fbpx
Wikipedia

Imitation

Imitation (from Latin imitatio, "a copying, imitation"[1]) is a behavior whereby an individual observes and replicates another's behavior. Imitation is also a form of that leads to the "development of traditions, and ultimately our culture. It allows for the transfer of information (behaviors, customs, etc.) between individuals and down generations without the need for genetic inheritance."[2] The word imitation can be applied in many contexts, ranging from animal training to politics.[3] The term generally refers to conscious behavior; subconscious imitation is termed mirroring.[4]

A toddler imitates his father.

Anthropology and social sciences

In anthropology, some theories hold that all cultures imitate ideas from one of a few original cultures or several cultures whose influence overlaps geographically. Evolutionary diffusion theory holds that cultures influence one another, but that similar ideas can be developed in isolation.

Scholars[5] as well as popular authors[6][7] have argued that the role of imitation in humans is unique among animals. However, this claim has been recently challenged by scientific research which observed social learning and imitative abilities in animals.

Psychologist Kenneth Kaye showed[8][9] that the ability of infants to match the sounds or gestures of an adult depends on an interactive process of turn-taking over many successive trials, in which adults' instinctive behavior plays as great a role as that of the infant. These writers assume that evolution would have selected imitative abilities as fit because those who were good at it had a wider arsenal of learned behavior at their disposal, including tool-making and language.

However, research also suggests that imitative behaviors and other social learning processes are only selected for when outnumbered or accompanied by asocial learning processes: an over-saturation of imitation and imitating individuals leads humans to collectively copy inefficient strategies and evolutionarily maladaptive behaviors, thereby reducing flexibility to new environmental contexts that require adaptation.[10] Research suggests imitative social learning hinders the acquisition of knowledge in novel environments and in situations where asocial learning is faster and more advantageous.[11][12]

In the mid-20th century, social scientists began to study how and why people imitate ideas. Everett Rogers pioneered innovation diffusion studies, identifying factors in adoption and profiles of adopters of ideas.[13] Imitation mechanisms play a central role in both analytical and empirical models of collective human behavior.[14]

Neuroscience

Humans are capable of imitating movements, actions, skills, behaviors, gestures, pantomimes, mimics, vocalizations, sounds, speech, etc. and that we have particular "imitation systems" in the brain is old neurological knowledge dating back to Hugo Karl Liepmann. Liepmann's model 1908 "Das hierarchische Modell der Handlungsplanung" (the hierarchical model of action planning) is still valid. On studying the cerebral localization of function, Liepmann postulated that planned or commanded actions were prepared in the parietal lobe of the brain's dominant hemisphere, and also frontally. His most important pioneering work is when extensively studying patients with lesions in these brain areas, he discovered that the patients lost (among other things) the ability to imitate. He was the one who coined the term "apraxia" and differentiated between ideational and ideomotor apraxia. It is in this basic and wider frame of classical neurological knowledge that the discovery of the mirror neuron has to be seen. Though mirror neurons were first discovered in macaques, their discovery also relates to humans.[15]

Human brain studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) revealed a network of regions in the inferior frontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex which are typically activated during imitation tasks.[16] It has been suggested that these regions contain mirror neurons similar to the mirror neurons recorded in the macaque monkey.[17] However, it is not clear if macaques spontaneously imitate each other in the wild.

Neurologist V. S. Ramachandran argues that the evolution of mirror neurons were important in the human acquisition of complex skills such as language and believes the discovery of mirror neurons to be a most important advance in neuroscience.[18] However, little evidence directly supports the theory that mirror neuron activity is involved in cognitive functions such as empathy or learning by imitation.[19]

Evidence is accumulating that bottlenose dolphins employ imitation to learn hunting and other skills from other dolphins.[20][21]

Japanese monkeys have been seen to spontaneously begin washing potatoes after seeing humans washing them.[22]

Mirror neuron system

Research has been conducted to locate where in the brain specific parts and neurological systems are activated when humans imitate behaviors and actions of others, discovering a mirror neuron system. This neuron system allows a person to observe and then recreate the actions of others. Mirror neurons are premotor and parietal cells in the macaque brain that fire when the animal performs a goal directed action and when it sees others performing the same action."[23] Evidence suggests that the mirror neuron system also allows people to comprehend and understand the intentions and emotions of others.[24] Problems of the mirror neuron system may be correlated with the social inadequacies of autism. There have been many studies done showing that children with autism, compared with typically-developing children, demonstrate reduced activity in the frontal mirror neuron system area when observing or imitating facial emotional expressions. Of course, the higher the severity of the disease, the lower the activity in the mirror neuron system is.[23]

Animal behavior

Scientists debate whether animals can consciously imitate the unconscious incitement from sentinel animals, whether imitation is uniquely human, or whether humans do a complex version of what other animals do.[25][26] The current controversy is partly definitional. Thorndike uses "learning to do an act from seeing it done."[27] It has two major shortcomings: first, by using "seeing" it restricts imitation to the visual domain and excludes, e.g., vocal imitation and, second, it would also include mechanisms such as priming, contagious behavior and social facilitation,[28] which most scientist distinguish as separate forms of observational learning. Thorpe suggested defining imitation as "the copying of a novel or otherwise improbable act or utterance, or some act for which there is clearly no instinctive tendency."[29] This definition is favored by many scholars, though questions have been raised how strictly the term "novel" has to be interpreted and how exactly a performed act has to match the demonstration to count as a copy.

Hayes and Hayes (1952) used the "do-as-I-do" procedure to demonstrate the imitative abilities of their trained chimpanzee "Viki."[30] Their study was repeatedly criticized for its subjective interpretations of their subjects' responses. Replications of this study[31] found much lower matching degrees between subjects and models. However, imitation research focusing on the copying fidelity got new momentum from a study by Voelkl and Huber.[32] They analyzed the motion trajectories of both model and observer monkeys and found a high matching degree in their movement patterns.

Paralleling these studies, comparative psychologists provided tools or apparatuses that could be handled in different ways. Heyes[33][34] and co-workers reported evidence for imitation in rats that pushed a lever in the same direction as their models, though later on they withdrew their claims due to methodological problems in their original setup.[35] By trying to design a testing paradigm that is less arbitrary than pushing a lever to the left or to the right, Custance and co-workers[36] introduced the "artificial fruit" paradigm, where a small object could be opened in different ways to retrieve food placed inside—not unlike a hard-shelled fruit. Using this paradigm, scientists reported evidence for imitation in monkeys[37][38] and apes.[39][40][41] There remains a problem with such tool (or apparatus) use studies: what animals might learn in such studies need not be the actual behavior patterns (i.e., the actions) that were observed. Instead they might learn about some effects in the environment (i.e., how the tool moves, or how the apparatus works).[42] This type of observational learning, which focuses on results, not actions, has been dubbed emulation (see Emulation (observational learning)).

In an article written by Carl Zimmer, he looked into a study being done by Derek Lyons, focusing on human evolution, in which he studied a chimpanzee. He first started with showing the chimpanzee how to retrieve food from a box. The chimpanzee soon caught on and did exactly what the scientist just did. They wanted to see if the chimpanzee's brain functioned just like a human brain, so they replicated the experiment using 16 children, following the same procedure; once the children saw how it was done, they followed the same exact steps.[43]

Imitation in animals

Blackbird imitating the vehicle motion alarm of a local garbage truck in Brastad, Sweden.

Imitation in animals is a study in the field of social learning where learning behavior is observed in animals specifically how animals learn and adapt through imitation. Ethologists can classify imitation in animals by the learning of certain behaviors from conspecifics.[44] More specifically, these behaviors are usually unique to the species and can be complex in nature and can benefit the individual's survival.[44]

Some scientists believe true imitation is only produced by humans, arguing that simple learning though sight is not enough to sustain as a being who can truly imitate.[45] Thorpe defines true imitation as "the copying of a novel or otherwise improbable act or utterance, or some act for which there is clearly no instinctive tendency," which is highly debated for its portrayal of imitation as a mindless repeating act.[45] True imitation is produced when behavioral, visual and vocal imitation is achieved, not just the simple reproduction of exclusive behaviors.[45] Imitation is not a simple reproduction of what one sees; rather it incorporates intention and purpose.[45] Animal imitation can range from survival purpose; imitating as a function of surviving or adapting, to unknown possible curiosity, which vary between different animals and produce different results depending on the measured intelligence of the animal.[45]

There is considerable evidence to support true imitation in animals.[46] Experiments performed on apes, birds and more specifically the Japanese quail have provided positive results to imitating behavior, demonstrating imitation of opaque behavior.[46] However the problem that lies is in the discrepancies between what is considered true imitation in behavior.[46] Birds have demonstrated visual imitation, where the animal simply does as it sees.[46] Studies on apes however have proven more advanced results in imitation, being able to remember and learn from what they imitate.[46] Studies have demonstrated far more positive results with behavioral imitation in primates and birds than any other type of animal.[46] Imitation in non-primate mammals and other animals have been proven difficult to conclude solid positive results for and poses a difficult question to scientists on why that is so.[46]

Theories

There are two types of theories of imitation, transformational and associative. Transformational theories suggest that the information that is required to display certain behavior is created internally through cognitive processes and observing these behaviors provides incentive to duplicate them.[47] Meaning we already have the codes to recreate any behavior and observing it results in its replication. Albert Bandura's "social cognitive theory" is one example of a transformational theory.[48] Associative, or sometimes referred to as "contiguity",[49] theories suggest that the information required to display certain behaviors does not come from within ourselves but solely from our surroundings and experiences.[47] These theories have not yet provided testable predictions in the field of social learning in animals and have yet to conclude strong results.[47]

New developments

There have been three major developments in the field of animal imitation. The first, behavioral ecologists and experimental psychologists found there to be adaptive patterns in behaviors in different vertebrate species in biologically important situations.[50] The second, primatologists and comparative psychologists have found imperative evidence that suggest true learning through imitation in animals.[50] The third, population biologists and behavioral ecologists created experiments that demand animals to depend on social learning in certain manipulated environments.[50]

Child development

Developmental psychologist Jean Piaget noted that children in a developmental phase he called the sensorimotor stage (a period which lasts up to the first two years of a child) begin to imitate observed actions.[51] This is an important stage in the development of a child because the child is beginning to think symbolically, associating behaviors with actions, thus setting the child up for the development of further symbolic thinking. Imitative learning also plays a crucial role in the development of cognitive and social communication behaviors, such as language, play, and joint attention. Imitation serves as both a learning and a social function because new skills and knowledge are acquired, and communication skills are improved by interacting in social and emotional exchanges. It is shown, however, that "children with autism exhibit significant deficits in imitation that are associated with impairments in other social communication skills."[52] To help children with autism, reciprocal imitation training (RIT) is used. It is a naturalistic imitation intervention that helps teach the social benefits of imitation during play by increasing child responsiveness and by increasing imitative language.[52]

Reinforcement learning, both positive and negative, and punishment, are used by people that children imitate to either promote or discontinue behavior. If a child imitates a certain type of behavior or action and the consequences are rewarding, the child is very likely to continue performing the same behavior or action. The behavior "has been reinforced (i.e. strengthened)".[53][self-published source?] However, if the imitation is not accepted and approved by others, then the behavior will be weakened.

