fbpx
Wikipedia

Introspection

Introspection is the examination of one's own conscious thoughts and feelings.[1] In psychology, the process of introspection relies on the observation of one's mental state, while in a spiritual context it may refer to the examination of one's soul.[2] Introspection is closely related to human self-reflection and self-discovery and is contrasted with external observation.

It generally provides a privileged access to one's own mental states,[3] not mediated by other sources of knowledge, so that individual experience of the mind is unique. Introspection can determine any number of mental states including: sensory, bodily, cognitive, emotional and so forth.[3]

Introspection has been a subject of philosophical discussion for thousands of years. The philosopher Plato asked, "…why should we not calmly and patiently review our own thoughts, and thoroughly examine and see what these appearances in us really are?"[4][5] While introspection is applicable to many facets of philosophical thought it is perhaps best known for its role in epistemology; in this context introspection is often compared with perception, reason, memory, and testimony as a source of knowledge.[6]

In psychology edit

Wundt edit

It has often been claimed that Wilhelm Wundt, the father of experimental psychology, was the first to adopt introspection to experimental psychology[1] though the methodological idea had been presented long before, as by 18th century German philosopher-psychologists such as Alexander Gottlieb Baumgarten or Johann Nicolaus Tetens.[7] Also, Wundt's views on introspection must be approached with great care.[8] Wundt was influenced by notable physiologists, such as Gustav Fechner, who used a kind of controlled introspection as a means to study human sensory organs. Building upon the pre-existing use of introspection in physiology, Wundt believed the method of introspection was the ability to observe an experience, not just the logical reflection or speculations which some others interpreted his meaning to be.[9] Wundt imposed exacting control over the use of introspection in his experimental laboratory at the University of Leipzig,[1] making it possible for other scientists to replicate his experiments elsewhere, a development that proved essential to the development of psychology as a modern, peer-reviewed scientific discipline. Such exact purism was typical of Wundt and he instructed all introspection observations be performed under these same instructions: "1) the Observer must, if possible, be in a position to determine beforehand the entrance of the process to be observed. 2) the introspectionist must, as far as possible, grasp the phenomenon in a state of strained attention and follow its course. 3) Every observation must, in order to make certain, be capable of being repeated several times under the same conditions and 4) the conditions under which the phenomenon appears must be found out by the variation of the attendant circumstances and when this was done the various coherent experiments must be varied according to a plan partly by eliminating certain stimuli and partly by grading their strength and quality".[9]

Titchener edit

Edward Titchener was an early pioneer in experimental psychology and student of Wilhelm Wundt.[1] After earning his doctorate under the tutelage of Wundt at the University of Leipzig, he made his way to Cornell University, where he established his own laboratory and research.[1] When Titchener arrived at Cornell in 1894, psychology was still a fledgling discipline, especially in the United States, and Titchener was a key figure in bringing Wundt's ideas to America. However, Titchener misrepresented some of Wundt's ideas to the American psychological establishment, especially in his account of introspection which, Titchener taught, only served a purpose in the qualitative analysis of consciousness into its various parts,[1] while Wundt saw it as a means to quantitatively measure the whole of conscious experience.[1] Titchener was exclusively interested in the individual components that comprise conscious experience, while Wundt, seeing little purpose in the analysis of individual components, focused on synthesis of these components. Ultimately, Titchener's ideas would form the basis of the short-lived psychological theory of structuralism.[1]

Historical misconceptions edit

American historiography of introspection, according to some authors,[10][11] is dominated by three misconceptions. In particular, historians of psychology tend to argue 1) that introspection once was the dominant method of psychological inquiry, 2) that behaviorism, and in particular John B. Watson, is responsible for discrediting introspection as a valid method, and 3) that scientific psychology completely abandoned introspection as a result of those critiques.[10] However, introspection has not been the dominant method. It is believed to be so because Edward Titchener's student Edwin G. Boring, in his influential historical accounts of experimental psychology, privileged Titchener's views while giving little credit to original sources.[10] Introspection has been critiqued by many other psychologists, including Wilhelm Wundt, and Knight Dunlap who in his article "The Case Against Introspection", presents an argument against self-observation that is not primarily rooted in behaviorist epistemology. Introspection is still widely used in psychology, but now implicitly, as self-report surveys, interviews and some fMRI studies are based on introspection.[11]: 4  It is not the method but rather its name that has been dropped from the dominant psychological vocabulary.

