fbpx
Wikipedia

Swarm behaviour

Swarm behaviour, or swarming, is a collective behaviour exhibited by entities, particularly animals, of similar size which aggregate together, perhaps milling about the same spot or perhaps moving en masse or migrating in some direction. It is a highly interdisciplinary topic.[1]

A flock of auklets exhibit swarm behaviour

As a term, swarming is applied particularly to insects, but can also be applied to any other entity or animal that exhibits swarm behaviour. The term flocking or murmuration can refer specifically to swarm behaviour in birds, herding to refer to swarm behaviour in tetrapods, and shoaling or schooling to refer to swarm behaviour in fish. Phytoplankton also gather in huge swarms called blooms, although these organisms are algae and are not self-propelled the way animals are. By extension, the term "swarm" is applied also to inanimate entities which exhibit parallel behaviours, as in a robot swarm, an earthquake swarm, or a swarm of stars.

From a more abstract point of view, swarm behaviour is the collective motion of a large number of self-propelled entities.[2] From the perspective of the mathematical modeller, it is an emergent behaviour arising from simple rules that are followed by individuals and does not involve any central coordination. Swarm behaviour is also studied by active matter physicists as a phenomenon which is not in thermodynamic equilibrium, and as such requires the development of tools beyond those available from the statistical physics of systems in thermodynamic equilibrium. In this regard, swarming has been compared to the mathematics of superfluids, specifically in the context of starling flocks (murmuration).[3]

Swarm behaviour was first simulated on a computer in 1986 with the simulation program boids.[4] This program simulates simple agents (boids) that are allowed to move according to a set of basic rules. The model was originally designed to mimic the flocking behaviour of birds, but it can be applied also to schooling fish and other swarming entities.

Models edit

In recent decades, scientists have turned to modeling swarm behaviour to gain a deeper understanding of the behaviour.

Mathematical models edit

 
In the metric distance model of a fish school (left), the focal fish (yellow) pays attention to all fish within the small zone of repulsion (red), the zone of alignment (lighter red) and the larger zone of attraction (lightest red). In the topological distance model (right), the focal fish only pays attention to the six or seven closest fish (green), regardless of their distance.
External images
  Boids simulation
  iFloys simulation
  Efloys simulation

Early studies of swarm behaviour employed mathematical models to simulate and understand the behaviour. The simplest mathematical models of animal swarms generally represent individual animals as following three rules:

  • Move in the same direction as their neighbours
  • Remain close to their neighbours
  • Avoid collisions with their neighbours

The boids computer program, created by Craig Reynolds in 1986, simulates swarm behaviour following the above rules.[4] Many subsequent and current models use variations on these rules, often implementing them by means of concentric "zones" around each animal. In the "zone of repulsion", very close to the animal, the focal animal will seek to distance itself from its neighbours to avoid collision. Slightly further away, in the "zone of alignment", the focal animal will seek to align its direction of motion with its neighbours. In the outermost "zone of attraction", which extends as far away from the focal animal as it is able to sense, the focal animal will seek to move towards a neighbour.

The shape of these zones will necessarily be affected by the sensory capabilities of a given animal. For example, the visual field of a bird does not extend behind its body. Fish rely on both vision and on hydrodynamic perceptions relayed through their lateral lines, while Antarctic krill rely both on vision and hydrodynamic signals relayed through antennae.

However recent studies of starling flocks have shown that each bird modifies its position, relative to the six or seven animals directly surrounding it, no matter how close or how far away those animals are.[5] Interactions between flocking starlings are thus based on a topological, rather than a metric, rule. It remains to be seen whether this applies to other animals. Another recent study, based on an analysis of high-speed camera footage of flocks above Rome and assuming minimal behavioural rules, has convincingly simulated a number of aspects of flock behaviour.[6][7][8][9]

Evolutionary models edit

In order to gain insight into why animals evolve swarming behaviours, scientists have turned to evolutionary models that simulate populations of evolving animals. Typically these studies use a genetic algorithm to simulate evolution over many generations. These studies have investigated a number of hypotheses attempting to explain why animals evolve swarming behaviours, such as the selfish herd theory[10][11][12][13][14] the predator confusion effect,[15][16] the dilution effect,[17][18] and the many eyes theory.[19]

Agents edit

  • Mach, Robert; Schweitzer, Frank (2003). "Multi-Agent Model of Biological Swarming". Advances In Artificial Life. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Vol. 2801. pp. 810–820. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.87.8022. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-39432-7_87. ISBN 978-3-540-20057-4.

Self-organization edit

 
Flocking birds are an example of self-organization in biology

Emergence edit

The concept of emergence—that the properties and functions found at a hierarchical level are not present and are irrelevant at the lower levels–is often a basic principle behind self-organizing systems.[20] An example of self-organization in biology leading to emergence in the natural world occurs in ant colonies. The queen does not give direct orders and does not tell the ants what to do.[citation needed] Instead, each ant reacts to stimuli in the form of chemical scents from larvae, other ants, intruders, food and buildup of waste, and leaves behind a chemical trail, which, in turn, provides a stimulus to other ants. Here each ant is an autonomous unit that reacts depending only on its local environment and the genetically encoded rules for its variety. Despite the lack of centralized decision making, ant colonies exhibit complex behaviours and have even been able to demonstrate the ability to solve geometric problems. For example, colonies routinely find the maximum distance from all colony entrances to dispose of dead bodies.

Stigmergy edit

A further key concept in the field of swarm intelligence is stigmergy.[21][22] Stigmergy is a mechanism of indirect coordination between agents or actions. The principle is that the trace left in the environment by an action stimulates the performance of a next action, by the same or a different agent. In that way, subsequent actions tend to reinforce and build on each other, leading to the spontaneous emergence of coherent, apparently systematic activity. Stigmergy is a form of self-organization. It produces complex, seemingly intelligent structures, without need for any planning, control, or even direct communication between the agents. As such it supports efficient collaboration between extremely simple agents, who lack any memory, intelligence or even awareness of each other.[22]

Swarm intelligence edit

Swarm intelligence is the collective behaviour of decentralized, self-organized systems, natural or artificial. The concept is employed in work on artificial intelligence. The expression was introduced by Gerardo Beni and Jing Wang in 1989, in the context of cellular robotic systems.[23]

Swarm intelligence systems are typically made up of a population of simple agents such as boids interacting locally with one another and with their environment. The agents follow very simple rules, and although there is no centralized control structure dictating how individual agents should behave, local, and to a certain degree random, interactions between such agents lead to the emergence of intelligent global behaviour, unknown to the individual agents.

Swarm intelligence research is multidisciplinary. It can be divided into natural swarm research studying biological systems and artificial swarm research studying human artefacts. There is also a scientific stream attempting to model the swarm systems themselves and understand their underlying mechanisms, and an engineering stream focused on applying the insights developed by the scientific stream to solve practical problems in other areas.[24]

Algorithms edit

Swarm algorithms follow a Lagrangian approach or an Eulerian approach.[25] The Eulerian approach views the swarm as a field, working with the density of the swarm and deriving mean field properties. It is a hydrodynamic approach, and can be useful for modelling the overall dynamics of large swarms.[26][27][28] However, most models work with the Lagrangian approach, which is an agent-based model following the individual agents (points or particles) that make up the swarm. Individual particle models can follow information on heading and spacing that is lost in the Eulerian approach.[25][29]

Ant colony optimization edit

External image
  Swarmanoid robots find shortest path over double bridge[30]

Ant colony optimization is a widely used algorithm which was inspired by the behaviours of ants, and has been effective solving discrete optimization problems related to swarming.[31] The algorithm was initially proposed by Marco Dorigo in 1992,[32][33] and has since been diversified to solve a wider class of numerical problems. Species that have multiple queens may have a queen leaving the nest along with some workers to found a colony at a new site, a process akin to swarming in honeybees.[34][35]

  • Ants are behaviourally unsophisticated; collectively they perform complex tasks. Ants have highly developed sophisticated sign-based communication.
  • Ants communicate using pheromones; trails are laid that can be followed by other ants.
  • Routing problem ants drop different pheromones used to compute the "shortest" path from source to destination(s).
  • Rauch, EM; Millonas, MM; Chialvo, DR (1995). "Pattern formation and functionality in swarm models". Physics Letters A. 207 (3–4): 185. arXiv:adap-org/9507003. Bibcode:1995PhLA..207..185R. doi:10.1016/0375-9601(95)00624-c. S2CID 120567147.

Self-propelled particles edit

External videos
  SPP model interactive simulation[36]
– needs Java

The concept of self-propelled particles (SPP) was introduced in 1995 by Tamás Vicsek et al.[37] as a special case of the boids model introduced in 1986 by Reynolds.[4] An SPP swarm is modelled by a collection of particles that move with a constant speed and respond to random perturbations by adopting at each time increment the average direction of motion of the other particles in their local neighbourhood.[38]

Simulations demonstrate that a suitable "nearest neighbour rule" eventually results in all the particles swarming together, or moving in the same direction. This emerges, even though there is no centralized coordination, and even though the neighbours for each particle constantly change over time.[37] SPP models predict that swarming animals share certain properties at the group level, regardless of the type of animals in the swarm.[39] Swarming systems give rise to emergent behaviours which occur at many different scales, some of which are both universal and robust. It has become a challenge in theoretical physics to find minimal statistical models that capture these behaviours.[40][41]

Particle swarm optimization edit

Particle swarm optimization is another algorithm widely used to solve problems related to swarms. It was developed in 1995 by Kennedy and Eberhart and was first aimed at simulating the social behaviour and choreography of bird flocks and fish schools.[42][43] The algorithm was simplified and it was observed to be performing optimization. The system initially seeds a population with random solutions. It then searches in the problem space through successive generations using stochastic optimization to find the best solutions. The solutions it finds are called particles. Each particle stores its position as well as the best solution it has achieved so far. The particle swarm optimizer tracks the best local value obtained so far by any particle in the local neighbourhood. The remaining particles then move through the problem space following the lead of the optimum particles. At each time iteration, the particle swarm optimiser accelerates each particle toward its optimum locations according to simple mathematical rules. Particle swarm optimization has been applied in many areas. It has few parameters to adjust, and a version that works well for a specific applications can also work well with minor modifications across a range of related applications.[44] A book by Kennedy and Eberhart describes some philosophical aspects of particle swarm optimization applications and swarm intelligence.[45] An extensive survey of applications is made by Poli.[46][47]

Altruism edit

Researchers in Switzerland have developed an algorithm based on Hamilton's rule of kin selection. The algorithm shows how altruism in a swarm of entities can, over time, evolve and result in more effective swarm behaviour.[48][49]

Biological swarming edit

 
Linear cluster of Ampyx priscus

The earliest evidence of swarm behaviour in animals dates back about 480 million years. Fossils of the trilobite Ampyx priscus have been recently described as clustered in lines along the ocean floor. The animals were all mature adults, and were all facing the same direction as though they had formed a conga line or a peloton. It has been suggested they line up in this manner to migrate, much as spiny lobsters migrate in single-file queues;[50] it has also been suggested that the formation is the precursor for mating,[51] as with the fly Leptoconops torrens. The findings suggest animal collective behaviour has very early evolutionary origins.[52]

Examples of biological swarming are found in bird flocks,[53] fish schools,[54][55] insect swarms,[56] bacteria swarms,[57][58] molds,[59] molecular motors,[60] quadruped herds[61] and people.[62][63][64][65]

Social insects edit

Swarm of nematocera, flying around a treetop

The behaviour of social insects (insects that live in colonies, such as ants, bees, wasps and termites) has always been a source of fascination for children, naturalists and artists. Individual insects seem to do their own thing without any central control, yet the colony as a whole behaves in a highly coordinated manner.[66] Researchers have found that cooperation at the colony level is largely self-organized. The group coordination that emerges is often just a consequence of the way individuals in the colony interact. These interactions can be remarkably simple, such as one ant merely following the trail left by another ant. Yet put together, the cumulative effect of such behaviours can solve highly complex problems, such as locating the shortest route in a network of possible paths to a food source. The organised behaviour that emerges in this way is sometimes called swarm intelligence, a form of biological emergence.[66]

Ants edit

A swarm of weaver ants (Oecophylla smaragdina) transporting a dead gecko

Individual ants do not exhibit complex behaviours, yet a colony of ants collectively achieves complex tasks such as constructing nests, taking care of their young, building bridges and foraging for food. A colony of ants can collectively select (i.e. send most workers towards) the best, or closest, food source from several in the vicinity.[67] Such collective decisions are achieved using positive feedback mechanisms. Selection of the best food source is achieved by ants following two simple rules. First, ants which find food return to the nest depositing a pheromone chemical. More pheromone is laid for higher quality food sources.[68] Thus, if two equidistant food sources of different qualities are found simultaneously, the pheromone trail to the better one will be stronger. Ants in the nest follow another simple rule, to favor stronger trails, on average. More ants then follow the stronger trail, so more ants arrive at the high quality food source, and a positive feedback cycle ensures, resulting in a collective decision for the best food source. If there are two paths from the ant nest to a food source, then the colony usually selects the shorter path. This is because the ants that first return to the nest from the food source are more likely to be those that took the shorter path. More ants then retrace the shorter path, reinforcing the pheromone trail.[69]

Army ants, unlike most ant species, do not construct permanent nests; an army ant colony moves almost incessantly over the time it exists, remaining in an essentially perpetual state of swarming. Several lineages have independently evolved the same basic behavioural and ecological syndrome, often referred to as "legionary behaviour", and may be an example of convergent evolution.[70]

The successful techniques used by ant colonies have been studied in computer science and robotics to produce distributed and fault-tolerant systems for solving problems. This area of biomimetics has led to studies of ant locomotion, search engines that make use of "foraging trails", fault-tolerant storage and networking algorithms.[71]

Honey bees edit

 
Bees swarming on a tree

In temperate climates, honey bees usually form swarms in late spring. A swarm typically contains about half the workers together with the old queen, while the new queen stays back with the remaining workers in the original hive. When honey bees emerge from a hive to form a swarm, they may gather on a branch of a tree or on a bush only a few meters from the hive. The bees cluster about the queen and send out 20–50 scouts to find suitable new nest locations. The scouts are the most experienced foragers in the cluster. If a scout finds a suitable location, she returns to the cluster and promotes it by dancing a version of the waggle dance. This dance conveys information about the quality, direction, and distance of the new site. The more excited she is about her findings, the more vigorously she dances. If she can convince others they may take off and check the site she found. If they approve they may promote it as well. In this decision-making process, scouts check several sites, often abandoning their own original site to promote the superior site of another scout. Several different sites may be promoted by different scouts at first. After some hours and sometimes days, a preferred location eventually emerges from this decision-making process. When all scouts agree on the final location, the whole cluster takes off and swarms to it. Sometimes, if no decision is reached, the swarm will separate, some bees going in one direction; others, going in another. This usually results in failure, with both groups dying. A new location is typically a kilometre or more from the original hive, though some species, e.g., Apis dorsata,[72] may establish new colonies within as little as 500 meters from the natal nest. This collective decision-making process is remarkably successful in identifying the most suitable new nest site and keeping the swarm intact. A good hive site has to be large enough to accommodate the swarm (about 15 litres in volume), has to be well-protected from the elements, receive an optimal amount of sunshine, be some height above the ground, have a small entrance and be capable of resisting ant infestation - that is why tree cavities are often selected.[73][74][75][76][77]

Non-social insects edit

Unlike social insects, swarms of non-social insects that have been studied primarily seem to function in contexts such as mating, feeding, predator avoidance, and migration.

Moths edit

Moths may exhibit synchronized mating, during which pheromones released by females initiate searching and swarming behavior in males.[78] Males sense pheromones with sensitive antennae and may track females as far as several kilometers away.[79] Swarm mating involves female choice and male competition. Only one male in the swarm—typically the first—will successfully copulate.[80] Females maximize fitness benefits and minimize cost by governing the onset and magnitude of pheromone deployed. Too little pheromone will not attract a mate, too much allows less fit males to sense the signal.[81] After copulation, females lay the eggs on a host plant. Quality of host plant may be a factor influencing the location of swarming and egg-laying. In one case, researchers observed pink-striped oakworm moths (Anisota virginiensis) swarming at a carrion site, where decomposition likely increased soil nutrient levels and host plant quality.[82]

Flies edit

Midges, such as Tokunagayusurika akamusi, form swarms, dancing in the air. Swarming serves multiple purposes, including the facilitation of mating by attracting females to approach the swarm, a phenomenon known as lek mating. Such cloud-like swarms often form in early evening when the sun is getting low, at the tip of a bush, on a hilltop, over a pool of water, or even sometimes above a person. The forming of such swarms is not out of instinct, but an adaptive behavior – a "consensus" – between the individuals within the swarms. It is also suggested that swarming is a ritual, because there is rarely any male midge by itself and not in a swarm. This could have formed due to the benefit of lowering inbreeding by having males of various genes gathering in one spot.[83] The genus Culicoides, also known as biting midges, have displayed swarming behavior which are believed to cause confusion in predators.[84]

Cockroaches edit

Cockroaches leave chemical trails in their feces as well as emitting airborne pheromones for mating. Other cockroaches will follow these trails to discover sources of food and water, and also discover where other cockroaches are hiding. Thus, groups of cockroaches can exhibit emergent behaviour,[85] in which group or swarm behaviour emerges from a simple set of individual interactions.

Cockroaches are mainly nocturnal and will run away when exposed to light. A study tested the hypothesis that cockroaches use just two pieces of information to decide where to go under those conditions: how dark it is and how many other cockroaches there are. The study conducted by José Halloy and colleagues at the Free University of Brussels and other European institutions created a set of tiny robots that appear to the roaches as other roaches and can thus alter the roaches' perception of critical mass. The robots were also specially scented so that they would be accepted by the real roaches.[86]

Locusts edit

 
A 19th century depiction of a swarm of desert locusts

Locusts are the swarming phase of the short-horned grasshoppers of the family Acrididae. Some species can breed rapidly under suitable conditions and subsequently become gregarious and migratory. They form bands as nymphs and swarms as adults—both of which can travel great distances, rapidly stripping fields and greatly damaging crops. The largest swarms can cover hundreds of square miles and contain billions of locusts. A locust can eat its own weight (about 2 grams) in plants every day. That means one million locusts can eat more than one tonne of food each day, and the largest swarms can consume over 100,000 tonnes each day.[87]

Swarming in locusts has been found to be associated with increased levels of serotonin which causes the locust to change colour, eat much more, become mutually attracted, and breed much more easily. Researchers propose that swarming behaviour is a response to overcrowding and studies have shown that increased tactile stimulation of the hind legs or, in some species, simply encountering other individuals causes an increase in levels of serotonin. The transformation of the locust to the swarming variety can be induced by several contacts per minute over a four-hour period.[88][89][90][91] Notably, an innate predisposition to aggregate has been found in hatchlings of the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria, independent of their parental phase.[92]

An individual locust's response to a loss of alignment in the group appears to increase the randomness of its motion, until an aligned state is again achieved. This noise-induced alignment appears to be an intrinsic characteristic of collective coherent motion.[93]

Migratory behavior edit

 
Cluster of monarch butterflies. Monarch butterflies migrate to Santa Cruz, California, where they overwinter

Insect migration is the seasonal movement of insects, particularly those by species of dragonflies, beetles, butterflies, and moths. The distance can vary from species to species, but in most cases these movements involve large numbers of individuals. In some cases the individuals that migrate in one direction may not return and the next generation may instead migrate in the opposite direction. This is a significant difference from bird migration.

