fbpx
Wikipedia

Gestalt psychology

Gestalt psychology, gestaltism, or configurationism is a school of psychology that emerged in the early twentieth century in Austria and Germany as a theory of perception that was a rejection of basic principles of Wilhelm Wundt's and Edward Titchener's elementalist and structuralist psychology.[1][2][3]

As used in Gestalt psychology, the German word Gestalt (/ɡəˈʃtælt, -ˈʃtɑːlt, -ˈʃtɔːlt, -ˈstɑːlt, -ˈstɔːlt/ gə-SHTA(H)LT, -⁠STAHLT, -⁠S(H)TAWLT,[4][5] German: [ɡəˈʃtalt] ; meaning "form"[6]) is interpreted as "pattern" or "configuration".[7] Gestalt psychologists emphasize that organisms perceive entire patterns or configurations, not merely individual components.[7] The view is sometimes summarized using the adage, "the whole is more than the sum of its parts."[8]: 13 

Gestalt psychology was founded on works by Max Wertheimer, Wolfgang Köhler, and Kurt Koffka.[7]

Origin and history edit

Max Wertheimer (1880–1943), Kurt Koffka (1886–1941), and Wolfgang Köhler (1887–1967) founded Gestalt psychology in the early 20th century.[8]: 113–116  The dominant view in psychology at the time was structuralism, exemplified by the work of Hermann von Helmholtz (1821–1894), Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920), and Edward B. Titchener (1867–1927).[9][10]: 3  Structuralism was rooted firmly in British empiricism[9][10]: 3  and was based on three closely interrelated theories:

  1. "atomism," also known as "elementalism,"[10]: 3  the view that all knowledge, even complex abstract ideas, is built from simple, elementary constituents
  2. "sensationalism," the view that the simplest constituents—the atoms of thought—are elementary sense impressions
  3. "associationism," the view that more complex ideas arise from the association of simpler ideas.[10]: 3 [11]

Together, these three theories give rise to the view that the mind constructs all perceptions and even abstract thoughts strictly from lower-level sensations that are related solely by being associated closely in space and time.[9] The Gestaltists took issue with this widespread "atomistic" view that the aim of psychology should be to break consciousness down into putative basic elements.[6]

In contrast, the Gestalt psychologists believed that breaking psychological phenomena down into smaller parts would not lead to understanding psychology.[8]: 13  The Gestalt psychologists believed, instead, that the most fruitful way to view psychological phenomena is as organized, structured wholes.[8]: 13  They argued that the psychological "whole" has priority and that the "parts" are defined by the structure of the whole, rather than vice versa. One could say that the approach was based on a macroscopic view of psychology rather than a microscopic approach.[12] Gestalt theories of perception are based on human nature being inclined to understand objects as an entire structure rather than the sum of its parts.[13]

Wertheimer had been a student of Austrian philosopher, Christian von Ehrenfels (1859–1932), a member of the School of Brentano. Von Ehrenfels introduced the concept of Gestalt to philosophy and psychology in 1890, before the advent of Gestalt psychology as such.[14][9] Von Ehrenfels observed that a perceptual experience, such as perceiving a melody or a shape, is more than the sum of its sensory components.[9] He claimed that, in addition to the sensory elements of the perception, there is something extra. Although in some sense derived from the organization of the component sensory elements, this further quality is an element in its own right. He called it Gestalt-qualität or "form-quality."

For instance, when one hears a melody, one hears the notes plus something in addition to them that binds them together into a tune – the Gestalt-qualität. It is this Gestalt-qualität that, according to von Ehrenfels, allows a tune to be transposed to a new key, using completely different notes, while still retaining its identity. The idea of a Gestalt-qualität has roots in theories by David Hume, Johann Wolfgang von Goethe, Immanuel Kant, David Hartley, and Ernst Mach. Both von Ehrenfels and Edmund Husserl seem to have been inspired by Mach's work Beiträge zur Analyse der Empfindungen (Contributions to the Analysis of Sensations, 1886), in formulating their very similar concepts of gestalt and figural moment, respectively.[14]

By 1914, the first published references to Gestalt theory could be found in a footnote of Gabriele von Wartensleben's application of Gestalt theory to personality. She was a student at Frankfurt Academy for Social Sciences, who interacted deeply with Wertheimer and Köhler.[15]

Through a series of experiments, Wertheimer discovered that a person observing a pair of alternating bars of light can, under the right conditions, experience the illusion of movement between one location and the other. He noted that this was a perception of motion absent any moving object. That is, it was pure phenomenal motion. He dubbed it phi ("phenomenal") motion.[14][16] Wertheimer's publication of these results in 1912[17] marks the beginning of Gestalt psychology.[16] In comparison to von Ehrenfels and others who had used the term "gestalt" earlier in various ways, Wertheimer's unique contribution was to insist that the "gestalt" is perceptually primary. The gestalt defines the parts from which it is composed, rather than being a secondary quality that emerges from those parts.[16] Wertheimer took the more radical position that "what is given me by the melody does not arise ... as a secondary process from the sum of the pieces as such. Instead, what takes place in each single part already depends upon what the whole is", (1925/1938). In other words, one hears the melody first and only then may perceptually divide it up into notes. Similarly, in vision, one sees the form of the circle first—it is given "im-mediately" (i.e., its apprehension is not mediated by a process of part-summation). Only after this primary apprehension might one notice that it is made up of lines or dots or stars.

The two men who served as Wertheimer's subjects in the phi experiments were Köhler and Koffka. Köhler was an expert in physical acoustics, having studied under physicist Max Planck (1858–1947), but had taken his degree in psychology under Carl Stumpf (1848–1936). Koffka was also a student of Stumpf's, having studied movement phenomena and psychological aspects of rhythm. In 1917, Köhler (1917/1925) published the results of four years of research on learning in chimpanzees. Köhler showed, contrary to the claims of most other learning theorists, that animals can learn by "sudden insight" into the "structure" of a problem, over and above the associative and incremental manner of learning that Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) and Edward Lee Thorndike (1874–1949) had demonstrated with dogs and cats, respectively.

In 1921, Koffka published a Gestalt-oriented text on developmental psychology, Growth of the Mind. With the help of American psychologist Robert Ogden, Koffka introduced the Gestalt point of view to an American audience in 1922 by way of a paper in Psychological Bulletin. It contains criticisms of then-current explanations of a number of problems of perception, and the alternatives offered by the Gestalt school. Koffka moved to the United States in 1924, eventually settling at Smith College in 1927. In 1935, Koffka published his Principles of Gestalt Psychology. This textbook laid out the Gestalt vision of the scientific enterprise as a whole. Science, he said, is not the simple accumulation of facts. What makes research scientific is the incorporation of facts into a theoretical structure. The goal of the Gestaltists was to integrate the facts of inanimate nature, life, and mind into a single scientific structure. This meant that science would have to accommodate not only what Koffka called the quantitative facts of physical science but the facts of two other "scientific categories": questions of order and questions of Sinn, a German word which has been variously translated as significance, value, and meaning. Without incorporating the meaning of experience and behavior, Koffka believed that science would doom itself to trivialities in its investigation of human beings.

Having survived the Nazis up to the mid-1930s,[18] all the core members of the Gestalt movement were forced out of Germany to the United States by 1935.[19] Köhler published another book, Dynamics in Psychology, in 1940 but thereafter the Gestalt movement suffered a series of setbacks. Koffka died in 1941 and Wertheimer in 1943. Wertheimer's long-awaited book on mathematical problem-solving, Productive Thinking, was published posthumously in 1945, but Köhler was left to guide the movement without his two long-time colleagues.[note 1]

Gestalt therapy edit

Gestalt psychology should not be confused with the Gestalt therapy, which is only peripherally linked to Gestalt psychology. The founders of Gestalt therapy, Fritz and Laura Perls, had worked with Kurt Goldstein, a neurologist who had applied principles of Gestalt psychology to the functioning of the organism. Laura Perls had been a Gestalt psychologist before she became a psychoanalyst and before she began developing Gestalt therapy together with Fritz Perls.[20] The extent to which Gestalt psychology influenced Gestalt therapy is disputed, however. In any case, it is not identical to Gestalt psychology. On the one hand, Laura Perls preferred not to use the term "Gestalt" to name the emerging new therapy, because she thought that the Gestalt psychologists would object to it;[21] on the other hand, Fritz and Laura Perls clearly adopted some of Goldstein's work.[22] Thus, though recognizing the historical connection and the influence, most Gestalt psychologists emphasize that Gestalt therapy is not a form of Gestalt psychology.

Mary Henle noted in her presidential address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association (1975): "What Perls has done has been to take a few terms from Gestalt psychology, stretch their meaning beyond recognition, mix them with notions—often unclear and often incompatible—from the depth psychologies, existentialism, and common sense, and he has called the whole mixture gestalt therapy. His work has no substantive relation to scientific Gestalt psychology. To use his own language, Fritz Perls has done 'his thing'; whatever it is, it is not Gestalt psychology"[23] With her analysis, however, she restricts herself explicitly to only three of Perls' books from 1969 and 1972, leaving out Perls' earlier work, and Gestalt therapy in general as a psychotherapy method.[22]

There have been clinical applications of Gestalt psychology in the psychotherapeutic field long before Perls'ian Gestalt therapy, in group psychoanalysis (Foulkes), Adlerian individual psychology, by Gestalt psychologists in psychotherapy like Erwin Levy, Abraham S. Luchins, by Gestalt psychologically oriented psychoanalysts in Italy (Canestrari and others), and there have been newer developments foremost in Europe. For example, a strictly Gestalt psychology-based therapeutic method is Gestalt Theoretical Psychotherapy, developed by the German Gestalt psychologist and psychotherapist Hans-Jürgen Walter and his colleagues in Germany, Austria (Gerhard Stemberger and colleagues) and Switzerland. Other countries, especially Italy, have seen similar developments.

Contributions edit

Gestalt psychology made many contributions to the body of psychology. The Gestaltists were the first to demonstrate empirically and document many facts about perception—including facts about the perception of movement, the perception of contour, perceptual constancy, and perceptual illusions.[14] Wertheimer's discovery of the phi phenomenon is one example of such a contribution.[24] In addition to discovering perceptual phenomena, the contributions of Gestalt psychology include: (a) a unique theoretical framework and methodology, (b) a set of perceptual principles, (c) a well-known set of perceptual grouping laws, (d) a theory of problem solving based on insight, and (e) a theory of memory. The following subsections discuss these contributions in turn.

Theoretical framework and methodology edit

The Gestalt psychologists practiced a set of theoretical and methodological principles that attempted to redefine the approach to psychological research. This is in contrast to investigations developed at the beginning of the 20th century, based on traditional scientific methodology, which divided the object of study into a set of elements that could be analyzed separately with the objective of reducing the complexity of this object.

