fbpx
Wikipedia

In-group favoritism

In-group favoritism, sometimes known as in-group–out-group bias, in-group bias, intergroup bias, or in-group preference, is a pattern of favoring members of one's in-group over out-group members. This can be expressed in evaluation of others, in allocation of resources, and in many other ways.[1][2]

This effect has been researched by many psychologists and linked to many theories related to group conflict and prejudice. The phenomenon is primarily viewed from a social psychology standpoint. Studies have shown that in-group favoritism arises as a result of the formation of cultural groups.[3][4] These cultural groups can be divided based on seemingly trivial observable traits, but with time, populations grow to associate certain traits with certain behavior, increasing covariation. This then incentivizes in-group bias.

Two prominent theoretical approaches to the phenomenon of in-group favoritism are realistic conflict theory and social identity theory. Realistic conflict theory proposes that intergroup competition, and sometimes intergroup conflict, arises when two groups have opposing claims to scarce resources. In contrast, social identity theory posits a psychological drive for positively distinct social identities as the general root cause of in-group favoring behavior.

Origins of the research tradition Edit

In 1906, the sociologist William Sumner posited that humans are a species that join together in groups by their very nature. However, he also maintained that humans had an innate tendency to favor their own group over others, proclaiming how "each group nourishes its own pride and vanity, boasts itself superior, exists in its own divinities, and looks with contempt on outsiders".[5] This is seen on the group level with ingroup–outgroup bias. When experienced in larger groups such as tribes, ethnic groups, or nations, it is referred to as ethnocentrism.

Explanations Edit

Competition Edit

Realistic conflict theory (or realistic group conflict) posits that competition between groups for resources is the cause of in-group bias and the corresponding negative treatment of members of the out-group. Muzafer Sherif's Robbers Cave Experiment is the most widely known demonstration of realistic conflict theory. In the experiment, 22 eleven-year-old boys with similar backgrounds were studied in a mock summer camp situation, with researchers posing as camp personnel.

The boys were divided into two equal groups and encouraged to bond, with the aim of fostering an in-group mentality. The researchers then introduced a series of competitive activities which pitted groups against each other for a valuable prize. Hostility and out-group negativity ensued.[6] Lastly, researchers attempted to reverse the hostility by engaging the boys in situations of mutual interdependence, an effort which eventually resulted in relative harmony between the two groups.

Sherif concluded from this experiment that negative attitudes toward out-groups arise when groups compete for limited resources.[6] However, he also theorized that inter-group frictions could be reduced and positive relations created,[6] but only in the presence of an overarching goal, which could only be achieved with the two groups' cooperation.[6][1]

Self-esteem Edit

According to social identity theory, one of the key determinants of group biases is the need to improve self-esteem. The desire to view one's self positively is transferred onto the group, creating a tendency to view one's own group in a positive light, and by comparison, outside groups in a negative light.[7] That is, individuals will find a reason, no matter how insignificant, to prove to themselves why their own group is superior. This phenomenon was pioneered and studied most extensively by Henri Tajfel, a British social psychologist who looked at the psychological root of in-group/out-group bias. To study this in the lab, Tajfel and colleagues created minimal groups (see minimal group paradigm), which occur when "complete strangers are formed into groups using the most trivial criteria imaginable". In Tajfel's studies, participants were split into groups by flipping a coin, and each group then was told to appreciate a certain style of painting none of the participants were familiar with when the experiment began. What Tajfel and his colleagues discovered was that—regardless of the facts that a) participants did not know each other, b) their groups were completely meaningless, and c) none of the participants had any inclination as to which "style" they like better—participants almost always "liked the members of their own group better and they rated the members of their in-group as more likely to have pleasant personalities". By having a more positive impression of individuals in the in-group, individuals are able to boost their own self-esteem as members of that group.[1]

Robert Cialdini and his research team looked at the number of university T-shirts being worn on college campuses following either a win or loss at the football game. They found that the Monday after a win, there were more T-shirts being worn, on average, than following a loss.[1][8]

In another set of studies, done in the 1980s by Jennifer Crocker and colleagues using the minimal group paradigm, individuals with high self-esteem who suffered a threat to the self-concept exhibited greater ingroup biases than did people with low self-esteem who suffered a threat to the self-concept.[9] While some studies have supported this notion of a negative correlation between self-esteem and in-group bias,[10] other researchers have found that individuals with low self-esteem showed more bias toward both in-group and out-group members.[9] Some studies have even shown that high-self-esteem groups showed more bias than did lower self-esteem groups.[11] This research may suggest that there is an alternative explanation and additional reasoning as to the relationship between self-esteem and in-group/out-group biases. Alternatively, it is possible that researchers have used the wrong sort of self-esteem measures to test the link between self-esteem and in-group bias (global personal self-esteem rather than specific social self-esteem).[12]

Biological basis as an effect of oxytocin Edit

In a meta-analysis and review of the effect of oxytocin on social behavior done by Carsten De Dreu, the research reviewed shows that oxytocin enables the development of trust, specifically towards individuals with similar characteristics—categorized as 'in-group' members—promoting cooperation with and favoritism towards such individuals.[13] This bias of oxytocin-induced goodwill towards those with features and characteristics perceived to be similar may have evolved as a biological basis for sustaining in-group cooperation and protection, fitting with the Darwinian insight that acts of self-sacrifice and cooperation contribute to the functioning of the group and hence improve the odds of survival for members of said group.[13]

Race can be used as an example of in-group and out-group tendencies because society often categorizes individuals into groups based on race (Caucasian, African American, Latino, etc.). One study that examined race and empathy found that participants receiving nasally administered oxytocin had stronger reactions to pictures of in-group members making pained faces than to pictures of out-group members with the same expression.[14] This shows that oxytocin may be implicated in our ability to empathize with individuals of different races, with individuals of one race potentially biased towards helping individuals of the same race than individuals of another race when they are experiencing pain.

Oxytocin has also been implicated in lying when lying would prove beneficial to other in-group members. In a study where such a relationship was examined, it was found that when individuals were administered oxytocin, rates of dishonesty in the participants' responses increased for their in-group members when a beneficial outcome for their group was expected.[15] Both of these examples show the tendency to act in ways that benefit in-group members.

Self-identity and social identity Edit

As noted in two recent theoretical reviews,[16] the theoretical basis for the inclusion of self-identity in the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior has many similarities to social identity theory [17] and its extension, self-categorization theory.[18] According to social identity theory, an important component of the self-concept is derived from memberships in social groups and categories. When people define and evaluate themselves in terms of a self-inclusive social category (e.g., sex, class, team) two processes come into play: (1) categorization, which perceptually accentuates differences between the in-group and out-group, and similarities among in-group members (including the self) on stereotypical dimensions; and (2) self-enhancement which, because the self-concept is defined in terms of group membership, seeks behaviorally and perceptually to favor the in-group over the out-group. Social identities are cognitively represented as group prototypes that describe and prescribe beliefs, attitudes, feelings and behaviors that optimize a balance between minimization of in-group differences and maximization of intergroup differences.

More specifically, according to social identity theory, there is a continuum between personal and social identity shifts along this continuum that determine the extent to which group-related or personal characteristics influence a person's feelings and actions.[19] If a particular social identity is a salient basis for self-conception, then the self is assimilated to the perceived in-group prototype which can be thought of as a set of perceived in-group norms such that self-perception, beliefs, attitudes, feelings and behaviors are defined in terms of the group prototype. Thus, social identities should influence behavior through the mediating role of group norms. People will be more likely to engage in a particular behavior if it is in accord with the norms of a behaviorally relevant group membership, particularly if the identity is a salient basis for self-definition. If the group membership is not salient, then people's behavior and feelings should be in accord with their own personal and idiosyncratic characteristics rather than group norms.

