fbpx
Wikipedia

Instinct

Instinct is the inherent inclination of a living organism towards a particular complex behaviour, containing both innate (inborn) and learned elements. The simplest example of an instinctive behaviour is a fixed action pattern (FAP), in which a very short to medium length sequence of actions, without variation, are carried out in response to a corresponding clearly defined stimulus.

A leatherback turtle hatchling makes its way to the open ocean.

Any behaviour is instinctive if it is performed without being based upon prior experience (that is, in the absence of learning), and is therefore an expression of innate biological factors. Sea turtles, newly hatched on a beach, will instinctively move toward the ocean. A marsupial climbs into its mother's pouch upon being born. Other examples include animal fighting, animal courtship behaviour, internal escape functions, and the building of nests. Though an instinct is defined by its invariant innate characteristics, details of its performance can be changed by experience; for example, a dog can improve its listening skills by practice.

A wet dog instinctively shakes the water from its fur

Instincts are inborn complex patterns of behaviour that exist in most members of the species, and should be distinguished from reflexes, which are simple responses of an organism to a specific stimulus, such as the contraction of the pupil in response to bright light or the spasmodic movement of the lower leg when the knee is tapped. The absence of volitional capacity must not be confused with an inability to modify fixed action patterns. For example, people may be able to modify a stimulated fixed action pattern by consciously recognizing the point of its activation and simply stop doing it, whereas animals without a sufficiently strong volitional capacity may not be able to disengage from their fixed action patterns, once activated.[1]

Instinctual behaviour in humans has been studied, and is a controversial topic.

Early theorists

Jean Henri Fabre

Jean Henri Fabre (1823–1915) is said to be the first person to study small animals (that weren't birds) and insects, and he specifically specialized in the instincts of insects.[2][3] Fabre considered an instinct to be a linked set of behaviours that an organism undergoes unconsciously in response to external conditions.[4]

Insect and animal behaviour

Fabre concluded a significant difference between humans and animals is that animals cannot reason.[2] He came to this conclusion after observing how insects and wild birds continued to repeat a certain behaviour in response to a novel situation.[2] While these instinctive behaviours appeared complex, the insects and animals did not adjust their behaviour despite it not helping them in that novel situation.[2][4]

The following are some insect and animal behaviours that Fabre observed and labelled "instinctive",[2] for they do not involve reasoning:

Fixed patterns

Fabre believed instincts were "fixed patterns", meaning these linked sets of behaviours do not change in response to novel environmental situations.[4][5] One specific example that helped him arrive at this conclusion is his study of various wasp species.[3][4] All of the wasp species he studied performed a certain pattern of behaviour when catching their prey, which Fabre called a fixed pattern.[3][4] Then Fabre intervened in the wasps' process of catching prey, and only one of the species adjusted their behaviour in response to this unfamiliar interception.[4] Fabre explained this contradiction by arguing that any individuals which stray from the norms of their species are merely an exception,[2][3] while also admitting that there could be some room for growth within a species' instincts.[4]

Fabre's belief that instincts are fixed opposes the theory of evolution. He rejected that one species could evolve into another, and also rejected that the consciousness humans possess could be achieved through the evolution of unconscious traits.[2][4]

Wilhelm Wundt

Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920) is known for founding the first psychology laboratory, which occurred in 1879 at the University of Leipzig.[6][7] He was able to draw conclusions about instinct from his careful observations of both animal and human behaviour.[7][8]

Unconscious processes

To better explain Wundt's research, Claudia Wassman analyzed a large collection of sources. This included some of the earlier journals Wundt wrote, which pondered the idea of unconsciousness more than his later and more known research.[9] Her paper concludes that he believed unconscious processes (which he called "instinctive movements") were the result of sensations and emotions, and these unconscious processes were building blocks towards consciousness.[8][9][10]

Facial expressions

An example of what Wundt studied to arrive at his conclusions regarding unconscious processes includes the facial expressions babies made in response to the sensations of sweet, sour, and bitter tastes.[8] He concluded these facial expressions were the result of the babies trying to avoid unpleasant emotions because there was something unpleasant in their mouths, and that these instincts (which he uses interchangeably with reflexive movements) only became innate because past generations learned it and it benefited their survival.[8]

Natural selection

The process by which Wundt explained the existence of instincts is natural selection. More specifically, his research suggests natural selection causes small changes in the nervous system over time.[8][10] These changes bring about hereditary drives in organisms, which are then responsible for any unconscious processes.[8][10] Another thing to note is that Wundt used the terms unconscious processes, reflexive movements, and instinctive movements interchangeably, often grouping them together.[8][9]

Sigmund Freud

Sigmund Freud considered that mental images of bodily needs, expressed in the form of mental desires, are called instincts.[11]

William McDougall

In the early 20th century, there was recognized a "union of instinct and emotion".[12] William McDougall held that many instincts have their respective associated specific emotions.[13] As research became more rigorous and terms better defined, instinct as an explanation for human behaviour became less common. In 1932, McDougall argued that the word instinct is more suitable for describing animal behaviour, while he recommended the word propensity for goal-directed combinations of the many innate human abilities, which are loosely and variably linked, in a way that shows strong plasticity.[14]

Abraham Maslow

In the 1950s, the psychologist Abraham Maslow argued that humans no longer have instincts because we have the ability to override them in certain situations. He felt that what is called instinct is often imprecisely defined, and really amounts to strong "drives". For Maslow, an instinct is something which cannot be overridden, and therefore while the term may have applied to humans in the past, it no longer does.[15]

Konrad Lorenz

An interest in innate behaviours arose again in the 1950s with Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen, who made the distinction between instinct and learned behaviours. Our modern understanding of instinctual behaviour in animals owes much to their work. For instance, there exists a sensitive period for a bird in which it learns the identity of its mother. Konrad Lorenz famously had a goose imprint on his boots. Thereafter the goose would follow whomever wore the boots. This suggests that the identity of the goose's mother was learned, but the goose's behaviour towards what it perceived as its mother was instinctive.

