fbpx
Wikipedia

Impulsivity

In psychology, impulsivity (or impulsiveness) is a tendency to act on a whim, displaying behavior characterized by little or no forethought, reflection, or consideration of the consequences.[1] Impulsive actions are typically "poorly conceived, prematurely expressed, unduly risky, or inappropriate to the situation that often result in undesirable consequences,"[2] which imperil long-term goals and strategies for success.[3] Impulsivity can be classified as a multifactorial construct.[4] A functional variety of impulsivity has also been suggested, which involves action without much forethought in appropriate situations that can and does result in desirable consequences. "When such actions have positive outcomes, they tend not to be seen as signs of impulsivity, but as indicators of boldness, quickness, spontaneity, courageousness, or unconventionality."[2][5] Thus, the construct of impulsivity includes at least two independent components: first, acting without an appropriate amount of deliberation,[2] which may or may not be functional; and second, choosing short-term gains over long-term ones.[6]

Orbitofrontal cortex, part of the prefrontal cortex that shapes decision-making

Impulsivity is both a facet of personality and a major component of various disorders, including FASD, ADHD,[7] substance use disorders,[8][9] bipolar disorder,[10] antisocial personality disorder,[11] and borderline personality disorder.[10] Abnormal patterns of impulsivity have also been noted instances of acquired brain injury[12] and neurodegenerative diseases.[13] Neurobiological findings suggest that there are specific brain regions involved in impulsive behavior,[14][15][16] although different brain networks may contribute to different manifestations of impulsivity,[17][18] and that genetics may play a role.[19]

Many actions contain both impulsive and compulsive features, but impulsivity and compulsivity are functionally distinct. Impulsivity and compulsivity are interrelated in that each exhibits a tendency to act prematurely or without considered thought and often include negative outcomes.[20][21] Compulsivity may be on a continuum with compulsivity on one end and impulsivity on the other, but research has been contradictory on this point.[22] Compulsivity occurs in response to a perceived risk or threat, impulsivity occurs in response to a perceived immediate gain or benefit,[20] and, whereas compulsivity involves repetitive actions, impulsivity involves unplanned reactions.

Impulsivity is a common feature of the conditions of gambling and alcohol addiction. Research has shown that individuals with either of these addictions discount delayed money at higher rates than those without, and that the presence of gambling and alcohol abuse lead to additive effects on discounting.[23]

Impulse

An impulse is a wish or urge, particularly a sudden one. It can be considered as a normal and fundamental part of human thought processes, but also one that can become problematic, as in a condition like obsessive-compulsive disorder,[24][unreliable medical source?] borderline personality disorder, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, or in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders.

The ability to control impulses, or more specifically control the desire to act on them, is an important factor in personality and socialization. Deferred gratification, also known as impulse control is an example of this, concerning impulses primarily relating to things that a person wants or desires. Delayed gratification comes when one avoids acting on initial impulses. Delayed gratification has been studied in relation to childhood obesity. Resisting the urge to act on impulses is important to teach children, because it teaches the value of delayed gratification.[25]

Many psychological problems are characterized by a loss of control or a lack of control in specific situations. Usually, this lack of control is part of a pattern of behavior that also involves other maladaptive thoughts and actions, such as substance abuse problems or sexual disorders like the paraphilias (e.g. pedophilia and exhibitionism). When loss of control is only a component of a disorder, it usually does not have to be a part of the behavior pattern, and other symptoms must also be present for the diagnosis to be made. (Franklin[26][unreliable medical source?])

The five traits that can lead to impulsive actions

For many years it was understood that impulsivity is a trait but with further analysis it can be found that there were five traits that can lead to impulsive actions: positive urgency, negative urgency, sensation seeking, lack of planning, and lack of perseverance.[27][28][29][30]

Associated behavioral and societal problems

Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder

Attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a multiple component disorder involving inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR)[31] breaks ADHD into three subtypes according to the behavioral symptoms: Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Predominantly Inattentive Type, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Predominantly Hyperactive-Impulsive Type, and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Combined Type.

Predominantly hyperactive-impulsive type symptoms may include fidgeting and squirming in seats, talking nonstop, dashing around and touching or playing with anything in sight, having trouble sitting still during dinner/school/story time, being constantly in motion, and having difficulty doing quiet tasks or activities.

Other manifestations primarily of impulsivity include being very impatient, having difficulty waiting for things they want or waiting their turns in games, often interrupting conversations or others' activities, or blurting out inappropriate comments, showing their emotions without restraint, and act without regard for consequences.

Prevalence of the disorder worldwide is estimated to be between 4% and 10%, with reports as low as 2.2% and as high as 17.8%. Variation in rate of diagnoses may be attributed to differences between populations (i.e. culture), and differences in diagnostic methodologies.[32] Prevalence of ADHD among females is less than half that of males, and females more commonly fall into the inattentive subtype.[33]

Despite an upward trend in diagnoses of the inattentive subtype of ADHD, impulsivity is commonly considered to be the central feature of ADHD, and the impulsive and combined subtypes are the major contributors to the societal costs associated with ADHD.[33][34] The estimated cost of illness for a child with ADHD is $14,576 (in 2005 dollars) annually.[35] Prevalence of ADHD among prison populations is significantly higher than that of the normal population.[36]

In both adults[37] and children,[38][39] ADHD has a high rate of comorbidity with other mental health disorders such as learning disability, conduct disorder, anxiety disorder, major depressive disorder, bipolar disorder, and substance use disorders.

The precise genetic and environmental factors contributing to ADHD are relatively unknown, but endophenotypes offer a potential middle ground between genes and symptoms.[40] ADHD is commonly linked to "core" deficits involving "executive function," "delay aversion," or "activation/arousal" theories that attempt to explain ADHD through its symptomology.[40] Endophenotypes, on the other hand, purport to identify potential behavioral markers that correlate with specific genetic etiology. There is some evidence to support deficits in response inhibition as one such marker. Problems inhibiting prepotent responses are linked with deficits in pre-frontal cortex (PFC) functioning, which is a common dysfunction associated with ADHD and other impulse-control disorders.[41][42]

Evidence-based psychopharmacological and behavioral interventions exist for ADHD.[43]

Substance abuse

Impulsivity appears to be linked to all stages of substance abuse.[44][45]

The acquisition phase of substance abuse involves the escalation from single use to regular use.[44] Impulsivity may be related to the acquisition of substance abuse because of the potential role that instant gratification provided by the substance may offset the larger future benefits of abstaining from the substance, and because people with impaired inhibitory control may not be able to overcome motivating environmental cues, such as peer pressure.[46] "Similarly, individuals that discount the value of delayed reinforcers begin to abuse alcohol, marijuana, and cigarettes early in life, while also abusing a wider array of illicit drugs compared to those who discounted delayed reinforcers less."[47]

Escalation or dysregulation is the next and more severe phase of substance abuse. In this phase individuals "lose control" of their addiction with large levels of drug consumption and binge drug use. Animal studies suggest that individuals with higher levels of impulsivity may be more prone to the escalation stage of substance abuse.[44]

Impulsivity is also related to the abstinence, relapse, and treatment stages of substance abuse. People who scored high on the Barratt Impulsivity Scale (BIS) were more likely to stop treatment for cocaine abuse.[48] Additionally, they adhered to treatment for a shorter duration than people that scored low on impulsivity.[48] Also, impulsive people had greater cravings for drugs during withdrawal periods and were more likely to relapse. This effect was shown in a study where smokers that test high on the BIS had increased craving in response to smoking cues, and gave into the cravings more quickly than less impulsive smokers.[49] Taken as a whole the current research suggests that impulsive individuals are less likely to abstain from drugs and more likely to relapse earlier than less impulsive individuals.[44]

While it is important to note the effect of impulsivity on substance abuse, the reciprocating effect whereby substance abuse can increase impulsivity has also been researched and documented.[44] The promoting effect of impulsivity on substance abuse and the effect of substance abuse on increased impulsivity creates a positive feedback loop that maintains substance seeking behaviors. It also makes conclusions about the direction of causality difficult. This phenomenon has been shown to be related to several substances, but not all. For example, alcohol has been shown to increase impulsivity while amphetamines have had mixed results.[44]

Substance use disorder treatments include prescription of medications such as acamprosate, buprenorphine, disulfiram, LAAM, methadone, and naltrexone,[50] as well as effective psychotherapeutic treatment like behavioral couples therapy, CBT, contingency management, motivational enhancement therapy, and relapse prevention.[50]

Eating

Impulsive overeating spans from an episode of indulgence by an otherwise healthy person to chronic binges by a person with an eating disorder.[citation needed]

Consumption of a tempting food by non-clinical individuals increases when self-regulatory resources are previously depleted by another task, suggesting that it is caused by a breakdown in self control.[51] Impulsive eating of unhealthy snack foods appears to be regulated by individual differences in impulsivity when self-control is weak and by attitudes towards the snack and towards healthy eating when self-control is strong.[52] There is also evidence that greater food consumption occurs when people are in a sad mood, although it is possible that this is due more to emotional regulation than to a lack of self-control.[53] In these cases, overeating will only take place if the food is palatable to the person, and if so individual differences in impulsivity can predict the amount of consumption.[54]

Chronic overeating is a behavioral component of binge eating disorder, compulsive overeating, and bulimia nervosa. These diseases are more common for women and may involve eating thousands of calories at a time. Depending on which of these disorders is the underlying cause, an episode of overeating can have a variety of different motivations. Characteristics common among these three disorders include low self-esteem, depression, eating when not physically hungry, preoccupation with food, eating alone due to embarrassment, and feelings of regret or disgust after an episode. In these cases, overeating is not limited to palatable foods.[55]

Impulsivity differentially affects disorders involving the overcontrol of food intake (such as anorexia nervosa) and disorders involving the lack of control of food intake (such as bulimia nervosa). Cognitive impulsivity, such as risk-taking, is a component of many eating disorders, including those that are restrictive.[56] However, only people with disorders involving episodes of overeating have elevated levels of motoric impulsivity, such as reduced response inhibition capacity.[56]

One theory suggests that binging provides a short-term escape from feelings of sadness, anger, or boredom, although it may contribute to these negative emotions in the long-term.[57] Another theory suggests that binge eating involves reward seeking, as evidenced by decreased serotonin binding receptors of binge-eating women compared to matched-weight controls[58] and predictive value of heightened reward sensitivity/drive in dysfunctional eating.[59]

Treatments for clinical-grade overeating include cognitive behavioral therapy to teach people how to track and change their eating habits and actions, interpersonal psychotherapy to help people analyze the contribution of their friends and family in their disorder, and pharmacological therapies including antidepressants and SSRIs.[60]

Impulse buying

Impulse buying consists of purchasing a product or service without any previous intent to make that purchase.[61] It has been speculated to account for as much as eighty percent of all purchases[62] in the United States.[relevant?]

There are several theories pertaining to impulsive buying. One theory suggests that it is exposure combining with the speed that a reward can be obtained that influences an individual to choose lesser immediate rewards over greater rewards that can be obtained later.[63] For example, a person might choose to buy a candy bar because they are in the candy aisle even though they had decided earlier that they would not buy candy while in the store.

Another theory is one of self-regulation[57] which suggests that the capacity to refrain from impulsive buying is a finite resource. As this capacity is depleted with repeated acts of restraint susceptibility to purchasing other items on impulse increases.[citation needed]

Finally, a third theory suggests an emotional and behavioral tie between the purchaser and the product which drives both the likelihood of an impulsive purchase as well as the degree that a person will retroactively be satisfied with that purchase result.[64][65] Some studies have shown a large number of individuals are happy with purchases made on impulse (41% in one study[66]) which is explained as a preexisting emotional attachment which has a positive relationship both with the likelihood of initiating the purchase as well as mitigating post purchase satisfaction.[65] As an example, when purchasing team-related college paraphernalia a large percentage of those purchases are made on impulse and are tied to the degree with which a person has positive ties to that team.[65]

Impulsive buying is seen both as an individual trait in which each person has a preconditioned or hereditary allotment, as well as a situational construct which is mitigated by such things as emotion in the moment of the purchase and the preconditioned ties an individual has with the product.[57][65]

Psychotherapy and pharmacological treatments have been shown to be helpful interventions for patients with impulsive-compulsive buying disorder.[67] Psychotherapy interventions include the use of desensitization techniques,[68] self-help books[69] or attending a support group.[69] Pharmacological interventions include the use of SSRIs, such as fluvoxamine,[70][71] citalopram,[72][73] escitalopram,[74] and naltrexone.[75][76]

Impulse control disorders not elsewhere classified

Impulse control disorder (ICDs) are a class of DSM diagnoses that do not fall into the other diagnostic categories of the manual (e.g. substance use disorders), and that are characterized by extreme difficulty controlling impulses or urges despite negative consequences.[31] Individuals suffering from an impulse control disorder frequently experience five stages of symptoms: compelling urge or desire, failure to resist the urge, a heightened sense of arousal, succumbing to the urge (which usually yields relief from tension), and potential remorse or feelings of guilt after the behavior is completed.[77] Specific disorders included within this category include intermittent explosive disorder, kleptomania, pathological gambling, pyromania, trichotillomania (hair-pulling disorder), and impulse control disorders not otherwise specified (ICD NOS). ICD NOS includes other significant difficulties that seem to be related to impulsivity but do not meet the criteria for a specific DSM diagnosis.[31]

There has been much debate over whether or not the ICDs deserve a diagnostic category of their own, or whether they are in fact phenomenologically and epidemiologically related to other major psychiatric conditions like obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), affective disorders, and addictive disorders.[78] In fact, the ICD classification is likely to change with the release of the DSM-V in May 2013.[79] In this new revision the ICD NOS will likely be reduced or removed; proposed revisions include reclassifying trichotillomania (to be renamed hair-pulling disorder) and skin-picking disorder as obsessive-compulsive and related disorders, moving intermittent explosive disorder under the diagnostic heading of disruptive, impulse control, and conduct disorders, and gambling disorder may be included in addiction and related disorders.[79]

The role of impulsivity in the ICDs varies. Research on kleptomania and pyromania is lacking, though there is some evidence that greater kleptomania severity is tied to poor executive functioning.[80]

Trichotillomania and skin-picking disorder seem to be disorders that primarily involve motor impulsivity,[81][82] and will likely be classified in the DSM-V within the obsessive-compulsive and related disorders category.[79]

Pathological gambling, in contrast, seems to involve many diverse aspects of impulsivity and abnormal reward circuitry (similar to substance use disorders) that has led to it being increasingly conceptualized as a non-substance or behavioral addiction.[83] Evidence elucidating the role of impulsivity in pathological gambling is accumulating, with pathological gambling samples demonstrating greater response impulsivity, choice impulsivity, and reflection impulsivity than comparison control samples.[83] Additionally, pathological gamblers tend to demonstrate greater response perseveration (compulsivity) and risky decisionmaking in laboratory gambling tasks compared to controls, though there is no strong evidence suggesting that attention and working memory are impaired in pathological gamblers.[83] These relations between impulsivity and pathological gambling are confirmed by brain function research: pathological gamblers demonstrate less activation in the frontal cortical regions (implicated in impulsivity) compared to controls during behavioral tasks tapping response impulsivity, compulsivity, and risk/reward.[83] Preliminary, though variable, findings also suggest that striatal activation is different between gamblers and controls, and that neurotransmitter differences (e.g. dopamine, serotonin, opioids, glutamate, norepinephrine) may exist as well.[83]

Individuals with intermittent explosive disorder, also known as impulsive aggression, have exhibited serotonergic abnormalities and show differential activation in response to emotional stimuli and situations.[84] Notably, intermittent explosive disorder is not associated with a higher likelihood of diagnosis with any of the other ICDs but is highly comorbid with disruptive behavior disorders in childhood.[84] Intermittent explosive disorder is likely to be re-classified in the DSM-V under the heading of disruptive, impulse control, and conduct disorders.[79]

These sorts of impulse control disorders are most often treated using certain types of psychopharamcological interventions (e.g. antidepressants) and behavioral treatments like cognitive behavioral therapy.[citation needed]

Theories of impulsivity

Ego (cognitive) depletion

According to the ego (or cognitive) depletion theory of impulsivity, self-control refers to the capacity for altering one's own responses, especially to bring them into line with standards such as ideals, values, morals, and social expectations, and to support the pursuit of long-term goals.[85] Self-control enables a person to restrain or override one response, thereby making a different response possible.[85] A major tenet of the theory is that engaging in acts of self-control draws from a limited "reservoir" of self-control that, when depleted, results in reduced capacity for further self-regulation.[86][87] Self-control is viewed as analogous to a muscle: Just as a muscle requires strength and energy to exert force over a period of time, acts that have high self-control demands also require strength and energy to perform.[88] Similarly, as muscles become fatigued after a period of sustained exertion and have reduced capacity to exert further force, self-control can also become depleted when demands are made of self-control resources over a period of time. Baumeister and colleagues termed the state of diminished self-control strength ego depletion (or cognitive depletion).[87]

The strength model of self-control asserts that:

  • Just as exercise can make muscles stronger, there are signs that regular exertions of self-control can improve willpower strength.[89] These improvements typically take the form of resistance to depletion, in the sense that performance at self-control tasks deteriorates at a slower rate.[85] Targeted efforts to control behavior in one area, such as spending money or exercise, lead to improvements in unrelated areas, such as studying or household chores. And daily exercises in self-control, such as improving posture, altering verbal behavior, and using one's nondominant hand for simple tasks, gradually produce improvements in self-control as measured by laboratory tasks.[85] The finding that these improvements carry over into tasks vastly different from the daily exercises shows that the improvements are not due to simply increasing skill or acquiring self-efficacy from practice.[85]
  • Just as athletes begin to conserve their remaining strength when their muscles begin to tire, so do self-controllers when some of their self-regulatory resources have been expended. The severity of behavioral impairment during depletion depends in part on whether the person expects further challenges and demands.[85] When people expect to have to exert self-control later, they will curtail current performance more severely than if no such demands are anticipated.[90]
  • Consistent with the conservation hypothesis, people can exert self-control despite ego depletion if the stakes are high enough. Offering cash incentives or other motives for good performance counteracts the effects of ego depletion.[91] This may seem surprising but in fact it may be highly adaptive. Given the value and importance of the capacity for self-control, it would be dangerous for a person to lose that capacity completely, and so ego depletion effects may occur because people start conserving their remaining strength.[85] When people do exert themselves on the second task, they deplete the resource even more, as reflected in severe impairments on a third task that they have not anticipated.[90]

Empirical tests of the ego-depletion effect typically adopt dual-task paradigm.[86][92][93] Participants assigned to an experimental ego-depletion group are required to engage in two consecutive tasks requiring self-control.[88] Control participants are also required to engage in two consecutive tasks, but only the second task requires self-control. The strength model predicts that the performance of the experimental-group on the second self-control task will be impaired relative to that of the control group. This is because the finite self-control resources of the experimental participants will be diminished after the initial self-control task, leaving little to draw on for the second task.[85]

The effects of ego depletion do not appear to be a product of mood or arousal. In most studies, mood and arousal has not been found to differ between participants who exerted self-control and those who did not.[86][94] Likewise, mood and arousal was not related to final self-control performance.[94] The same is true for more specific mood items, such as frustration, irritation, annoyance, boredom, or interest as well. Feedback about success and failure of the self-control efforts does not appear to affect performance.[95] In short, the decline in self-control performance after exerting self-control appears to be directly related to the amount of self-control exerted and cannot be easily explained by other, well-established psychological processes.[94]

Automatic vs. controlled processes/cognitive control

Dual process theory states that mental processes operate in two separate classes: automatic and controlled. In general, automatic processes are those that are experiential in nature, occur without involving higher levels of cognition,[96] and are based on prior experiences or informal heuristics. Controlled decisions are effortful and largely conscious processes in which an individual weighs alternatives and makes a more deliberate decision.[citation needed]

  • Automatic Process: Automatic processes have four main features.[97] They occur unintentionally or without a conscious decision, the cost of the decision is very low in mental resources, they cannot be easily stopped, and they occur without conscious thought on the part of the individual making them.
  • Controlled Process: Controlled processes also have four main features[97] that are very close to the opposite in spectrum from their automatic counterparts. Controlled processes occur intentionally, they require the expenditure of cognitive resources, the individual making the decision can stop the process voluntarily, and the mental process is a conscious one.

Dual process theories at one time considered any single action/thought as either being automatic or controlled.[97] However, currently they are seen as operating more along a continuum as most impulsive actions will have both controlled and automatic attributes.[97] Automatic processes are classified according to whether they are meant to inhibit or to facilitate a thought process.[98] For example, in one study[99] researchers offered individuals a choice between a 1 in 10 chance of winning a prize and a 10 in 100 chance. Many participants chose one of the choices over the other without identifying that the chances inherent in each were the same as they saw either only 10 chances total as more beneficial, or of having 10 chances to win as more beneficial. In effect impulsive decisions can be made as prior information and experiences dictate one of the courses of action is more beneficial when in actuality careful consideration would better enable the individual to make a more informed and improved decision.[citation needed]

Intertemporal choice

Intertemporal choice is defined as "decisions with consequences that play out over time".[100] This is often assessed using the relative value people assign to rewards at different points in time, either by asking experimental subjects to choose between alternatives or examining behavioral choices in a naturalistic setting.[citation needed]

Intertemporal choice is commonly measured in the laboratory using a "delayed discounting" paradigm, which measures the process of devaluing rewards and punishments that happen in the future.[100] In this paradigm, subjects must choose between a smaller reward delivered soon and a larger reward delivered at a delay in the future. Choosing the smaller-sooner reward is considered impulsive. By repeatedly making these choices, indifference points can be estimated. For example, if someone chose $70 now over $100 in a week, but chose the $100 in a week over $60 now, it can be inferred that they are indifferent between $100 in a week and an intermediate value between $60 and $70. A delay discounting curve can be obtained for each participant by plotting their indifference points with different reward amounts and time delays. Individual differences in discounting curves are affected by personality characteristics such as self-reports of impulsivity and locus of control; personal characteristics such as age, gender, IQ, race, and culture; socioeconomic characteristics such as income and education; and many other variables.[101] to drug addiction.[102][103] Lesions of the nucleus accumbens core subregion[104] or basolateral amygdala[105] produce shifts towards choosing the smaller-sooner reward, suggesting the involvement of these brain regions in the preference for delayed reinforcers. There is also evidence that the orbitofrontal cortex is involved in delay discounting, although there is currently debate on whether lesions in this region result in more or less impulsivity.[106]

Economic theory suggests that optimal discounting involves the exponential discounting of value over time. This model assumes that people and institutions should discount the value of rewards and punishments at a constant rate according to how delayed they are in time.[100] While economically rational, recent evidence suggests that people and animals do not discount exponentially. Many studies suggest that humans and animals discount future values according to a hyperbolic discounting curve where the discount factor decreases with the length of the delay (for example, waiting from today to tomorrow involves more loss of value than waiting from twenty days to twenty-one days). Further evidence for non-constant delay discounting is suggested by the differential involvement of various brain regions in evaluating immediate versus delayed consequences. Specifically, the prefrontal cortex is activated when choosing between rewards at a short delay or a long delay, but regions associated with the dopamine system are additionally activated when the option of an immediate reinforcer is added.[107] Additionally, intertemporal choices differ from economic models because they involve anticipation (which may involve a neurological "reward" even if the reinforcer is delayed), self-control (and the breakdown of it when faced with temptations), and representation (how the choice is framed may influence desirability of the reinforcer),[100] none of which are accounted for by a model that assumes economic rationality.[citation needed]

One facet of intertemporal choice is the possibility for preference reversal, when a tempting reward becomes more highly valued than abstaining only when immediately available.[3] For example, when sitting home alone, a person may report that they value the health benefit of not smoking a cigarette over the effect of smoking one. However, later at night when the cigarette is immediately available, their subjective value of the cigarette may rise and they may choose to smoke it.[citation needed]

A theory called the "primrose path" is intended to explain how preference reversal can lead to addiction in the long run.[108] As an example, a lifetime of sobriety may be more highly valued than a lifetime of alcoholism, but, at the same time, one drink now may be more highly valued than not drinking now. Because it is always "now," the drink is always chosen, and a paradoxical effect occurs whereby the more-valued long-term alternative is not achieved because the more-valued short-term alternative is always chosen. This is an example of complex ambivalence,[109] when a choice is made not between two concrete alternatives but between one immediate and tangible alternative (i.e. having a drink) and one delayed and abstract alternative (i.e. sobriety).