Monarch butterflies are especially noted for their lengthy annual migration. In North America they make massive southward migrations starting in August until the first frost. A northward migration takes place in the spring. The monarch is the only butterfly that migrates both north and south as the birds do on a regular basis. But no single individual makes the entire round trip. Female monarchs deposit eggs for the next generation during these migrations.[94] The length of these journeys exceeds the normal lifespan of most monarchs, which is less than two months for butterflies born in early summer. The last generation of the summer enters into a non-reproductive phase known as diapause and may live seven months or more.[95] During diapause, butterflies fly to one of many overwintering sites. The generation that overwinters generally does not reproduce until it leaves the overwintering site sometime in February and March. It is the second, third and fourth generations that return to their northern locations in the United States and Canada in the spring. How the species manages to return to the same overwintering spots over a gap of several generations is still a subject of research; the flight patterns appear to be inherited, based on a combination of the position of the sun in the sky[96] and a time-compensated Sun compass that depends upon a circadian clock that is based in their antennae.[97][98]

Birds edit

 
Recent studies of starling flocks have shown that each bird modifies its position, relative to the six or seven animals directly surrounding it, no matter how close or how far away those animals are.[5]

  Murmurations of starlings

[99]

Bird migration edit

 
Large bird typically migrate in V echelon formations. There are significant aerodynamic gains. All birds can see ahead, and towards one side, making a good arrangement for protection.

Approximately 1800 of the world's 10,000 bird species are long-distance migrants.[100] The primary motivation for migration appears to be food; for example, some hummingbirds choose not to migrate if fed through the winter. Also, the longer days of the northern summer provide extended time for breeding birds to feed their young. This helps diurnal birds to produce larger clutches than related non-migratory species that remain in the tropics. As the days shorten in autumn, the birds return to warmer regions where the available food supply varies little with the season. These advantages offset the high stress, physical exertion costs, and other risks of the migration such as predation.

Many birds migrate in flocks. For larger birds, it is assumed that flying in flocks reduces energy costs. The V formation is often supposed to boost the efficiency and range of flying birds, particularly over long migratory routes. All the birds except the first fly in the upwash from one of the wingtip vortices of the bird ahead. The upwash assists each bird in supporting its own weight in flight, in the same way a glider can climb or maintain height indefinitely in rising air. Geese flying in a V formation save energy by flying in the updraft of the wingtip vortex generated by the previous animal in the formation. Thus, the birds flying behind do not need to work as hard to achieve lift. Studies show that birds in a V formation place themselves roughly at the optimum distance predicted by simple aerodynamic theory.[101] Geese in a V-formation may conserve 12–20% of the energy they would need to fly alone.[102][103] Red knots and dunlins were found in radar studies to fly 5 km per hour faster in flocks than when they were flying alone.[104] The birds flying at the tips and at the front are rotated in a timely cyclical fashion to spread flight fatigue equally among the flock members. The formation also makes communication easier and allows the birds to maintain visual contact with each other.

Common starlings
External videos
  Lobster Migration scene
– from The Trials of Life

Other animals may use similar drafting techniques when migrating. Lobsters, for example, migrate in close single-file formation "lobster trains", sometimes for hundreds of miles.

The Mediterranean and other seas present a major obstacle to soaring birds, which must cross at the narrowest points. Massive numbers of large raptors and storks pass through areas such as Gibraltar, Falsterbo, and the Bosphorus at migration times. More common species, such as the European honey buzzard, can be counted in hundreds of thousands in autumn. Other barriers, such as mountain ranges, can also cause funnelling, particularly of large diurnal migrants. This is a notable factor in the Central American migratory bottleneck. This concentration of birds during migration can put species at risk. Some spectacular migrants have already gone extinct, the most notable being the passenger pigeon. During migration the flocks were a mile (1.6 km) wide and 300 miles (500 km) long, taking several days to pass and containing up to a billion birds.

Marine life edit

Fish edit

 
Schooling predator fish size up schooling anchovies
External image
  Foraging efficiency[105]

The term "shoal" can be used to describe any group of fish, including mixed-species groups, while "school" is used for more closely knit groups of the same species swimming in a highly synchronised and polarised manner.

Fish derive many benefits from shoaling behaviour including defence against predators (through better predator detection and by diluting the chance of capture), enhanced foraging success, and higher success in finding a mate.[106] It is also likely that fish benefit from shoal membership through increased hydrodynamic efficiency.[107]

Fish use many traits to choose shoalmates. Generally they prefer larger shoals, shoalmates of their own species, shoalmates similar in size and appearance to themselves, healthy fish, and kin (when recognised). The "oddity effect" posits that any shoal member that stands out in appearance will be preferentially targeted by predators. This may explain why fish prefer to shoal with individuals that resemble them. The oddity effect would thus tend to homogenise shoals.[108]

One puzzling aspect of shoal selection is how a fish can choose to join a shoal of animals similar to themselves, given that it cannot know its own appearance. Experiments with zebrafish have shown that shoal preference is a learned ability, not innate. A zebrafish tends to associate with shoals that resemble shoals in which it was reared, a form of imprinting.[109]

Other open questions of shoaling behaviour include identifying which individuals are responsible for the direction of shoal movement. In the case of migratory movement, most members of a shoal seem to know where they are going. In the case of foraging behaviour, captive shoals of golden shiner (a kind of minnow) are led by a small number of experienced individuals who knew when and where food was available.[110]

Radakov estimated herring schools in the North Atlantic can occupy up to 4.8 cubic kilometres (1.2 cu mi) with fish densities between 0.5 and 1.0 fish/cubic metre, totalling several billion fish in one school.[111]

  • Partridge BL (1982) Scientific American, June:114–123.
  • Parrish JK, Viscido SV, Grunbaum D (2002). "Self-Organized Fish Schools: An Examination of Emergent Properties" (PDF). Biol. Bull. 202 (3): 296–305. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.116.1548. doi:10.2307/1543482. JSTOR 1543482. PMID 12087003. S2CID 377484.

Fish migration edit

External image
  Video clip of the "Sardine run"[112]

Between May and July huge numbers of sardines spawn in the cool waters of the Agulhas Bank and then follow a current of cold water northward along the east coast of South Africa. This great migration, called the sardine run, creates spectacular feeding frenzies along the coastline as marine predators, such as dolphins, sharks and gannets attack the schools.

Krill edit

 
Swarming krill

Most krill, small shrimp-like crustaceans, form large swarms, sometimes reaching densities of 10,000–60,000 individual animals per cubic metre.[113][114][115] Swarming is a defensive mechanism, confusing smaller predators that would like to pick out single individuals. The largest swarms are visible from space and can be tracked by satellite.[116] One swarm was observed to cover an area of 450 square kilometres (175 square miles) of ocean, to a depth of 200 meters (650 feet) and was estimated to contain over 2 million tons of krill.[117] Recent research suggests that krill do not simply drift passively in these currents but actually modify them.[117] Krill typically follow a diurnal vertical migration. By moving vertically through the ocean on a 12-hour cycle, the swarms play a major part in mixing deeper, nutrient-rich water with nutrient-poor water at the surface.[117] Until recently it has been assumed that they spend the day at greater depths and rise during the night toward the surface. It has been found that the deeper they go, the more they reduce their activity,[118] apparently to reduce encounters with predators and to conserve energy.

Later work suggested that swimming activity in krill varied with stomach fullness. Satiated animals that had been feeding at the surface swim less actively and therefore sink below the mixed layer.[119] As they sink they produce faeces which may mean that they have an important role to play in the Antarctic carbon cycle. Krill with empty stomachs were found to swim more actively and thus head towards the surface. This implies that vertical migration may be a bi- or tri-daily occurrence. Some species form surface swarms during the day for feeding and reproductive purposes even though such behaviour is dangerous because it makes them extremely vulnerable to predators.[120] Dense swarms may elicit a feeding frenzy among fish, birds and mammal predators, especially near the surface. When disturbed, a swarm scatters, and some individuals have even been observed to moult instantaneously, leaving the exuvia behind as a decoy.[121] In 2012, Gandomi and Alavi presented what appears to be a successful stochastic algorithm for modelling the behaviour of krill swarms. The algorithm is based on three main factors: " (i) movement induced by the presence of other individuals (ii) foraging activity, and (iii) random diffusion."[122]

Copepods edit

 
This copepod has its antenna spread (click to enlarge). The antenna detects the pressure wave of an approaching fish.

Copepods are a group of tiny crustaceans found in the sea and lakes. Many species are planktonic (drifting in sea waters), and others are benthic (living on the ocean floor). Copepods are typically 1 to 2 millimetres (0.04 to 0.08 in) long, with a teardrop shaped body and large antennae. Although like other crustaceans they have an armoured exoskeleton, they are so small that in most species this thin armour, and the entire body, is almost totally transparent. Copepods have a compound, median single eye, usually bright red, in the centre of the transparent head.

Copepods also swarm. For example, monospecific swarms have been observed regularly around coral reefs and sea grass, and in lakes. Swarms densities were about one million copepods per cubic metre. Typical swarms were one or two metres in diameter, but some exceeded 30 cubic metres. Copepods need visual contact to keep together, and they disperse at night.[123]

Spring produces blooms of swarming phytoplankton which provide food for copepods. Planktonic copepods are usually the dominant members of the zooplankton, and are in turn major food organisms for many other marine animals. In particular, copepods are prey to forage fish and jellyfish, both of which can assemble in vast, million-strong swarms. Some copepods have extremely fast escape responses when a predator is sensed and can jump with high speed over a few millimetres (see animated image below).

Planktonic copepods are important to the carbon cycle. Some scientists say they form the largest animal biomass on earth.[124] They compete for this title with Antarctic krill. Because of their smaller size and relatively faster growth rates, however, and because they are more evenly distributed throughout more of the world's oceans, copepods almost certainly contribute far more to the secondary productivity of the world's oceans, and to the global ocean carbon sink than krill, and perhaps more than all other groups of organisms together. The surface layers of the oceans are currently believed to be the world's largest carbon sink, absorbing about 2 billion tonnes of carbon a year, the equivalent to perhaps a third of human carbon emissions, thus reducing their impact. Many planktonic copepods feed near the surface at night, then sink into deeper water during the day to avoid visual predators. Their moulted exoskeletons, faecal pellets and respiration at depth all bring carbon to the deep sea.

Algal blooms edit

Many single-celled organisms called phytoplankton live in oceans and lakes. When certain conditions are present, such as high nutrient or light levels, these organisms reproduce explosively. The resulting dense swarm of phytoplankton is called an algal bloom. Blooms can cover hundreds of square kilometres and are easily seen in satellite images. Individual phytoplankton rarely live more than a few days, but blooms can last weeks.[125][126]

Plants edit

Scientists have attributed swarm behavior to plants for hundreds of years. In his 1800 book, Phytologia: or, The philosophy of agriculture and gardening, Erasmus Darwin wrote that plant growth resembled swarms observed elsewhere in nature.[127] While he was referring to more broad observations of plant morphology, and was focused on both root and shoot behavior, recent research has supported this claim.

Roots, in particular, display observable swarm behavior, growing in patterns that exceed the statistical threshold for random probability, and indicate the presence of communication between individual root apexes. The primary function of plant roots is the uptake of soil nutrients, and it is this purpose which drives swarm behavior. Plants growing in close proximity have adapted their growth to assure optimal nutrient availability. This is accomplished by growing in a direction that optimizes the distance between nearby roots, thereby increasing their chance of exploiting untapped nutrient reserves. The action of this behavior takes two forms: maximization of distance from, and repulsion by, neighboring root apexes.[128] The transition zone of a root tip is largely responsible for monitoring for the presence of soil-borne hormones, signaling responsive growth patterns as appropriate. Plant responses are often complex, integrating multiple inputs to inform an autonomous response. Additional inputs that inform swarm growth includes light and gravity, both of which are also monitored in the transition zone of a root's apex.[129] These forces act to inform any number of growing "main" roots, which exhibit their own independent releases of inhibitory chemicals to establish appropriate spacing, thereby contributing to a swarm behavior pattern. Horizontal growth of roots, whether in response to high mineral content in soil or due to stolon growth, produces branched growth that establish to also form their own, independent root swarms.[130]

Bacteria edit

Swarming also describes groupings of some kinds of predatory bacteria such as myxobacteria. Myxobacteria swarm together in "wolf packs", actively moving using a process known as bacterial gliding and keeping together with the help of intercellular molecular signals.[57][131]

Mammals edit

 
Sheep dogs (here a Border Collie) control the flocking behaviour of sheep.
 
Bats swarming out of a cave in Thailand
  • Parrish JK, Edelstein-Keshet L (1999). (PDF). Science. 284 (5411): 99–101. Bibcode:1999Sci...284...99P. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.560.5229. doi:10.1126/science.284.5411.99. PMID 10102827. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 July 2011.

People edit

 
Police protect Nick Altrock from an adoring crowd during baseball's 1906 World Series
External images
  Mexican wave simulation[132]
  Rhythmic applause simulation[133]

A collection of people can also exhibit swarm behaviour, such as pedestrians[134] or soldiers swarming the parapets[dubious ]. In Cologne, Germany, two biologists from the University of Leeds demonstrated flock like behaviour in humans. The group of people exhibited similar behavioural pattern to a flock, where if five percent of the flock changed direction the others would follow. If one person was designated as a predator and everyone else was to avoid him, the flock behaved very much like a school of fish.[135][136] Understanding how humans interact in crowds is important if crowd management is to effectively avoid casualties at football grounds, music concerts and subway stations.[137]

The mathematical modelling of flocking behaviour is a common technology, and has found uses in animation. Flocking simulations have been used in many films[138] to generate crowds which move realistically. Tim Burton's Batman Returns was the first movie to make use of swarm technology for rendering, realistically depicting the movements of a group of bats using the boids system. The Lord of the Rings film trilogy made use of similar technology, known as Massive, during battle scenes. Swarm technology is particularly attractive because it is cheap, robust, and simple.

An ant-based computer simulation using only six interaction rules has also been used to evaluate aircraft boarding behaviour.[139] Airlines have also used ant-based routing in assigning aircraft arrivals to airport gates. An airline system developed by Douglas A. Lawson uses swarm theory, or swarm intelligence—the idea that a colony of ants works better than one alone. Each pilot acts like an ant searching for the best airport gate. "The pilot learns from his experience what's the best for him, and it turns out that that's the best solution for the airline," Lawson explains. As a result, the "colony" of pilots always go to gates they can arrive and depart quickly. The program can even alert a pilot of plane back-ups before they happen. "We can anticipate that it's going to happen, so we'll have a gate available," says Lawson.[140]

Swarm behaviour occurs also in traffic flow dynamics, such as the traffic wave. Bidirectional traffic can be observed in ant trails.[141][142] In recent years this behaviour has been researched for insight into pedestrian and traffic models.[143][144] Simulations based on pedestrian models have also been applied to crowds which stampede because of panic.[145]

Herd behaviour in marketing has been used to explain the dependencies of customers' mutual behaviour. The Economist reported a recent conference in Rome on the subject of the simulation of adaptive human behaviour.[146] It shared mechanisms to increase impulse buying and get people "to buy more by playing on the herd instinct." The basic idea is that people will buy more of products that are seen to be popular, and several feedback mechanisms to get product popularity information to consumers are mentioned, including smart card technology and the use of Radio Frequency Identification Tag technology. A "swarm-moves" model was introduced by a Florida Institute of Technology researcher, which is appealing to supermarkets because it can "increase sales without the need to give people discounts."

  • Helbing D, Keltsch J, Molnar P (1997). "Modelling the evolution of human trail systems". Nature. 388 (6637): 47–50. arXiv:cond-mat/9805158. Bibcode:1997Natur.388...47H. doi:10.1038/40353. PMID 9214501. S2CID 4364517.
  • Helbing D, Farkas I, Vicsek T (2000). "Simulating dynamical features of escape panic". Nature. 407 (6803): 487–490. arXiv:cond-mat/0009448. Bibcode:2000Natur.407..487H. doi:10.1038/35035023. PMID 11028994. S2CID 310346.
  • Helbing D, Farkas IJ, Vicsek T (2000). "Freezing by heating in a driven mesoscopic system". Physical Review Letters. 84 (6): 1240–1243. arXiv:cond-mat/9904326. Bibcode:2000PhRvL..84.1240H. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.84.1240. PMID 11017488. S2CID 18649078.

Robotics edit

 
Kilobot thousand-robot swarm developed by Radhika Nagpal and Michael Rubenstein at Harvard University.

The application of swarm principles to robots is called swarm robotics, while swarm intelligence refers to the more general set of algorithms.

External videos
  A Swarm of Nano Quadrotors – YouTube[147]
  March of the microscopic robots Nature Video, YouTube

Partially inspired by colonies of insects such as ants and bees, researchers are modelling the behaviour of swarms of thousands of tiny robots which together perform a useful task, such as finding something hidden, cleaning, or spying. Each robot is quite simple, but the emergent behaviour of the swarm is more complex.[1] The whole set of robots can be considered as one single distributed system, in the same way an ant colony can be considered a superorganism, exhibiting swarm intelligence. The largest swarms so far created is the 1024 robot Kilobot swarm.[148] Other large swarms include the iRobot swarm, the SRI International/ActivMedia Robotics Centibots project,[149] and the Open-source Micro-robotic Project swarm, which are being used to research collective behaviours.[150][151] Swarms are also more resistant to failure. Whereas one large robot may fail and ruin a mission, a swarm can continue even if several robots fail. This could make them attractive for space exploration missions, where failure is normally extremely costly.[152] In addition to ground vehicles, swarm robotics includes also research of swarms of aerial robots[147][153] and heterogeneous teams of ground and aerial vehicles.[154][155]

In contrast macroscopic robots, colloidal particles at microscale can also be adopted as agents to perform collective behaviors to conduct tasks using mechanical and physical approaches, such as reconfigurable tornado-like microswarm[156] mimicking schooling fish,[157] hierarchical particle species[158] mimicking predating behavior of mammals, micro-object manipulation using a transformable microswarm.[159] The fabrication of such colloidal particles is usually based on chemical synthesis.

Military edit

 
Contrast between guerrilla ambush and true swarming (Edwards-2003)

Military swarming is a behaviour where autonomous or partially autonomous units of action attack an enemy from several different directions and then regroup. Pulsing, where the units shift the point of attack, is also a part of military swarming. Military swarming involves the use of a decentralized force against an opponent, in a manner that emphasizes mobility, communication, unit autonomy and coordination or synchronization.[160] Historically military forces used principles of swarming without really examining them explicitly, but now active research consciously examines military doctrines that draw ideas from swarming.

Merely because multiple units converge on a target, they are not necessarily swarming. Siege operations do not involve swarming, because there is no manoeuvre; there is convergence but on the besieged fortification. Nor do guerrilla ambushes constitute swarms, because they are "hit-and-run". Even though the ambush may have several points of attack on the enemy, the guerillas withdraw when they either have inflicted adequate damage, or when they are endangered.

