fbpx
Wikipedia

Inbreeding avoidance

Inbreeding avoidance, or the inbreeding avoidance hypothesis, is a concept in evolutionary biology that refers to the prevention of the deleterious effects of inbreeding. Animals only rarely exhibit inbreeding avoidance.[1] The inbreeding avoidance hypothesis posits that certain mechanisms develop within a species, or within a given population of a species, as a result of assortative mating and natural and sexual selection, in order to prevent breeding among related individuals. Although inbreeding may impose certain evolutionary costs, inbreeding avoidance, which limits the number of potential mates for a given individual, can inflict opportunity costs.[2] Therefore, a balance exists between inbreeding and inbreeding avoidance. This balance determines whether inbreeding mechanisms develop and the specific nature of such mechanisms.[3]

A 2007 study showed that inbred mice had significantly reduced survival when they were reintroduced into a natural habitat.[4]

Inbreeding can result in inbreeding depression, which is the reduction of fitness of a given population due to inbreeding. Inbreeding depression occurs via appearance of disadvantageous traits due to the pairing of deleterious recessive alleles in a mating pair's progeny.[5] When two related individuals mate, the probability of deleterious recessive alleles pairing in the resulting offspring is higher as compared to when non-related individuals mate because of increased homozygosity. However, inbreeding also gives opportunity for genetic purging of deleterious alleles that otherwise would continue to exist in population and could potentially increase in frequency over time. Another possible negative effect of inbreeding is weakened immune system due to less diverse immunity alleles as a result of outbreeding depression.[6]

A review of the genetics of inbreeding depression in wild animal and plant populations, as well as in humans, led to the conclusion that inbreeding depression and its opposite, heterosis (hybrid vigor), are predominantly caused by the presence of recessive deleterious alleles in populations.[7] Inbreeding, including self-fertilization in plants and automictic parthenogenesis (thelytoky) in hymenoptera, tends to lead to the harmful expression of deleterious recessive alleles (inbreeding depression). Cross-fertilization between unrelated individuals ordinarily leads to the masking of deleterious recessive alleles in progeny.[8][9]

Many studies have demonstrated that homozygous individuals are often disadvantaged with respect to heterozygous individuals.[10] For example, a study conducted on a population of South African cheetahs demonstrated that the lack of genetic variability among individuals in the population has resulted in negative consequences for individuals, such as a greater rate of juvenile mortality and spermatozoal abnormalities.[11] When heterozygotes possess a fitness advantage relative to a homozygote, a population with a large number of homozygotes will have a relatively reduced fitness, thus leading to inbreeding depression. Through these described mechanisms, the effects of inbreeding depression are often severe enough to cause the evolution of inbreeding avoidance mechanisms.[12]

Mechanisms edit

Inbreeding avoidance mechanisms have evolved in response to selection against inbred offspring. Inbreeding avoidance occurs in nature by at least four mechanisms: kin recognition, dispersal, extra-pair/extra-group copulations, and delayed maturation/reproductive suppression.[3][12] These mechanisms are not mutually exclusive and more than one can occur in a population at a given time.

Kin recognition edit

 
Golden hamsters have been shown to use their own phenotypes as a template in order to differentiate between kin and non-kin via olfaction.
 
Mean time (±s.d.) females spent courting in front of the non-kin (left bars) and brothers (right bars). Given is the time females spent in the choice zone measuring 7×19 cm in front of the males' compartments for outbred (n=9) and inbred fish (n=7) as well as for all females (n=16). Each test lasted 1800 s. n.s., non-significant, **p<0.01.

Kin recognition is the mechanism by which individuals identify and avoid mating with closely related conspecifics. There have been numerous documented examples of instances in which individuals are shown to find closely related conspecifics unattractive. In one set of studies, researchers formed artificial relative and non-relative mate-pairs (artificial meaning they preferentially paired individuals to mate for the purposes of the experiments) and compared the reproductive results of the two groups. In these studies, paired relatives demonstrated reduced reproduction and higher mating reluctance when compared with non-relatives.[12][13][14][15] For example, in a study by Simmons in field crickets, female crickets exhibited greater mating latency for paired siblings and half-siblings than with non-siblings.[13] In another set of studies, researchers allowed individuals to choose their mates from conspecifics that lie on a spectrum of relatedness. In this set, individuals were more likely to choose non-related over related conspecifics.[12][14][16] For example, in a study by Krackow et al., male wild house mice were set up in an arena with four separate openings leading to cages with bedding from conspecifics. The conspecifics exhibited a range of relatedness to the test subjects, and the males significantly preferred the bedding of non-siblings to the bedding of related females.[14]

Studies have shown that kin recognition is more developed in species in which dispersal patterns facilitate frequent adult kin encounters.[12]

There is a significant amount of variation in the mechanisms used for kin recognition. These mechanisms include recognition based on association or familiarity, an individual's own phenotypic cues, chemical cues, and the MHC genes. In association/familiarity mechanisms, individuals learn the phenotypic profiles of their kin and use this template for kin recognition.[12] Many species accomplish this by becoming "familiar" with their siblings, litter mates, or nestmates. These species rely on offspring being reared in close proximity to achieve kin recognition. This is called the Westermarck effect.[17] For example, Holmes and Sherman conducted a comparative study in Arctic ground squirrels and Belding's ground squirrels. They manipulated the reared groups to include both siblings and cross-fostered nestmates and found that in both species the individuals were equally aggressive toward their nestmates, regardless of kinship.[18] In certain species where social groups are highly stable, relatedness and association between infants and other individuals are usually highly correlated.[12][19] Therefore, degree of association can be used as a meter for kin recognition.

Individuals can also use their own characteristics or phenotype as a template in kin recognition. For example, in one study, Mateo and Johnston had golden hamsters reared with only non-kin then later had them differentiate between odors of related and non-related individuals without any postnatal encounters with kin. The hamsters were able to discriminate between the odors, demonstrating the use of their own phenotype for the purpose of kin recognition.[20] This study also provides an example of a species utilizing chemical cues for kin recognition.