On the other hand, the self-identity theory poses that the self is often a reflection of expected norms and practices in a person's social role. At the center of it is the proposition that the self is made up of multi-faceted and differentiated components that exist in an organized manner for the sake of filling in roles in society.[20] People are able to create an identity for themselves only through talking to others, and often what roles they are taking on differ from one group to another. These differing roles and positions people fill are a result of their interactions with others and are called role identities. Role identities may be self-realized, or may be facts like being a mother, a social worker, or a blood donor. Role identities lead people to act in certain ways due to assumed expectations for the roles. Because there is satisfaction in complying with expectations of the role, there is often distress behind an inability to appear congruent to one's identity as defined by societal norms. There is also an existing hierarchy of importance for roles that individuals take on, and according to the hierarchical standing of roles, people become more representative of roles that stand higher hierarchically, according to them.

Identity salience, the likelihood of role identities being invoked in different situations, is the result of role identities being placed hierarchically in different orders from person to person. People who hold the same roles may act differently because some roles are valued over others.[21] For example, a working mother may have less time to spend with her child as opposed to a mother that does not work. Behaviors are reflective of the identities that are held higher hierarchically by people, so people act out in self-worth and self-meaning according to these hierarchies.[22] Someone who holds the identity of being a psychologist higher than the identity of being a linguist will find that while he/she may become competitive when meeting another person that is better at psychology than he/she, he/she won't care when in contact with someone who is much better at being a linguist than he/she. In a similar way, social relationships are influenced by this salience. Self-identity often places individuals in social contexts and a commitment to the role within that context becomes a big part of perpetrating the idea of self. It also finds people relating more to others that hold similar role identities at the top of their hierarchies.

Because people have self-concepts that are derived from a role they define for themselves within the context of a group, when staying within their roles, intergroup similarities are accentuated while intergroup differences are diminished.[23] In an attempt to assimilate oneself according to the tendencies of a group, often people reconfigure their intragroup representations or identities. Certain prototypes form about these groups that reaffirm rules that members of the group are encouraged to follow. Shared information and views are discussed more often than novel and unshared information within a group, therefore a norm is established where the majority views are perpetuated and others silenced.[24] This norm is fluid and changing according to different contexts, but those within the group who want to keep up with the majority views in all matters have to keep an active role in affirming the views of the in-group in contest to out-groups.

Evolution of in-groups Edit

Formation of cultural groups Edit

Studies have shown that in-group favoritism arises endogenously, through the formation of cultural groups.[3] Symbolic markers in certain conditions can result in trivial groupings developing into cultural groups. The formation of such cultural groups then results in a higher degree of in-group favoritism.

Efferson, Lalive and Fehr published such a study in 2008, utilizing a series of coordination games to mimic cooperation between individuals. The study found that cultural groups were able to form endogenously through creation of a linkage between a payoff-relevant behavior and a payoff-irrelevant marker. Subsequently, in-group favoritism occurred in ensuing social interactions.[3]

Participants were first divided into one of several populations of 10 people, and then further divided into subpopulations of 5. Each group had different payoff for coordinating on one of 2 choices, behavior A or behavior B. In group 1, participants were awarded 41 points for coordinating (choosing A themselves and choosing another participant who also chose A) on A and 21 for coordinating on B. The payoffs were switched in the 2nd group. In both groups participants were awarded just 1 point for mis-coordinating. During each turn participants were also allowed to choose a payoff-irrelevant marker (circle or triangle). Players from both subpopulations were mixed to create a coordination problem, and every turn, an unidentified player from each subpopulation would be randomly switched.

The experiment created a situation in which participants were strongly incentivized to develop a sense of expected behaviors in his or her subpopulation, but occasionally would find themselves in a totally new situation in which their behaviors were not in-line with social norms.[3]

The results showed that players generally developed an inclination to pair behavior with a marker, especially if it had resulted in a positive payoff. As linkages at an individual level increase, covariation (of marker and behavior) at an aggregate level also increases. In the experiment, there was a significant increase in participants requesting for partners with the same-shape choice as it progressed, although the initial choice of shape had no effect on payoffs. Toward the end of the experiment, this number stood at a substantial 87%, indicating the presence of in-group favoritism.

Their study supported the hypothesis that the formation of cultural groups alters selective pressure facing individuals, and thus leads to certain behavioral traits being advantageous.[4][3] Thus, if such selective pressures were present in past civilizations, where membership in a certain group is correlated with a certain behavioral norm, the emergence of in-group biases where it is beneficial to act in differing manners to members of the same group is certainly plausible.[3]

Gender differences Edit

Automatic bias for own gender Edit

Rudman and Goodwin conducted research on gender bias that measured gender preferences without directly asking the participants. Subjects at Purdue and Rutgers University participated in computerized tasks that measured automatic attitudes based on how quickly a person categorizes pleasant and unpleasant attributes with each gender. Such a task was done to discover whether people associate pleasant words (good, happy, and sunshine) with women, and unpleasant words (bad, trouble, and pain) with men.[25]

This research found that while both women and men have more favorable views of women, women's in-group biases were 4.5 times stronger[25] than those of men and only women (not men) showed cognitive balance among in-group bias, identity, and self-esteem, revealing that men lack a mechanism that bolsters automatic preference for their own gender.[25]

Competition Edit

Using a publics-goods game, Van Vugt, De Cremer, and Janssen found that men contributed more to their group in the face of outside competition from another group; there was no distinct difference amongst women's contributions.[26]

Ethnicity-based favoritism Edit

In 2001 Fershtman and Gneezy found that men showed in-group biases in a "trust" game based on ethnicity, whereas this tendency was not present in women.[27] The study aims to identify ethnic discrimination in Israeli Jewish society, and was conducted on 996 Israeli undergraduates. Groups were separated based on whether the participant's name was typically ethnically Eastern or Ashkenazic. Similar to a dictator game, subjects were instructed to divide a sum of money (20 NIS) between themselves and another player. Player A was told that any money sent over to Player B would be tripled, and Player B would receive details of the experiment, including the name of Player A and the transferred sum. Subsequently, Player B would have a choice of whether to send any money back.

The experiment found that despite sharing similar average transfer values (10.63 for women and 11.42 for men), women did not display significant in-group biases when it came to recipients with either Ashkenazic or Eastern sounding names. However, a bias against Eastern sounding names was present amongst men.[27]

Furthermore, men showed more bias for Ashkenazic men compared to women, but the opposite was true for Eastern names.[27] This result may seem counter-intuitive, as participants appear to share more in common if they were both male. Thus, we would expect Eastern females to be more marginalized, but is actually consistent with other studies which studied discrimination against Afro-American women.[28]

Developmental age Edit

In 2008 Fehr, Bernhard, and Rockenbach, in a study conducted on children, found that boys displayed in-group favoritism from ages 3–8, whereas girls did not display such tendencies.[29] The experiment involved usage of an "envy game", a modified version of the dictator game. A possible explanation posited by researchers relied on an evolutionary basis.[29]

They theorized that parochialism and favoring members of the same group may have been particularly advantageous as it strengthened the individuals group position in intergroup conflicts.[29] As males were the ones who were frequently at the forefront of such conflicts in the past, and thus bore the majority of the costs of conflicts in terms of injury or death, evolution may have favored a greater sensitivity in males in situations which resulted in an advantageous payoff for their in-group. Thus males tended to show in-group biases from a younger age than females, as was evident in the experiment.[29]

Real-world examples Edit

Armenian Genocide denial Edit

A 2013 study found that Turks with a stronger in-group bias were less likely to acknowledge the in-group responsibility for the Armenian genocide.[30]

2008 US Presidential elections Edit

A study conducted during the 2008 Presidential elections showcased how group identities were dynamic.[31] The study was carried out among 395 Democrats from Cambridge, MA, using an Economics dictator game. Subjects were given $6 to divide between themselves and another person. The recipients remained anonymous, apart from which candidate they supported in the Democratic Primaries.

Data were collected in three separate periods. June 10 to 18 (after Hillary Clinton's concession speech on June 7); August 9 to 14, before the Democratic National Convention on the 25; and September 2 to 5, in the buildup to the Presidential elections. The results showed that men displayed significant in-group favoritism from June all the way to the DNC in August. This in-group bias, however, was not present in September. Women displayed no significant in-group favoritism throughout.

