fbpx
Wikipedia

Altruism

Altruism is the principle and practice of concern for the well-being and/or happiness of other humans or animals above oneself. While objects of altruistic concern vary, it is an important moral value in many cultures and religions. It may be considered a synonym of selflessness, the opposite of selfishness.

Giving alms to the poor is often considered an altruistic action.

The word altruism was popularized (and possibly coined) by the French philosopher Auguste Comte in French, as altruisme, for an antonym of egoism.[1] He derived it from the Italian altrui, which in turn was derived from Latin alteri, meaning "other people" or "somebody else".[2]

Altruism, as observed in populations of organisms, is when an individual performs an action at a cost to themselves (in terms of e.g. pleasure and quality of life, time, probability of survival or reproduction) that benefits, directly or indirectly, another individual, without the expectation of reciprocity or compensation for that action.

Altruism can be distinguished from feelings of loyalty or concern for the common good. The latter are predicated upon social relationships, whilst altruism does not consider relationships. Whether "true" altruism is possible in human psychology is a subject of debate. The theory of psychological egoism suggests that no act of sharing, helping, or sacrificing can be truly altruistic, as the actor may receive an intrinsic reward in the form of personal gratification. The validity of this argument depends on whether such intrinsic rewards qualify as "benefits".

The term altruism may also refer to an ethical doctrine that claims that individuals are morally obliged to benefit others. Used in this sense, it is usually contrasted with egoism, which claims individuals are morally obligated to serve themselves first.

Effective altruism is the use of evidence and reason to determine the most effective ways to benefit others.

The notion of altruism edit

The concept of altruism has a history in philosophical and ethical thought. The term was coined in the 19th century by the founding sociologist and philosopher of science Auguste Comte, and has become a major topic for psychologists (especially evolutionary psychology researchers), evolutionary biologists, and ethologists. Whilst ideas about altruism from one field can affect the other fields, the different methods and focuses of these fields always lead to different perspectives on altruism. In simple terms, altruism is caring about the welfare of other people and acting to help them, above oneself.

Scientific viewpoints edit

Anthropology edit

Marcel Mauss's essay The Gift contains a passage called "Note on alms". This note describes the evolution of the notion of alms (and by extension of altruism) from the notion of sacrifice. In it, he writes:

Alms are the fruits of a moral notion of the gift and of fortune on the one hand, and of a notion of sacrifice, on the other. Generosity is an obligation, because Nemesis avenges the poor and the gods for the superabundance of happiness and wealth of certain people who should rid themselves of it. This is the ancient morality of the gift, which has become a principle of justice. The gods and the spirits accept that the share of wealth and happiness that has been offered to them and had been hitherto destroyed in useless sacrifices should serve the poor and children.

Evolutionary explanations edit

 
Giving alms to beggar children

In the Science of ethology (the study of animal behaviour), and more generally in the study of social evolution, altruism refers to behavior by an individual that increases the fitness of another individual while decreasing the fitness of the actor.[3] In evolutionary psychology this term may be applied to a wide range of human behaviors such as charity, emergency aid, help to coalition partners, tipping, courtship gifts, production of public goods, and environmentalism.[4]

Theories of apparently altruistic behavior were accelerated[clarification needed] by the need to produce ideas compatible with evolutionary origins. Two related strands of research on altruism have emerged from traditional evolutionary analyses and evolutionary game theory: a mathematical model and analysis of behavioral strategies.

Some of the proposed mechanisms are:

  • Kin selection.[5] That animals and humans are more altruistic towards close kin than to distant kin and non-kin has been confirmed in numerous studies across many different cultures. Even subtle cues indicating kinship may unconsciously increase altruistic behavior. One kinship cue is facial resemblance. One study found that slightly altering photographs to resemble the faces of study participants more closely increased the trust the participants expressed regarding depicted persons. Another cue is having the same family name, especially if rare, which has been found to increase helpful behavior. Another study found more cooperative behavior, the greater the number of perceived kin in a group. Using kinship terms in political speeches increased audience agreement with the speaker in one study. This effect was powerful for firstborns, who are typically close to their families.[4]
  • Vested interests. People are likely to suffer if their friends, allies and those from similar social ingroups suffer or disappear. Helping such group members may, therefore, also benefit the altruist. Making ingroup membership more noticeable increases cooperativeness. Extreme self-sacrifice towards the ingroup may be adaptive if a hostile outgroup threatens the entire ingroup.[4]
  • Reciprocal altruism.[6] See also Reciprocity (evolution).
    • Direct reciprocity.[7] Research shows that it can be beneficial to help others if there is a chance that they will reciprocate the help. The effective tit for tat strategy is one game theoretic example. Many people seem to be following a similar strategy by cooperating if and only if others cooperate in return.[4]
      One consequence is that people are more cooperative with one another if they are more likely to interact again in the future. People tend to be less cooperative if they perceive that the frequency of helpers in the population is lower. They tend to help less if they see non-cooperativeness by others, and this effect tends to be stronger than the opposite effect of seeing cooperative behaviors. Simply changing the cooperative framing of a proposal may increase cooperativeness, such as calling it a "Community Game" instead of a "Wall Street Game".[4]
      A tendency towards reciprocity implies that people feel obligated to respond if someone helps them. This has been used by charities that give small gifts to potential donors hoping to induce reciprocity. Another method is to announce publicly that someone has given a large donation. The tendency to reciprocate can even generalize, so people become more helpful toward others after being helped. On the other hand, people will avoid or even retaliate against those perceived not to be cooperating. People sometimes mistakenly fail to help when they intended to, or their helping may not be noticed, which may cause unintended conflicts. As such, it may be an optimal strategy to be slightly forgiving of and have a slightly generous interpretation of non-cooperation.[4]
      People are more likely to cooperate on a task if they can communicate with one another first. This may be due to better cooperativeness assessments or promises exchange. They are more cooperative if they can gradually build trust instead of being asked to give extensive help immediately. Direct reciprocity and cooperation in a group can be increased by changing the focus and incentives from intra-group competition to larger-scale competitions, such as between groups or against the general population. Thus, giving grades and promotions based only on an individual's performance relative to a small local group, as is common, may reduce cooperative behaviors in the group.[4]
    • Indirect reciprocity.[8] Because people avoid poor reciprocators and cheaters, a person's reputation is important. A person esteemed for their reciprocity is more likely to receive assistance, even from individuals they have not directly interacted with before.[4]
    • Strong reciprocity.[9] This form of reciprocity is expressed by people who invest more resources in cooperation and punishment than what is deemed optimal based on established theories of altruism.
    • Pseudo-reciprocity.[10] An organism behaves altruistically and the recipient does not reciprocate but has an increased chance of acting in a way that is selfish but also as a byproduct benefits the altruist.
  • Costly signaling and the handicap principle.[11] Altruism, by diverting resources from the altruist, can act as an "honest signal" of available resources and the skills to acquire them. This may signal to others that the altruist is a valuable potential partner. It may also signal interactive and cooperative intentions, since someone who does not expect to interact further in the future gains nothing from such costly signaling. While it's uncertain if costly signaling can predict long-term cooperative traits, people tend to trust helpers more. Costly signaling loses its value when everyone shares identical traits, resources, and cooperative intentions, but it gains significance as population variability in these aspects increases.[4]
Hunters who share meat display a costly signal of ability. The research found that good hunters have higher reproductive success and more adulterous relations even if they receive no more of the hunted meat than anyone else. Similarly, holding large feasts and giving large donations are ways of demonstrating one's resources. Heroic risk-taking has also been interpreted as a costly signal of ability.[4]
 
Volunteers assist Hurricane victims at the Houston Astrodome, following Hurricane Katrina.
Both indirect reciprocity and costly signaling depend on reputation value and tend to make similar predictions. One is that people will be more helpful when they know that their helping behavior will be communicated to people they will interact with later, publicly announced, discussed, or observed by someone else. This has been documented in many studies. The effect is sensitive to subtle cues, such as people being more helpful when there were stylized eyespots instead of a logo on a computer screen.[dubious ] Weak reputational cues such as eyespots may become unimportant if there are stronger cues present and may lose their effect with continued exposure unless reinforced with real reputational effects.[4] Public displays such as public weeping for dead celebrities and participation in demonstrations may be influenced by a desire to be seen as generous. People who know that they are publicly monitored sometimes even wastefully donate the money they know is not needed by the recipient because of reputational concerns.[12]
Typically, women find altruistic men to be attractive partners. When women look for a long-term partner, altruism may be a trait they prefer as it may indicate that the prospective partner is also willing to share resources with her and her children. Men perform charitable acts in the early stages of a romantic relationship or simply when in the presence of an attractive woman. While both sexes state that kindness is the most preferable trait in a partner, there is some evidence that men place less value on this than women and that women may not be more altruistic in the presence of an attractive man. Men may even avoid altruistic women in short-term relationships, which may be because they expect less success.[4][12]
People may compete for the social benefit of a burnished reputation, which may cause competitive altruism. On the other hand, in some experiments, a proportion of people do not seem to care about reputation and do not help more, even if this is conspicuous. This may be due to reasons such as psychopathy or that they are so attractive that they need not be seen as altruistic. The reputational benefits of altruism occur in the future compared to the immediate costs of altruism. While humans and other organisms generally place less value on future costs/benefits as compared to those in the present, some have shorter time horizons than others, and these people tend to be less cooperative.[4]
Explicit extrinsic rewards and punishments have sometimes been found to have a counterintuitively inverse effect on behaviors when compared to intrinsic rewards. This may be because such extrinsic incentives may replace (partially or in whole) intrinsic and reputational incentives, motivating the person to focus on obtaining the extrinsic rewards, which may make the thus-incentivized behaviors less desirable. People prefer altruism in others when it appears to be due to a personality characteristic rather than overt reputational concerns; simply pointing out that there are reputational benefits of action may reduce them. This may be used as a derogatory tactic against altruists ("you're just virtue signalling"), especially by those who are non-cooperators. A counterargument is that doing good due to reputational concerns is better than doing no good.[4]
  • Group selection. It has controversially been argued by some evolutionary scientists such as David Sloan Wilson that natural selection can act at the level of non-kin groups to produce adaptations that benefit a non-kin group, even if these adaptations are detrimental at the individual level. Thus, while altruistic persons may under some circumstances be outcompeted by less altruistic persons at the individual level, according to group selection theory, the opposite may occur at the group level where groups consisting of the more altruistic persons may outcompete groups consisting of the less altruistic persons. Such altruism may only extend to ingroup members while directing prejudice and antagonism against outgroup members (see also in-group favoritism). Many other evolutionary scientists have criticized group selection theory.[13]
 
Helping the homeless in New York City

Such explanations do not imply that humans consciously calculate how to increase their inclusive fitness when doing altruistic acts. Instead, evolution has shaped psychological mechanisms, such as emotions, that promote certain altruistic behaviors.[4]

The benefits for the altruist may be increased, and the costs reduced by being more altruistic towards certain groups. Research has found that people are more altruistic to kin than to no-kin, to friends than strangers, to those attractive than to those unattractive, to non-competitors than competitors, and to members in-groups than to members of out-groups.[4]

The study of altruism was the initial impetus behind George R. Price's development of the Price equation, a mathematical equation used to study genetic evolution. An interesting example of altruism is found in the cellular slime moulds, such as Dictyostelium mucoroides. These protists live as individual amoebae until starved, at which point they aggregate and form a multicellular fruiting body in which some cells sacrifice themselves to promote the survival of other cells in the fruiting body.[14]

Selective investment theory proposes that close social bonds, and associated emotional, cognitive, and neurohormonal mechanisms, evolved to facilitate long-term, high-cost altruism between those closely depending on one another for survival and reproductive success.[15]

Such cooperative behaviors have sometimes been seen as arguments for left-wing politics, for example, by the Russian zoologist and anarchist Peter Kropotkin in his 1902 book Mutual Aid: A Factor of Evolution and Moral Philosopher Peter Singer in his book A Darwinian Left.