The major histocompatibility complex genes, or MHC genes, have been implicated in kin recognition.[21] One idea is that the MHC genes code for a specific pheromone profile for each individual, which are used to discriminate between kin and non-kin conspecifics. Several studies have demonstrated the involvement of the MHC genes in kin recognition. For example, Manning et al. conducted a study in house mice that looked at the species's behavior of communal nesting, or nursing one's own pups as well as the pups of other individuals. As Manning et al. state, kin selection theory predicts that the house mice will selectively nurse the pups of their relatives in order to maximize inclusive fitness. Manning et al. demonstrate that the house mice utilize the MHC genes in the process of discriminating between kin by preferring individuals who share the same allelic forms the MHC genes.[22]

Human kin recognition edit

The possible use of olfaction-biased mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance was examined in three different types of study.[23] The results indicated that olfaction may help mediate the development during childhood of incest avoidance (the Westermarck effect).

Post-copulatory inbreeding avoidance in mice edit

Experiments using in vitro fertilization in the mouse, provided evidence of sperm selection at the gametic level.[24] When sperm of sibling and non-sibling males were mixed, a fertilization bias towards the sperm of the non-sibling males was observed. The results were interpreted as egg-driven sperm selection against related sperm.

Inbreeding avoidance in plants edit

Experiments were performed with the dioecious plant Silene latifolia to test whether post-pollination selection favors less related pollen donors and reduces inbreeding.[25] The results showed that in S. latifolia, and presumably in other plant systems with inbreeding depression, pollen or embryo selection after multiple-donor pollination may reduce inbreeding.

Dispersal edit

 
In Gombe Stream National Park, male chimpanzees remain in their natal community while females disperse to other groups.
 
The likelihood of mating with kin decreases with respect to natal dispersal distance (m). The bold line shows the fitted values where natal dispersal distance was fitted as a predictor to inbreeding (f≥0.03125); dashed lines show the 95% confidence interval for the fit. The horizontal line represents the overall population average likelihood of inbreeding.

Some species will adopt dispersal as a way to separate close relatives and prevent inbreeding.[12] The initial dispersal route species may take is known as natal dispersal, whereby individuals move away from the area of birth. Subsequently, species may then resort to breeding dispersal, whereby individuals move from one non-natal group to another. Nelson-Flower et al. (2012) conducted a study on southern pied babblers and found that individuals may travel farther distances from natal groups than from non-natal groups.[26] This may be attributed to the possibility of encountering kin within local ranges when dispersing. The extent to which an individual in a particular species will disperse depends on whether the benefits of dispersing can outweigh both the costs of inbreeding and the costs of dispersal. Long‐distance movements can bear mortality risks and energetic costs.[27]

Sex-biased dispersal edit

In many cases of dispersal, one sex shows a greater tendency to disperse from their natal area than the opposite sex.[28] The extent of bias for a particular sex is dependent on numerous factors which include, but are not limited to: mating system, social organization, inbreeding and dispersal costs, and physiological factors.[27][28][29][30] When the costs and benefits of dispersal are symmetric for both males and females, then no sex-biased dispersal is expected to be observed in species.[27]

Female dispersal edit

Birds tend to adopt monogamous mating systems in which the males remain in their natal groups to defend familiar territories with high resource quality.[28] Females generally have high energy expenditure when producing offspring, therefore inbreeding is costly for the females in terms of offspring survival and reproductive success. Females will then benefit more by dispersing and choosing amongst these territorial males. In addition, according to the Oedipus hypothesis, daughters of female birds can cheat their mothers through brood parasitism, therefore females will evict the females from the nest, forcing their daughters to disperse. Female dispersal is not seen only in birds; males may remain philopatric in mammals when the average adult male residency in a breeding group exceeds the average age for female maturation and conception.[30] For example, in a community of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park, males tend to remain in their natal community for the duration of their lives, while females typically move to other communities as soon as they reach maturity.[31]

Male dispersal edit

Male dispersal is more common in mammals with cooperative breeding and polygynous systems. Australian marsupial juvenile males have a greater tendency to disperse from their natal groups, while the females remain philopatric.[32] In Antechinus, this is due to the fact that males die immediately after mating; therefore when they disperse to mate, they often meet with female natal groups with zero males present. Furthermore, the Oedipus hypothesis also states that fathers in polygynous systems will evict sons with the potential to cuckold them.[28] Polygynous mating systems also influence intrasexual competition between males, where in cases where males can guard multiple females and exert their dominance, subordinate males are often forced to disperse to other non-natal groups.

When species adopt alternative inbreeding avoidance mechanisms, they can indirectly influence whether a species will disperse. Their choice for non-natal group males then selects for male dispersal.

Delayed maturation edit

The delayed sexual maturation of offspring in the presence of parents is another mechanism by which individuals avoid inbreeding. Delayed maturation scenarios can involve the removal of the original, opposite-sex parent, as is the case in female lions that exhibit estrus earlier following the replacement of their fathers with new males. Another form of delayed maturation involves parental presence that inhibits reproductive activity, such as in mature marmosets offspring that are reproductively suppressed in the presence of opposite sex parents and siblings in their social groups.[12] Reproductive suppression occurs when sexually mature individuals in a group are prevented from reproducing due to behavioral or chemical stimuli from other group members that suppress breeding behavior.[33] Social cues from the surrounding environment often dictate when reproductive activity is suppressed and involves interactions between same-sex adults. If the current conditions for reproduction are unfavorable, such as when presented with only inbreeding as a means to reproduce, individuals may increase their lifetime reproductive success by timing their reproductive attempts to occur during more favorable conditions. This can be achieved by individuals suppressing their reproductive activity in poor reproduction conditions.