The theoretical principles are the following:

  • Principle of totality—Conscious experience must be considered globally (by taking into account all the physical and mental aspects of the individual simultaneously) because the nature of the mind demands that each component be considered as part of a system of dynamic relationships. Wertheimer described holism as fundamental to Gestalt psychology,[9] writing "There are wholes, the behavior of which is not determined by that of their individual elements, but where the part-processes are themselves determined by the intrinsic nature of the whole."[25] In other words, a perceptual whole is different from what one would predict based on only its individual parts.[9] Moreover, the nature of a part depends upon the whole in which it is embedded.[9] Köhler, for example, writes "In psychology...we have wholes which, instead of being the sum of parts existing independently, give their parts specific functions or properties that can only be defined in relation to the whole in question."[26] Thus, the maxim that the whole is more than the sum of its parts is not a precise description of the Gestaltist view.[9] Rather, "The whole is something else than the sum of its parts, because summing is a meaningless procedure, whereas the whole-part relationship is meaningful."[27]

Based on the principles above the following methodological principles are defined:

  • Phenomenon experimental analysis—In relation to the Totality Principle any psychological research should take phenomena as a starting point and not be solely focused on sensory qualities.
  • Biotic experiment—The Gestalt psychologists established a need to conduct real experiments that sharply contrasted with and opposed classic laboratory experiments. This signified experimenting in natural situations, developed in real conditions, in which it would be possible to reproduce, with higher fidelity, what would be habitual for a subject.[28]

Properties edit

The key principles of gestalt systems are emergence, reification, multistability and invariance.[29]

Reification edit

 
Reification

Reification is the constructive or generative aspect of perception, by which the experienced percept contains more explicit spatial information than the sensory stimulus on which it is based.

For instance, a triangle is perceived in picture A, though no triangle is there. In pictures B and D the eye recognizes disparate shapes as "belonging" to a single shape, in C a complete three-dimensional shape is seen, where in actuality no such thing is drawn.

Reification can be explained by progress in the study of illusory contours, which are treated by the visual system as "real" contours.

Multistability edit

 
The Necker cube and the Rubin vase, two examples of multistability

Multistability (or multistable perception) is the tendency of ambiguous perceptual experiences to pop back and forth unstably between two or more alternative interpretations. This is seen, for example, in the Necker cube and Rubin's Figure/Vase illusion shown here. Other examples include the three-legged blivet and artist M. C. Escher's artwork and the appearance of flashing marquee lights moving first one direction and then suddenly the other. Again, Gestalt psychology does not explain how images appear multistable, only that they do.

Invariance edit

 
Invariance

Invariance is the property of perception whereby simple geometrical objects are recognized independent of rotation, translation, and scale; as well as several other variations such as elastic deformations, different lighting, and different component features. For example, the objects in A in the figure are all immediately recognized as the same basic shape, which is immediately distinguishable from the forms in B. They are even recognized despite perspective and elastic deformations as in C, and when depicted using different graphic elements as in D. Computational theories of vision, such as those by David Marr, have provided alternate explanations of how perceived objects are classified.

Emergence, reification, multistability, and invariance are not necessarily separable modules to model individually, but they could be different aspects of a single unified dynamic mechanism.[30]

Figure-ground organization edit

The perceptual field (what an organism perceives) is organized. Figure-ground organization is one form of perceptual organization.[16] Figure-ground organization is the interpretation of perceptual elements in terms of their shapes and relative locations in the layout of surfaces in the 3-D world.[16] Figure-ground organization structures the perceptual field into a figure (standing out at the front of the perceptual field) and a background (receding behind the figure).[31] Pioneering work on figure-ground organization was carried out by the Danish psychologist Edgar Rubin. The Gestalt psychologists demonstrated that we tend to perceive as figures those parts of our perceptual fields that are convex, symmetric, small, and enclosed.[16]

Prägnanz edit

Like figure-ground organization, perceptual grouping (sometimes called perceptual segregation[32]) is a form of perceptual organization.[16] Organisms perceive some parts of their perceptual fields as "hanging together" more tightly than others.[16] They use this information for object detection.[32] Perceptual grouping is the process that determines what these "pieces" of the perceptual field are.[16]

The Gestaltists were the first psychologists to systematically study perceptual grouping.[32] According to Gestalt psychologists, the fundamental principle of perceptual grouping is the law of Prägnanz.[32] (The law of Prägnanz is also known as the law of good Gestalt.) Prägnanz is a German word that directly translates to "pithiness" and implies salience, conciseness, and orderliness.[33] The law of Prägnanz says that we tend to experience things as regular, orderly, symmetrical, and simple. As Koffka put it, "Of several geometrically possible organizations that one will actually occur which possesses the best, simplest and most stable shape."[34]

The law of Prägnanz implies that, as individuals perceive the world, they eliminate complexity and unfamiliarity so they can observe reality in its most simplistic form. Eliminating extraneous stimuli helps the mind create meaning. This meaning created by perception implies a global regularity, which is often mentally prioritized over spatial relations. The law of good Gestalt focuses on the idea of conciseness, which is what all of Gestalt theory is based on.[35]

A major aspect of Gestalt psychology is that it implies that the mind understands external stimuli as wholes rather than as the sums of their parts. The wholes are structured and organized using grouping laws.

Gestalt psychologists attempted to discover refinements of the law of Prägnanz, and this involved writing down laws that, hypothetically, allow us to predict the interpretation of sensation, what is often called "gestalt laws".[36] Wertheimer defined a few principles that explain the ways humans perceive objects. Those principles were based on similarity, proximity, continuity.[37] The Gestalt concept is based on perceiving reality in its simplest form.[38] The various laws are called laws or principles, depending on the paper where they appear—but for simplicity's sake, this article uses the term laws. These laws took several forms, such as the grouping of similar, or proximate, objects together, within this global process. These laws deal with the sensory modality of vision. However, there are analogous laws for other sensory modalities including auditory, tactile, gustatory, and olfactory (Bregman – GP). The visual Gestalt principles of grouping were introduced in Wertheimer (1923). Through the 1930s and 1940s Wertheimer, Kohler and Koffka formulated many of the laws of grouping through the study of visual perception.

Law of proximity edit

 
Law of proximity

The law of proximity states that when an individual perceives an assortment of objects, they perceive objects that are close to each other as forming a group. For example, in the figure illustrating the law of proximity, there are 72 circles, but we perceive the collection of circles in groups. Specifically, we perceive that there is a group of 36 circles on the left side of the image and three groups of 12 circles on the right side of the image. This law is often used in advertising logos to emphasize which aspects of events are associated.[35][39]

Law of similarity edit

 
Law of similarity

The law of similarity states that elements within an assortment of objects are perceptually grouped together if they are similar to each other. This similarity can occur in the form of shape, colour, shading or other qualities. For example, the figure illustrating the law of similarity portrays 36 circles all equal distance apart from one another forming a square. In this depiction, 18 of the circles are shaded dark, and 18 of the circles are shaded light. We perceive the dark circles as grouped together and the light circles as grouped together, forming six horizontal lines within the square of circles. This perception of lines is due to the law of similarity.[39]

Law of closure edit

 
Law of closure

Gestalt psychologists believed that humans tend to perceive objects as complete rather than focusing on the gaps that the object might contain.[40] For example, a circle has good Gestalt in terms of completeness. However, we will also perceive an incomplete circle as a complete circle.[31] That tendency to complete shapes and figures is called closure.[31] The law of closure states that individuals perceive objects such as shapes, letters, pictures, etc., as being whole when they are not complete. Specifically, when parts of a whole picture are missing, our perception fills in the visual gap. Research shows that the reason the mind completes a regular figure that is not perceived through sensation is to increase the regularity of surrounding stimuli. For example, the figure that depicts the law of closure portrays what we perceive as a circle on the left side of the image and a rectangle on the right side of the image. However, gaps are present in the shapes. If the law of closure did not exist, the image would depict an assortment of different lines with different lengths, rotations, and curvatures—but with the law of closure, we perceptually combine the lines into whole shapes.[35][39][41]

Law of symmetry edit

 
Law of symmetry

The law of symmetry states that the mind perceives objects as being symmetrical and forming around a center point. It is perceptually pleasing to divide objects into an even number of symmetrical parts. Therefore, when two symmetrical elements are unconnected the mind perceptually connects them to form a coherent shape. Similarities between symmetrical objects increase the likelihood that objects are grouped to form a combined symmetrical object. For example, the figure depicting the law of symmetry shows a configuration of square and curled brackets. When the image is perceived, we tend to observe three pairs of symmetrical brackets rather than six individual brackets.[35][39]

Law of common fate edit

The law of common fate states that objects are perceived as lines that move along the smoothest path. Experiments using the visual sensory modality found that the movement of elements of an object produces paths that individuals perceive that the objects are on. We perceive elements of objects to have trends of motion, which indicate the path that the object is on. The law of continuity implies the grouping together of objects that have the same trend of motion and are therefore on the same path. For example, if there is an array of dots and half the dots are moving upward while the other half are moving downward, we would perceive the upward moving dots and the downward moving dots as two distinct units.[33]

Law of continuity edit

 
Law of continuity

The law of continuity (also known as the law of good continuation) states that elements of objects tend to be grouped together, and therefore integrated into perceptual wholes if they are aligned within an object. In cases where there is an intersection between objects, individuals tend to perceive the two objects as two single uninterrupted entities. Stimuli remain distinct even with overlap. We are less likely to group elements with sharp abrupt directional changes as being one object. For example, the figure depicting the law of continuity shows a configuration of two crossed keys. When the image is perceived, we tend to perceive the key in the background as a single uninterrupted key instead of two separate halves of a key.[35]

Law of past experience edit

The law of past experience implies that under some circumstances visual stimuli are categorized according to past experience. If objects tend to be observed within close proximity, or small temporal intervals, the objects are more likely to be perceived together. For example, the English language contains 26 letters that are grouped to form words using a set of rules. If an individual reads an English word they have never seen, they use the law of past experience to interpret the letters "L" and "I" as two letters beside each other, rather than using the law of closure to combine the letters and interpret the object as an uppercase U.[33]

Music edit

An example of the Gestalt movement in effect, as it is both a process and result, is a music sequence. People are able to recognise a sequence of perhaps six or seven notes, despite them being transposed into a different tuning or key.[42] An early theory of gestalt grouping principles in music was composer-theorist James Tenney's Meta+Hodos (1961).[43] Auditory Scene Analysis as developed by Albert Bregman further extends a gestalt approach to the analysis of sound perception.