Neurobiology edit

Jorge Moll and Jordan Grafman, neuroscientists at the National Institutes of Health and LABS-D'Or Hospital Network, provided the first evidence for the neural bases of altruistic giving in normal healthy volunteers, using functional magnetic resonance imaging. In their research,[16] they showed that both pure monetary rewards and charitable donations activated the mesolimbic reward pathway, a primitive part of the brain that usually responds to food and sex. However, when volunteers generously placed the interests of others before their own by making charitable donations, another brain circuit was selectively activated: the subgenual cortex/septal region. These structures are related to[vague] social attachment and bonding in other species. The experiment suggested that altruism is not a higher moral faculty overpowering innate selfish desires, but a fundamental, ingrained, and enjoyable trait in the brain.[17] One brain region, the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex/basal forebrain, contributes to learning altruistic behavior, especially in people with trait[clarification needed] empathy.[18] The same study identified a link between[vague] giving to charity and the promotion[clarification needed] of social bonding.[19]

Bill Harbaugh, a University of Oregon economist, in an fMRI scanner test conducted with his psychologist colleague Dr. Ulrich Mayr, reached the same conclusions as Jorge Moll and Jordan Grafman about giving to charity, although they were able to divide the study group into two groups: "egoists" and "altruists". One of their discoveries was that, though rarely, even some of the considered "egoists" sometimes gave more than expected because that would help others, leading to the conclusion that there are other factors in charity, such as a person's environment and values.[19]

A recent meta-analysis of fMRI studies conducted by Shawn Rhoads, Jo Cutler, and Abigail Marsh analyzed the results of prior studies of generosity in which participants could freely choose to give or not give resources to someone else.[20] The results of this study confirmed that altruism is supported by distinct mechanisms from giving motivated by reciprocity or by fairness. This study also confirmed that the right ventral striatum is recruited during altruistic giving, as well as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, bilateral anterior cingulate cortex, and bilateral anterior insula, which are regions previously implicated in empathy.

Abigail Marsh has conducted studies of real-world altruists that have also identified an important role for the amygdala in human altruism. In real-world altruists, such as people who have donated kidneys to strangers, the amygdala is larger than in typical adults. Altruists' amygdalas are also more responsive than those of typical adults to the sight of others' distress, which is thought to reflect an empathic response to distress.[21][22] This structure may also be involved in altruistic choices due to its role in encoding the value of outcomes for others.[23] This is consistent with the findings of research in non-human animals, which has identified neurons within the amygdala that specifically encode the value of others' outcomes, activity in which appears to drive altruistic choices in monkeys.[24][25]

Psychology edit

The International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences defines psychological altruism as "a motivational state to increase another's welfare". Psychological altruism is contrasted with psychological egoism, which refers to the motivation to increase one's welfare.[26] In keeping with this, research in real-world altruists, including altruistic kidney donors, bone marrow donors, humanitarian aid workers, and heroic rescuers findings that these altruists are primarily distinguished from other adults by unselfish traits and decision-making patterns. This suggests that human altruism reflects genuinely high valuation of others' outcomes.[27]

There has been some debate on whether humans are capable of psychological altruism.[28] Some definitions specify a self-sacrificial nature to altruism and a lack of external rewards for altruistic behaviors.[29] However, because altruism ultimately benefits the self in many cases, the selflessness of altruistic acts is difficult to prove. The social exchange theory postulates that altruism only exists when the benefits outweigh the costs to the self.[30]

Daniel Batson, a psychologist, examined this question and argued against the social exchange theory. He identified four significant motives: to ultimately benefit the self (egoism), to ultimately benefit the other person (altruism), to benefit a group (collectivism), or to uphold a moral principle (principlism). Altruism that ultimately serves selfish gains is thus differentiated from selfless altruism, but the general conclusion has been that empathy-induced altruism can be genuinely selfless.[31] The empathy-altruism hypothesis states that psychological altruism exists and is evoked by the empathic desire to help someone suffering. Feelings of empathic concern are contrasted with personal distress, which compels people to reduce their unpleasant emotions and increase their positive ones by helping someone in need. Empathy is thus not selfless since altruism works either as a way to avoid those negative, unpleasant feelings and have positive, pleasant feelings when triggered by others' need for help or as a way to gain social reward or avoid social punishment by helping. People with empathic concern help others in distress even when exposure to the situation could be easily avoided, whereas those lacking in empathic concern avoid allowing it unless it is difficult or impossible to avoid exposure to another's suffering.[26]

Helping behavior is seen in humans from about two years old when a toddler can understand subtle emotional cues.[32]

 
Peace Corps trainees swearing in as volunteers in Cambodia, 4 April 2007

In psychological research on altruism, studies often observe altruism as demonstrated through prosocial behaviors such as helping, comforting, sharing, cooperation, philanthropy, and community service.[29] People are most likely to help if they recognize that a person is in need and feel personal responsibility for reducing the person's distress. The number of bystanders witnessing pain or suffering affects the likelihood of helping (the Bystander effect). More significant numbers of bystanders decrease individual feelings of responsibility.[26][33] However, a witness with a high level of empathic concern is likely to assume personal responsibility entirely regardless of the number of bystanders.[26]

Many studies have observed the effects of volunteerism (as a form of altruism) on happiness and health and have consistently found that those who exhibit volunteerism also have better current and future health and well-being.[34][35] In a study of older adults, those who volunteered had higher life satisfaction and will to live, and less depression, anxiety, and somatization.[36] Volunteerism and helping behavior have not only been shown to improve mental health but physical health and longevity as well, attributable to the activity and social integration it encourages.[34][37][38] One study examined the physical health of mothers who volunteered over 30 years and found that 52% of those who did not belong to a volunteer organization experienced a major illness while only 36% of those who did volunteer experienced one.[39] A study on adults aged 55 and older found that during the four-year study period, people who volunteered for two or more organizations had a 63% lower likelihood of dying. After controlling for prior health status, it was determined that volunteerism accounted for a 44% reduction in mortality.[40] Merely being aware of kindness in oneself and others is also associated with greater well-being. A study that asked participants to count each act of kindness they performed for one week significantly enhanced their subjective happiness. Happier people are kinder and more grateful, kinder people are happier and more grateful and more grateful people are happier and kinder, the study suggests.[41]

While research supports the idea that altruistic acts bring about happiness, it has also been found to work in the opposite direction—that happier people are also kinder. The relationship between altruistic behavior and happiness is bidirectional. Studies found that generosity increases linearly from sad to happy affective states.[42]

Feeling over-taxed by the needs of others has negative effects on health and happiness.[38] For example, one study on volunteerism found that feeling overwhelmed by others' demands had an even stronger negative effect on mental health than helping had a positive one (although positive effects were still significant).[43]

Pathological altruism edit

Pathological altruism is altruism taken to an unhealthy extreme, such that it either harms the altruistic person or the person's well-intentioned actions cause more harm than good.

The term "pathological altruism" was popularised by the book Pathological Altruism.

Examples include depression and burnout seen in healthcare professionals, an unhealthy focus on others to the detriment of one's own needs, hoarding of animals, and ineffective philanthropic and social programs that ultimately worsen the situations they are meant to aid.[44]

Sociology edit

"Sociologists have long been concerned with how to build the good society".[45] The structure of our societies and how individuals come to exhibit charitable, philanthropic, and other pro-social, altruistic actions for the common good is a commonly researched topic within the field. The American Sociology Association (ASA) acknowledges public sociology saying, "The intrinsic scientific, policy, and public relevance of this field of investigation in helping to construct 'good societies' is unquestionable".[45] This type of sociology seeks contributions that aid popular and theoretical understandings of what motivates altruism and how it is organized, and promotes an altruistic focus in order to benefit the world and people it studies.

How altruism is framed, organized, carried out, and what motivates it at the group level is an area of focus that sociologists investigate in order to contribute back to the groups it studies and "build the good society". The motivation of altruism is also the focus of study; for example, one study links the occurrence of moral outrage to altruistic compensation of victims.[46] Studies show that generosity in laboratory and in online experiments is contagious – people imitate the generosity they observe in others.[47]

Religious viewpoints edit

Most, if not all, of the world's religions promote altruism as a very important moral value. Buddhism, Christianity, Hinduism, Islam, Jainism, Judaism, and Sikhism, etc., place particular emphasis on altruistic morality.

Buddhism edit

 
Monks collecting alms

Altruism figures prominently in Buddhism. Love and compassion are components of all forms of Buddhism, and are focused on all beings equally: love is the wish that all beings be happy, and compassion is the wish that all beings be free from suffering. "Many illnesses can be cured by the one medicine of love and compassion. These qualities are the ultimate source of human happiness, and the need for them lies at the very core of our being" (Dalai Lama).[48][49]

The notion of altruism is modified in such a world-view, since the belief is that such a practice promotes the practitioner's own happiness: "The more we care for the happiness of others, the greater our own sense of well-being becomes" (Dalai Lama).[48]

In the context of larger ethical discussions on moral action and judgment, Buddhism is characterized by the belief that negative (unhappy) consequences of our actions derive not from punishment or correction based on moral judgment, but from the law of karma, which functions like a natural law of cause and effect. A simple illustration of such cause and effect is the case of experiencing the effects of what one causes: if one causes suffering, then as a natural consequence one would experience suffering; if one causes happiness, then as a natural consequence one would experience happiness.[citation needed]

Jainism edit

 
Sculpture depicting the Jain concept of ahimsa (non-injury)

The fundamental principles of Jainism revolve around altruism, not only for[ambiguous] humans but for all sentient beings. Jainism preaches ahimsa – to live and let live, not harming sentient beings, i.e. uncompromising reverence for all life. It also considers all living things to be equal[specify]. The first Tirthankara, Rishabhdev, introduced the concept of altruism for all living beings, from extending knowledge and experience to others to donation, giving oneself up for others, non-violence, and compassion for all living things.[citation needed]

The principle of nonviolence seeks to minimize karmas which limit the capabilities of the soul. Jainism views every soul as worthy of respect because it has the potential to become Siddha (God in Jainism). Because all living beings possess a soul, great care and awareness is essential in one's actions. Jainism emphasizes the equality of all life, advocating harmlessness towards all, whether the creatures are great or small. This policy extends even to microscopic organisms. Jainism acknowledges that every person has different capabilities and capacities to practice and therefore accepts different levels of compliance for ascetics and householders.[citation needed]

Christianity edit

Thomas Aquinas interprets "You should love your neighbour as yourself"[50] as meaning that love for ourselves is the exemplar of love for others.[51] Considering that "the love with which a man loves himself is the form and root of friendship" he quotes Aristotle that "the origin of friendly relations with others lies in our relations to ourselves",.[52] Aquinas concluded that though we are not bound to love others more than ourselves, we naturally seek the common good, the good of the whole, more than any private good, the good of a part. However, he thought we should love God more than ourselves and our neighbours, and more than our bodily life—since the ultimate purpose of loving our neighbour is to share in eternal beatitude: a more desirable thing than bodily well-being. In coining the word "altruism", as stated above, Comte was probably opposing this Thomistic doctrine, which is present in some theological schools within Catholicism. The aim and focus of Christian life is a life that glorifies God, with obeying christ's command to treat others equally, caring for them and understanding eternity in heaven is what Jesus Resurrection at calvary was all about.

Many biblical authors draw a strong connection between love of others and love of God. 1 John 4 states that for one to love God one must love his fellowman, and that hatred of one's fellowman is the same as hatred of God. Thomas Jay Oord has argued in several books that altruism is but one possible form of love. An altruistic action is not always a loving action. Oord defines altruism as acting for the other's good, and he agrees with feminists who note that sometimes love requires acting for one's own good when the other's demands undermine overall well-being.

German philosopher Max Scheler distinguishes two ways in which the strong can help the weak. One way is a sincere expression of Christian love, "motivated by a powerful feeling of security, strength, and inner salvation, of the invincible fullness of one's own life and existence".[53]: 88–89  Another way is merely "one of the many modern substitutes for love,... nothing but the urge to turn away from oneself and to lose oneself in other people's business".[53]: 95–96  At its worst, Scheler says, "love for the small, the poor, the weak, and the oppressed is really disguised hatred, repressed envy, an impulse to detract, etc., directed against the opposite phenomena: wealth, strength, power, largesse."[53]: 96–97 

Islam edit

In the Arabic language, "'iythar" (إيثار) means "preferring others to oneself".[54]

On the topic of donating blood to non-Muslims (a controversial topic within the faith), the Shia religious professor, Fadhil al-Milani has provided theological evidence that makes it positively justifiable. In fact, he considers it a form of religious sacrifice and ithar (altruism).[55]

For Sufis, 'iythar means devotion to others through complete forgetfulness of one's own concerns, where concern for others is deemed as a demand made by God on the human body, considered to be property of God alone. The importance of 'iythar (aka īthār) lies in sacrifice for the sake of the greater good; Islam considers those practicing īthār as abiding by the highest degree of nobility.[56] This is similar to the notion of chivalry. A constant concern for God results in a careful attitude towards people, animals, and other things in this world.[57]

Judaism edit

Judaism defines altruism as the desired goal of creation. Rabbi Abraham Isaac Kook stated that love is the most important attribute in humanity.[58] Love is defined as bestowal, or giving, which is the intention of altruism. This can be altruism towards humanity that leads to altruism towards the creator or God. Kabbalah defines God as the force of giving in existence. Rabbi Moshe Chaim Luzzatto focused on the "purpose of creation" and how the will of God was to bring creation into perfection and adhesion with this force of giving.[59]

Modern Kabbalah developed by Rabbi Yehuda Ashlag, in his writings about the future generation, focuses on how society could achieve an altruistic social framework.[60]: 120–130  Ashlag proposed that such a framework is the purpose of creation, and everything that happens is to raise humanity to the level of altruism, love for one another. Ashlag focused on society and its relation to divinity.[60]: 175–180 

Sikhism edit

Altruism is essential to the Sikh religion. The central faith in Sikhism is that the greatest deed anyone can do is to imbibe and live the godly qualities like love, affection, sacrifice, patience, harmony, and truthfulness. Sevā, or selfless service to the community for its own sake, is an important concept in Sikhism.[61]

The fifth Guru, Arjun Dev, sacrificed his life to uphold "22 carats of pure truth, the greatest gift to humanity", the Guru Granth. The ninth Guru, Tegh Bahadur, sacrificed his head to protect weak and defenseless people against atrocity.