Inbreeding avoidance between philopatric offspring and their parents/siblings severely restricts breeding opportunities of subordinates living in their social groups. A study by O'Riain et al. (2000) examined meerkats social groups and factors affecting reproductive suppression in subordinate females. They found that in family groups, the absence of a dominant individual of either sex led to reproductive quiescence. Reproductive activity only resumed upon another sexually mature female obtaining dominance, and immigration of an unrelated male. Reproduction required both the presence of an unrelated opposite-sex partner, which acted as appropriate stimulus on reproductively suppressed subordinates that were quiescent in the presence of the original dominant individual.[33]

 
DNA analysis has shown that 60% of offspring in splendid fairywrens nests were sired through extra-pair copulations, rather than from resident males.[12]

Extra-pair copulations edit

In various species, females benefit by mating with multiple males, thus producing more offspring of higher genetic diversity and potentially quality. Females that are pair bonded to a male of poor genetic quality, as can be the case in inbreeding, are more likely to engage in extra-pair copulations in order to improve their reproductive success and the survivability of their offspring.[34] This improved quality in offspring is generated from either the intrinsic effects of good genes, or from interactions between compatible genes from the parents. In inbreeding, loss of heterozygosity contributes to the overall decreased reproductive success, but when individuals engage in extra-pair copulations, mating between genetically dissimilar individuals leads to increased heterozygosity.[35]

Extra-pair copulations involve a number of costs and benefits for both male and female animals. For males, extra-pair copulation involves spending more time away from the original pairing in search of other females. This risks the original female being fertilized by other males while the original male is searching for partners, leading to a loss of paternity. The tradeoff for this cost depends entirely on whether the male is able to fertilize the other females’ eggs in the extra-pair copulation. For females, extra-pair copulations ensure egg fertilization, and provide enhanced genetic variety with compatible sperm that avoid expression of damaging recessive genes that come with inbreeding.[36] Through extra-pair mating, females are able to maximize the genetic variability of their offspring, providing protection against environmental changes that may otherwise target more homozygous populations that inbreeding often produces.[37]

Whether a female engages in extra-pair copulations for the sake of inbreeding avoidance depends on whether the costs of extra-pair copulation outweigh the costs of inbreeding. In extra-pair copulations, both inbreeding costs and pair-bond male loss (leading to the loss of paternal care) must be considered with the benefits of reproductive success that extra-pair copulation provides. When paternal care is absent or has little influence on offspring survivability, it is generally favorable for females to engage in extra-pair mating to increase reproductive success and avoid inbreeding.[34]

Gaps edit

Inbreeding avoidance has been studied via three main methods: (1) observing individual behavior in the presence and absence of close kin, (2) contrasting costs of avoidance with costs of tolerating close inbreeding, and (3) comparing observed and random frequencies of close inbreeding.[38] No method is perfect, giving rise to questions about the completeness and consistency of the inbreeding avoidance hypothesis.[38][39] Although the first option, individual behavioral observation, is preferred and most widely used, there is still debate over whether it can provide definitive evidence for inbreeding avoidance.

A majority of the literature on inbreeding avoidance was published at least 15 years ago, allowing for growth and development of the study through contemporary experimental methods and technology. Molecular techniques such as DNA fingerprinting have become more advanced and accessible, improving the efficiency and accuracy of measuring relatedness.[12] Studying inbreeding avoidance in carnivores has garnered increased interest due to ongoing work to explain their social behaviors.[40]

