fbpx
Wikipedia

Scaly-breasted munia

The scaly-breasted munia or spotted munia (Lonchura punctulata), known in the pet trade as nutmeg mannikin or spice finch, is a sparrow-sized estrildid finch native to tropical Asia. A species of the genus Lonchura, it was formally described and named by Carl Linnaeus in 1758. Its name is based on the distinct scale-like feather markings on the breast and belly. The adult is brown above and has a dark conical bill. The species has 11 subspecies across its range, which differ slightly in size and color.

Scaly-breasted munia
L. p. punctulata (India)
Scientific classification
Domain: Eukaryota
Kingdom: Animalia
Phylum: Chordata
Class: Aves
Order: Passeriformes
Family: Estrildidae
Genus: Lonchura
Species:
L. punctulata
Binomial name
Lonchura punctulata
Native range
Synonyms
  • Loxia punctulata Linnaeus, 1758

This munia eats mainly grass seeds apart from berries and small insects. They forage in flocks and communicate with soft calls and whistles. The species is highly social and may sometimes roost with other species of munias. This species is found in tropical plains and grasslands. Breeding pairs construct dome-shaped nests using grass or bamboo leaves.

The species is endemic to Asia and occurs from India and Sri Lanka east to Indonesia and the Philippines (where it is called mayang pakíng). It has been introduced into many other parts of the world, and feral populations have established in Puerto Rico and Hispaniola, as well as parts of Australia, and the United States of America, with sightings in California. The bird is listed as of least concern by the International Union for Conservation of Nature (IUCN).

Taxonomy edit

In 1743 the English naturalist George Edwards included an illustration and a description of the scaly-breasted munia in the first volume of his A Natural History of Uncommon Birds. He used the English name "Gowry Bird". Edwards based his hand-coloured etching on a specimen at the London home of Charles du Bois, treasurer to the East India Company.[2] When in 1758 the Swedish naturalist Carl Linnaeus updated his Systema Naturae for the tenth edition, he placed the scaly-breasted munia with the crossbills in the genus Loxia. Linnaeus included a brief description, coined the binomial name Loxia punctulata and cited Edwards' work.[3] Linnaeus specified the locality as "Asia" but this was restricted to Kolkata (Calcutta) by E. C. Stuart Baker in 1926.[4][5] The species is now placed in the genus Lonchura that was introduced by the English naturalist William Henry Sykes in 1832.[6][7] The genus name Lonchura combines the Ancient Greek lonkhē meaning "spear-head" or "lance" with oura meaning "tail". The specific epithet is from Modern Latin punctulatus meaning "spotted" or "dotted".[8]

Over its large range there are 11 recognised subspecies. These include the nominate form found in the plains of the Indian Subcontinent, including Pakistan, India, Iran, Nepal, Bangladesh and Sri Lanka. The name lineoventer was formerly used for the Indian population. Other populations include subundulata from the eastern Himalayas, yunnanensis of southern China, topela of Thailand, cabanisi of the Philippines and fretensis of Singapore and Sumatra. Island populations include nisoria (Java, Bali, Lombok, Sumbawa), particeps (Sulawesi), baweana (Bawean Island), sumbae (Sumba) and blasii (Flores, Timor and Tanimbar).[7]

The subspecies holmesi (southeast Borneo) is sometimes recognised.[9]

Description edit

 
juvenile L. p. punctulata, Sri Lanka

The scaly-breasted munia is about 11–12 centimetres (4.3–4.7 in) long and weighs 12–16 grams (0.026–0.035 lb). The adult has a stubby dark bill typical of grain eating birds, brown upperparts and a dark brown head. The underparts are white with dark scale markings. The sexes are similar, although males have darker markings on the underside and a darker throat than females.[10]

Immature birds have pale brown upperparts, lack the dark head found in adults, and have uniform buff underparts that can be confused with juveniles of other munia species such as the tricolored munia (Lonchura malacca) across the Asian and island populations and the black-throated munia (Lonchura kelaarti) in parts of India or Sri Lanka.[10][11]

Distribution and habitat edit

 
The scaly-breasted munia (subspecies topela[12]) has established in parts of eastern Australia such as Queensland

Scaly-breasted munias are found in a range of habitats but are usually close to water and grassland. In India, they are especially common in paddy fields where they are considered a minor pest on account of their feeding on grain. They are found mainly on the plains, but can be observed in the foothills of the Himalayas, in which they may be present at altitudes near 2,500 m (1.6 mi), and in the Nilgiris, where they are found at altitudes up to 2,100 m (6,900 ft) during the summer. In Pakistan, they are restricted to a narrow region from Swat in the west to Lahore, avoiding the desert zone, and then occurring again in India east of an area between Ludhiana and Mount Abu.[13] The species has also been observed in Kashmir, though this is rare.[14][15]

Outside their native range, escaped birds frequently establish themselves in areas with a suitable climate and can then colonize new areas nearby. Escaped cage-birds established in the wild and such populations have been recorded in the West Indies (Puerto Rico since 1971),[16] Hawaii (since 1883[17]),[18] Japan[19] and southern United States, mainly in Florida and California.[20][21] In Oahu, Hawaii, they compete for habitats with the tricolored munia and tend to be rare where this competitor is present.[18] The species has been introduced to other parts of the world due to its popularity as a cage bird and populations have established in the wild.[22][23]

Behaviour and ecology edit

Sociality edit

Scaly-breasted munias form flocks of as many as 100 birds. Individuals communicate with calls that include a short whistle, variations of kitty-kitty-kitty, and a sharp chipping alarm note.[11][23] They sometimes flick their tails and wings vertically or horizontally while hopping about. The tail flicking motion may have evolved from a locomotory intention movement. The exaggerated version of the tail flicking movement may have undergone ritualization. As a social signal, tail flicking in several other species acts as a signal indicating the intent to fly and helps keep flocks together.[23][24]

When roosting communally, scaly-breasted munia sit side by side in close contact with each other. The outermost bird often jostles towards the center. Birds in a flock sometimes preen each other, with the soliciting bird usually showing its chin. Allopreening is usually limited to the face and neck.[23] The scaly-breasted munia is rarely hostile but birds will sometimes quarrel without any ritualized posturing.[23]

Breeding edit

 
Egg of scaly-breasted munia

The breeding season is during the summer rainy season (mainly June to August and also in October season in India) but can vary. Laboratory studies have found that long day illumination and high humidity trigger gonadal growth.[25] The song of the male is very soft but complex and variable, audible only at close range. This song described as a jingle consists of a series of high notes followed by a croaky rattle and ending in a slurred whistle. When singing the male sits in what is called the slope posture—erect with the head feathers raised.[23]