In the late seventeenth century, Guru Gobind Singh (the tenth Guru in Sikhism), was at war with the Mughal rulers to protect the people of different faiths when a fellow Sikh, Bhai Kanhaiya, attended the troops of the enemy.[62] He gave water to both friends and foes who were wounded on the battlefield. Some of the enemy began to fight again and some Sikh warriors were annoyed by Bhai Kanhaiya as he was helping their enemy. Sikh soldiers brought Bhai Kanhaiya before Guru Gobind Singh, and complained of his action that they considered counterproductive to their struggle on the battlefield. "What were you doing, and why?" asked the Guru. "I was giving water to the wounded because I saw your face in all of them", replied Bhai Kanhaiya. The Guru responded, "Then you should also give them ointment to heal their wounds. You were practicing what you were coached in the house of the Guru."

Under the tutelage of the Guru, Bhai Kanhaiya subsequently founded a volunteer corps for altruism, which is still engaged today in doing good to others and in training new recruits for this service.[63]

Hinduism edit

In Hinduism Selflessness (Atmatyag), Love (Prema), Kindness (Daya), and Forgiveness (Kshama) are considered as the highest acts of humanity or "Manushyattva". Giving alms to the beggars or poor people is considered as a divine act or "Punya" and Hindus believe it will free their souls from guilt or "Paapa" and will led them to heaven or "Swarga" in afterlife. Altruism is also the central act of various Hindu mythology and religious poems and songs. Mass donation of clothes to poor people (Vastraseva), or blood donation camp or mass food donation (Annaseva) for poor people is common in various Hindu religious ceremonies.[citation needed]

The Bhagavad Gita supports the doctrine of karma yoga (achieving oneness with God through action) & "Nishkam Karma" or action without expectation / desire for personal gain which can be said to encompass altruism. Altruistic acts are generally celebrated and very well received in Hindu literature and are central to Hindu morality.[64]

Philosophy edit

There is a wide range of philosophical views on humans' obligations or motivations to act altruistically. Proponents of ethical altruism maintain that individuals are morally obligated to act altruistically.[65] The opposing view is ethical egoism, which maintains that moral agents should always act in their own self-interest. Both ethical altruism and ethical egoism contrast with utilitarianism, which maintains that each agent should act in order to maximise the efficacy of their function and the benefit to both themselves and their co-inhabitants.

A related concept in descriptive ethics is psychological egoism, the thesis that humans always act in their own self-interest and that true altruism is impossible. Rational egoism is the view that rationality consists in acting in one's self-interest (without specifying how this affects one's moral obligations).

Effective altruism edit

Effective altruism is a philosophy and social movement that uses evidence and reasoning to determine the most effective ways to benefit others.[66] Effective altruism encourages individuals to consider all causes and actions and to act in the way that brings about the greatest positive impact, based upon their values.[67] It is the broad, evidence-based, and cause-neutral approach that distinguishes effective altruism from traditional altruism or charity.[68] Effective altruism is part of the larger movement towards evidence-based practices.

While a substantial proportion of effective altruists have focused on the nonprofit sector, the philosophy of effective altruism applies more broadly to prioritizing the scientific projects, companies, and policy initiatives which can be estimated to save lives, help people, or otherwise have the biggest benefit.[69] People associated with the movement include philosopher Peter Singer,[70] Facebook co founder Dustin Moskovitz,[71] Cari Tuna,[72] Oxford-based researchers William MacAskill[73] and Toby Ord,[74] and professional poker player Liv Boeree.[75]

Genetics edit

OXTR, CD38, COMT, DRD4, DRD5, IGF2, and GABRB2 are candidate genes for influencing altruistic behavior.[76][further explanation needed]

Digital altruism edit

Digital altruism is the notion that some are willing to freely share information based on the principle of reciprocity and in the belief that in the end, everyone benefits from sharing information via the Internet.[77]

There are three types of digital altruism: (1) "everyday digital altruism", involving expedience, ease, moral engagement, and conformity; (2) "creative digital altruism", involving creativity, heightened moral engagement, and cooperation; and (3) "co-creative digital altruism" involving creativity, moral engagement, and meta cooperative efforts.[77]

