fbpx
Wikipedia

Aggression

Aggression is hostile and antagonistic behavior, often with the intent to cause harm, although it can be channeled into creative and practical outlets for some.[1] It may occur either reactively or without provocation. In humans, aggression can be caused by various triggers. For example, built-up frustration due to blocked goals or perceived disrespect.[2] Human aggression can be classified into direct and indirect aggression; whilst the former is characterized by physical or verbal behavior intended to cause harm to someone, the latter is characterized by behavior intended to harm the social relations of an individual or group.[3][4][5]

Many mammals, such as the dog, bare their teeth as a sign of aggression; a form of aposematism.
Two warthogs preparing to fight

In definitions commonly used in the social sciences and behavioral sciences, aggression is an action or response by an individual that delivers something unpleasant to another person.[6] Some definitions include that the individual must intend to harm another person.[7]

In an interdisciplinary perspective, aggression is regarded as "an ensemble of mechanism formed during the course of evolution in order to assert oneself, relatives or friends against others, to gain or to defend resources (ultimate causes) by harmful damaging means. These mechanisms are often motivated by emotions like fear, frustration, anger, feelings of stress, dominance or pleasure (proximate causes). Sometimes aggressive behavior serves as a stress relief or a subjective feeling of power."[8][9] Predatory or defensive behavior between members of different species may not be considered aggression in the same sense.

Aggression can take a variety of forms, which may be expressed physically, or communicated verbally or non-verbally: including anti-predator aggression, defensive aggression (fear-induced), predatory aggression, dominance aggression, inter-male aggression, resident-intruder aggression, maternal aggression, species-specific aggression, sex-related aggression, territorial aggression, isolation-induced aggression, irritable aggression, and brain-stimulation-induced aggression (hypothalamus). There are two subtypes of human aggression: (1) controlled-instrumental subtype (purposeful or goal-oriented); and (2) reactive-impulsive subtype (often elicits uncontrollable actions that are inappropriate or undesirable). Aggression differs from what is commonly called assertiveness, although the terms are often used interchangeably among laypeople (as in phrases such as "an aggressive salesperson").[10]

Overview edit

Dollard et al. (1939) proposed that aggression was due to frustration, which was described as an unpleasant emotion resulting from any interference with achieving a rewarding goal.[11] Berkowitz[12] extended this frustration–aggression hypothesis and proposed that it is not so much the frustration as the unpleasant emotion that evokes aggressive tendencies, and that all aversive events produce negative affect and thereby aggressive tendencies, as well as fear tendencies. Besides conditioned stimuli, Archer categorized aggression-evoking (as well as fear-evoking) stimuli into three groups; namely, pain, novelty, and frustration, although he also described "looming", which refers to an object rapidly moving towards the visual sensors of a subject, and can be categorized as "intensity."[13]

Aggression can have adaptive benefits or negative effects. Aggressive behavior is an individual or collective social interaction that is a hostile behavior with the intention of inflicting damage or harm.[3][4] Two broad categories of aggression are commonly distinguished. One includes affective (emotional) and hostile, reactive, or retaliatory aggression that is a response to provocation, and the other includes instrumental, goal-oriented or predatory, in which aggression is used as a means to achieve a goal.[14] An example of hostile aggression would be a person who punches someone who insulted him or her. An instrumental form of aggression would be armed robbery. Research on violence from a range of disciplines lend some support to a distinction between affective and predatory aggression.[15] However, some researchers question the usefulness of a hostile versus instrumental distinction in humans, despite its ubiquity in research, because most real-life cases involve mixed motives and interacting causes.[16]

A number of classifications and dimensions of aggression have been suggested. These depend on such things as whether the aggression is verbal or physical; whether or not it involves relational aggression such as covert bullying and social manipulation;[17] whether harm to others is intended or not; whether it is carried out actively or expressed passively; and whether the aggression is aimed directly or indirectly. Classification may also encompass aggression-related emotions (e.g. anger) and mental states (e.g. impulsivity, hostility).[18] Aggression may occur in response to non-social as well as social factors, and can have a close relationship with stress coping style.[19] Aggression may be displayed in order to intimidate.

The operative definition of aggression may be affected by moral or political views. Examples are the axiomatic moral view called the non-aggression principle and the political rules governing the behavior of one country toward another.[20] Likewise in competitive sports, or in the workplace, some forms of aggression may be sanctioned and others not (see Workplace aggression).[21] Aggressive behaviors are associated with adjustment problems and several psychopathological symptoms such as Antisocial Personality Disorder, Borderline Personality Disorder, and Intermittent Explosive Disorder.[22]

Biological approaches conceptualize aggression as an internal energy released by external stimuli, a product of evolution through natural selection, part of genetics, a product of hormonal fluctuations. Psychological approaches conceptualize aggression as a destructive instinct, a response to frustration, an affect excited by a negative stimulus, a result of observed learning of society and diversified reinforcement, a resultant of variables that affect personal and situational environments.[23][24]

Etymology edit

The term aggression comes from the Latin word aggressio, meaning attack. The Latin was itself a joining of ad- and gradi-, which meant step at. The first known use dates back to 1611, in the sense of an unprovoked attack.[25] A psychological sense of "hostile or destructive behavior" dates back to a 1912 English translation of Sigmund Freud's writing.[26] Alfred Adler theorized about an "aggressive drive" in 1908. Child raising experts began to refer to aggression, rather than anger, from the 1930s.[27]

Ethology edit

 
Male elephant seals fighting

Ethologists study aggression as it relates to the interaction and evolution of animals in natural settings. In such settings aggression can involve bodily contact such as biting, hitting or pushing, but most conflicts are settled by threat displays and intimidating thrusts that cause no physical harm. This form of aggression may include the display of body size, antlers, claws or teeth; stereotyped signals including facial expressions; vocalizations such as bird song; the release of chemicals; and changes in coloration.[28] The term agonistic behaviour is sometimes used to refer to these forms of behavior.

Most ethologists believe that aggression confers biological advantages. Aggression may help an animal secure territory, including resources such as food and water. Aggression between males often occurs to secure mating opportunities, and results in selection of the healthier/more vigorous animal. Aggression may also occur for self-protection or to protect offspring.[29] Aggression between groups of animals may also confer advantage; for example, hostile behavior may force a population of animals into a new territory, where the need to adapt to a new environment may lead to an increase in genetic flexibility.[30]

Between species and groups edit

The most apparent type of interspecific aggression is that observed in the interaction between a predator and its prey. However, according to many researchers, predation is not aggression. A cat does not hiss or arch its back when pursuing a rat, and the active areas in its hypothalamus resemble those that reflect hunger rather than those that reflect aggression.[31] However, others refer to this behavior as predatory aggression, and point out cases that resemble hostile behavior, such as mouse-killing by rats.[32] In aggressive mimicry a predator has the appearance of a harmless organism or object attractive to the prey; when the prey approaches, the predator attacks.

An animal defending against a predator may engage in either "fight or flight" or "tend and befriend" in response to predator attack or threat of attack, depending on its estimate of the predator's strength relative to its own. Alternative defenses include a range of antipredator adaptations, including alarm signals. An example of an alarm signal is nerol, a chemical which is found in the mandibular glands of Trigona fulviventris individuals.[33] Release of nerol by T. fulviventris individuals in the nest has been shown to decrease the number of individuals leaving the nest by fifty percent, as well as increasing aggressive behaviors like biting.[33] Alarm signals like nerol can also act as attraction signals; in T. fulviventris, individuals that have been captured by a predator may release nerol to attract nestmates, who will proceed to attack or bite the predator.[33]

Aggression between groups is determined partly by willingness to fight, which depends on a number of factors including numerical advantage, distance from home territories, how often the groups encounter each other, competitive abilities, differences in body size, and whose territory is being invaded.[34] Also, an individual is more likely to become aggressive if other aggressive group members are nearby.[35] One particular phenomenon – the formation of coordinated coalitions that raid neighbouring territories to kill conspecifics – has only been documented in two species in the animal kingdom: 'common' chimpanzees and humans.[36]

Within a group edit

Aggression between conspecifics in a group typically involves access to resources and breeding opportunities. One of its most common functions is to establish a dominance hierarchy. This occurs in many species by aggressive encounters between contending males when they are first together in a common environment.[37] Usually the more aggressive animals become the more dominant.[38][39] In test situations, most of the conspecific aggression ceases about 24 hours after the group of animals is brought together.[37][40] Aggression has been defined from this viewpoint as "behavior which is intended to increase the social dominance of the organism relative to the dominance position of other organisms".[41] Losing confrontations may be called social defeat, and winning or losing is associated with a range of practical and psychological consequences.[42]

Conflicts between animals occur in many contexts, such as between potential mating partners, between parents and offspring, between siblings and between competitors for resources. Group-living animals may dispute over the direction of travel or the allocation of time to joint activities. Various factors limit the escalation of aggression, including communicative displays, conventions, and routines. In addition, following aggressive incidents, various forms of conflict resolution have been observed in mammalian species, particularly in gregarious primates. These can mitigate or repair possible adverse consequences, especially for the recipient of aggression who may become vulnerable to attacks by other members of a group. Conciliatory acts vary by species and may involve specific gestures or simply more proximity and interaction between the individuals involved. However, conflicts over food are rarely followed by post conflict reunions, even though they are the most frequent type in foraging primates.[43]

Other questions that have been considered in the study of primate aggression, including in humans, is how aggression affects the organization of a group, what costs are incurred by aggression, and why some primates avoid aggressive behavior.[44] For example, bonobo chimpanzee groups are known for low levels of aggression within a partially matriarchal society. Captive animals including primates may show abnormal levels of social aggression and self-harm that are related to aspects of the physical or social environment; this depends on the species and individual factors such as gender, age and background (e.g. raised wild or captive).[45]

Aggression, fear and curiosity edit

Within ethology, it has long been recognized that there is a relation between aggression, fear, and curiosity.[46] A cognitive approach to this relationship puts aggression in the broader context of inconsistency reduction, and proposes that aggressive behavior is caused by an inconsistency between a desired, or expected, situation and the actually perceived situation (e.g., "frustration"), and functions to forcefully manipulate the perception into matching the expected situation.[47][13][48] In this approach, when the inconsistency between perception and expectancy is small, learning as a result of curiosity reduces inconsistency by updating expectancy to match perception. If the inconsistency is larger, fear or aggressive behavior may be employed to alter the perception in order to make it match expectancy, depending on the size of the inconsistency as well as the specific context. Uninhibited fear results in fleeing, thereby removing the inconsistent stimulus from the perceptual field and resolving the inconsistency. In some cases thwarted escape may trigger aggressive behavior in an attempt to remove the thwarting stimulus.[48]

Evolutionary explanations edit

Like many behaviors, aggression can be examined in terms of its ability to help an animal itself survive and reproduce, or alternatively to risk survival and reproduction. This cost–benefit analysis can be looked at in terms of evolution. However, there are profound differences in the extent of acceptance of a biological or evolutionary basis for human aggression.[49]

According to the male warrior hypothesis, intergroup aggression represents an opportunity for men to gain access to mates, territory, resources and increased status. As such, conflicts may have created selection evolutionary pressures for psychological mechanisms in men to initiate intergroup aggression.[50][51]

Violence and conflict edit

Aggression can involve violence that may be adaptive under certain circumstances in terms of natural selection. This is most obviously the case in terms of attacking prey to obtain food, or in anti-predatory defense. It may also be the case in competition between members of the same species or subgroup, if the average reward (e.g. status, access to resources, protection of self or kin) outweighs average costs (e.g. injury, exclusion from the group, death). There are some hypotheses of specific adaptions for violence in humans under certain circumstances, including for homicide, but it is often unclear what behaviors may have been selected for and what may have been a byproduct, as in the case of collective violence.[52][53][54][55]

Although aggressive encounters are ubiquitous in the animal kingdom, with often high stakes, most encounters that involve aggression may be resolved through posturing, or displaying and trial of strength. Game theory is used to understand how such behaviors might spread by natural selection within a population, and potentially become 'Evolutionary Stable Strategies'. An initial model of resolution of conflicts is the hawk-dove game. Others include the Sequential assessment model and the Energetic war of attrition. These try to understand not just one-off encounters but protracted stand-offs, and mainly differ in the criteria by which an individual decides to give up rather than risk loss and harm in physical conflict (such as through estimates of resource holding potential).[56]

Gender edit

General edit

Gender plays an important role in human aggression. There are multiple theories that seek to explain findings that males and females of the same species can have differing aggressive behaviors. One review concluded that male aggression tended to produce pain or physical injury whereas female aggression tended towards psychological or social harm.[57]

In general, sexual dimorphism can be attributed to greater intraspecific competition in one sex, either between rivals for access to mates and/or to be chosen by mates. This may stem from the other gender being constrained by providing greater parental investment, in terms of factors such as gamete production, gestation, lactation, or upbringing of young. Although there is much variation in species, generally the more physically aggressive sex is the male, particularly in mammals. In species where parental care by both sexes is required, there tends to be less of a difference. When the female can leave the male to care for the offspring, then females may be the larger and more physically aggressive. Competitiveness despite parental investment has also been observed in some species.[58] A related factor is the rate at which males and females are able to mate again after producing offspring, and the basic principles of sexual selection are also influenced by ecological factors affecting the ways or extent to which one sex can compete for the other. The role of such factors in human evolution is controversial.

The pattern of male and female aggression is argued to be consistent with evolved sexually-selected behavioral differences, while alternative or complementary views emphasize conventional social roles stemming from physical evolved differences.[59] Aggression in women may have evolved to be, on average, less physically dangerous and more covert or indirect.[60][61] However, there are critiques for using animal behavior to explain human behavior, especially in the application of evolutionary explanations to contemporary human behavior, including differences between the genders.[62]

According to the 2015 International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, sex differences in aggression is one of the most robust and oldest findings in psychology.[63] Past meta-analyses in the encyclopedia found males regardless of age engaged in more physical and verbal aggression while small effect for females engaging in more indirect aggression such as rumor spreading or gossiping.[63] It also found males tend to engage in more unprovoked aggression at higher frequency than females.[63] This analysis also conforms with the Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology which reviewed past analysis which found men to use more verbal and physical aggression with the difference being greater in the physical type.[64] There are more recent findings that show that differences in male and female aggression appear at about two years of age, though the differences in aggression are more consistent in middle-aged children and adolescence. Tremblay, Japel and Pérusse (1999) asserted that physically aggressive behaviors such as kicking, biting and hitting are age-typical expressions of innate and spontaneous reactions to biological drives such as anger, hunger, and affiliation.[65] Girls' relational aggression, meaning non-physical or indirect, tends to increase after age two while physical aggression decreases. There was no significant difference in aggression between males and females before two years of age.[66] A possible explanation for this could be that girls develop language skills more quickly than boys, and therefore have better ways of verbalizing their wants and needs. They are more likely to use communication when trying to retrieve a toy with the words "Ask nicely" or "Say please."[67]

According to the journal of Aggressive Behaviour, an analysis across 9 countries found boys reported more in the use of physical aggression.[66] At the same time no consistent sex differences emerged within relational aggression.[66] It has been found that girls are more likely than boys to use reactive aggression and then retract, but boys are more likely to increase rather than to retract their aggression after their first reaction. Studies show girls' aggressive tactics included gossip, ostracism, breaking confidences, and criticism of a victim's clothing, appearance, or personality, whereas boys engage in aggression that involves a direct physical and/or verbal assault.[68] This could be due to the fact that girls' frontal lobes develop earlier than boys, allowing them to self-restrain.[67]

One factor that shows insignificant differences between male and female aggression is in sports. In sports, the rate of aggression in both contact and non-contact sports is relatively equal. Since the establishment of Title IX, female sports have increased in competitiveness and importance, which could contribute to the evening of aggression and the "need to win" attitude between both genders. Among sex differences found in adult sports were that females have a higher scale of indirect hostility while men have a higher scale of assault.[69] Another difference found is that men have up to 20 times higher levels of testosterone than women.

In intimate relationships edit

Some studies suggest that romantic involvement in adolescence decreases aggression in males and females, but decreases at a higher rate in females. Females will seem more desirable to their mate if they fit in with society and females that are aggressive do not usually fit well in society. They can often be viewed as antisocial. Female aggression is not considered the norm in society and going against the norm can sometimes prevent one from getting a mate.[70] However, studies have shown that an increasing number of women are getting arrested for domestic violence charges. In many states, women now account for a quarter to a third of all domestic violence arrests, up from less than 10 percent a decade ago. The new statistics reflect a reality documented in research: women are perpetrators as well as victims of family violence.[71] However, another equally possible explanation is a case of improved diagnostics: it has become more acceptable for men to report female domestic violence to the authorities while at the same time actual female domestic violence has not increased at all.[72] This could be the case in a situation where men had become less ashamed of reporting female violence against themsuch a situation could conceivably lead to an increasing number of women being arrested despite the actual number of violent women remaining the same.

In addition, males in competitive sports are often advised by their coaches not to be in intimate relationships based on the premises that they become more docile and less aggressive during an athletic event. The circumstances in which males and females experience aggression are also different. A study showed that social anxiety and stress was positively correlated with aggression in males, meaning as stress and social anxiety increases so does aggression. Furthermore, a male with higher social skills has a lower rate of aggressive behavior than a male with lower social skills. In females, higher rates of aggression were only correlated with higher rates of stress. Other than biological factors that contribute to aggression there are physical factors as well.[73]

Physiological factors edit

Regarding sexual dimorphism, humans fall into an intermediate group with moderate sex differences in body size but relatively large testes. This is a typical pattern of primates where several males and females live together in a group and the male faces an intermediate number of challenges from other males compared to exclusive polygyny and monogamy but frequent sperm competition.[74]

Evolutionary psychology and sociobiology have also discussed and produced theories for some specific forms of male aggression such as sociobiological theories of rape and theories regarding the Cinderella effect. Another evolutionary theory explaining gender differences in aggression is the Male Warrior hypothesis, which explains that males have psychologically evolved for intergroup aggression in order to gain access to mates, resources, territory and status.[50][51]

Physiology edit

Brain pathways edit

Many researchers focus on the brain to explain aggression. Numerous circuits within both neocortical and subcortical structures play a central role in controlling aggressive behavior, depending on the species, and the exact role of pathways may vary depending on the type of trigger or intention.[75][4]

In mammals, the hypothalamus and periaqueductal gray of the midbrain are critical areas, as shown in studies on cats, rats, and monkeys. These brain areas control the expression of both behavioral and autonomic components of aggression in these species, including vocalization. Electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus causes aggressive behavior[76] and the hypothalamus has receptors that help determine aggression levels based on their interactions with serotonin and vasopressin.[77] In rodents, activation of estrogen receptor-expressing neurons in the ventrolateral portion of the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMHvl) was found to be sufficient to initiate aggression in both males and females.[78][79] Midbrain areas involved in aggression have direct connections with both the brainstem nuclei controlling these functions, and with structures such as the amygdala and prefrontal cortex.

Stimulation of the amygdala results in augmented aggressive behavior in hamsters,[80][81] while lesions of an evolutionarily homologous area in the lizard greatly reduce competitive drive and aggression (Bauman et al. 2006).[82] In rhesus monkeys, neonatal lesions in the amygdala or hippocampus results in reduced expression of social dominance, related to the regulation of aggression and fear.[83] Several experiments in attack-primed Syrian golden hamsters, for example, support the claim of circuitry within the amygdala being involved in control of aggression.[81] The role of the amygdala is less clear in primates and appears to depend more on situational context, with lesions leading to increases in either social affiliatory or aggressive responses. Amygdalotomy, which involves removing or destroying parts of the amygdala, has been performed on people to reduce their violent behaviour.

