fbpx
Wikipedia

Dream

A dream is a succession of images, ideas, emotions, and sensations that usually occur involuntarily in the mind during certain stages of sleep.[1] Humans spend about two hours dreaming per night,[2] and each dream lasts around 5 to 20 minutes, although the dreamer may perceive the dream as being much longer than this.[3]

A fantasy painting of a dream, showing a man walking on a road under the starry sky

The content and function of dreams have been topics of scientific, philosophical and religious interest throughout recorded history. Dream interpretation, practiced by the Babylonians in the third millennium BCE[4] and even earlier by the ancient Sumerians,[5][6] figures prominently in religious texts in several traditions, and has played a lead role in psychotherapy.[7][8] The scientific study of dreams is called oneirology.[9] Most modern dream study focuses on the neurophysiology of dreams and on proposing and testing hypotheses regarding dream function. It is not known where in the brain dreams originate, if there is a single origin for dreams or if multiple regions of the brain are involved, or what the purpose of dreaming is for the body or mind.

The human dream experience and what to make of it has undergone sizable shifts over the course of history.[10][11] Long ago, according to writings from Mesopotamia and Ancient Egypt, dreams dictated post-dream behaviors to an extent sharply reduced in later millennia. These ancient writings about dreams highlight visitation dreams, where a dream figure, usually a deity or a prominent forebear, commands the dreamer to take specific actions and may predict future events.[12][13][14] Framing the dream experience varies across cultures as well as through time.

Dreaming and sleep are intertwined. Dreams occur mainly in the rapid-eye movement (REM) stage of sleep—when brain activity is high and resembles that of being awake. Because REM sleep is detectable in many species, and because research suggests that all mammals experience REM,[15] linking dreams to REM sleep has led to conjectures that animals dream. However, humans dream during non-REM sleep, also, and not all REM awakenings elicit dream reports.[16] To be studied, a dream must first be reduced to a verbal report, which is an account of the subject's memory of the dream, not the subject's dream experience itself. So, dreaming by non-humans is currently unprovable, as is dreaming by human fetuses and pre-verbal infants.[17]

Subjective experience and content

 
Usha Dreaming Aniruddha (oleographic print) Raja Ravi Varma (1848–1906)

Preserved writings from early Mediterranean civilizations indicate a relatively abrupt change in subjective dream experience between Bronze Age antiquity and the beginnings of the classical era.[18]

In visitation dreams reported in ancient writings, dreamers were largely passive in their dreams, and visual content served primarily to frame authoritative auditory messaging.[19][10][20] Gudea, the king of the Sumerian city-state of Lagash (reigned c. 2144–2124 BCE), rebuilt the temple of Ningirsu as the result of a dream in which he was told to do so.[6] After antiquity, the passive hearing of visitation dreams largely gave way to visualized narratives in which the dreamer becomes a character who actively participates.

From the 1940s to 1985, Calvin S. Hall collected more than 50,000 dream reports at Western Reserve University. In 1966, Hall and Robert Van de Castle published The Content Analysis of Dreams, in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.[21] Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the world demonstrated similarity in their dream content. The only residue of antiquity's authoritative dream figure in the Hall and Van de Castle listing of dream characters is the inclusion of God in the category of prominent persons.[22] Hall's complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid-1990s by his protégé William Domhoff. More recent studies of dream reports, while providing more detail, continue to cite the Hall study favorably.[23]

 
A soldier dreams: the trenches of WWI. Jan Styka (1858–1925).

In the Hall study, the most common emotion experienced in dreams was anxiety. Other emotions included abandonment, anger, fear, joy, and happiness. Negative emotions were much more common than positive ones.[21] The Hall data analysis showed that sexual dreams occur no more than 10% of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid-teens.[21] Another study showed that 8% of both men's and women's dreams have sexual content.[24] In some cases, sexual dreams may result in orgasms or nocturnal emissions. These are colloquially known as "wet dreams".[25]

The visual nature of dreams is generally highly phantasmagoric; that is, different locations and objects continuously blend into each other. The visuals (including locations, people, and objects) are generally reflective of a person's memories and experiences, but conversation can take on highly exaggerated and bizarre forms. Some dreams may even tell elaborate stories wherein the dreamer enters entirely new, complex worlds and awakes with ideas, thoughts and feelings never experienced prior to the dream.

People who are blind from birth do not have visual dreams. Their dream contents are related to other senses like hearing, touch, smell and taste, whichever are present since birth.[26]

Neurophysiology

Dream study is popular with scientists exploring the mind–brain problem. Some "propose to reduce aspects of dream phenomenology to neurobiology."[27] But current science cannot specify dream physiology in detail. Protocols in most nations restrict human brain research to non-invasive procedures. In the United States, invasive brain procedures with a human subject are allowed only when these are deemed necessary in surgical treatment to address medical needs of the same human subject.[28] Non-invasive measures of brain activity like electroencephalogram (EEG) voltage averaging or cerebral blood flow cannot identify small but influential neuronal populations.[29] Also, fMRI signals are too slow to explain how brains compute in real time.[30]

Scientists researching some brain functions can work around current restrictions by examining animal subjects. As stated by the Society for Neuroscience, "Because no adequate alternatives exist, much of this research must [sic] be done on animal subjects."[31] However, since animal dreaming can be only inferred, not confirmed, animal studies yield no hard facts to illuminate the neurophysiology of dreams. Examining human subjects with brain lesions can provide clues, but the lesion method cannot discriminate between the effects of destruction and disconnection and cannot target specific neuronal groups in heterogeneous regions like the brain stem.[29]

Generation

Denied precision tools, obliged to depend on imaging, much dream research has succumbed to the law of the instrument. Studies detect an increase of blood flow in a specific brain region and then credit that region with a role in generating dreams. But pooling study results has led to the newer conclusion that dreaming involves large numbers of regions and pathways, which likely are different for different dream events.[32]

Image creation in the brain involves significant neural activity downstream from eye intake, and it is theorized that "the visual imagery of dreams is produced by activation during sleep of the same structures that generate complex visual imagery in waking perception."[33]

Dreams present a running narrative rather than exclusively visual imagery. Following their work with split-brain subjects, Gazzaniga and LeDoux postulated, without attempting to specify the neural mechanisms, a "left-brain interpreter" that seeks to create a plausible narrative from whatever electro-chemical signals reach the brain's left hemisphere. Sleep research has determined that some brain regions fully active during waking are, during REM sleep, activated only in a partial or fragmentary way.[34] Drawing on this knowledge, textbook author James W. Kalat explains, "[A] dream represents the brain's effort to make sense of sparse and distorted information.... The cortex combines this haphazard input with whatever other activity was already occurring and does its best to synthesize a story that makes sense of the information."[35] Neuroscientist Indre Viskontas is even more blunt, calling often bizarre dream content "just the result of your interpreter trying to create a story out of random neural signaling."[36]

Theories on function

For humans in the pre-classical era, and continuing for some non-literate populations into modern times, dreams are believed to have functioned as revealers of truths sourced during sleep from gods or other external entities.[37][11] Ancient Egyptians believed that dreams were the best way to receive divine revelation, and thus they would induce (or "incubate") dreams. They went to sanctuaries and slept on special "dream beds" in hope of receiving advice, comfort, or healing from the gods.[14] From a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement, provide some benefit for natural selection to take place, or at least have no negative impact on fitness. Robert (1886),[38] a physician from Hamburg, was the first who suggested that dreams are a need and that they have the function to erase (a) sensory impressions that were not fully worked up, and (b) ideas that were not fully developed during the day. In dreams, incomplete material is either removed (suppressed) or deepened and included into memory. Freud, whose dream studies focused on interpreting dreams, not explaining how or why humans dream, disputed Robert's hypothesis[39] and proposed that dreams preserve sleep by representing as fulfilled those wishes that otherwise would awaken the dreamer.[40] Freud wrote that dreams "serve the purpose of prolonging sleep instead of waking up. Dreams are the GUARDIANS of sleep and not its disturbers."[41]

 
Grandmother and Granddaughter Dream (1839 or 1840). Taras Shevchenko

A turning point in theorizing about dream function came in 1953, when Science published the Aserinsky and Kleitman paper[42] establishing REM sleep as a distinct phase of sleep and linking dreams to REM sleep.[43] Until and even after publication of the Solms 2000 paper that certified the separability of REM sleep and dream phenomena,[16] many studies purporting to uncover the function of dreams have in fact been studying not dreams but measurable REM sleep.

Theories of dream function since the identification of REM sleep include:

Hobson's and McCarley's 1977 activation-synthesis hypothesis, which proposed "a functional role for dreaming sleep in promoting some aspect of the learning process...."[44] In 2010 a Harvard study was published showing experimental evidence that dreams were correlated with improved learning.[45]

Crick's and Mitchison's 1983 "reverse learning" theory, which states that dreams are like the cleaning-up operations of computers when they are offline, removing (suppressing) parasitic nodes and other "junk" from the mind during sleep.[46][47]

Hartmann's 1995 proposal that dreams serve a "quasi-therapeutic" function, enabling the dreamer to process trauma in a safe place.[48]

Revonsuo's 2000 threat simulation hypothesis, whose premise is that during much of human evolution, physical and interpersonal threats were serious, giving reproductive advantage to those who survived them. Dreaming aided survival by replicating these threats and providing the dreamer with practice in dealing with them.[49]

Eagleman's and Vaughn's 2021 defensive activation theory, which says that, given the brain's neuroplasticity, dreams evolved as a visual hallucinatory activity during sleep's extended periods of darkness, busying the occipital lobe and thereby protecting it from possible appropriation by other, non-vision, sense operations.[50]

Erik Hoel proposes, based on artificial neural networks, that dreams prevent overfitting to past experiences; that is, they enable the dreamer to learn from novel situations.[51][52]

Religious and other cultural contexts

Dreams figure prominently in major world religions. The dream experience for early humans, according to one interpretation, gave rise to the notion of a human "soul,"[53] a central element in much religious thought. J. W. Dunne wrote:

But there can be no reasonable doubt that the idea of a soul must have first arisen in the mind of primitive man as a result of observation of his dreams. Ignorant as he was, he could have come to no other conclusion but that, in dreams, he left his sleeping body in one universe and went wandering off into another. It is considered that, but for that savage, the idea of such a thing as a 'soul' would never have even occurred to mankind....[54]

Hindu

In the Mandukya Upanishad, part of the Veda scriptures of Indian Hinduism, a dream is one of three states that the soul experiences during its lifetime, the other two states being the waking state and the sleep state.[55] The earliest Upanishads, written before 300 BCE, emphasize two meanings of dreams. The first says that dreams are merely expressions of inner desires. The second is the belief of the soul leaving the body and being guided until awakened.

Abrahamic

 
Jacob's dream of a ladder of angels, c. 1690. Michael Willmann

In Judaism, dreams are considered part of the experience of the world that can be interpreted and from which lessons can be garnered. It is discussed in the Talmud, Tractate Berachot 55–60.

The ancient Hebrews connected their dreams heavily with their religion, though the Hebrews were monotheistic and believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone. Hebrews also differentiated between good dreams (from God) and bad dreams (from evil spirits). The Hebrews, like many other ancient cultures, incubated dreams in order to receive a divine revelation. For example, the Hebrew prophet Samuel would "lie down and sleep in the temple at Shiloh before the Ark and receive the word of the Lord". Most of the dreams in the Bible are in the Book of Genesis.[56]

Christians mostly shared the beliefs of the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural character because the Old Testament includes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration. The most famous of these dream stories was Jacob's dream of a ladder that stretches from Earth to Heaven. Many Christians preach that God can speak to people through their dreams. The famous glossary, the Somniale Danielis, written in the name of Daniel, attempted to teach Christian populations to interpret their dreams.