In 2014 the U. S. Office of Naval Research released a video showing tests of a swarm of small autonomous drone attack boats that can steer and take coordinated offensive action as a group.[161]

Gallery edit

Myths edit

  • There is a popular myth that lemmings commit mass suicide by swarming off cliffs when they migrate. Driven by strong biological urges, some species of lemmings may migrate in large groups when population density becomes too great. Lemmings can swim and may choose to cross a body of water in search of a new habitat. In such cases, many may drown if the body of water is so wide as to stretch their physical capability to the limit. This fact combined with some unexplained fluctuations in the population of Norwegian lemmings gave rise to the myth.[165]
  • Piranha have a reputation as fearless fish that swarm in ferocious and predatory packs. However, recent research, which started "with the premise that they school as a means of cooperative hunting", discovered that they were in fact rather fearful fish, like other fish, who schooled for protection from their predators, such as cormorants, caimans and dolphins. A researcher described them as "basically like regular fish with large teeth".[166]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ a b Bouffanais, Roland (2016). Design and Control of Swarm Dynamics. SpringerBriefs in Complexity (First ed.). Springer. doi:10.1007/978-981-287-751-2. ISBN 978-981-287-750-5.
  2. ^ O'Loan; Evans (1998). "Alternating steady state in one-dimensional flocking". Journal of Physics A: Mathematical and General. 32 (8): L99–L105. arXiv:cond-mat/9811336. Bibcode:1999JPhA...32L..99O. doi:10.1088/0305-4470/32/8/002. S2CID 7642063.
  3. ^ Attanasi, A.; Cavagna, A.; Del Castello, L.; Giardina, I.; Grigera, T. S.; Jelić, A.; Melillo, S.; Parisi, L.; Pohl, O.; Shen, E.; Viale, M. (September 2014). "Information transfer and behavioural inertia in starling flocks". Nature Physics. 10 (9): 691–696. arXiv:1303.7097. Bibcode:2014NatPh..10..691A. doi:10.1038/nphys3035. PMC 4173114. PMID 25264452.
  4. ^ a b c Reynolds CW (1987). "Flocks, herds and schools: A distributed behavioral model". Proceedings of the 14th annual conference on Computer graphics and interactive techniques. Vol. 21. pp. 25–34. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.103.7187. doi:10.1145/37401.37406. ISBN 978-0-89791-227-3. S2CID 546350. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  5. ^ a b Ballerini M, Cabibbo N, Candelier R, Cavagna A, Cisbani E, Giardina I, Lecomte V, Orlandi A, Parisi G, Procaccini A, Viale M, Zdravkovic V (2008). "Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: Evidence from a field study". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 (4): 1232–7. arXiv:0709.1916. Bibcode:2008PNAS..105.1232B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0711437105. PMC 2234121. PMID 18227508.
  6. ^ Hildenbrandt H, Carere C, Hemelrijk CK (2010). "Self-organized aerial displays of thousands of starlings: a model". Behavioral Ecology. 21 (6): 1349–1359. arXiv:0908.2677. doi:10.1093/beheco/arq149.
  7. ^ Hemelrijk CK, Hildenbrandt H (2011). "Some causes of the variable shape of flocks of birds". PLOS ONE. 6 (8): e22479. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...622479H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0022479. PMC 3150374. PMID 21829627.
  8. ^ "Zwermen en scholen - Swarming - Permanente expo - Bezoek onze expo's & workshops! - Science LinX - Rijksuniversiteit Groningen". 10 November 2007.
  9. ^ "Onderzoek aan de Faculteit Wiskunde en Natuurwetenschappen - Faculteit Wiskunde en Natuurwetenschappen - Over ons - Rijksuniversiteit Groningen". 25 October 2012.
  10. ^ Yang, W.; Schmickl, T. (2019). "Collective Motion as an Ultimate Effect in Crowded Selfish Herds". Scientific Reports. 9 (1): 6618. Bibcode:2019NatSR...9.6618Y. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-43179-6. PMC 6488663. PMID 31036873.
  11. ^ Olson RS, Knoester DB, Adami C (2013). "Critical interplay between density-dependent predation and evolution of the selfish herd". Proceedings of the 15th annual conference on Genetic and evolutionary computation. Gecco '13. pp. 247–254. doi:10.1145/2463372.2463394. ISBN 9781450319638. S2CID 14414033.
  12. ^ Ward CR, Gobet F, Kendall G (2001). "Evolving collective behavior in an artificial ecology". Artificial Life. 7 (2): 191–209. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.108.3956. doi:10.1162/106454601753139005. PMID 11580880. S2CID 12133884.
  13. ^ Reluga TC, Viscido S (2005). "Simulated evolution of selfish herd behavior". Journal of Theoretical Biology. 234 (2): 213–225. Bibcode:2005JThBi.234..213R. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2004.11.035. PMID 15757680.
  14. ^ Wood AJ, Ackland GJ (2007). "Evolving the selfish herd: emergence of distinct aggregating strategies in an individual-based model". Proc Biol Sci. 274 (1618): 1637–1642. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0306. PMC 2169279. PMID 17472913.
  15. ^ Olson RS, Hintze A, Dyer FC, Knoester DB, Adami C (2013). "Predator confusion is sufficient to evolve swarming behaviour". J. R. Soc. Interface. 10 (85): 20130305. doi:10.1098/rsif.2013.0305. PMC 4043163. PMID 23740485.
  16. ^ Demsar J, Hemelrijk CK, Hildenbrandt H, Bajec IL (2015). "Simulating predator attacks on schools: Evolving composite tactics" (PDF). Ecological Modelling. 304: 22–33. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2015.02.018. hdl:11370/0bfcbb69-a101-4ec1-833a-df301e49d8ef. S2CID 46988508.
  17. ^ Tosh CR (2011). "Which conditions promote negative density dependent selection on prey aggregations?" (PDF). Journal of Theoretical Biology. 281 (1): 24–30. Bibcode:2011JThBi.281...24T. doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.04.014. PMID 21540037.
  18. ^ Ioannou CC, Guttal V, Couzin ID (2012). "Predatory Fish Select for Coordinated Collective Motion in Virtual Prey". Science. 337 (6099): 1212–1215. Bibcode:2012Sci...337.1212I. doi:10.1126/science.1218919. PMID 22903520. S2CID 10203872.
  19. ^ Olson RS, Haley PB, Dyer FC, Adami C (2015). "Exploring the evolution of a trade-off between vigilance and foraging in group-living organisms". Royal Society Open Science. 2 (9): 150135. arXiv:1408.1906. Bibcode:2015RSOS....250135O. doi:10.1098/rsos.150135. PMC 4593673. PMID 26473039.
  20. ^ . 14 September 2008. Archived from the original on 3 July 2016. Retrieved 6 October 2009.
  21. ^ Parunak, H. v D. (2003). "Making swarming happen" In: Proceedings of Conference on Swarming and Network Enabled Command, Control, Communications, Computers, Intelligence, Surveillance and Reconnaissance (C4ISR), McLean, Virginia, USA, 3 January 2003.
  22. ^ a b Marsh L.; Onof C. (2008). "Stigmergic epistemology, stigmergic cognition" (PDF). Cognitive Systems Research. 9 (1): 136–149. doi:10.1016/j.cogsys.2007.06.009. S2CID 23140721.
  23. ^ Beni, G., Wang, J. Swarm Intelligence in Cellular Robotic Systems, Proceed. NATO Advanced Workshop on Robots and Biological Systems, Tuscany, Italy, June 26–30 (1989)
  24. ^ Dorigo, M; Birattari, M (2007). "Swarm intelligence". Scholarpedia. 2 (9): 1462. Bibcode:2007SchpJ...2.1462D. doi:10.4249/scholarpedia.1462.
  25. ^ a b Li, YX; Lukeman, R; Edelstein-Keshet, L (2007). "Minimal mechanisms for school formation in self-propelled particles" (PDF). Physica D: Nonlinear Phenomena. 237 (5): 699–720. Bibcode:2008PhyD..237..699L. doi:10.1016/j.physd.2007.10.009.
  26. ^ Toner J and Tu Y (1995) "Long-range order in a two-dimensional xy model: how birds fly together" Physical Revue Letters, 75 (23)(1995), 4326–4329.
  27. ^ Topaz C, Bertozzi A (2004). "Swarming patterns in a two-dimensional kinematic model for biological groups". SIAM J Appl Math. 65 (1): 152–174. Bibcode:2004APS..MAR.t9004T. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.88.3071. doi:10.1137/S0036139903437424. S2CID 18468679.
  28. ^ Topaz C, Bertozzi A, Lewis M (2006). "A nonlocal continuum model for biological aggregation". Bull Math Biol. 68 (7): 1601–1623. arXiv:q-bio/0504001. doi:10.1007/s11538-006-9088-6. PMID 16858662. S2CID 14750061.
  29. ^ Carrillo, J; Fornasier, M; Toscani, G (2010). "Particle, kinetic, and hydrodynamic models of swarming" (PDF). Mathematical Modeling of Collective Behavior in Socio-Economic and Life Sciences. Modeling and Simulation in Science, Engineering and Technology. Vol. 3. pp. 297–336. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.193.5047. doi:10.1007/978-0-8176-4946-3_12. ISBN 978-0-8176-4945-6.
  30. ^ "Swarmanoid project".
  31. ^ Ant colony optimization Retrieved 15 December 2010.
  32. ^ A. Colorni, M. Dorigo et V. Maniezzo, Distributed Optimization by Ant Colonies, actes de la première conférence européenne sur la vie artificielle, Paris, Elsevier Publishing, 134–142, 1991.
  33. ^ M. Dorigo, Optimization, Learning and Natural Algorithms, PhD thesis, Politecnico di Milano, Italie, 1992.
  34. ^ Hölldobler & Wilson (1990), pp. 143–179
  35. ^ DORIGO, M.; DI CARO, G.; GAMBERELLA, L. M. (1999). Ant Algorithms for Discrete Optimization, Artificial Life. MIT Press.
  36. ^ Self driven particle model 2012-10-14 at the Wayback Machine Interactive simulations, 2005, University of Colorado. Retrieved 10 April 2011.
  37. ^ a b Vicsek T, Czirok A, Ben-Jacob E, Cohen I, Shochet O (1995). "Novel type of phase transition in a system of self-driven particles". Physical Review Letters. 75 (6): 1226–1229. arXiv:cond-mat/0611743. Bibcode:1995PhRvL..75.1226V. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.75.1226. PMID 10060237. S2CID 15918052.
  38. ^ Czirók A, Vicsek T (2006). "Collective behavior of interacting self-propelled particles". Physica A. 281 (1–4): 17–29. arXiv:cond-mat/0611742. Bibcode:2000PhyA..281...17C. doi:10.1016/S0378-4371(00)00013-3. S2CID 14211016.
  39. ^ Buhl J, Sumpter DJT, Couzin D, Hale JJ, Despland E, Miller ER, Simpson SJ, et al. (2006). (PDF). Science. 312 (5778): 1402–1406. Bibcode:2006Sci...312.1402B. doi:10.1126/science.1125142. PMID 16741126. S2CID 359329. Archived from the original (PDF) on 29 September 2011. Retrieved 13 April 2011.
  40. ^ Toner J, Tu Y, Ramaswamy S (2005). (PDF). Annals of Physics. 318 (1): 170–244. Bibcode:2005AnPhy.318..170T. doi:10.1016/j.aop.2005.04.011. Archived from the original (PDF) on 18 July 2011. Retrieved 13 April 2011.
  41. ^ Bertin, E; Droz; Grégoire, G (2009). "Hydrodynamic equations for self-propelled particles: microscopic derivation and stability analysis". J. Phys. A. 42 (44): 445001. arXiv:0907.4688. Bibcode:2009JPhA...42R5001B. doi:10.1088/1751-8113/42/44/445001. S2CID 17686543.
  42. ^ Kennedy, J.; Eberhart, R. (1995). "Particle Swarm Optimization". Proceedings of IEEE International Conference on Neural Networks. Vol. IV. pp. 1942–1948.
  43. ^ Kennedy, J. (1997). "The particle swarm: social adaptation of knowledge". Proceedings of IEEE International Conference on Evolutionary Computation. pp. 303–308.
  44. ^ Hu X Particle swarm optimization: Tutorial. Retrieved 15 December 2010.
  45. ^ Kennedy, J.; Eberhart, R.C. (2001). Swarm Intelligence. Morgan Kaufmann. ISBN 978-1-55860-595-4.
  46. ^ Poli, R. (2007). (PDF). Technical Report CSM-469. Archived from the original (PDF) on 16 July 2011. Retrieved 15 December 2010.
  47. ^ Poli, R. (2008). "Analysis of the publications on the applications of particle swarm optimisation" (PDF). Journal of Artificial Evolution and Applications. 2008: 1–10. doi:10.1155/2008/685175.
  48. ^ Altruism helps swarming robots fly better 2012-09-15 at the Wayback Machine genevalunch.com, 4 May 2011.
  49. ^ Waibel, M; Floreano, D; Keller, L (2011). "A quantitative test of Hamilton's rule for the evolution of altruism". PLOS Biology. 9 (5): 1000615. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000615. PMC 3086867. PMID 21559320.
  50. ^ Herrnkind, W (1969). "Queuing behavior of spiny lobsters". Science. 164 (3886): 1425–1427. Bibcode:1969Sci...164.1425H. doi:10.1126/science.164.3886.1425. PMID 5783720. S2CID 10324354.
  51. ^ National Geographic, 17 October 2019.
  52. ^ Vannier, J; Vidal, M; Marchant, R; El Hariri, K; Kouraiss, K; Pittet, B; El Albani, A; Mazurier, A; Martin, E (2019). "Collective behaviour in 480-million-year-old trilobite arthropods from Morocco". Scientific Reports. 9 (1): 14941. Bibcode:2019NatSR...914941V. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-51012-3. PMC 6797724. PMID 31624280.
  53. ^ Feare C (1984) The Starling, Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-217705-6.
  54. ^ Partridge BL (1982). (PDF). Scientific American. Vol. 246, no. 6. pp. 114–123. Bibcode:1982SciAm.246f.114P. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0682-114. PMID 7201674. Archived from the original (PDF) on 3 July 2011.
  55. ^ Hubbard S, Babak P, Sigurdsson S, Magnusson K (2004). "A model of the formation of fish schools and migrations of fish". Ecol. Model. 174 (4): 359–374. doi:10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2003.06.006.
  56. ^ Rauch E, Millonas M, Chialvo D (1995). "Pattern formation and functionality in swarm models". Physics Letters A. 207 (3–4): 185–193. arXiv:adap-org/9507003. Bibcode:1995PhLA..207..185R. doi:10.1016/0375-9601(95)00624-C. S2CID 120567147.
  57. ^ a b Allison C, Hughes C (1991). "Bacterial swarming: an example of prokaryotic differentiation and multicellular behaviour". Science Progress. 75 (298 Pt 3–4): 403–422. PMID 1842857.
  58. ^ Ben-Jacob E, Cohen I, Shochet O, Czirok A, Vicsek T (1995). "Cooperative Formation of Chiral Patterns during Growth of Bacterial Colonies". Physical Review Letters. 75 (15): 2899–2902. Bibcode:1995PhRvL..75.2899B. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.75.2899. PMID 10059433.
  59. ^ Rappel WJ, Nicol A, Sarkissian A, Levine H, Loomis WF (1999). "Self-organized vortex state in two-dimensional Dictyostelium dynamics". Physical Review Letters. 83 (6): 1247–1250. arXiv:patt-sol/9811001. Bibcode:1999PhRvL..83.1247R. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.83.1247. S2CID 1590827.
  60. ^ Chowdhury, D (2006). "Collective effects in intra-cellular molecular motor transport: coordination, cooperation and competition". Physica A. 372 (1): 84–95. arXiv:physics/0605053. Bibcode:2006PhyA..372...84C. doi:10.1016/j.physa.2006.05.005. S2CID 14822256.
  61. ^ Parrish JK and Hamner WM (eds) (1997) Animal Groups in Three Dimensions Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-46024-8.
  62. ^ Helbing D, Keltsch J, Molnar P (1997). "Modelling the evolution of human trail systems". Nature. 388 (6637): 47–50. arXiv:cond-mat/9805158. Bibcode:1997Natur.388...47H. doi:10.1038/40353. PMID 9214501. S2CID 4364517.
  63. ^ Helbing D, Farkas I, Vicsek T (2000). "Simulating dynamical features of escape panic". Nature. 407 (6803): 487–490. arXiv:cond-mat/0009448. Bibcode:2000Natur.407..487H. doi:10.1038/35035023. PMID 11028994. S2CID 310346.
  64. ^ Helbing D, Farkas IJ, Vicsek T (2000). "Freezing by heating in a driven mesoscopic system". Physical Review Letters. 84 (6): 1240–1243. arXiv:cond-mat/9904326. Bibcode:2000PhRvL..84.1240H. doi:10.1103/PhysRevLett.84.1240. PMID 11017488. S2CID 18649078.
  65. ^
    • National Geographic. Feature article, July 2007.
    • Beekman M, Sword GA and Simpson SK (2008) Biological Foundations of Swarm Intelligence. In Swarm intelligence: introduction and applications, Eds Blum C and Merkle D. シュプリンガー・ジャパン株式会社, Page 3–43. ISBN 978-3-540-74088-9
    • Parrish JK, Edelstein-Keshet L (1999). (PDF). Science. 284 (5411): 99–101. Bibcode:1999Sci...284...99P. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.560.5229. doi:10.1126/science.284.5411.99. PMID 10102827. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 July 2011.
  66. ^ a b Bonabeau E and Theraulaz G (2008) "Swarm Smarts". In Your Future with Robots Scientific American Special Editions.
  67. ^ Czaczkes, T.J.; Grüter, C.; Ratnieks, F. L. W. (2015). "Trail pheromones: an integrative view of their role in colony organisation". Annual Review of Entomology. 60: 581–599. doi:10.1146/annurev-ento-010814-020627. PMID 25386724. S2CID 37972066.
  68. ^ Beckers, R.; Deneubourg, J. L.; Goss, S (1993). "Modulation of trail laying in the ant Lasius niger (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) and its role in the collective selection of a food source". Journal of Insect Behavior. 6 (6): 751–759. Bibcode:1993JIBeh...6..751B. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.161.9541. doi:10.1007/BF01201674. S2CID 14670150.
  69. ^ Goss, S.; Aron, S.; Deneubourg, J. L.; Pasteels, J. M. (1989). "Self-organized shortcuts in the Argentine ant" (PDF). Naturwissenschaften. 76 (12): 579–581. Bibcode:1989NW.....76..579G. doi:10.1007/BF00462870. S2CID 18506807.
  70. ^ Brady, Seán G. (2003). "Evolution of the army ant syndrome: The origin and long-term evolutionary stasis of a complex of behavioral and reproductive adaptations". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 100 (11): 6575–9. Bibcode:2003PNAS..100.6575B. doi:10.1073/pnas.1137809100. PMC 164488. PMID 12750466.
  71. ^ Dicke E, Byde A, Cliff D, Layzell P (2004). "An ant-inspired technique for storage area network design". In A. J. Ispeert, M. Murata, N. Wakamiya (eds.). Proceedings of Biologically Inspired Approaches to Advanced Information Technology: First International Workshop, BioADIT 2004 LNCS 3141. pp. 364–379. ISBN 9783540233398.
