fbpx
Wikipedia

Animal communication

Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals (sender or senders) to one or more other animals (receiver or receivers) that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers.[1][2] Information may be sent intentionally, as in a courtship display, or unintentionally, as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones. Information may be transferred to an "audience" of several receivers.[3] Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior, sociology, neurology and animal cognition. Many aspects of animal behavior, such as symbolic name use, emotional expression, learning and sexual behavior, are being understood in new ways.

Great egret (Ardea alba) in a courtship display communicating the desire to find a mate.

When the information from the sender changes the behavior of a receiver, the information is referred to as a "signal". Signalling theory predicts that for a signal to be maintained in the population, both the sender and receiver should usually receive some benefit from the interaction. Signal production by senders and the perception and subsequent response of receivers are thought to coevolve.[4] Signals often involve multiple mechanisms, e.g. both visual and auditory, and for a signal to be understood the coordinated behaviour of both sender and receiver require careful study.

Modes Edit

 
A lamb investigates a rabbit, an example of interspecific communication using body posture and olfaction.

Visual Edit

  • Gestures: Most animals understand communication through a visual display of distinctive body parts or bodily movements. Animals will reveal or accentuate a body part to relay certain information. The parent herring gull displays its bright yellow bill on the ground next over its chick when it has returned to the nest with food. The chicks exhibit a begging response by tapping the red spot on the lower mandible of the parent herring gull's bill. This signal stimulates the parent to regurgitate food and completes the feeding signal. The distinctive morphological feature accentuated in this communication is the parent's red-spotted bill, while the tapping towards the ground makes the red spot visible to the chick, demonstrating a distinctive movement.[5] Frans de Waal studied bonobos and chimps to understand if language was somehow evolved by gestures. He found that both apes and humans only use intentional gestures to communicate.[6]
  • Facial expression: Another important signal of emotion in animal communication are facial gestures. Blue and Yellow Macaws were studied to understand how they reacted to interactions with a familiar animal care taker. Studies show that Blue and Yellow Macaws demonstrated a significant amount of blushing frequently during mutual interactions with a caretaker.[7] In another experiment, Jeffrey Mogil studied facial expression in mice in response to increments of increasing pain. He found that mice exhibited five recognizable face expressions: orbital tightening, nose and cheek bulge, and changes in ear and whisker carriage.[8]
  • Gaze-following: Social animals use gaze-following as a form of communication through monitoring head and eye orientation in other mammals.[9] Studies have been conducted on apes, monkeys, dogs, birds, wolves and tortoises, and have focused on two different tasks: "follow[ing] another's gaze into distant space" and "follow[ing] another's gaze geometrically around a visual barrier e.g. by repositioning themselves to follow a gaze cue when faced with a barrier blocking their view".[10] A broad range of animals have been proven to exhibit the latter, however only apes, dogs, wolves, and corvids (ravens) have been able to follow another's gaze into distant space. Marmosets and ibis were unable to demonstrate "geometric gaze following". Researchers do not yet have a clear picture of the cognitive basis of gaze following, but developmental evidence indicates that "simple" gaze following and "geometric" gaze following probably rely on different cognitive mechanisms.[9]
  • Color change: Color change can be separated into changes that occur during growth and development, and those triggered by mood, social context, or abiotic factors such as temperature. The latter are seen in many taxa. Some cephalopods, such as the octopus and the cuttlefish, have specialized skin cells (chromatophores) that can change the apparent colour, opacity, and reflectiveness of their skin.[11] In addition to their use for camouflage, rapid changes in skin color are used while hunting and in courtship rituals.[12] Cuttlefish may display two entirely different signals simultaneously from opposite sides of their body. When a male cuttlefish courts a female in the presence of other males, he displays a male pattern facing the female and a female pattern facing away, to deceive other males.[13] Some color signals occur in cycles. For example, when a female olive baboon begins to ovulate, her anogenital area swells and turns a bright red/pink. This signals to males that she is ready to mate.[14] Humboldt squid are bioluminescent and thus capable of communicating visually in dark ocean environments.[15]
  • Bioluminescent communication: Communication by the production of light occurs commonly in vertebrates and invertebrates in the oceans, particularly at depths (e.g. angler fish). Two well known forms of land bioluminescence occur in fireflies and glow worms. Other insects, insect larvae, annelids, arachnids and even species of fungi possess bioluminescent abilities. Some bioluminescent animals produce the light themselves whereas others have a symbiotic relationship with bioluminescent bacteria.[16] Animals exhibit bioluminescent light to lure in prey, attract a mate, or protect themselves from potential predators.[17] (See also: List of bioluminescent organisms)

Auditory Edit

 
Bird calls can serve as alarms or keep members of a flock in contact, while the longer and more complex bird songs are associated with courtship and mating.[18]
Humpback Whale singing at Southern Ocean feeding grounds.

Many animals communicate through vocalization. Vocal communication serves many purposes, including mating rituals, warning calls, conveying location of food sources, and social learning. In a number of species, males perform calls during mating rituals as a form of competition against other males and to signal to females. Examples include frogs, hammer-headed bats, red deer, humpback whales, elephant seals, and songbirds.[19][20][21] Other instances of vocal communication include the alarm calls of the Campbell monkey,[22] the territorial calls of gibbons, and the use of frequency in greater spear-nosed bats to distinguish between groups.[23] The vervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of its four different predators, and the reactions of other monkeys vary appropriately according to the call. For example, if an alarm call signals a python, the monkeys climb into the trees, whereas the "eagle" alarm causes monkeys to seek a hiding place on the ground.[24] Prairie dogs also use complex calls that signal predator differences. According to Con Slobodchikoff and others, prairie dog calls communicate the type, size, and speed of an approaching predator.[25][26][27][28]  Whale vocalizations have been found to have different dialects based on social learning.[29][30] Mammalian acoustic culture was first discovered in southern resident orcas in 1978.[31][32]

Not all animals use vocalization as a means of auditory communication. Many arthropods rub specialized body parts together to produce sound. This is known as stridulation. Crickets and grasshoppers are well known for this, but many others use stridulation as well, including crustaceans, spiders, scorpions, wasps, ants, beetles, butterflies, moths, millipedes, and centipedes. Another means of auditory communication is the vibration of swim bladders in bony fish. The structure of swim bladders and the attached sonic muscles varies greatly across bony fish families, resulting in a wide variety of sounds.[33] Striking body parts together can also produce auditory signals. A well-known example of this is the tail tip vibration of rattlesnakes as a warning signal. Other examples include bill clacking in birds, wing clapping in manakin courtship displays, and chest beating in gorillas.[34]

 
An alert motionless groundhog whistles when alarmed to warn other groundhogs.

Burrowing animals species are known to whistle to communicate threats, and sometimes mood. Species such as the marmot species including the groundhog (woodchuck) the alpine marmot show this trait. Whistling is used by animals such as prairie dogs to communicate threats, with prairie dogs having one of the most complex communication systems in the animal kingdom. Prairie dogs are able to communicate an animal's speed, shape, size, species, and for humans specific attire and if the human is carrying a gun.[35] This method of communication is usually done by having a sentry stand on two feet surveying for potential threats while the rest of the pack finds food. Once a threat has been identified the sentry sounds a whistle alarm, (sometimes describing the threat) at which point the pack retreats to their burrows. The intensity of the threat is usually determined by how long the sentry whistles. The sentry continues to whistle the alarm until the entirety of the pack have gone to safety at which point the sentry returns to the burrow.[36]

Olfactory Edit

 
Flehmen response in a tiger

Despite being the oldest method of communication, chemical communication is one of the least understood forms due in part to the sheer abundance of chemicals in our environment and the difficulty of detecting and measuring all the chemicals in a sample.[34] The ability to detect chemicals in the environment serves many functions, a crucial one being the detection of food, a function that first arose in single-celled organisms (bacteria) living in the oceans during the early days of life on Earth.[34] As this function evolved, organisms began to differentiate between chemicals compounds emanating from resources, conspecifics (same species; i.e., mates and kin), and heterospecifics (different species; i.e., competitors and predators).[34]

For instance, a small minnow species may do well to avoid habitat with a detectable concentration of chemical cue associated with a predator species such as northern pike.[37] Minnows with the ability to perceive the presence of predators before they are close enough to be seen and then respond with adaptive behavior (such as hiding) are more likely to survive and reproduce.[38] Atlantic salmon go a step further than detecting a predator's cue: when an individual is damaged by a predator, it releases a chemical cue to its conspecifics.[39] As has also been observed in other species, acidification and changes in pH physically disrupt these chemical cues, which has various implications for animal behavior.[39][40]

Scent marking and scent rubbing are common forms of olfactory communication in mammals.[41][42] An example of scent rubbing by an animal can be seen from bears, bears do this as a way to mark territory or let others know they are there and to stay away.[43]

Electric Edit

Electrocommunication is a rare form of communication in animals. It is seen primarily in aquatic animals, though some land mammals, notably the platypus and echidnas, sense electric fields that might be used for communication.[44]

Weakly electric fishes provide an example of electrocommunication, together with electrolocation. These fish use an electric organ to generate an electric field, which is detected by electroreceptors. Differences in the waveform and frequency of changes in the field convey information on species, sex, and identity. These electric signals can be generated in response to hormones, circadian rhythms, and interactions with other fish. They can also serve to mediate social hierarchy amongst species that have a social order.[45] Some predators, such as sharks and rays, are able to eavesdrop on these electrogenic fish through passive electroreception.[46]

Touch Edit

For more on the mechanism for touch, see Somatosensory system and Mechanoreceptors

Touch is a key factor in many social interactions. Here are some examples:

    • Fighting: In a fight, touch may be used to challenge an opponent and to coordinate movements during the fight. It may also be used by the loser to indicate submission.[47]
    • Mating: Mammals often initiate mating by grooming, stroking or rubbing against each other. This provides the opportunity to apply chemical signals and to assess those excreted by the potential mate. Touch may also announce the intention of the male to mount the female, as when a male kangaroo grabs the tail of a female. During mating, touch stimuli are important for pair positioning, coordination and genital stimulation.[48]
    • Social integration: Touch is widely used for social integration, a use that is typified by the social grooming of one animal by another. Social grooming has several functions; it removes parasites and debris from the groomed animal, it reaffirms the social bond or hierarchical relationship between the animals, and it gives the groomer an opportunity to examine olfactory cues on the groomed individual, perhaps adding additional ones. This behaviour has been observed in social insects, birds and mammals.[49]
    • Foraging: Some ant species recruit fellow workers to new food finds by first tapping them with their antennae and forelegs, then leading them to the food source while keeping physical contact. "Patrollers" leave the nest to check for danger nearby and return to recruit "foragers" by making physical contact.[50] Another example of this is the waggle dance of honey bees.[34]
    • Huddling: Prolonged physical contact or huddling also serves social integration. Huddling promotes heat exchange, together with the transfer of olfactory or tactile information.[51] Some organisms live in constant contact in a colony, for example colonial corals. When individuals are linked tightly in this way an entire colony can react on the aversive or alarm movements made by only a few individuals.[52] In several herbivorous insect nymphs and larvae, aggregations where there is prolonged contact play a major role in group coordination. These aggregations may take the form of a procession or a rosette.[53]

Seismic Edit

Seismic communication is the exchange of information using self-generated vibrational signals transmitted via a substrate such as the soil, water, spider webs, plant stems, or a blade of grass. This form of communication has several advantages, for example it can be sent regardless of light and noise levels, and it usually has a short range and short persistence, which may reduce the danger of detection by predators. The use of seismic communication is found in many taxa, including frogs, kangaroo rats, mole rats, bees, nematode worms, and others. Tetrapods usually make seismic waves by drumming on the ground with a body part, a signal that is sensed by the sacculus of the receiver.[54] The sacculus is an organ in the inner ear containing a membranous sac that is used for balance, but can also detect seismic waves in animals that use this form of communication. Vibrations may be combined with other sorts of communication.[55]

Thermal Edit

 
A python (top) and rattlesnake illustrating the positions of the pit organs. Red arrows indicate the pit organs whereas black arrows indicate the nostril.

A number of different snakes have the ability to sense infrared (IR) thermal radiation, which allows these reptiles to derive thermal images from the radiant heat emitted by predators or prey at wavelengths between 5 and 30 μm. The accuracy of this sense is such that a blind rattlesnake can target its strike to the vulnerable body parts of a prey animal.[56] It was previously thought that the pit organs evolved primarily as prey detectors, but it is now believed that they may also be used to control body temperature.[57]

The facial pits enabling thermoregulation underwent parallel evolution in pitvipers and some boas and pythons, having evolved once in pitvipers and multiple times in boas and pythons.[58] The electrophysiology of the structure is similar between lineages, but it differs in gross structure anatomy. Most superficially, pitvipers possess one large pit organ on either side of the head, between the eye and the nostril (loreal pit), while boas and pythons have three or more comparatively smaller pits lining the upper and sometimes the lower lip, in or between the scales. Those of the pitvipers are the more advanced, having a suspended sensory membrane as opposed to a simple pit structure. Within the family Viperidae, the pit organ is seen only in the subfamily Crotalinae: the pitvipers. Despite the detection of IR radiation, the pits' IR mechanism is dissimilar to photoreceptors; while photoreceptors detect light via photochemical reactions, the protein in the facial pits of snakes is a temperature sensitive ion channel. It senses infrared signals through a mechanism involving warming of the pit organ, rather than chemical reaction to light.[59] This is consistent with the thin pit membrane, which allows incoming IR radiation to quickly and precisely warm a given ion channel and trigger a nerve impulse, as well as vascularize the pit membrane to rapidly cool the ion channel back to its original "resting" or "inactive" temperature.[59]

Common vampire bats (Desmodus rotundus) have specialized IR sensors in their nose-leaf.[60] Vampire bats are the only mammals that feed exclusively on blood. The IR sense enables Desmodus to localize homeothermic animals such as cattle and horses within a range of about 10 to 15 cm. This infrared perception may be used in detecting regions of maximal blood flow on targeted prey.