The broad area of the cortex known as the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is crucial for self-control and inhibition of impulses, including inhibition of aggression and emotions. Reduced activity of the prefrontal cortex, in particular its medial and orbitofrontal portions, has been associated with violent/antisocial aggression.[84] In addition, reduced response inhibition has been found in violent offenders, compared to non-violent offenders.[75]

The role of the chemicals in the brain, particularly neurotransmitters, in aggression has also been examined. This varies depending on the pathway, the context and other factors such as gender. A deficit in serotonin has been theorized to have a primary role in causing impulsivity and aggression. At least one epigenetic study supports this supposition.[85] Nevertheless, low levels of serotonin transmission may explain a vulnerability to impulsiveness, potential aggression, and may have an effect through interactions with other neurochemical systems. These include dopamine systems which are generally associated with attention and motivation toward rewards, and operate at various levels. Norepinephrine, also known as noradrenaline, may influence aggression responses both directly and indirectly through the hormonal system, the sympathetic nervous system or the central nervous system (including the brain). It appears to have different effects depending on the type of triggering stimulus, for example social isolation/rank versus shock/chemical agitation which appears not to have a linear relationship with aggression. Similarly, GABA, although associated with inhibitory functions at many CNS synapses, sometimes shows a positive correlation with aggression, including when potentiated by alcohol.[86][87]

The hormonal neuropeptides vasopressin and oxytocin play a key role in complex social behaviours in many mammals such as regulating attachment, social recognition, and aggression. Vasopressin has been implicated in male-typical social behaviors which includes aggression. Oxytocin may have a particular role in regulating female bonds with offspring and mates, including the use of protective aggression. Initial studies in humans suggest some similar effects.[88][89]

In human, aggressive behavior has been associated with abnormalities in three principal regulatory systems in the body serotonin systems, catecholamine systems, and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Abnormalities in these systems also are known to be induced by stress, either severe, acute stress or chronic low-grade stress[90]

Testosterone edit

Early androgenization has an organizational effect on the developing brains of both males and females, making more neural circuits that control sexual behavior as well as intermale and interfemale aggression become more sensitive to testosterone.[91] There are noticeable sex differences in aggression. Testosterone is present to a lesser extent in females, who may be more sensitive to its effects. Animal studies have also indicated a link between incidents of aggression and the individual level of circulating testosterone. However, results in relation to primates, particularly humans, are less clear cut and are at best only suggestive of a positive association in some contexts.[92]

In humans, there is a seasonal variation in aggression associated with changes in testosterone.[93] For example, in some primate species, such as rhesus monkeys and baboons, females are more likely to engage in fights around the time of ovulation as well as right before menstruation.[91] If the results were the same in humans as they are in rhesus monkeys and baboons, then the increase in aggressive behaviors during ovulation is explained by the decline in estrogen levels. This makes normal testosterone levels more effective.[94] Castrated mice and rats exhibit lower levels of aggression. Males castrated as neonates exhibit low levels of aggression even when given testosterone throughout their development.

Challenge hypothesis edit

The challenge hypothesis outlines the dynamic relationship between plasma testosterone levels and aggression in mating contexts in many species. It proposes that testosterone is linked to aggression when it is beneficial for reproduction, such as in mate guarding and preventing the encroachment of intrasexual rivals. The challenge hypothesis predicts that seasonal patterns in testosterone levels in a species are a function of mating system (monogamy versus polygyny), paternal care, and male-male aggression in seasonal breeders. This pattern between testosterone and aggression was first observed in seasonally breeding birds, such as the song sparrow, where testosterone levels rise modestly with the onset of the breeding season to support basic reproductive functions.[95] The hypothesis has been subsequently expanded and modified to predict relationships between testosterone and aggression in other species. For example, chimpanzees, which are continuous breeders, show significantly raised testosterone levels and aggressive male-male interactions when receptive and fertile females are present.[96] Currently, no research has specified a relationship between the modified challenge hypothesis and human behavior, or the human nature of concealed ovulation, although some suggest it may apply.[93]

Effects on the nervous system edit

 
Testosterone to Estradiol conversion

Another line of research has focused on the proximate effects of circulating testosterone on the nervous system, as mediated by local metabolism within the brain. Testosterone can be metabolized to estradiol by the enzyme aromatase, or to dihydrotestosterone (DHT) by 5α-reductase.[97]

Aromatase is highly expressed in regions involved in the regulation of aggressive behavior, such as the amygdala and hypothalamus. In studies using genetic knockout techniques in inbred mice, male mice that lacked a functional aromatase enzyme displayed a marked reduction in aggression. Long-term treatment with estradiol partially restored aggressive behavior, suggesting that the neural conversion of circulating testosterone to estradiol and its effect on estrogen receptors influences inter-male aggression. In addition, two different estrogen receptors, ERα and ERβ, have been identified as having the ability to exert different effects on aggression in mice. However, the effect of estradiol appears to vary depending on the strain of mouse, and in some strains it reduces aggression during long days (16 h of light), while during short days (8 h of light) estradiol rapidly increases aggression.[97]

Another hypothesis is that testosterone influences brain areas that control behavioral reactions. Studies in animal models indicate that aggression is affected by several interconnected cortical and subcortical structures within the so-called social behavior network. A study involving lesions and electrical-chemical stimulation in rodents and cats revealed that such a neural network consists of the medial amygdala, medial hypothalamus and periaqueductal grey (PAG), and it positively modulates reactive aggression.[98] Moreover, a study done in human subjects showed that prefrontal-amygdala connectivity is modulated by endogenous testosterone during social emotional behavior.[99]

In human studies, testosterone-aggression research has also focused on the role of the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). This brain area is strongly associated with impulse control and self-regulation systems that integrate emotion, motivation, and cognition to guide context-appropriate behavior.[100] Patients with localized lesions to the OFC engage in heightened reactive aggression.[101] Aggressive behavior may be regulated by testosterone via reduced medial OFC engagement following social provocation.[100] When measuring participants' salivary testosterone, higher levels can predict subsequent aggressive behavioral reactions to unfairness faced during a task. Moreover, brain scanning with fMRI shows reduced activity in the medial OFC during such reactions. Such findings may suggest that a specific brain region, the OFC, is a key factor in understanding reactive aggression.

General associations with behavior edit

Scientists have for a long time been interested in the relationship between testosterone and aggressive behavior. In most species, males are more aggressive than females. Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males. In humans, males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females. The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens which happen at the same time as testosterone levels rise. Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by a lumbar puncture which is not done for research purposes. Studies therefore have often instead used more unreliable measurements from blood or saliva.[102]

The Handbook of Crime Correlates, a review of crime studies, states most studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex. However, nearly all studies of juvenile delinquency and testosterone are not significant. Most studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as antisocial behavior and alcoholism. Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior/feelings and testosterone. About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship.[102]

Studies of testosterone levels of male athletes before and after a competition revealed that testosterone levels rise shortly before their matches, as if in anticipation of the competition, and are dependent on the outcome of the event: testosterone levels of winners are high relative to those of losers. No specific response of testosterone levels to competition was observed in female athletes, although a mood difference was noted.[103] In addition, some experiments have failed to find a relationship between testosterone levels and aggression in humans.[104][22][105]

The possible correlation between testosterone and aggression could explain the "roid rage" that can result from anabolic steroid use,[106][107] although an effect of abnormally high levels of steroids does not prove an effect at physiological levels.

Dehydroepiandrosterone edit

Dehydroepiandrosterone (DHEA) is the most abundant circulating androgen hormone and can be rapidly metabolized within target tissues into potent androgens and estrogens. Gonadal steroids generally regulate aggression during the breeding season, but non-gonadal steroids may regulate aggression during the non-breeding season. Castration of various species in the non-breeding season has no effect on territorial aggression. In several avian studies, circulating DHEA has been found to be elevated in birds during the non-breeding season. These data support the idea that non-breeding birds combine adrenal and/or gonadal DHEA synthesis with neural DHEA metabolism to maintain territorial behavior when gonadal testosterone secretion is low. Similar results have been found in studies involving different strains of rats, mice, and hamsters. DHEA levels also have been studied in humans and may play a role in human aggression. Circulating DHEAS (its sulfated ester) levels rise during adrenarche (≈7 years of age) while plasma testosterone levels are relatively low. This implies that aggression in pre-pubertal children with aggressive conduct disorder might be correlated with plasma DHEAS rather than plasma testosterone, suggesting an important link between DHEAS and human aggressive behavior.[97]

Glucocorticoids edit

Glucocorticoid hormones have an important role in regulating aggressive behavior. In adult rats, acute injections of corticosterone promote aggressive behavior and acute reduction of corticosterone decreases aggression; however, a chronic reduction of corticosterone levels can produce abnormally aggressive behavior. In addition, glucocorticoids affect development of aggression and establishment of social hierarchies. Adult mice with low baseline levels of corticosterone are more likely to become dominant than are mice with high baseline corticosterone levels.[97]

Glucocorticoids are released by the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis in response to stress, of which cortisol is the most prominent in humans. Results in adults suggest that reduced levels of cortisol, linked to lower fear or a reduced stress response, can be associated with more aggression. However, it may be that proactive aggression is associated with low cortisol levels while reactive aggression may be accompanied by elevated levels. Differences in assessments of cortisol may also explain a diversity of results, particularly in children.[92]

The HPA axis is related to the general fight-or-flight response or acute stress reaction, and the role of catecholamines such as epinephrine, popularly known as adrenaline.

Pheromones edit

In many animals, aggression can be linked to pheromones released between conspecifics. In mice, major urinary proteins (Mups) have been demonstrated to promote innate aggressive behavior in males,[108][109] and can be mediated by neuromodulatory systems.[110] Mups activate olfactory sensory neurons in the vomeronasal organ (VNO), a subsystem of the nose known to detect pheromones via specific sensory receptors, of mice[109] and rats.[111] Pheremones have also been identified in fruit flies, detected by neurons in the antenna, that send a message to the brain eliciting aggression; it has been noted that aggression pheremones have not been identified in humans.[112]

Genetics edit

In general, differences in a continuous phenotype such as aggression are likely to result from the action of a large number of genes each of small effect, which interact with each other and the environment through development and life.

In a non-mammalian example of genes related to aggression, the fruitless gene in fruit flies is a critical determinant of certain sexually dimorphic behaviors, and its artificial alteration can result in a reversal of stereotypically male and female patterns of aggression in fighting. However, in what was thought to be a relatively clear case, inherent complexities have been reported in deciphering the connections between interacting genes in an environmental context and a social phenotype involving multiple behavioral and sensory interactions with another organism.[113]

In mice, candidate genes for differentiating aggression between the sexes are the Sry (sex determining region Y) gene, located on the Y chromosome and the Sts (steroid sulfatase) gene. The Sts gene encodes the steroid sulfatase enzyme, which is pivotal in the regulation of neurosteroid biosynthesis. It is expressed in both sexes, is correlated with levels of aggression among male mice, and increases dramatically in females after parturition and during lactation, corresponding to the onset of maternal aggression.[81] At least one study has found a possible epigenetic signature (i.e. decreased methylation at a specific CpG site on the promoter region) of the serotonin receptor 5-HT3a that is associated with maternal aggression among human subjects.[85]

Mice with experimentally elevated sensitivity to oxidative stress (through inhibition of copper-zinc superoxide dismutase, SOD1 activity) were tested for aggressive behavior.[114] Males completely deficient in SOD1 were found to be more aggressive than both wild-type males and males that express 50% of this antioxidant enzyme. They were also faster to attack another male. The causal connection between SOD1 deficiency and increased aggression is not yet understood.

In humans, there is good evidence that the basic human neural architecture underpinning the potential for flexible aggressive responses is influenced by genes as well as environment. In terms of variation between individual people, more than 100 twin and adoption studies have been conducted in recent decades examining the genetic basis of aggressive behavior and related constructs such as conduct disorders. According to a meta-analysis published in 2002, approximately 40% of variation between individuals is explained by differences in genes, and 60% by differences in environment (mainly non-shared environmental influences rather than those that would be shared by being raised together). However, such studies have depended on self-report or observation by others including parents, which complicates interpretation of the results. The few laboratory-based analyses have not found significant amounts of individual variation in aggression explicable by genetic variation in the human population. Furthermore, linkage and association studies that seek to identify specific genes, for example that influence neurotransmitter or hormone levels, have generally resulted in contradictory findings characterized by failed attempts at replication. One possible factor is an allele (variant) of the MAO-A gene which, in interaction with certain life events such as childhood maltreatment (which may show a main effect on its own), can influence development of brain regions such as the amygdala and as a result some types of behavioral response may be more likely. The generally unclear picture has been compared to equally difficult findings obtained in regard to other complex behavioral phenotypes.[115][116] For example, both 7R and 5R, ADHD-linked VNTR alleles of dopamine receptor D4 gene are directly associated with the incidence of proactive aggression in the men with no history of ADHD.[117]

Society and culture edit

Humans share aspects of aggression with non-human animals, and have specific aspects and complexity related to factors such as genetics, early development, social learning and flexibility, culture and morals. Konrad Lorenz stated in his 1963 classic, On Aggression, that human behavior is shaped by four main, survival-seeking animal drives. Taken together, these drives—hunger, fear, reproduction, and aggression—achieve natural selection.[118] E. O. Wilson elaborated in On Human Nature that aggression is, typically, a means of gaining control over resources. Aggression is, thus, aggravated during times when high population densities generate resource shortages.[119] According to Richard Leakey and his colleagues, aggression in humans has also increased by becoming more interested in ownership and by defending his or her property.[120] However, UNESCO adopted the Seville Statement of Violence in 1989 that refuted claims, by evolutionary scientists, that genetics by itself was the sole cause of aggression.[121][122]

Social and cultural aspects may significantly interfere with the distinct expression of aggressiveness. For example, a high population density, when associated with a decrease of available resources, might be a significant intervening variable for the occurrence of violent acts.[123]

Culture edit

Culture is one factor that plays a role in aggression. Tribal or band societies existing before or outside of modern states have sometimes been depicted as peaceful 'noble savages'. The ǃKung people were described as 'The Harmless People' in a popular work by Elizabeth Marshall Thomas in 1958,[124] while Lawrence Keeley's 1996 War Before Civilization suggested that regular warfare without modern technology was conducted by most groups throughout human history, including most Native American tribes.[125] Studies of hunter-gatherers show a range of different societies. In general, aggression, conflict and violence sometimes occur, but direct confrontation is generally avoided and conflict is socially managed by a variety of verbal and non-verbal methods. Different rates of aggression or violence, currently or in the past, within or between groups, have been linked to the structuring of societies and environmental conditions influencing factors such as resource or property acquisition, land and subsistence techniques, and population change.[126]

American psychologist Peter Gray hypothesizes that band hunter-gatherer societies are able to reduce aggression while maintaining relatively peaceful, egalitarian relations between members through various methods, such as fostering a playful spirit in all areas of life, the use of humor to counter the tendency of any one person to dominate the group, and non-coercive or "indulgent" child-rearing practices. Gray likens hunter-gatherer bands to social play groups, while stressing that such play is not frivolous or even easy at all times.[127] According to Gray, "Social play—that is, play involving more than one player—is necessarily egalitarian. It always requires a suspension of aggression and dominance along with a heightened sensitivity to the needs and desires of the other players".[128]

Joan Durrant at the University of Manitoba writes that a number of studies have found physical punishment to be associated with "higher levels of aggression against parents, siblings, peers and spouses", even when controlling for other factors.[129] According to Elizabeth Gershoff at the University of Texas at Austin, the more that children are physically punished, the more likely they are as adults to act violently towards family members, including intimate partners.[130] In countries where physical punishment of children is perceived as being more culturally accepted, it is less strongly associated with increased aggression; however, physical punishment has been found to predict some increase in child aggression regardless of culture.[131] While these associations do not prove causality, a number of longitudinal studies suggest that the experience of physical punishment has a direct causal effect on later aggressive behaviors.[129] In examining several longitudinal studies that investigated the path from disciplinary spanking to aggression in children from preschool age through adolescence, Gershoff concluded: "Spanking consistently predicted increases in children's aggression over time, regardless of how aggressive children were when the spanking occurred".[132] similar results were found by Catherine Taylor at Tulane University in 2010.[133] Family violence researcher Murray A. Straus argues, "There are many reasons this evidence has been ignored. One of the most important is the belief that spanking is more effective than nonviolent discipline and is, therefore, sometimes necessary, despite the risk of harmful side effects".[134]

Analyzing aggression culturally or politically is complicated by the fact that the label 'aggressive' can itself be used as a way of asserting a judgement from a particular point of view.[according to whom?] Whether a coercive or violent method of social control is perceived as aggression – or as legitimate versus illegitimate aggression – depends on the position of the relevant parties in relation to the social order of their culture. This in turn can relate to factors such as: norms for coordinating actions and dividing resources; what is considered self-defense or provocation; attitudes towards 'outsiders', attitudes towards specific groups such as women, disabled people or those with lower status; the availability of alternative conflict resolution strategies; trade interdependence and collective security pacts; fears and impulses; and ultimate goals regarding material and social outcomes.[123]

Cross-cultural research has found differences in attitudes towards aggression in different cultures. In one questionnaire study of university students, in addition to men overall justifying some types of aggression more than women, United States respondents justified defensive physical aggression more readily than Japanese or Spanish respondents, whereas Japanese students preferred direct verbal aggression (but not indirect) more than their American and Spanish counterparts.[135] Within American culture, southern men were shown in a study on university students to be more affected and to respond more aggressively than northerners when randomly insulted after being bumped into, which was theoretically related to a traditional culture of honor in the Southern United States, or "saving face."[136] Other cultural themes sometimes applied to the study of aggression include individualistic versus collectivist styles, which may relate, for example, to whether disputes are responded to with open competition or by accommodating and avoiding conflicts. In a study including 62 countries school principals reported aggressive student behavior more often the more individualist, and hence less collectivist, their country's culture.[137] Other comparisons made in relation to aggression or war include democratic versus authoritarian political systems and egalitarian versus stratified societies.[123] The economic system known as capitalism has been viewed by some as reliant on the leveraging of human competitiveness and aggression in pursuit of resources and trade, which has been considered in both positive and negative terms.[138] Attitudes about the social acceptability of particular acts or targets of aggression are also important factors. This can be highly controversial, as for example in disputes between religions or nation states, for example in regard to the Arab–Israeli conflict.[139][140]

Media edit

Some scholars believe that behaviors like aggression may be partially learned by watching and imitating people's behavior, while other researchers have concluded that the media may have some small effects on aggression.[141] There is also research questioning this view.[142] For instance, a long-term outcome study of youth found no long-term relationship between playing violent video games and youth violence or bullying.[143] One study suggested there is a smaller effect of violent video games on aggression than has been found with television violence on aggression. This effect is positively associated with type of game violence and negatively associated to time spent playing the games.[144] The author concluded that insufficient evidence exists to link video game violence with aggression. However, another study suggested links to aggressive behavior.[145]

Children edit

The frequency of physical aggression in humans peaks at around 2–3 years of age. It then declines gradually on average.[146][147] These observations suggest that physical aggression is not only a learned behavior but that development provides opportunities for the learning and biological development of self-regulation. However, a small subset of children fail to acquire all the necessary self-regulatory abilities and tend to show atypical levels of physical aggression across development. These may be at risk for later violent behavior or, conversely, lack of aggression that may be considered necessary within society. Some findings suggest that early aggression does not necessarily lead to aggression later on, however, although the course through early childhood is an important predictor of outcomes in middle childhood. In addition, physical aggression that continues is likely occurring in the context of family adversity, including socioeconomic factors. Moreover, 'opposition' and 'status violations' in childhood appear to be more strongly linked to social problems in adulthood than simply aggressive antisocial behavior.[148][149] Social learning through interactions in early childhood has been seen as a building block for levels of aggression which play a crucial role in the development of peer relationships in middle childhood.[150] Overall, an interplay of biological, social and environmental factors can be considered.[151] Some research indicates that changes in the weather can increase the likelihood of children exhibiting deviant behavior.[152]

Typical expectations edit

  • Young children preparing to enter kindergarten need to develop the socially important skill of being assertive. Examples of assertiveness include asking others for information, initiating conversation, or being able to respond to peer pressure.
  • In contrast, some young children use aggressive behavior, such as hitting or biting, as a form of communication.
  • Aggressive behavior can impede learning as a skill deficit, while assertive behavior can facilitate learning. However, with young children, aggressive behavior is developmentally appropriate and can lead to opportunities of building conflict resolution and communication skills.
  • By school age, children should learn more socially appropriate forms of communicating such as expressing themselves through verbal or written language; if they have not, this behavior may signify a disability or developmental delay.

Aggression triggers edit

The Bobo doll experiment was conducted by Albert Bandura in 1961. In this work, Bandura found that children exposed to an aggressive adult model acted more aggressively than those who were exposed to a nonaggressive adult model. This experiment suggests that anyone who comes in contact with and interacts with children can affect the way they react and handle situations.[153]

Summary points from recommendations by national associations
  • American Academy of Pediatrics (2011): "The best way to prevent aggressive behavior is to give your child a stable, secure home life with firm, loving discipline and full-time supervision during the toddler and preschool years. Everyone who cares for your child should be a good role model and agree on the rules he's expected to observe as well as the response to use if he disobeys."[154]
  • National Association of School Psychologists (2008): "Proactive aggression is typically reasoned, unemotional, and focused on acquiring some goal. For example, a bully wants peer approval and victim submission, and gang members want status and control. In contrast, reactive aggression is frequently highly emotional and is often the result of biased or deficient cognitive processing on the part of the student."[155]

Gender edit

Gender is a factor that plays a role in both human and animal aggression. Males are historically believed to be generally more physically aggressive than females from an early age,[156][157] and men commit the vast majority of murders (Buss 2005). This is one of the most robust and reliable behavioral sex differences, and it has been found across many different age groups and cultures. However, some empirical studies have found the discrepancy in male and female aggression to be more pronounced in childhood and the gender difference in adults to be modest when studied in an experimental context.[57] Still, there is evidence that males are quicker to aggression (Frey et al. 2003) and more likely than females to express their aggression physically.[158] When considering indirect forms of non-violent aggression, such as relational aggression and social rejection, some scientists argue that females can be quite aggressive, although female aggression is rarely expressed physically.[159][160][161] An exception is intimate partner violence that occurs among couples who are engaged, married, or in some other form of intimate relationship.