See also edit

Notes edit

  1. ^
    • "altruism (n .)". Online Etymology Dictionary. Douglas Harper. Retrieved 27 May 2021.
    • Teske, Nathan (2009). Political Activists in America: The Identity Construction Model of Political Participation. University Park, Pa.: Pennsylvania State University Press. p. 101. ISBN 9780271035468.
  2. ^ Ciciloni, Ferdinando (1825). A Grammar of the Italian Language. London: John Murray. p. 64.
  3. ^ Bell, Graham (2008). Selection: the mechanism of evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 367–368. ISBN 978-0-19-856972-5.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Barcaly, Pat (2011). "The evolution of charitable behaviour and the power of reputation". In Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
  5. ^ Okasha, Samir (2013). "Kin Selection and Inclusive Fitness". Biological Altruism. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
  6. ^ Trivers, R.L. (1971). "The evolution of reciprocal altruism". Quarterly Review of Biology. 46: 35–57. doi:10.1086/406755. S2CID 19027999.
  7. ^ Axelrod, R; Hamilton, W.D. (27 March 1981). "The evolution of cooperation". Science. 211 (4489): 1390–1396. Bibcode:1981Sci...211.1390A. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.147.9644. doi:10.1126/science.7466396. PMID 7466396.
  8. ^ Nowak, Martin; Sigmund, Karl (October 2005). "Evolution of indirect reciprocity" (PDF). Nature. 437 (27): 1291–1298. Bibcode:2005Natur.437.1291N. doi:10.1038/nature04131. PMID 16251955. S2CID 3153895.
  9. ^ Gintis, Herbert (September 2000). "Strong Reciprocity and Human Sociality". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 206 (2): 169–179. Bibcode:2000JThBi.206..169G. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.335.7226. doi:10.1006/jtbi.2000.2111. hdl:10419/105717. PMID 10966755. S2CID 9260305.
  10. ^ Hammerstein, Peter, ed. (2003). "By-product Benefits, Reciprocity, and Pseudoreciprocity in Mutualism". Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. MIT Press. pp. 203–222. doi:10.7551/mitpress/3232.003.0013. ISBN 978-0-262-08326-3.
  11. ^ Zahavi, A. (1995). "Altruism as a handicap – The limitations of kin selection and reciprocity". Journal of Avian Biology. 26 (1): 1–3. doi:10.2307/3677205. JSTOR 3677205.
  12. ^ a b Iredal, Wendy; van Vugt, Mark (2011). "Altruism as showing off: a signaling perspective on promoting green behavior and acts of kindness". In Roberts, S. Craig (ed.). Applied Evolutionary Psychology. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586073.001.0001. ISBN 9780199586073.
  13. ^ Neyfakh, Leon (17 April 2011). . Boston Globe. Archived from the original on 19 July 2011.
  14. ^ Hudson, Richard Ellis; Aukema, Juliann Eve; Rispe, Claude; Roze, Denis (July 2002). "Altruism, cheating, and anticheater adaptations in cellular slime molds". The American Naturalist. 160 (1): 31–43. doi:10.1086/340613. ISSN 1537-5323. PMID 18707497. Retrieved 1 February 2024.
  15. ^ Brown, S.L.; Brown, R.M. (2006). (PDF). Psychological Inquiry. 17: 1–29. doi:10.1207/s15327965pli1701_01. S2CID 144718661. Archived from the original (PDF) on 14 July 2014.
  16. ^ Moll, Jorge; Krueger, Frank; Zahn, Roland; Pardini, Matteo; de Oliveira-Souza, Ricardo; Grafman, Jordan (17 October 2006). "Human fronto–mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 103 (42): 15623–15628. Bibcode:2006PNAS..10315623M. doi:10.1073/pnas.0604475103. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 1622872. PMID 17030808.
  17. ^ Vedantam, Shankar (28 May 2007). "If It Feels Good to Be Good, It Might Be Only Natural". The Washington Post. Retrieved 23 April 2010.
  18. ^
    • Lockwood, Patricia L; Apps, Matthew A J; Valton, Vincent; Viding, Essi; Roiser, Jonathan P (2016). "Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 113 (35): 9763–8. Bibcode:2016PNAS..113.9763L. doi:10.1073/pnas.1603198113. PMC 5024617. PMID 27528669. . fMRI revealed that activity in a posterior portion of the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex/basal forebrain (sgACC) drives learning only when we are acting in a prosocial context
    • Lay summary in: "Finding the Brain's Generosity Center". Neuroscience News. 15 August 2016.
  19. ^ a b Svoboda, Elizabeth (5 September 2013). "Scientists Are Finding That We Are Hard-Wired for Giving". University of Notre Dame. Retrieved 7 August 2017.
  20. ^ Rhoads, S. A. (2021). "A feature-based network analysis and fMRI meta-analysis reveal three distinct types of prosocial decisions". academic.oup.com. Retrieved 30 December 2023.
  21. ^ Marsh, Abigail A.; Stoycos, Sarah A.; Brethel-Haurwitz, Kristin M.; Robinson, Paul; VanMeter, John W.; Cardinale, Elise M. (21 October 2014). "Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary altruists". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 111 (42): 15036–15041. doi:10.1073/pnas.1408440111. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 4210306. PMID 25225374.
  22. ^ Brethel-Haurwitz, Kristin M.; O'Connell, Katherine; Cardinale, Elise M.; Stoianova, Maria; Stoycos, Sarah A.; Lozier, Leah M.; VanMeter, John W.; Marsh, Abigail A. (25 October 2017). "Amygdala–midbrain connectivity indicates a role for the mammalian parental care system in human altruism". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 284 (1865): 20171731. doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.1731. ISSN 0962-8452. PMC 5666102. PMID 29070724.
  23. ^ Rhoads, Shawn A; O'Connell, Katherine; Berluti, Kathryn; Ploe, Montana L; Elizabeth, Hannah S; Amormino, Paige; Li, Joanna L; Dutton, Mary Ann; VanMeter, Ashley Skye; Marsh, Abigail A (13 June 2023). "Neural responses underlying extraordinary altruists' generosity for socially distant others". PNAS Nexus. 2 (7): pgad199. doi:10.1093/pnasnexus/pgad199. ISSN 2752-6542. PMC 10321390. PMID 37416875.
  24. ^ Dal Monte, Olga; Chu, Cheng C. J.; Fagan, Nicholas A.; Chang, Steve W. C. (April 2020). "Specialized medial prefrontal–amygdala coordination in other-regarding decision preference". Nature Neuroscience. 23 (4): 565–574. doi:10.1038/s41593-020-0593-y. hdl:2318/1730693. ISSN 1546-1726.
  25. ^ Putnam, P. T. (2023). "Dissociation of vicarious and experienced rewards by coupling frequency within the same neural pathway". Neuron. 111 (16): 2513–2522.
  26. ^ a b c d Darity, William A. Jr., ed. (2008). "Altruism". International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences. Vol. 1 (2nd ed.). Detroit: Macmillan Reference USA. pp. 87–88.
  27. ^ Rhoads, Shawn A.; Vekaria, Kruti M.; O’Connell, Katherine; Elizabeth, Hannah S.; Rand, David G.; Kozak Williams, Megan N.; Marsh, Abigail A. (31 March 2023). "Unselfish traits and social decision-making patterns characterize six populations of real-world extraordinary altruists". Nature Communications. 14 (1): 1807. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-37283-5. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 10066349. PMID 37002205.
  28. ^ Batson, C. (2011). Altruism in humans. New York, N.Y. U.S.: Oxford University Press.
  29. ^ a b Batson, C. Daniel (2012). "A history of prosocial behavior research". In Kruglanski, Arie W.; Stroebe, Wolfgang (eds.). Handbook of the history of social psychology. New York, NY: Psychology Press. pp. 243–264. ISBN 9781848728684.
  30. ^ Maner, J. K.; Luce, C. L.; Neuberg, S. L.; Cialdini, R. B.; Brown, S.; Sagarin, B. J. (2002). "The effects of perspective taking on motivations for helping: Still no evidence for altruism". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 28 (11): 1601–1610. doi:10.1177/014616702237586. S2CID 146125891.
  31. ^ Batson, C. Daniel; Ahmad, Nadia; Stocks, E. L. (2011). "Four forms of prosocial motivation: Egoism, altruism, collectivism, and principlism". In Dunning, David (ed.). Social motivation. New York, NY: Psychology Press. pp. 103–126. ISBN 9781136847202.
  32. ^ Svetlova, M.; Nichols, S. R.; Brownell, C. A. (2010). "Toddlers prosocial behavior: From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping". Child Development. 81 (6): 1814–1827. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01512.x. PMC 3088085. PMID 21077866.
  33. ^ Hudson, James M.; Bruckman, Amy S. (2004). "The Bystander Effect: A Lens for Understanding Patterns of Participation". Journal of the Learning Sciences. 13 (2): 165–195. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.72.4881. doi:10.1207/s15327809jls1302_2. S2CID 16442298.
  34. ^ a b Musick, M. A.; Wilson, J. (2003). "Volunteering and depression: The role of psychological and social resources in different age groups". Social Science & Medicine. 56 (2): 259–269. doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00025-4. PMID 12473312.
  35. ^ Koenig, L. B.; McGue, M.; Krueger, R. F.; Bouchard (2007). "Religiousness, antisocial behavior, and altruism: Genetic and environmental mediation". Journal of Personality. 75 (2): 265–290. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00439.x. PMID 17359239.
  36. ^ Hunter, K. I.; Hunter, M. W. (1980). "Psychosocial differences between elderly volunteers and non-volunteers". The International Journal of Aging & Human Development. 12 (3): 205–213. doi:10.2190/0H6V-QPPP-7JK4-LR38. PMID 7216525. S2CID 42991434.
  37. ^
    • Kayloe, J. C.; Krause, M. (1985). "RARE FIND: or The value of volunteerism". Psychosocial Rehabilitation Journal. 8 (4): 49–56. doi:10.1037/h0099659.
    • Brown, S. L.; Brown, R.; House, J. S.; Smith, D. M. (2008). "Coping with spousal loss: Potential buffering effects of self-reported helping behavior". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 34 (6): 849–861. doi:10.1177/0146167208314972. PMID 18344495. S2CID 42983453.
  38. ^ a b Post, S. G. (2005). "Altruism, Happiness, and Health: It's Good to Be Good". International Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 12 (2): 66–77. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.485.8406. doi:10.1207/s15327558ijbm1202_4. PMID 15901215. S2CID 12544814.
  39. ^ Moen, P.; Dempster-Mcclain, D.; Williams, R. M. (1992). "Successful aging: A life-course perspective on women's multiple roles and health". American Journal of Sociology. 97 (6): 1612–1638. doi:10.1086/229941. S2CID 4828775.
  40. ^ Oman, D.; Thoresen, C. E.; McMahon, K. (1999). "Volunteerism and mortality among the community-dwelling elderly". Journal of Health Psychology. 4 (3): 301–316. doi:10.1177/135910539900400301. PMID 22021599.
  41. ^ Otake, K.; Shimai, S.; Tanaka-Matsumi, J.; Otsui, K.; Fredrickson, B. L. (2006). "Happy people become happier through kindness: A counting kindnesses intervention". Journal of Happiness Studies. 7 (3): 361–375. doi:10.1007/s10902-005-3650-z. PMC 1820947. PMID 17356687.
  42. ^ Underwood, B.; Froming, W. J.; Moore, B. S. (1977). "Mood, attention, and altruism: A search for mediating variables". Developmental Psychology. 13 (5): 541–542. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.13.5.541.
  43. ^ Schwartz, C.; Meisenhelder, J.; Ma, Y.; Reed, G. (2003). "Altruistic Social Interest Behaviors Are Associated With Better Mental Health". Psychosomatic Medicine. 65 (5): 778–785. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.529.7780. doi:10.1097/01.PSY.0000079378.39062.D4. PMID 14508020. S2CID 20644442.
  44. ^ Pathological Altruism. Oxford University Press. 19 December 2011. ISBN 9780199738571.
  45. ^ a b . American Sociological Association. Archived from the original on 3 May 2012.
  46. ^ Thulin, E.W.; Bicchieri, C. (2016). "I'm so angry I could help you: Moral outrage as a driver of victim compensation". Social Philosophy & Policy. 32 (2): 146–160. doi:10.1017/S0265052516000145. S2CID 148548711.
  47. ^
    • Tsvetkova, Milena; Macy, Michael (2015). "The Contagion of Prosocial Behavior and the Emergence of Voluntary-Contribution Communities". Social Phenomena: From Data Analysis to Models. Springer International Publishing. pp. 117–134. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-14011-7_7. ISBN 978-3-319-14010-0.
    • Tagiew, Rustam; Ignatov, Dmitry (2016). "Gift Ratios in Laboratory Experiments" (PDF). CEUR Workshop Proceedings. 1627: 82–93. Archived (PDF) from the original on 9 October 2022.
  48. ^ a b . Archived from the original on 3 October 2009.
  49. ^ The phrase "core of our being" is Freudian; see Bettina Bock von Wülfingen (2013). "Freud's 'Core of our Being' Between Cytology and Psychoanalysis". Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte. 36 (3): 226–244. doi:10.1002/bewi.201301604. PMID 32545937.
  50. ^ Leviticus 19 and Matthew 22
  51. ^ Thomas Aquinas, Summa Theologica, II:II Quaestio 25, Article 4
  52. ^ Aristotle, Nicomachean Ethics IX.4 1166a1
  53. ^ a b c Scheler, Max (1961). Ressentiment.
  54. ^ 'iythar Google Translate
  55. ^ Schmiedel, Ulrich; Smith, Graeme (15 February 2018). Religion in the European Refugee Crisis. Springer. pp. 295–96. ISBN 978-3-319-67961-7.
  56. ^ Gülen, M. Fethullah (2004). Key Concepts in the Practice of Sufism: Emerald Hills of the Heart. Rutherford, N.J.: Fountain. pp. 10–11. ISBN 978-1-932099-75-1.
  57. ^ Neusner, Jacob Eds (2005). Altruism in World Religions. Washington, D.C.: Georgetown Univ. Press. pp. 79–80. ISBN 978-1-58901-065-9.
  58. ^ Kook, Abraham Isaac; Bokser, Ben Zion (1978). Abraham Isaac Kook: The lights of penitence, The moral principles, Lights of holiness, essays, letters, and poems. Paulist Press. pp. 135–136. ISBN 978-0-8091-2159-5.
  59. ^ Luzzatto, Moshe Ḥayyim (1997). The way of God. Feldheim Publishers. pp. 37–38. ISBN 978-0-87306-769-0.
  60. ^ a b Ashlag, Yehuda (2006). Building the Future Society. Thornhill, Canada: Laitman Kabbalah Publishers. ISBN 978-965-7065-34-1.
  61. ^ Cole, W. Owen; Sambhi, Piara Singh (1990). A Popular Dictionary of Sikhism. Curzon Press. pp. 38–39, 84. Retrieved 2 December 2018.
  62. ^ Cunningham, Joseph Davey (1918). "A History of the Sikhs". Oxford University Press. p. ix. Retrieved 30 November 2018.
  63. ^ Ralhan, O. P. (1997). The great gurus of the Sikhs. New Delhi: Anmol Publications Pvt Ltd. p. 253. ISBN 978-81-7488-479-4.
  64. ^ Sivananda, Swami. Phaladhikaranam, Topic 8, Sutras 38–41.
  65. ^ Kraut, Richard (2020), "Altruism", in Zalta, Edward N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2020 ed.), Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, retrieved 16 January 2024
  66. ^ MacAskill, William (31 January 2017). "Effective Altruism: Introduction". Essays in Philosophy. 18 (1): 2. doi:10.7710/1526-0569.1580. ISSN 1526-0569.
  67. ^ Matthews, Dylan (24 April 2015). "You have $8 billion. You want to do as much good as possible. What do you do?". Vox. Retrieved 27 April 2015.
  68. ^ Bennett, Nicole; Carter, Ashley; Resney, Romney; Woods, Wendy. "How Tech Entrepreneurs Are Disrupting Philanthropy". BCG Perspectives. Boston Consulting Group. Retrieved 11 March 2017.
  69. ^ MacAskill, William (2015). Doing Good Better. Avery. ISBN 978-1592409105.
  70. ^ Walters, Helen (19 September 2013). "The why and how of effective altruism: Peter Singer's talk visualized". TED Blog.
  71. ^ "Cari Tuna and Dustin Moskovitz: Young Silicon Valley billionaires pioneer new approach to philanthropy". The Washington Post. 26 December 2014.
  72. ^ Callahan, Favid (12 September 2013). "Meet Cari Tuna, the Woman Giving Away Dustin Moskovitz's Facebook Fortune". Inside Philanthropy. Retrieved 1 March 2018.
  73. ^ Thompson, Derek (15 June 2015). "The Greatest Good". The Atlantic.
  74. ^ "Peter Singer: "The Most Good You Can Do" | Talks at Google". YouTube.
  75. ^
    • "News: Liv Boeree on Effective Altruism". www.pokerstrategy.com. Retrieved 11 April 2017.
    • . www.livboeree.com. Archived from the original on 11 April 2017. Retrieved 11 April 2017.
  76. ^ Thompson, GJ; Hurd, PL; Crespi, BJ (2013). "Genes underlying altruism". Biol Lett. 9 (6): 20130395. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2013.0395. PMC 3871336. PMID 24132092.
  77. ^ a b Klisanin, Dana (2011). "Is the Internet Giving Rise to New Forms of Altruism" (PDF). Media Psychology Review. 3 (1): 1–11. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 October 2022.

References edit

  • Axelrod, Robert (1984). The Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Basic Books. ISBN 978-0-465-02121-5.
  • Batson, Charles (1991). The Altruism Question. Mahwah: L. Erlbaum, Associates. ISBN 978-0-8058-0245-0.
  • Comte, Auguste (1891) [1852]. Catechisme positiviste (Catechism of Positivism). Translated by Congreve, R. London: Kegan Paul.
  • Dawkins, Richard (1989). The Selfish Gene. Oxford Oxfordshire: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-286092-7.
  • Fehr, E.; Fischbacher, U. (2003). "The nature of human altruism". Nature. 425 (6960): 785–791. Bibcode:2003Natur.425..785F. doi:10.1038/nature02043. PMID 14574401. S2CID 4305295.
  • Knox, T. (1999). "The volunteer's folly and socio-economic man: some thoughts on altruism, rationality, and community". Journal of Socio-Economics. 28 (4): 475–967. doi:10.1016/S1053-5357(99)00045-1.
  • Kropotkin, Peter (1902). Mutual Aid: A Factor of Evolution.
  • Madsen, E. A.; Tunney, R. J.; Fieldman, G.; Plotkin, H. C.; Dunbar, R. I. M.; Richardson, J. M.; McFarland, D. (2007). "Kinship and altruism: A cross-cultural experimental study". British Journal of Psychology. 98 (Pt 2): 339–359. doi:10.1348/000712606X129213. PMID 17456276. S2CID 18056028.
  • Monk-Turner, E.; Blake, V.; Chniel, F.; Forbes, S.; Lensey, L.; Madzuma, J. (2002). "Helping hands: A study of altruistic behavior". Gender Issues. 20 (4): 65–70. doi:10.1007/s12147-002-0024-2. S2CID 144977956.
  • Nietzsche, Friedrich (1886). Beyond Good and Evil.
  • Nowak, M. A. (2006). "Five Rules for the Evolution of Cooperation". Science. 314 (5805): 1560–1563. Bibcode:2006Sci...314.1560N. doi:10.1126/science.1133755. PMC 3279745. PMID 17158317.
  • Oliner, Samuel P.; Oliner, Pearl M. (1995). Towards a Caring Society: Ideas into Action. West Port, Conn.: Praeger.
  • Oord, Thomas (2004). Science of Love. Philadelphia: Templeton Foundation Press. ISBN 978-1-932031-70-6.
  • Oord, Thomas (2007). The Altruism Reader. Philadelphia: Templeton Foundation Press. ISBN 978-1-59947-127-3.
  • Oord, Thomas (2010). Defining Love. Grand Rapids: Brazos Press. ISBN 978-1-58743-257-6.
  • Proudhon, Pierre-Joseph (1847). The Philosophy of Poverty.
  • Ridley, Matt (1996). The Origins of Virtue.
  • Spooner, Lysander (1882). Natural Law.
  • Wedekind, C.; Milinski, M. (1996). "Human Cooperation in the simultaneous and the alternating Prisoner's Dilemma: Pavlov versus Generous Tit-for-tat". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 93 (7): 2686–2689. Bibcode:1996PNAS...93.2686W. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.7.2686. PMC 39691. PMID 11607644.
  • Wright, Robert (1995). The Moral Animal. New York: Vintage Books. ISBN 978-0-679-76399-4.