The experiment suggested that group identities are flexible and can change over time.[31] Researchers theorized that in-group bias was strong in June, as the competition to be the Democratic nominee in the elections was still recent and thus salient. A lack of actual electoral conflict (against the Republicans) caused perception of salient groupings to remain throughout August.[31] Only in September did the in-group favoritism subside as a superordinate goal shared between groups was now present.

Wikipedia Edit

Research analyzing articles about 35 inter-group conflicts (e.g., Falklands War) by comparing the corresponding language versions of Wikipedia (e.g., English, Spanish) found evidence for in-group favoritism: While the "in-group" was systematically preferred and presented in a more favorable light, the "out-group" was presented as more immoral and more responsible for the conflict.[32] There were substantial variations between conflicts, however, and additional analyses revealed in-group favoritism to be more pronounced in more recent conflicts and in articles written predominantly by "in-group" members.[32]

Versus out-group negativity Edit

Social psychologists have long made the distinction between ingroup favoritism and outgroup negativity, where outgroup negativity is the act of punishing or placing burdens upon the outgroup.[19] Indeed, a significant body of research exists that attempts to identify the relationship between ingroup favoritism and outgroup negativity, as well as conditions that will lead to outgroup negativity.[33][34][35] For example, Struch and Schwartz found support for the predictions of belief congruence theory.[36] The belief congruence theory concerns itself with the degree of similarity in beliefs, attitudes, and values perceived to exist between individuals. This theory also states that dissimilarity increases negative orientations towards others. When applied to racial discrimination, the belief congruence theory argues that the perceived dissimilarity of beliefs has more of an impact on racial discrimination than does race itself.

Research finds evidence of in-group bias in police investigations[37] and judicial decisions.[38]

Biological relationship Edit

Oxytocin is not only correlated with the preferences of individuals to associate with members of their own group, but it is also evident during conflicts between members of different groups. During conflict, individuals receiving nasally administered oxytocin demonstrate more frequent defense-motivated responses toward in-group members than out-group members. Further, oxytocin was correlated with participant desire to protect vulnerable in-group members, despite that individual's attachment to the conflict.[39] Similarly, it has been demonstrated that when oxytocin is administered, individuals alter their subjective preferences in order to align with in-group ideals over out-group ideals.[40] These studies demonstrate that oxytocin is associated with intergroup dynamics.

Further, oxytocin influences the responses of individuals in a particular group to those of another group. The in-group bias is evident in smaller groups; however, it can also be extended to groups as large as one's entire country leading toward a tendency of strong national zeal. A study done in the Netherlands showed that oxytocin increased the in-group favoritism of their nation while decreasing acceptance of members of other ethnicities and foreigners.[41] People also show more affection for their country's flag while remaining indifferent to other cultural objects when exposed to oxytocin.[42] It has thus been hypothesized that this hormone may be a factor in xenophobic tendencies secondary to this effect. Thus, oxytocin appears to affect individuals at an international level where the in-group becomes a specific "home" country and the out-group grows to include all other countries.

In-group derogation Edit

Cross-cultural studies have found that in-group derogation, the tendency to criticize members of one's own group or culture more harshly than members of outside groups, is more common among members of disadvantaged and minority groups than among members of the majority or dominant group. According to Ma-Kellams, Spencer-Rodgers and Peng, system justification theory seeks to explain why "minorities sometimes endorse system-justifying views of their group". They said their research into in-group favoritism and derogation partially supported this theory, but that the theory failed to address all of the nuances.[43]

Ma-Kellams et al. also found that, compared to individualist cultures, people from collectivist cultures, such as East Asian cultures, tended to judge their own group members less favorably than they judged outsiders, whereas people from individualist cultures were inclined to judge members of their own group more favorably than they judged outsiders.[43] Social identity theory[citation needed] and Freudian theorists explain in-group derogation as the result of a negative self-image, which they believe is then extended to the group.[43] Ma-Kellams et al. theorized that "ingroup derogation may be more culturally normative and less troubling for East Asians" as evidenced by the fact that East Asians were also likely to report high levels of positive affect (emotion) towards members of their in-group, demonstrating ambivalence towards the unfavorable characteristics they had acknowledged about their in-group. According to Ma-Kellam et al., culturally-ingrained attitudes and beliefs, rather than low self-esteem, may play a role in collectivist cultures' in-group derogation, due to their ability to tolerate holding seemingly contradictory views.[43]