Naturally, children are surrounded by many different types of people that influence their actions and behaviors, including parents, family members, teachers, peers, and even characters on television programs. These different types of individuals that are observed are called models. According to Saul McLeod, "these models provide examples of masculine and feminine behavior to observe and imitate."[53] Children imitate the behavior they have observed from others, regardless of the gender of the person and whether or not the behavior is gender appropriate. However, it has been proven that children will reproduce the behavior that "its society deems appropriate for its sex."[53]

Infants

Infants have the ability to reveal an understanding of certain outcomes before they occur, therefore in this sense they can somewhat imitate what they have perceived. Andrew N. Meltzoff, ran a series of tasks involving 14-month-old infants to imitate actions they perceived from adults. In this gathering he had concluded that the infants, before trying to reproduce the actions they wish to imitate, somehow revealed an understanding of the intended goal even though they failed to replicate the result wished to be imitated. These task implicated that the infants knew the goal intended.[54] Gergely, Bekkering, and Király (2002) figured that infants not only understand the intended goal but also the intentions of the person they were trying to imitate engaging in "rational imitation", as described by Tomasello, Carpenter and others [54]

It has long been claimed that newborn humans imitate bodily gestures and facial expressions as soon as their first few days of life.[55][56] For example, in a study conducted at the Mailman Centre for Child Development at the University of Miami Medical School, 74 newborn babies (with a mean age of 36 hours) were tested to see if they were able to imitate a smile, a frown and a pout, and a wide-open mouth and eyes. An observer stood behind the experimenter (so he/she couldn't see what facial expressions were being made by the experimenter) and watched only the babies' facial expressions, recording their results. Just by looking only at the babies' faces, the observer was more often able to correctly guess what facial expression was being presented to the child by the experimenter.[57] After the results were calculated, "the researchers concluded that...babies have an innate ability to compare an expression they see with their own sense of muscular feedback from making the movements to match that expression."[57]

However, the idea that imitation is an inborn ability has been recently challenged. A research group from the University of Queensland in Australia carried out the largest-ever longitudinal study of neonatal imitation in humans. One hundred and nine newborns were shown a variety of gestures including tongue protrusion, mouth opening, happy and sad facial expressions, at four time points between one week and 9 weeks of age. The results failed to reveal compelling evidence that newborns imitate: Infants were just as likely to produce matching and non-matching gestures in response to what they saw.[58]

At around eight months, infants will start to copy their child care providers' movements when playing pat-a-cake and peek-a-boo, as well as imitating familiar gestures, such as clapping hands together or patting a doll's back. At around 18 months, infants will then begin to imitate simple actions they observe adults doing, such as taking a toy phone out of a purse and saying "hello", pretending to sweep with a child-sized broom, as well as imitating using a toy hammer.[citation needed]

Toddlers

At around 30–36 months, toddlers will start to imitate their parents by pretending to get ready for work and school and saying the last word(s) of what an adult just said. For example, toddlers may say "bowl" or "a bowl" after they hear someone say, "That's a bowl." They may also imitate the way family members communicate by using the same gestures and words. For example, a toddler will say, "Mommy bye-bye" after the father says, "Mommy went bye-bye."[59]

Toddlers love to imitate their parents and help when they can; imitation helps toddlers learn, and through their experiences lasting impressions are made. 12- to 36-month-olds learn by doing, not by watching, and so it is often recommended to be a good role model and caretaker by showing them simple tasks like putting on socks or holding a spoon.[60]

Duke developmental psychologist Carol Eckerman did a study on toddlers imitating toddlers and found that at the age of 2 children involve themselves in imitation play to communicate with one another. This can be seen within a culture or across different cultures. 3 common imitative patterns Eckerman found were reciprocal imitation, follow-the-leader, and lead-follow.[61]

Kenneth Kaye's "apprenticeship" theory of imitation rejected assumptions that other authors had made about its development. His research showed that there is no one simple imitation skill with its own course of development. What changes is the type of behavior imitated.[62]

An important agenda for infancy is the progressive imitation of higher levels of use of signs, until the ultimate achievement of symbols. The principal role played by parents in this process is their provision of salient models within the facilitating frames that channel the infant's attention and organize his imitative efforts.

Gender and age differences

 
A small boy of Matera, Italy, unconsciously repeats the gesture of his grandmother's hands, ca. 1948 – ca. 1955

Imitation and imitative behaviors do not manifest ubiquitously and evenly in all human individuals; some individuals rely more on imitated information than others.[63] Although imitation is very useful when it comes to cognitive learning with toddlers, research has shown that there are some gender and age differences when it comes to imitation. Research done to judge imitation in toddlers 2–3 years old shows that when faced with certain conditions "2-year-olds displayed more motor imitation than 3-year-olds, and 3-year-olds displayed more verbal-reality imitation than 2-year-olds. Boys displayed more motor imitation than girls."[64]

No other research is more controversial pertaining gender differences in toddler imitation than renowned psychologist, Bandura's, bobo doll experiments.[65] The goal of the experiment was to see what happens to toddlers when exposed to aggressive and non-aggressive adults, would the toddlers imitate the behavior of the adults and if so, which gender is more likely to imitate the aggressive adult. In the beginning of the experiment Bandura had several predictions that actually came true. Children exposed to violent adults will imitate the actions of that adult when the adult is not present, boys who had observed an adult of the opposite sex act aggressively are less likely to act violently than those who witnessed a male adult act violently. In fact "boys who observed an adult male behaving violently were more influenced than those who had observed a female model behavior aggressively". One fascinating observation was that while boys are likely to imitate physical acts of violence, girls are likely to imitate verbal acts of violence.

Negative imitation

Imitation plays a major role on how a toddler interprets the world. Much of a child's understanding is derived from imitation, due to a lack of verbal skill imitation in toddlers for communication.[citation needed] It is what connects them to the communicating world, as they continue to grow they begin to learn more. This may mean that it is crucial for parents to be cautious as to how they act and behave around their toddlers. Imitation is the toddlers way of confirming and dis-conforming socially acceptable actions in society. Actions like washing dishes, cleaning up the house and doing chores are actions you want your toddlers to imitate. Imitating negative things is something that is never beyond young toddlers. If they are exposed to cursing and violence, it is going to be what the child views as the norm of their world, since imitation is the "mental activity that helps to formulate the conceptions of the world for toddlers".[66] So it is important for parents to be careful what they say or do in front of their children.[citation needed]

Autism

Children with autism exhibit significant impairment in imitation skills.[52] Imitation deficits have been reported on a variety of tasks including symbolic and non-symbolic body movements, symbolic and functional object use, vocalizations, and facial expressions.[52] In contrast, typically-developing children can copy a broad range of novel (as well as familiar) rules from a very early age.[67] Problems with imitation discriminate children with autism from those with other developmental disorders as early as age 2 and continue into adulthood.[68]

Children with autism exhibit significant deficits in imitation that are associated with impairments in other social communication skills. It is unclear whether imitation is mediating these relationships directly, or whether they are due to some other developmental variable that is also reflected in the measurement of imitation skills.[52]

On the contrary, research from the early 21st century suggests that people affected with forms of high-functioning autism easily interact with one another by using a more analytically-centered communication approach rather than an imitative cue-based approach,[69] suggesting that reduced imitative capabilities do not affect abilities for expressive social behavior but only the understanding of said social behavior. Social communication is not negatively affected when said communication involves less or no imitation. Children with autism may have significant problems understanding typical social communication not because of inherent social deficits, but because of differences in communication style which affect reciprocal understanding.[70][71]

Autistic individuals are also shown to possess increased analytical, cognitive, and visual processing,[72][73][74] suggesting that they have no true impairments in observing the actions of others but may decide not to imitate them because they do not analytically understand them.[75] A 2016 study has shown that involuntary, spontaneous facial mimicry – which supposedly depends on the mirror neuron system – is intact in individuals with autism, contrasting with previous studies and suggesting that the mirror neuron system is not inherently broken in autistic individuals.[76]

Automatic imitation

The automatic imitation comes very fast when a stimulus is given to replicate. The imitation can match the commands with the visual stimulus (compatible) or it cannot match the commands with the visual stimulus (incompatible). For example: 'Simon Says', a game played with children where they are told to follow the commands given by the adult. In this game, the adult gives the commands and shows the actions; the commands given can either match the action to be done or it will not match the action. The children who imitate the adult who has given the command with the correct action will stay in the game. The children who imitate the command with the wrong action will go out of the game, and this is where the child's automatic imitation comes into play. Psychologically, the visual stimulus being looked upon by the child is being imitated faster than the imitation of the command. In addition, the response times were faster in compatible scenarios than in incompatible scenarios.[77]

Children are surrounded by many different people, day by day. Their parents make a big impact on them, and usually what the children do is what they have seen their parent do. In this article they found that a child, simply watching its mother sweep the floor, right after soon picks up on it and starts to imitate the mother by sweeping the floor. By the children imitating, they are really teaching themselves how to do things without instruction from the parent or guardian. Toddlers love to play the game of house. They picked up on this game of house by television, school or at home; they play the game how they see it. The kids imitate their parents or anybody in their family. In the article it says it is so easy for them to pick up on the things they see on an everyday basis.[citation needed]

Over-imitation

Over-imitation is "the tendency of young children to copy all of an adult model's actions, even components that are irrelevant for the task at hand."[78] According to this human and cross-cultural phenomenon, a child has a strong tendency to automatically encode the deliberate action of an adult as causally meaningful even when the child observes evidence that proves that its performance is unnecessary. It is suggested that over-imitation "may be critical to the transmission of human culture." Experiments done by Lyons et al. (2007) has shown that when there are obvious pedagogical cues, children tend to imitate step by step, including many unnecessary steps; without pedagogical cues, children will simply skip those useless steps.[79]

However, another study suggests that children do not just "blindly follow the crowd" since they can also be just as discriminating as adults in choosing whether an unnecessary action should be copied or not.[80] They may imitate additional but unnecessary steps to a novel process if the adult demonstrations are all the same. However, in cases where one out of four adults showed a better technique, only 40% actually copied the extra step, as described by Evans, Carpenter and others.[81] Children’s imitation is selective, also known as “selective imitation”. Studies have shown that children tend to imitate older, competitive, and trustworthy individuals.[82]

Deferred imitation

Piaget coined the term deferred imitation and suggested that it arises out of the child's increasing ability to "form mental representations of behavior performed by others."[51] Deferred imitation is also "the ability to reproduce a previously witnessed action or sequence of actions in the absence of current perceptual support for the action."[2] Instead of copying what is currently occurring, individuals repeat the action or behavior later on. It appears that infants show an improving ability for deferred imitation as they get older, especially by 24 months. By 24 months, infants are able to imitate action sequences after a delay of up to three months, meaning that "they're able to generalize knowledge they have gained from one test environment to another and from one test object to another."[2]

A child's deferred imitation ability "to form mental representations of actions occurring in everyday life and their knowledge of communicative gestures" has also been linked to earlier productive language development.[83] Between 9 (preverbal period) and 16 months (verbal period), deferred imitation performance on a standard actions-on-objects task was consistent in one longitudinal study testing participants' ability to complete a target action, with high achievers at 9 months remaining so at 16 months. Gestural development at 9 months was also linked to productive language at 16 months. Researchers now believe that early deferred imitation ability is indicative of early declarative memory, also considered a predictor of productive language development.