External links edit

  •   Media related to Altruism at Wikimedia Commons
  • Altruism on In Our Time at the BBC
  • "The Good Show". Radiolab. Season 9. Episode 1. New York. 14 December 2011. WNYC.
  • Kraut, Richard (2020). "Altruism". Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
  • Okasha, Samir (2013). "Biological Altruism". Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.

altruism, otherism, redirects, here, philosophical, concept, other, philosophy, concept, behavioral, ecology, biology, ethical, doctrine, ethics, principle, practice, concern, well, being, happiness, other, humans, animals, above, oneself, while, objects, altr. Otherism redirects here For the philosophical concept see Other philosophy For the concept in behavioral ecology see Altruism biology For the ethical doctrine see Altruism ethics Altruism is the principle and practice of concern for the well being and or happiness of other humans or animals above oneself While objects of altruistic concern vary it is an important moral value in many cultures and religions It may be considered a synonym of selflessness the opposite of selfishness Giving alms to the poor is often considered an altruistic action The word altruism was popularized and possibly coined by the French philosopher Auguste Comte in French as altruisme for an antonym of egoism 1 He derived it from the Italian altrui which in turn was derived from Latin alteri meaning other people or somebody else 2 Altruism as observed in populations of organisms is when an individual performs an action at a cost to themselves in terms of e g pleasure and quality of life time probability of survival or reproduction that benefits directly or indirectly another individual without the expectation of reciprocity or compensation for that action Altruism can be distinguished from feelings of loyalty or concern for the common good The latter are predicated upon social relationships whilst altruism does not consider relationships Whether true altruism is possible in human psychology is a subject of debate The theory of psychological egoism suggests that no act of sharing helping or sacrificing can be truly altruistic as the actor may receive an intrinsic reward in the form of personal gratification The validity of this argument depends on whether such intrinsic rewards qualify as benefits The term altruism may also refer to an ethical doctrine that claims that individuals are morally obliged to benefit others Used in this sense it is usually contrasted with egoism which claims individuals are morally obligated to serve themselves first Effective altruism is the use of evidence and reason to determine the most effective ways to benefit others Contents 1 The notion of altruism 2 Scientific viewpoints 2 1 Anthropology 2 2 Evolutionary explanations 2 2 1 Neurobiology 2 2 2 Psychology 2 2 2 1 Pathological altruism 2 3 Sociology 3 Religious viewpoints 3 1 Buddhism 3 2 Jainism 3 3 Christianity 3 4 Islam 3 5 Judaism 3 6 Sikhism 3 7 Hinduism 4 Philosophy 4 1 Effective altruism 5 Genetics 6 Digital altruism 7 See also 8 Notes 9 References 10 External linksThe notion of altruism editThe concept of altruism has a history in philosophical and ethical thought The term was coined in the 19th century by the founding sociologist and philosopher of science Auguste Comte and has become a major topic for psychologists especially evolutionary psychology researchers evolutionary biologists and ethologists Whilst ideas about altruism from one field can affect the other fields the different methods and focuses of these fields always lead to different perspectives on altruism In simple terms altruism is caring about the welfare of other people and acting to help them above oneself Scientific viewpoints editAnthropology edit This section needs expansion You can help by adding to it July 2023 See also Alms and Altruism ethics Marcel Mauss s essay The Gift contains a passage called Note on alms This note describes the evolution of the notion of alms and by extension of altruism from the notion of sacrifice In it he writes Alms are the fruits of a moral notion of the gift and of fortune on the one hand and of a notion of sacrifice on the other Generosity is an obligation because Nemesis avenges the poor and the gods for the superabundance of happiness and wealth of certain people who should rid themselves of it This is the ancient morality of the gift which has become a principle of justice The gods and the spirits accept that the share of wealth and happiness that has been offered to them and had been hitherto destroyed in useless sacrifices should serve the poor and children Evolutionary explanations edit Main articles Altruism biology Evolution of morality and Evolutionary ethics nbsp Giving alms to beggar childrenIn the Science of ethology the study of animal behaviour and more generally in the study of social evolution altruism refers to behavior by an individual that increases the fitness of another individual while decreasing the fitness of the actor 3 In evolutionary psychology this term may be applied to a wide range of human behaviors such as charity emergency aid help to coalition partners tipping courtship gifts production of public goods and environmentalism 4 Theories of apparently altruistic behavior were accelerated clarification needed by the need to produce ideas compatible with evolutionary origins Two related strands of research on altruism have emerged from traditional evolutionary analyses and evolutionary game theory a mathematical model and analysis of behavioral strategies Some of the proposed mechanisms are Kin selection 5 That animals and humans are more altruistic towards close kin than to distant kin and non kin has been confirmed in numerous studies across many different cultures Even subtle cues indicating kinship may unconsciously increase altruistic behavior One kinship cue is facial resemblance One study found that slightly altering photographs to resemble the faces of study participants more closely increased the trust the participants expressed regarding depicted persons Another cue is having the same family name especially if rare which has been found to increase helpful behavior Another study found more cooperative behavior the greater the number of perceived kin in a group Using kinship terms in political speeches increased audience agreement with the speaker in one study This effect was powerful for firstborns who are typically close to their families 4 Vested interests People are likely to suffer if their friends allies and those from similar social ingroups suffer or disappear Helping such group members may therefore also benefit the altruist Making ingroup membership more noticeable increases cooperativeness Extreme self sacrifice towards the ingroup may be adaptive if a hostile outgroup threatens the entire ingroup 4 Reciprocal altruism 6 See also Reciprocity evolution Direct reciprocity 7 Research shows that it can be beneficial to help others if there is a chance that they will reciprocate the help The effective tit for tat strategy is one game theoretic example Many people seem to be following a similar strategy by cooperating if and only if others cooperate in return 4 One consequence is that people are more cooperative with one another if they are more likely to interact again in the future People tend to be less cooperative if they perceive that the frequency of helpers in the population is lower They tend to help less if they see non cooperativeness by others and this effect tends to be stronger than the opposite effect of seeing cooperative behaviors Simply changing the cooperative framing of a proposal may increase cooperativeness such as calling it a Community Game instead of a Wall Street Game 4 A tendency towards reciprocity implies that people feel obligated to respond if someone helps them This has been used by charities that give small gifts to potential donors hoping to induce reciprocity Another method is to announce publicly that someone has given a large donation The tendency to reciprocate can even generalize so people become more helpful toward others after being helped On the other hand people will avoid or even retaliate against those perceived not to be cooperating People sometimes mistakenly fail to help when they intended to or their helping may not be noticed which may cause unintended conflicts As such it may be an optimal strategy to be slightly forgiving of and have a slightly generous interpretation of non cooperation 4 People are more likely to cooperate on a task if they can communicate with one another first This may be due to better cooperativeness assessments or promises exchange They are more cooperative if they can gradually build trust instead of being asked to give extensive help immediately Direct reciprocity and cooperation in a group can be increased by changing the focus and incentives from intra group competition to larger scale competitions such as between groups or against the general population Thus giving grades and promotions based only on an individual s performance relative to a small local group as is common may reduce cooperative behaviors in the group 4 Indirect reciprocity 8 Because people avoid poor reciprocators and cheaters a person s reputation is important A person esteemed for their reciprocity is more likely to receive assistance even from individuals they have not directly interacted with before 4 Strong reciprocity 9 This form of reciprocity is expressed by people who invest more resources in cooperation and punishment than what is deemed optimal based on established theories of altruism Pseudo reciprocity 10 An organism behaves altruistically and the recipient does not reciprocate but has an increased chance of acting in a way that is selfish but also as a byproduct benefits the altruist Costly signaling and the handicap principle 11 Altruism by diverting resources from the altruist can act as an honest signal of available resources and the skills to acquire them This may signal to others that the altruist is a valuable potential partner It may also signal interactive and cooperative intentions since someone who does not expect to interact further in the future gains nothing from such costly signaling While it s uncertain if costly signaling can predict long term cooperative traits people tend to trust helpers more Costly signaling loses its value when everyone shares identical traits resources and cooperative intentions but it gains significance as population variability in these aspects increases 4 Hunters who share meat display a costly signal of ability The research found that good hunters have higher reproductive success and more adulterous relations even if they receive no more of the hunted meat than anyone else Similarly holding large feasts and giving large donations are ways of demonstrating one s resources Heroic risk taking has also been interpreted as a costly signal of ability 4 nbsp Volunteers assist Hurricane victims at the Houston Astrodome following Hurricane Katrina Both indirect reciprocity and costly signaling depend on reputation value and tend to make similar predictions One is that people will be more helpful when they know that their helping behavior will be communicated to people they will interact with later publicly announced discussed or observed by someone else This has been documented in many studies The effect is sensitive to subtle cues such as people being more helpful when there were stylized eyespots instead of a logo on a computer screen dubious discuss Weak reputational cues such as eyespots may become unimportant if there are stronger cues present and may lose their effect with continued exposure unless reinforced with real reputational effects 4 Public displays such as public weeping for dead celebrities and participation in demonstrations may be influenced by a desire to be seen as generous People who know that they are publicly monitored sometimes even wastefully donate the money they know is not needed by the recipient because of reputational concerns 12 Typically women find altruistic men to be attractive partners When women look for a long term partner altruism may be a trait they prefer as it may indicate that the prospective partner is also willing to share resources with her and her children Men perform charitable acts in the early stages of a romantic relationship or simply when in the presence of an attractive woman While both sexes state that kindness is the most preferable trait in a partner there is some evidence that men place less value on this than women and that women may not be more altruistic in the presence of an attractive man Men may even avoid altruistic women in short term relationships which may be because they expect less success 4 12 People may compete for the social benefit of a burnished reputation which may cause competitive altruism On the other hand in some experiments a proportion of people do not seem to care about reputation and do not help more even if this is conspicuous This may be due to reasons such as psychopathy or that they are so attractive that they need not be seen as altruistic The reputational benefits of altruism occur in the future compared to the immediate costs of altruism While humans and other organisms generally place less value on future costs benefits as compared to those in the present some have shorter time horizons than others and these people tend to be less cooperative 4 Explicit extrinsic rewards and punishments have sometimes been found to have a counterintuitively inverse effect on behaviors when compared to intrinsic rewards This may be because such extrinsic incentives may replace partially or in whole intrinsic and reputational incentives motivating the person to focus on obtaining the extrinsic rewards which may make the thus incentivized behaviors less desirable People prefer altruism in others when it appears to be due to a personality characteristic rather than overt reputational concerns simply pointing out that there are reputational benefits of action may reduce them This may be used as a derogatory tactic against altruists you re just virtue signalling especially by those who are non cooperators A counterargument is that doing good due to reputational concerns is better than doing no good 4 Group selection It has controversially been argued by some evolutionary scientists such as David Sloan Wilson that natural selection can act at the level of non kin groups to produce adaptations that benefit a non kin group even if these adaptations are detrimental at the individual level Thus while altruistic persons may