Although females are less likely than males to initiate physical violence, they can express aggression by using a variety of non-physical means. Exactly which method women use to express aggression is something that varies from culture to culture. On Bellona Island, a culture based on male dominance and physical violence, women tend to get into conflicts with other women more frequently than with men. When in conflict with males, instead of using physical means, they make up songs mocking the man, which spread across the island and humiliate him. If a woman wanted to kill a man, she would either convince her male relatives to kill him or hire an assassin. Although these two methods involve physical violence, both are forms of indirect aggression, since the aggressor herself avoids getting directly involved or putting herself in immediate physical danger.[162]

See also the sections on testosterone and evolutionary explanations for gender differences above.

Situational factors edit

There has been some links between those prone to violence and their alcohol use. Those who are prone to violence and use alcohol are more likely to carry out violent acts.[163] Alcohol impairs judgment, making people much less cautious than they usually are (MacDonald et al. 1996). It also disrupts the way information is processed (Bushman 1993, 1997; Bushman & Cooper 1990).

Pain and discomfort also increase aggression. Even the simple act of placing one's hands in hot water can cause an aggressive response. Hot temperatures have been implicated as a factor in a number of studies. One study completed in the midst of the civil rights movement found that riots were more likely on hotter days than cooler ones (Carlsmith & Anderson 1979). Students were found to be more aggressive and irritable after taking a test in a hot classroom (Anderson et al. 1996, Rule, et al. 1987). Drivers in cars without air conditioning were also found to be more likely to honk their horns (Kenrick & MacFarlane 1986), which is used as a measure of aggression and has shown links to other factors such as generic symbols of aggression or the visibility of other drivers.[164]

Frustration is another major cause of aggression. The Frustration aggression theory states that aggression increases if a person feels that he or she is being blocked from achieving a goal (Aronson et al. 2005). One study found that the closeness to the goal makes a difference. The study examined people waiting in line and concluded that the 2nd person was more aggressive than the 12th one when someone cut in line (Harris 1974). Unexpected frustration may be another factor. In a separate study to demonstrate how unexpected frustration leads to increased aggression, Kulik & Brown (1979) selected a group of students as volunteers to make calls for charity donations. One group was told that the people they would call would be generous and the collection would be very successful. The other group was given no expectations. The group that expected success was more upset when no one was pledging than the group who did not expect success (everyone actually had horrible success). This research suggests that when an expectation does not materialize (successful collections), unexpected frustration arises which increases aggression.

There is some evidence to suggest that the presence of violent objects such as a gun can trigger aggression. In a study done by Leonard Berkowitz and Anthony Le Page (1967), college students were made angry and then left in the presence of a gun or badminton racket. They were then led to believe they were delivering electric shocks to another student, as in the Milgram experiment. Those who had been in the presence of the gun administered more shocks. It is possible that a violence-related stimulus increases the likelihood of aggressive cognitions by activating the semantic network.

A new proposal links military experience to anger and aggression, developing aggressive reactions and investigating these effects on those possessing the traits of a serial killer. Castle and Hensley state, "The military provides the social context where servicemen learn aggression, violence, and murder."[165] Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is also a serious issue in the military, also believed to sometimes lead to aggression in soldiers who are suffering from what they witnessed in battle. They come back to the civilian world and may still be haunted by flashbacks and nightmares, causing severe stress. In addition, it has been claimed that in the rare minority who are claimed to be inclined toward serial killing, violent impulses may be reinforced and refined in war, possibly creating more effective murderers.[166]

As a positive adaptation theory edit

Some recent scholarship has questioned traditional psychological conceptualizations of aggression as universally negative.[41][167] Most traditional psychological definitions of aggression focus on the harm to the recipient of the aggression, implying this is the intent of the aggressor; however this may not always be the case.[168] From this alternate view, although the recipient may or may not be harmed, the perceived intent is to increase the status of the aggressor, not necessarily to harm the recipient.[169] Such scholars contend that traditional definitions of aggression have no validity because of how challenging it is to study directly.[170]

From this view, rather than concepts such as assertiveness, aggression, violence and criminal violence existing as distinct constructs, they exist instead along a continuum with moderate levels of aggression being most adaptive.[41] Such scholars do not consider this a trivial difference, noting that many traditional researchers' aggression measurements may measure outcomes lower down in the continuum, at levels which are adaptive, yet they generalize their findings to non-adaptive levels of aggression, thus losing precision.[171]