References edit

  1. ^ de Boer, Raïssa A.; Vega-Trejo, Regina; Kotrschal, Alexander; Fitzpatrick, John L. (July 2021). "Meta-analytic evidence that animals rarely avoid inbreeding". Nature Ecology & Evolution. 5 (7): 949–964. doi:10.1038/s41559-021-01453-9. ISSN 2397-334X. PMID 33941905. S2CID 233718913.
  2. ^ Waser PM, Austad SN, Keane B (1986). "When should animals tolerate inbreeding?". American Naturalist. 128 (4): 529–537. doi:10.1086/284585. S2CID 84806782.
  3. ^ a b Archie EA, Hollister-Smith JA, Poole JH, Lee PC, Moss CJ, Maldonado JE, Alberts SC (2007). "Behavioural inbreeding avoidance in wild African elephants". Molecular Ecology. 16 (19): 4138–4148. doi:10.1111/j.1365-294x.2007.03483.x. PMID 17784925. S2CID 1535829.
  4. ^ Jiménez JA, Hughes KA, Alaks G, Graham L, Lacy RC (October 1994). "An experimental study of inbreeding depression in a natural habitat". Science. 266 (5183): 271–273. Bibcode:1994Sci...266..271J. doi:10.1126/science.7939661. PMID 7939661.
  5. ^ Mohammad Afzal (January 1983). "Consanguinity effects on Intelligence Quotient and neonatal behaviors of Ansari muslim children".
  6. ^ Sommer, S. (2005). "The importance of immune gene variability (MHC) in evolutionary ecology and conservation". Frontiers in Zoology. 2: 16. doi:10.1186/1742-9994-2-16. PMC 1282567. PMID 16242022.
  7. ^ Charlesworth D, Willis JH (2009). "The genetics of inbreeding depression". Nat. Rev. Genet. 10 (11): 783–96. doi:10.1038/nrg2664. PMID 19834483. S2CID 771357.
  8. ^ Bernstein H, Hopf FA, Michod RE (1987). "The molecular basis of the evolution of sex". Molecular Genetics of Development. Advances in Genetics. Vol. 24. pp. 323–70. doi:10.1016/s0065-2660(08)60012-7. ISBN 9780120176243. PMID 3324702. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  9. ^ Michod, R.E. (1994). "Eros and Evolution: A Natural Philosophy of Sex" Addison-Wesley Publishing Company, Reading, Massachusetts. ISBN 978-0201442328
  10. ^ Crnokrak P, Roff DA (1999). "Inbreeding depression in the wild". Heredity. 83 (3): 260–270. doi:10.1038/sj.hdy.6885530. PMID 10504423.
  11. ^ O'Brien SJ, Roelke ME, Marker L (1985). "Genetic basis for species vulnerability in the cheetah". Science. 227 (4693): 1428–1434. Bibcode:1985Sci...227.1428O. doi:10.1126/science.2983425. PMID 2983425. S2CID 14341795.
  12. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Pusey A, Wolf M (1996). "Inbreeding avoidance in animals". Trends Ecol Evol. 11 (5): 201–206. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(96)10028-8. PMID 21237809.
  13. ^ a b Simmons, L.W. (1989) Kin recognition and its influence on mating preferences of the field cricket, Gryffus bimaculatus (de Geer), Anim. Behav. 38,68-77
  14. ^ a b c Krackow, S. and Matuschak, B. (1991) Mate choice for non-siblings in wild house mice: evidence from a choice test and a reproductive test, Ethology 88,99-108
  15. ^ Bollinger, E.K. et al. (1991) Avoidance of inbreeding in the meadow vole (Microtus pennsylvanicus), .I Mammal. 72, 419-421
  16. ^ Keane, B. (1990) The effect of relatedness on reproductive success and mate choice in the white-footed mouse, Peromyscus leucopus, Anim. Behav. 39,264-273
  17. ^ Wolf, A.P. Westermarck Redivivus. Annual Review of Anthropology 22: 157-175, 1993
  18. ^ Holmes WG, Sherman PW (1982) The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels. American Zoologist, 22,491?517.
  19. ^ Pusey, A.E. (1990) Mechanisms of inbreeding avoidance in nonhuman primates, in Pedophilia: Biosocial Dimensions (Feirman, J.R., ed.), pp. 201-220, Springer-Verlag
  20. ^ Mateo JM, Johnston RE (2000) Kin recognition and the ‘armpit effect’: evidence of self-referent phenotype matching. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 267, 695?700.
  21. ^ Jerram L. Brown and Amy Eklund The American Naturalist Vol. 143, No. 3 (Mar., 1994), pp. 435-461 Published by: The University of Chicago Press
  22. ^ Manning CJ, Wakeland EK, Potts WK (1992). "Communal nesting patterns in mice implicate MHC genes in kin recognition". Nature. 360 (6404): 581–583. Bibcode:1992Natur.360..581M. doi:10.1038/360581a0. PMID 1461279. S2CID 4335928.
  23. ^ Weisfeld GE, Czilli T, Phillips KA, Gall JA, Lichtman CM (2003). "Possible olfaction-based mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance". J Exp Child Psychol. 85 (3): 279–95. doi:10.1016/s0022-0965(03)00061-4. PMID 12810039.
  24. ^ Firman RC, Simmons LW (2015). "Gametic interactions promote inbreeding avoidance in house mice". Ecol. Lett. 18 (9): 937–43. doi:10.1111/ele.12471. PMID 26154782.
  25. ^ Teixeira S, Foerster K, Bernasconi G (2009). "Evidence for inbreeding depression and post-pollination selection against inbreeding in the dioecious plant Silene latifolia". Heredity (Edinb). 102 (2): 101–12. doi:10.1038/hdy.2008.86. PMID 18698334.
  26. ^ Nelson-Flower MJ, Hockey PA, O'Ryan C, Ridley AR (2012). "Inbreeding avoidance mechanisms: dispersal dynamics in cooperatively breeding southern pied babblers". Journal of Animal Ecology. 81 (4): 876–883. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2012.01983.x. PMID 22471769.
  27. ^ a b c Perrin N, Mazalov V (1999). "Dispersal and inbreeding avoidance". The American Naturalist. 154 (3): 282–292. doi:10.1086/303236. PMID 10506544. S2CID 42975760.
  28. ^ a b c d Pusey AE (1987). "Sex-biased dispersal and inbreeding avoidance in birds and mammals". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 2 (10): 295–299. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(87)90081-4. PMID 21227869.
  29. ^ Lehmann L, Perrin N (2003). "Inbreeding avoidance through kin recognition: choosy females boost male dispersal". The American Naturalist. 162 (5): 638–652. doi:10.1086/378823. PMID 14618541. S2CID 25356698.
  30. ^ a b Clutton-Brock TH (1989). "Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammals". Nature. 337 (6202): 70–72. Bibcode:1989Natur.337...70C. doi:10.1038/337070a0. PMID 2909891. S2CID 4343108.
  31. ^ Pusey AE (1980). "Inbreeding avoidance in chimpanzees". Animal Behaviour. 28 (2): 543–552. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(80)80063-7. S2CID 53164905.
  32. ^ Cockburn A, Scott MP, Scotts DJ (1985). "Inbreeding avoidance and male-biased natal dispersal in Antechinus spp. (Marsupialia: Dasyuridae)". Animal Behaviour. 33 (3): 908–915. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(85)80025-7. S2CID 53206203.
  33. ^ a b O'Riain MJ, Bennett NC, Brotherton PN, McIlrath GM, Clutton-Brock TH (2000b). "Reproductive suppression and inbreeding avoidance in wild populations of co-operatively breeding meerkats Suricata suricatta". Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48 (6): 471–477. doi:10.1007/s002650000249. S2CID 25928636.
  34. ^ a b Petrie M, Kempenaers B (1998). "Extra-pair paternity in birds: Explaining variation between species and populations". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 13 (2): 52–57. doi:10.1016/s0169-5347(97)01232-9. PMID 21238200.
  35. ^ Foerster K, Delhey K, Johnsen A, Lifjeld JT, Kempenaers B (Oct 2003). "Females increase offspring heterozygosity and fitness through extra-pair matings". Nature. 425 (6959): 714–7. Bibcode:2003Natur.425..714F. doi:10.1038/nature01969. PMID 14562103. S2CID 4311810.
  36. ^ Alcock, John. 1998. Animal Behavior. Sixth Edition. 429-519.
  37. ^ Morell S (1998). "A new look at monogamy". Science. 281 (5385): 1982–1983. doi:10.1126/science.281.5385.1982. PMID 9767050. S2CID 31391458.
  38. ^ a b Part, T. (1996). Problems with testing inbreeding avoidance: the case of the collared flycatcher. Evolution, 1625-1630.
  39. ^ Moore J, Ali R (1984). "Are dispersal and inbreeding avoidance related?". Animal Behaviour. 32 (1): 94–112. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.584.5226. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(84)80328-0. S2CID 7674864.
  40. ^ Keane B, Creel SR, Waser PM (1996). "No evidence of inbreeding avoidance or inbreeding depression in a social carnivore". Behavioral Ecology. 7 (4): 480–489. doi:10.1093/beheco/7.4.480.