under some circumstances be outcompeted by less altruistic persons at the individual level according to group selection theory the opposite may occur at the group level where groups consisting of the more altruistic persons may outcompete groups consisting of the less altruistic persons Such altruism may only extend to ingroup members while directing prejudice and antagonism against outgroup members see also in group favoritism Many other evolutionary scientists have criticized group selection theory 13 nbsp Helping the homeless in New York CitySuch explanations do not imply that humans consciously calculate how to increase their inclusive fitness when doing altruistic acts Instead evolution has shaped psychological mechanisms such as emotions that promote certain altruistic behaviors 4 The benefits for the altruist may be increased and the costs reduced by being more altruistic towards certain groups Research has found that people are more altruistic to kin than to no kin to friends than strangers to those attractive than to those unattractive to non competitors than competitors and to members in groups than to members of out groups 4 The study of altruism was the initial impetus behind George R Price s development of the Price equation a mathematical equation used to study genetic evolution An interesting example of altruism is found in the cellular slime moulds such as Dictyostelium mucoroides These protists live as individual amoebae until starved at which point they aggregate and form a multicellular fruiting body in which some cells sacrifice themselves to promote the survival of other cells in the fruiting body 14 Selective investment theory proposes that close social bonds and associated emotional cognitive and neurohormonal mechanisms evolved to facilitate long term high cost altruism between those closely depending on one another for survival and reproductive success 15 Such cooperative behaviors have sometimes been seen as arguments for left wing politics for example by the Russian zoologist and anarchist Peter Kropotkin in his 1902 book Mutual Aid A Factor of Evolution and Moral Philosopher Peter Singer in his book A Darwinian Left Neurobiology edit See also Compassion Jorge Moll and Jordan Grafman neuroscientists at the National Institutes of Health and LABS D Or Hospital Network provided the first evidence for the neural bases of altruistic giving in normal healthy volunteers using functional magnetic resonance imaging In their research 16 they showed that both pure monetary rewards and charitable donations activated the mesolimbic reward pathway a primitive part of the brain that usually responds to food and sex However when volunteers generously placed the interests of others before their own by making charitable donations another brain circuit was selectively activated the subgenual cortex septal region These structures are related to vague social attachment and bonding in other species The experiment suggested that altruism is not a higher moral faculty overpowering innate selfish desires but a fundamental ingrained and enjoyable trait in the brain 17 One brain region the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex basal forebrain contributes to learning altruistic behavior especially in people with trait clarification needed empathy 18 The same study identified a link between vague giving to charity and the promotion clarification needed of social bonding 19 Bill Harbaugh a University of Oregon economist in an fMRI scanner test conducted with his psychologist colleague Dr Ulrich Mayr reached the same conclusions as Jorge Moll and Jordan Grafman about giving to charity although they were able to divide the study group into two groups egoists and altruists One of their discoveries was that though rarely even some of the considered egoists sometimes gave more than expected because that would help others leading to the conclusion that there are other factors in charity such as a person s environment and values 19 A recent meta analysis of fMRI studies conducted by Shawn Rhoads Jo Cutler and Abigail Marsh analyzed the results of prior studies of generosity in which participants could freely choose to give or not give resources to someone else 20 The results of this study confirmed that altruism is supported by distinct mechanisms from giving motivated by reciprocity or by fairness This study also confirmed that the right ventral striatum is recruited during altruistic giving as well as the ventromedial prefrontal cortex bilateral anterior cingulate cortex and bilateral anterior insula which are regions previously implicated in empathy Abigail Marsh has conducted studies of real world altruists that have also identified an important role for the amygdala in human altruism In real world altruists such as people who have donated kidneys to strangers the amygdala is larger than in typical adults Altruists amygdalas are also more responsive than those of typical adults to the sight of others distress which is thought to reflect an empathic response to distress 21 22 This structure may also be involved in altruistic choices due to its role in encoding the value of outcomes for others 23 This is consistent with the findings of research in non human animals which has identified neurons within the amygdala that specifically encode the value of others outcomes activity in which appears to drive altruistic choices in monkeys 24 25 Psychology edit The International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences defines psychological altruism as a motivational state to increase another s welfare Psychological altruism is contrasted with psychological egoism which refers to the motivation to increase one s welfare 26 In keeping with this research in real world altruists including altruistic kidney donors bone marrow donors humanitarian aid workers and heroic rescuers findings that these altruists are primarily distinguished from other adults by unselfish traits and decision making patterns This suggests that human altruism reflects genuinely high valuation of others outcomes 27 There has been some debate on whether humans are capable of psychological altruism 28 Some definitions specify a self sacrificial nature to altruism and a lack of external rewards for altruistic behaviors 29 However because altruism ultimately benefits the self in many cases the selflessness of altruistic acts is difficult to prove The social exchange theory postulates that altruism only exists when the benefits outweigh the costs to the self 30 Daniel Batson a psychologist examined this question and argued against the social exchange theory He identified four significant motives to ultimately benefit the self egoism to ultimately benefit the other person altruism to benefit a group collectivism or to uphold a moral principle principlism Altruism that ultimately serves selfish gains is thus differentiated from selfless altruism but the general conclusion has been that empathy induced altruism can be genuinely selfless 31 The empathy altruism hypothesis states that psychological altruism exists and is evoked by the empathic desire to help someone suffering Feelings of empathic concern are contrasted with personal distress which compels people to reduce their unpleasant emotions and increase their positive ones by helping someone in need Empathy is thus not selfless since altruism works either as a way to avoid those negative unpleasant feelings and have positive pleasant feelings when triggered by others need for help or as a way to gain social reward or avoid social punishment by helping People with empathic concern help others in distress even when exposure to the situation could be easily avoided whereas those lacking in empathic concern avoid allowing it unless it is difficult or impossible to avoid exposure to another s suffering 26 Helping behavior is seen in humans from about two years old when a toddler can understand subtle emotional cues 32 nbsp Peace Corps trainees swearing in as volunteers in Cambodia 4 April 2007In psychological research on altruism studies often observe altruism as demonstrated through prosocial behaviors such as helping comforting sharing cooperation philanthropy and community service 29 People are most likely to help if they recognize that a person is in need and feel personal responsibility for reducing the person s distress The number of bystanders witnessing pain or suffering affects the likelihood of helping the Bystander effect More significant numbers of bystanders decrease individual feelings of responsibility 26 33 However a witness with a high level of empathic concern is likely to assume personal responsibility entirely regardless of the number of bystanders 26 Many studies have observed the effects of volunteerism as a form of altruism on happiness and health and have consistently found that those who exhibit volunteerism also have better current and future health and well being 34 35 In a study of older adults those who volunteered had higher life satisfaction and will to live and less depression anxiety and somatization 36 Volunteerism and helping behavior have not only been shown to improve mental health but physical health and longevity as well attributable to the activity and social integration it encourages 34 37 38 One study examined the physical health of mothers who volunteered over 30 years and found that 52 of those who did not belong to a volunteer organization experienced a major illness while only 36 of those who did volunteer experienced one 39 A study on adults aged 55 and older found that during the four year study period people who volunteered for two or more organizations had a 63 lower likelihood of dying After controlling for prior health status it was determined that volunteerism accounted for a 44 reduction in mortality 40 Merely being aware of kindness in oneself and others is also associated with greater well being A study that asked participants to count each act of kindness they performed for one week significantly enhanced their subjective happiness Happier people are kinder and more grateful kinder people are happier and more grateful and more grateful people are happier and kinder the study suggests 41 While research supports the idea that altruistic acts bring about happiness it has also been found to work in the opposite direction that happier people are also kinder The relationship between altruistic behavior and happiness is bidirectional Studies found that generosity increases linearly from sad to happy affective states 42 Feeling over taxed by the needs of others has negative effects on health and happiness 38 For example one study on volunteerism found that feeling overwhelmed by others demands had an even stronger negative effect on mental health than helping had a positive one although positive effects were still significant 43 Pathological altruism edit Pathological altruism is altruism taken to an unhealthy extreme such that it either harms the altruistic person or the person s well intentioned actions cause more harm than good The term pathological altruism was popularised by the book Pathological Altruism Examples include depression and burnout seen in healthcare professionals an unhealthy focus on others to the detriment of one s own needs hoarding of animals and ineffective philanthropic and social programs that ultimately worsen the situations they are meant to aid 44 Sociology edit See also Public sociology Sociologists have long been concerned with how to build the good society 45 The structure of our societies and how individuals come to exhibit charitable philanthropic and other pro social altruistic actions for the common good is a commonly researched topic within the field The American Sociology Association ASA acknowledges public sociology saying The intrinsic scientific policy and public relevance of this field of investigation in helping to construct good societies is unquestionable 45 This type of sociology seeks contributions that aid popular and theoretical understandings of what motivates altruism and how it is organized and promotes an altruistic focus in order to benefit the world and people it studies How altruism is framed organized carried out and what motivates it at the group level is an area of focus that sociologists investigate in order to contribute back to the groups it studies and build the good society The motivation of altruism is also the focus of study for example one study links the occurrence of moral outrage to altruistic compensation of victims 46 Studies show that generosity in laboratory and in online experiments is contagious people imitate the generosity they observe in others 47 Religious viewpoints editSee also Evolutionary origin of religion Most if not all of the world s religions promote altruism as a very important moral value Buddhism Christianity Hinduism Islam Jainism Judaism and Sikhism etc place particular emphasis on altruistic morality Buddhism edit nbsp Monks collecting almsAltruism figures prominently in Buddhism Love and compassion are components of all forms of Buddhism and are focused on all beings equally love is the wish that all beings be happy and compassion is the wish that all beings be free from suffering Many illnesses can be cured by the one medicine of love and compassion These qualities are the ultimate source of human happiness and the need for them lies at the very core of our being Dalai Lama 48 49 The notion of altruism is modified in such a world view since the belief is that such a practice promotes the practitioner s own happiness The more we care for the happiness of others the greater our own sense of well being becomes Dalai Lama 48 In the context of larger ethical discussions on moral action and judgment Buddhism is characterized by the belief that negative unhappy consequences of our actions derive not from punishment or correction based on moral judgment but from the law of karma which functions like a natural law of cause and effect A simple illustration of such cause and effect is the case of experiencing the effects of what one causes if one causes suffering then as a natural consequence one would experience suffering if one causes happiness then as a natural consequence one would experience happiness citation needed Jainism edit See also Ahimsa in Jainism nbsp Sculpture depicting the Jain concept of ahimsa non injury The fundamental principles of Jainism revolve around altruism not only for ambiguous humans but for all sentient beings Jainism preaches ahimsa to live and let live not harming sentient beings i e uncompromising reverence for all life It also considers