fbpx
Wikipedia

Physical attractiveness

Physical attractiveness is the degree to which a person's physical features are considered aesthetically pleasing or beautiful. The term often implies sexual attractiveness or desirability, but can also be distinct from either. There are many factors which influence one person's attraction to another, with physical aspects being one of them. Physical attraction itself includes universal perceptions common to all human cultures such as facial symmetry,[9] sociocultural dependent attributes and personal preferences unique to a particular individual.[10]

Venus de Milo at the Louvre has been described as a "classical vision of beauty".[1][2][3][a]
Michelangelo's David is considered a symbol of young male beauty and strength.
Ishtar, Mesopotamian goddess of sexual love and war. The goddess has been associated with sexuality, love, and fertility.[5][6][7]
Xi Shi (西施), born 506 BC, was one of the Four Great Beauties of ancient China.[8]

In many cases, humans subconsciously attribute positive characteristics, such as intelligence and honesty, to physically attractive people, a psychological phenomenon called the Halo effect.[11] From research done in the United States and United Kingdom, it was found that objective[12] measures of physical attractiveness and intelligence are positively correlated and that the association between the two attributes is stronger among men than among women.[13] Evolutionary psychologists have tried to answer why individuals who are more physically attractive should also, on average, be more intelligent, and have put forward the notion that both general intelligence and physical attractiveness may be indicators of underlying genetic fitness.[13] A person's physical characteristics can signal cues to fertility and health,[14][15][16] with statistical modeling studies showing that the facial shape variables that reflect aspects of physiological health, including body fat and blood pressure, also influence observers' perceptions of health.[17] Attending to these factors increases reproductive success, furthering the representation of one's genes in the population.[18]

Heterosexual men tend to be attracted to women who have a youthful appearance and exhibit features such as a symmetrical face,[19] full breasts, full lips, and a low waist–hip ratio.[20][21] Heterosexual women tend to be attracted to men who are taller than they themselves are and who display a high degree of facial symmetry, masculine facial dimorphism, upper body strength, broad shoulders, a relatively narrow waist, and a V-shaped torso.[22][23][24][25]

General contributing factors edit

 
The Mannerist movement was not afraid to exaggerate body proportions for an effect considered attractive; Juno in a niche, engraving by Jacopo Caraglio, probably of a drawing by Rosso Fiorentino, 1526

Generally, physical attractiveness can be viewed from a number of perspectives; with universal perceptions being common to all human cultures, cultural and social aspects, and individual subjective preferences. The perception of attractiveness can have a significant effect on how people are judged in terms of employment or social opportunities, friendship, sexual behavior, and marriage.[26]

Some physical features are attractive in both men and women, particularly bodily[27] and facial symmetry,[28][29][30][31] although one contrary report suggests that "absolute flawlessness" with perfect symmetry can be "disturbing".[32] Symmetry may be evolutionarily beneficial as a sign of health because asymmetry "signals past illness or injury".[33] One study suggested people were able to "gauge beauty at a subliminal level" by seeing only a glimpse of a picture for one-hundredth of a second.[33] Other important factors include youthfulness, skin clarity and smoothness of skin; and "vivid color" in the eyes and hair.[28] However, there are numerous differences based on gender.

A 1921 study of the reports of college students regarding those traits in individuals which make for attractiveness and repulsiveness argued that static traits, such as beauty or ugliness of features, hold a position subordinate to groups of physical elements like expressive behavior, affectionate disposition, grace of manner, aristocratic bearing, social accomplishments and personal habits.[34]

Grammer and colleagues have identified eight "pillars" of beauty: youthfulness, symmetry, averageness, sex-hormone markers, body odor, motion, skin complexion and hair texture.[35] Traditionally in Samoa, body fat was acceptable or attractive.[36]

Facial features edit

An Italian study published in 2008 studied the positions of the 50 soft-tissue landmarks of the faces of 324 white Northern Italian adolescent boys and girls to compare the features of a group of 93 "beautiful" individuals selected by a commercial casting agency with those of a reference group with normal dentofacial dimensions and proportions. The research found that, in comparison with the reference group, the attractive adolescents tended to have the following characteristics:

  • the ratio between the volume of the forehead and that of the total face was larger;
  • the nasal volume was smaller;
  • the distance between outer canthi was larger;
  • total facial height and depth were reduced.

Some tendencies differed by age and sex:

  • the facial volume was smaller in older attractive boys than in their peers, but bigger in attractive girls;
  • the faces of older attractive adolescents were less rounded (bigger ratio between facial area and volume), but the reverse was true for girls of any age;
  • attractive older boys had smaller angles of facial convexity with more acute profiles, while in girls the reverse pattern was found;
  • the nasolabial angle was reduced in girls, but in older boys the effect was reversed;
  • older attractive boys tended to have more prominent chins.

The study concluded that attractive adolescents had more neotenous and juvenile features, but older attractive boys also showed tendencies towards sexual dimorphism.[37]

Contrary to common misconception, one study finds that non-severe facial scarring increases male attractiveness for short-term relationships.[38]

Symmetry edit

Symmetrical faces and bodies may be signs of good inheritance to women of child-bearing age seeking to create healthy offspring. Studies suggest women are less attracted to men with asymmetrical faces,[39] and symmetrical faces correlate with long term mental performance[40] and are an indication that a man has experienced "fewer genetic and environmental disturbances such as diseases, toxins, malnutrition or genetic mutations" while growing.[40] Since achieving symmetry is a difficult task during human growth, requiring billions of cell reproductions while maintaining a parallel structure, achieving symmetry is a visible signal of genetic health.

Studies have also suggested that women at peak fertility were more likely to fantasize about men with greater facial symmetry,[41] and other studies have found that male symmetry was the only factor that could significantly predict the likelihood of a woman experiencing orgasm during sex. Women with partners possessing greater symmetry reported significantly more copulatory female orgasms than were reported by women with partners possessing low symmetry, even with many potential confounding variables controlled.[42] This finding has been found to hold across different cultures. It has been argued that masculine facial dimorphism (in men) and symmetry in faces are signals advertising genetic quality in potential mates.[43] Low facial and body fluctuating asymmetry may indicate good health and intelligence, which are desirable features.[44] Studies have found that women who perceive themselves as being more physically attractive are more likely to favor men with a higher degree of facial symmetry, than are women who perceive themselves as being less physically attractive.[45] It has been found that symmetrical females and males have a tendency to begin to have sexual intercourse at an earlier age, to have more sexual partners, and to have more one-night stands. They are also more likely to engage in infidelity.[46] A study of quarterbacks in the American National Football League found a positive correlation between facial symmetry and salaries.[29]

Body scent edit

Double-blind studies found that women prefer the scent of men who are rated as facially attractive.[47] For example, both males and females were more attracted to the natural scent of individuals who had been rated by consensus as facially attractive.[48] Additionally, it has also been shown that women have a preference for the scent of men with more symmetrical faces, and that women's preference for the scent of more symmetrical men is strongest during the most fertile period of their menstrual cycle.[49] Within the set of normally cycling women, individual women's preference for the scent of men with high facial symmetry correlated with their probability of conception.[49] Men's body odor is also affected by their diet, with women expressing preferences for male body odor associated with increased dietary fruit and vegetable and protein content, and reduced carbohydrate content.[50]

Genetics edit

Studies have explored the genetic basis behind such issues as facial symmetry and body scent and how they influence physical attraction. In one study in which women wore men's T-shirts, researchers found that women were more attracted to the bodily scents in shirts of men who had a different type of gene section within the DNA called major histocompatibility complex (MHC).[51] MHC is a large gene area within the DNA of vertebrates which encodes proteins dealing with the immune system[52] and which influences individual bodily odors.[53] One hypothesis is that humans are naturally attracted by the sense of smell and taste to others with dissimilar MHC sections, perhaps to avoid subsequent inbreeding while increasing the genetic diversity of offspring.[52] Furthermore, there are studies showing that women's natural attraction for men with dissimilar immune profiles can be distorted with use of birth control pills.[53] Other research findings involving the genetic foundations of attraction suggest that MHC heterozygosity positively correlates with male facial attractiveness. Women judge the faces of men who are heterozygous at all three MHC loci to be more attractive than the faces of men who are homozygous at one or more of these loci. Additionally, a second experiment with genotyped women raters, found these preferences were independent of the degree of MHC similarity between the men and the female rater. With MHC heterozygosity independently seen as a genetic advantage, the results suggest that facial attractiveness in men may be a measure of genetic quality.[54][55] General genetic heterozygosity has been demonstrated to be related to attractiveness in that people with mixed genetic backgrounds (i.e., mixed race people) as seen as more attractive than people with a more similar genetic parents. (i.e., single race people).[56] However, some studies have not found that mixed race individuals are rated as more attractive, and one found that only certain mixes were rated as more attractive; this study argued that equating race with genetics was incorrect and argued for social influences as the cause.[57]

Youthfulness edit

A 2010 study by OkCupid (a US dating site) on 200,000 of its male and female users found that heterosexual women  – except those during their early to mid-twenties – are open to relationships with both somewhat older and somewhat younger men; they have a larger potential dating pool than men until age 26. At age 20, women, in a "dramatic change", begin sending private messages to significantly older men. At age 29, they become "even more open to older men". Male desirability to women peaks in the late 20s and does not fall below the average for all men until 36.[58] Other research indicates that women, irrespective of their own age, are attracted to men who are the same age or older.[59]

For the Romans especially, "beardlessness" and "smooth young bodies" were considered beautiful to both men and women.[60] For Greek and Roman men, the most desirable traits of boys were their "youth" and "hairlessness". Pubescent boys were considered a socially appropriate object of male desire, while post-pubescent boys were considered to be "ἔξωροι" or "past the prime".[60] This was largely in the context of pederasty (adult male interest in adolescent boys). Today, men and women's attitudes towards male beauty has changed. For example, body hair on men may even be preferred (see below).

A 1984 study said that gay men tend to prefer gay men of the same age as ideal partners, but there was a statistically significant effect (p < 0.05) of masculinity-femininity. The study said that more feminine men tended to prefer relatively older men than themselves and more masculine men tended to prefer relatively younger men than themselves.[61]

Cross-cultural data shows that the reproductive success of women is tied to their youth and physical attractiveness[62] such as the pre-industrial Sami where the most reproductively successful women were 15 years younger than their man.[63] One study covering 37 cultures showed that, on average, a woman was 2.5 years younger than her male partner, with the age difference in Nigeria and Zambia being at the far extreme of 6.5 to 7.5 years. As men age, they tend to seek a mate who is ever younger.[64]

25% of eHarmony's male customers over the age of 50 request to only be matched with women younger than 40.[65] The 2010 OkCupid study found that female desirability to its male users peaks at age 21, and falls below the average for all women at 31. After age 26, men have a larger potential dating pool than women on the site; and by age 48, their pool is almost twice as large. The median 31-year-old male user searches for women aged 22-to-35, while the median 42-year-old male searches for women 27-to-45. The age skew is even greater with messages to other users; the median 30-year-old male messages teenage girls as often as women his own age, while mostly ignoring women a few years older than him. Excluding the 10% most and 10% least beautiful women, women's attractiveness does not change between 18 and 40. If extremes are included, however, "there's no doubt that younger [women] are more physically attractive – indeed in many ways beauty and youth are inextricable. That's why most of the models you see in magazines are teenagers".[58]

Pheromones (detected by female hormone markers) reflects female fertility and the reproductive value mean.[66] As females age, the estrogen-to-androgen production ratio changes and results in female faces to appear more and more masculine (thus appearing less "attractive").[66] In a small (n=148) study performed in the United States, using male college students at one university, the mean age expressed as ideal for a wife was found to be 16.87 years old, while 17.76 was the mean ideal age for a brief sexual encounter. However, the study sets up a framework where "taboos against sex with young girls" are purposely diminished, and biased their sample by removing any participant over the age of 30, with a mean participant age of 19.83.[67] In a study of penile tumescence, men were found most aroused by pictures of young adult females.[68]

Signals of fertility in women are often also seen as signals of youth. The evolutionary perspective proposes the idea that when it comes to sexual reproduction, the minimal parental investment required by men gives them the ability and want to simply reproduce 'as much as possible.'[69] It therefore makes sense that men are attracted to the features in women which signal youthfulness, and thus fertility.[69] Their chances of reproductive success are much higher than they would be should they pair with someone older—and therefore less fertile.

This may explain why combating age declines in attractiveness occurs from a younger age in women than in men. For example, the removal of one's body hair is considered a very feminine thing to do.[70] This can be explained by the fact that aging results in raised levels of testosterone and thus, body hair growth. Shaving reverts one's appearance to a more youthful stage[70] and although this may not be an honest signal, men will interpret this as a reflection of increased fertile value. Research supports this, showing hairlessness is considered sexually attractive by men.[71]

Leg-to-body ratio edit

 
 
These drawings of two male and two female figures is a remake of the drawing of the extremes of leg-to-body ratio (LBR), as used in the experiment by Swami et al. (2006) to find out what LBR is considered the most attractive for British men and women. The male figure with the lowest LBR and shortest legs at left had the highest average attractiveness ratings whereas the male figure with the highest LBR and longest legs at right had the lowest ratings from . The female figure with the lowest LBR and shortest legs at left had the lowest average attractiveness ratings whereas the figure with the highest LBR and longest legs at right had the highest average attractiveness ratings[72]

"Leg-to-body ratio" is seen as an indicator of physical attractiveness but there appears to be no single accepted definition of leg-length: the 'perineum-to-floor' measure[b] is the most frequently used, but arguably the distance from the ankle bone to the outer hip bone is more rigorous.[73] With the latter metric, the most attractive male leg-to-body ratio (judged by American women) is 1:1.[73] A Japanese study using the former metric found the same result for male attractiveness but women with longer legs than the rest of their body were judged to be more attractive.[74] Excessive deviations from the mean were seen as indicative of disease.[74] A study using Polish participants found that legs 5% longer than the average for both sexes was considered most attractive.[75] The study concluded this preference might stem from the influence of long-legged runway models.[76] Another study using British and American participants found "mid-ranging" leg-to-body ratios to be most ideal.[77]

A study by Swami et al. of British male and female undergraduates showed a preference for men with legs as long as the rest of their body and women with 40% longer legs than the rest of their body.[72] The researcher concluded that this preference might be influenced by American culture, in which long-legged women are portrayed as more attractive.[72]

Marco Bertamini criticized the Swami et al. study for using a picture of the same person with digitally altered leg lengths which he felt would make the modified image appear unrealistic.[78] Bertamini also criticized the Swami study for only changing the leg length while keeping the arm length constant.[78] After accounting for these concerns in his own study, Bertamini, using stick figures, also found a preference for women with proportionately longer legs than men.[78] When Bertamini investigated the issue of possible sexual dimorphism of leg length, he found two sources that indicated that men usually have slightly proportionately longer legs than women or that differences in leg length proportion may not exist between men and women.[79] Following this review of existing literature on the subject, he conducted his own calculations using data from 1774 men and 2208 women. Using this data, he similarly found that men usually have slightly proportionately longer legs than women or that differences in leg length proportion may not exist between men and women. These findings made him rule out the possibility that a preference for women with proportionately longer legs than men is due proportionately longer legs being a secondary sex characteristic of women.[79]

Genitalia edit

A 2006 study of 25,594 heterosexual men found that "men who perceived themselves as having a large penis were more satisfied with their own appearance".[80]

A 2014 study criticized previous studies based on the fact that they relied on images and used terms such as "small", "medium", and "large" when asking for female preference. The new study used 3D models of penises from sizes of 4 inches (10 cm) long and 2.5 inches (6.4 cm) in circumference to 8.5 inches (22 cm) long and 7 inches (18 cm) in circumference and let the women "view and handle" them. It was found that women overestimated the actual size of the penises they experimented with when asked in a follow-up survey. The study concluded that "women on average preferred the 6.5-inch (17 cm) penis in length both for long-term and for one-time partners. Penises with larger girth were preferred for one-time partners."[81]

Evidence from various cultures suggests that heterosexual men tend to find the sight of women's genitalia to be sexually arousing.[82]

Skin color edit

 
Les Deux Perles Fernand Le Quesne (The two pearls, 1889). This painting was intended to "contrast a Caucasian with an African beauty". In the painting, the black woman represents the beauty of a black pearl and the white woman represents the beauty of a white pearl.[83]

Manual laborers who spent extended periods of time outside developed a darker skin tone due to exposure to the sun. As a consequence, an association between dark skin and the lower classes developed. Light skin became an aesthetic ideal because it symbolized wealth. "Over time society attached various meanings to these colored differences. Including assumptions about a person's race, socioeconomic class, intelligence, and physical attractiveness."[84]

Some research has suggested that redder and yellower skin tones,[85] reflecting higher levels of oxygenated blood,[86] carotenoid and to a lesser extent melanin pigment, and net dietary intakes of fruit and vegetables,[87] appear healthier, and therefore more attractive.[88] However, there is little direct evidence that skin colour is actually related to health or immune system strength.[89]

A historical preference for lighter-skinned women has been documented across many cultures.[90][91] However, the accuracy of this research has been questioned by other authors.[92] Experimental studies show that white Western men are more attracted to dark-tanned women, rather than medium-tanned or pale women.[93][94][95] A 2010 study found a preference for lighter-skinned (but not lightest) women in New Zealand and California.[96] However, other research has found that African-American males and females consider medium complexion as more attractive than lighter or darker skin, while white and Hispanic women seek to tan their skin in order to increase their attractiveness to the opposite sex.[97] There is a direct correlation between being tan and self-perceived attractiveness, especially among young women.[98]

The relationship between attractiveness and skin colour may also intersect with ethnicity and prior experience.[99] Skin color preferences may shift over time, as in Western culture, where tanned skin used to be associated with the sun-exposed manual labor of the lower-class, but since the mid-20th century it has generally been considered more attractive and healthier than before, with sun tanning becoming fashionable.[100][101][102][103][104] In the African state of Mali, skin bleaching is common as it is thought to improve one's social standing and attractiveness to the opposite sex, although there has also been vocal opposition to this notion from pop culture icons.[105]

Skin radiance or glowing skin may influence perception of beauty and physical attractiveness.[106][107]

Hands edit

Hands have been found to be physically attractive.[108] The type of hands that are physically attractive are those with longer index and ring fingers.[109] Men have a smaller index-to-ring-finger ratio than women. The gender differences in the ratio between the index and ring fingers are said to be influenced by exposure to testosterone within the womb.[110] In a study where participants were shown computer-based images of hands, male participants found feminine hands with a smaller index finger less attractive. Whereas females found masculine hands with a longer ring finger more attractive. The study suggests that finger length has an effect on physical attraction because it gives indication of the desirable sex-hormone dependent traits which one may possess.[109] Another study found that averageness, healthiness of the skin, how fat the hands appear to be, and the grooming of the hands, all affect the attractiveness of hands.[111] What is meant by averageness is the degree to which the hands look like an average of the hands in the population. Average-looking hands give an indication of an individual's health (because there are no abnormalities). The healthier-looking the skin on the hands, the more attractive they appear. Reasons given for this say skin health may reflect an individual's overall health. Healthy skin can show that someone is free from illness because some illnesses have a bad effect on the look of skin. These features are found attractive because they show that the person has good genes and is therefore a suitable mate to reproduce with. Skin health may also give an indication of socioeconomic status, as rough hands may indicate a low-paying, laborious job. Low socioeconomic status might show that someone does not have resources to provide for the offspring, and is therefore less attractive. The more fat the hands appear, the less attractive they are. This is because of the co-morbidity associated with obesity. If someone is overfat, they may have another disease, which means they may not be able to produce healthy offspring. The attractiveness of the hands also gives an indication of other features of the individual, people with more attractive hands have been found to be taller and slimmer.[108] In most of these hand attractiveness studies, only white, European hands were used, and the participants were 18–26 years old. So, the attractiveness of non-white hands and of different age groups was not tested. Also, the people who rated the hand attractiveness were white Europeans, so their ratings may not represent how individuals of other skin colours and cultures would rate the hands.

Height edit

Females' sexual attraction towards males may be determined by the height of the man.[112] The online dating Website eHarmony only matches women with men taller than themselves, because of complaints from women matched with shorter men.[65]

Other studies have shown that heterosexual women often prefer men taller than they are, rather than a man with above average height. While women usually desire men to be at least the same height as themselves or taller, several other factors also determine male attractiveness, and the male-taller norm is not universal.[113]> For example, taller women are more likely to relax the "taller male" norm than shorter women.[114] Furthermore, professor Adam Eyre-Walker, from the University of Sussex, has stated that there is, as yet, no evidence that these preferences are evolutionary preferences, as opposed to merely cultural preferences.[115] Still, the cultural perceived attractiveness preferences for taller men are powerful and confirmed by multiple studies. One study of speed-daters by Stulp found that "women were most likely to choose [men] 25 cm taller than themselves, whereas men were most likely to choose women only 7 cm shorter than themselves".[116]

Additionally, women seem more receptive to an erect posture than men, though both prefer it as an element within beauty.[117] According to one study (Yee N., 2002), gay men who identify as "only tops" tend to prefer shorter men, while gay men who identify as "only bottoms" tend to prefer taller men.[118]

In romances in Middle English literature, all of the "ideal" male heroes are tall, and the vast majority of the "valiant" male heroes are tall too.[119]

Most men tend to be taller than their female partners.[21] It has been found that, in Western societies, most men prefer women shorter than themselves.[120] Having said this, height is a more important factor for a woman when choosing a man than it is for a man choosing a woman.[121] Western men tend to view women taller than themselves as less attractive,[120] and many people view heterosexual couples where the woman is taller to be less ideal.[120] Women who are 0.7 to 1.7 standard deviations below the mean female height have been reported to be the most reproductively successful,[115] since fewer tall women get married compared to shorter women.[120] However, in other ethnic groups, such as the Hadza people from Tanzania, a study has found that height is irrelevant in choosing a mate.[113] Another study found the same preference in rural Gambia.[120]

In Middle English literature, "tallness" is a characteristic of ideally beautiful women.[122] The British Fashion Model Agents Association (BFMA) says that female models should be at least 5 ft 8 in (1.73 m) tall.[123]

Body language edit

Standing postures edit

 
A statue of Venus standing in a contrapposto pose with her weight borne predominantly on one leg. As shown here, this posture accentuates the curvature of her figure.

Standing in a contrapposto posture (with bodyweight predominantly supported by one leg which is either straight, or very slightly bent, and with the other leg slightly bent) has been found to be more attractive looking than standing in a more plain, upright posture. This was found to be the case for both men and women. This posture may lower a person's observable waist-hip ratio and make their hips look wider and their waists thinner. For women especially, this can accentuate the curvature of their figure on one side of their body and make them seem more attractive. Such poses have been used in historical sculpture to emphasize an ideal of physical beauty.[124][125] It has also been demonstrated that the contrapposto posture in women elicits more neural activity in brain areas linked to perception and attractiveness assessments than a standing position.[126]

Movement patterns edit

The way an individual moves can indicate health and even age and influence attractiveness.[127] A study reflecting the views of 700 individuals and that involved animated representations of people walking, found that the physical attractiveness of women increased by about 50 percent when they walked with a hip sway. Similarly, the perceived attractiveness of males doubled when they moved with a swagger in their shoulders.[128]

Male-specific factors edit

 
Arnold Schwarzenegger, one of the most notable figures in bodybuilding, 1974

Women, on average, tend to be more attracted to men who have a relatively narrow waist, a V-shaped torso, wide chest and broad shoulders. Women also tend to be more attracted to men who are taller and larger than they are, and display a high degree of facial symmetry, as well as relatively masculine facial dimorphism.[22][23] Women, regardless of sexual orientation, tend to be more interested in a partner's physical attractiveness than men.[129][130][131]

Sexual dimorphism edit

The degree of differences between male and female anatomical traits is called sexual dimorphism. Female respondents in the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle were significantly more likely to choose a masculine face than those in menses and luteal phases,[132] (or in those taking hormonal contraception).[22][23][133][134] This distinction supports the sexy son hypothesis, which posits that it is evolutionarily advantageous for women to select potential fathers who are more genetically attractive,[135] rather than the best caregivers.[136] However, women's likeliness to exert effort to view male faces does not seem to depend on their masculinity, but to a general increase with women's testosterone levels.[137]

It is suggested that the masculinity of facial features is a reliable indication of good health, or, alternatively, that masculine-looking males are more likely to achieve high status.[138] However, the correlation between attractive facial features and health has been questioned.[139] Sociocultural factors, such as self-perceived attractiveness, status in a relationship and degree of gender-conformity, have been reported to play a role in female preferences for male faces.[140] Studies have found that women who perceive themselves as physically attractive are more likely to choose men with masculine facial dimorphism, than are women who perceive themselves as physically unattractive.[45] In men, facial masculinity significantly correlates with facial symmetry – it has been suggested that both are signals of developmental stability and genetic health.[141] One study called into question the importance of facial masculinity in physical attractiveness in men, arguing that when perceived health, which is factored into facial masculinity, is discounted it makes little difference in physical attractiveness.[142] In a cross-country study involving 4,794 women in their early twenties, a difference was found in women's average "masculinity preference" between countries.[143][144]

A study found that the same genetic factors cause facial masculinity in both males and females such that a male with a more masculine face would likely have a sister with a more masculine face due to the siblings having shared genes. The study also found that, although female faces that were more feminine were judged to be more attractive, there was no association between male facial masculinity and male facial attractiveness for female judges. With these findings, the study reasoned that if a woman were to reproduce with a man with a more masculine face, then her daughters would also inherit a more masculine face, making the daughters less attractive. The study concluded that there must be other factors that advantage the genetics for masculine male faces to offset their reproductive disadvantage in terms of "health", "fertility" and "facial attractiveness" when the same genetics are present in females. The study reasoned that the "selective advantage" for masculine male faces must "have (or had)" been due to some factor that is not directly tied to female perceptions of male facial attractiveness.[145]

In a study of 447 gay men in China, researchers said that tops preferred feminized male faces, bottoms preferred masculinized male faces and versatiles had no preference for either feminized or masculinized male faces.[146]

In pre-modern Chinese literature, the ideal man in caizi jiaren romances was said to have "rosy lips, sparkling white teeth" and a "jasper-like face" (Chinese: 唇紅齒白、面若冠玉).[147][148]

In Middle English literature, a beautiful man should have a long, broad and strong face.[149]

Waist-to-chest ratio edit

The physique of a slim waist, broad shoulders and muscular chest are often found to be attractive to both females and males.[150] Further research has shown that, when choosing a mate, the traits females look for indicate higher social status, such as dominance, resources, and protection.[151] An indicator of health in males (a contributing factor to physical attractiveness) is the android fat distribution pattern which is categorized as more fat distributed on the upper body and abdomen, commonly referred to as the "V shape."[151] When asked to rate other men, both heterosexual and homosexual men found low waist-to-chest ratios (WCR) to be more attractive on other men, with the gay men showing a preference for lower WCR (more V-shaped) than the straight men.[152]

Other researchers found waist-to-chest ratio the largest determinant of male attractiveness, with body mass index and waist-to-hip ratio not as significant.[153]

Women focus primarily on the ratio waist to chest or more specifically waist to shoulder. This is analogous to the waist to hip ratio (WHR) that men prefer. Some studies have shown that attractive bodily traits in the eyes of a heterosexual woman would include a tall, athletic physique, with wide shoulders, and a slim waist area.[154] Research has additionally shown that college males had a better satisfaction with their body than college females.[155] The research also found that when a college female's waist to hip ratio went up, their body image satisfaction decreased.[155]

Some research has shown that body weight may have a stronger effect than WHR when it comes to perceiving attractiveness of the opposite sex. It was found that waist to hip ratio played a smaller role in body preference than body weight in regards to both sexes.[156]

Psychologists Viren Swami and Martin J. Tovee compared female preference for male attractiveness cross culturally, between Britain and Malaysia. They found that females placed more importance on WCR (and therefore body shape) in urban areas of Britain and Malaysia, while females in rural areas placed more importance on BMI (therefore weight and body size). Both WCR and BMI are indicative of male status and ability to provide for offspring, as noted by evolutionary theory.[157]

Females have been found to desire males that are normal weight and have the average WHR for a male. Females view these males as attractive and healthy. Males who had the average WHR but were overweight or underweight are not perceived as attractive to females. This suggests that WHR is not a major factor in male attractiveness, but a combination of body weight and a typical male WHR seem to be the most attractive. Research has shown that men who have a higher waist to hip ratio and a higher salary are perceived as more attractive to women.[158]

Flat abdomen edit

A 1982 study found that an abdomen that protrudes was the "least attractive" trait for men.[159]

In Middle English literature, a beautiful man should have a flat abdomen.[160]

Musculature edit

 
Ronnie Coleman posing
 
Ronnie Coleman posing in 2009

Men's bodies portrayed in magazines marketed to men are more muscular than the men's bodies portrayed in magazines marketed to women. From this, some have concluded that men perceive a more muscular male body to be ideal, as distinct from a woman's ideal male, which is less muscular than what men perceive to be ideal.[161] This is due to the within-gender prestige granted by increased muscularity and within-gender competition for increased muscularity.[161] Men perceive the attractiveness of their own musculature by how closely their bodies resemble the "muscle man."[162] This "muscle man" ideal is characterized by large muscular arms, especially biceps, a large muscular chest that tapers to their waist and broad shoulders.[162] Among Australian university students, the male body composition found to be most attractive (12.16 kg fat, 63.27 kg muscle) was in line with the composition that was perceived as healthiest, and was well within the healthy range.[163]

In a study of stated profile preferences on Match.com, a greater percentage of gay men than lesbians selected their ideal partner's body type as "Athletic and Toned" as opposed to the other two options of "Average" or "Overweight".[164]

In pre-modern Chinese literature, such as in Romance of the Western Chamber, a type of masculinity called "scholar masculinity" is depicted wherein the "ideal male lover" is "weak, vulnerable, feminine, and pedantic".[147]

In Middle English literature, a beautiful man typically has thick, broad shoulders, a square and muscular chest, a muscular back, strong sides that taper to a small waist, large hands and arms and legs with huge muscles.[165]

Body hair edit

Studies based in the United States, New Zealand, and China have shown that women rate men with no trunk (chest and abdominal) hair as most attractive, and that attractiveness ratings decline as hairiness increases.[166][167] Another study, however, found that moderate amounts of trunk hair on men was most attractive, to the sample of British and Sri Lankan women.[168] Further, a degree of hirsuteness (hairiness) and a waist-to-shoulder ratio of 0.6 is often preferred when combined with a muscular physique.[168]

In a study using Finnish women, women with hairy fathers were more likely to prefer hairy men, suggesting that preference for hairy men is the result of either genetics or imprinting.[169] Among gay men, another study reported gay males who identify as "only tops" prefer less hairy men, while gay males who identify as "only bottoms" prefer more hairy men.[118]

Facial hair edit

One study shows that men with facial hair covering the cheeks, upper lip, and lower jaw were perceived as more physically attractive than men with patchy facial hair.[170] In this study, men's facial hair was split into four categories, each differing in the thickness and coverage: very light, light, medium, and heavy. Light facial hair was rated as the most attractive, followed by medium, heavy, and the least attractive was 'very light'. This study suggests that some facial hair is better than none because it shows masculine development, as beard growth requires the conversion of testosterone. An earlier study found that women from Western and Oceanic cultures are more attracted to clean-shaven faces than beards. However, they also rated full-bearded men as having higher status than clean-shaven men.[171]

Jawline edit

An attractive male jaw angle may have the following features:[citation needed]

Because it generates the gonial angle, the ramus has a function in attractiveness.[citation needed] The gonial or jaw angle is the angle formed by the ramus and the rest of the mandible. It can range from 90 to 140 degrees, with the population average being 128 +/- 2.36 degrees for males and 126 +/-2.41 degrees for females. Larger, more obtuse gonial angles result in a rounder, feminine appearance, whereas more acute gonial angles result in square, masculine faces.[172][173]

This exact ratio is not seen as ideal in every ethnic group. Most research shows that attractive bigonial width and Ramus measurements have similarities, but the jutting square chin is a prominently European-heritage trait – which means it should not be held as a universal indicator of attractiveness.[174][175] Men with low submental fat were viewed to have "better jawlines" and a more "youthful look".[176]

Female-specific factors edit

Research indicates that heterosexual men tend to be attracted to young[64] and beautiful women[177] with bodily symmetry.[178] Rather than decreasing it, modernity has only increased the emphasis men place on women's looks.[179] Evolutionary psychologists attribute such attraction to an evaluation of the fertility potential in a prospective mate.[64]

Facial features edit

General edit

 
A University of Toronto study found that the facial proportions of Jessica Alba were close to the average of all female profiles.
 