  72. ^ Oldroyd, BP (1998). "Colony relatedness in aggregations of Apis dorsata Fabricius (Hymenoptera, Apidae)". Insectes Sociaux. 47: 94–95. doi:10.1007/s000400050015. S2CID 40346464.
  73. ^ Milius, Susan Swarm Savvy 27 September 2012 at the Wayback Machine, How bees, ants and other animals avoid dumb collective decisions; Science News, May 9th, 2009; Vol.175 #10 (p. 16)
  74. ^ Bee Swarms Follow High-speed 'Streaker' Bees To Find A New Nest; ScienceDaily (Nov. 24, 2008)
  75. ^ Seeley, Thomas D.; Visscher, P. Kirk (2003). (PDF). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 54 (5): 511–520. doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0664-6. S2CID 16948152. Archived from the original (PDF) on 31 January 2009. Retrieved 14 December 2010.
  76. ^ Morse, R.A. (1963). "Swarm orientation in honeybees". Science. 141 (3578): 357–358. Bibcode:1963Sci...141..357M. doi:10.1126/science.141.3578.357. PMID 17815993. S2CID 46516976.
  77. ^ Seeley, Thomas (2003). "Consensus building during nest-site selection in honey bee swarms: The expiration of dissent". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 53 (6): 417–424. doi:10.1007/s00265-003-0598-z. S2CID 34798300.
  78. ^ Stepien, T.L.; Zmurchok, C.; Hengenius, J.B.; Caja Rivera, R.M.; D'Orsogna, M.R.; Lindsay, A.E. (2000). "Moth Mating: Modeling Female Pheromone Calling and Male Navigational Strategies to Optimize Reproductive Success". Applied Sciences. 10 (18): 6543. doi:10.3390/app10186543.
  79. ^ Badeke, B.; Haverkamp, A.; Sachse, S.A. (2016). "A challenge for a male noctuid moth? Discerning the female sex pheromone against the background of plant volatiles". Frontiers in Physiology. 7: 143. doi:10.3389/fphys.2016.00143. PMC 4843018. PMID 27199761.
  80. ^ Greenfield, M.D. (1981). "Moth sex pheromones: an evolutionary perspective". The Florida Entomologist. 64 (1): 4–17. doi:10.2307/3494597. JSTOR 3494597.
  81. ^ Umbers, K.D.L.; Symonds, M.R.E.; Kokko, H. (2015). "The Mothematics of female pheromone signaling: Strategies for aging virgins". American Naturalist. 185 (3): 417–432. doi:10.1086/679614. hdl:1885/13166. PMID 25674695. S2CID 13846468.
  82. ^ Mason, D.S.; Baruzzi, C. (2019). "Love in strange places". Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment. 17 (3): 184. Bibcode:2019FrEE...17..184M. doi:10.1002/fee.2027.
  83. ^ "Midges". MDC Discover Nature. Retrieved 19 November 2019.
  84. ^ Kirkeby, Carsten (30 June 2018). "Observations of female and mixed sex swarming behaviour in Culicoides LATREILLE, 1809 (Diptera: Ceratopogonidae)". Polish Journal of Entomology. 87 (2): 191–197. doi:10.2478/pjen-2018-0014.
  85. ^ Jennifer Viegas. . Discovery Channel. Archived from the original on 4 July 2008. Retrieved 10 June 2006.
  86. ^ Lemonick, Michael D. (15 November 2007). . Time. Archived from the original on 16 November 2007.
  87. ^ National Geographic. Retrieved 12 December 2010.
  88. ^ "Locust swarms 'high' on serotonin". 29 January 2009 – via BBC.
  89. ^ Rogers SM, Matheson T, Despland E, Dodgson T, Burrows M, Simpson SJ (November 2003). "Mechanosensory-induced behavioural gregarization in the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria". J. Exp. Biol. 206 (Pt 22): 3991–4002. doi:10.1242/jeb.00648. PMID 14555739.
  90. ^ Stevenson, PA (2009). "The Key to Pandora's Box". Science. 323 (5914): 594–5. doi:10.1126/science.1169280. PMID 19179520. S2CID 39306643.
  91. ^ Blocking 'happiness' chemical may prevent locust plagues, New Scientist, 2009-01-29, accessed 2009-01-31
  92. ^ Moshe Guershon; Amir Ayali (May 2012). "Innate phase behavior in the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria". Insect Science. 19 (6): 649–656. Bibcode:2012InsSc..19..649G. doi:10.1111/j.1744-7917.2012.01518.x. S2CID 83412818.
  93. ^ Yates, CA; Erban, R; Escudero, C; Couzin, ID; Buhl, J; Kevrekidis, IG; Maini, PK; Sumpter, DJ (2009). "Inherent noise can facilitate coherence in collective swarm motion". Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106 (14): 5464–9. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.5464Y. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811195106. PMC 2667078. PMID 19336580.
  94. ^ Pyle, Robert Michael, "National Audubon Society Field Guide to North American Butterflies", p712-713, Alfred A. Knopf, New York, ISBN 0-394-51914-0
  95. ^ "Monarch, Danaus plexippus". Archived from the original on 15 December 2012. Retrieved 27 August 2008.
  96. ^ Gugliotta, Guy (2003): Butterflies Guided By Body Clocks, Sun Scientists Shine Light on Monarchs' Pilgrimage 2006-03-05 at the Wayback Machine. Washington Post, May 23, 2003, page A03. Retrieved 2006-JAN-07.
  97. ^ Merlin C, Gegear RJ, Reppert SM (2009). "Antennal Circadian Clocks Coordinate Sun Compass Orientation in Migratory Monarch Butterflies". Science. 325 (5948): 1700–1704. Bibcode:2009Sci...325.1700M. doi:10.1126/science.1176221. PMC 2754321. PMID 19779201.
  98. ^ Kyriacou CP (2009). "Unraveling Traveling". Science. 325 (5948): 1629–1630. doi:10.1126/science.1178935. PMID 19779177. S2CID 206522416.
  99. ^ Nagy, M; Akos Zs, Biro D; Vicsek, T (2010). (PDF). Nature. 464 (7290): 890–893. arXiv:1010.5394. Bibcode:2010Natur.464..890N. doi:10.1038/nature08891. PMID 20376149. S2CID 4430488. Archived from the original (PDF) on 6 July 2010. Supplementary pdf
  100. ^ Sekercioglu, C.H. (2007). "Conservation ecology: area trumps mobility in fragment bird extinctions". Current Biology. 17 (8): R283–R286. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.02.019. PMID 17437705. S2CID 744140.
  101. ^ Drag Reduction from Formation Flight. Flying Aircraft in Bird-Like Formations Could Significantly Increase Range; Defense Technical Information Center; April 2002; Retrieved February 27, 2008
  102. ^ Hummel D.; Beukenberg M. (1989). "Aerodynamische Interferenzeffekte beim Formationsfl ug von Vogeln". J. Ornithol. 130 (1): 15–24. doi:10.1007/BF01647158. S2CID 823269.
  103. ^ Cutts, C. J. & J R Speakman (1994). "Energy savings in formation flight of Pink-footed Geese" (PDF). J. Exp. Biol. 189 (1): 251–261. doi:10.1242/jeb.189.1.251. PMID 9317742.
  104. ^ Newton, I. (2008). The Migration Ecology of Birds. Elselvier. ISBN 978-0-12-517367-4.
  105. ^ Pitcher et al. 1982.
  106. ^ Pitcher TJ and Parish JK (1993) "Functions of shoaling behaviour in teleosts" In: Pitcher TJ (ed) Behaviour of teleost fishes. Chapman and Hall, New York, pp 363–440
  107. ^ Hoare DJ, Krause J, Peuhkuri N and Godin JGJ (2000) Body size and shoaling in fish Journal of Fish Biology, 57(6) 1351-1366.
  108. ^ Snekser JL, Ruhl N, Bauer K, McRobert SP (2010). (PDF). International Journal of Comparative Psychology. 23: 70–81. doi:10.46867/IJCP.2010.23.01.04. Archived from the original (PDF) on 25 July 2011.
  109. ^ Engeszer RE, Ryan MJ, Parichy DM (2004). "Learned Social Preference in Zebrafish" (PDF). Current Biology. 14 (10): 881–884. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2004.04.042. PMID 15186744. S2CID 18741014.
  110. ^ Reebs, S.G. (2000). "Can a minority of informed leaders determine the foraging movements of a fish shoal?". Animal Behaviour. 59 (2): 403–9. doi:10.1006/anbe.1999.1314. PMID 10675263. S2CID 4945309.
  111. ^ Radakov DV (1973) Schooling in the ecology of fish. Israel Program for Scientific Translation, translated by Mill H. Halsted Press, New York. ISBN 978-0-7065-1351-6
  112. ^ Photographer: Mark van Coller
  113. ^ Hamner, WM; Hamner, PP; Strand, SW; Gilmer, RW (1983). "Behavior of Antarctic Krill, Euphausia superba: Chemoreception, Feeding, Schooling and Molting'". Science. 220 (4595): 433–5. Bibcode:1983Sci...220..433H. doi:10.1126/science.220.4595.433. PMID 17831417. S2CID 22161686.
  114. ^ U. Kils; P. Marshall (1995). "Der Krill, wie er schwimmt und frisst – neue Einsichten mit neuen Methoden ("The Antarctic krill – how it swims and feeds – new insights with new methods")". In I. Hempel; G. Hempel (eds.). Biologie der Polarmeere – Erlebnisse und Ergebnisse (Biology of the Polar Oceans Experiences and Results). Fischer Verlag. pp. 201–210. ISBN 978-3-334-60950-7.
  115. ^ R. Piper (2007). Extraordinary Animals: An Encyclopedia of Curious and Unusual Animals. Greenwood Press. ISBN 978-0-313-33922-6.
  116. ^ Hoare, Ben (2009). Animal Migration. London: Natural History Museum. p. 107. ISBN 978-0-565-09243-6.
  117. ^ a b c Hoare, Ben (2009). Animal Migration. London: Natural History Museum. p. 107. ISBN 978-0-565-09243-6
  118. ^ J.S. Jaffe; M.D. Ohmann; A. de Robertis (1999). (PDF). Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences. 56 (11): 2000–10. doi:10.1139/cjfas-56-11-2000. S2CID 228567512. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 July 2011.
  119. ^ Geraint A. Tarling & Magnus L. Johnson (2006). "Satiation gives krill that sinking feeling". Current Biology. 16 (3): 83–4. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.044. PMID 16461267.
  120. ^ Howard, D.: "Krill", pp. 133–140 in Karl, H.A. et al. (eds): Beyond the Golden Gate – Oceanography, Geology, Biology, and Environmental Issues in the Gulf of the Farallones, USGS Circular 1198, 2001. URLs last accessed 2010-06-04.
  121. ^ D. Howard. "Krill in Cordell Bank National Marine Sanctuary". NOAA. Retrieved 15 June 2005.
  122. ^ Gandomi, A.H.; Alavi, A.H. (2012). "Krill Herd Algorithm: A New Bio-Inspired Optimization Algorithm". Communications in Nonlinear Science and Numerical Simulation. 17 (12): 4831–4845. Bibcode:2012CNSNS..17.4831G. doi:10.1016/j.cnsns.2012.05.010.
  123. ^ Hamner, WM; Carleton, JH (1979). "Copepod swarms: Attributes and role in coral reef ecosystems". Limnol. Oceanogr. 24 (1): 1–14. Bibcode:1979LimOc..24....1H. doi:10.4319/lo.1979.24.1.0001.
  124. ^ Johannes Dürbaum & Thorsten Künnemann (5 November 1997). . Carl von Ossietzky University of Oldenburg. Archived from the original on 26 May 2010. Retrieved 8 December 2009.
  125. ^ Lindsey R and Scott M (2010) What are phytoplankton NASA Earth Observatory.
  126. ^ Harmful algal blooms in the Great Lakes 2010-06-16 at the Wayback Machine 2009, NOAA, Center of Excellence for Great Lakes and Human Health.
  127. ^ Darwin, Erasmus (1 January 1800). Phytologia: Or, The Philosophy of Agriculture and Gardening. With the Theory of Draining Morasses and with an Improved Construction of the Drill Plough. P. Byrne.
  128. ^ Ciszak, Marzena; Comparini, Diego; Mazzolai, Barbara; Baluska, Frantisek; Arecchi, F. Tito; Vicsek, Tamás; Mancuso, Stefano (17 January 2012). "Swarming Behavior in Plant Roots". PLOS ONE. 7 (1): e29759. Bibcode:2012PLoSO...729759C. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029759. ISSN 1932-6203. PMC 3260168. PMID 22272246.
  129. ^ Baluška, František; Mancuso, Stefano; Volkmann, Dieter; Barlow, Peter W. (1 July 2010). "Root apex transition zone: a signalling–response nexus in the root". Trends in Plant Science. 15 (7): 402–408. doi:10.1016/j.tplants.2010.04.007. PMID 20621671.
  130. ^ J., Trewavas, A. (2014). Plant behaviour and intelligence. Oxford university press. ISBN 9780199539543. OCLC 961862730.{{cite book}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  131. ^ Reichenbach H (2001). "Myxobacteria, producers of novel bioactive substances". J Ind Microbiol Biotechnol. 27 (3): 149–56. doi:10.1038/sj.jim.7000025. PMID 11780785. S2CID 34964313.
  132. ^ Farkas I, Helbing D, Vicsek T (2002). (PDF). Nature. 419 (6903): 131–132. arXiv:cond-mat/0210073. Bibcode:2002Natur.419..131F. doi:10.1038/419131a. PMID 12226653. S2CID 4309609. Archived from the original (PDF) on 10 July 2007.
  133. ^ Neda Z, Ravasz E, Brechet Y, Vicsek T, Barabasi AL (2002). (PDF). Physical Review E. 61 (6): 6987–6992. arXiv:cond-mat/0006423. Bibcode:2000PhRvE..61.6987N. doi:10.1103/physreve.61.6987. PMID 11088392. S2CID 14135891. Archived from the original (PDF) on 11 June 2011.
  134. ^ Helbing, D; Keltsch, J; Molnar, P (1997). "Modelling the evolution of human trail systems". Nature. 388 (6637): 47–50. arXiv:cond-mat/9805158. Bibcode:1997Natur.388...47H. doi:10.1038/40353. PMID 9214501. S2CID 4364517.
  135. ^ "http://psychcentral.com/news/2008/02/15/herd-mentality-explained/1922.html 29 November 2014 at the Wayback Machine". Retrieved on October 31st 2008.
  136. ^ "Danger in numbers during Haj". The National. 6 September 2009.
  137. ^ Couzin ID, Krause J (2003). (PDF). Advances in the Study of Behavior. Vol. 32. pp. 1–75. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(03)01001-5. ISBN 978-0-12-004532-7. Archived from the original (PDF) on 13 March 2012. Retrieved 14 April 2011.
  138. ^ Gabbai, J.M.E. (2005). (Thesis). Manchester: University of Manchester Doctoral Thesis. Archived from the original on 19 December 2014. Retrieved 11 July 2009.
  139. ^ Livermore R (2008) "A multi-agent system approach to a simulation study comparing the performance of aircraft boarding using pre-assigned seating and free-for-all strategies" Open University, Technical report No 2008/25.
  140. ^ "Planes, Trains and Ant Hills: Computer scientists simulate activity of ants to reduce airline delays" 2010-11-24 at the Wayback Machine Science Daily, 1 April 2008.
  141. ^ Burd, Martin; N. Aranwela (February 2003). "Head-on encounter rates and walking speed of foragers in leaf-cutting ant traffic". Insectes Sociaux. 50 (1): 3–8. doi:10.1007/s000400300001. S2CID 23876486.
  142. ^ Ribeiro, Pedro; André Frazão Helene; Gilberto Xavier; Carlos Navas; Fernando Leite Ribeiro (1 April 2009). Dornhaus, Anna (ed.). "Ants can learn to forage on one-way trails". PLOS ONE. 4 (4): e5024. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.5024R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005024. PMC 2659768. PMID 19337369.
  143. ^ John, Alexander; Andreas Schadschneider; Debashish Chowdhury; Katsuhiro Nishinari (March 2008). "Characteristics of ant-inspired traffic flow". Swarm Intelligence. 2 (1): 25–41. arXiv:0903.1434. doi:10.1007/s11721-008-0010-8. S2CID 18350336.
  144. ^ Are we nearly there yet? Motorists could learn a thing or two from ants The Economist, 10 July 2009.
  145. ^ Helbing, Dirk; Farkas, Illés; Vicsek, Tamás (2000). "Simulating dynamical features of escape panic". Nature. 407 (6803): 487–490. arXiv:cond-mat/0009448. Bibcode:2000Natur.407..487H. doi:10.1038/35035023. PMID 11028994. S2CID 310346.
  146. ^ "Swarming the shelves: How shops can exploit people's herd mentality to increase sales?". The Economist. 11 November 2006. p. 90.
  147. ^ a b Kushleyev, Alex; Mellinger, Daniel; Powers, Caitlin; Kumar, Vijay (2013). "Towards a swarm of agile micro quadrotors". Autonomous Robots. 35 (4): 287–300. doi:10.1007/s10514-013-9349-9. S2CID 18340816.
  148. ^ . Archived from the original on 26 October 2014.
  149. ^ . ActivMedia Robotics. Archived from the original on 14 July 2011.
  150. ^ "Open-source micro-robotic project". Retrieved 28 October 2007.
  151. ^ . iRobot Corporation. Archived from the original on 27 September 2007. Retrieved 28 October 2007.
  152. ^ Knapp, Louise (21 December 2000). "Look, Up in the Sky: Robofly". Wired. Retrieved 25 September 2008.
  153. ^ Saska, Martin; Jan, Vakula; Libor, Preucil (2014). Swarms of micro aerial vehicles stabilized under a visual relative localization. IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (ICRA).
  154. ^ Saska, Martin; Vonasek, Vojtech; Krajnik, Tomas; Preucil, Libor (2014). "Coordination and navigation of heterogeneous MAV–UGV formations localized by a hawk-eye-like approach under a model predictive control scheme" (PDF). International Journal of Robotics Research. 33 (10): 1393–1412. doi:10.1177/0278364914530482. S2CID 1195374.
  155. ^ Saska, Martin; Vonasek, Vojtech; Krajnik, Tomas; Preucil, Libor (2012). Coordination and Navigation of Heterogeneous UAVs-UGVs Teams Localized by a Hawk-Eye Approach. IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems (IROS).
  156. ^ Ji, Fengtong; Jin, Dongdong; Wang, Ben; Zhang, Li (23 June 2020). "Light-Driven Hovering of a Magnetic Microswarm in Fluid". ACS Nano. 14 (6): 6990–6998. doi:10.1021/acsnano.0c01464. ISSN 1936-0851. PMID 32463226. S2CID 218976382.
  157. ^ Hughes, Robin (22 February 2007), Barracuda Tornado, retrieved 7 February 2022
  158. ^ Mou, Fangzhi; Li, Xiaofeng; Xie, Qi; Zhang, Jianhua; Xiong, Kang; Xu, Leilei; Guan, Jianguo (20 December 2019). "Active Micromotor Systems Built from Passive Particles with Biomimetic Predator–Prey Interactions". ACS Nano. 14 (1): 406–414. doi:10.1021/acsnano.9b05996. ISSN 1936-0851. PMID 31860277. S2CID 209435036.
  159. ^ Yu, Jiangfan; Wang, Ben; Du, Xingzhou; Wang, Qianqian; Zhang, Li (21 August 2018). "Ultra-extensible ribbon-like magnetic microswarm". Nature Communications. 9 (1): 3260. Bibcode:2018NatCo...9.3260Y. doi:10.1038/s41467-018-05749-6. ISSN 2041-1723. PMC 6104072. PMID 30131487.
  160. ^ Edwards, Sean J.A. (2000). Swarming on the Battlefield: Past, Present, and Future. Rand Monograph MR-1100. Rand Corporation. ISBN 978-0-8330-2779-5.
  161. ^ U.S. Navy could 'swarm' foes with robot boats, CNN, 13 October 2014.
  162. ^ "Dive and Discover: Scientific Expedition 10: Antarctica". Retrieved 3 September 2008.
  163. ^ Crowd modelling: Simulating the behaviour of crowds of people, or swarms of animals, has both frivolous and important uses The Economist, 5 March 2009.
  164. ^ Fisher, Len (2009) The perfect swarm: the science of complexity in everyday life Page 57. Basic Books. ISBN 978-0-465-01884-0
  165. ^ Woodford, Riley. . Archived from the original on 3 January 2010.{{cite web}}: CS1 maint: unfit URL (link)
  166. ^ Red-Bellied Piranha Is Really Yellow New York Times, 24 May 2005.