Recent developments edit

Partly as a result of Titchener's misrepresentation, the use of introspection diminished after his death and the subsequent decline of structuralism.[1] Later psychological movements, such as functionalism and behaviorism, rejected introspection for its lack of scientific reliability among other factors.[1] Functionalism originally arose in direct opposition to structuralism, opposing its narrow focus on the elements of consciousness[1] and emphasizing the purpose of consciousness and other psychological behavior. Behaviorism's objection to introspection focused much more on its unreliability and subjectivity which conflicted with behaviorism's focus on measurable behavior.[1][12]

The more recently established cognitive psychology movement has to some extent accepted introspection's usefulness in the study of psychological phenomena, though generally only in experiments pertaining to internal thought conducted under experimental conditions. For example, in the "think aloud protocol", investigators cue participants to speak their thoughts aloud in order to study an active thought process without forcing an individual to comment on the process itself.[13]

Already in the 18th century authors had criticized the use of introspection, both for knowing one's own mind and as a method for psychology. David Hume pointed out that introspecting a mental state tends to alter the very state itself; a German author, Christian Gottfried Schütz, noted that introspection is often described as mere "inner sensation", but actually requires also attention, that introspection does not get at unconscious mental states, and that it cannot be used naively - one needs to know what to look for. Immanuel Kant added that, if they are understood too narrowly, introspective experiments are impossible. Introspection delivers, at best, hints about what goes on in the mind; it does not suffice to justify knowledge claims about the mind.[14] Similarly, the idea continued to be discussed between John Stuart Mill and Auguste Comte. Recent psychological research on cognition and attribution has asked people to report on their mental processes, for instance to say why they made a particular choice or how they arrived at a judgment. In some situations, these reports are clearly confabulated.[15] For example, people justify choices they have not in fact made.[16] Such results undermine the idea that those verbal reports are based on direct introspective access to mental content. Instead, judgements about one's own mind seem to be inferences from overt behavior, similar to judgements made about another person.[15] However, it is hard to assess whether these results only apply to unusual experimental situations, or if they reveal something about everyday introspection.[17] The theory of the adaptive unconscious suggests that a very large proportion of mental processes, even "high-level" processes like goal-setting and decision-making, are inaccessible to introspection.[18] Indeed, it is questionable how confident researchers can be in their own introspections.

One of the central implications of dissociations between consciousness and meta-consciousness is that individuals, presumably including researchers, can misrepresent their experiences to themselves. Jack and Roepstorff assert, '...there is also a sense in which subjects simply cannot be wrong about their own experiential states.' Presumably they arrived at this conclusion by drawing on the seemingly self-evident quality of their own introspections, and assumed that it must equally apply to others. However, when we consider research on the topic, this conclusion seems less self-evident. If, for example, extensive introspection can cause people to make decisions that they later regret, then one very reasonable possibility is that the introspection caused them to 'lose touch with their feelings'. In short, empirical studies suggest that people can fail to appraise adequately (i.e. are wrong about) their own experiential states.

Another question in regards to the veracious accountability of introspection is if researchers lack the confidence in their own introspections and those of their participants, then how can it gain legitimacy? Three strategies are accountable: identifying behaviors that establish credibility, finding common ground that enables mutual understanding, and developing a trust that allows one to know when to give the benefit of the doubt. That is to say, that words are only meaningful if validated by one's actions; When people report strategies, feelings or beliefs, their behaviors must correspond with these statements if they are to be believed.[19]

Even when their introspections are uninformative, people still give confident descriptions of their mental processes, being "unaware of their unawareness".[20] This phenomenon has been termed the introspection illusion and has been used to explain some cognitive biases[21] and belief in some paranormal phenomena.[22] When making judgements about themselves, subjects treat their own introspections as reliable, whereas they judge other people based on their behavior.[23] This can lead to illusions of superiority. For example, people generally see themselves as less conformist than others, and this seems to be because they do not introspect any urge to conform.[24] Another reliable finding is that people generally see themselves as less biased than everyone else, because they are not likely to introspect any biased thought processes.[23]

One experiment tried to give their subjects access to others' introspections. They made audio recordings of subjects who had been told to say whatever came into their heads as they answered a question about their own bias.[23] Although subjects persuaded themselves they were unlikely to be biased, their introspective reports did not sway the assessments of observers. When subjects were explicitly told to avoid relying on introspection, their assessments of their own bias became more realistic.[23]

In religion edit

Judaism edit

In Judaism, particularly in the teachings of the practitioners of Mussar a person could achieve progress in perfecting their character traits through a daily "Cheshbon Hanefesh," or Accounting of the Soul. In the practice of Cheshbon Hanefesh, a person introspects about themselves, their day, their faults, progress, and so on, and over time can use the data and process to change behavior and thoughts. Introspection is encouraged during the penitent season in the month of Elul in order to correct the year's sins through repentance, which in Judaism begins with recalling and recognizing them.