Problem solving and insight edit

Gestalt psychology contributed to the scientific study of problem solving.[24] In fact, the early experimental work of the Gestaltists in Germany[note 2] marks the beginning of the scientific study of problem solving. Later this experimental work continued through the 1960s and early 1970s with research conducted on relatively simple (but novel for participants) laboratory tasks of problem solving.[note 3][44]

Given Gestalt psychology's focus on the whole, it was natural for Gestalt psychologists to study problem-solving from the perspective of insight, seeking to understand the process by which organisms sometimes suddenly transition from having no idea how to solve a problem to instantly understanding the whole problem and its solution.[8]: 13  In a famous set of experiments, Köhler gave chimpanzees some boxes and placed food high off the ground; after some time, the chimpanzees appeared to suddenly realize that they could stack the boxes on top of each other to reach the food.[45]: 362 

Max Wertheimer distinguished two kinds of thinking: productive thinking and reproductive thinking.[8]: 456 [46][45]: 361  Productive thinking is solving a problem based on insight—a quick, creative, unplanned response to situations and environmental interaction. Reproductive thinking is solving a problem deliberately based on previous experience and knowledge. Reproductive thinking proceeds algorithmically—a problem solver reproduces a series of steps from memory, knowing that they will lead to a solution—or by trial and error.[45]: 361 

Karl Duncker, another Gestalt psychologist who studied problem solving,[45]: 370  coined the term functional fixedness for describing the difficulties in both visual perception and problem solving that arise from the fact that one element of a whole situation already has a (fixed) function that has to be changed in order to perceive something or find the solution to a problem.[47]

Abraham Luchins also studied problem solving from the perspective of Gestalt psychology. He is well known for his research on the role of mental set (Einstellung effect), which he demonstrated using a series of problems having to do with refilling water jars.[45]: 383 

Another Gestalt psychologist, Perkins, believes insight deals with three processes:

  1. Unconscious leap in thinking.[36]
  2. The increased amount of speed in mental processing.
  3. The amount of short-circuiting that occurs in normal reasoning.[48][49][50]

Views going against the Gestalt psychology are:

  1. Nothing-special view
  2. Neo-gestalt view
  3. The Three-Process View

Fuzzy-trace theory of memory edit

Fuzzy-trace theory, a process model of explaining and reasoning, was also derived from Gestalt psychology. Fuzzy-trace theory posits that we encode information into two separate traces: verbatim and gist. Information stored in verbatim and is exact for detail (the individual parts of a pattern, for example) while information stored in gist is semantic and conceptual (what we perceive the pattern to be). The effects seen in Gestalt psychology can be attributed to the way we encode information as gist.[51][52]

Legacy edit

Gestalt psychology struggled to precisely define terms like Prägnanz, to make specific behavioural predictions, and to articulate testable models of underlying neural mechanisms.[9] It was criticized as being merely descriptive.[53] These shortcomings led, by the mid-20th century, to growing dissatisfaction with Gestaltism and a subsequent decline in its impact on psychology.[9] Despite this decline, Gestalt psychology has formed the basis of much further research into the perception of patterns and objects[54] and of research into behaviour, thinking, problem solving and psychopathology.

Support from cybernetics and neurology edit

In the 1940s and 1950s, laboratory research in neurology and what became known as cybernetics on the mechanism of frogs' eyes indicate that perception of 'gestalts' (in particular gestalts in motion) is perhaps more primitive and fundamental than 'seeing' as such:

A frog hunts on land by vision... He has no fovea, or region of greatest acuity in vision, upon which he must centre a part of the image... The frog does not seem to see or, at any rate, is not concerned with the detail of stationary parts of the world around him. He will starve to death surrounded by food if it is not moving. His choice of food is determined only by size and movement. He will leap to capture any object the size of an insect or worm, providing it moves like one. He can be fooled easily not only by a piece of dangled meat but by any moving small object... He does remember a moving thing provided it stays within his field of vision and he is not distracted.[55]
The lowest-level concepts related to visual perception for a human being probably differ little from the concepts of a frog. In any case, the structure of the retina in mammals and in human beings is the same as in amphibians. The phenomenon of distortion of perception of an image stabilised on the retina gives some idea of the concepts of the subsequent levels of the hierarchy. This is a very interesting phenomenon. When a person looks at an immobile object, "fixes" it with his eyes, the eyeballs do not remain absolutely immobile; they make small involuntary movements. As a result, the image of the object on the retina is constantly in motion, slowly drifting and jumping back to the point of maximum sensitivity. The image "marks time" in the vicinity of this point.[56]

In the 1990s, Andranik Tangian developed a model of artificial perception that implemented a principle of correlativity, which operationalized the Gestalt psychology laws in their interaction. The model finds structures in data without knowing the structures, similarly to segregating elements in abstract painting—like curves, contours and spots—without identifying them with known objects. The approach is based on the least complex data representations in the sense of Kolmogorov, i.e. requiring the least memory storage, which is regarded as saving the brain energy. The least complexity criterion leads to multi-level data representations in terms of generative patterns and their transformations, using proximities, similarities, symmetries, common fate grouping, continuities, etc. The idea that perception is data representation rather than "physical" recognition is illustrated by the effect of several voices produced by a single physical body—a loudspeaker membrane, whereas the effect of a single tone is produced by several physical bodies—organ pipes tuned as a chord and activated by a single key. It is shown that the physical causality in certain observations can be revealed through optimal data representations, and this nature–information duality is explained by the fact that both nature and information are subordinated to the same principle of efficiency. In some situations, the least complex data representations use the patterns already stored in the memory, demonstrating the dependence of perception on previous knowledge—in line with the Gestalt psychology law of past experience. Such an intelligent perception is opposed to the naïve perception that is based exclusively on direct percepts and is therefore context-dependent. The model is applied to automatic notation of music—recognition of interval structures in chords and polyphonic voices (with no reference to pitch, thereby relying on interval hearing instead of absolute hearing) as well as rhythms under variable tempo, approaching the capabilities of trained musicians. The model is also relevant to visual scene analysis and explains some modes of abstract thinking.[57][58][59]

Quantum cognition modelling edit

Similarities between Gestalt phenomena and quantum mechanics have been pointed out by, among others, chemist Anton Amann, who commented that "similarities between Gestalt perception and quantum mechanics are on a level of a parable" yet may give useful insight nonetheless.[60] Physicists David Bohm and Basil Hiley, together with philosopher Paavo Pylkkänen, have suggested that mind and matter both emerge from an "implicate order".[61][62]

Use in contemporary social psychology edit

The halo effect can be explained through the application of Gestalt theories to social information processing.[63][13] The constructive theories of social cognition are applied to the expectations of individuals. They have been perceived in this manner and the person judging the individual is continuing to view them in this positive manner.[13] Gestalt's theories of perception enforces that individual's tendency to perceive actions and characteristics as a whole rather than isolated parts,[13] therefore humans are inclined to build a coherent and consistent impression of objects and behaviors in order to achieve an acceptable shape and form. The halo effect is what forms patterns for individuals,[13] the halo effect being classified as a cognitive bias which occurs during impression formation.[63] The halo effect can also be altered by physical characteristics, social status and many other characteristics.[64] As well, the halo effect can have real repercussions on the individual's perception of reality, either negatively or positively, meaning to construct negative or positive images about other individuals or situations, something that could lead to self-fulfilling prophesies, stereotyping, or even discrimination.[13]

Contemporary cognitive and perceptual psychology edit

Some of the central criticisms of Gestaltism are based on the preference Gestaltists are deemed to have for theory over data, and a lack of quantitative research supporting Gestalt ideas. This is not necessarily a fair criticism as highlighted by a recent collection of quantitative research on Gestalt perception.[65] Researchers continue to test hypotheses about the mechanisms underlying Gestalt principles such as the principle of similarity.[66]

Other important criticisms concern the lack of definition and support for the many physiological assumptions made by gestaltists[67] and lack of theoretical coherence in modern Gestalt psychology.[65]

In some scholarly communities, such as cognitive psychology and computational neuroscience, gestalt theories of perception are criticized for being descriptive rather than explanatory in nature. For this reason, they are viewed by some as redundant or uninformative. For example, a textbook on visual perception states that, "The physiological theory of the gestaltists has fallen by the wayside, leaving us with a set of descriptive principles, but without a model of perceptual processing. Indeed, some of their 'laws' of perceptual organisation today sound vague and inadequate. What is meant by a 'good' or 'simple' shape, for example?"[53]

One historian of psychology, David J. Murray, has argued that Gestalt psychologists first discovered many principles later championed by cognitive psychology, including schemas and prototypes.[68] Another psychologist has argued that the Gestalt psychologists made a lasting contribution by showing how the study of illusions can help scientists understand essential aspects of how the visual system normally functions, not merely how it breaks down.[10]: 16 

Use in design edit

 
Several grouping principles are employed in this map: similarity allows the reader to selectively isolate cities, rivers, or state boundaries; closure allows the dashed boundary lines to be perceived as continuous borders; proximity makes the collection of river segments appear as a single watershed; and continuity helps the reader "see" whole states even when boundaries are obscured under rivers.

The gestalt laws are used in several visual design fields, such as user interface design and cartography. The laws of similarity and proximity can, for example, be used as guides for placing radio buttons. They may also be used in designing computers and software for more intuitive human use. Examples include the design and layout of a desktop's shortcuts in rows and columns.[39]

In map design, principles of Prägnanz or grouping are crucial for implying a conceptual order to the portrayed geographic features, thus facilitating the intended use of the map.[69] The Law of Similarity is employed by selecting similar map symbols for similar kinds of features or features with similar properties; the Law of Proximity is crucial to identifying geographic patterns and regions; and the Laws of Closure and Continuity allow users to recognize features that may be obscured by other features (such as when a road goes over a river).

See also edit

Notes edit

  1. ^ For more on the history of Gestalt psychology, see Ash, M. G. (1995). Gestalt psychology in German culture, 1890–1967: Holism and the quest for objectivity. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press..
  2. ^ For example, Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.
  3. ^ For example Duncker's "X-ray" problem; Ewert & Lambert's "disk" problem in 1932, later known as Tower of Hanoi.