Frank Beach

In a conference in 1960, chaired by Frank Beach, a pioneer in comparative psychology, and attended by luminaries in the field, the term instinct was restricted in its application.[citation needed] During the 1960s and 1970s, textbooks still contained some discussion of instincts in reference to human behaviour. By the year 2000, a survey of the 12 best selling textbooks in introductory psychology revealed only one reference to instincts, and that was in regard to Sigmund Freud's referral to the "id" instincts.[citation needed] In this sense, the term instinct appeared to have become outmoded for introductory textbooks on human psychology.

The book Instinct: An Enduring Problem in Psychology (1961)[16] selected a range of writings about the topic.

Richard Herrnstein

In a classic paper published in 1972,[17] the psychologist Richard Herrnstein wrote: "A comparison of McDougall's theory of instinct and Skinner's reinforcement theory—representing nature and nurture—shows remarkable, and largely unrecognized, similarities between the contending sides in the nature–nurture debate as applied to the analysis of behavior."

F. B. Mandal proposed a set of criteria by which a behaviour might be considered instinctual: (a) be automatic, (b) be irresistible, (c) occur at some point in development, (d) be triggered by some event in the environment, (e) occur in every member of the species, (f) be unmodifiable, and (g) govern behaviour for which the organism needs no training (although the organism may profit from experience and to that degree the behaviour is modifiable).[18]

In Information Behavior: An Evolutionary Instinct (2010, pp. 35–42), Amanda Spink notes that "currently in the behavioral sciences instinct is generally understood as the innate part of behavior that emerges without any training or education in humans." She claims that the viewpoint that information behaviour has an instinctive basis is grounded in the latest thinking on human behaviour. Furthermore, she notes that "behaviors such as cooperation, sexual behavior, child rearing and aesthetics are [also] seen as 'evolved psychological mechanisms' with an instinctive basis."[19][20][21] Spink adds that Steven Pinker similarly asserts that language acquisition is instinctive in humans in his book The Language Instinct (1994). In 1908, William McDougall wrote about the "instinct of curiosity" and its associated "emotion of wonder",[22] though Spink's book does not mention this.

M. S. Blumberg in 2017 examined the use of the word instinct, and found it varied significantly.[23]

In humans

The existence of the simplest instincts in humans is a widely debated topic.[citation needed] Among possible examples of instinct-influenced behaviour in humans are the following.

  1. Congenital preparedness for developing fear of snakes and spiders was found in six-month-old babies.[24]
  2. Infant cry is believed to be a manifestation of instinct. The infant cannot otherwise protect itself for survival during its long period of maturation. The maternal and paternal bond manifest particularly in response to the infant cry. Its mechanism has been partly elucidated by observations with functional MRI of the parent's brain.[25][26]
  3. The herd instinct is found in human children and chimpanzee infants, but is apparently absent in the young orangutans.[27]
  4. Hormones are linked to specific forms of human behaviour, such as sexuality.[28] High levels of testosterone are often associated in a person (male or female) with aggressiveness,.[29][30] Decrease in testosterone level after the birth of a child was found among fathers.[31][32]
  5. Hygiene behaviour in humans was suggested to be partly instinctive, based on emotions such as disgust.[33][34]
  6. Maternal bond or maternal instinct is when a mother develops a relationship to a child to provide for its well-being. Maternal oxytocin is the hormone and neuropeptide thought to be responsible for predisposing women to showing bonding behavior and bonding.[35][36]
  7. Self-preservation in people generally is when we have the instinct to survive.[37]
  8. Fight-or-flight response in human beings has been said to be a particular response to the arising harmful event, attack or threat to survival.[38]
  9. Cooperation behavior or social instinct has been postulated as an instinct necessary for the future survival of people.[39]
  10. Resistance towards change is the difficulty experienced by a person when they are trying to push against the suggestions made to change behavior or accept certain treatments regardless of whether it will improve their condition or not, it allows for instinctive gratification.[40]

Reflexes

A gecko hunts the pointer of a mouse, confused with prey.

Examples of behaviours that do not require thought include many reflexes. The stimulus in a reflex may not require brain activity but instead may travel to the spinal cord as a message that is then transmitted back through the body, tracing a path called the reflex arc. Reflexes are similar to fixed action patterns in that most reflexes meet the criteria of a fixed action pattern. However, a fixed action pattern can be processed in the brain as well; a male stickleback's instinctive aggression towards anything red during his mating season is such an example. Examples of instinctive behaviours in humans include many of the primitive reflexes, such as rooting and suckling, behaviours which are present in mammals. In rats, it has been observed that innate responses are related to specific chemicals, and these chemicals are detected by two organs located in the nose: the vomeronasal organ (VNO) and the main olfactory epithelium (MOE).[41]

Maturational

Some instinctive behaviours depend on maturational processes to appear. For instance, we commonly refer to birds "learning" to fly. However, young birds have been experimentally reared in devices that prevent them from moving their wings until they reached the age at which their cohorts were flying. These birds flew immediately and normally when released, showing that their improvement resulted from neuromuscular maturation and not true learning.[42]

In evolution

Imprinting provides one example of instinct.[23] This complex response may involve visual, auditory, and olfactory cues in the environment surrounding an organism. In some cases, imprinting attaches an offspring to its parent, which is a reproductive benefit to offspring survival.[43][44] If an offspring has attachment to a parent, it is more likely to stay nearby under parental protection. Attached offspring are also more likely to learn from a parental figure when interacting closely. (Reproductive benefits are a driving force behind natural selection.)

