fbpx
Wikipedia

Konrad Lorenz

Konrad Zacharias Lorenz (German pronunciation: [ˈkɔnʁaːt tsaxaˈʁiːas ˈloːʁɛnts] ; 7 November 1903 – 27 February 1989) was an Austrian zoologist, ethologist, and ornithologist. He shared the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine with Nikolaas Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch. He is often regarded as one of the founders of modern ethology, the study of animal behavior. He developed an approach that began with an earlier generation, including his teacher Oskar Heinroth.[1]

Konrad Lorenz
Lorenz in 1978
Born
Konrad Zacharias Lorenz

(1903-11-07)7 November 1903
Died27 February 1989(1989-02-27) (aged 85)
Vienna, Austria
NationalityAustrian
Alma materColumbia University
University of Vienna (MD, PhD)
Awards
Scientific career
FieldsEthology

Lorenz studied instinctive behavior in animals, especially in greylag geese and jackdaws. Working with geese, he investigated the principle of imprinting, the process by which some nidifugous birds (i.e. birds that leave their nest early) bond instinctively with the first moving object that they see within the first hours of hatching. Although Lorenz did not discover the topic, he became widely known for his descriptions of imprinting as an instinctive bond. In 1936, he met Tinbergen, and the two collaborated in developing ethology as a separate sub-discipline of biology. A Review of General Psychology survey, published in 2002, ranked Lorenz the 65th most cited scholar of the 20th century in the technical psychology journals, introductory psychology textbooks, and survey responses.[2]

Lorenz's work was interrupted by the onset of World War II and in 1941 he was recruited into the German Army as a medic.[3] In 1944, he was sent to the Eastern Front where he was captured by the Soviet Red Army and spent four years as a German prisoner of war in Soviet Armenia. After the war, he regretted his membership in the Nazi Party.[4]

Lorenz wrote numerous books, some of which, such as King Solomon's Ring, On Aggression, and Man Meets Dog, became popular reading. His last work Here I Am – Where Are You? is a summary of his life's work and focuses on his famous studies of greylag geese.

Biography edit

 
Lorenz in 1904 with his elder brother

Lorenz was the son of Adolf Lorenz, a wealthy and distinguished surgeon, and his wife Emma (née Lecher), a physician who had been her husband's assistant.[5] The family lived on a large estate at Altenberg, and had a city apartment in Vienna.[6] He was educated at the Public Schottengymnasium of the Benedictine monks in Vienna.

In his autobiographical essay, published in 1973 in Les Prix Nobel (winners of the prizes are requested to provide such essays), Lorenz credits his career to his parents, who "were supremely tolerant of my inordinate love for animals", and to his childhood encounter with Selma Lagerlöf's The Wonderful Adventures of Nils, which filled him with a great enthusiasm about wild geese."[7]

At the request of his father, Adolf Lorenz, he began a premedical curriculum in 1922 at Columbia University,[8] but he returned to Vienna in 1923 to continue his studies at the University of Vienna. He graduated as Doctor of Medicine (MD) in 1928 and became an assistant professor at the Institute of Anatomy until 1935. He finished his zoological studies in 1933 and received his second doctorate (PhD).[9]

While still a student, Lorenz began developing what would become a large menagerie, ranging from domestic to exotic animals. In his popular book King Solomon's Ring, Lorenz recounts that while studying at the University of Vienna he kept a variety of animals at his parents' apartment, ranging from fish to a capuchin monkey named Gloria.[9]

In 1936, at an international scientific symposium on instinct, Lorenz met his great friend and colleague Nikolaas Tinbergen. Together they studied geese—wild, domestic, and hybrid. One result of these studies was that Lorenz "realized that an overpowering increase in the drives of feeding as well as of copulation and a waning of more differentiated social instincts is characteristic of very many domestic animals". Lorenz began to suspect and fear "that analogous processes of deterioration may be at work with civilized humanity." This observation of bird hybrids caused Lorenz to believe that domestication resulting from urbanisation in humans might also cause dysgenic effects, and to argue in two papers that the Nazi eugenics policies against this were therefore scientifically justified.[10]

 
Lorenz as a Soviet POW in 1944

In 1940 he became a professor of psychology at the University of Königsberg. He was drafted into the Wehrmacht in 1941. He sought to be a motorcycle mechanic, but instead he was assigned as a military psychologist, conducting racial studies on humans in occupied Poznań under Rudolf Hippius. The objective was to study the biological characteristics of "German-Polish half-breeds" to determine whether they 'benefitted' from the same work ethics as 'pure' Germans.[11][12] The degree to which Lorenz participated in the project is unknown, but the project director Hippius referred a couple of times to Lorenz as an "examining psychologist".[13]

Lorenz later described that he once saw transports of concentration camp inmates at Fort VII near Poznań, which made him "fully realize the complete inhumanity of the Nazis".[14]

He was sent to the Russian front in 1944 where he quickly became a prisoner of war in the Soviet Union from 1944 to 1948. In captivity in Soviet Armenia,[15] he continued to work as a medic and "became tolerably fluent in Russian and got quite friendly with some Russians, mostly doctors."[16] When he was repatriated, he was allowed to keep the manuscript of a book he had been writing, and his pet starling. He arrived back in Altenberg (his family home, near Vienna) both "with manuscript and bird intact." The manuscript became his 1973 book Behind the Mirror.[3]

The Max Planck Society established the Lorenz Institute for Behavioral Physiology in Buldern, Germany, in 1950. In his memoirs Lorenz described the chronology of his war years differently from what historians have been able to document after his death. He himself claimed that he was captured in 1942, where in reality he was only sent to the front and captured in 1944, leaving out entirely his involvement with the Poznań project.[3]

In 1958, Lorenz transferred to the Max Planck Institute for Behavioral Physiology in Seewiesen. He shared the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine "for discoveries in individual and social behavior patterns" with two other important early ethologists, Nikolaas Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch. In 1969, he became the first recipient of the Prix mondial Cino Del Duca. He was a friend and student of renowned biologist Sir Julian Huxley (grandson of "Darwin's bulldog", Thomas Henry Huxley). Famed psychoanalyst Ralph Greenson and Sir Peter Scott were good friends. Lorenz and Karl Popper were childhood friends; many years after they met, during the celebration of Popper's 80 years, they wrote together a book entitled Die Zukunft ist offen.[17]

He retired from the Max Planck Institute in 1973 but continued to research and publish from Altenberg and Grünau im Almtal in Austria. He died on 27 February 1989 in Altenberg.