There are two types of slope posture, a pre-copulatory one and an ordinary one. The pre-copulatory behavior of scaly-breasted munia includes a sequence of actions. The first involves either the male or female playing with nest-material. As soon as a bird has arranged the nest material in its bill, it begins to fly around in a zigzag path. Once a bird lands close to its partner, the male bends towards the female and wipes its bill. The male then sings with movements of the body. The female invites mounting with tail quivering.[11][23] The nest is a large domed structure loosely woven from blades of grass, bamboo or other leaves with a side entrance and is placed in a tree or under the eaves of a house. A study in southern India found the preferred nesting trees to be Toddalia asiatica, Gymnosporia montana and Acacia chundra, especially short and bushy ones in areas with low canopy cover. The nest opening is located to face downwind of the most frequent wind direction.[26] In northern India, they preferred isolated Acacia nilotica in non-urban areas but used Thuja orientalis and Polyalthia longifolia in urban gardens.[27]

Scaly-breasted munia clutches usually contain 4 to 6 eggs, but can contain up to 10. Both sexes build the nest and incubate the eggs, which hatch in 10 to 16 days.[14][28]

Food and foraging edit

 
Adult feeding young

The scaly-breasted munia feeds mainly on grass seeds, small berries such as those of Lantana and insects.[29] Although the bill is suited for crushing small grains, they do not show lateral movements of the lower mandible which help European greenfinches in dehusking seeds.[30] Like some other munias, they may also feed on algae, a rich protein source, prior to the breeding season.[31]

The ease of maintaining these birds in captivity has made them popular for studying behavior and physiology. Feeding behavior can be predicted by the optimal foraging theory, where animals minimize time and energy spent to maximize food intake. This theory has been tested by studying the strategies used by scaly-breasted munias to increase their feeding efficacy.[32]

Flock size tradeoffs edit

Studies on foraging have examined the effect of group size in reducing time spent on predator vigilance, thereby increasing feeding efficiency. According to the "many-eyes" hypothesis,[33] a reduction in the individual time spent on vigilance against threats in larger groups allows for more time to be spent on searching for food and feeding. Vigilance is greatest among solitary individuals and reduces as the group size increases to about four. The birds collect seeds more quickly in larger groups, reflecting a decrease in individual vigilance, a decrease in handling time, and an increase in both search speed and focus when foraging.[34]

 
A foraging group

Individuals may also take advantage of group foraging by "joining" members that have found food. The options to seek food or to join others that have discovered food involves information sharing and has been studied through what are termed "producer-scrounger models".[35] A cost associated with group foraging is increased resource competition, which in turn may reduce anti-predatory vigilance due to the intensity of foraging.[36] Some studies show that increased competition results in a decreased feeding rate.[37]

Foraging models edit

When foraging, scaly-breasted munia can search as individuals or search for others that have found food and join them. The economic consequences of the decision to join others has been modeled in two ways: the producer-scrounger model and the information sharing model. These models are based on hypotheses that differ in the degree of compatibility that is assumed between the two food and joining opportunity search modes.[38]

The information sharing model assumes that individuals search concurrently for finding and joining opportunities while the producer-scrounger model assumes that the search modes are mutually exclusive.[38] Hopping with the head facing up and downward are observed to be statistically associated with the frequencies of a bird's joining and finding, respectively. When the expected stable frequency of the scrounger tactic was altered by changing the availability of seeds, the relative frequency of hopping with the head up changed accordingly. When the seed distribution made the scrounger tactic unprofitable, the frequency of hopping with the head up diminished and appears to support the predictions of the producer-scrounger model.[39]

Studies show that scaly-breasted munias tend to adopt the scrounger tactic when food is more clumped and when the group size increases. When most foragers adopt scrounging, the time taken to discover new food patches is greater.[40]

Vigilance edit

Most social foragers must search for food while also avoiding predators. It has been suggested that individuals that play scrounger could also, by virtue of their head position, be alert for predators and hence contribute to antipredatory vigilance. If the scrounger tactic is compatible with antipredatory vigilance, then an increase in antipredatory vigilance should lead to the detection of more joining opportunities, and hence more joining. When stationary, the head-up tactic has been shown to be associated with antipredatory vigilance. However scanning while hopping does not aid in vigilance and it is thought that the scrounger tactic is incompatible with antipredatory vigilance in the scaly-breasted munia.[41]

Specialized foraging edit

Scaly-breasted munias have variable competitive behaviors that allow them to exploit scarce resources. There are two foraging alternatives: producers that make the food available and scroungers that steal food found by the producers. Studies show that these choices lead to a stable equilibrium within a group. When individuals are free to choose between producer and scrounger, frequency dependent selection results in a stable mixture of both behaviors where each receives similar payoff. Studies indicate that if most of the population consists of producers, then scrounging behavior is favored by natural selection because there is plenty of food to steal. On the other hand, if most birds exhibit scrounging then the competition for stealing is so great that producing is favored.[42][43]

 
A pair feeding on grains

Three hypotheses might account for consistent foraging specializations across individuals: food source variation, phenotypic differences, and frequency dependent-choice. The food source variation hypothesis predicts that individuals will specialize when the use of two skills is more costly than specialist foraging. The phenotypic differences hypothesis proposes that individuals differ in their ability to use each foraging skill and stably specialize on the most profitable one. The pattern of specialization is expected to be stable although the number of individuals that use a given skill depends on the phenotypic composition of the flock. The frequency dependent choice hypothesis also proposes that individuals specialize on the most profitable skill, but the profitability of each alternative decreases as the number of phenotypically identical foragers gradually specialize on each skill when initially given two equally profitable alternatives. At equilibrium, individual payoffs should be independent of the pattern of specialization. Individuals in flocks adjusted their use of the two skills and two birds in each flock specialized on a different skill resulting in a variant of both the food source variation hypothesis and frequency dependent choice hypothesis.[44]

Aviary experiments conducted with captive flocks of scaly-breasted munia have tested whether producers and scroungers reach the predicted stable equilibrium frequency (see Evolutionarily stable strategy) when individuals are free to choose either behavior. The numbers choosing either producers and scrounger strategies have been shown to converge on stable frequencies while demonstrating that variation in tactics arise through frequency dependent pay-offs from the choice of different feeding strategies.[45]

Furthermore, foraging birds may feed actively on the substrate or pick grains dropped on the ground and these strategies may be chosen according to the situation. Early departures occur more often when expected searching time decreases and when competition intensity increases. Competition intensity is expected to increase when more scroungers are present or when patches are smaller.[46]

Prey crypsis edit

Since producers search for food and scroungers wait for opportunities to join, prey crypsis imposes a producer specific cost that shifts the producer scrounger equilibria towards more scrounging. Prey crypsis resulted in increased latency to eat the seed and increased number of detection errors.[47] Moreover, the presence of a competitor negatively affected foraging efficiency under cryptic backgrounds. The foraging efficiency of individuals that had previously foraged with a competitor on cryptic seeds remained low even after the competitor had been removed. Thus, the costs of foraging on cryptic prey may be greater for social foragers than for solitary foragers.[48]

