fbpx
Wikipedia

Courtship display

A courtship display is a set of display behaviors in which an animal, usually a male, attempts to attract a mate; the mate exercises choice, so sexual selection acts on the display. These behaviors often include ritualized movement ("dances"), vocalizations, mechanical sound production, or displays of beauty, strength, or agonistic ability.[1][2][3][4][5][6]

Male blackbuck, Antilope cervicapra, courting a female

Male display

 
Male peacock spider, Maratus volans, courtship display

In some species, males will perform ritualized movements to attract females. The male six-plumed bird-of-paradise (Parotia lawesii) exemplifies male courtship display with its ritualized "ballerina dance" and unique occipital and breast feathers that serve to stimulate the female visual system.[7] In Drosophila subobscura, male courtship display is seen through the male's intricate wing scissoring patterns and rapid sidestepping.[8] These stimulations, along with many other factors, result in subsequent copulation or rejection.

In other species, males may exhibit courtship displays that serve as both visual and auditory stimulation. For example, the male Anna's hummingbird (Calypte anna) and calliope hummingbird (Stellula calliope) perform two types of courtship displays involving a combination of visual and vocal display—a stationary shuttle display[9] and dive display.[10][11] When engaging in the stationary shuttle display, the male displays a flared gorget and hovers in front of the female, moving from side to side while rotating his body and tail. The rhythmic movements of the male's wings produce a distinctive buzzing sound.[9] When conducting a dive display, the male typically ascends approximately 20–35 m (66–115 ft) in the air then abruptly turns and descends in a dive-like fashion. As the male flies over the female, he rotates his body and spreads his tail feathers, which flutter and collide to produce a short, buzzing sound.[9]

 
A male satin bowerbird makes and uses a bower to attract potential mates.

In addition, some animals attempt to attract females through the construction and decoration of unique structures. This technique can be seen in the satin bowerbird (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus) of Australia, males of which build and decorate nest-like structures called "bowers".[12] Bowers are decorated with bright and colourful objects (typically blue in colour) to attract and stimulate visiting females.[12] Typically, males who acquire the largest number of decorations tend to have greater success in mating.[13]

In some species, males initiate courtship rituals only after mounting the female. Courtship may even continue after copulation has been completed.[14] In this system, the ability of the female to choose her mate is limited. This process, known as copulatory courtship, is prevalent in many insect species.[15]

In most species, the male sex initiates courtship displays in precopulatory sexual selection. Performing a display allows the male to present his traits or abilities to a female. Mate choice, in this context, is driven by females; direct or indirect benefits to the female often determine which males reproduce and which do not.

Direct benefits may accrue to the female during male courtship displays. Females can raise their own fitness if they respond to courtship behavior that signals benefits to the female rather than the fitness of the male. For example, choosing to mate with males that produce local signals would require less energy for a female as she searches for a mate.[16] Males may compete by imposing lower mating costs on the female or even providing material or offspring contributions to the female.

Indirect benefits are benefits that may not directly affect the parents' fitness but instead increase the fitness of the offspring. Since the offspring of a female will inherit half of the genetic information from the male counterpart, those traits she saw as attractive will be passed on, producing fit offspring. In this case, males may compete during courtship by displaying desirable traits to pass on to offspring.

Female display

Female courtship display is less common in nature as a female would have to invest a lot of energy into both exaggerated traits and in their energetically expensive gametes.[17] However, situations in which males are the sexually selective sex in a species do occur in nature. Male choice in reproduction can arise if males are the sex in a species that are in short supply, for example, if there is a female bias in the operational sex ratio. This could arise in mating systems where reproducing comes at an energy cost to males.[18][19] Such energy costs can include the effort associated in obtaining nuptial gifts for the female[20] or performing long courtship or copulatory behaviors.[21] An added cost from these time and energy investments may come in the form of increased male mortality rates, putting further strain on males attempting to reproduce.

In pipefish (Syngnathus typhle), females use a temporary ornament, a striped pattern, to both attract males and intimidate rival females. In this case, the female of a species developed a sexually selected signal which serves a dual function of being both attractive to mates and deterring rivals.[22]

Multi-modal signal processing

 
Bosc's fringe-toed lizard Acanthodactylus boskianus love bite

Many species of animals engage in some type of courtship display to attract a mate, such as dancing, the creation of sounds, and physical displays. However, many species are not limited to only one of these behaviors. The males of a species across many taxa create complex multi-component signals that have an effect on more than one sensory modality, also known as multi-modal signals.[23][24][25][26] There are two leading hypotheses about the adaptive significance of multi-modal signal processing. The multiple message hypothesis states that each signal that a male exhibits will contribute to a possible mate's perception of the male. The redundant signal hypothesis states that the male exhibits multiple signals that portray the same "message" to the female, with each extra signal acting as a fall-back plan for the male should there be a signaling error.[27] The choosy sex may only evaluate one, or a couple, of traits at a given time when interpreting complex signals from the opposite sex. Alternatively, the choosy sex may attempt to process all of the signals at once to facilitate evaluation of the opposite sex.[28][29]

The process of multi-modal signaling is believed to help facilitate the courtship process in many species. One such species in which multi-modal signaling is seen to improve mating success is the green tree frog (Hyla cinerea). Many anuran amphibians, such as the green tree frog, may use visual cues as well as auditory signals to increase their chances of impressing a mate.[24] When the calls of the tree frogs were held equal, it was determined that females tended to overlook an auditory-only stimulus in favor of males who combined auditory/visual multi-modal signals. It was seen that female green tree frogs preferred when males coupled the visual display with the auditory communication, concluding that male green tree frogs that are visually accessible can increase their probability of mating success.[24]

Peacock spiders (Maratus volans) are exceptionally sexually dimorphic in appearance and signaling behavior. During courtship, male peacock spiders compete using both visual displays and vibratory signals for intersexual communication.[25] Because of the intense sexual selection on male peacock spiders, the reproductive success of an individual relies heavily on a male spider's ability to combine visual and vibratory displays during courtship. The combination of these displays in courtship offers support both to the redundant signal and multiple messages hypotheses for the evolution of multi-modal signaling in species.[30]

Multi-modal signaling is not limited to males. Females in certain species have more than one trait or characteristic that they use in a courtship display to attract mates. In dance flies (Rhamphomyia longicauda), females have two ornaments — inflatable abdominal sacs and pinnate tibial scales — that they use as courtship displays in mating swarms. Intermediate variations of such female-specific ornaments are sexually selected for by male dance flies in wild populations. These ornaments may also be a signal of high fecundity in females.[31]

Mutual display

 
Male and female long-billed curlew, Numenius americanus, mutual courtship display

Often, males and females will perform synchronized or responsive courtship displays in a mutual fashion. With many socially monogamous species such as birds, their duet facilitates pre-copulatory reassurance of pair bonding and strengthens post-copulatory dedication to the development of offspring (e.g., great crested grebe, Podiceps cristatus).[32] For example, male and female crested auklets, Aethia cristatella, will cackle at one another as a vocal form of mutual display that serves to strengthen a bond between the two.[33] In some cases, males may pair up to perform mutual, cooperative displays in order to increase courtship success and attract females. This phenomenon can be seen with long-tailed manakins, Chiroxiphia linearis.[34]

Wild turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo) also engage in co-operative displays in which small groups of males (typically brothers) work together to attract females and deter other competitive males.[35] In many cases, only one male within the group will mate, typically the dominant male.[36] To explain this behaviour, Hamilton's theory of kin selection suggests that subordinate males receive indirect benefits by helping related males copulate successfully.[37]

Sexual ornaments

Sexual ornaments can serve to increase attractiveness[38] and indicate good genes and higher levels of fitness.[39] When exposed to exaggerated male traits, some females may respond by increasing maternal investments. For example, female canaries have been shown to produce larger and denser eggs in response to male supranormal song production.[40]

Sexual conflict

Sexual conflict is the phenomenon in which the interests of males and females in reproduction are not the same: they are often quite different:[41]

  • Males: their interest is to mate with a large number of completely faithful females, thus spreading their genes widely throughout a population.
  • Females: their interest is to mate with a large number of fit males, thus producing a large quantity of fit and varied offspring.