Personal life edit

Lorenz married his childhood friend, Margarethe Gebhardt, a gynaecologist, daughter of a market gardener who lived near the Lorenz family;[18] they had a son and two daughters. He lived at the Lorenz family estate, which included a "fantastical neo-baroque mansion", previously owned by his father.[6][19]

Ethology edit

Lorenz is recognized as one of the founding fathers of the field of ethology, the study of animal behavior. He is best known for his research of the principle of attachment, or imprinting, through which in some species a bond is formed between a newborn animal and its caregiver. This principle had been discovered by Douglas Spalding in the 19th century, and Lorenz's mentor Oskar Heinroth had also worked on the topic, but Lorenz's description of Prägung, imprinting, in nidifugous birds such as greylag geese in his 1935 book Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels ("The Companion in the Environment of Birds") became the foundational description of the phenomenon.[13]

Here, Lorenz used Jakob von Uexküll's concept of Umwelt to understand how the limited perception of animals filtered out certain phenomena with which they interacted instinctively. For example, a young goose instinctively bonds with the first moving stimulus it perceives, whether it be its mother, or a person. Lorenz showed that this behavior of imprinting is what allows the goose to learn to recognize members of its own species, enabling them to be the object of subsequent behavior patterns such as mating.[20] He developed a theory of instinctive behavior that saw behavior patterns as largely innate but triggered through environmental stimuli, for example the hawk/goose effect. He argued that animals have an inner drive to carry out instinctive behaviors, and that if they do not encounter the right stimulus they will eventually engage in the behavior with an inappropriate stimulus.[21]

Lorenz's approach to ethology derived from a skepticism towards the studies of animal behavior done in laboratory settings. He considered that in order to understand the mechanisms of animal behavior, it was necessary to observe their full range of behaviors in their natural context. Lorenz did not carry out much traditional fieldwork but observed animals near his home. His method involved empathizing with animals, often using anthropomorphization to imagine their mental states. He believed that animals were capable of experiencing many of the same emotions as humans.[20][22]

Tinbergen, Lorenz's friend with whom he conjointly received the Nobel prize, summarized Lorenz's major contribution to ethology as making behavior a topic of biological inquiry, considering behavior a part of an animal's evolutionary equipment.[23] Tinbergen and Lorenz contributed to making Ethology a recognized sub-discipline within Biology and founded the first specialized journal of the field "Ethology" (originally "Zeitschift für Tierpsychologie")[20]

Politics edit

Nazism edit

Lorenz joined the Nazi Party in 1938 and accepted a university chair under the Nazi regime. In his application for party membership he wrote, "I'm able to say that my whole scientific work is devoted to the ideas of the National Socialists." His publications during that time led in later years to allegations that his scientific work had been contaminated by Nazi sympathies. His published writing during the Nazi period included support for Nazi ideas of "racial hygiene" couched in pseudoscientific metaphors.[24][25][26][27][28][29]

In his autobiography, Lorenz wrote:

The same individual geese on which we conducted these experiments, first aroused my interest in the process of domestication. They were F1 hybrids of wild Greylags and domestic geese and they showed surprising deviations from the normal social and sexual behaviour of the wild birds. I realised that an overpowering increase in the drives of feeding as well as of copulation and a waning of more differentiated social instincts is characteristic of very many domestic animals. I was frightened – as I still am – by the thought that analogous genetical processes of deterioration may be at work with civilized humanity. Moved by this fear, I did a very ill-advised thing soon after the Germans had invaded Austria: I wrote about the dangers of domestication and, in order to be understood, I couched my writing in the worst of nazi-terminology. I do not want to extenuate this action. I did, indeed, believe that some good might come of the new rulers. The precedent narrow-minded catholic regime in Austria induced better and more intelligent men than I was to cherish this naive hope. Practically all my friends and teachers did so, including my own father who certainly was a kindly and humane man. None of us as much as suspected that the word "selection", when used by these rulers, meant murder. I regret those writings not so much for the undeniable discredit they reflect on my person as for their effect of hampering the future recognition of the dangers of domestication.[3]

After the war, Lorenz denied having been a party member,[30] until his membership application was made public; and he denied having known the extent of the genocide, despite his position as a psychologist in the Office of Racial Policy.[31] He was also shown to have made anti-Semitic jokes on 'Jewish characteristics' in letters to his mentor Heinroth.[32] In 2015, the University of Salzburg posthumously rescinded an honorary doctorate awarded to Lorenz in 1983, citing his party membership and his assertions in his application that he was "always a National Socialist", and that his work "stands to serve National Socialist thought". The university also accused him of using his work to spread "basic elements of the racist ideology of National Socialism".[33][34]

Ecology edit

During the final years of his life, Lorenz supported the fledgling Austrian Green Party and in 1984 became the figurehead of the Konrad Lorenz Volksbegehren, a grass-roots movement that was formed to prevent the building of a power plant at the Danube near Hainburg an der Donau and thus the destruction of the surrounding woodland.

Contributions and legacy edit

 
With Nikolaas Tinbergen (left), 1978

Lorenz has been called 'The father of ethology', by Niko Tinbergen.[35] Perhaps Lorenz's most important contribution to ethology was his idea that behavior patterns can be studied as anatomical organs.[36] This concept forms the foundation of ethological research.[35][37] However, Richard Dawkins called Lorenz a "'good of the species' man",[38] stating that the idea of group selection was "so deeply ingrained"[38] in Lorenz's thinking that he "evidently did not realize that his statements contravened orthodox Darwinian theory."[38]

Together with Nikolaas Tinbergen, Lorenz developed the idea of an innate releasing mechanism to explain instinctive behaviors (fixed action patterns). They experimented with "supernormal stimuli" such as giant eggs or dummy bird beaks which they found could release the fixed action patterns more powerfully than the natural objects for which the behaviors were adapted. Influenced by the ideas of William McDougall, Lorenz developed this into a "psychohydraulic" model of the motivation of behavior, which tended towards group selectionist ideas, which were influential in the 1960s. Another of his contributions to ethology is his work on imprinting. His influence on a younger generation of ethologists; and his popular works, were important in bringing ethology to the attention of the general public.

Lorenz claimed that there was widespread contempt for the descriptive sciences. He attributed this to the denial of perception as the source of all scientific knowledge: "a denial that has been elevated to the status of religion."[39] He wrote that in comparative behavioral research, "it is necessary to describe various patterns of movement, record them, and above all, render them unmistakably recognizable."[40]

There are three research institutions named after Lorenz in Austria: the Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research (KLI) was housed in Lorenz' family mansion at Altenberg before moving to Klosterneuburg in 2013 Discover the KLI; the Konrad Lorenz Forschungsstelle (KLF) at his former field station in Grünau; and the Konrad Lorenz Institute of Ethology, an external research facility of the University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna.

Vision of the challenges facing humanity edit

 
With Nikolaas Tinbergen (right), 1978

Lorenz predicted the relationship between market economics and the threat of ecological catastrophe. In his 1973 book, Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins, Lorenz addresses the following paradox:

All the advantages that man has gained from his ever-deepening understanding of the natural world that surrounds him, his technological, chemical and medical progress, all of which should seem to alleviate human suffering... tends instead to favor humanity's destruction[41]

Lorenz adopts an ecological model to attempt to grasp the mechanisms behind this contradiction. Thus "all species... are adapted to their environment... including not only inorganic components... but all the other living beings that inhabit the locality." p31.

Fundamental to Lorenz's theory of ecology is the function of negative feedback mechanisms, which, in hierarchical fashion, dampen impulses that occur beneath a certain threshold. The thresholds themselves are the product of the interaction of contrasting mechanisms. Thus pain and pleasure act as checks on each other:

To gain a desired prey, a dog or wolf will do things that, in other contexts, they would shy away from: run through thorn bushes, jump into cold water and expose themselves to risks which would normally frighten them. All these inhibitory mechanisms... act as a counterweight to the effects of learning mechanisms... The organism cannot allow itself to pay a price which is not worth paying. p53.