Similarities between humans and non-human animals in intertemporal choice have been studied. Pigeons[110] and rats[111] also discount hyperbolically; tamarin monkeys do not wait more than eight seconds to triple the amount of a food reward.[112] The question arises as to whether this is a difference of homology or analogy—that is, whether the same underlying process underlies human-animal similarities or whether different processes are manifesting in similar patterns of results.[citation needed]

Inhibitory control

Inhibitory control, often conceptualized as an executive function, is the ability to inhibit or hold back a prepotent response.[113] It is theorized that impulsive behavior reflects a deficit in this ability to inhibit a response; impulsive people may find it more difficult to inhibit action whereas non-impulsive people may find it easier to do so.[113] There is evidence that, in normal adults, commonly used behavioral measures of inhibitory control correlate with standard self-report measures of impulsivity.[114]

Inhibitory control may itself be multifaceted, evidenced by numerous distinct inhibition constructs that can be measured in different ways, and relate to specific types of psychopathology.[115] Joel Nigg developed a useful working taxonomy of these different types of inhibition, drawing heavily from the fields of cognitive and personality psychology[115] Nigg's eight proposed types of inhibition include the following:

Executive Inhibition

Interference control

Suppression of a stimulus that elicits an interfering response, enabling a person to complete the primary response. Interference control can also refer to suppressing distractors.[115]

Interference control has been measured using cognitive tasks like the stroop test, flanker tasks, dual task interference, and priming tasks.[116] Personality researchers have used the Rothbart effortful control measures and the conscientiousness scale of the Big Five as inventory measures of interference control. Based on imaging and neural research it is theorized that the anterior cingulate, the dorsolateral prefrontal/premotor cortex, and the basal ganglia are related to interference control.[117][118]

Cognitive inhibition

Cognitive inhibition is the suppression of unwanted or irrelevant thoughts to protect working memory and attention resources.[115]

Cognitive inhibition is most often measured through tests of directed ignoring, self-report on one's intrusive thoughts, and negative priming tasks. As with interference control, personality psychologists have measured cognitive inhibition using the Rothbart Effortful Control scale and the Big Five Conscientiousness scale. The anterior cingulate, the prefrontal regions, and the association cortex seem to be involved in cognitive inhibition.[115]

Behavioral inhibition

Behavioral Inhibition is the suppression of prepotent response.[115]

Behavioral inhibition is usually measured using the Go/No Go task, Stop signal task, and reports of suppression of attentional orienting. Surveys that are theoretically relevant to behavioral inhibition include the Rothbart effortful control scale, and the Big Five Conscientiousness dimension.[115] The rationale behind the use of behavioral measures like the Stop signal task is that "go" processes and "stop processes" are independent, and that, upon "go" and "stop" cues, they "race" against each other; if the go process wins the race, the prepotent response is executed, whereas if the stop processes wins the race, the response is withheld. In this context, impulsivity is conceptualized as a relatively slow stop process.[119] The brain regions involved in behavioral inhibition appear to be the lateral and orbital prefrontal regions along with premotor processes.

Oculomotor Inhibition

Oculomotor Inhibition is the effortful suppression of reflexive saccade.[115]

Oculomotor inhibition is tested using antisaccade and oculomotor tasks. Also, the Rothbart effortful control measure and the Big Five Conscientiousness dimension are thought to tap some of the effortful processes underlying the ability to suppress saccade. The frontal eye fields and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex are involved in oculomotor inhibition.[115]

Motivational inhibition

In response to punishment

Motivational inhibition and response in the face of punishment can be measured using tasks tapping inhibition of primary response, modified go/no go tasks, inhibition of competing response, and emotional Stroop tasks.[115] Personality psychologists also use the Gray behavioral inhibition system measure, the Eysenck scale for neurotic introversion, and the Zuckerman Neuroticism-Anxiety scale.[115] The Septal-hippocampal formation, cingulate, and motor systems seem to be the brain areas most involved in response to punishment.[115]

In response to novelty

Response to novelty has been measured using the Kagan behavioral inhibition system measure and scales of neurotic introversion.[115] The amygdaloid system is implicated in novelty response.[115]

Automatic inhibition of attention

Recently inspected stimuli

Suppression of recently inspected stimuli for both attention and oculomotor saccade is usually measured using attentional and oculomotor inhibition of return tests. The superior colliculus and the midbrain, oculomotor pathway are involved in suppression of stimuli.[115]

Neglected stimuli

Information at locations that are not presently being attended to is suppressed, while attending elsewhere.[115]

This involves measures of covert attentional orienting and neglect, along with personality scales on neuroticism.[115] The posterior association cortex and subcortical pathways are implicated in this sort of inhibition.[115]

Action/Inaction goals

Recent psychology research also yields out the condition of impulsivity in relation to peoples' general goal setting. It is possible these action and inaction goals are underlying people's behavioral differences in their daily lives since they can demonstrate "patterns comparable to natural variation in overall activity levels".[120] More specifically, the level of impulsivity and mania people have might positive correlated with favorable attitudes about and goals of general action while negatively respond to favorable attitudes about and goals of general inaction.

Assessment of impulsivity

Personality tests and reports

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) is one of the oldest and most widely used measures of impulsive personality traits. The first BIS was developed in 1959 by Dr. Ernest Barratt.[121] It has been revised extensively to achieve two major goals: (1) to identify a set of "impulsiveness" items that was orthogonal to a set of "anxiety" items as measured by the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale (MAS) or the Cattell Anxiety Scale, and (2) to define impulsiveness within the structure of related personality traits like Eysenck's Extraversion dimension or Zuckerman's Sensation-Seeking dimension, especially the disinhibition subfactor.[121] The BIS-11 with 30 items was developed in 1995.[122] According to Patton and colleagues, there are 3 subscales (Attentional Impulsiveness, Motor Impulsiveness, and Non-Planning Impulsiveness) with six factors:[122]

  1. Attention: "focusing on a task at hand".
  2. Motor impulsiveness: "acting on the spur of the moment".
  3. Self-control: "planning and thinking carefully".
  4. Cognitive complexity: "enjoying challenging mental tasks".
  5. Perseverance: "a consistent life style".
  6. Cognitive instability: "thought insertion and racing thoughts".

Eysenck Impulsiveness Scale

The Eysenck Impulsiveness Scale (EIS)[123] is a 54-item yes/no questionnaire designed to measure impulsiveness. Three subscales are computed from this measure: Impulsiveness, Venturesomeness, and Empathy. Impulsiveness is defined as "behaving without thinking and without realizing the risk involved in the behavior".[124] Venturesomeness is conceptualized as "being conscious of the risk of the behavior but acting anyway"[124] The questionnaire was constructed through factor analysis to contain items that most highly loaded on impulsiveness and venturesomeness.[124] The EIS is a widely used and well-validated measure.[124]

Dickman Impulsivity Inventory

The Dickman Impulsivity Inventory was first developed in 1990 by Scott J. Dickman. This scale is based on Dickman's proposal that there are two types of impulsivity that are significantly different from one another.[125] This includes functional impulsivity which is characterized by quick decision making when it is optimal, a trait that is often considered to be a source of pride. The scale also includes dysfunctional impulsivity which is characterized by making quick decisions when it is not optimal. This type of impulsivity is most often associated with life difficulties including substance abuse problems and other negative outcomes.[126]

This scale includes 63 items of which 23 are related to dysfunctional impulsivity, 17 are related to functional impulsivity, and 23 are filler questions that relate to neither construct.[126] This scale has been developed into a version for use with children[127] as well as into several languages. Dickman showed there is no correlation between these two tendencies across individuals, and they also have different cognitive correlates.[125]

UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale

The UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale[128] is a 45-item self-report questionnaire that was designed to measure impulsivity across dimensions of the Five Factor Model of personality. The UPPS includes 4 sub-scales: lack of premeditation, urgency, lack of perseverance, and sensation-seeking.

UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P)[129] is a revised version of the UPPS, including 59 items. It assesses an additional personality pathway to impulsive behavior, Positive Urgency, in addition to the four pathways assessed in the original version of the scale: Urgency (now Negative Urgency), (lack of) Premeditation, (lack of) Perseverance, and Sensation Seeking

UPPS-P short version (UPPS-Ps)[130] is 20-item scale that evaluates five different impulsivity facets (4 items per dimension).

UPPS-R Interview[131] is a semi-structured interview that measures the degree to which individuals exhibit the various components of impulsivity assessed by the UPPS-P.

Lifetime History of Impulsive Behaviors

Lifetime History of Impulsive Behaviors (LHIB)[132] is a 53-item questionnaire designed to assess lifetime history of impulsive behavior (as opposed to impulsive tendencies) as well as the level of distress and impairment associated with these behaviors.[133] The assessment battery was designed to measure the following six dimensions: (a) impulsivity, (b) sensation seeking, (c) trait anxiety, (d) state depression, (e) empathy, and (f) social desirability. The LHIB consists of scales for clinically significant impulsivity, non-clinically significant impulsivity, and impulsivity related distress/impairment.[133]

Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System

Behavioral Inhibition System/Behavioral Activation System (BIS/BAS)[134] was developed based on the Gray's biopsychological theory of personality which suggests that there are two general motivational systems that underlie behavior and affect: BIS and BAS. This 20-item self-report questionnaire is designed to assess dispositional BIS and BAS sensitivities.

Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale

Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale (IPAS)[135] is a 30-item self-report questionnaire. Half of the items describe impulsive aggression and half the items describe premeditated aggression. Aggressive behavior has traditionally been classified into two distinct subtypes, impulsive or premeditated. Impulsive aggression is defined as a hair-trigger aggressive response to provocation with loss of behavioral control.[135] Premeditated aggression is defined as a planned or conscious aggressive act, not spontaneous or related to an agitated state.[135] The IPAS is designed to characterize aggressive behavior as predominately impulsive or predominately premeditated in nature.[135] Those subjects who clustered on the impulsive factor showed a broad range of emotional and cognitive impairments; those who clustered on the premeditated factor showed a greater inclination for aggression and anti-social behaviour.[135][136]

Padua Inventory

The Padua Inventory (PI) consists of 60 items describing common obsessional and compulsive behavior and allows investigation of such problems in normal and clinical subjects.[137]

Behavioral paradigms

A wide variety of behavioral tests have been devised for the assessment of impulsivity in both clinical and experimental settings. While no single test is a perfect predictor or a sufficient replacement for an actual clinical diagnosis, when used in conjunction with parent/teacher reports, behavioral surveys, and other diagnostic criteria, the utility of behavioral paradigms lies in their ability to narrow in on specific, discrete aspects of the impulsivity umbrella. Quantifying specific deficits is of use to the clinician and the experimenter, both of whom are generally concerned with obtaining objectively measurable treatment effects.[citation needed]

Marshmallow test

One widely recognizable test for impulsivity is the delay of gratification paradigm commonly known as the 'marshmallow test'.[63] Developed in the 1960s to assess 'willpower' and self-control in preschoolers, the marshmallow test consists of placing a single marshmallow in front of a child and informing them that they will be left alone in the room for some duration. The child is told that if the marshmallow remains uneaten when the experimenter returns, they will be awarded a second marshmallow, both of which can then be eaten.[citation needed][138]

Despite its simplicity and ease of administration, evidence from longitudinal studies suggests that the number of seconds preschoolers wait to obtain the second marshmallow is predictive of higher SAT scores, better social and emotional coping in adolescence, higher educational achievement, and less cocaine/crack use.[139][140][141]

Delay discounting

Like the marshmallow test, delay discounting is also a delay of gratification paradigm.[142] It is designed around the principle that the subjective value of a reinforcer decreases, or is 'discounted,' as the delay to reinforcement increases. Subjects are given varying choices between smaller, immediate rewards and larger, delayed rewards. By manipulating reward magnitude and/or reward delay over multiple trials, 'indifference' points can be estimated whereby choosing the small, immediate reward, or the large, delayed reward are about equally likely. Subjects are labeled impulsive when their indifference points decline more steeply as a function of delay compared to the normal population (i.e. greater preference for immediate reward). Unlike the marshmallow test, delay discounting does not require verbal instruction and can be implemented on non-human animals.[143]

Go/no-go and stop-signal reaction time tasks

Two common tests of response inhibition used in humans are the go/no-go task, and a slight variant known as the stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) test. During a go/no-task, the participant is trained over multiple trials to make a particular response (e.g., a key-press) when presented with a 'go' signal. On some trials, a 'stop' signal is presented just prior to, or simultaneously with the 'go' signal, and the subject must inhibit the impending response.

The SSRT test is similar, except that the 'stop' signal is presented after the 'go' signal. This small modification increases the difficulty of inhibiting the 'go' response, because the participant has typically already initiated the 'go' response by the time the 'stop' signal is presented.[144] The participant is instructed to respond as fast as possible to the 'go' signal while maintaining the highest possible inhibition accuracy (on no-go trials). During the task, the time at which the 'stop' signal is presented (the stop signal delay or SSD) is dynamically adjusted to match the time after the 'go' signal at which the participant is just able/unable to inhibit their 'go' response. If the participant fails to inhibit their 'go' response, the 'stop' signal is moved slightly closer to the original 'go' signal, and if the participant successfully inhibits their 'go' response, the 'stop' signal is moved slightly ahead in time. The SSRT is thus measured as the average 'go' response time minus the average 'stop' signal presentation time (SSD).

Balloon Analogue Risk Task

The balloon analogue risk task (BART) was designed to assess risk-taking behavior.[145] Subjects are presented with a computer depiction of a balloon that can be incrementally inflated by pressing a response key. As the balloon inflates, the subject accumulates rewards with each new key-press. The balloon is programmed with a constant probability of popping. If the balloon pops, all rewards for that balloon are lost, or the subject may choose to stop inflating and 'bank' the reward for that balloon at any time. Therefore, more key-presses equate to greater reward, but also greater probability of popping and cancelling rewards for that trial. The BART assumes that those with an affinity for 'risk-taking' are more likely to pop the balloon, earning less reward overall than the typical population.[citation needed]

Iowa Gambling Task

The Iowa gambling task (IGT) is a test originally meant to measure decision making specifically within individuals who have ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage.[146] The concept of impulsivity as relates to the IGT is one in which impulsive decisions are a function of an individual's lack of ability to make rational decisions over time due to an over amplification of emotional/somatic reward.[147] In the IGT individuals are provided four decks of cards to choose from. Two of these decks provide much higher rewards but the deductions are also much higher while the second two decks have lower rewards per card but also much lower deductions. Over time anyone who chooses predominantly from the high rewards decks will lose money while those who choose from the smaller rewards decks will gain money.

The IGT uses hot and cold processes in its concept of decision making.[147] Hot decision making involves emotional responses to the material presented based on motivation related to reward and punishment. Cold processes occur when an individual uses rational cognitive determinations when making decisions. Combined an individual should gain a positive emotional reaction when choices have beneficial consequences and will have negative emotional responses tied to choices that have greater negative consequences. In general, healthy responders to the IGT will begin to drift to the lower gain decks as they realize that they are gaining more money than they lose both through an ability to recognize that one is more consistently providing rewards as well as through the emotions related to winning consistently. However, those who have emotional deficits will fail to recognize that they are losing money over time and will continue to be more influenced by the exhilaration of higher value rewards without being influenced by the negative emotions of the loses associated with them.[citation needed]

For more information concerning these process refer to the Somatic marker hypothesis

Differential Reinforcement of Low Response Rate Task

Differential reinforcement of low response rate (DRL) described by Ferster and Skinner[148] is used to encourage low rates of responding. It is derived from research in operant conditioning that provides an excellent opportunity to measure the hyperactive child's ability to inhibit behavioral responding. Hyperactive children were relatively unable to perform efficiently on the task, and this deficit endured regardless of age, IQ, or experimental condition.[149] Therefore, it can be used to discriminate accurately between teacher rated and parent rated hyperactive and nonhyperactive children. In this procedure, responses that occur before a set time interval has passed are not reinforced and reset the time required between behaviors.[citation needed]

In a study, a child was taken to the experimental room and told that they were going to play a game in which they had a chance to win a lot of M&M's. Every time they made the light of the reward indicator by pressing a red button, they would earn an M&M's. However, they had to wait a while (6 seconds) before they could press it to get another point. If they had pressed the button too soon, then they would have not gotten a point, and the light would not go on, and they had to wait a while before they could press it to get another point.[citation needed]

Researchers have also observed that subjects in a time-based situation will often engage in a sequence or chain of behaviors between reinforceable responses.[149] This is because this collateral behavior sequence helps the subject "wait out" the required temporal delay between responses.[citation needed]

Other

Other common impulsivity tasks include the Continuous performance task (CPT), 5-choice serial reaction time task (5-CSRTT), Stroop task, and Matching Familiar Figures Task.

Pharmacology and neurobiology

Neurobiological findings

Although the precise neural mechanisms underlying disorders of impulse control are not fully known, the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is the brain region most ubiquitously implicated in impulsivity.[150] Damage to the prefrontal cortex has been associated with difficulties preparing to act, switching between response alternatives, and inhibiting inappropriate responses.[144] Recent research has uncovered additional regions of interest, as well as highlighted particular subregions of the PFC, that can be tied to performance in specific behavioral tasks.[citation needed]

Delay discounting

Excitotoxic lesions in the nucleus accumbens core have been shown to increase preference for the smaller, immediate reward, whereas lesions to the nucleus accumbens shell have had no observable effect. Additionally, lesions of the basolateral amygdala, a region tied closely to the PFC, negatively affect impulsive choice similarly to what is observed in the nucleus accumbens core lesions.[106] Moreover, dorsal striatum may also be involved in impulsive choice in an intricate manner.[151]

Go/No-go and Stop-signal reaction time test

The orbitofrontal cortex is now thought to play a role in disinhibiting,[152] and injury to other brain structures, such as to the right inferior frontal gyrus, a specific subregion of the PFC, has been associated with deficits in stop-signal inhibition.[153]

5-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task (5-CSRTT) and Differential Reinforcement of Low rates (DRL)

As with delay discounting, lesion studies have implicated the core region of the nucleus accumbens in response inhibition for both DRL and 5-CSRTT. Premature responses in the 5-CSRTT may also be modulated by other systems within the ventral striatum.[citation needed] In the 5-CSRTT, lesions of the anterior cingulate cortex have been shown to increase impulsive responding, and lesions to the prelimbic cortex impair attentional performance.[154]

Iowa Gambling Task

Patients with damage to the ventromedial frontal cortex exhibit poor decision-making and persist in making risky choices in the Iowa Gambling Task.[146][155]

Neurochemical and pharmacological findings

The primary pharmacological treatments for ADHD are methylphenidate (Ritalin) and amphetamine. Both methylphenidate and amphetamines block re-uptake of dopamine and norepinephrine into the pre-synaptic neuron, acting to increase post-synaptic levels of dopamine and norepinephrine. Of these two monoamines, increased availability of dopamine is considered the primary cause for the ameliorative effects of ADHD medications, whereas increased levels of norepinephrine may be efficacious only to the extent that it has downstream, indirect effects on dopamine.[156] The effectiveness of dopamine re-uptake inhibitors in treating the symptoms of ADHD has led to the hypothesis that ADHD may arise from low tonic levels of dopamine (particularly in the fronto-limbic circuitry), but evidence in support of this theory is mixed.[157][158]

Genetics

There are several difficulties when it comes to trying to identify a gene for complex traits such as impulsivity, such as genetic heterogeneity. Another difficulty is that the genes in question might sometimes show incomplete penetrance, "where a given gene variant does not always cause the phenotype".[159] Much of the research on the genetics of impulsivity-related disorders, such as ADHD, is based on family or linkage studies.[160] There are several genes of interest that have been studied in an attempt to find the major genetic contributors to impulsivity. Some of these genes are:

  • DAT1 is the dopamine transporter gene which is responsible for the active reuptake of dopamine from the neural synapse. DAT1 polymorphisms have been shown to be linked to hyperactivity and ADHD.[161]
  • DRD4 is the dopamine D4 receptor gene and is associated with ADHD and novelty seeking behaviors.[159][162] It has been proposed that novelty seeking is associated with impulsivity. Mice deficient for DRD4 have shown less behavioral responses to novelty.[163]
  • 5HT2A is the serotonin receptor gene. The serotonin 2A receptor gene has been associated with both hyper locomotion, ADHD, as well as impulsivity. Subjects with a particular polymorphism of the 5HT2A gene made more commission errors during a punishment-reward condition in a go/no-go task.[164]
  • HTR2B a serotonin receptor gene.[165]
  • CTNNA2 encodes for a brain-expressed α-catenin that has been associated with Excitement-Seeking in a genome-wide association study (GWAS) of 7860 individuals.[19]

Intervention

Interventions to impact impulsivity generally

While impulsivity can take on pathological forms (e.g. substance use disorder, ADHD), there are less severe, non-clinical forms of problematic impulsivity in many people's daily lives. Research on the different facets of impulsivity can inform small interventions to change decision making and reduce impulsive behavior[166] For example, changing cognitive representations of rewards (e.g. making long term rewards seem more concrete) and/or creating situations of "precommitment" (eliminating the option of changing one's mind later) can reduce the preference for immediate reward seen in delay discounting.[166]

Brain training

Brain training interventions include laboratory-based interventions (e.g. training using tasks like go/no go) as well as community, family, and school based interventions that are ecologically valid (e.g. teaching techniques for regulating emotions or behaviors) and can be used with individuals with non-clinical levels of impulsivity.[167] Both sorts of interventions are aimed at improving executive functioning and self-control capacities, with different interventions specifically targeting different aspects of executive functioning like inhibitory control, working memory, or attention.[167] Emerging evidence suggests that brain training interventions may succeed in impacting executive function, including inhibitory control.[168] Inhibitory control training specifically is accumulating evidence that it can help individuals resist temptation to consume high calorie food[169] and drinking behavior.[170] Some have voiced concerns that the favorable results of studies testing working memory training should be interpreted with caution, claiming that conclusions regarding changes to abilities are measured using single tasks, inconsistent use of working memory tasks, no-contact control groups, and subjective measurements of change.[171]

Treatment of specific disorders of impulsivity

Behavioral, psychosocial, and psychopharmacological treatments for disorders involving impulsivity are common.