References edit

  1. ^ Mather, George (2006). Foundations of Perception. Psychology Press. p. 32. ISBN 978-0-86377-834-6.
  2. ^ "Gestalt psychology". Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 2 January 2023.
  3. ^ Baker, David B., ed. (13 January 2012). The Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology: Global Perspectives. Oxford University Press, USA. p. 576. ISBN 978-0-19-536655-6.
  4. ^ Wells, John (3 April 2008). Longman Pronunciation Dictionary (3rd ed.). Pearson Longman. ISBN 978-1-4058-8118-0.
  5. ^ "Definition of gestalt | Dictionary.com". www.dictionary.com. Retrieved 2 January 2023.
  6. ^ a b "Gestalt". The Columbia Encyclopedia. Columbia University Press. 2018. ISBN 9781786848468. from the original on 22 July 2019. Retrieved 1 August 2019.
  7. ^ a b c "Gestalt psychology". Britannica Concise Encyclopedia. Encyclopaedia Britannica, Inc. 1 May 2008. p. 756. ISBN 9781593394929. from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
  8. ^ a b c d e f Sternberg, Robert J.; Sternberg, Karin (2012). Cognitive Psychology (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-1-133-31391-5.
  9. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Wagemans, Johan; Feldman, Jacob; Gepshtein, Sergei; Kimchi, Ruth; Pomerantz, James R.; van der Helm, Peter A.; van Leeuwen, Cees (2012). "A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: II. Conceptual and theoretical foundations". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (6): 1218–1252. doi:10.1037/a0029334. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 3728284. PMID 22845750.
  10. ^ a b c d e Kolers, Paul A. (1972). Aspects of Motion Perception: International Series of Monographs in Experimental Psychology. New York: Pergamon. ISBN 978-1-4831-7113-5.
  11. ^ Hamlyn, D. W. (1957). The Psychology of Perception: A Philosophical Examination of Gestalt Theory and Derivative Theories of Perception (eBook ed.). London: Routledge. pp. 88–89. ISBN 978-1-315-47329-1. from the original on 21 October 2020. Retrieved 19 October 2019.
  12. ^ Verstegen, Ian (2010). "Gestalt Psychology". The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. pp. 1–3. doi:10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0386. ISBN 9780470479216.
  13. ^ a b c d e f Pohl, Rüdiger F. (22 July 2016). Cognitive Illusions: Intriguing Phenomena in Judgement, Thinking and Memory. Psychology Press. ISBN 9781317448280. from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
  14. ^ a b c d Smith, Barry (1988). . In Smith, Barry (ed.). Foundations of Gestalt Theory. Vienna: Philosophia Verlag. pp. 11–81. Archived from (PDF) on 22 February 2012. Retrieved 12 October 2019.
  15. ^ King, D. Brett; Wertheimer, Michael (1 January 2005). Max Wertheimer and Gestalt Theory. Transaction Publishers. ISBN 978-1-4128-2826-0. from the original on 26 February 2022. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
  16. ^ a b c d e f g h i j Wagemans, Johan; Elder, James H.; Kubovy, Michael; Palmer, Stephen E.; Peterson, Mary A.; Singh, Manish; von der Heydt, Rüdiger (2012). "A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (6): 1172–1217. doi:10.1037/a0029333. ISSN 1939-1455. PMC 3482144. PMID 22845751.
  17. ^ Wertheimer, Max (1912). "Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung" (PDF). Zeitschrift für Psychologie (in German). 61: 161–265. (PDF) from the original on 23 November 2018. Retrieved 14 October 2019. Available in translation as Wertheimer, Max (2012). "Experimental Studies on Seeing Motion". In Spillman, Lothar (ed.). On Perceived Motion and Figural Organization. Michael Wertheimer, K. W. Watkins (trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT.
  18. ^ Henle, M (1978). "One man against the Nazis: Wolfgang Köhler" (PDF). American Psychologist. 33 (10): 939–944. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.33.10.939. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002B-9E5F-5. from the original on 26 February 2022. Retrieved 16 October 2019.
  19. ^ Henle, M (1984). "Robert M. Ogden and gestalt psychology in America". Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences. 20 (1): 9–19. doi:10.1002/1520-6696(198401)20:1<9::aid-jhbs2300200103>3.0.co;2-u. PMID 11608590.
  20. ^ Bocian, B. (2010). Fritz Perls in Berlin 1893 - 1933: Expressionism - Psychoanalysis - Judaism. EHP - Edition Humanistische Psychology. p. 190. ISBN 978-3-89797-068-7.
  21. ^ Joe Wysong/Edward Rosenfeld (eds): An Oral History of Gestalt Therapy, Highland, New York 1982, The Gestalt Journal Press, p. 12.
  22. ^ a b Barlow, Allen R. (Fall 1981). "Gestalt—Antecedent Influence or Historical Accident". The Gestalt Journal. 4 (2). Retrieved 2 January 2023.
  23. ^ Mary Henle 1975: Gestalt Psychology and Gestalt Therapy 5 February 2012 at the Wayback Machine; Presidential Address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association, Chicago, September 1975. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 14, pp 23-32.
  24. ^ a b Gobet, Fernand (2017), "Entrenchment, Gestalt formation, and chunking", in Schmid, H. J. (ed.), Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning: How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge, American Psychological Association, pp. 245–267, doi:10.1037/15969-012, ISBN 978-3110341300, from the original on 26 February 2022, retrieved 14 October 2019
  25. ^ Wertheimer, Max (1938) [Original work published 1924]. "Gestalt theory". In Ellis, Willis D. (ed.). A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. pp. 2. Retrieved 16 October 2019.
  26. ^ Köhler, Wolfgang (1971) [Original work published 1930]. "Human perception". In Henle, Mary (ed.). The Selected Papers of Wolfgang Köhler. Liveright. p. 145. ISBN 978-0-87140-253-0.
  27. ^ Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace. pp. 176. Retrieved 13 October 2019.
  28. ^ William Ray Woodward, Robert Sonné Cohen – World views and scientific discipline formation: science studies in the German Democratic Republic: papers from a German-American summer institute, 1988
  29. ^ Steven., Lehar (2003). . Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers. ISBN 0805841768. OCLC 52051454. Archived from the original on 19 July 2011. Retrieved 19 August 2005.
  30. ^ . Sharp.bu.edu. Archived from the original on 17 February 2012. Retrieved 6 April 2012.
  31. ^ a b c Brennan, James F.; Houde, Keith A. (26 October 2017). History and Systems of Psychology. Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/9781316827178. ISBN 9781107178670. S2CID 142935847.
  32. ^ a b c d Eysenck, Michael W. (2006). Fundamentals of Cognition. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. pp. 62–64. ISBN 978-1-84169-374-3.
  33. ^ a b c Todorovic, Dejan (2008). "Gestalt Principles". Scholarpedia. 3 (12): 5345. Bibcode:2008SchpJ...3.5345T. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.5345.
  34. ^ Koffka, K. (1935). Principles Of Gestalt Psychology. New York: Harcourt, Brace. Retrieved 13 October 2019.
  35. ^ a b c d e Stevenson, Herb. "Emergence: The Gestalt Approach to Change". Unleashing Executive and Orzanizational Potential. from the original on 21 June 2012. Retrieved 7 April 2012.
  36. ^ a b Sternberg, Robert (2003). Cognitive psychology (3rd ed.). Belmont, CA: Thomson/Wadsworth. ISBN 978-0-15-508535-0. OCLC 883578008.
  37. ^ Craighead, W. Edward; Nemeroff, Charles B. (19 April 2004). "Gestalt psychology". The Concise Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology and Behavioral Science. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 401–404. ISBN 9780471220367. from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
  38. ^ Reber, Arthur S. (1985). "Gestalt psychology". The Penguin Dictionary of Psychology. Viking. ISBN 9780670801367. from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 2 October 2020.
  39. ^ a b c d e Soegaard, Mads. Gestalt Principles of form Perception. Interaction Design. from the original on 10 April 2012. Retrieved 8 April 2012.
  40. ^ Hamlyn, D. W. (27 March 2017). The Psychology of Perception. doi:10.4324/9781315473291. ISBN 9781315473291.
  41. ^ "Why Your Brain Thinks These Dots Are a Dog". Gizmodo UK. from the original on 24 September 2015. Retrieved 23 March 2018.
  42. ^ Ellis, Willis D. (1999). A source book of Gestalt psychology. Vol. 2. Psychology Press.
  43. ^ Tenney, James. (1961) 2015. “Meta+Hodos.” In From Scratch: Writings in Music Theory. Edited by Larry Polansky, Lauren Pratt, Robert Wannamaker, and Michael Winter. Urbana: University of Illinois Press. 13–96.
  44. ^ Mayer, R. E. (1992). Thinking, problem solving, cognition. Second edition. New York: W. H. Freeman and Company.
  45. ^ a b c d e Kellogg, Ronald T. (2003). Cognitive Psychology (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. ISBN 0-7619-2130-3.
  46. ^ Wertheimer, Max (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper.
  47. ^ Duncker, Karl (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking] (in German). Berlin: Julius Springer.
  48. ^ Perkins, D.N. (1981). The Mind's Best Work. Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-57624-7. from the original on 26 February 2022. Retrieved 30 January 2022.
  49. ^ Weisberg, R.W. (1986). Creativity: Genius and Other Myths. Psychology Series. W.H. Freeman. ISBN 978-0-7167-1768-3. from the original on 26 February 2022. Retrieved 30 January 2022.
  50. ^ Weisberg, 1995
  51. ^ Reyna, Valerie (2012). "A new institutionism: Meaning, memory, and development in Fuzzy-Trace Theory". Judgment and Decision Making. 7 (3): 332–359. doi:10.1017/S1930297500002291. PMC 4268540. PMID 25530822.
  52. ^ Barghout, Lauren (2014). "Visual Taxometric Approach to Image Segmentation Using Fuzzy-Spatial Taxon Cut Yields Contextually Relevant Regions". Information Processing and Management of Uncertainty in Knowledge-Based Systems. Communications in Computer and Information Science. 443: 163–173. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-08855-6_17. ISBN 978-3-319-08854-9.
  53. ^ a b Bruce, V.; Green, P.; Georgeson, M. (1996). Visual perception: Physiology, psychology and ecology (3rd ed.). LEA. p. 110.
  54. ^ Carlson, Neil R.; Heth, C. Donald (2010). Psychology: The Science of Behaviour. Ontario: Pearson Education Canada. pp. 20–22.
  55. ^ Lettvin, J.Y., Maturana, H.R., Pitts, W.H., and McCulloch, W.S. (1961). Two Remarks on the Visual System of the Frog. In Sensory Communication edited by Walter Rosenblith, MIT Press and John Wiley and Sons: New York
  56. ^ Valentin Fedorovich Turchin – The phenomenon of science – a cybernetic approach to human evolution – Columbia University Press, 1977
  57. ^ Tanguiane (Tangian), Andranick (1993). Tanguiane, Andranick S (ed.). Artificial Perception and Music Recognition. Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence. Vol. 746. Berlin-Heidelberg: Springer. doi:10.1007/BFb0019384. ISBN 978-3-540-57394-4. S2CID 20835144. from the original on 15 April 2021. Retrieved 21 February 2021.
  58. ^ Tanguiane (Tangian), Andranick (1994). "Principle of correlativity of perception and its applications to music recognition". Music Perception. 11 (4): 465–502. doi:10.2307/40285634. JSTOR 40285634. from the original on 26 February 2022. Retrieved 21 February 2021.
  59. ^ Tangian, Andranik (2021). How do we think: Modeling interactions of perception and memory. KIT Scientific Working Papers. Vol. 166. Karlsruhe: Karlsruhe Institute of Technology (KIT). doi:10.5445/IR/1000133287. ISSN 2194-1629. S2CID 237995668.
  60. ^ Amann, Anton (1993). "The Gestalt problem in quantum theory: Generation of molecular shape by the environment". Synthese. Springer Science and Business Media LLC. 97 (1): 125–156. doi:10.1007/bf01255834. ISSN 0039-7857. S2CID 46973964.
  61. ^ B.J. Hiley: Particles, fields, and observers, Volume I The Origins of Life, Part 1 Origin and Evolution of Life, Section II The Physical and Chemical Basis of Life, pp. 87–106 (PDF 4 March 2016 at the Wayback Machine)
  62. ^ Hiley, Basil J.; Pylkkänen, Paavo (2001). "Naturalizing the mind in a quantum framework". In Pylkkänen, P.; Vadén, T. (eds.). Dimensions of Conscious Experience. Advances in consciousness research. J. Benjamins Pub. pp. 119–144. ISBN 978-90-272-5157-2. from the original on 30 January 2022. Retrieved 30 January 2022.
  63. ^ a b Nauts, Sanne; Langner, Oliver; Huijsmans, Inge; Vonk, Roos; Wigboldus, Daniël H. J. (2014). "Forming Impressions of Personality: A Replication and Review of Evidence for a Primacy-of-Warmth Effect in Impression Formation". Social Psychology. 45 (3): 153–163. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000179. hdl:2066/128034. ISSN 1864-9335. S2CID 7693277.
  64. ^ Sigall, Harold; Ostrove, Nancy (1975). "Beautiful but dangerous: Effects of offender attractiveness and nature of the crime on juridic judgment". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 31 (3): 410–414. doi:10.1037/h0076472. ISSN 0022-3514.
  65. ^ a b Jäkel F.; Singh M.; Wichmann F. A.; Herzog, M. H. (2016), "An overview of quantitative approaches in Gestalt perception.", Vision Research, 126: 3–8, doi:10.1016/j.visres.2016.06.004, PMID 27353224
  66. ^ Yu, Dian; Tam, Derek; Franconeri, Steven L. (2019). "Gestalt similarity groupings are not constructed in parallel". Cognition. 182: 8–13. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2018.08.006. ISSN 0010-0277. PMID 30212653. S2CID 52269830.
  67. ^ Schultz, Duane (2013). A History of Modern Psychology. Burlington: Elsevier Science. p. 291. ISBN 978-1483270081.
  68. ^ Murray, David J. (1995). Gestalt Psychology and the Cognitive Revolution. New York: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-320714-9.
  69. ^ Tait, Alex (1 April 2018). "Visual Hierarchy and Layout". Geographic Information Science & Technology Body of Knowledge. University Consortium for Geographic Information Science. 2018 (Q2). doi:10.22224/gistbok/2018.2.4. ISSN 2577-2848.
  • Heider, Grace M. (1977). "More about Hull and Koffka". American Psychologist. American Psychological Association. 32 (5): 383. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.32.5.383.a. ISSN 1935-990X.