In nature, these mechanisms tend towards a 'stable state' among the living beings of an ecology:

A closer examination shows that these beings... not only do not damage each other, but often constitute a community of interests. It is obvious that the predator is strongly interested in the survival of that species, animal or vegetable, which constitutes its prey. ... It is not uncommon that the prey species derives specific benefits from its interaction with the predator species... pp31–33.

Lorenz states that humanity is the one species not bound by these mechanisms, being the only one that has defined its own environment:

[The pace of human ecology] is determined by the progress of man's technology (p35)... human ecology (economy) is governed by mechanisms of POSITIVE feedback, defined as a mechanism which tends to encourage behavior rather than to attenuate it (p43). Positive feedback always involves the danger of an 'avalanche' effect... One particular kind of positive feedback occurs when individuals OF THE SAME SPECIES enter into competition among themselves... For many animal species, environmental factors keep... intraspecies selection from [leading to] disaster... But there is no force which exercises this type of healthy regulatory effect on humanity's cultural development; unfortunately for itself, humanity has learned to overcome all those environmental forces which are external to itself p44.

Regarding aggression in human beings, Lorenz states:

Let us imagine that an absolutely unbiased investigator on another planet, perhaps on Mars, is examining human behavior on earth, with the aid of a telescope whose magnification is too small to enable him to discern individuals and follow their separate behavior, but large enough for him to observe occurrences such as migrations of peoples, wars, and similar great historical events. He would never gain the impression that human behavior was dictated by intelligence, still less by responsible morality. If we suppose our extraneous observer to be a being of pure reason, devoid of instincts himself and unaware of the way in which all instincts in general and aggression in particular can miscarry, he would be at a complete loss how to explain history at all. The ever-recurrent phenomena of history do not have reasonable causes. It is a mere commonplace to say that they are caused by what common parlance so aptly terms "human nature." Unreasoning and unreasonable human nature causes two nations to compete, though no economic necessity compels them to do so; it induces two political parties or religions with amazingly similar programs of salvation to fight each other bitterly, and it impels an Alexander or a Napoleon to sacrifice millions of lives in his attempt to unite the world under his scepter. We have been taught to regard some of the persons who have committed these and similar absurdities with respect, even as "great" men, we are wont to yield to the political wisdom of those in charge, and we are all so accustomed to these phenomena that most of us fail to realize how abjectly stupid and undesirable the historical mass behavior of humanity actually is [42]

Lorenz does not see human independence from natural ecological processes as necessarily bad. He states that:

A completely new [ecology] which corresponds in every way to [humanity's] desires... could, theoretically, prove as durable as that which would have existed without his intervention (36).

However, the principle of competition, typical of Western societies, destroys any chance of this:

The competition between human beings destroys with cold and diabolic brutality... Under the pressure of this competitive fury we have not only forgotten what is useful to humanity as a whole, but even that which is good and advantageous to the individual. [...] One asks, which is more damaging to modern humanity: the thirst for money or consuming haste... in either case, fear plays a very important role: the fear of being overtaken by one's competitors, the fear of becoming poor, the fear of making wrong decisions or the fear of not being up to snuff... (pp. 45–47)

Philosophical speculations edit

In his 1973 book Behind the Mirror: A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge, Lorenz considers the old philosophical question of whether our senses correctly inform us about the world as it is, or provide us only with an illusion. His answer comes from evolutionary biology. Only traits that help us survive and reproduce are transmitted. If our senses gave us wrong information about our environment, we would soon be extinct. Therefore, we can be sure that our senses give us correct information, for otherwise we would not be here to be deceived.

Honours and awards edit

  • Elected a member of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1957[43]
  • Austrian Decoration for Science and Art in 1964
  • Elected a Foreign Member of the Royal Society (ForMemRS) in 1964[1]
  • Elected a member of the United States National Academy of Sciences in 1966[44]
  • Kalinga Prize for the Popularization of Science in 1969
  • Gold Medal of the Humboldt Society in 1972Rupke, Nicolaas (2008). Alexander Von Humboldt: A Metabiography. University of Chicago Press. p. 151. ISBN 978-0-226-73149-0.
  • Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1973[3]
  • Elected a member of the American Philosophical Society in 1974[45]
  • Honorary Doctorate, University of Salzburg, 1983, revoked in 2015[46]
  • Grand Cross with Star and Sash of the Order of Merit of the Federal Republic of Germany (Großes Verdienstkreuz mit Stern und Schulterband) in 1984
  • Bavarian Maximilian Order for Science and Art in 1984

Works edit

Lorenz's best-known books are King Solomon's Ring and On Aggression, both written for a popular audience. His scientific work appeared mainly in journal articles, written in German; it became widely known to English-speaking scientists through its description in Tinbergen's 1951 book The Study of Instinct, though many of his papers were later published in English translation in the two volumes titled Studies in Animal and Human Behavior.

  • King Solomon's Ring (1949) (Er redete mit dem Vieh, den Vögeln und den Fischen, 1949)
  • Man Meets Dog (1950) (So kam der Mensch auf den Hund, 1950)
  • Evolution and Modification of Behaviour (1965)
  • On Aggression (1966) (Das sogenannte Böse. Zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression, 1963)
  • Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume I (1970)
  • Studies in Animal and Human Behavior, Volume II (1971)
  • Motivation of Human and Animal Behavior: An Ethological View. With Paul Leyhausen (1973). New York: D. Van Nostrand Co. ISBN 0-442-24886-5
  • Behind the Mirror: A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge (1973) (Die Rückseite des Spiegels. Versuch einer Naturgeschichte menschlichen Erkennens, 1973)
  • Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins (1974) (Die acht Todsünden der zivilisierten Menschheit, 1973)
  • The Year of the Greylag Goose (1979) (Das Jahr der Graugans, 1979)
  • The Foundations of Ethology (1982)
  • The Waning of Humaneness (1987) (Der Abbau des Menschlichen, 1983)
  • Here I Am – Where Are You? – A Lifetime's Study of the Uncannily Human Behaviour of the Greylag Goose. (1988). Translated by Robert D. Martin from Hier bin ich – wo bist du?
  • The Natural Science of the Human Species: An Introduction to Comparative Behavioral Research – The Russian Manuscript (1944–1948) (1995)