Christianity edit

In Eastern Christianity some concepts addressing human needs, such as sober introspection (nepsis), require watchfulness of the human heart and the conflicts of the human nous, heart or mind. Noetic understanding can not be achieved by rational or discursive thought (i.e. systemization).[citation needed]

Jainism edit

Jains practise pratikraman (Sanskrit "introspection"), a process of repentance of wrongdoings during their daily life, and remind themselves to refrain from doing so again. Devout Jains often do Pratikraman at least twice a day.[citation needed] Many practice Pratikraman on holy days such as Samvatsari, or Forgiveness Day.[25]

Hinduism edit

Introspection is encouraged in schools such as Advaita Vedanta; in order for one to know their own true nature, they need to reflect and introspect on their true nature—which is what meditation is. Especially, Swami Chinmayananda emphasised the role of introspection in five stages, outlined in his book "Self Unfoldment."

In fiction edit

Introspection (also referred to as Rufus dialogue, interior monologue, self-talk) is the fiction-writing mode used to convey a character's thoughts. As explained by Renni Browne and Dave King, "One of the great gifts of literature is that it allows for the expression of unexpressed thoughts…"[26]

According to Nancy Kress, a character's thoughts can greatly enhance a story: deepening characterization, increasing tension, and widening the scope of a story.[27] As outlined by Jack M. Bickham, thought plays a critical role in both scene and sequel.[28]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l Schultz, D. P.; Schultz, S. E. (2012). A history of modern psychology (10th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. pp. 67–77, 88–100. ISBN 978-1-133-31624-4.
  2. ^ "psychology | Origin and meaning of psychology by Online Etymology Dictionary". www.etymonline.com. Retrieved 2020-09-05.
  3. ^ a b "Encyclopedia of Consciousness". January 2009: 187–199. doi:10.1016/B978-012373873-8.00071-2. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  4. ^ Theaetetus, 155.
  5. ^ J. Perner et al. (2007). "Introspection & remembering", Synthese, Springer.
  6. ^ Epistemology. (2005). In Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved fromhttp://plato.stanford.edu/entries/epistemology/#SOU.
  7. ^ Cf. Thomas Sturm, Kant und die Wissenschaften vom Menschen (Paderborn: Mentis, 2009), ch. 2.
  8. ^ Danziger, Kurt (1980). "The History of Introspection Reconsidered". Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences. 16 (3): 241–262. doi:10.1002/1520-6696(198007)16:3<241::aid-jhbs2300160306>3.0.co;2-o. PMID 11610711.
  9. ^ a b Asthana, Hari Shanker (June 2015). "Wilhelm Wundt". Psychological Studies. 60 (2): 244–248. doi:10.1007/s12646-014-0295-1. S2CID 189774028.
  10. ^ a b c Costal, A (2006). "'Introspectionism' and the mythical origins of scientific psychology". Consciousness and Cognition. 15 (4): 634–654. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.008. PMID 17174788. S2CID 17381488.
  11. ^ a b Clegg, Joshua (2013). Self-observation in the social science. New Jersey: Transaction.
  12. ^ Wilson, Robert Andrew; Keil, Frank C., eds. (2001). The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences. Cambridge, Massachusetts. ISBN 9780262731447.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: location missing publisher (link)
  13. ^ Hayes, S. C. (1986). "The case of the silent dog—Verbal reports and the analysis of rules: A review of Ericsson and Simon's Protocol Analysis: Verbal Reports as Data". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 45 (3): 351–363. doi:10.1901/jeab.1986.45-351. PMC 1348244.
  14. ^ Cf. Thomas Sturm, Kant und die Wissenschaften vom Menschen (Paderborn: Mentis, 2009), chapters 2 and 4.
  15. ^ a b Nisbett, Richard E.; Timothy D. Wilson (1977). "Telling more than we can know: Verbal reports on mental processes" (PDF). Psychological Review. 84 (3): 231–259. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.84.3.231. hdl:2027.42/92167. S2CID 7742203., reprinted in David Lewis Hamilton, ed. (2005). Social cognition: key readings. Psychology Press. ISBN 978-0-86377-591-8.