See also edit

References edit

  1. ^ Maremmani I., Avella M.T., Novi M., Bacciardi S., Maremmani A.G.I. Aggressive Behavior and Substance Use Disorder: The Heroin Use Disorder as a Case Study. Addict. Disord. Treat.. 2020;19(3):161-173. doi:10.1097/ADT.0000000000000199
  2. ^ DeBono, Amber; Muraven, Mark (1 November 2014). "Rejection perceptions: feeling disrespected leads to greater aggression than feeling disliked". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 55: 43–52. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2014.05.014. ISSN 0022-1031.
  3. ^ a b De Almeida, Rosa Maria Martins; Cabral, João Carlos Centurion; Narvaes, Rodrigo (2015). "Behavioural, hormonal and neurobiological mechanisms of aggressive behaviour in human and nonhuman primates". Physiology & Behavior. 143: 121–35. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.02.053. PMID 25749197. S2CID 27711931.
  4. ^ a b c Miczek, Klaus A.; Almeida, Rosa M. M. de; Kravitz, Edward A.; Rissman, Emilie F.; Boer, Sietse F. de; Raine, Adrian (31 October 2007). "Neurobiology of Escalated Aggression and Violence". Journal of Neuroscience. 27 (44): 11803–11806. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3500-07.2007. ISSN 0270-6474. PMC 2667097. PMID 17978016.
  5. ^ Hollowood, Lorna (2022). "Micro refers to its subtle delivery not its impact". rcn.org.uk. Royal College of Nursing. Microaggressions are something that somebody says to you; they can be a way that you've been made to feel. They are often subtle behaviours, but their effects are far from subtle
  6. ^ Buss, A. H. (1961). The psychology of aggression. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.[page needed]
  7. ^ Anderson, Craig A.; Bushman, Brad J. (2002). "Human Aggression". Annual Review of Psychology. 53: 27–51. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135231. PMID 11752478. S2CID 227846.
  8. ^ Wahl, Klaus (2020). The Radical Right. Biopsychosocial Roots and International Variations. London: Palgrave Macmillan. p. 47. ISBN 978-3-030-25130-7. OCLC 1126278982.
  9. ^ Wahl, Klaus (2013). Aggression und Gewalt. Ein biologischer, psychologischer und sozialwissenschaftlicher Überblick. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag. p. 2. ISBN 978-3-8274-3120-2. OCLC 471933605.
  10. ^ Akert, R.M., Aronson, E., & Wilson, T.D. (2010). Social Psychology (7th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.
  11. ^ Dollard, J.; Doob, L.W.; Miller, N.E.; Mowrer, O.H.; Sears, R.R. (1939). Frustration and Aggression. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  12. ^ Berkowitz, L. (1987). "Frustrations, appraisals, and aversively stimulated aggression". Aggressive Behavior. 14 (1): 3–11. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.106.1.59. PMID 2667009.
  13. ^ a b Archer, J. (1976). "The organization of aggression and fear in vertebrates". In Bateson, P.P.G.; Klopfer, P.H. (eds.). Perspectives in Ethology (Vol.2). New York, NY: Plenum. pp. 231–298.
  14. ^ Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  15. ^ McEllistrem, Joseph E. (2004). "Affective and predatory violence: A bimodal classification system of human aggression and violence". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 10 (1): 1–30. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2003.06.002.
  16. ^ Bushman, Brad J.; Anderson, Craig A. (2001). "Is it time to pull the plug on hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy?". Psychological Review. 108 (1): 273–9. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.273. PMID 11212630. S2CID 14223373.
  17. ^ Ellie L. Young, David A. Nelson, America B. Hottle, Brittney Warburton, and Bryan K. Young (2010) Relational Aggression Among Students Principal Leadership, October, copyright the National Association of Secondary School Principals
  18. ^ Ramírez, J.M.; Andreu, J.M. (2006). "Aggression, and some related psychological constructs (anger, hostility, and impulsivity) Some comments from a research project". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (3): 276–91. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.015. PMID 16081158. S2CID 17678048.
  19. ^ Veenema, Alexa H.; Neumann, Inga D. (2007). "Neurobiological Mechanisms of Aggression and Stress Coping: A Comparative Study in Mouse and Rat Selection Lines". Brain, Behavior and Evolution. 70 (4): 274–85. doi:10.1159/000105491. PMID 17914259. S2CID 25067952.
  20. ^ Simons, Marlise (May 2010). "International Court May Define Aggression as Crime". The New York Times.
  21. ^ Nathaniel Snow Violence and Aggression in Sports: An In-Depth Look (Part One) (Part 2Part 3) Bleacher Report, 23 March 2010
  22. ^ a b Coccaro, Emil F.; Beresford, Brendan; Minar, Philip; Kaskow, Jon; Geracioti, Thomas (2007). "CSF testosterone: Relationship to aggression, impulsivity, and venturesomeness in adult males with personality disorder". Journal of Psychiatric Research. 41 (6): 488–92. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.04.009. PMID 16765987.
  23. ^ Barbara Krahé (11 February 2013). The Social Psychology of Aggression: 2nd Edition. Psychology Press. p. 16. ISBN 978-1-136-17772-9.
  24. ^ Elizabeth Kande Englander (30 January 2003). Understanding Violence. Psychology Press. pp. 55–86. ISBN 978-1-135-65676-8.
  25. ^ Merriam-Webster: Aggression Retrieved 10 January 2012
  26. ^ Online Etymology Dictionary: Aggression Retrieved 10 January 2012
  27. ^ Stearns, D. C. (2003). Anger and aggression. Encyclopedia of Children and Childhood: In History and Society. Paula S. Fass (Ed.). Macmillan Reference Books
  28. ^ Van Staaden, M.J, Searcy, W.A. & Hanlon, R.T. 'Signaling Aggression' in Aggression Academic Press, Stephen F. Goodwin, 2011
  29. ^ Maestripieri, Dario (1992). "Functional aspects of maternal aggression in mammals". Canadian Journal of Zoology. 70 (6): 1069–77. doi:10.1139/z92-150.
  30. ^ Psychology- The Science of Behaviour, pg 420, Neil R Clarkson (4th Edition)
  31. ^ Gleitman, Henry, Alan J. Fridlund, and Daniel Reisberg. Psychology. 6th ed. New York: W W Norton and Company, 2004. 431–432.
  32. ^ Gendreau, PL & Archer, J. 'Subtypes of Aggression in Humans and Animals', in Developmental Origins of Aggression, 2005, The Guilford Press.
  33. ^ a b c Johnson, L. K.; Wiemer, D. F. (1982). "Nerol: An alarm substance of the stingless bee, Trigona fulviventris (Hymenoptera: Apidae)". Journal of Chemical Ecology. 8 (9): 1167–81. doi:10.1007/BF00990750. PMID 24413960. S2CID 25056451.
  34. ^ Rusch, H.; Gavrilets, S. (2017). "The logic of animal intergroup conflict: A review". Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization. 178: 1014–1030. doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2017.05.004. S2CID 49359988.
  35. ^ Tanner, C. J (2006). "Numerical assessment affects aggression and competitive ability: A team-fighting strategy for the ant Formica xerophila". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 273 (1602): 2737–42. doi:10.1098/rspb.2006.3626. JSTOR 25223670. PMC 1635503. PMID 17015327.
  36. ^ Mitani, John C.; Watts, David P.; Amsler, Sylvia J. (2010). "Lethal intergroup aggression leads to territorial expansion in wild chimpanzees". Current Biology. 20 (12): R507–8. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.04.021. PMID 20620900. S2CID 6493161.
  37. ^ a b Issa, F. A.; Adamson, D. J.; Edwards, D. H. (1999). "Dominance hierarchy formation in juvenile crayfish procambarus clarkii". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 202 (24): 3497–506. doi:10.1242/jeb.202.24.3497. PMID 10574728.
  38. ^ Heitor, Filipa; Oom, Maria do Mar; Vicente, Luís (2006). "Social relationships in a herd of Sorraia horses". Behavioural Processes. 73 (2): 170–7. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2006.05.004. PMID 16815645. S2CID 24008518.
  39. ^ Cant, Michael A.; Llop, Justine B.; Field, Jeremy (2006). "Individual Variation in Social Aggression and the Probability of Inheritance: Theory and a Field Test" (PDF). The American Naturalist. 167 (6): 837–52. doi:10.1086/503445. hdl:10871/26263. PMID 16615035. S2CID 12094679.
  40. ^ Bragin, A. V.; Osadchuk, L. V.; Osadchuk, A. V. (2006). "The experimental model of establishment and maintenance of social hierarchy in laboratory mice". Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deiatelnosti Imeni I P Pavlova. 56 (3): 412–9. PMID 16869278.
  41. ^ a b c Ferguson, Christopher J.; Beaver, Kevin M. (2009). "Natural born killers: The genetic origins of extreme violence". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 14 (5): 286–94. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2009.03.005.
  42. ^ Hsu, Yuying; Earley, Ryan L.; Wolf, Larry L. (2005). "Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: Mechanisms and contest outcomes". Biological Reviews. 81 (1): 33–74. doi:10.1017/S146479310500686X. PMID 16460581. S2CID 14284236.
  43. ^ Aureli, Filippo; Cords, Marina; Van Schaik, Carel P. (2002). "Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals: A predictive framework". Animal Behaviour. 64 (3): 325–43. doi:10.1006/anbe.2002.3071. S2CID 54361369.
  44. ^ Silverberg, James; J. Patrick Gray (1992) Aggression and Peacefulness in Humans and Other Primates ISBN 0-19-507119-0
  45. ^ Honess, P.E.; Marin, C.M. (2006). "Enrichment and aggression in primates". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (3): 413–36. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.05.002. PMID 16055188. S2CID 33130527.
  46. ^ Hinde, R.A. (1970). Animal Behaviour: A Synthesis of Ethology and Comparative Psychology (2nd Ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
  47. ^ Hebb, D.O. (1949). The Organisation of Behavior. New York, NY: Wiley.
  48. ^ a b van Kampen, H.S. (2019). "The principle of consistency and the cause and function of behaviour". Behavioural Processes. 159: 42–54. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.013. PMID 30562561. S2CID 56478466.
  49. ^ Somit, A. (1990). "Humans, Chimps, and Bonobos: The Biological Bases of Aggression, War, and Peacemaking". Journal of Conflict Resolution. 34 (3): 553–82. doi:10.1177/0022002790034003008. JSTOR 174228. S2CID 145380530.
  50. ^ a b McDonald, M. M.; Navarrete, C. D.; Van Vugt, M. (2012). "Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict: The male warrior hypothesis". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 367 (1589): 670–9. doi:10.1098/rstb.2011.0301. JSTOR 41433544. PMC 3260849. PMID 22271783.
  51. ^ a b Vugt, Mark Van; Cremer, David De; Janssen, Dirk P. (2007). "Gender Differences in Cooperation and Competition". Psychological Science. 18 (1): 19–23. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.518.3529. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01842.x. PMID 17362372. S2CID 3566509.
  52. ^ Buss, D.M. (2005). The murderer next door: Why the mind Is designed to kill. New York: Penguin Press.
  53. ^ McCall, Grant S.; Shields, Nancy (2008). "Examining the evidence from small-scale societies and early prehistory and implications for modern theories of aggression and violence". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 13 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2007.04.001.
  54. ^ Buss, D. M.; Duntley, J. D. (2006). "The evolution of aggression". In Schaller, M.; Simpson, J. A.; Kenrick, D. T. (eds.). Evolution and Social Psychology. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 263–86.
  55. ^ Durrant, Russil (2011). "Collective violence: An evolutionary perspective". Aggression and Violent Behavior. 16 (5): 428–36. doi:10.1016/j.avb.2011.04.014.
  56. ^ Briffa, Mark (2010). "Territoriality and Aggression". Nature Education Knowledge. 3 (10): 81.
  57. ^ a b Eagly, Alice H.; Steffen, Valerie J. (1986). "Gender and aggressive behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature". Psychological Bulletin. 100 (3): 309–30. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.100.3.309. PMID 3797558.
  58. ^ Clutton-Brock, T. H.; Hodge, S. J.; Spong, G.; Russell, A. F.; Jordan, N. R.; Bennett, N. C.; Sharpe, L. L.; Manser, M. B. (2006). "Intrasexual competition and sexual selection in cooperative mammals". Nature. 444 (7122): 1065–8. Bibcode:2006Natur.444.1065C. doi:10.1038/nature05386. PMID 17183322. S2CID 4397323.
  59. ^ Archer, John (2009). "Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression?" (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 32 (3–4): 249–66, discussion 266–311. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09990951. PMID 19691899.
  60. ^ Campbell, Anne (1999). "Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and women's intrasexual aggression". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 22 (2): 203–14, discussion 214–52. doi:10.1017/s0140525x99001818. PMID 11301523. S2CID 1081104.
  61. ^ The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology, edited by David M. Buss, John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 2005. Chapter 21 by Anne Campbell.[page needed]
  62. ^ Zuk, M. "Sexual Selections: What We Can and Can't Learn about Sex from Animals." University of California Press, 2002[page needed]
  63. ^ a b c Del Giudice, Marco (2015). "Gender Differences in Personality and Social Behavior". International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 750–6. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.25100-3. hdl:2318/1852940. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  64. ^ Campbell, Anne (2012). "Sex differences in aggression". In Barrett, Louise; Dunbar, Robin (eds.). Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. pp. 365–82. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568308.013.0025. ISBN 978-0-19-856830-8.
  65. ^ Lussier, Patrick; Corrado, Raymond; Tzoumakis, Stacy (2012). "Gender Differences in Physical Aggression and Associated Developmental Correlates in a Sample of Canadian Preschoolers". Behavioral Sciences & the Law. 30 (5): 643–71. doi:10.1002/bsl.2035. PMID 22996132.
  66. ^ a b c Lansford, Jennifer E.; Skinner, Ann T.; Sorbring, Emma; Giunta, Laura Di; Deater-Deckard, Kirby; Dodge, Kenneth A.; Malone, Patrick S.; Oburu, Paul; Pastorelli, Concetta; Tapanya, Sombat; Uribe Tirado, Liliana Maria; Zelli, Arnaldo; Al-Hassan, Suha M.; Peña Alampay, Liane; Bacchini, Dario; Bombi, Anna Silvia; Bornstein, Marc H.; Chang, Lei (2012). "Boys' and Girls' Relational and Physical Aggression in Nine Countries". Aggressive Behavior. 38 (4): 298–308. doi:10.1002/ab.21433. PMC 3736589. PMID 23935227.
  67. ^ a b Hay, Dale F.; Nash, Alison; Caplan, Marlene; Swartzentruber, Jan; Ishikawa, Fumiko; Vespo, Jo Ellen (2011). "The emergence of gender differences in physical aggression in the context of conflict between young peers". British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 29 (2): 158–75. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835x.2011.02028.x. PMID 21592146. S2CID 206006344.
  68. ^ Hess, Nicole H.; Hagen, Edward H. (2006). "Sex differences in indirect aggression". Evolution and Human Behavior. 27 (3): 231–45. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.11.001.
  69. ^ Keeler, Linda A (2007). "The differences in sport aggression, life aggression, and life assertion among adult male and female collision, contact, and non-contact sport athletes". Journal of Sport Behavior. 30 (1): 57–76.
  70. ^ Xie, Hongling; Drabick, Deborah A.G.; Chen, Diane (2011). "Developmental trajectories of aggression from late childhood through adolescence: Similarities and differences across gender". Aggressive Behavior. 37 (5): 387–404. doi:10.1002/ab.20404. PMC 4332584. PMID 21748751.
  71. ^ Young, Cathy (26 November 1999). "Feminists Play the Victim Game". The New York Times. Retrieved 6 December 2012.
  72. ^ "What To Do If Your Partner Has A Steamroller Personality". from the original on 7 September 2021.
  73. ^ Al-Ali, Majed. M.; Singh, Ajai Pratap; Smekal, Vladimir (2011). "Social anxiety in relation to social skills, aggression, and stress among male and female commercial institute students". Education. 132 (2): 351–61.
  74. ^ Low, Bobbi S. (2012). "Ecological and socio-cultural impacts on mating and marriage systems". In Barrett, Louise; Dunbar, Robin (eds.). Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. pp. 449–62. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198568308.013.0030. ISBN 978-0-19-856830-8.
  75. ^ a b Meijers, J.; Harte, J. M.; Meynen, G.; Cuijpers, P. (1 February 2017). "Differences in executive functioning between violent and non-violent offenders". Psychological Medicine. 47 (10): 1784–1793. doi:10.1017/S0033291717000241. ISSN 0033-2917. PMID 28173890. S2CID 4481557.
  76. ^ Kruk, Menno R.; Van Der Poel, A.M.; Meelis, W.; Hermans, J.; Mostert, P.G.; Mos, J.; Lohman, A.H.M. (1983). "Discriminant analysis of the localization of aggression-inducing electrode placements in the hypothalamus of male rats". Brain Research. 260 (1): 61–79. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(83)90764-3. PMID 6681724. S2CID 3231548.
  77. ^ Ferris, C. F.; Melloni, R. H. Jr.; Koppel, G; Perry, K. W.; Fuller, R. W.; Delville, Y (1997). "Vasopressin/serotonin interactions in the anterior hypothalamus control aggressive behavior in golden hamsters". The Journal of Neuroscience. 17 (11): 4331–40. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.17-11-04331.1997. PMC 6573530. PMID 9151749.
  78. ^ Lee, H (2014). "Scalable control of mounting and attack by Esr1+ neurons in the ventromedial hypothalamus". Nature. 509 (7502): 627–32. Bibcode:2014Natur.509..627L. doi:10.1038/nature13169. PMC 4098836. PMID 24739975.
  79. ^ Hashikawa, K (2017). "Esr1+ cells in the ventromedial hypothalamus control female aggression". Nat. Neurosci. 11 (20): 1580–1590. doi:10.1038/nn.4644. PMC 5953764. PMID 28920934.
  80. ^ Potegal, M.; Hebert, M.; Decoster, M.; Meyerhoff, J. L. (1996). "Brief, high-frequency stimulation of the corticomedial amygdala induces a delayed and prolonged increase of aggressiveness in male Syrian golden hamsters". Behavioral Neuroscience. 110 (2): 401–12. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.110.2.401. PMID 8731066.
  81. ^ a b c Potegal, M.; Ferris, C.F.; Hebert, M.; Meyerhoff, J.; Skaredoff, L. (1996). "Attack priming in female Syrian golden hamsters is associated with a c-fos-coupled process within the corticomedial amygdala". Neuroscience. 75 (3): 869–80. doi:10.1016/0306-4522(96)00236-9. PMID 8951880. S2CID 24136851.
  82. ^ Greenberg, Neil; Scott, Michelle; Crews, David (1984). "Role of the amygdala in the reproductive and aggressive behavior of the lizard, Anolis carolinensis". Physiology & Behavior. 32 (1): 147–51. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(84)90088-X. PMID 6538977. S2CID 9987359.
  83. ^ Bauman, M. D.; Toscano, J. E.; Mason, W. A.; Lavenex, P.; Amaral, D. G. (2006). "The expression of social dominance following neonatal lesions of the amygdala or hippocampus in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta)" (PDF). Behavioral Neuroscience. 120 (4): 749–60. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.120.4.749. PMID 16893283.
  84. ^ Paus, T. 'Mapping Brain Development' in Developmental Origins of Aggression, 2005, The Guilford Press.
  85. ^ a b Schechter, Daniel S.; Moser, Dominik A.; Pointet, Virginie C.; Aue, Tatjana; Stenz, Ludwig; Paoloni-Giacobino, Ariane; Adouan, Wafae; Manini, Aurélia; Suardi, Francesca; Vital, Marylene; Sancho Rossignol, Ana; Cordero, Maria I.; Rothenberg, Molly; Ansermet, François; Rusconi Serpa, Sandra; Dayer, Alexandre G. (2016). "The association of serotonin receptor 3A methylation with maternal violence exposure, neural activity, and child aggression". Behavioural Brain Research. 325 (Pt B): 268–277. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2016.10.009. PMID 27720744.
  86. ^ Caramaschi, Doretta; De Boer, Sietse F.; De Vries, Han; Koolhaas, Jaap M. (2008). "Development of violence in mice through repeated victory along with changes in prefrontal cortex neurochemistry" (PDF). Behavioural Brain Research. 189 (2): 263–72. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.01.003. hdl:11370/f683911d-3c8b-4ba6-a6bb-12cbaf11dead. PMID 18281105. S2CID 14888253.
  87. ^ Pihl, RO & Benkelfat, C. 'Neuromodulators in the Development and Expression of Inhibition and Aggression' in Developmental_origins_of_aggression.html?id=XmSfJEl2v4sC Developmental Origins of Aggression, 2005, The Guilford Press.
  88. ^ Heinrichs, M; Domes, G (2008). "Neuropeptides and social behaviour: Effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans". Advances in Vasopressin and Oxytocin – from Genes to Behaviour to Disease. Progress in Brain Research. Vol. 170. pp. 337–50. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)00428-7. ISBN 978-0-444-53201-5. PMID 18655894.
  89. ^ Campbell, Anne (2008). "Attachment, aggression and affiliation: The role of oxytocin in female social behavior" (PDF). Biological Psychology. 77 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.001. PMID 17931766. S2CID 33228118.
  90. ^ Walton, Kenneth G.; Levitsky, Debra K. (2003). "Effects of the Transcendental Meditation Program on Neuroendocrine Abnormalities Associated with Aggression and Crime". Journal of Offender Rehabilitation. 36 (1–4): 67–87. doi:10.1300/J076v36n01_04. S2CID 144374302.
  91. ^ a b Carlson, N. 'Hormonal Control of Aggressive Behavior' Chapter 11 in [Physiology of Behavior],2013, Pearson Education Inc.
  92. ^ a b Van Goozen, S. 'Hormones and the Developmental Origins of Aggression' Chapter 14 in Developmental Origins of Aggression, 2005, The Guilford Press.
  93. ^ a b Archer, John (2006). "Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of the challenge hypothesis". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 30 (3): 319–45. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.12.007. PMID 16483890. S2CID 26405251.
  94. ^ "Three Important Physical Ovulation Symptoms" from BabyMed.com
  95. ^ Wingfield, John C.; Ball, Gregory F.; Dufty, Alfred M.; Hegner, Robert E.; Ramenofsky, Marilyn (1987). "Testosterone and Aggression in Birds". American Scientist. 75 (6): 602–8. Bibcode:1987AmSci..75..602W.
  96. ^ Muller, Martin N; Wrangham, Richard W (2004). "Dominance, aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees: A test of the 'challenge hypothesis'". Animal Behaviour. 67: 113–23. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.03.013. S2CID 8041587.
  97. ^ a b c d Soma, Kiran K.; Scotti, Melissa-Ann L.; Newman, Amy E.M.; Charlier, Thierry D.; Demas, Gregory E. (2008). "Novel mechanisms for neuroendocrine regulation of aggression". Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 29 (4): 476–89. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2007.12.003. PMID 18280561. S2CID 32650274.
  98. ^ Siegel, Allan; Bhatt, Suresh; Bhatt, Rekha; Zalcman, Steven (2007). "The Neurobiological Bases for Development of Pharmacological Treatments of Aggressive Disorders". Current Neuropharmacology. 5 (2): 135–47. doi:10.2174/157015907780866929. PMC 2435345. PMID 18615178.
  99. ^ Volman, I.; Toni, I.; Verhagen, L.; Roelofs, K. (2011). "Endogenous Testosterone Modulates Prefrontal-Amygdala Connectivity during Social Emotional Behavior". Cerebral Cortex. 21 (10): 2282–90. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr001. PMC 3169658. PMID 21339377.
  100. ^ a b Mehta, Pranjal H.; Beer, Jennifer (2010). "Neural Mechanisms of the Testosterone–Aggression Relation: The Role of Orbitofrontal Cortex". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 22 (10): 2357–68. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.518.2751. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21389. PMID 19925198. S2CID 710598.
  101. ^ Siever, Larry J. (2008). "Neurobiology of Aggression and Violence". American Journal of Psychiatry. 165 (4): 429–42. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111774. PMC 4176893. PMID 18346997.
  102. ^ a b Handbook of Crime Correlates; Lee Ellis, Kevin M. Beaver, John Wright; 2009; Academic Press
  103. ^ Mazur, Allan; Booth, Alan (1998). "Testosterone and dominance in men". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 21 (3): 353–63, discussion 363–97. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.421.3005. doi:10.1017/s0140525x98001228. PMID 10097017. S2CID 9462611.
  104. ^ Albert, D.J.; Walsh, M.L.; Jonik, R.H. (1993). "Aggression in humans: What is its biological foundation?". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 17 (4): 405–25. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80117-4. PMID 8309650. S2CID 28557481.
  105. ^ Constantino, John N.; Grosz, Daniel; Saenger, Paul; Chandler, Donald W.; Nandi, Reena; Earls, Felton J. (1993). "Testosterone and Aggression in Children". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 32 (6): 1217–22. doi:10.1097/00004583-199311000-00015. PMID 8282667.
  106. ^ Pibiri, Fabio; Nelson, Marianela; Carboni, Giovanni; Pinna, Graziano (2006). "Neurosteroids regulate mouse aggression induced by anabolic androgenic steroids". NeuroReport. 17 (14): 1537–41. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000234752.03808.b2. PMID 16957604. S2CID 42991833.
  107. ^ Choi, P. Y. L.; Parrott, A. C.; Cowan, D. (1990). "High-dose anabolic steroids in strength athletes: Effects upon hostility and aggression". Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical and Experimental. 5 (4): 349–56. doi:10.1002/hup.470050407. S2CID 37157824.
  108. ^ "Aggression protein found in mice". BBC News. 5 December 2007. Retrieved 26 September 2009.
  109. ^ a b Chamero P; Marton TF; Logan DW; et al. (December 2007). "Identification of protein pheromones that promote aggressive behaviour". Nature. 450 (7171): 899–902. Bibcode:2007Natur.450..899C. doi:10.1038/nature05997. PMID 18064011. S2CID 4398766.
  110. ^ Smith, R. S.; Hu, R.; Desouza, A.; Eberly, C. L.; Krahe, K.; Chan, W.; Araneda, R. C. (2015). "Differential Muscarinic Modulation in the Olfactory Bulb". Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (30): 10773–85. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0099-15.2015. PMC 4518052. PMID 26224860.
  111. ^ Krieger, J.; Schmitt, A.; Lobel, D.; Gudermann, T.; Schultz, G.; Breer, H.; Boekhoff, I. (1999). "Selective Activation of G Protein Subtypes in the Vomeronasal Organ upon Stimulation with Urine-derived Compounds". Journal of Biological Chemistry. 274 (8): 4655–62. doi:10.1074/jbc.274.8.4655. PMID 9988702.
  112. ^ Caltech press release, 2009
  113. ^ Siwicki, Kathleen K; Kravitz, Edward A (2009). "Fruitless, doublesex and the genetics of social behavior in Drosophila melanogaster". Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 19 (2): 200–6. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2009.04.001. PMC 2716404. PMID 19541474.
  114. ^ Garratt M, Brooks RC (January 2015). "A genetic reduction in antioxidant function causes elevated aggression in mice". J. Exp. Biol. 218 (Pt 2): 223–7. doi:10.1242/jeb.112011. PMID 25524980.
  115. ^ Perusse, D. & Gendreau, P. 'Genetics and the Development of Aggression' in Developmental Origins of Aggression, 2005, The Guilford Press.[page needed]
  116. ^ Derringer, Jaime; Krueger, Robert F.; Irons, Daniel E.; Iacono, William G. (2010). "Harsh Discipline, Childhood Sexual Assault, and MAOA Genotype: An Investigation of Main and Interactive Effects on Diverse Clinical Externalizing Outcomes". Behavior Genetics. 40 (5): 639–48. doi:10.1007/s10519-010-9358-9. PMC 2912157. PMID 20364435.
  117. ^ Cherepkova, Elena V; Maksimov, Vladimir N; Aftanas, Lyubomir I; Menshanov, Petr N (2015). "Genotype and haplotype frequencies of the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism in the men with no history of ADHD, convicted of violent crimes". Journal of Criminal Justice. 43 (6): 464–469. doi:10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2015.10.002.
  118. ^ Konrad Lorenz, On Aggression (1963).[page needed]
  119. ^ E.O. Wilson, On Human Nature (Harvard, 1978) pp.101–107.
  120. ^ Leakey, R.,& Lewin, R. (1978). People of the lake. New York: Anchor Press/Doubleday.
  121. ^ UNESCO, (1989). The Seville Statement, Retrieved: http://www.unesco.org/cpp/uk/declarations/seville.pdf 12 August 2013 at the Wayback Machine
  122. ^ UNESCO Prize for Peace Education, (1989), Retrieved:http://www.demilitarisation.org/IMG/article_PDF/Seville-Statement-UNESCO-1989_a143.pdf
  123. ^ a b c Bond, MH. (2004) 'Aggression and culture', in Encyclopedia of applied psychology, Volume 1.
  124. ^ Thomas, E.M. (1958). The harmless people. New York: Vintage Books.[page needed]
  125. ^ Keeley, L.H. (1996). War Before Civilization: The myth of the peaceful savage. New York: Oxford University Press.
  126. ^ Lomas, W. (2009) Conflict, Violence, and Conflict Resolution in Hunting and Gathering Societies Totem: The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthropology, Volume 17, Issue 1, Article 13
  127. ^ Gray, Peter (Spring 2009). "Play as a Foundation for Hunter-Gatherer Social Existence" (PDF). American Journal of Play. 1 (4): 476–522.
  128. ^ Gray, Peter (16 May 2011). "How Hunter-Gatherers Maintained Their Egalitarian Ways". Psychology Today.
  129. ^ a b Durrant, J.; Ensom, R. (2012). "Physical punishment of children: Lessons from 20 years of research". Canadian Medical Association Journal. 184 (12): 1373–7. doi:10.1503/cmaj.101314. PMC 3447048. PMID 22311946.
  130. ^ Gershoff, E.T. (2008). (PDF). Columbus, OH: Center for Effective Discipline. p. 16. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 January 2016. Retrieved 15 December 2015.
  131. ^ "Corporal Punishment" (2008). International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences.
  132. ^ Gershoff, Elizabeth T. (2013). "Spanking and Child Development: We Know Enough Now to Stop Hitting Our Children". Child Development Perspectives. 7 (3): 133–137. doi:10.1111/cdep.12038. PMC 3768154. PMID 24039629.
  133. ^ Taylor, C. A.; Manganello, J. A.; Lee, S. J.; Rice, J. C. (2010). "Mothers' Spanking of 3-Year-Old Children and Subsequent Risk of Children's Aggressive Behavior". Pediatrics. 125 (5): e1057–65. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-2678. PMC 5094178. PMID 20385647.
  134. ^ Straus, Murray A.; Douglas, Emily M.; Madeiros, Rose Ann (2013). The Primordial Violence: Spanking Children, Psychological Development, Violence, and Crime. New York: Routledge. p. 81. ISBN 978-1-84872-953-7.
  135. ^ Fujihara, Takehiro; Kohyama, Takaya; Andreu, J. Manuel; Ramirez, J. Martin (1999). "Justification of interpersonal aggression in Japanese, American, and Spanish students". Aggressive Behavior. 25 (3): 185–95. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2337(1999)25:3<185::AID-AB3>3.0.CO;2-K. S2CID 145166936.
  136. ^ Cohen, Dov; Nisbett, Richard E.; Bowdle, Brian F.; Schwarz, Norbert (1996). "Insult, aggression, and the southern culture of honor: An "experimental ethnography."" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 70 (5): 945–59. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.70.5.945. hdl:2027.42/92155. PMID 8656339.
  137. ^ Bergmüller, Silvia (2013). "The Relationship Between Cultural Individualism-Collectivism and Student Aggression Across 62 Countries". Aggressive Behavior. 39 (3): 182–200. doi:10.1002/ab.21472. PMID 23494751.
  138. ^ Nolan, P. (2007) Capitalism and freedom: the contradictory character of globalisation From page 2. Anthem Studies in Development and Globalization, Anthem Press
  139. ^ Sherer, Moshe; Karnieli-Miller, Orit (2004). "Aggression and violence among Jewish and Arab Youth in Israel". International Journal of Intercultural Relations. 28 (2): 93–109. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2004.03.004.
  140. ^ Amjad, Naumana; Wood, Alex M. (2009). "Identifying and changing the normative beliefs about aggression which lead young Muslim adults to join extremist anti-Semitic groups in Pakistan". Aggressive Behavior. 35 (6): 514–9. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.332.6476. doi:10.1002/ab.20325. PMID 19790255.
  141. ^ Akert, M. Robin, Aronson, E., and Wilson, D.T. "Social Psychology", 5th Edition. Pearson Education, Inc. 2005[page needed]
  142. ^ Freedman, J. (2002). Media violence and its effect on aggression: Assessing the scientific evidence. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.
  143. ^ Christopher J. Ferguson, (2010) "Video Games and Youth Violence: A Prospective Analysis in Adolescents 25 May 2012 at the Wayback Machine", Journal of Youth and Adolescence
  144. ^ Sherry, J. (2001). "The effects of violent video games on aggression. A meta-analysis". Human Communication Research. 27 (3): 409–31. doi:10.1093/hcr/27.3.409. S2CID 6322160.
  145. ^ Anderson, Craig A.; Dill, Karen E. (2000). "Video games and aggressive thoughts, feelings, and behavior in the laboratory and in life". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (4): 772–90. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.1006.1548. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.4.772. PMID 10794380.
  146. ^ Tremblay, Richard E. (2000). "The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood: What have we learned in the past century?". International Journal of Behavioral Development. 24 (2): 129–41. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.487.7070. doi:10.1080/016502500383232. S2CID 145617291.
  147. ^ Bongers, Ilja L.; Koot, Hans M.; Van Der Ende, Jan; Verhulst, Frank C. (2004). "Developmental Trajectories of Externalizing Behaviors in Childhood and Adolescence". Child Development. 75 (5): 1523–37. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00755.x. PMID 15369529. S2CID 12514300.
  148. ^ NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. (2004). "Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood: Predictors, correlates, and outcomes". Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 69 (4): vii, 1–129. doi:10.1111/j.0037-976X.2004.00312.x. PMID 15667346.
  149. ^ Bongers, I. L.; Koot, H. M.; Van Der Ende, J.; Verhulst, F. C. (2007). "Predicting young adult social functioning from developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviour". Psychological Medicine. 38 (7): 989–99. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.572.4309. doi:10.1017/S0033291707002309. PMID 18047767. S2CID 6218999.
  150. ^ Schellenberg, R. (2000). "Aggressive personality: When does it develop and why?". Virginia Counselors Journal. 26: 67–76.
  151. ^ Tremblay, Richard E; Hartup, Willard W.; Archer, John, eds. (2005). Developmental Origins of Aggression. New York: The Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-59385-110-1.
  152. ^ Dabb, C (May 1997). The relationship between weather and children's behavior: a study of teacher perceptions. USU Thesis.
  153. ^ Bandura, Albert; Ross, Dorothea; Ross, Sheila A. (1961). "Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models". The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 63 (3): 575–82. doi:10.1037/h0045925. PMID 13864605. S2CID 18361226.
  154. ^ American Academy of Pediatrics (2011) Ages & Stages: Aggressive Behavior HealthChildren.org, retrieved January 2012
  155. ^ National Association of School Psychologists (2008)
  156. ^ Coie, J.D. & Dodge, K.A. (1997). Aggression and antisocial behavior. In W. Damon & N. Eisenberg (Eds). Handbook of Child Psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional and personality development
  157. ^ Maccoby. E.E. & Jacklin. C.N. (1974). The psychology of sex differences, Stanford: Stanford University Press.
  158. ^ Björkqvist, Kaj; Österman, Karin; Lagerspetz, Kirsti M. J. (1994). (PDF). Aggressive Behavior. 20: 27–33. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.453.7106. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1994)20:1<27::aid-ab2480200105>3.0.co;2-q. Archived from the original (PDF) on 3 December 2011. Retrieved 6 December 2018.
  159. ^ Archer, John (2004). "Sex Differences in Aggression in Real-World Settings: A Meta-Analytic Review". Review of General Psychology. 8 (4): 291–322. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.8.4.291. S2CID 26394462.
  160. ^ Card, Noel A.; Stucky, Brian D.; Sawalani, Gita M.; Little, Todd D. (2008). "Direct and Indirect Aggression During Childhood and Adolescence: A Meta-Analytic Review of Gender Differences, Intercorrelations, and Relations to Maladjustment". Child Development. 79 (5): 1185–229. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01184.x. PMID 18826521. S2CID 7942628.
  161. ^ Hines, Denise A.; Saudino, Kimberly J. (2003). "Gender Differences in Psychological, Physical, and Sexual Aggression Among College Students Using the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales". Violence and Victims. 18 (2): 197–217. doi:10.1891/vivi.2003.18.2.197. PMID 12816404. S2CID 28687366.
  162. ^ Björkqvist, Kaj (1994). "Sex differences in physical, verbal, and indirect aggression: A review of recent research". Sex Roles. 30 (3–4): 177–88. doi:10.1007/BF01420988. S2CID 142759440.
  163. ^ Navis, Charlene; Brown, Stephen L.; Heim, Derek (2008). "Predictors of injurious assault committed during or after drinking alcohol: A case–control study of young offenders". Aggressive Behavior. 34 (2): 167–74. doi:10.1002/ab.20231. PMID 17922526.
  164. ^ Turner, Charles W.; Layton, John F.; Simons, Lynn S. (1975). "Naturalistic studies of aggressive behavior: Aggressive stimuli, victim visibility, and horn honking". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 31 (6): 1098–107. doi:10.1037/h0076960. PMID 1142063. S2CID 36562997.
  165. ^ Castle, T.; Hensley, C (2002). "Serial Killers with Military Experience: Applying Learning Theory to Serial Murder". International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 46 (4): 453–65. doi:10.1177/0306624X02464007. PMID 12150084. S2CID 35278358.
  166. ^ blennow; Manhem (20 January 2021). "CSF studies in violent offenders". Journal of Neural Transmission. 108: 869–878 – via Forsman A.
  167. ^ Dolan, Eric W. (30 September 2023). "Contrary to popular belief, recent psychology findings suggest aggression isn't always tied to a lack of self-control". PsyPost. Retrieved 2 October 2023.
  168. ^ Smith, P. (2007). "Why has aggression been thought of as maladaptive?". Aggression and Adaptation: the Bright Side to Bad Behavior. Routledge. pp. 65–83. ISBN 978-1-135-59375-9.
  169. ^ Hawley, Patricia H.; Vaughn, Brian E. (2003). "Aggression and Adaptive Functioning: The Bright Side to Bad Behavior". Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 49 (3): 239–42. doi:10.1353/mpq.2003.0012. JSTOR 23096055. S2CID 54998386.
  170. ^ Bjørkly, Stâl (2006), "Psychological Theories of Aggression: Principles and Application to Practice", Violence in Mental Health Settings, New York, NY: Springer New York, pp. 27–46, doi:10.1007/978-0-387-33965-8_2, ISBN 978-0-387-33964-1, retrieved 5 May 2021
  171. ^ Ferguson, Christopher J. (2010). "Blazing angels or resident evil? Can violent video games be a force for good?" (PDF). Review of General Psychology. 14 (2): 68–81. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.360.3176. doi:10.1037/a0018941. S2CID 3053432.