See also

References

  1. ^ imitation, Online etymology dictionary
  2. ^ a b c Hopper, Lydia M. (2010). . Psychologist. 23 (4): 294–7. Archived from the original on August 3, 2014. Retrieved March 8, 2021.
  3. ^ Moss, Ellen; Strayer, F. F. (1988). "Imitation is the Greatest form of Flattery". PsycCRITIQUES. 33 (11): 970–971. doi:10.1037/026218.
  4. ^ Chartrand, T.; Bargh, J. (1999). "The Chameleon Effect: The Perception-Behavior Link and Social Interaction" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. New York University. 76 (6): 893–910. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893. PMID 10402679. Retrieved September 28, 2014.
  5. ^ George Herbert Mead, Mind, Self, and Society; Kenneth Kaye, The Mental and Social Life of Babies, U. Chicago Press 1982[page needed]
  6. ^ Susan Blackmore, The Meme Machine, Oxford U. Press 2007[page needed]
  7. ^ Jared Diamond, The Third Chimpanzee, HarperCollins 1992.[page needed]
  8. ^ In M. Bullowa, ed. Before Speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication, Cambridge U. Press 1979[page needed]
  9. ^ Kaye, Kenneth; Marcus, Janet (1981). "Infant imitation: The sensory-motor agenda". Developmental Psychology. 17 (3): 258. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.258.
  10. ^ Rendell, Luke; Fogarty, Laurel; Laland, Kevin N. (2010). "Rogers' Paradox Recast and Resolved: Population Structure and the Evolution of Social Learning Strategies". Evolution. 64 (2): 534–548. doi:10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00817.x. ISSN 1558-5646. PMID 19674093. S2CID 2898779.
  11. ^ Laland, Kevin N.; Williams, Kerry (1998). "Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy". Behavioral Ecology. 9 (5): 493–499. doi:10.1093/beheco/9.5.493. ISSN 1045-2249.
  12. ^ Johnstone, R. A.; Dall, S. R. X.; Giraldeau, Luc–Alain; Valone, Thomas J.; Templeton, Jennifer J. (November 29, 2002). "Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences. 357 (1427): 1559–1566. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1065. PMC 1693065. PMID 12495513.
  13. ^ Rogers, EM (2010). "Diffusion of innovations". Simon and Schuster.
  14. ^ Castellano, Claudio; Fortunato, Santo; Loreto, Vittorio (May 11, 2009). "Statistical physics of social dynamics". Reviews of Modern Physics. 81 (2): 591–646. arXiv:0710.3256. Bibcode:2009RvMP...81..591C. doi:10.1103/RevModPhys.81.591. S2CID 118376889.
  15. ^ Bonini, Luca; Ferrari, Pier Francesco (2011). "Evolution of mirror systems: a simple mechanism for complex cognitive functions". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1225 (1): 166–175. Bibcode:2011NYASA1225..166B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06002.x. PMC 3113469. PMID 21535003.
  16. ^ Iacoboni, M.; Woods, RP; Brass, M; Bekkering, H; Mazziotta, JC; Rizzolatti, G (1999). "Cortical Mechanisms of Human Imitation". Science. 286 (5449): 2526–8. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.555.8075. doi:10.1126/science.286.5449.2526. PMID 10617472.
  17. ^ Rizzolatti, Giacomo; Craighero, Laila (2004). "The Mirror-Neuron System". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 27: 169–92. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230. PMID 15217330. S2CID 1729870.
  18. ^ Ramachandran, V.S. "Mirror Neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind 'the great leap forward' in human evolution". Edge Foundation.
  19. ^ Dinstein, Ilan; Thomas, Cibu; Behrmann, Marlene; Heeger, David J. (2008). "A mirror up to nature". Current Biology. 18 (1): R13–8. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.004. PMC 2517574. PMID 18177704.
  20. ^ Krützen, Michael; Mann, Janet; Heithaus, Michael R.; Connor, Richard C.; Bejder, Lars; Sherwin, William B. (2005). "Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 102 (25): 8939–43. Bibcode:2005PNAS..102.8939K. doi:10.1073/pnas.0500232102. PMC 1157020. PMID 15947077.
  21. ^ . WDCS, the Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society. Archived from the original on January 14, 2009.
  22. ^ Boesch, Christophe; Tomasello, Michael (December 1998). (PDF). Current Anthropology. 39 (5): 591–614. doi:10.1086/204785. S2CID 55562574. Archived from the original (PDF) on February 26, 2019.
  23. ^ a b Iacoboni, Marco; Dapretto, Mirella (2006). "The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 7 (12): 942–51. doi:10.1038/nrn2024. PMID 17115076. S2CID 9463011.
  24. ^ Geiger, Alexander; Bente, Gary; Lammers, Sebastian; Tepest, Ralf; Roth, Daniel; Bzdok, Danilo; Vogeley, Kai (November 15, 2019). "Distinct functional roles of the mirror neuron system and the mentalizing system". NeuroImage. 202: 116102. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116102. ISSN 1053-8119. PMID 31446128. S2CID 201125892.
  25. ^ Zentall, Thomas R. (2006). "Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms". Animal Cognition. 9 (4): 335–53. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0039-2. PMID 17024510. S2CID 16183221.
  26. ^ Mead, George Herbert (2015) [1934]. "PART II: MIND 8. Imitation and the Origin of Language". In Morris, Charles W.; Huebner, Daniel R.; Joas, Hans (eds.). Mind, Self & Society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. pp. 58–59. doi:10.7208/chicago/9780226112879.001.0001. ISBN 978-0-226-11273-2. [T]he 'herding' instinct, if reduced down to something concrete in the action of the form itself...this may lead to a stampede in the herd. Something of that sort is involved in the so-called 'sentinel.' One animal, a little more sensitive than the others, lifts his head and starts to run away, and the other animals do tend to move with the sentinel form...[Y]ou [humans] unconsciously imitate [dialects]. The same thing is also true of various other [primate] mannerisms...That is what we call 'imitation,' and what is curious is that there is practically no indication of such behavior on the part of lower [life]forms. You can teach a sparrow to sing as a canary but you have to keep that sparrow constantly listening to a canary. It does not take place readily...in general the taking over of the processes of others is not natural to lower forms. Imitation seems to belong to the human form, where it has reached some sort of independent conscious existence.
  27. ^ Thorndike, Edward L. (1898). "Animal intelligence: An experimental study of the associative processes in animals". The Psychological Review: Monograph Supplements. 2 (4): i–109. doi:10.1037/h0092987. hdl:2027/njp.32101045694773. S2CID 143644299.
  28. ^ Heyes, Cecilia M.; Galef, Bennett G., eds. (1996). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. San Diego: Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-08-054131-0.[page needed]
  29. ^ Thorpe, William Homan (1963). Learning and Instinct in Animals. London: Methuen. OCLC 225309.[page needed]
  30. ^ Hayes, Keith J.; Hayes, Catherine (1952). "Imitation in a home-raised chimpanzee". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 45 (5): 450–9. doi:10.1037/h0053609. PMID 13000013.
  31. ^ Custance, Deborah M.; Whiten, Andrew; Bard, Kim A. (1995). "Can Young Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) Imitate Arbitrary Actions? Hayes & Hayes (1952) Revisited". Behaviour. 132 (11): 837–59. doi:10.1163/156853995X00036. JSTOR 4535304.
  32. ^ Voelkl, Bernhard; Huber, Ludwig (2007). Ferrari, Pier (ed.). "Imitation as Faithful Copying of a Novel Technique in Marmoset Monkeys". PLOS ONE. 2 (7): e611. Bibcode:2007PLoSO...2..611V. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0000611. PMC 1905941. PMID 17622356.  
  33. ^ Heyes, C. M.; Dawson, G. R.; Nokes, T. (1992). "Imitation in rats: Initial responding and transfer evidence". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 45 (3): 229–40. doi:10.1080/14640749208401018 (inactive December 31, 2022).{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of December 2022 (link)
  34. ^ Heyes, C. M.; Dawson, G. R. (1990). "A demonstration of observational learning in rats using a bidirectional control". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 42 (1): 59–71. doi:10.1080/14640749008401871 (inactive December 31, 2022). PMID 2326494.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: DOI inactive as of December 2022 (link)
  35. ^ Heyes, C.M.; Ray, E.D.; Mitchell, C.J.; Nokes, T. (2000). "Stimulus Enhancement: Controls for Social Facilitation and Local Enhancement". Learning and Motivation. 31 (2): 83–98. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.520.2135. doi:10.1006/lmot.1999.1041.
  36. ^ Custance, Deborah; Whiten, Andrew; Fredman, Tamar (1999). "Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 113: 13–23. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.13.
  37. ^ Bugnyar, Thomas; Huber, Ludwig (1997). "Push or pull: An experimental study on imitation in marmosets". Animal Behaviour. 54 (4): 817–31. doi:10.1006/anbe.1996.0497. PMID 9344436. S2CID 40005662.
  38. ^ Voelkl, Bernhard; Huber, Ludwig (2000). "True imitation in marmosets". Animal Behaviour. 60 (2): 195–202. doi:10.1006/anbe.2000.1457. PMID 10973721. S2CID 31623649.
  39. ^ Whiten, Andrew; Custance, Deborah M.; Gomez, Juan-Carlos; Teixidor, Patricia; Bard, Kim A. (1996). "Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 110 (1): 3–14. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.110.1.3. PMID 8851548.
  40. ^ Stoinski, Tara S.; Wrate, Joanna L.; Ure, Nicky; Whiten, Andrew (2001). "Imitative learning by captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) in a simulated food-processing task". Journal of Comparative Psychology. 115 (3): 272–81. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.115.3.272. PMID 11594496.