Environment is an important factor in the evolution of innate behaviour. A hypothesis of Michael McCollough, a positive psychologist, explains that environment plays a key role in human behaviours such as forgiveness and revenge. This hypothesis theorizes that various social environments cause either forgiveness or revenge to prevail. McCollough relates his theory to game theory.[45] In a tit-for-tat strategy, cooperation and retaliation are comparable to forgiveness and revenge. The choice between the two can be beneficial or detrimental, depending on what the partner-organism chooses. Though this psychological example of game theory does not have such directly measurable results, it provides an interesting theory of unique thought. From a more biological standpoint, the brain's limbic system operates as the main control-area for response to certain stimuli, including a variety of instinctual behaviour. The limbic system processes external stimuli related to emotions, social activity, and motivation, which propagates a behavioural response. Some behaviours include maternal care, aggression, defense, and social hierarchy. These behaviours are influenced by sensory input — sight, sound, touch, and smell.

Within the circuitry of the limbic system, there are various places where evolution could have taken place, or could take place in the future. For example, many rodents have receptors in the vomeronasal organ that respond explicitly to predator stimuli that specifically relate to that individual species of rodent. The reception of a predatory stimulus usually creates a response of defense or fear.[41] Mating in rats follows a similar mechanism. The vomeronasal organ and the main olfactory epithelium, together called the olfactory system, detect pheromones from the opposite sex. These signals then travel to the medial amygdala, which disperses the signal to a variety of brain parts. The pathways involved with innate circuitry are extremely specialized and specific.[41] Various organs and sensory receptors play parts in this complex process.

Instinct is a phenomenon that can be investigated from a multitude of angles: genetics, limbic system, nervous pathways, and environment.[46] Researchers can study levels of instincts, from molecular to groups of individuals. Extremely specialized systems have evolved, resulting in individuals which exhibit behaviours without learning them.