See also Edit

References Edit

  1. ^ a b c d Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. (2010). Social psychology. 7th ed. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.
  2. ^ Taylor, Donald M.; Doria, Janet R. (April 1981). "Self-serving and group-serving bias in attribution". Journal of Social Psychology. 113 (2): 201–211. doi:10.1080/00224545.1981.9924371.
  3. ^ a b c d e f Efferson, Charles; Lalive, Rafael; Fehr, Ernst (2008-09-26). "The Coevolution of Cultural Groups and Ingroup Favoritism" (PDF). Science. 321 (5897): 1844–1849. Bibcode:2008Sci...321.1844E. doi:10.1126/science.1155805. PMID 18818361. S2CID 32927015.
  4. ^ a b Fu, Feng; Tarnita, Corina E.; Christakis, Nicholas A.; Wang, Long; Rand, David G.; Nowak, Martin A. (2012-06-21). "Evolution of in-group favoritism". Scientific Reports. 2: 460. Bibcode:2012NatSR...2E.460F. doi:10.1038/srep00460. PMC 3380441. PMID 22724059.
  5. ^ Sumner, William Graham, 1840–1910. (23 September 2014). Folkways : a study of the sociological importance of usages, manners, customs, mores, and morals (Reprint, 2014 ed.). p. 13. ISBN 978-1-5024-6917-5. OCLC 1003045387.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  6. ^ a b c d Sherif, M.; Harvey, O.J.; White, B.J.; Hood, W. & Sherif, C.W. (1961). Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robbers Cave Experiment. Norman, OK: The University Book Exchange. pp. 155–184.
  7. ^ Billig, Michael; Tajfel, Henri (1973). "Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behaviour". European Journal of Social Psychology. 3 (1): 27–52. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420030103.
  8. ^ Cialdini, Robert B.; Borden, Richard J.; Thorne, Avril; Walker, Marcus Randall; Freeman, Stephen; Sloan, Lloyd Reynolds (1974). "Basking in Reflected Glory: Three (Football) Field Studies". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 34 (3): 366–375. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.3.366. S2CID 13191936.
  9. ^ a b Crocker, Jennifer; Thompson, Leigh L.; McGraw, Kathleen M.; Ingerman, Cindy (May 1987). "Downward Comparison, Prejudice, and Evaluations of Others: Effects of Self-Esteem and Threat". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 52 (5): 907–916. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.5.907. PMID 3585702.
  10. ^ Abrams, Dominic; Hogg, Michael A. (1988). "Comments on the motivational status of self-esteem in social identity and intergroup discrimination". European Journal of Social Psychology. 18 (4): 317–334. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420180403.
  11. ^ Sachdev, Itesh; Bourhis, Richard Y. (September 1987). "Status differentials and intergroup behaviour". European Journal of Social Psychology. 17 (3): 277–293. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420170304.
  12. ^ Rubin, Mark; Hewstone, Miles (February 1998). "Social Identity Theory's Self-Esteem Hypothesis: A Review and Some Suggestions for Clarification". Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2 (1): 40–62. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0201_3. hdl:1959.13/930907. PMID 15647150. S2CID 40695727.
  13. ^ a b De Dreu, Carsten K.W. (2012). "Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups: An integrative review and research agenda". Hormones and Behavior. 61 (3): 419–428. doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.009. PMID 22227278. S2CID 140120921.
  14. ^ Sheng F, Liu Y, Zhou B, Zhou W, Han S (February 2013). "Oxytocin modulates the racial bias in neural responses to others' suffering". Biological Psychology. 92 (2): 380–6. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.018. PMID 23246533. S2CID 206109148.
  15. ^ Shalvi S, De Dreu CK (April 2014). "Oxytocin promotes group-serving dishonesty". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 111 (15): 5503–7. Bibcode:2014PNAS..111.5503S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1400724111. PMC 3992689. PMID 24706799.
  16. ^ Hogg, Michael A.; Terry, Deborah J.; White, Katherine M. (1995). "A Tale of Two Theories: A Critical Comparison of Identity Theory with Social Identity Theory". Social Psychology Quarterly. 58 (4): 255–269. doi:10.2307/2787127. JSTOR 2787127.
  17. ^ Tajfel, Henri (1974-04-01). "Social identity and intergroup behaviour". Social Science Information. 13 (2): 65–93. doi:10.1177/053901847401300204. S2CID 143666442.
  18. ^ Turner, John C. (1985). "Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior". In Lawler, E. J. (ed.). Advances in Group Processes: Theory and Research, Vol.2. Greenwich, CT: JAI. pp. 77–122. ISBN 978-0-89232-524-5. OCLC 925165493.
  19. ^ a b Tajfel, Henri; Turner, John C. (1979). "An integrative theory of intergroup conflict". In Austin, W. G.; Worchel, S. (eds.). The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Brooks-Cole. pp. 33–47. ISBN 978-0-8185-0278-1.
  20. ^ Stryker, Sheldon; Serpe, Richard T. (1982). "Commitment, Identity Salience, and Role Behavior: Theory and Research Example". In Ickes, William; Knowles, Eric S. (eds.). Personality, Roles, and Social Behavior. Springer New York. pp. 199–218. doi:10.1007/978-1-4613-9469-3_7. ISBN 978-1-4613-9471-6.
  21. ^ Nuttbrock, Larry; Freudiger, Patricia (1991). "Identity Salience and Motherhood: A Test of Stryker's Theory". Social Psychology Quarterly. 54 (2): 146–157. doi:10.2307/2786932. JSTOR 2786932.
  22. ^ Callero, Peter L. (1985). "Role-Identity Salience". Social Psychology Quarterly. 48 (3): 203–215. doi:10.2307/3033681. JSTOR 3033681.
  23. ^ Tajfel, H. (1959). "Quantitative Judgement in Social Perception". British Journal of Psychology. 50 (1): 16–29. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1959.tb00677.x. PMID 13628966.
  24. ^ Larson, James R.; Foster-Fishman, Pennie G.; Keys, Christopher B. (1994). "Discussion of shared and unshared information in decision-making groups". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (3): 446–461. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.446.
  25. ^ a b c Rudman, Laurie A.; Goodwin, Stephanie A. (2004). "Gender differences in automatic in-group bias: Why do women like women more than men like men?" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (4): 494–509. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.494. PMID 15491274. (PDF) from the original on July 18, 2014. Retrieved September 2, 2016.
  26. ^ Van Vugt, Mark; De Cremer, David; Janssen, Dirk P. (2007). "Gender differences in cooperation and competition: the male-warrior hypothesis". Psychological Science. 18 (1): 19–23. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x. PMID 17362372. S2CID 3566509.
  27. ^ a b c Fershtman, Chaim; Gneezy, Uri (2001). "Discrimination in a Segmented Society: An Experimental Approach". The Quarterly Journal of Economics. 116 (1): 351–377. doi:10.1162/003355301556338. S2CID 10389797.
  28. ^ Epstein, Cynthia Fuchs (1973). "Positive Effects of the Multiple Negative: Explaining the Success of Black Professional Women". American Journal of Sociology. 78 (4): 912–935. doi:10.1086/225410. JSTOR 2776611. S2CID 144493603.
  29. ^ a b c d Fehr, Ernst; Bernhard, Helen; Rockenbach, Bettina (2008). (PDF). Nature. 454 (7208): 1079–1083. Bibcode:2008Natur.454.1079F. doi:10.1038/nature07155. PMID 18756249. S2CID 4430780. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2020-02-12. Retrieved 2019-02-05.
  30. ^ Bilali, Rezarta (2013). "National Narrative and Social Psychological Influences in Turks' Denial of the Mass Killings of Armenians as Genocide: Understanding Denial". Journal of Social Issues. 69 (1): 16–33. doi:10.1111/josi.12001. Interestingly, in-group glorification and perceived threat differentially predicted Turks' construals of harm inflicted during the mass violence. In-group glorification minimized perceived in-group perpetration (i.e., predicted less perceived harm inflicted by Turks), whereas in-group threat elevated the perceived in-group victimization. These results are in line with the idea that in-group glorification is driven by identity-enhancing motives (i.e., perceiving the in-group in a positive light via minimizing in-group perpetration).
  31. ^ a b c Rand, David G.; Pfeiffer, Thomas; Dreber, Anna; Sheketoff, Rachel W.; Wernerfelt, Nils C.; Benkler, Yochai (2009-04-14). "Dynamic remodeling of in-group bias during the 2008 presidential election". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (15): 6187–6191. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.6187R. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811552106. PMC 2664153. PMID 19332775.
  32. ^ a b Oeberst, Aileen; Beck, Ina; Matschke, Christina; Ihme, Toni Alexander; Cress, Ulrike (December 2019). "Collectively biased representations of the past: Ingroup Bias in Wikipedia articles about intergroup conflicts". British Journal of Social Psychology. 59 (4): 791–818. doi:10.1111/bjso.12356. ISSN 0144-6665. PMID 31788823.
  33. ^ Bourhis, Richard Y.; Gagnon, André (2003). "Social Orientations in the Minimal Group Paradigm". In Brown, R.; Gaertner, S. L. (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers Ltd. pp. 89–111. doi:10.1002/9780470693421.ch5. ISBN 978-0-470-69342-1.
  34. ^ Mummendey, Amélie; Otten, Sabine (2003). "Aversive Discrimination". In Brown, R.; Gaertner, S. L. (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers Ltd. pp. 112–132. doi:10.1002/9780470693421.ch6. ISBN 978-0-470-69342-1.
  35. ^ Turner, John C.; Reynolds, Katherine J. (2003). "The Social Identity Perspective in Intergroup Relations: Theories, Themes, and Controversies". In Brown, R.; Gaertner, S. L. (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers Ltd. pp. 133–152. doi:10.1002/9780470693421.ch7. ISBN 978-0-470-69342-1.
  36. ^ Struch, Naomi; Schwartz, Shalom H. (March 1989). "Intergroup Aggression: Its Predictors and Distinctness From In-Group Bias". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56 (3): 364–373. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.3.364. PMID 2926634. S2CID 7328410.
  37. ^ West, Jeremy (February 2018). "Racial Bias in Police Investigations" (PDF). Working Paper.
  38. ^ Depew, Briggs; Eren, Ozkan; Mocan, Naci (2017). "Judges, Juveniles, and In-Group Bias" (PDF). Journal of Law and Economics. 60 (2): 209–239. doi:10.1086/693822. S2CID 147631237.
  39. ^ De Dreu CK, Shalvi S, Greer LL, Van Kleef GA, Handgraaf MJ (2012). "Oxytocin motivates non-cooperation in intergroup conflict to protect vulnerable in-group members". PLOS ONE. 7 (11): e46751. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...746751D. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0046751. PMC 3492361. PMID 23144787.
  40. ^ Stallen M, De Dreu CK, Shalvi S, Smidts A, Sanfey AG (2012). "The herding hormone: oxytocin stimulates in-group conformity". Psychological Science. 23 (11): 1288–92. doi:10.1177/0956797612446026. PMID 22991128. S2CID 16255677.
  41. ^ De Dreu CK, Greer LL, Van Kleef GA, Shalvi S, Handgraaf MJ (January 2011). "Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (4): 1262–6. Bibcode:2011PNAS..108.1262D. doi:10.1073/pnas.1015316108. PMC 3029708. PMID 21220339.
  42. ^ Ma X, Luo L, Geng Y, Zhao W, Zhang Q, Kendrick KM (2014). "Oxytocin increases liking for a country's people and national flag but not for other cultural symbols or consumer products". Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 8: 266. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00266. PMC 4122242. PMID 25140135.
  43. ^ a b c d Ma-Kellams, Christine; Spencer-Rodgers, Julie; Peng, Kaiping (2011). "I Am Against Us? Unpacking Cultural Differences in Ingroup Favoritism via Dialecticism". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 37 (1): 15–27. doi:10.1177/0146167210388193. PMID 21084525. S2CID 18906927.