  41. ^ Whiten, Andrew; Horner, Victoria; Litchfield, Carla A.; Marshall-Pescini, Sarah (2004). "How do apes ape?". Animal Learning & Behavior. 32 (1): 36–52. doi:10.3758/BF03196005. PMID 15161139.
  42. ^ Tennie, Claudio; Call, Josep; Tomasello, Michael (2006). "Push or Pull: Imitation vs. Emulation in Great Apes and Human Children". Ethology. 112 (12): 1159–69. doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01269.x. S2CID 56228726.
  43. ^ Zimmer, Carl (December 13, 2005). "Children Learn by Monkey See, Monkey Do. Chimps Don't". The New York Times.
  44. ^ a b Howard, Maureen; Keenen, Michael (Spring 1993). "Outline for a Functional Analysis of Imitation in Animals". The Physiological Record. 43 (2): 185 – via Business Source Complete.
  45. ^ a b c d e Zentall, Thomas R. (2001). "Imitation in Animals: Evidence, Function, and Mechanisms". Cybernetics and Systems. 32 (1–2): 53–96. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.459.375. doi:10.1080/019697201300001812. S2CID 30219876.
  46. ^ a b c d e f g Zentall, Thomas R. (2003). "Imitation by animals: How do they do it?". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 12 (3): 91–5. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01237. JSTOR 20182847. S2CID 145596513.
  47. ^ a b c Heyes, Cecilia M.; Ray, Elizabeth D. (2000). "What is the Significance of Imitation in Animals?". In Slater, Peter J.B.; Rosenblatt, Jay S.; Snowdon, Charles T.; Roper, Timothy J. (eds.). Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol. 29. pp. 215–45. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60106-0. ISBN 978-0-12-004529-7.
  48. ^ Bandura, A. Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall, 1986.
  49. ^ Heyes, C. "Transformational and Associative Theories of Imitation". MIT Press, 2002, p. 504-505.
  50. ^ a b c Galef, Bennett G. (1998). "Recent progress in studies of imitation and social learning in animals". In Sabourin, Michel; Craik, Fergus; Robert, Michèle (eds.). Advances in psychological science, Vol. 2: Biological and cognitive aspects. Hove, England: Psychology Press. pp. 275–99. ISBN 978-0-86377-471-3.
  51. ^ a b Carlson, Neil R.; Buskist, William; Enzle, Michael E.; Heth, C Donald (2005). Psychology: The Science of Behaviour (3rd ed.). Pearson Education Canada. p. 384. ISBN 978-0-205-40386-8.
  52. ^ a b c d e Ingersoll, Brooke (2008). "The Social Role of Imitation in Autism". Infants & Young Children. 21 (2): 107–19. doi:10.1097/01.IYC.0000314482.24087.14. S2CID 10001799.
  53. ^ a b c McLeod, S. A. (2011). "Bandura: Social learning theory". Simply Psychology. Retrieved November 10, 2012.
  54. ^ a b Tomasello, Michael (2006). "Rational Imitation in 12-Month-Old Infants Christiane Schwier, Catharine van Maanen, Malinda Carpenter". Infancy. 10 (3): 303–11. doi:10.1207/s15327078in1003_6. S2CID 41793867.
  55. ^ Meltzoff, A. N. & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75-78. DOI: 10.1126/science.198.4312.75
  56. ^ Field, T.; Woodson, R; Greenberg, R; Cohen, D (October 8, 1982). "Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates". Science. 218 (4568): 179–181. Bibcode:1982Sci...218..179F. doi:10.1126/science.7123230. ISSN 0036-8075. PMID 7123230.
  57. ^ a b "Newborns found able to imitate facial expressions". The New York Times. October 12, 1982. Retrieved November 14, 2012.
  58. ^ Oostenbroek, Janine; Suddendorf, Thomas; Nielsen, Mark; Redshaw, Jonathan; Kennedy-Costantini, Siobhan; Davis, Jacqueline; Clark, Sally; Slaughter, Virginia (May 2016). "Comprehensive Longitudinal Study Challenges the Existence of Neonatal Imitation in Humans". Current Biology. 26 (10): 1334–1338. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.047. PMID 27161497. S2CID 3918288.
  59. ^ "Foundation: Imitation: California infant/toddler learning & development foundations". California Department of Education. 2012. Retrieved November 10, 2012.
  60. ^ Stiefel, Chana (2012). "What Your Child Learns By Imitating You". Parents Magazine.
  61. ^ Malcom, Kelly (June 30, 2000). "Studies shed light on toddler development".
  62. ^ Kaye, Kenneth (1982). "Imitation". The Mental and Social Life of Babies. pp. 155–88. ISBN 978-0-226-42848-2.
  63. ^ Toyokawa, Wataru; Saito, Yoshimatsu; Kameda, Tatsuya (May 1, 2017). "Individual differences in learning behaviours in humans: Asocial exploration tendency does not predict reliance on social learning". Evolution and Human Behavior. 38 (3): 325–333. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2016.11.001. ISSN 1090-5138.
  64. ^ Patrick, Karen; Richman, Charles L. (1985). "Imitation in Toddlers as a Function of Motor and Verbal Aspects of Modeling". The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 146 (4): 507–18. doi:10.1080/00221325.1985.10532470. PMID 3835229.
  65. ^ Cherry, Kendra. "Bobo Doll Experiment". About.com.
  66. ^ Hay, Dennis C.; Young, Andrew W.; Ellis, Andrew W. (November 1991). "Routes through the Face Recognition System". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 43 (4): 761–791. doi:10.1080/14640749108400957. ISSN 0272-4987.
  67. ^ Subiaul, Francys; Lurie, Herbert; Romansky, Kathryn; Klein, Tovah; Holmes, David; Terrace, Herbert (2007). "Cognitive imitation in typically-developing 3- and 4-year olds and individuals with autism". Cognitive Development. 22 (2): 230. Bibcode:1996CogD...11..491S. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.003. PMC 2818295. PMID 20148122.
  68. ^ Rogers, Sally J.; Hepburn, Susan L.; Stackhouse, Tracy; Wehner, Elizabeth (2003). "Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 44 (5): 763–81. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00162. PMID 12831120. S2CID 17936792.
  69. ^ Crompton, Catherine J.; Sharp, Martha; Axbey, Harriet; Fletcher-Watson, Sue; Flynn, Emma G.; Ropar, Danielle (2020). "Neurotype-Matching, but Not Being Autistic, Influences Self and Observer Ratings of Interpersonal Rapport". Frontiers in Psychology. 11: 2961. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.586171. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 7645034. PMID 33192918.
  70. ^ Milton, Damian E.M. (October 1, 2012). "On the ontological status of autism: the 'double empathy problem'". Disability & Society. 27 (6): 883–887. doi:10.1080/09687599.2012.710008. ISSN 0968-7599. S2CID 54047060.
  71. ^ DeThorne, Laura S. (March 1, 2020). "Revealing the Double Empathy Problem". The ASHA Leader. 25 (3): 58–65. doi:10.1044/leader.ftr2.25042020.58. S2CID 216359201. Retrieved November 8, 2021.
  72. ^ Soulières, Isabelle; Dawson, Michelle; Samson, Fabienne; Barbeau, Elise B.; Sahyoun, Chérif P.; Strangman, Gary E.; Zeffiro, Thomas A.; Mottron, Laurent (2009). "Enhanced visual processing contributes to matrix reasoning in autism". Human Brain Mapping. 30 (12): 4082–4107. doi:10.1002/hbm.20831. PMC 2787806. PMID 19530215.
  73. ^ Plaisted, Kate; O'Riordan, Michelle; Baron-Cohen, Simon (July 1998). "Enhanced Discrimination of Novel, Highly Similar Stimuli by Adults with Autism During a Perceptual Learning Task". Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 39 (5): 765–775. doi:10.1017/s0021963098002601. ISSN 0021-9630. PMID 9690939.
  74. ^ O'Riordan, Michelle; Plaisted, Kate (November 1, 2001). "Enhanced discrimination in autism". The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A. 54 (4): 961–979. doi:10.1080/713756000. ISSN 0272-4987. PMID 11765744. S2CID 144120552.
  75. ^ Baron-Cohen, Simon; Belmonte, Matthew K. (July 21, 2005). "AUTISM: A Window Onto the Development of the Social and the Analytic Brain". Annual Review of Neuroscience. 28 (1): 109–126. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144137. ISSN 0147-006X. PMID 16033325.
  76. ^ Schulte-Rüther, Martin; Otte, Ellen; Adigüzel, Kübra; Firk, Christine; Herpertz-Dahlmann, Beate; Koch, Iring; Konrad, Kerstin (June 28, 2016). "Intact mirror mechanisms for automatic facial emotions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Intact mirror mechanisms in Austism". Autism Research. 10 (2): 298–310. doi:10.1002/aur.1654.
  77. ^ Heyes, Cecilia (2011). "Automatic imitation". Psychological Bulletin. 137 (3): 463–83. doi:10.1037/a0022288. PMID 21280938. S2CID 6975248.
  78. ^ Lyons, Derek E.; Keil, Frank C. (April 23, 2013). Navigating the Social World. Oxford University Press. pp. 145–149. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.003.0026. ISBN 9780199890712.
  79. ^ Lyons, Derek E.; Young, Andrew G.; Keil, Frank C. (December 11, 2007). "The hidden structure of overimitation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 104 (50): 19751–19756. Bibcode:2007PNAS..10419751L. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704452104. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 2148370. PMID 18056814.
  80. ^ Bower, Bruce (May 15, 2018). "Kids are selective imitators, not extreme copycats". Science News. Retrieved May 18, 2018.
  81. ^ Evans, Cara L.; Laland, Kevin N.; Carpenter, Malinda; Kendal, Rachel L. (December 17, 2017). "Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly "optimal-" rather than "over-" imitators" (PDF). Developmental Science. 6 (1): e12637. doi:10.1111/desc.12637. hdl:10023/16702. ISSN 1363-755X. PMID 29250871.
  82. ^ Koenig, M. A.; Clement, F.; Harris, P. L. (October 1, 2004). "Trust in Testimony: Children's Use of True and False Statements". Psychological Science. 15 (10): 694–698. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x. ISSN 0956-7976. PMID 15447641. S2CID 2116025.
  83. ^ Sundqvist, Annette; Nordqvist, Emelie; Koch, Felix-Sebastian; Heimann, Mikael (November 2016). "Early declarative memory predicts productive language: A longitudinal study of deferred imitation and communication at 9 and 16months". Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 151: 109–119. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.015. PMID 26925719.