This has many consequences. Courtship displays allow the mate performing the selection to have a means on which to base the copulatory decision. If a female chooses more than one male, then sperm competition comes into play. This is competition between sperm to fertilize an egg, which is very competitive as only a single sperm will achieve union.[42] In some insects, the male injects a cocktail of chemicals in seminal fluid together with sperm. The chemicals kill off older sperm from any previous mates, up-regulates the female's egg-laying rate, and reduces her desire to re-mate with another male. The cocktail also shortens the female's lifespan, also reducing her likelihood of mating with other males.[43] Also, some females can get rid of the previous male's sperm.[44]

After mating has taken place, males perform various actions to prevent females from mating again. What action is performed depends on the animal. In some species, the male produces a mating plug after insemination. In some hymenoptera, the male provides a huge quantity of sperm, enough to last the female's entire life. In some birds and mammals, the male may participate in agonistic behaviors with other candidate males.[41][45][46]

Agonistic behavior and courtship

Although rare, agonistic behavior between males and females during courtship displays is seen in nature. Intraspecific agonistic behavior that results in the death of a combatant is rare because of the associated risk of death or injury. However, agonistic behavior that turns dangerous does occur.

In some species, physical traits that are sexually selected for in male courtship displays may also be used in agonistic behavior between two males for a mate. In fiddler crabs (genus Uca), males have been sexually selected to have one enlarged claw, which can take up anywhere from a third to a half of their total body mass, and one regular claw. Although the enlarged claw is believed to have developed for use in combat for territorial defense, it is not uncommon for males to employ this claw in battle for a mate.[47] Even though this claw developed as a weapon, it is also closely linked with the crabs' courtship display: it is waved in a certain pattern to attract females for mating.[48]

Agonistic behavior in courtship displays is not limited to male-male interactions. In many primate species, males direct agonistic behavior toward females prior to courtship behaviors. Such behavior can include aggressive vocalizations, displays, and physical aggression.[49] In the western gorilla (Gorilla gorilla), dominant males exhibit agonistic behavior toward female gorillas at very high rates, with the majority of those interactions being courtship-related. Most documented cases of male gorilla aggression toward females is courtship related and is used primarily as a strategy to prevent females from migrating to another male.[50]

In many cases, male courtship displays will cause forms of contest competition to develop. This is often seen within lek mating systems. For example, males will seek to obtain a certain spot or position to perform their courtship display. The best spots are regions of high contention as many males want them for themselves. Because of this direct conflict, agonistic[clarification needed] encounters between males are fairly common.[citation needed]

Extended courtship period

Mating is preceded by a courtship/pairing period in many animal mating systems. It is during this period that sexually mature animals select their partners for reproduction.[51] This courtship period, which involves displays to attract a mate by a member of a species, is usually short, lasting anywhere from 15 minutes to a few days. However, certain animals may undergo an extended courtship period, lasting as long as two months.[52]

One such exception is the emperor penguin (Aptenodytes forsteri). Emperor penguins engage in an extended courtship period that can last up to two months, the longest of any Arctic seabird. Their courtship period accounts for 16% of the total time they spend breeding, whereas in their closest relatives, the king penguin (Aptenodytes patagonicus), the courtship period takes up just three percent of their breeding cycle.[52][53]

Energetic costs

 
Greater sage-grouse lek males perform a courtship "strutting" display for visiting females.

Courtship displays typically involve some sort of metabolic cost to the animal performing it.[11] The energy expended to perform courtship behaviour can vary among species. Some animals engage in displays that expend little energy, as seen in the salamander (Desmognathus ochrophaeus).[54] Under laboratory settings, courtship behaviours in this species, although complex and involving the release of pheromones,[55] represent as little as approximately one percent of its daily calorie intake.[54]

In contrast, species that engage in prolonged or elaborate displays expend considerable amounts of energy and run the risk of developing fatigue. To prepare and prevent such a risk, some animals may gain weight before a courtship period, only to lose the weight afterward. An example of this can be seen in the greater sage-grouse (Centrocercus urophasianus). During the peak of their breeding season, which lasts up to three months during spring,[56] leks are frequently visited by groups of up to seventy females.[57] In response to such a large presence of females, males engage in a strutting display up to six to ten times per minute[58][59] for approximately three to four hours per day.[57] This frequent and repetitive behaviour can result in energy expenditures of up to 2524 kJ/day compared to the inactive males that typically expend 1218 kJ/day.[11]

Environmental factors

Various environmental factors, such as temperature, photoperiod, resource and light availability, have an effect on the timing and effectiveness of courtship displays in certain species of animals.[52]

In guppies (Poecilia reticulata), variation in the light environment plays a huge role in their ability to attract mates.[60] Guppy males alter both their 'courtship mode', whether they perform a full courtship display or try to 'engage' in sneak copulations, and distance from females as light intensity changes.[61] Courtship mode also varies with light spectrum and relates to predation risk.[62] On average, male guppies seek out and spend more time in the environment in which their color pattern is the most visible. Males, in the light environment that made them most visible, copulated with the most females.[60]

In emperor penguins (Aptenodytes forsteri), resource availability determines when male emperor penguins will be able to return to their breeding grounds to initiate their courtship rituals.[52] The greater the concentration of resources in their feeding ground, the quicker they will be able to restore their body reserves for winter, and the sooner they will be able to return to their breeding grounds. An early return to their breeding grounds comes with an increased likelihood of finding a mate.[63]

The effectiveness of Hirtodrosophila mycetophaga mating displays is influenced by the color of the bracket fungus that it mates and courts upon;[64] these flies choose brackets that are lighter, making their displays more visible to the opposite sex.[64]

Evolutionary significance

 
Male and female Pigeon Guillemot, Cepphus columba, mutual courtship display

There are multiple hypotheses about how courtship displays may have evolved in animals, including the Fisherian runaway model and the good genes hypothesis.

As explained by the Fisherian runaway model, sexually dimorphic males with exaggerated ornamentation may have been sexually selected for in species with female choice. Fitness of these males would increase, resulting in the proliferation of males with such ornamentation over time.[65] This means that a gene or set of genes will be favoured by female choice over time. This would explain why and how such elaborate traits develop within certain species. However, as time goes on and generations pass, the survival advantage associated with one trait may dissipate due to extreme exaggeration to the point that it decreases fitness.

The "good genes" hypothesis proposes that female selection of a mate is dependent on whether or not the male has genes that would increase the quality of the offspring of the female. In some cases, exaggerated male ornamentation may be indicative to a choosing female that a male who is able to place such a large investment in trait somewhat counterintuitive to survival would carry good genes.[66] For example, the costs associated with bright and complex plumage can be high. Only males with good genes are able to support a large investment into the development of such traits, which, in turn displays their high fitness.

