fbpx
Wikipedia

Ethology

Ethology is a branch of zoology that studies the behaviour of non-human animals. It has its scientific roots in the work of Charles Darwin and of American and German ornithologists of the late 19th and early 20th century, including Charles O. Whitman, Oskar Heinroth, and Wallace Craig. The modern discipline of ethology is generally considered to have begun during the 1930s with the work of the Dutch biologist Nikolaas Tinbergen and the Austrian biologists Konrad Lorenz and Karl von Frisch, the three winners of the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine. Ethology combines laboratory and field science, with a strong relation to neuroanatomy, ecology, and evolutionary biology.

Honeybee workers perform the waggle dance to indicate the range and direction of food.
Great crested grebes perform a complex synchronised courtship display.
Male impalas fighting during the rut

Etymology edit

The modern term ethology derives from the Greek language: ἦθος, ethos meaning "character" and -λογία, -logia meaning "the study of". The term was first popularized by the American entomologist William Morton Wheeler in 1902.[1]

History edit

The beginnings of ethology edit

 
Charles Darwin (1809–1882) explored the expression of emotions in animals.

Ethologists have been concerned particularly with the evolution of behaviour and its understanding in terms of natural selection. In one sense, the first modern ethologist was Charles Darwin, whose 1872 book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals influenced many ethologists. He pursued his interest in behaviour by encouraging his protégé George Romanes, who investigated animal learning and intelligence using an anthropomorphic method, anecdotal cognitivism, that did not gain scientific support.[2]

Other early ethologists, such as Eugène Marais, Charles O. Whitman, Oskar Heinroth, Wallace Craig and Julian Huxley, instead concentrated on behaviours that can be called instinctive in that they occur in all members of a species under specified circumstances.[3][4][1] Their starting point for studying the behaviour of a new species was to construct an ethogram, a description of the main types of behaviour with their frequencies of occurrence. This provided an objective, cumulative database of behaviour.[1]

Growth of the field edit

Due to the work of Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen, ethology developed strongly in continental Europe during the years prior to World War II.[1] After the war, Tinbergen moved to the University of Oxford, and ethology became stronger in the UK, with the additional influence of William Thorpe, Robert Hinde, and Patrick Bateson at the University of Cambridge.[5]

Lorenz, Tinbergen, and von Frisch were jointly awarded the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1973 for their work of developing ethology.[6]

Ethology is now a well-recognized scientific discipline, with its own journals such as Animal Behaviour, Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Animal Cognition, Behaviour, Behavioral Ecology and Ethology. In 1972, the International Society for Human Ethology was founded along with its journal, The Human Ethology Bulletin.[citation needed]

Social ethology edit

In 1972, the English ethologist John H. Crook distinguished comparative ethology from social ethology, and argued that much of the ethology that had existed so far was really comparative ethology—examining animals as individuals—whereas, in the future, ethologists would need to concentrate on the behaviour of social groups of animals and the social structure within them.[7]

E. O. Wilson's book Sociobiology: The New Synthesis appeared in 1975,[8] and since that time, the study of behaviour has been much more concerned with social aspects. It has been driven by the Darwinism associated with Wilson, Robert Trivers, and W. D. Hamilton. The related development of behavioural ecology has helped transform ethology.[9] Furthermore, a substantial rapprochement with comparative psychology has occurred, so the modern scientific study of behaviour offers a spectrum of approaches. In 2020, Tobias Starzak and Albert Newen from the Institute of Philosophy II at the Ruhr University Bochum postulated that animals may have beliefs.[10]

Determinants of behaviour edit

Behaviour is determined by three major factors, namely inborn instincts, learning, and environmental factors. The latter include abiotic and biotic factors. Abiotic factors such as temperature or light conditions have dramatic effects on animals, especially if they are ectothermic or nocturnal. Biotic factors include members of the same species (e.g. sexual behavior), predators (fight or flight), or parasites and diseases.[citation needed]

Instinct edit

 
Kelp gull chicks peck at red spot on mother's beak to stimulate regurgitating reflex

Webster's Dictionary defines instinct as "A largely inheritable and unalterable tendency of an organism to make a complex and specific response to environmental stimuli without involving reason".[11]

Fixed action patterns edit

An important development, associated with the name of Konrad Lorenz though probably due more to his teacher, Oskar Heinroth, was the identification of fixed action patterns. Lorenz popularized these as instinctive responses that would occur reliably in the presence of identifiable stimuli called sign stimuli or "releasing stimuli". Fixed action patterns are now considered to be instinctive behavioural sequences that are relatively invariant within the species and that almost inevitably run to completion.[12]

One example of a releaser is the beak movements of many bird species performed by newly hatched chicks, which stimulates the mother to regurgitate food for her offspring.[13] Other examples are the classic studies by Tinbergen on the egg-retrieval behaviour and the effects of a "supernormal stimulus" on the behaviour of graylag geese.[14][15]

One investigation of this kind was the study of the waggle dance ("dance language") in bee communication by Karl von Frisch.[16]

Learning edit

Habituation edit

Habituation is a simple form of learning and occurs in many animal taxa. It is the process whereby an animal ceases responding to a stimulus. Often, the response is an innate behavior. Essentially, the animal learns not to respond to irrelevant stimuli. For example, prairie dogs (Cynomys ludovicianus) give alarm calls when predators approach, causing all individuals in the group to quickly scramble down burrows. When prairie dog towns are located near trails used by humans, giving alarm calls every time a person walks by is expensive in terms of time and energy. Habituation to humans is therefore an important adaptation in this context.[17][18][19]

Associative learning edit

Associative learning in animal behaviour is any learning process in which a new response becomes associated with a particular stimulus.[20] The first studies of associative learning were made by the Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov, who observed that dogs trained to associate food with the ringing of a bell would salivate on hearing the bell.[21]

Imprinting edit

 
Imprinting in a moose.