  16. ^ Johansson, Petter; Lars Hall; Sverker Sikström; Betty Tärning; Andreas Lind (2006). "How something can be said about telling more than we can know: On choice blindness and introspection". Consciousness and Cognition. 15 (4): 673–692. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2006.09.004. PMID 17049881. S2CID 14863202.
  17. ^ White, Peter A. (1988). "Knowing more about what we can tell: 'Introspective access' and causal report accuracy 10 years later". British Journal of Psychology. 79 (1): 13–45. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1988.tb02271.x.
  18. ^ Wilson, Timothy D.; Elizabeth W. Dunn (2004). "Self-Knowledge: Its Limits, Value, and Potential for Improvement". Annual Review of Psychology. 55: 493–518. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141954. PMID 14744224.
  19. ^ Schooler, Jonathan W. (2002). "Establishing a legitimate relationship with introspection". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 6 (9): 371–372. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01970-8. PMID 12200175. S2CID 45055944.
  20. ^ Wilson, Timothy D.; Yoav Bar-Anan (August 22, 2008). "The Unseen Mind". Science. 321 (5892): 1046–1047. doi:10.1126/science.1163029. PMID 18719269. S2CID 11434647.
  21. ^ Pronin, Emily (January 2007). "Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (1): 37–43. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.001. ISSN 1364-6613. PMID 17129749. S2CID 2754235.
  22. ^ Wegner, Daniel M. (2008). (PDF). In John Baer; James C. Kaufman; Roy F. Baumeister (eds.). Are we free?: psychology and free will. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-518963-6. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-01-20. Retrieved 2008-07-02.
  23. ^ a b c d Pronin, Emily; Matthew B. Kugler (July 2007). "Valuing thoughts, ignoring behavior: The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 43 (4): 565–578. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.05.011. ISSN 0022-1031.
  24. ^ Pronin, Emily; Jonah Berger; Sarah Molouki (2007). "Alone in a Crowd of Sheep: Asymmetric Perceptions of Conformity and Their Roots in an Introspection Illusion". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 92 (4): 585–595. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.4.585. ISSN 0022-3514. PMID 17469946.
  25. ^ "Jains say it with Michchhami Dukkadam; Sthanakwasi Jains who observe Samvatsari today will break fasts and Derawasi Jains to perform the rituals on Friday & Saturday". DNA (Daily News & Analysis). Athena Information Solutions Pvt. Ltd. 2 September 2011.
  26. ^ Browne, Renni; King, David (2004). Self-Editing for Fiction Writers: How to Edit Yourself into Print. New York, NY: HarperCollins Publishers Inc. ISBN 978-0-06-054569-7.
  27. ^ Kress, Nancy (2003). "Make "Em Think". Writer's Digest (August): 38.
  28. ^ Bickham, Jack M. (1993). Scene & Structure. Cincinnati, OH: Writer's Digest Books. pp. 12–22, 50–58. ISBN 978-0-89879-551-6.

Further reading edit

  • Boring, Edwin G. (1953). (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 50 (3): 169–189. doi:10.1037/h0090793. PMID 13056096. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-06-10. Retrieved 2009-07-17.
  • Gillespie, Alex (2006). "Descartes' demon: A dialogical analysis of 'Meditations on First Philosophy" (PDF). Theory & Psychology. 16 (6): 761–781. doi:10.1177/0959354306070527. hdl:1893/705. S2CID 144046196.
  • Gillespie, Alex (2007). Valsiner, Jaan; Rosa, Alberto (eds.). The social basis of self-reflection. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 678–691.
  • Jack, Anthony; Roepstorff, Andreas, eds. (2003). Trusting the subject?: The use of introspective evidence in cognitive science. Imprint Academic. ISBN 978-0-907845-56-0.
  • Wilson, Timothy (2002). Strangers to Ourselves: Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious. Cambridge: Belknap Press. ISBN 978-0-674-00936-3.
  • Wilson, Timothy D. Wilson; Sara D. Hodges (1992). "Attitudes as Temporary Constructions". In Leonard L. Martin; Abraham Tesser (eds.). The Construction of social judgments. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 978-0-8058-1149-0.