Sources edit

  • Blum C and Merkle D (2008) Swarm intelligence: introduction and applications Springer. ISBN 978-3-540-74088-9.
  • Camazine S, Deneubourg JL, Franks NR, Sneyd J, Theraulaz G and Bonabeau E (2003) Self-Organization in Biological Systems Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-11624-2.
  • Fisher L (2009) The perfect swarm: the science of complexity in everyday life Basic Books. ISBN 978-0-465-01884-0.
  • Kennedy JF, Kennedy J, Eberhart RC and Shi Y (2001) Swarm intelligence Morgan Kaufmann. ISBN 978-1-55860-595-4.
  • Krause, J (2005) Living in Groups Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-850818-2
  • Lim CP, Jain LC and Dehuri S (2009) Innovations in Swarm Intelligence Springer. ISBN 978-3-642-04224-9.
  • Miller, Peter (2010) The Smart Swarm: How understanding flocks, schools, and colonies can make us better at communicating, decision making, and getting things done Penguin, ISBN 978-1-58333-390-7
  • Nedjah N and Mourelle LdM (2006) Swarm intelligent systems Springer. ISBN 978-3-540-33868-0.
  • Sumpter, David JT (2010) Collective Animal Behavior Princeton University Press. ISBN 978-0-691-14843-4.
  • Vicsek A, Zafeiris A (2012). "Collective motion". Physics Reports. 517 (3–4): 71–140. arXiv:1010.5017. Bibcode:2012PhR...517...71V. doi:10.1016/j.physrep.2012.03.004. S2CID 119109873.