See also

References

  1. ^ Lorenz, Konrad (1977). Behind the Mirror: A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. ISBN 978-0-15-111699-7.
  2. ^ a b c d e f g Pasteur, Georges (July 1994). "Jean Henri Fabre". Scientific American. 271 (1): 74–80. Bibcode:1994SciAm.271a..74P. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0794-74. ISSN 0036-8733. S2CID 43232778.
  3. ^ a b c d Wheeler, William Morton (1916), "Jean-Henri Fabre", Journal of Animal Behavior, 6: 74–80, doi:10.1037/h0070333, retrieved 2022-05-07
  4. ^ a b c d e f g h Yavetz, Ido (1988). "Jean Henri Fabre and Evolution: Indifference or Blind Hatred?". History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences. 10 (1): 3–36. ISSN 0391-9714. JSTOR 23328997.
  5. ^ Raffles, Hugh (2010). Insectopedia. New York: Pantheon Books. ISBN 978-0-375-42386-4.
  6. ^ Kim, Alan (2016), "Wilhelm Maximilian Wundt", in Zalta, Edward N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall 2016 ed.), Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, retrieved 2022-05-06
  7. ^ a b Munger, Margaret P. (2003). The History of Psychology: Fundamental Questions. Oxford University Press. pp. 296–308. ISBN 0-19-515154-2. OCLC 49558592.
  8. ^ a b c d e f g Wassmann, C. (2008-10-23). "Physiological Optics, Cognition and Emotion: A Novel Look at the Early Work of Wilhelm Wundt". Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences. 64 (2): 213–249. doi:10.1093/jhmas/jrn058. ISSN 0022-5045. PMID 18948411.
  9. ^ a b c Fahrenberg, Jochen (2019). Wilhelm Wundt (1832–1920): Introduction, Quotations, Reception, Commentaries, Attempts at Reconstruction. pp. 35–63. ISBN 978-3-95853-574-9. OCLC 1164647262.
  10. ^ a b c Hamlin, Alice Julia (1897). "IV.—An Attempt at a Psychology of Instinct". Mind. VI (1): 59–70. doi:10.1093/mind/vi.1.59. ISSN 0026-4423.
  11. ^ Hjelle, Larry; Ziegler, Daniel (1981). Personality Theories: Basic Assumptions, Research, and Applications. McGraw-Hill. p. 494. ISBN 9780070290631.
  12. ^ James Rowland Angell (1906). "The Important Human Instincts", Chapter 16 in Psychology: An Introductory Study of the Structure and Function of Human Consciousness, third edition, revised. New York: Henry Holt and Company. https://brocku.ca/MeadProject/Angell/Angell_1906/Angell_1906_p.html
  13. ^ McDougall, W. (1928). An Introduction to Social Psychology, 21st edition, Methuen & Co. Ltd, London, p. vii.
  14. ^ McDougall, W. (1932). The Energies of Men: A Study of the Fundamentals of Dynamic Psychology, second edition, Methuen & Co. Ltd, London, p. 99.
  15. ^ Maslow, Abraham H. (1954). "Instinct Theory Reexamined". Motivation and Personality. New York: Harper & Row.
  16. ^ Birney, R. C., Teevan, R. C. (1961). Instinct: An Enduring Problem in Psychology, Van Nostrand, Princeton, New Jersey.[page needed]
  17. ^ Herrnstein, R. J. (1972). "Nature as Nurture: Behaviorism and the Instinct Doctrine". Behaviorism. 1 (1): 23–52. JSTOR 27758791.
  18. ^ Mandal, F. B. (2010). Textbook of Animal Behaviour. PHI Learning. p. 47. ISBN 978-81-203-4035-0.
  19. ^ Buss, D. (2008). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (3rd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
  20. ^ Dickens, W. T.; Cohen, J. L. (2003). "Instinct and Choice: A Framework for Analysis". In Garcia Coll, C. (ed.). Nature and Nurture: The Complex Interplay of Genetic and Environmental Influences on Human Behavior and Development. Mahwah, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
  21. ^ Geary, D. C. (2004). The Origin of Mind: Evolution of Brain, Cognition, and General Intelligence. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.
  22. ^ McDougall, W. (1928). An Introduction to Social Psychology, 21st edition, Methuen & Co. Ltd, London, p. xxii.
  23. ^ a b Blumberg, Mark S. (2017). "Development evolving:The origins and meanings of instinct". Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. 8 (1–2): e1371. doi:10.1002/wcs.1371. ISSN 1939-5078. PMC 5182125. PMID 27906515.
  24. ^ Hoehl, Stefanie; Hellmer, Kahl; Johansson, Maria; Gredebäck, Gustaf (2017). "Itsy Bitsy Spider…: Infants React with Increased Arousal to Spiders and Snakes". Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 1710. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01710. PMC 5651927. PMID 29093687.
  25. ^ Bornstein, Marc H.; Putnick, Diane L.; Rigo, Paola; Esposito, Gianluca; Swain, James E.; Suwalsky, Joan T. D.; Su, Xueyun; Du, Xiaoxia; Zhang, Kaihua; Cote, Linda R.; De Pisapia, Nicola; Venuti, Paola (2017). "Neurobiology of culturally common maternal responses to infant cry". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 114 (45): E9465–E9473. doi:10.1073/pnas.1712022114. PMC 5692572. PMID 29078366.
  26. ^ Li, Ting; Horta, Marilyn; Mascaro, Jennifer S.; Bijanki, Kelly; Arnal, Luc H.; Adams, Melissa; Barr, Roland G.; Rilling, James K. (2018). "Explaining individual variation in paternal brain responses to infant cries". Physiology & Behavior. 193 (A): 43–54. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.12.033. PMC 6015531. PMID 29730041.
  27. ^ Haun, Daniel B.M.; Rekers, Yvonne; Tomasello, Michael (2012). "Majority-Biased Transmission in Chimpanzees and Human Children, but Not Orangutans". Current Biology. 22 (8): 727–731. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.006. PMID 22503497.
  28. ^ Garland T, Zhao M, Saltzman W (August 2016). "Hormones and the Evolution of Complex Traits: Insights from Artificial Selection on Behavior". Integrative and Comparative Biology. 56 (2): 207–24. doi:10.1093/icb/icw040. PMC 5964798. PMID 27252193.
  29. ^ Dabbs, J. M.; Frady, R. L.; Carr, T. S.; Besch, N. F. (1987). "Saliva testosterone and criminal violence in young adult prison inmates". Psychosomatic Medicine. 49 (2): 174–182. doi:10.1097/00006842-198703000-00007. PMID 3575604. S2CID 39757740.
  30. ^ Dabbs, James; Hargrove, Marian F. (1997). "Age, Testosterone, and Behavior Among Female Prison Inmates". Psychosomatic Medicine. 59 (5): 477–480. doi:10.1097/00006842-199709000-00003. PMID 9316179. S2CID 19900226.
  31. ^ Gettler, L. T.; McDade, T. W.; Feranil, A. B.; Kuzawa, C. W. (2011). "Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 108 (39): 16194–16199. doi:10.1073/pnas.1105403108. PMC 3182719. PMID 21911391.
  32. ^ Grebe, Nicholas M.; Sarafin, Ruth E.; Strenth, Chance R.; Zilioli, Samuele (2019). "Pair-bonding, fatherhood, and the role of testosterone: A meta-analytic review". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 98: 221–233. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.01.010. PMID 30639674. S2CID 58635068.
  33. ^ Curtis, Valerie; Aunger, Robert; deBarra, Mícheál (2011). "Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour". Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366 (1563): 389–401. doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0117. PMC 3013466. PMID 21199843.
  34. ^ Curtis, Valerie; Biran, Adam (2001). "Dirt, disgust, and disease. Is hygiene in our genes?". Perspectives in Biology and Medicine. 44 (1): 17–31. doi:10.1353/pbm.2001.0001. PMID 11253302. S2CID 15675303.
  35. ^ Chiras DD (2012). Human Biology (7th ed.). Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learning. p. 262. ISBN 978-0-7637-8345-7.
  36. ^ Human Evolutionary Biology. Cambridge University Press. 2010. p. 282. ISBN 978-1-139-78900-4.
  37. ^ Green, Scholes, Marci, Marc (30 November 2003). Attachment and Human Survival (1st ed.). Milton Park, Abingdon: United Kingdom: Routledge. ISBN 978-1855759596. Retrieved 30 September 2022.
  38. ^ Cannon, Walter (1932). Wisdom of the Body. United States: W.W. Norton & Company. ISBN 978-0393002058.
  39. ^ Raihani, Nichola (31 August 2021). The Social Instinct: How Cooperation Shaped the World. St. Martin's Press. ISBN 978-1250262820. Retrieved 30 September 2022.
  40. ^ "The Concept of Resistance in Self Psychology". 50 (1): 66–74. doi:10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1996.50.1.66. PMID 8867576. Retrieved 30 September 2021. {{cite journal}}: Cite journal requires |journal= (help)
  41. ^ a b c Sokolowski, Katie; Corbin, Joshua G. (2012). "Wired for behaviors: From development to function of innate limbic system circuitry". Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 5: 55. doi:10.3389/fnmol.2012.00055. PMC 3337482. PMID 22557946.
  42. ^ Campbell, Neil A.; Reece, Jane B. (2002). Biology (6th ed.). San Francisco: Addison Wesley. ISBN 978-0-201-75054-6.[page needed]
  43. ^ Jaynes, Julian (1957). "Imprinting: The interaction of learned and innate behavior: II. The critical period". Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 50 (1): 6–10. doi:10.1037/h0044716. PMID 13406129.
  44. ^ Kim, Young-Joon; Žitňan, Dušan; Galizia, C. Giovanni; Cho, Kook-Ho; Adams, Michael E. (2006). "A Command Chemical Triggers an Innate Behavior by Sequential Activation of Multiple Peptidergic Ensembles". Current Biology. 16 (14): 1395–1407. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.027. PMID 16860738. S2CID 14745330.
  45. ^ Hogan, Michael (2010). "Beyond revenge: The evolution of the forgiveness instinct, by Michael Mc Cullough". The Journal of Positive Psychology. 5: 97–100. doi:10.1080/17439760903509614. S2CID 144005845.
  46. ^ Liang T, Brinkman BAW (2022) Evolution of innate behavioral strategies through competitive population dynamics. PLoS Comput Biol 18(3): e1009934. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1009934