group, favoritism, this, article, about, favoring, group, behavior, cognition, strategic, sense, other, phenomena, related, psychological, group, formation, ingroups, outgroups, this, article, needs, additional, citations, verification, please, help, improve, . This article is about favoring of one s own group s behavior and cognition in a strategic sense For other phenomena related to psychological group formation see Ingroups and outgroups This article needs additional citations for verification Please help improve this article by adding citations to reliable sources Unsourced material may be challenged and removed Find sources In group favoritism news newspapers books scholar JSTOR March 2023 Learn how and when to remove this template message In group favoritism sometimes known as in group out group bias in group bias intergroup bias or in group preference is a pattern of favoring members of one s in group over out group members This can be expressed in evaluation of others in allocation of resources and in many other ways 1 2 This effect has been researched by many psychologists and linked to many theories related to group conflict and prejudice The phenomenon is primarily viewed from a social psychology standpoint Studies have shown that in group favoritism arises as a result of the formation of cultural groups 3 4 These cultural groups can be divided based on seemingly trivial observable traits but with time populations grow to associate certain traits with certain behavior increasing covariation This then incentivizes in group bias Two prominent theoretical approaches to the phenomenon of in group favoritism are realistic conflict theory and social identity theory Realistic conflict theory proposes that intergroup competition and sometimes intergroup conflict arises when two groups have opposing claims to scarce resources In contrast social identity theory posits a psychological drive for positively distinct social identities as the general root cause of in group favoring behavior Contents 1 Origins of the research tradition 2 Explanations 2 1 Competition 2 2 Self esteem 2 3 Biological basis as an effect of oxytocin 2 4 Self identity and social identity 3 Evolution of in groups 3 1 Formation of cultural groups 4 Gender differences 4 1 Automatic bias for own gender 4 2 Competition 4 3 Ethnicity based favoritism 4 4 Developmental age 5 Real world examples 5 1 Armenian Genocide denial 5 2 2008 US Presidential elections 5 3 Wikipedia 6 Versus out group negativity 6 1 Biological relationship 7 In group derogation 8 See also 9 ReferencesOrigins of the research tradition EditIn 1906 the sociologist William Sumner posited that humans are a species that join together in groups by their very nature However he also maintained that humans had an innate tendency to favor their own group over others proclaiming how each group nourishes its own pride and vanity boasts itself superior exists in its own divinities and looks with contempt on outsiders 5 This is seen on the group level with ingroup outgroup bias When experienced in larger groups such as tribes ethnic groups or nations it is referred to as ethnocentrism Explanations EditCompetition Edit Realistic conflict theory or realistic group conflict posits that competition between groups for resources is the cause of in group bias and the corresponding negative treatment of members of the out group Muzafer Sherif s Robbers Cave Experiment is the most widely known demonstration of realistic conflict theory In the experiment 22 eleven year old boys with similar backgrounds were studied in a mock summer camp situation with researchers posing as camp personnel The boys were divided into two equal groups and encouraged to bond with the aim of fostering an in group mentality The researchers then introduced a series of competitive activities which pitted groups against each other for a valuable prize Hostility and out group negativity ensued 6 Lastly researchers attempted to reverse the hostility by engaging the boys in situations of mutual interdependence an effort which eventually resulted in relative harmony between the two groups Sherif concluded from this experiment that negative attitudes toward out groups arise when groups compete for limited resources 6 However he also theorized that inter group frictions could be reduced and positive relations created 6 but only in the presence of an overarching goal which could only be achieved with the two groups cooperation 6 1 Self esteem Edit According to social identity theory one of the key determinants of group biases is the need to improve self esteem The desire to view one s self positively is transferred onto the group creating a tendency to view one s own group in a positive light and by comparison outside groups in a negative light 7 That is individuals will find a reason no matter how insignificant to prove to themselves why their own group is superior This phenomenon was pioneered and studied most extensively by Henri Tajfel a British social psychologist who looked at the psychological root of in group out group bias To study this in the lab Tajfel and colleagues created minimal groups see minimal group paradigm which occur when complete strangers are formed into groups using the most trivial criteria imaginable In Tajfel s studies participants were split into groups by flipping a coin and each group then was told to appreciate a certain style of painting none of the participants were familiar with when the experiment began What Tajfel and his colleagues discovered was that regardless of the facts that a participants did not know each other b their groups were completely meaningless and c none of the participants had any inclination as to which style they like better participants almost always liked the members of their own group better and they rated the members of their in group as more likely to have pleasant personalities By having a more positive impression of individuals in the in group individuals are able to boost their own self esteem as members of that group 1 Robert Cialdini and his research team looked at the number of university T shirts being worn on college campuses following either a win or loss at the football game They found that the Monday after a win there were more T shirts being worn on average than following a loss 1 8 In another set of studies done in the 1980s by Jennifer Crocker and colleagues using the minimal group paradigm individuals with high self esteem who suffered a threat to the self concept exhibited greater ingroup biases than did people with low self esteem who suffered a threat to the self concept 9 While some studies have supported this notion of a negative correlation between self esteem and in group bias 10 other researchers have found that individuals with low self esteem showed more bias toward both in group and out group members 9 Some studies have even shown that high self esteem groups showed more bias than did lower self esteem groups 11 This research may suggest that there is an alternative explanation and additional reasoning as to the relationship between self esteem and in group out group biases Alternatively it is possible that researchers have used the wrong sort of self esteem measures to test the link between self esteem and in group bias global personal self esteem rather than specific social self esteem 12 Biological basis as an effect of oxytocin Edit Main article Oxytocin In a meta analysis and review of the effect of oxytocin on social behavior done by Carsten De Dreu the research reviewed shows that oxytocin enables the development of trust specifically towards individuals with similar characteristics categorized as in group members promoting cooperation with and favoritism towards such individuals 13 This bias of oxytocin induced goodwill towards those with features and characteristics perceived to be similar may have evolved as a biological basis for sustaining in group cooperation and protection fitting with the Darwinian insight that acts of self sacrifice and cooperation contribute to the functioning of the group and hence improve the odds of survival for members of said group 13 Race can be used as an example of in group and out group tendencies because society often categorizes individuals into groups based on race Caucasian African American Latino etc One study that examined race and empathy found that participants receiving nasally administered oxytocin had stronger reactions to pictures of in group members making pained faces than to pictures of out group members with the same expression 14 This shows that oxytocin may be implicated in our ability to empathize with individuals of different races with individuals of one race potentially biased towards helping individuals of the same race than individuals of another race when they are experiencing pain Oxytocin has also been implicated in lying when lying would prove beneficial to other in group members In a study where such a relationship was examined it was found that when individuals were administered oxytocin rates of dishonesty in the participants responses increased for their in group members when a beneficial outcome for their group was expected 15 Both of these examples show the tendency to act in ways that benefit in group members Self identity and social identity Edit Main article Social identity theory As noted in two recent theoretical reviews 16 the theoretical basis for the inclusion of self identity in the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior has many similarities to social identity theory 17 and its extension self categorization theory 18 According to social identity theory an important component of the self concept is derived from memberships in social groups and