Autocommunication Edit

Autocommunication is a type of communication in which the sender and receiver are the same individual. The sender emits a signal that is altered by the environment and eventually is received by the same individual. The altered signal provides information that can indicate food, predators or conspecifics. Because the sender and receiver are the same animal, selection pressure maximizes signal efficacy, i.e. the degree to which an emitted signal is correctly identified by a receiver despite propagation distortion and noise. There are some species, such as the pacific herring, which have evolved to intercept these messages from their predators. They are able to use it as an early warning sign and respond defensively.[61] There are two types of autocommunication. The first is active electrolocation, where the organism emits an electrical pulse through its electric organ and senses the projected geometrical property of the object. This is found in the electric fish Gymnotiformes (knifefishes) and Mormyridae(elephantfish).[62] The second type of autocommunication is echolocation, found in bats and toothed whales. Echolocation involves emitting sounds and interpreting the vibrations that return from objects.[63] In bats, echolocation also serves the purpose of mapping their environment. They are capable of recognizing a space they've been in before without any visible light because they can memorize patterns in the feedback they get from echolocation.[64]

Functions Edit

There are many functions of animal communication. However, some have been studied in more detail than others. This includes:

  • Communication during contests: Animal communication plays a vital role in determining the winner of contest over a resource. Many species have distinct signals that signal aggression or willingness to attack or signals to convey retreat during competitions over food, territories, or mates.[65]
Two 'Red Deer roaring, most likely to establish dominance during a rut. However, males also use loud roaring to keep track of harems of females.
    • Mating rituals: Animals produce signals to attract the attention of a possible mate or to solidify pair bonds. These signals frequently involve the display of body parts or postures. For example, a gazelle will assume characteristic poses to initiate mating. Mating signals can also include the use of olfactory signals or mating calls unique to a species. Animals that form lasting pair bonds often have symmetrical displays that they make to each other. Famous examples are the mutual presentation of reeds by great crested grebes studied by Julian Huxley, the triumph displays shown by many species of geese and penguins on their nest sites, and the spectacular courtship displays by birds of paradise.[citation needed] "Copulation calls" in mammals can indicate a female's breeding status or attract other mates.[66]
    • Ownership/territorial: Signals used to claim or defend a territory, food, or a mate. Polygynous lizards (Anolis carolinensis) will show greater signs of aggression from farther distances between males than between females when defending a territory or mate. It is believed that males have evolved to remain distant from each other due to higher reproductive consequences as opposed to females.[67]
    • Redirected aggression: Some animals who are fearful of stimuli will attack anyone near them.[68]
    • Food-related signals: Many animals make "food calls" to attract a mate, offspring, or other members of a social group to a food source. Perhaps the most elaborate food-related signal is the Waggle dance of honeybees studied by Karl von Frisch. One well-known example of begging of offspring in a clutch or litter is altricial songbirds. Young ravens will signal to older ravens when they encounter new or untested food.  Rhesus macaques will send food calls to inform other monkeys of a food source to avoid punishment.  Pheromones are released by many social insects to lead the other members of the society to the food source. For example, ants leave a pheromone trail on the ground that can be followed by other ants to lead them to the food source.
    • Alarm calls: Alarm calls communicate the threat of a predator. This allows all members of a social group (and sometimes other species) to respond accordingly. This may include running for cover, becoming immobile, or gathering into a group to reduce the risk of attack.[69] Alarm signals are not always vocalizations. Crushed ants will release an alarm pheromone to attract more ants and send them into an attack state.[70]
    • Meta-communication: Signals that will modify the meaning of subsequent signals. One example is the 'play face' in dogs which signals that a subsequent aggressive signal is part of a play fight rather than a serious aggressive episode.

Interpretation of animal behaviour Edit

As described above, many animal gestures, postures, and sounds, convey meaning to nearby animals. These signals are often easier to describe than to interpret. It is tempting, especially with domesticated animals and apes, to anthropomorphize, that is, to interpret animal actions in human terms, but this can be quite misleading; for example, an ape's "smile" is often a sign of aggression. Also, the same gesture may have different meanings depending on context within which it occurs. For example, a domestic dog's tail wag and posture may be used in different ways to convey many meanings as illustrated in Charles Darwin's The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals published in 1872. Some of Darwin's illustrations are reproduced here.

Interspecific communication Edit

 
Sentry prairie dog surveying potential dangers and threats
 
Sentry prairie dog alerts other prairie dogs to a threat

Much animal communication is intraspecific, that is, it occurs between members of the same species. As for interspecific communication, that between predator and prey is of particular interest.

Prey to predator Edit

If a prey animal moves, makes a noise or vibrations, or emits a smell in such a way that a predator can detect it, it is communicating with its predator.[71] This is consistent with the definition of "communication" given above. This type of communication is known as interceptive eavesdropping if a predator intercepts a message intended for conspecifics.

There are however, some actions of prey species are clearly directed to actual or potential predators. A good example is warning coloration: species such as wasps that are capable of harming potential predators are often brightly coloured, and this modifies the behavior of the predator, who either instinctively or as the result of experience will avoid attacking such an animal. Some forms of mimicry fall in the same category: for example hoverflies are coloured in the same way as wasps, and although they are unable to sting, the strong avoidance of wasps by predators gives the hoverfly some protection. There are also behavioural changes that act in a similar way to warning colouration. For example, canines such as wolves and coyotes may adopt an aggressive posture, such as growling with their teeth bared, to indicate they will fight if necessary, and rattlesnakes use their well-known rattle to warn potential predators of their venomous bite. Sometimes, a behavioural change and warning colouration will be combined, as in certain species of amphibians which have most of their body coloured to blend with their surroundings, except for a brightly coloured belly. When confronted with a potential threat, they show their belly, indicating that they are poisonous in some way.

Another example of prey to predator communication is the pursuit-deterrent signal. Pursuit-deterrent signals occur when prey indicates to a predator that pursuit would be unprofitable because the signaler is prepared to escape. Pursuit-deterrent signals provide a benefit to both the signaler and receiver; they prevent the sender from wasting time and energy fleeing, and they prevent the receiver from investing in a costly pursuit that is unlikely to result in capture. Such signals can advertise prey's ability to escape, and reflect phenotypic condition (quality advertisement), or can advertise that the prey has detected the predator (perception advertisement).[71] Pursuit-deterrent signals have been reported for a wide variety of taxa, including fish (Godin and Davis, 1995), lizards (Cooper etc. al., 2004), ungulates (Caro, 1995), rabbits (Holley 1993), primates (Zuberbuhler et al. 1997), rodents (Shelley and Blumstein 2005, Clark, 2005), and birds (Alvarez, 1993, Murphy, 2006, 2007). A familiar example of quality advertisement pursuit-deterrent signal is stotting (sometimes called pronking), a pronounced combination of stiff-legged running while simultaneously jumping shown by some antelopes such as Thomson's gazelle in the presence of a predator. At least 11 hypotheses for stotting have been proposed. A leading theory today is that it alerts predators that the element of surprise has been lost. Predators like cheetahs rely on surprise attacks, proven by the fact that chases are rarely successful when antelope stot. Predators do not waste energy on a chase that will likely be unsuccessful (optimal foraging behavior). Quality advertisement can be communicated by modes other than visual. The banner-tailed kangaroo rat produces several complex foot-drumming patterns in a number of different contexts, one of which is when it encounters a snake. The foot-drumming may alert nearby offspring but most likely conveys vibrations through the ground that the rat is too alert for a successful attack, thus preventing the snake's predatory pursuit.[72]

Predator to prey Edit

 
The humpback anglerfish angles for small fish by deceptively dangling a bioluminescent lure in front of its jaws.

Typically, predators attempt to reduce communication to prey as this will generally reduce the effectiveness of their hunting. However, some forms of predator to prey communication occur in ways that change the behavior of the prey and make their capture easier, i.e. deception by the predator. A well-known example is the angler fish, an ambush predator which waits for its prey to come to it. It has a fleshy bioluminescent growth protruding from its forehead which it dangles in front of its jaws. Smaller fish attempt to take the lure, placing themselves in a better position for the angler fish to catch them. Another example of deceptive communication is observed in the genus of jumping spiders (Myrmarachne). These spiders are commonly referred to as "antmimicking spiders" because of the way they wave their front legs in the air to simulate antennae.

Human/animal Edit

Various ways in which humans interpret the behavior of animals, or give commands to them, are consistent with the definition of interspecies communication. Skillful interpretation of animal communications may be critical to the welfare of animals that are being cared for or trained by humans. Winjngaarden suggests IIC as a way to communicate with animals. IIC is useful because it allows the flexibility of people and animals to essentially understand.[73] For example, behavior indicating pain need to be recognized. Indeed, the survival of both the animal and its human caretaker may be at stake if, for example, a human fails to recognize a signal for imminent attack. It is also important to take into account that non-human animal species may interpret the signals of humans differently than humans themselves. For instance, a pointing command refers to a location rather than an object in dogs.[74]

Since the late 90s, one scientist, Sean Senechal, has been developing, studying, and using the learned visible, expressive language in dogs and horses. By teaching these animals a gestural (human made) American Sign Language-like language, the animals have been found to use the new signs on their own to get what they need.[75] The recent experiments on animal language are perhaps the most sophisticated attempt yet to establish human/animal communication, though their relation to natural animal communication is uncertain.

Other aspects Edit

Evolution Edit

The importance of communication is evident from the highly elaborate morphology, behaviour and physiology that some animals have evolved to facilitate this. These include some of the most striking structures in the animal kingdom, such as the peacock's tail, the antlers of a stag and the frill of the frill-necked lizard, but also include even the modest red spot on a European herring gull's bill. Highly elaborate behaviours have evolved for communication such as the dancing of cranes, the pattern changes of cuttlefish, and the gathering and arranging of materials by bowerbirds. Other evidence for the importance of communication in animals is the prioritisation of physiological features to this function. For example, birdsong appears to have brain structures entirely devoted to its production. All these adaptations require evolutionary explanation.

There are two aspects to the required explanation:

  • identifying a route by which an animal that lacked the relevant feature or behaviour could acquire it;
  • identifying the selective pressure that makes it adaptive for animals to develop structures that facilitate communication, emit communications, and respond to them.

Significant contributions to the first of these problems were made by Konrad Lorenz and other early ethologists. By comparing related species within groups, they showed that movements and body parts that in the primitive forms had no communicative function could be "captured" in a context where communication would be functional for one or both partners, and could evolve into a more elaborate, specialised form. For example, Desmond Morris showed in a study of grass finches that a beak-wiping response occurred in a range of species, serving a preening function, but that in some species this had been elaborated into a courtship signal.[76]

The second problem has been more controversial. The early ethologists assumed that communication occurred for the good of the species as a whole, but this would require a process of group selection which is believed to be mathematically impossible in the evolution of sexually reproducing animals. Altruism towards an unrelated group is not widely accepted in the scientific community, but rather can be seen as reciprocal altruism, expecting the same behaviour from others, a benefit of living in a group. Sociobiologists argued that behaviours that benefited a whole group of animals might emerge as a result of selection pressures acting solely on the individual. A gene-centered view of evolution proposes that behaviours that enabled a gene to become wider established within a population would become positively selected for, even if their effect on individuals or the species as a whole was detrimental;[77]

 
The apparently excessive eye-spot signalling by the male peacock tail may be runaway selection

In the case of communication, an important discussion by John Krebs and Richard Dawkins established hypotheses for the evolution of such apparently altruistic or mutualistic communications as alarm calls and courtship signals to emerge under individual selection. This led to the realization that communication might not always be "honest" (indeed, there are some obvious examples where it is not, as in mimicry). The possibility of evolutionarily stable dishonest communication has been the subject of much controversy, with Amotz Zahavi in particular arguing that it cannot exist in the long term. Sociobiologists have also been concerned with the evolution of apparently excessive signaling structures such as the peacock's tail; it is widely thought that these can only emerge as a result of sexual selection, which can create a positive feedback process that leads to the rapid exaggeration of a characteristic that confers an advantage in a competitive mate-selection situation.