External links

instinct, other, uses, disambiguation, inherent, inclination, living, organism, towards, particular, complex, behaviour, containing, both, innate, inborn, learned, elements, simplest, example, instinctive, behaviour, fixed, action, pattern, which, very, short,. For other uses see Instinct disambiguation Instinct is the inherent inclination of a living organism towards a particular complex behaviour containing both innate inborn and learned elements The simplest example of an instinctive behaviour is a fixed action pattern FAP in which a very short to medium length sequence of actions without variation are carried out in response to a corresponding clearly defined stimulus A leatherback turtle hatchling makes its way to the open ocean Any behaviour is instinctive if it is performed without being based upon prior experience that is in the absence of learning and is therefore an expression of innate biological factors Sea turtles newly hatched on a beach will instinctively move toward the ocean A marsupial climbs into its mother s pouch upon being born Other examples include animal fighting animal courtship behaviour internal escape functions and the building of nests Though an instinct is defined by its invariant innate characteristics details of its performance can be changed by experience for example a dog can improve its listening skills by practice A wet dog instinctively shakes the water from its fur Instincts are inborn complex patterns of behaviour that exist in most members of the species and should be distinguished from reflexes which are simple responses of an organism to a specific stimulus such as the contraction of the pupil in response to bright light or the spasmodic movement of the lower leg when the knee is tapped The absence of volitional capacity must not be confused with an inability to modify fixed action patterns For example people may be able to modify a stimulated fixed action pattern by consciously recognizing the point of its activation and simply stop doing it whereas animals without a sufficiently strong volitional capacity may not be able to disengage from their fixed action patterns once activated 1 Instinctual behaviour in humans has been studied and is a controversial topic Contents 1 Early theorists 1 1 Jean Henri Fabre 1 1 1 Insect and animal behaviour 1 1 2 Fixed patterns 1 2 Wilhelm Wundt 1 2 1 Unconscious processes 1 2 2 Facial expressions 1 2 3 Natural selection 1 3 Sigmund Freud 1 4 William McDougall 1 5 Abraham Maslow 1 6 Konrad Lorenz 1 7 Frank Beach 1 8 Richard Herrnstein 2 In humans 3 Reflexes 4 Maturational 5 In evolution 6 See also 7 References 8 External linksEarly theorists Edit Primitive reflexes Jean Henri Fabre Edit Jean Henri Fabre 1823 1915 is said to be the first person to study small animals that weren t birds and insects and he specifically specialized in the instincts of insects 2 3 Fabre considered an instinct to be a linked set of behaviours that an organism undergoes unconsciously in response to external conditions 4 Insect and animal behaviour Edit Fabre concluded a significant difference between humans and animals is that animals cannot reason 2 He came to this conclusion after observing how insects and wild birds continued to repeat a certain behaviour in response to a novel situation 2 While these instinctive behaviours appeared complex the insects and animals did not adjust their behaviour despite it not helping them in that novel situation 2 4 The following are some insect and animal behaviours that Fabre observed and labelled instinctive 2 for they do not involve reasoning Maternal instincts Metamorphosis Mimicry Molting Playing dead TaxisFixed patterns Edit Fabre believed instincts were fixed patterns meaning these linked sets of behaviours do not change in response to novel environmental situations 4 5 One specific example that helped him arrive at this conclusion is his study of various wasp species 3 4 All of the wasp species he studied performed a certain pattern of behaviour when catching their prey which Fabre called a fixed pattern 3 4 Then Fabre intervened in the wasps process of catching prey and only one of the species adjusted their behaviour in response to this unfamiliar interception 4 Fabre explained this contradiction by arguing that any individuals which stray from the norms of their species are merely an exception 2 3 while also admitting that there could be some room for growth within a species instincts 4 Fabre s belief that instincts are fixed opposes the theory of evolution He rejected that one species could evolve into another and also rejected that the consciousness humans possess could be achieved through the evolution of unconscious traits 2 4 Wilhelm Wundt Edit Wilhelm Wundt 1832 1920 is known for founding the first psychology laboratory which occurred in 1879 at the University of Leipzig 6 7 He was able to draw conclusions about instinct from his careful observations of both animal and human behaviour 7 8 Unconscious processes Edit To better explain Wundt s research Claudia Wassman analyzed a large collection of sources This included some of the earlier journals Wundt wrote which pondered the idea of unconsciousness more than his later and more known research 9 Her paper concludes that he believed unconscious processes which he called instinctive movements were the result of sensations and emotions and these unconscious processes were building blocks towards consciousness 8 9 10 Facial expressions Edit An example of what Wundt studied to arrive at his conclusions regarding unconscious processes includes the facial expressions babies made in response to the sensations of sweet sour and bitter tastes 8 He concluded these facial expressions were the result of the babies trying to avoid unpleasant emotions because there was something unpleasant in their mouths and that these instincts which he uses interchangeably with reflexive movements only became innate because past generations learned it and it benefited their survival 8 Natural selection Edit The process by which Wundt explained the existence of instincts is natural selection More specifically his research suggests natural selection causes small changes in the nervous system over time 8 10 These changes bring about hereditary drives in organisms which are then responsible for any unconscious processes 8 10 Another thing to note is that Wundt used the terms unconscious processes reflexive movements and instinctive movements interchangeably often grouping them together 8 9 Sigmund Freud Edit