Imprinting enables the young to discriminate the members of their own species, vital for reproductive success. This important type of learning only takes place in a very limited period of time. Konrad Lorenz observed that the young of birds such as geese and chickens followed their mothers spontaneously from almost the first day after they were hatched, and he discovered that this response could be imitated by an arbitrary stimulus if the eggs were incubated artificially and the stimulus were presented during a critical period that continued for a few days after hatching.[22]

Cultural learning edit

Observational learning edit
Imitation edit

Imitation is an advanced behavior whereby an animal observes and exactly replicates the behavior of another. The National Institutes of Health reported that capuchin monkeys preferred the company of researchers who imitated them to that of researchers who did not. The monkeys not only spent more time with their imitators but also preferred to engage in a simple task with them even when provided with the option of performing the same task with a non-imitator.[23] Imitation has been observed in recent research on chimpanzees; not only did these chimps copy the actions of another individual, when given a choice, the chimps preferred to imitate the actions of the higher-ranking elder chimpanzee as opposed to the lower-ranking young chimpanzee.[24]

Stimulus and local enhancement edit

Animals can learn using observational learning but without the process of imitation. One way is stimulus enhancement in which individuals become interested in an object as the result of observing others interacting with the object.[25] Increased interest in an object can result in object manipulation which allows for new object-related behaviours by trial-and-error learning. Haggerty (1909) devised an experiment in which a monkey climbed up the side of a cage, placed its arm into a wooden chute, and pulled a rope in the chute to release food. Another monkey was provided an opportunity to obtain the food after watching a monkey go through this process on four occasions. The monkey performed a different method and finally succeeded after trial-and-error.[26] Another example familiar to some cat and dog owners is the ability of their animals to open doors. The action of humans operating the handle to open the door results in the animals becoming interested in the handle and then by trial-and-error, they learn to operate the handle and open the door.

In local enhancement, a demonstrator attracts an observer's attention to a particular location.[27] Local enhancement has been observed to transmit foraging information among birds, rats and pigs.[28] The stingless bee (Trigona corvina) uses local enhancement to locate other members of their colony and food resources.[29]

Social transmission edit

A well-documented example of social transmission of a behaviour occurred in a group of macaques on Hachijojima Island, Japan. The macaques lived in the inland forest until the 1960s, when a group of researchers started giving them potatoes on the beach: soon, they started venturing onto the beach, picking the potatoes from the sand, and cleaning and eating them.[8] About one year later, an individual was observed bringing a potato to the sea, putting it into the water with one hand, and cleaning it with the other. This behaviour was soon expressed by the individuals living in contact with her; when they gave birth, this behaviour was also expressed by their young—a form of social transmission.[30]

Teaching edit

Teaching is a highly specialized aspect of learning in which the "teacher" (demonstrator) adjusts their behaviour to increase the probability of the "pupil" (observer) achieving the desired end-result of the behaviour. For example, orcas are known to intentionally beach themselves to catch pinniped prey.[31] Mother orcas teach their young to catch pinnipeds by pushing them onto the shore and encouraging them to attack the prey. Because the mother orca is altering her behaviour to help her offspring learn to catch prey, this is evidence of teaching.[31] Teaching is not limited to mammals. Many insects, for example, have been observed demonstrating various forms of teaching to obtain food. Ants, for example, will guide each other to food sources through a process called "tandem running," in which an ant will guide a companion ant to a source of food.[32] It has been suggested that the pupil ant is able to learn this route to obtain food in the future or teach the route to other ants. This behaviour of teaching is also exemplified by crows, specifically New Caledonian crows. The adults (whether individual or in families) teach their young adolescent offspring how to construct and utilize tools. For example, Pandanus branches are used to extract insects and other larvae from holes within trees.[33]

Mating and the fight for supremacy edit

 
Courtship display of a sarus crane

Individual reproduction is the most important phase in the proliferation of individuals or genes within a species: for this reason, there exist complex mating rituals, which can be very complex even if they are often regarded as fixed action patterns. The stickleback's complex mating ritual, studied by Tinbergen, is regarded as a notable example.[34]

Often in social life, animals fight for the right to reproduce, as well as social supremacy. A common example of fighting for social and sexual supremacy is the so-called pecking order among poultry. Every time a group of poultry cohabitate for a certain time length, they establish a pecking order. In these groups, one chicken dominates the others and can peck without being pecked. A second chicken can peck all the others except the first, and so on. Chickens higher in the pecking order may at times be distinguished by their healthier appearance when compared to lower level chickens.[citation needed] While the pecking order is establishing, frequent and violent fights can happen, but once established, it is broken only when other individuals enter the group, in which case the pecking order re-establishes from scratch.[35]

Social behaviour edit

Several animal species, including humans, tend to live in groups. Group size is a major aspect of their social environment. Social life is probably a complex and effective survival strategy. It may be regarded as a sort of symbiosis among individuals of the same species: a society is composed of a group of individuals belonging to the same species living within well-defined rules on food management, role assignments and reciprocal dependence.[citation needed]

When biologists interested in evolution theory first started examining social behaviour, some apparently unanswerable questions arose, such as how the birth of sterile castes, like in bees, could be explained through an evolving mechanism that emphasizes the reproductive success of as many individuals as possible, or why, amongst animals living in small groups like squirrels, an individual would risk its own life to save the rest of the group. These behaviours may be examples of altruism.[36] Not all behaviours are altruistic, as indicated by the table below. For example, revengeful behaviour was at one point claimed to have been observed exclusively in Homo sapiens. However, other species have been reported to be vengeful including chimpanzees,[37] as well as anecdotal reports of vengeful camels.[38]

Classification of social behaviours[citation needed]
Type of behaviour Effect on the donor Effect on the receiver
Egoistic Neutral to Increases fitness Decreases fitness
Cooperative Neutral to Increases fitness Neutral to Increases fitness
Altruistic Decreases fitness Neutral to Increases fitness
Revengeful Decreases fitness Decreases fitness

Altruistic behaviour has been explained by the gene-centred view of evolution.[39][40]