Iain R. Edgar has researched the role of dreams in Islam.[57] He has argued that dreams play an important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims, since dream interpretation is the only way that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the death of the last prophet, Muhammad.[58] According to Edgar, Islam classifies three types of dreams. Firstly, there is the true dream (al-ru’ya), then the false dream, which may come from the devil (shaytan), and finally, the meaningless everyday dream (hulm). This last dream could be brought forth by the dreamer's ego or base appetite based on what they experienced in the real world. The true dream is often indicated by Islam's hadith tradition.[58] In one narration by Aisha, the wife of the Prophet, it is said that the Prophet's dreams would come true like the ocean's waves.[58] Just as in its predecessors, the Quran also recounts the story of Joseph and his unique ability to interpret dreams.[58]

Buddhist

In Buddhism, ideas about dreams are similar to the classical and folk traditions in South Asia. The same dream is sometimes experienced by multiple people, as in the case of the Buddha-to-be, before he is leaving his home. It is described in the Mahāvastu that several of the Buddha's relatives had premonitory dreams preceding this. Some dreams are also seen to transcend time: the Buddha-to-be has certain dreams that are the same as those of previous Buddhas, the Lalitavistara states. In Buddhist literature, dreams often function as a "signpost" motif to mark certain stages in the life of the main character.[59]

Buddhist views about dreams are expressed in the Pāli Commentaries and the Milinda Pañhā.[59]

Other

 
Dreaming of the Tiger Spring (虎跑夢泉) Statue at Hupao Spring (Hupaomengquan) in Hangzhou, Zhejiang, China.

In Chinese history, people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body during slumber to journey in a dream realm, while the other remained in the body.[60] This belief and dream interpretation had been questioned since early times, such as by the philosopher Wang Chong (27–97 CE).[60]

The Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into "good," which were sent by the gods, and "bad," sent by demons.[61] A surviving collection of dream omens entitled Iškar Zaqīqu records various dream scenarios as well as prognostications of what will happen to the person who experiences each dream, apparently based on previous cases.[6][62] Some list different possible outcomes, based on occasions in which people experienced similar dreams with different results.[6] The Greeks shared their beliefs with the Egyptians on how to interpret good and bad dreams, and the idea of incubating dreams. Morpheus, the Greek god of dreams, also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept at shrines and temples. The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically visited the dreamers, where they entered through a keyhole, exiting the same way after the divine message was given.

Antiphon wrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BCE. In that century, other cultures influenced Greeks to develop the belief that souls left the sleeping body.[63] Hippocrates (469–399 BCE) had a simple dream theory: during the day, the soul receives images; during the night, it produces images. Greek philosopher Aristotle (384–322 BCE) believed dreams caused physiological activity. He thought dreams could analyze illness and predict diseases. Marcus Tullius Cicero, for his part, believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a dreamer had during the preceding days.[64] Cicero's Somnium Scipionis described a lengthy dream vision, which in turn was commented on by Macrobius in his Commentarii in Somnium Scipionis.

Herodotus in his The Histories, writes "The visions that occur to us in dreams are, more often than not, the things we have been concerned about during the day."[65]

The Dreaming is a common term within the animist creation narrative of indigenous Australians for a personal, or group, creation and for what may be understood as the "timeless time" of formative creation and perpetual creating.[66]

Some Indigenous American tribes and Mexican populations believe that dreams are a way of visiting and having contact with their ancestors.[67] Some Native American tribes have used vision quests as a rite of passage, fasting and praying until an anticipated guiding dream was received, to be shared with the rest of the tribe upon their return.[68][69]

Interpretation

 
Joseph Interprets Pharaoh's Dream c. 1896–1902. Jacques Joseph Tissot (1836–1902).

Beginning in the late 19th century, Austrian neurologist Sigmund Freud, founder of psychoanalysis, theorized that dreams reflect the dreamer's unconscious mind and specifically that dream content is shaped by unconscious wish fulfillment. He argued that important unconscious desires often relate to early childhood memories and experiences.[7] Carl Jung and others expanded on Freud's idea that dream content reflects the dreamer's unconscious desires.

Dream interpretation can be a result of subjective ideas and experiences. One study found that most people believe that "their dreams reveal meaningful hidden truths".[70] The researchers surveyed students in the United States, South Korea, and India, and found that 74% of Indians, 65% of South Koreans and 56% of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight into their unconscious beliefs and desires. This Freudian view of dreaming was believed significantly more than theories of dreaming that attribute dream content to memory consolidation, problem-solving, or as a byproduct of unrelated brain activity. The same study found that people attribute more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake. Americans were more likely to report that they would intentionally miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flying (while awake), and that they would be as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the night before their flight as if there was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to take. Participants in the study were more likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake. They were more likely to view a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream about someone they disliked, for example, and were more likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as meaningful than a negative dream about a person they liked.

According to surveys, it is common for people to feel their dreams are predicting subsequent life events.[71] Psychologists have explained these experiences in terms of memory biases, namely a selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively fitted onto life experiences.[71] The multi-faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to find connections between dream content and real events.[72] The term "veridical dream" has been used to indicate dreams that reveal or contain truths not yet known to the dreamer, whether future events or secrets.[73]

In one experiment, subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary. This prevented the selective memory effect, and the dreams no longer seemed accurate about the future.[74] Another experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with apparently precognitive dreams. This diary described events from the person's life, as well as some predictive dreams and some non-predictive dreams. When subjects were asked to recall the dreams they had read, they remembered more of the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones.[75]

Images and literature

Graphic artists, writers and filmmakers all have found dreams to offer a rich vein for creative expression. In the West, artists' depictions of dreams in Renaissance and Baroque art often were related to Biblical narrative. Especially preferred by visual artists were the Jacob's Ladder dream in Genesis and St. Joseph's dreams in the Gospel according to Matthew.

Many later graphic artists have depicted dreams, including Japanese woodblock artist Hokusai (1760–1849) and Western European painters Rousseau (1844–1910), Picasso (1881–1973), and Dali (1904–1989).

In literature, dream frames were frequently used in medieval allegory to justify the narrative; The Book of the Duchess[76] and The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman[77] are two such dream visions. Even before them, in antiquity, the same device had been used by Cicero and Lucian of Samosata.

 
The cheshire cat, John Tenniel (1820–1914), illustration in Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, 1866 edition.

Dreams have also featured in fantasy and speculative fiction since the 19th century. One of the best-known dream worlds is Wonderland from Lewis Carroll's Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, as well as Looking-Glass Land from its sequel, Through the Looking-Glass. Unlike many dream worlds, Carroll's logic is like that of actual dreams, with transitions and flexible causality.

Other fictional dream worlds include the Dreamlands of H. P. Lovecraft's Dream Cycle[78] and The Neverending Story's[79] world of Fantastica, which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams, the Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness. Dreamworlds, shared hallucinations and other alternate realities feature in a number of works by Philip K. Dick, such as The Three Stigmata of Palmer Eldritch and Ubik. Similar themes were explored by Jorge Luis Borges, for instance in The Circular Ruins.

Modern popular culture often conceives of dreams, as did Freud, as expressions of the dreamer's deepest fears and desires.[80] In speculative fiction, the line between dreams and reality may be blurred even more in service to the story.[81] Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated (Dreamscape, 1984; the Nightmare on Elm Street films, 1984–2010; Inception, 2010) or even come literally true (as in The Lathe of Heaven, 1971).[80]

Lucidity

Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one's state while dreaming. In this state the dreamer may often have some degree of control over their own actions within the dream or even the characters and the environment of the dream. Dream control has been reported to improve with practiced deliberate lucid dreaming, but the ability to control aspects of the dream is not necessary for a dream to qualify as "lucid"—a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they are dreaming.[82] The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified.[83]

"Oneironaut" is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream.

In 1975, psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a communication from a dreamer experiencing a lucid dream. On April 12, 1975, after agreeing to move his eyes left and right upon becoming lucid, the subject and Hearne's co-author on the resulting article, Alan Worsley, successfully carried out this task.[84] Years later, psychophysiologist Stephen LaBerge conducted similar work including:

  • Using eye signals to map the subjective sense of time in dreams.
  • Comparing the electrical activity of the brain while singing awake and while dreaming.
  • Studies comparing in-dream sex, arousal, and orgasm.[85]

Communication between two dreamers has also been documented. The processes involved included EEG monitoring, ocular signaling, incorporation of reality in the form of red light stimuli and a coordinating website. The website tracked when both dreamers were dreaming and sent the stimulus to one of the dreamers where it was incorporated into the dream. This dreamer, upon becoming lucid, signaled with eye movements; this was detected by the website whereupon the stimulus was sent to the second dreamer, invoking incorporation into that dreamer's dream.[86]

Recollection

 
Raphael's dream (1821). Johannes Riepenhausen and Franz Riepenhausen.

The recollection of dreams is extremely unreliable, though it is a skill that can be trained. Dreams can usually be recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming.[87] Women tend to have more frequent dream recall than men.[87] Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized by relatively little affect, and factors such as salience, arousal, and interference play a role in dream recall. Often, a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus. The salience hypothesis proposes that dream content that is salient, that is, novel, intense, or unusual, is more easily remembered. There is considerable evidence that vivid, intense, or unusual dream content is more frequently recalled.[88] A dream journal can be used to assist dream recall, for personal interest or psychotherapy purposes.

Adults report remembering around two dreams per week, on average.[89][90] Unless a dream is particularly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it, the content of the dream is typically not remembered.[91] Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall. Using the permitted non-invasive technologies, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electromyography (EMG), researchers have been able to identify basic dream imagery,[92] dream speech activity[93] and dream motor behavior (such as walking and hand movements).[94][95]

In line with the salience hypothesis, there is considerable evidence that people who have more vivid, intense or unusual dreams show better recall. There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is related to recall. Specifically, people who have vivid and unusual experiences during the day tend to have more memorable dream content and hence better dream recall. People who score high on measures of personality traits associated with creativity, imagination, and fantasy, such as openness to experience, daydreaming, fantasy proneness, absorption, and hypnotic susceptibility, tend to show more frequent dream recall.[88] There is also evidence for continuity between the bizarre aspects of dreaming and waking experience. That is, people who report more bizarre experiences during the day, such as people high in schizotypy (psychosis proneness), have more frequent dream recall and also report more frequent nightmares.[88]

Miscellany

Illusion of reality

Some philosophers have proposed that what we think of as the "real world" could be or is an illusion (an idea known as the skeptical hypothesis about ontology). The first recorded mention of the idea was in the 4th century BCE by Zhuangzi, and in Eastern philosophy, the problem has been named the "Zhuangzi Paradox."

He who dreams of drinking wine may weep when morning comes; he who dreams of weeping may in the morning go off to hunt. While he is dreaming he does not know it is a dream, and in his dream he may even try to interpret a dream. Only after he wakes does he know it was a dream. And someday there will be a great awakening when we know that this is all a great dream. Yet the stupid believe they are awake, busily and brightly assuming they understand things, calling this man ruler, that one herdsman—how dense! Confucius and you are both dreaming! And when I say you are dreaming, I am dreaming, too. Words like these will be labeled the Supreme Swindle. Yet, after ten thousand generations, a great sage may appear who will know their meaning, and it will still be as though he appeared with astonishing speed.[96]

The idea also is discussed in Hindu and Buddhist writings.[97] It was formally introduced to Western philosophy by Descartes in the 17th century in his Meditations on First Philosophy.