Further reading

  • Zentall, Thomas R. (2006). "Imitation: Definitions, evidence, and mechanisms". Animal Cognition. 9 (4): 335–53. doi:10.1007/s10071-006-0039-2. PMID 17024510. S2CID 16183221.
  • Liepmann, H. (1900). Das Krankheitsbild der Apraxie (motorische Asymbolie). Berlin: S. Karger Publ.
  • Liepmann, H. (1905). Über die Störungen des Handelns bei Gehirnkranken. Berlin: S. Karger Verlag.
  • Liepmann, H. (1908). Drei Aufsätze aus dem Apraxiegebiet. Berlin: S. Karger Publ.
  • Liepmann, H. (1920). "Apraxie". Ergebn Ges Med. 1: 516–43. NAID 10008100327.

External links

  • M. Metzmacher, 1995. La transmission du chant chez le Pinson des arbres (Fringilla c. coelebs) : phase sensible et rôle des tuteurs chez les oiseaux captifs. Alauda, 63 : 123 – 134.
  • M. Metzmacher, 2016. Imitations et transmission culturelle dans le chant du Pinson des arbres Fringilla coelebs ? Alauda, 84 : 203-220.

imitation, this, article, about, behaviour, humans, animals, other, uses, disambiguation, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, this, article, adding, citations, reliable, sources, unsourced, material, challenged, re. This article is about the behaviour in humans and animals For other uses see Imitation disambiguation This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources Imitation news newspapers books scholar JSTOR October 2018 Learn how and when to remove this template message Imitation from Latin imitatio a copying imitation 1 is a behavior whereby an individual observes and replicates another s behavior Imitation is also a form of that leads to the development of traditions and ultimately our culture It allows for the transfer of information behaviors customs etc between individuals and down generations without the need for genetic inheritance 2 The word imitation can be applied in many contexts ranging from animal training to politics 3 The term generally refers to conscious behavior subconscious imitation is termed mirroring 4 A toddler imitates his father Contents 1 Anthropology and social sciences 2 Neuroscience 3 Mirror neuron system 4 Animal behavior 4 1 Imitation in animals 4 1 1 Theories 4 1 2 New developments 5 Child development 5 1 Infants 5 2 Toddlers 5 3 Gender and age differences 5 4 Negative imitation 5 5 Autism 6 Automatic imitation 7 Over imitation 8 Deferred imitation 9 See also 10 References 11 Further reading 12 External linksAnthropology and social sciences EditIn anthropology some theories hold that all cultures imitate ideas from one of a few original cultures or several cultures whose influence overlaps geographically Evolutionary diffusion theory holds that cultures influence one another but that similar ideas can be developed in isolation Scholars 5 as well as popular authors 6 7 have argued that the role of imitation in humans is unique among animals However this claim has been recently challenged by scientific research which observed social learning and imitative abilities in animals Psychologist Kenneth Kaye showed 8 9 that the ability of infants to match the sounds or gestures of an adult depends on an interactive process of turn taking over many successive trials in which adults instinctive behavior plays as great a role as that of the infant These writers assume that evolution would have selected imitative abilities as fit because those who were good at it had a wider arsenal of learned behavior at their disposal including tool making and language However research also suggests that imitative behaviors and other social learning processes are only selected for when outnumbered or accompanied by asocial learning processes an over saturation of imitation and imitating individuals leads humans to collectively copy inefficient strategies and evolutionarily maladaptive behaviors thereby reducing flexibility to new environmental contexts that require adaptation 10 Research suggests imitative social learning hinders the acquisition of knowledge in novel environments and in situations where asocial learning is faster and more advantageous 11 12 In the mid 20th century social scientists began to study how and why people imitate ideas Everett Rogers pioneered innovation diffusion studies identifying factors in adoption and profiles of adopters of ideas 13 Imitation mechanisms play a central role in both analytical and empirical models of collective human behavior 14 Neuroscience EditHumans are capable of imitating movements actions skills behaviors gestures pantomimes mimics vocalizations sounds speech etc and that we have particular imitation systems in the brain is old neurological knowledge dating back to Hugo Karl Liepmann Liepmann s model 1908 Das hierarchische Modell der Handlungsplanung the hierarchical model of action planning is still valid On studying the cerebral localization of function Liepmann postulated that planned or commanded actions were prepared in the parietal lobe of the brain s dominant hemisphere and also frontally His most important pioneering work is when extensively studying patients with lesions in these brain areas he discovered that the patients lost among other things the ability to imitate He was the one who coined the term apraxia and differentiated between ideational and ideomotor apraxia It is in this basic and wider frame of classical neurological knowledge that the discovery of the mirror neuron has to be seen Though mirror neurons were first discovered in macaques their discovery also relates to humans 15 Human brain studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI revealed a network of regions in the inferior frontal cortex and inferior parietal cortex which are typically activated during imitation tasks 16 It has been suggested that these regions contain mirror neurons similar to the mirror neurons recorded in the macaque monkey 17 However it is not clear if macaques spontaneously imitate each other in the wild Neurologist V S Ramachandran argues that the evolution of mirror neurons were important in the human acquisition of complex skills such as language and believes the discovery of mirror neurons to be a most important advance in neuroscience 18 However little evidence directly supports the theory that mirror neuron activity is involved in cognitive functions such as empathy or learning by imitation 19 Evidence is accumulating that bottlenose dolphins employ imitation to learn hunting and other skills from other dolphins 20 21 Japanese monkeys have been seen to spontaneously begin washing potatoes after seeing humans washing them 22 Mirror neuron system EditResearch has been conducted to locate where in the brain specific parts and neurological systems are activated when humans imitate behaviors and actions of others discovering a mirror neuron system This neuron system allows a person to observe and then recreate the actions of others Mirror neurons are premotor and parietal cells in the macaque brain that fire when the animal performs a goal directed action and when it sees others performing the same action 23 Evidence suggests that the mirror neuron system also allows people to comprehend and understand the intentions and emotions of others 24 Problems of the mirror neuron system may be correlated with the social inadequacies of autism There have been many studies done showing that children with autism compared with typically developing children demonstrate reduced activity in the frontal mirror neuron system area when observing or imitating facial emotional expressions Of course the higher the severity of the disease the lower the activity in the mirror neuron system is 23 Animal behavior EditScientists debate whether animals can consciously imitate the unconscious incitement from sentinel animals whether imitation is uniquely human or whether humans do a complex version of what other animals do 25 26 The current controversy is partly definitional Thorndike uses learning to do an act from seeing it done 27 It has two major shortcomings first by using seeing it restricts imitation to the visual domain and excludes e g vocal imitation and second it would also include mechanisms such as priming contagious behavior and social facilitation 28 which most scientist distinguish as separate forms of observational learning Thorpe suggested defining imitation as the copying of a novel or otherwise improbable act or utterance or some act for which there is clearly no instinctive tendency 29 This definition is favored by many scholars though questions have been raised how strictly the term novel has to be interpreted and how exactly a performed act has to match the demonstration to count as a copy Hayes and Hayes 1952 used the do as I do procedure to demonstrate the imitative abilities of their trained chimpanzee Viki 30 Their study was repeatedly criticized for its subjective interpretations of their subjects responses Replications of this study 31 found much lower matching degrees between subjects and models However imitation research focusing on the copying fidelity got new momentum from a study by Voelkl and Huber 32 They analyzed the motion trajectories of both model and observer monkeys and found a high matching degree in their movement patterns Paralleling these studies comparative psychologists provided tools or apparatuses that could be handled in different ways Heyes 33 34 and co workers reported evidence for imitation in rats that pushed a lever in the same direction as their models though later on they withdrew their claims due to methodological problems in their original setup 35 By trying to design a testing paradigm that is less arbitrary than pushing a lever to the left or to the right Custance and co workers 36 introduced the artificial fruit paradigm where a small object could be opened in different ways to retrieve food placed inside not unlike a hard shelled fruit Using this paradigm scientists reported evidence for imitation in monkeys 37 38 and apes 39 40 41 There remains a problem with such tool or apparatus use studies what animals might learn in such studies need not be the actual behavior patterns i e the actions that were observed Instead they might learn about some effects in the environment i e how the tool moves or how the apparatus works 42 This type of observational learning which focuses on results not actions has been dubbed emulation see Emulation observational learning In an article written by Carl Zimmer he looked into a study being done by Derek Lyons focusing on human evolution in which he studied a chimpanzee He first started with showing the chimpanzee how to retrieve food from a box The chimpanzee soon caught on and did exactly what the scientist just did They wanted to see if the chimpanzee s brain functioned just like a human brain so they replicated the experiment using 16 children following the same procedure once the children saw how it was done they followed the same exact steps 43 Imitation in animals Edit source source source source source source source source source source source source source source Blackbird imitating the vehicle motion alarm of a local garbage truck in Brastad Sweden Imitation in animals is a study in the field of social learning where learning behavior is observed in animals specifically how animals learn and adapt through imitation Ethologists can classify imitation in animals by the learning of certain behaviors from conspecifics 44 More specifically these behaviors are usually unique to the species and can be complex in nature and can benefit the individual s survival 44 Some scientists believe true imitation is only produced by humans arguing that simple learning though sight is not enough to sustain as a being who can truly imitate 45 Thorpe defines true imitation as the copying of a novel or otherwise improbable act or utterance or some act for which there is clearly no instinctive tendency which is highly debated for its portrayal of imitation as a mindless repeating act 45 True imitation is produced when behavioral visual and vocal imitation is achieved not just the simple reproduction of exclusive behaviors 45 Imitation is not a simple reproduction of what one sees rather it incorporates intention and purpose 45 Animal imitation can range from survival purpose imitating as a function of surviving or adapting to unknown possible curiosity which vary between different animals and produce different results depending on the measured intelligence of the animal 45 There is considerable evidence to support true imitation in animals 46 Experiments