External links edit

  • New York Times article on investigations into swarming
  • From the Wolfram Demonstrations Project – requires CDF player (free):
    • Model of a Firefly Swarm.
    • Garbage Collection by Ants
    • Beverton and Merging Schools of Fish
    • Propp Circles

swarm, behaviour, swarm, redirects, here, other, uses, swarm, disambiguation, swarming, collective, behaviour, exhibited, entities, particularly, animals, similar, size, which, aggregate, together, perhaps, milling, about, same, spot, perhaps, moving, masse, m. Swarm redirects here For other uses see Swarm disambiguation Swarm behaviour or swarming is a collective behaviour exhibited by entities particularly animals of similar size which aggregate together perhaps milling about the same spot or perhaps moving en masse or migrating in some direction It is a highly interdisciplinary topic 1 A flock of auklets exhibit swarm behaviour As a term swarming is applied particularly to insects but can also be applied to any other entity or animal that exhibits swarm behaviour The term flocking or murmuration can refer specifically to swarm behaviour in birds herding to refer to swarm behaviour in tetrapods and shoaling or schooling to refer to swarm behaviour in fish Phytoplankton also gather in huge swarms called blooms although these organisms are algae and are not self propelled the way animals are By extension the term swarm is applied also to inanimate entities which exhibit parallel behaviours as in a robot swarm an earthquake swarm or a swarm of stars From a more abstract point of view swarm behaviour is the collective motion of a large number of self propelled entities 2 From the perspective of the mathematical modeller it is an emergent behaviour arising from simple rules that are followed by individuals and does not involve any central coordination Swarm behaviour is also studied by active matter physicists as a phenomenon which is not in thermodynamic equilibrium and as such requires the development of tools beyond those available from the statistical physics of systems in thermodynamic equilibrium In this regard swarming has been compared to the mathematics of superfluids specifically in the context of starling flocks murmuration 3 Swarm behaviour was first simulated on a computer in 1986 with the simulation program boids 4 This program simulates simple agents boids that are allowed to move according to a set of basic rules The model was originally designed to mimic the flocking behaviour of birds but it can be applied also to schooling fish and other swarming entities Contents 1 Models 1 1 Mathematical models 1 2 Evolutionary models 1 3 Agents 1 4 Self organization 1 5 Emergence 1 6 Stigmergy 1 7 Swarm intelligence 1 8 Algorithms 1 8 1 Ant colony optimization 1 8 2 Self propelled particles 1 8 3 Particle swarm optimization 1 8 4 Altruism 2 Biological swarming 2 1 Social insects 2 1 1 Ants 2 1 2 Honey bees 2 2 Non social insects 2 2 1 Moths 2 2 2 Flies 2 2 3 Cockroaches 2 2 4 Locusts 2 2 5 Migratory behavior 2 3 Birds 2 3 1 Bird migration 2 4 Marine life 2 4 1 Fish 2 4 2 Fish migration 2 4 3 Krill 2 4 4 Copepods 2 4 5 Algal blooms 2 5 Plants 2 6 Bacteria 2 7 Mammals 3 People 4 Robotics 5 Military 6 Gallery 7 Myths 8 See also 9 References 9 1 Sources 10 External linksModels editSee also Collective animal behaviour In recent decades scientists have turned to modeling swarm behaviour to gain a deeper understanding of the behaviour Mathematical models edit nbsp In the metric distance model of a fish school left the focal fish yellow pays attention to all fish within the small zone of repulsion red the zone of alignment lighter red and the larger zone of attraction lightest red In the topological distance model right the focal fish only pays attention to the six or seven closest fish green regardless of their distance External images nbsp Boids simulation nbsp iFloys simulation nbsp Efloys simulation Early studies of swarm behaviour employed mathematical models to simulate and understand the behaviour The simplest mathematical models of animal swarms generally represent individual animals as following three rules Move in the same direction as their neighbours Remain close to their neighbours Avoid collisions with their neighbours The boids computer program created by Craig Reynolds in 1986 simulates swarm behaviour following the above rules 4 Many subsequent and current models use variations on these rules often implementing them by means of concentric zones around each animal In the zone of repulsion very close to the animal the focal animal will seek to distance itself from its neighbours to avoid collision Slightly further away in the zone of alignment the focal animal will seek to align its direction of motion with its neighbours In the outermost zone of attraction which extends as far away from the focal animal as it is able to sense the focal animal will seek to move towards a neighbour The shape of these zones will necessarily be affected by the sensory capabilities of a given animal For example the visual field of a bird does not extend behind its body Fish rely on both vision and on hydrodynamic perceptions relayed through their lateral lines while Antarctic krill rely both on vision and hydrodynamic signals relayed through antennae However recent studies of starling flocks have shown that each bird modifies its position relative to the six or seven animals directly surrounding it no matter how close or how far away those animals are 5 Interactions between flocking starlings are thus based on a topological rather than a metric rule It remains to be seen whether this applies to other animals Another recent study based on an analysis of high speed camera footage of flocks above Rome and assuming minimal behavioural rules has convincingly simulated a number of aspects of flock behaviour 6 7 8 9 Evolutionary models edit In order to gain insight into why animals evolve swarming behaviours scientists have turned to evolutionary models that simulate populations of evolving animals Typically these studies use a genetic algorithm to simulate evolution over many generations These studies have investigated a number of hypotheses attempting to explain why animals evolve swarming behaviours such as the selfish herd theory 10 11 12 13 14 the predator confusion effect 15 16 the dilution effect 17 18 and the many eyes theory 19 Agents edit Main article Agent based model in biology See also Agent based models Intelligent agent Autonomous agent and Quorum sensing Mach Robert Schweitzer Frank 2003 Multi Agent Model of Biological Swarming Advances In Artificial Life Lecture Notes in Computer Science Vol 2801 pp 810 820 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 87 8022 doi 10 1007 978 3 540 39432 7 87 ISBN 978 3 540 20057 4 Self organization edit nbsp Flocking birds are an example of self organization in biology See also Self organization and Biological organisation Emergence edit Main article Emergence The concept of emergence that the properties and functions found at a hierarchical level are not present and are irrelevant at the lower levels is often a basic principle behind self organizing systems 20 An example of self organization in biology leading to emergence in the natural world occurs in ant colonies The queen does not give direct orders and does not tell the ants what to do citation needed Instead each ant reacts to stimuli in the form of chemical scents from larvae other ants intruders food and buildup of waste and leaves behind a chemical trail which in turn provides a stimulus to other ants Here each ant is an autonomous unit that reacts depending only on its local environment and the genetically encoded rules for its variety Despite the lack of centralized decision making ant colonies exhibit complex behaviours and have even been able to demonstrate the ability to solve geometric problems For example colonies routinely find the maximum distance from all colony entrances to dispose of dead bodies Stigmergy edit Main article Stigmergy A further key concept in the field of swarm intelligence is stigmergy 21 22 Stigmergy is a mechanism of indirect coordination between agents or actions The principle is that the trace left in the environment by an action stimulates the performance of a next action by the same or a different agent In that way subsequent actions tend to reinforce and build on each other leading to the spontaneous emergence of coherent apparently systematic activity Stigmergy is a form of self organization It produces complex seemingly intelligent structures without need for any planning control or even direct communication between the agents As such it supports efficient collaboration between extremely simple agents who lack any memory intelligence or even awareness of each other 22 Swarm intelligence edit Main article Swarm intelligence Swarm intelligence is the collective behaviour of decentralized self organized systems natural or artificial The concept is employed in work on artificial intelligence The expression was introduced by Gerardo Beni and Jing Wang in 1989 in the context of cellular robotic systems 23 Swarm intelligence systems are typically made up of a population of simple agents such as boids interacting locally with one another and with their environment The agents follow very simple rules and although there is no centralized control structure dictating how individual agents should behave local and to a certain degree random interactions between such agents lead to the emergence of intelligent global behaviour unknown to the individual agents Swarm intelligence research is multidisciplinary It can be divided into natural swarm research studying biological systems and artificial swarm research studying human artefacts There is also a scientific stream attempting to model the swarm systems themselves and understand their underlying mechanisms and an engineering stream focused on applying the insights developed by the scientific stream to solve practical problems in other areas 24 Algorithms edit Swarm algorithms follow a Lagrangian approach or an Eulerian approach 25 The Eulerian approach views the swarm as a field working with the density of the swarm and deriving mean field properties It is a hydrodynamic approach and can be useful for modelling the overall dynamics of large swarms 26 27 28 However most models work with the Lagrangian approach which is an agent based model following the individual agents points or particles that make up the swarm Individual particle models can follow information on heading and spacing that is lost in the Eulerian approach 25 29 Ant colony optimization edit Main article Ant colony optimization algorithm External image nbsp Swarmanoid robots find shortest path over double bridge 30 Ant colony optimization is a widely used algorithm which was inspired by the behaviours of ants and has been effective solving discrete optimization problems related to swarming 31 The algorithm was initially proposed by Marco Dorigo in 1992 32 33 and has since been diversified to solve a wider class of numerical problems Species that have multiple queens may have a queen leaving the nest along with some workers to found a colony at a new site a process akin to swarming in honeybees 34 35 Ants are behaviourally unsophisticated collectively they perform complex tasks Ants have highly developed sophisticated sign based communication Ants communicate using pheromones trails are laid that can be followed by other ants Routing problem ants drop different pheromones used to compute the shortest path from source to destination s Rauch EM Millonas MM Chialvo DR 1995 Pattern formation and functionality in swarm models Physics Letters A 207 3 4 185 arXiv adap org 9507003 Bibcode 1995PhLA 207 185R doi 10 1016 0375 9601 95 00624 c S2CID 120567147 Self propelled particles edit Main article Self propelled particles External videos nbsp SPP model interactive simulation 36 needs Java The concept of self propelled particles SPP was introduced in 1995 by Tamas Vicsek et al 37 as a special case of the boids model introduced in 1986 by Reynolds 4 An SPP swarm is modelled by a collection of particles that move with a constant speed and respond to random perturbations by adopting at each time increment the average direction of motion of the other particles in their local neighbourhood 38 Simulations demonstrate that a suitable nearest neighbour rule eventually results in all the particles swarming together or moving in the same direction This emerges even though there is no centralized coordination and even though the neighbours for each particle constantly change over time 37 SPP models predict that swarming animals share certain properties at the group level regardless of the type of animals in the swarm 39 Swarming systems give rise to emergent behaviours which occur at many different scales some of which are both universal and robust It has become a challenge in theoretical physics to find minimal statistical models that capture these behaviours 40 41 Particle swarm optimization edit Main article Particle swarm optimization Particle swarm optimization is another algorithm widely used to solve problems related to swarms It was developed in 1995 by Kennedy and Eberhart and was first aimed at simulating the social behaviour and choreography of bird flocks and fish schools 42 43 The algorithm was simplified and it was observed to be performing optimization The system initially seeds a population with random solutions It then searches in the problem space through successive generations using stochastic optimization to find the best solutions The solutions it finds are called particles Each particle stores its position as well as the best solution it has achieved so far The particle swarm optimizer tracks the best local value obtained so far by any particle in the local neighbourhood The remaining particles then move through the problem space following the lead of the optimum particles At each time iteration the particle swarm optimiser accelerates each particle toward its optimum locations according to simple mathematical rules Particle swarm optimization has been applied in many areas It has few parameters to adjust and a version that works well for a specific applications can also work well with minor modifications across a range of related applications 44 A book by Kennedy and Eberhart describes some philosophical aspects of particle swarm optimization applications and swarm intelligence 45 An extensive survey of applications is made by Poli 46 47 Altruism edit Researchers in Switzerland have developed an algorithm based on Hamilton s rule of kin selection The algorithm shows how altruism in a swarm of entities can over time evolve and result in more effective swarm behaviour 48 49 Biological swarming edit nbsp Linear cluster of Ampyx priscus The earliest evidence of swarm behaviour in animals dates back about 480 million years Fossils of the trilobite Ampyx priscus have been recently described as clustered in lines along the ocean floor The animals were all mature adults and were all facing the same direction as though they had formed a conga line or a peloton It has been suggested they line up in this manner to migrate much as spiny lobsters migrate in single file queues 50 it has also been suggested that the formation is the precursor for mating 51 as with the fly Leptoconops torrens The findings suggest animal collective behaviour has very early evolutionary origins 52 Examples of biological swarming are found in bird flocks 53 fish schools 54 55 insect swarms 56 bacteria swarms 57 58 molds 59 molecular motors 60 quadruped herds 61 and people 62 63 64 65 Social insects edit source source source source Swarm of nematocera flying around a treetop The behaviour of social insects insects that live in colonies such as ants bees wasps and termites has always been a source of fascination for children naturalists and artists Individual insects seem to do their own thing without any central control yet the colony as a whole behaves in a highly coordinated manner 66 Researchers have found that cooperation at the colony level is largely self organized The group coordination that emerges is often just a consequence of the way individuals in the colony interact These interactions can be remarkably simple such as one ant merely following the trail left by another ant Yet put together the cumulative effect of such behaviours can solve highly complex problems such as locating the shortest route in a network of possible paths to a food source The organised behaviour that emerges in this way is sometimes called swarm intelligence a form of biological emergence 66 Ants edit See also Ant colony Ant colony optimization algorithm Ant mill and Ant robotics source source source source source source source source A swarm of weaver ants Oecophylla smaragdina transporting a dead gecko Individual ants do not exhibit complex behaviours yet a colony of ants collectively achieves complex tasks such as constructing nests taking care of their young building bridges and foraging for food A colony of ants can collectively select i e send most workers towards the best or closest food source from several in the vicinity 67 Such collective decisions are achieved using positive feedback mechanisms Selection of the best food source is achieved by ants following two simple rules First ants which find food return to the nest depositing a pheromone chemical More pheromone is laid for higher quality food sources 68 Thus if two equidistant food sources of different qualities are found simultaneously the pheromone trail to the better one will be stronger Ants in the nest follow another simple rule to favor stronger trails on average More ants then follow the stronger trail so more ants arrive at the high quality food source and a positive feedback cycle ensures resulting in a collective decision for the best food source If there are two paths from the ant nest to a food source then the colony usually selects the shorter path This is because the ants that first return to the nest from the food source are more likely to be those that took the shorter path More ants then retrace the shorter path reinforcing the pheromone trail 69 Army ants unlike most ant species do not construct permanent nests an army ant colony moves almost incessantly over the time it exists remaining in an essentially perpetual state of swarming Several lineages have independently evolved the same basic behavioural and ecological syndrome often referred to as legionary behaviour and may be an example of convergent evolution 70 The successful techniques used by ant colonies have been studied in computer science and robotics to produce distributed and fault tolerant systems for solving problems This area of biomimetics has led to studies of ant locomotion search engines that make use of foraging trails fault tolerant storage and networking algorithms 71 Honey bees edit nbsp Bees swarming on a tree Main articles Bees algorithm and Swarming honey bee In temperate climates honey bees usually form swarms in late spring A swarm typically contains about half the workers together with the old queen while the new queen stays back with the remaining workers in the original hive When honey bees emerge from a hive to form a swarm they may gather on a branch of a tree or on a bush only a few meters from the hive The bees cluster about the queen and send out 20 50 scouts to find suitable new nest locations The scouts are the most experienced foragers in the cluster If a scout finds a suitable location she returns to the cluster and promotes it by dancing a version of the waggle dance This dance conveys information about the quality direction and distance of the new site The more excited she is about her findings the more vigorously she dances If she can convince others they may take off and check the site she found If they approve they may promote it as well In this decision making process scouts check several sites often abandoning their own original site to promote the superior site of another scout Several different sites may be promoted by different scouts at first After some hours and sometimes days a preferred location eventually emerges from this decision making process When all scouts agree on the final location the whole cluster takes off and swarms to it Sometimes if no decision is reached the swarm will separate some bees going in one direction others going in another This usually results in failure with both groups dying A new location is typically a kilometre or more from the original hive though some species e g Apis dorsata 72 may establish new colonies within as little as 500 meters from the natal nest This collective decision making process is remarkably successful in identifying the most suitable new nest site and keeping the swarm intact A good hive site has to be large enough to accommodate the swarm about 15 litres in volume has to be well protected from the elements receive an optimal amount of sunshine be some height above the ground have a small entrance and be capable of resisting ant infestation that is why tree cavities are often selected 73 74 75 76 77 Non social insects edit Unlike social insects swarms of non social insects that have been studied primarily seem to function in contexts such as mating feeding predator avoidance and migration Moths edit Moths may exhibit synchronized mating during which pheromones released by females initiate searching and swarming behavior in males 78 Males sense pheromones with sensitive antennae and may track females as far as several kilometers away 79 Swarm mating involves female choice and male competition Only one male in the swarm typically the first will successfully copulate 80 Females maximize fitness benefits and minimize cost by governing the onset and magnitude of pheromone deployed Too little pheromone will not attract a mate too much allows less fit males to sense the signal 81 After copulation females lay the eggs on a host plant Quality of host plant may be a factor influencing the location of swarming and egg laying In one case researchers observed pink striped oakworm moths Anisota virginiensis swarming at a carrion site where decomposition likely increased soil nutrient levels and host plant quality 82 Flies edit Midges such as Tokunagayusurika akamusi form swarms dancing in the air Swarming serves multiple purposes including the facilitation of mating by attracting females to approach the swarm a phenomenon known as lek mating Such cloud like swarms often form in early evening when the sun is getting low at the tip of a bush on a hilltop over a pool of water or even sometimes above a person The forming of such swarms is not out of instinct but an adaptive behavior a consensus between the individuals within the swarms It is also suggested that swarming is a ritual because there is rarely any male midge by itself and not in a swarm This could have formed due to the benefit of lowering inbreeding by having males of various genes gathering in one spot 83 The genus Culicoides also known as biting midges have displayed swarming behavior which are believed to cause confusion in predators 84 Cockroaches edit Cockroaches leave chemical trails in their feces as well as emitting airborne pheromones for mating Other cockroaches will follow these trails to discover sources of food and water and also discover where other cockroaches are hiding Thus groups of cockroaches can exhibit emergent behaviour 85 in which group or swarm behaviour emerges from a simple set of individual interactions Cockroaches are mainly nocturnal and will run away when exposed to light A study tested the hypothesis that cockroaches use just two pieces of information to decide where to go under those conditions how dark it is and how many other cockroaches there are The study conducted by Jose Halloy and colleagues at the Free University of Brussels and other European institutions created a set of tiny robots that appear to the roaches as other roaches and can thus alter the roaches perception of critical mass The robots were also specially scented so that they would be accepted by the real roaches 86 Locusts edit See also Marching locusts nbsp A 19th century depiction of a swarm of desert locusts Locusts are the swarming phase of the short horned grasshoppers of the family Acrididae Some species can breed rapidly under suitable conditions and subsequently become gregarious and migratory They form bands as nymphs and swarms as adults both of which can travel great distances rapidly stripping fields and greatly damaging crops The largest swarms can cover hundreds of square miles and contain billions of locusts A locust can eat its own weight about 2 grams in plants every day That means one million locusts can eat more than one tonne of food each day and the largest swarms can consume over 100 000 tonnes each day 87 Swarming in locusts has been found to be associated with increased levels of serotonin which causes the locust to change colour eat much more become mutually attracted and breed much more easily Researchers propose that swarming behaviour is a response to overcrowding and studies have shown that increased tactile stimulation of the hind legs or in some species simply encountering other individuals causes an increase in levels of serotonin The transformation of the locust to the swarming variety can be induced by several contacts per minute over a four hour period 88 89 90 91 Notably an innate predisposition to aggregate has been found in hatchlings of the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria independent of their parental phase 92 An individual locust s response to a loss of alignment in the group appears to increase the randomness of its motion until an aligned state is again achieved This noise induced alignment appears to be an intrinsic characteristic of collective coherent motion 93 Migratory behavior edit nbsp Cluster of monarch butterflies Monarch butterflies migrate to Santa Cruz California where they overwinter Main article Insect migration See also Lepidoptera migration Insect migration is the seasonal movement of insects particularly those by species of dragonflies beetles butterflies and moths The distance can vary from species to species but in most cases these movements involve large numbers of individuals In some cases the individuals that migrate in one direction may not return and the next generation may instead migrate in the opposite direction This is a significant difference from bird migration Monarch butterflies are especially noted for their lengthy annual migration In North America they make massive southward migrations starting in August until the first frost A northward migration takes place in the spring The monarch is the only butterfly that migrates both north and south as the birds do on a regular basis But no single individual makes the entire round trip Female monarchs deposit eggs for the next generation during these migrations 94 The length of these journeys exceeds the normal lifespan of most monarchs which is less than two months for butterflies born in early summer The last generation of the summer enters into a non reproductive phase known as diapause and may live seven months or more 95 During diapause butterflies fly to one of many overwintering sites The generation that overwinters generally does not reproduce until it leaves the overwintering site sometime in February and March It is the second third and fourth generations that return to their northern locations in the United States and Canada in the spring How the species manages to return to the same overwintering spots over a gap of several generations is still a subject of research the flight patterns appear to be inherited based on a combination of the position of the sun in the sky 96 and a time compensated Sun compass that depends upon a circadian clock that is based in their antennae 97 98 Birds edit nbsp Recent studies of starling flocks have shown that each bird modifies its position relative to