Resource defence edit

Recent models of economic defence in a group-foraging context predict that the frequency of aggressive interactions should decline as resource density increases.[49][50][51] Studies with scaly-breasted munia show that the intensity of aggressive encounters was highest when patch location was signaled, and the effect of changing resource density depended on whether patch location was signaled or not. Signaling patch location was equivalent to making the resources more spatially predictable. Changing patch density had no effect on the number of aggressive encounters when the location of food was not signaled. When food location was signaled, increasing patch density resulted in the predicted decrease in the number of aggressive encounters.[52]

Conservation edit

The scaly-breasted munia is an abundant species and classified as least concern on the IUCN Red List.[1] The species occupies an extremely large range, and its population, while still unquantified, is large and stable. The scaly-breasted munia is not globally threatened and is common to very common throughout most of its range. However, some populations are dwindled due to the increase of bird cagings.[53]

In many areas it is regarded as an agricultural pest, feeding in large flocks on cultivated cereals such as rice.[54] In Southeast Asia, the scaly-breasted munia is trapped in large numbers for Buddhist ceremonies, but most birds are later released.[55]

References edit

  1. ^ a b BirdLife International (2016). "Lonchura punctulata". IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. 2016: e.T22719821A94646304. doi:10.2305/IUCN.UK.2016-3.RLTS.T22719821A94646304.en. Retrieved 19 November 2021.
  2. ^ Edwards, George (1743). A Natural History of Uncommon Birds. Vol. Part 1. London: Printed for the author at the College of Physicians. p. 40, Plate 40.
  3. ^ Linnaeus, Carl (1758). Systema Naturae per regna tria naturae, secundum classes, ordines, genera, species, cum characteribus, differentiis, synonymis, locis (in Latin). Vol. 1 (10th ed.). Holmiae (Stockholm): Laurentii Salvii. p. 173.
  4. ^ Baker, E.C. Stuart (1926). The Fauna of British India Birds including Ceylon and Burma. Birds. Vol. 3 (2nd ed.). London: Taylor and Francis. p. 91.
  5. ^ Paynter, Raymond A. Jr, ed. (1968). Check-List of Birds of the World. Vol. 14. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Museum of Comparative Zoology.
  6. ^ Sykes, William Henry (1832). "Catalogue of birds of the raptorial and insessorial orders (systematically arranged,) observed in the Dukhun". Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London. 2 (18): 77–99 [94].
  7. ^ a b Gill, Frank; Donsker, David; Rasmussen, Pamela, eds. (July 2021). "Waxbills, parrotfinches, munias, whydahs, Olive Warbler, accentors, pipits". IOC World Bird List Version 11.2. International Ornithologists' Union. Retrieved 13 July 2021.
  8. ^ Jobling, J. A. (2010). The Helm Dictionary of Scientific Bird Names. London: Christopher Helm. pp. 229, 324. ISBN 978-1-4081-2501-4.
  9. ^ Clements, J. F.; T. S. Schulenberg; M. J. Iliff; B.L. Sullivan; C. L. Wood & D. Roberson (2013). The eBird/Clements checklist of birds of the world: Version 6.8. The Cornell Lab of Ornithology.
  10. ^ a b Rasmussen, P.C. & Anderton, J.C. (2005). Birds of South Asia. The Ripley Guide. Vol. Volume 2. Smithsonian Institution and Lynx Edicions. p. 673. ISBN 978-84-87334-66-5.
  11. ^ a b c Restall, R. (1997). Munias and Mannikins. Yale University Press. pp. 97–105. ISBN 978-0-300-07109-2.
  12. ^ Forshaw J; Mark Shephard; Anthony Pridham. Grassfinches in Australia. Csiro Publishing. pp. 267–268.
  13. ^ Abbass, D.; Rais, M.; Ghalib, S.A. & Khan, M.Z. (2010). "First Record of Spotted Munia (Lonchura punctulata) from Karachi". Pakistan Journal of Zoology. 42 (4): 503–505.
  14. ^ a b Ali, S. & Ripley, S.D. (1999). Handbook of the Birds of India and Pakistan. Vol. Volume 10 (Second ed.). New Delhi: Oxford University Press. pp. 119–121. ISBN 978-0-19-563708-3.
  15. ^ Akhtar, S.A.; Rao, P.; Tiwari, J.K.; Javed, S. (1992). "Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata (Linn.) from Dachigam National Park, Jammu and Kashmir". Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society. 89 (1): 129.
  16. ^ Moreno, J.A. (1997). "Review of the Subspecific Status and Origin of Introduced Finches in Puerto Rico". Caribbean Journal of Science. 33 (3–4): 233–238.
  17. ^ Moulton, M.P. (1993). "The All-or-None Pattern in Introduced Hawaiian Passeriforms: The role of competition sustained". The American Naturalist. 141 (1): 105–119. doi:10.1086/285463. JSTOR 2462765. S2CID 84341527.
  18. ^ a b Moulton, M. P.; Allen, L. J. S. & Ferris, D. K. (1992). "Competition, resource use and habitat selection in two introduced Hawaiian Mannikins". Biotropica. 24 (1): 77–85. doi:10.2307/2388475. JSTOR 2388475.
  19. ^ Eguchi, K. & Amano, H.E. (2004). "Invasive Birds in Japan" (PDF). Global Environmental Research. 8 (1): 29–39.
  20. ^ Duncan, R.A. (2009). "The status of the nutmeg mannikin (Lonchura punctulata) in the extreme western panhandle of Florida" (PDF). Florida Field Naturalist. 37 (3): 96–97.
  21. ^ Garrett, K.L. (2000). "The juvenile nutmeg mannikin: identification of a little brown bird" (PDF). Western Birds. 31 (2): 130–131.
  22. ^ Burton, M.; Burton, R. (2002). International Wildlife Encyclopedia. New York, NY: Marshal Cavendish. ISBN 9780761472865.
  23. ^ a b c d e f g Moynihan, M. & Hall, M.F. (1954). "Hostile, Sexual, and Other Social Behaviour Patterns of the Spice Finch (Lonchura punctulata) in captivity". Behaviour. 7 (1): 33–76. doi:10.1163/156853955X00021.
  24. ^ Baptista, L.F.; Lawson, R.; Visser, E.; Bell, D. A. (1999). "Relationships of some mannikins and waxbills in the estrildidae". Journal für Ornithologie. 140 (2): 179–192. doi:10.1007/BF01653597. S2CID 29184906.
  25. ^ Sikdar, M.; Kar, A. & Prakash, P. (1992). "Role of humidity in the seasonal reproduction of male spotted munia, Lonchura punctulata". Journal of Experimental Zoology. 264 (1): 82–84. doi:10.1002/jez.1402640112.
  26. ^ Gokula, V. (2001). "Nesting ecology of the Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata in Mudumalai Wildlife Sanctuary (South India)". Acta Ornithologica. 36 (1): 1–5. doi:10.3161/068.036.0107. S2CID 84260813.
  27. ^ Sharma, R.C.; Bhatt, D. & Sharma, R.K. (2004). "Breeding success of the tropical Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata in urbanized and forest habitats". Ornithological Science. 3 (2): 113–117. doi:10.2326/osj.3.113.
