control and reasoning can be re mediated with effective rehabilitation and could improve the capacity of injured persons to resolve everyday problems 10 Interpersonal everyday problem solving is dependent upon personal motivational and contextual components One such component is the emotional valence of real world problems which can either impede or aid problem solving performance Researchers have focused on the role of emotions in problem solving 11 demonstrating that poor emotional control can disrupt focus on the target task impede problem resolution and lead to negative outcomes such as fatigue depression and inertia 12 In conceptualization clarification needed human problem solving consists of two related processes problem orientation and the motivational attitudinal affective approach to problematic situations and problem solving skills People s strategies cohere with their goals 13 and stem from the process of comparing oneself with others Cognitive sciences edit Among the first experimental psychologists to study problem solving were the Gestaltists in Germany such as Karl Duncker in The Psychology of Productive Thinking 1935 14 Perhaps best known is the work of Allen Newell and Herbert A Simon 15 Experiments in the 1960s and early 1970s asked participants to solve relatively simple well defined but not previously seen laboratory tasks 16 17 These simple problems such as the Tower of Hanoi admitted optimal solutions that could be found quickly allowing researchers to observe the full problem solving process Researchers assumed that these model problems would elicit the characteristic cognitive processes by which more complex real world problems are solved An outstanding problem solving technique found by this research is the principle of decomposition 18 Computer science edit This section needs expansion You can help by adding to it September 2018 Much of computer science and artificial intelligence involves designing automated systems to solve a specified type of problem to accept input data and calculate a correct or adequate response reasonably quickly Algorithms are recipes or instructions that direct such systems written into computer programs Steps for designing such systems include problem determination heuristics root cause analysis de duplication analysis diagnosis and repair Analytic techniques include linear and nonlinear programming queuing systems and simulation 19 A large perennial obstacle is to find and fix errors in computer programs debugging Logic edit Formal logic concerns issues like validity truth inference argumentation and proof In a problem solving context it can be used to formally represent a problem as a theorem to be proved and to represent the knowledge needed to solve the problem as the premises to be used in a proof that the problem has a solution The use of computers to prove mathematical theorems using formal logic emerged as the field of automated theorem proving in the 1950s It included the use of heuristic methods designed to simulate human problem solving as in the Logic Theory Machine developed by Allen Newell Herbert A Simon and J C Shaw as well as algorithmic methods such as the resolution principle developed by John Alan Robinson In addition to its use for finding proofs of mathematical theorems automated theorem proving has also been used for program verification in computer science In 1958 John McCarthy proposed the advice taker to represent information in formal logic and to derive answers to questions using automated theorem proving An important step in this direction was made by Cordell Green in 1969 who used a resolution theorem prover for question answering and for such other applications in artificial intelligence as robot planning The resolution theorem prover used by Cordell Green bore little resemblance to human problem solving methods In response to criticism of that approach from researchers at MIT Robert Kowalski developed logic programming and SLD resolution 20 which solves problems by problem decomposition He has advocated logic for both computer and human problem solving 21 and computational logic to improve human thinking 22 Engineering edit When products or processes fail problem solving techniques can be used to develop corrective actions that can be taken to prevent further failures Such techniques can also be applied to a product or process prior to an actual failure event to predict analyze and mitigate a potential problem in advance Techniques such as failure mode and effects analysis can proactively reduce the likelihood of problems In either the reactive or the proactive case it is necessary to build a causal explanation through a process of diagnosis In deriving an explanation of effects in terms of causes abduction generates new ideas or hypotheses asking how deduction evaluates and refines hypotheses based on other plausible premises asking why and induction justifies a hypothesis with empirical data asking how much 23 The objective of abduction is to determine which hypothesis or proposition to test not which one to adopt or assert 24 In the Peircean logical system the logic of abduction and deduction contribute to our conceptual understanding of a phenomenon while the logic of induction adds quantitative details empirical substantiation to our conceptual knowledge 25 Forensic engineering is an important technique of failure analysis that involves tracing product defects and flaws Corrective action can then be taken to prevent further failures Reverse engineering attempts to discover the original problem solving logic used in developing a product by disassembling the product and developing a plausible pathway to creating and assembling its parts 26 Military science edit In military science problem solving is linked to the concept of end states the conditions or situations which are the aims of the strategy 27 xiii E 2 Ability to solve problems is important at any military rank but is essential at the command and control level It results from deep qualitative and quantitative understanding of possible scenarios Effectiveness in this context is an evaluation of results to what extent the end states were accomplished 27 IV 24 Planning is the process of determining how to effect those end states 27 IV 1 Processes editSome models of problem solving involve identifying a goal and then a sequence of subgoals towards achieving this goal Andersson who introduced the ACT R model of cognition modelled this collection of goals and subgoals as a goal stack in which the mind contains a stack of goals and subgoals to be completed and a single task being carried out at any time 28 51 Knowledge of how to solve one problem can be applied to another problem in a process known as transfer 28 56 Problem solving strategies editSee also Category Problem solving skills Problem solving strategies are steps to overcoming the obstacles to achieving a goal The iteration of such strategies over the course of solving a problem is the problem solving cycle 29 Common steps in this cycle include recognizing the problem defining it developing a strategy to fix it organizing knowledge and resources available monitoring progress and evaluating the effectiveness of the solution Once a solution is achieved another problem usually arises and the cycle starts again Insight is the sudden aha solution to a problem the birth of a new idea to