External links edit

gestalt, psychology, confused, with, psychotherapy, fritz, perls, gestalt, therapy, this, article, multiple, issues, please, help, improve, discuss, these, issues, talk, page, learn, when, remove, these, template, messages, this, article, needs, additional, ci. Not to be confused with the psychotherapy of Fritz Perls Gestalt therapy This article has multiple issues Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page Learn how and when to remove these template messages This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Gestalt psychology news newspapers books scholar JSTOR November 2016 Learn how and when to remove this template message The neutrality of this article is disputed Relevant discussion may be found on the talk page Please do not remove this message until conditions to do so are met November 2016 Learn how and when to remove this template message Learn how and when to remove this template message Gestalt psychology gestaltism or configurationism is a school of psychology that emerged in the early twentieth century in Austria and Germany as a theory of perception that was a rejection of basic principles of Wilhelm Wundt s and Edward Titchener s elementalist and structuralist psychology 1 2 3 As used in Gestalt psychology the German word Gestalt ɡ e ˈ ʃ t ae l t ˈ ʃ t ɑː l t ˈ ʃ t ɔː l t ˈ s t ɑː l t ˈ s t ɔː l t ge SHTA H LT STAHLT S H TAWLT 4 5 German ɡeˈʃtalt meaning form 6 is interpreted as pattern or configuration 7 Gestalt psychologists emphasize that organisms perceive entire patterns or configurations not merely individual components 7 The view is sometimes summarized using the adage the whole is more than the sum of its parts 8 13 Gestalt psychology was founded on works by Max Wertheimer Wolfgang Kohler and Kurt Koffka 7 Contents 1 Origin and history 1 1 Gestalt therapy 2 Contributions 2 1 Theoretical framework and methodology 2 2 Properties 2 2 1 Reification 2 2 2 Multistability 2 2 3 Invariance 2 3 Figure ground organization 2 4 Pragnanz 2 4 1 Law of proximity 2 4 2 Law of similarity 2 4 3 Law of closure 2 4 4 Law of symmetry 2 4 5 Law of common fate 2 4 6 Law of continuity 2 4 7 Law of past experience 2 4 8 Music 2 5 Problem solving and insight 2 6 Fuzzy trace theory of memory 3 Legacy 3 1 Support from cybernetics and neurology 3 2 Quantum cognition modelling 3 3 Use in contemporary social psychology 3 4 Contemporary cognitive and perceptual psychology 3 5 Use in design 4 See also 5 Notes 6 References 7 External linksOrigin and history editFurther information History of psychology Gestalt psychology Max Wertheimer 1880 1943 Kurt Koffka 1886 1941 and Wolfgang Kohler 1887 1967 founded Gestalt psychology in the early 20th century 8 113 116 The dominant view in psychology at the time was structuralism exemplified by the work of Hermann von Helmholtz 1821 1894 Wilhelm Wundt 1832 1920 and Edward B Titchener 1867 1927 9 10 3 Structuralism was rooted firmly in British empiricism 9 10 3 and was based on three closely interrelated theories atomism also known as elementalism 10 3 the view that all knowledge even complex abstract ideas is built from simple elementary constituents sensationalism the view that the simplest constituents the atoms of thought are elementary sense impressions associationism the view that more complex ideas arise from the association of simpler ideas 10 3 11 Together these three theories give rise to the view that the mind constructs all perceptions and even abstract thoughts strictly from lower level sensations that are related solely by being associated closely in space and time 9 The Gestaltists took issue with this widespread atomistic view that the aim of psychology should be to break consciousness down into putative basic elements 6 In contrast the Gestalt psychologists believed that breaking psychological phenomena down into smaller parts would not lead to understanding psychology 8 13 The Gestalt psychologists believed instead that the most fruitful way to view psychological phenomena is as organized structured wholes 8 13 They argued that the psychological whole has priority and that the parts are defined by the structure of the whole rather than vice versa One could say that the approach was based on a macroscopic view of psychology rather than a microscopic approach 12 Gestalt theories of perception are based on human nature being inclined to understand objects as an entire structure rather than the sum of its parts 13 Wertheimer had been a student of Austrian philosopher Christian von Ehrenfels 1859 1932 a member of the School of Brentano Von Ehrenfels introduced the concept of Gestalt to philosophy and psychology in 1890 before the advent of Gestalt psychology as such 14 9 Von Ehrenfels observed that a perceptual experience such as perceiving a melody or a shape is more than the sum of its sensory components 9 He claimed that in addition to the sensory elements of the perception there is something extra Although in some sense derived from the organization of the component sensory elements this further quality is an element in its own right He called it Gestalt qualitat or form quality For instance when one hears a melody one hears the notes plus something in addition to them that binds them together into a tune the Gestalt qualitat It is this Gestalt qualitat that according to von Ehrenfels allows a tune to be transposed to a new key using completely different notes while still retaining its identity The idea of a Gestalt qualitat has roots in theories by David Hume Johann Wolfgang von Goethe Immanuel Kant David Hartley and Ernst Mach Both von Ehrenfels and Edmund Husserl seem to have been inspired by Mach s work Beitrage zur Analyse der Empfindungen Contributions to the Analysis of Sensations 1886 in formulating their very similar concepts of gestalt and figural moment respectively 14 By 1914 the first published references to Gestalt theory could be found in a footnote of Gabriele von Wartensleben s application of Gestalt theory to personality She was a student at Frankfurt Academy for Social Sciences who interacted deeply with Wertheimer and Kohler 15 Through a series of experiments Wertheimer discovered that a person observing a pair of alternating bars of light can under the right conditions experience the illusion of movement between one location and the other He noted that this was a perception of motion absent any moving object That is it was pure phenomenal motion He dubbed it phi phenomenal motion 14 16 Wertheimer s publication of these results in 1912 17 marks the beginning of Gestalt psychology 16 In comparison to von Ehrenfels and others who had used the term gestalt earlier in various ways Wertheimer s unique contribution was to insist that the gestalt is perceptually primary The gestalt defines the parts from which it is composed rather than being a secondary quality that emerges from those parts 16 Wertheimer took the more radical position that what is given me by the melody does not arise as a secondary process from the sum of the pieces as such Instead what takes place in each single part already depends upon what the whole is 1925 1938 In other words one hears the melody first and only then may perceptually divide it up into notes Similarly in vision one sees the form of the circle first it is given im mediately i e its apprehension is not mediated by a process of part summation Only after this primary apprehension might one notice that it is made up of lines or dots or stars The two men who served as Wertheimer s subjects in the phi experiments were Kohler and Koffka Kohler was an expert in physical acoustics having studied under physicist Max Planck 1858 1947 but had taken his degree in psychology under Carl Stumpf 1848 1936 Koffka was also a student of Stumpf s having studied movement phenomena and psychological aspects of rhythm In 1917 Kohler 1917 1925 published the results of four years of research on learning in chimpanzees Kohler showed contrary to the claims of most other learning theorists that animals can learn by sudden insight into the structure of a problem over and above the associative and incremental manner of learning that Ivan Pavlov 1849 1936 and Edward Lee Thorndike 1874 1949 had demonstrated with dogs and cats respectively In 1921 Koffka published a Gestalt oriented text on developmental psychology Growth of the Mind With the help of American psychologist Robert Ogden Koffka introduced the Gestalt point of view to an American audience in 1922 by way of a paper in Psychological Bulletin It contains criticisms of then current explanations of a number of problems of perception and the alternatives offered by the Gestalt school Koffka moved to the United States in 1924 eventually settling at Smith College in 1927 In 1935 Koffka published his Principles of Gestalt Psychology This textbook laid out the Gestalt vision of the scientific enterprise as a whole Science he said is not the simple accumulation of facts What makes research scientific is the incorporation of facts into a theoretical structure The goal of the Gestaltists was to integrate the facts of inanimate nature life and mind into a single scientific structure This meant that science would have to accommodate not only what Koffka called the quantitative facts of physical science but the facts of two other scientific categories questions of order and questions of Sinn a German word which has been variously translated as significance value and meaning Without incorporating the meaning of experience and behavior Koffka believed that science would doom itself to trivialities in its investigation of human beings Having survived the Nazis up to the mid 1930s 18 all the core members of the Gestalt movement were forced out of Germany to the United States by 1935 19 Kohler published another book Dynamics in Psychology in 1940 but thereafter the Gestalt movement suffered a series of setbacks Koffka died in 1941 and Wertheimer in 1943 Wertheimer s long awaited book on mathematical problem solving Productive Thinking was published posthumously in 1945 but Kohler was left to guide the movement without his two long time colleagues note 1 Gestalt therapy edit Gestalt psychology should not be confused with the Gestalt therapy which is only peripherally linked to Gestalt psychology The founders of Gestalt therapy Fritz and Laura Perls had worked with Kurt Goldstein a neurologist who had applied principles of Gestalt psychology to the functioning of the organism Laura Perls had been a Gestalt psychologist before she became a psychoanalyst and before she began developing Gestalt therapy together with Fritz Perls 20 The extent to which Gestalt psychology influenced Gestalt therapy is disputed however In any case it is not identical to Gestalt psychology On the one hand Laura Perls preferred not to use the term Gestalt to name the emerging new therapy because she thought that the Gestalt psychologists would object to it 21 on the other hand Fritz and Laura Perls clearly adopted some of Goldstein s work 22 Thus though recognizing the historical connection and the influence most Gestalt psychologists emphasize that Gestalt therapy is not a form of Gestalt psychology Mary Henle noted in her presidential address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association 1975 What Perls has done has been to take a few terms from Gestalt psychology stretch their meaning beyond recognition mix them with notions often unclear and often incompatible from the depth psychologies existentialism and common sense and he has called the whole mixture gestalt therapy His work has no substantive relation to scientific Gestalt psychology To use his own language Fritz Perls has done his thing whatever it is it is not Gestalt psychology 23 With her analysis however she restricts herself explicitly to only three of Perls books from 1969 and 1972 leaving out Perls earlier work and Gestalt therapy in general as a psychotherapy method 22 There have been clinical applications of Gestalt psychology in the psychotherapeutic field long before Perls ian Gestalt therapy in group psychoanalysis Foulkes Adlerian individual psychology by Gestalt psychologists in psychotherapy like Erwin Levy Abraham S Luchins by Gestalt psychologically oriented psychoanalysts in Italy Canestrari and others and there have been newer developments foremost in Europe For example a strictly Gestalt psychology based therapeutic method is Gestalt Theoretical Psychotherapy developed by the German Gestalt psychologist and psychotherapist Hans Jurgen Walter and his colleagues in Germany Austria Gerhard Stemberger and colleagues and Switzerland Other countries especially Italy have seen similar developments Contributions editGestalt psychology made many contributions to the body of psychology The Gestaltists were the first to demonstrate empirically and document many facts about perception including facts about the perception of movement the perception of contour perceptual constancy and perceptual illusions 14 Wertheimer s discovery of the phi phenomenon is one example of such a contribution 24 In addition to discovering perceptual phenomena the contributions of Gestalt psychology include a a unique theoretical framework and methodology b a set of perceptual principles c a well known set of perceptual grouping laws d a theory of problem solving based on insight and e a theory of memory The following subsections discuss these contributions in turn Theoretical framework and methodology edit