Absent-minded transgression

Dreams of absent-minded transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absent-mindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop (one classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking with intense feelings of guilt. One study found a positive association between having these dreams and successfully stopping the behavior.[98]

Non-REM dreams

Hypnogogic and hypnopompic dreams, dreamlike states shortly after falling asleep and shortly before awakening, and dreams during stage 2 of NREM-sleep, also occur, but are shorter than REM-dreams.[99][100]

Daydreams

 
Dante Meditating, 1852, by Joseph Noel Paton.

A daydream is a visionary fantasy, especially one of happy, pleasant thoughts, hopes or ambitions, imagined as coming to pass, and experienced while awake.[101] There are many different types of daydreams, and there is no consistent definition amongst psychologists.[101] The general public also uses the term for a broad variety of experiences. Research by Harvard psychologist Deirdre Barrett has found that people who experience vivid dreamlike mental images reserve the word for these, whereas many other people refer to milder imagery, realistic future planning, review of memories or just "spacing out"—i.e. one's mind going relatively blank—when they talk about "daydreaming".[102][103]

While daydreaming has long been derided as a lazy, non-productive pastime, it is now commonly acknowledged that daydreaming can be constructive in some contexts.[104] There are numerous examples of people in creative or artistic careers, such as composers, novelists and filmmakers, developing new ideas through daydreaming. Similarly, research scientists, mathematicians and physicists have developed new ideas by daydreaming about their subject areas.

Hallucination

A hallucination, in the broadest sense of the word, is a perception in the absence of a stimulus. In a stricter sense, hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state, in the absence of external stimuli, and have qualities of real perception, in that they are vivid, substantial, and located in external objective space. The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related phenomena of dreaming, which does not involve wakefulness.

Nightmare

 
Woman having a nightmare. Jean-Pierre Simon (1764–1810 or 1813).

A nightmare is an unpleasant dream that can cause a strong negative emotional response from the mind, typically fear or horror, but also despair, anxiety and great sadness. The dream may contain situations of danger, discomfort, psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a state of distress and may be unable to return to sleep for a prolonged period of time.[105]

Night terror

A night terror, also known as a sleep terror or pavor nocturnus, is a parasomnia disorder that predominantly affects children, causing feelings of terror or dread. Night terrors should not be confused with nightmares, which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear.[106]

Déjà vu

One theory of déjà vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to having dreamed about a similar situation or place, and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or the place while awake.[107]