Psychopharmacological intervention

Psychopharmacological intervention in disorders of impulsivity has shown evidence of positive effects; common pharmacological interventions include the use of stimulant medication, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) and other antidepressants.[78] ADHD has a well-established evidence base supporting the use of stimulant medication for the reduction of ADHD symptoms.[172] Pathological gambling has also been studied in drug trials, and there is evidence that gambling is responsive to SSRIs and other antidepressants.[78] Evidence based pharmacological treatment for trichotillomania is not yet available, with mixed results of studies investigating the use of SSRIs, though cognitive behavioral therapy has shown positive effects.[78] Intermittent explosive disorder is most often treated with mood stabilizers, SSRIs, beta blockers, alpha agonists, and anti-psychotics (all of which have shown positive effects).[78] There is evidence that some pharmacological interventions are efficacious in treating substance-use disorders, though their use can depend on the type of substance that is abused.[50] Pharmacological treatments for substance-use disorders include acamprosate, buprenorphine, disulfiram, LAAM, methadone, and naltrexone.[50]

Behavioral interventions

Behavioral interventions also have a fairly strong evidence base in impulse-control disorders.[78] In ADHD, the behavioral interventions of behavioral parent training, behavioral classroom management, and intensive peer-focused behavioral interventions in recreational settings meet stringent guidelines qualifying them for evidence based treatment status.[173] In addition, a recent meta-analysis of evidence-based ADHD treatment found organization training to be a well-established treatment method.[174] Empirically validated behavioral treatments for substance use disorder are fairly similar across substance use disorders, and include behavioral couples therapy, CBT, contingency management, motivational enhancement therapy, and relapse prevention.[50] Pyromania and kleptomania are understudied (due in large part to the illegality of the behaviors), though there is some evidence that psychotherapeutic interventions (CBT, short term counseling, day treatment programs) are efficacious in treating pyromania, while kleptomania seems to be best addressed using SSRIs.[78] Additionally, therapies including CBT, family therapy, and social skill training have shown positive effects on explosive aggressive behaviors.[78]