inbreeding, avoidance, this, article, relies, excessively, references, primary, sources, please, improve, this, article, adding, secondary, tertiary, sources, find, sources, news, newspapers, books, scholar, jstor, january, 2018, learn, when, remove, this, tem. This article relies excessively on references to primary sources Please improve this article by adding secondary or tertiary sources Find sources Inbreeding avoidance news newspapers books scholar JSTOR January 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message Inbreeding avoidance or the inbreeding avoidance hypothesis is a concept in evolutionary biology that refers to the prevention of the deleterious effects of inbreeding Animals only rarely exhibit inbreeding avoidance 1 The inbreeding avoidance hypothesis posits that certain mechanisms develop within a species or within a given population of a species as a result of assortative mating and natural and sexual selection in order to prevent breeding among related individuals Although inbreeding may impose certain evolutionary costs inbreeding avoidance which limits the number of potential mates for a given individual can inflict opportunity costs 2 Therefore a balance exists between inbreeding and inbreeding avoidance This balance determines whether inbreeding mechanisms develop and the specific nature of such mechanisms 3 A 2007 study showed that inbred mice had significantly reduced survival when they were reintroduced into a natural habitat 4 Inbreeding can result in inbreeding depression which is the reduction of fitness of a given population due to inbreeding Inbreeding depression occurs via appearance of disadvantageous traits due to the pairing of deleterious recessive alleles in a mating pair s progeny 5 When two related individuals mate the probability of deleterious recessive alleles pairing in the resulting offspring is higher as compared to when non related individuals mate because of increased homozygosity However inbreeding also gives opportunity for genetic purging of deleterious alleles that otherwise would continue to exist in population and could potentially increase in frequency over time Another possible negative effect of inbreeding is weakened immune system due to less diverse immunity alleles as a result of outbreeding depression 6 A review of the genetics of inbreeding depression in wild animal and plant populations as well as in humans led to the conclusion that inbreeding depression and its opposite heterosis hybrid vigor are predominantly caused by the presence of recessive deleterious alleles in populations 7 Inbreeding including self fertilization in plants and automictic parthenogenesis thelytoky in hymenoptera tends to lead to the harmful expression of deleterious recessive alleles inbreeding depression Cross fertilization between unrelated individuals ordinarily leads to the masking of deleterious recessive alleles in progeny 8 9 Many studies have demonstrated that homozygous individuals are often disadvantaged with respect to heterozygous individuals 10 For example a study conducted on a population of South African cheetahs demonstrated that the lack of genetic variability among individuals in the population has resulted in negative consequences for individuals such as a greater rate of juvenile mortality and spermatozoal abnormalities 11 When heterozygotes possess a fitness advantage relative to a homozygote a population with a large number of homozygotes will have a relatively reduced fitness thus leading to inbreeding depression Through these described mechanisms the effects of inbreeding depression are often severe enough to cause the evolution of inbreeding avoidance mechanisms 12 Contents 1 Mechanisms 1 1 Kin recognition 1 1 1 Human kin recognition 1 2 Post copulatory inbreeding avoidance in mice 1 3 Inbreeding avoidance in plants 1 4 Dispersal 1 4 1 Sex biased dispersal 1 4 1 1 Female dispersal 1 4 1 2 Male dispersal 1 5 Delayed maturation 1 6 Extra pair copulations 2 Gaps 3 ReferencesMechanisms editInbreeding avoidance mechanisms have evolved in response to selection against inbred offspring Inbreeding avoidance occurs in nature by at least four mechanisms kin recognition dispersal extra pair extra group copulations and delayed maturation reproductive suppression 3 12 These mechanisms are not mutually exclusive and more than one can occur in a population at a given time Kin recognition edit Main article Kin recognition nbsp Golden hamsters have been shown to use their own phenotypes as a template in order to differentiate between kin and non kin via olfaction nbsp Mean time s d females spent courting in front of the non kin left bars and brothers right bars Given is the time females spent in the choice zone measuring 7 19 cm in front of the males compartments for outbred n 9 and inbred fish n 7 as well as for all females n 16 Each test lasted 1800 s n s non significant p lt 0 01 Kin recognition is the mechanism by which individuals identify and avoid mating with closely related conspecifics There have been numerous documented examples of instances in which individuals are shown to find closely related conspecifics unattractive In one set of studies researchers formed artificial relative and non relative mate pairs artificial meaning they preferentially paired individuals to mate for the purposes of the experiments and compared the reproductive results of the two groups In these studies paired relatives demonstrated reduced reproduction and higher mating reluctance when compared with non relatives 12 13 14 15 For example in a study by Simmons in field crickets female crickets exhibited greater mating latency for paired siblings and half siblings than with non siblings 13 In another set of studies researchers allowed individuals to choose their mates from conspecifics that lie on a spectrum of relatedness In this set individuals were more likely to choose non related over related conspecifics 12 14 16 For example in a study by Krackow et al male wild house mice were set up in an arena with four separate openings leading to cages with bedding from conspecifics The conspecifics exhibited a range of relatedness to the test subjects and the males significantly preferred the bedding of non siblings to the bedding of related females 14 Studies have shown that kin recognition is more developed in species in which dispersal patterns facilitate frequent adult kin encounters 12 There is a significant amount of variation in the mechanisms used for kin recognition These mechanisms include recognition based on association or familiarity an individual s own phenotypic cues chemical cues and the MHC genes In association familiarity mechanisms individuals learn the phenotypic profiles of their