performed on apes birds and more specifically the Japanese quail have provided positive results to imitating behavior demonstrating imitation of opaque behavior 46 However the problem that lies is in the discrepancies between what is considered true imitation in behavior 46 Birds have demonstrated visual imitation where the animal simply does as it sees 46 Studies on apes however have proven more advanced results in imitation being able to remember and learn from what they imitate 46 Studies have demonstrated far more positive results with behavioral imitation in primates and birds than any other type of animal 46 Imitation in non primate mammals and other animals have been proven difficult to conclude solid positive results for and poses a difficult question to scientists on why that is so 46 Theories Edit There are two types of theories of imitation transformational and associative Transformational theories suggest that the information that is required to display certain behavior is created internally through cognitive processes and observing these behaviors provides incentive to duplicate them 47 Meaning we already have the codes to recreate any behavior and observing it results in its replication Albert Bandura s social cognitive theory is one example of a transformational theory 48 Associative or sometimes referred to as contiguity 49 theories suggest that the information required to display certain behaviors does not come from within ourselves but solely from our surroundings and experiences 47 These theories have not yet provided testable predictions in the field of social learning in animals and have yet to conclude strong results 47 New developments Edit There have been three major developments in the field of animal imitation The first behavioral ecologists and experimental psychologists found there to be adaptive patterns in behaviors in different vertebrate species in biologically important situations 50 The second primatologists and comparative psychologists have found imperative evidence that suggest true learning through imitation in animals 50 The third population biologists and behavioral ecologists created experiments that demand animals to depend on social learning in certain manipulated environments 50 Child development EditDevelopmental psychologist Jean Piaget noted that children in a developmental phase he called the sensorimotor stage a period which lasts up to the first two years of a child begin to imitate observed actions 51 This is an important stage in the development of a child because the child is beginning to think symbolically associating behaviors with actions thus setting the child up for the development of further symbolic thinking Imitative learning also plays a crucial role in the development of cognitive and social communication behaviors such as language play and joint attention Imitation serves as both a learning and a social function because new skills and knowledge are acquired and communication skills are improved by interacting in social and emotional exchanges It is shown however that children with autism exhibit significant deficits in imitation that are associated with impairments in other social communication skills 52 To help children with autism reciprocal imitation training RIT is used It is a naturalistic imitation intervention that helps teach the social benefits of imitation during play by increasing child responsiveness and by increasing imitative language 52 Reinforcement learning both positive and negative and punishment are used by people that children imitate to either promote or discontinue behavior If a child imitates a certain type of behavior or action and the consequences are rewarding the child is very likely to continue performing the same behavior or action The behavior has been reinforced i e strengthened 53 self published source However if the imitation is not accepted and approved by others then the behavior will be weakened Naturally children are surrounded by many different types of people that influence their actions and behaviors including parents family members teachers peers and even characters on television programs These different types of individuals that are observed are called models According to Saul McLeod these models provide examples of masculine and feminine behavior to observe and imitate 53 Children imitate the behavior they have observed from others regardless of the gender of the person and whether or not the behavior is gender appropriate However it has been proven that children will reproduce the behavior that its society deems appropriate for its sex 53 Infants Edit Infants have the ability to reveal an understanding of certain outcomes before they occur therefore in this sense they can somewhat imitate what they have perceived Andrew N Meltzoff ran a series of tasks involving 14 month old infants to imitate actions they perceived from adults In this gathering he had concluded that the infants before trying to reproduce the actions they wish to imitate somehow revealed an understanding of the intended goal even though they failed to replicate the result wished to be imitated These task implicated that the infants knew the goal intended 54 Gergely Bekkering and Kiraly 2002 figured that infants not only understand the intended goal but also the intentions of the person they were trying to imitate engaging in rational imitation as described by Tomasello Carpenter and others 54 It has long been claimed that newborn humans imitate bodily gestures and facial expressions as soon as their first few days of life 55 56 For example in a study conducted at the Mailman Centre for Child Development at the University of Miami Medical School 74 newborn babies with a mean age of 36 hours were tested to see if they were able to imitate a smile a frown and a pout and a wide open mouth and eyes An observer stood behind the experimenter so he she couldn t see what facial expressions were being made by the experimenter and watched only the babies facial expressions recording their results Just by looking only at the babies faces the observer was more often able to correctly guess what facial expression was being presented to the child by the experimenter 57 After the results were calculated the researchers concluded that babies have an innate ability to compare an expression they see with their own sense of muscular feedback from making the movements to match that expression 57 However the idea that imitation is an inborn ability has been recently challenged A research group from the University of Queensland in Australia carried out the largest ever longitudinal study of neonatal imitation in humans One hundred and nine newborns were shown a variety of gestures including tongue protrusion mouth opening happy and sad facial expressions at four time points between one week and 9 weeks of age The results failed to reveal compelling evidence that newborns imitate Infants were just as likely to produce matching and non matching gestures in response to what they saw 58 At around eight months infants will start to copy their child care providers movements when playing pat a cake and peek a boo as well as imitating familiar gestures such as clapping hands together or patting a doll s back At around 18 months infants will then begin to imitate simple actions they observe adults doing such as taking a toy phone out of a purse and saying hello pretending to sweep with a child sized broom as well as imitating using a toy hammer citation needed Toddlers Edit At around 30 36 months toddlers will start to imitate their parents by pretending to get ready for work and school and saying the last word s of what an adult just said For example toddlers may say bowl or a bowl after they hear someone say That s a bowl They may also imitate the way family members communicate by using the same gestures and words For example a toddler will say Mommy bye bye after the father says Mommy went bye bye 59 Toddlers love to imitate their parents and help when they can imitation helps toddlers learn and through their experiences lasting impressions are made 12 to 36 month olds learn by doing not by watching and so it is often recommended to be a good role model and caretaker by showing them simple tasks like putting on socks or holding a spoon 60 Duke developmental psychologist Carol Eckerman did a study on toddlers imitating toddlers and found that at the age of 2 children involve themselves in imitation play to communicate with one another This can be seen within a culture or across different cultures 3 common imitative patterns Eckerman found were reciprocal imitation follow the leader and lead follow 61 Kenneth Kaye s apprenticeship theory of imitation rejected assumptions that other authors had made about its development His research showed that there is no one simple imitation skill with its own course of development What changes is the type of behavior imitated 62 An important agenda for infancy is the progressive imitation of higher levels of use of signs until the ultimate achievement of symbols The principal role played by parents in this process is their provision of salient models within the facilitating frames that channel the infant s attention and organize his imitative efforts Gender and age differences Edit A small boy of Matera Italy unconsciously repeats the gesture of his grandmother s hands ca 1948 ca 1955 Imitation and imitative behaviors do not manifest ubiquitously and evenly in all human individuals some individuals rely more on imitated information than others 63 Although imitation is very useful when it comes to cognitive learning with toddlers research has shown that there are some gender and age differences when it comes to imitation Research done to judge imitation in toddlers 2 3 years old shows that when faced with certain conditions 2 year olds displayed more motor imitation than 3 year olds and 3 year olds displayed more verbal reality imitation than 2 year olds Boys displayed more motor imitation than girls 64 No other research is more controversial pertaining gender differences in toddler imitation than renowned psychologist Bandura s bobo doll experiments 65 The goal of the experiment was to see what happens to toddlers when exposed to aggressive and non aggressive adults would the toddlers imitate the behavior of the adults and if so which gender is more likely to imitate the aggressive adult In the beginning of the experiment Bandura had several predictions that actually came true Children exposed to violent adults will imitate the actions of that adult when the adult is not present boys who had observed an adult of the opposite sex act aggressively are less likely to act violently than those who witnessed a male adult act violently In fact boys who observed an adult male behaving violently were more influenced than those who had observed a female model behavior aggressively One fascinating observation was that while boys are likely to imitate physical acts of violence girls are likely to imitate verbal acts of violence Negative imitation Edit Imitation plays a major role on how a toddler interprets the world Much of a child s understanding is derived from imitation due to a lack of verbal skill imitation in toddlers for communication citation needed It is what connects them to the communicating world as they continue to grow they begin to learn more This may mean that it is crucial for parents to be cautious as to how they act and behave around their toddlers Imitation is the toddlers way of confirming and dis conforming socially acceptable actions in society Actions like washing dishes cleaning up the house and doing chores are actions you want your toddlers to imitate Imitating negative things is something that is never beyond young toddlers If they are exposed to cursing and violence it is going to be what the child views as the norm of their world since imitation is the mental activity that helps to formulate the conceptions of the world for toddlers 66 So it is important for parents to be careful what they say or do in front of their children citation needed Autism Edit Children with autism exhibit significant impairment in imitation skills 52 Imitation deficits have been reported on a variety of tasks including symbolic and non symbolic body movements symbolic and functional object use vocalizations and facial expressions 52 In contrast typically developing children can copy a broad range of novel as well as familiar