categories When people define and evaluate themselves in terms of a self inclusive social category e g sex class team two processes come into play 1 categorization which perceptually accentuates differences between the in group and out group and similarities among in group members including the self on stereotypical dimensions and 2 self enhancement which because the self concept is defined in terms of group membership seeks behaviorally and perceptually to favor the in group over the out group Social identities are cognitively represented as group prototypes that describe and prescribe beliefs attitudes feelings and behaviors that optimize a balance between minimization of in group differences and maximization of intergroup differences More specifically according to social identity theory there is a continuum between personal and social identity shifts along this continuum that determine the extent to which group related or personal characteristics influence a person s feelings and actions 19 If a particular social identity is a salient basis for self conception then the self is assimilated to the perceived in group prototype which can be thought of as a set of perceived in group norms such that self perception beliefs attitudes feelings and behaviors are defined in terms of the group prototype Thus social identities should influence behavior through the mediating role of group norms People will be more likely to engage in a particular behavior if it is in accord with the norms of a behaviorally relevant group membership particularly if the identity is a salient basis for self definition If the group membership is not salient then people s behavior and feelings should be in accord with their own personal and idiosyncratic characteristics rather than group norms On the other hand the self identity theory poses that the self is often a reflection of expected norms and practices in a person s social role At the center of it is the proposition that the self is made up of multi faceted and differentiated components that exist in an organized manner for the sake of filling in roles in society 20 People are able to create an identity for themselves only through talking to others and often what roles they are taking on differ from one group to another These differing roles and positions people fill are a result of their interactions with others and are called role identities Role identities may be self realized or may be facts like being a mother a social worker or a blood donor Role identities lead people to act in certain ways due to assumed expectations for the roles Because there is satisfaction in complying with expectations of the role there is often distress behind an inability to appear congruent to one s identity as defined by societal norms There is also an existing hierarchy of importance for roles that individuals take on and according to the hierarchical standing of roles people become more representative of roles that stand higher hierarchically according to them Identity salience the likelihood of role identities being invoked in different situations is the result of role identities being placed hierarchically in different orders from person to person People who hold the same roles may act differently because some roles are valued over others 21 For example a working mother may have less time to spend with her child as opposed to a mother that does not work Behaviors are reflective of the identities that are held higher hierarchically by people so people act out in self worth and self meaning according to these hierarchies 22 Someone who holds the identity of being a psychologist higher than the identity of being a linguist will find that while he she may become competitive when meeting another person that is better at psychology than he she he she won t care when in contact with someone who is much better at being a linguist than he she In a similar way social relationships are influenced by this salience Self identity often places individuals in social contexts and a commitment to the role within that context becomes a big part of perpetrating the idea of self It also finds people relating more to others that hold similar role identities at the top of their hierarchies Because people have self concepts that are derived from a role they define for themselves within the context of a group when staying within their roles intergroup similarities are accentuated while intergroup differences are diminished 23 In an attempt to assimilate oneself according to the tendencies of a group often people reconfigure their intragroup representations or identities Certain prototypes form about these groups that reaffirm rules that members of the group are encouraged to follow Shared information and views are discussed more often than novel and unshared information within a group therefore a norm is established where the majority views are perpetuated and others silenced 24 This norm is fluid and changing according to different contexts but those within the group who want to keep up with the majority views in all matters have to keep an active role in affirming the views of the in group in contest to out groups Evolution of in groups EditFormation of cultural groups Edit Studies have shown that in group favoritism arises endogenously through the formation of cultural groups 3 Symbolic markers in certain conditions can result in trivial groupings developing into cultural groups The formation of such cultural groups then results in a higher degree of in group favoritism Efferson Lalive and Fehr published such a study in 2008 utilizing a series of coordination games to mimic cooperation between individuals The study found that cultural groups were able to form endogenously through creation of a linkage between a payoff relevant behavior and a payoff irrelevant marker Subsequently in group favoritism occurred in ensuing social interactions 3 Participants were first divided into one of several populations of 10 people and then further divided into subpopulations of 5 Each group had different payoff for coordinating on one of 2 choices behavior A or behavior B In group 1 participants were awarded 41 points for coordinating choosing A themselves and choosing another participant who also chose A on A and 21 for coordinating on B The payoffs were switched in the 2nd group In both groups participants were awarded just 1 point for mis coordinating During each turn participants were also allowed to choose a payoff irrelevant marker circle or triangle Players from both subpopulations were mixed to create a coordination problem and every turn an unidentified player from each subpopulation would be randomly switched The experiment created a situation in which participants were strongly incentivized to develop a sense of expected behaviors in his or her subpopulation but occasionally would find themselves in a totally new situation in which their behaviors were not in line with social norms 3 The results showed that players generally developed an inclination to pair behavior with a marker especially if it had resulted in a positive payoff As linkages at an individual level increase covariation of marker and behavior at an aggregate level also increases In the experiment there was a significant increase in participants requesting for partners with the same shape choice as it progressed although the initial choice of shape had no effect on payoffs Toward the end of the experiment this number stood at a substantial 87 indicating the presence of in group favoritism Their study supported the hypothesis that the formation of cultural groups alters selective pressure facing individuals and thus leads to certain behavioral traits being advantageous 4 3 Thus if such selective pressures were present in past civilizations where membership in a certain group is correlated with a certain behavioral norm the emergence of in group biases where it is beneficial to act in differing manners to members of the same group is certainly plausible 3 Gender differences EditAutomatic bias for own gender Edit Rudman and Goodwin conducted research on gender bias that measured gender preferences without directly asking the participants Subjects at Purdue and Rutgers University participated in computerized tasks that measured automatic attitudes based on how quickly a person categorizes pleasant and unpleasant attributes with each gender Such a task was done to discover whether people associate pleasant words good happy and sunshine with women and unpleasant words bad trouble and pain with men 25 This research found that while both women and men have more favorable views of women women s in group biases were 4 5 times stronger 25 than those of men and only women not men showed cognitive balance among in group bias identity and self esteem revealing that men lack a mechanism that bolsters automatic preference for their own gender 25 Competition Edit Using a publics goods game Van Vugt De Cremer and Janssen found that men contributed more to their group in the face of outside competition from another group there was no distinct difference amongst women s contributions 26 Ethnicity based favoritism Edit In 2001 Fershtman and Gneezy found that men showed in group biases in a trust game based on ethnicity whereas this tendency was not present in women 27 The study aims to identify ethnic discrimination in Israeli Jewish society and was conducted on 996 Israeli undergraduates Groups were separated based on whether the participant s name was typically ethnically Eastern or Ashkenazic Similar to a dictator game subjects were instructed to divide a sum of money 20 NIS between themselves and another player Player A was told that any money sent over to Player B would be tripled