  28. ^ Lamba, B.S. (1974). "Nest construction technique of the Spotted Munia, Lonchura punctulata". Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society. 71 (3): 613–616.
  29. ^ Mehta, P. (1997). "Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata feeding on scat?". Newsletter for Birdwatchers. 37 (1): 16.
  30. ^ Nuijens, F.W.; Zweers, G.A. (1997). "Characters discriminating two seed husking mechanisms in finches (Fringillidae: Carduelinae) and estrildids (Passeridae: Estrildinae)". Journal of Morphology. 232 (1): 1–33. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-4687(199704)232:1<1::AID-JMOR1>3.0.CO;2-G. PMID 29852621. S2CID 46921231.
  31. ^ Avery, M. L. (1980). "Diet and breeding seasonality among a population of sharp-tailed munias, Lonchura striata, in Malaysia" (PDF). The Auk. 97: 160–166. doi:10.1093/auk/97.1.160.
  32. ^ Stephens, D.W. (2007). A comprehensive guide to optimal foraging theory. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
  33. ^ Pulliam, R.H. (1973). "On the advantages of flocking". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 38 (2): 419–422. Bibcode:1973JThBi..38..419P. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(73)90184-7. PMID 4734745.
  34. ^ Beauchamp, G. & Livoreil, B. (1997). "The effect of group size on vigilance and feeding rate in spice finches (Lonchura punctulata)". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 75 (9): 1526–1531. doi:10.1139/z97-776.
  35. ^ Giraldeau, L.A.; Beauchamp, G. (1999). "Food exploitation: searching for the optimal joining policy". Trends in Ecology and Evolution. 14 (3): 102–106. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01542-0. PMID 10322509.
  36. ^ Rieucau, G. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2009). "Group size effect caused by food competition in nutmeg mannikins (Lonchura punctulata)". Behavioral Ecology. 20 (2): 421–425. doi:10.1093/beheco/arn144.
  37. ^ Gauvin, S. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2004). "Nutmeg mannikins (Lonchura punctulata) reduce their feeding rates in response to simulated competition". Oecologia. 139 (1): 150–156. Bibcode:2004Oecol.139..150G. doi:10.1007/s00442-003-1482-2. PMID 14722748. S2CID 21144047.
  38. ^ a b Giraldeau, L.-A. & Beauchamp, G. (1999). "Food exploitation: searching for the optimal joining policy". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 14 (3): 102–106. doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(98)01542-0. PMID 10322509.
  39. ^ Coolen, I.; Giraldeau, L.-A.; Lavoie, M. (2001). "Head position as an indication of producer and scrounger tactics in a ground-feeding bird". Animal Behaviour. 61 (5): 895–903. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1678. S2CID 53145727.
  40. ^ Coolen, I. (2002). "Increasing foraging group size increases scrounger use and reduces searching efficiency in nutmeg mannikins (Lonchura punctulata)". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 52 (3): 232–238. doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0500-4. S2CID 28537757.
  41. ^ Coolen, I. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2003). "Incompatibility between antipredatory vigilance and scrounger tactic in nutmeg mannikins, Lonchura punctulata". Animal Behaviour. 66 (4): 657–664. doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2236. S2CID 53152430.
  42. ^ Davies, N. (2012). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Competing for Resources: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 130–131. ISBN 978-1-4051-1416-5.
  43. ^ Barnard, C.J. & Sibly, R.M. (1981). "Producers and scroungers: A general model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows". Animal Behaviour. 29 (2): 543–550. doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80117-0. S2CID 53170850.
  44. ^ Beauchamp, G.; Giraldeau, L.-A. & Ennis, N. (1997). "Experimental evidence for the maintenance of foraging specializations by frequency-dependent choice in flocks of spice finches". Ethology Ecology & Evolution. 9 (2): 105–117. doi:10.1080/08927014.1997.9522890.
  45. ^ Mottley, K. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2000). "Experimental evidence that group foragers can converge on predicted producer–scrounger equilibria" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 60 (3): 341–350. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1474. PMID 11007643. S2CID 35238033.
  46. ^ Beauchamp, G. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (1997). "Patch exploitation in a producer-scrounger system: test of a hypothesis using flocks of spice finches (Lonchura punctulata)". Behavioral Ecology. 8 (1): 54–59. doi:10.1093/beheco/8.1.54.
  47. ^ Barrette, M. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2006). "Prey crypticity reduces the proportion of group members searching for food". Animal Behaviour. 71 (5): 1183–1189. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.10.008. S2CID 53146661.
  48. ^ Courant, S. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2008). "Conspecific presence makes exploiting cryptic prey more difficult in wild-caught nutmeg mannikins". Animal Behaviour. 75 (3): 1101–1108. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.08.023. S2CID 54398287.
  49. ^ Broom, M. & Ruxton, G.D. (1998). "Evolutionarily stable stealing: game theory applied to kleptoparasitism". Behavioral Ecology. 9 (4): 397–403. doi:10.1093/beheco/9.4.397.
  50. ^ Sirot, E. (1999). "An evolutionarily stable strategy for aggressiveness in feeding groups". Behavioral Ecology. 11 (4): 351–356. doi:10.1093/beheco/11.4.351.
  51. ^ Dubois, F. (2002). "Resource defense in a group-foraging context". Behavioral Ecology. 14 (1): 2–9. doi:10.1093/beheco/14.1.2.
  52. ^ Dubois, F. & Giraldeau, L.-A. (2004). "Reduced resource defence in an uncertain world: an experimental test using captive nutmeg mannikins". Animal Behaviour. 68 (1): 21–25. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.06.025. S2CID 54349286.
  53. ^ "Trade in wild birds going 'unchecked' in Vietnam: new report". Mongabay Environmental News. September 25, 2017.
  54. ^ Bomford, M.; Sinclair, R. (2002). "Australian research on bird pests: impact, management and future directions". Emu. 102: 29–45. doi:10.1071/MU01028. S2CID 83464835.
  55. ^ Collar, N.; Newton, I.; Clement, P. & Arkhipov, V. (2010). "Scaly-breasted Munia Lonchura punctulata". In Del Hoyo, J.; Elliott, A. & Christie, D. (eds.). Handbook of the Birds of the World. Vol. Volume 15. Finches. Barcelona: Lynx Edicions. ISBN 978-84-96553-68-2.