Benefits and costs of group living edit

One advantage of group living is decreased predation. If the number of predator attacks stays the same despite increasing prey group size, each prey has a reduced risk of predator attacks through the dilution effect.[9][page needed] Further, according to the selfish herd theory, the fitness benefits associated with group living vary depending on the location of an individual within the group. The theory suggests that conspecifics positioned at the centre of a group will reduce the likelihood predations while those at the periphery will become more vulnerable to attack.[41] In groups, prey can also actively reduce their predation risk through more effective defence tactics, or through earlier detection of predators through increased vigilance.[9]

Another advantage of group living is an increased ability to forage for food. Group members may exchange information about food sources, facilitating the process of resource location.[9][page needed] Honeybees are a notable example of this, using the waggle dance to communicate the location of flowers to the rest of their hive.[42] Predators also receive benefits from hunting in groups, through using better strategies and being able to take down larger prey.[9][page needed]

Some disadvantages accompany living in groups. Living in close proximity to other animals can facilitate the transmission of parasites and disease, and groups that are too large may also experience greater competition for resources and mates.[43]

Group size edit

Theoretically, social animals should have optimal group sizes that maximize the benefits and minimize the costs of group living. However, in nature, most groups are stable at slightly larger than optimal sizes.[9][page needed] Because it generally benefits an individual to join an optimally-sized group, despite slightly decreasing the advantage for all members, groups may continue to increase in size until it is more advantageous to remain alone than to join an overly full group.[44]

Tinbergen's four questions for ethologists edit

Niko Tinbergen argued that ethology needed to include four kinds of explanation in any instance of behaviour:[45][46]

  • Function – How does the behaviour affect the animal's chances of survival and reproduction? Why does the animal respond that way instead of some other way?
  • Causation – What are the stimuli that elicit the response, and how has it been modified by recent learning?
  • Development – How does the behaviour change with age, and what early experiences are necessary for the animal to display the behaviour?
  • Evolutionary history – How does the behaviour compare with similar behaviour in related species, and how might it have begun through the process of phylogeny?