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c Krebs, J. R.; Sjolander, S.; Sjolander, S. (1992). "Konrad Zacharias Lorenz. 7 November 1903 – 27 February 1989". Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society. 38: 210–228. doi:10.1098/rsbm.1992.0011. PMID 11616215.
  2. ^ Haggbloom, Steven J.; Warnick, Renee; Warnick, Jason E.; Jones, Vinessa K.; Yarbrough, Gary L.; Russell, Tenea M.; Borecky, Chris M.; McGahhey, Reagan; Powell, John L. III; Beavers, Jamie; Monte, Emmanuelle (2002). "The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century". Review of General Psychology. 6 (2): 139–52. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.586.1913. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.6.2.139. S2CID 145668721.
  3. ^ a b c d e "Konrad Lorenz – Biographical, The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1973". Nobel prize The Official Web Site of The Nobel Prize. Retrieved 7 November 2013.
  4. ^ Campbell, Donald T. (1975). "Reintroducing Konrad Lorenz to Psychology". In Evans, R. I. (ed.). Konrad Lorenz: The Man and His Ideas. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. p. 106. ISBN 978-0-15-147285-7.
  5. ^ Konrad Lorenz, Alec Nisbett, Dent, 1976, p. 15
  6. ^ a b Sullivan, Walter (March 1989). "Konrad Lorenz, Pioneer in Study of Animals' Behavior, Dies at 85". The New York Times.
  7. ^ Nisbett, Alec (1976). Konrad Lorenz. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. p. 72. ISBN 978-0-15-147286-4.
  8. ^ Columbia College (Columbia University). Office of Alumni Affairs and Development; Columbia College (Columbia University) (1989). Columbia College today. Columbia University Libraries. New York, N.Y. : Columbia College, Office of Alumni Affairs and Development.
  9. ^ a b Lorenz, Konrad (2007). King Solomon's Ring (3rd ed.). London: Routledge. pp. 4–5. ISBN 978-0-415-26747-2.
  10. ^ Klopfer, Peter (1994). "Konrad Lorenz and the National Socialists: On the Politics of Ethology". International Journal of Comparative Psychology. 7 (4): 202–208. doi:10.46867/C4P30R. S2CID 141222261.
  11. ^ Deichmann, Ute (1992). Biologists under Hitler: Expulsion, Careers, Research. Frankfurt/Main, New York: Harvard University Press. pp. 261–264. ISBN 978-0-674-07404-0.
  12. ^ Klopfer, Peter (1994). "Konrad Lorenz and the National Socialists: On the Politics of Ethology". International Journal of Comparative Psychology. 7 (4). doi:10.46867/C4P30R. ISSN 0889-3667. S2CID 141222261.
  13. ^ a b Burkhardt, R. W. (2005). Patterns of behavior: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen, and the founding of ethology. University of Chicago Press.
  14. ^ Alec Nisbett, Konrad Lorenz (1976), ISBN 0-15-147286-6, page 94.
  15. ^ Sullivan, Walter (1 March 1989). "Konrad Lorenz, Pioneer in Study Of Animals' Behavior, Dies at 85". The New York Times.
  16. ^ "Konrad Lorenz: Biographical". nobelprize.org.
  17. ^ Karl Popper and Konrad Lorenz, Die Zukunft ist offen: das Altenberger Gespräch, mit den Texten des Wiener Popper-Symposiums, ed. Franz Kreuzer (Munich: Piper, 1985). Reprinted by Chicago: Northwestern University, 2011. ISBN 978-3-492-00640-8 For an English-language discussion of the Vienna of Popper and Lorenz's early years, see Malachi Haim Hacohen, Karl Popper – The Formative Years, 1902–1945: Politics and Philosophy in Interwar Vienna (Cambridge University Press, 2002), 31–34. ISBN 978-0-521-89055-7 Google books
  18. ^ Konrad Lorenz, Alec Nisbett, Dent, 1976, p. 22
  19. ^ Anatomy of Restlessness- Uncollected Writings, Bruce Chatwin, Picador, 1997, p. 141
  20. ^ a b c Allen, Colin (1997). Species of mind: the philosophy and biology of cognitive ethology. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-51108-7. OCLC 42328566.
  21. ^ Yount, L. (2009). A to Z of Biologists. Infobase Publishing. pp. 183–184
  22. ^ Vicedo, M. (2009). "The father of ethology and the foster mother of ducks: Konrad Lorenz as an Expert on Motherhood". Isis. 100 (2): 263–291. doi:10.1086/599553. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002A-B0E9-E. PMID 19653490. S2CID 45373309.
  23. ^ Tinbergen, N. (1963). On Aims and Methods of Ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 20(4), 410–433.
  24. ^ Eisenberg, L. (2005). "Which Image for Lorenz?". American Journal of Psychiatry. 162 (9): 1760. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.9.1760. PMID 16135651.
  25. ^ Sax, Boria (2007). "Konrad Lorenz and the Mythology of Science". In Aftandilian, Dave; et al. (eds.). What Are Animals to Us? Approaches from Science, Religion, Folklore, Literature and Art. Knoxville: U. of Tennessee Press. pp. 269–276. ISBN 978-1-57233-472-4.
  26. ^ Sax, Boria (1997). "What is a 'Jewish Dog'?: Konrad Z. Lorenz and the Cult of Wildness". Society and Animals. 5 (1): 3–21. doi:10.1163/156853097X00196.
  27. ^ Föger, B., & Taschwer, K. (2001). Die andere Seite des Spiegels: Konrad Lorenz und der Nationalsozialismus. Czernin-Verlag.
  28. ^ Kalikow, T. J. (1983). "Konrad Lorenz's ethological theory: Explanation and ideology, 1938–1943". Journal of the History of Biology. 16 (1): 39–73. doi:10.1007/bf00186675. PMID 11611248. S2CID 26788185.
  29. ^ Kalikow, T. J. (1978). Konrad Lorenz's "brown past": A reply to Alec Nisbett.
  30. ^ World Jewish Congress (18 December 2015). "Late Austrian scientist Konrad Lorenz stripped of doctorate for lying about Nazi past". World Jewish Congress. Retrieved 20 June 2021.
  31. ^ Klopfer 1994; Deichmann 1992.
  32. ^ Klopfer 1994.
  33. ^ Anonymous, "Austrian university strips Nobel Prize winner Konrad Lorenz of doctorate due to Nazi past", Associated Press, 17 December 2015.
  34. ^ "Uni Salzburg entzieht Konrad Lorenz die Ehrendoktorwürde". Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung (FAZ). 18 December 2015. Retrieved 19 December 2015.
  35. ^ a b Tinbergen, N. (1963). "On aims and methods of ethology". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 20 (4): 410–433. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x.
  36. ^ Lorenz, Konrad (1937). "On the formation of the concept of instinct". Die Naturwissenschaften. 25 (19): 289–300. Bibcode:1937NW.....25..289L. doi:10.1007/BF01492648. S2CID 41134631.
  37. ^ Dawkins, Richard (1982). The Extended Phenotype. Oxford: Oxford University Press. p. 2. ISBN 978-0-19-286088-0.
  38. ^ a b c Dawkins, Richard (1976). The Selfish Gene (1st ed.). Oxford University Press. pp. 9, 72. ISBN 978-0-19-857519-1.
  39. ^ Lorenz, Konrad (1979). The Year of the Greylag Goose. London: Eyre Methuen. p. 6.
  40. ^ Lorenz (1979), p. 7.
  41. ^ Gli otto peccati capitali della nostra civiltà [Civilized Man's Eight Deadly Sins]. Milano: Adelphi Edizioni. 1974. p. 26. The citation is translated from the Italian version of the book.
  42. ^ LORENZ, Konrad. On Aggression. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1966. Translated by Marjorie Kerr Wilson. Originally published in Austria under the title DAS SOGENANNTE BÖSE. Zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression. Viena: Dr. G. Borotha-Schoeler Verlag, 1963, p. 263.
  43. ^ "Konrad Zacharias Lorenz". American Academy of Arts & Sciences. Retrieved 10 August 2022.
  44. ^ "Konrad Lorenz". www.nasonline.org. Retrieved 10 August 2022.
  45. ^ "APS Member History". search.amphilsoc.org. Retrieved 10 August 2022.
  46. ^ "Titel "erschlichen": Uni Salzburg entzieht Konrad Lorenz die Ehrendoktorwürde". Faz.net – via www.faz.net.