simplify a complex situation Solutions found through insight are often more incisive than those from step by step analysis A quick solution process requires insight to select productive moves at different stages of the problem solving cycle Unlike Newell and Simon s formal definition of a move problem there is no consensus definition of an insight problem 30 Some problem solving strategies include 31 Abstraction solving the problem in a tractable model system to gain insight into the real system Analogy adapting the solution to a previous problem which has similar features or mechanisms Brainstorming especially among groups of people suggesting a large number of solutions or ideas and combining and developing them until an optimum solution is found Bypasses transform the problem into another problem that is easier to solve bypassing the barrier then transform that solution back to a solution to the original problem Critical thinking analysis of available evidence and arguments to form a judgement via rational skeptical and unbiased evaluation Divide and conquer breaking down a large complex problem into smaller solvable problems Help seeking obtaining external assistance to deal with obstacles Hypothesis testing assuming a possible explanation to the problem and trying to prove or in some contexts disprove the assumption Lateral thinking approaching solutions indirectly and creatively Means ends analysis choosing an action at each step to move closer to the goal Morphological analysis assessing the output and interactions of an entire systemObservation Question edit in the natural sciences is an act or instance of noticing or perceiving and the acquisition of information from a primary source is an utterance which serves as a request for information Proof of impossibility try to prove that the problem cannot be solved The point where the proof fails will be the starting point for solving it Reduction transforming the problem into another problem for which solutions exist Research employing existing ideas or adapting existing solutions to similar problems Root cause analysis identifying the cause of a problem Trial and error testing possible solutions until the right one is foundProblem solving methods editSee also Category Problem solving methods and Category Problem structuring methods A3 problem solving Structured problem improvement approach Design thinking Processes by which design concepts are developed Eight Disciplines Problem Solving Eight Disciplines of Team Oriented Problem Solving MethodPages displaying short descriptions of redirect targets GROW model Method for goal setting and problem solving Help seeking Theory in psychology How to Solve It Book by George Polya Lateral thinking Manner of solving problems OODA loop Observe orient decide act cycle PDCA Iterative design and management method used in business Root cause analysis Method of identifying the fundamental causes of faults or problems RPR problem diagnosis problem diagnosis method designed to determine the root cause of IT problemsPages displaying wikidata descriptions as a fallback TRIZ Problem solving tools Scientific method is an empirical method for acquiring knowledge that has characterized the development of science Swarm intelligence Collective behavior of decentralized self organized systems System dynamics Study of non linear complex systemsCommon barriers editCommon barriers to problem solving include mental constructs that impede an efficient search for solutions Five of the most common identified by researchers are confirmation bias mental set functional fixedness unnecessary constraints and irrelevant information Confirmation bias edit Main article Confirmation bias Confirmation bias is an unintentional tendency to collect and use data which favors preconceived notions Such notions may be incidental rather than motivated by important personal beliefs the desire to be right may be sufficient motivation 32 Scientific and technical professionals also experience confirmation bias One online experiment for example suggested that professionals within the field of psychological research are likely to view scientific studies that agree with their preconceived notions more favorably than clashing studies 33 According to Raymond Nickerson one can see the consequences of confirmation bias in real life situations which range in severity from inefficient government policies to genocide Nickerson argued that those who killed people accused of witchcraft demonstrated confirmation bias with motivation citation needed Researcher Michael Allen found evidence for confirmation bias with motivation in school children who worked to manipulate their science experiments to produce favorable results 34 However confirmation bias does not necessarily require motivation In 1960 Peter Cathcart Wason conducted an experiment in which participants first viewed three numbers and then created a hypothesis in the form of a rule that could have been used to create that triplet of numbers When testing their hypotheses participants tended to only create additional triplets of numbers that would confirm their hypotheses and tended not to create triplets that would negate or disprove their hypotheses 35 Mental set edit Main article Mental set Mental set is the inclination to re use a previously successful solution rather than search for new and better solutions It is a reliance on habit It was first articulated by Abraham S Luchins in the 1940s with his well known water jug experiments 36 Participants were asked to fill one jug with a specific amount of water by using other jugs with different maximum capacities After Luchins gave a set of jug problems that could all be solved by a single technique he then introduced a problem that could be solved by the same technique but also by a novel and simpler method His participants tended to use the accustomed technique oblivious of the simpler alternative 37 This was again demonstrated in Norman Maier s 1931 experiment which challenged participants to solve a problem by using a familiar tool pliers in an unconventional manner Participants were often unable to view the object in a way that strayed from its typical use a type of mental set known as functional fixedness see the following section Rigidly clinging to a mental set is called fixation which can deepen to an obsession or preoccupation with attempted strategies that are repeatedly unsuccessful 38 In the late 1990s researcher Jennifer Wiley found that professional expertise in a field can create a mental set perhaps leading to fixation 38 Groupthink in which each individual takes on the mindset of the rest of the group can produce and exacerbate mental set 39 Social pressure leads to everybody thinking the same thing and reaching the same conclusions Functional fixedness edit Main article Functional fixedness Functional fixedness is the tendency to view an object as having only one function and to be unable to conceive of any novel use as in the Maier pliers experiment described above Functional fixedness is a specific form of mental set and is one of the most common forms of cognitive bias in daily life As an example imagine a man wants to kill a bug in his house but the only thing at hand is a can of air freshener He may start searching for something to kill the bug instead of squashing it with the can thinking only of its main