One theory to explain the evolution of traits like a peacock's tail is 'runaway selection'. This requires two traits—a trait that exists, like the bright tail, and a preexisting bias in the female to select for that trait. Females prefer the more elaborate tails, and thus those males are able to mate successfully. Exploiting the psychology of the female, a positive feedback loop is enacted and the tail becomes bigger and brighter. Eventually, the evolution will level off because the survival costs to the male do not allow for the trait to be elaborated any further.[78] Two theories exist to explain runaway selection. The first is the good genes hypothesis. This theory states that an elaborate display is an honest signal of fitness and truly is a better mate. The second is the handicap hypothesis. This explains that the peacock's tail is a handicap, requiring energy to keep and makes it more visible to predators. Thus, the signal is costly to maintain, and remains an honest indicator of the signaler's condition. Another assumption is that the signal is more costly for low quality males to produce than for higher quality males to produce. This is simply because the higher quality males have more energy reserves available to allocate to costly signaling.[4]

Cognitive aspects Edit

Ethologists and sociobiologists have characteristically analysed animal communication in terms of more or less automatic responses to stimuli, without raising the question of whether the animals concerned understand the meaning of the signals they emit and receive. That is a key question in animal cognition. There are some signalling systems that seem to demand a more advanced understanding. A much discussed example is the use of alarm calls by vervet monkeys. Robert Seyfarth and Dorothy Cheney showed that these animals emit different alarm calls in the presence of different predators (leopards, eagles, and snakes), and the monkeys that hear the calls respond appropriately—but that this ability develops over time, and also takes into account the experience of the individual emitting the call. Metacommunication, discussed above, also seems to require a more sophisticated cognitive process.

It has been reported [79] that bottlenose dolphins can recognize identity information from whistles even when otherwise stripped of the characteristics of the whistle; making dolphins the only animals other than humans that have been shown to transmit identity information independent of the caller's voice or location. The paper concludes that:

The fact that signature whistle shape carries identity information independent from voice features presents the possibility to use these whistles as referential signals, either addressing individuals or referring to them, similar to the use of names in humans. Given the cognitive abilities of bottlenose dolphins, their vocal learning and copying skills, and their fission–fusion social structure, this possibility is an intriguing one that demands further investigation.

— V. M. Janik, et al. [79]

Human behaviour Edit

Another controversial issue is the extent to which human behaviours resemble animal communication, or whether all such communication has disappeared as a result of our linguistic capacity. Some of our bodily features—eyebrows, beards and moustaches, deep adult male voices, perhaps female breasts—strongly resemble adaptations to producing signals. Ethologists such as Irenäus Eibl-Eibesfeldt have argued that facial gestures such as smiling, grimacing, and the eyebrow flash on greeting are universal human communicative signals that can be related to corresponding signals in other primates. Given how recently spoken language has emerged, it is very likely that human body language does include some more or less involuntary responses that have a similar origin to the communication we have.[80]

Humans also often seek to mimic animals' communicative signals in order to interact with them. For example, cats have a mild affiliative response of slowly closing their eyes; humans often mimic this signal towards a pet cat to establish a tolerant relationship. Stroking, petting and rubbing pet animals are all actions that probably work through their natural patterns of interspecific communication.

Dogs have shown an ability to understand human communication. In object choice tasks, dogs utilize human communicative gestures such as pointing and direction of gaze in order to locate hidden food and toys.[81] However, in contrast to humans pointing has a different meaning for dogs as it refers to a direction or location.[82] It has also been shown that dogs exhibit a left gaze bias when looking at human faces, indicating that they are capable of reading human emotions.[83] Dogs do not make use of direction of gaze or exhibit left gaze bias with other dogs.

A new approach in the 21st century in the field of animal communication uses applied behavioural analysis, specifically functional communication training. This form of training previously has been used in schools and clinics with humans with special needs, such as children with autism, to help them develop language. Sean Senechal at the AnimalSign Center has been using an approach similar to functional communication training with domesticated animals, such as dogs since 2004 and horses since 2000, with encouraging results and benefits to the animals and people. Functional communication training for animals, Senechal calls "Animal Sign Language". This includes teaching communication through gestures (like simplified American sign language), Picture Exchange Communication System, tapping, and vocalisation. The process for animals includes simplified and modified techniques.[citation needed]

Linguistics Edit

External video
  Do animals have language? - Michele Bishop, TED Ed, 4:54, September 10, 2015[84]

For linguistics, the interest of animal communication systems lies in their similarities to and differences from human language:

    1. Human languages are characterized for having a double articulation (in the characterization of French linguist André Martinet). It means that complex linguistic expressions can be broken down in meaningful elements (such as morphemes and words), which in turn are composed of smallest phonetic elements that affect meaning, called phonemes. Animal signals, however, do not exhibit this dual structure.
    2. In general, animal utterances are responses to external stimuli, and do not refer to matters removed in time and space. Matters of relevance at a distance, such as distant food sources, tend to be indicated to other individuals by body language instead, for example wolf activity before a hunt, or the information conveyed in honeybee dance language. It is therefore unclear to what extent utterances are automatic responses and to what extent deliberate intent plays a part.
    3. In contrast to human language, animal communication systems are usually not able to express conceptual generalizations. (Cetaceans and some primates may be notable exceptions).[85]
    4. Human languages combine elements to produce new messages (a property known as creativity). One factor in this is that much human language growth is based upon conceptual ideas and hypothetical structures, both being far greater capabilities in humans than animals. This appears far less common in animal communication systems, although current research into animal culture is still an ongoing process with many new discoveries. In 2009 it was reported that affixation may play a role in the call meanings of Campbell's mona monkey.[86]

Errors in communication Edit

There becomes possibility for error within communication between animals when certain circumstances apply.[87] These circumstances could include distance between the two communicating subjects, as well as the complexity of the signal that is being communicated to the "listener" of the situation. It may not always be clear to the "listener" where the location of the communication is coming from, as the "singer" can sometimes deceive them and create more error.[88]