Further reading edit

  • R. Douglas Fields, "The Roots of Human Aggression: Experiments in humans and animals have started to identify how violent behaviors begin in the brain", Scientific American, vol. 320, no. 5 (May 2019), pp. 64–71. "Decisions to take aggressive action are risky and bring into play specific neural circuits." (p. 66.)

External links edit

  • Aggression and Violent Behavior, a Review Journal
  • International Society for Research on Aggression (ISRA)
  • Problems in the Concepts and Definitions of Aggression, Violence and some Related Terms by Johan van der Dennen, originally published in 1980
  • Aggression and brain asymmetry 12 August 2017 at the Wayback Machine

aggression, aggressive, aggressive, behavior, redirect, here, other, uses, aggressive, disambiguation, disambiguation, aggressive, behavior, journal, hostile, antagonistic, behavior, often, with, intent, cause, harm, although, channeled, into, creative, practi. Aggressive and Aggressive behavior redirect here For other uses see Aggressive disambiguation Aggression disambiguation and Aggressive Behavior journal Aggression is hostile and antagonistic behavior often with the intent to cause harm although it can be channeled into creative and practical outlets for some 1 It may occur either reactively or without provocation In humans aggression can be caused by various triggers For example built up frustration due to blocked goals or perceived disrespect 2 Human aggression can be classified into direct and indirect aggression whilst the former is characterized by physical or verbal behavior intended to cause harm to someone the latter is characterized by behavior intended to harm the social relations of an individual or group 3 4 5 Many mammals such as the dog bare their teeth as a sign of aggression a form of aposematism Two warthogs preparing to fightIn definitions commonly used in the social sciences and behavioral sciences aggression is an action or response by an individual that delivers something unpleasant to another person 6 Some definitions include that the individual must intend to harm another person 7 In an interdisciplinary perspective aggression is regarded as an ensemble of mechanism formed during the course of evolution in order to assert oneself relatives or friends against others to gain or to defend resources ultimate causes by harmful damaging means These mechanisms are often motivated by emotions like fear frustration anger feelings of stress dominance or pleasure proximate causes Sometimes aggressive behavior serves as a stress relief or a subjective feeling of power 8 9 Predatory or defensive behavior between members of different species may not be considered aggression in the same sense Aggression can take a variety of forms which may be expressed physically or communicated verbally or non verbally including anti predator aggression defensive aggression fear induced predatory aggression dominance aggression inter male aggression resident intruder aggression maternal aggression species specific aggression sex related aggression territorial aggression isolation induced aggression irritable aggression and brain stimulation induced aggression hypothalamus There are two subtypes of human aggression 1 controlled instrumental subtype purposeful or goal oriented and 2 reactive impulsive subtype often elicits uncontrollable actions that are inappropriate or undesirable Aggression differs from what is commonly called assertiveness although the terms are often used interchangeably among laypeople as in phrases such as an aggressive salesperson 10 Contents 1 Overview 2 Etymology 3 Ethology 3 1 Between species and groups 3 2 Within a group 3 3 Aggression fear and curiosity 4 Evolutionary explanations 4 1 Violence and conflict 4 2 Gender 4 2 1 General 4 2 2 In intimate relationships 4 2 3 Physiological factors 5 Physiology 5 1 Brain pathways 5 2 Testosterone 5 2 1 Challenge hypothesis 5 2 2 Effects on the nervous system 5 2 3 General associations with behavior 5 3 Dehydroepiandrosterone 5 4 Glucocorticoids 5 5 Pheromones 6 Genetics 7 Society and culture 7 1 Culture 7 1 1 Media 7 2 Children 7 2 1 Typical expectations 7 2 2 Aggression triggers 7 3 Gender 7 4 Situational factors 7 5 As a positive adaptation theory 8 See also 9 References 10 Further reading 11 External linksOverview editDollard et al 1939 proposed that aggression was due to frustration which was described as an unpleasant emotion resulting from any interference with achieving a rewarding goal 11 Berkowitz 12 extended this frustration aggression hypothesis and proposed that it is not so much the frustration as the unpleasant emotion that evokes aggressive tendencies and that all aversive events produce negative affect and thereby aggressive tendencies as well as fear tendencies Besides conditioned stimuli Archer categorized aggression evoking as well as fear evoking stimuli into three groups namely pain novelty and frustration although he also described looming which refers to an object rapidly moving towards the visual sensors of a subject and can be categorized as intensity 13 Aggression can have adaptive benefits or negative effects Aggressive behavior is an individual or collective social interaction that is a hostile behavior with the intention of inflicting damage or harm 3 4 Two broad categories of aggression are commonly distinguished One includes affective emotional and hostile reactive or retaliatory aggression that is a response to provocation and the other includes instrumental goal oriented or predatory in which aggression is used as a means to achieve a goal 14 An example of hostile aggression would be a person who punches someone who insulted him or her An instrumental form of aggression would be armed robbery Research on violence from a range of disciplines lend some support to a distinction between affective and predatory aggression 15 However some researchers question the usefulness of a hostile versus instrumental distinction in humans despite its ubiquity in research because most real life cases involve mixed motives and interacting causes 16 A number of classifications and dimensions of aggression have been suggested These depend on such things as whether the aggression is verbal or physical whether or not it involves relational aggression such as covert bullying and social manipulation 17 whether harm to others is intended or not whether it is carried out actively or expressed passively and whether the aggression is aimed directly or indirectly Classification may also encompass aggression related emotions e g anger and mental states e g impulsivity hostility 18 Aggression may occur in response to non social as well as social factors and can have a close relationship with stress coping style 19 Aggression may be displayed in order to intimidate The operative definition of aggression may be affected by moral or political views Examples are the axiomatic moral view called the non aggression principle and the political rules governing the behavior of one country toward another 20 Likewise in competitive sports or in the workplace some forms of aggression may be sanctioned and others not see Workplace aggression 21 Aggressive behaviors are associated with adjustment problems and several psychopathological symptoms such as Antisocial Personality Disorder Borderline Personality Disorder and Intermittent Explosive Disorder 22 Biological approaches conceptualize aggression as an internal energy released by external stimuli a product of evolution through natural selection part of genetics a product of hormonal fluctuations Psychological approaches conceptualize aggression as a destructive instinct a response to frustration an affect excited by a negative stimulus a result of observed learning of society and diversified reinforcement a resultant of variables that affect personal and situational environments 23 24 Etymology editThe term aggression comes from the Latin word aggressio meaning attack The Latin was itself a joining of ad and gradi which meant step at The first known use dates back to 1611 in the sense of an unprovoked attack 25 A psychological sense of hostile or destructive behavior dates back to a 1912 English translation of Sigmund Freud s writing 26 Alfred Adler theorized about an aggressive drive in 1908 Child raising experts began to refer to aggression rather than anger from the 1930s 27 Ethology edit nbsp Male elephant seals fightingEthologists study aggression as it relates to the interaction and evolution of animals in natural settings In such settings aggression can involve bodily contact such as biting hitting or pushing but most conflicts are settled by threat displays and intimidating thrusts that cause no physical harm This form of aggression may include the display of body size antlers claws or teeth stereotyped signals including facial expressions vocalizations such as bird song the release of chemicals and changes in coloration 28 The term agonistic behaviour is sometimes used to refer to these forms of behavior Most ethologists believe that aggression confers biological advantages Aggression may help an animal secure territory including resources such as food and water Aggression between males often occurs to secure mating opportunities and results in selection of the healthier more vigorous animal Aggression may also occur for self protection or to protect offspring 29 Aggression between groups of animals may also confer advantage for example hostile behavior may force a population of animals into a new territory where the need to adapt to a new environment may lead to an increase in genetic flexibility 30 Between species and groups edit The most apparent type of interspecific aggression is that observed in the interaction between a predator and its prey However according to many researchers predation is not aggression A cat does not hiss or arch its back when pursuing a rat and the active areas in its hypothalamus resemble those that reflect hunger rather than those that reflect aggression 31 However others refer to this behavior as predatory aggression and point out cases that resemble hostile behavior such as mouse killing by rats 32 In aggressive mimicry a predator has the appearance of a harmless organism or object attractive to the prey when the prey approaches the predator attacks An animal defending against a predator may engage in either fight or flight or tend and befriend in response to predator attack or threat of attack depending on its estimate of the predator s strength relative to its own Alternative defenses include a range of antipredator adaptations including alarm signals An example of an alarm signal is nerol a chemical which is found in the mandibular glands of Trigona fulviventris individuals 33 Release of nerol by T fulviventris individuals in the nest has been shown to decrease the number of individuals leaving the nest by fifty percent as well as increasing aggressive behaviors like biting 33 Alarm signals like nerol can also act as attraction signals in T fulviventris individuals that have been captured by a predator may release nerol to attract nestmates who will proceed to attack or bite the predator 33 Aggression between groups is determined partly by willingness to fight which depends on a number of factors including numerical advantage distance from home territories how often the groups encounter each other competitive abilities differences in body size and whose territory is being invaded 34 Also an individual is more likely to become aggressive if other aggressive group members are nearby 35 One particular phenomenon the formation of coordinated coalitions that raid neighbouring territories to kill conspecifics has only been documented in two species in the animal kingdom common chimpanzees and humans 36 Within a group edit Aggression between conspecifics in a group typically involves access to resources and breeding opportunities One of its most common functions is to establish a dominance hierarchy This occurs in many species by aggressive encounters between contending males when they are first together in a common environment 37 Usually the more aggressive animals become the more dominant 38 39 In test situations most of the conspecific aggression ceases about 24 hours after the group of animals is brought together 37 40 Aggression has been defined from this viewpoint as behavior which is intended to increase the social dominance of the organism relative to the dominance position of other organisms 41 Losing confrontations may be called social defeat and winning or losing is associated with a range of practical and psychological consequences 42 Conflicts between animals occur in many contexts such as between potential mating partners between parents and offspring between siblings and between competitors for resources Group living animals may dispute over the direction of travel or the allocation of time to joint activities Various factors limit the escalation of aggression including communicative displays conventions and routines In addition following aggressive incidents various forms of conflict resolution have been observed in mammalian species particularly in gregarious primates These can mitigate or repair possible adverse consequences especially for the recipient of aggression who may become vulnerable to attacks by other members of a group Conciliatory acts vary by species and may involve specific gestures or simply more proximity and interaction between the individuals involved However conflicts over food are rarely followed by post conflict reunions even though they are the most frequent type in foraging primates 43 Other questions that have been considered in the study of primate aggression including in humans is how aggression affects the organization of a group what costs are incurred by aggression and why some primates avoid aggressive behavior 44 For example bonobo chimpanzee groups are known for low levels of aggression within a partially matriarchal society Captive animals including primates may show abnormal levels of social aggression and self harm that are related to aspects of the physical or social environment this depends on the species and individual factors such as gender age and background e g raised wild or captive 45 Aggression fear and curiosity edit Within ethology it has long been recognized that there is a relation between aggression fear and curiosity 46 A cognitive approach to this relationship puts aggression in the broader context of inconsistency reduction and proposes that aggressive behavior is caused by an inconsistency between a desired or expected situation and the actually perceived situation e g frustration and functions to forcefully manipulate the perception into matching the expected situation 47 13 48 In this approach when the inconsistency between perception and expectancy is small learning as a result of curiosity reduces inconsistency by updating expectancy to match perception If the inconsistency is larger fear or aggressive behavior may be employed to alter the perception in order to make it match expectancy depending on the size of the inconsistency as well as the specific context Uninhibited fear results in fleeing thereby removing the inconsistent stimulus from the perceptual field and resolving the inconsistency In some cases thwarted escape may trigger aggressive behavior in an attempt to remove the thwarting stimulus 48 Evolutionary explanations editLike many behaviors aggression can be examined in terms of its ability to help an animal itself survive and reproduce or alternatively to risk survival and reproduction This cost benefit analysis can be looked at in terms of evolution However there are profound differences in the extent of acceptance of a biological or evolutionary basis for human aggression 49 According to the male warrior hypothesis intergroup aggression represents an opportunity for men to gain access to mates territory resources and increased status As such conflicts may have created selection evolutionary pressures for psychological mechanisms in men to initiate intergroup aggression 50 51 Violence and conflict edit Aggression can involve violence that may be adaptive under certain circumstances in terms of natural selection This is most obviously the case in terms of attacking prey to obtain food or in anti predatory defense It may also be the case in competition between members of the same species or subgroup if the average reward e g status access to resources protection of self or kin outweighs average costs e g injury exclusion from the group death There are some hypotheses of specific adaptions for violence in humans under certain circumstances including for homicide but it is often unclear what behaviors may have been selected for and what may have been a byproduct as in the case of collective violence 52 53 54 55 Although aggressive encounters are ubiquitous in the animal kingdom with often high stakes most encounters that involve aggression may be resolved through posturing or displaying and trial of strength Game theory is used to understand how such behaviors might spread by natural selection within a population and potentially become Evolutionary Stable Strategies An initial model of resolution of conflicts is the hawk dove game Others include the Sequential assessment model and the Energetic war of attrition These try to understand not just one off encounters but protracted stand offs and mainly differ in the criteria by which an individual decides to give up rather than risk loss and harm in physical conflict such as through estimates of resource holding potential 56 Gender edit General edit Gender plays an important role in human aggression There are multiple theories that seek to explain findings that males and females of the same species can have differing aggressive behaviors One review concluded that male aggression tended to produce pain or physical injury whereas female aggression tended towards psychological or social harm 57 In general sexual dimorphism can be attributed to greater intraspecific competition in one sex either between rivals for access to mates and or to be chosen by mates This may stem from the other gender being constrained by providing greater parental investment in terms of factors such as gamete production gestation lactation or upbringing of young Although there is much variation in species generally the more physically aggressive sex is the male particularly in mammals In species where parental care by both sexes is required there tends to be less of a difference When the female can leave the male to care for the offspring then females may be the larger and more physically aggressive Competitiveness despite parental investment has also been observed in some species 58 A related factor is the rate at which males and females are able to mate again after producing offspring and the basic principles of sexual selection are also influenced by ecological factors affecting the ways or extent to which one sex can compete for the other The role of such factors in human evolution is controversial The pattern of male and female aggression is argued to be consistent with evolved sexually selected behavioral differences while alternative or complementary views emphasize conventional social roles stemming from physical evolved differences 59 Aggression in women may have evolved to be on average less physically dangerous and more covert or indirect 60 61 However there are critiques for using animal behavior to explain human behavior especially in the application of evolutionary explanations to contemporary human behavior including differences between the genders 62 According to the 2015 International Encyclopedia of the Social amp Behavioral Sciences sex differences in aggression is one of the most robust and oldest findings in psychology 63 Past meta analyses in the encyclopedia found males regardless of age engaged in more physical and verbal aggression while small effect for females engaging in more indirect aggression such as rumor spreading or gossiping 63 It also found males tend to engage in more unprovoked aggression at higher frequency than females 63 This analysis also conforms with the Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology which reviewed past analysis which found men to use more verbal and physical aggression with the difference being greater in the physical type 64 There are more recent findings that show that differences in male and female aggression appear at about two years of age though the differences in aggression are more consistent in middle aged children and adolescence Tremblay Japel and Perusse 1999 asserted that physically aggressive behaviors such as kicking biting and hitting are age typical expressions of innate and spontaneous reactions to biological drives such as anger hunger and affiliation 65 Girls relational aggression meaning non physical or indirect tends to increase after age two while physical aggression decreases There was no significant difference in aggression between males and females before two years of age 66 A possible explanation for this could be that girls develop language skills more quickly than boys and therefore have better ways of verbalizing their wants and needs They are more likely to use communication when trying to retrieve a toy with the words Ask nicely or Say please 67 According to the journal of Aggressive Behaviour an analysis across 9 countries found boys reported more in the use of physical aggression 66 At the same time no consistent sex differences emerged within relational aggression 66 It has been found that girls are more likely than boys to use reactive aggression and then retract but boys are more likely to increase rather than to retract their aggression after their first reaction Studies show girls aggressive tactics included gossip ostracism breaking confidences and criticism of a victim s clothing appearance or personality whereas boys engage in aggression that involves a direct physical and or verbal assault 68 This could be due to the fact that girls frontal lobes develop earlier than boys allowing them to self restrain 67 One factor that shows insignificant differences between male and female aggression is in sports In sports the rate of aggression in both contact and non contact sports is relatively equal Since the establishment of Title IX female sports have increased in competitiveness and importance which could contribute to the evening of aggression and the need to win attitude between both genders Among sex differences found in adult sports were that females have a higher scale of indirect hostility while men have a higher scale of assault 69 Another difference found is that men have up to 20 times higher levels of testosterone than women In intimate relationships edit Some studies suggest that romantic involvement in adolescence decreases aggression in males and females but decreases at a higher rate in females Females will seem more desirable to their mate if they fit in with society and females that are aggressive do not usually fit well in society They can often be viewed as antisocial Female aggression is not considered the norm in society and going against the norm can sometimes prevent one from getting a mate 70 However studies have shown that an increasing number of women are getting arrested for domestic violence charges In many states women now account for a quarter to a third of all domestic violence arrests up from less than 10 percent a decade ago The new statistics reflect a reality documented in research women are perpetrators as well as victims of family violence 71 However another equally possible explanation is a case of improved diagnostics it has become more acceptable for men to report female domestic violence to the authorities while at the same time actual female domestic violence has not increased at all 72 This could be the case in a situation where men had become less ashamed of reporting female violence against them such a situation could conceivably lead to an increasing number of women being arrested despite the actual number of violent women remaining the same In addition males in competitive sports are often advised by their coaches not to be in intimate relationships based on the premises that they become more docile and less aggressive during an athletic event The circumstances in which males and females experience aggression are also different A study showed that social anxiety and stress was positively correlated with aggression in males meaning as stress and social anxiety increases so does aggression Furthermore a male with higher social skills has a lower rate of aggressive behavior than a male with lower social skills In females higher rates of aggression were only correlated with higher rates of stress Other than biological factors that contribute to aggression