The Gestalt psychologists practiced a set of theoretical and methodological principles that attempted to redefine the approach to psychological research This is in contrast to investigations developed at the beginning of the 20th century based on traditional scientific methodology which divided the object of study into a set of elements that could be analyzed separately with the objective of reducing the complexity of this object The theoretical principles are the following Principle of totality Conscious experience must be considered globally by taking into account all the physical and mental aspects of the individual simultaneously because the nature of the mind demands that each component be considered as part of a system of dynamic relationships Wertheimer described holism as fundamental to Gestalt psychology 9 writing There are wholes the behavior of which is not determined by that of their individual elements but where the part processes are themselves determined by the intrinsic nature of the whole 25 In other words a perceptual whole is different from what one would predict based on only its individual parts 9 Moreover the nature of a part depends upon the whole in which it is embedded 9 Kohler for example writes In psychology we have wholes which instead of being the sum of parts existing independently give their parts specific functions or properties that can only be defined in relation to the whole in question 26 Thus the maxim that the whole is more than the sum of its parts is not a precise description of the Gestaltist view 9 Rather The whole is something else than the sum of its parts because summing is a meaningless procedure whereas the whole part relationship is meaningful 27 Principle of psychophysical isomorphism Kohler hypothesized that there is a correlation between conscious experience and cerebral activity 16 Based on the principles above the following methodological principles are defined Phenomenon experimental analysis In relation to the Totality Principle any psychological research should take phenomena as a starting point and not be solely focused on sensory qualities Biotic experiment The Gestalt psychologists established a need to conduct real experiments that sharply contrasted with and opposed classic laboratory experiments This signified experimenting in natural situations developed in real conditions in which it would be possible to reproduce with higher fidelity what would be habitual for a subject 28 Properties edit The key principles of gestalt systems are emergence reification multistability and invariance 29 Reification edit See also Reification fallacy nbsp ReificationReification is the constructive or generative aspect of perception by which the experienced percept contains more explicit spatial information than the sensory stimulus on which it is based For instance a triangle is perceived in picture A though no triangle is there In pictures B and D the eye recognizes disparate shapes as belonging to a single shape in C a complete three dimensional shape is seen where in actuality no such thing is drawn Reification can be explained by progress in the study of illusory contours which are treated by the visual system as real contours Multistability edit nbsp The Necker cube and the Rubin vase two examples of multistabilityMultistability or multistable perception is the tendency of ambiguous perceptual experiences to pop back and forth unstably between two or more alternative interpretations This is seen for example in the Necker cube and Rubin s Figure Vase illusion shown here Other examples include the three legged blivet and artist M C Escher s artwork and the appearance of flashing marquee lights moving first one direction and then suddenly the other Again Gestalt psychology does not explain how images appear multistable only that they do Invariance edit nbsp InvarianceInvariance is the property of perception whereby simple geometrical objects are recognized independent of rotation translation and scale as well as several other variations such as elastic deformations different lighting and different component features For example the objects in A in the figure are all immediately recognized as the same basic shape which is immediately distinguishable from the forms in B They are even recognized despite perspective and elastic deformations as in C and when depicted using different graphic elements as in D Computational theories of vision such as those by David Marr have provided alternate explanations of how perceived objects are classified Emergence reification multistability and invariance are not necessarily separable modules to model individually but they could be different aspects of a single unified dynamic mechanism 30 Figure ground organization edit The perceptual field what an organism perceives is organized Figure ground organization is one form of perceptual organization 16 Figure ground organization is the interpretation of perceptual elements in terms of their shapes and relative locations in the layout of surfaces in the 3 D world 16 Figure ground organization structures the perceptual field into a figure standing out at the front of the perceptual field and a background receding behind the figure 31 Pioneering work on figure ground organization was carried out by the Danish psychologist Edgar Rubin The Gestalt psychologists demonstrated that we tend to perceive as figures those parts of our perceptual fields that are convex symmetric small and enclosed 16 Pragnanz edit Main article Principles of grouping Like figure ground organization perceptual grouping sometimes called perceptual segregation 32 is a form of perceptual organization 16 Organisms perceive some parts of their perceptual fields as hanging together more tightly than others 16 They use this information for object detection 32 Perceptual grouping is the process that determines what these pieces of the perceptual field are 16 The Gestaltists were the first psychologists to systematically study perceptual grouping 32 According to Gestalt psychologists the fundamental principle of perceptual grouping is the law of Pragnanz 32 The law of Pragnanz is also known as the law of good Gestalt Pragnanz is a German word that directly translates to pithiness and implies salience conciseness and orderliness 33 The law of Pragnanz says that we tend to experience things as regular orderly symmetrical and simple As Koffka put it Of several geometrically possible organizations that one will actually occur which possesses the best simplest and most stable shape 34 The law of Pragnanz implies that as individuals perceive the world they eliminate complexity and unfamiliarity so they can observe reality in its most simplistic form Eliminating extraneous stimuli helps the mind create meaning This meaning created by perception implies a global regularity which is often mentally prioritized over spatial relations The law of good Gestalt focuses on the idea of conciseness which is what all of Gestalt theory is based on 35 A major aspect of Gestalt psychology is that it implies that the mind understands external stimuli as wholes rather than as the sums of their parts The wholes are structured and organized using grouping laws Gestalt psychologists attempted to discover refinements of the law of Pragnanz and this involved writing down laws that hypothetically allow us to predict the interpretation of sensation what is often called gestalt laws 36 Wertheimer defined a few principles that explain the ways humans perceive objects Those principles were based on similarity proximity continuity 37 The Gestalt concept is based on perceiving reality in its simplest form 38 The various laws are called laws or principles depending on the paper where they appear but for simplicity s sake this article uses the term laws These laws took several forms such as the grouping of similar or proximate objects together within this global process These laws deal with the sensory modality of vision However there are analogous laws for other sensory modalities including auditory tactile gustatory and olfactory Bregman GP The visual Gestalt principles of grouping were introduced in Wertheimer 1923 Through the 1930s and 1940s Wertheimer Kohler and Koffka formulated many of the laws of grouping through the study of visual perception Law of proximity edit nbsp Law of proximityThe law of proximity states that when an individual perceives an assortment of objects they perceive objects that are close to each other as forming a group For example in the figure illustrating the law of proximity there are 72 circles but we perceive the collection of circles in groups Specifically we perceive that there is a group of 36 circles on the left side of the image and three groups of 12 circles on the right side of the image This law is often used in advertising logos to emphasize which aspects of events are associated 35 39 Law of similarity edit nbsp Law of similarityThe law of similarity states that elements within an assortment of objects are perceptually grouped together if they are similar to each other This similarity can occur in the form of shape colour shading or other qualities For example the figure illustrating the law of similarity portrays 36 circles all equal distance apart from one another forming a square In this depiction 18 of the circles are shaded dark and 18 of the circles are shaded light We perceive the dark circles as grouped together and the light circles as grouped together forming six horizontal lines within the square of circles This perception of lines is due to the law of similarity 39 Law of closure edit nbsp Law of closureGestalt psychologists believed that humans tend to perceive objects as complete rather than focusing on the gaps that the object might contain 40 For example a circle has good Gestalt in terms of completeness However we will also perceive an incomplete circle as a complete circle 31 That tendency to complete shapes and figures is called closure 31 The law of closure states that individuals perceive objects such as shapes letters pictures etc as being whole when they are not complete Specifically when parts of a whole picture are missing our perception fills in the visual gap Research shows that the reason the mind completes a regular figure that is not perceived through sensation is to increase the regularity of surrounding stimuli For example the figure that depicts the law of closure portrays what we perceive as a circle on the left side of the image and a rectangle on the right side of the image However gaps are present in the shapes If the law of closure did not exist the image would depict an assortment of different lines with different lengths rotations and curvatures but with the law of closure we perceptually combine the lines into whole shapes 35 39 41 Law of symmetry edit nbsp Law of symmetryThe law of symmetry states that the mind perceives objects as being symmetrical and forming around a center point It is perceptually pleasing to divide objects into an even number of symmetrical parts Therefore when two symmetrical elements are unconnected the mind perceptually connects them to form a coherent shape Similarities between symmetrical objects increase the likelihood that objects are grouped to form a combined symmetrical object For example the figure depicting the law of symmetry shows a configuration of square and curled brackets When the image is perceived we tend to observe three pairs of symmetrical brackets rather than six individual brackets 35 39 Law of common fate edit The law of common fate states that objects are perceived as lines that move along the smoothest path Experiments using the visual sensory modality found that the movement of elements of an object produces paths that individuals perceive that the objects are on We perceive elements of objects to have trends of motion which indicate the path that the object is on The law of continuity implies the grouping together of objects that have the same trend of motion and are therefore on the same path For example if there is an array of dots and half the dots are moving upward while the other half are moving downward we would perceive the upward moving dots and the downward moving dots as two distinct units 33 Law of continuity edit nbsp Law of continuityThe law of continuity also known as the law of good continuation states that elements of objects tend to be grouped together and therefore integrated into perceptual wholes if they are aligned within an object In cases where there is an intersection between objects individuals tend to perceive the two objects as two single uninterrupted entities Stimuli remain distinct even with overlap We are less likely to group elements with sharp abrupt directional changes as being one object For example the figure depicting the law of continuity shows a configuration of two crossed keys When the image is perceived we tend to perceive the key in the background as a single uninterrupted key instead of two separate halves of a key 35 Law of past experience edit The law of past experience implies that under some circumstances visual stimuli are categorized according to past experience If objects tend to be observed within close proximity or small temporal intervals the objects are more likely to be perceived together For example the English language contains 26 letters that are grouped to form words using a set of rules If an individual reads an English word they have never seen