External links edit

Konrad & Adolf Lorenz Museum KALM https://www.kalm.at

konrad, lorenz, konrad, zacharias, lorenz, german, pronunciation, ˈkɔnʁaːt, tsaxaˈʁiːas, ˈloːʁɛnts, november, 1903, february, 1989, austrian, zoologist, ethologist, ornithologist, shared, 1973, nobel, prize, physiology, medicine, with, nikolaas, tinbergen, kar. Konrad Zacharias Lorenz German pronunciation ˈkɔnʁaːt tsaxaˈʁiːas ˈloːʁɛnts 7 November 1903 27 February 1989 was an Austrian zoologist ethologist and ornithologist He shared the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine with Nikolaas Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch He is often regarded as one of the founders of modern ethology the study of animal behavior He developed an approach that began with an earlier generation including his teacher Oskar Heinroth 1 Konrad LorenzForMemRSLorenz in 1978BornKonrad Zacharias Lorenz 1903 11 07 7 November 1903Vienna Austria HungaryDied27 February 1989 1989 02 27 aged 85 Vienna AustriaNationalityAustrianAlma materColumbia UniversityUniversity of Vienna MD PhD AwardsForMemRS 1964 1 Kalinga Prize 1969 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1973 Scientific careerFieldsEthology Lorenz studied instinctive behavior in animals especially in greylag geese and jackdaws Working with geese he investigated the principle of imprinting the process by which some nidifugous birds i e birds that leave their nest early bond instinctively with the first moving object that they see within the first hours of hatching Although Lorenz did not discover the topic he became widely known for his descriptions of imprinting as an instinctive bond In 1936 he met Tinbergen and the two collaborated in developing ethology as a separate sub discipline of biology A Review of General Psychology survey published in 2002 ranked Lorenz the 65th most cited scholar of the 20th century in the technical psychology journals introductory psychology textbooks and survey responses 2 Lorenz s work was interrupted by the onset of World War II and in 1941 he was recruited into the German Army as a medic 3 In 1944 he was sent to the Eastern Front where he was captured by the Soviet Red Army and spent four years as a German prisoner of war in Soviet Armenia After the war he regretted his membership in the Nazi Party 4 Lorenz wrote numerous books some of which such as King Solomon s Ring On Aggression and Man Meets Dog became popular reading His last work Here I Am Where Are You is a summary of his life s work and focuses on his famous studies of greylag geese Contents 1 Biography 2 Personal life 3 Ethology 4 Politics 4 1 Nazism 4 2 Ecology 5 Contributions and legacy 5 1 Vision of the challenges facing humanity 5 2 Philosophical speculations 6 Honours and awards 7 Works 8 See also 9 References 10 External linksBiography edit nbsp Lorenz in 1904 with his elder brother Lorenz was the son of Adolf Lorenz a wealthy and distinguished surgeon and his wife Emma nee Lecher a physician who had been her husband s assistant 5 The family lived on a large estate at Altenberg and had a city apartment in Vienna 6 He was educated at the Public Schottengymnasium of the Benedictine monks in Vienna In his autobiographical essay published in 1973 in Les Prix Nobel winners of the prizes are requested to provide such essays Lorenz credits his career to his parents who were supremely tolerant of my inordinate love for animals and to his childhood encounter with Selma Lagerlof s The Wonderful Adventures of Nils which filled him with a great enthusiasm about wild geese 7 At the request of his father Adolf Lorenz he began a premedical curriculum in 1922 at Columbia University 8 but he returned to Vienna in 1923 to continue his studies at the University of Vienna He graduated as Doctor of Medicine MD in 1928 and became an assistant professor at the Institute of Anatomy until 1935 He finished his zoological studies in 1933 and received his second doctorate PhD 9 While still a student Lorenz began developing what would become a large menagerie ranging from domestic to exotic animals In his popular book King Solomon s Ring Lorenz recounts that while studying at the University of Vienna he kept a variety of animals at his parents apartment ranging from fish to a capuchin monkey named Gloria 9 In 1936 at an international scientific symposium on instinct Lorenz met his great friend and colleague Nikolaas Tinbergen Together they studied geese wild domestic and hybrid One result of these studies was that Lorenz realized that an overpowering increase in the drives of feeding as well as of copulation and a waning of more differentiated social instincts is characteristic of very many domestic animals Lorenz began to suspect and fear that analogous processes of deterioration may be at work with civilized humanity This observation of bird hybrids caused Lorenz to believe that domestication resulting from urbanisation in humans might also cause dysgenic effects and to argue in two papers that the Nazi eugenics policies against this were therefore scientifically justified 10 nbsp Lorenz as a Soviet POW in 1944 In 1940 he became a professor of psychology at the University of Konigsberg He was drafted into the Wehrmacht in 1941 He sought to be a motorcycle mechanic but instead he was assigned as a military psychologist conducting racial studies on humans in occupied Poznan under Rudolf Hippius The objective was to study the biological characteristics of German Polish half breeds to determine whether they benefitted from the same work ethics as pure Germans 11 12 The degree to which Lorenz participated in the project is unknown but the project director Hippius referred a couple of times to Lorenz as an examining psychologist 13 Lorenz later described that he once saw transports of concentration camp inmates at Fort VII near Poznan which made him fully realize the complete inhumanity of the Nazis 14 He was sent to the Russian front in 1944 where he quickly became a prisoner of war in the Soviet Union from 1944 to 1948 In captivity in Soviet Armenia 15 he continued to work as a medic and became tolerably fluent in Russian and got quite friendly with some Russians mostly doctors 16 When he was repatriated he was allowed to keep the manuscript of a book he had been writing and his pet starling He arrived back in Altenberg his family home near Vienna both with manuscript and bird intact The manuscript became his 1973 book Behind the Mirror 3 The Max Planck Society established the Lorenz Institute for Behavioral Physiology in Buldern Germany in 1950 In his memoirs Lorenz described the chronology of his war years differently from what historians have been able to document after his death He himself claimed that he was captured in 1942 where in reality he was only sent to the front and captured in 1944 leaving out entirely his involvement with the Poznan project 3 In 1958 Lorenz transferred to the Max Planck Institute for Behavioral Physiology in Seewiesen He shared the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine for discoveries in individual and social behavior patterns with two other important early ethologists Nikolaas Tinbergen and Karl von Frisch In 1969 he became the first recipient of the Prix mondial Cino Del Duca He was a friend and student of renowned biologist Sir Julian Huxley grandson of Darwin s bulldog Thomas Henry Huxley Famed psychoanalyst Ralph Greenson and Sir Peter Scott were good friends Lorenz and Karl Popper were childhood friends many years after they met during the celebration of Popper s 80 years they wrote together a book entitled Die Zukunft ist offen 17 He retired from the Max Planck Institute in 1973 but continued to research and publish from Altenberg and Grunau im Almtal in Austria He died on 27 February 1989 in Altenberg Personal life editLorenz married his childhood friend Margarethe Gebhardt a gynaecologist daughter of a market gardener who lived near the Lorenz