See also

References

  1. ^ VandenBos, G. R. (2007). APA dictionary of psychology. Washington, DC: APA. p. 470. ISBN 9781591473800.
  2. ^ a b c Daruna, J. H.; Barnes, P. A. (1993). "A neurodevelopmental view of impulsivity". In McCown, William George; Johnson, Judith L.; Shure, Myrna B. (eds.). The Impulsive Client: Theory, Research, and Treatment. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 23–37. doi:10.1037/10500-002. ISBN 978-1-55798-208-7.
  3. ^ a b Madden, Gregory J.; Johnson, Patrick S. (2010). "A Delay-Discounting Primer". In Madden, Gregory Jude; Bickel, Warren K. (eds.). Impulsivity: The Behavioral and Neurological Science of Discounting. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. pp. 11–37. ISBN 978-1-4338-0477-9.
  4. ^ Evenden, J. L. (1999). "Varieties of impulsivity". Psychopharmacology. 146 (4): 348–61. doi:10.1007/PL00005481. PMID 10550486. S2CID 5972342.
  5. ^ Dickman, Scott J. (1990). "Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: Personality and cognitive correlates". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 58 (1): 95–102. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.1.95. PMID 2308076. S2CID 34912306.
  6. ^ Rachlin, Howard (2000). "Self-Control as an Abstraction of Environmental Feedback". The Science of Self-Control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. p. 183. ISBN 978-0-674-00093-3.
  7. ^ Nigg, Joel T. (2001). "Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder?". Psychological Bulletin. 127 (5): 571–98. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.5.571. PMID 11548968.
  8. ^ Lane, Scott D.; Cherek, Don R.; Rhoades, Howard M.; Pietras, Cynthia J.; Tcheremissine, Oleg V. (2003). "Relationships Among Laboratory and Psychometric Measures of Impulsivity: Implications in Substance Abuse and Dependence". Addictive Disorders & Their Treatment. 2 (2): 33–40. doi:10.1097/00132576-200302020-00001. S2CID 143129215.
  9. ^ Madden, G.J.; Petry, N.M.; Badger, G.J.; Bickel, W. K. (1997). "Impulsive and self-control choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control patients: Drug and monetary rewards". Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology. 5 (3): 256–62. doi:10.1037/1064-1297.5.3.256. PMID 9260073.
  10. ^ a b Henry, Chantal; Mitropoulou, Vivian; New, Antonia S; Koenigsberg, Harold W; Silverman, Jeremy; Siever, Larry J (2001). "Affective instability and impulsivity in borderline personality and bipolar II disorders: Similarities and differences". Journal of Psychiatric Research. 35 (6): 307–12. doi:10.1016/S0022-3956(01)00038-3. PMID 11684137.
  11. ^ Horn, N.R.; Dolan, M.; Elliott, R.; Deakin, J.F.W.; Woodruff, P.W.R. (2003). "Response inhibition and impulsivity: An fMRI study". Neuropsychologia. 41 (14): 1959–66. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(03)00077-0. PMID 14572528. S2CID 31957781.
  12. ^ Dixon, Mark R.; Jacobs, Eric A.; Sanders, Scott; Guercio, John M.; Soldner, James; Parker-Singler, Susan; Robinson, Ashton; Small, Stacey; Dillen, Jeffrey E. (2005). "Impulsivity, self-control, and delay discounting in persons with acquired brain injury". Behavioral Interventions. 20 (1): 101–20. doi:10.1002/bin.173.
  13. ^ Gleichgerrcht, Ezequiel; Ibáñez, Agustín; Roca, María; Torralva, Teresa; Manes, Facundo (2010). "Decision-making cognition in neurodegenerative diseases". Nature Reviews Neurology. 6 (11): 611–23. doi:10.1038/nrneurol.2010.148. PMID 21045795. S2CID 13333018.
  14. ^ Corsini, Raymond Joseph (1999). The Dictionary of Psychology. Psychology Press. p. 476. ISBN 1-58391-028-X.
  15. ^ Berlin, H. A.; Rolls, E. T.; Kischka, U (2004). "Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions". Brain. 127 (5): 1108–26. doi:10.1093/brain/awh135. PMID 14985269.
  16. ^ Salmond, C.H.; Menon, D.K.; Chatfield, D.A.; Pickard, J.D.; Sahakian, B.J. (2005). "Deficits in Decision-Making in Head Injury Survivors". Journal of Neurotrauma. 22 (6): 613–22. doi:10.1089/neu.2005.22.613. PMID 15941371.
  17. ^ Hampton WH, Venkatraman V, Olson IR (2017). "Dissociable frontostriatal white matter connectivity underlies reward and motor impulsivity". NeuroImage. 150 (4): 336–343. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.021. PMC 5494201. PMID 28189592.
  18. ^ Whelan, Robert; Conrod, Patricia J; Poline, Jean-Baptiste; Lourdusamy, Anbarasu; Banaschewski, Tobias; Barker, Gareth J; Bellgrove, Mark A; Büchel, Christian; et al. (2012). "Adolescent impulsivity phenotypes characterized by distinct brain networks". Nature Neuroscience. 15 (6): 920–5. doi:10.1038/nn.3092. PMID 22544311. S2CID 18808674.
  19. ^ a b Terracciano, A; Esko, T; Sutin, A R; De Moor, M H M; Meirelles, O; Zhu, G; Tanaka, T; Giegling, I; et al. (2011). "Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies identifies common variants in CTNNA2 associated with excitement-seeking". Translational Psychiatry. 1 (10): e49–. doi:10.1038/tp.2011.42. PMC 3309493. PMID 22833195.
  20. ^ a b Berlin, Heather A.; Hollander, Eric (July 1, 2008). "Understanding the Differences Between Impulsivity and Compulsivity". Psychiatric Times. Psychiatric Times Vol 25 No 8. 25 (8). from the original on December 3, 2013.
  21. ^ Oldham, J.M.; Hollander, E.; Skodol, A.E. (1996). Impulsivity and Compulsivity. Washington D.C.: American Psychiatric Press.[page needed]
  22. ^ Engel, Scott G.; Corneliussen, Stephanie J.; Wonderlich, Stephen A.; Crosby, Ross D.; Le Grange, Daniel; Crow, Scott; Klein, Marjorie; Bardone-Cone, Anna; et al. (2005). "Impulsivity and compulsivity in bulimia nervosa". International Journal of Eating Disorders. 38 (3): 244–51. doi:10.1002/eat.20169. PMID 16211626.
  23. ^ Andrade, Leonardo F.; Alessi, Sheila M.; Petry, Nancy M. (2013). "The Effects of Alcohol Problems and Smoking on Delay Discounting in Individuals with Gambling Problems". Journal of Psychoactive Drugs. Informa UK Limited. 45 (3): 241–248. doi:10.1080/02791072.2013.803645. ISSN 0279-1072. PMC 3816387. PMID 24175489.
  24. ^ "Acting on Impulse". Psychology Today. Retrieved 20 February 2016.
  25. ^ Caleza, Carolina; Yañez-Vico, Rosa M.; Mendoza, Asunción; Iglesias-Linares, Alejandro (2016-02-01). "Childhood Obesity and Delayed Gratification Behavior: A Systematic Review of Experimental Studies". The Journal of Pediatrics. 169: 201–207.e1. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2015.10.008. ISSN 0022-3476. PMID 26563536.
  26. ^ Psychological Impulse Control Disorders
  27. ^ Smith, Gregory T.; Cyders, Melissa A. (June 2016). "Integrating affect and impulsivity: The role of positive and negative urgency in substance use risk". Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 163 (Suppl 1): S3–S12. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.08.038. PMC 4911536. PMID 27306729.
  28. ^ Fischer, Sarah; Smith, Gregory T.; Anderson, Kristen G. (May 2003). "Clarifying the role of impulsivity in bulimia nervosa". International Journal of Eating Disorders. 33 (4): 406–411. doi:10.1002/eat.10165. PMID 12658670.
  29. ^ Smith, Gregory T.; Guller, Leila; Zapolski, Tamika C. B. (July 2013). "A Comparison of Two Models of Urgency: Urgency Predicts Both Rash Action and Depression in Youth". Clinical Psychological Science. 1 (3): 266–275. doi:10.1177/2167702612470647. PMC 4238932. PMID 25419495.
  30. ^ Dvorak, Robert D.; Pearson, Matthew R.; Kuvaas, Nicholas J. (May 2013). "The Five-Factor Model of Impulsivity-Like Traits and Emotional Lability in Aggressive Behavior: Impulsivity, Emotional Lability, and Aggression". Aggressive Behavior. 39 (3): 222–228. doi:10.1002/ab.21474. PMC 4625555. PMID 23471690.
  31. ^ a b c Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Vol. 1 (4th ed.). American Psychiatric Association. 2000. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890423349. ISBN 978-0-89042-334-9.[page needed]
  32. ^ Skounti, Maria; Philalithis, Anastas; Galanakis, Emmanouil (21 December 2006). "Variations in prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder worldwide". European Journal of Pediatrics. 166 (2): 117–123. doi:10.1007/s00431-006-0299-5. PMID 17033803. S2CID 20342282.
  33. ^ a b Froehlich, Tanya E.; Lanphear, BP; Epstein, JN; Barbaresi, WJ; Katusic, SK; Kahn, RS (2007). "Prevalence, Recognition, and Treatment of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in a National Sample of US Children". Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 161 (9): 857–64. doi:10.1001/archpedi.161.9.857. PMID 17768285.
  34. ^ Solanto, Mary V.; Abikoff, Howard; Sonuga-Barke, Edmund; Schachar, Russell; Logan, Gordon D.; Wigal, Tim; Hechtman, Lily; Hinshaw, Stephen; Turkel, Elihu (2001). "The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: A supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study of AD/HD". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 29 (3): 215–28. doi:10.1023/A:1010329714819. PMID 11411784. S2CID 23953111.
  35. ^ Pelham, William E.; Foster, E. Michael; Robb, Jessica A. (2007). "The Economic Impact of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adolescents". Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 32 (6): 711–27. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsm022. PMID 17556402.
  36. ^ Rösler, M.; Retz, W.; Retz-Junginger, P.; Hengesch, G.; Schneider, M.; Supprian, T.; Schwitzgebel, P.; Pinhard, K.; et al. (2004). "Prevalence of attention deficit–/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and comorbid disorders in young male prison inmates". European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 254 (6): 365–71. doi:10.1007/s00406-004-0516-z. PMID 15538605. S2CID 8737929.
  37. ^ Kessler, R. C.; Adler, L.; Barkley, R.; Biederman, J.; Conners, C. K.; Demler, O.; Faraone, S. V.; Greenhill, L. L.; et al. (2006). "The Prevalence and Correlates of Adult ADHD in the United States: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication". American Journal of Psychiatry. 163 (4): 716–23. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.716. PMC 2859678. PMID 16585449.
  38. ^ Wilens, Timothy E.; Biederman, Joseph; Brown, Sarah; Tanguay, Sarah; Monuteaux, Michael C.; Blake, Christie; Spencer, Thomas J. (2002). "Psychiatric Comorbidity and Functioning in Clinically Referred Preschool Children and School-Age Youths with ADHD". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 41 (3): 262–8. doi:10.1097/00004583-200203000-00005. PMID 11886020.
  39. ^ Scahill, L; Schwab-Stone, M (2000). "Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children". Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 9 (3): 541–55, vii. doi:10.1016/S1056-4993(18)30106-8. PMID 10944656.
  40. ^ a b Aron, Adam R.; Poldrack, Russell A. (2005). "The Cognitive Neuroscience of Response Inhibition: Relevance for Genetic Research in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Biological Psychiatry. 57 (11): 1285–92. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.026. PMID 15950000. S2CID 41786909.
  41. ^ Ridderinkhof, K. Richard; Van Den Wildenberg, Wery P.M.; Segalowitz, Sidney J.; Carter, Cameron S. (2004). "Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning". Brain and Cognition. 56 (2): 129–40. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016. PMID 15518930. S2CID 16820592.
  42. ^ Casey, B.J.; Castellanos, F. Xavier; Giedd, Jay N.; Marsh, Wendy L.; Hamburger, Susan D.; Schubert, Anne B.; Vauss, Yolanda C.; Vaituzis, A. Catherine; et al. (1997). "Implication of Right Frontostriatal Circuitry in Response Inhibition and Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 36 (3): 374–83. doi:10.1097/00004583-199703000-00016. PMID 9055518.
  43. ^ DeVries, Melissa (2010-12-09). "Book Review: Pliszka, S. R., UTHSCSA Treating ADHD and Comorbid Disorders: Psychosocial and Psychopharmacological Interventions. New York: Guilford, 2008". Journal of Attention Disorders. 15 (1): 92–93. doi:10.1177/1087054710362216. ISSN 1087-0547. S2CID 143753779.
  44. ^ a b c d e f Perry, Jennifer L.; Carroll, Marilyn E. (2008). "The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse". Psychopharmacology. 200 (1): 1–26. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1173-0. PMID 18600315. S2CID 24587409.
  45. ^ De Wit, Harriet (2009). "Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: A review of underlying processes". Addiction Biology. 14 (1): 22–31. doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2008.00129.x. PMC 3640851. PMID 18855805.
  46. ^ De Wit, H; Richards, JB (2004). "Dual determinants of drug use in humans: Reward and impulsivity". Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 50: 19–55. PMID 15160637.
  47. ^ Kollins, Scott H (2003). "Delay discounting is associated with substance use in college students". Addictive Behaviors. 28 (6): 1167–73. doi:10.1016/S0306-4603(02)00220-4. PMID 12834659.
  48. ^ a b Moeller, F.Gerard; Dougherty, Donald M.; Barratt, Ernest S.; Schmitz, Joy M.; Swann, Alan C.; Grabowski, John (2001). "The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment". Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 21 (4): 193–8. doi:10.1016/S0740-5472(01)00202-1. PMID 11777668.
  49. ^ Doran, Neal; Spring, Bonnie; McChargue, Dennis (2007). "Effect of impulsivity on craving and behavioral reactivity to smoking cues". Psychopharmacology. 194 (2): 279–88. doi:10.1007/s00213-007-0832-x. PMID 17594080. S2CID 35948752.
  50. ^ a b c d e McGovern, Mark P; Carroll, Kathleen M (2003). "Evidence-based practices for substance use disorders". Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 26 (4): 991–1010. doi:10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00073-X. PMC 3678283. PMID 14711132.
  51. ^ Vohs, K. D.; Heatherton, T. F. (2000). "Self-Regulatory Failure: A Resource-Depletion Approach". Psychological Science. 11 (3): 249–54. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00250. PMID 11273412. S2CID 18307595.
  52. ^ Honkanen, Pirjo; Olsen, Svein Ottar; Verplanken, Bas; Tuu, Ho Huy (2012). "Reflective and impulsive influences on unhealthy snacking. The moderating effects of food related self-control". Appetite. 58 (2): 616–22. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.019. PMID 22138115. S2CID 39135358.
  53. ^ Tice, Dianne M.; Bratslavsky, Ellen; Baumeister, Roy F. (2001). "Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it!". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (1): 53–67. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.53. PMID 11195891. S2CID 10596913.
  54. ^ Hofmann, Wilhelm; Friese, Malte; Roefs, Anne (2009). "Three ways to resist temptation: The independent contributions of executive attention, inhibitory control, and affect regulation to the impulse control of eating behavior". Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 45 (2): 431–5. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.09.013. S2CID 51986961.
  55. ^ Heatherton, Todd F.; Baumeister, Roy F. (1991). "Binge eating as escape from self-awareness". Psychological Bulletin. 110 (1): 86–108. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.110.1.86. PMID 1891520. S2CID 1232881.
  56. ^ a b Rosval, Lindsay; Steiger, Howard; Bruce, Kenneth; Israël, Mimi; Richardson, Jodie; Aubut, Melanie (2006). "Impulsivity in women with eating disorders: Problem of response inhibition, planning, or attention?". International Journal of Eating Disorders. 39 (7): 590–3. doi:10.1002/eat.20296. PMID 16826575.
  57. ^ a b c Vohs, Kathleen D.; Faber, Ronald J. (2007). "Spent Resources: Self-Regulatory Resource Availability Affects Impulse Buying". Journal of Consumer Research. 33 (4): 537–47. doi:10.1086/510228.
  58. ^ Kuikka, Jyrki; Tammela, Liisa; Karhunen, Leila; Rissanen, Aila; Bergström, Kim; Naukkarinen, Hannu; Vanninen, Esko; Karhu, Jari; et al. (2001). "Reduced serotonin transporter binding in binge eating women". Psychopharmacology. 155 (3): 310–4. doi:10.1007/s002130100716. PMID 11432694. S2CID 7812191.
  59. ^ Loxton, N.J., Dawe, S. (2004). Alcohol abuse and dysfunctional eating in women: The contribution of reward and punishment sensitivity and family of origin. In press.[verification needed] As cited in: Dawe, Sharon; Loxton, Natalie J (2004). "The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders". Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 28 (3): 343–51. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.03.007. PMID 15225976. S2CID 24435589.
  60. ^ Agras, W. Stewart; Bohon, Cara (2021-05-07). "Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for the Eating Disorders". Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 17 (1): 417–438. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-081219-110907. ISSN 1548-5943. PMID 33962536. S2CID 233998712.
  61. ^ Beatty, Sharon E.; Ferrell, M. Elizabeth (1998). "Impulse buying: Modeling its precursors". Journal of Retailing. 74 (2): 169–91. doi:10.1016/S0022-4359(99)80092-X.
  62. ^ Smith, D. (1996). The joy of candy. National Petroleum News Supplement, S2.
  63. ^ a b Mischel, Walter; Ebbesen, Ebbe B.; Raskoff Zeiss, Antonette (1972). "Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 21 (2): 204–18. doi:10.1037/h0032198. PMID 5010404.
  64. ^ Strack, Fritz; Werth, Lioba; Deutsch, Roland (2006). "Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Consumer Behavior". Journal of Consumer Psychology. 16 (3): 205–16. doi:10.1207/s15327663jcp1603_2.
  65. ^ a b c d Kwon, Harry H.; Armstrong, Ketra L. (2006). "Impulse Purchases of Sport Team Licensed Merchandise: What Matters?". Journal of Sport Management. 20 (1): 101–119. doi:10.1123/jsm.20.1.101.
  66. ^ Rook, Dennis W. (1987). "The Buying Impulse". Journal of Consumer Research. 14 (2): 189–99. doi:10.1086/209105. JSTOR 2489410.
  67. ^ Hollander, E; Stein, D (2006). Impulse-control disorders. Arlington: American Psychiatric Publishing.[page needed]
  68. ^ Bernik, MA; Akerman, D; Amaral, JA; Braun, RC (1996). "Cue exposure in compulsive buying". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 57 (2): 90. PMID 8591975.
  69. ^ a b Catalano, EM; Sonenberg, N (1993). Consuming passions: help for compulsive shoppers. Oakland: NewHarbinger Publications.[page needed]
  70. ^ Black, DW; Monahan, P; Gabel, J (1997). "Fluvoxamine in the treatment of compulsive buying". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 58 (4): 159–63. doi:10.4088/JCP.v58n0404. PMID 9164426.
  71. ^ Black, Donald; Gabel, Janelle; Hansen, Jeffrey; Schlosser, Steven (2000). "A Double-Blind Comparison of Fluvoxamine Versus Placebo in the Treatment of Compulsive Buying Disorder". Annals of Clinical Psychiatry. 12 (4): 205–11. doi:10.1023/A:1009030425631. PMID 11140921. S2CID 189937538.
  72. ^ Koran, Lorrin M.; Bullock, Kim D.; Hartston, Heidi J.; Elliott, Michael A.; d'Andrea, Vincent (2002). "Citalopram Treatment of Compulsive Shopping". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 63 (8): 704–8. doi:10.4088/JCP.v63n0808. PMID 12197451.
  73. ^ Guy, LS (1976). ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology, Publication ADM 76 338. Washington, DC: Department of Health, Education and Welfare.[page needed]
  74. ^ Koran, Lorrin M.; Aboujaoude, Elias N.; Solvason, Brent; Gamel, Nona N.; Smith, Emily H. (2007). "Escitalopram for Compulsive Buying Disorder". Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology. 27 (2): 225–7. doi:10.1097/01.jcp.0000264975.79367.f4. PMID 17414257. S2CID 41883042.
  75. ^ Kim, Suck Won; Dwight, MM; McElroy, SL (1998). "Opioid Antagonists in the Treatment of Impulse-Control Disorders". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 59 (4): 165–71. doi:10.4088/JCP.v59n0403. PMID 9590666. S2CID 7161577.
  76. ^ Grant, Jon E. (2003). "Three cases of compulsive buying treated with naltrexone". International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice. 7 (3): 223–5. doi:10.1080/13651500310003219.
  77. ^ Hollander, E.; Stein, D.J., eds. (2005). Clinical manual of impulse-control disorders. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Press. pp. 4–8.
  78. ^ a b c d e f g h Dell'Osso, Bernardo; Altamura, A. Carlo; Allen, Andrea; Marazziti, Donatella; Hollander, Eric (2006). "Epidemiologic and clinical updates on impulse control disorders: A critical review". European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 256 (8): 464–75. doi:10.1007/s00406-006-0668-0. PMC 1705499. PMID 16960655.
  79. ^ a b c d . www.dsm5.org. Archived from the original on January 30, 2013.
  80. ^ Grant, J; Odlaug, B; Wozniak, J (2007). "Neuropsychological functioning in kleptomania☆". Behaviour Research and Therapy. 45 (7): 1663–70. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.08.013. PMID 17007811.
  81. ^ Fineberg, Naomi A; Potenza, Marc N; Chamberlain, Samuel R; Berlin, Heather A; Menzies, Lara; Bechara, Antoine; Sahakian, Barbara J; Robbins, Trevor W; et al. (2009). "Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors, from Animal Models to Endophenotypes: A Narrative Review". Neuropsychopharmacology. 35 (3): 591–604. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.185. PMC 3055606. PMID 19940844.
  82. ^ Odlaug, Brian L.; Chamberlain, Samuel R.; Grant, Jon E. (2010). "Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in pathologic skin picking". Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry. 34 (1): 208–11. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2009.11.008. PMID 19913592. S2CID 207409063.
  83. ^ a b c d e Leeman, Robert F.; Potenza, Marc N. (2011). "Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders: A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity". Psychopharmacology. 219 (2): 469–90. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2550-7. PMC 3249521. PMID 22057662.
  84. ^ a b Coccaro, Emil F. (2012). "Intermittent Explosive Disorder as a Disorder of Impulsive Aggression for DSM-5". American Journal of Psychiatry. 169 (6): 577–88. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11081259. PMID 22535310.
  85. ^ a b c d e f g h Baumeister, Roy F.; Vohs, Kathleen D.; Tice, Dianne M. (2007). "The Strength Model of Self-Control". Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (6): 351–5. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x. S2CID 7414142.
  86. ^ a b c Baumeister, Roy F.; Bratslavsky, Ellen; Muraven, Mark; Tice, Dianne M. (1998). "Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource?". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (5): 1252–65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252. PMID 9599441.
  87. ^ a b Muraven, Mark; Baumeister, Roy F. (2000). "Self-regulation and depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle?". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (2): 247–59. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247. PMID 10748642. S2CID 10962693.
  88. ^ a b Hagger, Martin S.; Wood, Chantelle; Stiff, Chris; Chatzisarantis, Nikos L. D. (2010). "Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: A meta-analysis". Psychological Bulletin. 136 (4): 495–525. doi:10.1037/a0019486. PMID 20565167. S2CID 22705578.
  89. ^ Baumeister, Roy F.; Gailliot, Matthew; Dewall, C. Nathan; Oaten, Megan (2006). "Self-Regulation and Personality: How Interventions Increase Regulatory Success, and How Depletion Moderates the Effects of Traits on Behavior". Journal of Personality. 74 (6): 1773–801. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2006.00428.x. PMID 17083666.