kin and use this template for kin recognition 12 Many species accomplish this by becoming familiar with their siblings litter mates or nestmates These species rely on offspring being reared in close proximity to achieve kin recognition This is called the Westermarck effect 17 For example Holmes and Sherman conducted a comparative study in Arctic ground squirrels and Belding s ground squirrels They manipulated the reared groups to include both siblings and cross fostered nestmates and found that in both species the individuals were equally aggressive toward their nestmates regardless of kinship 18 In certain species where social groups are highly stable relatedness and association between infants and other individuals are usually highly correlated 12 19 Therefore degree of association can be used as a meter for kin recognition Individuals can also use their own characteristics or phenotype as a template in kin recognition For example in one study Mateo and Johnston had golden hamsters reared with only non kin then later had them differentiate between odors of related and non related individuals without any postnatal encounters with kin The hamsters were able to discriminate between the odors demonstrating the use of their own phenotype for the purpose of kin recognition 20 This study also provides an example of a species utilizing chemical cues for kin recognition The major histocompatibility complex genes or MHC genes have been implicated in kin recognition 21 One idea is that the MHC genes code for a specific pheromone profile for each individual which are used to discriminate between kin and non kin conspecifics Several studies have demonstrated the involvement of the MHC genes in kin recognition For example Manning et al conducted a study in house mice that looked at the species s behavior of communal nesting or nursing one s own pups as well as the pups of other individuals As Manning et al state kin selection theory predicts that the house mice will selectively nurse the pups of their relatives in order to maximize inclusive fitness Manning et al demonstrate that the house mice utilize the MHC genes in the process of discriminating between kin by preferring individuals who share the same allelic forms the MHC genes 22 Human kin recognition edit The possible use of olfaction biased mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance was examined in three different types of study 23 The results indicated that olfaction may help mediate the development during childhood of incest avoidance the Westermarck effect Post copulatory inbreeding avoidance in mice edit Experiments using in vitro fertilization in the mouse provided evidence of sperm selection at the gametic level 24 When sperm of sibling and non sibling males were mixed a fertilization bias towards the sperm of the non sibling males was observed The results were interpreted as egg driven sperm selection against related sperm Inbreeding avoidance in plants edit Experiments were performed with the dioecious plant Silene latifolia to test whether post pollination selection favors less related pollen donors and reduces inbreeding 25 The results showed that in S latifolia and presumably in other plant systems with inbreeding depression pollen or embryo selection after multiple donor pollination may reduce inbreeding Dispersal edit nbsp In Gombe Stream National Park male chimpanzees remain in their natal community while females disperse to other groups nbsp The likelihood of mating with kin decreases with respect to natal dispersal distance m The bold line shows the fitted values where natal dispersal distance was fitted as a predictor to inbreeding f 0 03125 dashed lines show the 95 confidence interval for the fit The horizontal line represents the overall population average likelihood of inbreeding Some species will adopt dispersal as a way to separate close relatives and prevent inbreeding 12 The initial dispersal route species may take is known as natal dispersal whereby individuals move away from the area of birth Subsequently species may then resort to breeding dispersal whereby individuals move from one non natal group to another Nelson Flower et al 2012 conducted a study on southern pied babblers and found that individuals may travel farther distances from natal groups than from non natal groups 26 This may be attributed to the possibility of encountering kin within local ranges when dispersing The extent to which an individual in a particular species will disperse depends on whether the benefits of dispersing can outweigh both the costs of inbreeding and the costs of dispersal Long distance movements can bear mortality risks and energetic costs 27 Sex biased dispersal edit In many cases of dispersal one sex shows a greater tendency to disperse from their natal area than the opposite sex 28 The extent of bias for a particular sex is dependent on numerous factors which include but are not limited to mating system social organization inbreeding and dispersal costs and physiological factors 27 28 29 30 When the costs and benefits of dispersal are symmetric for both males and females then no sex biased dispersal is expected to be observed in species 27 Female dispersal edit Birds tend to adopt monogamous mating systems in which the males remain in their natal groups to defend familiar territories with high resource quality 28 Females generally have high energy expenditure when producing offspring therefore inbreeding is costly for the females in terms of offspring survival and reproductive success Females will then benefit more by dispersing and choosing amongst these territorial males In addition according to the Oedipus hypothesis daughters of female birds can cheat their mothers through brood parasitism therefore females will evict the females from the nest forcing their daughters to disperse Female dispersal is not seen only in birds males may remain philopatric in mammals when the average adult male residency in a breeding group exceeds the average age for female maturation and conception 30 For example in a community of chimpanzees in Gombe National Park males tend to remain in their natal community for the duration of their lives while females typically move to other communities as soon as they reach maturity 31 Male dispersal edit Male dispersal is more common in mammals with cooperative breeding and polygynous systems Australian marsupial juvenile males have a greater tendency to disperse from their natal groups while the females remain philopatric 32 In Antechinus this is due to the fact that males die immediately after mating therefore when they disperse to mate they often meet with female natal groups with zero males present Furthermore the Oedipus hypothesis