family 18 they had a son and two daughters He lived at the Lorenz family estate which included a fantastical neo baroque mansion previously owned by his father 6 19 Ethology editFurther information Imprinting psychology and Ritualization Lorenz is recognized as one of the founding fathers of the field of ethology the study of animal behavior He is best known for his research of the principle of attachment or imprinting through which in some species a bond is formed between a newborn animal and its caregiver This principle had been discovered by Douglas Spalding in the 19th century and Lorenz s mentor Oskar Heinroth had also worked on the topic but Lorenz s description of Pragung imprinting in nidifugous birds such as greylag geese in his 1935 book Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels The Companion in the Environment of Birds became the foundational description of the phenomenon 13 Here Lorenz used Jakob von Uexkull s concept of Umwelt to understand how the limited perception of animals filtered out certain phenomena with which they interacted instinctively For example a young goose instinctively bonds with the first moving stimulus it perceives whether it be its mother or a person Lorenz showed that this behavior of imprinting is what allows the goose to learn to recognize members of its own species enabling them to be the object of subsequent behavior patterns such as mating 20 He developed a theory of instinctive behavior that saw behavior patterns as largely innate but triggered through environmental stimuli for example the hawk goose effect He argued that animals have an inner drive to carry out instinctive behaviors and that if they do not encounter the right stimulus they will eventually engage in the behavior with an inappropriate stimulus 21 Lorenz s approach to ethology derived from a skepticism towards the studies of animal behavior done in laboratory settings He considered that in order to understand the mechanisms of animal behavior it was necessary to observe their full range of behaviors in their natural context Lorenz did not carry out much traditional fieldwork but observed animals near his home His method involved empathizing with animals often using anthropomorphization to imagine their mental states He believed that animals were capable of experiencing many of the same emotions as humans 20 22 Tinbergen Lorenz s friend with whom he conjointly received the Nobel prize summarized Lorenz s major contribution to ethology as making behavior a topic of biological inquiry considering behavior a part of an animal s evolutionary equipment 23 Tinbergen and Lorenz contributed to making Ethology a recognized sub discipline within Biology and founded the first specialized journal of the field Ethology originally Zeitschift fur Tierpsychologie 20 Politics editNazism edit Lorenz joined the Nazi Party in 1938 and accepted a university chair under the Nazi regime In his application for party membership he wrote I m able to say that my whole scientific work is devoted to the ideas of the National Socialists His publications during that time led in later years to allegations that his scientific work had been contaminated by Nazi sympathies His published writing during the Nazi period included support for Nazi ideas of racial hygiene couched in pseudoscientific metaphors 24 25 26 27 28 29 In his autobiography Lorenz wrote The same individual geese on which we conducted these experiments first aroused my interest in the process of domestication They were F1 hybrids of wild Greylags and domestic geese and they showed surprising deviations from the normal social and sexual behaviour of the wild birds I realised that an overpowering increase in the drives of feeding as well as of copulation and a waning of more differentiated social instincts is characteristic of very many domestic animals I was frightened as I still am by the thought that analogous genetical processes of deterioration may be at work with civilized humanity Moved by this fear I did a very ill advised thing soon after the Germans had invaded Austria I wrote about the dangers of domestication and in order to be understood I couched my writing in the worst of nazi terminology I do not want to extenuate this action I did indeed believe that some good might come of the new rulers The precedent narrow minded catholic regime in Austria induced better and more intelligent men than I was to cherish this naive hope Practically all my friends and teachers did so including my own father who certainly was a kindly and humane man None of us as much as suspected that the word selection when used by these rulers meant murder I regret those writings not so much for the undeniable discredit they reflect on my person as for their effect of hampering the future recognition of the dangers of domestication 3 After the war Lorenz denied having been a party member 30 until his membership application was made public and he denied having known the extent of the genocide despite his position as a psychologist in the Office of Racial Policy 31 He was also shown to have made anti Semitic jokes on Jewish characteristics in letters to his mentor Heinroth 32 In 2015 the University of Salzburg posthumously rescinded an honorary doctorate awarded to Lorenz in 1983 citing his party membership and his assertions in his application that he was always a National Socialist and that his work stands to serve National Socialist thought The university also accused him of using his work to spread basic elements of the racist ideology of National Socialism 33 34 Ecology edit During the final years of his life Lorenz supported the fledgling Austrian Green Party and in 1984 became the figurehead of the Konrad Lorenz Volksbegehren a grass roots movement that was formed to prevent the building of a power plant at the Danube near Hainburg an der Donau and thus the destruction of the surrounding woodland Contributions and legacy edit nbsp With Nikolaas Tinbergen left 1978 Lorenz has been called The father of ethology by Niko Tinbergen 35 Perhaps Lorenz s most important contribution to ethology was his idea that behavior patterns can be studied as anatomical organs 36 This concept forms the foundation of ethological research 35 37 However Richard Dawkins called Lorenz a good of the species man 38 stating that the idea of group selection was so deeply ingrained 38 in Lorenz s thinking that he evidently did not realize that his statements contravened orthodox Darwinian theory 38 Together with Nikolaas Tinbergen Lorenz developed the idea of an innate releasing mechanism to explain instinctive behaviors fixed action patterns They experimented with supernormal stimuli such as giant eggs or dummy bird beaks which they found could release the fixed action patterns more powerfully than the natural objects for which the behaviors were adapted Influenced by the ideas of William McDougall Lorenz developed this into a psychohydraulic model of the motivation of behavior which tended towards group selectionist ideas which were influential in the 1960s Another of his contributions to ethology is his work on imprinting His influence on a younger generation of ethologists and his popular works were important in bringing ethology to the attention of the general public Lorenz claimed that there was widespread contempt for the descriptive sciences He attributed this to the denial of perception as the source of all scientific knowledge a denial that has been elevated to the status of religion 39 He wrote that in comparative behavioral research it is necessary to describe various patterns of movement record them and above all render them unmistakably recognizable 40 There are three research institutions named after Lorenz in Austria the Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research KLI was housed in Lorenz family mansion at Altenberg before moving to Klosterneuburg in 2013 Discover the KLI the Konrad