An alternative is the sensory exploitation hypothesis, which supposes that sexual preferences are the result of preexisting sensory biases, such as that for supernormal stimuli. These could drive the evolution of courtship displays.[67]

See also

References

  1. ^ Girard, Madeline B.; Kasumovic, Michael M.; Elias, Damian O. (2011). "Multi-modal courtship in the peacock spider, Maratus volans (O.P.-Cambridge, 1874)". PLOS ONE. 6 (9): e25390. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...625390G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025390. PMC 3181266. PMID 21980440.
  2. ^ Riede, Tobias; Forstmeier, Wolfgang; Kempenaers, Bart; Goller, Franz (2015). "The functional morphology of male courtship displays in the Pectoral Sandpiper (Calidris melanotos)". The Auk. 132: 65–77. doi:10.1642/AUK-14-25.1. S2CID 86155377.
  3. ^ Koch, Rebecca E.; Krakauer, Alan H.; Patricelli, Gail L. (2015). "Investigating female mate choice for mechanical sounds in the male Greater Sage-Grouse" (PDF). The Auk. 132 (2): 349–358. doi:10.1642/auk-14-63.1. S2CID 30509108.
  4. ^ Beauchamp, A. J. (2014). (PDF). Notornis. 61 (1): 27–34. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2018-02-17. Retrieved 2018-02-13.
  5. ^ Martin, A. R.; Da Silva, V. M F; Rothery, P. (2008). "Object carrying as socio-sexual display in an aquatic mammal". Biology Letters. 4 (3): 243–245. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2008.0067. PMC 2610054. PMID 18364306.
  6. ^ Lim, Matthew L. M.; Li, Daiqin (2004). "Courtship and male-male agonistic behaviour of Cosmophasis umbratica Simon, an ornate jumping spider (Araneae: Salticidae) from Singapore" (PDF). Raffles Bulletin of Zoology. 52 (2): 435–448.
  7. ^ Wilts, Bodo D.; Michielsen, Kristel; De Raedt, Hans; Stavenga, Doekele G. (2014). "Sparkling feather reflections of a bird-of-paradise explained by finite-difference time-domain modeling". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 111 (12): 4363–4368. Bibcode:2014PNAS..111.4363W. doi:10.1073/pnas.1323611111. PMC 3970480. PMID 24591592.
  8. ^ Maynard Smith, J (1956). "Fertility, mating behaviour and sexual selection in Drosophila subobscura". Journal of Genetics. 54 (2): 261–279. doi:10.1007/bf02982781. S2CID 43641513.
  9. ^ a b c Clark, Christopher James (2011-04-01). "Wing, tail, and vocal contributions to the complex acoustic signals of courting Calliope hummingbirds". Current Zoology. 57 (2): 187–196. doi:10.1093/czoolo/57.2.187. ISSN 1674-5507.
  10. ^ Clark, Christopher James (2009-09-07). "Courtship dives of Anna's hummingbird offer insights into flight performance limits". Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 276 (1670): 3047–3052. doi:10.1098/rspb.2009.0508. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 2817121. PMID 19515669.
  11. ^ a b c Clark, Christopher J. (2012). "The role of power versus energy in courtship: what is the 'energetic cost' of a courtship display?". Animal Behaviour. 84 (1): 269–277. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.012. S2CID 45711419.
  12. ^ a b Borgia, Gerald (1985). "Bower quality, number of decorations and mating success of male satin bowerbirds (Ptilonorhynchus violaceus): an experimental analysis". Animal Behaviour. 33 (1): 266–271. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(85)80140-8. S2CID 53192968.
  13. ^ Madden, Joah R. (2003). "Male Spotted Bowerbirds Preferentially Choose, Arrange and Proffer Objects That Are Good Predictors of Mating Success". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 53 (5): 263–268. doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0582-7. JSTOR 4602214. S2CID 44048073.
  14. ^ Eberhard, W.G. (1991). "Copulatory courtship and cryptic female choice in insects". Biology Reviews. 66: 1–31. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.1991.tb01133.x. S2CID 85822041.
  15. ^ Hoikkala, A. (2000). "Copulatory courtship in drosophila birchii and D. serrata, species recognition and sexual selection" (PDF). Journal of Insect Behavior. 13 (3): 361–373. doi:10.1023/a:1007710218609. S2CID 25532996.[dead link]
  16. ^ Wagner, William E. Jr. (2011). "Direct Benefits and the Evolution of Female Mating Preferences: Conceptual Problems, Potential Solutions, and a Field Cricket". Advances in the Study of Behavior. 43: 273. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-380896-7.00006-X. S2CID 80868197.
  17. ^ Lehtonen, Jussi; Parker, Geoff A.; Schärer, Lukas (May 2016). "Why anisogamy drives ancestral sex roles". Evolution. 70 (5): 1129–1135. doi:10.1111/evo.12926. PMID 27110661. S2CID 2107146.
  18. ^ Kokko, Hanna; Monaghan, Pat (March 2001). "Predicting the direction of sexual selection". Ecology Letters. 4 (2): 159–165. doi:10.1046/j.1461-0248.2001.00212.x.
  19. ^ Parker, G.A. (1983). "Mate quality and mating decisions". In Bateson, P. (ed.). Mate Choice. Cambridge University Press. pp. 141–164. ISBN 978-0-521-27207-0.
  20. ^ Simmons, L.W. (July 1990). "Nuptial feeding in tettigoniids male costs and the rates of fecundity increase". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 27 (1): 43–47. doi:10.1007/BF00183312. JSTOR 4600442. S2CID 24088701.
  21. ^ Saeki, Yoriko; Kruse, Kipp C.; Switzer, Paul V. (September 2005). "Physiological costs of mate guarding in the Japanese beetle (Popillia japonica Newman)". Ethology. 111 (9): 863–877. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2005.01106.x.
  22. ^ Berglund, A.; Rosenqvist, G. (2008). "An intimidating ornament in a female pipefish". Behavioral Ecology. 20 (1): 54–59. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn114.
  23. ^ Bee, Mark A. (April 2012). "Sound source perception in anuran amphibians". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 22 (2): 301–310. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.12.014. PMC 3338885. PMID 22265243.
  24. ^ a b c Laird, Krispen L.; Clements, Paul; Hunter, Kimberly L.; Taylor, Ryan C. (2016). "Multimodal signaling improves mating success in the green tree frog (Hyla cinerea), but may not help small males". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 70 (9): 1517–1525. doi:10.1007/s00265-016-2160-9. S2CID 15612885.
  25. ^ a b Girard, Madeline B.; Kasumovic, Michael M.; Elias, Damian O. (2011). "Multi-modal courtship in the peacock spider, Maratus volans (O.P.-Cambridge, 1874)". PLOS ONE. 6 (9): e25390. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...625390G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0025390. PMC 3181266. PMID 21980440.
  26. ^ Mikula, P.; Toszogyova, A.; Albrecht, T (2022). "A global analysis of aerial displays in passerines revealed an effect of habitat, mating system and migratory traits". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 289 (1973): 20220370. doi:10.1098/rspb.2022.0370. PMC 9019522. PMID 35440206.
  27. ^ Hebets, Eileen A.; Papaj, Daniel R. (2004). "Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 57 (3): 197–214. doi:10.1007/s00265-004-0865-7. S2CID 16494821.
  28. ^ Rowe, Candy (1999). "Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals". Animal Behaviour. 58 (5): 921–931. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1242. PMID 10564594. S2CID 1372407.
  29. ^ Candolin, U. (November 2003). "The use of multiple cues in mate choice" (PDF). Biological Reviews. 78 (4): 575–595. doi:10.1017/S1464793103006158. hdl:1975/138. PMID 14700392. S2CID 8577794.
  30. ^ Girard, Madeline B.; Elias, Damian O.; Kasumovic, Michael M. (2015). "Female preference for multi-modal courtship: multiple signals are important for male mating success in peacock spiders". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 282 (1820): 20152222. doi:10.1098/rspb.2015.2222. PMC 4685782. PMID 26631566.