function of deodorizing Tim German and Clark Barrett describe this barrier subjects become fixed on the design function of the objects and problem solving suffers relative to control conditions in which the object s function is not demonstrated 40 Their research found that young children s limited knowledge of an object s intended function reduces this barrier 41 Research has also discovered functional fixedness in educational contexts as an obstacle to understanding functional fixedness may be found in learning concepts as well as in solving chemistry problems 42 There are several hypotheses in regards to how functional fixedness relates to problem solving 43 It may waste time delaying or entirely preventing the correct use of a tool Unnecessary constraints edit Unnecessary constraints are arbitrary boundaries imposed unconsciously on the task at hand which foreclose a productive avenue of solution The solver may become fixated on only one type of solution as if it were an inevitable requirement of the problem Typically this combines with mental set clinging to a previously successful method 44 page needed Visual problems can also produce mentally invented constraints 45 page needed A famous example is the dot problem nine dots arranged in a three by three grid pattern must be connected by drawing four straight line segments without lifting pen from paper or backtracking along a line The subject typically assumes the pen must stay within the outer square of dots but the solution requires lines continuing beyond this frame and researchers have found a 0 solution rate within a brief allotted time 46 This problem has produced the expression think outside the box 47 page needed Such problems are typically solved via a sudden insight which leaps over the mental barriers often after long toil against them 48 This can be difficult depending on how the subject has structured the problem in their mind how they draw on past experiences and how well they juggle this information in their working memory In the example envisioning the dots connected outside the framing square requires visualizing an unconventional arrangement which is a strain on working memory 47 Irrelevant information edit See also Information overload and Mass media Irrelevant information is a specification or data presented in a problem that is unrelated to the solution 44 If the solver assumes that all information presented needs to be used this often derails the problem solving process making relatively simple problems much harder 49 For example Fifteen percent of the people in Topeka have unlisted telephone numbers You select 200 names at random from the Topeka phone book How many of these people have unlisted phone numbers 47 page needed The obvious answer is 15 but in fact none of the unlisted people would be listed among the 200 This kind of trick question is often used in aptitude tests or cognitive evaluations 50 Though not inherently difficult they require independent thinking that is not necessarily common Mathematical word problems often include irrelevant qualitative or numerical information as an extra challenge Avoiding barriers by changing problem representation edit The disruption caused by the above cognitive biases can depend on how the information is represented 50 visually verbally or mathematically A classic example is the Buddhist monk problem A Buddhist monk begins at dawn one day walking up a mountain reaches the top at sunset meditates at the top for several days until one dawn when he begins to walk back to the foot of the mountain which he reaches at sunset Making no assumptions about his starting or stopping or about his pace during the trips prove that there is a place on the path which he occupies at the same hour of the day on the two separate journeys The problem cannot be addressed in a verbal context trying to describe the monk s progress on each day It becomes much easier when the paragraph is represented mathematically by a function one visualizes a graph whose horizontal axis is time of day and whose vertical axis shows the monk s position or altitude on the path at each time Superimposing the two journey curves which traverse opposite diagonals of a rectangle one sees they must cross each other somewhere The visual representation by graphing has resolved the difficulty Similar strategies can often improve problem solving on tests 44 51 Other barriers for individuals edit People who are engaged in problem solving tend to overlook subtractive changes even those that are critical elements of efficient solutions example needed This tendency to solve by first only or mostly creating or adding elements rather than by subtracting elements or processes is shown to intensify with higher cognitive loads such as information overload 52 Dreaming problem solving without waking consciousness editPeople can also solve problems while they are asleep There are many reports of scientists and engineers who solved problems in their dreams For example Elias Howe inventor of the sewing machine figured out the structure of the bobbin from a dream 53 The chemist August Kekule was considering how benzene arranged its six carbon and hydrogen atoms Thinking about the problem he dozed off and dreamt of dancing atoms that fell into a snakelike pattern which led him to discover the benzene ring As Kekule wrote in his diary One of the snakes seized hold of its own tail and the form whirled mockingly before my eyes As if by a flash of lightning I awoke and this time also I spent the rest of the night in working out the consequences of the hypothesis 54 There also are empirical studies of how people can think consciously about a problem before going to sleep and then solve the problem with a dream image Dream researcher William C Dement told his undergraduate class of 500 students that he wanted them to think about an infinite series whose first elements were OTTFF to see if they could deduce the principle behind it and to say what the next elements of the series would be 55 page needed He asked them to think about this problem every night for 15 minutes before going to sleep and to write down any dreams that they then had They were instructed to think about the problem again for 15 minutes when they awakened in the morning The sequence OTTFF is the first letters of the numbers one two three four five The next five elements of the series are SSENT six seven eight nine ten Some of the students solved the puzzle by reflecting on their dreams One example was a student who reported the following dream 55 page needed I was standing in an art gallery looking at the paintings on the wall As I walked down the hall I began to count the paintings one two three four five As I came to the sixth and seventh the paintings had been ripped from their frames I stared at the empty frames with a peculiar feeling that some mystery was about to be solved Suddenly I realized that the sixth and seventh spaces were the solution to the problem With more than 500 undergraduate students 87 dreams were judged to be related to the problems students were assigned 53 directly related and 34 indirectly related Yet of the people who had dreams that apparently solved the problem only seven were actually able to consciously know the solution The rest 46 out of 53 thought they did not know the solution Mark Blechner conducted this experiment and obtained