all living things to be equal specify The first Tirthankara Rishabhdev introduced the concept of altruism for all living beings from extending knowledge and experience to others to donation giving oneself up for others non violence and compassion for all living things citation needed The principle of nonviolence seeks to minimize karmas which limit the capabilities of the soul Jainism views every soul as worthy of respect because it has the potential to become Siddha God in Jainism Because all living beings possess a soul great care and awareness is essential in one s actions Jainism emphasizes the equality of all life advocating harmlessness towards all whether the creatures are great or small This policy extends even to microscopic organisms Jainism acknowledges that every person has different capabilities and capacities to practice and therefore accepts different levels of compliance for ascetics and householders citation needed Christianity edit Thomas Aquinas interprets You should love your neighbour as yourself 50 as meaning that love for ourselves is the exemplar of love for others 51 Considering that the love with which a man loves himself is the form and root of friendship he quotes Aristotle that the origin of friendly relations with others lies in our relations to ourselves 52 Aquinas concluded that though we are not bound to love others more than ourselves we naturally seek the common good the good of the whole more than any private good the good of a part However he thought we should love God more than ourselves and our neighbours and more than our bodily life since the ultimate purpose of loving our neighbour is to share in eternal beatitude a more desirable thing than bodily well being In coining the word altruism as stated above Comte was probably opposing this Thomistic doctrine which is present in some theological schools within Catholicism The aim and focus of Christian life is a life that glorifies God with obeying christ s command to treat others equally caring for them and understanding eternity in heaven is what Jesus Resurrection at calvary was all about Many biblical authors draw a strong connection between love of others and love of God 1 John 4 states that for one to love God one must love his fellowman and that hatred of one s fellowman is the same as hatred of God Thomas Jay Oord has argued in several books that altruism is but one possible form of love An altruistic action is not always a loving action Oord defines altruism as acting for the other s good and he agrees with feminists who note that sometimes love requires acting for one s own good when the other s demands undermine overall well being German philosopher Max Scheler distinguishes two ways in which the strong can help the weak One way is a sincere expression of Christian love motivated by a powerful feeling of security strength and inner salvation of the invincible fullness of one s own life and existence 53 88 89 Another way is merely one of the many modern substitutes for love nothing but the urge to turn away from oneself and to lose oneself in other people s business 53 95 96 At its worst Scheler says love for the small the poor the weak and the oppressed is really disguised hatred repressed envy an impulse to detract etc directed against the opposite phenomena wealth strength power largesse 53 96 97 Islam edit In the Arabic language iythar إيثار means preferring others to oneself 54 On the topic of donating blood to non Muslims a controversial topic within the faith the Shia religious professor Fadhil al Milani has provided theological evidence that makes it positively justifiable In fact he considers it a form of religious sacrifice and ithar altruism 55 For Sufis iythar means devotion to others through complete forgetfulness of one s own concerns where concern for others is deemed as a demand made by God on the human body considered to be property of God alone The importance of iythar aka ithar lies in sacrifice for the sake of the greater good Islam considers those practicing ithar as abiding by the highest degree of nobility 56 This is similar to the notion of chivalry A constant concern for God results in a careful attitude towards people animals and other things in this world 57 Judaism edit Judaism defines altruism as the desired goal of creation Rabbi Abraham Isaac Kook stated that love is the most important attribute in humanity 58 Love is defined as bestowal or giving which is the intention of altruism This can be altruism towards humanity that leads to altruism towards the creator or God Kabbalah defines God as the force of giving in existence Rabbi Moshe Chaim Luzzatto focused on the purpose of creation and how the will of God was to bring creation into perfection and adhesion with this force of giving 59 Modern Kabbalah developed by Rabbi Yehuda Ashlag in his writings about the future generation focuses on how society could achieve an altruistic social framework 60 120 130 Ashlag proposed that such a framework is the purpose of creation and everything that happens is to raise humanity to the level of altruism love for one another Ashlag focused on society and its relation to divinity 60 175 180 Sikhism edit Altruism is essential to the Sikh religion The central faith in Sikhism is that the greatest deed anyone can do is to imbibe and live the godly qualities like love affection sacrifice patience harmony and truthfulness Seva or selfless service to the community for its own sake is an important concept in Sikhism 61 The fifth Guru Arjun Dev sacrificed his life to uphold 22 carats of pure truth the greatest gift to humanity the Guru Granth The ninth Guru Tegh Bahadur sacrificed his head to protect weak and defenseless people against atrocity In the late seventeenth century Guru Gobind Singh the tenth Guru in Sikhism was at war with the Mughal rulers to protect the people of different faiths when a fellow Sikh Bhai Kanhaiya attended the troops of the enemy 62 He gave water to both friends and foes who were wounded on the battlefield Some of the enemy began to fight again and some Sikh warriors were annoyed by Bhai Kanhaiya as he was helping their enemy Sikh soldiers brought Bhai Kanhaiya before Guru Gobind Singh and complained of his action that they considered counterproductive to their struggle on the battlefield What were you doing and why asked the Guru I was giving water to the wounded because I saw your face in all of them replied Bhai Kanhaiya The Guru responded Then you should also give them ointment to heal their wounds You were practicing what you were coached in the house of the Guru Under the tutelage of the Guru Bhai Kanhaiya subsequently founded a volunteer corps for altruism which is still engaged today in doing good to others and in training new recruits for this service 63 Hinduism edit In Hinduism Selflessness Atmatyag Love Prema Kindness Daya and Forgiveness Kshama are considered as the highest acts of humanity or Manushyattva Giving alms to the beggars or poor people is considered as a divine act or Punya and Hindus believe it will free their souls from guilt or Paapa and will led them to heaven or Swarga in afterlife Altruism is also the central act of various Hindu mythology and religious poems and songs Mass donation of clothes to poor people Vastraseva or blood donation camp or mass food donation Annaseva for poor people is common in various Hindu religious ceremonies citation needed The Bhagavad Gita supports the doctrine of karma yoga achieving oneness with God through action amp Nishkam Karma or action without expectation desire for personal gain which can be said to encompass altruism Altruistic acts are generally celebrated and very well received in Hindu literature and are central to Hindu morality 64 Philosophy editMain article Altruism ethics This section does not cite any sources Please help improve this section by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed July 2023 Learn how and when to remove this template message There is a wide range of philosophical views on humans obligations or motivations to act altruistically Proponents of ethical altruism maintain that individuals are morally obligated to act altruistically 65 The opposing view is ethical egoism which maintains that moral agents should always act in their own self interest Both ethical altruism and ethical egoism contrast with utilitarianism which maintains that each agent should act in order to maximise the efficacy of their function and the benefit to both themselves and their co inhabitants A related concept in descriptive ethics is psychological egoism the thesis that humans always act in their own self interest and that true altruism is impossible Rational egoism is the view that rationality consists in acting in one s self interest without specifying how this affects one s moral obligations Effective altruism edit See also Effective altruism Earning to give and Giving What We Can Effective altruism is a philosophy and social movement that uses evidence and reasoning to determine the most effective ways to benefit others 66 Effective altruism encourages individuals to consider all causes and actions and to act in the way that brings about the greatest positive impact based upon their values 67 It is the broad evidence based and cause neutral approach that distinguishes effective altruism from traditional altruism or charity 68 Effective altruism is part of the larger movement towards evidence based practices While a substantial proportion of effective altruists have focused on the nonprofit sector the philosophy of effective altruism applies more broadly to prioritizing the scientific projects companies and policy initiatives which can be estimated to save lives help people or otherwise have the biggest benefit 69 People associated with the movement include philosopher Peter Singer 70 Facebook co founder Dustin Moskovitz 71 Cari Tuna 72 Oxford based researchers William MacAskill 73 and Toby Ord 74 and professional poker player Liv Boeree 75 Genetics editOXTR CD38 COMT DRD4 DRD5 IGF2 and GABRB2 are candidate genes for influencing altruistic behavior 76 further explanation needed Digital altruism editDigital altruism is the notion that some are willing to freely share information based on the principle of reciprocity and in the belief that in the end everyone benefits from sharing information via the Internet 77 There are three types of digital altruism 1 everyday digital altruism involving expedience ease moral engagement and conformity 2 creative digital altruism involving creativity heightened moral engagement and cooperation and 3 co creative digital altruism involving creativity moral engagement and meta cooperative efforts 77 See also editAltruria California Utopian commune in California Charitable organization Nonprofit organization with charitable purpose Consideration Concept in the common law of contracts Egotism Drive to maintain and enhance favorable views of oneself Family economics Application of economic concepts to the study of the family Gene centered view of evolution Theory of the selfish gene Golden Rule Principle of treating others as one wants to be treated Humanity virtue Virtue linked with basic ethics Misanthropy General dislike of humanity Mutual aid organization theory Voluntary exchange of resources and services for mutual benefit Non nobis solum Latin for not for ourselves alone Philanthropy Private efforts to increase public good Prisoner s dilemma Standard example in game theory Prosocial behavior Intent to benefit others Random act of kindness Nonpremeditated act to cheer up another Reciprocal altruism Form of behaviour between organisms Sign of contradiction Person who experiences strong hostility due to divine connection as defined in Catholic theology Social preferences Human tendency to care about social outcomes Social psychology Study of social effects on people s thoughts feelings and behaviors Social support Support systems for individuals Solidarity sociology Unity of feeling or action on a common interestPages displaying short descriptions of redirect targets Spite game theory Tragedy of the commons Self interests causing depletion of a shared resourceNotes edit altruism n Online Etymology Dictionary Douglas Harper Retrieved 27 May 2021 Teske Nathan 2009 Political Activists in America The Identity Construction Model of Political Participation University Park Pa Pennsylvania State University Press p 101 ISBN 9780271035468 Ciciloni Ferdinando 1825 A Grammar of the Italian Language London John Murray p 64 Bell Graham 2008 Selection the mechanism of evolution Oxford Oxford University Press pp 367 368 ISBN 978 0 19 856972 5 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Barcaly Pat 2011 The evolution of charitable behaviour and the power of reputation In Roberts S Craig ed Applied Evolutionary Psychology Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199586073 001 0001 ISBN 9780199586073 Okasha Samir 2013 Kin Selection and Inclusive Fitness Biological Altruism Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Trivers R L 1971 The evolution of reciprocal altruism Quarterly Review of Biology 46 35 57 doi 10 1086 406755 S2CID 19027999 Axelrod R Hamilton W D 27 March 1981 The evolution of cooperation Science 211 4489 1390 1396 Bibcode 1981Sci 211 1390A CiteSeerX 10 1 1 147 9644 doi 10 1126 science 7466396 PMID 7466396 Nowak Martin Sigmund Karl October 2005 Evolution of indirect reciprocity PDF Nature 437 27 1291 1298 Bibcode 2005Natur 437 1291N doi 10 1038 nature04131 PMID 16251955 S2CID 3153895 Gintis Herbert September 2000 Strong Reciprocity and Human Sociality Journal of Theoretical Biology 206 2 169 179 Bibcode 2000JThBi 206 169G CiteSeerX 10 1 1 335 7226 doi 10 1006 jtbi 2000 2111 hdl 10419 105717 PMID 10966755 S2CID 9260305 Hammerstein Peter ed 2003 By product Benefits Reciprocity and Pseudoreciprocity in Mutualism Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation MIT Press pp 203 222 doi 10 7551 mitpress 3232 003 0013 ISBN 978 0 262 08326 3 Zahavi A 1995 Altruism as a handicap The limitations of kin selection and reciprocity Journal of Avian Biology 26 1 1 3 doi 10 2307 3677205 JSTOR 3677205 a b Iredal Wendy van Vugt Mark 2011 Altruism as showing off a signaling perspective on promoting green behavior and acts of kindness In Roberts S Craig ed Applied Evolutionary Psychology Oxford University Press doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199586073 001 0001 ISBN 9780199586073 Neyfakh Leon 17 April 2011 Where does good come from Boston Globe Archived from the original on 19 July 2011 Hudson Richard Ellis Aukema Juliann Eve Rispe Claude Roze