fbpx
Wikipedia

Mimicry

In evolutionary biology, mimicry is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object, often an organism of another species. Mimicry may evolve between different species, or between individuals of the same species. Often, mimicry functions to protect a species from predators, making it an anti-predator adaptation.[1] Mimicry evolves if a receiver (such as a predator) perceives the similarity between a mimic (the organism that has a resemblance) and a model (the organism it resembles) and as a result changes its behaviour in a way that provides a selective advantage to the mimic.[2] The resemblances that evolve in mimicry can be visual, acoustic, chemical, tactile, or electric, or combinations of these sensory modalities.[2][3] Mimicry may be to the advantage of both organisms that share a resemblance, in which case it is a form of mutualism; or mimicry can be to the detriment of one, making it parasitic or competitive. The evolutionary convergence between groups is driven by the selective action of a signal-receiver or dupe.[4] Birds, for example, use sight to identify palatable insects and butterflies,[5] whilst avoiding the noxious ones. Over time, palatable insects may evolve to resemble noxious ones, making them mimics and the noxious ones models. In the case of mutualism, sometimes both groups are referred to as "co-mimics". It is often thought that models must be more abundant than mimics, but this is not so.[6] Mimicry may involve numerous species; many harmless species such as hoverflies are Batesian mimics of strongly defended species such as wasps, while many such well-defended species form Müllerian mimicry rings, all resembling each other. Mimicry between prey species and their predators often involves three or more species.[7]

Plate from Henry Walter Bates (1862) illustrating Batesian mimicry between Dismorphia species (top row, third row) and various Ithomiini (Nymphalidae, second row, bottom row)

In its broadest definition, mimicry can include non-living models. The specific terms masquerade and mimesis are sometimes used when the models are inanimate.[8][3][9] For example, animals such as flower mantises, planthoppers, comma and geometer moth caterpillars resemble twigs, bark, leaves, bird droppings or flowers.[3][6][10][11] Many animals bear eyespots, which are hypothesized to resemble the eyes of larger animals. They may not resemble any specific organism's eyes, and whether or not animals respond to them as eyes is also unclear.[12] Nonetheless, eyespots are the subject of a rich contemporary literature.[13][14][15] The model is usually another species, except in automimicry, where members of the species mimic other members, or other parts of their own bodies, and in inter-sexual mimicry, where members of one sex mimic members of the other.[6]

Mimesis in Ctenomorphodes chronus, camouflaged as a eucalyptus twig

Mimicry can result in an evolutionary arms race if mimicry negatively affects the model, and the model can evolve a different appearance from the mimic.[6]p161 Mimicry should not be confused with other forms of convergent evolution that occurs when species come to resemble each other by adapting to similar lifestyles that have nothing to do with a common signal receiver. Mimics may have different models for different life cycle stages, or they may be polymorphic, with different individuals imitating different models, such as in Heliconius butterflies. Models themselves may have more than one mimic, though frequency-dependent selection favours mimicry where models outnumber mimics. Models tend to be relatively closely related organisms,[16] but mimicry of vastly different species is also known. Most known mimics are insects,[3] though many other examples including vertebrates are also known. Plants and fungi may also be mimics, though less research has been carried out in this area.[17][18][19][20]

Etymology edit

Use of the word mimicry dates to 1637. It derives from the Greek term mimetikos, "imitative", in turn from mimetos, the verbal adjective of mimeisthai, "to imitate". Originally used to describe people, "mimetic" was used in zoology from 1851, "mimicry" from 1861.[21]

Classification edit

Many types of mimicry have been described. An overview of each follows, highlighting the similarities and differences between the various forms. Classification is often based on function with respect to the mimic (e.g., avoiding harm). Some cases may belong to more than one class, e.g., automimicry and aggressive mimicry are not mutually exclusive, as one describes the species relationship between model and mimic, while the other describes the function for the mimic (obtaining food). The terminology used is not without debate and attempts to clarify have led to new terms being included. The term "masquerade" is sometimes used when the model is inanimate but it is differentiated from "crypsis" in its strict sense[22] by the potential response of the signal receiver. In crypsis the receiver is assumed to not respond while a masquerader confuses the recognition system of the receiver that would otherwise seek the signaller. In the other forms of mimicry, the signal is not filtered out by the sensory system of the receiver.[23] These are not mutually exclusive and in the evolution of wasp-like appearance, it has been argued that insects evolve to masquerade wasps since predatory wasps do not attack each other but this mimetic resemblance also deters vertebrate predators.[24]

Defensive edit

 
Macroxiphus sp katydid mimics an ant

Defensive or protective mimicry takes place when organisms are able to avoid harmful encounters by deceiving enemies into treating them as something else.