See also

References

  1. ^ "Dream". The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language, Fourth Edition. 2000. Retrieved 7 May 2009.
  2. ^ "Brain Basics: Understanding Sleep". National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. 2006. from the original on 11 October 2007. Retrieved 16 December 2007.
  3. ^ Lee Ann Obringer (2006). How Dream Works. from the original on 18 April 2006. Retrieved 4 May 2006.
  4. ^ Krippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, Andre Percia de (2002). Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. p. 9. ISBN 0-7914-5257-3. Clay tablets have been found, dating to about 2500 B.C.E., that contain interpretive material for Babylonian and Assyrian dreamers.
  5. ^ Seligman, K (1948). Magic, Supernaturalism and Religion. New York: Random House.
  6. ^ a b c d Black, Jeremy; Green, Anthony (1992). Gods, Demons and Symbols of Ancient Mesopotamia: An Illustrated Dictionary. Austin: University of Texas Press. pp. 71–72, 89–90. ISBN 0714117056.
  7. ^ a b Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon.
  8. ^ Schredl, Michael; Bohusch, Claudia; Kahl, Johanna; Mader, Andrea; Somesan, Alexandra (2000). "The Use of Dreams in Psychotherapy". The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research. 9 (2): 81–87.
  9. ^ Kavanau, J.L. (2000). "Sleep, memory maintenance, and mental disorders". Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 12 (2): 199–208. doi:10.1176/jnp.12.2.199. PMID 11001598.
  10. ^ a b Dodds, E. R. (1951). The Greeks and the Irrational. Berkeley: University of California Press. p. 105. The Greeks never spoke as we do of having a dream, but always of seeing a dream....
  11. ^ a b Packer, Sharon (2002). Dreams in Myth, Medicine, and Movies. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. p. 85. ISBN 0-275-97243-7. …[M]any more ancient cultures think that dreams are imposed by a force that resides outside the individual.
  12. ^ Macrobius (1952) [430]. Commentary on the Dream of Scipio. Translated by W. H. Stahl. New York: Columbia University Press. p. 90. We call a dream oracular in which a parent, or a pious or revered man, or a priest, or even a god clearly reveals what will or will not transpire, and what action to take or to avoid.
  13. ^ Dodds (1951), referring to the type of dream described by Macrobius: "This last type is not, I think, at all common in our own dream-experience. But there is considerable evidence that dreams of this sort were familiar in antiquity." (p. 107).
  14. ^ a b Krippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, André Percia de (2002). Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them. Albany: State University of New York Press. p. 10. ISBN 0-7914-5257-3. The Egyptian papyrus of Deral-Madineh was written about 1300 B.C.E. and gives instructions on how to obtain a dream message from a god.
  15. ^ Lesku, J. A.; Meyer, L. C. R.; Fuller, A.; Maloney, S. K.; Dell'Omo, G.; Vyssotski, A. L.; Rattenborg, N. C. (2011). "Ostriches sleep like platypuses". PLOS ONE. 6 (8): 1–7. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...623203L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023203. PMC 3160860. PMID 21887239.
  16. ^ a b Solms, Mark (2000). "Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 843–850. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003988. PMID 11515144. S2CID 7264870. Dreaming and REM sleep are incompletely correlated. Between 5 and 30% of REM awakenings do not elicit dream reports; and at least 5–10% of NREM awakenings do elicit dream reports that are indistinguishable from REM....
  17. ^ Bulkeley, Kelly (2008). Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history. p. 14. ISBN 978-0-8147-9956-7. Do animals dream? We currently have no means of proving it one way or the other, just as we have no way to determine whether human fetuses and newborns are genuinely dreaming before they develop the ability to speak and relate their experiences.
  18. ^ Damasio, Antonio (2010). Self Comes to Mind. New York: Pantheon Books. p. 289. ISBN 978-0-307-37875-0. …I sympathize with Julian Jaynes's claim that something of great import may have happened to the human mind during the relatively brief interval of time between the events narrated in the Iliad and those that make up the Odyssey.
  19. ^ Nielsen, Tore A. (1991), "Reality Dreams and Their Effects on Spiritual Belief: A Revision of Animism Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.), Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms, Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 233–264, ISBN 0-89503-056-X
  20. ^ Atwan, Robert (1981). "The Interpretation of Dreams, The Origin of Consciousness, and the Birth of Tragedy". Research Communication in Psychology, Psychiatry and Behavior. 6 (2): 163–182.
  21. ^ a b c Hall, C., & Van de Castle, R. (1966). The Content Analysis of Dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Content Analysis Explained 12 April 2007 at the Wayback Machine
  22. ^ Schneider, Adam; Domhoff, G. William. "The Classification and Coding of Characters". University of California at Santa Cruz. Retrieved 17 July 2021.
  23. ^ Schredl, Michael; Ciric, Petra; Götz, Simon; Wittmann, Lutz (November 2004). "Typical Dreams: Stability and Gender Differences". The Journal of Psychology. 138 (6): 485–494. doi:10.3200/JRLP.138.6.485-494. PMID 15612605. S2CID 13554573.
  24. ^ Zadra, A., "1093: Sex dreams: what do men and women dream about?" 27 September 2007 at the Wayback Machine, Sleep Volume 30, Abstract Supplement, 2007 A376.
  25. ^ "Badan Pusat Statistik "Indonesia Young Adult Reproductive Health Survey 2002–2004" p. 27" (PDF). (PDF) from the original on 9 December 2012. Retrieved 4 April 2013.
  26. ^ . March 2012. Archived from the original on 24 January 2013. Retrieved 10 May 2013.
  27. ^ Hobson, J. Allan; Pace-Schott, Edward F.; Stickgold, Robert (2000). "Dream science 2000: A response to commentaries on Dreaming and the Brain". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 1019. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00954025. S2CID 144729368.
  28. ^ Chiong, Winston; Leonard, Matthew K.; Chang, Edward F. (2018). "Neurosurgical Patients as Human Research Subjects: Ethical Considerations in Intracranial Electrophysiology Research". Neurosurgery. 83 (1): 29–37. doi:10.1093/neuros/nyx361. PMC 5777911. PMID 28973530.
  29. ^ a b Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). "Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states". Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(6), 793–842.
  30. ^ Ringach, Dario L. (30 July 2009). "The limits of fMRI". Speaking of Research. Retrieved 18 July 2021.
  31. ^ "Policies on the Use of Animals and Humans in Research". Society for Neuroscience. Retrieved 18 July 2021.
  32. ^ Uttal, William R. (2013). Reliability in Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. p. 4. Similarly, modern neuroscience research is increasingly showing that activation areas on the brain associated with a cognitive process are far more widely distributed than had been thought only a decade or so ago. Indeed, it now seems likely that most of the brain is active in almost any cognitive process.
  33. ^ Solms, Mark (2000). "Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 843–850, discussion 904–1121. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003988. PMID 11515144. S2CID 7264870.
  34. ^ Braun, A. R.; Balkin, T. J; Wesensten, N. J.; Carson, R. E.; Varga, M.; Baldwin, P.; Selbie, S.; Belenky, G.; Herscovitch, P. (1997). "Regional cerebral blood flow through the sleep-wake cycle". Brain. Oxford University Press. 120: 1173–1197. doi:10.1093/brain/120.7.1173. PMID 9236630.
  35. ^ Kalat, James W. (2015). Biological Psychology (12 ed.). Boston: Cengage. p. 288. ISBN 978-1305105409.
  36. ^ Viskontas, Indre (2017). Brain Myths Exploded: Lessons from Neuroscience. Chantilly, VA: The Teaching Company. p. 393.
  37. ^ Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien (1923). "Chapter III Dreams". Primitive Mentality. Translated by Lilian A. Clare. New York: Macmillan. pp. 97–121.
  38. ^ Robert, W. Der Traum als Naturnothwendigkeit erklärt. Zweite Auflage, Hamburg: Seippel, 1886.
  39. ^ Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 188. The view adopted by Robert [1886, 9 f.] that the purpose of dreams is to unburden our memory of the useless impressions of daytime [cf. pp. 105 f.] is plainly no longer tenable....
  40. ^ Rycroft, Charles. A Critical Dictionary of Psychoanalysis. London: Penguin Books, 1995, p. 41.
  41. ^ Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 253.
  42. ^ Aserinsky, Eugene; Kleitman, Nathaniel (1953). "Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility, and Concomitant Phenomena, during Sleep". Science. 118 (3062): 273–274. Bibcode:1953Sci...118..273A. doi:10.1126/science.118.3062.273. PMID 13089671.
  43. ^ Smith, Robert C. (1991), "The Meaning of Dreams: A Current Warning Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.), Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms, Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 127–146, ISBN 0-89503-056-X
  44. ^ Hobson, J. Allan; McCarley, Robert W. (December 1977). "The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 134 (12): 1335–1348. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570. The dream process is thus seen as having its origin in sensorimotor systems, with little or no primary ideational, volitional, or emotional content. This concept is markedly different from that of the "dream thoughts" or wishes seen by Freud as the primary stimulus for the dream.
  45. ^ Benjamin, Victoria. "Study Links Dreaming to Increased Memory Performance". The Harvard Crimson. The Harvard Crimson. Retrieved 27 January 2022.
  46. ^ Evans, C.; Newman, E. (1964). "Dreaming: An analogy from computers". New Scientist. 419: 577–579.
  47. ^ Crick, F.; Mitchison, G. (1983). "The function of dream sleep". Nature. 304 (5922): 111–114. Bibcode:1983Natur.304..111C. doi:10.1038/304111a0. PMID 6866101. S2CID 41500914.
  48. ^ Hartmann, Ernest (1995). "Making Connections in a Safe Place: Is Dreaming Psychotherapy?". Dreaming. 5 (4): 213–228. doi:10.1037/h0094437.
  49. ^ Revonsuo, A. (2000). "The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 877–901. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00004015. PMID 11515147. S2CID 145340071.
  50. ^ Eagleman, David M.; Vaughn, Don A. (May 2021). "The Defensive Activation Theory: REM Sleep as a Mechanism to Prevent Takeover of the Visual Cortex". Frontiers in Neuroscience. 15: 632853. doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.632853. PMC 8176926. PMID 34093109.
  51. ^ "Weird dreams train us for the unexpected, says new theory". the Guardian. 14 May 2021. Retrieved 5 January 2023.
  52. ^ Hoel, Erik (14 May 2021). "The overfitted brain: Dreams evolved to assist generalization". Patterns. 2 (5): 100244. doi:10.1016/j.patter.2021.100244. ISSN 2666-3899.
  53. ^ Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien (1923). "Chapter III Dreams". Primitive Mentality. Translated by Lilian A. Clare. New York: Macmillan. p. 98. ...[I]n dreams,...man passes from the one world to the other without being aware of it. Such is in fact the ordinary idea of the dream to primitive peoples. The "soul" leaves its tenement for the time being. It frequently goes very far away; it communes with spirits or with ghosts. At the moment of awakening it returns to take its place in the body once more.
  54. ^ Dunne, J. W. (1950) [1927]. An Experiment with Time. London: Faber. p. 23.
  55. ^ Krishnananda, Swami (16 November 1996). . Archived from the original on 9 April 2015. Retrieved 26 March 2015.
  56. ^ Bar, Shaul (2001). A letter that has not been read: Dreams in the Hebrew Bible. Hebrew Union College Press. Retrieved 4 April 2013.
  57. ^ Edgar, Iain (2011). . Oxford: Berghahn Books. p. 178. ISBN 978-0-85745-235-1. Archived from the original on 29 September 2011. Retrieved 9 March 2012.
  58. ^ a b c d Edgar, Iain R.; Henig, David (September 2010). (PDF). History and Anthropology. 21 (3): 251–262. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1012.7334. doi:10.1080/02757206.2010.496781. S2CID 144463607. Archived from the original (PDF) on 8 November 2017. Retrieved 26 October 2017.
  59. ^ a b Young, S. (2003). . The encyclopedia of South Asian Folklore. Vol. 13. p. 7. Archived from the original on 25 February 2018. Retrieved 24 February 2018 – via Indian Folklife.
  60. ^ a b Bulkeley, Kelly (2008). Dreaming in the world's religions: A comparative history. pp. 71–73. ISBN 978-0-8147-9956-7.
  61. ^ Oppenheim, L.A. (1966). Mantic Dreams in the Ancient Near East in G. E. Von Grunebaum & R. Caillois (Eds.), The Dream and Human Societies (pp. 341–350). London, England: Cambridge University Press.
  62. ^ Nils P. Heessel : Divinatorische Texte I : ... oneiromantische Omina. Harrassowitz Verlag, 2007.
  63. ^ O'Neil, C.W. (1976). Dreams, culture and the individual. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.
  64. ^ Cicero, De Republica, 6.10
  65. ^ Herodotus (1998). The Histories. Oxford University Press. p. 414.
  66. ^ Uluru – Kata Tjuta National Park: Tjukurpa – Anangu culture 11 July 2009 at the Wayback Machine environment.gov.au, June 23, 2006
  67. ^ Tedlock, B. (1981). "Quiche Maya dream Interpretation". Ethos. 9 (4): 313–350. doi:10.1525/eth.1981.9.4.02a00050.
  68. ^ Webb, Craig (1995). . The DREAMS Foundation. Archived from the original on 5 March 2016. Retrieved 30 March 2008.
  69. ^ . Dream Encyclopedia. Archived from the original on 15 April 2012. Retrieved 10 April 2012.
  70. ^ Morewedge, Carey K.; Norton, Michael I. (2009). (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (2): 249–264. doi:10.1037/a0013264. PMID 19159131. S2CID 5706448. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 November 2020.
  71. ^ a b Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81. ISBN 978-1-57392-979-0.
  72. ^ Gilovich, Thomas (1991). How We Know What Isn't So: the fallibility of human reason in everyday life. Simon & Schuster. pp. 177–180. ISBN 978-0-02-911706-4.
  73. ^ . www.llewellyn.com. Archived from the original on 18 October 2016. Retrieved 16 October 2016.
  74. ^ Alcock, James E. (1981). Parapsychology: Science or Magic?: a psychological perspective. Oxford: Pergamon Press. ISBN 978-0-08-025773-0. via Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81. ISBN 978-1-57392-979-0.
  75. ^ Madey, Scott; Thomas Gilovich (1993). "Effects of Temporal Focus on the Recall of Expectancy-Consistent and Expectancy-Inconsistent Information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65 (3): 458–468. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.3.458. PMID 8410650. via Kida, Thomas (2006). Don't Believe Everything You Think: The 6 Basic Mistakes We Make in Thinking. Prometheus Books. ISBN 978-1-59102-408-8.
  76. ^ . Washington State University. Archived from the original on 14 November 2012. Retrieved 24 May 2012.
  77. ^ . The History Guide. Archived from the original on 6 June 2012. Retrieved 24 May 2012.
  78. ^ Lovecraft, Howard Phillips (1995). The Dream Cycle of H. P. Lovecraft: Dreams of Terror and Death. Ballantine Books. ISBN 978-0-345-38421-8. from the original on 3 September 2013.
  79. ^ . Archived from the original on 1 June 2012. Retrieved 24 May 2012.
  80. ^ a b Van Riper, A. Bowdoin (2002). Science in popular culture: a reference guide. Westport: Greenwood Press. pp. 56–57. ISBN 978-0-313-31822-1.
  81. ^ Van Riper, op. cit., p. 57.
  82. ^ Lucid dreaming FAQ 13 March 2007 at the Wayback Machine by The Lucidity Institute at Psych Web.
  83. ^ Watanabe, T. (2003). "Lucid Dreaming: Its Experimental Proof and Psychological Conditions". J Int Soc Life Inf Sci. 21 (1). ISSN 1341-9226.
  84. ^ (PDF). 5 September 1975. Archived from the original (PDF) on 26 April 2012. Retrieved 21 October 2013.
  85. ^ LaBerge, S. (2014). Lucid dreaming: Paradoxes of dreaming consciousness. In E. Cardeña, S. Lynn, S. Krippner (Eds.), Varieties of anomalous experience: Examining the scientific evidence (2nd ed.) (pp. 145–173). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/14258-006
  86. ^ Olson, Parmy. "Saying 'Hi' Through A Dream: How The Internet Could Make Sleeping More Social". Forbes. from the original on 31 May 2016. Retrieved 24 April 2016.
  87. ^ a b "The Science Behind Dreams and Nightmares". NPR.org. Npr.org. from the original on 22 August 2013. Retrieved 4 April 2013.
  88. ^ a b c Watson, David (2003). "To dream, perchance to remember: Individual differences in dream recall". Personality and Individual Differences. 34 (7): 1271–1286. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00114-9.
  89. ^ "Why Do Some People Always Remember Their Dreams, While Others Almost Never Do?". Discover Magazine. 2019. Retrieved 10 February 2021.
  90. ^ Herlin, Bastien; Leu-Semenescu, Smaranda; Chaumereuil, Charlotte; Arnulf, Isabelle (December 2015). "Evidence that non-dreamers do dream: a REM sleep behaviour disorder model". Journal of Sleep Research. 24 (6): 602–609. doi:10.1111/jsr.12323. PMID 26307463. Adults report, on average, 1–2.8 dream recalls per week in a dream questionnaire and 2.38 dream recalls per week when a home dream diary is completed
  91. ^ Hobson, J.A.; McCarly, R.W. (1977). (PDF). American Journal of Psychiatry. 134 (12): 1335–1348. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570. S2CID 10396934. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 July 2017.
  92. ^ Morelle, Rebecca (4 April 2013). "Scientists 'read dreams' using brain scans". BBC News. from the original on 27 April 2016. Retrieved 24 April 2016.
  93. ^ Mcguigan, F. (2012). The Psychophysiology of Thinking: Studies of Covert Processes. Elsevier. ISBN 978-0-323-14700-2.
  94. ^ Cvetkovic, Dean; Cosic, Irena (2011). States of Consciousness: Experimental Insights into Meditation, Waking, Sleep and Dreams. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN 978-3-642-18047-7.
  95. ^ Oldis, Daniel (4 February 2016). "Can We Turn Our Dreams Into Watchable Movies?". The Huffington Post. from the original on 4 August 2016. Retrieved 2016-08-20.
  96. ^ 莊子, 齊物論, 12. Zhuàngzi, "Discussion on making all things equal," 12. from Zhuàngzi, Burton Watson trans., Chuang Tzu (New York: Columbia University Press, 1996), 43. ISBN 978-0-231-10595-8 [1]
  97. ^ Kher, Chitrarekha V. (1992). Buddhism As Presented by the Brahmanical Systems. Sri Satguru Publications. ISBN 978-81-7030-293-3.
  98. ^ Hajek P, Belcher M (1991). "Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom". J Abnorm Psychol. 100 (4): 487–491. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.487. PMID 1757662.
  99. ^ The Science of Dreaming: 9 Key Points
  100. ^ Dormio: A Targeted Dream Incubation Device - PMC - NCBI
  101. ^ a b Klinger, Eric (October 1987). Psychology Today.
  102. ^ Barrett, D.L. (1979). "The Hypnotic Dream: Its Content in Comparison to Nocturnal Dreams and Waking Fantasy". Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 88 (5): 584–591. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.88.5.584.
  103. ^ Barrett, D.L. "Fantasizers and Dissociaters: Two types of High Hypnotizables, Two Imagery Styles". in R. Kusendorf, N. Spanos, & B. Wallace (Eds.) Hypnosis and Imagination. New York: Baywood, 1996. and, Barrett, D.L. "Dissociaters, Fantasizers, and their Relation to Hypnotizability" in Barrett, D.L. (Ed.) Hypnosis and Hypnotherapy, (2 vol.): Vol. 1: History, theory and general research, Vol. 2: Psychotherapy research and applications, New York: Praeger/Greenwood, 2010.
  104. ^ Tierney, John (28 June 2010). "Discovering the Virtues of a Wandering Mind". The New York Times. from the original on 21 April 2017.
  105. ^ American Psychiatric Association (2000), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, TR, p. 631
  106. ^ Hockenbury, Don H.; Hockenbury, Sandra E. (2010). Discovering psychology (5th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. p. 157. ISBN 978-1-4292-1650-0.
  107. ^ Lohff, David C. (2004). The Dream Directory: The Comprehensive Guide to Analysis and Interpretation. Running Press. ISBN 978-0-7624-1962-3.

Further reading

  • Dreaming (journal)
  • Jung, Carl (1934). The Practice of Psychotherapy. "The Practical Use of Dream-analysis". New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. pp. 139–. ISBN 978-0-7100-1645-4.
  • Jung, Carl (2002). Dreams (Routledge Classics). New York: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-26740-3.
  • Harris, William V. (2009) Dreams and Еxperience in Classical Antiquity. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press.