there are physical factors as well 73 Physiological factors edit Regarding sexual dimorphism humans fall into an intermediate group with moderate sex differences in body size but relatively large testes This is a typical pattern of primates where several males and females live together in a group and the male faces an intermediate number of challenges from other males compared to exclusive polygyny and monogamy but frequent sperm competition 74 Evolutionary psychology and sociobiology have also discussed and produced theories for some specific forms of male aggression such as sociobiological theories of rape and theories regarding the Cinderella effect Another evolutionary theory explaining gender differences in aggression is the Male Warrior hypothesis which explains that males have psychologically evolved for intergroup aggression in order to gain access to mates resources territory and status 50 51 Physiology editBrain pathways edit Many researchers focus on the brain to explain aggression Numerous circuits within both neocortical and subcortical structures play a central role in controlling aggressive behavior depending on the species and the exact role of pathways may vary depending on the type of trigger or intention 75 4 In mammals the hypothalamus and periaqueductal gray of the midbrain are critical areas as shown in studies on cats rats and monkeys These brain areas control the expression of both behavioral and autonomic components of aggression in these species including vocalization Electrical stimulation of the hypothalamus causes aggressive behavior 76 and the hypothalamus has receptors that help determine aggression levels based on their interactions with serotonin and vasopressin 77 In rodents activation of estrogen receptor expressing neurons in the ventrolateral portion of the ventromedial hypothalamus VMHvl was found to be sufficient to initiate aggression in both males and females 78 79 Midbrain areas involved in aggression have direct connections with both the brainstem nuclei controlling these functions and with structures such as the amygdala and prefrontal cortex Stimulation of the amygdala results in augmented aggressive behavior in hamsters 80 81 while lesions of an evolutionarily homologous area in the lizard greatly reduce competitive drive and aggression Bauman et al 2006 82 In rhesus monkeys neonatal lesions in the amygdala or hippocampus results in reduced expression of social dominance related to the regulation of aggression and fear 83 Several experiments in attack primed Syrian golden hamsters for example support the claim of circuitry within the amygdala being involved in control of aggression 81 The role of the amygdala is less clear in primates and appears to depend more on situational context with lesions leading to increases in either social affiliatory or aggressive responses Amygdalotomy which involves removing or destroying parts of the amygdala has been performed on people to reduce their violent behaviour The broad area of the cortex known as the prefrontal cortex PFC is crucial for self control and inhibition of impulses including inhibition of aggression and emotions Reduced activity of the prefrontal cortex in particular its medial and orbitofrontal portions has been associated with violent antisocial aggression 84 In addition reduced response inhibition has been found in violent offenders compared to non violent offenders 75 The role of the chemicals in the brain particularly neurotransmitters in aggression has also been examined This varies depending on the pathway the context and other factors such as gender A deficit in serotonin has been theorized to have a primary role in causing impulsivity and aggression At least one epigenetic study supports this supposition 85 Nevertheless low levels of serotonin transmission may explain a vulnerability to impulsiveness potential aggression and may have an effect through interactions with other neurochemical systems These include dopamine systems which are generally associated with attention and motivation toward rewards and operate at various levels Norepinephrine also known as noradrenaline may influence aggression responses both directly and indirectly through the hormonal system the sympathetic nervous system or the central nervous system including the brain It appears to have different effects depending on the type of triggering stimulus for example social isolation rank versus shock chemical agitation which appears not to have a linear relationship with aggression Similarly GABA although associated with inhibitory functions at many CNS synapses sometimes shows a positive correlation with aggression including when potentiated by alcohol 86 87 The hormonal neuropeptides vasopressin and oxytocin play a key role in complex social behaviours in many mammals such as regulating attachment social recognition and aggression Vasopressin has been implicated in male typical social behaviors which includes aggression Oxytocin may have a particular role in regulating female bonds with offspring and mates including the use of protective aggression Initial studies in humans suggest some similar effects 88 89 In human aggressive behavior has been associated with abnormalities in three principal regulatory systems in the body serotonin systems catecholamine systems and the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis Abnormalities in these systems also are known to be induced by stress either severe acute stress or chronic low grade stress 90 Testosterone edit See also Testosterone Aggression Early androgenization has an organizational effect on the developing brains of both males and females making more neural circuits that control sexual behavior as well as intermale and interfemale aggression become more sensitive to testosterone 91 There are noticeable sex differences in aggression Testosterone is present to a lesser extent in females who may be more sensitive to its effects Animal studies have also indicated a link between incidents of aggression and the individual level of circulating testosterone However results in relation to primates particularly humans are less clear cut and are at best only suggestive of a positive association in some contexts 92 In humans there is a seasonal variation in aggression associated with changes in testosterone 93 For example in some primate species such as rhesus monkeys and baboons females are more likely to engage in fights around the time of ovulation as well as right before menstruation 91 If the results were the same in humans as they are in rhesus monkeys and baboons then the increase in aggressive behaviors during ovulation is explained by the decline in estrogen levels This makes normal testosterone levels more effective 94 Castrated mice and rats exhibit lower levels of aggression Males castrated as neonates exhibit low levels of aggression even when given testosterone throughout their development Challenge hypothesis edit The challenge hypothesis outlines the dynamic relationship between plasma testosterone levels and aggression in mating contexts in many species It proposes that testosterone is linked to aggression when it is beneficial for reproduction such as in mate guarding and preventing the encroachment of intrasexual rivals The challenge hypothesis predicts that seasonal patterns in testosterone levels in a species are a function of mating system monogamy versus polygyny paternal care and male male aggression in seasonal breeders This pattern between testosterone and aggression was first observed in seasonally breeding birds such as the song sparrow where testosterone levels rise modestly with the onset of the breeding season to support basic reproductive functions 95 The hypothesis has been subsequently expanded and modified to predict relationships between testosterone and aggression in other species For example chimpanzees which are continuous breeders show significantly raised testosterone levels and aggressive male male interactions when receptive and fertile females are present 96 Currently no research has specified a relationship between the modified challenge hypothesis and human behavior or the human nature of concealed ovulation although some suggest it may apply 93 Effects on the nervous system edit nbsp Testosterone to Estradiol conversionAnother line of research has focused on the proximate effects of circulating testosterone on the nervous system as mediated by local metabolism within the brain Testosterone can be metabolized to estradiol by the enzyme aromatase or to dihydrotestosterone DHT by 5a reductase 97 Aromatase is highly expressed in regions involved in the regulation of aggressive behavior such as the amygdala and hypothalamus In studies using genetic knockout techniques in inbred mice male mice that lacked a functional aromatase enzyme displayed a marked reduction in aggression Long term treatment with estradiol partially restored aggressive behavior suggesting that the neural conversion of circulating testosterone to estradiol and its effect on estrogen receptors influences inter male aggression In addition two different estrogen receptors ERa and ERb have been identified as having the ability to exert different effects on aggression in mice However the effect of estradiol appears to vary depending on the strain of mouse and in some strains it reduces aggression during long days 16 h of light while during short days 8 h of light estradiol rapidly increases aggression 97 Another hypothesis is that testosterone influences brain areas that control behavioral reactions Studies in animal models indicate that aggression is affected by several interconnected cortical and subcortical structures within the so called social behavior network A study involving lesions and electrical chemical stimulation in rodents and cats revealed that such a neural network consists of the medial amygdala medial hypothalamus and periaqueductal grey PAG and it positively modulates reactive aggression 98 Moreover a study done in human subjects showed that prefrontal amygdala connectivity is modulated by endogenous testosterone during social emotional behavior 99 In human studies testosterone aggression research has also focused on the role of the orbitofrontal cortex OFC This brain area is strongly associated with impulse control and self regulation systems that integrate emotion motivation and cognition to guide context appropriate behavior 100 Patients with localized lesions to the OFC engage in heightened reactive aggression 101 Aggressive behavior may be regulated by testosterone via reduced medial OFC engagement following social provocation 100 When measuring participants salivary testosterone higher levels can predict subsequent aggressive behavioral reactions to unfairness faced during a task Moreover brain scanning with fMRI shows reduced activity in the medial OFC during such reactions Such findings may suggest that a specific brain region the OFC is a key factor in understanding reactive aggression General associations with behavior edit Scientists have for a long time been interested in the relationship between testosterone and aggressive behavior In most species males are more aggressive than females Castration of males usually has a pacifying effect on aggressive behavior in males In humans males engage in crime and especially violent crime more than females The involvement in crime usually rises in the early teens to mid teens which happen at the same time as testosterone levels rise Research on the relationship between testosterone and aggression is difficult since the only reliable measurement of brain testosterone is by a lumbar puncture which is not done for research purposes Studies therefore have often instead used more unreliable measurements from blood or saliva 102 The Handbook of Crime Correlates a review of crime studies states most studies support a link between adult criminality and testosterone although the relationship is modest if examined separately for each sex However nearly all studies of juvenile delinquency and testosterone are not significant Most studies have also found testosterone to be associated with behaviors or personality traits linked with criminality such as antisocial behavior and alcoholism Many studies have also been done on the relationship between more general aggressive behavior feelings and testosterone About half the studies have found a relationship and about half no relationship 102 Studies of testosterone levels of male athletes before and after a competition revealed that testosterone levels rise shortly before their matches as if in anticipation of the competition and are dependent on the outcome of the event testosterone levels of winners are high relative to those of losers No specific response of testosterone levels to competition was observed in female athletes although a mood difference was noted 103 In addition some experiments have failed to find a relationship between testosterone levels and aggression in humans 104 22 105 The possible correlation between testosterone and aggression could explain the roid rage that can result from anabolic steroid use 106 107 although an effect of abnormally high levels of steroids does not prove an effect at physiological levels Dehydroepiandrosterone edit Dehydroepiandrosterone DHEA is the most abundant circulating androgen hormone and can be rapidly metabolized within target tissues into potent androgens and estrogens Gonadal steroids generally regulate aggression during the breeding season but non gonadal steroids may regulate aggression during the non breeding season Castration of various species in the non breeding season has no effect on territorial aggression In several avian studies circulating DHEA has been found to be elevated in birds during the non breeding season These data support the idea that non breeding birds combine adrenal and or gonadal DHEA synthesis with neural DHEA metabolism to maintain territorial behavior when gonadal testosterone secretion is low Similar results have been found in studies involving different strains of rats mice and hamsters DHEA levels also have been studied in humans and may play a role in human aggression Circulating DHEAS its sulfated ester levels rise during adrenarche 7 years of age while plasma testosterone levels are relatively low This implies that aggression in pre pubertal children with aggressive conduct disorder might be correlated with plasma DHEAS rather than plasma testosterone suggesting an important link between DHEAS and human aggressive behavior 97 Glucocorticoids edit Glucocorticoid hormones have an important role in regulating aggressive behavior In adult rats acute injections of corticosterone promote aggressive behavior and acute reduction of corticosterone decreases aggression however a chronic reduction of corticosterone levels can produce abnormally aggressive behavior In addition glucocorticoids affect development of aggression and establishment of social hierarchies Adult mice with low baseline levels of corticosterone are more likely to become dominant than are mice with high baseline corticosterone levels 97 Glucocorticoids are released by the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal HPA axis in response to stress of which cortisol is the most prominent in humans Results in adults suggest that reduced levels of cortisol linked to lower fear or a reduced stress response can be associated with more aggression However it may be that proactive aggression is associated with low cortisol levels while reactive aggression may be accompanied by elevated levels Differences in assessments of cortisol may also explain a diversity of results particularly in children 92 The HPA axis is related to the general fight or flight response or acute stress reaction and the role of catecholamines such as epinephrine popularly known as adrenaline Pheromones edit In many animals aggression can be linked to pheromones released between conspecifics In mice major urinary proteins Mups have been demonstrated to promote innate aggressive behavior in males 108 109 and can be mediated by neuromodulatory systems 110 Mups activate olfactory sensory neurons in the vomeronasal organ VNO a subsystem of the nose known to detect pheromones via specific sensory receptors of mice 109 and rats 111 Pheremones have also been identified in fruit flies detected by neurons in the antenna that send a message to the brain eliciting aggression it has been noted that aggression pheremones have not been identified in humans 112 Genetics editMain article Genetics of aggression In general differences in a continuous phenotype such as aggression are likely to result from the action of a large number of genes each of small effect which interact with each other and the environment through development and life In a non mammalian example of genes related to aggression the fruitless gene in fruit flies is a critical determinant of certain sexually dimorphic behaviors and its artificial alteration can result in a reversal of stereotypically male and female patterns of aggression in fighting However in what was thought to be a relatively clear case inherent complexities have been reported in deciphering the connections between interacting genes in an environmental context and a social phenotype involving multiple behavioral and sensory interactions with another organism 113 In mice candidate genes for differentiating aggression between the sexes are the Sry sex determining region Y gene located on the Y chromosome and the Sts steroid sulfatase gene The Sts gene encodes the steroid sulfatase enzyme which is pivotal in the regulation of neurosteroid biosynthesis It is expressed in both sexes is correlated with levels of aggression among male mice and increases dramatically in females after parturition and during lactation corresponding to the onset of maternal aggression 81 At least one study has found a possible epigenetic signature i e decreased methylation at a specific CpG site on the promoter region of the serotonin receptor 5 HT3a that is associated with maternal aggression among human subjects 85 Mice with experimentally elevated sensitivity to oxidative stress through inhibition of copper zinc superoxide dismutase SOD1 activity were tested for aggressive behavior 114 Males completely deficient in SOD1 were found to be more aggressive than both wild type males and males that express 50 of this antioxidant enzyme They were also faster to attack another male The causal connection between SOD1 deficiency and increased aggression is not yet understood In humans there is good evidence that the basic human neural architecture underpinning the potential for flexible aggressive responses is influenced by genes as well as environment In terms of variation between individual people more than 100 twin and adoption studies have been conducted in recent decades examining the genetic basis of aggressive behavior and related constructs such as conduct disorders According to a meta analysis published in 2002 approximately 40 of variation between individuals is explained by differences in genes and 60 by differences in environment mainly non shared environmental influences rather than those that would be shared by being raised together However such studies have depended on self report or observation by others including parents which complicates interpretation of the results The few laboratory based analyses have not found significant amounts of individual variation in aggression explicable by genetic variation in the human population Furthermore linkage and association studies that seek to identify specific genes for example that influence neurotransmitter or hormone levels have generally resulted in contradictory findings characterized by failed attempts at replication One possible factor is an allele variant of the MAO A gene which in interaction with certain life events such as childhood maltreatment which may show a main effect on its own can influence development of brain regions such as the amygdala and as a result some types of behavioral response may be more likely The generally unclear picture has been compared to equally difficult findings obtained in regard to other complex behavioral phenotypes 115 116 For example both 7R and 5R ADHD linked VNTR alleles of dopamine receptor D4 gene are directly associated with the incidence of proactive aggression in the men with no history of ADHD 117 Society and culture editHumans share aspects of aggression with non human animals and have specific aspects and complexity related to factors such as genetics early development social learning and flexibility culture and morals Konrad Lorenz stated in his 1963 classic On Aggression that human behavior is shaped by four main survival seeking animal drives Taken together these drives hunger fear reproduction and aggression achieve natural selection 118 E O Wilson elaborated in On Human Nature that aggression is typically a means of gaining control over resources Aggression is thus aggravated during times when high population densities generate resource shortages 119 According to Richard Leakey and his colleagues aggression in humans has also increased by becoming more interested in ownership and by defending his or her property 120 However UNESCO adopted the Seville Statement of Violence in 1989 that refuted claims by evolutionary scientists that genetics by itself was the sole cause of aggression 121 122 Social and cultural aspects may significantly interfere with the distinct expression of aggressiveness For example a high population density when associated with a decrease of available resources might be a significant intervening variable for the occurrence of violent acts 123 Culture edit Culture is one factor that plays a role in aggression Tribal or band societies existing before or outside of modern states have sometimes been depicted as peaceful noble savages The ǃKung people were described as The Harmless People in a popular work by Elizabeth Marshall Thomas in 1958 124 while Lawrence Keeley s 1996 War Before Civilization suggested that regular warfare without modern technology was conducted by most groups throughout human history including most Native American tribes 125 Studies of hunter gatherers show a range of different societies In general aggression conflict and violence sometimes occur but direct confrontation is generally avoided and conflict is socially managed by a variety of verbal and non verbal methods Different rates of aggression or violence currently or in the past within or between groups have been linked to the structuring of societies and environmental conditions influencing factors such as resource or property acquisition land and subsistence techniques and population change 126 American psychologist Peter Gray hypothesizes that band hunter gatherer societies are able to reduce aggression while maintaining relatively peaceful egalitarian relations between members through various methods such as fostering a playful spirit in all areas of life the use of humor to counter the tendency of any one person to dominate the group and non coercive or indulgent child rearing practices Gray likens hunter gatherer bands to social play groups while stressing that such play is not frivolous or even easy at all times 127 According to Gray Social play that is play involving more than one player is necessarily egalitarian It always requires a suspension of aggression and dominance along with a heightened sensitivity to the needs and desires of the other players 128 Joan Durrant at the University of Manitoba writes that a number of studies have found physical punishment to be associated with higher levels of aggression against parents siblings peers and spouses even when controlling for other factors 129 According to Elizabeth Gershoff at the University of Texas at Austin the more that children are physically punished the more likely they are as adults to act violently towards family members including intimate partners 130 In countries where physical punishment of children is perceived as being more culturally accepted it is less strongly associated with increased aggression however physical punishment has been found to predict some increase in child aggression regardless of culture 131 While these associations do not prove causality a number of longitudinal studies suggest that the experience of physical punishment has a direct causal effect on later aggressive behaviors 129 In examining several longitudinal studies that investigated the path from disciplinary spanking to aggression in children from preschool age through adolescence Gershoff concluded Spanking consistently predicted increases in children s aggression over time regardless of how aggressive children were when the spanking occurred 132 similar results were found by Catherine Taylor at Tulane University in 2010 