Namie Amuro inspired the small-face fad in Japan which caused Japanese women to buy beauty products such as masks and creams to try to obtain a small face like hers.[180]
 
An 1889 U.S. newspaper ad for arsenic complexion wafers decried blotches, moles, pimples, freckles, and "all female irregularities".[181] Arsenic was known to be poisonous during the Victorian era.[182]

Research has attempted to determine which facial features communicate attractiveness. Facial symmetry has been shown to be considered attractive in women,[183][184] and men have been found to prefer full lips,[185] high forehead, broad face, small chin, small nose, short and narrow jaw, high cheekbones,[39][186] clear and smooth skin, and wide-set eyes.[64] The shape of the face in terms of "how everything hangs together" is an important determinant of beauty.[187] Women with thick, dark limbal rings in their eyes have also been found to be more attractive. The explanation given is that because the ring tends to fade with age and medical problems, a prominent limbal ring gives an honest indicator of youth.[188]

In Persian literature, beautiful women are said to have noses like hazelnuts.[189] In Arabian society in the Middle Ages, a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have straight and fine noses.[190]In Jewish Rabbinic literature, the rabbis considered a delicate nose to be the ideal type of nose for women.[191] In Japan, during the Edo period, a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have tall noses which were straight and not "too tall".[192]

In a cross-cultural study, more neotenized (i.e., youthful looking) female faces were found to be most attractive to men while less neotenized female faces were found to be less attractive to men, regardless of the females' actual age.[193] In a study of Italian women who have won beauty competitions, it was found that their faces had more "babyish" (pedomorphic) traits than those of the "normal" women used as a reference.[194]

In a cross-cultural study, Marcinkowska et al. said that 18-to-45-year-old heterosexual men in all 28 countries surveyed preferred photographs of 18-to-24-year-old white women whose faces were feminized using facial image editing software over faces of 18-to-24-year-old white women that were masculinized using that software, but there were differences in preferences for femininity across countries. The higher the National Health Index (based on eight national health statistics taken from the World Health Organization Statistical Information Service using data from 2002 to 2006) of a country, the more were the feminized faces preferred over the masculinized faces. Among the countries surveyed, Japan had the highest femininity preference and Nepal had the lowest femininity preference.[195]

Michael R. Cunningham of the Department of Psychology at the University of Louisville found, using a panel of East Asian, Hispanic and White judges, that the female faces tended to be judged as more attractive if they had a mixture of youthful and sexually mature features. Hispanic and East Asian women were judged as more attractive than White and Black women, and they happened to possess more of the attributes defined as attractive, however the authors noted that it would be inaccurate to conclude that any ethnic group was more attractive than the other, based on their sample.[196] Using a panel of African Americans and whites as judges, Cunningham found more neotenous faces were perceived as having both higher "femininity" and "sociability".[90] The authors found no evidence of ethnocentric bias in the Asian or White samples, as Asians and Whites did not differ significantly in preference for neonate cues, and positive ratings of white women did not increase with exposure to Western media.[197]

Rather than finding evidence for purely "neonate" faces being most appealing, Cunningham found faces with "sexually-mature" features at the "periphery" of the face combined with "neonate" features in the "center of the face" most appealing in women.[90] Upon analyzing the results of his study, Cunningham concluded that preference for "neonate features may display the least cross-cultural variability" in terms of "attractiveness ratings"[90] and, in another study, Cunningham concluded that there exists a large agreement on the characteristics of an attractive face.[198]

In computer face averaging tests, women with averaged faces have been shown to be considered more attractive.[31][199] This is possibly due to average features being more familiar and, therefore, more comfortable.[183]

Commenting on the prevalence of whiteness in supposed beauty ideals in his book White Lies: Race and the Myth of Whiteness, Maurice Berger states that the schematic rendering in the idealized face of a study conducted with American subjects had "straight hair," "light skin," "almond-shaped eyes," "thin, arched eyebrows," "a long, thin nose, closely set and tiny nostrils" and "a large mouth and thin lips",[200] though the author of the study stated that there was consistency between his results and those conducted on other races. Scholar Liu Jieyu says in the article White Collar Beauties, "The criterion of beauty is both arbitrary and gendered. The implicit consensus is that women who have fair skin and a slim figure with symmetrical facial features are pretty." He says that all of these requirements are socially constructed and force people to change themselves to fit these criteria.[201]

On average, symmetrical features are one ideal, while unusual, stand-out features are another.[202] A study performed by the University of Toronto found that the most attractive facial dimensions were those found in the average female face. However, that particular University of Toronto study looked only at white women.[203]

A 2011 study, by Wilkins, Chan and Kaiser found correlations between perceived femininity and attractiveness, that is, women's faces which were seen as more feminine were judged by both men and women to be more attractive.[204]

A component of the female beauty ideal in Persian literature is for women to have faces like a full moon.[189][205][206]

In Arabian society in the Middle Ages, a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have round faces which were like a "full moon".[190]

In Japan, during the Edo period, a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have oval faces. which were shaped like ovals.[192]

In Jewish Rabbinic literature, the rabbis considered full lips to be the ideal type of lips for women.[191]

Historically, in Chinese and Japanese literature, the feminine ideal was said to include small lips.[207] Women would paint their lips thinner and narrower to align with this ideal.[208][209]

A study that used Chinese, Malay and Indian judges said that Chinese women and Chinese men with retrusive mandibles (where the mouth is flat and in-line with the rest of the face) were judged to be the most attractive and Chinese men and Chinese women with a protruding mandible (where the jaw projects outward) were judged to be the least attractive.[210]

Classical Persian literature, paintings, and miniatures portrayed traits such as long black curly hair, a small mouth, long arched eyebrows, large almond shaped eyes, a small nose, and beauty spots as being beautiful for women.[211]

Eyes edit

A study that investigated whether or not an eyelid crease makes Chinese-descent women more attractive using photo-manipulated photographs of young Chinese-descent women's eyes found that the "medium upper eyelid crease" was considered most attractive by all three groups of both sexes: white people, Chinese and Taiwanese nationals together as a group, and Taiwanese and Chinese Americans together as a group. Similarly, all three groups of both genders found the absence of an eye crease to be least attractive on Chinese women.[212]

In the late sixteenth century, Japanese people considered epicanthic folds to be beautiful.[213]

A study that used Russian, American, Brazilian, Aché, and Hiwi raters, found that the only strong distinguisher between men and women's faces was wider eyes relative to facial height for women, and this trait consistently predicted attractiveness ratings for women.[214]

In Arabian society in the Middle Ages, a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have dark black eyes which are large and long and in the shape of almonds. Furthermore, the eyes should be lustrous, and they should have long eyelashes.[190]

A source written in 1823, said that a component of the Persian female beauty ideal was for women to have large eyes which are black in color.[205] In Persian literature, beautiful women are said to have eyes that are shaped like almonds.[189]

In Chinese, the phrase "lucent irises, lustrous teeth" (Chinese: 明 眸 皓 齒) is used to describe a beautiful woman with "clear eyes" and "well-aligned, white teeth",[215] and the phrase "moth-feeler eyebrows" (Chinese: 蛾眉) is used to denote a beautiful woman by describing her eyebrows as being thin and arched like moth antennae.[215] In the Chinese text The Grotto of the Immortals (Chinese: 遊 仙 窟) written during the Tang dynasty period, narrow eyes were the preferred type of eyes for women, and, in the Chinese text Jeweled Chamber Secrets (Chinese: 玉 房 秘 訣) from the Six Dynasties period, the ideal woman was described as having small eyes.[216]

In Japan, during the Edo period, one piece of evidence, the appearance of the "formal wife" of Tokugawa Iesada as determined by "bone anthropologist" Suzuki Hisashi, indicates that large eyes were considered attractive for women,[192] but,in another piece of evidence, the 1813 Japanese text Customs, Manners, and Fashions of the Capital (Japanese: 都 風 俗 化 粧 伝), indicates that large eyes were not considered attractive for women.[217]

Breasts edit

Research has shown that most heterosexual men enjoy the sight of female breasts, with a preference for large, firm breasts.[218][219] However, a contradictory study of British undergraduates found younger men preferred small breasts on women.[220] Smaller breasts were widely associated with youthfulness.[127] Cross-culturally, another study found "high variability" regarding the ideal breast size.[220] Some researchers in the United Kingdom have speculated that a preference for larger breasts may have developed in Western societies because women with larger breasts tend to have higher levels of the hormones estradiol and progesterone, which both promote fertility.[221]

A study by Groyecka et al., in which they examined Poles and Yali of New Guinea, demonstrated that men's judgements of breast appearance is affected by the occurrence of breast ptosis (i.e., sagginess, droopiness).[222] Greater breast ptosis (more sagging breasts) is perceived as less attractive and attributed to a woman of older age. These findings are coherent with previous research that link breast attractiveness with female youthfulness. Unlike breast size, breast ptosis seems to be a universal marker of female breast attractiveness.

A study showed that men prefer symmetrical breasts.[178][223] Breast symmetry may be particularly sensitive to developmental disturbances and the symmetry differences for breasts are large compared to other body parts. Women who have more symmetrical breasts tend to have more children.[224]

Historical literature often includes specific features of individuals or a gender that are considered desirable. These have often become a matter of convention, and should be interpreted with caution. In Arabian society in the Middle Ages, a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have small breasts.[190] In Persian literature, beautiful women are said to have breasts like pomegranates or lemons.[189] In the Chinese text Jeweled Chamber Secrets (Chinese: 玉 房 秘 訣) from the Six Dynasties period, the ideal woman was described as having firm breasts.[216] In Sanskrit literature, beautiful women are often said to have breasts so large that they cause the women to bend a little bit from their weight.[225] In Middle English literature, beautiful women should have small breasts that are round like an apple or a pear.[226]

Buttocks edit

 
A remake of a drawing used to research perceptions of the most attractive size of posterior and breasts for white and black women.

Biological anthropologist Helen E. Fisher of the Center for Human Evolution Studies in the Department of Anthropology of Rutgers University said that, "perhaps, the fleshy, rounded buttocks... attracted males during rear-entry intercourse."[227] In a recent study, using 3D models and eye-tracking technology Fisher's claim was tested and was shown that the slight thrusting out of a woman's back influence how attractive others perceive her to be and captures the gaze of both men and women.[228][229] Bobbi S. Low et al. of the School of Natural Resources and Environment at the University of Michigan, said the female "buttocks evolved in the context of females competing for the attention and parental commitment of powerful resource-controlling males" as an "honest display of fat reserves" that could not be confused with another type of tissue,[230] although T. M. Caro, professor in the Center for Population Biology and the Department of Wildlife, Fish, and Conservation Biology, at University of California, Davis, rejected that as being a necessary conclusion, stating that female fatty deposits on the hips improve "individual fitness of the female", regardless of sexual selection.[230]

In a 1995 study, black men were more likely than white men to use the words "big" or "large" to describe their conception of an attractive woman's posterior.[231] In a 2009 experiment to research what South African, British white and British African men considered to be the most attractive size of posterior and breasts for white and black women. This image shown here only shows the two extreme variations of size on black female figures used in the experiment. The left-hand figure received the highest average attractiveness rating from South African men while a figure with an intermediate size received the highest ratings from both white and black British men. The right-hand figure did not receive the highest average attractiveness rating from any group.[232]

Body mass edit

Body Mass Index (BMI) is an important determinant to the perception of beauty.[233] Even though the Western ideal is for a thin woman, some cultures prefer plumper women,[90][234] which has been argued to support that attraction for a particular BMI merely is a cultural artifact.[234] The attraction for a proportionate body also influences an appeal for erect posture.[235] One cross-cultural survey comparing body-mass preferences among 300 of the most thoroughly studied cultures in the world showed that 81% of cultures preferred a female body size that in English would be described as "plump".[236]

Availability of food influences which female body size is attractive which may have evolutionary reasons. Societies with food scarcities prefer larger female body size than societies that have plenty of food. In Western society, males who are hungry prefer a larger female body size than they do when not hungry.[237]

BMI has been criticised for conflating fat and muscle, and more recent studies have concentrated on body composition. Among Australian university students, the most attractive body composition for women (10.31 kg fat, 42.45 kg muscle) was found to be lower in fat than both the most healthy appearing composition, and below the healthy range.[163]

In the United States, women overestimate men's preferences for thinness in a mate. In one study, American women were asked to choose what their ideal build was and what they thought the build most attractive to men was. Women chose slimmer than average figures for both choices. When American men were independently asked to choose the female build most attractive to them, the men chose figures of average build. This indicates that women may be misled as to how thin men prefer women to be.[234] Some researchers believe women themselves may play a role in enforcing the thinness ideal, however they assert that this does not mean that the ideal does not ultimately trace its origins to patriarchical norms (the "male gaze").[238] Popenoe writes that societies with abundant food sources often value thin women, while those with food scarcity value women with fat bodies. In the West, women with thin bodies became the ideal in the 19th century, as the fat body became associated with criminality and backwardness.[239] Some implicated the fashion industry for the promulgation of the notion of thinness as attractive.[240][241]

East Asians have historically preferred women whose bodies had small features. For example, during the Spring and Autumn period of Chinese history, women in Chinese harems wanted to have a thin body in order to be attractive for the Chinese emperor. Later, during the Tang dynasty, a less thin body type was seen as most attractive for Chinese women.[242] In Arabian society in the Middle Ages, a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to be slender like a "cane" or a "twig".[190] In the Chinese text Jeweled Chamber Secrets (Chinese: 玉 房 秘 訣) from the Six Dynasties period, the ideal woman was described as not being "large-boned".[216]

In the Victorian era, women who adhered to Victorian ideals were expected to limit their food consumption to attain the ideal slim figure.[243] In Middle English literature, "slender" women are considered beautiful.[244]

According to research on females, women consider a full body with good distribution to be highly important to be considered attractive by men.[245]

Waist–hip ratio edit

A WHR of 0.7 for women has been shown to correlate strongly with general health and fertility. Women within the 0.7 range have optimal levels of estrogen and are less susceptible to major diseases such as diabetes, heart disease, and ovarian cancers.[246] Women with high WHR (0.80 or higher) have significantly lower pregnancy rates than women with lower WHRs (0.70–0.79), independent of their BMIs.[247][248] Female waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) has been proposed by evolutionary psychologists to be an important component of human male mate choice, because this trait is thought to provide a reliable cue to a woman's reproductive value.[249]

Both men and women judge women with smaller waist-to-hip ratios more attractive.[250] Ethnic groups vary with regard to their ideal waist-to-hip ratio for women,[251] ranging from 0.6 in China,[252] to 0.8 or 0.9 in parts of South America and Africa,[253][254][255] and divergent preferences based on ethnicity, rather than nationality, have also been noted.[256][257] A study found the Machiguenga people, an isolated indigenous South American ethnic group, prefer women with high WHR (0.9).[258] The preference for heavier women, has been interpreted to belong to societies where there is no risk of obesity.[259]

In Chinese, the phrase "willow waist" (Chinese: 柳 腰) is used to denote a beautiful woman by describing her waist as being slender like a willow branch.[215]

In the Victorian era, a small waist was considered the main trait of a beautiful woman.[243] The term "wasp waist" describes an extreme fashion silhouette, produced by a style of corset and girdle.

Feet size edit

According to some studies, most men prefer women with small feet,[260][261] such as in ancient China where foot binding was practiced.[262]

In Jewish Rabbinic literature, the rabbis considered small feet to be the ideal type of feet for women.[191]

Hair edit

Men have been found to prefer long-haired women.[64][263] [264] An evolutionary psychology explanation for this is that malnutrition and deficiencies in minerals and vitamins causes loss of hair or hair changes. Hair therefore indicates health and nutrition during the last 2–3 years. Lustrous hair is also often a cross-cultural preference.[127]

A component of the female beauty ideal in Persian literature is for women to have black hair,[265] which was also preferred in Arabian society in the Middle Ages.[190] In Middle English literature, curly hair is a necessary component of a beautiful woman.[266]

Fertility-driven attractiveness edit

There are some subtle changes in women's perceived attractiveness across the menstrual cycle. During their most fertile phase, we can observe some changes in women's behavior and physiology. A study conducted by G. Miller (2007) examined the amount of tip earnings by lap dancers across the menstrual cycle. He found that dancers received nearly US$15 more when they were near ovulation than during the rest of the month. This suggests that women either are more attractive during ovulation phase, or they experience a significant change in their behavior.[267] Some other studies have found that there are subtle differences in women's faces when in their fertile phase. Bobst and Lobmaier (2012) created 20 prototyped photographs, some of a female during ovulation and some during the luteal phase. Men were asked to choose the more attractive, the more caring and the more flirtatious faces. They found a significant preference for the follicular phase (ovulation). This suggests that subtle shape differences in faces occurring during the female's ovulation phase are sufficient to attract men more.[268] This idea is supported by another study, where a similar experiment was done. Men and women had to judge photographs of women's faces taken during their fertile phase. They were all rated more attractive than during non-fertile phase. There are some subtle visible cues to ovulation in women's faces, and they are perceived as more attractive, leading to the idea that it could be an adaptive mechanism to raise a female's mate value at that specific time (when probability of conception is at its highest).[267]

Women's attractiveness, as perceived by men and women, slightly differs across her menstrual cycle, being at peak when she is in her ovulation phase. Jones et al. (2008), focused on women's preferences for masculinity, apparent health and self-resemblance and found that it varies across the cycle. They explained that the function of the effects of menstrual cycle phase on preferences for apparent health and self-resemblance in faces is to increase the likelihood of pregnancy.[269]

Similarly, females prefer the scent of symmetrically faced men and men with masculine faces during fertile phases as well as stereotypical male displays such as social presence, and direct intrasexual competitiveness.[270]

During the follicular phase (fertile), females prefer more masculine traits (testosterone dependent traits such as face shape) than when in non-fertile phase. Those findings have been found in the voice, showing that females' preferences for more masculine voices over feminine voices increase the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle.[271]

But not only females' preferences vary across cycle, their behaviours as well. Effectively, men respond differently to females when they are on ovulatory cycle,[267] because females act differently. Women in the ovulatory phase are flirtier with males showing genetic fitness markers than in low fertile phase.[272] It has been shown in some studies that women high in estrogen are generally perceived to be more attractive than women with low levels of estrogen, based on women not wearing make-up. High estrogen level women may also be viewed as healthier or to have a more feminine face.[273]

Similarly, a study investigated the capacity of women to select high quality males based on their facial attractiveness. They found that facial attractiveness correlated with semen quality (good, normal, or bad depending on sperm morphology and motility). The more attractive a man's face is, linked to his sperm being of better quality.[274]

Sexual ornamentation edit

Sexual ornaments are seen in many organisms; in humans, females have sexual ornamentation in the form of breasts and buttocks. The physical attraction to sexual ornaments is associated with gynoid fat, as opposed to android fat, which is considered unattractive.[275] In human females, proximate causes of the development of sexual ornaments are associated with the predominance of estrogen in puberty. The activation of estrogen receptors around the female skeletal tissue causes gynoid fat to be deposited in the breasts, buttocks, hips and thighs, producing an overall typical female body shape.[276] Specifically, female breasts are considered more attractive when symmetrical, rather than asymmetrical,[277] as this is thought to reflect good developmental stability.[278]

Sexual ornaments are considered attractive features as they are thought to indicate high mate value, fertility,[279] and the ability to provide good care to offspring. They are sexually selected traits present for the purpose of honest signalling and capturing the visual attention of the opposite sex, most commonly associated with females capturing the visual attention of males. It has been proposed that these ornaments have evolved in order to advertise personal quality and reproductive value.[280] Honest signalling with sexual ornaments is associated with ultimate causation of these evolved traits. The evolution of these ornaments is also associated with female-female competition in order to gain material benefits provided by resourceful and high status males.[281] In humans, once these sexual ornaments develop, they are permanent. It is thought that this is associated with the long-term pair bonding humans engage in; human females engage in extended sexual activity outside of their fertile period.[282] This relates to another ultimate cause of sexual ornaments with function in obtaining non-genetic material benefits from males. In other animal species, even other primate species, these advertisements of reproductive value are not permanent. Usually, it is the point at which the female is at her most fertile, she displays sexual swellings.[283]

Adolescence is the period of time whereby humans experience puberty, and experience anatomical changes to their bodies through the increase of sex hormones released in the body. Adolescent exaggeration is the period of time at which sexual ornaments are maximised, and peak gynoid fat content is reached.[281] In human females, the mean age for this is approximately 16 years. Female breasts develop at this stage not only to prepare for reproduction, but also due to competition with other females in displaying their reproductive value and quality to males.[281]

Interpersonal attraction edit

In the social psychology perspective, interpersonal attraction is when someone experiences a positive attitude or evaluation regarding a particular person having potential of being a significant other. This includes "three components conventionally ascribed to attitudes: behavioral (tendency to approach the person), cognitive (positive beliefs about the person), and affective (positive feelings for the person)."[284] With these three components, psychology has created five elements of interpersonal attraction. These include proximity, similarity, physical attractiveness, reciprocity, and responsiveness.[285]


Neural correlates of perceiving attractiveness edit

Most studies of the brain activations associated with the perception of attractiveness show photographs of faces to their participants and let them or a comparable group of people rate the attractiveness of these faces. Such studies consistently find that activity in certain parts of the orbitofrontal cortex increases with increasing attractiveness of faces.[286][287][281][288][289] This neural response has been interpreted as a reaction on the rewarding nature of attractiveness, as similar increases in activation in the medial orbitofrontal cortex can be seen in response to smiling faces[290] and to statements of morally good actions.[281][289] While most of these studies have not assessed participants of both genders or homosexual individuals, evidence from one study including male and female hetero- and homosexual individuals indicate that some of the aforementioned increases in brain activity are restricted to images of faces of the gender which participants feel sexually attracted to.[291]

With regard to brain activation related to the perception of attractive bodies, one study with heterosexual participants suggests that activity in the nucleus accumbens and the anterior cingulate cortex increases with increasing attractiveness. The same study finds that for faces and bodies alike, the medial part of the orbitofrontal cortex responds with greater activity to both very attractive and very unattractive pictures.[292] Recent research has found that the perception of women with lower Waist-to-Hip Ratios and larger breasts triggers distinct patterns of temporal and spatial brain activation, differing from those associated with larger WHRs and smaller breasts.[293] Specifically, attractive WHRs and breast sizes influenced brain activity related to visual processing differently than less attractive WHRs and breast sizes, impacting both early and late stages of temporal processing in men and women.[293] Moreover, research on upper body size shows that posterior brain regions are involved in perceiving body forms of differing attractiveness due to variations male upper body size, while frontal brain regions are engaged when these perceptions are explicitly rated.[294] Such brain activities are unique to processing male, but not female, body sizes.

For both men and women, there appear to be universal criteria of attractiveness both within and across cultures and ethnic groups.[19][295] When considering long-term relationships, some studies have found that men place a higher emphasis on physical attractiveness in a partner than women do.[296][297][298][299][300] On the other hand, some studies have found few differences between men and women in terms of the weight they place on physical characteristics when they are choosing partners for short-term relationships,[301][302][303][304] in particular with regard to their implicit, as opposed to explicitly articulated, preferences.[305] Other recent studies continue to find sex differences for long-term relationships.[306][307][308][303] While still valuing physical attractiveness, women tend to prioritize a man's status over his physical attractiveness, while men prioritize physical attractiveness over status.[309] There is also one study suggesting that only men, not women, place greater priority on bodily compared to facial attractiveness when looking for a short-term as compared to a long-term partner.[310]

 
Bangladeshi bride exemplifying wedding day beauty

Some evolutionary psychologists, including David Buss, have argued that this long-term relationship difference may be a consequence of ancestral humans who selected partners based on secondary sexual characteristics, as well as general indicators of fitness which allowed for greater reproductive success as a result of higher fertility in those partners,[311] although a male's ability to provide resources for offspring was likely signaled less by physical features.[298] It is argued that the most prominent indicator of fertility in women is youth,[312][313][314] while the traits in a man which enhance reproductive success are proxies for his ability to accrue resources and protect.[314]

Studies have shown that women pay greater attention to physical traits than they do directly to earning capability or potential to commit,[315] including muscularity, fitness and masculinity of features; the latter preference was observed to vary during a woman's period, with women preferring more masculine features during the late-follicular (fertile) phase of the menstrual cycle.[271][316] Additionally, women process physical attractiveness differently, paying attention to both individual features and the aesthetic effect of the whole face.[317]

According to Bonnie Adrian, Taiwanese brides place great importance on physical attractiveness for their wedding photographs. These brides go through hours of makeup to transform themselves into socially constructed beauty. Adrian notes that female beauty standards and practices in Taiwan are quite different from those found in the West. Women in Taiwan tend to avoid tanning, while Taiwanese women with tanned skin tones would be considered the ideal in the Western world.[318]

According to strategic pluralism theory, men may have correspondingly evolved to pursue reproductive strategies that are contingent on their own physical attractiveness. More physically attractive men accrue reproductive benefits from spending more time seeking multiple mating partners and relatively less time investing in offspring. In contrast, the reproductive effort of physically less attractive men, who therefore will not have the same mating opportunities, is better allocated either to investing heavily in accruing resources, or investing in their mates and offspring and spending relatively less time seeking additional mates.[319]

Facial similarity and racial preferences edit

Research has suggested that people give high attractiveness ratings to same-sex people who look like them, but only in a non-sexual context. On the other hand, people generally do not find similar-looking people of the opposite sex to be sexually attractive. It is theorized that people may prefer sexual partners who look different from them, which is consistent with a pattern of inbreeding avoidance.[320][321][322] Although one study found that individuals show a preference for similar-looking mates, after a physiological stress test they preferred dis-similar looking mates.[323][324]

One study found no strong evidence for a same-race preference in heterosexual people, and they note that evidence from two similar studies is conflicting. Some racial groups show a strong preference for partners of a different race or ethnicity, and this preference may be gendered.[325][326] One study suggested that attraction to people with different ethnic features may be related to the effects of heterozygosity, which are thought to be a fitness advantage.[327]

A 2014 study found that racial preferences in Asian-Americans varied by gender: while heterosexual Asian women preferred to date white men, heterosexual Asian men preferred to date Asian women.[328]

A 2012 study suggests that imbalance in interracial marriage (white male-Asian female marriages are more than twice as common as the reverse) may be attributed to the fact that Asian women are perceived as more feminine, and therefore more attractive, than white women.[329] This study received support from a 2018 study which tested its hypothesis using images of Asian and white individuals.[330]

A 2012 study using Black and Caucasian subjects found that inter-ethnic and intra-ethnic differences in perceived male facial masculinity have no impact on attractiveness, and that skin color plays a more important role in attractiveness assessments of male faces within groups, but not between groups as a health signal.[331]

Group attractiveness effect edit

The group attractiveness effect is where a group's overall attractiveness rating is higher than the mean of each individuals' attractiveness rating. This occurs because people selectively attend to the most attractive group members[332] and thus they get the most attention. Men selectively attend to attractive people more than women, it has been suggested that this could be because men are less invested in their offspring, so they are less choosy when it comes to sexual partners, and therefore they are more easily attracted. As a result of selective attention, people end up giving a group rating which is biased as the rating focuses mainly on the attractive members and ignores the less attractive members. So the overall attractiveness rating is skewed towards more attractive. The group attractiveness effect was investigated in a study which got participants to rate the physical attractiveness of a group of females as a whole, individually in a group, and individually on their own.[333] Participants were asked to give a rating of 1–7, with 1 being very unattractive and 7 being very attractive. The group attractiveness effect has been replicated with males as the main group and also with a group of both males and females. This effect may not be demonstrated across all cultures because the sample only contained Dutch university students.