External links

  • Dreams on In Our Time at the BBC
  • Archive for Research in Archetypal Symbolism website
  • The International Association for the Study of Dreams
  • Dream at Curlie
  • Dixit, Jay (November 2007). "Dreams: Night School". Psychology Today. Retrieved December 1, 2018.
  • alt.dreams A long-running USENET forum wherein readers post and analyze dreams
  • LSDBase – online sleep research database documenting physiological effects of dreams through biofeedback

dream, other, uses, disambiguation, dream, succession, images, ideas, emotions, sensations, that, usually, occur, involuntarily, mind, during, certain, stages, sleep, humans, spend, about, hours, dreaming, night, each, dream, lasts, around, minutes, although, . For other uses see Dream disambiguation A dream is a succession of images ideas emotions and sensations that usually occur involuntarily in the mind during certain stages of sleep 1 Humans spend about two hours dreaming per night 2 and each dream lasts around 5 to 20 minutes although the dreamer may perceive the dream as being much longer than this 3 A fantasy painting of a dream showing a man walking on a road under the starry sky The content and function of dreams have been topics of scientific philosophical and religious interest throughout recorded history Dream interpretation practiced by the Babylonians in the third millennium BCE 4 and even earlier by the ancient Sumerians 5 6 figures prominently in religious texts in several traditions and has played a lead role in psychotherapy 7 8 The scientific study of dreams is called oneirology 9 Most modern dream study focuses on the neurophysiology of dreams and on proposing and testing hypotheses regarding dream function It is not known where in the brain dreams originate if there is a single origin for dreams or if multiple regions of the brain are involved or what the purpose of dreaming is for the body or mind The human dream experience and what to make of it has undergone sizable shifts over the course of history 10 11 Long ago according to writings from Mesopotamia and Ancient Egypt dreams dictated post dream behaviors to an extent sharply reduced in later millennia These ancient writings about dreams highlight visitation dreams where a dream figure usually a deity or a prominent forebear commands the dreamer to take specific actions and may predict future events 12 13 14 Framing the dream experience varies across cultures as well as through time Dreaming and sleep are intertwined Dreams occur mainly in the rapid eye movement REM stage of sleep when brain activity is high and resembles that of being awake Because REM sleep is detectable in many species and because research suggests that all mammals experience REM 15 linking dreams to REM sleep has led to conjectures that animals dream However humans dream during non REM sleep also and not all REM awakenings elicit dream reports 16 To be studied a dream must first be reduced to a verbal report which is an account of the subject s memory of the dream not the subject s dream experience itself So dreaming by non humans is currently unprovable as is dreaming by human fetuses and pre verbal infants 17 Contents 1 Subjective experience and content 2 Neurophysiology 3 Generation 4 Theories on function 5 Religious and other cultural contexts 5 1 Hindu 5 2 Abrahamic 5 3 Buddhist 5 4 Other 6 Interpretation 7 Images and literature 8 Lucidity 9 Recollection 10 Miscellany 10 1 Illusion of reality 10 2 Absent minded transgression 10 3 Non REM dreams 10 4 Daydreams 10 5 Hallucination 10 6 Nightmare 10 7 Night terror 10 8 Deja vu 11 See also 12 References 13 Further reading 14 External linksSubjective experience and contentFurther information Oneiromancy Usha Dreaming Aniruddha oleographic print Raja Ravi Varma 1848 1906 Preserved writings from early Mediterranean civilizations indicate a relatively abrupt change in subjective dream experience between Bronze Age antiquity and the beginnings of the classical era 18 In visitation dreams reported in ancient writings dreamers were largely passive in their dreams and visual content served primarily to frame authoritative auditory messaging 19 10 20 Gudea the king of the Sumerian city state of Lagash reigned c 2144 2124 BCE rebuilt the temple of Ningirsu as the result of a dream in which he was told to do so 6 After antiquity the passive hearing of visitation dreams largely gave way to visualized narratives in which the dreamer becomes a character who actively participates From the 1940s to 1985 Calvin S Hall collected more than 50 000 dream reports at Western Reserve University In 1966 Hall and Robert Van de Castle published The Content Analysis of Dreams in which they outlined a coding system to study 1 000 dream reports from college students 21 Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the world demonstrated similarity in their dream content The only residue of antiquity s authoritative dream figure in the Hall and Van de Castle listing of dream characters is the inclusion of God in the category of prominent persons 22 Hall s complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid 1990s by his protege William Domhoff More recent studies of dream reports while providing more detail continue to cite the Hall study favorably 23 A soldier dreams the trenches of WWI Jan Styka 1858 1925 In the Hall study the most common emotion experienced in dreams was anxiety Other emotions included abandonment anger fear joy and happiness Negative emotions were much more common than positive ones 21 The Hall data analysis showed that sexual dreams occur no more than 10 of the time and are more prevalent in young to mid teens 21 Another study showed that 8 of both men s and women s dreams have sexual content 24 In some cases sexual dreams may result in orgasms or nocturnal emissions These are colloquially known as wet dreams 25 The visual nature of dreams is generally highly phantasmagoric that is different locations and objects continuously blend into each other The visuals including locations people and objects are generally reflective of a person s memories and experiences but conversation can take on highly exaggerated and bizarre forms Some dreams may even tell elaborate stories wherein the dreamer enters entirely new complex worlds and awakes with ideas thoughts and feelings never experienced prior to the dream People who are blind from birth do not have visual dreams Their dream contents are related to other senses like hearing touch smell and taste whichever are present since birth 26 NeurophysiologyMain article Cognitive neuroscience of dreams Further information Neuroscience of sleep Dream study is popular with scientists exploring the mind brain problem Some propose to reduce aspects of dream phenomenology to neurobiology 27 But current science cannot specify dream physiology in detail Protocols in most nations restrict human brain research to non invasive procedures In the United States invasive brain procedures with a human subject are allowed only when these are deemed necessary in surgical treatment to address medical needs of the same human subject 28 Non invasive measures of brain activity like electroencephalogram EEG voltage averaging or cerebral blood flow cannot identify small but influential neuronal populations 29 Also fMRI signals are too slow to explain how brains compute in real time 30 Scientists researching some brain functions can work around current restrictions by examining animal subjects As stated by the Society for Neuroscience Because no adequate alternatives exist much of this research must sic be done on animal subjects 31 However since animal dreaming can be only inferred not confirmed animal studies yield no hard facts to illuminate the neurophysiology of dreams Examining human subjects with brain lesions can provide clues but the lesion method cannot discriminate between the effects of destruction and disconnection and cannot target specific neuronal groups in heterogeneous regions like the brain stem 29 Generation The Knight s Dream 1655 by Antonio de Pereda Denied precision tools obliged to depend on imaging much dream research has succumbed to the law of the instrument Studies detect an increase of blood flow in a specific brain region and then credit that region with a role in generating dreams But pooling study results has led to the newer conclusion that dreaming involves large numbers of regions and pathways which likely are different for different dream events 32 Image creation in the brain involves significant neural activity downstream from eye intake and it is theorized that the visual imagery of dreams is produced by activation during sleep of the same structures that generate complex visual imagery in waking perception 33 Dreams present a running narrative rather than exclusively visual imagery Following their work with split brain subjects Gazzaniga and LeDoux postulated without attempting to specify the neural mechanisms a left brain interpreter that seeks to create a plausible narrative from whatever electro chemical signals reach the brain s left hemisphere Sleep research has determined that some brain regions fully active during waking are during REM sleep activated only in a partial or fragmentary way 34 Drawing on this knowledge textbook author James W Kalat explains A dream represents the brain s effort to make sense of sparse and distorted information The cortex combines this haphazard input with whatever other activity was already occurring and does its best to synthesize a story that makes sense of the information 35 Neuroscientist Indre Viskontas is even more blunt calling often bizarre dream content just the result of your interpreter trying to create a story out of random neural signaling 36 Theories on functionMain article Oneirology Further information Rapid eye movement sleep For humans in the pre classical era and continuing for some non literate populations into modern times dreams are believed to have functioned as revealers of truths sourced during sleep from gods or other external entities 37 11 Ancient Egyptians believed that dreams were the best way to receive divine revelation and thus they would induce or incubate dreams They went to sanctuaries and slept on special dream beds in hope of receiving advice comfort or healing from the gods 14 From a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement provide some benefit for natural selection to take place or at least have no negative impact on fitness Robert 1886 38 a physician from Hamburg was the first who suggested that dreams are a need and that they have the function to erase a sensory impressions that were not fully worked up and b ideas that were not fully developed during the day In dreams incomplete material is either removed suppressed or deepened and included into memory Freud whose dream studies focused on interpreting dreams not explaining how or why humans dream disputed Robert s hypothesis 39 and proposed that dreams preserve sleep by representing as fulfilled those wishes that otherwise would awaken the dreamer 40 Freud wrote that dreams serve the purpose of prolonging sleep instead of waking up Dreams are the GUARDIANS of sleep and not its disturbers 41 Grandmother and Granddaughter Dream 1839 or 1840 Taras Shevchenko A turning point in theorizing about dream function came in 1953 when Science published the Aserinsky and Kleitman paper 42 establishing REM sleep as a distinct phase of sleep and linking dreams to REM sleep 43 Until and even after publication of the Solms 2000 paper that certified the separability of REM sleep and dream phenomena 16 many studies purporting to uncover the function of dreams have in fact been studying not dreams but measurable REM sleep Theories of dream function since the identification of REM sleep include Hobson s and McCarley s 1977 activation synthesis hypothesis which proposed a functional role for dreaming sleep in promoting some aspect of the learning process 44 In 2010 a Harvard study was published showing experimental evidence that dreams were correlated with improved learning 45 Crick s and Mitchison s 1983 reverse learning theory which states that dreams are like the cleaning up operations of computers when they are offline removing suppressing parasitic nodes and other junk from the mind during sleep 46 47 Hartmann s 1995 proposal that dreams serve a quasi therapeutic function enabling the dreamer to process trauma in a safe place 48 Revonsuo s 2000 threat simulation hypothesis whose premise is that during much of human evolution physical and interpersonal threats were serious giving reproductive advantage to those who survived them Dreaming aided survival by replicating these threats and providing the dreamer with practice in dealing with them 49 Eagleman s and Vaughn s 2021 defensive activation theory which says that given the brain s neuroplasticity dreams evolved as a visual hallucinatory activity during sleep s extended periods of darkness busying the occipital lobe and thereby protecting it from possible appropriation by other non vision sense operations 50 Erik Hoel proposes based on artificial neural networks that dreams prevent overfitting to past experiences that is they enable the dreamer to learn from novel situations 51 52 Religious and other cultural contextsDreams figure prominently in major world religions The dream experience for early humans according to one interpretation gave rise to the notion of a human soul 53 a central element in much religious thought J W Dunne wrote But there can be no reasonable doubt that the idea of a soul must have first arisen in the mind of primitive man as a result of observation of his dreams Ignorant as he