Denis July 2002 Altruism cheating and anticheater adaptations in cellular slime molds The American Naturalist 160 1 31 43 doi 10 1086 340613 ISSN 1537 5323 PMID 18707497 Retrieved 1 February 2024 Brown S L Brown R M 2006 Selective investment theory Recasting the functional significance of close relationships PDF Psychological Inquiry 17 1 29 doi 10 1207 s15327965pli1701 01 S2CID 144718661 Archived from the original PDF on 14 July 2014 Moll Jorge Krueger Frank Zahn Roland Pardini Matteo de Oliveira Souza Ricardo Grafman Jordan 17 October 2006 Human fronto mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103 42 15623 15628 Bibcode 2006PNAS 10315623M doi 10 1073 pnas 0604475103 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 1622872 PMID 17030808 Vedantam Shankar 28 May 2007 If It Feels Good to Be Good It Might Be Only Natural The Washington Post Retrieved 23 April 2010 Lockwood Patricia L Apps Matthew A J Valton Vincent Viding Essi Roiser Jonathan P 2016 Neurocomputational mechanisms of prosocial learning and links to empathy Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113 35 9763 8 Bibcode 2016PNAS 113 9763L doi 10 1073 pnas 1603198113 PMC 5024617 PMID 27528669 fMRI revealed that activity in a posterior portion of the subgenual anterior cingulate cortex basal forebrain sgACC drives learning only when we are acting in a prosocial contextLay summary in Finding the Brain s Generosity Center Neuroscience News 15 August 2016 a b Svoboda Elizabeth 5 September 2013 Scientists Are Finding That We Are Hard Wired for Giving University of Notre Dame Retrieved 7 August 2017 Rhoads S A 2021 A feature based network analysis and fMRI meta analysis reveal three distinct types of prosocial decisions academic oup com Retrieved 30 December 2023 Marsh Abigail A Stoycos Sarah A Brethel Haurwitz Kristin M Robinson Paul VanMeter John W Cardinale Elise M 21 October 2014 Neural and cognitive characteristics of extraordinary altruists Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 111 42 15036 15041 doi 10 1073 pnas 1408440111 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 4210306 PMID 25225374 Brethel Haurwitz Kristin M O Connell Katherine Cardinale Elise M Stoianova Maria Stoycos Sarah A Lozier Leah M VanMeter John W Marsh Abigail A 25 October 2017 Amygdala midbrain connectivity indicates a role for the mammalian parental care system in human altruism Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 284 1865 20171731 doi 10 1098 rspb 2017 1731 ISSN 0962 8452 PMC 5666102 PMID 29070724 Rhoads Shawn A O Connell Katherine Berluti Kathryn Ploe Montana L Elizabeth Hannah S Amormino Paige Li Joanna L Dutton Mary Ann VanMeter Ashley Skye Marsh Abigail A 13 June 2023 Neural responses underlying extraordinary altruists generosity for socially distant others PNAS Nexus 2 7 pgad199 doi 10 1093 pnasnexus pgad199 ISSN 2752 6542 PMC 10321390 PMID 37416875 Dal Monte Olga Chu Cheng C J Fagan Nicholas A Chang Steve W C April 2020 Specialized medial prefrontal amygdala coordination in other regarding decision preference Nature Neuroscience 23 4 565 574 doi 10 1038 s41593 020 0593 y hdl 2318 1730693 ISSN 1546 1726 Putnam P T 2023 Dissociation of vicarious and experienced rewards by coupling frequency within the same neural pathway Neuron 111 16 2513 2522 a b c d Darity William A Jr ed 2008 Altruism International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences Vol 1 2nd ed Detroit Macmillan Reference USA pp 87 88 Rhoads Shawn A Vekaria Kruti M O Connell Katherine Elizabeth Hannah S Rand David G Kozak Williams Megan N Marsh Abigail A 31 March 2023 Unselfish traits and social decision making patterns characterize six populations of real world extraordinary altruists Nature Communications 14 1 1807 doi 10 1038 s41467 023 37283 5 ISSN 2041 1723 PMC 10066349 PMID 37002205 Batson C 2011 Altruism in humans New York N Y U S Oxford University Press a b Batson C Daniel 2012 A history of prosocial behavior research In Kruglanski Arie W Stroebe Wolfgang eds Handbook of the history of social psychology New York NY Psychology Press pp 243 264 ISBN 9781848728684 Maner J K Luce C L Neuberg S L Cialdini R B Brown S Sagarin B J 2002 The effects of perspective taking on motivations for helping Still no evidence for altruism Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 28 11 1601 1610 doi 10 1177 014616702237586 S2CID 146125891 Batson C Daniel Ahmad Nadia Stocks E L 2011 Four forms of prosocial motivation Egoism altruism collectivism and principlism In Dunning David ed Social motivation New York NY Psychology Press pp 103 126 ISBN 9781136847202 Svetlova M Nichols S R Brownell C A 2010 Toddlers prosocial behavior From instrumental to empathic to altruistic helping Child Development 81 6 1814 1827 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2010 01512 x PMC 3088085 PMID 21077866 Hudson James M Bruckman Amy S 2004 The Bystander Effect A Lens for Understanding Patterns of Participation Journal of the Learning Sciences 13 2 165 195 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 72 4881 doi 10 1207 s15327809jls1302 2 S2CID 16442298 a b Musick M A Wilson J 2003 Volunteering and depression The role of psychological and social resources in different age groups Social Science amp Medicine 56 2 259 269 doi 10 1016 S0277 9536 02 00025 4 PMID 12473312 Koenig L B McGue M Krueger R F Bouchard 2007 Religiousness antisocial behavior and altruism Genetic and environmental mediation Journal of Personality 75 2 265 290 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 2007 00439 x PMID 17359239 Hunter K I Hunter M W 1980 Psychosocial differences between elderly volunteers and non volunteers The International Journal of Aging amp Human Development 12 3 205 213 doi 10 2190 0H6V QPPP 7JK4 LR38 PMID 7216525 S2CID 42991434 Kayloe J C Krause M 1985 RARE FIND or The value of volunteerism Psychosocial Rehabilitation Journal 8 4 49 56 doi 10 1037 h0099659 Brown S L Brown R House J S Smith D M 2008 Coping with spousal loss Potential buffering effects of self reported helping behavior Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 34 6 849 861 doi 10 1177 0146167208314972 PMID 18344495 S2CID 42983453 a b Post S G 2005 Altruism Happiness and Health It s Good to Be Good International Journal of Behavioral Medicine 12 2 66 77 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 485 8406 doi 10 1207 s15327558ijbm1202 4 PMID 15901215 S2CID 12544814 Moen P Dempster Mcclain D Williams R M 1992 Successful aging A life course perspective on women s multiple roles and health American Journal of Sociology 97 6 1612 1638 doi 10 1086 229941 S2CID 4828775 Oman D Thoresen C E McMahon K 1999 Volunteerism and mortality among the community dwelling elderly Journal of Health Psychology 4 3 301 316 doi 10 1177 135910539900400301 PMID 22021599 Otake K Shimai S Tanaka Matsumi J Otsui K Fredrickson B L 2006 Happy people become happier through kindness A counting kindnesses intervention Journal of Happiness Studies 7 3 361 375 doi 10 1007 s10902 005 3650 z PMC 1820947 PMID 17356687 Underwood B Froming W J Moore B S 1977 Mood attention and altruism A search for mediating variables Developmental Psychology 13 5 541 542 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 13 5 541 Schwartz C Meisenhelder J Ma Y Reed G 2003 Altruistic Social Interest Behaviors Are Associated With Better Mental Health Psychosomatic Medicine 65 5 778 785 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 529 7780 doi 10 1097 01 PSY 0000079378 39062 D4 PMID 14508020 S2CID 20644442 Pathological Altruism Oxford University Press 19 December 2011 ISBN 9780199738571 a b Altruism Morality and Social Solidarity American Sociological Association Archived from the original on 3 May 2012 Thulin E W Bicchieri C 2016 I m so angry I could help you Moral outrage as a driver of victim compensation Social Philosophy amp Policy 32 2 146 160 doi 10 1017 S0265052516000145 S2CID 148548711 Tsvetkova Milena Macy Michael 2015 The Contagion of Prosocial Behavior and the Emergence of Voluntary Contribution Communities Social Phenomena From Data Analysis to Models Springer International Publishing pp 117 134 doi 10 1007 978 3 319 14011 7 7 ISBN 978 3 319 14010 0 Tagiew Rustam Ignatov Dmitry 2016 Gift Ratios in Laboratory Experiments PDF CEUR Workshop Proceedings 1627 82 93 Archived PDF from the original on 9 October 2022 a b The Medicine of Altruism Archived from the original on 3 October 2009 The phrase core of our being is Freudian see Bettina Bock von Wulfingen 2013 Freud s Core of our Being Between Cytology and Psychoanalysis Berichte zur Wissenschaftsgeschichte 36 3 226 244 doi 10 1002 bewi 201301604 PMID 32545937 Leviticus 19 and Matthew 22 Thomas Aquinas Summa Theologica II II Quaestio 25 Article 4 Aristotle Nicomachean Ethics IX 4 1166a1 a b c Scheler Max 1961 Ressentiment iythar Google Translate Schmiedel Ulrich Smith Graeme 15 February 2018 Religion in the European Refugee Crisis Springer pp 295 96 ISBN 978 3 319 67961 7 Gulen M Fethullah 2004 Key Concepts in the Practice of Sufism Emerald Hills of the Heart Rutherford N J Fountain pp 10 11 ISBN 978 1 932099 75 1 Neusner Jacob Eds 2005 Altruism in World Religions Washington D C Georgetown Univ Press pp 79 80 ISBN 978 1 58901 065 9 Kook Abraham Isaac Bokser Ben Zion 1978 Abraham Isaac Kook The lights of penitence The moral principles Lights of holiness essays letters and poems Paulist Press pp 135 136 ISBN 978 0 8091 2159 5 Luzzatto Moshe Ḥayyim 1997 The way of God Feldheim Publishers pp 37 38 ISBN 978 0 87306 769 0 a b Ashlag Yehuda 2006 Building the Future Society Thornhill Canada Laitman Kabbalah Publishers ISBN 978 965 7065 34 1 Cole W Owen Sambhi Piara Singh 1990 A Popular Dictionary of Sikhism Curzon Press pp 38 39 84 Retrieved 2 December 2018 Cunningham Joseph Davey 1918 A History of the Sikhs Oxford University Press p ix Retrieved 30 November 2018 Ralhan O P 1997 The great gurus of the Sikhs New Delhi Anmol Publications Pvt Ltd p 253 ISBN 978 81 7488 479 4 Sivananda Swami Phaladhikaranam Topic 8 Sutras 38 41 Kraut Richard 2020 Altruism in Zalta Edward N ed The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Fall 2020 ed Metaphysics Research Lab Stanford University retrieved 16 January 2024 MacAskill William 31 January 2017 Effective Altruism Introduction Essays in Philosophy 18 1 2 doi 10 7710 1526 0569 1580 ISSN 1526 0569 Matthews Dylan 24 April 2015 You have 8 billion You want to do as much good as possible What do you do Vox Retrieved 27 April 2015 Bennett Nicole Carter Ashley Resney Romney Woods Wendy How Tech Entrepreneurs Are Disrupting Philanthropy BCG Perspectives Boston Consulting Group Retrieved 11 March 2017 MacAskill William 2015 Doing Good Better Avery ISBN 978 1592409105 Walters Helen 19 September 2013 The why and how of effective altruism Peter Singer s talk visualized TED Blog Cari Tuna and Dustin Moskovitz Young Silicon Valley billionaires pioneer new approach to philanthropy The Washington Post 26 December 2014 Callahan Favid 12 September 2013 Meet Cari Tuna the Woman Giving Away Dustin Moskovitz s Facebook Fortune Inside Philanthropy Retrieved 1 March 2018 Thompson Derek 15 June 2015 The Greatest Good The Atlantic Peter Singer The Most Good You Can Do Talks at Google YouTube News Liv Boeree on Effective Altruism www pokerstrategy com Retrieved 11 April 2017 Effective Altruism Liv Boeree www livboeree com Archived from the original on 11 April 2017 Retrieved 11 April 2017 Thompson GJ Hurd PL Crespi BJ 2013 Genes underlying altruism Biol Lett 9 6 20130395 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2013 0395 PMC 3871336 PMID 24132092 a b Klisanin Dana 2011 Is the Internet Giving Rise to New Forms of Altruism PDF Media Psychology Review 3 1 1 11 Archived from the original PDF on 9 October 2022 References editAxelrod Robert 1984 The Evolution of Cooperation New York Basic Books ISBN 978 0 465 02121 5 Batson Charles 1991 The Altruism Question Mahwah L Erlbaum Associates ISBN 978 0 8058 0245 0 Comte Auguste 1891 1852 Catechisme positiviste Catechism of Positivism Translated by Congreve R London Kegan Paul Dawkins Richard 1989 The Selfish Gene Oxford Oxfordshire Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 286092 7 Fehr E Fischbacher U 2003 The nature of human altruism Nature 425 6960 785 791 Bibcode 2003Natur 425 785F doi 10 1038 nature02043 PMID 14574401 S2CID 4305295 Knox T 1999 The volunteer s folly and socio economic man some thoughts on altruism rationality and community Journal of Socio Economics 28 4 475 967 doi 10 1016 S1053 5357 99 00045 1 Kropotkin Peter 1902 Mutual Aid A Factor of Evolution Madsen E A Tunney R J Fieldman G Plotkin H C Dunbar R I M Richardson J M McFarland D 2007 Kinship and altruism A cross cultural experimental study British Journal of Psychology 98 Pt 2 339 359 doi 10 1348 000712606X129213 PMID 17456276 S2CID 18056028 Monk Turner E Blake V Chniel F Forbes S Lensey L Madzuma J 2002 Helping hands A study of altruistic behavior Gender Issues 20 4 65 70 doi 10 1007 s12147 002 0024 2 S2CID 144977956 Nietzsche Friedrich 1886 Beyond Good and Evil Nowak M A 2006 Five Rules for the Evolution of Cooperation Science 314 5805 1560 1563 Bibcode 2006Sci 314 1560N doi 10 1126 science 1133755 PMC 3279745 PMID 17158317 Oliner Samuel P Oliner Pearl M 1995 Towards a Caring Society Ideas into Action West Port Conn Praeger Oord Thomas 2004 Science of Love Philadelphia Templeton Foundation Press ISBN 978 1 932031 70 6 Oord Thomas 2007 The Altruism Reader Philadelphia Templeton Foundation Press ISBN 978 1 59947 127 3 Oord Thomas 2010 Defining Love Grand Rapids Brazos Press ISBN 978 1 58743 257 6 Proudhon Pierre Joseph 1847 The Philosophy of Poverty Ridley Matt 1996 The Origins of Virtue Spooner Lysander 1882 Natural Law Wedekind C Milinski M 1996 Human Cooperation in the simultaneous and the alternating Prisoner s Dilemma Pavlov versus Generous Tit for tat Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 93 7 2686 2689 Bibcode 1996PNAS 93 2686W doi 10 1073 pnas 93 7 2686 PMC 39691 PMID 11607644 Wright Robert 1995 The Moral Animal New York Vintage Books ISBN 978 0 679 76399 4 External links edit nbsp Look up altruism in Wiktionary the free dictionary nbsp Wikiquote has quotations related to Altruism nbsp Media related to Altruism at Wikimedia Commons Altruism on In Our Time at the BBC The Good Show Radiolab Season 9 Episode 1 New York 14 December 2011 WNYC Kraut Richard 2020 Altruism Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Okasha Samir 2013 Biological Altruism Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Altruism amp oldid 1204258962, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.