External links edit

introspection, this, article, about, psychological, process, other, uses, disambiguation, examination, conscious, thoughts, feelings, psychology, process, introspection, relies, observation, mental, state, while, spiritual, context, refer, examination, soul, c. This article is about the psychological process For other uses see Introspection disambiguation Introspection is the examination of one s own conscious thoughts and feelings 1 In psychology the process of introspection relies on the observation of one s mental state while in a spiritual context it may refer to the examination of one s soul 2 Introspection is closely related to human self reflection and self discovery and is contrasted with external observation It generally provides a privileged access to one s own mental states 3 not mediated by other sources of knowledge so that individual experience of the mind is unique Introspection can determine any number of mental states including sensory bodily cognitive emotional and so forth 3 Introspection has been a subject of philosophical discussion for thousands of years The philosopher Plato asked why should we not calmly and patiently review our own thoughts and thoroughly examine and see what these appearances in us really are 4 5 While introspection is applicable to many facets of philosophical thought it is perhaps best known for its role in epistemology in this context introspection is often compared with perception reason memory and testimony as a source of knowledge 6 Contents 1 In psychology 1 1 Wundt 1 2 Titchener 1 3 Historical misconceptions 1 4 Recent developments 2 In religion 2 1 Judaism 2 2 Christianity 2 3 Jainism 2 4 Hinduism 3 In fiction 4 See also 5 References 6 Further reading 7 External linksIn psychology editWundt edit It has often been claimed that Wilhelm Wundt the father of experimental psychology was the first to adopt introspection to experimental psychology 1 though the methodological idea had been presented long before as by 18th century German philosopher psychologists such as Alexander Gottlieb Baumgarten or Johann Nicolaus Tetens 7 Also Wundt s views on introspection must be approached with great care 8 Wundt was influenced by notable physiologists such as Gustav Fechner who used a kind of controlled introspection as a means to study human sensory organs Building upon the pre existing use of introspection in physiology Wundt believed the method of introspection was the ability to observe an experience not just the logical reflection or speculations which some others interpreted his meaning to be 9 Wundt imposed exacting control over the use of introspection in his experimental laboratory at the University of Leipzig 1 making it possible for other scientists to replicate his experiments elsewhere a development that proved essential to the development of psychology as a modern peer reviewed scientific discipline Such exact purism was typical of Wundt and he instructed all introspection observations be performed under these same instructions 1 the Observer must if possible be in a position to determine beforehand the entrance of the process to be observed 2 the introspectionist must as far as possible grasp the phenomenon in a state of strained attention and follow its course 3 Every observation must in order to make certain be capable of being repeated several times under the same conditions and 4 the conditions under which the phenomenon appears must be found out by the variation of the attendant circumstances and when this was done the various coherent experiments must be varied according to a plan partly by eliminating certain stimuli and partly by grading their strength and quality 9 Titchener edit Edward Titchener was an early pioneer in experimental psychology and student of Wilhelm Wundt 1 After earning his doctorate under the tutelage of Wundt at the University of Leipzig he made his way to Cornell University where he established his own laboratory and research 1 When Titchener arrived at Cornell in 1894 psychology was still a fledgling discipline especially in the United States and Titchener was a key figure in bringing Wundt s ideas to America However Titchener misrepresented some of Wundt s ideas to the American psychological establishment especially in his account of introspection which Titchener taught only served a purpose in the qualitative analysis of consciousness into its various parts 1 while Wundt saw it as a means to quantitatively measure the whole of conscious experience 1 Titchener was exclusively interested in the individual components that comprise conscious experience while Wundt seeing little purpose in the analysis of individual components focused on synthesis of these components Ultimately Titchener s ideas would form the basis of the short lived psychological theory of structuralism 1 Historical misconceptions edit American historiography of introspection according to some authors 10 11 is dominated by three misconceptions In particular historians of psychology tend to argue 1 that introspection once was the dominant method of psychological inquiry 2 that behaviorism and in particular John B Watson is responsible for discrediting introspection as a valid method and 3 that scientific psychology completely abandoned