rules from a very early age 67 Problems with imitation discriminate children with autism from those with other developmental disorders as early as age 2 and continue into adulthood 68 Children with autism exhibit significant deficits in imitation that are associated with impairments in other social communication skills It is unclear whether imitation is mediating these relationships directly or whether they are due to some other developmental variable that is also reflected in the measurement of imitation skills 52 On the contrary research from the early 21st century suggests that people affected with forms of high functioning autism easily interact with one another by using a more analytically centered communication approach rather than an imitative cue based approach 69 suggesting that reduced imitative capabilities do not affect abilities for expressive social behavior but only the understanding of said social behavior Social communication is not negatively affected when said communication involves less or no imitation Children with autism may have significant problems understanding typical social communication not because of inherent social deficits but because of differences in communication style which affect reciprocal understanding 70 71 Autistic individuals are also shown to possess increased analytical cognitive and visual processing 72 73 74 suggesting that they have no true impairments in observing the actions of others but may decide not to imitate them because they do not analytically understand them 75 A 2016 study has shown that involuntary spontaneous facial mimicry which supposedly depends on the mirror neuron system is intact in individuals with autism contrasting with previous studies and suggesting that the mirror neuron system is not inherently broken in autistic individuals 76 Automatic imitation EditThe automatic imitation comes very fast when a stimulus is given to replicate The imitation can match the commands with the visual stimulus compatible or it cannot match the commands with the visual stimulus incompatible For example Simon Says a game played with children where they are told to follow the commands given by the adult In this game the adult gives the commands and shows the actions the commands given can either match the action to be done or it will not match the action The children who imitate the adult who has given the command with the correct action will stay in the game The children who imitate the command with the wrong action will go out of the game and this is where the child s automatic imitation comes into play Psychologically the visual stimulus being looked upon by the child is being imitated faster than the imitation of the command In addition the response times were faster in compatible scenarios than in incompatible scenarios 77 Children are surrounded by many different people day by day Their parents make a big impact on them and usually what the children do is what they have seen their parent do In this article they found that a child simply watching its mother sweep the floor right after soon picks up on it and starts to imitate the mother by sweeping the floor By the children imitating they are really teaching themselves how to do things without instruction from the parent or guardian Toddlers love to play the game of house They picked up on this game of house by television school or at home they play the game how they see it The kids imitate their parents or anybody in their family In the article it says it is so easy for them to pick up on the things they see on an everyday basis citation needed Over imitation EditOver imitation is the tendency of young children to copy all of an adult model s actions even components that are irrelevant for the task at hand 78 According to this human and cross cultural phenomenon a child has a strong tendency to automatically encode the deliberate action of an adult as causally meaningful even when the child observes evidence that proves that its performance is unnecessary It is suggested that over imitation may be critical to the transmission of human culture Experiments done by Lyons et al 2007 has shown that when there are obvious pedagogical cues children tend to imitate step by step including many unnecessary steps without pedagogical cues children will simply skip those useless steps 79 However another study suggests that children do not just blindly follow the crowd since they can also be just as discriminating as adults in choosing whether an unnecessary action should be copied or not 80 They may imitate additional but unnecessary steps to a novel process if the adult demonstrations are all the same However in cases where one out of four adults showed a better technique only 40 actually copied the extra step as described by Evans Carpenter and others 81 Children s imitation is selective also known as selective imitation Studies have shown that children tend to imitate older competitive and trustworthy individuals 82 Deferred imitation EditPiaget coined the term deferred imitation and suggested that it arises out of the child s increasing ability to form mental representations of behavior performed by others 51 Deferred imitation is also the ability to reproduce a previously witnessed action or sequence of actions in the absence of current perceptual support for the action 2 Instead of copying what is currently occurring individuals repeat the action or behavior later on It appears that infants show an improving ability for deferred imitation as they get older especially by 24 months By 24 months infants are able to imitate action sequences after a delay of up to three months meaning that they re able to generalize knowledge they have gained from one test environment to another and from one test object to another 2 A child s deferred imitation ability to form mental representations of actions occurring in everyday life and their knowledge of communicative gestures has also been linked to earlier productive language development 83 Between 9 preverbal period and 16 months verbal period deferred imitation performance on a standard actions on objects task was consistent in one longitudinal study testing participants ability to complete a target action with high achievers at 9 months remaining so at 16 months Gestural development at 9 months was also linked to productive language at 16 months Researchers now believe that early deferred imitation ability is indicative of early declarative memory also considered a predictor of productive language development See also EditAppropriation sociology Articulation sociology Associative Sequence Learning Cognitive imitation Copycat crime Copycat suicide Identification psychology Mimicry Royal Commission on Animal MagnetismReferences Edit imitation Online etymology dictionary a b c Hopper Lydia M 2010 Deferred imitation in children and apes Psychologist 23 4 294 7 Archived from the original on August 3 2014 Retrieved March 8 2021 Moss Ellen Strayer F F 1988 Imitation is the Greatest form of Flattery PsycCRITIQUES 33 11 970 971 doi 10 1037 026218 Chartrand T Bargh J 1999 The Chameleon Effect The Perception Behavior Link and Social Interaction PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology New York University 76 6 893 910 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 76 6 893 PMID 10402679 Retrieved September 28 2014 George Herbert Mead Mind Self and Society Kenneth Kaye The Mental and Social Life of Babies U Chicago Press 1982 page needed Susan Blackmore The Meme Machine Oxford U Press 2007 page needed Jared Diamond The Third Chimpanzee HarperCollins 1992 page needed In M Bullowa ed Before Speech The beginning of interpersonal communication Cambridge U Press 1979 page needed Kaye Kenneth Marcus Janet 1981 Infant imitation The sensory motor agenda Developmental Psychology 17 3 258 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 17 3 258 Rendell Luke Fogarty Laurel Laland Kevin N 2010 Rogers Paradox Recast and Resolved Population Structure and the Evolution of Social Learning Strategies Evolution 64 2 534 548 doi 10 1111 j 1558 5646 2009 00817 x ISSN 1558 5646 PMID 19674093 S2CID 2898779 Laland Kevin N Williams Kerry 1998 Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy Behavioral Ecology 9 5 493 499 doi 10 1093 beheco 9 5 493 ISSN 1045 2249 Johnstone R A Dall S R X Giraldeau Luc Alain Valone Thomas J Templeton Jennifer J November 29 2002 Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 357 1427 1559 1566 doi 10 1098 rstb 2002 1065 PMC 1693065 PMID 12495513 Rogers EM 2010 Diffusion of innovations Simon and Schuster Castellano Claudio Fortunato Santo Loreto Vittorio May 11 2009 Statistical physics of social dynamics Reviews of Modern Physics 81 2 591 646 arXiv 0710 3256 Bibcode 2009RvMP 81 591C doi 10 1103 RevModPhys 81 591 S2CID 118376889 Bonini Luca Ferrari Pier Francesco 2011 Evolution of mirror systems a simple mechanism for complex cognitive functions Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1225 1 166 175 Bibcode 2011NYASA1225 166B doi 10 1111 j 1749 6632 2011 06002 x PMC 3113469 PMID 21535003 Iacoboni M Woods RP Brass M Bekkering H Mazziotta JC Rizzolatti G 1999 Cortical Mechanisms of Human Imitation Science 286 5449 2526 8 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 555 8075 doi 10 1126 science 286 5449 2526 PMID 10617472 Rizzolatti Giacomo Craighero Laila 2004 The Mirror Neuron System Annual Review of Neuroscience 27 169 92 doi 10 1146 annurev neuro 27 070203 144230 PMID 15217330 S2CID 1729870 Ramachandran V S Mirror Neurons and imitation learning as the driving force behind the great leap forward in human evolution Edge Foundation Dinstein Ilan Thomas Cibu Behrmann Marlene Heeger David J 2008 A mirror up to nature Current Biology 18 1 R13 8 doi 10 1016 j cub 2007 11 004 PMC 2517574 PMID 18177704 Krutzen Michael Mann Janet Heithaus Michael R Connor Richard C Bejder Lars Sherwin William B 2005 Cultural transmission of tool use in bottlenose dolphins Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 102 25 8939 43 Bibcode 2005PNAS 102 8939K doi 10 1073 pnas 0500232102 PMC 1157020 PMID 15947077 Wild dolphins teaches others tail walking tricks WDCS the Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society Archived from the original on January 14 2009 Boesch Christophe Tomasello Michael December 1998 Chimpanzee and Human Cultures PDF Current Anthropology 39 5 591 614 doi 10 1086 204785 S2CID 55562574 Archived from the original PDF on February 26 2019 a b Iacoboni Marco Dapretto Mirella 2006 The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7 12 942 51 doi 10 1038 nrn2024 PMID 17115076 S2CID 9463011 Geiger Alexander Bente Gary Lammers Sebastian Tepest Ralf Roth Daniel Bzdok Danilo Vogeley Kai November 15 2019 Distinct functional roles of the mirror neuron system and the mentalizing system NeuroImage 202 116102 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2019 116102 ISSN 1053 8119 PMID 31446128 S2CID 201125892 Zentall Thomas R 2006 Imitation Definitions evidence and mechanisms Animal Cognition 9 4 335 53 doi 10 1007 s10071 006 0039 2 PMID 17024510 S2CID 16183221 Mead George Herbert 2015 1934 PART II MIND 8 Imitation and the Origin of Language In Morris Charles W Huebner Daniel R Joas Hans eds Mind Self amp Society Chicago The University of Chicago Press pp 58 59 doi 10 7208 chicago 9780226112879 001 0001 ISBN 978 0 226 11273 2 T he herding instinct if reduced down to something concrete in the action of the form itself this may lead to a stampede in the herd Something of that sort is involved in the so called sentinel One animal a little more sensitive than the others lifts his head and starts to run away and the other animals do tend to move with the sentinel form Y ou humans unconsciously imitate dialects The same thing is also true of various other primate mannerisms That is what we call imitation and what is curious is that there is practically no indication of such behavior on the part of lower life forms You can teach a sparrow to sing as a canary but you have to keep that sparrow constantly listening to a canary It does not take place readily in general the taking over of the processes of others is not natural to lower forms Imitation seems to belong to the human form where it has reached some sort of independent conscious existence Thorndike Edward L 1898 Animal intelligence An experimental study of the associative processes in animals The Psychological Review Monograph Supplements 2 4 i 