was he could have come to no other conclusion but that in dreams he left his sleeping body in one universe and went wandering off into another It is considered that but for that savage the idea of such a thing as a soul would never have even occurred to mankind 54 Hindu In the Mandukya Upanishad part of the Veda scriptures of Indian Hinduism a dream is one of three states that the soul experiences during its lifetime the other two states being the waking state and the sleep state 55 The earliest Upanishads written before 300 BCE emphasize two meanings of dreams The first says that dreams are merely expressions of inner desires The second is the belief of the soul leaving the body and being guided until awakened Abrahamic Jacob s dream of a ladder of angels c 1690 Michael Willmann In Judaism dreams are considered part of the experience of the world that can be interpreted and from which lessons can be garnered It is discussed in the Talmud Tractate Berachot 55 60 The ancient Hebrews connected their dreams heavily with their religion though the Hebrews were monotheistic and believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone Hebrews also differentiated between good dreams from God and bad dreams from evil spirits The Hebrews like many other ancient cultures incubated dreams in order to receive a divine revelation For example the Hebrew prophet Samuel would lie down and sleep in the temple at Shiloh before the Ark and receive the word of the Lord Most of the dreams in the Bible are in the Book of Genesis 56 Christians mostly shared the beliefs of the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural character because the Old Testament includes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration The most famous of these dream stories was Jacob s dream of a ladder that stretches from Earth to Heaven Many Christians preach that God can speak to people through their dreams The famous glossary the Somniale Danielis written in the name of Daniel attempted to teach Christian populations to interpret their dreams Iain R Edgar has researched the role of dreams in Islam 57 He has argued that dreams play an important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims since dream interpretation is the only way that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the death of the last prophet Muhammad 58 According to Edgar Islam classifies three types of dreams Firstly there is the true dream al ru ya then the false dream which may come from the devil shaytan and finally the meaningless everyday dream hulm This last dream could be brought forth by the dreamer s ego or base appetite based on what they experienced in the real world The true dream is often indicated by Islam s hadith tradition 58 In one narration by Aisha the wife of the Prophet it is said that the Prophet s dreams would come true like the ocean s waves 58 Just as in its predecessors the Quran also recounts the story of Joseph and his unique ability to interpret dreams 58 Buddhist In Buddhism ideas about dreams are similar to the classical and folk traditions in South Asia The same dream is sometimes experienced by multiple people as in the case of the Buddha to be before he is leaving his home It is described in the Mahavastu that several of the Buddha s relatives had premonitory dreams preceding this Some dreams are also seen to transcend time the Buddha to be has certain dreams that are the same as those of previous Buddhas the Lalitavistara states In Buddhist literature dreams often function as a signpost motif to mark certain stages in the life of the main character 59 Buddhist views about dreams are expressed in the Pali Commentaries and the Milinda Panha 59 Other Dreaming of the Tiger Spring 虎跑夢泉 Statue at Hupao Spring Hupaomengquan in Hangzhou Zhejiang China In Chinese history people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body during slumber to journey in a dream realm while the other remained in the body 60 This belief and dream interpretation had been questioned since early times such as by the philosopher Wang Chong 27 97 CE 60 The Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into good which were sent by the gods and bad sent by demons 61 A surviving collection of dream omens entitled Iskar Zaqiqu records various dream scenarios as well as prognostications of what will happen to the person who experiences each dream apparently based on previous cases 6 62 Some list different possible outcomes based on occasions in which people experienced similar dreams with different results 6 The Greeks shared their beliefs with the Egyptians on how to interpret good and bad dreams and the idea of incubating dreams Morpheus the Greek god of dreams also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept at shrines and temples The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically visited the dreamers where they entered through a keyhole exiting the same way after the divine message was given Antiphon wrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BCE In that century other cultures influenced Greeks to develop the belief that souls left the sleeping body 63 Hippocrates 469 399 BCE had a simple dream theory during the day the soul receives images during the night it produces images Greek philosopher Aristotle 384 322 BCE believed dreams caused physiological activity He thought dreams could analyze illness and predict diseases Marcus Tullius Cicero for his part believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a dreamer had during the preceding days 64 Cicero s Somnium Scipionis described a lengthy dream vision which in turn was commented on by Macrobius in his Commentarii in Somnium Scipionis Herodotus in his The Histories writes The visions that occur to us in dreams are more often than not the things we have been concerned about during the day 65 The Dreaming is a common term within the animist creation narrative of indigenous Australians for a personal or group creation and for what may be understood as the timeless time of formative creation and perpetual creating 66 Some Indigenous American tribes and Mexican populations believe that dreams are a way of visiting and having contact with their ancestors 67 Some Native American tribes have used vision quests as a rite of passage fasting and praying until an anticipated guiding dream was received to be shared with the rest of the tribe upon their return 68 69 InterpretationMain article Dream interpretation Further information Psychoanalysis and Precognition Joseph Interprets Pharaoh s Dream c 1896 1902 Jacques Joseph Tissot 1836 1902 Beginning in the late 19th century Austrian neurologist Sigmund Freud founder of psychoanalysis theorized that dreams reflect the dreamer s unconscious mind and specifically that dream content is shaped by unconscious wish fulfillment He argued that important unconscious desires often relate to early childhood memories and experiences 7 Carl Jung and others expanded on Freud s idea that dream content reflects the dreamer s unconscious desires Dream interpretation can be a result of subjective ideas and experiences One study found that most people believe that their dreams reveal meaningful hidden truths 70 The researchers surveyed students in the United States South Korea and India and found that 74 of Indians 65 of South Koreans and 56 of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight into their unconscious beliefs and desires This Freudian view of dreaming was believed significantly more than theories of dreaming that attribute dream content to memory consolidation problem solving or as a byproduct of unrelated brain activity The same study found that people attribute more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake Americans were more likely to report that they would intentionally miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flying while awake and that they would be as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the night before their flight as if there was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to take Participants in the study were more likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake They were more likely to view a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream about someone they disliked for example and were more likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as meaningful than a negative dream about a person they liked According to surveys it is common for people to feel their dreams are predicting subsequent life events 71 Psychologists have explained these experiences in terms of memory biases namely a selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively fitted onto life experiences 71 The multi faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to find connections between dream content and real events 72 The term veridical dream has been used to indicate dreams that reveal or contain truths not yet known to the dreamer whether future events or secrets 73 In one experiment subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary This prevented the selective memory effect and the dreams no longer seemed accurate about the future 74 Another experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with apparently precognitive dreams This diary described events from the person s life as well as some predictive dreams and some non predictive dreams When subjects were asked to recall the dreams they had read they remembered more of the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones 75 Images and literatureMain article Dream art Further information Dream world plot device Graphic artists writers and filmmakers all have found dreams to offer a rich vein for creative expression In the West artists depictions of dreams in Renaissance and Baroque art often were related to Biblical narrative Especially preferred by visual artists were the Jacob s Ladder dream in Genesis and St Joseph s dreams in the Gospel according to Matthew Nicolas Dipre Le songe de Jacob c 1500 Avignon Petit Palais Jose de Ribera 1591 1652 El sueno de Jacob from Prado in Google Earth Raphael Jacob s Dream 1518 Rembrandt Dream of Joseph 1645 Anton Raphael Mengs Traum des Hl Joseph 1773 or 1774 Many later graphic artists have depicted dreams including Japanese woodblock artist Hokusai 1760 1849 and Western European painters Rousseau 1844 1910 Picasso 1881 1973 and Dali 1904 1989 In literature dream frames were frequently used in medieval allegory to justify the narrative The Book of the Duchess 76 and The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman 77 are two such dream visions Even before them in antiquity the same device had been used by Cicero and Lucian of Samosata The cheshire cat John Tenniel 1820 1914 illustration in Alice s Adventures in Wonderland 1866 edition Dreams have also featured in fantasy and speculative fiction since the 19th century One of the best known dream worlds is Wonderland from Lewis Carroll s Alice s Adventures in Wonderland as well as Looking Glass Land from its sequel Through the Looking Glass Unlike many dream worlds Carroll s logic is like that of actual dreams with transitions and flexible causality Other fictional dream worlds include the Dreamlands of H P Lovecraft s Dream Cycle 78 and The Neverending Story s 79 world of Fantastica which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams the Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness Dreamworlds shared hallucinations and other alternate realities feature in a number of works by Philip K Dick such as The Three Stigmata of Palmer Eldritch and Ubik Similar themes were explored by Jorge Luis Borges for instance in The Circular Ruins Modern popular culture often conceives of dreams as did Freud as expressions of the dreamer s deepest fears and desires 80 In speculative fiction the line between dreams and reality may be blurred even more in service to the story 81 Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated Dreamscape 1984 the Nightmare on Elm Street films 1984 2010 Inception 2010 or even come literally true as in The Lathe of Heaven 1971 80 LucidityMain article Lucid dream Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one s state while dreaming In this state the dreamer may often have some degree of control over their own actions within the dream or even the characters and the environment of the dream Dream control has been reported to improve with practiced deliberate lucid dreaming but the ability to control aspects of the dream is not necessary for a dream to qualify as lucid a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they are dreaming 82 The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified 83 Oneironaut is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream In 1975 psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a communication from a dreamer experiencing a lucid dream On April 12 1975 after agreeing to move his eyes left and right upon becoming lucid the subject and Hearne s co author on the resulting article Alan Worsley successfully carried out this task 84 Years later psychophysiologist Stephen LaBerge conducted similar work including Using eye signals to map the subjective sense of time in dreams Comparing the electrical activity of the brain while singing awake and while dreaming Studies comparing in dream sex arousal and orgasm 85 Communication between two dreamers has also been documented The processes involved included EEG monitoring ocular signaling incorporation of reality in the form of red light stimuli and a coordinating website The website tracked when both dreamers were dreaming and sent the stimulus to one of the dreamers where it was incorporated