results similar to Dement s 56 page needed He found that while trying to solve the problem people had dreams in which the solution appeared to be obvious from the dream but it was rare for the dreamers to realize how their dreams had solved the puzzle Coaxing or hints did not get them to realize it although once they heard the solution they recognized how their dream had solved it For example one person in that OTTFF experiment dreamed 56 page needed There is a big clock You can see the movement The big hand of the clock was on the number six You could see it move up number by number six seven eight nine ten eleven twelve The dream focused on the small parts of the machinery You could see the gears inside In the dream the person counted out the next elements of the series six seven eight nine ten eleven twelve yet he did not realize that this was the solution of the problem His sleeping mindbrain jargon solved the problem but his waking mindbrain was not aware how Albert Einstein believed that much problem solving goes on unconsciously and the person must then figure out and formulate consciously what the mindbrain jargon has already solved He believed this was his process in formulating the theory of relativity The creator of the problem possesses the solution 57 Einstein said that he did his problem solving without words mostly in images The words or the language as they are written or spoken do not seem to play any role in my mechanism of thought The psychical entities which seem to serve as elements in thought are certain signs and more or less clear images which can be voluntarily reproduced and combined 58 Cognitive sciences two schools editProblem solving processes differ across knowledge domains and across levels of expertise 59 For this reason cognitive sciences findings obtained in the laboratory cannot necessarily generalize to problem solving situations outside the laboratory This has led to a research emphasis on real world problem solving since the 1990s This emphasis has been expressed quite differently in North America and Europe however Whereas North American research has typically concentrated on studying problem solving in separate natural knowledge domains much of the European research has focused on novel complex problems and has been performed with computerized scenarios 60 Europe edit In Europe two main approaches have surfaced one initiated by Donald Broadbent 61 in the United Kingdom and the other one by Dietrich Dorner 62 in Germany The two approaches share an emphasis on relatively complex semantically rich computerized laboratory tasks constructed to resemble real life problems The approaches differ somewhat in their theoretical goals and methodology The tradition initiated by Broadbent emphasizes the distinction between cognitive problem solving processes that operate under awareness versus outside of awareness and typically employs mathematically well defined computerized systems The tradition initiated by Dorner on the other hand has an interest in the interplay of the cognitive motivational and social components of problem solving and utilizes very complex computerized scenarios that contain up to 2 000 highly interconnected variables 63 North America edit In North America initiated by the work of Herbert A Simon on learning by doing in semantically rich domains 64 researchers began to investigate problem solving separately in different natural knowledge domains such as physics writing or chess playing rather than attempt to extract a global theory of problem solving 65 These researchers have focused on the development of problem solving within certain domains that is on the development of expertise 66 Areas that have attracted rather intensive attention in North America include calculation 67 computer skills 68 game playing 69 lawyers reasoning 70 managerial problem solving 71 mathematical problem solving 72 mechanical problem solving 73 personal problem solving 74 political decision making 75 problem solving in electronics 76 problem solving for innovations and inventions TRIZ 77 reading 78 social problem solving 11 writing 79 Characteristics of complex problems editComplex problem solving CPS is distinguishable from simple problem solving SPS In SPS there is a singular and simple obstacle In CPS there may be multiple simultaneous obstacles For example a surgeon at work has far more complex problems than an individual deciding what shoes to wear As elucidated by Dietrich Dorner and later expanded upon by Joachim Funke complex problems have some typical characteristics which include 1 complexity large numbers of items interrelations and decisions enumerability clarification needed heterogeneity specify connectivity hierarchy relation communication relation allocation relation clarification needed dynamics time considerations clarification needed temporal constraints temporal sensitivity clarification needed phase effects definition needed dynamic unpredictability specify intransparency lack of clarity of the situation commencement opacity definition needed continuation opacity definition needed polytely multiple goals 80 inexpressivenes specify opposition specify transience specify Collective problem solving editSee also Crowdsolving Collective action Collaborative intelligence Mass collaboration Collective wisdom The Wisdom of Crowds Distributed knowledge Online participation and Group decision making People solve problems on many different levels from the individual to the civilizational Collective problem solving refers to problem solving performed collectively Social issues and global issues can typically only be solved collectively The complexity of contemporary problems exceeds the cognitive capacity of any individual and requires different but complementary varieties of expertise and collective problem solving ability 81 Collective intelligence is shared or group intelligence that emerges from the collaboration collective efforts and competition of many individuals In collaborative problem solving people work together to solve real world problems Members of problem solving groups share a common concern a similar passion and or a commitment to their work Members can ask questions wonder and try to understand common issues They share expertise experiences tools and methods 82 Groups may be fluid based on need may only occur temporarily to finish an assigned task or may be more permanent depending on the nature of the problems For example in the educational context members of a group may all have input into the decision making process and a role in the learning process Members may be responsible for the thinking teaching and monitoring of all members in the group Group work may be coordinated among members so that each member makes an equal contribution to the whole work Members can identify and build on their individual strengths so that everyone can make a significant contribution to the task 83 Collaborative group work has the ability to promote critical thinking skills problem solving skills social skills and self esteem By using collaboration and communication members often learn from one another and construct meaningful knowledge that often leads to better learning outcomes than individual work 84 Collaborative groups require joint intellectual efforts between the members and involve social interactions to solve problems together The