they use the law of past experience to interpret the letters L and I as two letters beside each other rather than using the law of closure to combine the letters and interpret the object as an uppercase U 33 Music edit An example of the Gestalt movement in effect as it is both a process and result is a music sequence People are able to recognise a sequence of perhaps six or seven notes despite them being transposed into a different tuning or key 42 An early theory of gestalt grouping principles in music was composer theorist James Tenney s Meta Hodos 1961 43 Auditory Scene Analysis as developed by Albert Bregman further extends a gestalt approach to the analysis of sound perception Problem solving and insight edit Gestalt psychology contributed to the scientific study of problem solving 24 In fact the early experimental work of the Gestaltists in Germany note 2 marks the beginning of the scientific study of problem solving Later this experimental work continued through the 1960s and early 1970s with research conducted on relatively simple but novel for participants laboratory tasks of problem solving note 3 44 Given Gestalt psychology s focus on the whole it was natural for Gestalt psychologists to study problem solving from the perspective of insight seeking to understand the process by which organisms sometimes suddenly transition from having no idea how to solve a problem to instantly understanding the whole problem and its solution 8 13 In a famous set of experiments Kohler gave chimpanzees some boxes and placed food high off the ground after some time the chimpanzees appeared to suddenly realize that they could stack the boxes on top of each other to reach the food 45 362 Max Wertheimer distinguished two kinds of thinking productive thinking and reproductive thinking 8 456 46 45 361 Productive thinking is solving a problem based on insight a quick creative unplanned response to situations and environmental interaction Reproductive thinking is solving a problem deliberately based on previous experience and knowledge Reproductive thinking proceeds algorithmically a problem solver reproduces a series of steps from memory knowing that they will lead to a solution or by trial and error 45 361 Karl Duncker another Gestalt psychologist who studied problem solving 45 370 coined the term functional fixedness for describing the difficulties in both visual perception and problem solving that arise from the fact that one element of a whole situation already has a fixed function that has to be changed in order to perceive something or find the solution to a problem 47 Abraham Luchins also studied problem solving from the perspective of Gestalt psychology He is well known for his research on the role of mental set Einstellung effect which he demonstrated using a series of problems having to do with refilling water jars 45 383 Another Gestalt psychologist Perkins believes insight deals with three processes Unconscious leap in thinking 36 The increased amount of speed in mental processing The amount of short circuiting that occurs in normal reasoning 48 49 50 Views going against the Gestalt psychology are Nothing special view Neo gestalt view The Three Process ViewFuzzy trace theory of memory edit Fuzzy trace theory a process model of explaining and reasoning was also derived from Gestalt psychology Fuzzy trace theory posits that we encode information into two separate traces verbatim and gist Information stored in verbatim and is exact for detail the individual parts of a pattern for example while information stored in gist is semantic and conceptual what we perceive the pattern to be The effects seen in Gestalt psychology can be attributed to the way we encode information as gist 51 52 Legacy editGestalt psychology struggled to precisely define terms like Pragnanz to make specific behavioural predictions and to articulate testable models of underlying neural mechanisms 9 It was criticized as being merely descriptive 53 These shortcomings led by the mid 20th century to growing dissatisfaction with Gestaltism and a subsequent decline in its impact on psychology 9 Despite this decline Gestalt psychology has formed the basis of much further research into the perception of patterns and objects 54 and of research into behaviour thinking problem solving and psychopathology Support from cybernetics and neurology edit In the 1940s and 1950s laboratory research in neurology and what became known as cybernetics on the mechanism of frogs eyes indicate that perception of gestalts in particular gestalts in motion is perhaps more primitive and fundamental than seeing as such A frog hunts on land by vision He has no fovea or region of greatest acuity in vision upon which he must centre a part of the image The frog does not seem to see or at any rate is not concerned with the detail of stationary parts of the world around him He will starve to death surrounded by food if it is not moving His choice of food is determined only by size and movement He will leap to capture any object the size of an insect or worm providing it moves like one He can be fooled easily not only by a piece of dangled meat but by any moving small object He does remember a moving thing provided it stays within his field of vision and he is not distracted 55 The lowest level concepts related to visual perception for a human being probably differ little from the concepts of a frog In any case the structure of the retina in mammals and in human beings is the same as in amphibians The phenomenon of distortion of perception of an image stabilised on the retina gives some idea of the concepts of the subsequent levels of the hierarchy This is a very interesting phenomenon When a person looks at an immobile object fixes it with his eyes the eyeballs do not remain absolutely immobile they make small involuntary movements As a result the image of the object on the retina is constantly in motion slowly drifting and jumping back to the point of maximum sensitivity The image marks time in the vicinity of this point 56 In the 1990s Andranik Tangian developed a model of artificial perception that implemented a principle of correlativity which operationalized the Gestalt psychology laws in their interaction The model finds structures in data without knowing the structures similarly to segregating elements in abstract painting like curves contours and spots without identifying them with known objects The approach is based on the least complex data representations in the sense of Kolmogorov i e requiring the least memory storage which is regarded as saving the brain energy The least complexity criterion leads to multi level data representations in terms of generative patterns and their transformations using proximities similarities symmetries common fate grouping continuities etc The idea that perception is data representation rather than physical recognition is illustrated by the effect of several voices produced by a single physical body a loudspeaker membrane whereas the effect of a single tone is produced by several physical bodies organ pipes tuned as a chord and activated by a single key It is shown that the physical causality in certain observations can be revealed through optimal data representations and this nature information duality is explained by the fact that both nature and information are subordinated to the same principle of efficiency In some situations the least complex data representations use the patterns already stored in the memory demonstrating the dependence of perception on previous knowledge in line with the Gestalt psychology law of past experience Such an intelligent perception is opposed to the naive perception that is based exclusively on direct percepts and is therefore context dependent The model is applied to automatic notation of music recognition of interval structures in chords and polyphonic voices with no reference to pitch thereby relying on interval hearing instead of absolute hearing as well as rhythms under variable tempo approaching the capabilities of trained musicians The model is also relevant to visual scene analysis and explains some modes of abstract thinking 57 58 59 Quantum cognition modelling edit Main article Quantum cognition Gestalt perception Similarities between Gestalt phenomena and quantum mechanics have been pointed out by among others chemist Anton Amann who commented that similarities between Gestalt perception and quantum mechanics are on a level of a parable yet may give useful insight nonetheless 60 Physicists David Bohm and Basil Hiley together with philosopher Paavo Pylkkanen have suggested that mind and matter both emerge from an implicate order 61 62 Use in contemporary social psychology edit The halo effect can be explained through the application of Gestalt theories to social information processing 63 13 The constructive theories of social cognition are applied to the expectations of individuals They have been perceived in this manner and the person judging the individual is continuing to view them in this positive manner 13 Gestalt s theories of perception enforces that individual s tendency to perceive actions and characteristics as a whole rather than isolated parts 13 therefore humans are inclined to build a coherent and consistent impression of objects and behaviors in order to achieve an acceptable shape and form The halo effect is what forms patterns for individuals 13 the halo effect being classified as a cognitive bias which occurs during impression formation 63 The halo effect can also be altered by physical characteristics social status and many other characteristics 64 As well the halo effect can have real repercussions on the individual s perception of reality either negatively or positively meaning to construct negative or positive images about other individuals or situations something that could lead to self fulfilling prophesies stereotyping or even discrimination 13 Contemporary cognitive and perceptual psychology edit Some of the central criticisms of Gestaltism are based on the preference Gestaltists are deemed to have for theory over data and a lack of quantitative research supporting Gestalt ideas This is not necessarily a fair criticism as highlighted by a recent collection of quantitative research on Gestalt perception 65 Researchers continue to test hypotheses about the mechanisms underlying Gestalt principles such as the principle of similarity 66 Other important criticisms concern the lack of definition and support for the many physiological assumptions made by gestaltists 67 and lack of theoretical coherence in modern Gestalt psychology 65 In some scholarly communities such as cognitive psychology and computational neuroscience gestalt theories of perception are criticized for being descriptive rather than explanatory in nature For this reason they are viewed by some as redundant or uninformative For example a textbook on visual perception states that The physiological theory of the gestaltists has fallen by the wayside leaving us with a set of descriptive principles but without a model of perceptual processing Indeed some of their laws of perceptual organisation today sound vague and inadequate What is meant by a good or simple shape for example 53 One historian of psychology David J Murray has argued that Gestalt psychologists first discovered many principles later championed by cognitive psychology including schemas and prototypes 68 Another psychologist has argued that the Gestalt psychologists made a lasting contribution by showing how the study of illusions can help scientists understand essential aspects of how the visual system normally functions not merely how it breaks down 10 16 Use in design edit nbsp Several grouping principles are employed in this map similarity allows the reader to selectively isolate cities rivers or state boundaries closure allows the dashed boundary lines to be perceived as continuous borders proximity makes the collection of river segments appear as a single watershed and continuity helps the reader see whole states even when boundaries are obscured under rivers The gestalt laws are used in several visual design fields such as user interface design and cartography The laws of similarity and proximity can for example be used as guides for placing radio buttons They may also be used in designing computers and software for more intuitive human use Examples include the design and layout of a desktop s shortcuts in rows and columns 39 In map design principles of Pragnanz or grouping are crucial for implying a conceptual order to the portrayed geographic features thus facilitating the intended use of the map 69 The Law of Similarity is employed by selecting similar map symbols for similar kinds of features or features with similar properties the Law of Proximity is crucial to identifying geographic patterns and regions and the Laws of Closure and Continuity allow users to recognize features that may be obscured by other features such as when a road goes over a river See also editAugusto Garau Amodal perception Cognitive grammar Egregore Gestaltzerfall Graz School Hans Wallach Hermann Friedmann James J Gibson James Tenney Laws of association Mereology Optical illusion Pal Schiller Harkai Pattern recognition machine learning Pattern recognition psychology Phenomenology Principles of grouping Rudolf Arnheim Solomon Asch Structural information theory Topological data analysis Wolfgang Metzger Vera Felicidade de Almeida CamposNotes edit