Social effects edit

Perceptions of physical attractiveness contribute to generalized assumptions based on those attractions. Individuals assume that when someone is beautiful, then they have many other positive attributes that make the attractive person more likeable.[18] This is referred to as the halo effect, also known as the 'beautiful-is-good' effect.[18] Across cultures, what is beautiful is assumed to be good; attractive people are assumed to be more extroverted, popular, and happy. This could lead to a self-fulfilling prophecy, as, from a young age, attractive people receive more attention that helps them develop these characteristics.[334][335] In one study, beautiful people were found to be generally happier than less beautiful or plain people, perhaps because these outgoing personality traits are linked to happiness, or perhaps because beauty led to increased economic benefits which partially explained the increased happiness.[187] In another study testing first impressions in 56 female and 17 male participants at University of British Columbia, personality traits of physically attractive people were identified more positively and more accurately than those who were less physically attractive. It was explained that people pay closer attention to those they find physically beautiful or attractive, and thus perceiving attractive individuals with greater distinctive accuracy. The study believes this accuracy to be subjective to the eye of the beholder.[336] Recent results from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study confirmed the positive link between psychological well-being and attractiveness (higher facial attractiveness, lower BMI) and also found the complementary negative association with distress/depression. Even though connections and confounds with other variables could not be excluded, the effects of attractiveness in this study were the same size as the ones for other demographic variables.[337]

In developed western societies, women tend to be judged for their physical appearance over their other qualities and the pressure to engage in beauty work is much higher for women than men. Beauty work is defined as various beauty "practices individuals perform on themselves or others to elicit certain benefits from a specific social hierarchy."[338] Being "beautiful" has individual, social and institutional rewards.[338] Although marketers have started to target the "metro-sexual" male and produce hygiene and beauty products geared towards men, the expectations placed on them is less than women.[339] The time and money required for a man to achieve the same well-groomed appearance is much lower. Even in areas that men also face pressure to perform beauty work, such as haircuts/styling, the prices discrepancy for products and services are skewed. This phenomenon is called the "pink tax."[340][341]

However, attractiveness varies by society; in ancient China foot binding was practiced by confining young girls' feet in tightly bound shoes to prevent the feet from growing to normal size causing the women to have an attractive "lotus gait". In England, women used to wear corsets that severely constricted their breathing in order to achieve a visual effect of an exaggeratedly low waist-to-hip ratio.

People make judgments of physical attractiveness based on what they see, but also on what they know about the person. Specifically, perceptions of beauty are malleable such that information about the person's personality traits can influence one's assessment of another person's physical beauty. A 2007 study had participants first rate pictures for attractiveness. After doing distracting math problems, participants saw the pictures again, but with information about the person's personality. When participants learned that a person had positive personality characteristics (e.g., smart, funny, kind), that person was seen as more physically attractive.[342] Conversely, a person with negative personality characteristics (e.g., materialistic, rude, untrustworthy) was seen as less physically attractive. This was true for both females and males.

Physical attractiveness can have various social effects. For instance, humans tend to self-organize into couples where both parties have loosely similar attractiveness levels as judged by third parties.[343][344][345] A survey conducted by London Guildhall University of 11,000 people showed that those who subjectively describe themselves as physically attractive earn more income than others who would describe themselves as less attractive.[26] People who described themselves as less attractive earned, on average, 13% less than those who described themselves as more attractive, while the penalty for being overweight was around 5%. According to further research done on the correlation between looks and earnings in men, the punishment for unattractiveness is greater than the benefits of being attractive. However, in women the punishment is found to be equal to the benefits.[346] Another study suggests that more physically attractive people are significantly more likely on average to earn considerably higher wages. Differences in income due to attractiveness was much more pronounced for men rather than women, and held true for all ranges of income.[347] In the US the earnings disparities along the attractiveness continuum (net of controls) are similar or greater than the black-white disparity.[348] A study from 2020 found that social scientists who are judged as being more attractive receive higher public speaking fees than less attractive social scientists, whereas for natural scientists, relative unattractiveness is a comparative advantage in terms of public speaking fees.[349]

It is important to note that other factors such as self-confidence may explain or influence these findings as they are based on self-reported attractiveness as opposed to any sort of objective criteria; however, as one's self-confidence and self-esteem are largely learned from how one is regarded by their peers while maturing, even these considerations would suggest a significant role for physical appearance. One writer speculated that "the distress created in women by the spread of unattainable ideals of female beauty" might possibly be linked to increasing incidence of depression.[350]

Many have asserted that certain advantages tend to come to those who are perceived as being more attractive, including the ability to get better jobs and promotions; receiving better treatment from authorities and the legal system; having more choices in romantic or platonic partners and, therefore, more power in relationships; and marrying into families with more money.[30][187][334][335][351] Those who are attractive are treated and judged more positively than those who are considered unattractive, even by those who know them. Also, attractive individuals behave more positively than those who are unattractive.[352] One study found that teachers tend to expect that children who are attractive are more intelligent, and are more likely to progress further in school. They also consider these students to be more popular.[353] Voters choose political candidates who are more attractive over those who are less attractive.[354] Men and women use physical attractiveness as a measure of how "good" another person is.[355] In 1946, Soloman Asch coined the Implicit Personality Theory, meaning that the presence of one trait tends to imply the existence of other traits. This is also known as the halo effect. Research suggests that those who are physically attractive are thought to have more socially desirable personalities and lead better lives in general.[11] This is also known as the "what-is-beautiful-is-good effect" or physical attractiveness stereotype. Discrimination against or prejudice towards others based on their appearance is sometimes referred to as lookism (prejudice or discrimination based on physical appearance and especially physical appearance believed to fall short of societal notions of beauty).[356]

Some researchers conclude that little difference exists between men and women in terms of sexual behavior.[297][357] Other researchers disagree.[358] Symmetrically faced men and women have a tendency to begin to have sexual intercourse at an earlier age, to have more sexual partners, to engage in a wider variety of sexual activities, and to have more one-night stands. They are also prone to infidelity and are more likely to have open relationships.[46] Additionally, they have the most reproductive success. Therefore, their physical characteristics are most likely to be inherited by future generations.[359][360][361][362]

Concern for improving physical attractiveness has led many persons to consider alternatives such as cosmetic surgery. It has led scientists working with related disciplines such as computer imaging and mathematics to conduct research to suggest ways to surgically alter the distances between facial features in order to make a face conform more closely to the "agreed-upon standards of attractiveness" of an ideal face by using algorithms to suggest an alternative which still resembles the current face.[28] One research study found that cosmetic surgery as a way to "boost earnings" was "not profitable in a monetary sense."[187] Some research shows that physical attractiveness has a marginal effect on happiness.[363]

Misconceptions edit

The golden ratio edit

The golden ratio, also known as the golden proportion, was considered the perfect measurement of harmony, beauty and proportion in Ancient Greece. Researchers Mohammad Khursheed Alam, Nor Farid Mohd Noor, Rehana Basri, Tan Fo Yew and Tay Hui Wen conducted a study to test if the golden ratio was a contributor to perceptions of facial attractiveness in various ethnic groups. A total of 286 people aged 18 to 25 participated in the survey, including 100 Malay (50 male, 50 female), 100 Malaysian Chinese (50 male, 50 female), and 86 Malaysian Indian (36 male, 50 female). This study excluded subjects of mixed race, those with craniofacial deformities, and those who had previously received orthodontic treatment or had face surgery. The results showed that the golden ratio had no significant association with physical attractiveness.[364][365]

Ideal body shape for women edit

Some argue that body type preference is a mark of culture and regional beauty standards, and that there is no definitive "ideal body" for women, because it constantly shifts.[366][367][368] Some authors argue that body types have never been universal and that most evolutionary psychology studies on the "ideal female body" shape have been questioned or disproven due to external factors such as unreliable data and idealized western gender roles.[369][370][371][372] On the internet, communities can create their own niche beauty standards that differ from the cultural norm.[373][374][375] It has been argued that the "perfect body" is a matter of personal preference and exposure to regional media.[376][377]

See also edit

Notes edit

  1. ^ However, one expert suggested that her "almost matronly representation" was meant to convey an "impressive appearance" rather than "ideal female beauty".[4]
  2. ^ The "sitting body ratio" (SBR) is also quoted, where the trunk is measured with subject sitting on a flat table, and the leg-length determined by subtraction from standing height. This is almost the same as the perineum-to-floor distance but without the need to touch an intimate area.