and Player B would receive details of the experiment including the name of Player A and the transferred sum Subsequently Player B would have a choice of whether to send any money back The experiment found that despite sharing similar average transfer values 10 63 for women and 11 42 for men women did not display significant in group biases when it came to recipients with either Ashkenazic or Eastern sounding names However a bias against Eastern sounding names was present amongst men 27 Furthermore men showed more bias for Ashkenazic men compared to women but the opposite was true for Eastern names 27 This result may seem counter intuitive as participants appear to share more in common if they were both male Thus we would expect Eastern females to be more marginalized but is actually consistent with other studies which studied discrimination against Afro American women 28 Developmental age Edit In 2008 Fehr Bernhard and Rockenbach in a study conducted on children found that boys displayed in group favoritism from ages 3 8 whereas girls did not display such tendencies 29 The experiment involved usage of an envy game a modified version of the dictator game A possible explanation posited by researchers relied on an evolutionary basis 29 They theorized that parochialism and favoring members of the same group may have been particularly advantageous as it strengthened the individuals group position in intergroup conflicts 29 As males were the ones who were frequently at the forefront of such conflicts in the past and thus bore the majority of the costs of conflicts in terms of injury or death evolution may have favored a greater sensitivity in males in situations which resulted in an advantageous payoff for their in group Thus males tended to show in group biases from a younger age than females as was evident in the experiment 29 Real world examples EditArmenian Genocide denial Edit See also Armenian genocide denial A 2013 study found that Turks with a stronger in group bias were less likely to acknowledge the in group responsibility for the Armenian genocide 30 2008 US Presidential elections Edit A study conducted during the 2008 Presidential elections showcased how group identities were dynamic 31 The study was carried out among 395 Democrats from Cambridge MA using an Economics dictator game Subjects were given 6 to divide between themselves and another person The recipients remained anonymous apart from which candidate they supported in the Democratic Primaries Data were collected in three separate periods June 10 to 18 after Hillary Clinton s concession speech on June 7 August 9 to 14 before the Democratic National Convention on the 25 and September 2 to 5 in the buildup to the Presidential elections The results showed that men displayed significant in group favoritism from June all the way to the DNC in August This in group bias however was not present in September Women displayed no significant in group favoritism throughout The experiment suggested that group identities are flexible and can change over time 31 Researchers theorized that in group bias was strong in June as the competition to be the Democratic nominee in the elections was still recent and thus salient A lack of actual electoral conflict against the Republicans caused perception of salient groupings to remain throughout August 31 Only in September did the in group favoritism subside as a superordinate goal shared between groups was now present Wikipedia Edit Research analyzing articles about 35 inter group conflicts e g Falklands War by comparing the corresponding language versions of Wikipedia e g English Spanish found evidence for in group favoritism While the in group was systematically preferred and presented in a more favorable light the out group was presented as more immoral and more responsible for the conflict 32 There were substantial variations between conflicts however and additional analyses revealed in group favoritism to be more pronounced in more recent conflicts and in articles written predominantly by in group members 32 Versus out group negativity EditMain article Group conflict Social psychologists have long made the distinction between ingroup favoritism and outgroup negativity where outgroup negativity is the act of punishing or placing burdens upon the outgroup 19 Indeed a significant body of research exists that attempts to identify the relationship between ingroup favoritism and outgroup negativity as well as conditions that will lead to outgroup negativity 33 34 35 For example Struch and Schwartz found support for the predictions of belief congruence theory 36 The belief congruence theory concerns itself with the degree of similarity in beliefs attitudes and values perceived to exist between individuals This theory also states that dissimilarity increases negative orientations towards others When applied to racial discrimination the belief congruence theory argues that the perceived dissimilarity of beliefs has more of an impact on racial discrimination than does race itself Research finds evidence of in group bias in police investigations 37 and judicial decisions 38 Biological relationship Edit Oxytocin is not only correlated with the preferences of individuals to associate with members of their own group but it is also evident during conflicts between members of different groups During conflict individuals receiving nasally administered oxytocin demonstrate more frequent defense motivated responses toward in group members than out group members Further oxytocin was correlated with participant desire to protect vulnerable in group members despite that individual s attachment to the conflict 39 Similarly it has been demonstrated that when oxytocin is administered individuals alter their subjective preferences in order to align with in group ideals over out group ideals 40 These studies demonstrate that oxytocin is associated with intergroup dynamics Further oxytocin influences the responses of individuals in a particular group to those of another group The in group bias is evident in smaller groups however it can also be extended to groups as large as one s entire country leading toward a tendency of strong national zeal A study done in the Netherlands showed that oxytocin increased the in group favoritism of their nation while decreasing acceptance of members of other ethnicities and foreigners 41 People also show more affection for their country s flag while remaining indifferent to other cultural objects when exposed to oxytocin 42 It has thus been hypothesized that this hormone may be a factor in xenophobic tendencies secondary to this effect Thus oxytocin appears to affect individuals at an international level where the in group becomes a specific home country and the out group grows to include all other countries In group derogation EditCross cultural studies have found that in group derogation the tendency to criticize members of one s own group or culture more harshly than members of outside groups is more common among members of disadvantaged and minority groups than among members of the majority or dominant group According to Ma Kellams Spencer Rodgers and Peng system justification theory seeks to explain why minorities sometimes endorse system justifying views of their group They said their research into in group favoritism and derogation partially supported this theory but that the theory failed to address all of the nuances 43 Ma Kellams et al also found that compared to individualist cultures people from collectivist cultures such as East Asian cultures tended to judge their own group members less favorably than they judged outsiders whereas people from individualist cultures were inclined to judge members of their own group more favorably than they judged outsiders 43 Social identity theory citation needed and Freudian theorists explain in group derogation as the result of a negative self image which they believe is then extended to the group 43 Ma Kellams et al theorized that ingroup derogation may be more culturally normative and less troubling for East Asians as evidenced by the fact that East Asians were also likely to report high levels of positive affect emotion towards members of their in group demonstrating ambivalence towards the unfavorable characteristics they had acknowledged about their in group According to Ma Kellam et al culturally ingrained attitudes and beliefs rather than low self esteem may play a role in collectivist cultures in group derogation due to their ability to tolerate holding seemingly contradictory views 43 See also Edit nbsp Psychology portalCollective narcissism Common ingroup identity Cronyism Groupthink Linguistic intergroup bias Marginalization Marking your own homework Nepotism Old boy network Out group homogeneity Priming Psychological projection Protectionism Scapegoating Social projection Terror management theory XenophobiaReferences Edit a b c d Aronson E Wilson T D amp Akert R 2010 Social psychology 7th ed Upper Saddle River Prentice Hall Taylor Donald M Doria Janet R April 1981 Self serving and group serving bias in attribution Journal of Social Psychology 113 2 201 211 doi 10 1080 00224545 1981 9924371 a b c d e f Efferson Charles Lalive Rafael Fehr Ernst 2008 09 26 The Coevolution of Cultural Groups and Ingroup Favoritism PDF Science 321 5897 1844 1849 Bibcode 2008Sci 321 1844E doi 10 1126 science 1155805 PMID 18818361 S2CID 32927015 a b Fu Feng Tarnita Corina E Christakis Nicholas A Wang Long Rand David G Nowak Martin A 2012 06 21 Evolution of in group favoritism Scientific Reports 2 460 Bibcode 2012NatSR 2E 460F doi 10 1038 srep00460 PMC 3380441 PMID 22724059 Sumner William Graham 1840 1910 23 September 2014 Folkways a study of the sociological importance of