into the dream This dreamer upon becoming lucid signaled with eye movements this was detected by the website whereupon the stimulus was sent to the second dreamer invoking incorporation into that dreamer s dream 86 RecollectionFurther information Dream diary Raphael s dream 1821 Johannes Riepenhausen and Franz Riepenhausen The recollection of dreams is extremely unreliable though it is a skill that can be trained Dreams can usually be recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming 87 Women tend to have more frequent dream recall than men 87 Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized by relatively little affect and factors such as salience arousal and interference play a role in dream recall Often a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus The salience hypothesis proposes that dream content that is salient that is novel intense or unusual is more easily remembered There is considerable evidence that vivid intense or unusual dream content is more frequently recalled 88 A dream journal can be used to assist dream recall for personal interest or psychotherapy purposes Adults report remembering around two dreams per week on average 89 90 Unless a dream is particularly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it the content of the dream is typically not remembered 91 Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall Using the permitted non invasive technologies functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI and electromyography EMG researchers have been able to identify basic dream imagery 92 dream speech activity 93 and dream motor behavior such as walking and hand movements 94 95 In line with the salience hypothesis there is considerable evidence that people who have more vivid intense or unusual dreams show better recall There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is related to recall Specifically people who have vivid and unusual experiences during the day tend to have more memorable dream content and hence better dream recall People who score high on measures of personality traits associated with creativity imagination and fantasy such as openness to experience daydreaming fantasy proneness absorption and hypnotic susceptibility tend to show more frequent dream recall 88 There is also evidence for continuity between the bizarre aspects of dreaming and waking experience That is people who report more bizarre experiences during the day such as people high in schizotypy psychosis proneness have more frequent dream recall and also report more frequent nightmares 88 MiscellanyIllusion of reality Main article Dream argument Some philosophers have proposed that what we think of as the real world could be or is an illusion an idea known as the skeptical hypothesis about ontology The first recorded mention of the idea was in the 4th century BCE by Zhuangzi and in Eastern philosophy the problem has been named the Zhuangzi Paradox He who dreams of drinking wine may weep when morning comes he who dreams of weeping may in the morning go off to hunt While he is dreaming he does not know it is a dream and in his dream he may even try to interpret a dream Only after he wakes does he know it was a dream And someday there will be a great awakening when we know that this is all a great dream Yet the stupid believe they are awake busily and brightly assuming they understand things calling this man ruler that one herdsman how dense Confucius and you are both dreaming And when I say you are dreaming I am dreaming too Words like these will be labeled the Supreme Swindle Yet after ten thousand generations a great sage may appear who will know their meaning and it will still be as though he appeared with astonishing speed 96 The idea also is discussed in Hindu and Buddhist writings 97 It was formally introduced to Western philosophy by Descartes in the 17th century in his Meditations on First Philosophy Absent minded transgression Dreams of absent minded transgression DAMT are dreams wherein the dreamer absent mindedly performs an action that he or she has been trying to stop one classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking with intense feelings of guilt One study found a positive association between having these dreams and successfully stopping the behavior 98 Non REM dreams Hypnogogic and hypnopompic dreams dreamlike states shortly after falling asleep and shortly before awakening and dreams during stage 2 of NREM sleep also occur but are shorter than REM dreams 99 100 Daydreams Main article Daydream Dante Meditating 1852 by Joseph Noel Paton A daydream is a visionary fantasy especially one of happy pleasant thoughts hopes or ambitions imagined as coming to pass and experienced while awake 101 There are many different types of daydreams and there is no consistent definition amongst psychologists 101 The general public also uses the term for a broad variety of experiences Research by Harvard psychologist Deirdre Barrett has found that people who experience vivid dreamlike mental images reserve the word for these whereas many other people refer to milder imagery realistic future planning review of memories or just spacing out i e one s mind going relatively blank when they talk about daydreaming 102 103 While daydreaming has long been derided as a lazy non productive pastime it is now commonly acknowledged that daydreaming can be constructive in some contexts 104 There are numerous examples of people in creative or artistic careers such as composers novelists and filmmakers developing new ideas through daydreaming Similarly research scientists mathematicians and physicists have developed new ideas by daydreaming about their subject areas Hallucination Main article Hallucination A hallucination in the broadest sense of the word is a perception in the absence of a stimulus In a stricter sense hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state in the absence of external stimuli and have qualities of real perception in that they are vivid substantial and located in external objective space The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related phenomena of dreaming which does not involve wakefulness Nightmare Main article Nightmare Woman having a nightmare Jean Pierre Simon 1764 1810 or 1813 A nightmare is an unpleasant dream that can cause a strong negative emotional response from the mind typically fear or horror but also despair anxiety and great sadness The dream may contain situations of danger discomfort psychological or physical terror Sufferers usually awaken in a state of distress and may be unable to return to sleep for a prolonged period of time 105 Night terror Main article Night terror A night terror also known as a sleep terror or pavor nocturnus is a parasomnia disorder that predominantly affects children causing feelings of terror or dread Night terrors should not be confused with nightmares which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear 106 Deja vu Main article Deja vu One theory of deja vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to having dreamed about a similar situation or place and forgetting about it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or the place while awake 107 See also Psychology portalDream dictionary Dream incubation Dream of Macsen Wledig Dream pop Dream sequence Dream yoga Dreamcatcher Dreamwork False awakening Hatsuyume Incubus Lilith a Sumerian dream demoness List of dream diaries Mare folklore Mabinogion Sleep in animals Sleep paralysis Spirit spouse Succubus Works based on dreamsReferences Dream The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language Fourth Edition 2000 Retrieved 7 May 2009 Brain Basics Understanding Sleep National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke 2006 Archived from the original on 11 October 2007 Retrieved 16 December 2007 Lee Ann Obringer 2006 How Dream Works Archived from the original on 18 April 2006 Retrieved 4 May 2006 Krippner Stanley Bogzaran Fariba Carvalho Andre Percia de 2002 Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them Albany NY State University of New York Press p 9 ISBN 0 7914 5257 3 Clay tablets have been found dating to about 2500 B C E that contain interpretive material for Babylonian and Assyrian dreamers Seligman K 1948 Magic Supernaturalism and Religion New York Random House a b c d Black Jeremy Green Anthony 1992 Gods Demons and Symbols of Ancient Mesopotamia An Illustrated Dictionary Austin University of Texas Press pp 71 72 89 90 ISBN 0714117056 a b Freud Sigmund 1965 James Strachey ed The Interpretation of Dreams Translated by James Strachey New York Avon Schredl Michael Bohusch Claudia Kahl Johanna Mader Andrea Somesan Alexandra 2000 The Use of Dreams in Psychotherapy The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research 9 2 81 87 Kavanau J L 2000 Sleep memory maintenance and mental disorders Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 12 2 199 208 doi 10 1176 jnp 12 2 199 PMID 11001598 a b Dodds E R 1951 The Greeks and the Irrational Berkeley University of California Press p 105 The Greeks never spoke as we do of having a dream but always of seeing a dream a b Packer Sharon 2002 Dreams in Myth Medicine and Movies Westport CT Praeger Publishers p 85 ISBN 0 275 97243 7 M any more ancient cultures think that dreams are imposed by a force that resides outside the individual Macrobius 1952 430 Commentary on the Dream of Scipio Translated by W H Stahl New York Columbia University Press p 90 We call a dream oracular in which a parent or a pious or revered man or a priest or even a god clearly reveals what will or will not transpire and what action to take or to avoid Dodds 1951 referring to the type of dream described by Macrobius This last type is not I think at all common in our own dream experience But there is considerable evidence that dreams of this sort were familiar in antiquity p 107 a b Krippner Stanley Bogzaran Fariba Carvalho Andre Percia de 2002 Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them Albany State University of New York Press p 10 ISBN 0 7914 5257 3 The Egyptian papyrus of Deral Madineh was written about 1300 B C E and gives instructions on how to obtain a dream message from a god Lesku J A Meyer L C R Fuller A Maloney S K Dell Omo G Vyssotski A L Rattenborg N C 2011 Ostriches sleep like platypuses PLOS ONE 6 8 1 7 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 623203L doi 10 1371 journal pone 0023203 PMC 3160860 PMID 21887239 a b Solms Mark 2000 Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 6 843 850 doi 10 1017 S0140525X00003988 PMID 11515144 S2CID 7264870 Dreaming and REM sleep are incompletely correlated Between 5 and 30 of REM awakenings do not elicit dream reports and at least 5 10 of NREM awakenings do elicit dream reports that are indistinguishable from REM Bulkeley Kelly 2008 Dreaming in the world s religions A comparative history p 14 ISBN 978 0 8147 9956 7 Do animals dream We currently have no means of proving it one way or the other just as we have no way to determine whether human fetuses and newborns are genuinely dreaming before they develop the ability to speak and relate their experiences Damasio Antonio 2010 Self Comes to Mind New York Pantheon Books p 289 ISBN 978 0 307 37875 0 I sympathize with Julian Jaynes s claim that something of great import may have happened to the human mind during the relatively brief interval of time between the events narrated in the Iliad and those that make up the Odyssey Nielsen Tore A 1991 Reality Dreams and Their Effects on Spiritual Belief A Revision of Animism Theory in Gackenbach Jayne Sheikh Anees A eds Dream Images A Call to Mental Arms Amityville NY Baywood pp 233 264 ISBN 0 89503 056 X Atwan Robert 1981 The Interpretation of Dreams The Origin of Consciousness and the Birth of Tragedy Research Communication in Psychology Psychiatry and Behavior 6 2 163 182 a b c Hall C amp Van de Castle R 1966 The Content Analysis of Dreams New York Appleton Century Crofts Content Analysis Explained Archived 12 April 2007 at the Wayback Machine Schneider Adam Domhoff G William The Classification and Coding of Characters University of California at Santa Cruz Retrieved 17 July 2021 Schredl Michael Ciric Petra Gotz Simon Wittmann Lutz November 2004 Typical Dreams Stability and Gender Differences The Journal of Psychology 138 6 485 494 doi 10 3200 JRLP 138 6 485 494 PMID 15612605 S2CID 13554573 Zadra A 1093 Sex dreams what do men and women dream about Archived 27 September 2007 at the Wayback Machine Sleep Volume 30 Abstract Supplement 2007 A376 Badan Pusat Statistik Indonesia Young Adult Reproductive Health Survey 2002 2004 p 27 PDF Archived PDF from the original on 9 December 2012 Retrieved 4 April 2013 How do blind people dream The Body Odd March 2012 Archived from the original on 24 January 2013 Retrieved 10 May 2013 Hobson J Allan Pace Schott Edward F Stickgold Robert 2000 Dream science 2000 A response to commentaries on Dreaming and the Brain Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 6 1019 doi 10 1017 S0140525X00954025 S2CID 144729368 Chiong Winston Leonard Matthew K Chang Edward F 2018 Neurosurgical Patients as Human Research Subjects Ethical Considerations in Intracranial Electrophysiology Research Neurosurgery 83 1 29 37 doi 10 1093 neuros nyx361 PMC 5777911 PMID 28973530 a b Hobson J A Pace Schott E F amp Stickgold R 2000 Dreaming and the brain Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 6 793 842 Ringach Dario L 30 July 2009 The limits of fMRI Speaking of Research Retrieved 18 July 2021 Policies on the Use of Animals and Humans in Research Society for Neuroscience Retrieved 18 July 2021 Uttal William R 2013 Reliability in Cognitive