References edit

  1. ^ . Time. May 28, 1945. Archived from the original on August 11, 2009. Retrieved August 5, 2011. ... "the most perfect all-over beauty of all time." Runner-up: the Venus de Milo.
  2. ^ "Says Venus de Milo was not a Flapper; Osteopath Says She Was Neurasthenic, as Her Stomach Was Not in Proper Place" (PDF). The New York Times. April 29, 1922. Retrieved August 5, 2011. Venus de Milo ... That lady of renowned beauty...
  3. ^ CBS News Staff (August 5, 2011). "Venus". CBS News. Retrieved August 5, 2011. The classical vision of beauty exemplified in Greek art, such as the 2nd century B.C. Venus de Milo (a.k.a. Aphrodite of Milos), was an ideal carried through millennia, laying the basis for much of Western art's depictions of the human form.
  4. ^ Kousser R (2005). "Creating the Past: The Vénus de Milo and the Hellenistic Reception of Classical Greece". American Journal of Archaeology. 109 (2): 227–50. doi:10.3764/aja.109.2.227. S2CID 36871977.
  5. ^ Wilkinson P (1998). Illustrated Dictionary of Mythology. DK Pub. ISBN 9780789434135.
  6. ^ Day J (2004). "Does the Old Testament Refer to Sacred Prostitution and Did It Actual Exist in Ancient Israel?". In McCarthy C, Healey JF (eds.). Biblical and Near Eastern Essays: Studies in Honour of Kevin J. Cathcart. Cromwell Press. pp. 2–21. ISBN 978-0-8264-6690-7.
  7. ^ Singh NK (1997). Divine Prostitution. New Delhi: APH Publishing. pp. 4–6. ISBN 978-81-7024-821-7.
  8. ^ Perkins D (2013). Encyclopedia of China: History and Culture. Routledge. p. 581. ISBN 978-1135935627.
  9. ^ Grammer K, Thornhill R (October 1994). "Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness and sexual selection: the role of symmetry and averageness". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 108 (3): 233–42. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.108.3.233. PMID 7924253. S2CID 1205083. Retrieved May 4, 2019.
  10. ^ Zeigler-Hill V, L M Welling L, Shackelford TK (2015). Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology. United States: Springer Science+Business Media. p. 329. ISBN 978-3-319-12697-5. ...why, despite broad agreement, we see a wide variety of personal preferences.
  11. ^ a b Dion, Berscheid & Walster (1972).
  12. ^ Simpson, Jeffry A (1990). "Perception of physical attractiveness: Mechanisms involved in the maintenance of romantic relationships". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 6 (59): 1192–1201. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.6.1192. Retrieved November 19, 2022.
  13. ^ a b Kanazawa S (2011). "Intelligence and physical attractiveness". Intelligence. 39 (1): 7–14. doi:10.1016/j.intell.2010.11.003.
  14. ^ Stephen ID, Tan KW (2015). "Healthy body, healthy face? Evolutionary approaches to health perception.". In Sheppard E, Haque S (eds.). Culture and Cognition: A Collection of Critical Essays. Peter Lang International Publishers.
  15. ^ Brown WM, Price ME, Kang J, Pound N, Zhao Y, Yu H (September 2008). "Fluctuating asymmetry and preferences for sex-typical bodily characteristics". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105 (35): 12938–43. Bibcode:2008PNAS..10512938B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0710420105. PMC 2529114. PMID 18711125.
  16. ^ Bulczak, Grzegorz; Gugushvili, Alexi (March 17, 2023). "Physical attractiveness and cardiometabolic risk". American Journal of Human Biology. 35 (8): e23895. doi:10.1002/ajhb.23895. ISSN 1042-0533. PMID 36932650. S2CID 257604055.
  17. ^ Stephen ID, Hiew V, Coetzee V, Tiddeman BP, Perrett DI (2017). "Facial Shape Analysis Identifies Valid Cues to Aspects of Physiological Health in Caucasian, Asian, and African Populations". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 1883. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01883. PMC 5670498. PMID 29163270.
  18. ^ a b c Barelds-Dijkstra & Barelds (2008).
  19. ^ a b Briscoe J (January 17, 2004). "Haven't I seen you somewhere before?". The Guardian. London. Retrieved July 15, 2011. Evolutionary psychologists claim there is an underlying standard script for beauty – a foundation for what we find appealing that transcends culture and ethnicity. There are various absolutes. For instance, to judge someone beautiful, the eye requires symmetry
  20. ^ Fisher, Maryanne L. (2017). The Oxford Handbook of Women and Competition. Oxford University Press. p. 603. ISBN 978-0-19-937639-1.
  21. ^ a b Nettle D (September 2002). "Women's height, reproductive success and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in modern humans" (PDF). Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 269 (1503): 1919–23. doi:10.1098/rspb.2002.2111. PMC 1691114. PMID 12350254.
  22. ^ a b c Glassenberg et al. (2010).
  23. ^ a b c Perrett et al. (1998).
  24. ^ Sell A, Lukazsweski AW, Townsley M (December 2017). "Cues of upper body strength account for most of the variance in men's bodily attractiveness". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 284 (1869): 20171819. doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.1819. PMC 5745404. PMID 29237852.
  25. ^ Mautz BS, Wong BB, Peters RA, Jennions MD (April 2013). "Penis size interacts with body shape and height to influence male attractiveness". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 110 (17): 6925–30. Bibcode:2013PNAS..110.6925M. doi:10.1073/pnas.1219361110. PMC 3637716. PMID 23569234.
  26. ^ a b Lorenz K (2005). "Do Pretty People Earn More?". www.CNN.com.
  27. ^ Dammann G (August 20, 2008). "Rules of attraction". The Guardian. London. Retrieved July 15, 2011. scientists from Brunel University have revealed that physical attraction is all down to bodily symmetry
  28. ^ a b c Kershaw (2008).
  29. ^ a b Berri DJ (September 16, 2008). "Do Pretty-Boy Quarterbacks Make More Money?". The New York Times. Retrieved July 15, 2011. Research, though, has indicated that what we think of as facial attractiveness is really just facial symmetry
  30. ^ a b Willett E (October 29, 2008). . The Leader-Post (Regina). Archived from the original on November 10, 2012. Retrieved July 15, 2011. "people preferentially mate with, date, associate with, employ, and even vote for physically attractive individuals." ... Symmetry is one trait we find attractive (but only if the face is right-side up: your symmetric face will, alas, do nothing to help you attract a mate if you constantly stand on your head.
  31. ^ a b Murphy C (December 4, 2003). "In the eye of the beholder?". BBC News. Retrieved July 15, 2011. Art historians, anthropologists and human psychologists in general agree that it is the symmetry of a face, its perfect proportion, or indeed its averageness – where no feature stands out – that has consistently down the ages been deemed attractive. ...
  32. ^ Burkeman O (April 24, 2010). "This column will change your life: The beauty in imperfection". The Guardian. London. Retrieved December 27, 2012. Absolute flawlessness, it's long been observed, is disturbing. It offers no point of connection, and may help explain the "uncanny valley" effect, where almost-lifelike robots trigger revulsion in humans. ...
  33. ^ a b McKeen S (February 10, 2006). . The Edmonton Journal. Archived from the original on November 10, 2012. Retrieved July 15, 2011. Evolution taught us to lust after symmetry – a nicely balanced body and face – because asymmetry signals past illness or injury. We therefore define beauty quite elegantly, right down to the most ideal ratio of hips to breasts and upper lip to lower lip. Singh says one study showed that people were able to gauge beauty at a subliminal level, when shown pictures for a mere one-hundredth of a second. Another study showed babies prefer pretty faces.
  34. ^ Perrin FA (June 1921). "Physical Attractiveness and Repulsiveness". Journal of Experimental Psychology. 4 (3): 203–17. doi:10.1037/h0071949.
  35. ^ Grammer K, Sainani KL (October 2015). "Q&A: Karl Grammer. Innate attractions". Nature. 526 (7572): S11. doi:10.1038/526S11a. PMID 26444367.
  36. ^ Brewis, Alexandra A. (2011). Obesity: Cultural and Biocultural Perspectives. New Brunswick, London: Rutgers University Press. p. 132.
  37. ^ Sforza, Chiarella; Laino, Alberto; D'Alessio, Raoul; Grandi, Gaia; Tartaglia, Gianluca Martino; Ferrario, Virgilio Ferruccio (2008). "Soft-Tissue Facial Characteristics of Attractive and Normal Adolescent Boys and Girls" (PDF). Angle Orthodontist. 78 (5): 799–807. doi:10.2319/091207-431.1. PMID 18298221.
  38. ^ Burriss, Robert P.; Rowland, Hannah M.; Little, Anthony C. (January 1, 2009). "Facial scarring enhances men's attractiveness for short-term relationships". Personality and Individual Differences. 46 (2): 213–17. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.09.029 – via www.sciencedirect.com.
  39. ^ a b Feng C (December 6, 2002). . Stanford University. Archived from the original on January 13, 2012. Retrieved January 20, 2012.
  40. ^ a b Penke L, Bates TC, Gow AJ, Pattie A, Starr JM, Jones BC, Perrett DI, Deary IJ (November 2009). "Symmetric faces are a sign of successful cognitive aging". Evolution and Human Behavior. 30 (6): 429–37. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.06.001.
    • "Face shape clue to mental decline". BBC News. August 9, 2009.
  41. ^ Radford T (August 17, 2005). "How women dream of symmetrical men". The Guardian. London. Retrieved January 19, 2010. The research once again confirms a hypothesis that beauty is not merely in the eye of the beholder: it is an indicator of genetic fitness. From a choice of computer-generated faces, volunteers routinely choose the most symmetrical as the most attractive. Physical symmetry is interpreted as a sign of good inheritance. And therefore, the theory goes, women in a position to conceive would be more attracted to someone more likely to engender the healthiest offspring.
  42. ^ Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, Comer R (1995). "Human female orgasm and mate fluctuating asymmetry". Animal Behaviour. 50 (6): 1601–15. doi:10.1016/0003-3472(95)80014-X. S2CID 44103857.
  43. ^ Little AC, Jones BC, Waitt C, Tiddeman BP, Feinberg DR, Perrett DI, Apicella CL, Marlowe FW (May 2008). Reimchen T (ed.). "Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces: data across culture and species". PLOS ONE. 3 (5): e2106. Bibcode:2008PLoSO...3.2106L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0002106. PMC 2329856. PMID 18461131.
  44. ^ Zebrowitz L, Rhodes G (2004). (PDF). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior. 28 (3): 167–85. doi:10.1023/B:JONB.0000039648.30935.1b. S2CID 15981563. Archived from the original (PDF) on April 6, 2019. Retrieved March 4, 2012.
  45. ^ a b Little AC, Burt DM, Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI (January 2001). "Self-perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 268 (1462): 39–44. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1327. PMC 1087598. PMID 12123296.
  46. ^ a b Etcoff N (2000). Survival of the Prettiest: The Science of Beauty. Knopf Doubleday Publishing. pp. 50–53, 185–87. ISBN 9780385479424.
  47. ^ Haselton MG, Gangestad SW (April 2006). "Conditional expression of women's desires and men's mate guarding across the ovulatory cycle". Hormones and Behavior. 49 (4): 509–18. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2005.10.006. PMID 16403409. S2CID 7065777.
  48. ^ Rikowski A, Grammer K (May 1999). "Human body odour, symmetry and attractiveness". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 266 (1422): 869–74. doi:10.1098/rspb.1999.0717. PMC 1689917. PMID 10380676.
  49. ^ a b Gangestad SW, Thornhill R (May 1998). "Menstrual cycle variation in women's preferences for the scent of symmetrical men". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 265 (1399): 927–33. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0380. PMC 1689051. PMID 9633114.
  50. ^ Zuniga A, Stevenson RJ, Mahmut MK, Stephen ID (January 2017). "Diet quality and the attractiveness of male body odor". Evolution and Human Behavior. 38 (1): 136–43. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.08.002.
  51. ^ . CNN. April 13, 2009. Archived from the original on March 9, 2012. Retrieved July 25, 2011. ... when women are ovulating, they produce copulins, a scent that attracts men....
  52. ^ a b "Google Science Fair semi-finalist: I can taste your DNA". The Guardian. London. July 25, 2011. Retrieved July 25, 2011. the Major Histocompatibility Complex (MHC) is a large gene family found in most vertebrates....
  53. ^ a b Khan R (August 16, 2008). "Taking the pill might make your brother hawt?". Discover Magazine. Retrieved July 25, 2011. Previous studies in animals and humans show that genes in the major histocompatibility complex (MHC) influence individual odours and that females often prefer odour of MHC-dissimilar males, perhaps to increase offspring heterozygosity or reduce inbreeding. Women using oral hormonal contraceptives have been reported to have the opposite preference, raising the possibility that oral contraceptives alter female preference towards MHC similarity, with possible fertility costs.
  54. ^ Roberts SC, Little AC, Gosling LM, Perrett DI, Carter V, Jones BC, Penton-Voak I, Petrie M (May 2005). "MHC-heterozygosity and human facial attractiveness". Evol Hum Behav. 26 (3): 213–26. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2004.09.002.
  55. ^ Penn DJ, Damjanovich K, Potts WK (August 2002). "MHC heterozygosity confers a selective advantage against multiple-strain infections". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 99 (17): 11260–64. Bibcode:2002PNAS...9911260P. doi:10.1073/pnas.162006499. PMC 123244. PMID 12177415.
  56. ^ Lewis MB (January 1, 2010). "Why are mixed-race people perceived as more attractive?". Perception. 39 (1): 136–38. doi:10.1068/p6626. PMID 20301855. S2CID 15582617.
  57. ^ "The Biracial Advantage". Psychology Today.
  58. ^ a b Rudder C (February 16, 2010). "The Case For An Older Woman". OkTrends Dating Research from OkCupid. Retrieved April 28, 2012.
  59. ^ Antfolk J, Salo B, Alanko K, Bergen E, Corander J, Sandnabba NK, Santtila P (2015). "Women's and men's sexual preferences and activities with respect to the partner's age: evidence for female choice". Evolution & Human Behavior. 36 (1): 73–79. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.09.003.
  60. ^ a b Williams CA (1999). Roman homosexuality: ideologies of masculinity in classical antiquity. USA: Oxford University Press.
  61. ^ Boyden T, Carroll JS, Maier RA (1984). "Similarity and Attraction in Homosexual Males: The Effects of Age and Masculinity-Femininity". Sex Roles. 10 (11/12): 939–48. doi:10.1007/BF00288516. S2CID 145447590.
  62. ^ Hechter M (2011). Social Norms. Russell Sage Foundation. p. 300.
  63. ^ Biello D (December 5, 2007). "What is the Best Age Difference for Husband and Wife?". Scientific American.
  64. ^ a b c d e Buss (2003), pp. 51–54.
  65. ^ a b Reitman V (April 26, 2004). "We clicked". Los Angeles Times. Retrieved April 3, 2012.
  66. ^ a b Thornhill R, Gangestad SW (December 1999). "Facial attractiveness". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 3 (12): 452–60. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01403-5. PMID 10562724. S2CID 961347.
  67. ^ Young JA, Critelli JW, Keith KW (2005). "Male age preferences for short-term and long-term mating". Sexualities, Evolution & Gender. 7 (2): 83–93. doi:10.1080/14616660500035090.
  68. ^ Quinsey, V.L. The Etiology of Anomalous Sexual Preferences in Men. Queen's University Department of Psychology.
  69. ^ a b "Attraction and Relationships – The Journey from Initial Attachments to Romantic Love: Rozenberg Quarterly". rozenbergquarterly.com. Retrieved March 3, 2016.
  70. ^ a b Barber N (1995). "The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness: Sexual selection and human morphology". Ethology and Sociobiology. 16 (5): 395–424. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(95)00068-2.
  71. ^ Van den Berghe PL, Frost P (1986). "Skin color preference, sexual dimorphism and sexual selection: a case of gene-culture co-evolution?". Ethnic and Racial Studies. 9: 87–118. doi:10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516.
  72. ^ a b c Swami et al. (2006).
  73. ^ a b Thomas M. M. Versluys; Robert A. Foley; William J. Skylark (May 16, 2018). "The influence of leg-to-body ratio, arm-to-body ratio and intra-limb ratio on male human attractiveness". Royal Society Open Science. The Royal Society Publishing. 5 (5): 171790. Bibcode:2018RSOS....571790V. doi:10.1098/rsos.171790. PMC 5990728. PMID 29892373.
  74. ^ a b Kiire, S (2016). "Effect of Leg-to-Body Ratio on Body Shape Attractiveness". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 45 (4): 901–10. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0635-9. PMID 26474977. S2CID 40574546.
  75. ^ Sorokowskia P, Pawlowskib B (March 2008). "Adaptive preferences for leg length in a potential partner". Evol Hum Behav. 29 (2): 86–91. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.09.002.
  76. ^ Sorokowski P (2010). "Attractiveness of Legs Length in Poland and Great Britain" (PDF). J Hum Ecol. 31 (3): 148.
  77. ^ Frederick DA, Hadji-Michael M, Furnham A, Swami V (January 2010). "The influence of leg-to-body ratio (LBR) on judgments of female physical attractiveness: assessments of computer-generated images varying in LBR". Body Image. 7 (1): 51–55. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.09.001. PMID 19822462.
  78. ^ a b c Bertamini & Bennet (2009), p. 235.
  79. ^ a b Bertamini & Bennet (2009), pp. 245, 246.
  80. ^ Drummond MJ, Filiault SM (2007). "The long and the short of it: Gay men's perceptions of penis size". Gay & Lesbian Issues and Psychology Review. 3 (2): 121–29.
  81. ^ Rettner R (June 5, 2014). "For One Night Stands, Girth Matters". LiveScience. Retrieved December 15, 2014.
  82. ^ Sanderson, S.K. (2001). The Evolution of Human Sociality: A Darwinian Conflict Perspective US: Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc. p. 179. ISBN 0-8476-9534-4 (alk. paper)
  83. ^ Spielmann MH (1889). The Magazine of Art. London, Paris, New York, Melbourne: Cassell and Company Limited.
  84. ^ Jones, Trina (2000). Shades of Brown: The Law of Skin Color. Duke Law School.
  85. ^ Stephen ID, Law Smith MJ, Stirrat MR, Perrett DI (December 2009). "Facial Skin Coloration Affects Perceived Health of Human Faces". International Journal of Primatology. 30 (6): 845–57. doi:10.1007/s10764-009-9380-z. PMC 2780675. PMID 19946602.
  86. ^ Stephen ID, Coetzee V, Law Smith M, Perrett DI (2009). "Skin blood perfusion and oxygenation colour affect perceived human health". PLOS ONE. 4 (4): e5083. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.5083S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005083. PMC 2659803. PMID 19337378.
  87. ^ Stephen I, Coetzee V, Perrett DI (2011). "Carotenoid and melanin pigment coloration affect perceived human health". Evolution and Human Behavior. 32 (3): 216–27. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.003.
  88. ^ Tybur JM, Gangestad SW (December 2011). "Mate preferences and infectious disease: theoretical considerations and evidence in humans". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 366 (1583): 3375–88. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0136. PMC 3189358. PMID 22042915.
  89. ^ Tybur & Gangestad 2011: "Indeed, at the current time, little direct evidence addresses whether skin tone or colour reflects current infection levels or infectability. More research is clearly needed.",
  90. ^ a b c d e Cunningham et al. (1995).
  91. ^ Van den Berghe PL, Frost P (September 13, 2010). "Skin color preference, sexual dimorphism and sexual selection: A case of gene culture co-evolution?". Ethnic and Racial Studies. 9: 87–113. doi:10.1080/01419870.1986.9993516. an overwhelming cross-cultural preference for lighter skin
  92. ^ Polovick, Madeline (2017). "Orange Is the New Black": The Skin-Tanning Phenomenon and Its Influence on Perceptions of Race, Class, and Gender (Thesis). Senior Independent Study Theses. Retrieved January 31, 2023.
  93. ^ Polovick (2017), p. 14: "The researchers discovered that their findings did not support Van den Berghe and Frost’s hypothesis that men were more attracted to lighter-skinned women, especially as linked to a sign of fertility, because their data showed a preference for women with darker skin (measured by color parameters that indicated a skin value based on blue and green in comparison to red components)."
  94. ^ Polovick (2017), p. 12-13: "They chose to use tanned women as their research model because women have higher rates of tanning than men (Robinson et al. 1997b), and therefore are more likely to utilize tanned skin as an attractive trait. Their hypothesis was that both women and men would find a tanned female to be more physically and interpersonally attractive, healthier, taller, and thinner than a less tanned female. Only the men’s responses were statistically significant, demonstrating an overall preference for the dark-tanned female over then medium-tanned. Banerjee, Campo, and Greene found that their results contradict previous research that has shown a preference for medium tanned skin over dark-tanned skin (Broadstock et al. 1992). They also found that the men viewed the darker-tanned women as thinner" ... "They reason that women might tan more often than men because they believe men that find them more attractive that way; which, these perceptions of women by men were proven most likely accurate with their study."
  95. ^ Hurkman, Alexis Van (November 15, 2010). The Color Correction Handbook: Professional Techniques for Video and Cinema. Pearson Education. p. 30-IA5. ISBN 978-0-321-71974-4. "Smith, Cornelissen and Tovee found that, contrary to previous cross-cultural studies on the topic, the observers showed a marked preference for the darker-complexioned, tanned volunteers."
  96. ^ Dixson BJ, Dixson AF, Bishop PJ, Parish A (June 2010). "Human physique and sexual attractiveness in men and women: a New Zealand-U.S. comparative study". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 39 (3): 804. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9441-y. PMID 19139985. S2CID 33112678. In the current study, men rated images with lighter skin tones as most attractive, especially so in the Californian sample. In New Zealand, men rated the female images of average skin color as most attractive, followed by the image which was lightened by 10 units of brightness and 15 units of contrast.
  97. ^ Bulmer, Martin; Solomos, John (October 2, 2017). Race, Migration and Identity. Routledge. p. 229. ISBN 978-1-317-51969-0. "For example, in a recent study examining Latina female college students who identified as white, pale skin was viewed as unattractive compared to tanned skin (Stephens and Fernandez, 2012). Furthermore, having the ideal, tan skin colour was associated with being more desirable in peer and dating contexts; more sexually appealing to men ... For example, in a study of African American male and female college students, Coard, Breland and Raskin found that participants preferred a medium skin tone over a lighter or darker skin tone."
  98. ^ Verma, N. M. P.; Srivastava, Alpana (August 20, 2020). The Routledge Handbook of Exclusion, Inequality and Stigma in India. Taylor & Francis. p. 293. ISBN 978-1-000-09669-9. "A few studies have found that tanned skin is regarded as both more attractive and healthier than pale or very dark skin, and there is a direct correlation between the degree of tanning and perceived attractiveness, especially among young women."
  99. ^ Kleisner K, Kočnar T, Tureček P, Stella D, Akoko RM, Třebický V, Havlíček J (2017). "African and European perception of African female attractiveness". Evolution and Human Behavior. 38 (6): 744–55. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.07.002. S2CID 85453582.
  100. ^ Singer M, Beyer H (2008). Killer Commodities: Public Health and the Corporate Production of Harm. AltaMira Press. p. 151. ISBN 978-0-7591-0979-7. Harris investigated the history of the parasol... everywhere ordinary people were forbidden to protect themselves with such devices "pallid skin became a marker of upper-class status". At the beginning of the 20th century, in the United States, lighter-skinned people avoided the sun... Tanned skin was considered lower class.
  101. ^ Geller AC, Colditz G, Oliveria S, Emmons K, Jorgensen C, Aweh GN, Frazier AL (June 2002). "Use of sunscreen, sunburning rates, and tanning bed use among more than 10 000 US children and adolescents". Pediatrics. 109 (6): 1009–14. doi:10.1542/peds.109.6.1009. PMID 12042536.
  102. ^ Broadstock M, Borland R, Gason R (January 1992). "Effects of Suntan on Judgements of Healthiness and Attractiveness by Adolescents". J Appl Soc Psychol. 22 (2): 157–72. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1992.tb01527.x.
  103. ^ Leary MR, Jones JL (September 1993). "The Social Psychology of Tanning and Sunscreen Use: Self-Presentational Motives as a Predictor of Health Risk". J Appl Soc Psychol. 23 (17): 1390–1406. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1993.tb01039.x.
  104. ^ "Tan is 'In': Study Finds Light Brown More Attractive than Pale or Dark Skin". physorg.com. Retrieved June 14, 2015.
  105. ^ "The Heavy Cost of Light Skin". BBC News. April 18, 2000. Retrieved August 9, 2010.
  106. ^ Fink B, Grammer K, Thornhill R (March 2001). "Human (Homo sapiens) facial attractiveness in relation to skin texture and color". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 115 (1): 92–99. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.1.92. PMID 11334223.
  107. ^ Fink B, Matts PJ (April 2008). "The effects of skin colour distribution and topography cues on the perception of female facial age and health". Journal of the European Academy of Dermatology and Venereology. 22 (4): 493–98. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3083.2007.02512.x. PMID 18081752. S2CID 12964209.
  108. ^ a b Kościński, Krzysztof (May 13, 2011). "Determinants of hand attractiveness – a study involving digitally manipulated stimuli". Perception. 40 (6): 682–94. doi:10.1068/p6960. PMID 21936297. S2CID 25042722 – via Sage Journals.
  109. ^ a b Saino, Nicola; Romano, Maria; Innocenti, Paolo (July 2006). "Length of Index and Ring Fingers Differentially Influence Sexual Attractiveness of Men's and Women's Hands". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 60 (3): 447–54. doi:10.1007/s00265-006-0185-1. JSTOR 25063833. S2CID 25043274.
  110. ^ Manning, J.T.; Scutt, D; Wilson, J; Lewis-Jones, D I (November 1, 1998). "The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length: a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone, luteinizing hormone and oestrogen". Human Reproduction. 13 (11): 3000–04. doi:10.1093/humrep/13.11.3000. PMID 9853845 – via Oxford Academic.
  111. ^ Kościński, Krzysztof (March–April 2012). "Hand attractiveness – its determinants and associations with facial attractiveness". Behavioral Ecology. 23 (2): 334–42. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr190 – via Oxford academic.
  112. ^ Pierce, C.A. 1996; Cunningham, M.R. 1990; Pawlowski B, Dunbar RI, Lipowicz A 2000.
  113. ^ a b Sear & Marlowe (2009).
  114. ^ Courtiol A, Raymond M, Godelle B, Ferdy JB (August 2010). "Mate choice and human stature: homogamy as a unified framework for understanding mating preferences". Evolution; International Journal of Organic Evolution. 64 (8): 2189–203. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.00985.x. PMID 20199563. S2CID 4780206.
  115. ^ a b "Tall men 'top husband stakes'". BBC News. August 14, 2002. Retrieved October 15, 2009.
  116. ^ Stulp G, Buunk A, Kurzban R (2013). "The height of choosiness: mutual mate choice for stature results in suboptimal pair formation for both sexes". Animal Behaviour. 86 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.038. hdl:11370/66f4623c-20b8-46a0-9542-04c78c7ec6b5. S2CID 53171152.
  117. ^ Buss (2003), pp. 38–40.
  118. ^ a b Yee, N. (2002). Beyond Tops and Bottoms Correlations between Sex-Role Preference and Physical Preferences for Partners among Gay Men
  119. ^ Curry (1916), pp. 105, 107.
  120. ^ a b c d e Sear (2006).
  121. ^ Berger, John (1973). Ways of Seeing. British Broadcasting Corporation and Penguin Books. pp. 44–64.
  122. ^ Curry (1916), p. 101.
  123. ^ "Do I have what it takes to be a model?". British Fashion Model Agents Association.
  124. ^ Pazhoohi, Farid; Macedo, J, Antonio F.; Doyle, ames F.; Arantes, Joana (2020). "Waist‑to‑Hip Ratio as Supernormal Stimuli: Effect of Contrapposto Pose and Viewing Angle". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 49 (3): 837–847. doi:10.1007/s10508-019-01486-z. PMID 31214904. S2CID 195066235. Retrieved May 31, 2022.
  125. ^ Jacobs O, Pazhoohi F, Kingstone A (2023). "Contrapposto posture captures visual attention: An online gaze tracking experiment". Visual Cognition. 31 (2): 160–167. doi:10.1080/13506285.2023.2213904. S2CID 258852464.
  126. ^ Pazhoohi F, Arantes J, Kingstone A, Pinal D (2020). "Contrapposto pose increases attractiveness ratings and modulates observers' brain activity". Biological Psychology. 151: 107842. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107842. PMID 31958547. S2CID 210678442.
  127. ^ a b c Sugiyama (2005).
  128. ^ Johnson KL, Tassinary LG (March 2007). "Compatibility of basic social perceptions determines perceived attractiveness". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 104 (12): 5246–51. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.5246J. doi:10.1073/pnas.0608181104. PMC 1829294. PMID 17360395.
    • "Clues To Mysteries Of Physical Attractiveness Revealed". ScienceDaily (Press release). May 24, 2007.
  129. ^ Buss, D. M., & Barnes, M. (1986). Preferences in human mate selection. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(3), 559.
  130. ^ Frederick, D. A., & Haselton, M. G. (2007). Why is muscularity sexy? Tests of the fitness indicator hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin.
  131. ^ Bailey & Gaulin & Agyei & Gladue, J.M. & S & Y & B.A (1994). "Effects of gender and sexual orientation on evolutionary relevant aspects of human mating psychology". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 66 (6): 1081–93. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.6.1081. PMID 8046578.
  132. ^ Penton-Voak IS, Perrett DI (January 2000). "Female preference for male faces changes cyclically: Further evidence". Evol Hum Behav. 21 (1): 39–48. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00033-1.
  133. ^ Rhodes G (2006). "The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty". Annual Review of Psychology. 57 (1): 199–226. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208. PMID 16318594.
  134. ^ "Women's choice of men goes in cycles". BBC News. June 24, 1999. Retrieved November 4, 2016.
  135. ^ Dawkins R (1999). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press. ISBN 978-0-19-286092-7.
  136. ^ Kruger DJ, Fisher M, Jobling I (September 2003). "Proper and dark heroes as DADS and CADS: Alternative mating strategies in British Romantic literature". Human Nature. 14 (3): 305–17. doi:10.1007/s12110-003-1008-y. PMID 26190212. S2CID 45846130.
  137. ^ Wang H, Hahn AC, Fisher CI, DeBruine LM, Jones BC (December 2014). "Women's hormone levels modulate the motivational salience of facial attractiveness and sexual dimorphism" (PDF). Psychoneuroendocrinology. 50: 246–51. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.08.022. PMID 25244638. S2CID 1075853.
  138. ^ Fink B, Neave N, Seydel H (2007). "Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women". American Journal of Human Biology. 19 (1): 82–87. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20583. PMID 17160983. S2CID 31618973.
  139. ^ Rhodes G, Chan J, Zebrowitz LA, Simmons LW (August 2003). "Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health?". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 270 (Suppl 1): S93–95. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2003.0023. PMC 1698019. PMID 12952647.
  140. ^ Cellerino A (2003). "Psychobiology of facial attractiveness". Journal of Endocrinological Investigation. 26 (3 Suppl): 45–48. PMID 12834020.
  141. ^ Gangestad SW, Thornhill R (July 2003). "Facial masculinity and fluctuating asymmetry". Evol Hum Behav. 24 (4): 231–41. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(03)00017-5.
  142. ^ Scott IM, Pound N, Stephen ID, Clark AP, Penton-Voak IS (October 2010). "Does masculinity matter? The contribution of masculine face shape to male attractiveness in humans". PLOS ONE. 5 (10): e13585. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...513585S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0013585. PMC 2965103. PMID 21048972.
  143. ^ DeBruine LM, Jones BC, Crawford JR, Welling LL, Little AC (August 2010). "The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences: cross-cultural variation in women's preferences for masculinized male faces". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 277 (1692): 2405–10. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.2184. PMC 2894896. PMID 20236978.
  144. ^ Wen, Yi Feng; Wong, Hai Ming; Lin, Ruitao; Yin, Guosheng; McGrath, Colman (August 6, 2015). "Inter-Ethnic/Racial Facial Variations: A Systematic Review and Bayesian Meta-Analysis of Photogrammetric Studies". PLOS ONE. 10 (8): e0134525. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1034525W. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0134525. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 4527668. PMID 26247212.
  145. ^ Lee AJ, Mitchem DG, Wright MJ, Martin NG, Keller MC, Zietsch BP (February 2014). "Genetic factors that increase male facial masculinity decrease facial attractiveness of female relatives". Psychological Science. 25 (2): 476–84. doi:10.1177/0956797613510724. PMC 4205959. PMID 24379153.
  146. ^ Zheng L, Hart TA, Zheng Y (October 2013). "Attraction to male facial masculinity in gay men in China: relationship to intercourse preference positions and sociosexual behavior". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 42 (7): 1223–32. doi:10.1007/s10508-012-0057-x. PMID 23440561. S2CID 207090415.
  147. ^ a b Song G, Hird D (2013). Men and Masculinities in Contemporary China. Brill Publishers. p. 92. ISBN 978-90-04-26491-5.
  148. ^ Song G (2004). The Fragile Scholar: Power and Masculinity in Chinese Culture. Hong Kong University Press. p. 126. ISBN 978-962-209-620-2.
  149. ^ Curry (1916), p. 75.
  150. ^ Horvath T (February 1981). "Physical attractiveness: the influence of selected torso parameters". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 10 (1): 21–24. doi:10.1007/BF01542671. PMID 7212994. S2CID 7439609.
  151. ^ a b Braun MF, Bryan A (2006). "Female waist-to-hip and male waist-to-shoulder ratios as determinants of romantic partner desirability". Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 23 (5): 805–19. doi:10.1177/0265407506068264. S2CID 2940276.
  152. ^ Swami V, Tovée MJ (2008). "The Muscular Male: A Comparison of the Physical Attractiveness Preferences of Gay and Heterosexual Men". International Journal of Men's Health. 7 (1): 59–71. doi:10.3149/jmh.0701.59.
  153. ^ Fan J, Dai W, Liu F, Wu J (February 2005). "Visual perception of male body attractiveness". Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 272 (1560): 219–26. doi:10.1098/rspb.2004.2922. PMC 1634963. PMID 15705545.
  154. ^ Buss, David (October 2, 2015). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind. Psychology Press. p. 168. ISBN 978-1-317-34573-2.
  155. ^ a b Catikkas F (2011). "Physical correlates of college students' body image satisfaction levels". Social Behavior and Personality. 39 (4): 497–502. doi:10.2224/sbp.2011.39.4.497.
  156. ^ Furnham A, Tan T, McManus C (1997). (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 22 (4): 539–49. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(96)00241-3. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 28, 2016.
  157. ^ Swami V, Tovee MJ (December 2005). "Male physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia: A cross-cultural study". Body Image. 2 (4): 383–93. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.08.001. PMID 18089203.
  158. ^ Singh D (1995). "Female judgment of male attractiveness and desirability for relationships: Role of waist-to-hip ratio and financial status". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69 (6): 1089–1101. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1089. PMID 8531056.
  159. ^ Dececco J, Wright L (2013). The Bear Book: Readings in the History and Evolution of a Gay Male Subculture. New York: Routledge. p. 120. ISBN 978-1-56023-890-4.
  160. ^ Curry (1916), p. 116.
  161. ^ a b Frederick DA, Fessler DM, Haselton MG (March 2005). "Do representations of male muscularity differ in men's and women's magazines?". Body Image. 2 (1): 81–86. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.12.002. PMID 18089177. S2CID 11139349.
  162. ^ a b Johnston JR (2001). The American body in context: An anthology. US: Scholarly Resources, Inc.
  163. ^ a b Brierley ME, Brooks KR, Mond J, Stevenson RJ, Stephen ID (June 3, 2016). "The Body and the Beautiful: Health, Attractiveness and Body Composition in Men's and Women's Bodies". PLOS ONE. 11 (6): e0156722. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1156722B. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0156722. PMC 4892674. PMID 27257677.
  164. ^ Latinsky A (2012). "Public presentation of gendered bodies: A look at gay and lesbian online dating profiles". Sociation Today. 10 (2).
  165. ^ Curry (1916), pp. 112, 115, 118, 123.
  166. ^ Dixson BJ, Dixson AF, Bishop PJ, Parish A (June 2010). "Human physique and sexual attractiveness in men and women: a New Zealand-U.S. comparative study". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 39 (3): 798–806. doi:10.1007/s10508-008-9441-y. PMID 19139985. S2CID 33112678.
  167. ^ Dixson BJ, Dixson AF, Li B, Anderson MJ (2007). "Studies of human physique and sexual attractiveness: sexual preferences of men and women in China". American Journal of Human Biology. 19 (1): 88–95. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20584. PMID 17160976. S2CID 8868828.
  168. ^ a b Dixson AF, Halliwell G, East R, Wignarajah P, Anderson MJ (February 2003). "Masculine somatotype and hirsuteness as determinants of sexual attractiveness to women". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 32 (1): 29–39. doi:10.1023/A:1021889228469. PMID 12597270. S2CID 10169455.
  169. ^ Rantala MJ, Pölkki M, Rantala LM (2010). "Preference for human male body hair changes across the menstrual cycle and menopause". Behavioral Ecology. 21 (2): 419–23. doi:10.1093/beheco/arp206.
  170. ^ J. W. Dixson, Barnaby; J. Rantala, Markus (2016). "The Role of Facial and Body Hair Distribution in Women's Judgments of Men's Sexual Attractiveness". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 45 (4): 877–89. doi:10.1007/s10508-015-0588-z. PMID 26292838. S2CID 19988758 – via Springer link.
  171. ^ J.Dixon, Barnaby; L. Vasey, Paul (January 13, 2012). "Beards augment perceptions of men's age, social status, and aggressiveness, but not attractiveness". Behavioral Ecology. 23 (3): 481–90. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr214. hdl:10.1093/beheco/arr214 – via Oxford academic.
  172. ^ Holton, N E; Bonner, L L; Scott, J E; Marshall, S D; Franciscus, R G; Southard, T E (June 2015). "The ontogeny of the chin: an analysis of allometric and biomechanical scaling". Journal of Anatomy. 226 (6): 549–559. doi:10.1111/joa.12307. ISSN 0021-8782. PMC 4450959. PMID 25865897.
  173. ^ Mommaerts, Maurice Y. (2016). "The ideal male jaw angle – An Internet survey". Journal of Cranio-Maxillofacial Surgery. 44 (4): 381–391. doi:10.1016/j.jcms.2015.12.012. ISSN 1010-5182. PMID 26888465.
  174. ^ Wen, Y. F.; Wong, H. M.; Lin, R.; Yin, G.; McGrath, C. (2015). "Inter-Ethnic/Racial Facial Variations: A Systematic Review and Bayesian Meta-Analysis of Photogrammetric Studies". PLOS ONE. 10 (8): e0134525. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1034525W. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0134525. PMC 4527668. PMID 26247212.
  175. ^ Rhee, S. C. (2018). "Differences between Caucasian and Asian attractive faces". Skin Research and Technology. 24 (1): 73–79. doi:10.1111/srt.12392. PMID 28722243. S2CID 24279633.
  176. ^ Naini, Farhad B.; Cobourne, Martyn T.; McDonald, Fraser; Wertheim, David (December 2016). "Submental-Cervical Angle: Perceived Attractiveness and Threshold Values of Desire for Surgery". Journal of Maxillofacial and Oral Surgery. 15 (4): 469–477. doi:10.1007/s12663-015-0872-4. ISSN 0972-8279. PMC 5083688. PMID 27833339.
  177. ^ Browne KR (2006). "Sex, Power, and Dominance: The Evolutionary Psychology of Sexual Harassment". Managerial and Decision Economics. 27 (2–3): 145–58. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.484.5566. doi:10.1002/mde.1289.
  178. ^ a b Tattersall I (June 11, 2000). "Whatever Turns You On: A psychologist looks at sexual attraction and what it means for humankind. Geoffrey Miller". The New York Times: Book Review. Retrieved July 15, 2011. it turns out that symmetry of bodily structure is a fitness indicator, and symmetry is more easily detectable among large breasts than small ones.
  179. ^ Jackson LB (1992). Physical appearance and gender: sociobiological and sociocultural perspectives. State University of New York Press.
  180. ^ Miller L (2006). Beauty Up: Exploring Contemporary Japanese Body Aesthetics. Berkeley, California: University of California Press. p. 28. ISBN 978-0-520-24508-2.
  181. ^ "A Woman's Face is Her Fortune (advertisement)". The Helena Independent. November 9, 2000. p. 7.
  182. ^ Little, Becky (September 22, 2016). . National Geographic. Archived from the original on November 5, 2018.
  183. ^ a b Berscheid and Reis, 1998
  184. ^ Fink B, Penton-Voak IS (2002). "Evolutionary Psychology of Facial Attractiveness". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 11 (5): 154–58. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00190. S2CID 10217832.
  185. ^ Brizendine, Louann (2006). The female brain. Random House Digital, Inc. p. 63. ISBN 978-0-7679-2010-0.
  186. ^ Van Meter J (August 11, 2008). "About-Face". NY Mag. Retrieved July 30, 2012.
  187. ^ a b c d Jayson (2011).
  188. ^ "How Big Is Your Limbal Ring?". Psychology Today. Retrieved June 14, 2015.
  189. ^ a b c d Milani (1992), p. 187.
  190. ^ a b c d e f Lane (1883), pp. 214–216.
  191. ^ a b c Brayer, M.M. (1986). The Jewish Woman in Rabbinic Literature: A psychological perspective. Hoboken, New Jersey: Ktav Publishing House, Inc., ISBN 0-88125-071-6, p. 214.
  192. ^ a b c Kyo (2012), p. 6.
  193. ^ Jones D, Brace CL, Jankowiak W, Laland KN, Musselman LE, Langlois JH, Roggman LA, Pérusse D, Schweder B, Symons D (December 1995). "Sexual selection, physical attractiveness, and facial neoteny: cross-cultural evidence and implications". Current Anthropology. 36 (5): 723–48. doi:10.1086/204427. S2CID 52840802.
  194. ^ Sforza C, Laino A, D'Alessio R, Grandi G, Binelli M, Ferrario VF (January 2009). "Soft-tissue facial characteristics of attractive Italian women as compared to normal women". The Angle Orthodontist. 79 (1): 17–23. doi:10.2319/122707-605.1. PMID 19123721.
  195. ^ Marcinkowska UM, Kozlov MV, Cai H, Contreras-Garduño J, Dixson BJ, Oana GA, et al. (April 2014). "Cross-cultural variation in men's preference for sexual dimorphism in women's faces". Biology Letters. 10 (4): 20130850. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2013.0850. PMC 4013689. PMID 24789138.
  196. ^ Cunningham et al. (1995), p. 267: "Asian, Hispanic, and White participants were similar in giving higher ratings to targets with the neonate features of large eyes, greater distance between the eyes, and a smaller nose area (see Table 2). Women with the maturity qualities of higher cheek-bones and narrower faces and low levels of the male maturity cues of large chins and thick eyebrows were judged more positively."
    : See table 1, Study 1. "All groups of judges made more positive ratings of the Asian and Hispanic targets compared with the Black and White targets. Further analyses indicated that the Asian and Hispanic targets happened to possess significantly larger eye height, eye width, nose width, eyebrow height, smile width, and upper lip width than the White and Black women. Because the targets were chosen for their availability rather than randomly selected from their populations, and the absolute number of targets in each group was small, it would be incorrect to conclude that any ethnic group was more attractive than any other."
  197. ^ Cunningham et al. (1995), p. 271"The four-item measure of exposure to Western culture was not reliably associated with giving higher ratings to Whites (r = . 19, n s). The relation of rating Whites to frequency of viewing Western television, for example, was quite low(r=.01)."
  198. ^ Cunningham MR (May 1986). "Measuring the Physical in Physical Attractiveness: Quasi-Experiments on the Sociobiology of Female Facial Beauty". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 50 (5): 925–35. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.925. Research with Western subjects disclosed significant consistency in evaluating attractiveness (Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986; Iliife, 1960). The females judged to be most attractive may have such similar facial features that they were hard to distinguish one from another (Light, Hollander, & Kayra-Stuart, 1981). Cross-cultural investigations on the judgment of facial attractiveness tended to highlight societal differences, but rough agreements in facial aesthetic preferences were shown by Asian-American and white females (Wagatsuma & Kleinke, 1979), Chinese, Indian, and English females judging Greek males (Thakerar & Iwawaki, 1979), South African and American males and females (Morse, Gruzen, & Reis, 1976), and blacks and whites judging males and females from both races (Cross & Cross, 1971).
  199. ^ Buss (2003), pp. 54–55.
  200. ^ Berger M (1999). White lies: race and the myths of whiteness. Canada: Farrar, Strous and Giroux. ISBN 978-0-374-52715-0.
  201. ^ Liu J (2008). "Sexualized Labour? 'White-collar Beauties' in Provincial China.". In Jackson S, Liu J, Woo J (eds.). East Asian Sexualities: Modernity, Gender and New Sexual Cultures. London: Zed Books. pp. 85–103. ISBN 978-0-374-52715-0.
  202. ^ Hönekopp, J., Bartholomé, T., & Jansen, G. (2004). Facial Attractiveness, Symmetry, and Physical Fitness in Young Women. Human Nature, 15(2), 147–167.
  203. ^ "Perfect face dimensions measured". BBC News. December 18, 2009. Retrieved May 22, 2010.
  204. ^ Wilkins CL, Chan JF, Kaiser CR (October 2011). "Racial stereotypes and interracial attraction: phenotypic prototypicality and perceived attractiveness of Asians". Cultural Diversity & Ethnic Minority Psychology. 17 (4): 427–31. doi:10.1037/a0024733. PMID 21988581. see bottom-left of p. 43
  205. ^ a b Wilkes J, ed. (1823). Encyclopaedia Londinensis, or Universal Dictionary of Arts, Science and Literature. Vol. 19. John Wilkes. p. 173.
  206. ^ Howard N (1830). On Persian Poetry. Plymouth. p. 30.
  207. ^ Kyo (2012).
  208. ^ Buckley, Sandra (January 1, 2009). The Encyclopedia of Contemporary Japanese Culture. Taylor & Francis. ISBN 9780415481526.
  209. ^ Bonds, Alexandra B. (January 1, 2008). Beijing Opera Costumes: The Visual Communication of Character and Culture. University of Hawaii Press. ISBN 9780824829568.
  210. ^ Soh J, Chew MT, Wong HB (February 2007). "An Asian community's perspective on facial profile attractiveness". Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology. 35 (1): 18–24. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0528.2007.00304.x. PMID 17244134.
  211. ^ "Culture of Iran: Cosmetics, Styles & Beauty Concepts in Iran". www.iranchamber.com. Retrieved March 28, 2016.
  212. ^ Hwang HS, Spiegel JH (March 2014). "The effect of "single" vs "double" eyelids on the perceived attractiveness of Chinese women". Aesthetic Surgery Journal. 34 (3): 374–82. doi:10.1177/1090820X14523020. PMID 24604790.
  213. ^ Gill, R.D. (2004). Topsy-turvy 1585: A translation and explication of Luis Frois S.J.'s Tratado (treatise) listing 611 ways Europeans & Japanese are contrary. Paraverse Press. pp. 57–58. ISBN 0-9742618-1-5, ISBN 978-0-9742618-1-2
  214. ^ Elia IE (September 2013). "A foxy view of human beauty: implications of the farm fox experiment for understanding the origins of structural and experiential aspects of facial attractiveness". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 88 (3): 163–83. doi:10.1086/671486. PMID 24053070. S2CID 22293068.
  215. ^ a b c Kyo (2012), p. 15.
  216. ^ a b c Kyo (2012), p. 19.
  217. ^ Kyo (2012), p. 18.
  218. ^ Pazhoohi, F; Garza, R; Kingstone, A (2020). "Effects of Breast Size, Intermammary Cleft Distance (Cleavage) and Ptosis on Perceived Attractiveness, Health, Fertility and Age: Do Life History, Self-Perceived Mate Value and Sexism Attitude Play a Role?". Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology. 6: 75–92. doi:10.1007/s40750-020-00129-1. S2CID 256429772.
  219. ^ Buss (2005), p. 325.
  220. ^ a b Furnham A, Swami V (2007). "Perception of female buttocks and breast size in profile". Soc Behav Pers. 35 (1): 1–8. doi:10.2224/sbp.2007.35.1.1.
  221. ^ "Hourglass figure fertility link". bbc.co.uk. May 4, 2004. Retrieved June 14, 2015.
  222. ^ Groyecka A, Żelaźniewicz A, Misiak M, Karwowski M, Sorokowski P (July 2017). "Breast shape (ptosis) as a marker of a woman's breast attractiveness and age: Evidence from Poland and Papua". American Journal of Human Biology. 29 (4): e22981. doi:10.1002/ajhb.22981. PMID 28211217. S2CID 4576540.
  223. ^ Rodgers JE (2003). Sex: A Natural History. Macmillan. p. 102. ISBN 978-0-8050-7281-5.
  224. ^ Wade, T. J. (2010). "The Relationships between Symmetry and Attractiveness and Mating Relevant Decisions and Behavior: A Review". Symmetry. 2 (2): 1081–1098. Bibcode:2010Symm....2.1081W. doi:10.3390/sym2021081.
  225. ^ Pal, P. (1986). Indian Sculpture – Volume 1 – circa 500 B.C – A.D. 700. A catalogue of the Los Angeles County Museum of Art Collection. Berkeley, Los Angeles & London: University of California Press. p. 32. ISBN 9780520059917
  226. ^ Curry (1916), p. 113.
  227. ^ Fisher HE (1982). The Sex Contract: The Evolution of Human Behavior. New York: William Morrow & Company, Inc. ISBN 9780688006402.
  228. ^ Hawkins E (October 25, 2017). "Why arched backs are attractive". springer.com.
  229. ^ Pazhoohi F, Doyle JF, Macedo AF, Arantes J (2017). "Arching the Back (Lumbar Curvature) as a Female Sexual Proceptivity Signal: an Eye-Tracking Study". Evolutionary Psychological Science. 4 (2): 1–8. doi:10.1007/s40806-017-0123-7. S2CID 149046079.
  230. ^ a b Caro TM, Sellen DW (1990). "The Reproductive Advantages of Fat in Women" (PDF). Ethology and Sociobiology. 11 (5): 1–66. doi:10.1016/0162-3095(90)90005-Q. hdl:2027.42/28785.
  231. ^ Hebl MR, Heatherton TF (1997). "The Stigma of Obesity in Women: The Difference is Black and White". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 24 (4): 418. doi:10.1177/0146167298244008. S2CID 143697452.
  232. ^ Swami V, Jones J, Einon D, Furnham A (May 2009). "Men's preferences for women's profile waist-to-hip ratio, breast size, and ethnic group in Britain and South Africa". British Journal of Psychology. 100 (Pt 2): 313–25. doi:10.1348/000712608x329525. PMID 18625082.
  233. ^ Tovée MJ, Reinhardt S, Emery JL, Cornelissen PL (August 1998). "Optimum body-mass index and maximum sexual attractiveness". Lancet. 352 (9127): 548. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)79257-6. PMID 9716069. S2CID 35886360.
  234. ^ a b c Buss (2003), pp. 55, 56.
  235. ^ Furnham, Adrian, Gianna Caroline Fischer, Lauren Tanner, Melanie Dias, and Alastair McClelland 1998.
  236. ^ Brown PJ, Sweeney J (2009). "The Anthropology of Overweight, Obesity and the Body". AnthroNotes. 30 (1).
  237. ^ Nettle D (November 2009). "Ecological influences on human behavioural diversity: a review of recent findings". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 24 (11): 618–24. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2009.05.013. PMID 19683831.
  238. ^ Germov, John; Williams, Lauren (1999). Weighty Issues: Fatness and Thinness As Social Problems. Social Problems and Social Issues. Transaction Publishers. p. 125-127. ISBN 978-1-4128-4126-9.
  239. ^ Anderson, L. (2017). Deviance: Social Constructions and Blurred Boundaries. University of California Press. p. 390. ISBN 978-0-520-29237-6.
  240. ^ Hellmich N (September 26, 2006). "Do thin models warp girls' body image?". USA Today. Retrieved November 6, 2009. The widespread concern that model thinness has progressed from willowy to wasted has reached a threshold as evidenced by the recent actions of fashion show organizers.
  241. ^ Tierney J (January 18, 2007). . The New York Times. Archived from the original on January 21, 2007. Retrieved November 6, 2009. women's aesthetic judgments are so influenced by other women. Men prefer the wider hips, and most likely could [sic] care less about high heels and handbags. Yet for many women all these things are essential to marking their beauty status with other women (unpublished OpEd)
  242. ^ Witcomb GL, Arcelus J, Chen J (December 2013). "Can cognitive dissonance methods developed in the West for combatting the 'thin ideal' help slow the rapidly increasing prevalence of eating disorders in non-Western cultures?". Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry. 25 (6): 332–40. doi:10.3969/j.issn.1002-0829.2013.06.002. PMC 4054580. PMID 24991176.
  243. ^ a b Silver AK (2004). Victorian Literature and the Anorexic Body. UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 38, 48. ISBN 978-0-511-03051-2.
  244. ^ Curry (1916), p. 102.
  245. ^ Kilpela, Lisa Smith; Becker, Carolyn Black; Wesley, Nicole; Stewart, Tiffany (July 1, 2015). "Body Image in Adult Women: Moving Beyond the Younger Years". Advances in Eating Disorders (Abingdon, England ). 3 (2): 144–164. doi:10.1080/21662630.2015.1012728. ISSN 2166-2630. PMC 4452130. PMID 26052476.
  246. ^ Bjorn C (February 13, 2006). "The Rules of Attraction in the Game of Love". Live Science (Press release). Retrieved January 9, 2006.
  247. ^ Singh D (December 2002). "Female mate value at a glance: relationship of waist-to-hip ratio to health, fecundity and attractiveness" (PDF). Neuro Endocrinology Letters. 23. 23 (Suppl 4): 81–91. PMID 12496738.
  248. ^ Buss (2003), p. 56.
  249. ^ Wetsmana A, Marloweb F (July 1999). "How Universal Are Preferences for Female Waist-to-Hip Ratios? Evidence from the Hadza of Tanzania". Evolution and Human Behavior. 20 (4): 219–228. doi:10.1016/S1090-5138(99)00007-0.
  250. ^ Horvath T (1979). "Correlates of physical beauty in men and women". Social Behavior and Personality. 7 (2): 145–151. doi:10.2224/sbp.1979.7.2.145.
  251. ^ Fisher ML, Voracek M (June 2006). "The shape of beauty: determinants of female physical attractiveness". Journal of Cosmetic Dermatology. 5 (2): 190–4. doi:10.1111/j.1473-2165.2006.00249.x. PMID 17173598. S2CID 25660426.
  252. ^ Dixson BJ, Dixson AF, Li B, Anderson MJ (January 2007). "Studies of human physique and sexual attractiveness: sexual preferences of men and women in China". American Journal of Human Biology. 19 (1): 88–95. doi:10.1002/ajhb.20584. PMID 17160976. S2CID 8868828.
  253. ^ Marlowe F, Wetsman A (2001). (PDF). Personality and Individual Differences. 30 (3): 481–489. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.489.4169. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00039-8. Archived from the original (PDF) on June 30, 2014. Retrieved August 4, 2007.
  254. ^ Marlowe F, Apicella C, Reed D (November 2005). "Men's preferences for women's profile waist-to-hip ratio in two societies". Evol Hum Behav. 26 (6): 458–468. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.07.005. as PDF June 1, 2014, at the Wayback Machine
  255. ^ Dixson BJ, Dixson AF, Morgan B, Anderson MJ (June 2007). "Human physique and sexual attractiveness: sexual preferences of men and women in Bakossiland, Cameroon". Archives of Sexual Behavior. 36 (3): 369–75. doi:10.1007/s10508-006-9093-8. PMID 17136587. S2CID 40115821.
  256. ^ Freedman RE, Carter MM, Sbrocco T, Gray JJ (August 2007). "Do men hold African-American and Caucasian women to different standards of beauty?". Eating Behaviors. 8 (3): 319–33. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2006.11.008. PMC 3033406. PMID 17606