the six or seven animals directly surrounding it no matter how close or how far away those animals are 5 nbsp Murmurations of starlings Main article Flocking behaviour See also Flock birds Bird landings Bird strike Mixed species foraging flock and Mobbing behaviour 99 Bird migration edit nbsp Large bird typically migrate in V echelon formations There are significant aerodynamic gains All birds can see ahead and towards one side making a good arrangement for protection Main article Bird migration See also Reverse migration birds Approximately 1800 of the world s 10 000 bird species are long distance migrants 100 The primary motivation for migration appears to be food for example some hummingbirds choose not to migrate if fed through the winter Also the longer days of the northern summer provide extended time for breeding birds to feed their young This helps diurnal birds to produce larger clutches than related non migratory species that remain in the tropics As the days shorten in autumn the birds return to warmer regions where the available food supply varies little with the season These advantages offset the high stress physical exertion costs and other risks of the migration such as predation Many birds migrate in flocks For larger birds it is assumed that flying in flocks reduces energy costs The V formation is often supposed to boost the efficiency and range of flying birds particularly over long migratory routes All the birds except the first fly in the upwash from one of the wingtip vortices of the bird ahead The upwash assists each bird in supporting its own weight in flight in the same way a glider can climb or maintain height indefinitely in rising air Geese flying in a V formation save energy by flying in the updraft of the wingtip vortex generated by the previous animal in the formation Thus the birds flying behind do not need to work as hard to achieve lift Studies show that birds in a V formation place themselves roughly at the optimum distance predicted by simple aerodynamic theory 101 Geese in a V formation may conserve 12 20 of the energy they would need to fly alone 102 103 Red knots and dunlins were found in radar studies to fly 5 km per hour faster in flocks than when they were flying alone 104 The birds flying at the tips and at the front are rotated in a timely cyclical fashion to spread flight fatigue equally among the flock members The formation also makes communication easier and allows the birds to maintain visual contact with each other source source source source source source Common starlings External videos nbsp Lobster Migration scene from The Trials of Life Other animals may use similar drafting techniques when migrating Lobsters for example migrate in close single file formation lobster trains sometimes for hundreds of miles The Mediterranean and other seas present a major obstacle to soaring birds which must cross at the narrowest points Massive numbers of large raptors and storks pass through areas such as Gibraltar Falsterbo and the Bosphorus at migration times More common species such as the European honey buzzard can be counted in hundreds of thousands in autumn Other barriers such as mountain ranges can also cause funnelling particularly of large diurnal migrants This is a notable factor in the Central American migratory bottleneck This concentration of birds during migration can put species at risk Some spectacular migrants have already gone extinct the most notable being the passenger pigeon During migration the flocks were a mile 1 6 km wide and 300 miles 500 km long taking several days to pass and containing up to a billion birds Marine life edit Fish edit Main article Shoaling and schooling nbsp Schooling predator fish size up schooling anchovies External image nbsp Foraging efficiency 105 The term shoal can be used to describe any group of fish including mixed species groups while school is used for more closely knit groups of the same species swimming in a highly synchronised and polarised manner Fish derive many benefits from shoaling behaviour including defence against predators through better predator detection and by diluting the chance of capture enhanced foraging success and higher success in finding a mate 106 It is also likely that fish benefit from shoal membership through increased hydrodynamic efficiency 107 Fish use many traits to choose shoalmates Generally they prefer larger shoals shoalmates of their own species shoalmates similar in size and appearance to themselves healthy fish and kin when recognised The oddity effect posits that any shoal member that stands out in appearance will be preferentially targeted by predators This may explain why fish prefer to shoal with individuals that resemble them The oddity effect would thus tend to homogenise shoals 108 One puzzling aspect of shoal selection is how a fish can choose to join a shoal of animals similar to themselves given that it cannot know its own appearance Experiments with zebrafish have shown that shoal preference is a learned ability not innate A zebrafish tends to associate with shoals that resemble shoals in which it was reared a form of imprinting 109 Other open questions of shoaling behaviour include identifying which individuals are responsible for the direction of shoal movement In the case of migratory movement most members of a shoal seem to know where they are going In the case of foraging behaviour captive shoals of golden shiner a kind of minnow are led by a small number of experienced individuals who knew when and where food was available 110 Radakov estimated herring schools in the North Atlantic can occupy up to 4 8 cubic kilometres 1 2 cu mi with fish densities between 0 5 and 1 0 fish cubic metre totalling several billion fish in one school 111 See also Eel life history Partridge BL 1982 The structure and function of fish schools Scientific American June 114 123 Parrish JK Viscido SV Grunbaum D 2002 Self Organized Fish Schools An Examination of Emergent Properties PDF Biol Bull 202 3 296 305 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 116 1548 doi 10 2307 1543482 JSTOR 1543482 PMID 12087003 S2CID 377484 Fish migration edit Main article Fish migration See also Sardine run and Salmon run External image nbsp Video clip of the Sardine run 112 Between May and July huge numbers of sardines spawn in the cool waters of the Agulhas Bank and then follow a current of cold water northward along the east coast of South Africa This great migration called the sardine run creates spectacular feeding frenzies along the coastline as marine predators such as dolphins sharks and gannets attack the schools Krill edit nbsp Swarming krill Most krill small shrimp like crustaceans form large swarms sometimes reaching densities of 10 000 60 000 individual animals per cubic metre 113 114 115 Swarming is a defensive mechanism confusing smaller predators that would like to pick out single individuals The largest swarms are visible from space and can be tracked by satellite 116 One swarm was observed to cover an area of 450 square kilometres 175 square miles of ocean to a depth of 200 meters 650 feet and was estimated to contain over 2 million tons of krill 117 Recent research suggests that krill do not simply drift passively in these currents but actually modify them 117 Krill typically follow a diurnal vertical migration By moving vertically through the ocean on a 12 hour cycle the swarms play a major part in mixing deeper nutrient rich water with nutrient poor water at the surface 117 Until recently it has been assumed that they spend the day at greater depths and rise during the night toward the surface It has been found that the deeper they go the more they reduce their activity 118 apparently to reduce encounters with predators and to conserve energy Later work suggested that swimming activity in krill varied with stomach fullness Satiated animals that had been feeding at the surface swim less actively and therefore sink below the mixed layer 119 As they sink they produce faeces which may mean that they have an important role to play in the Antarctic carbon cycle Krill with empty stomachs were found to swim more actively and thus head towards the surface This implies that vertical migration may be a bi or tri daily occurrence Some species form surface swarms during the day for feeding and reproductive purposes even though such behaviour is dangerous because it makes them extremely vulnerable to predators 120 Dense swarms may elicit a feeding frenzy among fish birds and mammal predators especially near the surface When disturbed a swarm scatters and some individuals have even been observed to moult instantaneously leaving the exuvia behind as a decoy 121 In 2012 Gandomi and Alavi presented what appears to be a successful stochastic algorithm for modelling the behaviour of krill swarms The algorithm is based on three main factors i movement induced by the presence of other individuals ii foraging activity and iii random diffusion 122 Copepods edit nbsp This copepod has its antenna spread click to enlarge The antenna detects the pressure wave of an approaching fish See also Hunting copepods Copepods are a group of tiny crustaceans found in the sea and lakes Many species are planktonic drifting in sea waters and others are benthic living on the ocean floor Copepods are typically 1 to 2 millimetres 0 04 to 0 08 in long with a teardrop shaped body and large antennae Although like other crustaceans they have an armoured exoskeleton they are so small that in most species this thin armour and the entire body is almost totally transparent Copepods have a compound median single eye usually bright red in the centre of the transparent head Copepods also swarm For example monospecific swarms have been observed regularly around coral reefs and sea grass and in lakes Swarms densities were about one million copepods per cubic metre Typical swarms were one or two metres in diameter but some exceeded 30 cubic metres Copepods need visual contact to keep together and they disperse at night 123 Spring produces blooms of swarming phytoplankton which provide food for copepods Planktonic copepods are usually the dominant members of the zooplankton and are in turn major food organisms for many other marine animals In particular copepods are prey to forage fish and jellyfish both of which can assemble in vast million strong swarms Some copepods have extremely fast escape responses when a predator is sensed and can jump with high speed over a few millimetres see animated image below nbsp Photo School of herrings ram feeding on a swarm of copepods nbsp Animation showing how herrings hunting in a synchronised way can capture the very alert and evasive copepod click to view nbsp Swarms of jellyfish also prey on copepods Planktonic copepods are important to the carbon cycle Some scientists say they form the largest animal biomass on earth 124 They compete for this title with Antarctic krill Because of their smaller size and relatively faster growth rates however and because they are more evenly distributed throughout more of the world s oceans copepods almost certainly contribute far more to the secondary productivity of the world s oceans and to the global ocean carbon sink than krill and perhaps more than all other groups of organisms together The surface layers of the oceans are currently believed to be the world s largest carbon sink absorbing about 2 billion tonnes of carbon a year the equivalent to perhaps a third of human carbon emissions thus reducing their impact Many planktonic copepods feed near the surface at night then sink into deeper water during the day to avoid visual predators Their moulted exoskeletons faecal pellets and respiration at depth all bring carbon to the deep sea Algal blooms edit Many single celled organisms called phytoplankton live in oceans and lakes When certain conditions are present such as high nutrient or light levels these organisms reproduce explosively The resulting dense swarm of phytoplankton is called an algal bloom Blooms can cover hundreds of square kilometres and are easily seen in satellite images Individual phytoplankton rarely live more than a few days but blooms can last weeks 125 126 Plants edit Scientists have attributed swarm behavior to plants for hundreds of years In his 1800 book Phytologia or The philosophy of agriculture and gardening Erasmus Darwin wrote that plant growth resembled swarms observed elsewhere in nature 127 While he was referring to more broad observations of plant morphology and was focused on both root and shoot behavior recent research has supported this claim Roots in particular display observable swarm behavior growing in patterns that exceed the statistical threshold for random probability and indicate the presence of communication between individual root apexes The primary function of plant roots is the uptake of soil nutrients and it is this purpose which drives swarm behavior Plants growing in close proximity have adapted their growth to assure optimal nutrient availability This is accomplished by growing in a direction that optimizes the distance between nearby roots thereby increasing their chance of exploiting untapped nutrient reserves The action of this behavior takes two forms maximization of distance from and repulsion by neighboring root apexes 128 The transition zone of a root tip is largely responsible for monitoring for the presence of soil borne hormones signaling responsive growth patterns as appropriate Plant responses are often complex integrating multiple inputs to inform an autonomous response Additional inputs that inform swarm growth includes light and gravity both of which are also monitored in the transition zone of a root s apex 129 These forces act to inform any number of growing main roots which exhibit their own independent releases of inhibitory chemicals to establish appropriate spacing thereby contributing to a swarm behavior pattern Horizontal growth of roots whether in response to high mineral content in soil or due to stolon growth produces branched growth that establish to also form their own independent root swarms 130 Bacteria edit See also Swarming motility and Microbial intelligence Swarming also describes groupings of some kinds of predatory bacteria such as myxobacteria Myxobacteria swarm together in wolf packs actively moving using a process known as bacterial gliding and keeping together with the help of intercellular molecular signals 57 131 Mammals edit See also Herd Herd behaviour and Animal migration nbsp Sheep dogs here a Border Collie control the flocking behaviour of sheep nbsp Bats swarming out of a cave in Thailand Parrish JK Edelstein Keshet L 1999 Complexity pattern and evolutionary trade offs in animal aggregation PDF Science 284 5411 99 101 Bibcode 1999Sci 284 99P CiteSeerX 10 1 1 560 5229 doi 10 1126 science 284 5411 99 PMID 10102827 Archived from the original PDF on 20 July 2011 People edit nbsp Police protect Nick Altrock from an adoring crowd during baseball s 1906 World Series External images nbsp Mexican wave simulation 132 nbsp Rhythmic applause simulation 133 See also Crowd and Crowd simulation A collection of people can also exhibit swarm behaviour such as pedestrians 134 or soldiers swarming the parapets dubious discuss In Cologne Germany two biologists from the University of Leeds demonstrated flock like behaviour in humans The group of people exhibited similar behavioural pattern to a flock where if five percent of the flock changed direction the others would follow If one person was designated as a predator and everyone else was to avoid him the flock behaved very much like a school of fish 135 136 Understanding how humans interact in crowds is important if crowd management is to effectively avoid casualties at football grounds music concerts and subway stations 137 The mathematical modelling of flocking behaviour is a common technology and has found uses in animation Flocking simulations have been used in many films 138 to generate crowds which move realistically Tim Burton s Batman Returns was the first movie to make use of swarm technology for rendering realistically depicting the movements of a group of bats using the boids system The Lord of the Rings film trilogy made use of similar technology known as Massive during battle scenes Swarm technology is particularly attractive because it is cheap robust and simple An ant based computer simulation using only six interaction rules has also been used to evaluate aircraft boarding behaviour 139 Airlines have also used ant based routing in assigning aircraft arrivals to airport gates An airline system developed by Douglas A Lawson uses swarm theory or swarm intelligence the idea that a colony of ants works better than one alone Each pilot acts like an ant searching for the best airport gate The pilot learns from his experience what s the best for him and it turns out that that s the best solution for the airline Lawson explains As a result the colony of pilots always go to gates they can arrive and depart quickly The program can even alert a pilot of plane back ups before they happen We can anticipate that it s going to happen so we ll have a gate available says Lawson 140 Swarm behaviour occurs also in traffic flow dynamics such as the traffic wave Bidirectional traffic can be observed in ant trails 141 142 In recent years this behaviour has been researched for insight into pedestrian and traffic models 143 144 Simulations based on pedestrian models have also been applied to crowds which stampede because of panic 145 Herd behaviour in marketing has been used to explain the dependencies of customers mutual behaviour The Economist reported a recent conference in Rome on the subject of the simulation of adaptive human behaviour 146 It shared mechanisms to increase impulse buying and get people to buy more by playing on the herd instinct The basic idea is that people will buy more of products that are seen to be popular and several feedback mechanisms to get product popularity information to consumers are mentioned including smart card technology and the use of Radio Frequency Identification Tag technology A swarm moves model was introduced by a Florida Institute of Technology researcher which is appealing to supermarkets because it can increase sales without the need to give people discounts Helbing D Keltsch J Molnar P 1997 Modelling the evolution of human trail systems Nature 388 6637 47 50 arXiv cond mat 9805158 Bibcode 1997Natur 388 47H doi 10 1038 40353 PMID 9214501 S2CID 4364517 Helbing D Farkas I Vicsek T 2000 Simulating dynamical features of escape panic Nature 407 6803 487 490 arXiv cond mat 0009448 Bibcode 2000Natur 407 487H doi 10 1038 35035023 PMID 11028994 S2CID 310346 Helbing D Farkas IJ Vicsek T 2000 Freezing by heating in a driven mesoscopic system Physical Review Letters 84 6 1240 1243 arXiv cond mat 9904326 Bibcode 2000PhRvL 84 1240H doi 10 1103 PhysRevLett 84 1240 PMID 11017488 S2CID 18649078 Robotics editMain article Swarm robotics See also Ant robotics and Robotic materials nbsp Kilobot thousand robot swarm developed by Radhika Nagpal and Michael Rubenstein at Harvard University The application of swarm principles to robots is called swarm robotics while swarm intelligence refers to the more general set of algorithms External videos nbsp A Swarm of Nano Quadrotors YouTube 147 nbsp March of the microscopic robots Nature Video YouTube Partially inspired by colonies of insects such as ants and bees researchers are modelling the behaviour of swarms of thousands of tiny robots which together perform a useful task such as finding something hidden cleaning or spying Each robot is quite simple but the emergent behaviour of the swarm is more complex 1 The whole set of robots can be considered as one single distributed system in the same way an ant colony can be considered a superorganism exhibiting swarm intelligence The largest swarms so far created is the 1024 robot Kilobot swarm 148 Other large swarms include the iRobot swarm the SRI International ActivMedia Robotics Centibots project 149 and the Open source Micro robotic Project swarm which are being used to research collective behaviours 150 151 Swarms are also more resistant to failure Whereas one large robot may fail and ruin a mission a swarm can continue even if several robots fail This could make them attractive for space exploration missions where failure is normally extremely costly 152 In addition to ground vehicles swarm robotics includes also research of swarms of aerial robots 147 153 and heterogeneous teams of ground and aerial vehicles 154 155 In contrast macroscopic robots colloidal particles at microscale can also be adopted as agents to perform collective behaviors to conduct tasks using mechanical and physical approaches such as reconfigurable tornado like microswarm 156 mimicking schooling fish 157 hierarchical particle species 158 mimicking predating behavior of mammals micro object manipulation using a transformable microswarm 159 The fabrication of such colloidal particles is usually based on chemical synthesis Military edit nbsp Contrast between guerrilla ambush and true swarming Edwards 2003 Main article Swarming military Military swarming is a behaviour where autonomous or partially autonomous units of action attack an enemy from several different directions and then regroup Pulsing where the units shift the point of attack is also a part of military swarming Military swarming involves the use of a decentralized force against an opponent in a manner that emphasizes mobility communication unit autonomy and coordination or synchronization 160 Historically military forces used principles of swarming without really examining them explicitly but now active research consciously examines military doctrines that draw ideas from swarming Merely because multiple units converge on a target they are not necessarily swarming Siege operations do not involve swarming because there is no manoeuvre there is convergence but on the besieged fortification Nor do guerrilla ambushes constitute swarms because they are hit and run Even though the ambush may have several points of attack on the enemy the guerillas withdraw when they either have inflicted adequate damage or when they are endangered In 2014 the U S Office of Naval Research released a video showing tests of a swarm of small autonomous drone attack boats that can steer and take coordinated offensive action as a group 161 Gallery edit nbsp A swarm of migrating herrings nbsp A swarm of bees nbsp Salps arranged in chains form huge swarms 162 nbsp People swarming through an exit do not always behave like a fluid 163 164 nbsp A swarm of ladybirds nbsp A swarm of robots nbsp A swarm of earthquakes nbsp A swarm of ancient starsMyths editThere is a popular myth that lemmings commit mass suicide by swarming off cliffs when they migrate Driven by strong biological urges some species of lemmings may migrate in large groups when population density becomes too great Lemmings can swim and may choose to cross a body of water in search of a new habitat In such cases many may drown if the body of water is so wide as to stretch their physical capability to the limit This fact combined with some unexplained fluctuations in the population of Norwegian lemmings gave rise to the myth 165 Piranha have a reputation as fearless fish that swarm in ferocious and predatory packs However recent research which started with the premise that they school as a means of cooperative hunting discovered that they were in fact rather fearful fish like other fish who schooled for protection from their predators such as cormorants caimans and dolphins A researcher described them as basically like regular fish with large teeth 166 See also editActive matter Matter behavior at system scale Dyson swarm Hypothetical megastructure around a starPages displaying short descriptions of redirect targets List of collective nouns in English Mobile Bay jubilee Natural phenomenon that occurs in Mobile Bay Alabama United States Population cycle Swarm simulation open source agent based simulation toolkitPages displaying wikidata descriptions as a fallback Swirlonic state recently 2020 recognised new state of self propelled particles Traffic wave Type of highway congestion Swarmalators Agents that do swarming and synchronization simultaneously References edit a b Bouffanais Roland 2016 Design and Control of Swarm Dynamics SpringerBriefs in Complexity First ed Springer doi 10 1007 978 981 287 751 2 ISBN 978 981 287 750 5 O Loan Evans 1998 Alternating steady state in one dimensional flocking Journal of Physics A Mathematical and General 32 8 L99 L105 arXiv cond mat 9811336 Bibcode 1999JPhA 32L 99O doi 10 1088 0305 4470 32 8 002 S2CID 7642063 Attanasi A Cavagna A Del Castello L Giardina I Grigera T S Jelic A Melillo S Parisi L Pohl O Shen E Viale M September 2014 Information transfer and behavioural inertia in starling flocks Nature Physics 10 9 691 696 arXiv 1303 7097 Bibcode 2014NatPh 10 691A doi 10 1038 nphys3035 PMC 4173114 PMID 25264452 a b c Reynolds CW 1987 Flocks herds and schools A distributed behavioral model Proceedings of the 14th annual conference on Computer graphics and interactive techniques Vol 21 pp 25 34 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 103 7187 doi 10 1145 37401 37406 ISBN 978 0 89791 227 3 S2CID 546350 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a journal ignored help a b Ballerini M Cabibbo N Candelier R Cavagna A Cisbani E Giardina I Lecomte V Orlandi A Parisi G Procaccini A Viale M Zdravkovic V 2008 Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance Evidence from a field study Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105 4 1232 7 arXiv 0709 1916 Bibcode 2008PNAS 105 1232B doi 10 1073 pnas 0711437105 PMC 2234121 PMID 18227508 Hildenbrandt H Carere C Hemelrijk CK 2010 Self organized aerial displays of thousands of starlings a model Behavioral Ecology 21 6 1349 1359 arXiv 0908 2677 doi 10 1093 beheco arq149 Hemelrijk CK Hildenbrandt H 2011 Some causes of the variable shape of flocks of birds PLOS ONE 6 8 e22479 Bibcode 2011PLoSO 622479H doi 10 1371 journal pone 0022479 PMC 3150374 PMID 21829627 Zwermen en scholen Swarming Permanente expo Bezoek onze expo s amp workshops Science LinX Rijksuniversiteit Groningen 10 November 2007 Onderzoek aan de Faculteit Wiskunde en Natuurwetenschappen Faculteit Wiskunde en Natuurwetenschappen Over ons Rijksuniversiteit Groningen 25 October 2012 Yang W Schmickl T 2019 Collective Motion as an Ultimate Effect in Crowded Selfish Herds Scientific Reports 9 1 6618 Bibcode 2019NatSR 9 6618Y doi 10 1038 s41598 019 43179 6 PMC 6488663 PMID 31036873 Olson RS Knoester DB Adami C 2013 Critical interplay between density dependent predation and evolution of the selfish herd Proceedings of the 15th annual conference on Genetic and evolutionary computation Gecco 13 pp 247 254 doi 10 1145 2463372 2463394 ISBN 9781450319638 S2CID 14414033 Ward CR Gobet F Kendall G 2001 Evolving collective behavior in an artificial ecology Artificial Life 7 2 191 209 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 108 3956 doi 10 1162 106454601753139005 PMID 11580880 S2CID 12133884 Reluga TC Viscido S 2005 Simulated evolution of selfish herd behavior Journal of Theoretical Biology 234 2 213 225 Bibcode 2005JThBi 234 213R doi 10 1016 j jtbi 2004 11 035 PMID 15757680 Wood AJ Ackland GJ 2007 Evolving the selfish herd emergence of distinct aggregating strategies in an individual based model Proc Biol Sci 274 1618 1637 1642 doi 10 1098 rspb 2007 0306 PMC 2169279 PMID 17472913 Olson RS Hintze A Dyer FC Knoester DB Adami C 2013 Predator confusion is sufficient to evolve swarming behaviour J R Soc Interface 10 85 20130305 doi 10 1098 rsif 2013 0305 PMC 4043163 PMID 23740485 Demsar J Hemelrijk CK Hildenbrandt H Bajec IL 2015 Simulating predator attacks on schools Evolving composite tactics PDF Ecological Modelling 304 22 33 doi 10 1016 j ecolmodel 2015 02 018 hdl 11370 0bfcbb69 a101 4ec1 833a df301e49d8ef S2CID 46988508 Tosh CR 2011 Which conditions promote negative density