also states that fathers in polygynous systems will evict sons with the potential to cuckold them 28 Polygynous mating systems also influence intrasexual competition between males where in cases where males can guard multiple females and exert their dominance subordinate males are often forced to disperse to other non natal groups When species adopt alternative inbreeding avoidance mechanisms they can indirectly influence whether a species will disperse Their choice for non natal group males then selects for male dispersal Delayed maturation edit See also Animal sexual behaviour Mating systems The delayed sexual maturation of offspring in the presence of parents is another mechanism by which individuals avoid inbreeding Delayed maturation scenarios can involve the removal of the original opposite sex parent as is the case in female lions that exhibit estrus earlier following the replacement of their fathers with new males Another form of delayed maturation involves parental presence that inhibits reproductive activity such as in mature marmosets offspring that are reproductively suppressed in the presence of opposite sex parents and siblings in their social groups 12 Reproductive suppression occurs when sexually mature individuals in a group are prevented from reproducing due to behavioral or chemical stimuli from other group members that suppress breeding behavior 33 Social cues from the surrounding environment often dictate when reproductive activity is suppressed and involves interactions between same sex adults If the current conditions for reproduction are unfavorable such as when presented with only inbreeding as a means to reproduce individuals may increase their lifetime reproductive success by timing their reproductive attempts to occur during more favorable conditions This can be achieved by individuals suppressing their reproductive activity in poor reproduction conditions Inbreeding avoidance between philopatric offspring and their parents siblings severely restricts breeding opportunities of subordinates living in their social groups A study by O Riain et al 2000 examined meerkats social groups and factors affecting reproductive suppression in subordinate females They found that in family groups the absence of a dominant individual of either sex led to reproductive quiescence Reproductive activity only resumed upon another sexually mature female obtaining dominance and immigration of an unrelated male Reproduction required both the presence of an unrelated opposite sex partner which acted as appropriate stimulus on reproductively suppressed subordinates that were quiescent in the presence of the original dominant individual 33 nbsp DNA analysis has shown that 60 of offspring in splendid fairywrens nests were sired through extra pair copulations rather than from resident males 12 Extra pair copulations edit See also Polyandry In zoology Further information Extra pair copulation in non humans In various species females benefit by mating with multiple males thus producing more offspring of higher genetic diversity and potentially quality Females that are pair bonded to a male of poor genetic quality as can be the case in inbreeding are more likely to engage in extra pair copulations in order to improve their reproductive success and the survivability of their offspring 34 This improved quality in offspring is generated from either the intrinsic effects of good genes or from interactions between compatible genes from the parents In inbreeding loss of heterozygosity contributes to the overall decreased reproductive success but when individuals engage in extra pair copulations mating between genetically dissimilar individuals leads to increased heterozygosity 35 Extra pair copulations involve a number of costs and benefits for both male and female animals For males extra pair copulation involves spending more time away from the original pairing in search of other females This risks the original female being fertilized by other males while the original male is searching for partners leading to a loss of paternity The tradeoff for this cost depends entirely on whether the male is able to fertilize the other females eggs in the extra pair copulation For females extra pair copulations ensure egg fertilization and provide enhanced genetic variety with compatible sperm that avoid expression of damaging recessive genes that come with inbreeding 36 Through extra pair mating females are able to maximize the genetic variability of their offspring providing protection against environmental changes that may otherwise target more homozygous populations that inbreeding often produces 37 Whether a female engages in extra pair copulations for the sake of inbreeding avoidance depends on whether the costs of extra pair copulation outweigh the costs of inbreeding In extra pair copulations both inbreeding costs and pair bond male loss leading to the loss of paternal care must be considered with the benefits of reproductive success that extra pair copulation provides When paternal care is absent or has little influence on offspring survivability it is generally favorable for females to engage in extra pair mating to increase reproductive success and avoid inbreeding 34 Gaps editInbreeding avoidance has been studied via three main methods 1 observing individual behavior in the presence and absence of close kin 2 contrasting costs of avoidance with costs of tolerating close inbreeding and 3 comparing observed and random frequencies of close inbreeding 38 No method is perfect giving rise to questions about the completeness and consistency of the inbreeding avoidance hypothesis 38 39 Although the first option individual behavioral observation is preferred and most widely used there is still debate over whether it can provide definitive evidence for inbreeding avoidance A majority of the literature on inbreeding avoidance was published at least 15 years ago allowing for growth and development of the study through contemporary experimental methods and technology Molecular techniques such as DNA fingerprinting have become more advanced and accessible improving the efficiency and accuracy of measuring relatedness 12 Studying inbreeding avoidance in carnivores has garnered increased interest due to ongoing work to explain their social behaviors 40 References edit de Boer Raissa A Vega Trejo Regina Kotrschal Alexander Fitzpatrick John L July 2021 Meta analytic evidence that animals rarely avoid inbreeding Nature Ecology amp Evolution 5 7 949 964 doi 10 1038 s41559 021 01453 9 ISSN 2397 334X PMID 33941905 S2CID 233718913 Waser PM Austad SN Keane B 1986 When should animals tolerate inbreeding American Naturalist 128 4 529 537 doi 10 1086 284585 S2CID 84806782 