physical, attractiveness, degree, which, person, physical, features, considered, aesthetically, pleasing, beautiful, term, often, implies, sexual, attractiveness, desirability, also, distinct, from, either, there, many, factors, which, influence, person, attra. Physical attractiveness is the degree to which a person s physical features are considered aesthetically pleasing or beautiful The term often implies sexual attractiveness or desirability but can also be distinct from either There are many factors which influence one person s attraction to another with physical aspects being one of them Physical attraction itself includes universal perceptions common to all human cultures such as facial symmetry 9 sociocultural dependent attributes and personal preferences unique to a particular individual 10 Venus de Milo at the Louvre has been described as a classical vision of beauty 1 2 3 a Michelangelo s David is considered a symbol of young male beauty and strength Ishtar Mesopotamian goddess of sexual love and war The goddess has been associated with sexuality love and fertility 5 6 7 Xi Shi 西施 born 506 BC was one of the Four Great Beauties of ancient China 8 In many cases humans subconsciously attribute positive characteristics such as intelligence and honesty to physically attractive people a psychological phenomenon called the Halo effect 11 From research done in the United States and United Kingdom it was found that objective 12 measures of physical attractiveness and intelligence are positively correlated and that the association between the two attributes is stronger among men than among women 13 Evolutionary psychologists have tried to answer why individuals who are more physically attractive should also on average be more intelligent and have put forward the notion that both general intelligence and physical attractiveness may be indicators of underlying genetic fitness 13 A person s physical characteristics can signal cues to fertility and health 14 15 16 with statistical modeling studies showing that the facial shape variables that reflect aspects of physiological health including body fat and blood pressure also influence observers perceptions of health 17 Attending to these factors increases reproductive success furthering the representation of one s genes in the population 18 Heterosexual men tend to be attracted to women who have a youthful appearance and exhibit features such as a symmetrical face 19 full breasts full lips and a low waist hip ratio 20 21 Heterosexual women tend to be attracted to men who are taller than they themselves are and who display a high degree of facial symmetry masculine facial dimorphism upper body strength broad shoulders a relatively narrow waist and a V shaped torso 22 23 24 25 Contents 1 General contributing factors 1 1 Facial features 1 2 Symmetry 1 3 Body scent 1 4 Genetics 1 5 Youthfulness 1 6 Leg to body ratio 1 7 Genitalia 1 8 Skin color 1 9 Hands 1 10 Height 1 11 Body language 1 11 1 Standing postures 1 11 2 Movement patterns 2 Male specific factors 2 1 Sexual dimorphism 2 2 Waist to chest ratio 2 3 Flat abdomen 2 4 Musculature 2 5 Body hair 2 6 Facial hair 2 7 Jawline 3 Female specific factors 3 1 Facial features 3 1 1 General 3 1 2 Eyes 3 2 Breasts 3 3 Buttocks 3 4 Body mass 3 5 Waist hip ratio 3 6 Feet size 3 7 Hair 3 8 Fertility driven attractiveness 3 9 Sexual ornamentation 4 Interpersonal attraction 5 Neural correlates of perceiving attractiveness 6 Facial similarity and racial preferences 7 Group attractiveness effect 8 Social effects 9 Misconceptions 9 1 The golden ratio 9 2 Ideal body shape for women 10 See also 11 Notes 12 References 12 1 Principal sources 13 External linksGeneral contributing factors edit nbsp The Mannerist movement was not afraid to exaggerate body proportions for an effect considered attractive Juno in a niche engraving by Jacopo Caraglio probably of a drawing by Rosso Fiorentino 1526Generally physical attractiveness can be viewed from a number of perspectives with universal perceptions being common to all human cultures cultural and social aspects and individual subjective preferences The perception of attractiveness can have a significant effect on how people are judged in terms of employment or social opportunities friendship sexual behavior and marriage 26 Some physical features are attractive in both men and women particularly bodily 27 and facial symmetry 28 29 30 31 although one contrary report suggests that absolute flawlessness with perfect symmetry can be disturbing 32 Symmetry may be evolutionarily beneficial as a sign of health because asymmetry signals past illness or injury 33 One study suggested people were able to gauge beauty at a subliminal level by seeing only a glimpse of a picture for one hundredth of a second 33 Other important factors include youthfulness skin clarity and smoothness of skin and vivid color in the eyes and hair 28 However there are numerous differences based on gender A 1921 study of the reports of college students regarding those traits in individuals which make for attractiveness and repulsiveness argued that static traits such as beauty or ugliness of features hold a position subordinate to groups of physical elements like expressive behavior affectionate disposition grace of manner aristocratic bearing social accomplishments and personal habits 34 Grammer and colleagues have identified eight pillars of beauty youthfulness symmetry averageness sex hormone markers body odor motion skin complexion and hair texture 35 Traditionally in Samoa body fat was acceptable or attractive 36 Facial features edit An Italian study published in 2008 studied the positions of the 50 soft tissue landmarks of the faces of 324 white Northern Italian adolescent boys and girls to compare the features of a group of 93 beautiful individuals selected by a commercial casting agency with those of a reference group with normal dentofacial dimensions and proportions The research found that in comparison with the reference group the attractive adolescents tended to have the following characteristics the ratio between the volume of the forehead and that of the total face was larger the nasal volume was smaller the distance between outer canthi was larger total facial height and depth were reduced Some tendencies differed by age and sex the facial volume was smaller in older attractive boys than in their peers but bigger in attractive girls the faces of older attractive adolescents were less rounded bigger ratio between facial area and volume but the reverse was true for girls of any age attractive older boys had smaller angles of facial convexity with more acute profiles while in girls the reverse pattern was found the nasolabial angle was reduced in girls but in older boys the effect was reversed older attractive boys tended to have more prominent chins The study concluded that attractive adolescents had more neotenous and juvenile features but older attractive boys also showed tendencies towards sexual dimorphism 37 Contrary to common misconception one study finds that non severe facial scarring increases male attractiveness for short term relationships 38 Symmetry edit Symmetrical faces and bodies may be signs of good inheritance to women of child bearing age seeking to create healthy offspring Studies suggest women are less attracted to men with asymmetrical faces 39 and symmetrical faces correlate with long term mental performance 40 and are an indication that a man has experienced fewer genetic and environmental disturbances such as diseases toxins malnutrition or genetic mutations while growing 40 Since achieving symmetry is a difficult task during human growth requiring billions of cell reproductions while maintaining a parallel structure achieving symmetry is a visible signal of genetic health Studies have also suggested that women at peak fertility were more likely to fantasize about men with greater facial symmetry 41 and other studies have found that male symmetry was the only factor that could significantly predict the likelihood of a woman experiencing orgasm during sex Women with partners possessing greater symmetry reported significantly more copulatory female orgasms than were reported by women with partners possessing low symmetry even with many potential confounding variables controlled 42 This finding has been found to hold across different cultures It has been argued that masculine facial dimorphism in men and symmetry in faces are signals advertising genetic quality in potential mates 43 Low facial and body fluctuating asymmetry may indicate good health and intelligence which are desirable features 44 Studies have found that women who perceive themselves as being more physically attractive are more likely to favor men with a higher degree of facial symmetry than are women who perceive themselves as being less physically attractive 45 It has been found that symmetrical females and males have a tendency to begin to have sexual intercourse at an earlier age to have more sexual partners and to have more one night stands They are also more likely to engage in infidelity 46 A study of quarterbacks in the American National Football League found a positive correlation between facial symmetry and salaries 29 Body scent edit Main article Body odor Double blind studies found that women prefer the scent of men who are rated as facially attractive 47 For example both males and females were more attracted to the natural scent of individuals who had been rated by consensus as facially attractive 48 Additionally it has also been shown that women have a preference for the scent of men with more symmetrical faces and that women s preference for the scent of more symmetrical men is strongest during the most fertile period of their menstrual cycle 49 Within the set of normally cycling women individual women s preference for the scent of men with high facial symmetry correlated with their probability of conception 49 Men s body odor is also affected by their diet with women expressing preferences for male body odor associated with increased dietary fruit and vegetable and protein content and reduced carbohydrate content 50 Genetics edit See also Heterozygote advantage and Major histocompatibility complex and sexual selection Studies have explored the genetic basis behind such issues as facial symmetry and body scent and how they influence physical attraction In one study in which women wore men s T shirts researchers found that women were more attracted to the bodily scents in shirts of men who had a different type of gene section within the DNA called major histocompatibility complex MHC 51 MHC is a large gene area within the DNA of vertebrates which encodes proteins dealing with the immune system 52 and which influences individual bodily odors 53 One hypothesis is that humans are naturally attracted by the sense of smell and taste to others with dissimilar MHC sections perhaps to avoid subsequent inbreeding while increasing the genetic diversity of offspring 52 Furthermore there are studies showing that women s natural attraction for men with dissimilar immune profiles can be distorted with use of birth control pills 53 Other research findings involving the genetic foundations of attraction suggest that MHC heterozygosity positively correlates with male facial attractiveness Women judge the faces of men who are heterozygous at all three MHC loci to be more attractive than the faces of men who are homozygous at one or more of these loci Additionally a second experiment with genotyped women raters found these preferences were independent of the degree of MHC similarity between the men and the female rater With MHC heterozygosity independently seen as a genetic advantage the results suggest that facial attractiveness in men may be a measure of genetic quality 54 55 General genetic heterozygosity has been demonstrated to be related to attractiveness in that people with mixed genetic backgrounds i e mixed race people as seen as more attractive than people with a more similar genetic parents i e single race people 56 However some studies have not found that mixed race individuals are rated as more attractive and one found that only certain mixes were rated as more attractive this study argued that equating race with genetics was incorrect and argued for social influences as the cause 57 Youthfulness edit See also Age disparity in sexual relationships A 2010 study by OkCupid a US dating site on 200 000 of its male and female users found that heterosexual women except those during their early to mid twenties are open to relationships with both somewhat older and somewhat younger men they have a larger potential dating pool than men until age 26 At age 20 women in a dramatic change begin sending private messages to significantly older men At age 29 they become even more open to older men Male desirability to women peaks in the late 20s and does not fall below the average for all men until 36 58 Other research indicates that women irrespective of their own age are attracted to men who are the same age or older 59 For the Romans especially beardlessness and smooth young bodies were considered beautiful to both men and women 60 For Greek and Roman men the most desirable traits of boys were their youth and hairlessness Pubescent boys were considered a socially appropriate object of male desire while post pubescent boys were considered to be ἔ3wroi or past the prime 60 This was largely in the context of pederasty adult male interest in adolescent boys Today men and women s attitudes towards male beauty has changed For example body hair on men may even be preferred see below A 1984 study said that gay men tend to prefer gay men of the same age as ideal partners but there was a statistically significant effect p lt 0 05 of masculinity femininity The study said that more feminine men tended to prefer relatively older men than themselves and more masculine men tended to prefer relatively younger men than themselves 61 Cross cultural data shows that the reproductive success of women is tied to their youth and physical attractiveness 62 such as the pre industrial Sami where the most reproductively successful women were 15 years younger than their man 63 One study covering 37 cultures showed that on average a woman was 2 5 years younger than her male partner with the age difference in Nigeria and Zambia being at the far extreme of 6 5 to 7 5 years As men age they tend to seek a mate who is ever younger 64 25 of eHarmony s male customers over the age of 50 request to only be matched with women younger than 40 65 The 2010 OkCupid study found that female desirability to its male users peaks at age 21 and falls below the average for all women at 31 After age 26 men have a larger potential dating pool than women on the site and by age 48 their pool is almost twice as large The median 31 year old male user searches for women aged 22 to 35 while the median 42 year old male searches for women 27 to 45 The age skew is even greater with messages to other users the median 30 year old male messages teenage girls as often as women his own age while mostly ignoring women a few years older than him Excluding the 10 most and 10 least beautiful women women s attractiveness does not change between 18 and 40 If extremes are included however there s no doubt that younger women are more physically attractive indeed in many ways beauty and youth are inextricable That s why most of the models you see in magazines are teenagers 58 Pheromones detected by female hormone markers reflects female fertility and the reproductive value mean 66 As females age the estrogen to androgen production ratio changes and results in female faces to appear more and more masculine thus appearing less attractive 66 In a small n 148 study performed in the United States using male college students at one university the mean age expressed as ideal for a wife was found to be 16 87 years old while 17 76 was the mean ideal age for a brief sexual encounter However the study sets up a framework where taboos against sex with young girls are purposely diminished and biased their sample by removing any participant over the age of 30 with a mean participant age of 19 83 67 In a study of penile tumescence men were found most aroused by pictures of young adult females 68 Signals of fertility in women are often also seen as signals of youth The evolutionary perspective proposes the idea that when it comes to sexual reproduction the minimal parental investment required by men gives them the ability and want to simply reproduce as much as possible 69 It therefore makes sense that men are attracted to the features in women which signal youthfulness and thus fertility 69 Their chances of reproductive success are much higher than they would be should they pair with someone older and therefore less fertile This may explain why combating age declines in attractiveness occurs from a younger age in women than in men For example the removal of one s body hair is considered a very feminine thing to do 70 This can be explained by the fact that aging results in raised levels of testosterone and thus body hair growth Shaving reverts one s appearance to a more youthful stage 70 and although this may not be an honest signal men will interpret this as a reflection of increased fertile value Research supports this showing hairlessness is considered sexually attractive by men 71 Leg to body ratio edit Main article Body proportions Leg to body ratio nbsp nbsp These drawings of two male and two female figures is a remake of the drawing of the extremes of leg to body ratio LBR as used in the experiment by Swami et al 2006 to find out what LBR is considered the most attractive for British men and women The male figure with the lowest LBR and shortest legs at left had the highest average attractiveness ratings whereas the male figure with the highest LBR and longest legs at right had the lowest ratings from The female figure with the lowest LBR and shortest legs at left had the lowest average attractiveness ratings whereas the figure with the highest LBR and longest legs at right had the highest average attractiveness ratings 72 Leg to body ratio is seen as an indicator of physical attractiveness but there appears to be no single accepted definition of leg length the perineum to floor measure b is the most frequently used but arguably the distance from the ankle bone to the outer hip bone is more rigorous 73 With the latter metric the most attractive male leg to body ratio judged by American women is 1 1 73 A Japanese study using the former metric found the same result for male attractiveness but women with longer legs than the rest of their body were judged to be more attractive 74 Excessive deviations from the mean were seen as indicative of disease 74 A study using Polish participants found that legs 5 longer than the average for both sexes was considered most attractive 75 The study concluded this preference might stem from the influence of long legged runway models 76 Another study using British and American participants found mid ranging leg to body ratios to be most ideal 77 A study by Swami et al of British male and female undergraduates showed a preference for men with legs as long as the rest of their body and women with 40 longer legs than the rest of their body 72 The researcher concluded that this preference might be influenced by American culture in which long legged women are portrayed as more attractive 72 Marco Bertamini criticized the Swami et al study for using a picture of the same person with digitally altered leg lengths which he felt would make the modified image appear unrealistic 78 Bertamini also criticized the Swami study for only changing the leg length while keeping the arm length constant 78 After accounting for these concerns in his own study Bertamini using stick figures also found a preference for women with proportionately longer legs than men 78 When Bertamini investigated the issue of possible sexual dimorphism of leg length he found two sources that indicated that men usually have slightly proportionately longer legs than women or that differences in leg length proportion may not exist between men and women 79 Following this review of existing literature on the subject he conducted his own calculations using data from 1774 men and 2208 women Using this data he similarly found that men usually have slightly proportionately longer legs than women or that differences in leg length proportion may not exist between men and women These findings made him rule out the possibility that a preference for women with proportionately longer legs than men is due proportionately longer legs being a secondary sex characteristic of women 79 Genitalia edit A 2006 study of 25 594 heterosexual men found that men who perceived themselves as having a large penis were more satisfied with their own appearance 80 A 2014 study criticized previous studies based on the fact that they relied on images and used terms such as small medium and large when asking for female preference The new study used 3D models of penises from sizes of 4 inches 10 cm long and 2 5 inches 6 4 cm in circumference to 8 5 inches 22 cm long and 7 inches 18 cm in circumference and let the women view and handle them It was found that women overestimated the actual size of the penises they experimented with when asked in a follow up survey The study concluded that women on average preferred the 6 5 inch 17 cm penis in length both for long term and for one time partners Penises with larger girth were preferred for one time partners 81 Evidence from various cultures suggests that heterosexual men tend to find the sight of women s genitalia to be sexually arousing 82 Skin color edit See also Human skin color Social status and racism nbsp Les Deux Perles Fernand Le Quesne The two pearls 1889 This painting was intended to contrast a Caucasian with an African beauty In the painting the black woman represents the beauty of a black pearl and the white woman represents the beauty of a white pearl 83 Manual laborers who spent extended periods of time outside developed a darker skin tone due to exposure to the sun As a consequence an association between dark skin and the lower classes developed Light skin became an aesthetic ideal because it symbolized wealth Over time society attached various meanings to these colored differences Including assumptions about a person s race socioeconomic class intelligence and physical attractiveness 84 Some research has suggested that redder and yellower skin tones 85 reflecting higher levels of oxygenated blood 86 carotenoid and to a lesser extent melanin pigment and net dietary intakes of fruit and vegetables 87 appear healthier and therefore more attractive 88 However there is little direct evidence that skin colour is actually related to health or immune system strength 89 A historical preference for lighter skinned women has been documented across many cultures 90 91 However the accuracy of this research has been questioned by other authors 92 Experimental studies show that white Western men are more attracted to dark tanned women rather than medium tanned or pale women 93 94 95 A 2010 study found a preference for lighter skinned but not lightest women in New Zealand and California 96 However other research has found that African American males and females consider medium complexion as more attractive than lighter or darker skin while white and Hispanic women seek to tan their skin in order to increase their attractiveness to the opposite sex 97 There is a direct correlation between being tan and self perceived attractiveness especially among young women 98 The relationship between attractiveness and skin colour may also intersect with ethnicity and prior experience 99 Skin color preferences may shift over time as in Western culture where tanned skin used to be associated with the sun exposed manual labor of the lower class but since the mid 20th century it has generally been considered more attractive and healthier than before with sun tanning becoming fashionable 100 101 102 103 104 In the African state of Mali skin bleaching is common as it is thought to improve one s social standing and attractiveness to the opposite sex although there has also been vocal opposition to this notion from pop culture icons 105 Skin radiance or glowing skin may influence perception of beauty and physical attractiveness 106 107 Hands edit Hands have been found to be physically attractive 108 The type of hands that are physically attractive are those with longer index and ring fingers 109 Men have a smaller index to ring finger ratio than women The gender differences in the ratio between the index and ring fingers are said to be influenced by exposure to testosterone within the womb 110 In a study where participants were shown computer based images of hands male participants found feminine hands with a smaller index finger less attractive Whereas females found masculine hands with a longer ring finger more attractive The study suggests that finger length has an effect on physical attraction because it gives indication of the desirable sex hormone dependent traits which one may possess 109 Another study found that averageness healthiness of the skin how fat the hands appear to be and the grooming of the hands all affect the attractiveness of hands 111 What is meant by averageness is the degree to which the hands look like an average of the hands in the population Average looking hands give an indication of an individual s health because there are no abnormalities The healthier looking the skin on the hands the more attractive they appear Reasons given for this say skin health may reflect an individual s overall health Healthy skin can show that someone is free from illness because some illnesses have a bad effect on the look of skin These features are found attractive because they show that the person has good genes and is therefore a suitable mate to reproduce with Skin health may also give an indication of socioeconomic status as rough hands may indicate a low paying laborious job Low socioeconomic status might show that someone does not have resources to provide for the offspring and is therefore less attractive The more fat the hands appear the less attractive they are This is because of the co morbidity associated with obesity If someone is overfat they may have another disease which means they may not be able to produce healthy offspring The attractiveness of the hands also gives an indication of other features of the individual people with more attractive hands have been found to be taller and slimmer 108 In most of these hand attractiveness studies only white European hands were used and the participants were 18 26 years old So the attractiveness of non white hands and of different age groups was not tested Also the people who rated the hand attractiveness were white Europeans so their ratings may not represent how individuals of other skin colours and cultures would rate the hands Height edit Females sexual attraction towards males may be determined by the height of the man 112 The online dating Website eHarmony only matches women with men taller than themselves because of complaints from women matched with shorter men 65 Other studies have shown that heterosexual women often prefer men taller than they are rather than a man with above average height While women usually desire men to be at least the same height as themselves or taller several other factors also determine male attractiveness and the male taller norm is not universal 113 gt For example taller women are more likely to relax the taller male norm than shorter women 114 Furthermore professor Adam Eyre Walker from the University of Sussex has stated that there is as yet no evidence that these preferences are evolutionary preferences as opposed to merely cultural preferences 115 Still the cultural perceived attractiveness preferences for taller men are powerful and confirmed by multiple studies One study of speed daters by Stulp found that women were most likely to choose men 25 cm taller than themselves whereas men were most likely to choose women only 7 cm shorter than themselves 116 Additionally women seem more receptive to an erect posture than men though both prefer it as an element within beauty 117 According to one study Yee N 2002 gay men who identify as only tops tend to prefer shorter men while gay men who identify as only bottoms tend to prefer taller men 118 In romances in Middle English literature all of the ideal male heroes are tall and the vast majority of the valiant male heroes are tall too 119 Most men tend to be taller than their female partners 21 It has been found that in Western societies most men prefer women shorter than themselves 120 Having said this height is a more important factor for a woman when choosing a man than it is for a man choosing a woman 121 Western men tend to view women taller than themselves as less attractive 120 and many people view heterosexual couples where the woman is taller to be less ideal 120 Women who are 0 7 to 1 7 standard deviations below the mean female height have been reported to be the most reproductively successful 115 since fewer tall women get married compared to shorter women 120 However in other ethnic groups such as the Hadza people from Tanzania a study has found that height is irrelevant in choosing a mate 113 Another study found the same preference in rural Gambia 120 In Middle English literature tallness is a characteristic of ideally beautiful women 122 The British Fashion Model Agents Association BFMA says that female models should be at least 5 ft 8 in 1 73 m tall 123 Body language edit Standing postures edit nbsp A statue of Venus standing in a contrapposto pose with her weight borne predominantly on one leg As shown here this posture accentuates the curvature of her figure Standing in a contrapposto posture with bodyweight predominantly supported by one leg which is either straight or very slightly bent and with the other leg slightly bent has been found to be more attractive looking than standing in a more plain upright posture This was found to be the case for both men and women This posture may lower a person s observable waist hip ratio and make their hips look wider and their waists thinner For women especially this can accentuate the curvature of their figure on one side of their body and make them seem more attractive Such poses have been used in historical sculpture to emphasize an ideal of physical beauty 124 125 It has also been demonstrated that the contrapposto posture in women elicits more neural activity in brain areas linked to perception and attractiveness assessments than a standing position 126 Movement patterns edit The way an individual moves can indicate health and even age and influence attractiveness 127 A study reflecting the views of 700 individuals and that involved animated representations of people walking found that the physical attractiveness of women increased by about 50 percent when they walked with a hip sway Similarly the perceived attractiveness of males doubled when they moved with a swagger in their shoulders 128 Male specific factors edit nbsp Arnold Schwarzenegger one of the most notable figures in bodybuilding 1974Further information Masculine beauty ideal Women on average tend to be more attracted to men who have a relatively narrow waist a V shaped torso wide chest and broad shoulders Women also tend to be more attracted to men who are taller and larger than they are and display a high degree of facial symmetry as well as relatively masculine facial dimorphism 22 23 Women regardless of sexual orientation tend to be more interested in a partner s physical attractiveness than men 129 130 131 Sexual dimorphism edit Main article Sexual dimorphism The degree of differences between male and female anatomical traits is called sexual dimorphism Female respondents in the follicular phase of their menstrual cycle were significantly more likely to choose a masculine face than those in menses and luteal phases 132 or in those taking hormonal contraception 22 23 133 134 This distinction supports the sexy son hypothesis which posits that it is evolutionarily advantageous for women to select potential fathers who are more genetically attractive 135 rather than the best caregivers 136 However women s likeliness to exert effort to view male faces does not seem to depend on their masculinity but to a general increase with women s testosterone levels 137 It is suggested that the masculinity of facial features is a reliable indication of good health or alternatively that masculine looking males are more likely to achieve high status 138 However the correlation between attractive facial features and health has been questioned 139 Sociocultural factors such as self perceived attractiveness status in a relationship and degree of gender conformity have been reported to play a role in female preferences for male faces 140 Studies have found that women who perceive themselves as physically attractive are more likely to choose men with masculine facial dimorphism than are women who perceive themselves as physically unattractive 45 In men facial masculinity significantly correlates with facial symmetry it has been suggested that both are signals of developmental stability and genetic health 141 One study called into question the importance of facial masculinity in physical attractiveness in men arguing that when perceived health which is factored into facial masculinity is discounted it makes little difference in physical attractiveness 142 In a cross country study involving 4 794 women in their early twenties a difference was found in women s average masculinity preference between countries 143 144 A study found that the same genetic factors cause facial masculinity in both males and females such that a male with a more masculine face would likely have a sister with a more masculine face due to the siblings having shared genes The study also found that although female faces that were more feminine were judged to be more attractive there was no association between male facial masculinity and male facial attractiveness for female judges With these findings the study reasoned that if a woman were to reproduce with a man with a more masculine face then her daughters would also inherit a more masculine face making the daughters less attractive The study concluded that there must be other factors that advantage the genetics for masculine male faces to offset their reproductive disadvantage in terms of health fertility and facial attractiveness when the same genetics are present in females The study reasoned that the selective advantage for masculine male faces must have or had been due to some factor that is not directly tied to female perceptions of male facial attractiveness 145 In a study of 447 gay men in China researchers said that tops preferred feminized male faces bottoms preferred masculinized male faces and versatiles had no preference for either feminized or masculinized male faces 146 In pre modern Chinese literature the ideal man in caizi jiaren romances was said to have rosy lips sparkling white teeth and a jasper like face Chinese 唇紅齒白 面若冠玉 147 148 In Middle English literature a beautiful man should have a long broad and strong face 149 Waist to chest ratio edit The physique of a slim waist broad shoulders and muscular chest are often found to be attractive to both females and males 150 Further research has shown that when choosing a mate the traits females look for indicate