  31. ^ Wheeler, J.; Gwynne, D. T.; Bussière, L. F. (July 2012). "Stabilizing sexual selection for female ornaments in a dance fly". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 25 (7): 1233–1242. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02522.x. PMID 22551204.
  32. ^ Servedio, M. R.; Price, T. D.; Lande, R. (2013). "Evolution of displays within the pair bond". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 280 (1757): 20123020. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.3020. PMC 3619481. PMID 23427172.
  33. ^ Zubakin, V. A.; Volodin, I. A.; Klenova, A. V.; Zubakina, E. V.; Volodina, E. V.; Lapshina, E. N. (2010). "Behavior of crested auklets (Aethia cristatella, Charadriiformes, Alcidae) in the breeding season: Visual and acoustic displays". Biology Bulletin. 37 (8): 823–835. doi:10.1134/S1062359010080066. S2CID 9709011.
  34. ^ Lukianchuk, K. C.; Doucet, S. M. (2014). "Cooperative courtship display in Long-tailed Manakins Chiroxiphia linearis: predictors of courtship success revealed through full characterization of display". Journal of Ornithology. 155 (3): 729–743. doi:10.1007/s10336-014-1059-3. S2CID 15592813.
  35. ^ Krakauer, Alan H. (2005-03-03). "Kin selection and cooperative courtship in wild turkeys". Nature. 434 (7029): 69–72. Bibcode:2005Natur.434...69K. doi:10.1038/nature03325. ISSN 1476-4687. PMID 15744300. S2CID 1457512.
  36. ^ Watts, C. Robert; Stokes, Allen W. (1971). "The Social Order of Turkeys". Scientific American. 224 (6): 112–119. JSTOR 24922757.
  37. ^ Hamilton, W.D. (July 1964). "The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 7 (1): 1–16. Bibcode:1964JThBi...7....1H. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4. PMID 5875341.
  38. ^ Wheeler, J.; Gwynne, D. T.; Bussière, L. F. (2012-07-01). "Stabilizing sexual selection for female ornaments in a dance fly". Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 25 (7): 1233–1242. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02522.x. ISSN 1420-9101. PMID 22551204.
  39. ^ Tibbetts, Elizabeth A.; Forrest, Taylor; Vernier, Cassondra; Jinn, Judy; Madagame, Andrew (2015-11-01). "Socially selected ornaments and fitness: Signals of fighting ability in paper wasps are positively associated with survival, reproductive success, and rank". Evolution. 69 (11): 2917–2926. doi:10.1111/evo.12793. hdl:2027.42/134447. ISSN 1558-5646. PMID 26456701. S2CID 10079068.
  40. ^ Garcia-Fernandez, V.; Draganoiu, T.I.; Ung, D.; Lacroix, A.; Malacarne, G.; Leboucher, G. (2013). "Female canaries invest more in response to an exaggerated male trait". Animal Behaviour. 85 (3): 679–684. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.007. S2CID 53154422.
  41. ^ a b Arnqvist, G. & Rowe, L. 2005. Sexual conflict. Princeton, New Jersey. Princeton University Press.
  42. ^ Parker, Geoffrey A. 1970. Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in insects. Biological Reviews vol. 55 pp. 525–567.
  43. ^ Schilthuizen, Menno. 2001. Frogs, flies and dandelions: the making of species. Oxford University Press p92 ISBN 0-19-850392-X
  44. ^ Eberhard, W.G. 1996. Female control: sexual selection by cryptic female choice. Princeton, New Jersey. Princeton University Press.
  45. ^ Schilthuizen, Menno. 2001. Frogs, flies and dandelions: the making of species. Oxford University Press p. 92 ISBN 0-19-850392-X
  46. ^ Crudgington, H. & Siva-Jothy, M.T. 200. Genital damage, kicking and early death. Nature vol 407 pp. 855–856.
  47. ^ Perez, D. M. (2015). "Handedness in fiddler crab fights" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 110: 99–104. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.09.012. S2CID 53194236.
  48. ^ Kahn, A. T. (2014). "Female preferences for timing in a fiddler crab with synchronous courtship waving displays". Animal Behaviour. 98: 35–39. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.09.028. S2CID 44139315.
  49. ^ Muller, M. N. (2009). Sexual Coercion in Primates and Humans. Harvard University Press. pp. 3–22. ISBN 9780674033245.
  50. ^ Breuer, T. (2016). "Sexual coercion and courtship by male western gorillas" (PDF). Primates. 57 (1): 29–38. doi:10.1007/s10329-015-0496-9. PMID 26483073. S2CID 17684689.
  51. ^ West, K. (2009). "Animal behaviour: animal courtship". Chelsea House Publications.
  52. ^ a b c d Ancel, A. (2013). "The long engagement of the emperor penguin". Polar Biology. 36 (4): 573–577. doi:10.1007/s00300-013-1285-9. S2CID 15504155.
  53. ^ Isenmann, P. (1971). "Contribution a' l'e´thologie et a' l'e´cologie du manchot empereur (Aptenodytes forsteri Gray) a' la colonie de Pointe Ge´ologie (Terre Ade´lie)". L'Oiseau et la RFO. 40: 136–159.
  54. ^ a b Bennett, Albert F.; Houck, Lynne D. (1983-10-01). "The Energetic Cost of Courtship and Aggression in a Plethodontid Salamander". Ecology. 64 (5): 979–983. doi:10.2307/1937804. ISSN 1939-9170. JSTOR 1937804.
  55. ^ Houck, Lynne D.; Arnold, Stevan J.; Thisted, Ronald A. (1985). "A Statistical Study of Mate Choice: Sexual Selection in a Plethodontid Salamander (Desmognathus ochrophaeus)". Evolution. 39 (2): 370–386. doi:10.2307/2408370. JSTOR 2408370. PMID 28564228.
  56. ^ Vehrencamp, Sandra L.; Bradbury, Jack W.; Gibson, Robert M. (1989). "The energetic cost of display in male sage grouse". Animal Behaviour. 38 (5): 885–896. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(89)80120-4. S2CID 53195569.
  57. ^ a b Scott, John W. (1942). "Mating Behavior of the Sage Grouse". The Auk. 59 (4): 477–498. doi:10.2307/4079460. JSTOR 4079460.
  58. ^ Gibson, Robert M.; Bradbury, Jack W.; Vehrencamp, Sandra L. (1991-07-01). "Mate choice in lekking sage grouse revisited: the roles of vocal display, female site fidelity, and copying". Behavioral Ecology. 2 (2): 165–180. doi:10.1093/beheco/2.2.165. ISSN 1045-2249.
  59. ^ Young, Jessica R.; Hupp, Jerry W.; Bradbury, Jack W.; Braun, Clait E. (1994). "Phenotypic divergence of secondary sexual traits among sage grouse, Centrocercus urophasianus, populations". Animal Behaviour. 47 (6): 1353–1362. doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1183. S2CID 54287148.
  60. ^ a b Cole, G. L. (2016). "Male courtship decisions are influenced by light environment and female receptivity". Biological Sciences. 283 (1839): 1839. doi:10.1098/rspb.2016.0861. PMC 5046890. PMID 27683362.
  61. ^ Endler, J. A. (2016). "Predation, light intensity and courtship behaviour in Poecilia reticulata (Pisces: Poeciliidae)". Animal Behaviour. 35 (5): 1376–1385. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(87)80010-6. S2CID 53169408.
  62. ^ Gamble, S. (2003). "Environmental variation and the maintenance of polymorphism: the effect of ambient light spectrum on mating behaviour and sexual selection in guppies". Ecology Letters. 6 (5): 463–472. doi:10.1046/j.1461-0248.2003.00449.x.
  63. ^ Bried, J. (1999). "Why do aptenodytes penguins have high divorce rates?". The Auk. 116 (2): 504–512. doi:10.2307/4089382. JSTOR 4089382.
  64. ^ a b "The Evolutionary Biology of Colonizing Species". epdf.pub. Retrieved 2019-11-18.
  65. ^ Fisher, R. A. (1915). "The evolution of sexual preference". Eugenics Review. 7 (3): 184–192. PMC 2987134. PMID 21259607.
  66. ^ Yasui, Y. (1997). "A "Good-Sperm" model can explain the evolution of costly multiple mating by females". The American Naturalist. 149 (3): 573. doi:10.1086/286006. S2CID 84991216.
  67. ^ Ryan, Michael (2018). A taste for the beautiful: The evolution of attraction. Princeton University Press. pp. 3, 32, 41, 43–44, 155. ISBN 9780691167268.