knowledge shared during these interactions is acquired during communication negotiation and production of materials 85 Members actively seek information from others by asking questions The capacity to use questions to acquire new information increases understanding and the ability to solve problems 86 In a 1962 research report Douglas Engelbart linked collective intelligence to organizational effectiveness and predicted that proactively augmenting human intellect would yield a multiplier effect in group problem solving Three people working together in this augmented mode would seem to be more than three times as effective in solving a complex problem as is one augmented person working alone 87 Henry Jenkins a theorist of new media and media convergence draws on the theory that collective intelligence can be attributed to media convergence and participatory culture 88 He criticizes contemporary education for failing to incorporate online trends of collective problem solving into the classroom stating whereas a collective intelligence community encourages ownership of work as a group schools grade individuals Jenkins argues that interaction within a knowledge community builds vital skills for young people and teamwork through collective intelligence communities contributes to the development of such skills 89 Collective impact is the commitment of a group of actors from different sectors to a common agenda for solving a specific social problem using a structured form of collaboration After World War II the UN the Bretton Woods organization and the WTO were created Collective problem solving on the international level crystallized around these three types of organization from the 1980s onward As these global institutions remain state like or state centric it is unsurprising that they perpetuate state like or state centric approaches to collective problem solving rather than alternative ones 90 Crowdsourcing is a process of accumulating ideas thoughts or information from many independent participants with aim of finding the best solution for a given challenge Modern information technologies allow for many people to be involved and facilitate managing their suggestions in ways that provide good results 91 The Internet allows for a new capacity of collective including planetary scale problem solving 92 See also edit nbsp Philosophy portal nbsp Psychology portal nbsp Wikiquote has quotations related to Problem solving Actuarial science Statistics applied to risk in insurance and other financial products Analytical skill Crucial skill in all different fields of work and life Creative problem solving mental process of searching for an original and previously unknown solution to a problemPages displaying wikidata descriptions as a fallback Collective intelligence Group intelligence that emerges from collective efforts Community of practice Coworking Practice of independent contractors or scientists sharing office space without supervision Crowdsolving Sourcing services or funds from a groupPages displaying short descriptions of redirect targets Divergent thinking A process of generating creative ideas Grey problem IT service problem where the causing technology is unknown or unconfirmed making the problem solving difficult to allocatePages displaying wikidata descriptions as a fallback Innovation Practical implementation of improvements Instrumentalism Position in the philosophy of science Problem statement Description of an issue Problem structuring methods Shared intentionality Description of the concept of shared intentionality Structural fix solving a problem or resolving a conflict by bringing about structural changes in underlying structures that provoked or sustained these problemsPages displaying wikidata descriptions as a fallback Subgoal labeling Troubleshooting Form of problem solving often applied to repair failed products or processes Wicked problem Problem that is difficult or impossible to solveNotes edit a b Frensch Peter A Funke Joachim eds 2014 04 04 Complex Problem Solving doi 10 4324 9781315806723 ISBN 978 1 315 80672 3 a b Schacter D L Gilbert D T Wegner D M 2011 Psychology 2nd ed New York Worth Publishers p 376 Blanchard Fields F 2007 Everyday problem solving and emotion An adult developmental perspective Current Directions in Psychological Science 16 1 26 31 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2007 00469 x S2CID 145645352 Zimmermann Bernd 2004 On mathematical problem solving processes and history of mathematics ICME 10 Copenhagen Granvold Donald K 1997 Cognitive Behavioral Therapy with Adults In Brandell Jerrold R ed Theory and Practice in Clinical Social Work Simon and Schuster pp 189 ISBN 978 0 684 82765 0 Robertson S Ian 2001 Introduction to the study of problem solving Problem Solving Psychology Press ISBN 0 415 20300 7 Rubin M Watt S E Ramelli M 2012 Immigrants social integration as a function of approach avoidance orientation and problem solving style International Journal of Intercultural Relations 36 4 498 505 doi 10 1016 j ijintrel 2011 12 009 hdl 1959 13 931119 Goldstein F C Levin H S 1987 Disorders of reasoning and problem solving ability In M Meier A Benton L Diller eds Neuropsychological rehabilitation London Taylor amp Francis Group Vallacher Robin M Wegner Daniel 2012 Action Identification Theory Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology pp 327 348 doi 10 4135 9781446249215 n17 ISBN 978 0 85702 960 7 Margrett J A Marsiske M 2002 Gender differences in older adults everyday cognitive collaboration International Journal of Behavioral Development 26 1 45 59 doi 10 1080 01650250143000319 PMC 2909137 PMID 20657668 Antonucci T C Ajrouch K J Birditt K S 2013 The Convoy Model Explaining Social Relations From a Multidisciplinary Perspective The Gerontologist 54 1 82 92 doi 10 1093 geront gnt118 PMC 3894851 PMID 24142914 Rath Joseph F Simon Dvorah Langenbahn Donna M Sherr Rose Lynn Diller Leonard 2003 Group treatment of problem solving deficits in outpatients with traumatic brain injury A randomised outcome study Neuropsychological Rehabilitation 13 4 461 488 doi 10 1080 09602010343000039 S2CID 143165070 a b D Zurilla T J Goldfried M R 1971 Problem solving and behavior modification Journal of Abnormal Psychology 78 1 107 126 doi 10 1037 h0031360 PMID 4938262 D Zurilla T J Nezu A M 1982 Social problem solving in adults In P C Kendall ed Advances in cognitive behavioral research and therapy Vol 1 New York Academic Press pp 201 274 Rath J F Langenbahn D M Simon D Sherr R L Fletcher J Diller L 2004 The construct of problem solving in higher level neuropsychological assessment and rehabilitation 1 Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology 19 5 613 635 doi 10 1016 j acn 2003 08 006 PMID 15271407 Hoppmann Christiane A Blanchard Fields Fredda 2010 Goals and everyday problem solving Manipulating goal preferences in young and older adults Developmental Psychology 46 6 1433 1443 doi 10 1037 a0020676 PMID 20873926 Duncker Karl 1935 Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens The psychology of productive thinking in German Berlin Julius Springer Newell Allen Simon Herbert A 1972 Human problem solving Englewood Cliffs N J Prentice Hall For example X ray problem by Duncker Karl 1935 Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens The psychology of productive thinking in German Berlin Julius Springer Disk