133 Family violence researcher Murray A Straus argues There are many reasons this evidence has been ignored One of the most important is the belief that spanking is more effective than nonviolent discipline and is therefore sometimes necessary despite the risk of harmful side effects 134 Analyzing aggression culturally or politically is complicated by the fact that the label aggressive can itself be used as a way of asserting a judgement from a particular point of view according to whom Whether a coercive or violent method of social control is perceived as aggression or as legitimate versus illegitimate aggression depends on the position of the relevant parties in relation to the social order of their culture This in turn can relate to factors such as norms for coordinating actions and dividing resources what is considered self defense or provocation attitudes towards outsiders attitudes towards specific groups such as women disabled people or those with lower status the availability of alternative conflict resolution strategies trade interdependence and collective security pacts fears and impulses and ultimate goals regarding material and social outcomes 123 Cross cultural research has found differences in attitudes towards aggression in different cultures In one questionnaire study of university students in addition to men overall justifying some types of aggression more than women United States respondents justified defensive physical aggression more readily than Japanese or Spanish respondents whereas Japanese students preferred direct verbal aggression but not indirect more than their American and Spanish counterparts 135 Within American culture southern men were shown in a study on university students to be more affected and to respond more aggressively than northerners when randomly insulted after being bumped into which was theoretically related to a traditional culture of honor in the Southern United States or saving face 136 Other cultural themes sometimes applied to the study of aggression include individualistic versus collectivist styles which may relate for example to whether disputes are responded to with open competition or by accommodating and avoiding conflicts In a study including 62 countries school principals reported aggressive student behavior more often the more individualist and hence less collectivist their country s culture 137 Other comparisons made in relation to aggression or war include democratic versus authoritarian political systems and egalitarian versus stratified societies 123 The economic system known as capitalism has been viewed by some as reliant on the leveraging of human competitiveness and aggression in pursuit of resources and trade which has been considered in both positive and negative terms 138 Attitudes about the social acceptability of particular acts or targets of aggression are also important factors This can be highly controversial as for example in disputes between religions or nation states for example in regard to the Arab Israeli conflict 139 140 Media edit Main article Media violence research Some scholars believe that behaviors like aggression may be partially learned by watching and imitating people s behavior while other researchers have concluded that the media may have some small effects on aggression 141 There is also research questioning this view 142 For instance a long term outcome study of youth found no long term relationship between playing violent video games and youth violence or bullying 143 One study suggested there is a smaller effect of violent video games on aggression than has been found with television violence on aggression This effect is positively associated with type of game violence and negatively associated to time spent playing the games 144 The author concluded that insufficient evidence exists to link video game violence with aggression However another study suggested links to aggressive behavior 145 Children edit The frequency of physical aggression in humans peaks at around 2 3 years of age It then declines gradually on average 146 147 These observations suggest that physical aggression is not only a learned behavior but that development provides opportunities for the learning and biological development of self regulation However a small subset of children fail to acquire all the necessary self regulatory abilities and tend to show atypical levels of physical aggression across development These may be at risk for later violent behavior or conversely lack of aggression that may be considered necessary within society Some findings suggest that early aggression does not necessarily lead to aggression later on however although the course through early childhood is an important predictor of outcomes in middle childhood In addition physical aggression that continues is likely occurring in the context of family adversity including socioeconomic factors Moreover opposition and status violations in childhood appear to be more strongly linked to social problems in adulthood than simply aggressive antisocial behavior 148 149 Social learning through interactions in early childhood has been seen as a building block for levels of aggression which play a crucial role in the development of peer relationships in middle childhood 150 Overall an interplay of biological social and environmental factors can be considered 151 Some research indicates that changes in the weather can increase the likelihood of children exhibiting deviant behavior 152 Typical expectations edit Young children preparing to enter kindergarten need to develop the socially important skill of being assertive Examples of assertiveness include asking others for information initiating conversation or being able to respond to peer pressure In contrast some young children use aggressive behavior such as hitting or biting as a form of communication Aggressive behavior can impede learning as a skill deficit while assertive behavior can facilitate learning However with young children aggressive behavior is developmentally appropriate and can lead to opportunities of building conflict resolution and communication skills By school age children should learn more socially appropriate forms of communicating such as expressing themselves through verbal or written language if they have not this behavior may signify a disability or developmental delay Aggression triggers edit Physical fear of others Family difficulties Learning neurological or conduct behavior disorders Psychological traumaThe Bobo doll experiment was conducted by Albert Bandura in 1961 In this work Bandura found that children exposed to an aggressive adult model acted more aggressively than those who were exposed to a nonaggressive adult model This experiment suggests that anyone who comes in contact with and interacts with children can affect the way they react and handle situations 153 Summary points from recommendations by national associationsAmerican Academy of Pediatrics 2011 The best way to prevent aggressive behavior is to give your child a stable secure home life with firm loving discipline and full time supervision during the toddler and preschool years Everyone who cares for your child should be a good role model and agree on the rules he s expected to observe as well as the response to use if he disobeys 154 National Association of School Psychologists 2008 Proactive aggression is typically reasoned unemotional and focused on acquiring some goal For example a bully wants peer approval and victim submission and gang members want status and control In contrast reactive aggression is frequently highly emotional and is often the result of biased or deficient cognitive processing on the part of the student 155 Gender edit See also Sex and psychology Gender is a factor that plays a role in both human and animal aggression Males are historically believed to be generally more physically aggressive than females from an early age 156 157 and men commit the vast majority of murders Buss 2005 This is one of the most robust and reliable behavioral sex differences and it has been found across many different age groups and cultures However some empirical studies have found the discrepancy in male and female aggression to be more pronounced in childhood and the gender difference in adults to be modest when studied in an experimental context 57 Still there is evidence that males are quicker to aggression Frey et al 2003 and more likely than females to express their aggression physically 158 When considering indirect forms of non violent aggression such as relational aggression and social rejection some scientists argue that females can be quite aggressive although female aggression is rarely expressed physically 159 160 161 An exception is intimate partner violence that occurs among couples who are engaged married or in some other form of intimate relationship Although females are less likely than males to initiate physical violence they can express aggression by using a variety of non physical means Exactly which method women use to express aggression is something that varies from culture to culture On Bellona Island a culture based on male dominance and physical violence women tend to get into conflicts with other women more frequently than with men When in conflict with males instead of using physical means they make up songs mocking the man which spread across the island and humiliate him If a woman wanted to kill a man she would either convince her male relatives to kill him or hire an assassin Although these two methods involve physical violence both are forms of indirect aggression since the aggressor herself avoids getting directly involved or putting herself in immediate physical danger 162 See also the sections on testosterone and evolutionary explanations for gender differences above Situational factors edit See also Stereotype threat There has been some links between those prone to violence and their alcohol use Those who are prone to violence and use alcohol are more likely to carry out violent acts 163 Alcohol impairs judgment making people much less cautious than they usually are MacDonald et al 1996 It also disrupts the way information is processed Bushman 1993 1997 Bushman amp Cooper 1990 Pain and discomfort also increase aggression Even the simple act of placing one s hands in hot water can cause an aggressive response Hot temperatures have been implicated as a factor in a number of studies One study completed in the midst of the civil rights movement found that riots were more likely on hotter days than cooler ones Carlsmith amp Anderson 1979 Students were found to be more aggressive and irritable after taking a test in a hot classroom Anderson et al 1996 Rule et al 1987 Drivers in cars without air conditioning were also found to be more likely to honk their horns Kenrick amp MacFarlane 1986 which is used as a measure of aggression and has shown links to other factors such as generic symbols of aggression or the visibility of other drivers 164 Frustration is another major cause of aggression The Frustration aggression theory states that aggression increases if a person feels that he or she is being blocked from achieving a goal Aronson et al 2005 One study found that the closeness to the goal makes a difference The study examined people waiting in line and concluded that the 2nd person was more aggressive than the 12th one when someone cut in line Harris 1974 Unexpected frustration may be another factor In a separate study to demonstrate how unexpected frustration leads to increased aggression Kulik amp Brown 1979 selected a group of students as volunteers to make calls for charity donations One group was told that the people they would call would be generous and the collection would be very successful The other group was given no expectations The group that expected success was more upset when no one was pledging than the group who did not expect success everyone actually had horrible success This research suggests that when an expectation does not materialize successful collections unexpected frustration arises which increases aggression There is some evidence to suggest that the presence of violent objects such as a gun can trigger aggression In a study done by Leonard Berkowitz and Anthony Le Page 1967 college students were made angry and then left in the presence of a gun or badminton racket They were then led to believe they were delivering electric shocks to another student as in the Milgram experiment Those who had been in the presence of the gun administered more shocks It is possible that a violence related stimulus increases the likelihood of aggressive cognitions by activating the semantic network A new proposal links military experience to anger and aggression developing aggressive reactions and investigating these effects on those possessing the traits of a serial killer Castle and Hensley state The military provides the social context where servicemen learn aggression violence and murder 165 Post traumatic stress disorder PTSD is also a serious issue in the military also believed to sometimes lead to aggression in soldiers who are suffering from what they witnessed in battle They come back to the civilian world and may still be haunted by flashbacks and nightmares causing severe stress In addition it has been claimed that in the rare minority who are claimed to be inclined toward serial killing violent impulses may be reinforced and refined in war possibly creating more effective murderers 166 As a positive adaptation theory edit Some recent scholarship has questioned traditional psychological conceptualizations of aggression as universally negative 41 167 Most traditional psychological definitions of aggression focus on the harm to the recipient of the aggression implying this is the intent of the aggressor however this may not always be the case 168 From this alternate view although the recipient may or may not be harmed the perceived intent is to increase the status of the aggressor not necessarily to harm the recipient 169 Such scholars contend that traditional definitions of aggression have no validity because of how challenging it is to study directly 170 From this view rather than concepts such as assertiveness aggression violence and criminal violence existing as distinct constructs they exist instead along a continuum with moderate levels of aggression being most adaptive 41 Such scholars do not consider this a trivial difference noting that many traditional researchers aggression measurements may measure outcomes lower down in the continuum at levels which are adaptive yet they generalize their findings to non adaptive levels of aggression thus losing precision 171 See also editAggressionism Aggressive narcissism Bullying Child abuse Conflict disambiguation Displaced aggression Duelling Frustration Aggression Hypothesis Hero syndrome Homo homini lupus Microaggression Non aggression pact Non aggression principle Parental abuse by children Passive aggressive behavior Rage emotion Relational aggression Resource holding potential Revenge School bullying School violence Social defeatReferences edit Maremmani I Avella M T Novi M Bacciardi S Maremmani A G I Aggressive Behavior and Substance Use Disorder The Heroin Use Disorder as a Case Study Addict Disord Treat 2020 19 3 161 173 doi 10 1097 ADT 0000000000000199 DeBono Amber Muraven Mark 1 November 2014 Rejection perceptions feeling disrespected leads to greater aggression than feeling disliked Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 55 43 52 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2014 05 014 ISSN 0022 1031 a b De Almeida Rosa Maria Martins Cabral Joao Carlos Centurion Narvaes Rodrigo 2015 Behavioural hormonal and neurobiological mechanisms of aggressive behaviour in human and nonhuman primates Physiology amp Behavior 143 121 35 doi 10 1016 j physbeh 2015 02 053 PMID 25749197 S2CID 27711931 a b c Miczek Klaus A Almeida Rosa M M de Kravitz Edward A Rissman Emilie F Boer Sietse F de Raine Adrian 31 October 2007 Neurobiology of Escalated Aggression and Violence Journal of Neuroscience 27 44 11803 11806 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 3500 07 2007 ISSN 0270 6474 PMC 2667097 PMID 17978016 Hollowood Lorna 2022 Micro refers to its subtle delivery not its impact rcn org uk Royal College of Nursing Microaggressions are something that somebody says to you they can be a way that you ve been made to feel They are often subtle behaviours but their effects are far from subtle Buss A H 1961 The psychology of aggression Hoboken NJ John Wiley page needed Anderson Craig A Bushman Brad J 2002 Human Aggression Annual Review of Psychology 53 27 51 doi 10 1146 annurev psych 53 100901 135231 PMID 11752478 S2CID 227846 Wahl Klaus 2020 The Radical Right Biopsychosocial Roots and International Variations London Palgrave Macmillan p 47 ISBN 978 3 030 25130 7 OCLC 1126278982 Wahl Klaus 2013 Aggression und Gewalt Ein biologischer psychologischer und sozialwissenschaftlicher Uberblick Heidelberg Spektrum Akademischer Verlag p 2 ISBN 978 3 8274 3120 2 OCLC 471933605 Akert R M Aronson E amp Wilson T D 2010 Social Psychology 7th ed Upper Saddle River NJ Prentice Hall Dollard J Doob L W Miller N E Mowrer O H Sears R R 1939 Frustration and Aggression New Haven CT Yale University Press Berkowitz L 1987 Frustrations appraisals and aversively stimulated aggression Aggressive Behavior 14 1 3 11 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 106 1 59 PMID 2667009 a b Archer J 1976 The organization of aggression and fear in vertebrates In Bateson P P G Klopfer P H eds Perspectives in Ethology Vol 2 New York NY Plenum pp 231 298 Berkowitz L 1993 Aggression Its causes consequences and control New York NY McGraw Hill McEllistrem Joseph E 2004 Affective and predatory violence A bimodal classification system of human aggression and violence Aggression and Violent Behavior 10 1 1 30 doi 10 1016 j avb 2003 06 002 Bushman Brad J Anderson Craig A 2001 Is it time to pull the plug on hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy Psychological Review 108 1 273 9 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 108 1 273 PMID 11212630 S2CID 14223373 Ellie L Young David A Nelson America B Hottle Brittney Warburton and Bryan K Young 2010 Relational Aggression Among Students Principal Leadership October copyright the National Association of Secondary School Principals Ramirez J M Andreu J M 2006 Aggression and some related psychological constructs anger hostility and impulsivity Some comments from a research project Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 30 3 276 91 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2005 04 015 PMID 16081158 S2CID 17678048 Veenema Alexa H Neumann Inga D 2007 Neurobiological Mechanisms of Aggression and Stress Coping A Comparative Study in Mouse and Rat Selection Lines Brain Behavior and Evolution 70 4 274 85 doi 10 1159 000105491 PMID 17914259 S2CID 25067952 Simons Marlise May 2010 International Court May Define Aggression as Crime The New York Times Nathaniel Snow Violence and Aggression in Sports An In Depth Look Part One Part 2Part 3 Bleacher Report 23 March 2010 a b Coccaro Emil F Beresford Brendan Minar Philip Kaskow Jon Geracioti Thomas 2007 CSF testosterone Relationship to aggression impulsivity and venturesomeness in adult males with personality disorder Journal of Psychiatric Research 41 6 488 92 doi 10 1016 j jpsychires 2006 04 009 PMID 16765987 Barbara Krahe 11 February 2013 The Social Psychology of Aggression 2nd Edition Psychology Press p 16 ISBN 978 1 136 17772 9 Elizabeth Kande Englander 30 January 2003 Understanding Violence Psychology Press pp 55 86 ISBN 978 1 135 65676 8 Merriam Webster Aggression Retrieved 10 January 2012 Online Etymology Dictionary Aggression Retrieved 10 January 2012 Stearns D C 2003 Anger and aggression Encyclopedia of Children and Childhood In History and Society Paula S Fass Ed Macmillan Reference Books Van Staaden M J Searcy W A amp Hanlon R T Signaling Aggression in Aggression Academic Press Stephen F Goodwin 2011 Maestripieri Dario 1992 Functional aspects of maternal aggression in mammals Canadian Journal of Zoology 70 6 1069 77 doi 10 1139 z92 150 Psychology The Science of Behaviour pg 420 Neil R Clarkson 4th Edition Gleitman Henry Alan J Fridlund and Daniel Reisberg Psychology 6th ed New York W W Norton and Company 2004 431 432 Gendreau PL amp Archer J Subtypes of Aggression in Humans and Animals in Developmental Origins of Aggression 2005 The Guilford Press a b c Johnson L K Wiemer D F 1982 Nerol An alarm substance of the stingless bee Trigona fulviventris Hymenoptera Apidae Journal of Chemical Ecology 8 9 1167 81 doi 10 1007 BF00990750 PMID 24413960 S2CID 25056451 Rusch H Gavrilets S 2017 The logic of animal intergroup conflict A review Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization 178 1014 1030 doi 10 1016 j jebo 2017 05 004 S2CID 49359988 Tanner C J 2006 Numerical assessment affects aggression and competitive ability A team fighting strategy for the ant Formica xerophila Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 273 1602 2737 42 doi 10 1098 rspb 2006 3626 JSTOR 25223670 PMC 1635503 PMID 17015327 Mitani John C Watts David P Amsler Sylvia J 2010 Lethal intergroup aggression leads to territorial expansion in wild chimpanzees Current Biology 20 12 R507 8 doi 10 1016 j cub 2010 04 021 PMID 20620900 S2CID 6493161 a b Issa F A Adamson D J Edwards D H 1999 Dominance hierarchy formation in juvenile crayfish procambarus clarkii The Journal of Experimental Biology 202 24 3497 506 doi 10 1242 jeb 202 24 3497 PMID 10574728 Heitor Filipa Oom Maria do Mar Vicente Luis 2006 Social relationships in a herd of Sorraia horses Behavioural Processes 73 2 170 7 doi 10 1016 j beproc 2006 05 004 PMID 16815645 S2CID 24008518 Cant Michael A Llop Justine B Field Jeremy 2006 Individual Variation in Social Aggression and the Probability of Inheritance Theory and a Field Test PDF The American Naturalist 167 6 837 52 doi 10 1086 503445 hdl 10871 26263 PMID 16615035 S2CID 12094679 Bragin A V Osadchuk L V Osadchuk A V 2006 The experimental model of establishment and maintenance of social hierarchy in laboratory mice Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deiatelnosti Imeni I P Pavlova 56 3 412 9 PMID 16869278 a b c Ferguson Christopher J Beaver Kevin M 2009 Natural born killers The genetic origins of extreme violence Aggression and Violent Behavior 14 5 286 94 doi 10 1016 j avb 2009 03 005 Hsu Yuying Earley Ryan L Wolf Larry L 2005 Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience Mechanisms and contest outcomes Biological Reviews 81 1 33 74 doi 10 1017 S146479310500686X PMID 16460581 S2CID 14284236 Aureli Filippo Cords Marina Van Schaik Carel P 2002 Conflict resolution following aggression in gregarious animals A predictive framework Animal Behaviour 64 3 325 43 doi 10 1006 anbe 2002 3071 S2CID 54361369 Silverberg James J Patrick Gray 1992 Aggression and Peacefulness in Humans and Other Primates ISBN 0 19 507119 0 Honess P E Marin C M 2006 Enrichment and aggression in primates Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 30 3 413 36 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2005 05 002 PMID 16055188 S2CID 33130527 Hinde R A 1970 Animal Behaviour A Synthesis of Ethology and Comparative Psychology 2nd Ed New York NY McGraw Hill Hebb D O 1949 The Organisation of Behavior New York NY Wiley a b van Kampen H S 2019 The principle of consistency and the cause and function of behaviour Behavioural Processes 159 42 54 doi 10 1016 j beproc 2018 12 013 PMID 30562561 S2CID 56478466 Somit A 1990 Humans Chimps and Bonobos The Biological Bases of Aggression War and Peacemaking Journal of Conflict Resolution 34 3 553 82 doi 10 1177 0022002790034003008 JSTOR 174228 S2CID 145380530 a b McDonald M M Navarrete C D Van Vugt M 2012 Evolution and the psychology of intergroup conflict The male warrior hypothesis Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 367 1589 670 9 doi 10 1098 rstb 2011 0301 JSTOR 41433544 PMC 3260849 PMID 22271783 a b Vugt Mark Van Cremer David De Janssen Dirk P 2007 Gender Differences in Cooperation and Competition Psychological Science 18 1 19 23 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 518 3529 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 2007 01842 x PMID 17362372 S2CID 3566509 Buss D M 2005 The murderer next door Why the mind Is designed to kill New York Penguin Press McCall Grant S Shields Nancy 2008 Examining the evidence from small scale societies and early prehistory and implications for modern theories of aggression and violence Aggression and Violent Behavior 13 1 1 9 doi 10 1016 j avb 2007 04 001 Buss D M Duntley J D 2006 The evolution of aggression In Schaller M Simpson J A Kenrick D T eds Evolution and Social Psychology New York Psychology Press pp 263 86 Durrant Russil 