  90. ^ a b Muraven, Mark; Shmueli, Dikla; Burkley, Edward (2006). "Conserving self-control strength". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 91 (3): 524–37. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524. PMID 16938035.
  91. ^ Muraven, Mark; Slessareva, Elisaveta (2003). "Mechanisms of Self-Control Failure: Motivation and Limited Resources". Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 29 (7): 894–906. doi:10.1177/0146167203029007008. PMID 15018677. S2CID 16961238.
  92. ^ Muraven, Mark; Tice, Dianne M.; Baumeister, Roy F. (1998). "Self-control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (3): 774–89. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.774. PMID 9523419.
  93. ^ Finkel, Eli J.; Campbell, W. Keith; Brunell, Amy B.; Dalton, Amy N.; Scarbeck, Sarah J.; Chartrand, Tanya L. (2006). "High-maintenance interaction: Inefficient social coordination impairs self-regulation". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 91 (3): 456–75. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.456. PMID 16938030.
  94. ^ a b c Muraven, Mark (2012). "Ego Depletion: Theory and Evidence". In Ryan, Richard M. (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 111–26. ISBN 978-0-19-539982-0.
  95. ^ Wallace, Harry M.; Baumeister, Roy F. (2002). "The Effects of Success versus Failure Feedback on Further Self-Control". Self and Identity. 1: 35–41. doi:10.1080/152988602317232786. S2CID 144797743.
  96. ^ Amsel, Eric; Close, Jason; Sadler, Eric; Klaczynski, Paul A. (2009). "College Students' Awareness of Irrational Judgments on Gambling Tasks: A Dual-Process Account". The Journal of Psychology. 143 (3): 293–317. doi:10.3200/JRLP.143.3.293-317. PMID 19455857. S2CID 1997168.
  97. ^ a b c d Gawronski, Bertram; Creighton, Laura A. (2013). "Dual-Process Theories". In Carlston, Donal E. (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of Social Cognition. pp. 218–312. ISBN 978-0-19-973001-8.
  98. ^ Sherman, Jeffrey W.; Gawronski, Bertram; Gonsalkorale, Karen; Hugenberg, Kurt; Allen, Thomas J.; Groom, Carla J. (2008). "The self-regulation of automatic associations and behavioral impulses". Psychological Review. 115 (2): 314–35. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.314. PMID 18426292. S2CID 20186915.
  99. ^ Kirkpatrick, Lee A.; Epstein, Seymour (1992). "Cognitive-experiential self-theory and subjective probability: Further evidence for two conceptual systems". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 63 (4): 534–44. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.534. PMID 1447684.
  100. ^ a b c d Berns, Gregory S.; Laibson, David; Loewenstein, George (2007). "Intertemporal choice – toward an integrative framework". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 11 (11): 482–8. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.011. PMID 17980645. S2CID 22282339.
  101. ^ Hampton, W. (2018). "Things for Those Who Wait: Predictive Modeling Highlights Importance of Delay Discounting for Income Attainment". Frontiers in Psychology. 9 (1545): 1545. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01545. PMC 6129952. PMID 30233449.
  102. ^ MacKillop, J. (2011). "Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior". Psychopharmacology. 216 (3): 305–321. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0. PMC 3201846. PMID 21373791.
  103. ^ Odum, Amy L.; Baumann, Ana A. L. (2010). "Delay discounting: State and trait variable". In Madden, Gregory J.; Bickel, Warren K. (eds.). Impulsivity: The behavioral and neurological science of discounting. pp. 39–65. doi:10.1037/12069-002. ISBN 978-1-4338-0477-9.
  104. ^ Cardinal, R. N.; Pennicott, DR; Sugathapala, CL; Robbins, TW; Everitt, BJ (2001). "Impulsive Choice Induced in Rats by Lesions of the Nucleus Accumbens Core". Science. 292 (5526): 2499–501. doi:10.1126/science.1060818. PMID 11375482. S2CID 18020488.
  105. ^ Winstanley, C. A.; Theobald, DE; Cardinal, RN; Robbins, TW (2004). "Contrasting Roles of Basolateral Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex in Impulsive Choice". Journal of Neuroscience. 24 (20): 4718–22. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5606-03.2004. PMC 6729470. PMID 15152031.
  106. ^ a b Dalley, Jeffrey W.; Everitt, Barry J.; Robbins, Trevor W. (2011). "Impulsivity, Compulsivity, and Top-Down Cognitive Control". Neuron. 69 (4): 680–94. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020. PMID 21338879. S2CID 7952439.
  107. ^ McClure, S. M.; Laibson, DI; Loewenstein, G; Cohen, JD (2004). "Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards". Science. 306 (5695): 503–7. Bibcode:2004Sci...306..503M. doi:10.1126/science.1100907. PMID 15486304. S2CID 14663380.
  108. ^ Herrnstein, Richard J.; Prelec, Drazen (1992). "A Theory of Addiction". In Loewenstein, George; Elster, Jon (eds.). Choice Over Time. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. pp. 331–60. ISBN 978-0-87154-558-9.
  109. ^ Rachlin, Howard (2000). "Complex Ambivalence". The Science of Self-Control. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. pp. 57–81. ISBN 978-0-674-00093-3.
  110. ^ Ainslie, George; Herrnstein, R. J. (1981). "Preference reversal and delayed reinforcement". Animal Learning & Behavior. 9 (4): 476–82. doi:10.3758/BF03209777. S2CID 145693802.
  111. ^ Green, Leonard; Estle, Sara J (2003). "Preference reversals with food and water reinforcers in rats". Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 79 (2): 233–42. doi:10.1901/jeab.2003.79-233. PMC 1284932. PMID 12822689.
  112. ^ Hauser, Marc D.; Hallinan, Elizabeth V.; Stevens, Jeffrey R. (2005). "The ecology and evolution of patience in two New World monkeys". Biology Letters. 1 (2): 223–6. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0285. PMC 1626214. PMID 17148172.
  113. ^ a b Logan, Gordon D.; Schachar, Russell J.; Tannock, Rosemary (1997). "Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control". Psychological Science. 8 (1): 60–4. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00545.x. JSTOR 40062847. S2CID 145540424.
  114. ^ Enticott, Peter G.; Ogloff, James R.P.; Bradshaw, John L. (2006). "Associations between laboratory measures of executive inhibitory control and self-reported impulsivity". Personality and Individual Differences. 41 (2): 285–94. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.01.011.
  115. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r Nigg, Joel T. (2000). "On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy". Psychological Bulletin. 126 (2): 220–46. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220. PMID 10748641.
  116. ^ Gratton, Gabriele; Coles, Michael G. H.; Donchin, Emanuel (1992). "Optimizing the use of information: Strategic control of activation of responses". Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 121 (4): 480–506. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.480. PMID 1431740.
  117. ^ Diamond, A. (May 1997). Executive functions: Developmental perspective. Executive Function and Developmental Psychopathology. Toronto, Canada: University of Toronto Division of Child Psychiatry.
  118. ^ Cabeza, Roberto; Nyberg, Lars (1997). "Imaging Cognition: An Empirical Review of PET Studies with Normal Subjects". Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 9 (1): 1–26. doi:10.1162/jocn.1997.9.1.1. PMID 23968177. S2CID 33664797.
  119. ^ Logan, Gordon D.; Cowan, William B.; Davis, Kenneth A. (1984). "On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 10 (2): 276–91. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.10.2.276. PMID 6232345. S2CID 2595498.
  120. ^ Albarracin, D.; Handley, I. M.; Noguchi, K.; McCulloch, K. C.; Li, H.; Leeper, J.; Brown, R. D.; Earl, A.; Hart, W. (2008). "Increasing and decreasing motor and cognitive output: A model of general action and inaction goals". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 95 (3): 510–523. doi:10.1037/a0012833. PMC 3626413. PMID 18729691.
  121. ^ a b Barratt, Ernest S. (1959). "Anxiety and Impulsiveness Related to Psychomotor Efficiency". Perceptual and Motor Skills. 9 (3): 191–8. doi:10.2466/pms.1959.9.3.191. S2CID 143118554.
  122. ^ a b Patton, Jim H.; Stanford, Matthew S.; Barratt, Ernest S. (1995). "Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale". Journal of Clinical Psychology. 51 (6): 768–74. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1. PMID 8778124.
  123. ^ Eysenck, Sybil B.G.; Pearson, P.R.; Easting, G.; Allsopp, J.F. (1985). "Age norms for impulsiveness, venturesomeness and empathy in adults". Personality and Individual Differences. 6 (5): 613–9. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(85)90011-X.
  124. ^ a b c d Dean, Chelsea R. (2006). "Assessing self-injurious behaviors on a College Campus". Senior Honors Theses & Projects. Senior Honor Theses: Paper 59. from the original on 2013-12-03.[page needed]
  125. ^ a b Burnett Heyes S., Adam RJ., Urner M., van der Leer L., Bahrami B., Bays PM., Husain M.; Adam; Urner; Van Der Leer; Bahrami; Bays; Husain (22 May 2012). "Impulsivity and Rapid Decision-Making for Reward". Front. Psychol. 3: 153. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00153. PMC 3357492. PMID 22661960.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  126. ^ a b Dickman, S. J. (1990). Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: Personality and cognitive correlates. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(1), 95–102.
  127. ^ Brunas-Wagstaff, J., Tilley, A., Verity, M., Ford, S., & Thompson, D. (1997). Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity in children and their relationship to Eysenck's impulsiveness and venturesomeness dimensions. Personality and Individual Differences, 22(1), 19–35.
  128. ^ Whiteside, Stephen P.; Lynam, Donald R. (2001). "The Five Factor Model and impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity". Personality and Individual Differences. 30 (4): 669–89. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00064-7.
  129. ^ Cyders, Melissa A.; Smith, Gregory T.; Spillane, Nichea S.; Fischer, Sarah; Annus, Agnes M.; Peterson, Claire (2007). "Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior: Development and validation of a measure of positive urgency". Psychological Assessment. 19 (1): 107–18. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.19.1.107. PMID 17371126.
  130. ^ Billieux, Joël; Rochat, Lucien; Ceschi, Grazia; Carré, Arnaud; Offerlin-Meyer, Isabelle; Defeldre, Anne-Catherine; Khazaal, Yasser; Besche-Richard, Chrystel; Van Der Linden, Martial (2012). "Validation of a short French version of the UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale". Comprehensive Psychiatry. 53 (5): 609–15. doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.09.001. PMID 22036009.
  131. ^ Smith, G. T.; Fischer, S.; Cyders, M. A.; Annus, A. M.; Spillane, N. S. (2007). Toward a comprehensive model of multiple dispositions to rash action. Annual International Meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism. Chicago, IL.
  132. ^ Schmidt, Catherine A. (2000). Development and Validation of the Lifetime History of Impulsive Behaviors Interview and Self-Report Measures (PhD Thesis). OCLC 48501893.[page needed]
  133. ^ a b McCloskey, Michael S.; New, Antonia S.; Siever, Larry J.; Goodman, Marianne; Koenigsberg, Harold W.; Flory, Janine D.; Coccaro, Emil F. (2009). "Evaluation of behavioral impulsivity and aggression tasks as endophenotypes for borderline personality disorder". Journal of Psychiatric Research. 43 (12): 1036–48. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.01.002. PMC 2853811. PMID 19232640.
  134. ^ Carver, Charles S.; White, Teri L. (1994). "Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (2): 319–333. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.2.319. S2CID 6452203.
  135. ^ a b c d e Stanford, Matthew S.; Houston, Rebecca J.; Mathias, Charles W.; Villemarette-Pittman, Nicole R.; Helfritz, Laura E.; Conklin, Sarah M. (2003). "Characterizing Aggressive Behavior". Assessment. 10 (2): 183–90. doi:10.1177/1073191103010002009. PMID 12801190. S2CID 28427132.
  136. ^ Mathias, Charles W.; Stanford, Matthew S.; Marsh, Dawn M.; Frick, Paul J.; Moeller, F. Gerard; Swann, Alan C.; Dougherty, Donald M. (June 2007). "Characterizing aggressive behavior with the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale among adolescents with conduct disorder". Psychiatry Research. 151 (3): 231–242. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2006.11.001. PMC 1994790. PMID 17383014.
  137. ^ Burns, G.L. (1995). "Padua Inventory-Washington State University Revision". from the original on 2018-06-05.
  138. ^ Mischel, Walter; Ebbesen, Ebbe B.; Raskoff Zeiss, Antonette (1972). "Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 21 (2): 204–218. doi:10.1037/h0032198. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 5010404.
  139. ^ Mischel, Walter; Shoda, Yuichi; Peake, Philip K. (1988). "The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 54 (4): 687–96. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.4.687. PMID 3367285. S2CID 42623330.
  140. ^ Shoda, Yuichi; Mischel, Walter; Peake, Philip K. (1990). "Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions". Developmental Psychology. 26 (6): 978–86. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.6.978. S2CID 4259680.
  141. ^ Ayduk, Ozlem; Mendoza-Denton, Rodolfo; Mischel, Walter; Downey, Geraldine; Peake, Philip K.; Rodriguez, Monica (2000). "Regulating the interpersonal self: Strategic self-regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 79 (5): 776–92. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.776. PMID 11079241. S2CID 6345213.
  142. ^ Mazur, James E. (1987). "An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement". In Commons, Michael L.; Mazur, James E.; Nevin, John Anthony; et al. (eds.). The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value. Quantitative analyses of behavior: Vol. 5. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. pp. 55–73. ISBN 9781317838074.
  143. ^ Winstanley, Catharine A.; Dalley, Jeffrey W.; Theobald, David E. H.; Robbins, Trevor W. (November 2003). "Global 5-HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay-discounting task in rats". Psychopharmacology. 170 (3): 320–331. doi:10.1007/s00213-003-1546-3. PMID 12955303. S2CID 143605377.
  144. ^ a b Winstanley, Catharine A.; Eagle, Dawn M.; Robbins, Trevor W. (2006). "Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD: Translation between clinical and preclinical studies". Clinical Psychology Review. 26 (4): 379–95. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.01.001. PMC 1892795. PMID 16504359.
  145. ^ Lejuez, C. W.; Read, Jennifer P.; Kahler, Christopher W.; Richards, Jerry B.; Ramsey, Susan E.; Stuart, Gregory L.; Strong, David R.; Brown, Richard A. (2002). "Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking: The Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART)". Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied. 8 (2): 75–84. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.2.75. PMID 12075692. S2CID 7523498.
  146. ^ a b Bechara, Antoine; Damasio, Antonio R.; Damasio, Hanna; Anderson, Steven W. (1994). "Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex". Cognition. 50 (1–3): 7–15. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)90018-3. PMID 8039375. S2CID 204981454.
  147. ^ a b Buelow, Melissa T.; Suhr, Julie A. (2009). "Construct Validity of the Iowa Gambling Task". Neuropsychology Review. 19 (1): 102–14. doi:10.1007/s11065-009-9083-4. PMID 19194801. S2CID 8248057.
  148. ^ Ferster, C. B.; Skinner, B. F. Schedules of reinforcement. New York: 1957.[page needed]
  149. ^ a b Gordon, Michael (1979). "The assessment of impulsivity and mediating behaviors in hyperactive and nonhyperactive boys". Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 7 (3): 317–26. doi:10.1007/BF00916541. PMID 489853. S2CID 31953219.
  150. ^ Castellanos, F. Xavier; Tannock, Rosemary (2002). "Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: The search for endophenotypes". Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 3 (8): 617–28. doi:10.1038/nrn896. PMID 12154363. S2CID 3346437.
  151. ^ Kim, BaekSun; Im, Heh-In (2018). "The role of the dorsal striatum in choice impulsivity". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1451 (1): 92–111. doi:10.1111/nyas.13961. PMID 30277562. S2CID 52897511.
  152. ^ Berlin, H. A.; Rolls, ET; Kischka, U (2004). "Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions". Brain. 127 (5): 1108–26. doi:10.1093/brain/awh135. PMID 14985269.
  153. ^ Aron, Adam R.; Fletcher, Paul C.; Bullmore, Ed T.; Sahakian, Barbara J.; Robbins, Trevor W. (2003). "Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans". Nature Neuroscience. 6 (2): 115–6. doi:10.1038/nn1003. PMID 12536210. S2CID 10096947.
  154. ^ Muir, Janice L.; Everitt, Barry J.; Robbins, Trevor W. (1996). "The Cerebral Cortex of the Rat and Visual Attentional Function: Dissociable Effects of Mediofrontal, Cingulate, Anterior Dorsolateral, and Parietal Cortex Lesions on a Five-Choice Serial Reaction Time Task". Cerebral Cortex. 6 (3): 470–81. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.3.470. PMID 8670672.
  155. ^ Bechara, Antoine; Damasio, Hanna; Damasio, Antonio R.; Lee, Gregory P. (1999). "Different Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex to Decision-Making". The Journal of Neuroscience. 19 (13): 5473–81. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-13-05473.1999. PMC 6782338. PMID 10377356.
  156. ^ Wilens, Timothy E. (2006). "Mechanism of Action of Agents Used in Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 67 Suppl 8: 32–8. PMID 16961428. Archived from the original on 2013-06-28.
  157. ^ Dougherty, Darin D; Bonab, Ali A; Spencer, Thomas J; Rauch, Scott L; Madras, Bertha K; Fischman, Alan J (1999). "Dopamine transporter density in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder". The Lancet. 354 (9196): 2132–3. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(99)04030-1. PMID 10609822. S2CID 7420578.
  158. ^ Volkow, Nora D.; Wang, GJ; Kollins, SH; Wigal, TL; Newcorn, JH; Telang, F; Fowler, JS; Zhu, W; et al. (2009). "Evaluating Dopamine Reward Pathway in ADHD: Clinical Implications". JAMA. 302 (10): 1084–91. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.1308. PMC 2958516. PMID 19738093.
  159. ^ a b Wallis, D.; Russell, H. F.; Muenke, M. (2008). "Review: Genetics of Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Journal of Pediatric Psychology. 33 (10): 1085–99. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsn049. PMID 18522996.
  160. ^ Kempf, Lucas; Weinberger, Daniel R (2009). "Molecular Genetics and Bioinformatics: An Outline for Neuropsychological Genetics". In Goldberg, Terry E; Weinberger, Daniel R (eds.). The Genetics of Cognitive Neuroscience. pp. 3–26. ISBN 978-0-262-01307-9.
  161. ^ Brookes, K; Xu, X; Chen, W; Zhou, K; Neale, B; Lowe, N; Aneey, R; Franke, B; et al. (2006). "The analysis of 51 genes in DSM-IV combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Association signals in DRD4, DAT1 and 16 other genes". Molecular Psychiatry. 11 (10): 934–53. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001869. PMID 16894395. S2CID 6266643.
  162. ^ Ptacek, Radek; Kuzelova, Hana; Stefano, George B. (2011). "Dopamine D4 receptor gene DRD4 and its association with psychiatric disorders". Medical Science Monitor. 17 (9): RA215–20. doi:10.12659/MSM.881925. PMC 3560519. PMID 21873960.
  163. ^ Dulawa, Stephanie C.; Grandy, David K.; Low, Malcolm J.; Paulus, Martin P.; Geye, Mark A. (1999). "Dopamine D4 Receptor-Knock-Out Mice Exhibit Reduced Exploration of Novel Stimuli". The Journal of Neuroscience. 19 (21): 9550–6. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-21-09550.1999. PMC 6782928. PMID 10531457.
  164. ^ Nomura, Michio; Kusumi, Ichiro; Kaneko, Masayuki; Masui, Takuya; Daiguji, Makoto; Ueno, Takeji; Koyama, Tsukasa; Nomura, Yasuyuki (2006). "Involvement of a polymorphism in the 5-HT2A receptor gene in impulsive behavior". Psychopharmacology. 187 (1): 30–5. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0398-z. PMID 16767413. S2CID 12155339.
  165. ^ Bevilacqua, Laura; Doly, Stéphane; Kaprio, Jaakko; Yuan, Qiaoping; Tikkanen, Roope; Paunio, Tiina; Zhou, Zhifeng; Wedenoja, Juho; et al. (2010). "A population-specific HTR2B stop codon predisposes to severe impulsivity". Nature. 468 (7327): 1061–6. Bibcode:2010Natur.468.1061B. doi:10.1038/nature09629. PMC 3183507. PMID 21179162.
  166. ^ a b Ratner, Rebecca K.; Soman, Dilip; Zauberman, Gal; Ariely, Dan; Carmon, Ziv; Keller, Punam A.; Kim, B. Kyu; Lin, Fern; et al. (2008). "How behavioral decision research can enhance consumer welfare: From freedom of choice to paternalistic intervention". Marketing Letters. 19 (3–4): 383–397. doi:10.1007/s11002-008-9044-3. S2CID 5880506.
  167. ^ a b Berkman, Elliot T.; Graham, Alice M.; Fisher, Philip A. (2012). "Training Self-Control: A Domain-General Translational Neuroscience Approach". Child Development Perspectives. 6 (4): 374–384. doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00248.x. PMC 3722070. PMID 23894251.
  168. ^ Hofmann, Wilhelm; Schmeichel, Brandon J.; Baddeley, Alan D. (2012). "Executive functions and self-regulation". Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 16 (3): 174–80. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.01.006. PMID 22336729. S2CID 13309571.
  169. ^ Houben, Katrijn; Jansen, Anita (2011). "Training inhibitory control. A recipe for resisting sweet temptations". Appetite. 56 (2): 345–9. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.017. PMID 21185896. S2CID 10664026.
  170. ^ Houben, Katrijn; Nederkoorn, Chantal; Wiers, Reinout W.; Jansen, Anita (2011). "Resisting temptation: Decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition" (PDF). Drug and Alcohol Dependence. 116 (1–3): 132–6. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011. PMID 21288663.
  171. ^ Shipstead, Zach; Redick, Thomas S.; Engle, Randall W. (2012). "Is working memory training effective?". Psychological Bulletin. 138 (4): 628–54. doi:10.1037/a0027473. PMID 22409508. S2CID 14152894.
  172. ^ Swanson, James M.; Kraemer, Helena C.; Hinshaw, Stephen P.; Arnold, L. Eugene; Conners, C. Keith; Abikoff, Howard B.; Clevenger, Walter; Davies, Mark; et al. (2001). "Clinical Relevance of the Primary Findings of the MTA: Success Rates Based on Severity of ADHD and ODD Symptoms at the End of Treatment". Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 40 (2): 168–79. doi:10.1097/00004583-200102000-00011. PMID 11211365. S2CID 4638728.
  173. ^ Pelham, William E.; Fabiano, Gregory A. (2008). "Evidence-Based Psychosocial Treatments for Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder". Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology. 37 (1): 184–214. doi:10.1080/15374410701818681. PMID 18444058. S2CID 829396.
  174. ^ Evans, SW; Owens, JS; Bunford, N (2014). "Evidence-based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder". Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. 43 (4): 527–51. doi:10.1080/15374416.2013.850700. PMC 4025987. PMID 24245813.