problem later known as Tower of Hanoi by Ewert P H Lambert J F 1932 Part II The Effect of Verbal Instructions upon the Formation of a Concept The Journal of General Psychology 6 2 Informa UK Limited 400 413 doi 10 1080 00221309 1932 9711880 ISSN 0022 1309 Mayer R E 1992 Thinking problem solving cognition Second ed New York W H Freeman and Company Armstrong J Scott Denniston William B Jr Gordon Matt M 1975 The Use of the Decomposition Principle in Making Judgments PDF Organizational Behavior and Human Performance 14 2 257 263 doi 10 1016 0030 5073 75 90028 8 S2CID 122659209 Archived from the original PDF on 2010 06 20 Malakooti Behnam 2013 Operations and Production Systems with Multiple Objectives John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 1 118 58537 5 Kowalski Robert 1974 Predicate Logic as a Programming Language PDF Information Processing 74 Kowalski Robert 1979 Logic for Problem Solving PDF Artificial Intelligence Series Vol 7 Elsevier Science Publishing ISBN 0 444 00368 1 Kowalski Robert 2011 Computational Logic and Human Thinking How to be Artificially Intelligent PDF Cambridge University Press Staat Wim 1993 On abduction deduction induction and the categories Transactions of the Charles S Peirce Society 29 2 225 237 Sullivan Patrick F 1991 On Falsificationist Interpretations of Peirce Transactions of the Charles S Peirce Society 27 2 197 219 Ho Yu Chong 1994 Abduction Deduction Induction Is There a Logic of Exploratory Data Analysis PDF Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association New Orleans La Einstein s Secret to Amazing Problem Solving and 10 Specific Ways You Can Use It Litemind 2008 11 04 Archived from the original on 2017 06 21 Retrieved 2017 06 11 a b c Commander s Handbook for Strategic Communication and Communication Strategy PDF United States Joint Forces Command Joint Warfighting Center Suffolk Va 27 October 2009 Archived from the original PDF on April 29 2011 Retrieved 10 October 2016 a b Robertson S Ian 2017 Problem solving perspectives from cognition and neuroscience 2nd ed London Taylor amp Francis ISBN 978 1 317 49601 4 OCLC 962750529 Bransford J D Stein B S 1993 The ideal problem solver A guide for improving thinking learning and creativity 2nd ed New York W H Freeman Ash Ivan K Jee Benjamin D Wiley Jennifer 2012 Investigating Insight as Sudden Learning The Journal of Problem Solving 4 2 doi 10 7771 1932 6246 1123 ISSN 1932 6246 Chronicle Edward P MacGregor James N Ormerod Thomas C 2004 What Makes an Insight Problem The Roles of Heuristics Goal Conception and Solution Recoding in Knowledge Lean Problems PDF Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 30 1 14 27 doi 10 1037 0278 7393 30 1 14 ISSN 1939 1285 PMID 14736293 S2CID 15631498 Chu Yun MacGregor James N 2011 Human Performance on Insight Problem Solving A Review The Journal of Problem Solving 3 2 doi 10 7771 1932 6246 1094 ISSN 1932 6246 Wang Y Chiew V 2010 On the cognitive process of human problem solving PDF Cognitive Systems Research 11 1 Elsevier BV 81 92 doi 10 1016 j cogsys 2008 08 003 ISSN 1389 0417 S2CID 16238486 Nickerson Raymond S 1998 Confirmation bias A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises Review of General Psychology 2 2 176 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 2 2 175 S2CID 8508954 Hergovich Andreas Schott Reinhard Burger Christoph 2010 Biased Evaluation of Abstracts Depending on Topic and Conclusion Further Evidence of a Confirmation Bias Within Scientific Psychology Current Psychology 29 3 Springer Science and Business Media LLC 188 209 doi 10 1007 s12144 010 9087 5 ISSN 1046 1310 S2CID 145497196 Allen Michael 2011 Theory led confirmation bias and experimental persona Research in Science amp Technological Education 29 1 Informa UK Limited 107 127 Bibcode 2011RSTEd 29 107A doi 10 1080 02635143 2010 539973 ISSN 0263 5143 S2CID 145706148 Wason P C 1960 On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 12 3 129 140 doi 10 1080 17470216008416717 S2CID 19237642 Luchins Abraham S 1942 Mechanization in problem solving The effect of Einstellung Psychological Monographs 54 248 i 95 doi 10 1037 h0093502 Ollinger Michael Jones Gary Knoblich Gunther 2008 Investigating the Effect of Mental Set on Insight Problem Solving PDF Experimental Psychology 55 4 Hogrefe Publishing Group 269 282 doi 10 1027 1618 3169 55 4 269 ISSN 1618 3169 PMID 18683624 a b Wiley Jennifer 1998 Expertise as mental set The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving Memory amp Cognition 24 4 716 730 doi 10 3758 bf03211392 PMID 9701964 Cottam Martha L Dietz Uhler Beth Mastors Elena Preston Thomas 2010 Introduction to Political Psychology 2nd ed New York Psychology Press German Tim P Barrett H Clark 2005 Functional Fixedness in a Technologically Sparse Culture Psychological Science 16 1 SAGE Publications 1 5 doi 10 1111 j 0956 7976 2005 00771 x ISSN 0956 7976 PMID 15660843 S2CID 1833823 German Tim P Defeyter Margaret A 2000 Immunity to functional fixedness in young children Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 7 4 707 712 doi 10 3758 BF03213010 PMID 11206213 Furio C Calatayud M L Baracenas S Padilla O 2000 Functional fixedness and functional reduction as common sense reasonings in chemical equilibrium and in geometry and polarity of molecules Science Education 84 5 545 565 doi 10 1002 1098 237X 200009 84 5 lt 545 AID SCE1 gt 3 0 CO 2 1 Adamson Robert E 1952 Functional fixedness as related to problem solving A repetition of three experiments Journal of Experimental Psychology 44 4 288 291 doi 10 1037 h0062487 PMID 13000071 a b c Kellogg R T 2003 Cognitive psychology 2nd ed California Sage Publications Inc Meloy J R 1998 The Psychology of Stalking Clinical and Forensic Perspectives 2nd ed London England Academic Press MacGregor J N Ormerod T C Chronicle E P 2001 Information processing and insight A process model of performance on the nine dot and related problems Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition 27 1 176 201 doi 10 1037 0278 7393 27 1 176 PMID 11204097 a b c Weiten Wayne 2011 Psychology themes and variations 8th ed California Wadsworth Novick L R Bassok M 2005 Problem solving In Holyoak K J Morrison R G eds Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning New York N Y Cambridge University Press pp 321 349 Walinga Jennifer 2010 From walls to windows Using barriers as pathways to insightful solutions The Journal of Creative Behavior 44 3 143 167 doi 10 1002 j 2162 6057 2010 tb01331 x a b Walinga Jennifer Cunningham J Barton MacGregor James N 2011 Training insight problem solving through focus on barriers and assumptions The Journal of Creative Behavior 45 47 58 doi 10 1002 j 2162 6057 2011 tb01084 x Vlamings Petra H J M Hare Brian Call Joseph 2009 Reaching around barriers The performance of great apes and 3 5 year old children Animal Cognition 13 2 273 285 doi 10 1007 s10071 009 0265 5 PMC 2822225 PMID 19653018 Gupta Sujata 7 April 2021 People add by default even when subtraction makes more sense Science News Retrieved 10 May 2021 Adams Gabrielle S Converse Benjamin A Hales Andrew H Klotz Leidy E April 2021 People systematically overlook subtractive changes Nature 592 7853 258 261 Bibcode 2021Natur 592 258A doi 10 1038 s41586 021 03380 y ISSN 1476 4687 PMID 33828317 S2CID 233185662 Retrieved 10 May 2021 Kaempffert Waldemar B 1924 A Popular History of American Invention Vol 2 New York