These explanations are complementary rather than mutually exclusive—all instances of behaviour require an explanation at each of these four levels. For example, the function of eating is to acquire nutrients (which ultimately aids survival and reproduction), but the immediate cause of eating is hunger (causation). Hunger and eating are evolutionarily ancient and are found in many species (evolutionary history), and develop early within an organism's lifespan (development). It is easy to confuse such questions—for example, to argue that people eat because they are hungry and not to acquire nutrients—without realizing that the reason people experience hunger is because it causes them to acquire nutrients.[47]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b c d Matthews, Janice R.; Matthews, Robert W. (2009). Insect Behaviour. Springer. p. 13. ISBN 978-90-481-2388-9.
  2. ^ Keeley, Brian L. (2004). "Anthropomorphism, primatomorphism, mammalomorphism: understanding cross-species comparisons" (PDF). York University. p. 527. (PDF) from the original on 17 December 2008. Retrieved 19 December 2008.
  3. ^ "Guide to the Charles Otis Whitman Collection ca. 1911". lib.uchicago.edu. Retrieved 21 September 2022.
  4. ^ Schulze-Hagen, Karl; Birkhead, Timothy R. (1 January 2015). "The ethology and life history of birds: the forgotten contributions of Oskar, Magdalena and Katharina Heinroth". Journal of Ornithology. 156 (1): 9–18. doi:10.1007/s10336-014-1091-3. ISSN 2193-7206. S2CID 14170933.
  5. ^ Bateson, Patrick (1991). The Development and Integration of Behaviour: Essays in Honour of Robert Hinde. Cambridge University Press. p. 479. ISBN 978-0-521-40709-0.
  6. ^ "The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1973". Nobelprize.org. Retrieved 9 September 2016. The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1973 was awarded jointly to Karl von Frisch, Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen 'for their discoveries concerning organization and elicitation of individual and social behaviour patterns'.
  7. ^ Crook, John H.; Goss-Custard, J. D. (1972). "Social Ethology". Annual Review of Psychology. 23 (1): 277–312. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.23.020172.001425.
  8. ^ a b Wilson, Edward O. (2000). Sociobiology: the new synthesis. Harvard University Press. p. 170. ISBN 978-0-674-00089-6.
  9. ^ a b c d e f Davies, Nicholas B.; Krebs, John R.; West, Stuart A. (2012). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology (4th ed.). John Wiley & Sons. ISBN 978-1-4443-3949-9.
  10. ^ "What it means when animals have beliefs". ScienceDaily. 17 June 2020. Retrieved 18 June 2020.
  11. ^ "Instinct". Merriam-Webster Dictionary. 16 August 2023.
  12. ^ Campbell, N. A. (1996). "Chapter 50". Biology (4 ed.). Benjamin Cummings, New York. ISBN 978-0-8053-1957-6.
  13. ^ Bernstein, W. M. (2011). A Basic Theory of Neuropsychoanalysis. Karnac Books. p. 81. ISBN 978-1-85575-809-4.
  14. ^ Tinbergen, Niko (1951). The Study of Instinct. Oxford University Press, New York.
  15. ^ Tinbergen, Niko (1953). The Herring Gull's World. Collins, London.
  16. ^ Buchmann, Stephen (2006). Letters from the Hive: An Intimate History of Bees, Honey, and Humankind. Random House of Canada. p. 105. ISBN 978-0-553-38266-2.
  17. ^ Breed, Michael D. (2001). "Habituation". www.animalbehavioronline.com.
  18. ^ Keil, Frank C.; Wilson, Robert Andrew (2001). The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences. MIT Press. p. 184. ISBN 978-0-262-73144-7.
  19. ^ Bouton, M. E. (2007). . Sunderland. Archived from the original on 31 August 2016. Retrieved 9 September 2016.
  20. ^ "Associative learning". Encyclopædia Britannica. Retrieved 9 September 2014.
  21. ^ Hudmon, Andrew (2005). Learning and memory. Infobase Publishing. p. 35. ISBN 978-0-7910-8638-4.
  22. ^ Mercer, Jean (2006). Understanding attachment: parenting, child care, and emotional development. Greenwood Publishing Group. p. 19. ISBN 978-0-275-98217-1.
  23. ^ . National Institutes of Health. 13 August 2009. Archived from the original on 22 August 2009. Retrieved 8 November 2011.
  24. ^ Horner, Victoria; et al. (19 May 2010). "Prestige Affects Cultural Learning in Chimpanzees". PLOS ONE. 5 (5): e10625. Bibcode:2010PLoSO...510625H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010625. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 2873264. PMID 20502702.
  25. ^ Spence, K. W. (1937). "Experimental studies of learning and higher mental processes in infra-human primates". Psychological Bulletin. 34 (10): 806–850. doi:10.1037/h0061498.
  26. ^ Haggerty, M. E. (1909). "Imitation in monkeys". Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology. 19 (4): 337–455. doi:10.1002/cne.920190402.
  27. ^ Hoppitt, W.; Laland, K. N. (2013). Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models. Princeton University Press. Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-1-4008-4650-4.
  28. ^ Galef, B. G.; Giraldeau, L.-A. (2001). "Social influences on foraging in vertebrates: Causal mechanisms and adaptive functions". Animal Behaviour. 61 (1): 3–15. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1557. PMID 11170692. S2CID 38321280.
  29. ^ F.M.J. Sommerlandt; W. Huber; J. Spaethe (2014). "Social information in the Stingless Bee, Trigona corvina Cockerell (Hymenoptera: Apidae): The use of visual and olfactory cues at the food site". Sociobiology. 61 (4): 401–406. doi:10.13102/sociobiology.v61i4.401-406 (inactive 23 January 2024). ISSN 0361-6525.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of January 2024 (link)
  30. ^ "Japanese Macaque – Macaca fuscata". Blueplanetbiomes.org. Retrieved 8 November 2011.
  31. ^ a b Rendell, Luke; Whitehead, Hal (2001). "Culture in whales and dolphins" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (2): 309–324. doi:10.1017/s0140525x0100396x. PMID 11530544. S2CID 24052064.
  32. ^ Hoppitt, W. J.; Brown, G. R.; Kendal, R.; Rendell, L.; Thornton, A.; Webster, M. M.; Laland, K. N. (2008). "Lessons from animal teaching". Trends in Ecology & Evolution. 23 (9): 486–493. doi:10.1016/j.tree.2008.05.008. PMID 18657877.
  33. ^ Rutz, Christian; Bluff, Lucas A.; Reed, Nicola; Troscianko, Jolyon; Newton, Jason; Inger, Richard; Kacelnik, Alex; Bearhop, Stuart (September 2010). "The Ecological Significance of Tool Use in New Caledonian Crows". Science. 329 (5998): 1523–1526. Bibcode:2010Sci...329.1523R. doi:10.1126/science.1192053. PMID 20847272. S2CID 8888382.
  34. ^ Tinbergen, Niko; Van Iersel, J. J. A. (1947). "'Displacement Reactions' in the Three-Spined Stickleback". Behaviour. 1 (1): 56–63. doi:10.1163/156853948X00038. JSTOR 4532675.
  35. ^ Rajecki, D. W. (1988). "Formation of leap orders in pairs of male domestic chickens". Aggressive Behavior. 14 (6): 425–436. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1988)14:6<425::AID-AB2480140604>3.0.CO;2-#. S2CID 141664966.
  36. ^ Cummings, Mark; Zahn-Waxler, Carolyn; Iannotti, Ronald (1991). Altruism and aggression: biological and social origins. Cambridge University Press. p. 7. ISBN 978-0-521-42367-0.
  37. ^ McCullough, Michael E. (2008). Beyond Revenge: The Evolution of the Forgiveness Instinct. John Wiley & Sons. pp. 79–80. ISBN 978-0-470-26215-3.
  38. ^ De Waal, Frans (2001). The Ape and the Sushi Master: Cultural Reflections by a Primatologist. Basic Books. p. 338. ISBN 978-0-465-04176-3. Retrieved 27 December 2016.
  39. ^ Dawkins, Richard (January 1979). "Twelve Misunderstandings of Kin Selection". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 51 (2): 184–200. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1979.tb00682.x.
  40. ^ Ågren, J. Arvid (2016). "Selfish genetic elements and the gene's-eye view of evolution". Current Zoology. 62 (6): 659–665. doi:10.1093/cz/zow102. ISSN 1674-5507. PMC 5804262. PMID 29491953.
  41. ^ Hamilton, W. D. (1971). "Geometry for the Selfish Herd". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 31 (2): 295–311. Bibcode:1971JThBi..31..295H. doi:10.1016/0022-5193(71)90189-5. PMID 5104951.
  42. ^ Riley, J.; Greggers, U.; Smith, A.; Reynolds, D. R.; Menzel, R. (2005). "The flight paths of honeybees recruited by the waggle dance". Nature. 435 (7039): 205–207. Bibcode:2005Natur.435..205R. doi:10.1038/nature03526. PMID 15889092. S2CID 4413962.
  43. ^ Rathads, Triana (29 August 2007). . Science 360. Archived from the original on 8 May 2014. Retrieved 22 April 2014.
  44. ^ Sibley, R. M. (1983). "Optimal group size is unstable". Animal Behaviour. 31 (3): 947–948. doi:10.1016/s0003-3472(83)80250-4. S2CID 54387192.
  45. ^ Tinbergen, Niko (1963). "On aims and methods in ethology". Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 20 (4): 410–433. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.1963.tb01161.x.
  46. ^ MacDougall-Shackleton, Scott A. (27 July 2011). "The levels of analysis revisited". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 366 (1574): 2076–2085. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0363. PMC 3130367. PMID 21690126.
  47. ^ Barrett et al. (2002) Human Evolutionary Psychology. Princeton University Press. ISBN 9780691096223[page needed]

Further reading edit

  • Burkhardt, Richard W. Jr. "On the Emergence of Ethology as a Scientific Discipline." Conspectus of History 1.7 (1981).