Further reading

  • Evenden, J. L. (21 October 1999). "Varieties of impulsivity". Psychopharmacology. 146 (4): 348–361. doi:10.1007/PL00005481. PMID 10550486. S2CID 5972342.
  • Hollander, E.; Rosen, J. (March 2000). "Impulsivity". Journal of Psychopharmacology. 14 (2_suppl1): S39–S44. doi:10.1177/02698811000142S106. PMID 10888030. S2CID 243171966.
  • Moeller, F. Gerard; Barratt, Ernest S.; Dougherty, Donald M.; Schmitz, Joy M.; Swann, Alan C. (November 2001). "Psychiatric Aspects of Impulsivity". American Journal of Psychiatry. 158 (11): 1783–1793. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783. PMID 11691682.
  • Chamberlain, Samuel R; Sahakian, Barbara J (May 2007). "The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity". Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 20 (3): 255–261. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3280ba4989. PMID 17415079. S2CID 22198972.

External links

  •   Media related to Impulsivity at Wikimedia Commons
  • Impulsive Info

impulsivity, psychology, impulsivity, impulsiveness, tendency, whim, displaying, behavior, characterized, little, forethought, reflection, consideration, consequences, impulsive, actions, typically, poorly, conceived, prematurely, expressed, unduly, risky, ina. In psychology impulsivity or impulsiveness is a tendency to act on a whim displaying behavior characterized by little or no forethought reflection or consideration of the consequences 1 Impulsive actions are typically poorly conceived prematurely expressed unduly risky or inappropriate to the situation that often result in undesirable consequences 2 which imperil long term goals and strategies for success 3 Impulsivity can be classified as a multifactorial construct 4 A functional variety of impulsivity has also been suggested which involves action without much forethought in appropriate situations that can and does result in desirable consequences When such actions have positive outcomes they tend not to be seen as signs of impulsivity but as indicators of boldness quickness spontaneity courageousness or unconventionality 2 5 Thus the construct of impulsivity includes at least two independent components first acting without an appropriate amount of deliberation 2 which may or may not be functional and second choosing short term gains over long term ones 6 Orbitofrontal cortex part of the prefrontal cortex that shapes decision making Impulsivity is both a facet of personality and a major component of various disorders including FASD ADHD 7 substance use disorders 8 9 bipolar disorder 10 antisocial personality disorder 11 and borderline personality disorder 10 Abnormal patterns of impulsivity have also been noted instances of acquired brain injury 12 and neurodegenerative diseases 13 Neurobiological findings suggest that there are specific brain regions involved in impulsive behavior 14 15 16 although different brain networks may contribute to different manifestations of impulsivity 17 18 and that genetics may play a role 19 Many actions contain both impulsive and compulsive features but impulsivity and compulsivity are functionally distinct Impulsivity and compulsivity are interrelated in that each exhibits a tendency to act prematurely or without considered thought and often include negative outcomes 20 21 Compulsivity may be on a continuum with compulsivity on one end and impulsivity on the other but research has been contradictory on this point 22 Compulsivity occurs in response to a perceived risk or threat impulsivity occurs in response to a perceived immediate gain or benefit 20 and whereas compulsivity involves repetitive actions impulsivity involves unplanned reactions Impulsivity is a common feature of the conditions of gambling and alcohol addiction Research has shown that individuals with either of these addictions discount delayed money at higher rates than those without and that the presence of gambling and alcohol abuse lead to additive effects on discounting 23 Contents 1 Impulse 2 The five traits that can lead to impulsive actions 3 Associated behavioral and societal problems 3 1 Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder 3 2 Substance abuse 3 3 Eating 3 4 Impulse buying 3 5 Impulse control disorders not elsewhere classified 4 Theories of impulsivity 4 1 Ego cognitive depletion 4 2 Automatic vs controlled processes cognitive control 4 3 Intertemporal choice 4 4 Inhibitory control 4 4 1 Executive Inhibition 4 4 1 1 Interference control 4 4 1 2 Cognitive inhibition 4 4 1 3 Behavioral inhibition 4 4 1 4 Oculomotor Inhibition 4 4 2 Motivational inhibition 4 4 2 1 In response to punishment 4 4 2 2 In response to novelty 4 4 3 Automatic inhibition of attention 4 4 3 1 Recently inspected stimuli 4 4 3 2 Neglected stimuli 4 5 Action Inaction goals 5 Assessment of impulsivity 5 1 Personality tests and reports 5 1 1 Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 5 1 2 Eysenck Impulsiveness Scale 5 1 3 Dickman Impulsivity Inventory 5 1 4 UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale 5 1 5 Lifetime History of Impulsive Behaviors 5 1 6 Behavioral Inhibition System Behavioral Activation System 5 1 7 Impulsive Premeditated Aggression Scale 5 1 8 Padua Inventory 5 2 Behavioral paradigms 5 2 1 Marshmallow test 5 2 2 Delay discounting 5 2 3 Go no go and stop signal reaction time tasks 5 2 4 Balloon Analogue Risk Task 5 2 5 Iowa Gambling Task 5 2 6 Differential Reinforcement of Low Response Rate Task 5 2 7 Other 6 Pharmacology and neurobiology 6 1 Neurobiological findings 6 1 1 Delay discounting 6 1 2 Go No go and Stop signal reaction time test 6 1 3 5 Choice Serial Reaction Time Task 5 CSRTT and Differential Reinforcement of Low rates DRL 6 1 4 Iowa Gambling Task 6 2 Neurochemical and pharmacological findings 7 Genetics 8 Intervention 8 1 Interventions to impact impulsivity generally 8 1 1 Brain training 8 2 Treatment of specific disorders of impulsivity 8 2 1 Psychopharmacological intervention 8 2 2 Behavioral interventions 9 See also 10 References 11 Further reading 12 External linksImpulse EditAn impulse is a wish or urge particularly a sudden one It can be considered as a normal and fundamental part of human thought processes but also one that can become problematic as in a condition like obsessive compulsive disorder 24 unreliable medical source borderline personality disorder attention deficit hyperactivity disorder or in fetal alcohol spectrum disorders The ability to control impulses or more specifically control the desire to act on them is an important factor in personality and socialization Deferred gratification also known as impulse control is an example of this concerning impulses primarily relating to things that a person wants or desires Delayed gratification comes when one avoids acting on initial impulses Delayed gratification has been studied in relation to childhood obesity Resisting the urge to act on impulses is important to teach children because it teaches the value of delayed gratification 25 Many psychological problems are characterized by a loss of control or a lack of control in specific situations Usually this lack of control is part of a pattern of behavior that also involves other maladaptive thoughts and actions such as substance abuse problems or sexual disorders like the paraphilias e g pedophilia and exhibitionism When loss of control is only a component of a disorder it usually does not have to be a part of the behavior pattern and other symptoms must also be present for the diagnosis to be made Franklin 26 unreliable medical source The five traits that can lead to impulsive actions EditFor many years it was understood that impulsivity is a trait but with further analysis it can be found that there were five traits that can lead to impulsive actions positive urgency negative urgency sensation seeking lack of planning and lack of perseverance 27 28 29 30 Associated behavioral and societal problems EditAttention deficit hyperactivity disorder Edit Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ADHD is a multiple component disorder involving inattention impulsivity and hyperactivity The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders DSM IV TR 31 breaks ADHD into three subtypes according to the behavioral symptoms Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Predominantly Inattentive Type Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Predominantly Hyperactive Impulsive Type and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Combined Type Predominantly hyperactive impulsive type symptoms may include fidgeting and squirming in seats talking nonstop dashing around and touching or playing with anything in sight having trouble sitting still during dinner school story time being constantly in motion and having difficulty doing quiet tasks or activities Other manifestations primarily of impulsivity include being very impatient having difficulty waiting for things they want or waiting their turns in games often interrupting conversations or others activities or blurting out inappropriate comments showing their emotions without restraint and act without regard for consequences Prevalence of the disorder worldwide is estimated to be between 4 and 10 with reports as low as 2 2 and as high as 17 8 Variation in rate of diagnoses may be attributed to differences between populations i e culture and differences in diagnostic methodologies 32 Prevalence of ADHD among females is less than half that of males and females more commonly fall into the inattentive subtype 33 Despite an upward trend in diagnoses of the inattentive subtype of ADHD impulsivity is commonly considered to be the central feature of ADHD and the impulsive and combined subtypes are the major contributors to the societal costs associated with ADHD 33 34 The estimated cost of illness for a child with ADHD is 14 576 in 2005 dollars annually 35 Prevalence of ADHD among prison populations is significantly higher than that of the normal population 36 In both adults 37 and children 38 39 ADHD has a high rate of comorbidity with other mental health disorders such as learning disability conduct disorder anxiety disorder major depressive disorder bipolar disorder and substance use disorders The precise genetic and environmental factors contributing to ADHD are relatively unknown but endophenotypes offer a potential middle ground between genes and symptoms 40 ADHD is commonly linked to core deficits involving executive function delay aversion or activation arousal theories that attempt to explain ADHD through its symptomology 40 Endophenotypes on the other hand purport to identify potential behavioral markers that correlate with specific genetic etiology There is some evidence to support deficits in response inhibition as one such marker Problems inhibiting prepotent responses are linked with deficits in pre frontal cortex PFC functioning which is a common dysfunction associated with ADHD and other impulse control disorders 41 42 Evidence based psychopharmacological and behavioral interventions exist for ADHD 43 Substance abuse Edit Impulsivity appears to be linked to all stages of substance abuse 44 45 The acquisition phase of substance abuse involves the escalation from single use to regular use 44 Impulsivity may be related to the acquisition of substance abuse because of the potential role that instant gratification provided by the substance may offset the larger future benefits of abstaining from the substance and because people with impaired inhibitory control may not be able to overcome motivating environmental cues such as peer pressure 46 Similarly individuals that discount the value of delayed reinforcers begin to abuse alcohol marijuana and cigarettes early in life while also abusing a wider array of illicit drugs compared to those who discounted delayed reinforcers less 47 Escalation or dysregulation is the next and more severe phase of substance abuse In this phase individuals lose control of their addiction with large levels of drug consumption and binge drug use Animal studies suggest that individuals with higher levels of impulsivity may be more prone to the escalation stage of substance abuse 44 Impulsivity is also related to the abstinence relapse and treatment stages of substance abuse People who scored high on the Barratt Impulsivity Scale BIS were more likely to stop treatment for cocaine abuse 48 Additionally they adhered to treatment for a shorter duration than people that scored low on impulsivity 48 Also impulsive people had greater cravings for drugs during withdrawal periods and were more likely to relapse This effect was shown in a study where smokers that test high on the BIS had increased craving in response to smoking cues and gave into the cravings more quickly than less impulsive smokers 49 Taken as a whole the current research suggests that impulsive individuals are less likely to abstain from drugs and more likely to relapse earlier than less impulsive individuals 44 While it is important to note the effect of impulsivity on substance abuse the reciprocating effect whereby substance abuse can increase impulsivity has also been researched and documented 44 The promoting effect of impulsivity on substance abuse and the effect of substance abuse on increased impulsivity creates a positive feedback loop that maintains substance seeking behaviors It also makes conclusions about the direction of causality difficult This phenomenon has been shown to be related to several substances but not all For example alcohol has been shown to increase impulsivity while amphetamines have had mixed results 44 Substance use disorder treatments include prescription of medications such as acamprosate buprenorphine disulfiram LAAM methadone and naltrexone 50 as well as effective psychotherapeutic treatment like behavioral couples therapy CBT contingency management motivational enhancement therapy and relapse prevention 50 Eating Edit Impulsive overeating spans from an episode of indulgence by an otherwise healthy person to chronic binges by a person with an eating disorder citation needed Consumption of a tempting food by non clinical individuals increases when self regulatory resources are previously depleted by another task suggesting that it is caused by a breakdown in self control 51 Impulsive eating of unhealthy snack foods appears to be regulated by individual differences in impulsivity when self control is weak and by attitudes towards the snack and towards healthy eating when self control is strong 52 There is also evidence that greater food consumption occurs when people are in a sad mood although it is possible that this is due more to emotional regulation than to a lack of self control 53 In these cases overeating will only take place if the food is palatable to the person and if so individual differences in impulsivity can predict the amount of consumption 54 Chronic overeating is a behavioral component of binge eating disorder compulsive overeating and bulimia nervosa These diseases are more common for women and may involve eating thousands of calories at a time Depending on which of these disorders is the underlying cause an episode of overeating can have a variety of different motivations Characteristics common among these three disorders include low self esteem depression eating when not physically hungry preoccupation with food eating alone due to embarrassment and feelings of regret or disgust after an episode In these cases overeating is not limited to palatable foods 55 Impulsivity differentially affects disorders involving the overcontrol of food intake such as anorexia nervosa and disorders involving the lack of control of food intake such as bulimia nervosa Cognitive impulsivity such as risk taking is a component of many eating disorders including those that are restrictive 56 However only people with disorders involving episodes of overeating have elevated levels of motoric impulsivity such as reduced response inhibition capacity 56 One theory suggests that binging provides a short term escape from feelings of sadness anger or boredom although it may contribute to these negative emotions in the long term 57 Another theory suggests that binge eating involves reward seeking as evidenced by decreased serotonin binding receptors of binge eating women compared to matched weight controls 58 and predictive value of heightened reward sensitivity drive in dysfunctional eating 59 Treatments for clinical grade overeating include cognitive behavioral therapy to teach people how to track and change their eating habits and actions interpersonal psychotherapy to help people analyze the contribution of their friends and family in their disorder and pharmacological therapies including antidepressants and SSRIs 60 Impulse buying Edit Impulse buying consists of purchasing a product or service without any previous intent to make that purchase 61 It has been speculated to account for as much as eighty percent of all purchases 62 in the United States relevant There are several theories pertaining to impulsive buying One theory suggests that it is exposure combining with the speed that a reward can be obtained that influences an individual to choose lesser immediate rewards over greater rewards that can be obtained later 63 For example a person might choose to buy a candy bar because they are in the candy aisle even though they had decided earlier that they would not buy candy while in the store Another theory is one of self regulation 57 which suggests that the capacity to refrain from impulsive buying is a finite resource As this capacity is depleted with repeated acts of restraint susceptibility to purchasing other items on impulse increases citation needed Finally a third theory suggests an emotional and behavioral tie between the purchaser and the product which drives both the likelihood of an impulsive purchase as well as the degree that a person will retroactively be satisfied with that purchase result 64 65 Some studies have shown a large number of individuals are happy with purchases made on impulse 41 in one study 66 which is explained as a preexisting emotional attachment which has a positive relationship both with the likelihood of initiating the purchase as well as mitigating post purchase satisfaction 65 As an example when purchasing team related college paraphernalia a large percentage of those purchases are made on impulse and are tied to the degree with which a person has positive ties to that team 65 Impulsive buying is seen both as an individual trait in which each person has a preconditioned or hereditary allotment as well as a situational construct which is mitigated by such things as emotion in the moment of the purchase and the preconditioned ties an individual has with the product 57 65 Psychotherapy and pharmacological treatments have been shown to be helpful interventions for patients with impulsive compulsive buying disorder 67 Psychotherapy interventions include the use of desensitization techniques 68 self help books 69 or attending a support group 69 Pharmacological interventions include the use of SSRIs such as fluvoxamine 70 71 citalopram 72 73 escitalopram 74 and naltrexone 75 76 Impulse control disorders not elsewhere classified Edit Impulse control disorder ICDs are a class of DSM diagnoses that do not fall into the other diagnostic categories of the manual e g substance use disorders and that are characterized by extreme difficulty controlling impulses or urges despite negative consequences 31 Individuals suffering from an impulse control disorder frequently experience five stages of symptoms compelling urge or desire failure to resist the urge a heightened sense of arousal succumbing to the urge which usually yields relief from tension and potential remorse or feelings of guilt after the behavior is completed 77 Specific disorders included within this category include intermittent explosive disorder kleptomania pathological gambling pyromania trichotillomania hair pulling disorder and impulse control disorders not otherwise specified ICD NOS ICD NOS includes other significant difficulties that seem to be related to impulsivity but do not meet the criteria for a specific DSM diagnosis 31 There has been much debate over whether or not the ICDs deserve a diagnostic category of their own or whether they are in fact phenomenologically and epidemiologically related to other major psychiatric conditions like obsessive compulsive disorder OCD affective disorders and addictive disorders 78 In fact the ICD classification is likely to change with the release of the DSM V in May 2013 79 In this new revision the ICD NOS will likely be reduced or removed proposed revisions include reclassifying trichotillomania to be renamed hair pulling disorder and skin picking disorder as obsessive compulsive and related disorders moving intermittent explosive disorder under the diagnostic heading of disruptive impulse control and conduct disorders and gambling disorder may be included in addiction and related disorders 79 The role of impulsivity in the ICDs varies Research on kleptomania and pyromania is lacking though there is some evidence that greater kleptomania severity is tied to poor executive functioning 80 Trichotillomania and skin picking disorder seem to be disorders that primarily involve motor impulsivity 81 82 and will likely be classified in the DSM V within the obsessive compulsive and related disorders category 79 Pathological gambling in contrast seems to involve many diverse aspects of impulsivity and abnormal reward circuitry similar to substance use disorders that has led to it being increasingly conceptualized as a non substance or behavioral addiction 83 Evidence elucidating the role of impulsivity in pathological gambling is accumulating with pathological gambling samples demonstrating greater response impulsivity choice impulsivity and reflection impulsivity than comparison control samples 83 Additionally pathological gamblers tend to demonstrate greater response perseveration compulsivity and risky decisionmaking in laboratory gambling tasks compared to controls though there is no strong evidence suggesting that attention and working memory are impaired in pathological gamblers 83 These relations between impulsivity and pathological gambling are confirmed by brain function research pathological gamblers demonstrate less activation in the frontal cortical regions implicated in impulsivity compared to controls during behavioral tasks tapping response impulsivity compulsivity and risk reward 83 Preliminary though variable findings also suggest that striatal activation is different between gamblers and controls and that neurotransmitter differences e g dopamine serotonin opioids glutamate norepinephrine may exist as well 83 Individuals with intermittent explosive disorder also known as impulsive aggression have exhibited serotonergic abnormalities and show differential activation in response to emotional stimuli and situations 84 Notably intermittent explosive disorder is not associated with a higher likelihood of diagnosis with any of the other ICDs but is highly comorbid with disruptive behavior disorders in childhood 84 Intermittent explosive disorder is likely to be re classified in the DSM V under the heading of disruptive impulse control and conduct disorders 79 These sorts of impulse control disorders are most often treated using certain types of psychopharamcological interventions e g antidepressants and behavioral treatments like cognitive behavioral therapy citation needed Theories of impulsivity EditEgo cognitive depletion Edit According to the ego or cognitive depletion theory of impulsivity self control refers to the capacity for altering one s own responses especially to bring them into line with standards such as ideals values morals and social expectations and to support the pursuit of long term goals 85 Self control enables a person to restrain or override one response thereby making a different response possible 85 A major tenet of the theory is that engaging in acts of self control draws from a limited reservoir of self control that when depleted results in reduced capacity for further self regulation 86 87 Self control is viewed as analogous to a muscle Just as a muscle requires strength and energy to exert force over a period of time acts that have high self control demands also require strength and energy to perform 88 Similarly as muscles become fatigued after a period of sustained exertion and have reduced capacity to exert further force self control can also become depleted when demands are made of self control resources over a period of time Baumeister and colleagues termed the state of diminished self control strength ego depletion or cognitive depletion 87 The strength model of self control asserts that Just as exercise can make muscles stronger there are signs that regular exertions of self control can improve willpower strength 89 These improvements typically take the form of resistance to depletion in the sense that performance at self control tasks deteriorates at a slower rate 85 Targeted efforts to control behavior in one area such as spending money or exercise lead to improvements in unrelated areas such as studying or household chores And daily exercises in self control such as improving posture altering verbal behavior and using one s nondominant hand for simple tasks gradually produce improvements in self control as measured by laboratory tasks 85 The finding that these improvements carry over into tasks vastly different from the daily exercises shows that the improvements are not due to simply increasing skill or acquiring self efficacy from practice 85 Just as athletes begin to conserve their remaining strength when their muscles begin to tire so do self controllers when some of their self regulatory resources have been expended The severity of behavioral impairment during depletion depends in part on whether the person expects further challenges and demands 85 When people expect to have to exert self control later they will curtail current performance more severely than if no such demands are anticipated 90 Consistent with the conservation hypothesis people can exert self control despite ego depletion if the stakes are high enough Offering cash incentives or other motives for good performance counteracts the effects of ego depletion 91 This may seem surprising but in fact it may be highly adaptive Given the value and importance of the capacity for self control it would be dangerous for a person to lose that capacity completely and so ego depletion effects may occur because people start conserving their remaining strength 85 When people do exert themselves on the second task they deplete the resource even more as reflected in severe impairments on a third task that they have not anticipated 90 Empirical tests of the ego depletion effect typically adopt dual task paradigm 86 92 93 Participants assigned to an experimental ego depletion group are required to engage in two consecutive tasks requiring self control 88 Control participants are also required to engage in two consecutive tasks but only the second task requires self control The strength model predicts that the performance of the experimental group on the second self control task will be impaired relative to that of the control group This is because the finite self control resources of the experimental participants will be diminished after the initial self control task leaving little to draw on for the second task 85 The effects of ego depletion do not appear to be a product of mood or arousal In most studies mood and arousal has not been found to differ between participants who exerted self control and those who did not 86 94 Likewise mood and arousal was not related to final self control performance 94 The same is true for more specific mood items such as frustration irritation annoyance boredom or interest as well Feedback about success and failure of the self control efforts does not appear to affect performance 95 In short the decline in self control performance after exerting self control appears to be directly related to the amount of self control exerted and cannot be easily explained by other well established psychological processes 94 Automatic vs controlled processes cognitive control Edit Dual process theory states that mental processes operate in two separate classes automatic and controlled In general automatic processes are those that are experiential in nature occur without involving higher levels of cognition 96 and are based on prior experiences or informal heuristics Controlled decisions are effortful and largely conscious processes in which an individual weighs alternatives and makes a more deliberate decision citation needed Automatic Process Automatic processes have four main features 97 They occur unintentionally or without a conscious decision the cost of the decision is very low in mental resources they cannot be easily stopped and they occur without conscious thought on the part of the individual making them Controlled Process Controlled processes also have four main features 97 that are very close to the opposite in spectrum from their automatic counterparts Controlled processes occur intentionally they require the expenditure of cognitive resources the individual making the decision can stop the process voluntarily and the mental process is a conscious one Dual process theories at one time considered any single action thought as either being automatic or controlled 97 However currently they are seen as operating more along a continuum as most impulsive actions will have both controlled and automatic attributes 97 Automatic processes are classified according to whether they are meant to inhibit or to facilitate a thought process 98 For example in one study 99 researchers offered individuals a choice between a 1 in 10 chance of winning a prize and a 10 in 100 chance Many participants chose one of the choices over the other without identifying that the chances inherent in each were the same as they saw either only 10 chances total as more beneficial or of having 10 chances to win as more beneficial In effect impulsive decisions can be made as prior information and experiences dictate one of the courses of action is more beneficial when in actuality careful consideration would better enable the individual to make a more informed and improved decision citation needed Intertemporal choice Edit Intertemporal choice is defined as decisions with consequences that play out over time 100 This is often assessed using the relative value people assign to rewards at different points in time either by asking experimental subjects to choose between alternatives or examining behavioral choices in a naturalistic setting citation needed Intertemporal choice is commonly measured in the laboratory using a delayed discounting paradigm which measures the process of devaluing rewards and punishments that happen in the future 100 In this paradigm subjects must choose between a smaller reward delivered soon and a larger reward delivered at a delay in the future Choosing the smaller sooner reward is considered impulsive By repeatedly making these choices indifference points can be estimated For example if someone chose 70 now over 100 in a week but chose the 100 in a week over 60 now it can be inferred that they are indifferent between 100 in a week and an intermediate value between 60 and 70 A delay discounting curve can be obtained for each participant by plotting their indifference points with different reward amounts and time delays Individual differences in discounting curves are affected by personality characteristics such as self reports of impulsivity and locus of control personal characteristics such as age gender IQ race and culture socioeconomic characteristics such as income and education and many other variables 101 to drug addiction 102 103 Lesions of the nucleus accumbens core subregion 104 or basolateral amygdala 105 produce shifts towards choosing the smaller sooner reward suggesting the involvement of these brain regions in the preference for delayed reinforcers There is also evidence that the orbitofrontal cortex is involved in delay discounting although there is currently debate on whether lesions in this region result in more or less impulsivity 106 Economic theory suggests that optimal discounting involves the exponential discounting of value over time This model assumes that people and institutions should discount the value of rewards and punishments at a constant rate according to how delayed they are in time 100 While economically rational recent evidence suggests that people and animals do not discount exponentially Many studies suggest that humans and animals discount future values according to a hyperbolic discounting curve where the discount factor decreases with the length of the delay for example waiting from today to tomorrow involves more loss of value than waiting from twenty days to twenty one days Further evidence for non constant delay discounting is suggested by the differential involvement of various brain regions in evaluating immediate versus delayed consequences Specifically the prefrontal cortex is activated when choosing between rewards at a short delay or a long delay but regions associated with the dopamine system are additionally activated when the option of an immediate reinforcer is added 107 Additionally intertemporal choices differ from economic models because they involve anticipation which may involve a neurological reward even if the reinforcer is delayed self control and the breakdown of it when faced with temptations and representation how the choice is framed may influence desirability of the reinforcer 100 none of which are accounted for by a model that assumes economic rationality citation needed One facet of intertemporal choice is the possibility for preference reversal when a tempting reward becomes more highly valued than abstaining only when immediately available 3 For example when sitting home alone a person may report that they value the health benefit of not smoking a cigarette over the effect of smoking one However later at night when the cigarette is immediately available their subjective value of the cigarette may rise and they may choose to smoke it citation needed A theory called the primrose path is intended to explain how preference reversal can lead to addiction in the long run 108 As an example a lifetime of sobriety may be more highly valued than a lifetime of alcoholism but at the same time one drink now may be more highly valued than not drinking now Because it is always now the drink is always chosen and a paradoxical effect occurs whereby the more valued long term alternative is not achieved because the more valued short term alternative is always chosen This is an example of complex ambivalence 109 when a choice is made not between two concrete alternatives but between one immediate and tangible alternative i e having a drink and one delayed and abstract alternative i e sobriety Similarities between humans and non human animals in intertemporal choice have been studied Pigeons 110 and rats 111 also discount hyperbolically tamarin monkeys do not wait more than eight seconds to triple the amount of a food reward 112 The question arises as to whether this is a difference of homology or analogy that is whether the same underlying process underlies human animal similarities or whether different processes are manifesting in similar patterns of results citation needed Inhibitory control Edit Main article inhibitory control Inhibitory control often conceptualized as an executive function is the