Lorenz Forschungsstelle KLF at his former field station in Grunau and the Konrad Lorenz Institute of Ethology an external research facility of the University of Veterinary Medicine Vienna Vision of the challenges facing humanity edit nbsp With Nikolaas Tinbergen right 1978 Lorenz predicted the relationship between market economics and the threat of ecological catastrophe In his 1973 book Civilized Man s Eight Deadly Sins Lorenz addresses the following paradox All the advantages that man has gained from his ever deepening understanding of the natural world that surrounds him his technological chemical and medical progress all of which should seem to alleviate human suffering tends instead to favor humanity s destruction 41 Lorenz adopts an ecological model to attempt to grasp the mechanisms behind this contradiction Thus all species are adapted to their environment including not only inorganic components but all the other living beings that inhabit the locality p31 Fundamental to Lorenz s theory of ecology is the function of negative feedback mechanisms which in hierarchical fashion dampen impulses that occur beneath a certain threshold The thresholds themselves are the product of the interaction of contrasting mechanisms Thus pain and pleasure act as checks on each other To gain a desired prey a dog or wolf will do things that in other contexts they would shy away from run through thorn bushes jump into cold water and expose themselves to risks which would normally frighten them All these inhibitory mechanisms act as a counterweight to the effects of learning mechanisms The organism cannot allow itself to pay a price which is not worth paying p53 In nature these mechanisms tend towards a stable state among the living beings of an ecology A closer examination shows that these beings not only do not damage each other but often constitute a community of interests It is obvious that the predator is strongly interested in the survival of that species animal or vegetable which constitutes its prey It is not uncommon that the prey species derives specific benefits from its interaction with the predator species pp31 33 Lorenz states that humanity is the one species not bound by these mechanisms being the only one that has defined its own environment The pace of human ecology is determined by the progress of man s technology p35 human ecology economy is governed by mechanisms of POSITIVE feedback defined as a mechanism which tends to encourage behavior rather than to attenuate it p43 Positive feedback always involves the danger of an avalanche effect One particular kind of positive feedback occurs when individuals OF THE SAME SPECIES enter into competition among themselves For many animal species environmental factors keep intraspecies selection from leading to disaster But there is no force which exercises this type of healthy regulatory effect on humanity s cultural development unfortunately for itself humanity has learned to overcome all those environmental forces which are external to itself p44 Regarding aggression in human beings Lorenz states Let us imagine that an absolutely unbiased investigator on another planet perhaps on Mars is examining human behavior on earth with the aid of a telescope whose magnification is too small to enable him to discern individuals and follow their separate behavior but large enough for him to observe occurrences such as migrations of peoples wars and similar great historical events He would never gain the impression that human behavior was dictated by intelligence still less by responsible morality If we suppose our extraneous observer to be a being of pure reason devoid of instincts himself and unaware of the way in which all instincts in general and aggression in particular can miscarry he would be at a complete loss how to explain history at all The ever recurrent phenomena of history do not have reasonable causes It is a mere commonplace to say that they are caused by what common parlance so aptly terms human nature Unreasoning and unreasonable human nature causes two nations to compete though no economic necessity compels them to do so it induces two political parties or religions with amazingly similar programs of salvation to fight each other bitterly and it impels an Alexander or a Napoleon to sacrifice millions of lives in his attempt to unite the world under his scepter We have been taught to regard some of the persons who have committed these and similar absurdities with respect even as great men we are wont to yield to the political wisdom of those in charge and we are all so accustomed to these phenomena that most of us fail to realize how abjectly stupid and undesirable the historical mass behavior of humanity actually is 42 Lorenz does not see human independence from natural ecological processes as necessarily bad He states that A completely new ecology which corresponds in every way to humanity s desires could theoretically prove as durable as that which would have existed without his intervention 36 However the principle of competition typical of Western societies destroys any chance of this The competition between human beings destroys with cold and diabolic brutality Under the pressure of this competitive fury we have not only forgotten what is useful to humanity as a whole but even that which is good and advantageous to the individual One asks which is more damaging to modern humanity the thirst for money or consuming haste in either case fear plays a very important role the fear of being overtaken by one s competitors the fear of becoming poor the fear of making wrong decisions or the fear of not being up to snuff pp 45 47 Philosophical speculations edit In his 1973 book Behind the Mirror A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge Lorenz considers the old philosophical question of whether our senses correctly inform us about the world as it is or provide us only with an illusion His answer comes from evolutionary biology Only traits that help us survive and reproduce are transmitted If our senses gave us wrong information about our environment we would soon be extinct Therefore we can be sure that our senses give us correct information for otherwise we would not be here to be deceived Honours and awards editElected a member of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences in 1957 43 Austrian Decoration for Science and Art in 1964 Elected a Foreign Member of the Royal Society ForMemRS in 1964 1 Elected a member of the United States National Academy of Sciences in 1966 44 Kalinga Prize for the Popularization of Science in 1969 Gold Medal of the Humboldt Society in 1972Rupke Nicolaas 2008 Alexander Von Humboldt A Metabiography University of Chicago Press p 151 ISBN 978 0 226 73149 0 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1973 3 Elected a member of the American Philosophical Society in 1974 45 Honorary Doctorate University of Salzburg 1983 revoked in 2015 46 Grand Cross with Star and Sash of the Order of Merit of the Federal Republic of Germany Grosses Verdienstkreuz mit Stern und Schulterband in 1984 Bavarian Maximilian Order for Science and Art in 1984Works editLorenz s best known books are King Solomon s Ring and On Aggression both written for a popular audience His scientific work appeared mainly in journal articles written in German it became widely known to English speaking scientists through its description in Tinbergen s 1951 book The Study of Instinct though many of his papers were later published in English translation in the two volumes titled Studies in Animal and Human Behavior King Solomon s Ring 1949 Er redete mit dem Vieh den Vogeln und den Fischen 1949 Man Meets Dog 1950 So kam der Mensch auf den Hund 1950 Evolution and Modification of Behaviour 1965 On Aggression 1966 Das sogenannte Bose Zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression 1963 Studies in Animal and Human Behavior Volume I 1970 Studies in