External links edit

  •   Media related to Ethology at Wikimedia Commons

ethology, confused, with, ethnology, animal, behaviour, redirects, here, other, uses, animal, behaviour, disambiguation, journal, journal, branch, zoology, that, studies, behaviour, human, animals, scientific, roots, work, charles, darwin, american, german, or. Not to be confused with ethnology Animal behaviour redirects here For other uses see Animal behaviour disambiguation For the journal see Ethology journal Ethology is a branch of zoology that studies the behaviour of non human animals It has its scientific roots in the work of Charles Darwin and of American and German ornithologists of the late 19th and early 20th century including Charles O Whitman Oskar Heinroth and Wallace Craig The modern discipline of ethology is generally considered to have begun during the 1930s with the work of the Dutch biologist Nikolaas Tinbergen and the Austrian biologists Konrad Lorenz and Karl von Frisch the three winners of the 1973 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine Ethology combines laboratory and field science with a strong relation to neuroanatomy ecology and evolutionary biology Honeybee workers perform the waggle dance to indicate the range and direction of food Great crested grebes perform a complex synchronised courtship display Male impalas fighting during the rut Contents 1 Etymology 2 History 2 1 The beginnings of ethology 2 2 Growth of the field 2 3 Social ethology 3 Determinants of behaviour 3 1 Instinct 3 1 1 Fixed action patterns 3 2 Learning 3 2 1 Habituation 3 2 2 Associative learning 3 2 3 Imprinting 3 2 4 Cultural learning 3 2 4 1 Observational learning 3 2 4 2 Imitation 3 2 4 3 Stimulus and local enhancement 3 2 4 4 Social transmission 3 2 5 Teaching 4 Mating and the fight for supremacy 5 Social behaviour 5 1 Benefits and costs of group living 5 2 Group size 6 Tinbergen s four questions for ethologists 7 See also 8 References 9 Further reading 10 External linksEtymology editThe modern term ethology derives from the Greek language ἦ8os ethos meaning character and logia logia meaning the study of The term was first popularized by the American entomologist William Morton Wheeler in 1902 1 History editThe beginnings of ethology edit nbsp Charles Darwin 1809 1882 explored the expression of emotions in animals Ethologists have been concerned particularly with the evolution of behaviour and its understanding in terms of natural selection In one sense the first modern ethologist was Charles Darwin whose 1872 book The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals influenced many ethologists He pursued his interest in behaviour by encouraging his protege George Romanes who investigated animal learning and intelligence using an anthropomorphic method anecdotal cognitivism that did not gain scientific support 2 Other early ethologists such as Eugene Marais Charles O Whitman Oskar Heinroth Wallace Craig and Julian Huxley instead concentrated on behaviours that can be called instinctive in that they occur in all members of a species under specified circumstances 3 4 1 Their starting point for studying the behaviour of a new species was to construct an ethogram a description of the main types of behaviour with their frequencies of occurrence This provided an objective cumulative database of behaviour 1 Growth of the field edit Due to the work of Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen ethology developed strongly in continental Europe during the years prior to World War II 1 After the war Tinbergen moved to the University of Oxford and ethology became stronger in the UK with the additional influence of William Thorpe Robert Hinde and Patrick Bateson at the University of Cambridge 5 Lorenz Tinbergen and von Frisch were jointly awarded the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine in 1973 for their work of developing ethology 6 Ethology is now a well recognized scientific discipline with its own journals such as Animal Behaviour Applied Animal Behaviour Science Animal Cognition Behaviour Behavioral Ecology and Ethology In 1972 the International Society for Human Ethology was founded along with its journal The Human Ethology Bulletin citation needed Social ethology edit In 1972 the English ethologist John H Crook distinguished comparative ethology from social ethology and argued that much of the ethology that had existed so far was really comparative ethology examining animals as individuals whereas in the future ethologists would need to concentrate on the behaviour of social groups of animals and the social structure within them 7 E O Wilson s book Sociobiology The New Synthesis appeared in 1975 8 and since that time the study of behaviour has been much more concerned with social aspects It has been driven by the Darwinism associated with Wilson Robert Trivers and W D Hamilton The related development of behavioural ecology has helped transform ethology 9 Furthermore a substantial rapprochement with comparative psychology has occurred so the modern scientific study of behaviour offers a spectrum of approaches In 2020 Tobias Starzak and Albert Newen from the Institute of Philosophy II at the Ruhr University Bochum postulated that animals may have beliefs 10 Determinants of behaviour editBehaviour is determined by three major factors namely inborn instincts learning and environmental factors The latter include abiotic and biotic factors Abiotic factors such as temperature or light conditions have dramatic effects on animals especially if they are ectothermic or nocturnal Biotic factors include members of the same species e g sexual behavior predators fight or flight or parasites and diseases citation needed Instinct edit This section needs expansion You can help by adding to it December 2023 nbsp Kelp gull chicks peck at red spot on mother s beak to stimulate regurgitating reflexWebster s Dictionary defines instinct as A largely inheritable and unalterable tendency of an organism to make a complex and specific response to environmental stimuli without involving reason 11 Fixed action patterns edit Main article Fixed action pattern An important development associated with the name of Konrad Lorenz though probably due more to his teacher Oskar Heinroth was the identification of fixed action patterns Lorenz popularized these as instinctive responses that would occur reliably in the presence of identifiable stimuli called sign stimuli or releasing stimuli Fixed action patterns are now considered to be instinctive behavioural sequences that are relatively invariant within the species and that almost inevitably run to completion 12 One example of a releaser is the beak movements of many bird species performed by newly hatched chicks which stimulates the mother to regurgitate food for her offspring 13 Other examples are the classic studies by Tinbergen on the egg retrieval behaviour and the effects