External links

courtship, display, courtship, display, display, behaviors, which, animal, usually, male, attempts, attract, mate, mate, exercises, choice, sexual, selection, acts, display, these, behaviors, often, include, ritualized, movement, dances, vocalizations, mechani. A courtship display is a set of display behaviors in which an animal usually a male attempts to attract a mate the mate exercises choice so sexual selection acts on the display These behaviors often include ritualized movement dances vocalizations mechanical sound production or displays of beauty strength or agonistic ability 1 2 3 4 5 6 Male blackbuck Antilope cervicapra courting a female Contents 1 Male display 2 Female display 3 Multi modal signal processing 4 Mutual display 5 Sexual ornaments 6 Sexual conflict 7 Agonistic behavior and courtship 8 Extended courtship period 9 Energetic costs 10 Environmental factors 11 Evolutionary significance 12 See also 13 References 14 External linksMale display Edit Male peacock spider Maratus volans courtship display In some species males will perform ritualized movements to attract females The male six plumed bird of paradise Parotia lawesii exemplifies male courtship display with its ritualized ballerina dance and unique occipital and breast feathers that serve to stimulate the female visual system 7 In Drosophila subobscura male courtship display is seen through the male s intricate wing scissoring patterns and rapid sidestepping 8 These stimulations along with many other factors result in subsequent copulation or rejection In other species males may exhibit courtship displays that serve as both visual and auditory stimulation For example the male Anna s hummingbird Calypte anna and calliope hummingbird Stellula calliope perform two types of courtship displays involving a combination of visual and vocal display a stationary shuttle display 9 and dive display 10 11 When engaging in the stationary shuttle display the male displays a flared gorget and hovers in front of the female moving from side to side while rotating his body and tail The rhythmic movements of the male s wings produce a distinctive buzzing sound 9 When conducting a dive display the male typically ascends approximately 20 35 m 66 115 ft in the air then abruptly turns and descends in a dive like fashion As the male flies over the female he rotates his body and spreads his tail feathers which flutter and collide to produce a short buzzing sound 9 A male satin bowerbird makes and uses a bower to attract potential mates In addition some animals attempt to attract females through the construction and decoration of unique structures This technique can be seen in the satin bowerbird Ptilonorhynchus violaceus of Australia males of which build and decorate nest like structures called bowers 12 Bowers are decorated with bright and colourful objects typically blue in colour to attract and stimulate visiting females 12 Typically males who acquire the largest number of decorations tend to have greater success in mating 13 In some species males initiate courtship rituals only after mounting the female Courtship may even continue after copulation has been completed 14 In this system the ability of the female to choose her mate is limited This process known as copulatory courtship is prevalent in many insect species 15 In most species the male sex initiates courtship displays in precopulatory sexual selection Performing a display allows the male to present his traits or abilities to a female Mate choice in this context is driven by females direct or indirect benefits to the female often determine which males reproduce and which do not Direct benefits may accrue to the female during male courtship displays Females can raise their own fitness if they respond to courtship behavior that signals benefits to the female rather than the fitness of the male For example choosing to mate with males that produce local signals would require less energy for a female as she searches for a mate 16 Males may compete by imposing lower mating costs on the female or even providing material or offspring contributions to the female Indirect benefits are benefits that may not directly affect the parents fitness but instead increase the fitness of the offspring Since the offspring of a female will inherit half of the genetic information from the male counterpart those traits she saw as attractive will be passed on producing fit offspring In this case males may compete during courtship by displaying desirable traits to pass on to offspring Female display EditFemale courtship display is less common in nature as a female would have to invest a lot of energy into both exaggerated traits and in their energetically expensive gametes 17 However situations in which males are the sexually selective sex in a species do occur in nature Male choice in reproduction can arise if males are the sex in a species that are in short supply for example if there is a female bias in the operational sex ratio This could arise in mating systems where reproducing comes at an energy cost to males 18 19 Such energy costs can include the effort associated in obtaining nuptial gifts for the female 20 or performing long courtship or copulatory behaviors 21 An added cost from these time and energy investments may come in the form of increased male mortality rates putting further strain on males attempting to reproduce In pipefish Syngnathus typhle females use a temporary ornament a striped pattern to both attract males and intimidate rival females In this case the female of a species developed a sexually selected signal which serves a dual function of being both attractive to mates and deterring rivals 22 Multi modal signal processing Edit Bosc s fringe toed lizard Acanthodactylus boskianus love bite Many species of animals engage in some type of courtship display to attract a mate such as dancing the creation of sounds and physical displays However many species are not limited to only one of these behaviors The males of a species across many taxa create complex multi component signals that have an effect on more than one sensory modality also known as multi modal signals 23 24 25 26 There are two leading hypotheses about the adaptive significance of multi modal signal processing The multiple message hypothesis states that each signal that a male exhibits will contribute to a possible mate s perception of the male The redundant signal hypothesis states that the male exhibits multiple signals that portray the same message to the female with each extra signal acting as a fall back plan for the male should there be a signaling error 27 The choosy sex may only evaluate one or a couple of traits at a given time when interpreting complex signals from the opposite sex Alternatively the choosy sex may attempt to process all of the signals at once to facilitate evaluation of the opposite sex 28 29 The process of multi modal signaling is believed to help facilitate the courtship process in many species One such species in which multi modal signaling is seen to improve mating success is the green tree frog Hyla cinerea Many anuran amphibians such as the green tree frog may use visual cues as well as auditory signals to increase their chances of impressing a mate 24 When the calls of the tree frogs were held equal it was determined that females tended to overlook an auditory only stimulus in favor of males who combined auditory visual multi modal signals It was seen that female green tree frogs preferred when males coupled the visual display with the auditory communication concluding that male green tree frogs that are visually accessible can increase their probability of mating success 24 Peacock spiders Maratus volans are exceptionally sexually dimorphic in appearance and signaling behavior During courtship male peacock spiders compete using both visual displays and vibratory signals for intersexual communication 25 Because of the intense sexual selection on male peacock spiders the reproductive success of an individual relies heavily on a male spider s ability to combine visual and vibratory displays during courtship The combination of these displays in courtship offers support both to the redundant signal and multiple messages hypotheses for the evolution of multi modal signaling in species 30 Multi modal