Neuroscience Cambridge MA The MIT Press p 4 Similarly modern neuroscience research is increasingly showing that activation areas on the brain associated with a cognitive process are far more widely distributed than had been thought only a decade or so ago Indeed it now seems likely that most of the brain is active in almost any cognitive process Solms Mark 2000 Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different brain mechanisms Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 6 843 850 discussion 904 1121 doi 10 1017 S0140525X00003988 PMID 11515144 S2CID 7264870 Braun A R Balkin T J Wesensten N J Carson R E Varga M Baldwin P Selbie S Belenky G Herscovitch P 1997 Regional cerebral blood flow through the sleep wake cycle Brain Oxford University Press 120 1173 1197 doi 10 1093 brain 120 7 1173 PMID 9236630 Kalat James W 2015 Biological Psychology 12 ed Boston Cengage p 288 ISBN 978 1305105409 Viskontas Indre 2017 Brain Myths Exploded Lessons from Neuroscience Chantilly VA The Teaching Company p 393 Levy Bruhl Lucien 1923 Chapter III Dreams Primitive Mentality Translated by Lilian A Clare New York Macmillan pp 97 121 Robert W Der Traum als Naturnothwendigkeit erklart Zweite Auflage Hamburg Seippel 1886 Freud Sigmund 1965 James Strachey ed The Interpretation of Dreams Translated by James Strachey New York Avon p 188 The view adopted by Robert 1886 9 f that the purpose of dreams is to unburden our memory of the useless impressions of daytime cf pp 105 f is plainly no longer tenable Rycroft Charles A Critical Dictionary of Psychoanalysis London Penguin Books 1995 p 41 Freud Sigmund 1965 James Strachey ed The Interpretation of Dreams Translated by James Strachey New York Avon p 253 Aserinsky Eugene Kleitman Nathaniel 1953 Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility and Concomitant Phenomena during Sleep Science 118 3062 273 274 Bibcode 1953Sci 118 273A doi 10 1126 science 118 3062 273 PMID 13089671 Smith Robert C 1991 The Meaning of Dreams A Current Warning Theory in Gackenbach Jayne Sheikh Anees A eds Dream Images A Call to Mental Arms Amityville NY Baywood pp 127 146 ISBN 0 89503 056 X Hobson J Allan McCarley Robert W December 1977 The Brain as a Dream State Generator An Activation Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process The American Journal of Psychiatry 134 12 1335 1348 doi 10 1176 ajp 134 12 1335 PMID 21570 The dream process is thus seen as having its origin in sensorimotor systems with little or no primary ideational volitional or emotional content This concept is markedly different from that of the dream thoughts or wishes seen by Freud as the primary stimulus for the dream Benjamin Victoria Study Links Dreaming to Increased Memory Performance The Harvard Crimson The Harvard Crimson Retrieved 27 January 2022 Evans C Newman E 1964 Dreaming An analogy from computers New Scientist 419 577 579 Crick F Mitchison G 1983 The function of dream sleep Nature 304 5922 111 114 Bibcode 1983Natur 304 111C doi 10 1038 304111a0 PMID 6866101 S2CID 41500914 Hartmann Ernest 1995 Making Connections in a Safe Place Is Dreaming Psychotherapy Dreaming 5 4 213 228 doi 10 1037 h0094437 Revonsuo A 2000 The reinterpretation of dreams an evolutionary hypothesis of the function of dreaming Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23 6 877 901 doi 10 1017 S0140525X00004015 PMID 11515147 S2CID 145340071 Eagleman David M Vaughn Don A May 2021 The Defensive Activation Theory REM Sleep as a Mechanism to Prevent Takeover of the Visual Cortex Frontiers in Neuroscience 15 632853 doi 10 3389 fnins 2021 632853 PMC 8176926 PMID 34093109 Weird dreams train us for the unexpected says new theory the Guardian 14 May 2021 Retrieved 5 January 2023 Hoel Erik 14 May 2021 The overfitted brain Dreams evolved to assist generalization Patterns 2 5 100244 doi 10 1016 j patter 2021 100244 ISSN 2666 3899 Levy Bruhl Lucien 1923 Chapter III Dreams Primitive Mentality Translated by Lilian A Clare New York Macmillan p 98 I n dreams man passes from the one world to the other without being aware of it Such is in fact the ordinary idea of the dream to primitive peoples The soul leaves its tenement for the time being It frequently goes very far away it communes with spirits or with ghosts At the moment of awakening it returns to take its place in the body once more Dunne J W 1950 1927 An Experiment with Time London Faber p 23 Krishnananda Swami 16 November 1996 The Mandukya Upanishad Section 4 Archived from the original on 9 April 2015 Retrieved 26 March 2015 Bar Shaul 2001 A letter that has not been read Dreams in the Hebrew Bible Hebrew Union College Press Retrieved 4 April 2013 Edgar Iain 2011 The Dream in Islam From Qur anic Tradition to Jihadist Inspiration Oxford Berghahn Books p 178 ISBN 978 0 85745 235 1 Archived from the original on 29 September 2011 Retrieved 9 March 2012 a b c d Edgar Iain R Henig David September 2010 Istikhara The Guidance and practice of Islamic dream incubation through ethnographic comparison PDF History and Anthropology 21 3 251 262 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 1012 7334 doi 10 1080 02757206 2010 496781 S2CID 144463607 Archived from the original PDF on 8 November 2017 Retrieved 26 October 2017 a b Young S 2003 Dreams The encyclopedia of South Asian Folklore Vol 13 p 7 Archived from the original on 25 February 2018 Retrieved 24 February 2018 via Indian Folklife a b Bulkeley Kelly 2008 Dreaming in the world s religions A comparative history pp 71 73 ISBN 978 0 8147 9956 7 Oppenheim L A 1966 Mantic Dreams in the Ancient Near East in G E Von Grunebaum amp R Caillois Eds The Dream and Human Societies pp 341 350 London England Cambridge University Press Nils P Heessel Divinatorische Texte I oneiromantische Omina Harrassowitz Verlag 2007 O Neil C W 1976 Dreams culture and the individual San Francisco Chandler amp Sharp Cicero De Republica 6 10 Herodotus 1998 The Histories Oxford University Press p 414 Uluru Kata Tjuta National Park Tjukurpa Anangu culture Archived 11 July 2009 at the Wayback Machine environment gov au June 23 2006 Tedlock B 1981 Quiche Maya dream Interpretation Ethos 9 4 313 350 doi 10 1525 eth 1981 9 4 02a00050 Webb Craig 1995 Dreams Practical Meaning amp Applications The DREAMS Foundation Archived from the original on 5 March 2016 Retrieved 30 March 2008 Native American Dream Beliefs Dream Encyclopedia Archived from the original on 15 April 2012 Retrieved 10 April 2012 Morewedge Carey K Norton Michael I 2009 When dreaming is believing The motivated interpretation of dreams PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 96 2 249 264 doi 10 1037 a0013264 PMID 19159131 S2CID 5706448 Archived from the original PDF on 14 November 2020 a b Hines Terence 2003 Pseudoscience and the Paranormal Prometheus Books pp 78 81 ISBN 978 1 57392 979 0 Gilovich Thomas 1991 How We Know What Isn t So the fallibility of human reason in everyday life Simon amp Schuster pp 177 180 ISBN 978 0 02 911706 4 Llewellyn Worldwide Encyclopedia Term Veridical Dream www llewellyn com Archived from the original on 18 October 2016 Retrieved 16 October 2016 Alcock James E 1981 Parapsychology Science or Magic a psychological perspective Oxford Pergamon Press ISBN 978 0 08 025773 0 via Hines Terence 2003 Pseudoscience and the Paranormal Prometheus Books pp 78 81 ISBN 978 1 57392 979 0 Madey Scott Thomas Gilovich 1993 Effects of Temporal Focus on the Recall of Expectancy Consistent and Expectancy Inconsistent Information Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 65 3 458 468 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 65 3 458 PMID 8410650 via Kida Thomas 2006 Don t Believe Everything You Think The 6 Basic Mistakes We Make in Thinking Prometheus Books ISBN 978 1 59102 408 8 The book of the duchess Washington State University Archived from the original on 14 November 2012 Retrieved 24 May 2012 William Langland s The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman The History Guide Archived from the original on 6 June 2012 Retrieved 24 May 2012 Lovecraft Howard Phillips 1995 The Dream Cycle of H P Lovecraft Dreams of Terror and Death Ballantine Books ISBN 978 0 345 38421 8 Archived from the original on 3 September 2013 The Neverending Story Book Pictures Video Icons Archived from the original on 1 June 2012 Retrieved 24 May 2012 a b Van Riper A Bowdoin 2002 Science in popular culture a reference guide Westport Greenwood Press pp 56 57 ISBN 978 0 313 31822 1 Van Riper op cit p 57 Lucid dreaming FAQ Archived 13 March 2007 at the Wayback Machine by The Lucidity Institute at Psych Web Watanabe T 2003 Lucid Dreaming Its Experimental Proof and Psychological Conditions J Int Soc Life Inf Sci 21 1 ISSN 1341 9226 The University of Chicago Department of Psychiatry PDF 5 September 1975 Archived from the original PDF on 26 April 2012 Retrieved 21 October 2013 LaBerge S 2014 Lucid dreaming Paradoxes of dreaming consciousness In E Cardena S Lynn S Krippner Eds Varieties of anomalous experience Examining the scientific evidence 2nd ed pp 145 173 Washington DC American Psychological Association doi 10 1037 14258 006 Olson Parmy Saying Hi Through A Dream How The Internet Could Make Sleeping More Social Forbes Archived from the original on 31 May 2016 Retrieved 24 April 2016 a b The Science Behind Dreams and Nightmares NPR org Npr org Archived from the original on 22 August 2013 Retrieved 4 April 2013 a b c Watson David 2003 To dream perchance to remember Individual differences in dream recall Personality and Individual Differences 34 7 1271 1286 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 02 00114 9 Why Do Some People Always Remember Their Dreams While Others Almost Never Do Discover Magazine 2019 Retrieved 10 February 2021 Herlin Bastien Leu Semenescu Smaranda Chaumereuil Charlotte Arnulf Isabelle December 2015 Evidence that non dreamers do dream a REM sleep behaviour disorder model Journal of Sleep Research 24 6 602 609 doi 10 1111 jsr 12323 PMID 26307463 Adults report on average 1 2 8 dream recalls per week in a dream questionnaire and 2 38 dream recalls per week when a home dream diary is completed Hobson J A McCarly R W 1977 The brain as a dream state generator An activation synthesis hypothesis of the dream process PDF American Journal of Psychiatry 134 12 1335 1348 doi 10 1176 ajp 134 12 1335 PMID 21570 S2CID 10396934 Archived from the original PDF on 9 July 2017 Morelle Rebecca 4 April 2013 Scientists read dreams using brain scans BBC News Archived from the original on 27 April 2016 Retrieved 24 April 2016 Mcguigan F 2012 The Psychophysiology of Thinking Studies of Covert Processes Elsevier ISBN 978 0 323 14700 2 Cvetkovic Dean Cosic Irena 2011 States of Consciousness Experimental Insights into Meditation Waking Sleep and Dreams Springer Science amp Business Media ISBN 978 3 642 18047 7 Oldis Daniel 4 February 2016 Can We Turn Our Dreams Into Watchable Movies The Huffington Post Archived from the original on 4 August 2016 Retrieved 2016 08 20 莊子 齊物論 12 Zhuangzi Discussion on making all things equal 12 from Zhuangzi Burton Watson trans Chuang Tzu New York Columbia University Press 1996 43 ISBN 978 0 231 10595 8 1 Kher Chitrarekha V 1992 Buddhism As Presented by the Brahmanical Systems Sri Satguru Publications ISBN 978 81 7030 293 3 Hajek P Belcher M 1991 Dream of absent minded transgression an empirical study of a cognitive withdrawal symptom J Abnorm Psychol 100 4 487 491 doi 10 1037 0021 843X 100 4 487 PMID 1757662 The Science of Dreaming 9 Key Points Dormio A Targeted Dream Incubation Device PMC NCBI a b Klinger Eric October 1987 Psychology Today Barrett D L 1979 The Hypnotic Dream Its Content in Comparison to Nocturnal Dreams and Waking Fantasy Journal of Abnormal Psychology 88 5 584 591 doi 10 1037 0021 843x 88 5 584 Barrett D L Fantasizers and Dissociaters Two types of High Hypnotizables Two Imagery Styles in R Kusendorf N Spanos amp B Wallace Eds Hypnosis and Imagination New York Baywood 1996 and Barrett D L Dissociaters Fantasizers and their Relation to Hypnotizability in Barrett D L Ed Hypnosis and Hypnotherapy 2 vol Vol 1 History theory and general research Vol 2 Psychotherapy research and applications New York Praeger Greenwood 2010 Tierney John 28 June 2010 Discovering the Virtues of a Wandering Mind The New York Times Archived from the original on 21 April 2017 American Psychiatric Association 2000 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th ed TR p 631 Hockenbury Don H Hockenbury Sandra E 2010 Discovering psychology 5th ed New York Worth Publishers p 157 ISBN 978 1 4292 1650 0 Lohff David C 2004 The Dream Directory The Comprehensive Guide to Analysis and Interpretation Running Press ISBN 978 0 7624 1962 3 Further readingDreaming journal Jung Carl 1934 The Practice of Psychotherapy The Practical Use of Dream analysis New York Routledge amp Kegan Paul pp 139 ISBN 978 0 7100 1645 4 Jung Carl 2002 Dreams Routledge Classics New York Routledge ISBN 978 0 415 26740 3 Harris William V 2009 Dreams and Experience in Classical Antiquity Cambridge MA amp London Harvard University Press External links Wikimedia Commons has media related to Dreams Wikiquote has quotations related to Dream Dreams on In Our Time at the BBC Archive for Research in Archetypal Symbolism website The International Association for the Study of Dreams Dream at Curlie Dixit Jay November 2007 Dreams Night School Psychology Today Retrieved December 1 2018 alt dreams A long running USENET forum wherein readers post and analyze dreams LSDBase online sleep research database documenting physiological effects of dreams through biofeedback Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Dream amp oldid 1133808522, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.