of a supernormal stimulus on the behaviour of graylag geese 14 15 One investigation of this kind was the study of the waggle dance dance language in bee communication by Karl von Frisch 16 Learning edit Habituation edit Main article Habituation Habituation is a simple form of learning and occurs in many animal taxa It is the process whereby an animal ceases responding to a stimulus Often the response is an innate behavior Essentially the animal learns not to respond to irrelevant stimuli For example prairie dogs Cynomys ludovicianus give alarm calls when predators approach causing all individuals in the group to quickly scramble down burrows When prairie dog towns are located near trails used by humans giving alarm calls every time a person walks by is expensive in terms of time and energy Habituation to humans is therefore an important adaptation in this context 17 18 19 Associative learning edit Main article Association psychology Further information Classical conditioning and Operant conditioning Associative learning in animal behaviour is any learning process in which a new response becomes associated with a particular stimulus 20 The first studies of associative learning were made by the Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov who observed that dogs trained to associate food with the ringing of a bell would salivate on hearing the bell 21 Imprinting edit Main article Imprinting psychology nbsp Imprinting in a moose Imprinting enables the young to discriminate the members of their own species vital for reproductive success This important type of learning only takes place in a very limited period of time Konrad Lorenz observed that the young of birds such as geese and chickens followed their mothers spontaneously from almost the first day after they were hatched and he discovered that this response could be imitated by an arbitrary stimulus if the eggs were incubated artificially and the stimulus were presented during a critical period that continued for a few days after hatching 22 Cultural learning edit Main article Cultural transmission in animals Observational learning edit Main article Observational learning Imitation edit Main article Imitation Imitation is an advanced behavior whereby an animal observes and exactly replicates the behavior of another The National Institutes of Health reported that capuchin monkeys preferred the company of researchers who imitated them to that of researchers who did not The monkeys not only spent more time with their imitators but also preferred to engage in a simple task with them even when provided with the option of performing the same task with a non imitator 23 Imitation has been observed in recent research on chimpanzees not only did these chimps copy the actions of another individual when given a choice the chimps preferred to imitate the actions of the higher ranking elder chimpanzee as opposed to the lower ranking young chimpanzee 24 Stimulus and local enhancement edit Animals can learn using observational learning but without the process of imitation One way is stimulus enhancement in which individuals become interested in an object as the result of observing others interacting with the object 25 Increased interest in an object can result in object manipulation which allows for new object related behaviours by trial and error learning Haggerty 1909 devised an experiment in which a monkey climbed up the side of a cage placed its arm into a wooden chute and pulled a rope in the chute to release food Another monkey was provided an opportunity to obtain the food after watching a monkey go through this process on four occasions The monkey performed a different method and finally succeeded after trial and error 26 Another example familiar to some cat and dog owners is the ability of their animals to open doors The action of humans operating the handle to open the door results in the animals becoming interested in the handle and then by trial and error they learn to operate the handle and open the door In local enhancement a demonstrator attracts an observer s attention to a particular location 27 Local enhancement has been observed to transmit foraging information among birds rats and pigs 28 The stingless bee Trigona corvina uses local enhancement to locate other members of their colony and food resources 29 Social transmission edit See also Cultural transmission in animals A well documented example of social transmission of a behaviour occurred in a group of macaques on Hachijojima Island Japan The macaques lived in the inland forest until the 1960s when a group of researchers started giving them potatoes on the beach soon they started venturing onto the beach picking the potatoes from the sand and cleaning and eating them 8 About one year later an individual was observed bringing a potato to the sea putting it into the water with one hand and cleaning it with the other This behaviour was soon expressed by the individuals living in contact with her when they gave birth this behaviour was also expressed by their young a form of social transmission 30 Teaching edit See also Animal culture Teaching Teaching is a highly specialized aspect of learning in which the teacher demonstrator adjusts their behaviour to increase the probability of the pupil observer achieving the desired end result of the behaviour For example orcas are known to intentionally beach themselves to catch pinniped prey 31 Mother orcas teach their young to catch pinnipeds by pushing them onto the shore and encouraging them to attack the prey Because the mother orca is altering her behaviour to help her offspring learn to catch prey this is evidence of teaching 31 Teaching is not limited to mammals Many insects for example have been observed demonstrating various forms of teaching to obtain food Ants for example will guide each other to food sources through a process called tandem running in which an ant will guide a companion ant to a source of food 32 It has been suggested that the pupil ant is able to learn this route to obtain food in the future or teach the route to other ants This behaviour of teaching is also exemplified by crows specifically New Caledonian crows The adults whether individual or in families teach their young adolescent offspring how to construct and utilize tools For example Pandanus branches are used to extract insects and other larvae from holes within trees 33 Mating and the fight for supremacy edit nbsp Courtship display of a sarus craneIndividual reproduction is the most important phase in the proliferation of individuals or genes within a species for this reason there exist complex mating rituals which can be very complex even if they are often regarded as fixed action patterns The stickleback s complex mating ritual studied by Tinbergen is regarded as a notable example 34 Often in social life animals fight for the right to reproduce as well as social supremacy