Sigmund Freud considered that mental images of bodily needs expressed in the form of mental desires are called instincts 11 William McDougall Edit In the early 20th century there was recognized a union of instinct and emotion 12 William McDougall held that many instincts have their respective associated specific emotions 13 As research became more rigorous and terms better defined instinct as an explanation for human behaviour became less common In 1932 McDougall argued that the word instinct is more suitable for describing animal behaviour while he recommended the word propensity for goal directed combinations of the many innate human abilities which are loosely and variably linked in a way that shows strong plasticity 14 Abraham Maslow Edit In the 1950s the psychologist Abraham Maslow argued that humans no longer have instincts because we have the ability to override them in certain situations He felt that what is called instinct is often imprecisely defined and really amounts to strong drives For Maslow an instinct is something which cannot be overridden and therefore while the term may have applied to humans in the past it no longer does 15 Konrad Lorenz Edit An interest in innate behaviours arose again in the 1950s with Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas Tinbergen who made the distinction between instinct and learned behaviours Our modern understanding of instinctual behaviour in animals owes much to their work For instance there exists a sensitive period for a bird in which it learns the identity of its mother Konrad Lorenz famously had a goose imprint on his boots Thereafter the goose would follow whomever wore the boots This suggests that the identity of the goose s mother was learned but the goose s behaviour towards what it perceived as its mother was instinctive Frank Beach Edit In a conference in 1960 chaired by Frank Beach a pioneer in comparative psychology and attended by luminaries in the field the term instinct was restricted in its application citation needed During the 1960s and 1970s textbooks still contained some discussion of instincts in reference to human behaviour By the year 2000 a survey of the 12 best selling textbooks in introductory psychology revealed only one reference to instincts and that was in regard to Sigmund Freud s referral to the id instincts citation needed In this sense the term instinct appeared to have become outmoded for introductory textbooks on human psychology The book Instinct An Enduring Problem in Psychology 1961 16 selected a range of writings about the topic Richard Herrnstein Edit In a classic paper published in 1972 17 the psychologist Richard Herrnstein wrote A comparison of McDougall s theory of instinct and Skinner s reinforcement theory representing nature and nurture shows remarkable and largely unrecognized similarities between the contending sides in the nature nurture debate as applied to the analysis of behavior F B Mandal proposed a set of criteria by which a behaviour might be considered instinctual a be automatic b be irresistible c occur at some point in development d be triggered by some event in the environment e occur in every member of the species f be unmodifiable and g govern behaviour for which the organism needs no training although the organism may profit from experience and to that degree the behaviour is modifiable 18 In Information Behavior An Evolutionary Instinct 2010 pp 35 42 Amanda Spink notes that currently in the behavioral sciences instinct is generally understood as the innate part of behavior that emerges without any training or education in humans She claims that the viewpoint that information behaviour has an instinctive basis is grounded in the latest thinking on human behaviour Furthermore she notes that behaviors such as cooperation sexual behavior child rearing and aesthetics are also seen as evolved psychological mechanisms with an instinctive basis 19 20 21 Spink adds that Steven Pinker similarly asserts that language acquisition is instinctive in humans in his book The Language Instinct 1994 In 1908 William McDougall wrote about the instinct of curiosity and its associated emotion of wonder 22 though Spink s book does not mention this M S Blumberg in 2017 examined the use of the word instinct and found it varied significantly 23 In humans EditThe existence of the simplest instincts in humans is a widely debated topic citation needed Among possible examples of instinct influenced behaviour in humans are the following Congenital preparedness for developing fear of snakes and spiders was found in six month old babies 24 Infant cry is believed to be a manifestation of instinct The infant cannot otherwise protect itself for survival during its long period of maturation The maternal and paternal bond manifest particularly in response to the infant cry Its mechanism has been partly elucidated by observations with functional MRI of the parent s brain 25 26 The herd instinct is found in human children and chimpanzee infants but is apparently absent in the young orangutans 27 Hormones are linked to specific forms of human behaviour such as sexuality 28 High levels of testosterone are often associated in a person male or female with aggressiveness 29 30 Decrease in testosterone level after the birth of a child was found among fathers 31 32 Hygiene behaviour in humans was suggested to be partly instinctive based on emotions such as disgust 33 34 Maternal bond or maternal instinct is when a mother develops a relationship to a child to provide for its well being Maternal oxytocin is the hormone and neuropeptide thought to be responsible for predisposing women to showing bonding behavior and bonding 35 36 Self preservation in people generally is when we have the instinct to survive 37 Fight or flight response in human beings has been said to be a particular response to the arising harmful event attack or threat to survival 38 Cooperation behavior or social instinct has been postulated as an instinct necessary for the future survival of people 39 Resistance towards change is the difficulty experienced by a person when they are trying to push against the suggestions made to change behavior or accept certain treatments regardless of whether it will improve their condition or not it allows for instinctive gratification 40 Reflexes EditMain article Reflex source source source source source source source source source source source source source source A gecko hunts the pointer of a mouse confused with prey Examples of behaviours that do not require