109 doi 10 1037 h0092987 hdl 2027 njp 32101045694773 S2CID 143644299 Heyes Cecilia M Galef Bennett G eds 1996 Social Learning in Animals The Roots of Culture San Diego Academic Press ISBN 978 0 08 054131 0 page needed Thorpe William Homan 1963 Learning and Instinct in Animals London Methuen OCLC 225309 page needed Hayes Keith J Hayes Catherine 1952 Imitation in a home raised chimpanzee Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 45 5 450 9 doi 10 1037 h0053609 PMID 13000013 Custance Deborah M Whiten Andrew Bard Kim A 1995 Can Young Chimpanzees Pan Troglodytes Imitate Arbitrary Actions Hayes amp Hayes 1952 Revisited Behaviour 132 11 837 59 doi 10 1163 156853995X00036 JSTOR 4535304 Voelkl Bernhard Huber Ludwig 2007 Ferrari Pier ed Imitation as Faithful Copying of a Novel Technique in Marmoset Monkeys PLOS ONE 2 7 e611 Bibcode 2007PLoSO 2 611V doi 10 1371 journal pone 0000611 PMC 1905941 PMID 17622356 Heyes C M Dawson G R Nokes T 1992 Imitation in rats Initial responding and transfer evidence The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 45 3 229 40 doi 10 1080 14640749208401018 inactive December 31 2022 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint DOI inactive as of December 2022 link Heyes C M Dawson G R 1990 A demonstration of observational learning in rats using a bidirectional control The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 42 1 59 71 doi 10 1080 14640749008401871 inactive December 31 2022 PMID 2326494 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint DOI inactive as of December 2022 link Heyes C M Ray E D Mitchell C J Nokes T 2000 Stimulus Enhancement Controls for Social Facilitation and Local Enhancement Learning and Motivation 31 2 83 98 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 520 2135 doi 10 1006 lmot 1999 1041 Custance Deborah Whiten Andrew Fredman Tamar 1999 Social learning of an artificial fruit task in capuchin monkeys Cebus apella Journal of Comparative Psychology 113 13 23 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 113 1 13 Bugnyar Thomas Huber Ludwig 1997 Push or pull An experimental study on imitation in marmosets Animal Behaviour 54 4 817 31 doi 10 1006 anbe 1996 0497 PMID 9344436 S2CID 40005662 Voelkl Bernhard Huber Ludwig 2000 True imitation in marmosets Animal Behaviour 60 2 195 202 doi 10 1006 anbe 2000 1457 PMID 10973721 S2CID 31623649 Whiten Andrew Custance Deborah M Gomez Juan Carlos Teixidor Patricia Bard Kim A 1996 Imitative learning of artificial fruit processing in children Homo sapiens and chimpanzees Pan troglodytes Journal of Comparative Psychology 110 1 3 14 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 110 1 3 PMID 8851548 Stoinski Tara S Wrate Joanna L Ure Nicky Whiten Andrew 2001 Imitative learning by captive western lowland gorillas Gorilla gorilla gorilla in a simulated food processing task Journal of Comparative Psychology 115 3 272 81 doi 10 1037 0735 7036 115 3 272 PMID 11594496 Whiten Andrew Horner Victoria Litchfield Carla A Marshall Pescini Sarah 2004 How do apes ape Animal Learning amp Behavior 32 1 36 52 doi 10 3758 BF03196005 PMID 15161139 Tennie Claudio Call Josep Tomasello Michael 2006 Push or Pull Imitation vs Emulation in Great Apes and Human Children Ethology 112 12 1159 69 doi 10 1111 j 1439 0310 2006 01269 x S2CID 56228726 Zimmer Carl December 13 2005 Children Learn by Monkey See Monkey Do Chimps Don t The New York Times a b Howard Maureen Keenen Michael Spring 1993 Outline for a Functional Analysis of Imitation in Animals The Physiological Record 43 2 185 via Business Source Complete a b c d e Zentall Thomas R 2001 Imitation in Animals Evidence Function and Mechanisms Cybernetics and Systems 32 1 2 53 96 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 459 375 doi 10 1080 019697201300001812 S2CID 30219876 a b c d e f g Zentall Thomas R 2003 Imitation by animals How do they do it Current Directions in Psychological Science 12 3 91 5 doi 10 1111 1467 8721 01237 JSTOR 20182847 S2CID 145596513 a b c Heyes Cecilia M Ray Elizabeth D 2000 What is the Significance of Imitation in Animals In Slater Peter J B Rosenblatt Jay S Snowdon Charles T Roper Timothy J eds Advances in the Study of Behavior Vol 29 pp 215 45 doi 10 1016 S0065 3454 08 60106 0 ISBN 978 0 12 004529 7 Bandura A Social foundations of thought and action A social cognitive theory Prentice Hall 1986 Heyes C Transformational and Associative Theories of Imitation MIT Press 2002 p 504 505 a b c Galef Bennett G 1998 Recent progress in studies of imitation and social learning in animals In Sabourin Michel Craik Fergus Robert Michele eds Advances in psychological science Vol 2 Biological and cognitive aspects Hove England Psychology Press pp 275 99 ISBN 978 0 86377 471 3 a b Carlson Neil R Buskist William Enzle Michael E Heth C Donald 2005 Psychology The Science of Behaviour 3rd ed Pearson Education Canada p 384 ISBN 978 0 205 40386 8 a b c d e Ingersoll Brooke 2008 The Social Role of Imitation in Autism Infants amp Young Children 21 2 107 19 doi 10 1097 01 IYC 0000314482 24087 14 S2CID 10001799 a b c McLeod S A 2011 Bandura Social learning theory Simply Psychology Retrieved November 10 2012 a b Tomasello Michael 2006 Rational Imitation in 12 Month Old Infants Christiane Schwier Catharine van Maanen Malinda Carpenter Infancy 10 3 303 11 doi 10 1207 s15327078in1003 6 S2CID 41793867 Meltzoff A N amp Moore M K 1977 Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates Science 198 4312 75 78 DOI 10 1126 science 198 4312 75 Field T Woodson R Greenberg R Cohen D October 8 1982 Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates Science 218 4568 179 181 Bibcode 1982Sci 218 179F doi 10 1126 science 7123230 ISSN 0036 8075 PMID 7123230 a b Newborns found able to imitate facial expressions The New York Times October 12 1982 Retrieved November 14 2012 Oostenbroek Janine Suddendorf Thomas Nielsen Mark Redshaw Jonathan Kennedy Costantini Siobhan Davis Jacqueline Clark Sally Slaughter Virginia May 2016 Comprehensive Longitudinal Study Challenges the Existence of Neonatal Imitation in Humans Current Biology 26 10 1334 1338 doi 10 1016 j cub 2016 03 047 PMID 27161497 S2CID 3918288 Foundation Imitation California infant toddler learning amp development foundations California Department of Education 2012 Retrieved November 10 2012 Stiefel Chana 2012 What Your Child Learns By Imitating You Parents Magazine Malcom Kelly June 30 2000 Studies shed light on toddler development Kaye Kenneth 1982 Imitation The Mental and Social Life of Babies pp 155 88 ISBN 978 0 226 42848 2 Toyokawa Wataru Saito Yoshimatsu Kameda Tatsuya May 1 2017 Individual differences in learning behaviours in humans Asocial exploration tendency does not predict reliance on social learning Evolution and Human Behavior 38 3 325 333 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2016 11 001 ISSN 1090 5138 Patrick Karen Richman Charles L 1985 Imitation in Toddlers as a Function of Motor and Verbal Aspects of Modeling The Journal of Genetic Psychology 146 4 507 18 doi 10 1080 00221325 1985 10532470 PMID 3835229 Cherry Kendra Bobo Doll Experiment About com Hay Dennis C Young Andrew W Ellis Andrew W November 1991 Routes through the Face Recognition System The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 43 4 761 791 doi 10 1080 14640749108400957 ISSN 0272 4987 Subiaul Francys Lurie Herbert Romansky Kathryn Klein Tovah Holmes David Terrace Herbert 2007 Cognitive imitation in typically developing 3 and 4 year olds and individuals with autism Cognitive Development 22 2 230 Bibcode 1996CogD 11 491S doi 10 1016 j cogdev 2006 10 003 PMC 2818295 PMID 20148122 Rogers Sally J Hepburn Susan L Stackhouse Tracy Wehner Elizabeth 2003 Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44 5 763 81 doi 10 1111 1469 7610 00162 PMID 12831120 S2CID 17936792 Crompton Catherine J Sharp Martha Axbey Harriet Fletcher Watson Sue Flynn Emma G Ropar Danielle 2020 Neurotype Matching but Not Being Autistic Influences Self and Observer Ratings of Interpersonal Rapport Frontiers in Psychology 11 2961 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2020 586171 ISSN 1664 1078 PMC 7645034 PMID 33192918 Milton Damian E M October 1 2012 On the ontological status of autism the double empathy problem Disability amp Society 27 6 883 887 doi 10 1080 09687599 2012 710008 ISSN 0968 7599 S2CID 54047060 DeThorne Laura S March 1 2020 Revealing the Double Empathy Problem The ASHA Leader 25 3 58 65 doi 10 1044 leader ftr2 25042020 58 S2CID 216359201 Retrieved November 8 2021 Soulieres Isabelle Dawson Michelle Samson Fabienne Barbeau Elise B Sahyoun Cherif P Strangman Gary E Zeffiro Thomas A Mottron Laurent 2009 Enhanced visual processing contributes to matrix reasoning in autism Human Brain Mapping 30 12 4082 4107 doi 10 1002 hbm 20831 PMC 2787806 PMID 19530215 Plaisted Kate O Riordan Michelle Baron Cohen Simon July 1998 Enhanced Discrimination of Novel Highly Similar Stimuli by Adults with Autism During a Perceptual Learning Task Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 39 5 765 775 doi 10 1017 s0021963098002601 ISSN 0021 9630 PMID 9690939 O Riordan Michelle Plaisted Kate November 1 2001 Enhanced discrimination in autism The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 54 4 961 979 doi 10 1080 713756000 ISSN 0272 4987 PMID 11765744 S2CID 144120552 Baron Cohen Simon Belmonte Matthew K July 21 2005 AUTISM A Window Onto the Development of the Social and the Analytic Brain Annual Review of Neuroscience 28 1 109 126 doi 10 1146 annurev neuro 27 070203 144137 ISSN 0147 006X PMID 16033325 Schulte Ruther Martin Otte Ellen Adiguzel Kubra Firk Christine Herpertz Dahlmann Beate Koch Iring Konrad Kerstin June 28 2016 Intact mirror mechanisms for automatic facial emotions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder Intact mirror mechanisms in Austism Autism Research 10 2 298 310 doi 10 1002 aur 1654 Heyes Cecilia 2011 Automatic imitation Psychological Bulletin 137 3 463 83 doi 10 1037 a0022288 PMID 21280938 S2CID 6975248 Lyons Derek E Keil Frank C April 23 2013 Navigating the Social World Oxford University Press pp 145 149 doi 10 1093 acprof oso 9780199890712 003 0026 ISBN 9780199890712 Lyons Derek E Young Andrew G Keil Frank C December 11 2007 The hidden structure of overimitation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104 50 19751 19756 Bibcode 2007PNAS 10419751L doi 10 1073 pnas 0704452104 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 2148370 PMID 18056814 Bower Bruce May 15 2018 Kids are selective imitators not extreme copycats Science News Retrieved May 18 2018 Evans Cara L Laland Kevin N Carpenter Malinda Kendal Rachel L December 17 2017 Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly optimal rather than over imitators PDF Developmental Science 6 1 e12637 doi 10 1111 desc 12637 hdl 10023 16702 ISSN 1363 755X PMID 29250871 Koenig M A Clement F Harris P L October 1 2004 Trust in Testimony Children s Use of True and False Statements Psychological Science 15 10 694 698 doi 10 1111 j 0956 7976 2004 00742 x ISSN 0956 7976 PMID 15447641 S2CID 2116025 Sundqvist Annette Nordqvist Emelie Koch Felix Sebastian Heimann Mikael November 2016 Early declarative memory predicts productive language A longitudinal study of deferred imitation and communication at 9 and 16months Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 151 109 119 doi 10 1016 j jecp 2016 01 015 PMID 26925719 Further reading EditZentall Thomas R 2006 Imitation Definitions evidence and mechanisms Animal Cognition 9 4 335 53 doi 10 1007 s10071 006 0039 2 PMID 17024510 S2CID 16183221 Liepmann H 1900 Das Krankheitsbild der Apraxie motorische Asymbolie Berlin S Karger Publ Liepmann H 1905 Uber die Storungen des Handelns bei Gehirnkranken Berlin S Karger Verlag Liepmann H 1908 Drei Aufsatze aus dem Apraxiegebiet Berlin S Karger Publ Liepmann H 1920 Apraxie Ergebn Ges Med 1 516 43 NAID 10008100327 External links Edit Look up Imitation in Wiktionary the free dictionary Wikimedia Commons has media related to Imitation Wikiquote has quotations related to Imitation M Metzmacher 1995 La transmission du chant chez le Pinson des arbres Fringilla c coelebs phase sensible et role des tuteurs chez les oiseaux captifs Alauda 63 123 134 M Metzmacher 2016 Imitations et transmission culturelle dans le chant du Pinson des arbres Fringilla coelebs Alauda 84 203 220 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Imitation amp oldid 1153201330, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.