introspection as a result of those critiques 10 However introspection has not been the dominant method It is believed to be so because Edward Titchener s student Edwin G Boring in his influential historical accounts of experimental psychology privileged Titchener s views while giving little credit to original sources 10 Introspection has been critiqued by many other psychologists including Wilhelm Wundt and Knight Dunlap who in his article The Case Against Introspection presents an argument against self observation that is not primarily rooted in behaviorist epistemology Introspection is still widely used in psychology but now implicitly as self report surveys interviews and some fMRI studies are based on introspection 11 4 It is not the method but rather its name that has been dropped from the dominant psychological vocabulary Recent developments edit See also Introspection illusion Partly as a result of Titchener s misrepresentation the use of introspection diminished after his death and the subsequent decline of structuralism 1 Later psychological movements such as functionalism and behaviorism rejected introspection for its lack of scientific reliability among other factors 1 Functionalism originally arose in direct opposition to structuralism opposing its narrow focus on the elements of consciousness 1 and emphasizing the purpose of consciousness and other psychological behavior Behaviorism s objection to introspection focused much more on its unreliability and subjectivity which conflicted with behaviorism s focus on measurable behavior 1 12 The more recently established cognitive psychology movement has to some extent accepted introspection s usefulness in the study of psychological phenomena though generally only in experiments pertaining to internal thought conducted under experimental conditions For example in the think aloud protocol investigators cue participants to speak their thoughts aloud in order to study an active thought process without forcing an individual to comment on the process itself 13 Already in the 18th century authors had criticized the use of introspection both for knowing one s own mind and as a method for psychology David Hume pointed out that introspecting a mental state tends to alter the very state itself a German author Christian Gottfried Schutz noted that introspection is often described as mere inner sensation but actually requires also attention that introspection does not get at unconscious mental states and that it cannot be used naively one needs to know what to look for Immanuel Kant added that if they are understood too narrowly introspective experiments are impossible Introspection delivers at best hints about what goes on in the mind it does not suffice to justify knowledge claims about the mind 14 Similarly the idea continued to be discussed between John Stuart Mill and Auguste Comte Recent psychological research on cognition and attribution has asked people to report on their mental processes for instance to say why they made a particular choice or how they arrived at a judgment In some situations these reports are clearly confabulated 15 For example people justify choices they have not in fact made 16 Such results undermine the idea that those verbal reports are based on direct introspective access to mental content Instead judgements about one s own mind seem to be inferences from overt behavior similar to judgements made about another person 15 However it is hard to assess whether these results only apply to unusual experimental situations or if they reveal something about everyday introspection 17 The theory of the adaptive unconscious suggests that a very large proportion of mental processes even high level processes like goal setting and decision making are inaccessible to introspection 18 Indeed it is questionable how confident researchers can be in their own introspections One of the central implications of dissociations between consciousness and meta consciousness is that individuals presumably including researchers can misrepresent their experiences to themselves Jack and Roepstorff assert there is also a sense in which subjects simply cannot be wrong about their own experiential states Presumably they arrived at this conclusion by drawing on the seemingly self evident quality of their own introspections and assumed that it must equally apply to others However when we consider research on the topic this conclusion seems less self evident If for example extensive introspection can cause people to make decisions that they later regret then one very reasonable possibility is that the introspection caused them to lose touch with their feelings In short empirical studies suggest that people can fail to appraise adequately i e are wrong about their own experiential states Another question in regards to the veracious accountability of introspection is if researchers lack the confidence in their own introspections and those of their participants then how can it gain legitimacy Three strategies are accountable identifying behaviors that establish credibility finding common ground that enables mutual understanding and developing a trust that allows one to know when to give the benefit of the doubt That is to say that words are only meaningful if validated by one