Charles Scribner s Sons p 385 Kekule August 1890 Benzolfest Rede Berichte der Deutschen Chemischen Gesellschaft 23 1302 1311 Benfey O 1958 Kekule and the birth of the structural theory of organic chemistry in 1858 Journal of Chemical Education 35 1 21 23 Bibcode 1958JChEd 35 21B doi 10 1021 ed035p21 a b Dement W C 1972 Some Must Watch While Some Just Sleep New York Freeman a b Blechner Mark J 2018 The Mindbrain and Dreams An Exploration of Dreaming Thinking and Artistic Creation New York Routledge Fromm Erika O 1998 Lost and found half a century later Letters by Freud and Einstein American Psychologist 53 11 1195 1198 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 53 11 1195 Einstein Albert 1954 A Mathematician s Mind Ideas and Opinions New York Bonanza Books p 25 Sternberg R J 1995 Conceptions of expertise in complex problem solving A comparison of alternative conceptions In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 295 321 Funke J 1991 Solving complex problems Human identification and control of complex systems In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 185 222 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Broadbent Donald E 1977 Levels hierarchies and the locus of control Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 29 2 181 201 doi 10 1080 14640747708400596 S2CID 144328372 Berry Dianne C Broadbent Donald E 1995 Implicit learning in the control of complex systems A reconsideration of some of the earlier claims In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 131 150 Dorner Dietrich 1975 Wie Menschen eine Welt verbessern wollten How people wanted to improve the world Bild der Wissenschaft in German 12 48 53 Dorner Dietrich 1985 Verhalten Denken und Emotionen Behavior thinking and emotions In Eckensberger L H Lantermann E D eds Emotion und Reflexivitat in German Munchen Germany Urban amp Schwarzenberg pp 157 181 Dorner Dietrich Wearing Alex J 1995 Complex problem solving Toward a computer simulated theory In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 65 99 Buchner A 1995 Theories of complex problem solving In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 27 63 Dorner D Kreuzig H W Reither F Staudel T eds 1983 Lohhausen Vom Umgang mit Unbestimmtheit und Komplexitat Lohhausen On dealing with uncertainty and complexity in German Bern Switzerland Hans Huber Ringelband O J Misiak C Kluwe R H 1990 Mental models and strategies in the control of a complex system In Ackermann D Tauber M J eds Mental models and human computer interaction Vol 1 Amsterdam Elsevier Science Publishers pp 151 164 Anzai K Simon H A 1979 The theory of learning by doing Psychological Review 86 2 124 140 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 86 2 124 PMID 493441 Bhaskar R Simon Herbert A 1977 Problem Solving in Semantically Rich Domains An Example from Engineering Thermodynamics Cognitive Science 1 2 Wiley 193 215 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog0102 3 ISSN 0364 0213 e g Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds 1991 Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Chase W G Simon H A 1973 Perception in chess Cognitive Psychology 4 55 81 doi 10 1016 0010 0285 73 90004 2 Chi M T H Feltovich P J Glaser R 1981 Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices Cognitive Science 5 2 121 152 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog0502 2 Anderson J R Boyle C B Reiser B J 1985 Intelligent tutoring systems Science 228 4698 456 462 Bibcode 1985Sci 228 456A doi 10 1126 science 228 4698 456 PMID 17746875 S2CID 62403455 Sokol S M McCloskey M 1991 Cognitive mechanisms in calculation In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 85 116 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Kay D S 1991 Computer interaction Debugging the problems In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 317 340 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Frensch P A Sternberg R J 1991 Skill related differences in game playing In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 343 381 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Amsel E Langer R Loutzenhiser L 1991 Do lawyers reason differently from psychologists A comparative design for studying expertise In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 223 250 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Wagner R K 1991 Managerial problem solving In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 159 183 PsycNET 1991 98396 005 Polya George 1945 How to Solve It Princeton University Press Schoenfeld A H 1985 Mathematical Problem Solving Orlando Fla Academic Press ISBN 978 1 4832 9548 0 Hegarty M 1991 Knowledge and processes in mechanical problem solving In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 253 285 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Heppner P P Krauskopf C J 1987 An information processing approach to personal problem solving The Counseling Psychologist 15 3 371 447 doi 10 1177 0011000087153001 S2CID 146180007 Voss J F Wolfe C R Lawrence J A Engle R A 1991 From representation to decision An analysis of problem solving in international relations In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 119 158 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 PsycNET 1991 98396 004 Lesgold A Lajoie S 1991 Complex problem solving in electronics In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 287 316 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Altshuller Genrich 1994 And Suddenly the Inventor Appeared Translated by Lev Shulyak Worcester Mass Technical Innovation Center ISBN 978 0 9640740 1 9 Stanovich K E Cunningham A E 1991 Reading as constrained reasoning In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 3 60 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Bryson M Bereiter C Scardamalia M Joram E 1991 Going beyond the problem as given Problem solving in expert and novice writers In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 61 84 ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds 1991 Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates ISBN 0 8058 0650 4 OCLC 23254443 Hung Woei 2013 Team based complex problem solving a collective cognition perspective Educational Technology Research and Development 61 3 365 384 doi 10 1007 s11423 013 9296 3 S2CID 62663840 Jewett Pamela MacPhee Deborah 2012 Adding Collaborative Peer Coaching to Our Teaching Identities The Reading Teacher 66 2 105 110 doi 10 1002 TRTR 01089 Wang Qiyun 2009 Design and Evaluation of a Collaborative Learning Environment Computers and Education 53 4 1138 1146 doi 10 1016 j compedu 2009 05 023 Wang Qiyan 2010 Using online shared workspaces to support group collaborative learning Computers and Education 55 3 1270 1276 doi 10 1016 j compedu 2010 05 023 Kai Wai Chu Samuel Kennedy David M 2011 Using Online Collaborative tools for groups to Co Construct Knowledge Online Information Review 35 4 581 597 doi 10 1108 14684521111161945 ISSN 1468 4527 S2CID 206388086 Legare Cristine Mills Candice Souza Andre Plummer Leigh Yasskin Rebecca 2013 The use of questions as problem solving strategies during early childhood Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114 1 63 7 doi 10 1016 j jecp 2012 07 002 PMID 23044374 Engelbart Douglas 1962 Team Cooperation Augmenting Human Intellect A Conceptual Framework Vol AFOSR 3223 Stanford Research Institute