ability to inhibit or hold back a prepotent response 113 It is theorized that impulsive behavior reflects a deficit in this ability to inhibit a response impulsive people may find it more difficult to inhibit action whereas non impulsive people may find it easier to do so 113 There is evidence that in normal adults commonly used behavioral measures of inhibitory control correlate with standard self report measures of impulsivity 114 Inhibitory control may itself be multifaceted evidenced by numerous distinct inhibition constructs that can be measured in different ways and relate to specific types of psychopathology 115 Joel Nigg developed a useful working taxonomy of these different types of inhibition drawing heavily from the fields of cognitive and personality psychology 115 Nigg s eight proposed types of inhibition include the following Executive Inhibition Edit Interference control Edit Suppression of a stimulus that elicits an interfering response enabling a person to complete the primary response Interference control can also refer to suppressing distractors 115 Interference control has been measured using cognitive tasks like the stroop test flanker tasks dual task interference and priming tasks 116 Personality researchers have used the Rothbart effortful control measures and the conscientiousness scale of the Big Five as inventory measures of interference control Based on imaging and neural research it is theorized that the anterior cingulate the dorsolateral prefrontal premotor cortex and the basal ganglia are related to interference control 117 118 Cognitive inhibition Edit Cognitive inhibition is the suppression of unwanted or irrelevant thoughts to protect working memory and attention resources 115 Cognitive inhibition is most often measured through tests of directed ignoring self report on one s intrusive thoughts and negative priming tasks As with interference control personality psychologists have measured cognitive inhibition using the Rothbart Effortful Control scale and the Big Five Conscientiousness scale The anterior cingulate the prefrontal regions and the association cortex seem to be involved in cognitive inhibition 115 Behavioral inhibition Edit Behavioral Inhibition is the suppression of prepotent response 115 Behavioral inhibition is usually measured using the Go No Go task Stop signal task and reports of suppression of attentional orienting Surveys that are theoretically relevant to behavioral inhibition include the Rothbart effortful control scale and the Big Five Conscientiousness dimension 115 The rationale behind the use of behavioral measures like the Stop signal task is that go processes and stop processes are independent and that upon go and stop cues they race against each other if the go process wins the race the prepotent response is executed whereas if the stop processes wins the race the response is withheld In this context impulsivity is conceptualized as a relatively slow stop process 119 The brain regions involved in behavioral inhibition appear to be the lateral and orbital prefrontal regions along with premotor processes Oculomotor Inhibition Edit Oculomotor Inhibition is the effortful suppression of reflexive saccade 115 Oculomotor inhibition is tested using antisaccade and oculomotor tasks Also the Rothbart effortful control measure and the Big Five Conscientiousness dimension are thought to tap some of the effortful processes underlying the ability to suppress saccade The frontal eye fields and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex are involved in oculomotor inhibition 115 Motivational inhibition Edit In response to punishment Edit Motivational inhibition and response in the face of punishment can be measured using tasks tapping inhibition of primary response modified go no go tasks inhibition of competing response and emotional Stroop tasks 115 Personality psychologists also use the Gray behavioral inhibition system measure the Eysenck scale for neurotic introversion and the Zuckerman Neuroticism Anxiety scale 115 The Septal hippocampal formation cingulate and motor systems seem to be the brain areas most involved in response to punishment 115 In response to novelty Edit Response to novelty has been measured using the Kagan behavioral inhibition system measure and scales of neurotic introversion 115 The amygdaloid system is implicated in novelty response 115 Automatic inhibition of attention Edit Recently inspected stimuli Edit Suppression of recently inspected stimuli for both attention and oculomotor saccade is usually measured using attentional and oculomotor inhibition of return tests The superior colliculus and the midbrain oculomotor pathway are involved in suppression of stimuli 115 Neglected stimuli Edit Information at locations that are not presently being attended to is suppressed while attending elsewhere 115 This involves measures of covert attentional orienting and neglect along with personality scales on neuroticism 115 The posterior association cortex and subcortical pathways are implicated in this sort of inhibition 115 Action Inaction goals Edit Recent psychology research also yields out the condition of impulsivity in relation to peoples general goal setting It is possible these action and inaction goals are underlying people s behavioral differences in their daily lives since they can demonstrate patterns comparable to natural variation in overall activity levels 120 More specifically the level of impulsivity and mania people have might positive correlated with favorable attitudes about and goals of general action while negatively respond to favorable attitudes about and goals of general inaction Assessment of impulsivity EditPersonality tests and reports Edit Barratt Impulsiveness Scale Edit The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale BIS is one of the oldest and most widely used measures of impulsive personality traits The first BIS was developed in 1959 by Dr Ernest Barratt 121 It has been revised extensively to achieve two major goals 1 to identify a set of impulsiveness items that was orthogonal to a set of anxiety items as measured by the Taylor Manifest Anxiety Scale MAS or the Cattell Anxiety Scale and 2 to define impulsiveness within the structure of related personality traits like Eysenck s Extraversion dimension or Zuckerman s Sensation Seeking dimension especially the disinhibition subfactor 121 The BIS 11 with 30 items was developed in 1995 122 According to Patton and colleagues there are 3 subscales Attentional Impulsiveness Motor Impulsiveness and Non Planning Impulsiveness with six factors 122 Attention focusing on a task at hand Motor impulsiveness acting on the spur of the moment Self control planning and thinking carefully Cognitive complexity enjoying challenging mental tasks Perseverance a consistent life style Cognitive instability thought insertion and racing thoughts Eysenck Impulsiveness Scale Edit The Eysenck Impulsiveness Scale EIS 123 is a 54 item yes no questionnaire designed to measure impulsiveness Three subscales are computed from this measure Impulsiveness Venturesomeness and Empathy Impulsiveness is defined as behaving without thinking and without realizing the risk involved in the behavior 124 Venturesomeness is conceptualized as being conscious of the risk of the behavior but acting anyway 124 The questionnaire was constructed through factor analysis to contain items that most highly loaded on impulsiveness and venturesomeness 124 The EIS is a widely used and well validated measure 124 Dickman Impulsivity Inventory Edit The Dickman Impulsivity Inventory was first developed in 1990 by Scott J Dickman This scale is based on Dickman s proposal that there are two types of impulsivity that are significantly different from one another 125 This includes functional impulsivity which is characterized by quick decision making when it is optimal a trait that is often considered to be a source of pride The scale also includes dysfunctional impulsivity which is characterized by making quick decisions when it is not optimal This type of impulsivity is most often associated with life difficulties including substance abuse problems and other negative outcomes 126 This scale includes 63 items of which 23 are related to dysfunctional impulsivity 17 are related to functional impulsivity and 23 are filler questions that relate to neither construct 126 This scale has been developed into a version for use with children 127 as well as into several languages Dickman showed there is no correlation between these two tendencies across individuals and they also have different cognitive correlates 125 UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale Edit The UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale 128 is a 45 item self report questionnaire that was designed to measure impulsivity across dimensions of the Five Factor Model of personality The UPPS includes 4 sub scales lack of premeditation urgency lack of perseverance and sensation seeking UPPS P Impulsive Behavior Scale UPPS P 129 is a revised version of the UPPS including 59 items It assesses an additional personality pathway to impulsive behavior Positive Urgency in addition to the four pathways assessed in the original version of the scale Urgency now Negative Urgency lack of Premeditation lack of Perseverance and Sensation SeekingUPPS P short version UPPS Ps 130 is 20 item scale that evaluates five different impulsivity facets 4 items per dimension UPPS R Interview 131 is a semi structured interview that measures the degree to which individuals exhibit the various components of impulsivity assessed by the UPPS P Lifetime History of Impulsive Behaviors Edit Lifetime History of Impulsive Behaviors LHIB 132 is a 53 item questionnaire designed to assess lifetime history of impulsive behavior as opposed to impulsive tendencies as well as the level of distress and impairment associated with these behaviors 133 The assessment battery was designed to measure the following six dimensions a impulsivity b sensation seeking c trait anxiety d state depression e empathy and f social desirability The LHIB consists of scales for clinically significant impulsivity non clinically significant impulsivity and impulsivity related distress impairment 133 Behavioral Inhibition System Behavioral Activation System Edit Behavioral Inhibition System Behavioral Activation System BIS BAS 134 was developed based on the Gray s biopsychological theory of personality which suggests that there are two general motivational systems that underlie behavior and affect BIS and BAS This 20 item self report questionnaire is designed to assess dispositional BIS and BAS sensitivities Impulsive Premeditated Aggression Scale Edit Impulsive Premeditated Aggression Scale IPAS 135 is a 30 item self report questionnaire Half of the items describe impulsive aggression and half the items describe premeditated aggression Aggressive behavior has traditionally been classified into two distinct subtypes impulsive or premeditated Impulsive aggression is defined as a hair trigger aggressive response to provocation with loss of behavioral control 135 Premeditated aggression is defined as a planned or conscious aggressive act not spontaneous or related to an agitated state 135 The IPAS is designed to characterize aggressive behavior as predominately impulsive or predominately premeditated in nature 135 Those subjects who clustered on the impulsive factor showed a broad range of emotional and cognitive impairments those who clustered on the premeditated factor showed a greater inclination for aggression and anti social behaviour 135 136 Padua Inventory Edit The Padua Inventory PI consists of 60 items describing common obsessional and compulsive behavior and allows investigation of such problems in normal and clinical subjects 137 Behavioral paradigms Edit A wide variety of behavioral tests have been devised for the assessment of impulsivity in both clinical and experimental settings While no single test is a perfect predictor or a sufficient replacement for an actual clinical diagnosis when used in conjunction with parent teacher reports behavioral surveys and other diagnostic criteria the utility of behavioral paradigms lies in their ability to narrow in on specific discrete aspects of the impulsivity umbrella Quantifying specific deficits is of use to the clinician and the experimenter both of whom are generally concerned with obtaining objectively measurable treatment effects citation needed Marshmallow test Edit One widely recognizable test for impulsivity is the delay of gratification paradigm commonly known as the marshmallow test 63 Developed in the 1960s to assess willpower and self control in preschoolers the marshmallow test consists of placing a single marshmallow in front of a child and informing them that they will be left alone in the room for some duration The child is told that if the marshmallow remains uneaten when the experimenter returns they will be awarded a second marshmallow both of which can then be eaten citation needed 138 Despite its simplicity and ease of administration evidence from longitudinal studies suggests that the number of seconds preschoolers wait to obtain the second marshmallow is predictive of higher SAT scores better social and emotional coping in adolescence higher educational achievement and less cocaine crack use 139 140 141 Delay discounting Edit Like the marshmallow test delay discounting is also a delay of gratification paradigm 142 It is designed around the principle that the subjective value of a reinforcer decreases or is discounted as the delay to reinforcement increases Subjects are given varying choices between smaller immediate rewards and larger delayed rewards By manipulating reward magnitude and or reward delay over multiple trials indifference points can be estimated whereby choosing the small immediate reward or the large delayed reward are about equally likely Subjects are labeled impulsive when their indifference points decline more steeply as a function of delay compared to the normal population i e greater preference for immediate reward Unlike the marshmallow test delay discounting does not require verbal instruction and can be implemented on non human animals 143 Go no go and stop signal reaction time tasks Edit Two common tests of response inhibition used in humans are the go no go task and a slight variant known as the stop signal reaction time SSRT test During a go no task the participant is trained over multiple trials to make a particular response e g a key press when presented with a go signal On some trials a stop signal is presented just prior to or simultaneously with the go signal and the subject must inhibit the impending response The SSRT test is similar except that the stop signal is presented after the go signal This small modification increases the difficulty of inhibiting the go response because the participant has typically already initiated the go response by the time the stop signal is presented 144 The participant is instructed to respond as fast as possible to the go signal while maintaining the highest possible inhibition accuracy on no go trials During the task the time at which the stop signal is presented the stop signal delay or SSD is dynamically adjusted to match the time after the go signal at which the participant is just able unable to inhibit their go response If the participant fails to inhibit their go response the stop signal is moved slightly closer to the original go signal and if the participant successfully inhibits their go response the stop signal is moved slightly ahead in time The SSRT is thus measured as the average go response time minus the average stop signal presentation time SSD Balloon Analogue Risk Task Edit The balloon analogue risk task BART was designed to assess risk taking behavior 145 Subjects are presented with a computer depiction of a balloon that can be incrementally inflated by pressing a response key As the balloon inflates the subject accumulates rewards with each new key press The balloon is programmed with a constant probability of popping If the balloon pops all rewards for that balloon are lost or the subject may choose to stop inflating and bank the reward for that balloon at any time Therefore more key presses equate to greater reward but also greater probability of popping and cancelling rewards for that trial The BART assumes that those with an affinity for risk taking are more likely to pop the balloon earning less reward overall than the typical population citation needed Iowa Gambling Task Edit The Iowa gambling task IGT is a test originally meant to measure decision making specifically within individuals who have ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage 146 The concept of impulsivity as relates to the IGT is one in which impulsive decisions are a function of an individual s lack of ability to make rational decisions over time due to an over amplification of emotional somatic reward 147 In the IGT individuals are provided four decks of cards to choose from Two of these decks provide much higher rewards but the deductions are also much higher while the second two decks have lower rewards per card but also much lower deductions Over time anyone who chooses predominantly from the high rewards decks will lose money while those who choose from the smaller rewards decks will gain money The IGT uses hot and cold processes in its concept of decision making 147 Hot decision making involves emotional responses to the material presented based on motivation related to reward and punishment Cold processes occur when an individual uses rational cognitive determinations when making decisions Combined an individual should gain a positive emotional reaction when choices have beneficial consequences and will have negative emotional responses tied to choices that have greater negative consequences In general healthy responders to the IGT will begin to drift to the lower gain decks as they realize that they are gaining more money than they lose both through an ability to recognize that one is more consistently providing rewards as well as through the emotions related to winning consistently However those who have emotional deficits will fail to recognize that they are losing money over time and will continue to be more influenced by the exhilaration of higher value rewards without being influenced by the negative emotions of the loses associated with them citation needed For more information concerning these process refer to the Somatic marker hypothesis Differential Reinforcement of Low Response Rate Task Edit Differential reinforcement of low response rate DRL described by Ferster and Skinner 148 is used to encourage low rates of responding It is derived from research in operant conditioning that provides an excellent opportunity to measure the hyperactive child s ability to inhibit behavioral responding Hyperactive children were relatively unable to perform efficiently on the task and this deficit endured regardless of age IQ or experimental condition 149 Therefore it can be used to discriminate accurately between teacher rated and parent rated hyperactive and nonhyperactive children In this procedure responses that occur before a set time interval has passed are not reinforced and reset the time required between behaviors citation needed In a study a child was taken to the experimental room and told that they were going to play a game in which they had a chance to win a lot of M amp M s Every time they made the light of the reward indicator by pressing a red button they would earn an M amp M s However they had to wait a while 6 seconds before they could press it to get another point If they had pressed the button too soon then they would have not gotten a point and the light would not go on and they had to wait a while before they could press it to get another point citation needed Researchers have also observed that subjects in a time based situation will often engage in a sequence or chain of behaviors between reinforceable responses 149 This is because this collateral behavior sequence helps the subject wait out the required temporal delay between responses citation needed Other Edit Other common impulsivity tasks include the Continuous performance task CPT 5 choice serial reaction time task 5 CSRTT Stroop task and Matching Familiar Figures Task Pharmacology and neurobiology EditNeurobiological findings Edit Although the precise neural mechanisms underlying disorders of impulse control are not fully known the prefrontal cortex PFC is the brain region most ubiquitously implicated in impulsivity 150 Damage to the prefrontal cortex has been associated with difficulties preparing to act switching between response alternatives and inhibiting inappropriate responses 144 Recent research has uncovered additional regions of interest as well as highlighted particular subregions of the PFC that can be tied to performance in specific behavioral tasks citation needed Delay discounting Edit Excitotoxic lesions in the nucleus accumbens core have been shown to increase preference for the smaller immediate reward whereas lesions to the nucleus accumbens shell have had no observable effect Additionally lesions of the basolateral amygdala a region tied closely to the PFC negatively affect impulsive choice similarly to what is observed in the nucleus accumbens core lesions 106 Moreover dorsal striatum may also be involved in impulsive choice in an intricate manner 151 Go No go and Stop signal reaction time test Edit The orbitofrontal cortex is now thought to play a role in disinhibiting 152 and injury to other brain structures such as to the right inferior frontal gyrus a specific subregion of the PFC has been associated with deficits in stop signal inhibition 153 5 Choice Serial Reaction Time Task 5 CSRTT and Differential Reinforcement of Low rates DRL Edit As with delay discounting lesion studies have implicated the core region of the nucleus accumbens in response inhibition for both DRL and 5 CSRTT Premature responses in the 5 CSRTT may also be modulated by other systems within the ventral striatum citation needed In the 5 CSRTT lesions of the anterior cingulate cortex have been shown to increase impulsive responding and lesions to the prelimbic cortex impair attentional performance 154 Iowa Gambling Task Edit Patients with damage to the ventromedial frontal cortex exhibit poor decision making and persist in making risky choices in the Iowa Gambling Task 146 155 Neurochemical and pharmacological findings Edit The primary pharmacological treatments for ADHD are methylphenidate Ritalin and amphetamine Both methylphenidate and amphetamines block re uptake of dopamine and norepinephrine into the pre synaptic neuron acting to increase post synaptic levels of dopamine and norepinephrine Of these two monoamines increased availability of dopamine is considered the primary cause for the ameliorative effects of ADHD medications whereas increased levels of norepinephrine may be efficacious only to the extent that it has downstream indirect effects on dopamine 156 The effectiveness of dopamine re uptake inhibitors in treating the symptoms of ADHD has led to the hypothesis that ADHD may arise from low tonic levels of dopamine particularly in the fronto limbic circuitry but evidence in support of this theory is mixed 157 158 Genetics EditThere are several difficulties when it comes to trying to identify a gene for complex traits such as impulsivity such as genetic heterogeneity Another difficulty is that the genes in question might sometimes show incomplete penetrance where a given gene variant does not always cause the phenotype 159 Much of the research on the genetics of impulsivity related disorders such as ADHD is based on family or linkage studies 160 There are several genes of interest that have been studied in an attempt to find the major genetic contributors to impulsivity Some of these genes are DAT1 is the dopamine transporter gene which is responsible for the active reuptake of dopamine from the neural synapse DAT1 polymorphisms have been shown to be linked to hyperactivity and ADHD 161 DRD4 is the dopamine D4 receptor gene and is associated with ADHD and novelty seeking behaviors 159 162 It has been proposed that novelty seeking is associated with impulsivity Mice deficient for DRD4 have shown less behavioral responses to novelty 163 5HT2A is the serotonin receptor gene The serotonin 2A receptor gene has been associated with both hyper locomotion ADHD as well as impulsivity Subjects with a particular polymorphism of the 5HT2A gene made more commission errors during a punishment reward condition in a go no go task 164 HTR2B a serotonin receptor gene 165 CTNNA2 encodes for a brain expressed a catenin that has been associated with Excitement Seeking in a genome wide association study GWAS of 7860 individuals 19 Intervention EditInterventions to impact impulsivity generally Edit While impulsivity can take on pathological forms e g substance use disorder ADHD there are less severe non clinical forms of problematic impulsivity in many people s daily lives Research on the different facets of impulsivity can inform small interventions to change decision making and reduce impulsive behavior 166 For example changing cognitive representations of rewards e g making long term rewards seem more concrete and or creating situations of precommitment eliminating the option of changing one s mind later can reduce the preference for immediate reward seen in delay discounting 166 Brain training Edit Brain training interventions include laboratory based interventions e g training using tasks like go no go as well as community family and school based interventions that are ecologically valid e g teaching techniques for regulating emotions or behaviors and can be used with individuals with non clinical levels of impulsivity 167 Both sorts of interventions are aimed at improving executive functioning and self control capacities with different interventions specifically targeting different aspects of executive functioning like inhibitory control working memory or attention 167 Emerging evidence suggests that brain training interventions may succeed in impacting executive function including inhibitory control 168 Inhibitory control training specifically is accumulating evidence that it can help individuals resist temptation to consume high calorie food 169 and drinking behavior 170 Some have voiced concerns that the favorable results of studies testing working memory training should be interpreted with caution claiming that conclusions regarding changes to abilities are measured using single tasks inconsistent use of working memory tasks no contact control groups and subjective measurements of change 171 Treatment of specific disorders of impulsivity Edit Behavioral psychosocial and psychopharmacological treatments for disorders involving impulsivity are common Psychopharmacological intervention Edit Psychopharmacological intervention in disorders of impulsivity has shown evidence of positive effects common pharmacological interventions include the use of stimulant medication selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors SSRIs and other antidepressants 78 ADHD has a well established evidence base supporting the use of stimulant medication for the reduction of ADHD symptoms 172 Pathological gambling has also been studied in drug trials and there is evidence that gambling is responsive to SSRIs and other antidepressants 78 Evidence based pharmacological treatment for trichotillomania is not yet available with mixed results of studies investigating the use of SSRIs though cognitive behavioral therapy has shown positive effects 78 Intermittent explosive disorder is most often treated with mood stabilizers SSRIs beta blockers alpha agonists and anti psychotics all of which have shown positive effects 78 There is evidence that some pharmacological interventions are efficacious in treating substance use disorders though their use can depend on the type of substance that is abused 50 Pharmacological treatments for substance use disorders include acamprosate buprenorphine disulfiram LAAM methadone and naltrexone 50 Behavioral interventions Edit Behavioral interventions also have a fairly strong evidence base in impulse control disorders 78 In ADHD the behavioral interventions of behavioral parent training behavioral classroom management and intensive peer focused behavioral interventions in recreational settings meet stringent guidelines qualifying them for evidence based treatment status 173 In addition a recent meta analysis of evidence based ADHD treatment found organization training to be a well established treatment method 174 Empirically validated behavioral treatments for substance use disorder are fairly similar across substance use disorders and include behavioral couples therapy CBT contingency management motivational enhancement therapy and relapse prevention 50 Pyromania and kleptomania are understudied due in large part to the illegality of the behaviors though there is some evidence that psychotherapeutic interventions CBT short term counseling day treatment programs are efficacious in treating pyromania while kleptomania seems to be best addressed using SSRIs 78 Additionally therapies including CBT family therapy and social skill training have shown positive effects on explosive aggressive behaviors 78 See also EditAffect ADHD Addiction Creativity Deferred gratification Drive theory Emotion Feeling Gaffe Instinct Impulse control disorder Sensation seeking Novelty seeking Alternative five model of personalityReferences Edit VandenBos G R 2007 APA dictionary of psychology Washington DC APA p 470 ISBN 9781591473800 a b c Daruna J H Barnes P A 1993 A neurodevelopmental view of impulsivity In McCown William George Johnson Judith L Shure Myrna B eds The Impulsive Client Theory Research and Treatment Washington DC American Psychological Association pp 23 37 doi 10 1037 10500 002 ISBN 978 1 55798 208 7 a b Madden Gregory J Johnson Patrick S 2010 A Delay Discounting Primer In Madden Gregory Jude Bickel Warren K eds Impulsivity The Behavioral and Neurological Science of Discounting Washington DC American Psychological Association pp 11 37 ISBN 978 1 4338 0477 9 Evenden J L 1999 Varieties of impulsivity Psychopharmacology 146 4 348 61 doi 10 1007 PL00005481 PMID 10550486 S2CID 5972342 Dickman Scott J 1990 Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity Personality and cognitive correlates Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 1 95 102 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 58 1 95 PMID 2308076 S2CID 34912306 Rachlin Howard 2000 Self Control as an Abstraction of Environmental Feedback The Science of Self Control Cambridge MA Harvard University Press p 183 ISBN 978 0 674 00093 3 Nigg Joel T 2001 Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder Psychological Bulletin 127 5 571 98 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 127 5 571 PMID 11548968 Lane Scott D Cherek Don R Rhoades Howard M Pietras Cynthia J Tcheremissine Oleg V 2003 Relationships Among Laboratory and Psychometric Measures of Impulsivity Implications in Substance Abuse and Dependence Addictive Disorders amp Their Treatment 2 2 33 40 doi 10 1097 00132576 200302020 00001 S2CID 143129215 Madden G J Petry N M Badger G J Bickel W K 1997 Impulsive and self control choices in opioid dependent patients and non drug using control patients Drug and monetary rewards Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology 5 3 256 62 doi 10 1037 1064 1297 5 3 256 PMID 9260073 a b Henry Chantal Mitropoulou Vivian New Antonia S Koenigsberg Harold W Silverman Jeremy Siever Larry J 2001 Affective instability and impulsivity in borderline personality and bipolar II disorders Similarities and differences Journal of Psychiatric Research 35 6 307 12 doi 10 1016 S0022 3956 01 00038 3 PMID 11684137 Horn N R Dolan M Elliott R Deakin J F W Woodruff P W R 2003 Response inhibition and impulsivity An fMRI study Neuropsychologia 41 14 1959 66 doi 10 1016 S0028 3932 03 00077 0 PMID 14572528 S2CID 31957781 Dixon Mark R Jacobs Eric A Sanders Scott Guercio John M Soldner James Parker Singler Susan Robinson Ashton Small Stacey Dillen Jeffrey E 2005 Impulsivity self control and delay discounting in persons with acquired brain injury Behavioral Interventions 20 1 101 20 doi 10 1002 bin 173 Gleichgerrcht Ezequiel Ibanez Agustin Roca Maria Torralva Teresa Manes Facundo 2010 Decision making cognition in neurodegenerative diseases Nature Reviews Neurology 6 11 611 23 doi 10 1038 nrneurol 2010 148 PMID 21045795 S2CID 13333018 Corsini Raymond Joseph 1999 The Dictionary of Psychology Psychology Press p 476 ISBN 1 58391 028 X Berlin H A Rolls E T Kischka U 2004 Impulsivity time perception emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions Brain 127 5 1108 26 doi 10 1093 brain awh135 PMID 14985269 Salmond C H Menon D K Chatfield D A Pickard J D Sahakian B J 2005 Deficits in Decision Making in Head Injury Survivors Journal of Neurotrauma 22 6 613 22 doi 10 1089 neu 2005 22 613 PMID 15941371 Hampton WH Venkatraman V Olson IR 2017 Dissociable frontostriatal white matter connectivity underlies reward and motor impulsivity NeuroImage 150 4 336 343 doi 10 1016 j neuroimage 2017 02 021 PMC 5494201 PMID 28189592 Whelan Robert Conrod Patricia J Poline Jean Baptiste Lourdusamy Anbarasu Banaschewski Tobias Barker Gareth J Bellgrove Mark A Buchel Christian et al 2012 Adolescent impulsivity phenotypes characterized by distinct brain networks Nature Neuroscience 15 6 920 5 doi 10 1038 nn 3092 PMID 22544311 S2CID 18808674 a b Terracciano A Esko T Sutin A R De Moor M H M Meirelles O Zhu G Tanaka T Giegling I et al 2011 Meta analysis of genome wide association studies identifies common variants in CTNNA2 associated with excitement seeking Translational Psychiatry 1 10 e49 doi 10 1038 tp 2011 42 PMC 3309493 PMID 22833195 a b Berlin Heather A Hollander Eric July 1 2008 Understanding the Differences Between Impulsivity and Compulsivity Psychiatric Times Psychiatric Times Vol 25 No 8 25 8 Archived from the original on December 3 2013 Oldham J M Hollander E Skodol A E 1996 Impulsivity and Compulsivity Washington D C American Psychiatric Press page needed Engel Scott G Corneliussen Stephanie J Wonderlich Stephen A Crosby Ross D Le Grange Daniel Crow Scott Klein Marjorie Bardone Cone Anna et al 2005 Impulsivity and compulsivity in bulimia nervosa International Journal of Eating Disorders 38 3 244 51 doi 10 1002 eat 20169 PMID 16211626 Andrade Leonardo F Alessi Sheila M Petry Nancy M 2013 The Effects of Alcohol Problems and Smoking on Delay Discounting in Individuals with Gambling Problems Journal of Psychoactive Drugs Informa UK Limited 45 3 241 248 doi 10 1080 02791072 2013 803645 ISSN 0279 1072 PMC 3816387 PMID 24175489 Acting on Impulse Psychology Today Retrieved 20 February 2016 Caleza Carolina Yanez Vico Rosa M Mendoza Asuncion Iglesias Linares Alejandro 2016 02 01 Childhood Obesity and Delayed Gratification Behavior A Systematic Review of Experimental Studies The Journal of Pediatrics 169 201 207 e1 doi 10 1016 j jpeds 2015 10 008 ISSN 0022 3476 PMID 26563536 Psychological Impulse Control Disorders Smith Gregory T Cyders Melissa A June 2016 Integrating affect and impulsivity The role of positive and negative urgency in substance use risk Drug and Alcohol Dependence 163 Suppl 1 S3 S12 doi 10 1016 j drugalcdep 2015 08 038 PMC 4911536 PMID 27306729 Fischer Sarah Smith Gregory T Anderson Kristen G May 2003 Clarifying the role of impulsivity in bulimia nervosa International Journal of Eating