signaling is not limited to males Females in certain species have more than one trait or characteristic that they use in a courtship display to attract mates In dance flies Rhamphomyia longicauda females have two ornaments inflatable abdominal sacs and pinnate tibial scales that they use as courtship displays in mating swarms Intermediate variations of such female specific ornaments are sexually selected for by male dance flies in wild populations These ornaments may also be a signal of high fecundity in females 31 Mutual display Edit Male and female long billed curlew Numenius americanus mutual courtship display Often males and females will perform synchronized or responsive courtship displays in a mutual fashion With many socially monogamous species such as birds their duet facilitates pre copulatory reassurance of pair bonding and strengthens post copulatory dedication to the development of offspring e g great crested grebe Podiceps cristatus 32 For example male and female crested auklets Aethia cristatella will cackle at one another as a vocal form of mutual display that serves to strengthen a bond between the two 33 In some cases males may pair up to perform mutual cooperative displays in order to increase courtship success and attract females This phenomenon can be seen with long tailed manakins Chiroxiphia linearis 34 Wild turkeys Meleagris gallopavo also engage in co operative displays in which small groups of males typically brothers work together to attract females and deter other competitive males 35 In many cases only one male within the group will mate typically the dominant male 36 To explain this behaviour Hamilton s theory of kin selection suggests that subordinate males receive indirect benefits by helping related males copulate successfully 37 Sexual ornaments EditSexual ornaments can serve to increase attractiveness 38 and indicate good genes and higher levels of fitness 39 When exposed to exaggerated male traits some females may respond by increasing maternal investments For example female canaries have been shown to produce larger and denser eggs in response to male supranormal song production 40 Sexual conflict EditSexual conflict is the phenomenon in which the interests of males and females in reproduction are not the same they are often quite different 41 Males their interest is to mate with a large number of completely faithful females thus spreading their genes widely throughout a population Females their interest is to mate with a large number of fit males thus producing a large quantity of fit and varied offspring This has many consequences Courtship displays allow the mate performing the selection to have a means on which to base the copulatory decision If a female chooses more than one male then sperm competition comes into play This is competition between sperm to fertilize an egg which is very competitive as only a single sperm will achieve union 42 In some insects the male injects a cocktail of chemicals in seminal fluid together with sperm The chemicals kill off older sperm from any previous mates up regulates the female s egg laying rate and reduces her desire to re mate with another male The cocktail also shortens the female s lifespan also reducing her likelihood of mating with other males 43 Also some females can get rid of the previous male s sperm 44 After mating has taken place males perform various actions to prevent females from mating again What action is performed depends on the animal In some species the male produces a mating plug after insemination In some hymenoptera the male provides a huge quantity of sperm enough to last the female s entire life In some birds and mammals the male may participate in agonistic behaviors with other candidate males 41 45 46 Agonistic behavior and courtship EditAlthough rare agonistic behavior between males and females during courtship displays is seen in nature Intraspecific agonistic behavior that results in the death of a combatant is rare because of the associated risk of death or injury However agonistic behavior that turns dangerous does occur In some species physical traits that are sexually selected for in male courtship displays may also be used in agonistic behavior between two males for a mate In fiddler crabs genus Uca males have been sexually selected to have one enlarged claw which can take up anywhere from a third to a half of their total body mass and one regular claw Although the enlarged claw is believed to have developed for use in combat for territorial defense it is not uncommon for males to employ this claw in battle for a mate 47 Even though this claw developed as a weapon it is also closely linked with the crabs courtship display it is waved in a certain pattern to attract females for mating 48 Agonistic behavior in courtship displays is not limited to male male interactions In many primate species males direct agonistic behavior toward females prior to courtship behaviors Such behavior can include aggressive vocalizations displays and physical aggression 49 In the western gorilla Gorilla gorilla dominant males exhibit agonistic behavior toward female gorillas at very high rates with the majority of those interactions being courtship related Most documented cases of male gorilla aggression toward females is courtship related and is used primarily as a strategy to prevent females from migrating to another male 50 In many cases male courtship displays will cause forms of contest competition to develop This is often seen within lek mating systems For example males will seek to obtain a certain spot or position to perform their courtship display The best spots are regions of high contention as many males want them for themselves Because of this direct conflict agonistic clarification needed encounters between males are fairly common citation needed Extended courtship period EditMating is preceded by a courtship pairing period in many animal mating systems It is during this period that sexually mature animals select their partners for reproduction 51 This courtship period which involves displays to attract a mate by a member of a species is usually short lasting anywhere from 15 minutes to a few days However certain animals may undergo an extended courtship period lasting as long as two months 52 One such exception is the emperor penguin Aptenodytes forsteri Emperor penguins engage in an extended courtship period that can last up to two months the longest of any Arctic seabird Their courtship period accounts for 16 of the total time they spend breeding whereas in their closest relatives the king penguin Aptenodytes patagonicus the courtship period takes up just three percent of their breeding cycle 52 53 Energetic costs Edit Greater sage grouse lek males perform a courtship strutting display for visiting females Courtship displays typically involve some sort of metabolic cost to the animal performing it 11 The energy expended to perform courtship behaviour can vary among species Some animals engage in displays that expend little energy as seen in the salamander Desmognathus ochrophaeus 54 Under laboratory settings courtship behaviours in this species although complex and involving the release of pheromones 55 represent as little as approximately one percent of its daily calorie intake 54 In contrast species that engage in prolonged or elaborate displays expend considerable amounts of energy and run the risk of developing fatigue To prepare and prevent such a risk some animals may gain weight before a courtship period only to lose the weight afterward An example of this can be seen in the greater sage grouse Centrocercus urophasianus During the peak of their breeding season which lasts up to three months during spring 56 leks are frequently visited by groups of up to seventy females 57 In response to such a large presence of females males engage in a strutting display up to six to ten times per minute 58 59 for approximately three to four hours per day 57 This frequent and repetitive behaviour can result in energy expenditures of up to 2524 kJ day compared to the inactive males that typically expend 1218 kJ day 11 Environmental factors EditVarious environmental factors such as temperature photoperiod resource and light availability have an effect on the timing and effectiveness of courtship displays in certain