a b Archie EA Hollister Smith JA Poole JH Lee PC Moss CJ Maldonado JE Alberts SC 2007 Behavioural inbreeding avoidance in wild African elephants Molecular Ecology 16 19 4138 4148 doi 10 1111 j 1365 294x 2007 03483 x PMID 17784925 S2CID 1535829 Jimenez JA Hughes KA Alaks G Graham L Lacy RC October 1994 An experimental study of inbreeding depression in a natural habitat Science 266 5183 271 273 Bibcode 1994Sci 266 271J doi 10 1126 science 7939661 PMID 7939661 Mohammad Afzal January 1983 Consanguinity effects on Intelligence Quotient and neonatal behaviors of Ansari muslim children Sommer S 2005 The importance of immune gene variability MHC in evolutionary ecology and conservation Frontiers in Zoology 2 16 doi 10 1186 1742 9994 2 16 PMC 1282567 PMID 16242022 Charlesworth D Willis JH 2009 The genetics of inbreeding depression Nat Rev Genet 10 11 783 96 doi 10 1038 nrg2664 PMID 19834483 S2CID 771357 Bernstein H Hopf FA Michod RE 1987 The molecular basis of the evolution of sex Molecular Genetics of Development Advances in Genetics Vol 24 pp 323 70 doi 10 1016 s0065 2660 08 60012 7 ISBN 9780120176243 PMID 3324702 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a journal ignored help Michod R E 1994 Eros and Evolution A Natural Philosophy of Sex Addison Wesley Publishing Company Reading Massachusetts ISBN 978 0201442328 Crnokrak P Roff DA 1999 Inbreeding depression in the wild Heredity 83 3 260 270 doi 10 1038 sj hdy 6885530 PMID 10504423 O Brien SJ Roelke ME Marker L 1985 Genetic basis for species vulnerability in the cheetah Science 227 4693 1428 1434 Bibcode 1985Sci 227 1428O doi 10 1126 science 2983425 PMID 2983425 S2CID 14341795 a b c d e f g h i j k Pusey A Wolf M 1996 Inbreeding avoidance in animals Trends Ecol Evol 11 5 201 206 doi 10 1016 0169 5347 96 10028 8 PMID 21237809 a b Simmons L W 1989 Kin recognition and its influence on mating preferences of the field cricket Gryffus bimaculatus de Geer Anim Behav 38 68 77 a b c Krackow S and Matuschak B 1991 Mate choice for non siblings in wild house mice evidence from a choice test and a reproductive test Ethology 88 99 108 Bollinger E K et al 1991 Avoidance of inbreeding in the meadow vole Microtus pennsylvanicus I Mammal 72 419 421 Keane B 1990 The effect of relatedness on reproductive success and mate choice in the white footed mouse Peromyscus leucopus Anim Behav 39 264 273 Wolf A P Westermarck Redivivus Annual Review of Anthropology 22 157 175 1993 Holmes WG Sherman PW 1982 The ontogeny of kin recognition in two species of ground squirrels American Zoologist 22 491 517 Pusey A E 1990 Mechanisms of inbreeding avoidance in nonhuman primates in Pedophilia Biosocial Dimensions Feirman J R ed pp 201 220 Springer Verlag Mateo JM Johnston RE 2000 Kin recognition and the armpit effect evidence of self referent phenotype matching Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 267 695 700 Jerram L Brown and Amy Eklund The American Naturalist Vol 143 No 3 Mar 1994 pp 435 461 Published by The University of Chicago Press Manning CJ Wakeland EK Potts WK 1992 Communal nesting patterns in mice implicate MHC genes in kin recognition Nature 360 6404 581 583 Bibcode 1992Natur 360 581M doi 10 1038 360581a0 PMID 1461279 S2CID 4335928 Weisfeld GE Czilli T Phillips KA Gall JA Lichtman CM 2003 Possible olfaction based mechanisms in human kin recognition and inbreeding avoidance J Exp Child Psychol 85 3 279 95 doi 10 1016 s0022 0965 03 00061 4 PMID 12810039 Firman RC Simmons LW 2015 Gametic interactions promote inbreeding avoidance in house mice Ecol Lett 18 9 937 43 doi 10 1111 ele 12471 PMID 26154782 Teixeira S Foerster K Bernasconi G 2009 Evidence for inbreeding depression and post pollination selection against inbreeding in the dioecious plant Silene latifolia Heredity Edinb 102 2 101 12 doi 10 1038 hdy 2008 86 PMID 18698334 Nelson Flower MJ Hockey PA O Ryan C Ridley AR 2012 Inbreeding avoidance mechanisms dispersal dynamics in cooperatively breeding southern pied babblers Journal of Animal Ecology 81 4 876 883 doi 10 1111 j 1365 2656 2012 01983 x PMID 22471769 a b c Perrin N Mazalov V 1999 Dispersal and inbreeding avoidance The American Naturalist 154 3 282 292 doi 10 1086 303236 PMID 10506544 S2CID 42975760 a b c d Pusey AE 1987 Sex biased dispersal and inbreeding avoidance in birds and mammals Trends in Ecology amp Evolution 2 10 295 299 doi 10 1016 0169 5347 87 90081 4 PMID 21227869 Lehmann L Perrin N 2003 Inbreeding avoidance through kin recognition choosy females boost male dispersal The American Naturalist 162 5 638 652 doi 10 1086 378823 PMID 14618541 S2CID 25356698 a b Clutton Brock TH 1989 Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammals Nature 337 6202 70 72 Bibcode 1989Natur 337 70C doi 10 1038 337070a0 PMID 2909891 S2CID 4343108 Pusey AE 1980 Inbreeding avoidance in chimpanzees Animal Behaviour 28 2 543 552 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 80 80063 7 S2CID 53164905 Cockburn A Scott MP Scotts DJ 1985 Inbreeding avoidance and male biased natal dispersal in Antechinus spp Marsupialia Dasyuridae Animal Behaviour 33 3 908 915 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 85 80025 7 S2CID 53206203 a b O Riain MJ Bennett NC Brotherton PN McIlrath GM Clutton Brock TH 2000b Reproductive suppression and inbreeding avoidance in wild populations of co operatively breeding meerkats Suricata suricatta Behav Ecol Sociobiol 48 6 471 477 doi 10 1007 s002650000249 S2CID 25928636 a b Petrie M Kempenaers B 1998 Extra pair paternity in birds Explaining variation between species and populations Trends in Ecology and Evolution 13 2 52 57 doi 10 1016 s0169 5347 97 01232 9 PMID 21238200 Foerster K Delhey K Johnsen A Lifjeld JT Kempenaers B Oct 2003 Females increase offspring heterozygosity and fitness through extra pair matings Nature 425 6959 714 7 Bibcode 2003Natur 425 714F doi 10 1038 nature01969 PMID 14562103 S2CID 4311810 Alcock John 1998 Animal Behavior Sixth Edition 429 519 Morell S 1998 A new look at monogamy Science 281 5385 1982 1983 doi 10 1126 science 281 5385 1982 PMID 9767050 S2CID 31391458 a b Part T 1996 Problems with testing inbreeding avoidance the case of the collared flycatcher Evolution 1625 1630 Moore J Ali R 1984 Are dispersal and inbreeding avoidance related Animal Behaviour 32 1 94 112 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 584 5226 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 84 80328 0 S2CID 7674864 Keane B Creel SR Waser PM 1996 No evidence of inbreeding avoidance or inbreeding depression in a social carnivore Behavioral Ecology 7 4 480 489 doi 10 1093 beheco 7 4 480 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Inbreeding avoidance amp oldid 1207386033, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.