Disorders 33 4 406 411 doi 10 1002 eat 10165 PMID 12658670 Smith Gregory T Guller Leila Zapolski Tamika C B July 2013 A Comparison of Two Models of Urgency Urgency Predicts Both Rash Action and Depression in Youth Clinical Psychological Science 1 3 266 275 doi 10 1177 2167702612470647 PMC 4238932 PMID 25419495 Dvorak Robert D Pearson Matthew R Kuvaas Nicholas J May 2013 The Five Factor Model of Impulsivity Like Traits and Emotional Lability in Aggressive Behavior Impulsivity Emotional Lability and Aggression Aggressive Behavior 39 3 222 228 doi 10 1002 ab 21474 PMC 4625555 PMID 23471690 a b c Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders Vol 1 4th ed American Psychiatric Association 2000 doi 10 1176 appi books 9780890423349 ISBN 978 0 89042 334 9 page needed Skounti Maria Philalithis Anastas Galanakis Emmanouil 21 December 2006 Variations in prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder worldwide European Journal of Pediatrics 166 2 117 123 doi 10 1007 s00431 006 0299 5 PMID 17033803 S2CID 20342282 a b Froehlich Tanya E Lanphear BP Epstein JN Barbaresi WJ Katusic SK Kahn RS 2007 Prevalence Recognition and Treatment of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in a National Sample of US Children Archives of Pediatrics amp Adolescent Medicine 161 9 857 64 doi 10 1001 archpedi 161 9 857 PMID 17768285 Solanto Mary V Abikoff Howard Sonuga Barke Edmund Schachar Russell Logan Gordon D Wigal Tim Hechtman Lily Hinshaw Stephen Turkel Elihu 2001 The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD HD A supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study of AD HD Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 29 3 215 28 doi 10 1023 A 1010329714819 PMID 11411784 S2CID 23953111 Pelham William E Foster E Michael Robb Jessica A 2007 The Economic Impact of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Children and Adolescents Journal of Pediatric Psychology 32 6 711 27 doi 10 1093 jpepsy jsm022 PMID 17556402 Rosler M Retz W Retz Junginger P Hengesch G Schneider M Supprian T Schwitzgebel P Pinhard K et al 2004 Prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ADHD and comorbid disorders in young male prison inmates European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 254 6 365 71 doi 10 1007 s00406 004 0516 z PMID 15538605 S2CID 8737929 Kessler R C Adler L Barkley R Biederman J Conners C K Demler O Faraone S V Greenhill L L et al 2006 The Prevalence and Correlates of Adult ADHD in the United States Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication American Journal of Psychiatry 163 4 716 23 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 163 4 716 PMC 2859678 PMID 16585449 Wilens Timothy E Biederman Joseph Brown Sarah Tanguay Sarah Monuteaux Michael C Blake Christie Spencer Thomas J 2002 Psychiatric Comorbidity and Functioning in Clinically Referred Preschool Children and School Age Youths with ADHD Journal of the American Academy of Child amp Adolescent Psychiatry 41 3 262 8 doi 10 1097 00004583 200203000 00005 PMID 11886020 Scahill L Schwab Stone M 2000 Epidemiology of ADHD in school age children Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America 9 3 541 55 vii doi 10 1016 S1056 4993 18 30106 8 PMID 10944656 a b Aron Adam R Poldrack Russell A 2005 The Cognitive Neuroscience of Response Inhibition Relevance for Genetic Research in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Biological Psychiatry 57 11 1285 92 doi 10 1016 j biopsych 2004 10 026 PMID 15950000 S2CID 41786909 Ridderinkhof K Richard Van Den Wildenberg Wery P M Segalowitz Sidney J Carter Cameron S 2004 Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control The role of prefrontal cortex in action selection response inhibition performance monitoring and reward based learning Brain and Cognition 56 2 129 40 doi 10 1016 j bandc 2004 09 016 PMID 15518930 S2CID 16820592 Casey B J Castellanos F Xavier Giedd Jay N Marsh Wendy L Hamburger Susan D Schubert Anne B Vauss Yolanda C Vaituzis A Catherine et al 1997 Implication of Right Frontostriatal Circuitry in Response Inhibition and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Journal of the American Academy of Child amp Adolescent Psychiatry 36 3 374 83 doi 10 1097 00004583 199703000 00016 PMID 9055518 DeVries Melissa 2010 12 09 Book Review Pliszka S R UTHSCSA Treating ADHD and Comorbid Disorders Psychosocial and Psychopharmacological Interventions New York Guilford 2008 Journal of Attention Disorders 15 1 92 93 doi 10 1177 1087054710362216 ISSN 1087 0547 S2CID 143753779 a b c d e f Perry Jennifer L Carroll Marilyn E 2008 The role of impulsive behavior in drug abuse Psychopharmacology 200 1 1 26 doi 10 1007 s00213 008 1173 0 PMID 18600315 S2CID 24587409 De Wit Harriet 2009 Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use A review of underlying processes Addiction Biology 14 1 22 31 doi 10 1111 j 1369 1600 2008 00129 x PMC 3640851 PMID 18855805 De Wit H Richards JB 2004 Dual determinants of drug use in humans Reward and impulsivity Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 50 19 55 PMID 15160637 Kollins Scott H 2003 Delay discounting is associated with substance use in college students Addictive Behaviors 28 6 1167 73 doi 10 1016 S0306 4603 02 00220 4 PMID 12834659 a b Moeller F Gerard Dougherty Donald M Barratt Ernest S Schmitz Joy M Swann Alan C Grabowski John 2001 The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention in treatment Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment 21 4 193 8 doi 10 1016 S0740 5472 01 00202 1 PMID 11777668 Doran Neal Spring Bonnie McChargue Dennis 2007 Effect of impulsivity on craving and behavioral reactivity to smoking cues Psychopharmacology 194 2 279 88 doi 10 1007 s00213 007 0832 x PMID 17594080 S2CID 35948752 a b c d e McGovern Mark P Carroll Kathleen M 2003 Evidence based practices for substance use disorders Psychiatric Clinics of North America 26 4 991 1010 doi 10 1016 S0193 953X 03 00073 X PMC 3678283 PMID 14711132 Vohs K D Heatherton T F 2000 Self Regulatory Failure A Resource Depletion Approach Psychological Science 11 3 249 54 doi 10 1111 1467 9280 00250 PMID 11273412 S2CID 18307595 Honkanen Pirjo Olsen Svein Ottar Verplanken Bas Tuu Ho Huy 2012 Reflective and impulsive influences on unhealthy snacking The moderating effects of food related self control Appetite 58 2 616 22 doi 10 1016 j appet 2011 11 019 PMID 22138115 S2CID 39135358 Tice Dianne M Bratslavsky Ellen Baumeister Roy F 2001 Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control If you feel bad do it Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80 1 53 67 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 80 1 53 PMID 11195891 S2CID 10596913 Hofmann Wilhelm Friese Malte Roefs Anne 2009 Three ways to resist temptation The independent contributions of executive attention inhibitory control and affect regulation to the impulse control of eating behavior Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 45 2 431 5 doi 10 1016 j jesp 2008 09 013 S2CID 51986961 Heatherton Todd F Baumeister Roy F 1991 Binge eating as escape from self awareness Psychological Bulletin 110 1 86 108 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 110 1 86 PMID 1891520 S2CID 1232881 a b Rosval Lindsay Steiger Howard Bruce Kenneth Israel Mimi Richardson Jodie Aubut Melanie 2006 Impulsivity in women with eating disorders Problem of response inhibition planning or attention International Journal of Eating Disorders 39 7 590 3 doi 10 1002 eat 20296 PMID 16826575 a b c Vohs Kathleen D Faber Ronald J 2007 Spent Resources Self Regulatory Resource Availability Affects Impulse Buying Journal of Consumer Research 33 4 537 47 doi 10 1086 510228 Kuikka Jyrki Tammela Liisa Karhunen Leila Rissanen Aila Bergstrom Kim Naukkarinen Hannu Vanninen Esko Karhu Jari et al 2001 Reduced serotonin transporter binding in binge eating women Psychopharmacology 155 3 310 4 doi 10 1007 s002130100716 PMID 11432694 S2CID 7812191 Loxton N J Dawe S 2004 Alcohol abuse and dysfunctional eating in women The contribution of reward and punishment sensitivity and family of origin In press verification needed As cited in Dawe Sharon Loxton Natalie J 2004 The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders Neuroscience amp Biobehavioral Reviews 28 3 343 51 doi 10 1016 j neubiorev 2004 03 007 PMID 15225976 S2CID 24435589 Agras W Stewart Bohon Cara 2021 05 07 Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for the Eating Disorders Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 17 1 417 438 doi 10 1146 annurev clinpsy 081219 110907 ISSN 1548 5943 PMID 33962536 S2CID 233998712 Beatty Sharon E Ferrell M Elizabeth 1998 Impulse buying Modeling its precursors Journal of Retailing 74 2 169 91 doi 10 1016 S0022 4359 99 80092 X Smith D 1996 The joy of candy National Petroleum News Supplement S2 a b Mischel Walter Ebbesen Ebbe B Raskoff Zeiss Antonette 1972 Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 21 2 204 18 doi 10 1037 h0032198 PMID 5010404 Strack Fritz Werth Lioba Deutsch Roland 2006 Reflective and Impulsive Determinants of Consumer Behavior Journal of Consumer Psychology 16 3 205 16 doi 10 1207 s15327663jcp1603 2 a b c d Kwon Harry H Armstrong Ketra L 2006 Impulse Purchases of Sport Team Licensed Merchandise What Matters Journal of Sport Management 20 1 101 119 doi 10 1123 jsm 20 1 101 Rook Dennis W 1987 The Buying Impulse Journal of Consumer Research 14 2 189 99 doi 10 1086 209105 JSTOR 2489410 Hollander E Stein D 2006 Impulse control disorders Arlington American Psychiatric Publishing page needed Bernik MA Akerman D Amaral JA Braun RC 1996 Cue exposure in compulsive buying The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 57 2 90 PMID 8591975 a b Catalano EM Sonenberg N 1993 Consuming passions help for compulsive shoppers Oakland NewHarbinger Publications page needed Black DW Monahan P Gabel J 1997 Fluvoxamine in the treatment of compulsive buying The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 58 4 159 63 doi 10 4088 JCP v58n0404 PMID 9164426 Black Donald Gabel Janelle Hansen Jeffrey Schlosser Steven 2000 A Double Blind Comparison of Fluvoxamine Versus Placebo in the Treatment of Compulsive Buying Disorder Annals of Clinical Psychiatry 12 4 205 11 doi 10 1023 A 1009030425631 PMID 11140921 S2CID 189937538 Koran Lorrin M Bullock Kim D Hartston Heidi J Elliott Michael A d Andrea Vincent 2002 Citalopram Treatment of Compulsive Shopping The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 63 8 704 8 doi 10 4088 JCP v63n0808 PMID 12197451 Guy LS 1976 ECDEU assessment manual for psychopharmacology Publication ADM 76 338 Washington DC Department of Health Education and Welfare page needed Koran Lorrin M Aboujaoude Elias N Solvason Brent Gamel Nona N Smith Emily H 2007 Escitalopram for Compulsive Buying Disorder Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 27 2 225 7 doi 10 1097 01 jcp 0000264975 79367 f4 PMID 17414257 S2CID 41883042 Kim Suck Won Dwight MM McElroy SL 1998 Opioid Antagonists in the Treatment of Impulse Control Disorders The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 59 4 165 71 doi 10 4088 JCP v59n0403 PMID 9590666 S2CID 7161577 Grant Jon E 2003 Three cases of compulsive buying treated with naltrexone International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice 7 3 223 5 doi 10 1080 13651500310003219 Hollander E Stein D J eds 2005 Clinical manual of impulse control disorders Arlington VA American Psychiatric Press pp 4 8 a b c d e f g h Dell Osso Bernardo Altamura A Carlo Allen Andrea Marazziti Donatella Hollander Eric 2006 Epidemiologic and clinical updates on impulse control disorders A critical review European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 256 8 464 75 doi 10 1007 s00406 006 0668 0 PMC 1705499 PMID 16960655 a b c d DSM 5 www dsm5 org Archived from the original on January 30 2013 Grant J Odlaug B Wozniak J 2007 Neuropsychological functioning in kleptomania Behaviour Research and Therapy 45 7 1663 70 doi 10 1016 j brat 2006 08 013 PMID 17007811 Fineberg Naomi A Potenza Marc N Chamberlain Samuel R Berlin Heather A Menzies Lara Bechara Antoine Sahakian Barbara J Robbins Trevor W et al 2009 Probing Compulsive and Impulsive Behaviors from Animal Models to Endophenotypes A Narrative Review Neuropsychopharmacology 35 3 591 604 doi 10 1038 npp 2009 185 PMC 3055606 PMID 19940844 Odlaug Brian L Chamberlain Samuel R Grant Jon E 2010 Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in pathologic skin picking Progress in Neuro Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 34 1 208 11 doi 10 1016 j pnpbp 2009 11 008 PMID 19913592 S2CID 207409063 a b c d e Leeman Robert F Potenza Marc N 2011 Similarities and differences between pathological gambling and substance use disorders A focus on impulsivity and compulsivity Psychopharmacology 219 2 469 90 doi 10 1007 s00213 011 2550 7 PMC 3249521 PMID 22057662 a b Coccaro Emil F 2012 Intermittent Explosive Disorder as a Disorder of Impulsive Aggression for DSM 5 American Journal of Psychiatry 169 6 577 88 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 2012 11081259 PMID 22535310 a b c d e f g h Baumeister Roy F Vohs Kathleen D Tice Dianne M 2007 The Strength Model of Self Control Current Directions in Psychological Science 16 6 351 5 doi 10 1111 j 1467 8721 2007 00534 x S2CID 7414142 a b c Baumeister Roy F Bratslavsky Ellen Muraven Mark Tice Dianne M 1998 Ego depletion Is the active self a limited resource Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 5 1252 65 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 5 1252 PMID 9599441 a b Muraven Mark Baumeister Roy F 2000 Self regulation and depletion of limited resources Does self control resemble a muscle Psychological Bulletin 126 2 247 59 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 126 2 247 PMID 10748642 S2CID 10962693 a b Hagger Martin S Wood Chantelle Stiff Chris Chatzisarantis Nikos L D 2010 Ego depletion and the strength model of self control A meta analysis Psychological Bulletin 136 4 495 525 doi 10 1037 a0019486 PMID 20565167 S2CID 22705578 Baumeister Roy F Gailliot Matthew Dewall C Nathan Oaten Megan 2006 Self Regulation and Personality How Interventions Increase Regulatory Success and How Depletion Moderates the Effects of Traits on Behavior Journal of Personality 74 6 1773 801 doi 10 1111 j 1467 6494 2006 00428 x PMID 17083666 a b Muraven Mark Shmueli Dikla Burkley Edward 2006 Conserving self control strength Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91 3 524 37 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 91 3 524 PMID 16938035 Muraven Mark Slessareva Elisaveta 2003 Mechanisms of Self Control Failure Motivation and Limited Resources Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 29 7 894 906 doi 10 1177 0146167203029007008 PMID 15018677 S2CID 16961238 Muraven Mark Tice Dianne M Baumeister Roy F 1998 Self control as a limited resource Regulatory depletion patterns Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74 3 774 89 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 74 3 774 PMID 9523419 Finkel Eli J Campbell W Keith Brunell Amy B Dalton Amy N Scarbeck Sarah J Chartrand Tanya L 2006 High maintenance interaction Inefficient social coordination impairs self regulation Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91 3 456 75 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 91 3 456 PMID 16938030 a b c Muraven Mark 2012 Ego Depletion Theory and Evidence In Ryan Richard M ed The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation Oxford Oxford University Press pp 111 26 ISBN 978 0 19 539982 0 Wallace Harry M Baumeister Roy F 2002 The Effects of Success versus Failure Feedback on Further Self Control Self and Identity 1 35 41 doi 10 1080 152988602317232786 S2CID 144797743 Amsel Eric Close Jason Sadler Eric Klaczynski Paul A 2009 College Students Awareness of Irrational Judgments on Gambling Tasks A Dual Process Account The Journal of Psychology 143 3 293 317 doi 10 3200 JRLP 143 3 293 317 PMID 19455857 S2CID 1997168 a b c d Gawronski Bertram Creighton Laura A 2013 Dual Process Theories In Carlston Donal E ed The Oxford Handbook of Social Cognition pp 218 312 ISBN 978 0 19 973001 8 Sherman Jeffrey W Gawronski Bertram Gonsalkorale Karen Hugenberg Kurt Allen Thomas J Groom Carla J 2008 The self regulation of automatic associations and behavioral impulses Psychological Review 115 2 314 35 doi 10 1037 0033 295X 115 2 314 PMID 18426292 S2CID 20186915 Kirkpatrick Lee A Epstein Seymour 1992 Cognitive experiential self theory and subjective probability Further evidence for two conceptual systems Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 63 4 534 44 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 63 4 534 PMID 1447684 a b c d Berns Gregory S Laibson David Loewenstein George 2007 Intertemporal choice toward an integrative framework Trends in Cognitive Sciences 11 11 482 8 doi 10 1016 j tics 2007 08 011 PMID 17980645 S2CID 22282339 Hampton W 2018 Things for Those Who Wait Predictive Modeling Highlights Importance of Delay Discounting for Income Attainment Frontiers in Psychology 9 1545 1545 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2018 01545 PMC 6129952 PMID 30233449 MacKillop J 2011 Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior Psychopharmacology 216 3 305 321 doi 10 1007 s00213 011 2229 0 PMC 3201846 PMID 21373791 Odum Amy L Baumann Ana A L 2010 Delay discounting State and trait variable In Madden Gregory J Bickel Warren K eds Impulsivity The behavioral and neurological science of discounting pp 39 65 doi 10 1037 12069 002 ISBN 978 1 4338 0477 9 Cardinal R N Pennicott DR Sugathapala CL Robbins TW Everitt BJ 2001 Impulsive Choice Induced in Rats by Lesions of the Nucleus Accumbens Core Science 292 5526 2499 501 doi 10 1126 science 1060818 PMID 11375482 S2CID 18020488 Winstanley C A Theobald DE Cardinal RN Robbins TW 2004 Contrasting Roles of Basolateral Amygdala and Orbitofrontal Cortex in Impulsive Choice Journal of Neuroscience 24 20 4718 22 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 5606 03 2004 PMC 6729470 PMID 15152031 a b Dalley Jeffrey W Everitt Barry J Robbins Trevor W 2011 Impulsivity Compulsivity and Top Down Cognitive Control Neuron 69 4 680 94 doi 10 1016 j neuron 2011 01 020 PMID 21338879 S2CID 7952439 McClure S M Laibson DI Loewenstein G Cohen JD 2004 Separate Neural Systems Value Immediate and Delayed Monetary Rewards Science 306 5695 503 7 Bibcode 2004Sci 306 503M doi 10 1126 science 1100907 PMID 15486304 S2CID 14663380 Herrnstein Richard J Prelec Drazen 1992 A Theory of Addiction In Loewenstein George Elster Jon eds Choice Over Time New York Russell Sage Foundation pp 331 60 ISBN 978 0 87154 558 9 Rachlin Howard 2000 Complex Ambivalence The Science of Self Control Cambridge Massachusetts Harvard University Press pp 57 81 ISBN 978 0 674 00093 3 Ainslie George Herrnstein R J 1981 Preference reversal and delayed reinforcement Animal Learning amp Behavior 9 4 476 82 doi 10 3758 BF03209777 S2CID 145693802 Green Leonard Estle Sara J 2003 Preference reversals with food and water reinforcers in rats Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior 79 2 233 42 doi 10 1901 jeab 2003 79 233 PMC 1284932 PMID 12822689 Hauser Marc D Hallinan Elizabeth V Stevens Jeffrey R 2005 The ecology and evolution of patience in two New World monkeys Biology Letters 1 2 223 6 doi 10 1098 rsbl 2004 0285 PMC 1626214 PMID 17148172 a b Logan Gordon D Schachar Russell J Tannock Rosemary 1997 Impulsivity and Inhibitory Control Psychological Science 8 1 60 4 doi 10 1111 j 1467 9280 1997 tb00545 x JSTOR 40062847 S2CID 145540424 Enticott Peter G Ogloff James R P Bradshaw John L 2006 Associations between laboratory measures of executive inhibitory control and self reported impulsivity Personality and Individual Differences 41 2 285 94 doi 10 1016 j paid 2006 01 011 a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q r Nigg Joel T 2000 On inhibition disinhibition in developmental psychopathology Views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy Psychological Bulletin 126 2 220 46 doi 10 1037 0033 2909 126 2 220 PMID 10748641 Gratton Gabriele Coles Michael G H Donchin Emanuel 1992 Optimizing the use of information Strategic control of activation of responses Journal of Experimental Psychology General 121 4 480 506 doi 10 1037 0096 3445 121 4 480 PMID 1431740 Diamond A May 1997 Executive functions Developmental perspective Executive Function and Developmental Psychopathology Toronto Canada University of Toronto Division of Child Psychiatry Cabeza Roberto Nyberg Lars 1997 Imaging Cognition An Empirical Review of PET Studies with Normal Subjects Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 9 1 1 26 doi 10 1162 jocn 1997 9 1 1 PMID 23968177 S2CID 33664797 Logan Gordon D Cowan William B Davis Kenneth A 1984 On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses A model and a method Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception and Performance 10 2 276 91 doi 10 1037 0096 1523 10 2 276 PMID 6232345 S2CID 2595498 Albarracin D Handley I M Noguchi K McCulloch K C Li H Leeper J Brown R D Earl A Hart W 2008 Increasing and decreasing motor and cognitive output A model of general action and inaction goals Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 95 3 510 523 doi 10 1037 a0012833 PMC 3626413 PMID 18729691 a b Barratt Ernest S 1959 Anxiety and Impulsiveness Related to Psychomotor Efficiency Perceptual and Motor Skills 9 3 191 8 doi 10 2466 pms 1959 9 3 191 S2CID 143118554 a b Patton Jim H Stanford Matthew S Barratt Ernest S 1995 Factor structure of the barratt impulsiveness scale Journal of Clinical Psychology 51 6 768 74 doi 10 1002 1097 4679 199511 51 6 lt 768 AID JCLP2270510607 gt 3 0 CO 2 1 PMID 8778124 Eysenck Sybil B G Pearson P R Easting G Allsopp J F 1985 Age norms for impulsiveness venturesomeness and empathy in adults Personality and Individual Differences 6 5 613 9 doi 10 1016 0191 8869 85 90011 X a b c d Dean Chelsea R 2006 Assessing self injurious behaviors on a College Campus Senior Honors Theses amp Projects Senior Honor Theses Paper 59 Archived from the original on 2013 12 03 page needed a b Burnett Heyes S Adam RJ Urner M van der Leer L Bahrami B Bays PM Husain M Adam Urner Van Der Leer Bahrami Bays Husain 22 May 2012 Impulsivity and Rapid Decision Making for Reward Front Psychol 3 153 doi 10 3389 fpsyg 2012 00153 PMC 3357492 PMID 22661960 a href Template Cite journal html title Template Cite journal cite journal a CS1 maint multiple names authors list link a b Dickman S J 1990 Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity Personality and cognitive correlates Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 58 1 95 102 Brunas Wagstaff J Tilley A Verity M Ford S amp Thompson D 1997 Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity in children and their relationship to Eysenck s impulsiveness and venturesomeness dimensions Personality and Individual Differences 22 1 19 35 Whiteside Stephen P Lynam Donald R 2001 The Five Factor Model and impulsivity Using a structural model of personality to understand impulsivity Personality and Individual Differences 30 4 669 89 doi 10 1016 S0191 8869 00 00064 7 Cyders Melissa A Smith Gregory T Spillane Nichea S Fischer Sarah Annus Agnes M Peterson Claire 2007 Integration of impulsivity and positive mood to predict risky behavior Development and validation of a measure of positive urgency Psychological Assessment 19 1 107 18 doi 10 1037 1040 3590 19 1 107 PMID 17371126 Billieux Joel Rochat Lucien Ceschi Grazia Carre Arnaud Offerlin Meyer Isabelle Defeldre Anne Catherine Khazaal Yasser Besche Richard Chrystel Van Der Linden Martial 2012 Validation of a short French version of the UPPS P Impulsive Behavior Scale Comprehensive Psychiatry 53 5 609 15 doi 10 1016 j comppsych 2011 09 001 PMID 22036009 Smith G T Fischer S Cyders M A Annus A M Spillane N S 2007 Toward a comprehensive model of multiple dispositions to rash action Annual International Meeting of the Research Society on Alcoholism Chicago IL Schmidt Catherine A 2000 Development and Validation of the Lifetime History of Impulsive Behaviors Interview and Self Report Measures PhD Thesis OCLC 48501893 page needed a b McCloskey Michael S New Antonia S Siever Larry J Goodman Marianne Koenigsberg Harold W Flory Janine D Coccaro Emil F 2009 Evaluation of behavioral impulsivity and aggression tasks as endophenotypes for borderline personality disorder Journal of Psychiatric Research 43 12 1036 48 doi 10 1016 j jpsychires 2009 01 002 PMC 2853811 PMID 19232640 Carver Charles S White Teri L 1994 Behavioral inhibition behavioral activation and affective responses to impending reward and punishment The BIS BAS Scales Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67 2 319 333 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 67 2 319 S2CID 6452203 a b c d e Stanford Matthew S Houston Rebecca J Mathias Charles W Villemarette Pittman Nicole R Helfritz Laura E Conklin Sarah M 2003 Characterizing Aggressive Behavior Assessment 10 2 183 90 doi 10 1177 1073191103010002009 PMID 12801190 S2CID 28427132 Mathias Charles W Stanford Matthew S Marsh Dawn M Frick Paul J Moeller F Gerard Swann Alan C Dougherty Donald M June 2007 Characterizing aggressive behavior with the Impulsive Premeditated Aggression Scale among adolescents with conduct disorder Psychiatry Research 151 3 231 242 doi 10 1016 j psychres 2006 11 001 PMC 1994790 PMID 17383014 Burns G L 1995 Padua Inventory Washington State University Revision Archived from the original on 2018 06 05 Mischel Walter Ebbesen Ebbe B Raskoff Zeiss Antonette 1972 Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 21 2 204 218 doi 10 1037 h0032198 ISSN 1939 1315 PMID 5010404 Mischel Walter Shoda Yuichi Peake Philip K 1988 The nature of adolescent competencies predicted by preschool delay of gratification Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54 4 687 96 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 54 4 687 PMID 3367285 S2CID 42623330 Shoda Yuichi Mischel Walter Peake Philip K 1990 Predicting adolescent cognitive and self regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification Identifying diagnostic conditions Developmental Psychology 26 6 978 86 doi 10 1037 0012 1649 26 6 978 S2CID 4259680 Ayduk Ozlem Mendoza Denton Rodolfo Mischel Walter Downey Geraldine Peake Philip K Rodriguez Monica 2000 Regulating the interpersonal self Strategic self regulation for coping with rejection sensitivity Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 5 776 92 doi 10 1037 0022 3514 79 5 776 PMID 11079241 S2CID 6345213 Mazur James E 1987 An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement In Commons Michael L Mazur James E Nevin John Anthony et al eds The effect of delay and of intervening events on reinforcement value Quantitative analyses of behavior Vol 5 Hillsdale NJ Erlbaum pp 55 73 ISBN 9781317838074 Winstanley Catharine A Dalley Jeffrey W Theobald David E H Robbins Trevor W November 2003 Global 5 HT depletion attenuates the ability of amphetamine to decrease impulsive choice on a delay discounting task in rats Psychopharmacology 170 3 320 331 doi 10 1007 s00213 003 1546 3 PMID 12955303 S2CID 143605377 a b Winstanley Catharine A Eagle Dawn M Robbins Trevor W 2006 Behavioral models of impulsivity in relation to ADHD Translation between clinical and preclinical studies Clinical Psychology Review 26 4 379 95 doi 10 1016 j cpr 2006 01 001 PMC 1892795 PMID 16504359 Lejuez C W Read Jennifer P Kahler Christopher W Richards Jerry B Ramsey Susan E Stuart Gregory L Strong David R Brown Richard A 2002 Evaluation of a behavioral measure of risk taking The Balloon Analogue Risk Task BART Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 8 2 75 84 doi 10 1037 1076 898X 8 2 75 PMID 12075692 S2CID 7523498 a b Bechara Antoine Damasio Antonio R Damasio Hanna Anderson Steven W 1994 Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex Cognition 50 1 3 7 15 doi 10 1016 0010 0277 94 90018 3 PMID 8039375 S2CID 204981454 a b Buelow Melissa T Suhr Julie A 2009 Construct Validity of the Iowa Gambling Task Neuropsychology Review 19 1 102 14 doi 10 1007 s11065 009 9083 4 PMID 19194801 S2CID 8248057 Ferster C B Skinner B F Schedules of reinforcement New York 1957 page needed a b Gordon Michael 1979 The assessment of impulsivity and mediating behaviors in hyperactive and nonhyperactive boys Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 7 3 317 26 doi 10 1007 BF00916541 PMID 489853 S2CID 31953219 Castellanos F Xavier Tannock Rosemary 2002 Neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder The search for endophenotypes Nature Reviews Neuroscience 3 8 617 28 doi 10 1038 nrn896 PMID 12154363 S2CID 3346437 Kim BaekSun Im Heh In 2018 The role of the dorsal striatum in choice impulsivity Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1451 1 92 111 doi 10 1111 nyas 13961 PMID 30277562 S2CID 52897511 Berlin H A Rolls ET Kischka U 2004 Impulsivity time perception emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions Brain 127 5 1108 26 doi 10 1093 brain awh135 PMID 14985269 Aron Adam R Fletcher Paul C Bullmore Ed T Sahakian Barbara J Robbins Trevor W 2003 Stop signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans Nature Neuroscience 6 2 115 6 doi 10 1038 nn1003 PMID 12536210 S2CID 10096947 Muir Janice L Everitt Barry J Robbins Trevor W 1996 The Cerebral Cortex of the Rat and Visual Attentional Function Dissociable Effects of Mediofrontal Cingulate Anterior Dorsolateral and Parietal Cortex Lesions on a Five Choice Serial Reaction Time Task Cerebral Cortex 6 3 470 81 doi 10 1093 cercor 6 3 470 PMID 8670672 Bechara Antoine Damasio Hanna Damasio Antonio R Lee Gregory P 1999 Different Contributions of the Human Amygdala and Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex to Decision Making The Journal of Neuroscience 19 13 5473 81 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 19 13 05473 1999 PMC 6782338 PMID 10377356 Wilens Timothy E 2006 Mechanism of Action of Agents Used in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67 Suppl 8 32 8 PMID 16961428 Archived from the original on 2013 06 28 Dougherty Darin D Bonab Ali A Spencer Thomas J Rauch Scott L Madras Bertha K Fischman Alan J 1999 Dopamine transporter density in patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder The Lancet 354 9196 2132 3 doi 10 1016 S0140 6736 99 04030 1 PMID 10609822 S2CID 7420578 Volkow Nora D Wang GJ Kollins SH Wigal TL Newcorn JH Telang F Fowler JS Zhu W et al 2009 Evaluating Dopamine Reward Pathway in ADHD Clinical Implications JAMA 302 10 1084 91 doi 10 1001 jama 2009 1308 PMC 2958516 PMID 19738093 a b Wallis D Russell H F Muenke M 2008 Review Genetics of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Journal of Pediatric Psychology 33 10 1085 99 doi 10 1093 jpepsy jsn049 PMID 18522996 Kempf Lucas Weinberger Daniel R 2009 Molecular Genetics and Bioinformatics An Outline for Neuropsychological Genetics In Goldberg Terry E Weinberger Daniel R eds The Genetics of Cognitive Neuroscience pp 3 26 ISBN 978 0 262 01307 9 Brookes K Xu X Chen W Zhou K Neale B Lowe N Aneey R Franke B et al 2006 The analysis of 51 genes in DSM IV combined type attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Association signals in DRD4 DAT1 and 16 other genes Molecular Psychiatry 11 10 934 53 doi 10 1038 sj mp 4001869 PMID 16894395 S2CID 6266643 Ptacek Radek Kuzelova Hana Stefano George B 2011 Dopamine D4 receptor gene DRD4 and its association with psychiatric disorders Medical Science Monitor 17 9 RA215 20 doi 10 12659 MSM 881925 PMC 3560519 PMID 21873960 Dulawa Stephanie C Grandy David K Low Malcolm J Paulus Martin P Geye Mark A 1999 Dopamine D4 Receptor Knock Out Mice Exhibit Reduced Exploration of Novel Stimuli The Journal of Neuroscience 19 21 9550 6 doi 10 1523 JNEUROSCI 19 21 09550 1999 PMC 6782928 PMID 10531457 Nomura Michio Kusumi Ichiro Kaneko Masayuki Masui Takuya Daiguji Makoto Ueno Takeji Koyama Tsukasa Nomura Yasuyuki 2006 Involvement of a polymorphism in the 5 HT2A receptor gene in impulsive behavior Psychopharmacology 187 1 30 5 doi 10 1007 s00213 006 0398 z PMID 16767413 S2CID 12155339 Bevilacqua Laura Doly Stephane Kaprio Jaakko Yuan Qiaoping Tikkanen Roope Paunio Tiina Zhou Zhifeng Wedenoja Juho et al 2010 A population specific HTR2B stop codon predisposes to severe impulsivity Nature 468 7327 1061 6 Bibcode 2010Natur 468 1061B doi 10 1038 nature09629 PMC 3183507 PMID 21179162 a b Ratner Rebecca K Soman Dilip Zauberman Gal Ariely Dan Carmon Ziv Keller Punam A Kim B Kyu Lin Fern et al 2008 How behavioral decision research can enhance consumer welfare From freedom of choice to paternalistic intervention Marketing Letters 19 3 4 383 397 doi 10 1007 s11002 008 9044 3 S2CID 5880506 a b Berkman Elliot T Graham Alice M Fisher Philip A 2012 Training Self Control A Domain General Translational Neuroscience Approach Child Development Perspectives 6 4 374 384 doi 10 1111 j 1750 8606 2012 00248 x PMC 3722070 PMID 23894251 Hofmann Wilhelm Schmeichel Brandon J Baddeley Alan D 2012 Executive functions and self regulation Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 3 174 80 doi 10 1016 j tics 2012 01 006 PMID 22336729 S2CID 13309571 Houben Katrijn Jansen Anita 2011 Training inhibitory control A recipe for resisting sweet temptations Appetite 56 2 345 9 doi 10 1016 j appet 2010 12 017 PMID 21185896 S2CID 10664026 Houben Katrijn Nederkoorn Chantal Wiers Reinout W Jansen Anita 2011 Resisting temptation Decreasing alcohol related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition PDF Drug and Alcohol Dependence 116 1 3 132 6 doi 10 1016 j drugalcdep 2010 12 011 PMID 21288663 Shipstead Zach Redick Thomas S Engle Randall W 2012 Is working memory training effective Psychological Bulletin 138 4 628 54 doi 10 1037 a0027473 PMID 22409508 S2CID 14152894 Swanson James M Kraemer Helena C Hinshaw Stephen P Arnold L Eugene Conners C Keith Abikoff Howard B Clevenger Walter Davies Mark et al 2001 Clinical Relevance of the Primary Findings of the MTA Success Rates Based on Severity of ADHD and ODD Symptoms at the End of Treatment Journal of the American Academy of Child amp Adolescent Psychiatry 40 2 168 79 doi 10 1097 00004583 200102000 00011 PMID 11211365 S2CID 4638728 Pelham William E Fabiano Gregory A 2008 Evidence Based Psychosocial Treatments for Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Journal of Clinical Child amp Adolescent Psychology 37 1 184 214 doi 10 1080 15374410701818681 PMID 18444058 S2CID 829396 Evans SW Owens JS Bunford N 2014 Evidence based psychosocial treatments for children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology 43 4 527 51 doi 10 1080 15374416 2013 850700 PMC 4025987 PMID 24245813 Further reading EditEvenden J L 21 October 1999 Varieties of impulsivity Psychopharmacology 146 4 348 361 doi 10 1007 PL00005481 PMID 10550486 S2CID 5972342 Hollander E Rosen J March 2000 Impulsivity Journal of Psychopharmacology 14 2 suppl1 S39 S44 doi 10 1177 02698811000142S106 PMID 10888030 S2CID 243171966 Moeller F Gerard Barratt Ernest S Dougherty Donald M Schmitz Joy M Swann Alan C November 2001 Psychiatric Aspects of Impulsivity American Journal of Psychiatry 158 11 1783 1793 doi 10 1176 appi ajp 158 11 1783 PMID 11691682 Chamberlain Samuel R Sahakian Barbara J May 2007 The neuropsychiatry of impulsivity Current Opinion in Psychiatry 20 3 255 261 doi 10 1097 YCO 0b013e3280ba4989 PMID 17415079 S2CID 22198972 External links Edit Media related to Impulsivity at Wikimedia Commons Impulsive Info Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Impulsivity amp oldid 1151236712, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.