higher social status such as dominance resources and protection 151 An indicator of health in males a contributing factor to physical attractiveness is the android fat distribution pattern which is categorized as more fat distributed on the upper body and abdomen commonly referred to as the V shape 151 When asked to rate other men both heterosexual and homosexual men found low waist to chest ratios WCR to be more attractive on other men with the gay men showing a preference for lower WCR more V shaped than the straight men 152 Other researchers found waist to chest ratio the largest determinant of male attractiveness with body mass index and waist to hip ratio not as significant 153 Women focus primarily on the ratio waist to chest or more specifically waist to shoulder This is analogous to the waist to hip ratio WHR that men prefer Some studies have shown that attractive bodily traits in the eyes of a heterosexual woman would include a tall athletic physique with wide shoulders and a slim waist area 154 Research has additionally shown that college males had a better satisfaction with their body than college females 155 The research also found that when a college female s waist to hip ratio went up their body image satisfaction decreased 155 Some research has shown that body weight may have a stronger effect than WHR when it comes to perceiving attractiveness of the opposite sex It was found that waist to hip ratio played a smaller role in body preference than body weight in regards to both sexes 156 Psychologists Viren Swami and Martin J Tovee compared female preference for male attractiveness cross culturally between Britain and Malaysia They found that females placed more importance on WCR and therefore body shape in urban areas of Britain and Malaysia while females in rural areas placed more importance on BMI therefore weight and body size Both WCR and BMI are indicative of male status and ability to provide for offspring as noted by evolutionary theory 157 Females have been found to desire males that are normal weight and have the average WHR for a male Females view these males as attractive and healthy Males who had the average WHR but were overweight or underweight are not perceived as attractive to females This suggests that WHR is not a major factor in male attractiveness but a combination of body weight and a typical male WHR seem to be the most attractive Research has shown that men who have a higher waist to hip ratio and a higher salary are perceived as more attractive to women 158 Flat abdomen edit A 1982 study found that an abdomen that protrudes was the least attractive trait for men 159 In Middle English literature a beautiful man should have a flat abdomen 160 Musculature edit nbsp Ronnie Coleman posing nbsp Ronnie Coleman posing in 2009See also Bodybuilding Men s bodies portrayed in magazines marketed to men are more muscular than the men s bodies portrayed in magazines marketed to women From this some have concluded that men perceive a more muscular male body to be ideal as distinct from a woman s ideal male which is less muscular than what men perceive to be ideal 161 This is due to the within gender prestige granted by increased muscularity and within gender competition for increased muscularity 161 Men perceive the attractiveness of their own musculature by how closely their bodies resemble the muscle man 162 This muscle man ideal is characterized by large muscular arms especially biceps a large muscular chest that tapers to their waist and broad shoulders 162 Among Australian university students the male body composition found to be most attractive 12 16 kg fat 63 27 kg muscle was in line with the composition that was perceived as healthiest and was well within the healthy range 163 In a study of stated profile preferences on Match com a greater percentage of gay men than lesbians selected their ideal partner s body type as Athletic and Toned as opposed to the other two options of Average or Overweight 164 In pre modern Chinese literature such as in Romance of the Western Chamber a type of masculinity called scholar masculinity is depicted wherein the ideal male lover is weak vulnerable feminine and pedantic 147 In Middle English literature a beautiful man typically has thick broad shoulders a square and muscular chest a muscular back strong sides that taper to a small waist large hands and arms and legs with huge muscles 165 Body hair edit Studies based in the United States New Zealand and China have shown that women rate men with no trunk chest and abdominal hair as most attractive and that attractiveness ratings decline as hairiness increases 166 167 Another study however found that moderate amounts of trunk hair on men was most attractive to the sample of British and Sri Lankan women 168 Further a degree of hirsuteness hairiness and a waist to shoulder ratio of 0 6 is often preferred when combined with a muscular physique 168 In a study using Finnish women women with hairy fathers were more likely to prefer hairy men suggesting that preference for hairy men is the result of either genetics or imprinting 169 Among gay men another study reported gay males who identify as only tops prefer less hairy men while gay males who identify as only bottoms prefer more hairy men 118 Facial hair edit One study shows that men with facial hair covering the cheeks upper lip and lower jaw were perceived as more physically attractive than men with patchy facial hair 170 In this study men s facial hair was split into four categories each differing in the thickness and coverage very light light medium and heavy Light facial hair was rated as the most attractive followed by medium heavy and the least attractive was very light This study suggests that some facial hair is better than none because it shows masculine development as beard growth requires the conversion of testosterone An earlier study found that women from Western and Oceanic cultures are more attracted to clean shaven faces than beards However they also rated full bearded men as having higher status than clean shaven men 171 Jawline edit This section may be too technical for most readers to understand Please help improve it to make it understandable to non experts without removing the technical details February 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message Main article Mandible An attractive male jaw angle may have the following features citation needed 115 130 in profile view interregional clarification needed width similar to facial width vertical position in frontal view at the corners of the mouth or at least not below the lower lip jawline slope in frontal view nearly parallel to with a maximum 15 downward deviation from a line extending from the lateral canthus to the alare ascending ramus slope 65 75 to the Frankfurt horizontal visible curvature in oblique viewBecause it generates the gonial angle the ramus has a function in attractiveness citation needed The gonial or jaw angle is the angle formed by the ramus and the rest of the mandible It can range from 90 to 140 degrees with the population average being 128 2 36 degrees for males and 126 2 41 degrees for females Larger more obtuse gonial angles result in a rounder feminine appearance whereas more acute gonial angles result in square masculine faces 172 173 This exact ratio is not seen as ideal in every ethnic group Most research shows that attractive bigonial width and Ramus measurements have similarities but the jutting square chin is a prominently European heritage trait which means it should not be held as a universal indicator of attractiveness 174 175 Men with low submental fat were viewed to have better jawlines and a more youthful look 176 Female specific factors editFurther information Feminine beauty ideal Research indicates that heterosexual men tend to be attracted to young 64 and beautiful women 177 with bodily symmetry 178 Rather than decreasing it modernity has only increased the emphasis men place on women s looks 179 Evolutionary psychologists attribute such attraction to an evaluation of the fertility potential in a prospective mate 64 Facial features edit General edit See also Cuteness and Averageness nbsp A University of Toronto study found that the facial proportions of Jessica Alba were close to the average of all female profiles nbsp Namie Amuro inspired the small face fad in Japan which caused Japanese women to buy beauty products such as masks and creams to try to obtain a small face like hers 180 nbsp An 1889 U S newspaper ad for arsenic complexion wafers decried blotches moles pimples freckles and all female irregularities 181 Arsenic was known to be poisonous during the Victorian era 182 Research has attempted to determine which facial features communicate attractiveness Facial symmetry has been shown to be considered attractive in women 183 184 and men have been found to prefer full lips 185 high forehead broad face small chin small nose short and narrow jaw high cheekbones 39 186 clear and smooth skin and wide set eyes 64 The shape of the face in terms of how everything hangs together is an important determinant of beauty 187 Women with thick dark limbal rings in their eyes have also been found to be more attractive The explanation given is that because the ring tends to fade with age and medical problems a prominent limbal ring gives an honest indicator of youth 188 In Persian literature beautiful women are said to have noses like hazelnuts 189 In Arabian society in the Middle Ages a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have straight and fine noses 190 In Jewish Rabbinic literature the rabbis considered a delicate nose to be the ideal type of nose for women 191 In Japan during the Edo period a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have tall noses which were straight and not too tall 192 In a cross cultural study more neotenized i e youthful looking female faces were found to be most attractive to men while less neotenized female faces were found to be less attractive to men regardless of the females actual age 193 In a study of Italian women who have won beauty competitions it was found that their faces had more babyish pedomorphic traits than those of the normal women used as a reference 194 In a cross cultural study Marcinkowska et al said that 18 to 45 year old heterosexual men in all 28 countries surveyed preferred photographs of 18 to 24 year old white women whose faces were feminized using facial image editing software over faces of 18 to 24 year old white women that were masculinized using that software but there were differences in preferences for femininity across countries The higher the National Health Index based on eight national health statistics taken from the World Health Organization Statistical Information Service using data from 2002 to 2006 of a country the more were the feminized faces preferred over the masculinized faces Among the countries surveyed Japan had the highest femininity preference and Nepal had the lowest femininity preference 195 Michael R Cunningham of the Department of Psychology at the University of Louisville found using a panel of East Asian Hispanic and White judges that the female faces tended to be judged as more attractive if they had a mixture of youthful and sexually mature features Hispanic and East Asian women were judged as more attractive than White and Black women and they happened to possess more of the attributes defined as attractive however the authors noted that it would be inaccurate to conclude that any ethnic group was more attractive than the other based on their sample 196 Using a panel of African Americans and whites as judges Cunningham found more neotenous faces were perceived as having both higher femininity and sociability 90 The authors found no evidence of ethnocentric bias in the Asian or White samples as Asians and Whites did not differ significantly in preference for neonate cues and positive ratings of white women did not increase with exposure to Western media 197 Rather than finding evidence for purely neonate faces being most appealing Cunningham found faces with sexually mature features at the periphery of the face combined with neonate features in the center of the face most appealing in women 90 Upon analyzing the results of his study Cunningham concluded that preference for neonate features may display the least cross cultural variability in terms of attractiveness ratings 90 and in another study Cunningham concluded that there exists a large agreement on the characteristics of an attractive face 198 In computer face averaging tests women with averaged faces have been shown to be considered more attractive 31 199 This is possibly due to average features being more familiar and therefore more comfortable 183 Commenting on the prevalence of whiteness in supposed beauty ideals in his book White Lies Race and the Myth of Whiteness Maurice Berger states that the schematic rendering in the idealized face of a study conducted with American subjects had straight hair light skin almond shaped eyes thin arched eyebrows a long thin nose closely set and tiny nostrils and a large mouth and thin lips 200 though the author of the study stated that there was consistency between his results and those conducted on other races Scholar Liu Jieyu says in the article White Collar Beauties The criterion of beauty is both arbitrary and gendered The implicit consensus is that women who have fair skin and a slim figure with symmetrical facial features are pretty He says that all of these requirements are socially constructed and force people to change themselves to fit these criteria 201 On average symmetrical features are one ideal while unusual stand out features are another 202 A study performed by the University of Toronto found that the most attractive facial dimensions were those found in the average female face However that particular University of Toronto study looked only at white women 203 A 2011 study by Wilkins Chan and Kaiser found correlations between perceived femininity and attractiveness that is women s faces which were seen as more feminine were judged by both men and women to be more attractive 204 A component of the female beauty ideal in Persian literature is for women to have faces like a full moon 189 205 206 In Arabian society in the Middle Ages a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have round faces which were like a full moon 190 In Japan during the Edo period a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have oval faces which were shaped like ovals 192 In Jewish Rabbinic literature the rabbis considered full lips to be the ideal type of lips for women 191 Historically in Chinese and Japanese literature the feminine ideal was said to include small lips 207 Women would paint their lips thinner and narrower to align with this ideal 208 209 A study that used Chinese Malay and Indian judges said that Chinese women and Chinese men with retrusive mandibles where the mouth is flat and in line with the rest of the face were judged to be the most attractive and Chinese men and Chinese women with a protruding mandible where the jaw projects outward were judged to be the least attractive 210 Classical Persian literature paintings and miniatures portrayed traits such as long black curly hair a small mouth long arched eyebrows large almond shaped eyes a small nose and beauty spots as being beautiful for women 211 Eyes edit A study that investigated whether or not an eyelid crease makes Chinese descent women more attractive using photo manipulated photographs of young Chinese descent women s eyes found that the medium upper eyelid crease was considered most attractive by all three groups of both sexes white people Chinese and Taiwanese nationals together as a group and Taiwanese and Chinese Americans together as a group Similarly all three groups of both genders found the absence of an eye crease to be least attractive on Chinese women 212 In the late sixteenth century Japanese people considered epicanthic folds to be beautiful 213 A study that used Russian American Brazilian Ache and Hiwi raters found that the only strong distinguisher between men and women s faces was wider eyes relative to facial height for women and this trait consistently predicted attractiveness ratings for women 214 In Arabian society in the Middle Ages a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have dark black eyes which are large and long and in the shape of almonds Furthermore the eyes should be lustrous and they should have long eyelashes 190 A source written in 1823 said that a component of the Persian female beauty ideal was for women to have large eyes which are black in color 205 In Persian literature beautiful women are said to have eyes that are shaped like almonds 189 In Chinese the phrase lucent irises lustrous teeth Chinese 明 眸 皓 齒 is used to describe a beautiful woman with clear eyes and well aligned white teeth 215 and the phrase moth feeler eyebrows Chinese 蛾眉 is used to denote a beautiful woman by describing her eyebrows as being thin and arched like moth antennae 215 In the Chinese text The Grotto of the Immortals Chinese 遊 仙 窟 written during the Tang dynasty period narrow eyes were the preferred type of eyes for women and in the Chinese text Jeweled Chamber Secrets Chinese 玉 房 秘 訣 from the Six Dynasties period the ideal woman was described as having small eyes 216 In Japan during the Edo period one piece of evidence the appearance of the formal wife of Tokugawa Iesada as determined by bone anthropologist Suzuki Hisashi indicates that large eyes were considered attractive for women 192 but in another piece of evidence the 1813 Japanese text Customs Manners and Fashions of the Capital Japanese 都 風 俗 化 粧 伝 indicates that large eyes were not considered attractive for women 217 Breasts edit See also Breast fetishism Research has shown that most heterosexual men enjoy the sight of female breasts with a preference for large firm breasts 218 219 However a contradictory study of British undergraduates found younger men preferred small breasts on women 220 Smaller breasts were widely associated with youthfulness 127 Cross culturally another study found high variability regarding the ideal breast size 220 Some researchers in the United Kingdom have speculated that a preference for larger breasts may have developed in Western societies because women with larger breasts tend to have higher levels of the hormones estradiol and progesterone which both promote fertility 221 A study by Groyecka et al in which they examined Poles and Yali of New Guinea demonstrated that men s judgements of breast appearance is affected by the occurrence of breast ptosis i e sagginess droopiness 222 Greater breast ptosis more sagging breasts is perceived as less attractive and attributed to a woman of older age These findings are coherent with previous research that link breast attractiveness with female youthfulness Unlike breast size breast ptosis seems to be a universal marker of female breast attractiveness A study showed that men prefer symmetrical breasts 178 223 Breast symmetry may be particularly sensitive to developmental disturbances and the symmetry differences for breasts are large compared to other body parts Women who have more symmetrical breasts tend to have more children 224 Historical literature often includes specific features of individuals or a gender that are considered desirable These have often become a matter of convention and should be interpreted with caution In Arabian society in the Middle Ages a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to have small breasts 190 In Persian literature beautiful women are said to have breasts like pomegranates or lemons 189 In the Chinese text Jeweled Chamber Secrets Chinese 玉 房 秘 訣 from the Six Dynasties period the ideal woman was described as having firm breasts 216 In Sanskrit literature beautiful women are often said to have breasts so large that they cause the women to bend a little bit from their weight 225 In Middle English literature beautiful women should have small breasts that are round like an apple or a pear 226 Buttocks edit Main article Cultural history of the buttocks nbsp A remake of a drawing used to research perceptions of the most attractive size of posterior and breasts for white and black women Biological anthropologist Helen E Fisher of the Center for Human Evolution Studies in the Department of Anthropology of Rutgers University said that perhaps the fleshy rounded buttocks attracted males during rear entry intercourse 227 In a recent study using 3D models and eye tracking technology Fisher s claim was tested and was shown that the slight thrusting out of a woman s back influence how attractive others perceive her to be and captures the gaze of both men and women 228 229 Bobbi S Low et al of the School of Natural Resources and Environment at the University of Michigan said the female buttocks evolved in the context of females competing for the attention and parental commitment of powerful resource controlling males as an honest display of fat reserves that could not be confused with another type of tissue 230 although T M Caro professor in the Center for Population Biology and the Department of Wildlife Fish and Conservation Biology at University of California Davis rejected that as being a necessary conclusion stating that female fatty deposits on the hips improve individual fitness of the female regardless of sexual selection 230 In a 1995 study black men were more likely than white men to use the words big or large to describe their conception of an attractive woman s posterior 231 In a 2009 experiment to research what South African British white and British African men considered to be the most attractive size of posterior and breasts for white and black women This image shown here only shows the two extreme variations of size on black female figures used in the experiment The left hand figure received the highest average attractiveness rating from South African men while a figure with an intermediate size received the highest ratings from both white and black British men The right hand figure did not receive the highest average attractiveness rating from any group 232 Body mass edit Body Mass Index BMI is an important determinant to the perception of beauty 233 Even though the Western ideal is for a thin woman some cultures prefer plumper women 90 234 which has been argued to support that attraction for a particular BMI merely is a cultural artifact 234 The attraction for a proportionate body also influences an appeal for erect posture 235 One cross cultural survey comparing body mass preferences among 300 of the most thoroughly studied cultures in the world showed that 81 of cultures preferred a female body size that in English would be described as plump 236 Availability of food influences which female body size is attractive which may have evolutionary reasons Societies with food scarcities prefer larger female body size than societies that have plenty of food In Western society males who are hungry prefer a larger female body size than they do when not hungry 237 BMI has been criticised for conflating fat and muscle and more recent studies have concentrated on body composition Among Australian university students the most attractive body composition for women 10 31 kg fat 42 45 kg muscle was found to be lower in fat than both the most healthy appearing composition and below the healthy range 163 In the United States women overestimate men s preferences for thinness in a mate In one study American women were asked to choose what their ideal build was and what they thought the build most attractive to men was Women chose slimmer than average figures for both choices When American men were independently asked to choose the female build most attractive to them the men chose figures of average build This indicates that women may be misled as to how thin men prefer women to be 234 Some researchers believe women themselves may play a role in enforcing the thinness ideal however they assert that this does not mean that the ideal does not ultimately trace its origins to patriarchical norms the male gaze 238 Popenoe writes that societies with abundant food sources often value thin women while those with food scarcity value women with fat bodies In the West women with thin bodies became the ideal in the 19th century as the fat body became associated with criminality and backwardness 239 Some implicated the fashion industry for the promulgation of the notion of thinness as attractive 240 241 East Asians have historically preferred women whose bodies had small features For example during the Spring and Autumn period of Chinese history women in Chinese harems wanted to have a thin body in order to be attractive for the Chinese emperor Later during the Tang dynasty a less thin body type was seen as most attractive for Chinese women 242 In Arabian society in the Middle Ages a component of the female beauty ideal was for women to be slender like a cane or a twig 190 In the Chinese text Jeweled Chamber Secrets Chinese 玉 房 秘 訣 from the Six Dynasties period the ideal woman was described as not being large boned 216 In the Victorian era women who adhered to Victorian ideals were expected to limit their food consumption to attain the ideal slim figure 243 In Middle English literature slender women are considered beautiful 244 According to research on females women consider a full body with good distribution to be highly important to be considered attractive by men 245 Waist hip ratio edit Main article Waist hip ratio A WHR of 0 7 for women has been shown to correlate strongly with general health and fertility Women within the 0 7 range have optimal levels of estrogen and are less susceptible to major diseases such as diabetes heart disease and ovarian cancers 246 Women with high WHR 0 80 or higher have significantly lower pregnancy rates than women with lower WHRs 0 70 0 79 independent of their BMIs 247 248 Female waist to hip ratio WHR has been proposed by evolutionary psychologists to be an important component of human male mate choice because this trait is thought to provide a reliable cue to a woman s reproductive value 249 Both men and women judge women with smaller waist to hip ratios more attractive 250 Ethnic groups vary with regard to their ideal waist to hip ratio for women 251 ranging from 0 6 in China 252 to 0 8 or 0 9 in parts of South America and Africa 253 254 255 and divergent preferences based on ethnicity rather than nationality have also been noted 256 257 A study found the Machiguenga people an isolated indigenous South American ethnic group prefer women with high WHR 0 9 258 The preference for heavier women has been interpreted to belong to societies where there is no risk of obesity 259 In Chinese the phrase willow waist Chinese 柳 腰 is used to denote a beautiful woman by describing her waist as being slender like a willow branch 215 In the Victorian era a small waist was considered the main trait of a beautiful woman 243 The term wasp waist describes an extreme fashion silhouette produced by a style of corset and girdle Feet size edit According to some studies most men prefer women with small feet 260 261 such as in ancient China where foot binding was practiced 262 In Jewish Rabbinic literature the rabbis considered small feet to be the ideal type of feet for women 191 Hair edit Men have been found to prefer long haired women 64 263 264 An evolutionary psychology explanation for this is that malnutrition and deficiencies in minerals and vitamins causes loss of hair or hair changes Hair therefore indicates health and nutrition during the last 2 3 years Lustrous hair is also often a cross cultural preference 127 A component of the female beauty ideal in Persian literature is for women to have black hair 265 which was also preferred in Arabian society in the Middle Ages 190 In Middle English literature curly hair is a necessary component of a beautiful woman 266 Fertility driven attractiveness edit There are some subtle changes in women s perceived attractiveness across the menstrual cycle During their most fertile phase we can observe some changes in women s behavior and physiology A study conducted by G Miller 2007 examined the amount of tip earnings by lap dancers across the menstrual cycle He found that dancers received nearly US 15 more when they were near ovulation than during the rest of the month This suggests that women either are more attractive during ovulation phase or they experience a significant change in their behavior 267 Some other studies have found that there are subtle differences in women s faces when in their fertile phase Bobst and Lobmaier 2012 created 20 prototyped photographs some of a female during ovulation and some during the luteal phase Men were asked to choose the more attractive the more caring and the more flirtatious faces They found a significant preference for the follicular phase ovulation This suggests that subtle shape differences in faces occurring during the female s ovulation phase are sufficient to attract men more 268 This idea is supported by another study where a similar experiment was done Men and women had to judge photographs of women s faces taken during their fertile phase They were all rated more attractive than during non fertile phase There are some subtle visible cues to ovulation in women s faces and they are perceived as more attractive leading to the idea that it could be an adaptive mechanism to raise a female s mate value at that specific time when probability of conception is at its highest 267 Women s attractiveness as perceived by men and women slightly differs across her menstrual cycle being at peak when she is in her ovulation phase Jones et al 2008 focused on women s preferences for masculinity apparent health and self resemblance and found that it varies across the cycle They explained that the function of the effects of menstrual cycle phase on preferences for apparent health and self resemblance in faces is to increase the likelihood of pregnancy 269 Similarly females prefer the scent of symmetrically faced men and men with masculine faces during fertile phases as well as stereotypical male displays such as social presence and direct intrasexual competitiveness 270 During the follicular phase fertile females prefer more masculine traits testosterone dependent traits such as face shape than when in non fertile phase Those findings have been found in the voice showing that females preferences for more masculine voices over feminine voices increase the fertile phase of the menstrual cycle 271 But not only females preferences vary across cycle their behaviours as well Effectively men respond differently to females when they are on ovulatory cycle 267 because females act differently Women in the ovulatory phase are flirtier with males showing genetic fitness markers than in low fertile phase 272 It has been shown in some studies that women high in estrogen are generally perceived to be more attractive than women with low levels of estrogen based on women not wearing make up High estrogen level women may also be viewed as healthier or to have a more feminine face 273 Similarly a study investigated the capacity of women to select high quality males based on their facial attractiveness They found that facial attractiveness correlated with semen quality good normal or bad depending on sperm morphology and motility The more attractive a man s face is linked to his sperm being of better quality 274 Sexual ornamentation edit Sexual ornaments are seen in many organisms in humans females have sexual ornamentation in the form of breasts and buttocks The physical attraction to sexual ornaments is associated with gynoid fat as opposed to android fat which is considered unattractive 275 In human females proximate causes of the development of sexual ornaments are associated with the predominance of estrogen in puberty The activation of estrogen receptors around the female skeletal tissue causes gynoid fat to be deposited in the breasts buttocks hips and thighs producing an overall typical female body shape 276 Specifically female breasts are considered more attractive when symmetrical rather than asymmetrical 277 as this is thought to reflect good developmental stability 278 Sexual ornaments are considered attractive features as they are thought to indicate high mate value fertility 279 and the ability to provide good care to offspring They are sexually selected traits present for the purpose of honest signalling and capturing the visual attention of the opposite sex most commonly associated with females capturing the visual attention of males It has been proposed that these ornaments have evolved in order to advertise personal quality and reproductive value 280 Honest signalling with sexual ornaments is associated with ultimate causation of these evolved traits The evolution of these ornaments is also associated with female female competition in order to gain material benefits provided by resourceful and high status males 281 In humans once these sexual ornaments develop they are permanent It is thought that this is associated with the long term pair bonding humans engage in human females engage in extended sexual activity outside of their fertile period 282 This relates to another ultimate cause of sexual ornaments with function in obtaining non genetic material benefits from males In other animal species even other primate species these advertisements of reproductive value are not permanent Usually it is the point at which the female is at her most fertile she displays sexual swellings 283 Adolescence is the period of time whereby humans experience puberty and experience anatomical changes to their bodies through the increase of sex hormones released in the body Adolescent exaggeration is the period of time at which sexual ornaments are maximised and peak gynoid fat content is reached 281 In human females the mean age for this is approximately 16 years Female breasts develop at this stage not only to prepare for reproduction but also due to competition with other females in displaying their reproductive value and quality to males 281 Interpersonal attraction editIn the social psychology perspective interpersonal attraction is when someone experiences a positive attitude or evaluation regarding a particular person having potential of being a significant other This includes three components conventionally ascribed to attitudes behavioral tendency to approach the person cognitive positive beliefs about the person and affective positive feelings for the person 284 With these three components psychology has created five elements of interpersonal attraction These include proximity similarity physical attractiveness reciprocity and responsiveness 285 Neural correlates of perceiving attractiveness editMost studies of the brain activations associated with the perception of attractiveness show photographs of faces to their participants and let them or a comparable group of people rate the attractiveness of these faces Such studies consistently find that activity in certain parts of the orbitofrontal cortex increases with increasing attractiveness of faces 286 287 281 288 289 This neural response has been interpreted as a reaction on the rewarding nature of attractiveness as similar increases in activation in the medial orbitofrontal cortex can be seen in response to smiling faces 290 and to statements of morally good actions 281 289 While most of these studies have not assessed participants of both genders