species of animals 52 In guppies Poecilia reticulata variation in the light environment plays a huge role in their ability to attract mates 60 Guppy males alter both their courtship mode whether they perform a full courtship display or try to engage in sneak copulations and distance from females as light intensity changes 61 Courtship mode also varies with light spectrum and relates to predation risk 62 On average male guppies seek out and spend more time in the environment in which their color pattern is the most visible Males in the light environment that made them most visible copulated with the most females 60 In emperor penguins Aptenodytes forsteri resource availability determines when male emperor penguins will be able to return to their breeding grounds to initiate their courtship rituals 52 The greater the concentration of resources in their feeding ground the quicker they will be able to restore their body reserves for winter and the sooner they will be able to return to their breeding grounds An early return to their breeding grounds comes with an increased likelihood of finding a mate 63 The effectiveness of Hirtodrosophila mycetophaga mating displays is influenced by the color of the bracket fungus that it mates and courts upon 64 these flies choose brackets that are lighter making their displays more visible to the opposite sex 64 Evolutionary significance Edit Male and female Pigeon Guillemot Cepphus columba mutual courtship display There are multiple hypotheses about how courtship displays may have evolved in animals including the Fisherian runaway model and the good genes hypothesis As explained by the Fisherian runaway model sexually dimorphic males with exaggerated ornamentation may have been sexually selected for in species with female choice Fitness of these males would increase resulting in the proliferation of males with such ornamentation over time 65 This means that a gene or set of genes will be favoured by female choice over time This would explain why and how such elaborate traits develop within certain species However as time goes on and generations pass the survival advantage associated with one trait may dissipate due to extreme exaggeration to the point that it decreases fitness The good genes hypothesis proposes that female selection of a mate is dependent on whether or not the male has genes that would increase the quality of the offspring of the female In some cases exaggerated male ornamentation may be indicative to a choosing female that a male who is able to place such a large investment in trait somewhat counterintuitive to survival would carry good genes 66 For example the costs associated with bright and complex plumage can be high Only males with good genes are able to support a large investment into the development of such traits which in turn displays their high fitness An alternative is the sensory exploitation hypothesis which supposes that sexual preferences are the result of preexisting sensory biases such as that for supernormal stimuli These could drive the evolution of courtship displays 67 See also EditDisplay zoology Lek biology Courtship Courtship disorder Mate choice Sexual selectionReferences Edit Girard Madeline B Kasumovic Michael M Elias Damian O 2011 Multi modal courtship in the peacock spider Maratus volans O P Cambridge 1874 PLOS ONE 6 9 e25390 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 625390G doi 10 1371 journal pone 0025390 PMC 3181266 PMID 21980440 Riede Tobias Forstmeier Wolfgang Kempenaers Bart Goller Franz 2015 The functional morphology of male courtship displays in the Pectoral Sandpiper Calidris melanotos The Auk 132 65 77 doi 10 1642 AUK 14 25 1 S2CID 86155377 Koch Rebecca E Krakauer Alan H Patricelli Gail L 2015 Investigating female mate choice for mechanical sounds in the male Greater Sage Grouse PDF The Auk 132 2 349 358 doi 10 1642 auk 14 63 1 S2CID 30509108 Beauchamp A J 2014 Calling and display by peacocks pavo cristatus at mansion house historic reserve kawau island New Zealand PDF Notornis 61 1 27 34 Archived from the original PDF on 2018 02 17 Retrieved 2018 02 13 Martin A R Da Silva V M F Rothery P 2008 Object carrying as socio sexual display in an aquatic mammal Biology Letters 4 3 243 245 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2008 0067 PMC 2610054 PMID 18364306 Lim Matthew L M Li Daiqin 2004 Courtship and male male agonistic behaviour of Cosmophasis umbratica Simon an ornate jumping spider Araneae Salticidae from Singapore PDF Raffles Bulletin of Zoology 52 2 435 448 Wilts Bodo D Michielsen Kristel De Raedt Hans Stavenga Doekele G 2014 Sparkling feather reflections of a bird of paradise explained by finite difference time domain modeling Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111 12 4363 4368 Bibcode 2014PNAS 111 4363W doi 10 1073 pnas 1323611111 PMC 3970480 PMID 24591592 Maynard Smith J 1956 Fertility mating behaviour and sexual selection in Drosophila subobscura Journal of Genetics 54 2 261 279 doi 10 1007 bf02982781 S2CID 43641513 a b c Clark Christopher James 2011 04 01 Wing tail and vocal contributions to the complex acoustic signals of courting Calliope hummingbirds Current Zoology 57 2 187 196 doi 10 1093 czoolo 57 2 187 ISSN 1674 5507 Clark Christopher James 2009 09 07 Courtship dives of Anna s hummingbird offer insights into flight performance limits Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B Biological Sciences 276 1670 3047 3052 doi 10 1098 rspb 2009 0508 ISSN 0962 8452 PMC 2817121 PMID 19515669 a b c Clark Christopher J 2012 The role of power versus energy in courtship what is the energetic cost of a courtship display Animal Behaviour 84 1 269 277 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2012 04 012 S2CID 45711419 a b Borgia Gerald 1985 Bower quality number of decorations and mating success of male satin bowerbirds Ptilonorhynchus violaceus an experimental analysis Animal Behaviour 33 1 266 271 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 85 80140 8 S2CID 53192968 Madden Joah R 2003 Male Spotted Bowerbirds Preferentially Choose Arrange and Proffer Objects That Are Good Predictors of Mating Success Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 53 5 263 268 doi 10 1007 s00265 003 0582 7 JSTOR 4602214 S2CID 44048073 Eberhard W G 1991 Copulatory courtship and cryptic female choice in insects Biology Reviews 66 1 31 doi 10 1111 j 1469 185X 1991 tb01133 x S2CID 85822041 Hoikkala A 2000 Copulatory courtship in drosophila birchii and D serrata species recognition and sexual selection PDF Journal of Insect Behavior 13 3 361 373 doi 10 1023 a 1007710218609 S2CID 25532996 dead link Wagner William E Jr 2011 Direct Benefits and the Evolution of Female Mating Preferences Conceptual Problems Potential Solutions and a Field Cricket Advances in the Study of Behavior 43 273 doi 10 1016 B978 0 12 380896 7 00006 X S2CID 80868197 Lehtonen Jussi Parker Geoff A Scharer Lukas May 2016 Why anisogamy drives ancestral sex roles Evolution 70 5 1129 1135 doi 10 1111 evo 12926 PMID 27110661 S2CID 2107146 Kokko Hanna Monaghan Pat March 2001 Predicting the direction of sexual selection Ecology Letters 4 2 159 165 doi 10 1046 j 1461 0248 2001 00212 x Parker G A 1983 Mate quality and mating decisions In Bateson P ed Mate Choice Cambridge University Press pp 141 164 ISBN 978 0 521 27207 0 Simmons L W July 1990 Nuptial feeding in tettigoniids male costs and the rates of fecundity increase Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 27 1 43 47 doi 10 1007 BF00183312 JSTOR 4600442 S2CID 24088701 Saeki Yoriko Kruse Kipp C Switzer Paul V September 2005 Physiological costs of mate guarding in the Japanese beetle Popillia japonica Newman Ethology 111 9 863 877 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 2005 01106 x Berglund A Rosenqvist G 2008 An intimidating ornament in a female pipefish Behavioral Ecology 20 1 54 59 doi 10 1093 beheco arn114 Bee Mark A April 2012 Sound source perception in anuran amphibians Current Opinion in Neurobiology 22 2 301 310 doi 10 1016 j conb 2011 12 014 PMC 3338885 PMID 22265243 a b c Laird Krispen L Clements Paul Hunter Kimberly L Taylor Ryan C 2016 Multimodal signaling improves mating success in the green tree frog Hyla cinerea but may not help small males Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 70 9 1517 1525 doi 