usages manners customs mores and morals Reprint 2014 ed p 13 ISBN 978 1 5024 6917 5 OCLC 1003045387 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b c d Sherif M Harvey O J White B J Hood W amp Sherif C W 1961 Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation The Robbers Cave Experiment Norman OK The University Book Exchange pp 155 184 Billig Michael Tajfel Henri 1973 Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behaviour European Journal of Social Psychology 3 1 27 52 doi 10 1002 ejsp 2420030103 Cialdini Robert B Borden Richard J Thorne Avril Walker Marcus Randall Freeman Stephen Sloan Lloyd Reynolds 1974 Basking in Reflected Glory Three Football Field Studies Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 34 3 366 375 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 34 3 366 S2CID 13191936 a b Crocker Jennifer Thompson Leigh L McGraw Kathleen M Ingerman Cindy May 1987 Downward Comparison Prejudice and Evaluations of Others Effects of Self Esteem and Threat Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 52 5 907 916 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 52 5 907 PMID 3585702 Abrams Dominic Hogg Michael A 1988 Comments on the motivational status of self esteem in social identity and intergroup discrimination European Journal of Social Psychology 18 4 317 334 doi 10 1002 ejsp 2420180403 Sachdev Itesh Bourhis Richard Y September 1987 Status differentials and intergroup behaviour European Journal of Social Psychology 17 3 277 293 doi 10 1002 ejsp 2420170304 Rubin Mark Hewstone Miles February 1998 Social Identity Theory s Self Esteem Hypothesis A Review and Some Suggestions for Clarification Personality and Social Psychology Review 2 1 40 62 doi 10 1207 s15327957pspr0201 3 hdl 1959 13 930907 PMID 15647150 S2CID 40695727 a b De Dreu Carsten K W 2012 Oxytocin modulates cooperation within and competition between groups An integrative review and research agenda Hormones and Behavior 61 3 419 428 doi 10 1016 j yhbeh 2011 12 009 PMID 22227278 S2CID 140120921 Sheng F Liu Y Zhou B Zhou W Han S February 2013 Oxytocin modulates the racial bias in neural responses to others suffering Biological Psychology 92 2 380 6 doi 10 1016 j biopsycho 2012 11 018 PMID 23246533 S2CID 206109148 Shalvi S De Dreu CK April 2014 Oxytocin promotes group serving dishonesty Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111 15 5503 7 Bibcode 2014PNAS 111 5503S doi 10 1073 pnas 1400724111 PMC 3992689 PMID 24706799 Hogg Michael A Terry Deborah J White Katherine M 1995 A Tale of Two Theories A Critical Comparison of Identity Theory with Social Identity Theory Social Psychology Quarterly 58 4 255 269 doi 10 2307 2787127 JSTOR 2787127 Tajfel Henri 1974 04 01 Social identity and intergroup behaviour Social Science Information 13 2 65 93 doi 10 1177 053901847401300204 S2CID 143666442 Turner John C 1985 Social categorization and the self concept A social cognitive theory of group behavior In Lawler E J ed Advances in Group Processes Theory and Research Vol 2 Greenwich CT JAI pp 77 122 ISBN 978 0 89232 524 5 OCLC 925165493 a b Tajfel Henri Turner John C 1979 An integrative theory of intergroup conflict In Austin W G Worchel S eds The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations Brooks Cole pp 33 47 ISBN 978 0 8185 0278 1 Stryker Sheldon Serpe Richard T 1982 Commitment Identity Salience and Role Behavior Theory and Research Example In Ickes William Knowles Eric S eds Personality Roles and Social Behavior Springer New York pp 199 218 doi 10 1007 978 1 4613 9469 3 7 ISBN 978 1 4613 9471 6 Nuttbrock Larry Freudiger Patricia 1991 Identity Salience and Motherhood A Test of Stryker s Theory Social Psychology Quarterly 54 2 146 157 doi 10 2307 2786932 JSTOR 2786932 Callero Peter L 1985 Role Identity Salience Social Psychology Quarterly 48 3 203 215 doi 10 2307 3033681 JSTOR 3033681 Tajfel H 1959 Quantitative Judgement in Social Perception British Journal of Psychology 50 1 16 29 doi 10 1111 j 2044 8295 1959 tb00677 x PMID 13628966 Larson James R Foster Fishman Pennie G Keys Christopher B 1994 Discussion of shared and unshared information in decision making groups Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 3 446 461 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 67 3 446 a b c Rudman Laurie A Goodwin Stephanie A 2004 Gender differences in automatic in group bias Why do women like women more than men like men PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 87 4 494 509 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 87 4 494 PMID 15491274 Archived PDF from the original on July 18 2014 Retrieved September 2 2016 Van Vugt Mark De Cremer David Janssen Dirk P 2007 Gender differences in cooperation and competition the male warrior hypothesis Psychological Science 18 1 19 23 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 2007 01842 x PMID 17362372 S2CID 3566509 a b c Fershtman Chaim Gneezy Uri 2001 Discrimination in a Segmented Society An Experimental Approach The Quarterly Journal of Economics 116 1 351 377 doi 10 1162 003355301556338 S2CID 10389797 Epstein Cynthia Fuchs 1973 Positive Effects of the Multiple Negative Explaining the Success of Black Professional Women American Journal of Sociology 78 4 912 935 doi 10 1086 225410 JSTOR 2776611 S2CID 144493603 a b c d Fehr Ernst Bernhard Helen Rockenbach Bettina 2008 Egalitarianism in young children PDF Nature 454 7208 1079 1083 Bibcode 2008Natur 454 1079F doi 10 1038 nature07155 PMID 18756249 S2CID 4430780 Archived from the original PDF on 2020 02 12 Retrieved 2019 02 05 Bilali Rezarta 2013 National Narrative and Social Psychological Influences in Turks Denial of the Mass Killings of Armenians as Genocide Understanding Denial Journal of Social Issues 69 1 16 33 doi 10 1111 josi 12001 Interestingly in group glorification and perceived threat differentially predicted Turks construals of harm inflicted during the mass violence In group glorification minimized perceived in group perpetration i e predicted less perceived harm inflicted by Turks whereas in group threat elevated the perceived in group victimization These results are in line with the idea that in group glorification is driven by identity enhancing motives i e perceiving the in group in a positive light via minimizing in group perpetration a b c Rand David G Pfeiffer Thomas Dreber Anna Sheketoff Rachel W Wernerfelt Nils C Benkler Yochai 2009 04 14 Dynamic remodeling of in group bias during the 2008 presidential election Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106 15 6187 6191 Bibcode 2009PNAS 106 6187R doi 10 1073 pnas 0811552106 PMC 2664153 PMID 19332775 a b Oeberst Aileen Beck Ina Matschke Christina Ihme Toni Alexander Cress Ulrike December 2019 Collectively biased representations of the past Ingroup Bias in Wikipedia articles about intergroup conflicts British Journal of Social Psychology 59 4 791 818 doi 10 1111 bjso 12356 ISSN 0144 6665 PMID 31788823 Bourhis Richard Y Gagnon Andre 2003 Social Orientations in the Minimal Group Paradigm In Brown R Gaertner S L eds Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology Intergroup Processes Oxford UK Blackwell Publishers Ltd pp 89 111 doi 10 1002 9780470693421 ch5 ISBN 978 0 470 69342 1 Mummendey Amelie Otten Sabine 2003 Aversive Discrimination In Brown R Gaertner S L eds Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology Intergroup Processes Oxford UK Blackwell Publishers Ltd pp 112 132 doi 10 1002 9780470693421 ch6 ISBN 978 0 470 69342 1 Turner John C Reynolds Katherine J 2003 The Social Identity Perspective in Intergroup Relations Theories Themes and Controversies In Brown R Gaertner S L eds Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology Intergroup Processes Oxford UK Blackwell Publishers Ltd pp 133 152 doi 10 1002 9780470693421 ch7 ISBN 978 0 470 69342 1 Struch Naomi Schwartz Shalom H March 1989 Intergroup Aggression Its Predictors and Distinctness From In Group Bias Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 56 3 364 373 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 56 3 364 PMID 2926634 S2CID 7328410 West Jeremy February 2018 Racial Bias in Police Investigations PDF Working Paper Depew Briggs Eren Ozkan Mocan Naci 2017 Judges Juveniles and In Group Bias PDF Journal of Law and Economics 60 2 209 239 doi 10 1086 693822 S2CID 147631237 De Dreu CK Shalvi S Greer LL Van Kleef GA Handgraaf MJ 2012 Oxytocin motivates non cooperation in intergroup conflict to protect vulnerable in group members PLOS ONE 7 11 e46751 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 746751D doi 10 1371 journal pone 0046751 PMC 3492361 PMID 23144787 Stallen M De Dreu CK Shalvi S Smidts A Sanfey AG 2012 The herding hormone oxytocin stimulates in group conformity Psychological Science 23 11 1288 92 doi 10 1177 0956797612446026 PMID 22991128 S2CID 16255677 De Dreu CK Greer LL Van Kleef GA Shalvi S Handgraaf MJ January 2011 Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 108 4 1262 6 Bibcode 2011PNAS 108 1262D doi 10 1073 pnas 1015316108 PMC 3029708 PMID 21220339 Ma X Luo L Geng Y Zhao W Zhang Q Kendrick KM 2014 Oxytocin increases liking for a country s people and national flag but not for other cultural symbols or consumer products Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience 8 266 doi 10 3389 fnbeh 2014 00266 PMC 4122242 PMID 25140135 a b c d Ma Kellams Christine Spencer Rodgers Julie Peng Kaiping 2011 I Am Against Us Unpacking Cultural Differences in Ingroup Favoritism via Dialecticism Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37 1 15 27 doi 10 1177 0146167210388193 PMID 21084525 S2CID 18906927 Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title In group favoritism amp oldid 1173424514, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.