thought include many reflexes The stimulus in a reflex may not require brain activity but instead may travel to the spinal cord as a message that is then transmitted back through the body tracing a path called the reflex arc Reflexes are similar to fixed action patterns in that most reflexes meet the criteria of a fixed action pattern However a fixed action pattern can be processed in the brain as well a male stickleback s instinctive aggression towards anything red during his mating season is such an example Examples of instinctive behaviours in humans include many of the primitive reflexes such as rooting and suckling behaviours which are present in mammals In rats it has been observed that innate responses are related to specific chemicals and these chemicals are detected by two organs located in the nose the vomeronasal organ VNO and the main olfactory epithelium MOE 41 Maturational EditSome instinctive behaviours depend on maturational processes to appear For instance we commonly refer to birds learning to fly However young birds have been experimentally reared in devices that prevent them from moving their wings until they reached the age at which their cohorts were flying These birds flew immediately and normally when released showing that their improvement resulted from neuromuscular maturation and not true learning 42 In evolution EditImprinting provides one example of instinct 23 This complex response may involve visual auditory and olfactory cues in the environment surrounding an organism In some cases imprinting attaches an offspring to its parent which is a reproductive benefit to offspring survival 43 44 If an offspring has attachment to a parent it is more likely to stay nearby under parental protection Attached offspring are also more likely to learn from a parental figure when interacting closely Reproductive benefits are a driving force behind natural selection Environment is an important factor in the evolution of innate behaviour A hypothesis of Michael McCollough a positive psychologist explains that environment plays a key role in human behaviours such as forgiveness and revenge This hypothesis theorizes that various social environments cause either forgiveness or revenge to prevail McCollough relates his theory to game theory 45 In a tit for tat strategy cooperation and retaliation are comparable to forgiveness and revenge The choice between the two can be beneficial or detrimental depending on what the partner organism chooses Though this psychological example of game theory does not have such directly measurable results it provides an interesting theory of unique thought From a more biological standpoint the brain s limbic system operates as the main control area for response to certain stimuli including a variety of instinctual behaviour The limbic system processes external stimuli related to emotions social activity and motivation which propagates a behavioural response Some behaviours include maternal care aggression defense and social hierarchy These behaviours are influenced by sensory input sight sound touch and smell Within the circuitry of the limbic system there are various places where evolution could have taken place or could take place in the future For example many rodents have receptors in the vomeronasal organ that respond explicitly to predator stimuli that specifically relate to that individual species of rodent The reception of a predatory stimulus usually creates a response of defense or fear 41 Mating in rats follows a similar mechanism The vomeronasal organ and the main olfactory epithelium together called the olfactory system detect pheromones from the opposite sex These signals then travel to the medial amygdala which disperses the signal to a variety of brain parts The pathways involved with innate circuitry are extremely specialized and specific 41 Various organs and sensory receptors play parts in this complex process Instinct is a phenomenon that can be investigated from a multitude of angles genetics limbic system nervous pathways and environment 46 Researchers can study levels of instincts from molecular to groups of individuals Extremely specialized systems have evolved resulting in individuals which exhibit behaviours without learning them See also EditDrive theory Ethology Genetic memory Heuristic Human ethology Innatism Prey drive hunting instinct Psychological nativism Rationality Sociobiology Unconscious mindReferences Edit Lorenz Konrad 1977 Behind the Mirror A Search for a Natural History of Human Knowledge New York Harcourt Brace Jovanovich ISBN 978 0 15 111699 7 a b c d e f g Pasteur Georges July 1994 Jean Henri Fabre Scientific American 271 1 74 80 Bibcode 1994SciAm 271a 74P doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0794 74 ISSN 0036 8733 S2CID 43232778 a b c d Wheeler William Morton 1916 Jean Henri Fabre Journal of Animal Behavior 6 74 80 doi 10 1037 h0070333 retrieved 2022 05 07 a b c d e f g h Yavetz Ido 1988 Jean Henri Fabre and Evolution Indifference or Blind Hatred History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 10 1 3 36 ISSN 0391 9714 JSTOR 23328997 Raffles Hugh 2010 Insectopedia New York Pantheon Books ISBN 978 0 375 42386 4 Kim Alan 2016 Wilhelm Maximilian Wundt in Zalta Edward N ed The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Fall 2016 ed Metaphysics Research Lab Stanford University retrieved 2022 05 06 a b Munger Margaret P 2003 The History of Psychology Fundamental Questions Oxford University Press pp 296 308 ISBN 0 19 515154 2 OCLC 49558592 a b c d e f g Wassmann C 2008 10 23 Physiological Optics Cognition and Emotion A Novel Look at the Early Work of Wilhelm Wundt Journal of the History of Medicine and Allied Sciences 64 2 213 249 doi 10 1093 jhmas jrn058 ISSN 0022 5045 PMID 18948411 a b c Fahrenberg Jochen 2019 Wilhelm Wundt 1832 1920 Introduction Quotations Reception Commentaries Attempts at Reconstruction pp 35 63 ISBN 978 3 95853 574 9 OCLC 1164647262 a b c Hamlin Alice Julia 1897 IV An Attempt at a Psychology of Instinct Mind VI 1 59 70 doi 10 1093 mind vi 1 59 ISSN 0026 4423 Hjelle Larry Ziegler Daniel 1981 Personality Theories Basic Assumptions Research and Applications McGraw Hill p 494 ISBN 9780070290631 James Rowland Angell 1906 The Important Human Instincts Chapter 16 in Psychology An Introductory Study of the Structure and Function of Human Consciousness third edition revised New York Henry Holt and Company https brocku ca MeadProject Angell Angell 1906 Angell 1906 p html McDougall W 1928 An Introduction to Social