See also Edit

References Edit

  1. ^ "Animal communication". Encyclopedia Britannica. Retrieved 2020-10-31.
  2. ^ Shah, Sonia (20 September 2023). "The Animals Are Talking. What Does It Mean? - Language was long understood as a human-only affair. New research suggests that isn't so. + comment". The New York Times. Archived from the original on 21 September 2023. Retrieved 21 September 2023.
  3. ^ Seyfarth, Robert M.; Cheney, Dorothy L. (2003-02-01). "Signalers and Receivers in Animal Communication". Annual Review of Psychology. 54 (1): 145–173. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145121. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 12359915.
  4. ^ a b Maynard-Smith and Harper, 2003
  5. ^ Tinbergen, N.; Perdeck, A. C. (1951-01-01). "On the Stimulus Situation Releasing the Begging Response in the Newly Hatched Herring Gull Chick (Larus Argentatus Argentatus Pont.)". Behaviour. 3 (1): 1–39. doi:10.1163/156853951X00197. ISSN 0005-7959.
  6. ^ Pollick, Amy S.; Waal, Frans B. M. de (2007-05-08). "Ape gestures and language evolution". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (19): 8184–8189. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.8184P. doi:10.1073/pnas.0702624104. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 1876592. PMID 17470779.
  7. ^ Bertin, Aline; Beraud, Arielle; Lansade, Léa; et al. (22 August 2018). "Facial display and blushing: Means of visual communication in blue-and-yellow macaws (Ara Ararauna)?". PLOS ONE. 13 (8). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0201762. ProQuest 2091762411.
  8. ^ Mogil, Jeffrey S. (April 2009). "Animal models of pain: progress and challenges". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 10 (4): 283–294. doi:10.1038/nrn2606. ISSN 1471-003X. PMID 19259101. S2CID 205504814.
  9. ^ a b Shepherd, Stephen V. (2010-03-19). "Following Gaze: Gaze-Following Behavior as a Window into Social Cognition". Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 4: 5. doi:10.3389/fnint.2010.00005. ISSN 1662-5145. PMC 2859805. PMID 20428494.
  10. ^ Range, Friederike; Virányi, Zsófia (2011-02-23). Wylie, Doug (ed.). "Development of Gaze Following Abilities in Wolves (Canis Lupus)". PLOS ONE. 6 (2): e16888. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...616888R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0016888. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3044139. PMID 21373192.
  11. ^ Cloney, R.A.; Florey, E. (1968). "Ultrastructure of cephalopod chromatophore organs". Z. Zellforsch. Mikrosk. Anat. 89 (2): 250–280. doi:10.1007/bf00347297. PMID 5700268. S2CID 26566732.
  12. ^ Hanlon, R.T.; Messenger, J.B. (1996). Cephalopod Behaviour. Cambridge University Press. p. 121. ISBN 978-0-521-64583-6.
  13. ^ Williams, Sarah (2012). . Science Now. Archived from the original on March 8, 2013. Retrieved March 16, 2013.
  14. ^ Motluk, Alison (2001). "Big Bottom". New Scientist. 19 (7).
  15. ^ Deep Sea Squid May Communicate Through Glowing Pigmentation, Researchers Find
  16. ^ Mason, Julia (July 18, 2018). "Glowing in the Deep". The Dish on Science.
  17. ^ "Bioluminescence | Smithsonian Ocean". ocean.si.edu. Retrieved 2020-10-25.
  18. ^ Ehrlich, Paul R.; David S. Dobkin & Darryl Wheye. ""Bird Voices" and "Vocal Development" from Birds of Stanford essays". Retrieved 9 Sep 2008.
  19. ^ Schwartzkopff, J (January 1977). "Auditory Communication in Lower Animals: Role of Auditory Physiology". Annual Review of Psychology. 28 (1): 61–84. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.28.020177.000425. ISSN 0066-4308. PMID 324382.
  20. ^ Slabbekoorn, Hans; Smith, Thomas B (2002-04-29). "Bird song, ecology and speciation". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 357 (1420): 493–503. doi:10.1098/rstb.2001.1056. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 1692962. PMID 12028787.
  21. ^ Mikula, P.; Valcu, M.; Brumm, H.; Bulla, M.; Forstmeier, W.; Petrusková, T.; Kempenaers, B. & Albrecht, T. (2021). "A global analysis of song frequency in passerines provides no support for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis but suggests a role for sexual selection". Ecology Letters. 24 (3): 477–486. doi:10.1111/ele.13662. PMID 33314573.
  22. ^ Zuberbühler, Klaus (2001). "Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell's monkeys, Cercopithecus campbelli" (PDF). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 5: 414–422.
  23. ^ Boughman, J W (1998-02-07). "Vocal learning by greater spear-nosed bats". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 265 (1392): 227–233. doi:10.1098/rspb.1998.0286. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 1688873. PMID 9493408.
  24. ^ Price, Tabitha; Wadewitz, Philip; Cheney, Dorothy; Seyfarth, Robert; Hammerschmidt, Kurt; Fischer, Julia (2015-08-19). "Vervets revisited: A quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity". Scientific Reports. 5 (1): 13220. Bibcode:2015NatSR...513220P. doi:10.1038/srep13220. ISSN 2045-2322. PMC 4541072. PMID 26286236.
  25. ^ Jabr, Ferris (2017-05-12). "Can Prairie Dogs Talk? (Published 2017)". The New York Times. ISSN 0362-4331. Retrieved 2020-10-25.
  26. ^ "New Language Discovered: Prairiedogese". NPR.org. Retrieved 2020-10-25.
  27. ^ . 2016-10-27. Archived from the original on 2016-10-27. Retrieved 2020-10-25.
  28. ^ "Prairie dogs' language decoded by scientists | CBC News". CBC. Retrieved 2020-10-25.
  29. ^ January 2006, Bjorn Carey 03 (3 January 2006). "Whales Found to Speak in Dialects". livescience.com. Retrieved 2020-10-25.
  30. ^ Weilgart, Linda; Whitehead, H. (1997-05-01). "Group-specific dialects and geographical variation in coda repertoire in South Pacific sperm whales". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 40 (5): 277–285. doi:10.1007/s002650050343. ISSN 1432-0762. S2CID 11845118.
  31. ^ Ford, John Kenneth Baker (1984). Call Traditions and Dialects of Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) in British Columbia. Retrospective Theses and Dissertations, 1919-2007 (Thesis). University of British Columbia. p. 284. doi:10.14288/1.0096602.
  32. ^ Souhaut M; Shields MW (2021). "Stereotyped whistles in southern resident killer whales". Aquatic Biology. PeerJ. 9: e12085. doi:10.7717/peerj.12085. PMC 8404572. PMID 34532160. Retrieved 2 January 2023.
  33. ^ Ladich, Friedrich (2001). "Sound-generating and -detecting motor system in catfish: Design of swimbladder muscles in doradids and pimelodids". The Anatomical Record. 263 (3): 297–306. doi:10.1002/ar.1105. ISSN 1097-0185. PMID 11455539. S2CID 24896586.
  34. ^ a b c d e Searcy, William A. (March 2013). "Principles of Animal Communication. Second Edition. By Jack W. Bradbury and Sandra L. Vehrencamp. Sunderland (Massachusetts): Sinauer Associates. $99.95. xiv + 697 p.; ill. + C-1–C-6 (credits) + I-1–I-47 (index). ISBN: 978-0-87893-045-6. 2011". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 88 (1): 48. doi:10.1086/669301. ISSN 0033-5770.
  35. ^ (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 June 2021. Retrieved 3 June 2021.
  36. ^ Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: I. The meaning of situationally variable alarm calls by Daneil T. Blumstein & Kenneth B. Armitage Department of Systematics and Ecology, University of Kansas. Published by Animal Behavior, via UCLA Life Sciences
  37. ^ Brown, Grant E.; Chivers, Douglas P.; Smith, R. Jan F. (1995-02-01). "Localized defecation by pike: a response to labelling by cyprinid alarm pheromone?". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 36 (2): 105–110. doi:10.1007/BF00170715. ISSN 1432-0762. S2CID 31875357.
  38. ^ Brown, Grant; Adrian, James; Patton, Todd; Chivers, Douglas (Dec 2001). "Fathead minnows learn to recognize predator odor when exposed to concentrations of artificial alarm pheromone below their behavioral-response threshold". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 79 (12): 2239–2245. doi:10.1139/z01-194. ProQuest 220512135 – via ProQuest.
  39. ^ a b "Acid Rain Is Totally So Last Century, Right? Not Exactly: A Canadian scientist explains how acid rain is still making its mark – IISD Experimental Lakes Area". 16 May 2018. Retrieved 2020-10-26.
  40. ^ du P. Bothma, J.; le Richet, E. A. N. (1995-04-01). "Evidence of the use of rubbing, scent-marking andscratching-posts by Kalahari leopards". Journal of Arid Environments. 29 (4): 511–517. Bibcode:1995JArEn..29..511D. doi:10.1016/S0140-1963(95)80023-9. ISSN 0140-1963.
  41. ^ Gosling, L. Morris; Roberts, S. Craig (2001). "Scent-marking by male mammals: cheat-proof signals to competitors and mates" (PDF). Advances in the Study of Behavior. 30: 169–217. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(01)80007-3. ISBN 9780120045303.
  42. ^ du P. Bothma, J.; Richet, E.A.N. le (1995). "Evidence of the use of rubbing, scent-marking andscratching-posts by Kalahari leopards". Journal of Arid Environments. 29 (4): 511–517. Bibcode:1995JArEn..29..511D. doi:10.1016/s0140-1963(95)80023-9.
  43. ^ Clapham, Melanie. Chemical signalling in brown bears, ursus arctos : an assessment of scent marking strategies and social function. OCLC 1065010384.
  44. ^ "Electrocommunication". archive.fo. 2012-05-29. Archived from the original on 2012-05-29. Retrieved 2020-10-26.
  45. ^ Worm, Martin; Kirschbaum, Frank; von der Emde, Gerhard (2017-09-13). "Social interactions between live and artificial weakly electric fish: Electrocommunication and locomotor behavior of Mormyrus rume proboscirostris towards a mobile dummy fish". PLOS ONE. 12 (9): e0184622. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1284622W. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0184622. PMC 5597219. PMID 28902915.
  46. ^ Donati, Elisa; Worm, Martin; Mintchev, Stefano; van der Wiel, Marleen; Benelli, Giovanni; von der Emde, Gerhard; Stefanini, Cesare (2016-12-01). "Investigation of Collective Behaviour and Electrocommunication in the Weakly Electric Fish, Mormyrus rume, through a biomimetic Robotic Dummy Fish". Bioinspiration & Biomimetics. 11 (6): 066009. Bibcode:2016BiBi...11f6009D. doi:10.1088/1748-3190/11/6/066009. ISSN 1748-3190. PMID 27906686. S2CID 26047199.
  47. ^ Kuo, Zing (Jan 1, 1960). "Studies on the basic factors in animal fighting: VI. Inter-species coexistence in birds". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 97: 15. ProQuest 1297122533 – via ProQuest.
  48. ^ Yamamoto, Maria Emilia; Araujo, Arrilton; Arruda, Maria de Fatima; Lima, Ana Karinne Moreira; Siqueira, Jose de Oliveira; Hattori, Wallisen Tadashi (2014-11-01). "Male and female breeding strategies in a cooperative primate". Behavioural Processes. Neotropical Behaviour. 109: 27–33. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2014.06.009. ISSN 0376-6357. PMID 25010563. S2CID 205979349.
  49. ^ Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010-02-01). "The social role of touch in humans and primates: Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. Touch, Temperature, Pain/Itch and Pleasure. 34 (2): 260–268. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001. ISSN 0149-7634. PMID 18662717. S2CID 30450770.
  50. ^ "Ants' body odor, physical contact get worker ants working, study finds". news.stanford.edu. Retrieved 2020-10-26.
  51. ^ Sukhchuluun, Gansukh; Zhang, Xue-Ying; Chi, Qing-Sheng; Wang, De-Hua (2018-05-18). "Huddling Conserves Energy, Decreases Core Body Temperature, but Increases Activity in Brandt's Voles (Lasiopodomys brandtii)". Frontiers in Physiology. 9: 563. doi:10.3389/fphys.2018.00563. ISSN 1664-042X. PMC 5968109. PMID 29867585.
  52. ^ "Coral Reef Protection: What Are Coral Reefs? | Habitat Protection | US EPA". archive.epa.gov. Retrieved 2020-10-26.
  53. ^ Cocroft, Reginald (October 2001). "Vibrational Communication and the Ecology of Group-Living, Herbivorous Insects". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 41: 1215–1221 – via Oxford Academic.
  54. ^ Hill, Peggy (October 2001). "Vibration and Animal Communication: A Review". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 41: 1135–1142 – via Oxford Academic.
  55. ^ Narins, Peter (April 1990). "Seismic Communication in Anuran Amphibians". BioScience. 40 (4): 268–274. doi:10.2307/1311263. JSTOR 1311263 – via Oxford Academic.
  56. ^ Kardong, Kenneth V.; Mackessy, Stephen P. (1991). "The Strike Behavior of a Congenitally Blind Rattlesnake". Journal of Herpetology. 25 (2): 208–211. doi:10.2307/1564650. ISSN 0022-1511. JSTOR 1564650.
  57. ^ Bakken, George S.; Krochmal, Aaron R. (2007-08-15). "The imaging properties and sensitivity of the facial pits of pitvipers as determined by optical and heat-transfer analysis". Journal of Experimental Biology. 210 (16): 2801–2810. doi:10.1242/jeb.006965. ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 17690227. S2CID 25037159.
  58. ^ Pough, F.; Andrews, Robin; Cadle, John; Crump, Martha; Savitzky, Alan; Wells, Kentwood (2003-01-01). Herpetology (Third ed.). New York: Prentice Hall.
  59. ^ a b Gracheva, Elena O.; Ingolia, Nicholas T.; Kelly, Yvonne M.; Cordero-Morales, Julio F.; Hollopeter, Gunther; Chesler, Alexander T.; Sánchez, Elda E.; Perez, John C.; Weissman, Jonathan S.; Julius, David (April 2010). "Molecular basis of infrared detection by snakes". Nature. 464 (7291): 1006–1011. Bibcode:2010Natur.464.1006G. doi:10.1038/nature08943. ISSN 1476-4687. PMC 2855400. PMID 20228791.
  60. ^ Kürten, L.; Schmidt, U.; Schäfer, K. (1984-06-01). "Warm and cold receptors in the nose of the vampire bat Desmodus rotundas". Naturwissenschaften. 71 (6): 327–328. Bibcode:1984NW.....71..327K. doi:10.1007/BF00396621. ISSN 1432-1904. PMID 6472483. S2CID 31899356.
  61. ^ Wilson, Ben; Dill, Lawrence M. (March 2002). "Pacific herring respond to stimulated odontocete echolocation sounds". Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences. 59 (3): 542. doi:10.1139/f02-029.
  62. ^ Haldar, Vivekananda; Chakraborty, Niladri (2017-07-01). "A novel evolutionary technique based on electrolocation principle of elephant nose fish and shark: fish electrolocation optimization". Soft Computing. 21 (14): 3827–3848. doi:10.1007/s00500-016-2033-1. ISSN 1433-7479. S2CID 207013387.
  63. ^ Jones, Gareth (2005-07-12). "Echolocation". Current Biology. 15 (13): R484–R488. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.06.051. ISSN 0960-9822. PMID 16005275. S2CID 235311777.
  64. ^ Vanderelst, Dieter; Steckel, Jan; Boen, Andre; Peremans, Herbert; Holderied, Marc W (2016-08-02). Eichenbaum, Howard (ed.). "Place recognition using batlike sonar". eLife. 5: e14188. doi:10.7554/eLife.14188. ISSN 2050-084X. PMC 4970868. PMID 27481189.
  65. ^ Stritih, Natasa; Kosi, Alenka (Nov 2017). "Olfactory signaling of aggressive intent in male-male contests of cave crickets (Tropglophilus neglectus; Orthoptera: Rhaphidophoridae)". PLOS ONE. 12 (11): e0187512. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1287512S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0187512. PMC 5675388. PMID 29112984. ProQuest 1961423777.
  66. ^ Clutton-Brock, Tim (2016-05-31). Mammal Societies. John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-119-09532-3.
  67. ^ Jenssen, Thomas A.; Orrell, Kimberly S.; Lovern, Matthew B. (2000). "Sexual Dimorphisms in Aggressive Signal Structure and Use by a Polygynous Lizard, Anolis carolinensis". Copeia. 2000 (1): 140–149. doi:10.1643/0045-8511(2000)2000[0140:SDIASS]2.0.CO;2. ISSN 0045-8511. JSTOR 1448245. S2CID 9813895.
  68. ^ Bowen, Jon; Heath, Sarah (2005). Behaviour problems in small animals : practical advice for the veterinary team. Edinburgh: Elsevier Saunders. p. 127. ISBN 978-0702027673. Retrieved 16 June 2022.
  69. ^ Cäsar, Cristiane; Byrne, Richard W.; Hoppitt, William; Young, Robert J.; Zuberbühler, Klaus (2012-08-01). "Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls". Animal Behaviour. 84 (2): 405–411. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.05.010. ISSN 0003-3472. S2CID 45749417.
  70. ^ Sasaki, Takao; Hölldobler, Bert; Millar, Jocelyn G.; Pratt, Stephen C. (2014-09-15). "A context-dependent alarm signal in the ant Temnothorax rugatulus". Journal of Experimental Biology. 217 (18): 3229–3236. doi:10.1242/jeb.106849. hdl:2286/R.I.28102. ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 25013103. S2CID 2102542.
  71. ^ a b Hasson, O. (October 1991). "Pursuit-deterrent signals: communication between prey and predator". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 6 (10): 325–329. doi:10.1016/0169-5347(91)90040-5. ISSN 0169-5347. PMID 21232498.
  72. ^ . Archived from the original on 2020-11-29. Retrieved 2020-10-28.
  73. ^ Wijngaarden, Vanessa (2023-02-08). "Interviewing Animals Through Animal Communicators: Potentials of Intuitive Interspecies Communication for Multispecies Methods". Society & Animals. -1 (aop): 1–21. doi:10.1163/15685306-bja10122. ISSN 1568-5306.
  74. ^ Tauzin, Tibor; Csík, Andor; Kis, Anna; Topál, József (2015). "What or where? The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs (Canis familiaris)" (PDF). Journal of Comparative Psychology. 129 (4): 334–338. doi:10.1037/a0039462. ISSN 1939-2087. PMID 26147704.
  75. ^ Sean Senechal: Dogs can sign, too. A breakthrough method of teaching your dog to communicate to you, 2009, Random House/Crown/TenSpeed Press
  76. ^ Morris, Desmond (1958). "The Comparative Ethology of Grassfinches (erythrurae) and Mannikins (amadinae)". Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London. 131 (3): 389–439. doi:10.1111/j.1096-3642.1958.tb00695.x. ISSN 1469-7998.
  77. ^ discussed at length by Richard Dawkins under the subject of his book The Selfish Gene
  78. ^ Chandler, Christopher H.; Ofria, Charles; Dworkin, Ian (2013). "Runaway Sexual Selection Leads to Good Genes". Evolution. 67 (1): 110–119. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2012.01750.x. ISSN 0014-3820. JSTOR 23327705. PMID 23289565. S2CID 15929198.
  79. ^ a b V. M. Janik, L. S. Sayigh, and R. S. Wells: "Signature whistle shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins", Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, vol. 103 no 21, May 23, 2006
  80. ^ Smith, Eric Alden (2017-09-29). Evolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior. Routledge. ISBN 978-1-351-52132-1.
  81. ^ Hare, Brian; Call, Josep; Tomasello, Michael (1998-01-01). "Communication of Food Location Between Human and Dog (Canis Familiaris)". Evolution of Communication. 2 (1): 137–159. doi:10.1075/eoc.2.1.06har. ISSN 1387-5337.
  82. ^ Tauzin, Tibor; Csík, Andor; Kis, Anna; Topál, József (November 2015). "What or where? The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs (Canis familiaris)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 129 (4): 334–338. doi:10.1037/a0039462. ISSN 1939-2087. PMID 26147704.
  83. ^ Guo, Kun; Meints, Kerstin; Hall, Charlotte; Hall, Sophie; Mills, Daniel (2009-05-01). "Left gaze bias in humans, rhesus monkeys and domestic dogs". Animal Cognition. 12 (3): 409–418. doi:10.1007/s10071-008-0199-3. ISSN 1435-9456. PMID 18925420. S2CID 5661394.
  84. ^ "Do animals have language? - Michele Bishop". TED Ed. 10 September 2015. Retrieved 11 September 2015.
  85. ^ Thompson, Roger K. R.; Oden, David L. (2000). "Categorical Perception and Conceptual Judgments by Nonhuman Primates: The Paleological Monkey and the Analogical Ape". Cognitive Science. 24 (3): 363–396. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog2403_2. ISSN 1551-6709.
  86. ^ "Rudiments of Language Discovered in Monkeys". Wired. ISSN 1059-1028. Retrieved 2020-10-31.
  87. ^ Real, Leslie A. (1994-12-15). Behavioral Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology. University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0-226-70595-8.
  88. ^ Gergely, Anna; Compton, Anna; Newberry, Ruth; Miklósi, Ádám (Apr 2016). "Social Interaction with an "Unidentified Moving Object" Elicits A-Not_B Error in Domestic Dogs". PLOS ONE. 11 (4): e0151600. Bibcode:2016PLoSO..1151600G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0151600. PMC 4830451. PMID 27073867. S2CID 16369609.