A common example of fighting for social and sexual supremacy is the so called pecking order among poultry Every time a group of poultry cohabitate for a certain time length they establish a pecking order In these groups one chicken dominates the others and can peck without being pecked A second chicken can peck all the others except the first and so on Chickens higher in the pecking order may at times be distinguished by their healthier appearance when compared to lower level chickens citation needed While the pecking order is establishing frequent and violent fights can happen but once established it is broken only when other individuals enter the group in which case the pecking order re establishes from scratch 35 Social behaviour editSeveral animal species including humans tend to live in groups Group size is a major aspect of their social environment Social life is probably a complex and effective survival strategy It may be regarded as a sort of symbiosis among individuals of the same species a society is composed of a group of individuals belonging to the same species living within well defined rules on food management role assignments and reciprocal dependence citation needed When biologists interested in evolution theory first started examining social behaviour some apparently unanswerable questions arose such as how the birth of sterile castes like in bees could be explained through an evolving mechanism that emphasizes the reproductive success of as many individuals as possible or why amongst animals living in small groups like squirrels an individual would risk its own life to save the rest of the group These behaviours may be examples of altruism 36 Not all behaviours are altruistic as indicated by the table below For example revengeful behaviour was at one point claimed to have been observed exclusively in Homo sapiens However other species have been reported to be vengeful including chimpanzees 37 as well as anecdotal reports of vengeful camels 38 Classification of social behaviours citation needed Type of behaviour Effect on the donor Effect on the receiverEgoistic Neutral to Increases fitness Decreases fitnessCooperative Neutral to Increases fitness Neutral to Increases fitnessAltruistic Decreases fitness Neutral to Increases fitnessRevengeful Decreases fitness Decreases fitnessAltruistic behaviour has been explained by the gene centred view of evolution 39 40 Benefits and costs of group living edit One advantage of group living is decreased predation If the number of predator attacks stays the same despite increasing prey group size each prey has a reduced risk of predator attacks through the dilution effect 9 page needed Further according to the selfish herd theory the fitness benefits associated with group living vary depending on the location of an individual within the group The theory suggests that conspecifics positioned at the centre of a group will reduce the likelihood predations while those at the periphery will become more vulnerable to attack 41 In groups prey can also actively reduce their predation risk through more effective defence tactics or through earlier detection of predators through increased vigilance 9 Another advantage of group living is an increased ability to forage for food Group members may exchange information about food sources facilitating the process of resource location 9 page needed Honeybees are a notable example of this using the waggle dance to communicate the location of flowers to the rest of their hive 42 Predators also receive benefits from hunting in groups through using better strategies and being able to take down larger prey 9 page needed Some disadvantages accompany living in groups Living in close proximity to other animals can facilitate the transmission of parasites and disease and groups that are too large may also experience greater competition for resources and mates 43 Group size edit Theoretically social animals should have optimal group sizes that maximize the benefits and minimize the costs of group living However in nature most groups are stable at slightly larger than optimal sizes 9 page needed Because it generally benefits an individual to join an optimally sized group despite slightly decreasing the advantage for all members groups may continue to increase in size until it is more advantageous to remain alone than to join an overly full group 44 Tinbergen s four questions for ethologists editMain article Tinbergen s four questions Niko Tinbergen argued that ethology needed to include four kinds of explanation in any instance of behaviour 45 46 Function How does the behaviour affect the animal s chances of survival and reproduction Why does the animal respond that way instead of some other way Causation What are the stimuli that elicit the response and how has it been modified by recent learning Development How does the behaviour change with age and what early experiences are necessary for the animal to display the behaviour Evolutionary history How does the behaviour compare with similar behaviour in related species and how might it have begun through the process of phylogeny These explanations are complementary rather than mutually exclusive all instances of behaviour require an explanation at each of these four levels For example the function of eating is to acquire nutrients which ultimately aids survival and reproduction but the immediate cause of eating is hunger causation Hunger and eating are evolutionarily ancient and are found in many species evolutionary history and develop early within an organism s lifespan development It is easy to confuse such questions for example to argue that people eat because they are hungry and not to acquire nutrients without realizing that the reason people experience hunger is because it causes them to acquire nutrients 47 See also edit nbsp Psychology portal nbsp Zoology portalAnimal behavior consultant Anthrozoology Behavioral ecology Cognitive ethology Deception in animals Human ethology List of abnormal behaviours in animals Tool use by non human animalsReferences edit a b c d Matthews Janice R Matthews Robert W 2009 Insect Behaviour Springer p 13 ISBN 978 90 481 2388 9 Keeley Brian L 2004 Anthropomorphism primatomorphism mammalomorphism understanding cross species comparisons PDF York University p 527 Archived PDF from the original on 17 December 2008 Retrieved 19 December 2008 Guide to the Charles Otis Whitman Collection ca 1911 lib uchicago edu Retrieved 21 September 2022 Schulze Hagen Karl Birkhead Timothy R 1 January 2015 The ethology and life history of birds the forgotten contributions of Oskar Magdalena and Katharina Heinroth Journal of Ornithology 156 1 9 18 doi 10 1007 s10336 014 1091 3 ISSN 2193 7206 S2CID 14170933 Bateson Patrick 1991 The Development and Integration of Behaviour Essays in Honour of Robert Hinde Cambridge University Press p 479 ISBN 978 0 521 40709 0 