s actions When people report strategies feelings or beliefs their behaviors must correspond with these statements if they are to be believed 19 Even when their introspections are uninformative people still give confident descriptions of their mental processes being unaware of their unawareness 20 This phenomenon has been termed the introspection illusion and has been used to explain some cognitive biases 21 and belief in some paranormal phenomena 22 When making judgements about themselves subjects treat their own introspections as reliable whereas they judge other people based on their behavior 23 This can lead to illusions of superiority For example people generally see themselves as less conformist than others and this seems to be because they do not introspect any urge to conform 24 Another reliable finding is that people generally see themselves as less biased than everyone else because they are not likely to introspect any biased thought processes 23 One experiment tried to give their subjects access to others introspections They made audio recordings of subjects who had been told to say whatever came into their heads as they answered a question about their own bias 23 Although subjects persuaded themselves they were unlikely to be biased their introspective reports did not sway the assessments of observers When subjects were explicitly told to avoid relying on introspection their assessments of their own bias became more realistic 23 In religion editJudaism edit In Judaism particularly in the teachings of the practitioners of Mussar a person could achieve progress in perfecting their character traits through a daily Cheshbon Hanefesh or Accounting of the Soul In the practice of Cheshbon Hanefesh a person introspects about themselves their day their faults progress and so on and over time can use the data and process to change behavior and thoughts Introspection is encouraged during the penitent season in the month of Elul in order to correct the year s sins through repentance which in Judaism begins with recalling and recognizing them Christianity edit In Eastern Christianity some concepts addressing human needs such as sober introspection nepsis require watchfulness of the human heart and the conflicts of the human nous heart or mind Noetic understanding can not be achieved by rational or discursive thought i e systemization citation needed Jainism edit Jains practise pratikraman Sanskrit introspection a process of repentance of wrongdoings during their daily life and remind themselves to refrain from doing so again Devout Jains often do Pratikraman at least twice a day citation needed Many practice Pratikraman on holy days such as Samvatsari or Forgiveness Day 25 Hinduism edit Introspection is encouraged in schools such as Advaita Vedanta in order for one to know their own true nature they need to reflect and introspect on their true nature which is what meditation is Especially Swami Chinmayananda emphasised the role of introspection in five stages outlined in his book Self Unfoldment In fiction editIntrospection also referred to as Rufus dialogue interior monologue self talk is the fiction writing mode used to convey a character s thoughts As explained by Renni Browne and Dave King One of the great gifts of literature is that it allows for the expression of unexpressed thoughts 26 According to Nancy Kress a character s thoughts can greatly enhance a story deepening characterization increasing tension and widening the scope of a story 27 As outlined by Jack M Bickham thought plays a critical role in both scene and sequel 28 See also edit nbsp Philosophy portal nbsp Psychology portalConceptual proliferation Generation effect Human self reflection Insight Intrapersonal communication Introversion Know thyself Mode literature Psychological mindedness Phenomenology philosophy Phenomenology psychology Pratikramana Psychonautics Psychophysics Rumination psychology Self awareness Self consciousness Self discovery Style fiction References edit a b c d e f g h i j k l Schultz D P Schultz S E 2012 A history of modern psychology 10th ed Belmont CA Wadsworth Cengage Learning pp 67 77 88 100 ISBN 978 1 133 31624 4 psychology Origin and meaning of psychology by Online Etymology Dictionary www etymonline com Retrieved 2020 09 05 a b Encyclopedia of Consciousness January 2009 187 199 doi 10 1016 B978 012373873 8 00071 2 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a Cite journal requires journal help Theaetetus 155 J Perner et al 2007 Introspection amp remembering Synthese Springer Epistemology 2005 In Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Retrieved fromhttp plato stanford edu entries epistemology SOU Cf Thomas Sturm Kant und die Wissenschaften vom Menschen Paderborn Mentis 2009 ch 2 Danziger Kurt 1980 The History of Introspection Reconsidered Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 16 3 241 262 doi 10 1002 1520 6696 198007 16 3 lt 241 aid jhbs2300160306 gt 3 0 co 2 o PMID 11610711 a b Asthana Hari Shanker June 2015 Wilhelm Wundt Psychological Studies 60 2 244 248 doi 10 1007 s12646 014 0295 1 S2CID 189774028 a b c Costal A 2006 Introspectionism and the mythical origins of scientific psychology Consciousness and Cognition 15 4 634 654 doi 10 1016 j concog 2006 09 008 PMID 17174788 