External links Edit

  • Animal Communicator – Documentary
  • Zoosemiotics: animal communication on the web 2005-10-25 at the Wayback Machine
  • The Animal Communication Project
  • International Bioacoustics Council research on animal language.
  • Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior research on animal vocalizations.
  • Animal Sounds different animal sounds to listen and download.
  • The British Library Sound Archive contains over 150,000 recordings of animal sounds and natural atmospheres from all over the world.

animal, communication, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, this, article, adding, citations, reliable, sources, unsourced, material, challenged, removed, find, sources, news, newspapers, books, scholar, jstor, nove. This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Animal communication news newspapers books scholar JSTOR November 2016 Learn how and when to remove this template message Animal communication is the transfer of information from one or a group of animals sender or senders to one or more other animals receiver or receivers that affects the current or future behavior of the receivers 1 2 Information may be sent intentionally as in a courtship display or unintentionally as in the transfer of scent from predator to prey with kairomones Information may be transferred to an audience of several receivers 3 Animal communication is a rapidly growing area of study in disciplines including animal behavior sociology neurology and animal cognition Many aspects of animal behavior such as symbolic name use emotional expression learning and sexual behavior are being understood in new ways Great egret Ardea alba in a courtship display communicating the desire to find a mate When the information from the sender changes the behavior of a receiver the information is referred to as a signal Signalling theory predicts that for a signal to be maintained in the population both the sender and receiver should usually receive some benefit from the interaction Signal production by senders and the perception and subsequent response of receivers are thought to coevolve 4 Signals often involve multiple mechanisms e g both visual and auditory and for a signal to be understood the coordinated behaviour of both sender and receiver require careful study Contents 1 Modes 1 1 Visual 1 2 Auditory 1 3 Olfactory 1 4 Electric 1 5 Touch 1 6 Seismic 1 7 Thermal 2 Autocommunication 3 Functions 4 Interpretation of animal behaviour 5 Interspecific communication 5 1 Prey to predator 5 2 Predator to prey 5 3 Human animal 6 Other aspects 6 1 Evolution 6 2 Cognitive aspects 6 3 Human behaviour 6 4 Linguistics 7 Errors in communication 8 See also 9 References 10 External linksModes Edit nbsp A lamb investigates a rabbit an example of interspecific communication using body posture and olfaction Visual Edit For information on the perception of visual signals see Visual perception Gestures Most animals understand communication through a visual display of distinctive body parts or bodily movements Animals will reveal or accentuate a body part to relay certain information The parent herring gull displays its bright yellow bill on the ground next over its chick when it has returned to the nest with food The chicks exhibit a begging response by tapping the red spot on the lower mandible of the parent herring gull s bill This signal stimulates the parent to regurgitate food and completes the feeding signal The distinctive morphological feature accentuated in this communication is the parent s red spotted bill while the tapping towards the ground makes the red spot visible to the chick demonstrating a distinctive movement 5 Frans de Waal studied bonobos and chimps to understand if language was somehow evolved by gestures He found that both apes and humans only use intentional gestures to communicate 6 Facial expression Another important signal of emotion in animal communication are facial gestures Blue and Yellow Macaws were studied to understand how they reacted to interactions with a familiar animal care taker Studies show that Blue and Yellow Macaws demonstrated a significant amount of blushing frequently during mutual interactions with a caretaker 7 In another experiment Jeffrey Mogil studied facial expression in mice in response to increments of increasing pain He found that mice exhibited five recognizable face expressions orbital tightening nose and cheek bulge and changes in ear and whisker carriage 8 Gaze following Social animals use gaze following as a form of communication through monitoring head and eye orientation in other mammals 9 Studies have been conducted on apes monkeys dogs birds wolves and tortoises and have focused on two different tasks follow ing another s gaze into distant space and follow ing another s gaze geometrically around a visual barrier e g by repositioning themselves to follow a gaze cue when faced with a barrier blocking their view 10 A broad range of animals have been proven to exhibit the latter however only apes dogs wolves and corvids ravens have been able to follow another s gaze into distant space Marmosets and ibis were unable to demonstrate geometric gaze following Researchers do not yet have a clear picture of the cognitive basis of gaze following but developmental evidence indicates that simple gaze following and geometric gaze following probably rely on different cognitive mechanisms 9 Color change Color change can be separated into changes that occur during growth and development and those triggered by mood social context or abiotic factors such as temperature The latter are seen in many taxa Some cephalopods such as the octopus and the cuttlefish have specialized skin cells chromatophores that can change the apparent colour opacity and reflectiveness of their skin 11 In addition to their use for camouflage rapid changes in skin color are used while hunting and in courtship rituals 12 Cuttlefish may display two entirely different signals simultaneously from opposite sides of their body When a male cuttlefish courts a female in the presence of other males he displays a male pattern facing the female and a female pattern facing away to deceive other males 13 Some color signals occur in cycles For example when a female olive baboon begins to ovulate her anogenital area swells and turns a bright red pink This signals to males that she is ready to mate 14 Humboldt squid are bioluminescent and thus capable of communicating visually in dark ocean environments 15 Bioluminescent communication Communication by the production of light occurs commonly in vertebrates and invertebrates in the oceans particularly at depths e g angler fish Two well known forms of land bioluminescence occur in fireflies and glow worms Other insects insect larvae annelids arachnids and even species of fungi possess bioluminescent abilities Some bioluminescent animals produce the light themselves whereas others have a symbiotic relationship with bioluminescent bacteria 16 Animals exhibit bioluminescent light to lure in prey attract a mate or protect themselves from potential predators 17 See also List of bioluminescent organisms Auditory Edit Main article Animal language nbsp Bird calls can serve as alarms or keep members of a flock in contact while the longer and more complex bird songs are associated with courtship and mating 18 nbsp Crows flocking source source A flock of American crows Problems playing this file See media help source source Humpback Whale singing at Southern Ocean feeding grounds Many animals communicate through vocalization Vocal communication serves many purposes including mating rituals warning calls conveying location of food sources and social learning In a number of species males perform calls during mating rituals as a form of competition against other males and to signal to females Examples include frogs hammer headed bats red deer humpback whales elephant seals and songbirds 19 20 21 Other instances of vocal communication include the alarm calls of the Campbell monkey 22 the territorial calls of gibbons and the use of frequency in greater spear nosed bats to distinguish between groups 23 The vervet monkey gives a distinct alarm call for each of its four different predators and the reactions of other monkeys vary appropriately according to the call For example if an alarm call signals a python the monkeys climb into the trees whereas the eagle alarm causes monkeys to seek a hiding place on the ground 24 Prairie dogs also use complex calls that signal predator differences According to Con Slobodchikoff and others prairie dog calls communicate the type size and speed of an approaching predator 25 26 27 28 Whale vocalizations have been found to have different dialects based on social learning 29 30 Mammalian acoustic culture was first discovered in southern resident orcas in 1978 31 32 Not all animals use vocalization as a means of auditory communication Many arthropods rub specialized body parts together to produce sound This is known as stridulation Crickets and grasshoppers are well known for this but many others use stridulation as well including crustaceans spiders scorpions wasps ants beetles butterflies moths millipedes and centipedes Another means of auditory communication is the vibration of swim bladders in bony fish The structure of swim bladders and the attached sonic muscles varies greatly across bony fish families resulting in a wide variety of sounds 33 Striking body parts together can also produce auditory signals A well known example of this is the tail tip vibration of rattlesnakes as a warning signal Other examples include bill clacking in birds wing clapping in manakin courtship displays and chest beating in gorillas 34 nbsp An alert motionless groundhog whistles when alarmed to warn other groundhogs Burrowing animals species are known to whistle to communicate threats and sometimes mood Species such as the marmot species including the groundhog woodchuck the alpine marmot show this trait Whistling is used by animals such as prairie dogs to communicate threats with prairie dogs having one of the most complex communication systems in the animal kingdom Prairie dogs are able to communicate an animal s speed shape size species and for humans specific attire and if the human is carrying a gun 35 This method of communication is usually done by having a sentry stand on two feet surveying for potential threats while the rest of the pack finds food Once a threat has been identified the sentry sounds a whistle alarm sometimes describing the threat at which point the pack retreats to their burrows The intensity of the threat is usually determined by how long the sentry whistles The sentry continues to whistle the alarm until the entirety of the pack have gone to safety at which point the sentry returns to the burrow 36 Olfactory Edit See also Olfactic communication nbsp Flehmen response in a tigerDespite being the oldest method of communication chemical communication is one of the least understood forms due in part to the sheer abundance of chemicals in our environment and the difficulty of detecting and measuring all the chemicals in a sample 34 The ability to detect chemicals in the environment serves many functions a crucial one being the detection of food a function that first arose in single celled organisms bacteria living in the oceans during the early days of life on Earth 34 As this function evolved organisms began to differentiate between chemicals compounds emanating from resources conspecifics same species i e mates and kin and heterospecifics different species i e competitors and predators 34 For instance a small minnow species may do well to avoid habitat with a detectable concentration of chemical cue associated with a predator species such as northern pike 37 Minnows with the ability to perceive the presence of predators before they are close enough to be seen and then respond with adaptive behavior such as hiding are more likely to survive and reproduce 38 Atlantic salmon go a step further than detecting a predator s cue when an individual is damaged by a predator it releases a chemical cue to its conspecifics 39 As has also been observed in other species acidification and changes in pH physically disrupt these chemical cues which has various implications for animal behavior 39 40 Scent marking and scent rubbing are common forms of olfactory communication in mammals 41 42 An example of scent rubbing by an animal can be seen from bears bears do this as a way to mark territory or let others know they are there and to stay away 43 Electric Edit Main article Electrocommunication Electrocommunication is a rare form of communication in animals It is seen primarily in aquatic animals though some land mammals notably the platypus and echidnas sense electric fields that might be used for communication 44 Weakly electric fishes provide an example of electrocommunication together with electrolocation These fish use an electric organ to generate an electric field which is detected by electroreceptors Differences in the waveform and frequency of changes in the field convey information on species sex and identity These electric signals can be generated in response to hormones circadian rhythms and interactions with other fish They can also serve to mediate social hierarchy amongst species that have a social order 45 Some predators such as sharks and rays are able to eavesdrop on these electrogenic fish through passive electroreception 46 Touch Edit For more on the mechanism for touch see Somatosensory system and MechanoreceptorsTouch is a key factor in many social interactions Here are some examples Fighting In a fight touch may be used to challenge an opponent and to coordinate movements during the fight It may also be used by the loser to indicate submission 47 Mating Mammals often initiate mating by grooming stroking or rubbing against each other This provides the opportunity to apply chemical signals and to assess those excreted by the potential mate Touch may also announce the intention of the male to mount the female as when a male kangaroo grabs the tail of a female During mating touch stimuli are important for pair positioning coordination and genital stimulation 48 Social integration Touch is widely used for social integration a use that is typified by the social grooming of one animal by another Social grooming has several functions it removes parasites and debris from the groomed animal it reaffirms the social bond or hierarchical relationship between the animals and it gives the groomer an opportunity to examine olfactory cues on the groomed individual perhaps adding additional ones This behaviour has been observed in social insects birds and mammals 49 Foraging Some ant species recruit fellow workers to new food finds by first tapping them with their antennae and forelegs then leading them to the food source while keeping physical contact Patrollers leave the nest to check for danger nearby and return to recruit foragers by making physical contact 50 Another example of this is the waggle dance of honey bees 34 Huddling Prolonged physical contact or huddling also serves social integration Huddling promotes heat exchange together with the transfer of olfactory or tactile information 51 Some organisms live in constant contact in a colony for example colonial corals When individuals are linked tightly in this way an entire