or homosexual individuals evidence from one study including male and female hetero and homosexual individuals indicate that some of the aforementioned increases in brain activity are restricted to images of faces of the gender which participants feel sexually attracted to 291 With regard to brain activation related to the perception of attractive bodies one study with heterosexual participants suggests that activity in the nucleus accumbens and the anterior cingulate cortex increases with increasing attractiveness The same study finds that for faces and bodies alike the medial part of the orbitofrontal cortex responds with greater activity to both very attractive and very unattractive pictures 292 Recent research has found that the perception of women with lower Waist to Hip Ratios and larger breasts triggers distinct patterns of temporal and spatial brain activation differing from those associated with larger WHRs and smaller breasts 293 Specifically attractive WHRs and breast sizes influenced brain activity related to visual processing differently than less attractive WHRs and breast sizes impacting both early and late stages of temporal processing in men and women 293 Moreover research on upper body size shows that posterior brain regions are involved in perceiving body forms of differing attractiveness due to variations male upper body size while frontal brain regions are engaged when these perceptions are explicitly rated 294 Such brain activities are unique to processing male but not female body sizes For both men and women there appear to be universal criteria of attractiveness both within and across cultures and ethnic groups 19 295 When considering long term relationships some studies have found that men place a higher emphasis on physical attractiveness in a partner than women do 296 297 298 299 300 On the other hand some studies have found few differences between men and women in terms of the weight they place on physical characteristics when they are choosing partners for short term relationships 301 302 303 304 in particular with regard to their implicit as opposed to explicitly articulated preferences 305 Other recent studies continue to find sex differences for long term relationships 306 307 308 303 While still valuing physical attractiveness women tend to prioritize a man s status over his physical attractiveness while men prioritize physical attractiveness over status 309 There is also one study suggesting that only men not women place greater priority on bodily compared to facial attractiveness when looking for a short term as compared to a long term partner 310 nbsp Bangladeshi bride exemplifying wedding day beautySome evolutionary psychologists including David Buss have argued that this long term relationship difference may be a consequence of ancestral humans who selected partners based on secondary sexual characteristics as well as general indicators of fitness which allowed for greater reproductive success as a result of higher fertility in those partners 311 although a male s ability to provide resources for offspring was likely signaled less by physical features 298 It is argued that the most prominent indicator of fertility in women is youth 312 313 314 while the traits in a man which enhance reproductive success are proxies for his ability to accrue resources and protect 314 Studies have shown that women pay greater attention to physical traits than they do directly to earning capability or potential to commit 315 including muscularity fitness and masculinity of features the latter preference was observed to vary during a woman s period with women preferring more masculine features during the late follicular fertile phase of the menstrual cycle 271 316 Additionally women process physical attractiveness differently paying attention to both individual features and the aesthetic effect of the whole face 317 According to Bonnie Adrian Taiwanese brides place great importance on physical attractiveness for their wedding photographs These brides go through hours of makeup to transform themselves into socially constructed beauty Adrian notes that female beauty standards and practices in Taiwan are quite different from those found in the West Women in Taiwan tend to avoid tanning while Taiwanese women with tanned skin tones would be considered the ideal in the Western world 318 According to strategic pluralism theory men may have correspondingly evolved to pursue reproductive strategies that are contingent on their own physical attractiveness More physically attractive men accrue reproductive benefits from spending more time seeking multiple mating partners and relatively less time investing in offspring In contrast the reproductive effort of physically less attractive men who therefore will not have the same mating opportunities is better allocated either to investing heavily in accruing resources or investing in their mates and offspring and spending relatively less time seeking additional mates 319 Facial similarity and racial preferences editSee also Race and sexuality Research has suggested that people give high attractiveness ratings to same sex people who look like them but only in a non sexual context On the other hand people generally do not find similar looking people of the opposite sex to be sexually attractive It is theorized that people may prefer sexual partners who look different from them which is consistent with a pattern of inbreeding avoidance 320 321 322 Although one study found that individuals show a preference for similar looking mates after a physiological stress test they preferred dis similar looking mates 323 324 One study found no strong evidence for a same race preference in heterosexual people and they note that evidence from two similar studies is conflicting Some racial groups show a strong preference for partners of a different race or ethnicity and this preference may be gendered 325 326 One study suggested that attraction to people with different ethnic features may be related to the effects of heterozygosity which are thought to be a fitness advantage 327 A 2014 study found that racial preferences in Asian Americans varied by gender while heterosexual Asian women preferred to date white men heterosexual Asian men preferred to date Asian women 328 A 2012 study suggests that imbalance in interracial marriage white male Asian female marriages are more than twice as common as the reverse may be attributed to the fact that Asian women are perceived as more feminine and therefore more attractive than white women 329 This study received support from a 2018 study which tested its hypothesis using images of Asian and white individuals 330 A 2012 study using Black and Caucasian subjects found that inter ethnic and intra ethnic differences in perceived male facial masculinity have no impact on attractiveness and that skin color plays a more important role in attractiveness assessments of male faces within groups but not between groups as a health signal 331 Group attractiveness effect editThe group attractiveness effect is where a group s overall attractiveness rating is higher than the mean of each individuals attractiveness rating This occurs because people selectively attend to the most attractive group members 332 and thus they get the most attention Men selectively attend to attractive people more than women it has been suggested that this could be because men are less invested in their offspring so they are less choosy when it comes to sexual partners and therefore they are more easily attracted As a result of selective attention people end up giving a group rating which is biased as the rating focuses mainly on the attractive members and ignores the less attractive members So the overall attractiveness rating is skewed towards more attractive The group attractiveness effect was investigated in a study which got participants to rate the physical attractiveness of a group of females as a whole individually in a group and individually on their own 333 Participants were asked to give a rating of 1 7 with 1 being very unattractive and 7 being very attractive The group attractiveness effect has been replicated with males as the main group and also with a group of both males and females This effect may not be demonstrated across all cultures because the sample only contained Dutch university students Social effects editPerceptions of physical attractiveness contribute to generalized assumptions based on those attractions Individuals assume that when someone is beautiful then they have many other positive attributes that make the attractive person more likeable 18 This is referred to as the halo effect also known as the beautiful is good effect 18 Across cultures what is beautiful is assumed to be good attractive people are assumed to be more extroverted popular and happy This could lead to a self fulfilling prophecy as from a young age attractive people receive more attention that helps them develop these characteristics 334 335 In one study beautiful people were found to be generally happier than less beautiful or plain people perhaps because these outgoing personality traits are linked to happiness or perhaps because beauty led to increased economic benefits which partially explained the increased happiness 187 In another study testing first impressions in 56 female and 17 male participants at University of British Columbia personality traits of physically attractive people were identified more positively and more accurately than those who were less physically attractive It was explained that people pay closer attention to those they find physically beautiful or attractive and thus perceiving attractive individuals with greater distinctive accuracy The study believes this accuracy to be subjective to the eye of the beholder 336 Recent results from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study confirmed the positive link between psychological well being and attractiveness higher facial attractiveness lower BMI and also found the complementary negative association with distress depression Even though connections and confounds with other variables could not be excluded the effects of attractiveness in this study were the same size as the ones for other demographic variables 337 In developed western societies women tend to be judged for their physical appearance over their other qualities and the pressure to engage in beauty work is much higher for women than men Beauty work is defined as various beauty practices individuals perform on themselves or others to elicit certain benefits from a specific social hierarchy 338 Being beautiful has individual social and institutional rewards 338 Although marketers have started to target the metro sexual male and produce hygiene and beauty products geared towards men the expectations placed on them is less than women 339 The time and money required for a man to achieve the same well groomed appearance is much lower Even in areas that men also face pressure to perform beauty work such as haircuts styling the prices discrepancy for products and services are skewed This phenomenon is called the pink tax 340 341 However attractiveness varies by society in ancient China foot binding was practiced by confining young girls feet in tightly bound shoes to prevent the feet from growing to normal size causing the women to have an attractive lotus gait In England women used to wear corsets that severely constricted their breathing in order to achieve a visual effect of an exaggeratedly low waist to hip ratio People make judgments of physical attractiveness based on what they see but also on what they know about the person Specifically perceptions of beauty are malleable such that information about the person s personality traits can influence one s assessment of another person s physical beauty A 2007 study had participants first rate pictures for attractiveness After doing distracting math problems participants saw the pictures again but with information about the person s personality When participants learned that a person had positive personality characteristics e g smart funny kind that person was seen as more physically attractive 342 Conversely a person with negative personality characteristics e g materialistic rude untrustworthy was seen as less physically attractive This was true for both females and males Physical attractiveness can have various social effects For instance humans tend to self organize into couples where both parties have loosely similar attractiveness levels as judged by third parties 343 344 345 A survey conducted by London Guildhall University of 11 000 people showed that those who subjectively describe themselves as physically attractive earn more income than others who would describe themselves as less attractive 26 People who described themselves as less attractive earned on average 13 less than those who described themselves as more attractive while the penalty for being overweight was around 5 According to further research done on the correlation between looks and earnings in men the punishment for unattractiveness is greater than the benefits of being attractive However in women the punishment is found to be equal to the benefits 346 Another study suggests that more physically attractive people are significantly more likely on average to earn considerably higher wages Differences in income due to attractiveness was much more pronounced for men rather than women and held true for all ranges of income 347 In the US the earnings disparities along the attractiveness continuum net of controls are similar or greater than the black white disparity 348 A study from 2020 found that social scientists who are judged as being more attractive receive higher public speaking fees than less attractive social scientists whereas for natural scientists relative unattractiveness is a comparative advantage in terms of public speaking fees 349 It is important to note that other factors such as self confidence may explain or influence these findings as they are based on self reported attractiveness as opposed to any sort of objective criteria however as one s self confidence and self esteem are largely learned from how one is regarded by their peers while maturing even these considerations would suggest a significant role for physical appearance One writer speculated that the distress created in women by the spread of unattainable ideals of female beauty might possibly be linked to increasing incidence of depression 350 Many have asserted that certain advantages tend to come to those who are perceived as being more attractive including the ability to get better jobs and promotions receiving better treatment from authorities and the legal system having more choices in romantic or platonic partners and therefore more power in relationships and marrying into families with more money 30 187 334 335 351 Those who are attractive are treated and judged more positively than those who are considered unattractive even by those who know them Also attractive individuals behave more positively than those who are unattractive 352 One study found that teachers tend to expect that children who are attractive are more intelligent and are more likely to progress further in school They also consider these students to be more popular 353 Voters choose political candidates who are more attractive over those who are less attractive 354 Men and women use physical attractiveness as a measure of how good another person is 355 In 1946 Soloman Asch coined the Implicit Personality Theory meaning that the presence of one trait tends to imply the existence of other traits This is also known as the halo effect Research suggests that those who are physically attractive are thought to have more socially desirable personalities and lead better lives in general 11 This is also known as the what is beautiful is good effect or physical attractiveness stereotype Discrimination against or prejudice towards others based on their appearance is sometimes referred to as lookism prejudice or discrimination based on physical appearance and especially physical appearance believed to fall short of societal notions of beauty 356 Some researchers conclude that little difference exists between men and women in terms of sexual behavior 297 357 Other researchers disagree 358 Symmetrically faced men and women have a tendency to begin to have sexual intercourse at an earlier age to have more sexual partners to engage in a wider variety of sexual activities and to have more one night stands They are also prone to infidelity and are more likely to have open relationships 46 Additionally they have the most reproductive success Therefore their physical characteristics are most likely to be inherited by future generations 359 360 361 362 Concern for improving physical attractiveness has led many persons to consider alternatives such as cosmetic surgery It has led scientists working with related disciplines such as computer imaging and mathematics to conduct research to suggest ways to surgically alter the distances between facial features in order to make a face conform more closely to the agreed upon standards of attractiveness of an ideal face by using algorithms to suggest an alternative which still resembles the current face 28 One research study found that cosmetic surgery as a way to boost earnings was not profitable in a monetary sense 187 Some research shows that physical attractiveness has a marginal effect on happiness 363 Misconceptions editThe golden ratio edit The golden ratio also known as the golden proportion was considered the perfect measurement of harmony beauty and proportion in Ancient Greece Researchers Mohammad Khursheed Alam Nor Farid Mohd Noor Rehana Basri Tan Fo Yew and Tay Hui Wen conducted a study to test if the golden ratio was a contributor to perceptions of facial attractiveness in various ethnic groups A total of 286 people aged 18 to 25 participated in the survey including 100 Malay 50 male 50 female 100 Malaysian Chinese 50 male 50 female and 86 Malaysian Indian 36 male 50 female This study excluded subjects of mixed race those with craniofacial deformities and those who had previously received orthodontic treatment or had face surgery The results showed that the golden ratio had no significant association with physical attractiveness 364 365 Ideal body shape for women edit This section possibly contains original research Please improve it by verifying the claims made and adding inline citations Statements consisting only of original research should be removed September 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message Some of this section s listed sources may not be reliable Please help improve this article by looking for better more reliable sources Unreliable citations may be challenged and removed September 2022 Learn how and when to remove this template message Some argue that body type preference is a mark of culture and regional beauty standards and that there is no definitive ideal body for women because it constantly shifts 366 367 368 Some authors argue that body types have never been universal and that most evolutionary psychology studies on the ideal female body shape have been questioned or disproven due to external factors such as unreliable data and idealized western gender roles 369 370 371 372 On the internet communities can create their own niche beauty standards that differ from the cultural norm 373 374 375 It has been argued that the perfect body is a matter of personal preference and exposure to regional media 376 377 See also editAdornment Accessory or ornament worn to enhance the beauty or status of the wearer Body proportions Proportions of the human body in art Artistic canons of body proportions Criteria used in formal figurative art Body shape General shape of a human body Male body shape Male musculo skeletal characteristics Female body shape Cumulative product of the human female skeletal structure and distribution of muscle and fat Circassian beauties Stereotypical beliefPages displaying short descriptions of redirect targets Cultural views on the navel Feminine beauty ideal Socially constructed norms The Honest Body Project Collection of pictures of real women Human physical appearance Look outward phenotype Koinophilia Hypothesis on normative mate selection Matching hypothesis Mate selection by social desirability Ovulatory shift hypothesis Hypothesis that female mating behavior changes throughout the menstrual cycle Sexual attraction Attraction on the basis of sexual desire Sexual capital Social value from sexual attractiveness Sexual fetishism Sexual arousal a person receives from an object or situationNotes edit However one expert suggested that her almost matronly representation was meant to convey an impressive appearance rather than ideal female beauty 4 The sitting body ratio SBR is also quoted where the trunk is measured with subject sitting on a flat table and the leg length determined by subtraction from standing height This is almost the same as the perineum to floor distance but without the need to touch an intimate area References edit People Just Deserts Time May 28 1945 Archived from the original on August 11 2009 Retrieved August 5 2011 the most perfect all over beauty of all time Runner up the Venus de Milo Says Venus de Milo was not a Flapper Osteopath Says She Was Neurasthenic as Her Stomach Was Not in Proper Place PDF The New York Times April 29 1922 Retrieved August 5 2011 Venus de Milo That lady of renowned beauty CBS News Staff August 5 2011 Venus CBS News Retrieved August 5 2011 The classical vision of beauty exemplified in Greek art such as the 2nd century B C Venus de Milo a k a Aphrodite of Milos was an ideal carried through millennia laying the basis for much of Western art s depictions of the human form Kousser R 2005 Creating the Past The Venus de Milo and the Hellenistic Reception of Classical Greece American Journal of Archaeology 109 2 227 50 doi 10 3764 aja 109 2 227 S2CID 36871977 Wilkinson P 1998 Illustrated Dictionary of Mythology DK Pub ISBN 9780789434135 Day J 2004 Does the Old Testament Refer to Sacred Prostitution and Did It Actual Exist in Ancient Israel In McCarthy C Healey JF eds Biblical and Near Eastern Essays Studies in Honour of Kevin J Cathcart Cromwell Press pp 2 21 ISBN 978 0 8264 6690 7 Singh NK 1997 Divine Prostitution New Delhi APH Publishing pp 4 6 ISBN 978 81 7024 821 7 Perkins D 2013 Encyclopedia of China History and Culture Routledge p 581 ISBN 978 1135935627 Grammer K Thornhill R October 1994 Human Homo sapiens facial attractiveness and sexual selection the role of symmetry and averageness Journal of Comparative Psychology 108 3 233 42 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 108 3 233 PMID 7924253 S2CID 1205083 Retrieved May 4 2019 Zeigler Hill V L M Welling L Shackelford TK 2015 Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology United States Springer Science Business Media p 329 ISBN 978 3 319 12697 5 why despite broad agreement we see a wide variety of personal preferences a b Dion Berscheid amp Walster 1972 Simpson Jeffry A 1990 Perception of physical attractiveness Mechanisms involved in the maintenance of romantic relationships Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 6 59 1192 1201 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 59 6 1192 Retrieved November 19 2022 a b Kanazawa S 2011 Intelligence and physical attractiveness Intelligence 39 1 7 14 doi 10 1016 j intell 2010 11 003 Stephen ID Tan KW 2015 Healthy body healthy face Evolutionary approaches to health perception In Sheppard E Haque S eds Culture and Cognition A Collection of Critical Essays Peter Lang International Publishers Brown WM Price ME Kang J Pound N Zhao Y Yu H September 2008 Fluctuating asymmetry and preferences for sex typical bodily characteristics Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105 35 12938 43 Bibcode 2008PNAS 10512938B doi 10 1073 pnas 0710420105 PMC 2529114 PMID 18711125 Bulczak Grzegorz Gugushvili Alexi March 17 2023 Physical attractiveness and cardiometabolic risk American Journal of Human Biology 35 8 e23895 doi 10 1002 ajhb 23895 ISSN 1042 0533 PMID 36932650 S2CID 257604055 Stephen ID Hiew V Coetzee V Tiddeman BP Perrett DI 2017 Facial Shape Analysis Identifies Valid Cues to Aspects of Physiological Health in Caucasian Asian and African Populations Frontiers in Psychology 8 1883 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2017 01883 PMC 5670498 PMID 29163270 a b c Barelds Dijkstra amp Barelds 2008 a b Briscoe J January 17 2004 Haven t I seen you somewhere before The Guardian London Retrieved July 15 2011 Evolutionary psychologists claim there is an underlying standard script for beauty a foundation for what we find appealing that transcends culture and ethnicity There are various absolutes For instance to judge someone beautiful the eye requires symmetry Fisher Maryanne L 2017 The Oxford Handbook of Women and Competition Oxford University Press p 603 ISBN 978 0 19 937639 1 a b Nettle D September 2002 Women s height reproductive success and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in modern humans PDF Proceedings Biological Sciences 269 1503 1919 23 doi 10 1098 rspb 2002 2111 PMC 1691114 PMID 12350254 a b c Glassenberg et al 2010 a b c Perrett et al 1998 Sell A Lukazsweski AW Townsley M December 2017 Cues of upper body strength account for most of the variance in men s bodily attractiveness Proceedings Biological Sciences 284 1869 20171819 doi 10 1098 rspb 2017 1819 PMC 5745404 PMID 29237852 Mautz BS Wong BB Peters RA Jennions MD April 2013 Penis size interacts with body shape and height to influence male attractiveness Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 110 17 6925 30 Bibcode 2013PNAS 110 6925M doi 10 1073 pnas 1219361110 PMC 3637716 PMID 23569234 a b Lorenz K 2005 Do Pretty People Earn More www CNN com Dammann G August 20 2008 Rules of attraction The Guardian London Retrieved July 15 2011 scientists from Brunel University have revealed that physical attraction is all down to bodily symmetry a b c Kershaw 2008 a b Berri DJ September 16 2008 Do Pretty Boy Quarterbacks Make More Money The New York Times Retrieved July 15 2011 Research though has indicated that what we think of as facial attractiveness is really just facial symmetry a b Willett E October 29 2008 A person s face can say a lot Helen s face is said to have launched a thousand ships while Medusa s could turn men to stone And even today we talk about individuals with a face that can stop a clock The Leader Post Regina Archived from the original on November 10 2012 Retrieved July 15 2011 people preferentially mate with date associate with employ and even vote for physically attractive individuals Symmetry is one trait we find attractive but only if the face is right side up your symmetric face will alas do nothing to help you attract a mate if you constantly stand on your head a b Murphy C December 4 2003 In the eye of the beholder BBC News Retrieved July 15 2011 Art historians anthropologists and human psychologists in general agree that it is the symmetry of a face its perfect proportion or indeed its averageness where no feature stands out that has consistently down the ages been deemed attractive Burkeman O April 24 2010 This column will change your life The beauty in imperfection The Guardian London Retrieved December 27 2012 Absolute flawlessness it s long been observed is disturbing It offers no point of connection and may help explain the uncanny valley effect where almost lifelike robots trigger revulsion in humans a b McKeen S February 10 2006 A beauty fix plumps up psyche and overall health The Edmonton Journal Archived from the original on November 10 2012 Retrieved July 15 2011 Evolution taught us to lust after symmetry a nicely balanced body and face because asymmetry signals past illness or injury We therefore define beauty quite elegantly right down to the most ideal ratio of hips to breasts and upper lip to lower lip Singh says one study showed that people were able to gauge beauty at a subliminal level when shown pictures for a mere one hundredth of a second Another study showed babies prefer pretty faces Perrin FA June 1921 Physical Attractiveness and Repulsiveness Journal of Experimental Psychology 4 3 203 17 doi 10 1037 h0071949 Grammer K Sainani KL October 2015 Q amp A Karl Grammer Innate attractions Nature 526 7572 S11 doi 10 1038 526S11a PMID 26444367 Brewis Alexandra A 2011 Obesity Cultural and Biocultural Perspectives New Brunswick London Rutgers University Press p 132 Sforza Chiarella Laino Alberto D Alessio Raoul Grandi Gaia Tartaglia Gianluca Martino Ferrario Virgilio Ferruccio 2008 Soft Tissue Facial Characteristics of Attractive and Normal Adolescent Boys and Girls PDF Angle Orthodontist 78 5 799 807 doi 10 2319 091207 431 1 PMID 18298221 Burriss Robert P Rowland Hannah M Little Anthony C January 1 2009 Facial scarring enhances men s attractiveness for short term relationships Personality and Individual Differences 46 2 213 17 doi 10 1016 j paid 2008 09 029 via www sciencedirect com a b Feng C December 6 2002 Looking Good The Psychology and Biology of Beauty Stanford University Archived from the original on January 13 2012 Retrieved January 20 2012 a b Penke L Bates TC Gow AJ Pattie A Starr JM Jones BC Perrett DI Deary IJ November 2009 Symmetric faces are a sign of successful cognitive aging Evolution and Human Behavior 30 6 429 37 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2009 06 001 Face shape clue to mental decline BBC News August 9 2009 Radford T August 17 2005 How women dream of symmetrical men The Guardian London Retrieved January 19 2010 The research once again confirms a hypothesis that beauty is not merely in the eye of the beholder it is an indicator of genetic fitness From a choice of computer generated faces volunteers routinely choose the most symmetrical as the most attractive Physical symmetry is interpreted as a sign of good inheritance And therefore the theory goes women in a position to conceive would be more attracted to someone more likely to engender the healthiest offspring Thornhill R Gangestad SW Comer R 1995 Human female orgasm and mate fluctuating asymmetry Animal Behaviour 50 6 1601 15 doi 10 1016 0003 3472 95 80014 X S2CID 44103857 Little AC Jones BC Waitt C Tiddeman BP Feinberg DR Perrett DI Apicella CL Marlowe FW May 2008 Reimchen T ed Symmetry is related to sexual dimorphism in faces data across culture and species PLOS ONE 3 5 e2106 Bibcode 2008PLoSO 3 2106L doi 10 1371 journal pone 0002106 PMC 2329856 PMID 18461131 Zebrowitz L Rhodes G 2004 Sensitivity to Bad Genes and the Anomalous Face Overgeneralization Effect Cue Validity Cute Utilization and Accuracy in Judging Intelligence and Health PDF Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 28 3 167 85 doi 10 1023 B JONB 0000039648 30935 1b S2CID 15981563 Archived from the original PDF on April 6 2019 Retrieved March 4 2012 a b Little AC Burt DM Penton Voak IS Perrett DI January 2001 Self perceived attractiveness influences human female preferences for sexual dimorphism and symmetry in male faces Proceedings Biological Sciences 268 1462 39 44 doi 10 1098 rspb 2000 1327 PMC 1087598 PMID 12123296 a b Etcoff N 2000 Survival of the Prettiest The Science of Beauty Knopf Doubleday Publishing pp 50 53 185 87 ISBN 9780385479424 Haselton MG Gangestad SW April 2006 Conditional expression of women s desires and men s mate guarding across the ovulatory cycle Hormones and Behavior 49 4 509 18 doi 10 1016 j yhbeh 2005 10 006 PMID 16403409 S2CID 7065777 Rikowski A Grammer K May 1999 Human body odour symmetry and attractiveness Proceedings Biological Sciences 266 1422 869 74 doi 10 1098 rspb 1999 0717 PMC 1689917 PMID 10380676 a b Gangestad SW Thornhill R May 1998 Menstrual cycle variation in women s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men Proceedings Biological Sciences 265 1399 927 33 doi 10 1098 rspb 1998 0380 PMC 1689051 PMID 9633114 Zuniga A Stevenson RJ Mahmut MK Stephen ID January 2017 Diet quality and the attractiveness of male body odor Evolution and Human Behavior 38 1 136 43 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2016 08 002 The laws of sexual attraction CNN April 13 2009 Archived from the original on March 9 2012 Retrieved July 25 2011 when women are ovulating they produce copulins a scent that attracts men a b Google Science Fair semi finalist I can taste your DNA The Guardian London July 25 2011 Retrieved July 25 2011 the Major Histocompatibility Complex MHC is a large gene family found in most vertebrates a b Khan R August 16 2008 Taking the pill might make your brother hawt Discover Magazine Retrieved July 25 2011 Previous studies in animals and humans show that genes in the major histocompatibility complex MHC influence individual odours and that females often prefer odour of MHC dissimilar males perhaps to increase offspring heterozygosity or reduce inbreeding Women using oral hormonal contraceptives have been reported to have the opposite preference raising the possibility that oral contraceptives alter female preference towards MHC similarity with possible fertility costs Roberts SC Little AC Gosling LM Perrett DI Carter V Jones BC Penton Voak I Petrie M May 2005 MHC heterozygosity and human facial attractiveness Evol Hum Behav 26 3 213 26 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2004 09 002 Penn DJ Damjanovich K Potts WK August 2002 MHC heterozygosity confers a selective advantage against multiple strain infections Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99 17 11260 64 Bibcode 2002PNAS 9911260P doi 10 1073 pnas 162006499 PMC 123244 PMID 12177415 Lewis MB January 1 2010 Why are mixed race people perceived as more attractive Perception 39 1 136 38 doi 10 1068 p6626 PMID 20301855 S2CID 15582617 The Biracial Advantage Psychology Today a b Rudder C February 16 2010 The Case For An Older Woman OkTrends Dating Research from OkCupid Retrieved April 28 2012 Antfolk J Salo B Alanko K Bergen E Corander J Sandnabba NK Santtila P 2015 Women s and men s sexual preferences and activities with respect to the partner s age evidence for female choice Evolution amp Human Behavior 36 1 73 79 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2014 09 003 a b Williams CA 1999 Roman homosexuality ideologies of masculinity in classical antiquity USA Oxford University Press Boyden T Carroll JS Maier RA 1984 Similarity and Attraction in Homosexual Males The Effects of Age and Masculinity Femininity Sex Roles 10 11 12 939 48 doi 10 1007 BF00288516 S2CID 145447590 Hechter M 2011 Social Norms Russell Sage Foundation p 300 Biello D December 5 2007 What is the Best Age Difference for Husband and Wife Scientific American a b c d e Buss 2003 pp 51 54 a b Reitman V April 26 2004 We clicked Los Angeles Times Retrieved April 3 2012 a b Thornhill R Gangestad SW December 1999 Facial attractiveness Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3 12 452 60 doi 10 1016 s1364 6613 99 01403 5 PMID 10562724 S2CID 961347 Young JA Critelli JW Keith KW 2005 Male age preferences for short term and long term mating Sexualities Evolution amp Gender 7 2 83 93 doi 10 1080 14616660500035090 Quinsey V L The Etiology of Anomalous Sexual Preferences in Men Queen s University Department of Psychology a b Attraction and Relationships The Journey from Initial Attachments to Romantic Love Rozenberg Quarterly rozenbergquarterly com Retrieved March 3 2016 a b Barber N 1995 The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness Sexual selection and human morphology