10 1007 s00265 016 2160 9 S2CID 15612885 a b Girard Madeline B Kasumovic Michael M Elias Damian O 2011 Multi modal courtship in the peacock spider Maratus volans O P Cambridge 1874 PLOS ONE 6 9 e25390 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 625390G doi 10 1371 journal pone 0025390 PMC 3181266 PMID 21980440 Mikula P Toszogyova A Albrecht T 2022 A global analysis of aerial displays in passerines revealed an effect of habitat mating system and migratory traits Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 289 1973 20220370 doi 10 1098 rspb 2022 0370 PMC 9019522 PMID 35440206 Hebets Eileen A Papaj Daniel R 2004 Complex signal function developing a framework of testable hypotheses Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 57 3 197 214 doi 10 1007 s00265 004 0865 7 S2CID 16494821 Rowe Candy 1999 Receiver psychology and the evolution of multicomponent signals Animal Behaviour 58 5 921 931 doi 10 1006 anbe 1999 1242 PMID 10564594 S2CID 1372407 Candolin U November 2003 The use of multiple cues in mate choice PDF Biological Reviews 78 4 575 595 doi 10 1017 S1464793103006158 hdl 1975 138 PMID 14700392 S2CID 8577794 Girard Madeline B Elias Damian O Kasumovic Michael M 2015 Female preference for multi modal courtship multiple signals are important for male mating success in peacock spiders Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 282 1820 20152222 doi 10 1098 rspb 2015 2222 PMC 4685782 PMID 26631566 Wheeler J Gwynne D T Bussiere L F July 2012 Stabilizing sexual selection for female ornaments in a dance fly Journal of Evolutionary Biology 25 7 1233 1242 doi 10 1111 j 1420 9101 2012 02522 x PMID 22551204 Servedio M R Price T D Lande R 2013 Evolution of displays within the pair bond Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 280 1757 20123020 doi 10 1098 rspb 2012 3020 PMC 3619481 PMID 23427172 Zubakin V A Volodin I A Klenova A V Zubakina E V Volodina E V Lapshina E N 2010 Behavior of crested auklets Aethia cristatella Charadriiformes Alcidae in the breeding season Visual and acoustic displays Biology Bulletin 37 8 823 835 doi 10 1134 S1062359010080066 S2CID 9709011 Lukianchuk K C Doucet S M 2014 Cooperative courtship display in Long tailed Manakins Chiroxiphia linearis predictors of courtship success revealed through full characterization of display Journal of Ornithology 155 3 729 743 doi 10 1007 s10336 014 1059 3 S2CID 15592813 Krakauer Alan H 2005 03 03 Kin selection and cooperative courtship in wild turkeys Nature 434 7029 69 72 Bibcode 2005Natur 434 69K doi 10 1038 nature03325 ISSN 1476 4687 PMID 15744300 S2CID 1457512 Watts C Robert Stokes Allen W 1971 The Social Order of Turkeys Scientific American 224 6 112 119 JSTOR 24922757 Hamilton W D July 1964 The genetical evolution of social behaviour I Journal of Theoretical Biology 7 1 1 16 Bibcode 1964JThBi 7 1H doi 10 1016 0022 5193 64 90038 4 PMID 5875341 Wheeler J Gwynne D T Bussiere L F 2012 07 01 Stabilizing sexual selection for female ornaments in a dance fly Journal of Evolutionary Biology 25 7 1233 1242 doi 10 1111 j 1420 9101 2012 02522 x ISSN 1420 9101 PMID 22551204 Tibbetts Elizabeth A Forrest Taylor Vernier Cassondra Jinn Judy Madagame Andrew 2015 11 01 Socially selected ornaments and fitness Signals of fighting ability in paper wasps are positively associated with survival reproductive success and rank Evolution 69 11 2917 2926 doi 10 1111 evo 12793 hdl 2027 42 134447 ISSN 1558 5646 PMID 26456701 S2CID 10079068 Garcia Fernandez V Draganoiu T I Ung D Lacroix A Malacarne G Leboucher G 2013 Female canaries invest more in response to an exaggerated male trait Animal Behaviour 85 3 679 684 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2013 01 007 S2CID 53154422 a b Arnqvist G amp Rowe L 2005 Sexual conflict Princeton New Jersey Princeton University Press Parker Geoffrey A 1970 Sperm competition and its evolutionary consequences in insects Biological Reviews vol 55 pp 525 567 Schilthuizen Menno 2001 Frogs flies and dandelions the making of species Oxford University Press p92 ISBN 0 19 850392 X Eberhard W G 1996 Female control sexual selection by cryptic female choice Princeton New Jersey Princeton University Press Schilthuizen Menno 2001 Frogs flies and dandelions the making of species Oxford University Press p 92 ISBN 0 19 850392 X Crudgington H amp Siva Jothy M T 200 Genital damage kicking and early death Nature vol 407 pp 855 856 Perez D M 2015 Handedness in fiddler crab fights PDF Animal Behaviour 110 99 104 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2015 09 012 S2CID 53194236 Kahn A T 2014 Female preferences for timing in a fiddler crab with synchronous courtship waving displays Animal Behaviour 98 35 39 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2014 09 028 S2CID 44139315 Muller M N 2009 Sexual Coercion in Primates and Humans Harvard University Press pp 3 22 ISBN 9780674033245 Breuer T 2016 Sexual coercion and courtship by male western gorillas PDF Primates 57 1 29 38 doi 10 1007 s10329 015 0496 9 PMID 26483073 S2CID 17684689 West K 2009 Animal behaviour animal courtship Chelsea House Publications a b c d Ancel A 2013 The long engagement of the emperor penguin Polar Biology 36 4 573 577 doi 10 1007 s00300 013 1285 9 S2CID 15504155 Isenmann P 1971 Contribution a l e thologie et a l e cologie du manchot empereur Aptenodytes forsteri Gray a la colonie de Pointe Ge ologie Terre Ade lie L Oiseau et la RFO 40 136 159 a b Bennett Albert F Houck Lynne D 1983 10 01 The Energetic Cost of Courtship and Aggression in a Plethodontid Salamander Ecology 64 5 979 983 doi 10 2307 1937804 ISSN 1939 9170 JSTOR 1937804 Houck Lynne D Arnold Stevan J Thisted Ronald A 1985 A Statistical Study of Mate Choice Sexual Selection in a Plethodontid Salamander Desmognathus ochrophaeus Evolution 39 2 370 386 doi 10 2307 2408370 JSTOR 2408370 PMID 28564228 Vehrencamp Sandra L Bradbury Jack W Gibson Robert M 1989 The energetic cost of display in male sage grouse Animal Behaviour 38 5 885 896 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 89 80120 4 S2CID 53195569 a b Scott John W 1942 Mating Behavior of the Sage Grouse The Auk 59 4 477 498 doi 10 2307 4079460 JSTOR 4079460 Gibson Robert M Bradbury Jack W Vehrencamp Sandra L 1991 07 01 Mate choice in lekking sage grouse revisited the roles of vocal display female site fidelity and copying Behavioral Ecology 2 2 165 180 doi 10 1093 beheco 2 2 165 ISSN 1045 2249 Young Jessica R Hupp Jerry W Bradbury Jack W Braun Clait E 1994 Phenotypic divergence of secondary sexual traits among sage grouse Centrocercus urophasianus populations Animal Behaviour 47 6 1353 1362 doi 10 1006 anbe 1994 1183 S2CID 54287148 a b Cole G L 2016 Male courtship decisions are influenced by light environment and female receptivity Biological Sciences 283 1839 1839 doi 10 1098 rspb 2016 0861 PMC 5046890 PMID 27683362 Endler J A 2016 Predation light intensity and courtship behaviour in Poecilia reticulata Pisces Poeciliidae Animal Behaviour 35 5 1376 1385 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 87 80010 6 S2CID 53169408 Gamble S 2003 Environmental variation and the maintenance of polymorphism the effect of ambient light spectrum on mating behaviour and sexual selection in guppies Ecology Letters 6 5 463 472 doi 10 1046 j 1461 0248 2003 00449 x Bried J 1999 Why do aptenodytes penguins have high divorce rates The Auk 116 2 504 512 doi 10 2307 4089382 JSTOR 4089382 a b The Evolutionary Biology of Colonizing Species epdf pub Retrieved 2019 11 18 Fisher R A 1915 The evolution of sexual preference Eugenics Review 7 3 184 192 PMC 2987134 PMID 21259607 Yasui Y 1997 A Good Sperm model can explain the evolution of costly multiple mating by females The American Naturalist 149 3 573 doi 10 1086 286006 S2CID 84991216 Ryan Michael 2018 A taste for the beautiful The evolution of attraction Princeton University Press pp 3 32 41 43 44 155 ISBN 9780691167268 External links Edithttp www stanford edu group stanfordbirds text essays Visual Displays html http www featherlightphoto com blog 2014 12 avian courtship display http www bbc co uk nature life Lawes 27s Parotia http www birdsofparadiseproject org http www bbc co uk programmes b006mywy episodes guide Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Courtship display amp oldid 1152868284, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.