S2CID 17381488 a b Clegg Joshua 2013 Self observation in the social science New Jersey Transaction Wilson Robert Andrew Keil Frank C eds 2001 The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences Cambridge Massachusetts ISBN 9780262731447 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint location missing publisher link Hayes S C 1986 The case of the silent dog Verbal reports and the analysis of rules A review of Ericsson and Simon s Protocol Analysis Verbal Reports as Data Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 45 3 351 363 doi 10 1901 jeab 1986 45 351 PMC 1348244 Cf Thomas Sturm Kant und die Wissenschaften vom Menschen Paderborn Mentis 2009 chapters 2 and 4 a b Nisbett Richard E Timothy D Wilson 1977 Telling more than we can know Verbal reports on mental processes PDF Psychological Review 84 3 231 259 doi 10 1037 0033 295x 84 3 231 hdl 2027 42 92167 S2CID 7742203 reprinted in David Lewis Hamilton ed 2005 Social cognition key readings Psychology Press ISBN 978 0 86377 591 8 Johansson Petter Lars Hall Sverker Sikstrom Betty Tarning Andreas Lind 2006 How something can be said about telling more than we can know On choice blindness and introspection Consciousness and Cognition 15 4 673 692 doi 10 1016 j concog 2006 09 004 PMID 17049881 S2CID 14863202 White Peter A 1988 Knowing more about what we can tell Introspective access and causal report accuracy 10 years later British Journal of Psychology 79 1 13 45 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8295 1988 tb02271 x Wilson Timothy D Elizabeth W Dunn 2004 Self Knowledge Its Limits Value and Potential for Improvement Annual Review of Psychology 55 493 518 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 55 090902 141954 PMID 14744224 Schooler Jonathan W 2002 Establishing a legitimate relationship with introspection Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6 9 371 372 doi 10 1016 S1364 6613 02 01970 8 PMID 12200175 S2CID 45055944 Wilson Timothy D Yoav Bar Anan August 22 2008 The Unseen Mind Science 321 5892 1046 1047 doi 10 1126 science 1163029 PMID 18719269 S2CID 11434647 Pronin Emily January 2007 Perception and misperception of bias in human judgment Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 1 37 43 doi 10 1016 j tics 2006 11 001 ISSN 1364 6613 PMID 17129749 S2CID 2754235 Wegner Daniel M 2008 Self is Magic PDF In John Baer James C Kaufman Roy F Baumeister eds Are we free psychology and free will New York Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 518963 6 Archived from the original PDF on 2017 01 20 Retrieved 2008 07 02 a b c d Pronin Emily Matthew B Kugler July 2007 Valuing thoughts ignoring behavior The introspection illusion as a source of the bias blind spot Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 43 4 565 578 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2006 05 011 ISSN 0022 1031 Pronin Emily Jonah Berger Sarah Molouki 2007 Alone in a Crowd of Sheep Asymmetric Perceptions of Conformity and Their Roots in an Introspection Illusion Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 92 4 585 595 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 92 4 585 ISSN 0022 3514 PMID 17469946 Jains say it with Michchhami Dukkadam Sthanakwasi Jains who observe Samvatsari today will break fasts and Derawasi Jains to perform the rituals on Friday amp Saturday DNA Daily News amp Analysis Athena Information Solutions Pvt Ltd 2 September 2011 Browne Renni King David 2004 Self Editing for Fiction Writers How to Edit Yourself into Print New York NY HarperCollins Publishers Inc ISBN 978 0 06 054569 7 Kress Nancy 2003 Make Em Think Writer s Digest August 38 Bickham Jack M 1993 Scene amp Structure Cincinnati OH Writer s Digest Books pp 12 22 50 58 ISBN 978 0 89879 551 6 Further reading editBoring Edwin G 1953 A history of introspection PDF Psychological Bulletin 50 3 169 189 doi 10 1037 h0090793 PMID 13056096 Archived from the original PDF on 2011 06 10 Retrieved 2009 07 17 Gillespie Alex 2006 Descartes demon A dialogical analysis of Meditations on First Philosophy PDF Theory amp Psychology 16 6 761 781 doi 10 1177 0959354306070527 hdl 1893 705 S2CID 144046196 Gillespie Alex 2007 Valsiner Jaan Rosa Alberto eds The social basis of self reflection Cambridge Cambridge University Press pp 678 691 Jack Anthony Roepstorff Andreas eds 2003 Trusting the subject The use of introspective evidence in cognitive science Imprint Academic ISBN 978 0 907845 56 0 Wilson Timothy 2002 Strangers to Ourselves Discovering the Adaptive Unconscious Cambridge Belknap Press ISBN 978 0 674 00936 3 Wilson Timothy D Wilson Sara D Hodges 1992 Attitudes as Temporary Constructions In Leonard L Martin Abraham Tesser eds The Construction of social judgments Lawrence Erlbaum Associates ISBN 978 0 8058 1149 0 External links edit nbsp Wikiquote has quotations related to Introspection nbsp Look up introspection in Wiktionary the free dictionary Introspection entry by Amy Kind in the Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy What is Introspection What Is Introspection In Psychology The Benefits Of Introspection Schwitzgebel Eric Introspection In Zalta Edward N ed Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Introspection amp oldid 1206973201, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.