External links edit

  • Pictures, videos and sound
  • Oriental Bird Images: Scaly-breasted Munia Selected images

scaly, breasted, munia, scaly, breasted, munia, spotted, munia, lonchura, punctulata, known, trade, nutmeg, mannikin, spice, finch, sparrow, sized, estrildid, finch, native, tropical, asia, species, genus, lonchura, formally, described, named, carl, linnaeus, . The scaly breasted munia or spotted munia Lonchura punctulata known in the pet trade as nutmeg mannikin or spice finch is a sparrow sized estrildid finch native to tropical Asia A species of the genus Lonchura it was formally described and named by Carl Linnaeus in 1758 Its name is based on the distinct scale like feather markings on the breast and belly The adult is brown above and has a dark conical bill The species has 11 subspecies across its range which differ slightly in size and color Scaly breasted muniaL p punctulata India Conservation statusLeast Concern IUCN 3 1 1 Scientific classificationDomain EukaryotaKingdom AnimaliaPhylum ChordataClass AvesOrder PasseriformesFamily EstrildidaeGenus LonchuraSpecies L punctulataBinomial nameLonchura punctulata Linnaeus 1758 Native rangeSynonymsLoxia punctulata Linnaeus 1758This munia eats mainly grass seeds apart from berries and small insects They forage in flocks and communicate with soft calls and whistles The species is highly social and may sometimes roost with other species of munias This species is found in tropical plains and grasslands Breeding pairs construct dome shaped nests using grass or bamboo leaves The species is endemic to Asia and occurs from India and Sri Lanka east to Indonesia and the Philippines where it is called mayang paking It has been introduced into many other parts of the world and feral populations have established in Puerto Rico and Hispaniola as well as parts of Australia and the United States of America with sightings in California The bird is listed as of least concern by the International Union for Conservation of Nature IUCN Contents 1 Taxonomy 2 Description 3 Distribution and habitat 4 Behaviour and ecology 4 1 Sociality 4 2 Breeding 5 Food and foraging 5 1 Flock size tradeoffs 5 2 Foraging models 5 3 Vigilance 5 4 Specialized foraging 5 5 Prey crypsis 5 6 Resource defence 6 Conservation 7 References 8 External linksTaxonomy editIn 1743 the English naturalist George Edwards included an illustration and a description of the scaly breasted munia in the first volume of his A Natural History of Uncommon Birds He used the English name Gowry Bird Edwards based his hand coloured etching on a specimen at the London home of Charles du Bois treasurer to the East India Company 2 When in 1758 the Swedish naturalist Carl Linnaeus updated his Systema Naturae for the tenth edition he placed the scaly breasted munia with the crossbills in the genus Loxia Linnaeus included a brief description coined the binomial name Loxia punctulata and cited Edwards work 3 Linnaeus specified the locality as Asia but this was restricted to Kolkata Calcutta by E C Stuart Baker in 1926 4 5 The species is now placed in the genus Lonchura that was introduced by the English naturalist William Henry Sykes in 1832 6 7 The genus name Lonchura combines the Ancient Greek lonkhe meaning spear head or lance with oura meaning tail The specific epithet is from Modern Latin punctulatus meaning spotted or dotted 8 Over its large range there are 11 recognised subspecies These include the nominate form found in the plains of the Indian Subcontinent including Pakistan India Iran Nepal Bangladesh and Sri Lanka The name lineoventer was formerly used for the Indian population Other populations include subundulata from the eastern Himalayas yunnanensis of southern China topela of Thailand cabanisi of the Philippines and fretensis of Singapore and Sumatra Island populations include nisoria Java Bali Lombok Sumbawa particeps Sulawesi baweana Bawean Island sumbae Sumba and blasii Flores Timor and Tanimbar 7 L p punctulata Linnaeus 1758 northern Pakistan India except northeast Nepal terai and Sri Lanka L p subundulata Godwin Austen 1874 Bhutan Bangladesh northeast India Assam and west Myanmar L p yunnanensis Parkes 1958 southern China southeast Xizang south Sichuan Yunnan and north Myanmar L p topela R Swinhoe 1863 southern Myanmar Thailand southeast China Taiwan Hainan Islands Laos Cambodia and Vietnam L p cabanisi Sharpe 1890 north and west Philippines Luzon Mindoro Calauit Palawan Panay Negros Cebu Mindanao and northern Borneo coastal west Sabah and Brunei L p fretensis Kloss 1931 south Malay Peninsula Singapore Sumatra and Nias Islands L p nisoria Temminck 1830 southern Borneo western amp southern Kalimantan Java Bali and western Lesser Sundas Lombok Sumbawa L p sumbae Mayr 1944 Sumba in western Lesser Sundas L p blasii Stresemann 1912 central amp eastern Lesser Sundas Flores east to Timor and Tanimbar Islands L p baweana Hoogerwerf 1963 Bawean Islands off northeast Java L p particeps Riley 1920 SulawesiThe subspecies holmesi southeast Borneo is sometimes recognised 9 Description edit nbsp juvenile L p punctulata Sri LankaThe scaly breasted munia is about 11 12 centimetres 4 3 4 7 in long and weighs 12 16 grams 0 026 0 035 lb The adult has a stubby dark bill typical of grain eating birds brown upperparts and a dark brown head The underparts are white with dark scale markings The sexes are similar although males have darker markings on the underside and a darker throat than females 10 Immature birds have pale brown upperparts lack the dark head found in adults and have uniform buff underparts that can be confused with juveniles of other munia species such as the tricolored munia Lonchura malacca across the Asian and island populations and the black throated munia Lonchura kelaarti in parts of India or Sri Lanka 10 11 Distribution and habitat edit nbsp The scaly breasted munia subspecies topela 12 has established in parts of eastern Australia such as QueenslandScaly breasted munias are found in a range of habitats but are usually close to water and grassland In India they are especially common in paddy fields where they are considered a minor pest on account of their feeding on grain They are found mainly on the plains but can be observed in the foothills of the Himalayas in which they may be present at altitudes near 2 500 m 1 6 mi and in the Nilgiris where they are found at altitudes up to 2 100 m 6 900 ft during the summer In Pakistan they are restricted to a narrow region from Swat in the west to Lahore avoiding the desert zone and then occurring again in India east of an area between Ludhiana and Mount Abu 13 The species has also been observed in Kashmir though this is rare 14 15 Outside their native range escaped birds frequently establish themselves in areas with a suitable climate and can then colonize new areas nearby Escaped cage birds established in the wild and such populations have been recorded in the West Indies Puerto Rico since 1971 16 Hawaii since 1883 17 18 Japan 19 and southern United States mainly in Florida and California 20 21 In Oahu Hawaii they compete for habitats with the tricolored munia and tend to be rare where this competitor is present 18 The species has been introduced to other parts of the world due to its popularity as a cage bird and populations have established in the wild 22 23 Behaviour and ecology