Flew Terry 2008 New Media an introduction Melbourne Oxford University Press Henry Jenkins Interactive audiences The collective intelligence of media fans PDF Archived from the original PDF on April 26 2018 Retrieved December 11 2016 Finger Matthias 2008 03 27 Which governance for sustainable development An organizational and institutional perspective In Park Jacob Conca Ken Finger Matthias eds The Crisis of Global Environmental Governance Towards a New Political Economy of Sustainability Routledge p 48 ISBN 978 1 134 05982 9 Guazzini Andrea Vilone Daniele Donati Camillo Nardi Annalisa Levnajic Zoran 10 November 2015 Modeling crowdsourcing as collective problem solving Scientific Reports 5 16557 arXiv 1506 09155 Bibcode 2015NatSR 516557G doi 10 1038 srep16557 PMC 4639727 PMID 26552943 Boroomand A Smaldino P E 2021 Hard Work Risk Taking and Diversity in a Model of Collective Problem Solving Journal of Artificial Societies and Social Simulation 24 4 doi 10 18564 jasss 4704 S2CID 240483312 Stefanovitch Nicolas Alshamsi Aamena Cebrian Manuel Rahwan Iyad 30 September 2014 Error and attack tolerance of collective problem solving The DARPA Shredder Challenge EPJ Data Science 3 1 doi 10 1140 epjds s13688 014 0013 1 hdl 21 11116 0000 0002 D39F D Further reading editBeckmann Jens F Guthke Jurgen 1995 Complex problem solving intelligence and learning ability In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 177 200 Brehmer Berndt 1995 Feedback delays in dynamic decision making In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 103 130 Brehmer Berndt Dorner D 1993 Experiments with computer simulated microworlds Escaping both the narrow straits of the laboratory and the deep blue sea of the field study Computers in Human Behavior 9 2 3 171 184 doi 10 1016 0747 5632 93 90005 D Dorner D 1992 Uber die Philosophie der Verwendung von Mikrowelten oder Computerszenarios in der psychologischen Forschung On the proper use of microworlds or computer scenarios in psychological research In Gundlach H ed Psychologische Forschung und Methode Das Versprechen des Experiments Festschrift fur Werner Traxel in German Passau Germany Passavia Universitats Verlag pp 53 87 Eyferth K Schomann M Widowski D 1986 Der Umgang von Psychologen mit Komplexitat On how psychologists deal with complexity Sprache amp Kognition in German 5 11 26 Funke Joachim 1993 Microworlds based on linear equation systems A new approach to complex problem solving and experimental results PDF In Strube G Wender K F eds The cognitive psychology of knowledge Amsterdam Elsevier Science Publishers pp 313 330 Funke Joachim 1995 Experimental research on complex problem solving PDF In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 243 268 Funke U 1995 Complex problem solving in personnel selection and training In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 219 240 Groner M Groner R Bischof W F 1983 Approaches to heuristics A historical review In Groner R Groner M Bischof W F eds Methods of heuristics Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 1 18 Hayes J 1980 The complete problem solver Philadelphia The Franklin Institute Press Huber O 1995 Complex problem solving as multistage decision making In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 151 173 Hubner Ronald 1989 Methoden zur Analyse und Konstruktion von Aufgaben zur kognitiven Steuerung dynamischer Systeme Methods for the analysis and construction of dynamic system control tasks PDF Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie in German 36 221 238 Hunt Earl 1991 Some comments on the study of complexity In Sternberg R J Frensch P A eds Complex problem solving Principles and mechanisms Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 383 395 ISBN 978 1 317 78386 2 Hussy W 1985 Komplexes Problemlosen Eine Sackgasse Complex problem solving a dead end Zeitschrift fur Experimentelle und Angewandte Psychologie in German 32 55 77 Kluwe R H 1993 Chapter 19 Knowledge and Performance in Complex Problem Solving The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge Advances in Psychology Vol 101 pp 401 423 doi 10 1016 S0166 4115 08 62668 0 ISBN 978 0 444 89942 2 Kluwe R H 1995 Single case studies and models of complex problem solving In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 269 291 Kolb S Petzing F Stumpf S 1992 Komplexes Problemlosen Bestimmung der Problemlosegute von Probanden mittels Verfahren des Operations Research ein interdisziplinarer Ansatz Complex problem solving determining the quality of human problem solving by operations research tools an interdisciplinary approach Sprache amp Kognition in German 11 115 128 Krems Josef F 1995 Cognitive flexibility and complex problem solving In Frensch P A Funke J eds Complex problem solving The European Perspective Hillsdale N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates pp 201 218 Melzak Z 1983 Bypasses A Simple Approach to Complexity London UK Wiley Muller H 1993 Komplexes Problemlosen Reliabilitat und Wissen Complex problem solving Reliability and knowledge in German Bonn Germany Holos Paradies M W Unger L W 2000 TapRooT The System for Root Cause Analysis Problem Investigation and Proactive Improvement Knoxville Tenn System Improvements Putz Osterloh Wiebke 1993 Chapter 15 Strategies for Knowledge Acquisition and Transfer of Knowledge in Dynamic Tasks The Cognitive Psychology of Knowledge Advances in Psychology Vol 101 pp 331 350 doi 10 1016 S0166 4115 08 62664 3 ISBN 978 0 444 89942 2 Riefer David M Batchelder William H 1988 Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes PDF Psychological Review 95 3 318 339 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 95 3 318 S2CID 14994393 Archived from the original PDF on 2018 11 25 Schaub H 1993 Modellierung der Handlungsorganisation in German Bern Switzerland Hans Huber Strauss B 1993 Konfundierungen beim Komplexen Problemlosen Zum Einfluss des Anteils der richtigen Losungen ArL auf das Problemloseverhalten in komplexen Situationen Confoundations in complex problem solving On the influence of the degree of correct solutions on problem solving in complex situations in German Bonn Germany Holos Strohschneider S 1991 Kein System von Systemen Kommentar zu dem Aufsatz Systemmerkmale als Determinanten des Umgangs mit dynamischen Systemen von Joachim Funke No system of systems Reply to the paper System features as determinants of behavior in dynamic task environments by Joachim Funke Sprache amp Kognition in German 10 109 113 Tonelli Marcello 2011 Unstructured Processes of Strategic Decision Making Saarbrucken Germany Lambert Academic Publishing ISBN 978 3 8465 5598 9 Van Lehn Kurt 1989 Problem solving and cognitive skill acquisition In Posner M I ed Foundations of cognitive science PDF Cambridge Mass MIT Press pp 527 579 Wisconsin Educational Media Association 1993 Information literacy A position paper on information problem solving WEMA Publications vol ED 376 817 Madison Wis a href Template Citation html title Template Citation citation a CS1 maint location missing publisher link Portions adapted from Michigan State Board of Education s Position Paper on Information Processing Skills 1992 External links edit nbsp Learning materials related to Solving Problems at Wikiversity Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Problem solving amp oldid 1216632140, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.