colony can react on the aversive or alarm movements made by only a few individuals 52 In several herbivorous insect nymphs and larvae aggregations where there is prolonged contact play a major role in group coordination These aggregations may take the form of a procession or a rosette 53 Seismic Edit Main article Seismic communication Seismic communication is the exchange of information using self generated vibrational signals transmitted via a substrate such as the soil water spider webs plant stems or a blade of grass This form of communication has several advantages for example it can be sent regardless of light and noise levels and it usually has a short range and short persistence which may reduce the danger of detection by predators The use of seismic communication is found in many taxa including frogs kangaroo rats mole rats bees nematode worms and others Tetrapods usually make seismic waves by drumming on the ground with a body part a signal that is sensed by the sacculus of the receiver 54 The sacculus is an organ in the inner ear containing a membranous sac that is used for balance but can also detect seismic waves in animals that use this form of communication Vibrations may be combined with other sorts of communication 55 Thermal Edit Main article Thermoreception nbsp A python top and rattlesnake illustrating the positions of the pit organs Red arrows indicate the pit organs whereas black arrows indicate the nostril A number of different snakes have the ability to sense infrared IR thermal radiation which allows these reptiles to derive thermal images from the radiant heat emitted by predators or prey at wavelengths between 5 and 30 mm The accuracy of this sense is such that a blind rattlesnake can target its strike to the vulnerable body parts of a prey animal 56 It was previously thought that the pit organs evolved primarily as prey detectors but it is now believed that they may also be used to control body temperature 57 The facial pits enabling thermoregulation underwent parallel evolution in pitvipers and some boas and pythons having evolved once in pitvipers and multiple times in boas and pythons 58 The electrophysiology of the structure is similar between lineages but it differs in gross structure anatomy Most superficially pitvipers possess one large pit organ on either side of the head between the eye and the nostril loreal pit while boas and pythons have three or more comparatively smaller pits lining the upper and sometimes the lower lip in or between the scales Those of the pitvipers are the more advanced having a suspended sensory membrane as opposed to a simple pit structure Within the family Viperidae the pit organ is seen only in the subfamily Crotalinae the pitvipers Despite the detection of IR radiation the pits IR mechanism is dissimilar to photoreceptors while photoreceptors detect light via photochemical reactions the protein in the facial pits of snakes is a temperature sensitive ion channel It senses infrared signals through a mechanism involving warming of the pit organ rather than chemical reaction to light 59 This is consistent with the thin pit membrane which allows incoming IR radiation to quickly and precisely warm a given ion channel and trigger a nerve impulse as well as vascularize the pit membrane to rapidly cool the ion channel back to its original resting or inactive temperature 59 Common vampire bats Desmodus rotundus have specialized IR sensors in their nose leaf 60 Vampire bats are the only mammals that feed exclusively on blood The IR sense enables Desmodus to localize homeothermic animals such as cattle and horses within a range of about 10 to 15 cm This infrared perception may be used in detecting regions of maximal blood flow on targeted prey Autocommunication EditAutocommunication is a type of communication in which the sender and receiver are the same individual The sender emits a signal that is altered by the environment and eventually is received by the same individual The altered signal provides information that can indicate food predators or conspecifics Because the sender and receiver are the same animal selection pressure maximizes signal efficacy i e the degree to which an emitted signal is correctly identified by a receiver despite propagation distortion and noise There are some species such as the pacific herring which have evolved to intercept these messages from their predators They are able to use it as an early warning sign and respond defensively 61 There are two types of autocommunication The first is active electrolocation where the organism emits an electrical pulse through its electric organ and senses the projected geometrical property of the object This is found in the electric fish Gymnotiformes knifefishes and Mormyridae elephantfish 62 The second type of autocommunication is echolocation found in bats and toothed whales Echolocation involves emitting sounds and interpreting the vibrations that return from objects 63 In bats echolocation also serves the purpose of mapping their environment They are capable of recognizing a space they ve been in before without any visible light because they can memorize patterns in the feedback they get from echolocation 64 Functions EditThere are many functions of animal communication However some have been studied in more detail than others This includes Communication during contests Animal communication plays a vital role in determining the winner of contest over a resource Many species have distinct signals that signal aggression or willingness to attack or signals to convey retreat during competitions over food territories or mates 65 source source Two Red Deer roaring most likely to establish dominance during a rut However males also use loud roaring to keep track of harems of females Mating rituals Animals produce signals to attract the attention of a possible mate or to solidify pair bonds These signals frequently involve the display of body parts or postures For example a gazelle will assume characteristic poses to initiate mating Mating signals can also include the use of olfactory signals or mating calls unique to a species Animals that form lasting pair bonds often have symmetrical displays that they make to each other Famous examples are the mutual presentation of reeds by great crested grebes studied by Julian Huxley the triumph displays shown by many species of geese and penguins on their nest sites and the spectacular courtship displays by birds of paradise citation needed Copulation calls in mammals can indicate a female s breeding status or attract other mates 66 Ownership territorial Signals used to claim or defend a territory food or a mate Polygynous lizards Anolis carolinensis will show greater signs of aggression from farther distances between males than between females when defending a territory or mate It is believed that males have evolved to remain distant from each other due to higher reproductive consequences as opposed to females 67 Redirected aggression Some animals who are fearful of stimuli will attack anyone near them 68 Food related signals Many animals make food calls to attract a mate offspring or other members of a social group to a food source Perhaps the most elaborate food related signal is the Waggle dance of honeybees studied by Karl von Frisch One well known example of begging of offspring in a clutch or litter is altricial songbirds Young ravens will signal to older ravens when they encounter new or untested food Rhesus macaques will send food calls to inform other monkeys of a food source to avoid punishment Pheromones are released by many social insects to lead the other members of the society to the food source For example ants leave a pheromone trail on the ground that can be followed by other ants to lead them to the food source Alarm calls Alarm calls communicate the threat of a predator This allows all members of a social group and sometimes other species to respond accordingly This may include running for cover becoming immobile or gathering into a group to reduce the risk of attack 69 Alarm signals are not always vocalizations Crushed ants will release an alarm pheromone to attract more ants and send them into an attack state 70 Meta communication Signals that will modify the meaning of subsequent signals One example is the play face in dogs which signals that a subsequent aggressive signal is part of a play fight rather than a serious aggressive episode Interpretation of animal behaviour EditAs described above many animal gestures postures and sounds convey meaning to nearby animals These signals are often easier to describe than to interpret It is tempting especially with domesticated animals and apes to anthropomorphize that is to interpret animal actions in human terms but this can be quite misleading for example an ape s smile is often a sign of aggression Also the same gesture may have different meanings depending on context within which it occurs For example a domestic dog s tail wag and posture may be used in different ways to convey many meanings as illustrated in Charles Darwin s The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals published in 1872 Some of Darwin s illustrations are reproduced here Examples of tail position indicating different emotions in dogs nbsp Small dog watching a cat on a table nbsp Dog approaching another dog with hostile intentions nbsp Dog in a humble and affectionate frame of mind nbsp Half bred shepherd dog nbsp Dog caressing his master Interspecific communication EditMain article Interspecies communication nbsp Sentry prairie dog surveying potential dangers and threats nbsp Sentry prairie dog alerts other prairie dogs to a threat Much animal communication is intraspecific that is it occurs between members of the same species As for interspecific communication that between predator and prey is of particular interest Prey to predator Edit If a prey animal moves makes a noise or vibrations or emits a smell in such a way that a predator can detect it it is communicating with its predator 71 This is consistent with the definition of communication given above This type of communication is known as interceptive eavesdropping if a predator intercepts a message intended for conspecifics There are however some actions of prey species are clearly directed to actual or potential predators A good example is warning coloration species such as wasps that are capable of harming potential predators are often brightly coloured and this modifies the behavior of the predator who either instinctively or as the result of experience will avoid attacking such an animal Some forms of mimicry fall in the same category for example hoverflies are coloured in the same way as wasps and although they are unable to sting the strong avoidance of wasps by predators gives the hoverfly some protection There are also behavioural changes that act in a similar way to warning colouration For example canines such as wolves and coyotes may adopt an aggressive posture such as growling with their teeth bared to indicate they will fight if necessary and rattlesnakes use their well known rattle to warn potential predators of their venomous bite Sometimes a behavioural change and warning colouration will be combined as in certain species of amphibians which have most of their body coloured to blend with their surroundings except for a brightly coloured belly When confronted with a potential threat they show their belly indicating that they are poisonous in some way Another example of prey to predator communication is the pursuit deterrent signal Pursuit deterrent signals occur when prey indicates to a predator that pursuit would be unprofitable because the signaler is prepared to escape Pursuit deterrent signals provide a benefit to both the signaler and receiver they prevent the sender from wasting time and energy fleeing and they prevent the receiver from investing in a costly pursuit that is unlikely to result in capture Such signals can advertise prey s ability to escape and reflect phenotypic condition quality advertisement or can advertise that the prey has detected the predator perception advertisement 71 Pursuit deterrent signals have been reported for a wide variety of taxa including fish Godin and Davis 1995 lizards Cooper etc al 2004 ungulates Caro 1995 rabbits Holley 1993 primates Zuberbuhler et al 1997 rodents Shelley and Blumstein 2005 Clark 2005 and birds Alvarez 1993 Murphy 2006 2007 A familiar example of quality advertisement pursuit deterrent signal is stotting sometimes called pronking a pronounced combination of stiff legged running while simultaneously jumping shown by some antelopes such as Thomson s gazelle in the presence of a predator At least 11 hypotheses for stotting have been proposed A leading theory today is that it alerts predators that the element of surprise has been lost Predators like cheetahs rely on surprise attacks proven by the fact that chases are rarely successful when antelope stot Predators do not waste energy on a chase that will likely be unsuccessful optimal foraging behavior Quality advertisement can be communicated by modes other than visual The banner tailed kangaroo rat produces several complex foot drumming patterns in a number of different contexts one of which is when it encounters a snake The foot drumming may alert nearby offspring but most likely conveys vibrations through the ground that the rat is too alert for a successful attack thus preventing the snake s predatory pursuit 72 Predator to prey Edit nbsp The humpback anglerfish angles for small fish by deceptively dangling a bioluminescent lure in front of its jaws Further information Deception in animals Typically predators attempt to reduce communication to prey as this will generally reduce the effectiveness of their hunting However some forms of predator to prey communication occur in ways that change the behavior of the prey and make their capture easier i e deception by the predator A well known example is the angler fish an ambush predator which waits for its prey to come to it It has a fleshy bioluminescent growth protruding from its forehead which it dangles in front of its jaws Smaller fish attempt to take the lure placing themselves in a better position for the angler fish to catch them Another example of deceptive communication is observed in the genus of jumping spiders Myrmarachne These spiders are commonly referred to as antmimicking spiders because of the way they wave their front legs in the air to simulate antennae Human animal Edit Main article Human animal communication Various ways in which humans interpret the behavior of animals or give commands to them are consistent with the definition of interspecies communication Skillful interpretation of animal communications may be critical to the welfare of animals that are being cared for or trained by humans Winjngaarden suggests IIC as a way to communicate with animals IIC is useful because it allows the flexibility of people and animals to essentially understand 73 For example behavior indicating pain need to be recognized Indeed the survival of both the animal and its human caretaker may be at stake if for example a human fails to recognize a signal for imminent attack It is also important to take into account that non human animal species may interpret the signals of humans differently than humans themselves For instance a pointing command refers to a