dependent selection on prey aggregations PDF Journal of Theoretical Biology 281 1 24 30 Bibcode 2011JThBi 281 24T doi 10 1016 j jtbi 2011 04 014 PMID 21540037 Ioannou CC Guttal V Couzin ID 2012 Predatory Fish Select for Coordinated Collective Motion in Virtual Prey Science 337 6099 1212 1215 Bibcode 2012Sci 337 1212I doi 10 1126 science 1218919 PMID 22903520 S2CID 10203872 Olson RS Haley PB Dyer FC Adami C 2015 Exploring the evolution of a trade off between vigilance and foraging in group living organisms Royal Society Open Science 2 9 150135 arXiv 1408 1906 Bibcode 2015RSOS 250135O doi 10 1098 rsos 150135 PMC 4593673 PMID 26473039 Hierarchy of Life 14 September 2008 Archived from the original on 3 July 2016 Retrieved 6 October 2009 Parunak H v D 2003 Making swarming happen In Proceedings of Conference on Swarming and Network Enabled Command Control Communications Computers Intelligence Surveillance and Reconnaissance C4ISR McLean Virginia USA 3 January 2003 a b Marsh L Onof C 2008 Stigmergic epistemology stigmergic cognition PDF Cognitive Systems Research 9 1 136 149 doi 10 1016 j cogsys 2007 06 009 S2CID 23140721 Beni G Wang J Swarm Intelligence in Cellular Robotic Systems Proceed NATO Advanced Workshop on Robots and Biological Systems Tuscany Italy June 26 30 1989 Dorigo M Birattari M 2007 Swarm intelligence Scholarpedia 2 9 1462 Bibcode 2007SchpJ 2 1462D doi 10 4249 scholarpedia 1462 a b Li YX Lukeman R Edelstein Keshet L 2007 Minimal mechanisms for school formation in self propelled particles PDF Physica D Nonlinear Phenomena 237 5 699 720 Bibcode 2008PhyD 237 699L doi 10 1016 j physd 2007 10 009 Toner J and Tu Y 1995 Long range order in a two dimensional xy model how birds fly together Physical Revue Letters 75 23 1995 4326 4329 Topaz C Bertozzi A 2004 Swarming patterns in a two dimensional kinematic model for biological groups SIAM J Appl Math 65 1 152 174 Bibcode 2004APS MAR t9004T CiteSeerX 10 1 1 88 3071 doi 10 1137 S0036139903437424 S2CID 18468679 Topaz C Bertozzi A Lewis M 2006 A nonlocal continuum model for biological aggregation Bull Math Biol 68 7 1601 1623 arXiv q bio 0504001 doi 10 1007 s11538 006 9088 6 PMID 16858662 S2CID 14750061 Carrillo J Fornasier M Toscani G 2010 Particle kinetic and hydrodynamic models of swarming PDF Mathematical Modeling of Collective Behavior in Socio Economic and Life Sciences Modeling and Simulation in Science Engineering and Technology Vol 3 pp 297 336 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 193 5047 doi 10 1007 978 0 8176 4946 3 12 ISBN 978 0 8176 4945 6 Swarmanoid project Ant colony optimization Retrieved 15 December 2010 A Colorni M Dorigo et V Maniezzo Distributed Optimization by Ant Colonies actes de la premiere conference europeenne sur la vie artificielle Paris Elsevier Publishing 134 142 1991 M Dorigo Optimization Learning and Natural Algorithms PhD thesis Politecnico di Milano Italie 1992 Holldobler amp Wilson 1990 pp 143 179 DORIGO M DI CARO G GAMBERELLA L M 1999 Ant Algorithms for Discrete Optimization Artificial Life MIT Press Self driven particle model Archived 2012 10 14 at the Wayback Machine Interactive simulations 2005 University of Colorado Retrieved 10 April 2011 a b Vicsek T Czirok A Ben Jacob E Cohen I Shochet O 1995 Novel type of phase transition in a system of self driven particles Physical Review Letters 75 6 1226 1229 arXiv cond mat 0611743 Bibcode 1995PhRvL 75 1226V doi 10 1103 PhysRevLett 75 1226 PMID 10060237 S2CID 15918052 Czirok A Vicsek T 2006 Collective behavior of interacting self propelled particles Physica A 281 1 4 17 29 arXiv cond mat 0611742 Bibcode 2000PhyA 281 17C doi 10 1016 S0378 4371 00 00013 3 S2CID 14211016 Buhl J Sumpter DJT Couzin D Hale JJ Despland E Miller ER Simpson SJ et al 2006 From disorder to order in marching locusts PDF Science 312 5778 1402 1406 Bibcode 2006Sci 312 1402B doi 10 1126 science 1125142 PMID 16741126 S2CID 359329 Archived from the original PDF on 29 September 2011 Retrieved 13 April 2011 Toner J Tu Y Ramaswamy S 2005 Hydrodynamics and phases of flocks PDF Annals of Physics 318 1 170 244 Bibcode 2005AnPhy 318 170T doi 10 1016 j aop 2005 04 011 Archived from the original PDF on 18 July 2011 Retrieved 13 April 2011 Bertin E Droz Gregoire G 2009 Hydrodynamic equations for self propelled particles microscopic derivation and stability analysis J Phys A 42 44 445001 arXiv 0907 4688 Bibcode 2009JPhA 42R5001B doi 10 1088 1751 8113 42 44 445001 S2CID 17686543 Kennedy J Eberhart R 1995 Particle Swarm Optimization Proceedings of IEEE International Conference on Neural Networks Vol IV pp 1942 1948 Kennedy J 1997 The particle swarm social adaptation of knowledge Proceedings of IEEE International Conference on Evolutionary Computation pp 303 308 Hu X Particle swarm optimization Tutorial Retrieved 15 December 2010 Kennedy J Eberhart R C 2001 Swarm Intelligence Morgan Kaufmann ISBN 978 1 55860 595 4 Poli R 2007 An analysis of publications on particle swarm optimisation applications PDF Technical Report CSM 469 Archived from the original PDF on 16 July 2011 Retrieved 15 December 2010 Poli R 2008 Analysis of the publications on the applications of particle swarm optimisation PDF Journal of Artificial Evolution and Applications 2008 1 10 doi 10 1155 2008 685175 Altruism helps swarming robots fly better Archived 2012 09 15 at the Wayback Machine genevalunch com 4 May 2011 Waibel M Floreano D Keller L 2011 A quantitative test of Hamilton s rule for the evolution of altruism PLOS Biology 9 5 1000615 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 1000615 PMC 3086867 PMID 21559320 Herrnkind W 1969 Queuing behavior of spiny lobsters Science 164 3886 1425 1427 Bibcode 1969Sci 164 1425H doi 10 1126 science 164 3886 1425 PMID 5783720 S2CID 10324354 Fossil conga lines reveal origins of animal swarms National Geographic 17 October 2019 Vannier J Vidal M Marchant R El Hariri K Kouraiss K Pittet B El Albani A Mazurier A Martin E 2019 Collective behaviour in 480 million year old trilobite arthropods from Morocco Scientific Reports 9 1 14941 Bibcode 2019NatSR 914941V doi 10 1038 s41598 019 51012 3 PMC 6797724 PMID 31624280 Feare C 1984 The Starling Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 217705 6 Partridge BL 1982 The structure and function of fish schools PDF Scientific American Vol 246 no 6 pp 114 123 Bibcode 1982SciAm 246f 114P doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0682 114 PMID 7201674 Archived from the original PDF on 3 July 2011 Hubbard S Babak P Sigurdsson S Magnusson K 2004 A model of the formation of fish schools and migrations of fish Ecol Model 174 4 359 374 doi 10 1016 j ecolmodel 2003 06 006 Rauch E Millonas M Chialvo D 1995 Pattern formation and functionality in swarm models Physics Letters A 207 3 4 185 193 arXiv adap org 9507003 Bibcode 1995PhLA 207 185R doi 10 1016 0375 9601 95 00624 C S2CID 120567147 a b Allison C Hughes C 1991 Bacterial swarming an example of prokaryotic differentiation and multicellular behaviour Science Progress 75 298 Pt 3 4 403 422 PMID 1842857 Ben Jacob E Cohen I Shochet O Czirok A Vicsek T 1995 Cooperative Formation of Chiral Patterns during Growth of Bacterial Colonies Physical Review Letters 75 15 2899 2902 Bibcode 1995PhRvL 75 2899B doi 10 1103 PhysRevLett 75 2899 PMID 10059433 Rappel WJ Nicol A Sarkissian A Levine H Loomis WF 1999 Self organized vortex state in two dimensional Dictyostelium dynamics Physical Review Letters 83 6 1247 1250 arXiv patt sol 9811001 Bibcode 1999PhRvL 83 1247R doi 10 1103 PhysRevLett 83 1247 S2CID 1590827 Chowdhury D 2006 Collective effects in intra cellular molecular motor transport coordination cooperation and competition Physica A 372 1 84 95 arXiv physics 0605053 Bibcode 2006PhyA 372 84C doi 10 1016 j physa 2006 05 005 S2CID 14822256 Parrish JK and Hamner WM eds 1997 Animal Groups in Three Dimensions Cambridge University Press ISBN 978 0 521 46024 8 Helbing D Keltsch J Molnar P 1997 Modelling the evolution of human trail systems Nature 388 6637 47 50 arXiv cond mat 9805158 Bibcode 1997Natur 388 47H doi 10 1038 40353 PMID 9214501 S2CID 4364517 Helbing D Farkas I Vicsek T 2000 Simulating dynamical features of escape panic Nature 407 6803 487 490 arXiv cond mat 0009448 Bibcode 2000Natur 407 487H doi 10 1038 35035023 PMID 11028994 S2CID 310346 Helbing D Farkas IJ Vicsek T 2000 Freezing by heating in a driven mesoscopic system Physical Review Letters 84 6 1240 1243 arXiv cond mat 9904326 Bibcode 2000PhRvL 84 1240H doi 10 1103 PhysRevLett 84 1240 PMID 11017488 S2CID 18649078 Swarm Theory National Geographic Feature article July 2007 Beekman M Sword GA and Simpson SK 2008 Biological Foundations of Swarm Intelligence In Swarm intelligence introduction and applications Eds Blum C and Merkle D シュプリンガー ジャパン株式会社 Page 3 43 ISBN 978 3 540 74088 9 Parrish JK Edelstein Keshet L 1999 Complexity pattern and evolutionary trade offs in animal aggregation PDF Science 284 5411 99 101 Bibcode 1999Sci 284 99P CiteSeerX 10 1 1 560 5229 doi 10 1126 science 284 5411 99 PMID 10102827 Archived from the original PDF on 20 July 2011 a b Bonabeau E and Theraulaz G 2008 Swarm Smarts In Your Future with Robots Scientific American Special Editions Czaczkes T J Gruter C Ratnieks F L W 2015 Trail pheromones an integrative view of their role in colony organisation Annual Review of Entomology 60 581 599 doi 10 1146 annurev ento 010814 020627 PMID 25386724 S2CID 37972066 Beckers R Deneubourg J L Goss S 1993 Modulation of trail laying in the ant Lasius niger Hymenoptera Formicidae and its role in the collective selection of a food source Journal of Insect Behavior 6 6 751 759 Bibcode 1993JIBeh 6 751B CiteSeerX 10 1 1 161 9541 doi 10 1007 BF01201674 S2CID 14670150 Goss S Aron S Deneubourg J L Pasteels J M 1989 Self organized shortcuts in the Argentine ant PDF Naturwissenschaften 76 12 579 581 Bibcode 1989NW 76 579G doi 10 1007 BF00462870 S2CID 18506807 Brady Sean G 2003 Evolution of the army ant syndrome The origin and long term evolutionary stasis of a complex of behavioral and reproductive adaptations Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100 11 6575 9 Bibcode 2003PNAS 100 6575B doi 10 1073 pnas 1137809100 PMC 164488 PMID 12750466 Dicke E Byde A Cliff D Layzell P 2004 An ant inspired technique for storage area network design In A J Ispeert M Murata N Wakamiya eds Proceedings of Biologically Inspired Approaches to Advanced Information Technology First International Workshop BioADIT 2004 LNCS 3141 pp 364 379 ISBN 9783540233398 Oldroyd BP 1998 Colony relatedness in aggregations of Apis dorsata Fabricius Hymenoptera Apidae Insectes Sociaux 47 94 95 doi 10 1007 s000400050015 S2CID 40346464 Milius Susan Swarm Savvy Archived 27 September 2012 at the Wayback Machine How bees ants and other animals avoid dumb collective decisions Science News May 9th 2009 Vol 175 10 p 16 Bee Swarms Follow High speed Streaker Bees To Find A New Nest ScienceDaily Nov 24 2008 Seeley Thomas D Visscher P Kirk 2003 Choosing a home how the scouts in a honey bee swarm perceive the completion of their group decision making PDF Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 54 5 511 520 doi 10 1007 s00265 003 0664 6 S2CID 16948152 Archived from the original PDF on 31 January 2009 Retrieved 14 December 2010 Morse R A 1963 Swarm orientation in honeybees Science 141 3578 357 358 Bibcode 1963Sci 141 357M doi 10 1126 science 141 3578 357 PMID 17815993 S2CID 46516976 Seeley Thomas 2003 Consensus building during nest site selection in honey bee swarms The expiration of dissent Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 53 6 417 424 doi 10 1007 s00265 003 0598 z S2CID 34798300 Stepien T L Zmurchok C Hengenius J B Caja Rivera R M D Orsogna M R Lindsay A E 2000 Moth Mating Modeling Female Pheromone Calling and Male Navigational Strategies to Optimize Reproductive Success Applied Sciences 10 18 6543 doi 10 3390 app10186543 Badeke B Haverkamp A Sachse S A 2016 A challenge for a male noctuid moth Discerning the female sex pheromone against the background of plant volatiles Frontiers in Physiology 7 143 doi 10 3389 fphys 2016 00143 PMC 4843018 PMID 27199761 Greenfield M D 1981 Moth sex pheromones an evolutionary perspective The Florida Entomologist 64 1 4 17 doi 10 2307 3494597 JSTOR 3494597 Umbers K D L Symonds M R E Kokko H 2015 The Mothematics of female pheromone signaling Strategies for aging virgins American Naturalist 185 3 417 432 doi 10 1086 679614 hdl 1885 13166 PMID 25674695 S2CID 13846468 Mason D S Baruzzi C 2019 Love in strange places Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 17 3 184 Bibcode 2019FrEE 17 184M doi 10 1002 fee 2027 Midges MDC Discover Nature Retrieved 19 November 2019 Kirkeby Carsten 30 June 2018 Observations of female and mixed sex swarming behaviour in Culicoides LATREILLE 1809 Diptera Ceratopogonidae Polish Journal of Entomology 87 2 191 197 doi 10 2478 pjen 2018 0014 Jennifer Viegas Cockroaches Make Group Decisions Discovery Channel Archived from the original on 4 July 2008 Retrieved 10 June 2006 Lemonick Michael D 15 November 2007 Robotic Roaches Do the Trick Time Archived from the original on 16 November 2007 Locust Locustidae National Geographic Retrieved 12 December 2010 Locust swarms high on serotonin 29 January 2009 via BBC Rogers SM Matheson T Despland E Dodgson T Burrows M Simpson SJ November 2003 Mechanosensory induced behavioural gregarization in the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria J Exp Biol 206 Pt 22 3991 4002 doi 10 1242 jeb 00648 PMID 14555739 Stevenson PA 2009 The Key to Pandora s Box Science 323 5914 594 5 doi 10 1126 science 1169280 PMID 19179520 S2CID 39306643 Blocking happiness chemical may prevent locust plagues New Scientist 2009 01 29 accessed 2009 01 31 Moshe Guershon Amir Ayali May 2012 Innate phase behavior in the desert locust Schistocerca gregaria Insect Science 19 6 649 656 Bibcode 2012InsSc 19 649G doi 10 1111 j 1744 7917 2012 01518 x S2CID 83412818 Yates CA Erban R Escudero C Couzin ID Buhl J Kevrekidis IG Maini PK Sumpter DJ 2009 Inherent noise can facilitate coherence in collective swarm motion Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 106 14 5464 9 Bibcode 2009PNAS 106 5464Y doi 10 1073 pnas 0811195106 PMC 2667078 PMID 19336580 Pyle Robert Michael National Audubon Society Field Guide to North American Butterflies p712 713 Alfred A Knopf New York ISBN 0 394 51914 0 Monarch Danaus plexippus Archived from the original on 15 December 2012 Retrieved 27 August 2008 Gugliotta Guy 2003 Butterflies Guided By Body Clocks Sun Scientists Shine Light on Monarchs Pilgrimage Archived 2006 03 05 at the Wayback Machine Washington Post May 23 2003 page A03 Retrieved 2006 JAN 07 Merlin C Gegear RJ Reppert SM 2009 Antennal Circadian Clocks Coordinate Sun Compass Orientation in Migratory Monarch Butterflies Science 325 5948 1700 1704 Bibcode 2009Sci 325 1700M doi 10 1126 science 1176221 PMC 2754321 PMID 19779201 Kyriacou CP 2009 Unraveling Traveling Science 325 5948 1629 1630 doi 10 1126 science 1178935 PMID 19779177 S2CID 206522416 Nagy M Akos Zs Biro D Vicsek T 2010 Hierarchical group dynamics in pigeon flocks PDF Nature 464 7290 890 893 arXiv 1010 5394 Bibcode 2010Natur 464 890N doi 10 1038 nature08891 PMID 20376149 S2CID 4430488 Archived from the original PDF on 6 July 2010 Supplementary pdf Sekercioglu C H 2007 Conservation ecology area trumps mobility in fragment bird extinctions Current Biology 17 8 R283 R286 doi 10 1016 j cub 2007 02 019 PMID 17437705 S2CID 744140 Drag Reduction from Formation Flight Flying Aircraft in Bird Like Formations Could Significantly Increase Range Defense Technical Information Center April 2002 Retrieved February 27 2008 Hummel D Beukenberg M 1989 Aerodynamische Interferenzeffekte beim Formationsfl ug von Vogeln J Ornithol 130 1 15 24 doi 10 1007 BF01647158 S2CID 823269 Cutts C J amp J R Speakman 1994 Energy savings in formation flight of Pink footed Geese PDF J Exp Biol 189 1 251 261 doi 10 1242 jeb 189 1 251 PMID 9317742 Newton I 2008 The Migration Ecology of Birds Elselvier ISBN 978 0 12 517367 4 Pitcher et al 1982 Pitcher TJ and Parish JK 1993 Functions of shoaling behaviour in teleosts In Pitcher TJ ed Behaviour of teleost fishes Chapman and Hall New York pp 363 440 Hoare DJ Krause J Peuhkuri N and Godin JGJ 2000 Body size and shoaling in fish Journal of Fish Biology 57 6 1351 1366 Snekser JL Ruhl N Bauer K McRobert SP 2010 The Influence of Sex and Phenotype on Shoaling Decisions in Zebrafish PDF International Journal of Comparative Psychology 23 70 81 doi 10 46867 IJCP 2010 23 01 04 Archived from the original PDF on 25 July 2011 Engeszer RE Ryan MJ Parichy DM 2004 Learned Social Preference in Zebrafish PDF Current Biology 14 10 881 884 doi 10 1016 j cub 2004 04 042 PMID 15186744 S2CID 18741014 Reebs S G 2000 Can a minority of informed leaders determine the foraging movements of a fish shoal Animal Behaviour 59 2 403 9 doi 10 1006 anbe 1999 1314 PMID 10675263 S2CID 4945309 Radakov DV 1973 Schooling in the ecology of fish Israel Program for Scientific Translation translated by Mill H Halsted Press New York ISBN 978 0 7065 1351 6 Photographer Mark van Coller Hamner WM Hamner PP Strand SW Gilmer RW 1983 Behavior of Antarctic Krill Euphausia superba Chemoreception Feeding Schooling and Molting Science 220 4595 433 5 Bibcode 1983Sci 220 433H doi 10 1126 science 220 4595 433 PMID 17831417 S2CID 22161686 U Kils P Marshall 1995 Der Krill wie er schwimmt und frisst neue Einsichten mit neuen Methoden The Antarctic krill how it swims and feeds new insights with new methods In I Hempel G Hempel eds Biologie der Polarmeere Erlebnisse und Ergebnisse Biology of the Polar Oceans Experiences and Results Fischer Verlag pp 201 210 ISBN 978 3 334 60950 7 R Piper 2007 Extraordinary Animals An Encyclopedia of Curious and Unusual Animals Greenwood Press ISBN 978 0 313 33922 6 Hoare Ben 2009 Animal Migration London Natural History Museum p 107 ISBN 978 0 565 09243 6 a b c Hoare Ben 2009 Animal Migration London Natural History Museum p 107 ISBN 978 0 565 09243 6 J S Jaffe M D Ohmann A de Robertis 1999 Sonar estimates of daytime activity levels of Euphausia pacifica in Saanich Inlet PDF Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 56 11 2000 10 doi 10 1139 cjfas 56 11 2000 S2CID 228567512 Archived from the original PDF on 20 July 2011 Geraint A Tarling amp Magnus L Johnson 2006 Satiation gives krill that sinking feeling Current Biology 16 3 83 4 doi 10 1016 j cub 2006 01 044 PMID 16461267 Howard D Krill pp 133 140 in Karl H A et al eds Beyond the Golden Gate Oceanography Geology Biology and Environmental Issues in the Gulf of the Farallones USGS Circular 1198 2001 URLs last accessed 2010 06 04 D Howard Krill in Cordell Bank National Marine Sanctuary NOAA Retrieved 15 June 2005 Gandomi A H Alavi A H 2012 Krill Herd Algorithm A New Bio Inspired Optimization Algorithm Communications in Nonlinear Science and Numerical Simulation 17 12 4831 4845 Bibcode 2012CNSNS 17 4831G doi 10 1016 j cnsns 2012 05 010 Hamner WM Carleton JH 1979 Copepod swarms Attributes and role in coral reef ecosystems Limnol Oceanogr 24 1 1 14 Bibcode 1979LimOc 24 1H doi 10 4319 lo 1979 24 1 0001 Johannes Durbaum amp Thorsten Kunnemann 5 November 1997 Biology of Copepods An Introduction Carl von Ossietzky University of Oldenburg Archived from the original on 26 May 2010 Retrieved 8 December 2009 Lindsey R and Scott M 2010 What are phytoplankton NASA Earth Observatory Harmful algal blooms in the Great Lakes Archived 2010 06 16 at the Wayback Machine 2009 NOAA Center of Excellence for Great Lakes and Human Health Darwin Erasmus 1 January 1800 Phytologia Or The Philosophy of Agriculture and Gardening With the Theory of Draining Morasses and with an Improved Construction of the Drill Plough P Byrne Ciszak Marzena Comparini Diego Mazzolai Barbara Baluska Frantisek Arecchi F Tito Vicsek Tamas Mancuso Stefano 17 January 2012 Swarming Behavior in Plant Roots PLOS ONE 7 1 e29759 Bibcode 2012PLoSO 729759C doi 10 1371 journal pone 0029759 ISSN 1932 6203 PMC 3260168 PMID 22272246 Baluska Frantisek Mancuso Stefano Volkmann Dieter Barlow Peter W 1 July 2010 Root apex transition zone a signalling response nexus in the root Trends in Plant Science 15 7 402 408 doi 10 1016 j tplants 2010 04 007 PMID 20621671 J Trewavas A 2014 Plant behaviour and intelligence Oxford university press ISBN 9780199539543 OCLC 961862730 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link Reichenbach H 2001 Myxobacteria producers of novel bioactive substances J Ind Microbiol Biotechnol 27 3 149 56 doi 10 1038 sj jim 7000025 PMID 11780785 S2CID 34964313 Farkas I Helbing D Vicsek T 2002 Mexican waves in an excitable medium PDF Nature 419 6903 131 132 arXiv cond mat 0210073 Bibcode 2002Natur 419 131F doi 10 1038 419131a PMID 12226653 S2CID 4309609 Archived from the original PDF on 10 July 2007 Neda Z Ravasz E Brechet Y Vicsek T Barabasi AL 2002 Physics of Rhythmic Applause PDF Physical Review E 61 6 6987 6992 arXiv cond mat 0006423 Bibcode 2000PhRvE 61 6987N doi 10 1103 physreve 61 6987 PMID 11088392 S2CID 14135891 Archived from the original PDF on 11 June 2011 Helbing D Keltsch J Molnar P 1997 Modelling the evolution of human trail systems Nature 388 6637 47 50 arXiv cond mat 9805158 Bibcode 1997Natur 388 47H doi 10 1038 40353 PMID 9214501 S2CID 4364517 http psychcentral com news 2008 02 15 herd mentality explained 1922 html Archived 29 November 2014 at the Wayback Machine Retrieved on October 31st 2008 Danger in numbers during Haj The National 6 September 2009 Couzin ID Krause J 2003 Self organization and collective behavior in vertebrates PDF Advances in the Study of Behavior Vol 32 pp 1 75 doi 10 1016 S0065 3454 03 01001 5 ISBN 978 0 12 004532 7 Archived from the original PDF on 13 March 2012 Retrieved 14 April 2011 Gabbai J M E 2005 Complexity and the Aerospace Industry Understanding Emergence by Relating Structure to Performance using Multi Agent Systems Thesis Manchester University of Manchester Doctoral Thesis Archived from the original on 19 December 2014 Retrieved 11 July 2009 Livermore R 2008 A multi agent system approach to a simulation study comparing the performance of aircraft boarding using pre assigned seating and free for all strategies Open University Technical report No 2008 25 Planes Trains and Ant Hills Computer scientists simulate activity of ants to reduce airline delays Archived 2010 11 24 at the Wayback Machine Science Daily 1 April 2008 Burd Martin N Aranwela February 2003 Head on encounter rates and walking speed of foragers in leaf cutting ant traffic Insectes Sociaux 50 1 3 8 doi 10 1007 s000400300001 S2CID 23876486 Ribeiro Pedro Andre Frazao Helene Gilberto Xavier Carlos Navas Fernando Leite Ribeiro 1 April 2009 Dornhaus Anna ed Ants can learn to forage on one way trails PLOS ONE 4 4 e5024 Bibcode 2009PLoSO 4 5024R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0005024 PMC 2659768 PMID 19337369 John Alexander Andreas Schadschneider Debashish Chowdhury Katsuhiro Nishinari March 2008 Characteristics of ant inspired traffic flow Swarm Intelligence 2 1 25 41 arXiv 0903 1434 doi 10 1007 s11721 008 0010 8 S2CID 18350336 Are we nearly there yet Motorists could learn a thing or two from ants The Economist 10 July 2009 Helbing Dirk Farkas Illes Vicsek Tamas 2000 Simulating dynamical features of escape panic Nature 407 6803 487 490 arXiv cond mat 0009448 Bibcode 2000Natur 407 487H doi 10 1038 35035023 PMID 11028994 S2CID 310346 Swarming the shelves How shops can exploit people s herd mentality to increase sales The Economist 11 November 2006 p 90 a b Kushleyev Alex Mellinger Daniel Powers Caitlin Kumar Vijay 2013 Towards a swarm of agile micro quadrotors Autonomous Robots 35 4 287 300 doi 10 1007 s10514 013 9349 9 S2CID 18340816 Self organizing Systems Research Group Archived from the original on 26 October 2014 Centibots 100 Robot Collaborative Reconnaissance Project ActivMedia Robotics Archived from the original on 14 July 2011 Open source micro robotic project Retrieved 28 October 2007 Swarm iRobot Corporation Archived from the original on 27 September 2007 Retrieved 28 October 2007 Knapp Louise 21 December 2000 Look Up in the Sky Robofly Wired Retrieved 25 September 2008 Saska Martin Jan Vakula Libor Preucil 2014 Swarms of micro aerial vehicles stabilized under a visual relative localization IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation ICRA Saska Martin Vonasek Vojtech Krajnik Tomas Preucil Libor 2014 Coordination and navigation of heterogeneous MAV UGV formations localized by a hawk eye like approach under a model predictive control scheme PDF International Journal of Robotics Research 33 10 1393 1412 doi 10 1177 0278364914530482 S2CID 1195374 Saska Martin Vonasek Vojtech Krajnik Tomas Preucil Libor 2012 Coordination and Navigation of Heterogeneous UAVs UGVs Teams Localized by a Hawk Eye Approach IEEE RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems IROS Ji Fengtong Jin Dongdong Wang Ben Zhang Li 23 June 2020 Light Driven Hovering of a Magnetic Microswarm in Fluid ACS Nano 14 6 6990 6998 doi 10 1021 acsnano 0c01464 ISSN 1936 0851 PMID 32463226 S2CID 218976382 Hughes Robin 22 February 2007 Barracuda Tornado retrieved 7 February 2022 Mou Fangzhi Li Xiaofeng Xie Qi Zhang Jianhua Xiong Kang Xu Leilei Guan Jianguo 20 December 2019 Active Micromotor Systems Built from Passive Particles with Biomimetic Predator Prey Interactions ACS Nano 14 1 406 414 doi 10 1021 acsnano 9b05996 ISSN 1936 0851 PMID 31860277 S2CID 209435036 Yu Jiangfan Wang Ben Du Xingzhou Wang Qianqian Zhang Li 21 August 2018 Ultra extensible ribbon like magnetic microswarm Nature Communications 9 1 3260 Bibcode 2018NatCo 9 3260Y doi 10 1038 s41467 018 05749 6 ISSN 2041 1723 PMC 6104072 PMID 30131487 Edwards Sean J A 2000 Swarming on the Battlefield Past Present and Future Rand Monograph MR 1100 Rand Corporation ISBN 978 0 8330 2779 5 U S Navy could swarm foes with robot boats CNN 13 October 2014 Dive and Discover Scientific Expedition 10 Antarctica Retrieved 3 September 2008 Crowd modelling Simulating the behaviour of crowds of people or swarms of animals has both frivolous and important uses The Economist 5 March 2009 Fisher Len 2009 The perfect swarm the science of complexity in everyday life Page 57 Basic Books ISBN 978 0 465 01884 0 Woodford Riley Lemming Suicide Myth Disney Film Faked Bogus Behavior Archived from the original on 3 January 2010 a href Template Cite web html title Template Cite web cite web a CS1 maint unfit URL link Red Bellied Piranha Is Really Yellow New York Times 24 May 2005 Sources edit Blum C and Merkle D 2008 Swarm intelligence introduction and applications Springer ISBN 978 3 540 74088 9 Camazine S Deneubourg JL Franks NR Sneyd J Theraulaz G and Bonabeau E 2003 Self Organization in Biological Systems Princeton University Press ISBN 978 0 691 11624 2 Fisher L 2009 The perfect swarm the science of complexity in everyday life Basic Books ISBN 978 0 465 01884 0 Kennedy JF Kennedy J Eberhart RC and Shi Y 2001 Swarm intelligence Morgan Kaufmann ISBN 978 1 55860 595 4 Krause J 2005 Living in Groups Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 850818 2 Lim CP Jain LC and Dehuri S 2009 Innovations in Swarm Intelligence Springer ISBN 978 3 642 04224 9 Miller Peter 2010 The Smart Swarm How understanding flocks schools and colonies can make us better at communicating decision making and getting things done Penguin ISBN 978 1 58333 390 7 Nedjah N and Mourelle LdM 2006 Swarm intelligent systems Springer ISBN 978 3 540 33868 0 Sumpter David JT 2010 Collective Animal Behavior Princeton University Press ISBN 978 0 691 14843 4 Vicsek A Zafeiris A 2012 Collective motion Physics Reports 517 3 4 71 140 arXiv 1010 5017 Bibcode 2012PhR 517 71V doi 10 1016 j physrep 2012 03 004 S2CID 119109873 External links editNew York Times article on investigations into swarming From the Wolfram Demonstrations Project requires CDF player free Model of a Firefly Swarm Garbage Collection by Ants Beverton and Merging Schools of Fish Propp Circles Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Swarm behaviour amp oldid 1222010412, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.