editSociality edit Scaly breasted munias form flocks of as many as 100 birds Individuals communicate with calls that include a short whistle variations of kitty kitty kitty and a sharp chipping alarm note 11 23 They sometimes flick their tails and wings vertically or horizontally while hopping about The tail flicking motion may have evolved from a locomotory intention movement The exaggerated version of the tail flicking movement may have undergone ritualization As a social signal tail flicking in several other species acts as a signal indicating the intent to fly and helps keep flocks together 23 24 When roosting communally scaly breasted munia sit side by side in close contact with each other The outermost bird often jostles towards the center Birds in a flock sometimes preen each other with the soliciting bird usually showing its chin Allopreening is usually limited to the face and neck 23 The scaly breasted munia is rarely hostile but birds will sometimes quarrel without any ritualized posturing 23 Breeding edit nbsp Egg of scaly breasted muniaThe breeding season is during the summer rainy season mainly June to August and also in October season in India but can vary Laboratory studies have found that long day illumination and high humidity trigger gonadal growth 25 The song of the male is very soft but complex and variable audible only at close range This song described as a jingle consists of a series of high notes followed by a croaky rattle and ending in a slurred whistle When singing the male sits in what is called the slope posture erect with the head feathers raised 23 There are two types of slope posture a pre copulatory one and an ordinary one The pre copulatory behavior of scaly breasted munia includes a sequence of actions The first involves either the male or female playing with nest material As soon as a bird has arranged the nest material in its bill it begins to fly around in a zigzag path Once a bird lands close to its partner the male bends towards the female and wipes its bill The male then sings with movements of the body The female invites mounting with tail quivering 11 23 The nest is a large domed structure loosely woven from blades of grass bamboo or other leaves with a side entrance and is placed in a tree or under the eaves of a house A study in southern India found the preferred nesting trees to be Toddalia asiatica Gymnosporia montana and Acacia chundra especially short and bushy ones in areas with low canopy cover The nest opening is located to face downwind of the most frequent wind direction 26 In northern India they preferred isolated Acacia nilotica in non urban areas but used Thuja orientalis and Polyalthia longifolia in urban gardens 27 Scaly breasted munia clutches usually contain 4 to 6 eggs but can contain up to 10 Both sexes build the nest and incubate the eggs which hatch in 10 to 16 days 14 28 Food and foraging edit nbsp Adult feeding youngThe scaly breasted munia feeds mainly on grass seeds small berries such as those of Lantana and insects 29 Although the bill is suited for crushing small grains they do not show lateral movements of the lower mandible which help European greenfinches in dehusking seeds 30 Like some other munias they may also feed on algae a rich protein source prior to the breeding season 31 The ease of maintaining these birds in captivity has made them popular for studying behavior and physiology Feeding behavior can be predicted by the optimal foraging theory where animals minimize time and energy spent to maximize food intake This theory has been tested by studying the strategies used by scaly breasted munias to increase their feeding efficacy 32 Flock size tradeoffs edit Studies on foraging have examined the effect of group size in reducing time spent on predator vigilance thereby increasing feeding efficiency According to the many eyes hypothesis 33 a reduction in the individual time spent on vigilance against threats in larger groups allows for more time to be spent on searching for food and feeding Vigilance is greatest among solitary individuals and reduces as the group size increases to about four The birds collect seeds more quickly in larger groups reflecting a decrease in individual vigilance a decrease in handling time and an increase in both search speed and focus when foraging 34 nbsp A foraging groupIndividuals may also take advantage of group foraging by joining members that have found food The options to seek food or to join others that have discovered food involves information sharing and has been studied through what are termed producer scrounger models 35 A cost associated with group foraging is increased resource competition which in turn may reduce anti predatory vigilance due to the intensity of foraging 36 Some studies show that increased competition results in a decreased feeding rate 37 Foraging models edit When foraging scaly breasted munia can search as individuals or search for others that have found food and join them The economic consequences of the decision to join others has been modeled in two ways the producer scrounger model and the information sharing model These models are based on hypotheses that differ in the degree of compatibility that is assumed between the two food and joining opportunity search modes 38 The information sharing model assumes that individuals search concurrently for finding and joining opportunities while the producer scrounger model assumes that the search modes are mutually exclusive 38 Hopping with the head facing up and downward are observed to be statistically associated with the frequencies of a bird s joining and finding respectively When the expected stable frequency of the scrounger tactic was altered by changing the availability of seeds the relative frequency of hopping with the head up changed accordingly When the seed distribution made the scrounger tactic unprofitable the frequency of hopping with the head up diminished and appears to support the predictions of the producer scrounger model 39 Studies show that scaly breasted munias tend to adopt the scrounger tactic when food is more clumped and when the group size increases When most foragers adopt scrounging the time taken to discover new food patches is greater 40 Vigilance edit Most social foragers must search for food while also avoiding predators It has been suggested that individuals that play scrounger could also by virtue of their head position be alert for predators and hence contribute to antipredatory vigilance If the scrounger tactic is compatible with antipredatory vigilance then an increase in antipredatory vigilance should lead to the detection of more joining opportunities and hence more joining When stationary the head up tactic has been shown to be associated with antipredatory vigilance However scanning while hopping does not aid in vigilance and it is thought that the scrounger tactic is incompatible with antipredatory vigilance in the scaly breasted munia 41 Specialized foraging edit Scaly breasted munias have variable competitive behaviors that allow them to exploit scarce resources There are two foraging alternatives producers that make the food available and scroungers that steal food found by the producers Studies show that these choices lead to a stable equilibrium within a group When individuals are free to choose between