location rather than an object in dogs 74 Since the late 90s one scientist Sean Senechal has been developing studying and using the learned visible expressive language in dogs and horses By teaching these animals a gestural human made American Sign Language like language the animals have been found to use the new signs on their own to get what they need 75 The recent experiments on animal language are perhaps the most sophisticated attempt yet to establish human animal communication though their relation to natural animal communication is uncertain Other aspects EditEvolution Edit Further information Signalling theory The importance of communication is evident from the highly elaborate morphology behaviour and physiology that some animals have evolved to facilitate this These include some of the most striking structures in the animal kingdom such as the peacock s tail the antlers of a stag and the frill of the frill necked lizard but also include even the modest red spot on a European herring gull s bill Highly elaborate behaviours have evolved for communication such as the dancing of cranes the pattern changes of cuttlefish and the gathering and arranging of materials by bowerbirds Other evidence for the importance of communication in animals is the prioritisation of physiological features to this function For example birdsong appears to have brain structures entirely devoted to its production All these adaptations require evolutionary explanation There are two aspects to the required explanation identifying a route by which an animal that lacked the relevant feature or behaviour could acquire it identifying the selective pressure that makes it adaptive for animals to develop structures that facilitate communication emit communications and respond to them Significant contributions to the first of these problems were made by Konrad Lorenz and other early ethologists By comparing related species within groups they showed that movements and body parts that in the primitive forms had no communicative function could be captured in a context where communication would be functional for one or both partners and could evolve into a more elaborate specialised form For example Desmond Morris showed in a study of grass finches that a beak wiping response occurred in a range of species serving a preening function but that in some species this had been elaborated into a courtship signal 76 The second problem has been more controversial The early ethologists assumed that communication occurred for the good of the species as a whole but this would require a process of group selection which is believed to be mathematically impossible in the evolution of sexually reproducing animals Altruism towards an unrelated group is not widely accepted in the scientific community but rather can be seen as reciprocal altruism expecting the same behaviour from others a benefit of living in a group Sociobiologists argued that behaviours that benefited a whole group of animals might emerge as a result of selection pressures acting solely on the individual A gene centered view of evolution proposes that behaviours that enabled a gene to become wider established within a population would become positively selected for even if their effect on individuals or the species as a whole was detrimental 77 nbsp The apparently excessive eye spot signalling by the male peacock tail may be runaway selectionIn the case of communication an important discussion by John Krebs and Richard Dawkins established hypotheses for the evolution of such apparently altruistic or mutualistic communications as alarm calls and courtship signals to emerge under individual selection This led to the realization that communication might not always be honest indeed there are some obvious examples where it is not as in mimicry The possibility of evolutionarily stable dishonest communication has been the subject of much controversy with Amotz Zahavi in particular arguing that it cannot exist in the long term Sociobiologists have also been concerned with the evolution of apparently excessive signaling structures such as the peacock s tail it is widely thought that these can only emerge as a result of sexual selection which can create a positive feedback process that leads to the rapid exaggeration of a characteristic that confers an advantage in a competitive mate selection situation One theory to explain the evolution of traits like a peacock s tail is runaway selection This requires two traits a trait that exists like the bright tail and a preexisting bias in the female to select for that trait Females prefer the more elaborate tails and thus those males are able to mate successfully Exploiting the psychology of the female a positive feedback loop is enacted and the tail becomes bigger and brighter Eventually the evolution will level off because the survival costs to the male do not allow for the trait to be elaborated any further 78 Two theories exist to explain runaway selection The first is the good genes hypothesis This theory states that an elaborate display is an honest signal of fitness and truly is a better mate The second is the handicap hypothesis This explains that the peacock s tail is a handicap requiring energy to keep and makes it more visible to predators Thus the signal is costly to maintain and remains an honest indicator of the signaler s condition Another assumption is that the signal is more costly for low quality males to produce than for higher quality males to produce This is simply because the higher quality males have more energy reserves available to allocate to costly signaling 4 Cognitive aspects Edit Ethologists and sociobiologists have characteristically analysed animal communication in terms of more or less automatic responses to stimuli without raising the question of whether the animals concerned understand the meaning of the signals they emit and receive That is a key question in animal cognition There are some signalling systems that seem to demand a more advanced understanding A much discussed example is the use of alarm calls by vervet monkeys Robert Seyfarth and Dorothy Cheney showed that these animals emit different alarm calls in the presence of different predators leopards eagles and snakes and the monkeys that hear the calls respond appropriately but that this ability develops over time and also takes into account the experience of the individual emitting the call Metacommunication discussed above also seems to require a more sophisticated cognitive process It has been reported 79 that bottlenose dolphins can recognize identity information from whistles even when otherwise stripped of the characteristics of the whistle making dolphins the only animals other than humans that have been shown to transmit identity information independent of the caller s voice or location The paper concludes that The fact that signature whistle shape carries identity information independent from voice features presents the possibility to use these whistles as referential signals either addressing individuals or referring to them similar to the use of names in humans Given the cognitive abilities of bottlenose dolphins their vocal learning and copying skills and their fission fusion social structure this possibility is an intriguing one that demands further investigation V M Janik et al 79 Human behaviour Edit Another controversial issue is the extent to which human behaviours resemble animal communication or whether all such communication has disappeared as a result of our linguistic capacity Some of our bodily features eyebrows beards and moustaches deep adult male voices perhaps female breasts strongly resemble adaptations to producing signals Ethologists such as Irenaus Eibl Eibesfeldt have argued that facial gestures such as smiling grimacing and the eyebrow flash on greeting are universal human communicative signals that can be related to corresponding signals in other primates Given how recently spoken language has emerged it is very likely that human body language does include some more or less involuntary responses that have a similar origin to the communication we have 80 Humans also often seek to mimic animals communicative signals in order to interact with them For example cats have a mild affiliative response of slowly closing their eyes humans often mimic this signal towards a pet cat to establish a tolerant relationship Stroking petting and rubbing pet animals are all actions that probably work through their natural patterns of interspecific communication Dogs have shown an ability to understand human communication In object choice tasks dogs utilize human communicative gestures such as pointing and direction of gaze in order to locate hidden food and toys 81 However in contrast to humans pointing has a different meaning for dogs as it refers to a direction or location 82 It has also been shown that dogs exhibit a left gaze bias when looking at human faces indicating that they are capable of reading human emotions 83 Dogs do not make use of direction of gaze or exhibit left gaze bias with other dogs A new approach in the 21st century in the field of animal communication uses applied behavioural analysis specifically functional communication training This form of training previously has been used in schools and clinics with humans with special needs such as children with autism to help them develop language Sean Senechal at the AnimalSign Center has been using an approach similar to functional communication training with domesticated animals such as dogs since 2004 and horses since 2000 with encouraging results and benefits to the animals and people Functional communication training for animals Senechal calls Animal Sign Language This includes teaching communication through gestures like simplified American sign language Picture Exchange Communication System tapping and vocalisation The process for animals includes simplified and modified techniques citation needed Linguistics Edit External video nbsp Do animals have language Michele Bishop TED Ed 4 54 September 10 2015 84 Main article Animal language For linguistics the interest of animal communication systems lies in their similarities to and differences from human language Human languages are characterized for having a double articulation in the characterization of French linguist Andre Martinet It means that complex linguistic expressions can be broken down in meaningful elements such as morphemes and words which in turn are composed of smallest phonetic elements that affect meaning called phonemes Animal signals however do not exhibit this dual structure In general animal utterances are responses to external stimuli and do not refer to matters removed in time and space Matters of relevance at a distance such as distant food sources tend to be indicated to other individuals by body language instead for example wolf activity before a hunt or the information conveyed in honeybee dance language It is therefore unclear to what extent utterances are automatic responses and to what extent deliberate intent plays a part In contrast to human language animal communication systems are usually not able to express conceptual generalizations Cetaceans and some primates may be notable exceptions 85 Human languages combine elements to produce new messages a property known as creativity One factor in this is that much human language growth is based upon conceptual ideas and hypothetical structures both being far greater capabilities in humans than animals This appears far less common in animal communication systems although current research into animal culture is still an ongoing process with many new discoveries In 2009 it was reported that affixation may play a role in the call meanings of Campbell s mona monkey 86 Errors in communication EditThere becomes possibility for error within communication between animals when certain circumstances apply 87 These circumstances could include distance between the two communicating subjects as well as the complexity of the signal that is being communicated to the listener of the situation It may not always be clear to the listener where the location of the communication is coming from as the singer can sometimes deceive them and create more error 88 See also Edit nbsp Animals portalAnimal consciousness Anthrozoology human animal studies Biocommunication Biosemiotics Body language Dear enemy effect and Nasty neighbour effect Deception in animals Degeneracy biology Emotion in animals Forms of activity and interpersonal relations Human animal communication International Society for Biosemiotic Studies Origin of language Origin of speech Sir Philip Sidney game Talking animal Zoomusicology ZoosemioticsReferences Edit Animal communication Encyclopedia Britannica Retrieved 2020 10 31 Shah Sonia 20 September 2023 The Animals Are Talking What Does It Mean Language was long understood as a human only affair New research suggests that isn t so comment The New York Times Archived from the original on 21 September 2023 Retrieved 21 September 2023 Seyfarth Robert M Cheney Dorothy L 2003 02 01 Signalers and Receivers in Animal Communication Annual Review of Psychology 54 1 145 173 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 54 101601 145121 ISSN 0066 4308 PMID 12359915 a b Maynard Smith and Harper 2003 Tinbergen N Perdeck A C 1951 01 01 On the Stimulus Situation Releasing the Begging Response in the Newly Hatched Herring Gull Chick Larus Argentatus Argentatus Pont Behaviour 3 1 1 39 doi 10 1163 156853951X00197 ISSN 0005 7959 Pollick Amy S Waal Frans B M de 2007 05 08 Ape gestures and language evolution Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 19 8184 8189 Bibcode 2007PNAS 104 8184P doi 10 1073 pnas 0702624104 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 1876592 PMID 17470779 Bertin Aline Beraud Arielle Lansade Lea et al 22 August 2018 Facial display and blushing Means of visual communication in blue and yellow macaws Ara Ararauna PLOS ONE 13 8 doi 10 1371 journal pone 0201762 ProQuest 2091762411 Mogil Jeffrey S April 2009 Animal models of pain progress and challenges Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10 4 283 294 doi 10 1038 nrn2606 ISSN 1471 003X PMID 19259101 S2CID 205504814 a b Shepherd Stephen V 2010 03 19 Following Gaze Gaze Following Behavior as a Window into Social Cognition Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 4 5 doi 10 3389 fnint 2010 00005 ISSN 1662 5145 PMC 2859805 PMID 20428494 Range Friederike Viranyi Zsofia 2011 02 23 Wylie Doug ed Development of Gaze Following Abilities in Wolves Canis Lupus PLOS ONE 6 2 e16888 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 616888R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0016888 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3044139 PMID 21373192 Cloney R A Florey E 1968 Ultrastructure of cephalopod chromatophore organs Z Zellforsch Mikrosk Anat 89 2 250 280 doi 10 1007 bf00347297 PMID 5700268 S2CID 26566732 Hanlon R T Messenger J B 1996 Cephalopod Behaviour Cambridge University Press p 121 ISBN 978 0 521 64583 6 Williams Sarah 2012 Two faced fish tricks competitors Science Now Archived from the original on March 8 2013 Retrieved March 16 2013 Motluk Alison 2001 Big Bottom New Scientist 19 7 Deep Sea Squid May Communicate Through Glowing Pigmentation Researchers Find Mason Julia July 18 2018 Glowing in the Deep The Dish on Science