Animal and Human Behavior Volume II 1971 Motivation of Human and Animal Behavior An Ethological View With Paul Leyhausen 1973 New York D Van Nostrand Co ISBN 0 442 24886 5 Behind the Mirror A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge 1973 Die Ruckseite des Spiegels Versuch einer Naturgeschichte menschlichen Erkennens 1973 Civilized Man s Eight Deadly Sins 1974 Die acht Todsunden der zivilisierten Menschheit 1973 The Year of the Greylag Goose 1979 Das Jahr der Graugans 1979 The Foundations of Ethology 1982 The Waning of Humaneness 1987 Der Abbau des Menschlichen 1983 Here I Am Where Are You A Lifetime s Study of the Uncannily Human Behaviour of the Greylag Goose 1988 Translated by Robert D Martin from Hier bin ich wo bist du The Natural Science of the Human Species An Introduction to Comparative Behavioral Research The Russian Manuscript 1944 1948 1995 See also editAnimal mobbing behavior Imprinting psychology References edit a b c Krebs J R Sjolander S Sjolander S 1992 Konrad Zacharias Lorenz 7 November 1903 27 February 1989 Biographical Memoirs of Fellows of the Royal Society 38 210 228 doi 10 1098 rsbm 1992 0011 PMID 11616215 Haggbloom Steven J Warnick Renee Warnick Jason E Jones Vinessa K Yarbrough Gary L Russell Tenea M Borecky Chris M McGahhey Reagan Powell John L III Beavers Jamie Monte Emmanuelle 2002 The 100 most eminent psychologists of the 20th century Review of General Psychology 6 2 139 52 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 586 1913 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 6 2 139 S2CID 145668721 a b c d e Konrad Lorenz Biographical The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1973 Nobel prize The Official Web Site of The Nobel Prize Retrieved 7 November 2013 Campbell Donald T 1975 Reintroducing Konrad Lorenz to Psychology In Evans R I ed Konrad Lorenz The Man and His Ideas New York Harcourt Brace Jovanovich p 106 ISBN 978 0 15 147285 7 Konrad Lorenz Alec Nisbett Dent 1976 p 15 a b Sullivan Walter March 1989 Konrad Lorenz Pioneer in Study of Animals Behavior Dies at 85 The New York Times Nisbett Alec 1976 Konrad Lorenz Harcourt Brace Jovanovich p 72 ISBN 978 0 15 147286 4 Columbia College Columbia University Office of Alumni Affairs and Development Columbia College Columbia University 1989 Columbia College today Columbia University Libraries New York N Y Columbia College Office of Alumni Affairs and Development a b Lorenz Konrad 2007 King Solomon s Ring 3rd ed London Routledge pp 4 5 ISBN 978 0 415 26747 2 Klopfer Peter 1994 Konrad Lorenz and the National Socialists On the Politics of Ethology International Journal of Comparative Psychology 7 4 202 208 doi 10 46867 C4P30R S2CID 141222261 Deichmann Ute 1992 Biologists under Hitler Expulsion Careers Research Frankfurt Main New York Harvard University Press pp 261 264 ISBN 978 0 674 07404 0 Klopfer Peter 1994 Konrad Lorenz and the National Socialists On the Politics of Ethology International Journal of Comparative Psychology 7 4 doi 10 46867 C4P30R ISSN 0889 3667 S2CID 141222261 a b Burkhardt R W 2005 Patterns of behavior Konrad Lorenz Niko Tinbergen and the founding of ethology University of Chicago Press Alec Nisbett Konrad Lorenz 1976 ISBN 0 15 147286 6 page 94 Sullivan Walter 1 March 1989 Konrad Lorenz Pioneer in Study Of Animals Behavior Dies at 85 The New York Times Konrad Lorenz Biographical nobelprize org Karl Popper and Konrad Lorenz Die Zukunft ist offen das Altenberger Gesprach mit den Texten des Wiener Popper Symposiums ed Franz Kreuzer Munich Piper 1985 Reprinted by Chicago Northwestern University 2011 ISBN 978 3 492 00640 8 For an English language discussion of the Vienna of Popper and Lorenz s early years see Malachi Haim Hacohen Karl Popper The Formative Years 1902 1945 Politics and Philosophy in Interwar Vienna Cambridge University Press 2002 31 34 ISBN 978 0 521 89055 7 Google books Konrad Lorenz Alec Nisbett Dent 1976 p 22 Anatomy of Restlessness Uncollected Writings Bruce Chatwin Picador 1997 p 141 a b c Allen Colin 1997 Species of mind the philosophy and biology of cognitive ethology Cambridge Mass MIT Press ISBN 978 0 262 51108 7 OCLC 42328566 Yount L 2009 A to Z of Biologists Infobase Publishing pp 183 184 Vicedo M 2009 The father of ethology and the foster mother of ducks Konrad Lorenz as an Expert on Motherhood Isis 100 2 263 291 doi 10 1086 599553 hdl 11858 00 001M 0000 002A B0E9 E PMID 19653490 S2CID 45373309 Tinbergen N 1963 On Aims and Methods of Ethology Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 20 4 410 433 Eisenberg L 2005 Which Image for Lorenz American Journal of Psychiatry 162 9 1760 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 162 9 1760 PMID 16135651 Sax Boria 2007 Konrad Lorenz and the Mythology of Science In Aftandilian Dave et al eds What Are Animals to Us Approaches from Science Religion Folklore Literature and Art Knoxville U of Tennessee Press pp 269 276 ISBN 978 1 57233 472 4 Sax Boria 1997 What is a Jewish Dog Konrad Z Lorenz and the Cult of Wildness Society and Animals 5 1 3 21 doi 10 1163 156853097X00196 Foger B amp Taschwer K 2001 Die andere Seite des Spiegels Konrad Lorenz und der Nationalsozialismus Czernin Verlag Kalikow T J 1983 Konrad Lorenz s ethological theory Explanation and ideology 1938 1943 Journal of the History of Biology 16 1 39 73 doi 10 1007 bf00186675 PMID 11611248 S2CID 26788185 Kalikow T J 1978 Konrad Lorenz s brown past A reply to Alec Nisbett World Jewish Congress 18 December 2015 Late Austrian scientist Konrad Lorenz stripped of doctorate for lying about Nazi past World Jewish Congress Retrieved 20 June 2021 Klopfer 1994 Deichmann 1992 Klopfer 1994 Anonymous Austrian university strips Nobel Prize winner Konrad Lorenz of doctorate due to Nazi past Associated Press 17 December 2015 Uni Salzburg entzieht Konrad Lorenz die Ehrendoktorwurde Frankfurter Allgemeine Zeitung FAZ 18 December 2015 Retrieved 19 December 2015 a b Tinbergen N 1963 On aims and methods of ethology Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 20 4 410 433 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 1963 tb01161 x Lorenz Konrad 1937 On the formation of the concept of instinct Die Naturwissenschaften 25 19 289 300 Bibcode 1937NW 25 289L doi 10 1007 BF01492648 S2CID 41134631 Dawkins Richard 1982 The Extended Phenotype Oxford Oxford University Press p 2 ISBN 978 0 19 286088 0 a b c Dawkins Richard 1976 The Selfish Gene 1st ed Oxford University Press pp 9 72 ISBN 978 0 19 857519 1 Lorenz Konrad 1979 The Year of the Greylag Goose London Eyre Methuen p 6 Lorenz 1979 p 7 Gli otto peccati capitali della nostra civilta Civilized Man s Eight Deadly Sins Milano Adelphi Edizioni 1974 p 26 The citation is translated from the Italian version of the book LORENZ Konrad On Aggression New York Harcourt Brace amp World 1966 Translated by Marjorie Kerr Wilson Originally published in Austria under the title DAS SOGENANNTE BOSE Zur Naturgeschichte der Aggression Viena Dr G Borotha Schoeler Verlag 1963 p 263 Konrad Zacharias Lorenz American Academy of Arts amp Sciences Retrieved 10 August 2022 Konrad Lorenz www nasonline org Retrieved 10 August 2022 APS Member History search amphilsoc org Retrieved 10 August 2022 Titel erschlichen Uni Salzburg entzieht Konrad Lorenz die Ehrendoktorwurde Faz net via www faz net External links edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Konrad Lorenz Konrad amp Adolf Lorenz Museum KALM https www kalm atKonrad Lorenz on Nobelprize org nbsp A chapter from On Aggression 1963 Review of Biologists Under Hitler Konrad Lorenz Institutes Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research in Altenberg Konrad Lorenz Research Station Grunau im Almtal Konrad Lorenz Institute for Ethology Konrad Lorenz at IMDb Portals nbsp History of science nbsp Biology nbsp Austria nbsp Biography Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Konrad Lorenz amp oldid 1220049378, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.