producer and scrounger frequency dependent selection results in a stable mixture of both behaviors where each receives similar payoff Studies indicate that if most of the population consists of producers then scrounging behavior is favored by natural selection because there is plenty of food to steal On the other hand if most birds exhibit scrounging then the competition for stealing is so great that producing is favored 42 43 nbsp A pair feeding on grainsThree hypotheses might account for consistent foraging specializations across individuals food source variation phenotypic differences and frequency dependent choice The food source variation hypothesis predicts that individuals will specialize when the use of two skills is more costly than specialist foraging The phenotypic differences hypothesis proposes that individuals differ in their ability to use each foraging skill and stably specialize on the most profitable one The pattern of specialization is expected to be stable although the number of individuals that use a given skill depends on the phenotypic composition of the flock The frequency dependent choice hypothesis also proposes that individuals specialize on the most profitable skill but the profitability of each alternative decreases as the number of phenotypically identical foragers gradually specialize on each skill when initially given two equally profitable alternatives At equilibrium individual payoffs should be independent of the pattern of specialization Individuals in flocks adjusted their use of the two skills and two birds in each flock specialized on a different skill resulting in a variant of both the food source variation hypothesis and frequency dependent choice hypothesis 44 Aviary experiments conducted with captive flocks of scaly breasted munia have tested whether producers and scroungers reach the predicted stable equilibrium frequency see Evolutionarily stable strategy when individuals are free to choose either behavior The numbers choosing either producers and scrounger strategies have been shown to converge on stable frequencies while demonstrating that variation in tactics arise through frequency dependent pay offs from the choice of different feeding strategies 45 Furthermore foraging birds may feed actively on the substrate or pick grains dropped on the ground and these strategies may be chosen according to the situation Early departures occur more often when expected searching time decreases and when competition intensity increases Competition intensity is expected to increase when more scroungers are present or when patches are smaller 46 Prey crypsis edit Since producers search for food and scroungers wait for opportunities to join prey crypsis imposes a producer specific cost that shifts the producer scrounger equilibria towards more scrounging Prey crypsis resulted in increased latency to eat the seed and increased number of detection errors 47 Moreover the presence of a competitor negatively affected foraging efficiency under cryptic backgrounds The foraging efficiency of individuals that had previously foraged with a competitor on cryptic seeds remained low even after the competitor had been removed Thus the costs of foraging on cryptic prey may be greater for social foragers than for solitary foragers 48 Resource defence edit Recent models of economic defence in a group foraging context predict that the frequency of aggressive interactions should decline as resource density increases 49 50 51 Studies with scaly breasted munia show that the intensity of aggressive encounters was highest when patch location was signaled and the effect of changing resource density depended on whether patch location was signaled or not Signaling patch location was equivalent to making the resources more spatially predictable Changing patch density had no effect on the number of aggressive encounters when the location of food was not signaled When food location was signaled increasing patch density resulted in the predicted decrease in the number of aggressive encounters 52 Conservation editThe scaly breasted munia is an abundant species and classified as least concern on the IUCN Red List 1 The species occupies an extremely large range and its population while still unquantified is large and stable The scaly breasted munia is not globally threatened and is common to very common throughout most of its range However some populations are dwindled due to the increase of bird cagings 53 In many areas it is regarded as an agricultural pest feeding in large flocks on cultivated cereals such as rice 54 In Southeast Asia the scaly breasted munia is trapped in large numbers for Buddhist ceremonies but most birds are later released 55 References edit a b BirdLife International 2016 Lonchura punctulata IUCN Red List of Threatened Species 2016 e T22719821A94646304 doi 10 2305 IUCN UK 2016 3 RLTS T22719821A94646304 en Retrieved 19 November 2021 Edwards George 1743 A Natural History of Uncommon Birds Vol Part 1 London Printed for the author at the College of Physicians p 40 Plate 40 Linnaeus Carl 1758 Systema Naturae per regna tria naturae secundum classes ordines genera species cum characteribus differentiis synonymis locis in Latin Vol 1 10th ed Holmiae Stockholm Laurentii Salvii p 173 Baker E C Stuart 1926 The Fauna of British India Birds including Ceylon and Burma Birds Vol 3 2nd ed London Taylor and Francis p 91 Paynter Raymond A Jr ed 1968 Check List of Birds of the World Vol 14 Cambridge Massachusetts Museum of Comparative Zoology Sykes William Henry 1832 Catalogue of birds of the raptorial and insessorial orders systematically arranged observed in the Dukhun Proceedings of the Zoological Society of London 2 18 77 99 94 a b Gill Frank Donsker David Rasmussen Pamela eds July 2021 Waxbills parrotfinches munias whydahs Olive Warbler accentors pipits IOC World Bird List Version 11 2 International Ornithologists Union Retrieved 13 July 2021 Jobling J A 2010 The Helm Dictionary of Scientific Bird Names London Christopher Helm pp 229 324 ISBN 978 1 4081 2501 4 Clements J F T S Schulenberg M J Iliff B L Sullivan C L Wood amp D Roberson 2013 The eBird Clements checklist of birds of the world Version 6 8 The Cornell Lab of Ornithology a b Rasmussen P C amp Anderton J C 2005 Birds of South Asia The Ripley Guide Vol Volume 2 Smithsonian Institution and Lynx Edicions p 673 ISBN 978 84 87334 66 5 a b c Restall R 1997 Munias and Mannikins Yale University Press pp 97 105 ISBN 978 0 300 07109 2 Forshaw J Mark Shephard Anthony Pridham Grassfinches in Australia Csiro Publishing pp 267 268 Abbass D Rais M Ghalib S A amp Khan M Z 2010 First Record of Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata from Karachi Pakistan Journal of Zoology 42 4 503 505 a b Ali S amp Ripley S D 1999 Handbook of the Birds of India and Pakistan Vol Volume 10 Second ed New Delhi Oxford University Press pp 119 121 ISBN 978 0 19 563708 3 Akhtar S A Rao P Tiwari J K Javed S 1992 Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata Linn from Dachigam National Park Jammu and Kashmir Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society 89 1 129 Moreno J A 1997 Review of the Subspecific Status and Origin of Introduced Finches in Puerto Rico Caribbean Journal of Science 33 3 4 233 238 Moulton M P 1993 The All or None Pattern in Introduced Hawaiian Passeriforms