For more on the history of Gestalt psychology see Ash M G 1995 Gestalt psychology in German culture 1890 1967 Holism and the quest for objectivity Cambridge England Cambridge University Press For example Duncker Karl 1935 Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens The psychology of productive thinking in German Berlin Julius Springer For example Duncker s X ray problem Ewert amp Lambert s disk problem in 1932 later known as Tower of Hanoi References edit Mather George 2006 Foundations of Perception Psychology Press p 32 ISBN 978 0 86377 834 6 Gestalt psychology Encyclopaedia Britannica Retrieved 2 January 2023 Baker David B ed 13 January 2012 The Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology Global Perspectives Oxford University Press USA p 576 ISBN 978 0 19 536655 6 Wells John 3 April 2008 Longman Pronunciation Dictionary 3rd ed Pearson Longman ISBN 978 1 4058 8118 0 Definition of gestalt Dictionary com www dictionary com Retrieved 2 January 2023 a b Gestalt The Columbia Encyclopedia Columbia University Press 2018 ISBN 9781786848468 Archived from the original on 22 July 2019 Retrieved 1 August 2019 a b c Gestalt psychology Britannica Concise Encyclopedia Encyclopaedia Britannica Inc 1 May 2008 p 756 ISBN 9781593394929 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 2 October 2020 a b c d e f Sternberg Robert J Sternberg Karin 2012 Cognitive Psychology 6th ed Belmont CA Cengage Learning ISBN 978 1 133 31391 5 a b c d e f g h i j k Wagemans Johan Feldman Jacob Gepshtein Sergei Kimchi Ruth Pomerantz James R van der Helm Peter A van Leeuwen Cees 2012 A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception II Conceptual and theoretical foundations Psychological Bulletin 138 6 1218 1252 doi 10 1037 a0029334 ISSN 1939 1455 PMC 3728284 PMID 22845750 a b c d e Kolers Paul A 1972 Aspects of Motion Perception International Series of Monographs in Experimental Psychology New York Pergamon ISBN 978 1 4831 7113 5 Hamlyn D W 1957 The Psychology of Perception A Philosophical Examination of Gestalt Theory and Derivative Theories of Perception eBook ed London Routledge pp 88 89 ISBN 978 1 315 47329 1 Archived from the original on 21 October 2020 Retrieved 19 October 2019 Verstegen Ian 2010 Gestalt Psychology The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology pp 1 3 doi 10 1002 9780470479216 corpsy0386 ISBN 9780470479216 a b c d e f Pohl Rudiger F 22 July 2016 Cognitive Illusions Intriguing Phenomena in Judgement Thinking and Memory Psychology Press ISBN 9781317448280 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 2 October 2020 a b c d Smith Barry 1988 Gestalt Theory An Essay in Philosophy In Smith Barry ed Foundations of Gestalt Theory Vienna Philosophia Verlag pp 11 81 Archived from the original PDF on 22 February 2012 Retrieved 12 October 2019 King D Brett Wertheimer Michael 1 January 2005 Max Wertheimer and Gestalt Theory Transaction Publishers ISBN 978 1 4128 2826 0 Archived from the original on 26 February 2022 Retrieved 2 October 2020 a b c d e f g h i j Wagemans Johan Elder James H Kubovy Michael Palmer Stephen E Peterson Mary A Singh Manish von der Heydt Rudiger 2012 A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception I Perceptual grouping and figure ground organization Psychological Bulletin 138 6 1172 1217 doi 10 1037 a0029333 ISSN 1939 1455 PMC 3482144 PMID 22845751 Wertheimer Max 1912 Experimentelle Studien uber das Sehen von Bewegung PDF Zeitschrift fur Psychologie in German 61 161 265 Archived PDF from the original on 23 November 2018 Retrieved 14 October 2019 Available in translation as Wertheimer Max 2012 Experimental Studies on Seeing Motion In Spillman Lothar ed On Perceived Motion and Figural Organization Michael Wertheimer K W Watkins trans Cambridge MA MIT Henle M 1978 One man against the Nazis Wolfgang Kohler PDF American Psychologist 33 10 939 944 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 33 10 939 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002B 9E5F 5 Archived from the original on 26 February 2022 Retrieved 16 October 2019 Henle M 1984 Robert M Ogden and gestalt psychology in America Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 20 1 9 19 doi 10 1002 1520 6696 198401 20 1 lt 9 aid jhbs2300200103 gt 3 0 co 2 u PMID 11608590 Bocian B 2010 Fritz Perls in Berlin 1893 1933 Expressionism Psychoanalysis Judaism EHP Edition Humanistische Psychology p 190 ISBN 978 3 89797 068 7 Joe Wysong Edward Rosenfeld eds An Oral History of Gestalt Therapy Highland New York 1982 The Gestalt Journal Press p 12 a b Barlow Allen R Fall 1981 Gestalt Antecedent Influence or Historical Accident The Gestalt Journal 4 2 Retrieved 2 January 2023 Mary Henle 1975 Gestalt Psychology and Gestalt Therapy Archived 5 February 2012 at the Wayback Machine Presidential Address to Division 24 at the meeting of the American Psychological Association Chicago September 1975 Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 14 pp 23 32 a b Gobet Fernand 2017 Entrenchment Gestalt formation and chunking in Schmid H J ed Entrenchment and the psychology of language learning How we reorganize and adapt linguistic knowledge American Psychological Association pp 245 267 doi 10 1037 15969 012 ISBN 978 3110341300 archived from the original on 26 February 2022 retrieved 14 October 2019 Wertheimer Max 1938 Original work published 1924 Gestalt theory In Ellis Willis D ed A Source Book of Gestalt Psychology London Routledge amp Kegan Paul pp 2 Retrieved 16 October 2019 Kohler Wolfgang 1971 Original work published 1930 Human perception In Henle Mary ed The Selected Papers of Wolfgang Kohler Liveright p 145 ISBN 978 0 87140 253 0 Koffka K 1935 Principles of Gestalt Psychology New York Harcourt Brace pp 176 Retrieved 13 October 2019 William Ray Woodward Robert Sonne Cohen World views and scientific discipline formation science studies in the German Democratic Republic papers from a German American summer institute 1988 Steven Lehar 2003 The World in your Head A Gestalt View of the Mechanism of Conscious Experience Mahwah N J Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers ISBN 0805841768 OCLC 52051454 Archived from the original on 19 July 2011 Retrieved 19 August 2005 Gestalt Isomorphism Sharp bu edu Archived from the original on 17 February 2012 Retrieved 6 April 2012 a b c Brennan James F Houde Keith A 26 October 2017 History and Systems of Psychology Cambridge University Press doi 10 1017 9781316827178 ISBN 9781107178670 S2CID 142935847 a b c d Eysenck Michael W 2006 Fundamentals of Cognition Hove UK Psychology Press pp 62 64 ISBN 978 1 84169 374 3 a b c Todorovic Dejan 2008 Gestalt Principles Scholarpedia 3 12 5345 Bibcode 2008SchpJ 3 5345T doi 10 4249 scholarpedia 5345 Koffka K 1935 Principles Of Gestalt Psychology New York Harcourt Brace Retrieved 13 October 2019 a b c d e Stevenson Herb Emergence The Gestalt Approach to Change Unleashing Executive and Orzanizational Potential Archived from the original on 21 June 2012 Retrieved 7 April 2012 a b Sternberg Robert 2003 Cognitive psychology 3rd ed Belmont CA Thomson Wadsworth ISBN 978 0 15 508535 0 OCLC 883578008 Craighead W Edward Nemeroff Charles B 19 April 2004 Gestalt psychology The Concise Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology and Behavioral Science John Wiley amp Sons pp 401 404 ISBN 9780471220367 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 2 October 2020 Reber Arthur S 1985 Gestalt psychology The Penguin Dictionary of Psychology Viking ISBN 9780670801367 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 2 October 2020 a b c d e Soegaard Mads Gestalt Principles of form Perception Interaction Design Archived from the original on 10 April 2012 Retrieved 8 April 2012 Hamlyn D W 27 March 2017 The Psychology of Perception doi 10 4324 9781315473291 ISBN 9781315473291 Why Your Brain Thinks These Dots Are a Dog Gizmodo UK Archived from the original on 24 September 2015 Retrieved 23 March 2018 Ellis Willis D 1999 A source book of Gestalt psychology Vol 2 Psychology Press Tenney James 1961 2015 Meta Hodos In From Scratch Writings in Music Theory Edited by Larry Polansky Lauren Pratt Robert Wannamaker and Michael Winter Urbana University of Illinois Press 13 96 Mayer R E 1992 Thinking problem solving cognition Second edition New York W H Freeman and Company a b c d e Kellogg Ronald T 2003 Cognitive Psychology 2nd ed Thousand Oaks CA SAGE Publications ISBN 0 7619 2130 3 Wertheimer Max 1945 Productive thinking New York Harper Duncker Karl 1935 Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens The psychology of productive thinking in German Berlin Julius Springer Perkins D N 1981 The Mind s Best Work Harvard University Press ISBN 978 0 674 57624 7 Archived from the original on 26 February 2022 Retrieved 30 January 2022 Weisberg R W 1986 Creativity Genius and Other Myths Psychology Series W H Freeman ISBN 978 0 7167 1768 3 Archived from the original on 26 February 2022 Retrieved 30 January 2022 Weisberg 1995 Reyna Valerie 2012 A new institutionism Meaning memory and development in Fuzzy Trace Theory Judgment and Decision Making 7 3 332 359 doi 10 1017 S1930297500002291 PMC 4268540 PMID 25530822 Barghout Lauren 2014 Visual Taxometric Approach to Image Segmentation Using Fuzzy Spatial Taxon Cut Yields Contextually Relevant Regions Information Processing and Management of Uncertainty in Knowledge Based Systems Communications in Computer and Information Science 443 163 173 doi 10 1007 978 3 319 08855 6 17 ISBN 978 3 319 08854 9 a b Bruce V Green P Georgeson M 1996 Visual perception Physiology psychology and ecology 3rd ed LEA p 110 Carlson Neil R Heth C Donald 2010 Psychology The Science of Behaviour Ontario Pearson Education Canada pp 20 22 Lettvin J Y Maturana H R Pitts W H and McCulloch W S 1961 Two Remarks on the Visual System of the Frog In Sensory Communication edited by Walter Rosenblith MIT Press and John Wiley and Sons New York Valentin Fedorovich Turchin The phenomenon of science a cybernetic approach to human evolution Columbia University Press 1977 Tanguiane Tangian Andranick 1993 Tanguiane Andranick S ed Artificial Perception and Music Recognition Lecture Notes in Artificial Intelligence Vol 746 Berlin Heidelberg Springer doi 10 1007 BFb0019384 ISBN 978 3 540 57394 4 S2CID 20835144 Archived from the original on 15 April 2021 Retrieved 21 February 2021 Tanguiane Tangian Andranick 1994 Principle of correlativity of perception and its applications to music recognition Music Perception 11 4 465 502 doi 10 2307 40285634 JSTOR 40285634 Archived from the original on 26 February 2022 Retrieved 21 February 2021 Tangian Andranik 2021 How do we think Modeling interactions of perception and memory KIT Scientific Working Papers Vol 166 Karlsruhe Karlsruhe Institute of Technology KIT doi 10 5445 IR 1000133287 ISSN 2194 1629 S2CID 237995668 Amann Anton 1993 The Gestalt problem in quantum theory Generation of molecular shape by the environment Synthese Springer Science and Business Media LLC 97 1 125 156 doi 10 1007 bf01255834 ISSN 0039 7857 S2CID 46973964 B J Hiley Particles fields and observers Volume I The Origins of Life Part 1 Origin and Evolution of Life Section II The Physical and Chemical Basis of Life pp 87 106 PDF Archived 4 March 2016 at the Wayback Machine Hiley Basil J Pylkkanen Paavo 2001 Naturalizing the mind in a quantum framework In Pylkkanen P Vaden T eds Dimensions of Conscious Experience Advances in consciousness research J Benjamins Pub pp 119 144 ISBN 978 90 272 5157 2 Archived from the original on 30 January 2022 Retrieved 30 January 2022 a b Nauts Sanne Langner Oliver Huijsmans Inge Vonk Roos Wigboldus Daniel H J 2014 Forming Impressions of Personality A Replication and Review of Evidence for a Primacy of Warmth Effect in Impression Formation Social Psychology 45 3 153 163 doi 10 1027 1864 9335 a000179 hdl 2066 128034 ISSN 1864 9335 S2CID 7693277 Sigall Harold Ostrove Nancy 1975 Beautiful but dangerous Effects of offender attractiveness and nature of the crime on juridic judgment Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31 3 410 414 doi 10 1037 h0076472 ISSN 0022 3514 a b Jakel F Singh M Wichmann F A Herzog M H 2016 An overview of quantitative approaches in Gestalt perception Vision Research 126 3 8 doi 10 1016 j visres 2016 06 004 PMID 27353224 Yu Dian Tam Derek Franconeri Steven L 2019 Gestalt similarity groupings are not constructed in parallel Cognition 182 8 13 doi 10 1016 j cognition 2018 08 006 ISSN 0010 0277 PMID 30212653 S2CID 52269830 Schultz Duane 2013 A History of Modern Psychology Burlington Elsevier Science p 291 ISBN 978 1483270081 Murray David J 1995 Gestalt Psychology and the Cognitive Revolution New York Prentice Hall ISBN 978 0 13 320714 9 Tait Alex 1 April 2018 Visual Hierarchy and Layout Geographic Information Science amp Technology Body of Knowledge University Consortium for Geographic Information Science 2018 Q2 doi 10 22224 gistbok 2018 2 4 ISSN 2577 2848 Heider Grace M 1977 More about Hull and Koffka American Psychologist American Psychological Association 32 5 383 doi 10 1037 0003 066x 32 5 383 a ISSN 1935 990X External links edit nbsp Psychology portalGestalt psychology on Encyclopaedia Britannica Journal Gestalt Theory An International Multidisciplinary Journal in full text open source International Society for Gestalt Theory and its Applications GTA Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Gestalt psychology amp oldid 1184777856, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.