Bioluminescence Smithsonian Ocean ocean si edu Retrieved 2020 10 25 Ehrlich Paul R David S Dobkin amp Darryl Wheye Bird Voices and Vocal Development from Birds of Stanford essays Retrieved 9 Sep 2008 Schwartzkopff J January 1977 Auditory Communication in Lower Animals Role of Auditory Physiology Annual Review of Psychology 28 1 61 84 doi 10 1146 annurev ps 28 020177 000425 ISSN 0066 4308 PMID 324382 Slabbekoorn Hans Smith Thomas B 2002 04 29 Bird song ecology and speciation Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 357 1420 493 503 doi 10 1098 rstb 2001 1056 ISSN 0962 8436 PMC 1692962 PMID 12028787 Mikula P Valcu M Brumm H Bulla M Forstmeier W Petruskova T Kempenaers B amp Albrecht T 2021 A global analysis of song frequency in passerines provides no support for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis but suggests a role for sexual selection Ecology Letters 24 3 477 486 doi 10 1111 ele 13662 PMID 33314573 Zuberbuhler Klaus 2001 Predator specific alarm calls in Campbell s monkeys Cercopithecus campbelli PDF Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 5 414 422 Boughman J W 1998 02 07 Vocal learning by greater spear nosed bats Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 265 1392 227 233 doi 10 1098 rspb 1998 0286 ISSN 0962 8452 PMC 1688873 PMID 9493408 Price Tabitha Wadewitz Philip Cheney Dorothy Seyfarth Robert Hammerschmidt Kurt Fischer Julia 2015 08 19 Vervets revisited A quantitative analysis of alarm call structure and context specificity Scientific Reports 5 1 13220 Bibcode 2015NatSR 513220P doi 10 1038 srep13220 ISSN 2045 2322 PMC 4541072 PMID 26286236 Jabr Ferris 2017 05 12 Can Prairie Dogs Talk Published 2017 The New York Times ISSN 0362 4331 Retrieved 2020 10 25 New Language Discovered Prairiedogese NPR org Retrieved 2020 10 25 YIPS BARKS AND CHIRPS THE LANGUAGE OF PRAIRIE DOGS 2016 10 27 Archived from the original on 2016 10 27 Retrieved 2020 10 25 Prairie dogs language decoded by scientists CBC News CBC Retrieved 2020 10 25 January 2006 Bjorn Carey 03 3 January 2006 Whales Found to Speak in Dialects livescience com Retrieved 2020 10 25 Weilgart Linda Whitehead H 1997 05 01 Group specific dialects and geographical variation in coda repertoire in South Pacific sperm whales Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 40 5 277 285 doi 10 1007 s002650050343 ISSN 1432 0762 S2CID 11845118 Ford John Kenneth Baker 1984 Call Traditions and Dialects of Killer Whales Orcinus Orca in British Columbia Retrospective Theses and Dissertations 1919 2007 Thesis University of British Columbia p 284 doi 10 14288 1 0096602 Souhaut M Shields MW 2021 Stereotyped whistles in southern resident killer whales Aquatic Biology PeerJ 9 e12085 doi 10 7717 peerj 12085 PMC 8404572 PMID 34532160 Retrieved 2 January 2023 Ladich Friedrich 2001 Sound generating and detecting motor system in catfish Design of swimbladder muscles in doradids and pimelodids The Anatomical Record 263 3 297 306 doi 10 1002 ar 1105 ISSN 1097 0185 PMID 11455539 S2CID 24896586 a b c d e Searcy William A March 2013 Principles of Animal Communication Second Edition By Jack W Bradbury and Sandra L Vehrencamp Sunderland Massachusetts Sinauer Associates 99 95 xiv 697 p ill C 1 C 6 credits I 1 I 47 index ISBN 978 0 87893 045 6 2011 The Quarterly Review of Biology 88 1 48 doi 10 1086 669301 ISSN 0033 5770 Prairie dog alarm calls encode labels about predator colors By N Slobodchiko V Andrea Paseka Jennifer L Verdolin Published 31 December 2008 via Springer Verlag PDF Archived from the original PDF on 2 June 2021 Retrieved 3 June 2021 Alarm calling in yellow bellied marmots I The meaning of situationally variable alarm calls by Daneil T Blumstein amp Kenneth B Armitage Department of Systematics and Ecology University of Kansas Published by Animal Behavior via UCLA Life Sciences Brown Grant E Chivers Douglas P Smith R Jan F 1995 02 01 Localized defecation by pike a response to labelling by cyprinid alarm pheromone Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 36 2 105 110 doi 10 1007 BF00170715 ISSN 1432 0762 S2CID 31875357 Brown Grant Adrian James Patton Todd Chivers Douglas Dec 2001 Fathead minnows learn to recognize predator odor when exposed to concentrations of artificial alarm pheromone below their behavioral response threshold Canadian Journal of Zoology 79 12 2239 2245 doi 10 1139 z01 194 ProQuest 220512135 via ProQuest a b Acid Rain Is Totally So Last Century Right Not Exactly A Canadian scientist explains how acid rain is still making its mark IISD Experimental Lakes Area 16 May 2018 Retrieved 2020 10 26 du P Bothma J le Richet E A N 1995 04 01 Evidence of the use of rubbing scent marking andscratching posts by Kalahari leopards Journal of Arid Environments 29 4 511 517 Bibcode 1995JArEn 29 511D doi 10 1016 S0140 1963 95 80023 9 ISSN 0140 1963 Gosling L Morris Roberts S Craig 2001 Scent marking by male mammals cheat proof signals to competitors and mates PDF Advances in the Study of Behavior 30 169 217 doi 10 1016 S0065 3454 01 80007 3 ISBN 9780120045303 du P Bothma J Richet E A N le 1995 Evidence of the use of rubbing scent marking andscratching posts by Kalahari leopards Journal of Arid Environments 29 4 511 517 Bibcode 1995JArEn 29 511D doi 10 1016 s0140 1963 95 80023 9 Clapham Melanie Chemical signalling in brown bears ursus arctos an assessment of scent marking strategies and social function OCLC 1065010384 Electrocommunication archive fo 2012 05 29 Archived from the original on 2012 05 29 Retrieved 2020 10 26 Worm Martin Kirschbaum Frank von der Emde Gerhard 2017 09 13 Social interactions between live and artificial weakly electric fish Electrocommunication and locomotor behavior of Mormyrus rume proboscirostris towards a mobile dummy fish PLOS ONE 12 9 e0184622 Bibcode 2017PLoSO 1284622W doi 10 1371 journal pone 0184622 PMC 5597219 PMID 28902915 Donati Elisa Worm Martin Mintchev Stefano van der Wiel Marleen Benelli Giovanni von der Emde Gerhard Stefanini Cesare 2016 12 01 Investigation of Collective Behaviour and Electrocommunication in the Weakly Electric Fish Mormyrus rume through a biomimetic Robotic Dummy Fish Bioinspiration amp Biomimetics 11 6 066009 Bibcode 2016BiBi 11f6009D doi 10 1088 1748 3190 11 6 066009 ISSN 1748 3190 PMID 27906686 S2CID 26047199 Kuo Zing Jan 1 1960 Studies on the basic factors in animal fighting VI Inter species coexistence in birds The Journal of Genetic Psychology 97 15 ProQuest 1297122533 via ProQuest Yamamoto Maria Emilia Araujo Arrilton Arruda Maria de Fatima Lima Ana Karinne Moreira Siqueira Jose de Oliveira Hattori Wallisen Tadashi 2014 11 01 Male and female breeding strategies in a cooperative primate Behavioural Processes Neotropical Behaviour 109 27 33 doi 10 1016 j beproc 2014 06 009 ISSN 0376 6357 PMID 25010563 S2CID 205979349 Dunbar R I M 2010 02 01 The social role of touch in humans and primates Behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews Touch Temperature Pain Itch and Pleasure 34 2 260 268 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2008 07 001 ISSN 0149 7634 PMID 18662717 S2CID 30450770 Ants body odor physical contact get worker ants working study finds news stanford edu Retrieved 2020 10 26 Sukhchuluun Gansukh Zhang Xue Ying Chi Qing Sheng Wang De Hua 2018 05 18 Huddling Conserves Energy Decreases Core Body Temperature but Increases Activity in Brandt s Voles Lasiopodomys brandtii Frontiers in Physiology 9 563 doi 10 3389 fphys 2018 00563 ISSN 1664 042X PMC 5968109 PMID 29867585 Coral Reef Protection What Are Coral Reefs Habitat Protection US EPA archive epa gov Retrieved 2020 10 26 Cocroft Reginald October 2001 Vibrational Communication and the Ecology of Group Living Herbivorous Insects Integrative and Comparative Biology 41 1215 1221 via Oxford Academic Hill Peggy October 2001 Vibration and Animal Communication A Review Integrative and Comparative Biology 41 1135 1142 via Oxford Academic Narins Peter April 1990 Seismic Communication in Anuran Amphibians BioScience 40 4 268 274 doi 10 2307 1311263 JSTOR 1311263 via Oxford Academic Kardong Kenneth V Mackessy Stephen P 1991 The Strike Behavior of a Congenitally Blind Rattlesnake Journal of Herpetology 25 2 208 211 doi 10 2307 1564650 ISSN 0022 1511 JSTOR 1564650 Bakken George S Krochmal Aaron R 2007 08 15 The imaging properties and sensitivity of the facial pits of pitvipers as determined by optical and heat transfer analysis Journal of Experimental Biology 210 16 2801 2810 doi 10 1242 jeb 006965 ISSN 0022 0949 PMID 17690227 S2CID 25037159 Pough F Andrews Robin Cadle John Crump Martha Savitzky Alan Wells Kentwood 2003 01 01 Herpetology Third ed New York Prentice Hall a b Gracheva Elena O Ingolia Nicholas T Kelly Yvonne M Cordero Morales Julio F Hollopeter Gunther Chesler Alexander T Sanchez Elda E Perez John C Weissman Jonathan S Julius David April 2010 Molecular basis of infrared detection by snakes Nature 464 7291 1006 1011 Bibcode 2010Natur 464 1006G doi 10 1038 nature08943 ISSN 1476 4687 PMC 2855400 PMID 20228791 Kurten L Schmidt U Schafer K 1984 06 01 Warm and cold receptors in the nose of the vampire bat Desmodus rotundas Naturwissenschaften 71 6 327 328 Bibcode 1984NW 71 327K doi 10 1007 BF00396621 ISSN 1432 1904 PMID 6472483 S2CID 31899356 Wilson Ben Dill Lawrence M March 2002 Pacific herring respond to stimulated odontocete echolocation sounds Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 59 3 542 doi 10 1139 f02 029 Haldar Vivekananda Chakraborty Niladri 2017 07 01 A novel evolutionary technique based on electrolocation principle of elephant nose fish and shark fish electrolocation optimization Soft Computing 21 14 3827 3848 doi 10 1007 s00500 016 2033 1 ISSN 1433 7479 S2CID 207013387 Jones Gareth 2005 07 12 Echolocation Current Biology 15 13 R484 R488 doi 10 1016 j cub 2005 06 051 ISSN 0960 9822 PMID 16005275 S2CID 235311777 Vanderelst Dieter Steckel Jan Boen Andre Peremans Herbert Holderied Marc W 2016 08 02 Eichenbaum Howard ed Place recognition using batlike sonar eLife 5 e14188 doi 10 7554 eLife 14188 ISSN 2050 084X PMC 4970868 PMID 27481189 Stritih Natasa Kosi Alenka Nov 2017 Olfactory signaling of aggressive intent in male male contests of cave crickets Tropglophilus neglectus Orthoptera Rhaphidophoridae PLOS ONE 12 11 e0187512 Bibcode 2017PLoSO 1287512S doi 10 1371 journal pone 0187512 PMC 5675388 PMID 29112984 ProQuest 1961423777 Clutton Brock Tim 2016 05 31 Mammal Societies John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 1 119 09532 3 Jenssen Thomas A Orrell Kimberly S Lovern Matthew B 2000 Sexual Dimorphisms in Aggressive Signal Structure and Use by a Polygynous Lizard Anolis carolinensis Copeia 2000 1 140 149 doi 10 1643 0045 8511 2000 2000 0140 SDIASS 2 0 CO 2 ISSN 0045 8511 JSTOR 1448245 S2CID 9813895 Bowen Jon Heath Sarah 2005 Behaviour problems in small animals practical advice for the veterinary team Edinburgh Elsevier Saunders p 127 ISBN 978 0702027673 Retrieved 16 June 2022 Casar Cristiane Byrne Richard W Hoppitt William Young Robert J Zuberbuhler Klaus 2012 08 01 Evidence for semantic communication in titi monkey alarm calls Animal Behaviour 84 2 405 411 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2012 05 010 ISSN 0003 3472 S2CID 45749417 Sasaki Takao Holldobler Bert Millar Jocelyn G Pratt Stephen C 2014 09 15 A context dependent alarm signal in the ant Temnothorax rugatulus Journal of Experimental Biology 217 18 3229 3236 doi 10 1242 jeb 106849 hdl 2286 R I 28102 ISSN 0022 0949 PMID 25013103 S2CID 2102542 a b Hasson O October 1991 Pursuit deterrent signals communication between prey and predator Trends in Ecology amp Evolution 6 10 325 329 doi 10 1016 0169 5347 91 90040 5 ISSN 0169 5347 PMID 21232498 Map of Life Vibrational communication in animals Archived from the original on 2020 11 29 Retrieved 2020 10 28 Wijngaarden Vanessa 2023 02 08 Interviewing Animals Through Animal Communicators Potentials of Intuitive Interspecies Communication for Multispecies Methods Society amp Animals 1 aop 1 21 doi 10 1163 15685306 bja10122 ISSN 1568 5306 Tauzin Tibor Csik Andor Kis Anna Topal Jozsef 2015 What or where The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs Canis familiaris PDF Journal of Comparative Psychology 129 4 334 338 doi 10 1037 a0039462 ISSN 1939 2087 PMID 26147704 Sean Senechal Dogs can sign too A breakthrough method of teaching your dog to communicate to you 2009 Random House Crown TenSpeed Press Morris Desmond 1958 The Comparative Ethology of Grassfinches erythrurae and Mannikins amadinae Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 131 3 389 439 doi 10 1111 j 1096 3642 1958 tb00695 x ISSN 1469 7998 discussed at length by Richard Dawkins under the subject of his book The Selfish Gene Chandler Christopher H Ofria Charles Dworkin Ian 2013 Runaway Sexual Selection Leads to Good Genes Evolution 67 1 110 119 doi 10 1111 j 1558 5646 2012 01750 x ISSN 0014 3820 JSTOR 23327705 PMID 23289565 S2CID 15929198 a b V M Janik L S Sayigh and R S Wells Signature whistle shape conveys identity information to bottlenose dolphins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences vol 103 no 21 May 23 2006 Smith Eric Alden 2017 09 29 Evolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior Routledge ISBN 978 1 351 52132 1 Hare Brian Call Josep Tomasello Michael 1998 01 01 Communication of Food Location Between Human and Dog Canis Familiaris Evolution of Communication 2 1 137 159 doi 10 1075 eoc 2 1 06har ISSN 1387 5337 Tauzin Tibor Csik Andor Kis Anna Topal Jozsef November 2015 What or where The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs Canis familiaris Journal of Comparative Psychology 129 4 334 338 doi 10 1037 a0039462 ISSN 1939 2087 PMID 26147704 Guo Kun Meints Kerstin Hall Charlotte Hall Sophie Mills Daniel 2009 05 01 Left gaze bias in humans rhesus monkeys and domestic dogs Animal Cognition 12 3 409 418 doi 10 1007 s10071 008 0199 3 ISSN 1435 9456 PMID 18925420 S2CID 5661394 Do animals have language Michele Bishop TED Ed 10 September 2015 Retrieved 11 September 2015 Thompson Roger K R Oden David L 2000 Categorical Perception and Conceptual Judgments by Nonhuman Primates The Paleological Monkey and the Analogical Ape Cognitive Science 24 3 363 396 doi 10 1207 s15516709cog2403 2 ISSN 1551 6709 Rudiments of Language Discovered in Monkeys Wired ISSN 1059 1028 Retrieved 2020 10 31 Real Leslie A 1994 12 15 Behavioral Mechanisms in Evolutionary Ecology University of Chicago Press ISBN 978 0 226 70595 8 Gergely Anna Compton Anna Newberry Ruth Miklosi Adam Apr 2016 Social Interaction with an Unidentified Moving Object Elicits A Not B Error in Domestic Dogs PLOS ONE 11 4 e0151600 Bibcode 2016PLoSO 1151600G doi 10 1371 journal pone 0151600 PMC 4830451 PMID 27073867 S2CID 16369609 External links Edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Animal communication Animal Communicator Documentary Zoosemiotics animal communication on the web Archived 2005 10 25 at the Wayback Machine The Animal Communication Project International Bioacoustics Council research on animal language Max Planck Institute of Animal Behavior research on animal vocalizations Animal Sounds different animal sounds to listen and download The British Library Sound Archive contains over 150 000 recordings of animal sounds and natural atmospheres from all over the world Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Animal communication amp oldid 1176770255, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.