The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1973 Nobelprize org Retrieved 9 September 2016 The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1973 was awarded jointly to Karl von Frisch Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen for their discoveries concerning organization and elicitation of individual and social behaviour patterns Crook John H Goss Custard J D 1972 Social Ethology Annual Review of Psychology 23 1 277 312 doi 10 1146 annurev ps 23 020172 001425 a b Wilson Edward O 2000 Sociobiology the new synthesis Harvard University Press p 170 ISBN 978 0 674 00089 6 a b c d e f Davies Nicholas B Krebs John R West Stuart A 2012 An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology 4th ed John Wiley amp Sons ISBN 978 1 4443 3949 9 What it means when animals have beliefs ScienceDaily 17 June 2020 Retrieved 18 June 2020 Instinct Merriam Webster Dictionary 16 August 2023 Campbell N A 1996 Chapter 50 Biology 4 ed Benjamin Cummings New York ISBN 978 0 8053 1957 6 Bernstein W M 2011 A Basic Theory of Neuropsychoanalysis Karnac Books p 81 ISBN 978 1 85575 809 4 Tinbergen Niko 1951 The Study of Instinct Oxford University Press New York Tinbergen Niko 1953 The Herring Gull s World Collins London Buchmann Stephen 2006 Letters from the Hive An Intimate History of Bees Honey and Humankind Random House of Canada p 105 ISBN 978 0 553 38266 2 Breed Michael D 2001 Habituation www animalbehavioronline com Keil Frank C Wilson Robert Andrew 2001 The MIT encyclopedia of the cognitive sciences MIT Press p 184 ISBN 978 0 262 73144 7 Bouton M E 2007 Learning and behavior A contemporary synthesis Sunderland Archived from the original on 31 August 2016 Retrieved 9 September 2016 Associative learning Encyclopaedia Britannica Retrieved 9 September 2014 Hudmon Andrew 2005 Learning and memory Infobase Publishing p 35 ISBN 978 0 7910 8638 4 Mercer Jean 2006 Understanding attachment parenting child care and emotional development Greenwood Publishing Group p 19 ISBN 978 0 275 98217 1 Imitation Promotes Social Bonding in Primates August 13 2009 News Release National Institutes of Health 13 August 2009 Archived from the original on 22 August 2009 Retrieved 8 November 2011 Horner Victoria et al 19 May 2010 Prestige Affects Cultural Learning in Chimpanzees PLOS ONE 5 5 e10625 Bibcode 2010PLoSO 510625H doi 10 1371 journal pone 0010625 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 2873264 PMID 20502702 Spence K W 1937 Experimental studies of learning and higher mental processes in infra human primates Psychological Bulletin 34 10 806 850 doi 10 1037 h0061498 Haggerty M E 1909 Imitation in monkeys Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology 19 4 337 455 doi 10 1002 cne 920190402 Hoppitt W Laland K N 2013 Social Learning An Introduction to Mechanisms Methods and Models Princeton University Press Princeton University Press ISBN 978 1 4008 4650 4 Galef B G Giraldeau L A 2001 Social influences on foraging in vertebrates Causal mechanisms and adaptive functions Animal Behaviour 61 1 3 15 doi 10 1006 anbe 2000 1557 PMID 11170692 S2CID 38321280 F M J Sommerlandt W Huber J Spaethe 2014 Social information in the Stingless Bee Trigona corvina Cockerell Hymenoptera Apidae The use of visual and olfactory cues at the food site Sociobiology 61 4 401 406 doi 10 13102 sociobiology v61i4 401 406 inactive 23 January 2024 ISSN 0361 6525 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint DOI inactive as of January 2024 link Japanese Macaque Macaca fuscata Blueplanetbiomes org Retrieved 8 November 2011 a b Rendell Luke Whitehead Hal 2001 Culture in whales and dolphins PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 2 309 324 doi 10 1017 s0140525x0100396x PMID 11530544 S2CID 24052064 Hoppitt W J Brown G R Kendal R Rendell L Thornton A Webster M M Laland K N 2008 Lessons from animal teaching Trends in Ecology amp Evolution 23 9 486 493 doi 10 1016 j tree 2008 05 008 PMID 18657877 Rutz Christian Bluff Lucas A Reed Nicola Troscianko Jolyon Newton Jason Inger Richard Kacelnik Alex Bearhop Stuart September 2010 The Ecological Significance of Tool Use in New Caledonian Crows Science 329 5998 1523 1526 Bibcode 2010Sci 329 1523R doi 10 1126 science 1192053 PMID 20847272 S2CID 8888382 Tinbergen Niko Van Iersel J J A 1947 Displacement Reactions in the Three Spined Stickleback Behaviour 1 1 56 63 doi 10 1163 156853948X00038 JSTOR 4532675 Rajecki D W 1988 Formation of leap orders in pairs of male domestic chickens Aggressive Behavior 14 6 425 436 doi 10 1002 1098 2337 1988 14 6 lt 425 AID AB2480140604 gt 3 0 CO 2 S2CID 141664966 Cummings Mark Zahn Waxler Carolyn Iannotti Ronald 1991 Altruism and aggression biological and social origins Cambridge University Press p 7 ISBN 978 0 521 42367 0 McCullough Michael E 2008 Beyond Revenge The Evolution of the Forgiveness Instinct John Wiley amp Sons pp 79 80 ISBN 978 0 470 26215 3 De Waal Frans 2001 The Ape and the Sushi Master Cultural Reflections by a Primatologist Basic Books p 338 ISBN 978 0 465 04176 3 Retrieved 27 December 2016 Dawkins Richard January 1979 Twelve Misunderstandings of Kin Selection Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 51 2 184 200 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 1979 tb00682 x Agren J Arvid 2016 Selfish genetic elements and the gene s eye view of evolution Current Zoology 62 6 659 665 doi 10 1093 cz zow102 ISSN 1674 5507 PMC 5804262 PMID 29491953 Hamilton W D 1971 Geometry for the Selfish Herd Journal of Theoretical Biology 31 2 295 311 Bibcode 1971JThBi 31 295H doi 10 1016 0022 5193 71 90189 5 PMID 5104951 Riley J Greggers U Smith A Reynolds D R Menzel R 2005 The flight paths of honeybees recruited by the waggle dance Nature 435 7039 205 207 Bibcode 2005Natur 435 205R doi 10 1038 nature03526 PMID 15889092 S2CID 4413962 Rathads Triana 29 August 2007 A Look at Animal Social Groups Science 360 Archived from the original on 8 May 2014 Retrieved 22 April 2014 Sibley R M 1983 Optimal group size is unstable Animal Behaviour 31 3 947 948 doi 10 1016 s0003 3472 83 80250 4 S2CID 54387192 Tinbergen Niko 1963 On aims and methods in ethology Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie 20 4 410 433 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 1963 tb01161 x MacDougall Shackleton Scott A 27 July 2011 The levels of analysis revisited Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 366 1574 2076 2085 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0363 PMC 3130367 PMID 21690126 Barrett et al 2002 Human Evolutionary Psychology Princeton University Press ISBN 9780691096223 page needed Further reading editBurkhardt Richard W Jr On the Emergence of Ethology as a Scientific Discipline Conspectus of History 1 7 1981 External links edit nbsp Media related to Ethology at Wikimedia Commons Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Ethology amp oldid 1206720750, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.