Psychology 21st edition Methuen amp Co Ltd London p vii McDougall W 1932 The Energies of Men A Study of the Fundamentals of Dynamic Psychology second edition Methuen amp Co Ltd London p 99 Maslow Abraham H 1954 Instinct Theory Reexamined Motivation and Personality New York Harper amp Row Birney R C Teevan R C 1961 Instinct An Enduring Problem in Psychology Van Nostrand Princeton New Jersey page needed Herrnstein R J 1972 Nature as Nurture Behaviorism and the Instinct Doctrine Behaviorism 1 1 23 52 JSTOR 27758791 Mandal F B 2010 Textbook of Animal Behaviour PHI Learning p 47 ISBN 978 81 203 4035 0 Buss D 2008 Evolutionary Psychology The New Science of the Mind 3rd ed Boston Allyn amp Bacon Dickens W T Cohen J L 2003 Instinct and Choice A Framework for Analysis In Garcia Coll C ed Nature and Nurture The Complex Interplay of Genetic and Environmental Influences on Human Behavior and Development Mahwah New Jersey Erlbaum Geary D C 2004 The Origin of Mind Evolution of Brain Cognition and General Intelligence Washington D C American Psychological Association McDougall W 1928 An Introduction to Social Psychology 21st edition Methuen amp Co Ltd London p xxii a b Blumberg Mark S 2017 Development evolving The origins and meanings of instinct Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science 8 1 2 e1371 doi 10 1002 wcs 1371 ISSN 1939 5078 PMC 5182125 PMID 27906515 Hoehl Stefanie Hellmer Kahl Johansson Maria Gredeback Gustaf 2017 Itsy Bitsy Spider Infants React with Increased Arousal to Spiders and Snakes Frontiers in Psychology 8 1710 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2017 01710 PMC 5651927 PMID 29093687 Bornstein Marc H Putnick Diane L Rigo Paola Esposito Gianluca Swain James E Suwalsky Joan T D Su Xueyun Du Xiaoxia Zhang Kaihua Cote Linda R De Pisapia Nicola Venuti Paola 2017 Neurobiology of culturally common maternal responses to infant cry Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114 45 E9465 E9473 doi 10 1073 pnas 1712022114 PMC 5692572 PMID 29078366 Li Ting Horta Marilyn Mascaro Jennifer S Bijanki Kelly Arnal Luc H Adams Melissa Barr Roland G Rilling James K 2018 Explaining individual variation in paternal brain responses to infant cries Physiology amp Behavior 193 A 43 54 doi 10 1016 j physbeh 2017 12 033 PMC 6015531 PMID 29730041 Haun Daniel B M Rekers Yvonne Tomasello Michael 2012 Majority Biased Transmission in Chimpanzees and Human Children but Not Orangutans Current Biology 22 8 727 731 doi 10 1016 j cub 2012 03 006 PMID 22503497 Garland T Zhao M Saltzman W August 2016 Hormones and the Evolution of Complex Traits Insights from Artificial Selection on Behavior Integrative and Comparative Biology 56 2 207 24 doi 10 1093 icb icw040 PMC 5964798 PMID 27252193 Dabbs J M Frady R L Carr T S Besch N F 1987 Saliva testosterone and criminal violence in young adult prison inmates Psychosomatic Medicine 49 2 174 182 doi 10 1097 00006842 198703000 00007 PMID 3575604 S2CID 39757740 Dabbs James Hargrove Marian F 1997 Age Testosterone and Behavior Among Female Prison Inmates Psychosomatic Medicine 59 5 477 480 doi 10 1097 00006842 199709000 00003 PMID 9316179 S2CID 19900226 Gettler L T McDade T W Feranil A B Kuzawa C W 2011 Longitudinal evidence that fatherhood decreases testosterone in human males Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108 39 16194 16199 doi 10 1073 pnas 1105403108 PMC 3182719 PMID 21911391 Grebe Nicholas M Sarafin Ruth E Strenth Chance R Zilioli Samuele 2019 Pair bonding fatherhood and the role of testosterone A meta analytic review Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 98 221 233 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2019 01 010 PMID 30639674 S2CID 58635068 Curtis Valerie Aunger Robert deBarra Micheal 2011 Disgust as an adaptive system for disease avoidance behaviour Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366 1563 389 401 doi 10 1098 rstb 2010 0117 PMC 3013466 PMID 21199843 Curtis Valerie Biran Adam 2001 Dirt disgust and disease Is hygiene in our genes Perspectives in Biology and Medicine 44 1 17 31 doi 10 1353 pbm 2001 0001 PMID 11253302 S2CID 15675303 Chiras DD 2012 Human Biology 7th ed Sudbury MA Jones amp Bartlett Learning p 262 ISBN 978 0 7637 8345 7 Human Evolutionary Biology Cambridge University Press 2010 p 282 ISBN 978 1 139 78900 4 Green Scholes Marci Marc 30 November 2003 Attachment and Human Survival 1st ed Milton Park Abingdon United Kingdom Routledge ISBN 978 1855759596 Retrieved 30 September 2022 Cannon Walter 1932 Wisdom of the Body United States W W Norton amp Company ISBN 978 0393002058 Raihani Nichola 31 August 2021 The Social Instinct How Cooperation Shaped the World St Martin s Press ISBN 978 1250262820 Retrieved 30 September 2022 The Concept of Resistance in Self Psychology 50 1 66 74 doi 10 1176 appi psychotherapy 1996 50 1 66 PMID 8867576 Retrieved 30 September 2021 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a Cite journal requires journal help a b c Sokolowski Katie Corbin Joshua G 2012 Wired for behaviors From development to function of innate limbic system circuitry Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience 5 55 doi 10 3389 fnmol 2012 00055 PMC 3337482 PMID 22557946 Campbell Neil A Reece Jane B 2002 Biology 6th ed San Francisco Addison Wesley ISBN 978 0 201 75054 6 page needed Jaynes Julian 1957 Imprinting The interaction of learned and innate behavior II The critical period Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 50 1 6 10 doi 10 1037 h0044716 PMID 13406129 Kim Young Joon Zitnan Dusan Galizia C Giovanni Cho Kook Ho Adams Michael E 2006 A Command Chemical Triggers an Innate Behavior by Sequential Activation of Multiple Peptidergic Ensembles Current Biology 16 14 1395 1407 doi 10 1016 j cub 2006 06 027 PMID 16860738 S2CID 14745330 Hogan Michael 2010 Beyond revenge The evolution of the forgiveness instinct by Michael Mc Cullough The Journal of Positive Psychology 5 97 100 doi 10 1080 17439760903509614 S2CID 144005845 Liang T Brinkman BAW 2022 Evolution of innate behavioral strategies through competitive population dynamics PLoS Comput Biol 18 3 e1009934 https doi org 10 1371 journal pcbi 1009934External links Edit Look up instinct in Wiktionary the free dictionary Wikiquote has quotations related to Instinct Laplanche Jean Pontalis Jean Bertrand 1973 Instinct or Drive The Language of Psycho analysis London Karnac Books pp 214 217 ISBN 978 0 946 43949 2 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Instinct amp oldid 1128238141, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.