2011 Collective violence An evolutionary perspective Aggression and Violent Behavior 16 5 428 36 doi 10 1016 j avb 2011 04 014 Briffa Mark 2010 Territoriality and Aggression Nature Education Knowledge 3 10 81 a b Eagly Alice H Steffen Valerie J 1986 Gender and aggressive behavior A meta analytic review of the social psychological literature Psychological Bulletin 100 3 309 30 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 100 3 309 PMID 3797558 Clutton Brock T H Hodge S J Spong G Russell A F Jordan N R Bennett N C Sharpe L L Manser M B 2006 Intrasexual competition and sexual selection in cooperative mammals Nature 444 7122 1065 8 Bibcode 2006Natur 444 1065C doi 10 1038 nature05386 PMID 17183322 S2CID 4397323 Archer John 2009 Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression PDF Behavioral and Brain Sciences 32 3 4 249 66 discussion 266 311 doi 10 1017 S0140525X09990951 PMID 19691899 Campbell Anne 1999 Staying alive Evolution culture and women s intrasexual aggression Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 2 203 14 discussion 214 52 doi 10 1017 s0140525x99001818 PMID 11301523 S2CID 1081104 The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology edited by David M Buss John Wiley amp Sons Inc 2005 Chapter 21 by Anne Campbell page needed Zuk M Sexual Selections What We Can and Can t Learn about Sex from Animals University of California Press 2002 page needed a b c Del Giudice Marco 2015 Gender Differences in Personality and Social Behavior International Encyclopedia of the Social amp Behavioral Sciences pp 750 6 doi 10 1016 B978 0 08 097086 8 25100 3 hdl 2318 1852940 ISBN 978 0 08 097087 5 Campbell Anne 2012 Sex differences in aggression In Barrett Louise Dunbar Robin eds Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology pp 365 82 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780198568308 013 0025 ISBN 978 0 19 856830 8 Lussier Patrick Corrado Raymond Tzoumakis Stacy 2012 Gender Differences in Physical Aggression and Associated Developmental Correlates in a Sample of Canadian Preschoolers Behavioral Sciences amp the Law 30 5 643 71 doi 10 1002 bsl 2035 PMID 22996132 a b c Lansford Jennifer E Skinner Ann T Sorbring Emma Giunta Laura Di Deater Deckard Kirby Dodge Kenneth A Malone Patrick S Oburu Paul Pastorelli Concetta Tapanya Sombat Uribe Tirado Liliana Maria Zelli Arnaldo Al Hassan Suha M Pena Alampay Liane Bacchini Dario Bombi Anna Silvia Bornstein Marc H Chang Lei 2012 Boys and Girls Relational and Physical Aggression in Nine Countries Aggressive Behavior 38 4 298 308 doi 10 1002 ab 21433 PMC 3736589 PMID 23935227 a b Hay Dale F Nash Alison Caplan Marlene Swartzentruber Jan Ishikawa Fumiko Vespo Jo Ellen 2011 The emergence of gender differences in physical aggression in the context of conflict between young peers British Journal of Developmental Psychology 29 2 158 75 doi 10 1111 j 2044 835x 2011 02028 x PMID 21592146 S2CID 206006344 Hess Nicole H Hagen Edward H 2006 Sex differences in indirect aggression Evolution and Human Behavior 27 3 231 45 doi 10 1016 j evolhumbehav 2005 11 001 Keeler Linda A 2007 The differences in sport aggression life aggression and life assertion among adult male and female collision contact and non contact sport athletes Journal of Sport Behavior 30 1 57 76 Xie Hongling Drabick Deborah A G Chen Diane 2011 Developmental trajectories of aggression from late childhood through adolescence Similarities and differences across gender Aggressive Behavior 37 5 387 404 doi 10 1002 ab 20404 PMC 4332584 PMID 21748751 Young Cathy 26 November 1999 Feminists Play the Victim Game The New York Times Retrieved 6 December 2012 What To Do If Your Partner Has A Steamroller Personality Archived from the original on 7 September 2021 Al Ali Majed M Singh Ajai Pratap Smekal Vladimir 2011 Social anxiety in relation to social skills aggression and stress among male and female commercial institute students Education 132 2 351 61 Low Bobbi S 2012 Ecological and socio cultural impacts on mating and marriage systems In Barrett Louise Dunbar Robin eds Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology pp 449 62 doi 10 1093 oxfordhb 9780198568308 013 0030 ISBN 978 0 19 856830 8 a b Meijers J Harte J M Meynen G Cuijpers P 1 February 2017 Differences in executive functioning between violent and non violent offenders Psychological Medicine 47 10 1784 1793 doi 10 1017 S0033291717000241 ISSN 0033 2917 PMID 28173890 S2CID 4481557 Kruk Menno R Van Der Poel A M Meelis W Hermans J Mostert P G Mos J Lohman A H M 1983 Discriminant analysis of the localization of aggression inducing electrode placements in the hypothalamus of male rats Brain Research 260 1 61 79 doi 10 1016 0006 8993 83 90764 3 PMID 6681724 S2CID 3231548 Ferris C F Melloni R H Jr Koppel G Perry K W Fuller R W Delville Y 1997 Vasopressin serotonin interactions in the anterior hypothalamus control aggressive behavior in golden hamsters The Journal of Neuroscience 17 11 4331 40 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 17 11 04331 1997 PMC 6573530 PMID 9151749 Lee H 2014 Scalable control of mounting and attack by Esr1 neurons in the ventromedial hypothalamus Nature 509 7502 627 32 Bibcode 2014Natur 509 627L doi 10 1038 nature13169 PMC 4098836 PMID 24739975 Hashikawa K 2017 Esr1 cells in the ventromedial hypothalamus control female aggression Nat Neurosci 11 20 1580 1590 doi 10 1038 nn 4644 PMC 5953764 PMID 28920934 Potegal M Hebert M Decoster M Meyerhoff J L 1996 Brief high frequency stimulation of the corticomedial amygdala induces a delayed and prolonged increase of aggressiveness in male Syrian golden hamsters Behavioral Neuroscience 110 2 401 12 doi 10 1037 0735 7044 110 2 401 PMID 8731066 a b c Potegal M Ferris C F Hebert M Meyerhoff J Skaredoff L 1996 Attack priming in female Syrian golden hamsters is associated with a c fos coupled process within the corticomedial amygdala Neuroscience 75 3 869 80 doi 10 1016 0306 4522 96 00236 9 PMID 8951880 S2CID 24136851 Greenberg Neil Scott Michelle Crews David 1984 Role of the amygdala in the reproductive and aggressive behavior of the lizard Anolis carolinensis Physiology amp Behavior 32 1 147 51 doi 10 1016 0031 9384 84 90088 X PMID 6538977 S2CID 9987359 Bauman M D Toscano J E Mason W A Lavenex P Amaral D G 2006 The expression of social dominance following neonatal lesions of the amygdala or hippocampus in rhesus monkeys Macaca mulatta PDF Behavioral Neuroscience 120 4 749 60 doi 10 1037 0735 7044 120 4 749 PMID 16893283 Paus T Mapping Brain Development in Developmental Origins of Aggression 2005 The Guilford Press a b Schechter Daniel S Moser Dominik A Pointet Virginie C Aue Tatjana Stenz Ludwig Paoloni Giacobino Ariane Adouan Wafae Manini Aurelia Suardi Francesca Vital Marylene Sancho Rossignol Ana Cordero Maria I Rothenberg Molly Ansermet Francois Rusconi Serpa Sandra Dayer Alexandre G 2016 The association of serotonin receptor 3A methylation with maternal violence exposure neural activity and child aggression Behavioural Brain Research 325 Pt B 268 277 doi 10 1016 j bbr 2016 10 009 PMID 27720744 Caramaschi Doretta De Boer Sietse F De Vries Han Koolhaas Jaap M 2008 Development of violence in mice through repeated victory along with changes in prefrontal cortex neurochemistry PDF Behavioural Brain Research 189 2 263 72 doi 10 1016 j bbr 2008 01 003 hdl 11370 f683911d 3c8b 4ba6 a6bb 12cbaf11dead PMID 18281105 S2CID 14888253 Pihl RO amp Benkelfat C Neuromodulators in the Development and Expression of Inhibition and Aggression in Developmental origins of aggression html id XmSfJEl2v4sC Developmental Origins of Aggression 2005 The Guilford Press Heinrichs M Domes G 2008 Neuropeptides and social behaviour Effects of oxytocin and vasopressin in humans Advances in Vasopressin and Oxytocin from Genes to Behaviour to Disease Progress in Brain Research Vol 170 pp 337 50 doi 10 1016 S0079 6123 08 00428 7 ISBN 978 0 444 53201 5 PMID 18655894 Campbell Anne 2008 Attachment aggression and affiliation The role of oxytocin in female social behavior PDF Biological Psychology 77 1 1 10 doi 10 1016 j biopsycho 2007 09 001 PMID 17931766 S2CID 33228118 Walton Kenneth G Levitsky Debra K 2003 Effects of the Transcendental Meditation Program on Neuroendocrine Abnormalities Associated with Aggression and Crime Journal of Offender Rehabilitation 36 1 4 67 87 doi 10 1300 J076v36n01 04 S2CID 144374302 a b Carlson N Hormonal Control of Aggressive Behavior Chapter 11 in Physiology of Behavior 2013 Pearson Education Inc a b Van Goozen S Hormones and the Developmental Origins of Aggression Chapter 14 in Developmental Origins of Aggression 2005 The Guilford Press a b Archer John 2006 Testosterone and human aggression An evaluation of the challenge hypothesis Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 30 3 319 45 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2004 12 007 PMID 16483890 S2CID 26405251 Three Important Physical Ovulation Symptoms from BabyMed com Wingfield John C Ball Gregory F Dufty Alfred M Hegner Robert E Ramenofsky Marilyn 1987 Testosterone and Aggression in Birds American Scientist 75 6 602 8 Bibcode 1987AmSci 75 602W Muller Martin N Wrangham Richard W 2004 Dominance aggression and testosterone in wild chimpanzees A test of the challenge hypothesis Animal Behaviour 67 113 23 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2003 03 013 S2CID 8041587 a b c d Soma Kiran K Scotti Melissa Ann L Newman Amy E M Charlier Thierry D Demas Gregory E 2008 Novel mechanisms for neuroendocrine regulation of aggression Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 29 4 476 89 doi 10 1016 j yfrne 2007 12 003 PMID 18280561 S2CID 32650274 Siegel Allan Bhatt Suresh Bhatt Rekha Zalcman Steven 2007 The Neurobiological Bases for Development of Pharmacological Treatments of Aggressive Disorders Current Neuropharmacology 5 2 135 47 doi 10 2174 157015907780866929 PMC 2435345 PMID 18615178 Volman I Toni I Verhagen L Roelofs K 2011 Endogenous Testosterone Modulates Prefrontal Amygdala Connectivity during Social Emotional Behavior Cerebral Cortex 21 10 2282 90 doi 10 1093 cercor bhr001 PMC 3169658 PMID 21339377 a b Mehta Pranjal H Beer Jennifer 2010 Neural Mechanisms of the Testosterone Aggression Relation The Role of Orbitofrontal Cortex Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22 10 2357 68 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 518 2751 doi 10 1162 jocn 2009 21389 PMID 19925198 S2CID 710598 Siever Larry J 2008 Neurobiology of Aggression and Violence American Journal of Psychiatry 165 4 429 42 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 2008 07111774 PMC 4176893 PMID 18346997 a b Handbook of Crime Correlates Lee Ellis Kevin M Beaver John Wright 2009 Academic Press Mazur Allan Booth Alan 1998 Testosterone and dominance in men Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 3 353 63 discussion 363 97 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 421 3005 doi 10 1017 s0140525x98001228 PMID 10097017 S2CID 9462611 Albert D J Walsh M L Jonik R H 1993 Aggression in humans What is its biological foundation Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 17 4 405 25 doi 10 1016 S0149 7634 05 80117 4 PMID 8309650 S2CID 28557481 Constantino John N Grosz Daniel Saenger Paul Chandler Donald W Nandi Reena Earls Felton J 1993 Testosterone and Aggression in Children Journal of the American Academy of Child amp Adolescent Psychiatry 32 6 1217 22 doi 10 1097 00004583 199311000 00015 PMID 8282667 Pibiri Fabio Nelson Marianela Carboni Giovanni Pinna Graziano 2006 Neurosteroids regulate mouse aggression induced by anabolic androgenic steroids NeuroReport 17 14 1537 41 doi 10 1097 01 wnr 0000234752 03808 b2 PMID 16957604 S2CID 42991833 Choi P Y L Parrott A C Cowan D 1990 High dose anabolic steroids in strength athletes Effects upon hostility and aggression Human Psychopharmacology Clinical and Experimental 5 4 349 56 doi 10 1002 hup 470050407 S2CID 37157824 Aggression protein found in mice BBC News 5 December 2007 Retrieved 26 September 2009 a b Chamero P Marton TF Logan DW et al December 2007 Identification of protein pheromones that promote aggressive behaviour Nature 450 7171 899 902 Bibcode 2007Natur 450 899C doi 10 1038 nature05997 PMID 18064011 S2CID 4398766 Smith R S Hu R Desouza A Eberly C L Krahe K Chan W Araneda R C 2015 Differential Muscarinic Modulation in the Olfactory Bulb Journal of Neuroscience 35 30 10773 85 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 0099 15 2015 PMC 4518052 PMID 26224860 Krieger J Schmitt A Lobel D Gudermann T Schultz G Breer H Boekhoff I 1999 Selective Activation of G Protein Subtypes in the Vomeronasal Organ upon Stimulation with Urine derived Compounds Journal of Biological Chemistry 274 8 4655 62 doi 10 1074 jbc 274 8 4655 PMID 9988702 Caltech Scientists Discover Aggression Promoting Pheromone in Flies Caltech press release 2009 Siwicki Kathleen K Kravitz Edward A 2009 Fruitless doublesex and the genetics of social behavior in Drosophila melanogaster Current Opinion in Neurobiology 19 2 200 6 doi 10 1016 j conb 2009 04 001 PMC 2716404 PMID 19541474 Garratt M Brooks RC January 2015 A genetic reduction in antioxidant function causes elevated aggression in mice J Exp Biol 218 Pt 2 223 7 doi 10 1242 jeb 112011 PMID 25524980 Perusse D amp Gendreau P Genetics and the Development of Aggression in Developmental Origins of Aggression 2005 The Guilford Press page needed Derringer Jaime Krueger Robert F Irons Daniel E Iacono William G 2010 Harsh Discipline Childhood Sexual Assault and MAOA Genotype An Investigation of Main and Interactive Effects on Diverse Clinical Externalizing Outcomes Behavior Genetics 40 5 639 48 doi 10 1007 s10519 010 9358 9 PMC 2912157 PMID 20364435 Cherepkova Elena V Maksimov Vladimir N Aftanas Lyubomir I Menshanov Petr N 2015 Genotype and haplotype frequencies of the DRD4 VNTR polymorphism in the men with no history of ADHD convicted of violent crimes Journal of Criminal Justice 43 6 464 469 doi 10 1016 j jcrimjus 2015 10 002 Konrad Lorenz On Aggression 1963 page needed E O Wilson On Human Nature Harvard 1978 pp 101 107 Leakey R amp Lewin R 1978 People of the lake New York Anchor Press Doubleday UNESCO 1989 The Seville Statement Retrieved http www unesco org cpp uk declarations seville pdf Archived 12 August 2013 at the Wayback Machine UNESCO Prize for Peace Education 1989 Retrieved http www demilitarisation org IMG article PDF Seville Statement UNESCO 1989 a143 pdf a b c Bond MH 2004 Aggression and culture in Encyclopedia of applied psychology Volume 1 Thomas E M 1958 The harmless people New York Vintage Books page needed Keeley L H 1996 War Before Civilization The myth of the peaceful savage New York Oxford University Press Lomas W 2009 Conflict Violence and Conflict Resolution in Hunting and Gathering Societies Totem The University of Western Ontario Journal of Anthropology Volume 17 Issue 1 Article 13 Gray Peter Spring 2009 Play as a Foundation for Hunter Gatherer Social Existence PDF American Journal of Play 1 4 476 522 Gray Peter 16 May 2011 How Hunter Gatherers Maintained Their Egalitarian Ways Psychology Today a b Durrant J Ensom R 2012 Physical punishment of children Lessons from 20 years of research Canadian Medical Association Journal 184 12 1373 7 doi 10 1503 cmaj 101314 PMC 3447048 PMID 22311946 Gershoff E T 2008 Report on Physical Punishment in the United States What Research Tells Us About Its Effects on Children PDF Columbus OH Center for Effective Discipline p 16 Archived from the original PDF on 27 January 2016 Retrieved 15 December 2015 Corporal Punishment 2008 International Encyclopedia of the Social Sciences Gershoff Elizabeth T 2013 Spanking and Child Development We Know Enough Now to Stop Hitting Our Children Child Development Perspectives 7 3 133 137 doi 10 1111 cdep 12038 PMC 3768154 PMID 24039629 Taylor C A Manganello J A Lee S J Rice J C 2010 Mothers Spanking of 3 Year Old Children and Subsequent Risk of Children s Aggressive Behavior Pediatrics 125 5 e1057 65 doi 10 1542 peds 2009 2678 PMC 5094178 PMID 20385647 Straus Murray A Douglas Emily M Madeiros Rose Ann 2013 The Primordial Violence Spanking Children Psychological Development Violence and Crime New York Routledge p 81 ISBN 978 1 84872 953 7 Fujihara Takehiro Kohyama Takaya Andreu J Manuel Ramirez J Martin 1999 Justification of interpersonal aggression in Japanese American and Spanish students Aggressive Behavior 25 3 185 95 doi 10 1002 SICI 1098 2337 1999 25 3 lt 185 AID AB3 gt 3 0 CO 2 K S2CID 145166936 Cohen Dov Nisbett Richard E Bowdle Brian F Schwarz Norbert 1996 Insult aggression and the southern culture of honor An experimental ethnography PDF Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 70 5 945 59 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 70 5 945 hdl 2027 42 92155 PMID 8656339 Bergmuller Silvia 2013 The Relationship Between Cultural Individualism Collectivism and Student Aggression Across 62 Countries Aggressive Behavior 39 3 182 200 doi 10 1002 ab 21472 PMID 23494751 Nolan P 2007 Capitalism and freedom the contradictory character of globalisation From page 2 Anthem Studies in Development and Globalization Anthem Press Sherer Moshe Karnieli Miller Orit 2004 Aggression and violence among Jewish and Arab Youth in Israel International Journal of Intercultural Relations 28 2 93 109 doi 10 1016 j ijintrel 2004 03 004 Amjad Naumana Wood Alex M 2009 Identifying and changing the normative beliefs about aggression which lead young Muslim adults to join extremist anti Semitic groups in Pakistan Aggressive Behavior 35 6 514 9 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 332 6476 doi 10 1002 ab 20325 PMID 19790255 Akert M Robin Aronson E and Wilson D T Social Psychology 5th Edition Pearson Education Inc 2005 page needed Freedman J 2002 Media violence and its effect on aggression Assessing the scientific evidence Toronto University of Toronto Press Christopher J Ferguson 2010 Video Games and Youth Violence A Prospective Analysis in Adolescents Archived 25 May 2012 at the Wayback Machine Journal of Youth and Adolescence Sherry J 2001 The effects of violent video games on aggression A meta analysis Human Communication Research 27 3 409 31 doi 10 1093 hcr 27 3 409 S2CID 6322160 Anderson Craig A Dill Karen E 2000 Video games and aggressive thoughts feelings and behavior in the laboratory and in life Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78 4 772 90 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 1006 1548 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 78 4 772 PMID 10794380 Tremblay Richard E 2000 The development of aggressive behaviour during childhood What have we learned in the past century International Journal of Behavioral Development 24 2 129 41 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 487 7070 doi 10 1080 016502500383232 S2CID 145617291 Bongers Ilja L Koot Hans M Van Der Ende Jan Verhulst Frank C 2004 Developmental Trajectories of Externalizing Behaviors in Childhood and Adolescence Child Development 75 5 1523 37 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2004 00755 x PMID 15369529 S2CID 12514300 NICHD Early Child Care Research Network 2004 Trajectories of physical aggression from toddlerhood to middle childhood Predictors correlates and outcomes Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 69 4 vii 1 129 doi 10 1111 j 0037 976X 2004 00312 x PMID 15667346 Bongers I L Koot H M Van Der Ende J Verhulst F C 2007 Predicting young adult social functioning from developmental trajectories of externalizing behaviour Psychological Medicine 38 7 989 99 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 572 4309 doi 10 1017 S0033291707002309 PMID 18047767 S2CID 6218999 Schellenberg R 2000 Aggressive personality When does it develop and why Virginia Counselors Journal 26 67 76 Tremblay Richard E Hartup Willard W Archer John eds 2005 Developmental Origins of Aggression New York The Guilford Press ISBN 978 1 59385 110 1 Dabb C May 1997 The relationship between weather and children s behavior a study of teacher perceptions USU Thesis Bandura Albert Ross Dorothea Ross Sheila A 1961 Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 63 3 575 82 doi 10 1037 h0045925 PMID 13864605 S2CID 18361226 American Academy of Pediatrics 2011 Ages amp Stages Aggressive Behavior HealthChildren org retrieved January 2012 National Association of School Psychologists 2008 Angry and Aggressive Students Coie J D amp Dodge K A 1997 Aggression and antisocial behavior In W Damon amp N Eisenberg Eds Handbook of Child Psychology Vol 3 Social emotional and personality development Maccoby E E amp Jacklin C N 1974 The psychology of sex differences Stanford Stanford University Press Bjorkqvist Kaj Osterman Karin Lagerspetz Kirsti M J 1994 Sex differences in covert aggression among adults PDF Aggressive Behavior 20 27 33 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 453 7106 doi 10 1002 1098 2337 1994 20 1 lt 27 aid ab2480200105 gt 3 0 co 2 q Archived from the original PDF on 3 December 2011 Retrieved 6 December 2018 Archer John 2004 Sex Differences in Aggression in Real World Settings A Meta Analytic Review Review of General Psychology 8 4 291 322 doi 10 1037 1089 2680 8 4 291 S2CID 26394462 Card Noel A Stucky Brian D Sawalani Gita M Little Todd D 2008 Direct and Indirect Aggression During Childhood and Adolescence A Meta Analytic Review of Gender Differences Intercorrelations and Relations to Maladjustment Child Development 79 5 1185 229 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8624 2008 01184 x PMID 18826521 S2CID 7942628 Hines Denise A Saudino Kimberly J 2003 Gender Differences in Psychological Physical and Sexual Aggression Among College Students Using the Revised Conflict Tactics Scales Violence and Victims 18 2 197 217 doi 10 1891 vivi 2003 18 2 197 PMID 12816404 S2CID 28687366 Bjorkqvist Kaj 1994 Sex differences in physical verbal and indirect aggression A review of recent research Sex Roles 30 3 4 177 88 doi 10 1007 BF01420988 S2CID 142759440 Navis Charlene Brown Stephen L Heim Derek 2008 Predictors of injurious assault committed during or after drinking alcohol A case control study of young offenders Aggressive Behavior 34 2 167 74 doi 10 1002 ab 20231 PMID 17922526 Turner Charles W Layton John F Simons Lynn S 1975 Naturalistic studies of aggressive behavior Aggressive stimuli victim visibility and horn honking Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 31 6 1098 107 doi 10 1037 h0076960 PMID 1142063 S2CID 36562997 Castle T Hensley C 2002 Serial Killers with Military Experience Applying Learning Theory to Serial Murder International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 46 4 453 65 doi 10 1177 0306624X02464007 PMID 12150084 S2CID 35278358 blennow Manhem 20 January 2021 CSF studies in violent offenders Journal of Neural Transmission 108 869 878 via Forsman A Dolan Eric W 30 September 2023 Contrary to popular belief recent psychology findings suggest aggression isn t always tied to a lack of self control PsyPost Retrieved 2 October 2023 Smith P 2007 Why has aggression been thought of as maladaptive Aggression and Adaptation the Bright Side to Bad Behavior Routledge pp 65 83 ISBN 978 1 135 59375 9 Hawley Patricia H Vaughn Brian E 2003 Aggression and Adaptive Functioning The Bright Side to Bad Behavior Merrill Palmer Quarterly 49 3 239 42 doi 10 1353 mpq 2003 0012 JSTOR 23096055 S2CID 54998386 Bjorkly Stal 2006 Psychological Theories of Aggression Principles and Application to Practice Violence in Mental Health Settings New York NY Springer New York pp 27 46 doi 10 1007 978 0 387 33965 8 2 ISBN 978 0 387 33964 1 retrieved 5 May 2021 Ferguson Christopher J 2010 Blazing angels or resident evil Can violent video games be a force for good PDF Review of General Psychology 14 2 68 81 CiteSeerX 10 1 1 360 3176 doi 10 1037 a0018941 S2CID 3053432 Further reading editR Douglas Fields The Roots of Human Aggression Experiments in humans and animals have started to identify how violent behaviors begin in the brain Scientific American vol 320 no 5 May 2019 pp 64 71 Decisions to take aggressive action are risky and bring into play specific neural circuits p 66 External links edit nbsp Look up aggression aggressive or aggressiveness in Wiktionary the free dictionary nbsp Wikiquote has quotations related to Aggression nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Aggression When Family Life Hurts Family experience of aggression in children Parentline plus 31 October 2010 Aggression and Violent Behavior a Review Journal International Society for Research on Aggression ISRA Problems in the Concepts and Definitions of Aggression Violence and some Related Terms by Johan van der Dennen originally published in 1980 Aggression and brain asymmetry Archived 12 August 2017 at the Wayback Machine Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Aggression amp oldid 1192735118, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.