The role of competition sustained The American Naturalist 141 1 105 119 doi 10 1086 285463 JSTOR 2462765 S2CID 84341527 a b Moulton M P Allen L J S amp Ferris D K 1992 Competition resource use and habitat selection in two introduced Hawaiian Mannikins Biotropica 24 1 77 85 doi 10 2307 2388475 JSTOR 2388475 Eguchi K amp Amano H E 2004 Invasive Birds in Japan PDF Global Environmental Research 8 1 29 39 Duncan R A 2009 The status of the nutmeg mannikin Lonchura punctulata in the extreme western panhandle of Florida PDF Florida Field Naturalist 37 3 96 97 Garrett K L 2000 The juvenile nutmeg mannikin identification of a little brown bird PDF Western Birds 31 2 130 131 Burton M Burton R 2002 International Wildlife Encyclopedia New York NY Marshal Cavendish ISBN 9780761472865 a b c d e f g Moynihan M amp Hall M F 1954 Hostile Sexual and Other Social Behaviour Patterns of the Spice Finch Lonchura punctulata in captivity Behaviour 7 1 33 76 doi 10 1163 156853955X00021 Baptista L F Lawson R Visser E Bell D A 1999 Relationships of some mannikins and waxbills in the estrildidae Journal fur Ornithologie 140 2 179 192 doi 10 1007 BF01653597 S2CID 29184906 Sikdar M Kar A amp Prakash P 1992 Role of humidity in the seasonal reproduction of male spotted munia Lonchura punctulata Journal of Experimental Zoology 264 1 82 84 doi 10 1002 jez 1402640112 Gokula V 2001 Nesting ecology of the Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata in Mudumalai Wildlife Sanctuary South India Acta Ornithologica 36 1 1 5 doi 10 3161 068 036 0107 S2CID 84260813 Sharma R C Bhatt D amp Sharma R K 2004 Breeding success of the tropical Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata in urbanized and forest habitats Ornithological Science 3 2 113 117 doi 10 2326 osj 3 113 Lamba B S 1974 Nest construction technique of the Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata Journal of the Bombay Natural History Society 71 3 613 616 Mehta P 1997 Spotted Munia Lonchura punctulata feeding on scat Newsletter for Birdwatchers 37 1 16 Nuijens F W Zweers G A 1997 Characters discriminating two seed husking mechanisms in finches Fringillidae Carduelinae and estrildids Passeridae Estrildinae Journal of Morphology 232 1 1 33 doi 10 1002 SICI 1097 4687 199704 232 1 lt 1 AID JMOR1 gt 3 0 CO 2 G PMID 29852621 S2CID 46921231 Avery M L 1980 Diet and breeding seasonality among a population of sharp tailed munias Lonchura striata in Malaysia PDF The Auk 97 160 166 doi 10 1093 auk 97 1 160 Stephens D W 2007 A comprehensive guide to optimal foraging theory Chicago The University of Chicago Press Pulliam R H 1973 On the advantages of flocking Journal of Theoretical Biology 38 2 419 422 Bibcode 1973JThBi 38 419P doi 10 1016 0022 5193 73 90184 7 PMID 4734745 Beauchamp G amp Livoreil B 1997 The effect of group size on vigilance and feeding rate in spice finches Lonchura punctulata Canadian Journal of Zoology 75 9 1526 1531 doi 10 1139 z97 776 Giraldeau L A Beauchamp G 1999 Food exploitation searching for the optimal joining policy Trends in Ecology and Evolution 14 3 102 106 doi 10 1016 S0169 5347 98 01542 0 PMID 10322509 Rieucau G amp Giraldeau L A 2009 Group size effect caused by food competition in nutmeg mannikins Lonchura punctulata Behavioral Ecology 20 2 421 425 doi 10 1093 beheco arn144 Gauvin S amp Giraldeau L A 2004 Nutmeg mannikins Lonchura punctulata reduce their feeding rates in response to simulated competition Oecologia 139 1 150 156 Bibcode 2004Oecol 139 150G doi 10 1007 s00442 003 1482 2 PMID 14722748 S2CID 21144047 a b Giraldeau L A amp Beauchamp G 1999 Food exploitation searching for the optimal joining policy Trends in Ecology amp Evolution 14 3 102 106 doi 10 1016 S0169 5347 98 01542 0 PMID 10322509 Coolen I Giraldeau L A Lavoie M 2001 Head position as an indication of producer and scrounger tactics in a ground feeding bird Animal Behaviour 61 5 895 903 doi 10 1006 anbe 2000 1678 S2CID 53145727 Coolen I 2002 Increasing foraging group size increases scrounger use and reduces searching efficiency in nutmeg mannikins Lonchura punctulata Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 52 3 232 238 doi 10 1007 s00265 002 0500 4 S2CID 28537757 Coolen I amp Giraldeau L A 2003 Incompatibility between antipredatory vigilance and scrounger tactic in nutmeg mannikins Lonchura punctulata Animal Behaviour 66 4 657 664 doi 10 1006 anbe 2003 2236 S2CID 53152430 Davies N 2012 An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology Competing for Resources Wiley Blackwell pp 130 131 ISBN 978 1 4051 1416 5 Barnard C J amp Sibly R M 1981 Producers and scroungers A general model and its application to captive flocks of house sparrows Animal Behaviour 29 2 543 550 doi 10 1016 S0003 3472 81 80117 0 S2CID 53170850 Beauchamp G Giraldeau L A amp Ennis N 1997 Experimental evidence for the maintenance of foraging specializations by frequency dependent choice in flocks of spice finches Ethology Ecology amp Evolution 9 2 105 117 doi 10 1080 08927014 1997 9522890 Mottley K amp Giraldeau L A 2000 Experimental evidence that group foragers can converge on predicted producer scrounger equilibria PDF Animal Behaviour 60 3 341 350 doi 10 1006 anbe 2000 1474 PMID 11007643 S2CID 35238033 Beauchamp G amp Giraldeau L A 1997 Patch exploitation in a producer scrounger system test of a hypothesis using flocks of spice finches Lonchura punctulata Behavioral Ecology 8 1 54 59 doi 10 1093 beheco 8 1 54 Barrette M amp Giraldeau L A 2006 Prey crypticity reduces the proportion of group members searching for food Animal Behaviour 71 5 1183 1189 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2005 10 008 S2CID 53146661 Courant S amp Giraldeau L A 2008 Conspecific presence makes exploiting cryptic prey more difficult in wild caught nutmeg mannikins Animal Behaviour 75 3 1101 1108 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2007 08 023 S2CID 54398287 Broom M amp Ruxton G D 1998 Evolutionarily stable stealing game theory applied to kleptoparasitism Behavioral Ecology 9 4 397 403 doi 10 1093 beheco 9 4 397 Sirot E 1999 An evolutionarily stable strategy for aggressiveness in feeding groups Behavioral Ecology 11 4 351 356 doi 10 1093 beheco 11 4 351 Dubois F 2002 Resource defense in a group foraging context Behavioral Ecology 14 1 2 9 doi 10 1093 beheco 14 1 2 Dubois F amp Giraldeau L A 2004 Reduced resource defence in an uncertain world an experimental test using captive nutmeg mannikins Animal Behaviour 68 1 21 25 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2003 06 025 S2CID 54349286 Trade in wild birds going unchecked in Vietnam new report Mongabay Environmental News September 25 2017 Bomford M Sinclair R 2002 Australian research on bird pests impact management and future directions Emu 102 29 45 doi 10 1071 MU01028 S2CID 83464835 Collar N Newton I Clement P amp Arkhipov V 2010 Scaly breasted Munia Lonchura punctulata In Del Hoyo J Elliott A amp Christie D eds Handbook of the Birds of the World Vol Volume 15 Finches Barcelona Lynx Edicions ISBN 978 84 96553 68 2 External links edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Lonchura punctulata Pictures videos and sound Oriental Bird Images Scaly breasted Munia Selected images Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Scaly breasted munia amp oldid 1198267538, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.