Ethology and Sociobiology 16 5 395 424 doi 10 1016 0162 3095 95 00068 2 Van den Berghe PL Frost P 1986 Skin color preference sexual dimorphism and sexual selection a case of gene culture co evolution Ethnic and Racial Studies 9 87 118 doi 10 1080 01419870 1986 9993516 a b c Swami et al 2006 a b Thomas M M Versluys Robert A Foley William J Skylark May 16 2018 The influence of leg to body ratio arm to body ratio and intra limb ratio on male human attractiveness Royal Society Open Science The Royal Society Publishing 5 5 171790 Bibcode 2018RSOS 571790V doi 10 1098 rsos 171790 PMC 5990728 PMID 29892373 a b Kiire S 2016 Effect of Leg to Body Ratio on Body Shape Attractiveness Archives of Sexual Behavior 45 4 901 10 doi 10 1007 s10508 015 0635 9 PMID 26474977 S2CID 40574546 Sorokowskia P Pawlowskib B March 2008 Adaptive preferences for leg length in a potential partner Evol Hum Behav 29 2 86 91 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2007 09 002 Sorokowski P 2010 Attractiveness of Legs Length in Poland and Great Britain PDF J Hum Ecol 31 3 148 Frederick DA Hadji Michael M Furnham A Swami V January 2010 The influence of leg to body ratio LBR on judgments of female physical attractiveness assessments of computer generated images varying in LBR Body Image 7 1 51 55 doi 10 1016 j bodyim 2009 09 001 PMID 19822462 a b c Bertamini amp Bennet 2009 p 235 a b Bertamini amp Bennet 2009 pp 245 246 Drummond MJ Filiault SM 2007 The long and the short of it Gay men s perceptions of penis size Gay amp Lesbian Issues and Psychology Review 3 2 121 29 Rettner R June 5 2014 For One Night Stands Girth Matters LiveScience Retrieved December 15 2014 Sanderson S K 2001 The Evolution of Human Sociality A Darwinian Conflict Perspective US Rowman amp Littlefield Publishers Inc p 179 ISBN 0 8476 9534 4 alk paper Spielmann MH 1889 The Magazine of Art London Paris New York Melbourne Cassell and Company Limited Jones Trina 2000 Shades of Brown The Law of Skin Color Duke Law School Stephen ID Law Smith MJ Stirrat MR Perrett DI December 2009 Facial Skin Coloration Affects Perceived Health of Human Faces International Journal of Primatology 30 6 845 57 doi 10 1007 s10764 009 9380 z PMC 2780675 PMID 19946602 Stephen ID Coetzee V Law Smith M Perrett DI 2009 Skin blood perfusion and oxygenation colour affect perceived human health PLOS ONE 4 4 e5083 Bibcode 2009PLoSO 4 5083S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0005083 PMC 2659803 PMID 19337378 Stephen I Coetzee V Perrett DI 2011 Carotenoid and melanin pigment coloration affect perceived human health Evolution and Human Behavior 32 3 216 27 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2010 09 003 Tybur JM Gangestad SW December 2011 Mate preferences and infectious disease theoretical considerations and evidence in humans Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 366 1583 3375 88 doi 10 1098 rstb 2011 0136 PMC 3189358 PMID 22042915 Tybur amp Gangestad 2011 Indeed at the current time little direct evidence addresses whether skin tone or colour reflects current infection levels or infectability More research is clearly needed a b c d e Cunningham et al 1995 Van den Berghe PL Frost P September 13 2010 Skin color preference sexual dimorphism and sexual selection A case of gene culture co evolution Ethnic and Racial Studies 9 87 113 doi 10 1080 01419870 1986 9993516 an overwhelming cross cultural preference for lighter skin Polovick Madeline 2017 Orange Is the New Black The Skin Tanning Phenomenon and Its Influence on Perceptions of Race Class and Gender Thesis Senior Independent Study Theses Retrieved January 31 2023 Polovick 2017 p 14 The researchers discovered that their findings did not support Van den Berghe and Frost s hypothesis that men were more attracted to lighter skinned women especially as linked to a sign of fertility because their data showed a preference for women with darker skin measured by color parameters that indicated a skin value based on blue and green in comparison to red components Polovick 2017 p 12 13 They chose to use tanned women as their research model because women have higher rates of tanning than men Robinson et al 1997b and therefore are more likely to utilize tanned skin as an attractive trait Their hypothesis was that both women and men would find a tanned female to be more physically and interpersonally attractive healthier taller and thinner than a less tanned female Only the men s responses were statistically significant demonstrating an overall preference for the dark tanned female over then medium tanned Banerjee Campo and Greene found that their results contradict previous research that has shown a preference for medium tanned skin over dark tanned skin Broadstock et al 1992 They also found that the men viewed the darker tanned women as thinner They reason that women might tan more often than men because they believe men that find them more attractive that way which these perceptions of women by men were proven most likely accurate with their study Hurkman Alexis Van November 15 2010 The Color Correction Handbook Professional Techniques for Video and Cinema Pearson Education p 30 IA5 ISBN 978 0 321 71974 4 Smith Cornelissen and Tovee found that contrary to previous cross cultural studies on the topic the observers showed a marked preference for the darker complexioned tanned volunteers Dixson BJ Dixson AF Bishop PJ Parish A June 2010 Human physique and sexual attractiveness in men and women a New Zealand U S comparative study Archives of Sexual Behavior 39 3 804 doi 10 1007 s10508 008 9441 y PMID 19139985 S2CID 33112678 In the current study men rated images with lighter skin tones as most attractive especially so in the Californian sample In New Zealand men rated the female images of average skin color as most attractive followed by the image which was lightened by 10 units of brightness and 15 units of contrast Bulmer Martin Solomos John October 2 2017 Race Migration and Identity Routledge p 229 ISBN 978 1 317 51969 0 For example in a recent study examining Latina female college students who identified as white pale skin was viewed as unattractive compared to tanned skin Stephens and Fernandez 2012 Furthermore having the ideal tan skin colour was associated with being more desirable in peer and dating contexts more sexually appealing to men For example in a study of African American male and female college students Coard Breland and Raskin found that participants preferred a medium skin tone over a lighter or darker skin tone Verma N M P Srivastava Alpana August 20 2020 The Routledge Handbook of Exclusion Inequality and Stigma in India Taylor amp Francis p 293 ISBN 978 1 000 09669 9 A few studies have found that tanned skin is regarded as both more attractive and healthier than pale or very dark skin and there is a direct correlation between the degree of tanning and perceived attractiveness especially among young women Kleisner K Kocnar T Turecek P Stella D Akoko RM Trebicky V Havlicek J 2017 African and European perception of African female attractiveness Evolution and Human Behavior 38 6 744 55 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2017 07 002 S2CID 85453582 Singer M Beyer H 2008 Killer Commodities Public Health and the Corporate Production of Harm AltaMira Press p 151 ISBN 978 0 7591 0979 7 Harris investigated the history of the parasol everywhere ordinary people were forbidden to protect themselves with such devices pallid skin became a marker of upper class status At the beginning of the 20th century in the United States lighter skinned people avoided the sun Tanned skin was considered lower class Geller AC Colditz G Oliveria S Emmons K Jorgensen C Aweh GN Frazier AL June 2002 Use of sunscreen sunburning rates and tanning bed use among more than 10 000 US children and adolescents Pediatrics 109 6 1009 14 doi 10 1542 peds 109 6 1009 PMID 12042536 Broadstock M Borland R Gason R January 1992 Effects of Suntan on Judgements of Healthiness and Attractiveness by Adolescents J Appl Soc Psychol 22 2 157 72 doi 10 1111 j 1559 1816 1992 tb01527 x Leary MR Jones JL September 1993 The Social Psychology of Tanning and Sunscreen Use Self Presentational Motives as a Predictor of Health Risk J Appl Soc Psychol 23 17 1390 1406 doi 10 1111 j 1559 1816 1993 tb01039 x Tan is In Study Finds Light Brown More Attractive than Pale or Dark Skin physorg com Retrieved June 14 2015 The Heavy Cost of Light Skin BBC News April 18 2000 Retrieved August 9 2010 Fink B Grammer K Thornhill R March 2001 Human Homo sapiens facial attractiveness in relation to skin texture and color Journal of Comparative Psychology 115 1 92 99 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 115 1 92 PMID 11334223 Fink B Matts PJ April 2008 The effects of skin colour distribution and topography cues on the perception of female facial age and health Journal of the European Academy of Dermatology and Venereology 22 4 493 98 doi 10 1111 j 1468 3083 2007 02512 x PMID 18081752 S2CID 12964209 a b Koscinski Krzysztof May 13 2011 Determinants of hand attractiveness a study involving digitally manipulated stimuli Perception 40 6 682 94 doi 10 1068 p6960 PMID 21936297 S2CID 25042722 via Sage Journals a b Saino Nicola Romano Maria Innocenti Paolo July 2006 Length of Index and Ring Fingers Differentially Influence Sexual Attractiveness of Men s and Women s Hands Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 60 3 447 54 doi 10 1007 s00265 006 0185 1 JSTOR 25063833 S2CID 25043274 Manning J T Scutt D Wilson J Lewis Jones D I November 1 1998 The ratio of 2nd to 4th digit length a predictor of sperm numbers and concentrations of testosterone luteinizing hormone and oestrogen Human Reproduction 13 11 3000 04 doi 10 1093 humrep 13 11 3000 PMID 9853845 via Oxford Academic Koscinski Krzysztof March April 2012 Hand attractiveness its determinants and associations with facial attractiveness Behavioral Ecology 23 2 334 42 doi 10 1093 beheco arr190 via Oxford academic Pierce C A 1996 Cunningham M R 1990 Pawlowski B Dunbar RI Lipowicz A 2000 a b Sear amp Marlowe 2009 Courtiol A Raymond M Godelle B Ferdy JB August 2010 Mate choice and human stature homogamy as a unified framework for understanding mating preferences Evolution International Journal of Organic Evolution 64 8 2189 203 doi 10 1111 j 1558 5646 2010 00985 x PMID 20199563 S2CID 4780206 a b Tall men top husband stakes BBC News August 14 2002 Retrieved October 15 2009 Stulp G Buunk A Kurzban R 2013 The height of choosiness mutual mate choice for stature results in suboptimal pair formation for both sexes Animal Behaviour 86 1 37 46 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2013 03 038 hdl 11370 66f4623c 20b8 46a0 9542 04c78c7ec6b5 S2CID 53171152 Buss 2003 pp 38 40 a b Yee N 2002 Beyond Tops and Bottoms Correlations between Sex Role Preference and Physical Preferences for Partners among Gay Men Curry 1916 pp 105 107 a b c d e Sear 2006 Berger John 1973 Ways of Seeing British Broadcasting Corporation and Penguin Books pp 44 64 Curry 1916 p 101 Do I have what it takes to be a model British Fashion Model Agents Association Pazhoohi Farid Macedo J Antonio F Doyle ames F Arantes Joana 2020 Waist to Hip Ratio as Supernormal Stimuli Effect of Contrapposto Pose and Viewing Angle Archives of Sexual Behavior 49 3 837 847 doi 10 1007 s10508 019 01486 z PMID 31214904 S2CID 195066235 Retrieved May 31 2022 Jacobs O Pazhoohi F Kingstone A 2023 Contrapposto posture captures visual attention An online gaze tracking experiment Visual Cognition 31 2 160 167 doi 10 1080 13506285 2023 2213904 S2CID 258852464 Pazhoohi F Arantes J Kingstone A Pinal D 2020 Contrapposto pose increases attractiveness ratings and modulates observers brain activity Biological Psychology 151 107842 doi 10 1016 j biopsycho 2020 107842 PMID 31958547 S2CID 210678442 a b c Sugiyama 2005 Johnson KL Tassinary LG March 2007 Compatibility of basic social perceptions determines perceived attractiveness Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 104 12 5246 51 Bibcode 2007PNAS 104 5246J doi 10 1073 pnas 0608181104 PMC 1829294 PMID 17360395 Clues To Mysteries Of Physical Attractiveness Revealed ScienceDaily Press release May 24 2007 Buss D M amp Barnes M 1986 Preferences in human mate selection Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50 3 559 Frederick D A amp Haselton M G 2007 Why is muscularity sexy Tests of the fitness indicator hypothesis Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin Bailey amp Gaulin amp Agyei amp Gladue J M amp S amp Y amp B A 1994 Effects of gender and sexual orientation on evolutionary relevant aspects of human mating psychology Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66 6 1081 93 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 66 6 1081 PMID 8046578 Penton Voak IS Perrett DI January 2000 Female preference for male faces changes cyclically Further evidence Evol Hum Behav 21 1 39 48 doi 10 1016 S1090 5138 99 00033 1 Rhodes G 2006 The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty Annual Review of Psychology 57 1 199 226 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 57 102904 190208 PMID 16318594 Women s choice of men goes in cycles BBC News June 24 1999 Retrieved November 4 2016 Dawkins R 1999 The selfish gene Oxford Oxford Univ Press ISBN 978 0 19 286092 7 Kruger DJ Fisher M Jobling I September 2003 Proper and dark heroes as DADS and CADS Alternative mating strategies in British Romantic literature Human Nature 14 3 305 17 doi 10 1007 s12110 003 1008 y PMID 26190212 S2CID 45846130 Wang H Hahn AC Fisher CI DeBruine LM Jones BC December 2014 Women s hormone levels modulate the motivational salience of facial attractiveness and sexual dimorphism PDF Psychoneuroendocrinology 50 246 51 doi 10 1016 j psyneuen 2014 08 022 PMID 25244638 S2CID 1075853 Fink B Neave N Seydel H 2007 Male facial appearance signals physical strength to women American Journal of Human Biology 19 1 82 87 doi 10 1002 ajhb 20583 PMID 17160983 S2CID 31618973 Rhodes G Chan J Zebrowitz LA Simmons LW August 2003 Does sexual dimorphism in human faces signal health Proceedings Biological Sciences 270 Suppl 1 S93 95 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2003 0023 PMC 1698019 PMID 12952647 Cellerino A 2003 Psychobiology of facial attractiveness Journal of Endocrinological Investigation 26 3 Suppl 45 48 PMID 12834020 Gangestad SW Thornhill R July 2003 Facial masculinity and fluctuating asymmetry Evol Hum Behav 24 4 231 41 doi 10 1016 S1090 5138 03 00017 5 Scott IM Pound N Stephen ID Clark AP Penton Voak IS October 2010 Does masculinity matter The contribution of masculine face shape to male attractiveness in humans PLOS ONE 5 10 e13585 Bibcode 2010PLoSO 513585S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0013585 PMC 2965103 PMID 21048972 DeBruine LM Jones BC Crawford JR Welling LL Little AC August 2010 The health of a nation predicts their mate preferences cross cultural variation in women s preferences for masculinized male faces Proceedings Biological Sciences 277 1692 2405 10 doi 10 1098 rspb 2009 2184 PMC 2894896 PMID 20236978 Wen Yi Feng Wong Hai Ming Lin Ruitao Yin Guosheng McGrath Colman August 6 2015 Inter Ethnic Racial Facial Variations A Systematic Review and Bayesian Meta Analysis of Photogrammetric Studies PLOS ONE 10 8 e0134525 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1034525W doi 10 1371 journal pone 0134525 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 4527668 PMID 26247212 Lee AJ Mitchem DG Wright MJ Martin NG Keller MC Zietsch BP February 2014 Genetic factors that increase male facial masculinity decrease facial attractiveness of female relatives Psychological Science 25 2 476 84 doi 10 1177 0956797613510724 PMC 4205959 PMID 24379153 Zheng L Hart TA Zheng Y October 2013 Attraction to male facial masculinity in gay men in China relationship to intercourse preference positions and sociosexual behavior Archives of Sexual Behavior 42 7 1223 32 doi 10 1007 s10508 012 0057 x PMID 23440561 S2CID 207090415 a b Song G Hird D 2013 Men and Masculinities in Contemporary China Brill Publishers p 92 ISBN 978 90 04 26491 5 Song G 2004 The Fragile Scholar Power and Masculinity in Chinese Culture Hong Kong University Press p 126 ISBN 978 962 209 620 2 Curry 1916 p 75 Horvath T February 1981 Physical attractiveness the influence of selected torso parameters Archives of Sexual Behavior 10 1 21 24 doi 10 1007 BF01542671 PMID 7212994 S2CID 7439609 a b Braun MF Bryan A 2006 Female waist to hip and male waist to shoulder ratios as determinants of romantic partner desirability Journal of Social and Personal Relationships 23 5 805 19 doi 10 1177 0265407506068264 S2CID 2940276 Swami V Tovee MJ 2008 The Muscular Male A Comparison of the Physical Attractiveness Preferences of Gay and Heterosexual Men International Journal of Men s Health 7 1 59 71 doi 10 3149 jmh 0701 59 Fan J Dai W Liu F Wu J February 2005 Visual perception of male body attractiveness Proceedings Biological Sciences 272 1560 219 26 doi 10 1098 rspb 2004 2922 PMC 1634963 PMID 15705545 Buss David October 2 2015 Evolutionary Psychology The New Science of the Mind Psychology Press p 168 ISBN 978 1 317 34573 2 a b Catikkas F 2011 Physical correlates of college students body image satisfaction levels Social Behavior and Personality 39 4 497 502 doi 10 2224 sbp 2011 39 4 497 Furnham A Tan T McManus C 1997 Waist to hip ratio and preferences for body shape A replication and extension PDF Personality and Individual Differences 22 4 539 49 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 96 00241 3 Archived from the original PDF on March 28 2016 Swami V Tovee MJ December 2005 Male physical attractiveness in Britain and Malaysia A cross cultural study Body Image 2 4 383 93 doi 10 1016 j bodyim 2005 08 001 PMID 18089203 Singh D 1995 Female judgment of male attractiveness and desirability for relationships Role of waist to hip ratio and financial status Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69 6 1089 1101 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 69 6 1089 PMID 8531056 Dececco J Wright L 2013 The Bear Book Readings in the History and Evolution of a Gay Male Subculture New York Routledge p 120 ISBN 978 1 56023 890 4 Curry 1916 p 116 a b Frederick DA Fessler DM Haselton MG March 2005 Do representations of male muscularity differ in men s and women s magazines Body Image 2 1 81 86 doi 10 1016 j bodyim 2004 12 002 PMID 18089177 S2CID 11139349 a b Johnston JR 2001 The American body in context An anthology US Scholarly Resources Inc a b Brierley ME Brooks KR Mond J Stevenson RJ Stephen ID June 3 2016 The Body and the Beautiful Health Attractiveness and Body Composition in Men s and Women s Bodies PLOS ONE 11 6 e0156722 Bibcode 2016PLoSO 1156722B doi 10 1371 journal pone 0156722 PMC 4892674 PMID 27257677 Latinsky A 2012 Public presentation of gendered bodies A look at gay and lesbian online dating profiles Sociation Today 10 2 Curry 1916 pp 112 115 118 123 Dixson BJ Dixson AF Bishop PJ Parish A June 2010 Human physique and sexual attractiveness in men and women a New Zealand U S comparative study Archives of Sexual Behavior 39 3 798 806 doi 10 1007 s10508 008 9441 y PMID 19139985 S2CID 33112678 Dixson BJ Dixson AF Li B Anderson MJ 2007 Studies of human physique and sexual attractiveness sexual preferences of men and women in China American Journal of Human Biology 19 1 88 95 doi 10 1002 ajhb 20584 PMID 17160976 S2CID 8868828 a b Dixson AF Halliwell G East R Wignarajah P Anderson MJ February 2003 Masculine somatotype and hirsuteness as determinants of sexual attractiveness to women Archives of Sexual Behavior 32 1 29 39 doi 10 1023 A 1021889228469 PMID 12597270 S2CID 10169455 Rantala MJ Polkki M Rantala LM 2010 Preference for human male body hair changes across the menstrual cycle and menopause Behavioral Ecology 21 2 419 23 doi 10 1093 beheco arp206 J W Dixson Barnaby J Rantala Markus 2016 The Role of Facial and Body Hair Distribution in Women s Judgments of Men s Sexual Attractiveness Archives of Sexual Behavior 45 4 877 89 doi 10 1007 s10508 015 0588 z PMID 26292838 S2CID 19988758 via Springer link J Dixon Barnaby L Vasey Paul January 13 2012 Beards augment perceptions of men s age social status and aggressiveness but not attractiveness Behavioral Ecology 23 3 481 90 doi 10 1093 beheco arr214 hdl 10 1093 beheco arr214 via Oxford academic Holton N E Bonner L L Scott J E Marshall S D Franciscus R G Southard T E June 2015 The ontogeny of the chin an analysis of allometric and biomechanical scaling Journal of Anatomy 226 6 549 559 doi 10 1111 joa 12307 ISSN 0021 8782 PMC 4450959 PMID 25865897 Mommaerts Maurice Y 2016 The ideal male jaw angle An Internet survey Journal of Cranio Maxillofacial Surgery 44 4 381 391 doi 10 1016 j jcms 2015 12 012 ISSN 1010 5182 PMID 26888465 Wen Y F Wong H M Lin R Yin G McGrath C 2015 Inter Ethnic Racial Facial Variations A Systematic Review and Bayesian Meta Analysis of Photogrammetric Studies PLOS ONE 10 8 e0134525 Bibcode 2015PLoSO 1034525W doi 10 1371 journal pone 0134525 PMC 4527668 PMID 26247212 Rhee S C 2018 Differences between Caucasian and Asian attractive faces Skin Research and Technology 24 1 73 79 doi 10 1111 srt 12392 PMID 28722243 S2CID 24279633 Naini Farhad B Cobourne Martyn T McDonald Fraser Wertheim David December 2016 Submental Cervical Angle Perceived Attractiveness and Threshold Values of Desire for Surgery Journal of Maxillofacial and Oral Surgery 15 4 469 477 doi 10 1007 s12663 015 0872 4 ISSN 0972 8279 PMC 5083688 PMID 27833339 Browne KR 2006 Sex Power and Dominance The Evolutionary Psychology of Sexual Harassment Managerial and Decision Economics 27 2 3 145 58 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 484 5566 doi 10 1002 mde 1289 a b Tattersall I June 11 2000 Whatever Turns You On A psychologist looks at sexual attraction and what it means for humankind Geoffrey Miller The New York Times Book Review Retrieved July 15 2011 it turns out that symmetry of bodily structure is a fitness indicator and symmetry is more easily detectable among large breasts than small ones Jackson LB 1992 Physical appearance and gender sociobiological and sociocultural perspectives State University of New York Press Miller L 2006 Beauty Up Exploring Contemporary Japanese Body Aesthetics Berkeley California University of California Press p 28 ISBN 978 0 520 24508 2 A Woman s Face is Her Fortune advertisement The Helena Independent November 9 2000 p 7 Little Becky September 22 2016 Arsenic Pills and Lead Foundation The History of Toxic Makeup National Geographic Archived from the original on November 5 2018 a b Berscheid and Reis 1998 Fink B Penton Voak IS 2002 Evolutionary Psychology of Facial Attractiveness Current Directions in Psychological Science 11 5 154 58 doi 10 1111 1467 8721 00190 S2CID 10217832 Brizendine Louann 2006 The female brain Random House Digital Inc p 63 ISBN 978 0 7679 2010 0 Van Meter J August 11 2008 About Face NY Mag Retrieved July 30 2012 a b c d Jayson 2011 How Big Is Your Limbal Ring Psychology Today Retrieved June 14 2015 a b c d Milani 1992 p 187 a b c d e f Lane 1883 pp 214 216 a b c Brayer M M 1986 The Jewish Woman in Rabbinic Literature A psychological perspective Hoboken New Jersey Ktav Publishing House Inc ISBN 0 88125 071 6 p 214 a b c Kyo 2012 p 6 Jones D Brace CL Jankowiak W Laland KN Musselman LE Langlois JH Roggman LA Perusse D Schweder B Symons D December 1995 Sexual selection physical attractiveness and facial neoteny cross cultural evidence and implications Current Anthropology 36 5 723 48 doi 10 1086 204427 S2CID 52840802 Sforza C Laino A D Alessio R Grandi G Binelli M Ferrario VF January 2009 Soft tissue facial characteristics of attractive Italian women as compared to normal women The Angle Orthodontist 79 1 17 23 doi 10 2319 122707 605 1 PMID 19123721 Marcinkowska UM Kozlov MV Cai H Contreras Garduno J Dixson BJ Oana GA et al April 2014 Cross cultural variation in men s preference for sexual dimorphism in women s faces Biology Letters 10 4 20130850 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2013 0850 PMC 4013689 PMID 24789138 Cunningham et al 1995 p 267 Asian Hispanic and White participants were similar in giving higher ratings to targets with the neonate features of large eyes greater distance between the eyes and a smaller nose area see Table 2 Women with the maturity qualities of higher cheek bones and narrower faces and low levels of the male maturity cues of large chins and thick eyebrows were judged more positively See table 1 Study 1 All groups of judges made more positive ratings of the Asian and Hispanic targets compared with the Black and White targets Further analyses indicated that the Asian and Hispanic targets happened to possess significantly larger eye height eye width nose width eyebrow height smile width and upper lip width than the White and Black women Because the targets were chosen for their availability rather than randomly selected from their populations and the absolute number of targets in each group was small it would be incorrect to conclude that any ethnic group was more attractive than any other Cunningham et al 1995 p 271 The four item measure of exposure to Western culture was not reliably associated with giving higher ratings to Whites r 19 n s The relation of rating Whites to frequency of viewing Western television for example was quite low r 01 Cunningham MR May 1986 Measuring the Physical in Physical Attractiveness Quasi Experiments on the Sociobiology of Female Facial Beauty Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50 5 925 35 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 50 5 925 Research with Western subjects disclosed significant consistency in evaluating attractiveness Hatfield amp Sprecher 1986 Iliife 1960 The females judged to be most attractive may have such similar facial features that they were hard to distinguish one from another Light Hollander amp Kayra Stuart 1981 Cross cultural investigations on the judgment of facial attractiveness tended to highlight societal differences but rough agreements in facial aesthetic preferences were shown by Asian American and white females Wagatsuma amp Kleinke 1979 Chinese Indian and English females judging Greek males Thakerar amp Iwawaki 1979 South African and American males and females Morse Gruzen amp Reis 1976 and blacks and whites judging males and females from both races Cross amp Cross 1971 Buss 2003 pp 54 55 Berger M 1999 White lies race and the myths of whiteness Canada Farrar Strous and Giroux ISBN 978 0 374 52715 0 Liu J 2008 Sexualized Labour White collar Beauties in Provincial China In Jackson S Liu J Woo J eds East Asian Sexualities Modernity Gender and New Sexual Cultures London Zed Books pp 85 103 ISBN 978 0 374 52715 0 Honekopp J Bartholome T amp Jansen G 2004 Facial Attractiveness Symmetry and Physical Fitness in Young Women Human Nature 15 2 147 167 Perfect face dimensions measured BBC News December 18 2009 Retrieved May 22 2010 Wilkins CL Chan JF Kaiser CR October 2011 Racial stereotypes and interracial attraction phenotypic prototypicality and perceived attractiveness of Asians Cultural Diversity amp Ethnic Minority Psychology 17 4 427 31 doi 10 1037 a0024733 PMID 21988581 see bottom left of p 43 a b Wilkes J ed 1823 Encyclopaedia Londinensis or Universal Dictionary of Arts Science and Literature Vol 19 John Wilkes p 173 Howard N 1830 On Persian Poetry Plymouth p 30 Kyo 2012 Buckley Sandra January 1 2009 The Encyclopedia of Contemporary Japanese Culture Taylor amp Francis ISBN 9780415481526 Bonds Alexandra B January 1 2008 Beijing Opera Costumes The Visual Communication of Character and Culture University of Hawaii Press ISBN 9780824829568 Soh J Chew MT Wong HB February 2007 An Asian community s perspective on facial profile attractiveness Community Dentistry and Oral Epidemiology 35 1 18 24 doi 10 1111 j 1600 0528 2007 00304 x PMID 17244134 Culture of Iran Cosmetics Styles amp Beauty Concepts in Iran www iranchamber com Retrieved March 28 2016 Hwang HS Spiegel JH March 2014 The effect of single vs double eyelids on the perceived attractiveness of Chinese women Aesthetic Surgery Journal 34 3 374 82 doi 10 1177 1090820X14523020 PMID 24604790 Gill R D 2004 Topsy turvy 1585 A translation and explication of Luis Frois S J s Tratado treatise listing 611 ways Europeans amp Japanese are contrary Paraverse Press pp 57 58 ISBN 0 9742618 1 5 ISBN 978 0 9742618 1 2 Elia IE September 2013 A foxy view of human beauty implications of the farm fox experiment for understanding the origins of structural and experiential aspects of facial attractiveness The Quarterly Review of Biology 88 3 163 83 doi 10 1086 671486 PMID 24053070 S2CID 22293068 a b c Kyo 2012 p 15 a b c Kyo 2012 p 19 Kyo 2012 p 18 Pazhoohi F Garza R Kingstone A 2020 Effects of Breast Size Intermammary Cleft Distance Cleavage and Ptosis on Perceived Attractiveness Health Fertility and Age Do Life History Self Perceived Mate Value and Sexism Attitude Play a Role Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 6 75 92 doi 10 1007 s40750 020 00129 1 S2CID 256429772 Buss 2005 p 325 a b Furnham A Swami V 2007 Perception of female buttocks and breast size in profile Soc Behav Pers 35 1 1 8 doi 10 2224 sbp 2007 35 1 1 Hourglass figure fertility link bbc co uk May 4 2004 Retrieved June 14 2015 Groyecka A Zelazniewicz A Misiak M Karwowski M Sorokowski P July 2017 Breast shape ptosis as a marker of a woman s breast attractiveness and age Evidence from Poland and Papua American Journal of Human Biology 29 4 e22981 doi 10 1002 ajhb 22981 PMID 28211217 S2CID 4576540 Rodgers JE 2003 Sex A Natural History Macmillan p 102 ISBN 978 0 8050 7281 5 Wade T J 2010 The Relationships between Symmetry and Attractiveness and Mating Relevant Decisions and Behavior A Review Symmetry 2 2 1081 1098 Bibcode 2010Symm 2 1081W doi 10 3390 sym2021081 Pal P 1986 Indian Sculpture Volume 1 circa 500 B C A D 700 A catalogue of the Los Angeles County Museum of Art Collection Berkeley Los Angeles amp London University of California Press p 32 ISBN 9780520059917 Curry 1916 p 113 Fisher HE 1982 The Sex Contract The Evolution of Human Behavior New York William Morrow amp Company Inc ISBN 9780688006402 Hawkins E October 25 2017 Why arched backs are attractive springer com Pazhoohi F Doyle JF Macedo AF Arantes J 2017 Arching the Back Lumbar Curvature as a Female Sexual Proceptivity Signal an Eye Tracking Study Evolutionary Psychological Science 4 2 1 8 doi 10 1007 s40806 017 0123 7 S2CID 149046079 a b Caro TM Sellen DW 1990 The Reproductive Advantages of Fat in Women PDF Ethology and Sociobiology 11 5 1 66 doi 10 1016 0162 3095 90 90005 Q hdl 2027 42 28785 Hebl MR Heatherton TF 1997 The Stigma of Obesity in Women The Difference is Black and White Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 24 4 418 doi 10 1177 0146167298244008 S2CID 143697452 Swami V Jones J Einon D Furnham A May 2009 Men s preferences for women s profile waist to hip ratio breast size and ethnic group in Britain and South Africa British Journal of Psychology 100 Pt 2 313 25 doi 10 1348 000712608x329525 PMID 18625082 Tovee MJ Reinhardt S Emery JL Cornelissen PL August 1998 Optimum body mass index and maximum sexual attractiveness Lancet 352 9127 548 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 05 79257 6 PMID 9716069 S2CID 35886360 a b c Buss 2003 pp 55 56 Furnham Adrian Gianna Caroline Fischer Lauren Tanner Melanie Dias and Alastair McClelland 1998 Brown PJ Sweeney J 2009 The Anthropology of Overweight Obesity and the Body AnthroNotes 30 1 Nettle D November 2009 Ecological influences on human behavioural diversity a review of recent findings Trends in Ecology amp Evolution 24 11 618 24 doi 10 1016 j tree 2009 05 013 PMID 19683831 Germov John Williams Lauren 1999 Weighty Issues Fatness and Thinness As Social Problems Social Problems and Social Issues Transaction Publishers p 125 127 ISBN 978 1 4128 4126 9 Anderson L 2017 Deviance Social Constructions and Blurred Boundaries University of California Press p 390 ISBN 978 0 520 29237 6 Hellmich N September 26 2006 Do thin models warp girls body image USA Today Retrieved November 6 2009 The widespread concern that model thinness has progressed from willowy to wasted has reached a threshold as evidenced by the recent actions of fashion show organizers Tierney J January 18 2007 The Waif From Ipanema The New York Times Archived from the original on January 21 2007 Retrieved November 6 2009 women s aesthetic judgments are so influenced by other women Men prefer the wider hips and most likely could sic care less about high heels and handbags Yet for many women all these things are essential to marking their beauty status with other women unpublished OpEd Witcomb GL Arcelus J Chen J December 2013 Can cognitive dissonance methods developed in the West for combatting the thin ideal help slow the rapidly increasing prevalence of eating disorders in non Western cultures Shanghai Archives of Psychiatry 25 6 332 40 doi 10 3969 j issn 1002 0829 2013 06 002 PMC 4054580 PMID 24991176 a b Silver AK 2004 Victorian Literature and the Anorexic Body UK Cambridge University Press pp 38 48 ISBN 978 0 511 03051 2 Curry 1916 p 102 Kilpela Lisa Smith Becker Carolyn Black Wesley Nicole Stewart Tiffany July 1 2015 Body Image in Adult Women Moving Beyond the Younger Years Advances in Eating Disorders Abingdon England 3 2 144 164 doi 10 1080 21662630 2015 1012728 ISSN 2166 2630 PMC 4452130 PMID 26052476 Bjorn C February 13 2006 The Rules of Attraction in the Game of Love Live Science Press release Retrieved January 9 2006 Singh D December 2002 Female mate value at a glance relationship of waist to hip ratio to health fecundity and attractiveness PDF Neuro Endocrinology Letters 23 23 Suppl 4 81 91 PMID 12496738 Buss 2003 p 56 Wetsmana A Marloweb F July 1999 How Universal Are Preferences for Female Waist to Hip Ratios Evidence from the Hadza of Tanzania Evolution and Human Behavior 20 4 219 228 doi 10 1016 S1090 5138 99 00007 0 Horvath T 1979 Correlates of physical beauty in men and women Social Behavior and Personality 7 2 145 151 doi 10 2224 sbp 1979 7 2 145 Fisher ML Voracek M June 2006 The shape of beauty determinants of female physical attractiveness Journal of Cosmetic Dermatology 5 2 190 4 doi 10 1111 j 1473 2165 2006 00249 x PMID 17173598 S2CID 25660426 Dixson BJ Dixson AF Li B Anderson MJ January 2007 Studies of human physique and sexual attractiveness sexual preferences of men and women in China American Journal of Human Biology 19 1 88 95 doi 10 1002 ajhb 20584 PMID 17160976 S2CID 8868828 Marlowe F Wetsman A 2001 Preferred waist to hip ratio and ecology PDF Personality and Individual Differences 30 3 481 489 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 489 4169 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 00 00039 8 Archived from the original PDF on June 30 2014 Retrieved August 4 2007 Marlowe F Apicella C Reed D November 2005 Men s preferences for women s profile waist to hip ratio in two societies Evol Hum Behav 26 6 458 468 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2005 07 005 as PDF Archived June 1 2014 at the Wayback Machine Dixson BJ Dixson AF Morgan B Anderson MJ June 2007 Human physique and sexual attractiveness sexual preferences of men and women in Bakossiland Cameroon Archives of Sexual Behavior 36 3 369 75 doi 10 1007 s10508 006 9093 8 PMID 17136587 S2CID 40115821 Freedman RE Carter MM Sbrocco T Gray JJ August 2007 Do men hold African American and Caucasian women to different standards of beauty Eating Behaviors 8 3 319 33 doi 10 1016 j eatbeh 2006 11 008 PMC 3033406 PMID 17606, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.