The first three such cases discussed here entail mimicry of animals protected by warning coloration:

The fourth case, Vavilovian mimicry, where weeds resemble crops, involves humans as the agent of selection.

Batesian edit

 
Common hawk-cuckoo resembles a predator, the shikra.[25]

In Batesian mimicry the mimic shares signals similar to the model, but does not have the attribute that makes it unprofitable to predators (e.g., unpalatability). In other words, a Batesian mimic is a sheep in wolf's clothing. It is named after Henry Walter Bates, an English naturalist whose work on butterflies in the Amazon rainforest (described in The Naturalist on the River Amazons) was pioneering in this field of study.[26][27] Mimics are less likely to be found out (for example by predators) when in low proportion to their model. This phenomenon is called negative frequency-dependent selection, and it applies in most forms of mimicry. Batesian mimicry can only be maintained if the harm caused to the predator by eating a model outweighs the benefit of eating a mimic. The nature of learning is weighted in favor of the mimics, for a predator that has a bad first experience with a model tends to avoid anything that looks like it for a long time, and does not re-sample soon to see whether the initial experience was a false negative. However, if mimics become more abundant than models, then the probability of a young predator having a first experience with a mimic increases. Such systems are therefore most likely to be stable where both the model and the mimic occur, and where the model is more abundant than the mimic.[28] This is not the case in Müllerian mimicry, which is described next.

 
Many insects including hoverflies and the wasp beetle are Batesian mimics of stinging wasps.

There are many Batesian mimics in the order Lepidoptera. Consul fabius and Eresia eunice imitate unpalatable Heliconius butterflies such as H. ismenius.[29] Limenitis arthemis imitate the poisonous pipevine swallowtail (Battus philenor). Several palatable moths produce ultrasonic click calls to mimic unpalatable tiger moths.[30][31][32][33] Octopuses of the genus Thaumoctopus (the mimic octopus) are able to intentionally alter their body shape and coloration to resemble dangerous sea snakes or lionfish.[34] In the Amazon, the helmeted woodpecker (Dryocopus galeatus), a rare species which lives in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil, Paraguay, and Argentina, has a similar red crest, black back, and barred underside to two larger woodpeckers: Dryocopus lineatus and Campephilus robustus. This mimicry reduces attacks on Dryocopus galeatus from other animals. Scientists had falsely believed that D. galeatus was a close cousin of the other two species, because of the visual similarity, and because the three species live in the same habitat and eat similar food.[35] Batesian mimicry also occurs in the plant kingdom, such as the chameleon vine, which adapts its leaf shape and colour to match that of the plant it is climbing, such that its edible leaves appear to be the less desirable leaves of its host.[36]

Müllerian edit

 
The Heliconius butterflies from the tropics of the Western Hemisphere are the classical model for Müllerian mimicry.[37]

Müllerian mimicry, named for the German naturalist Fritz Müller, describes a situation where two or more species have similar warning or aposematic signals and both share genuine anti-predation attributes (e.g. being unpalatable). At first, Bates could not explain why this should be so—if both were harmful why did one need to mimic another? Müller put forward the first explanation and mathematical model for this phenomenon: if a common predator confuses two species, individuals in both those species are more likely to survive.[38][39] This type of mimicry is unique in several respects. Firstly, both the mimic and the model benefit from the interaction, which could thus be classified as mutualism. The signal receiver also benefits by this system, despite being deceived about species identity, as it is able to generalize the pattern to potentially harmful encounters. The distinction between mimic and model that is clear in Batesian mimicry is also blurred. Where one species is scarce and another abundant, the rare species can be said to be the mimic. When both are present in similar numbers, however, it makes more sense to speak of each as a co-mimic than of distinct 'mimic' and 'model' species, as their warning signals tend to converge.[40] Also, the mimetic species may exist on a continuum from harmless to highly noxious, so Batesian mimicry grades smoothly into Müllerian convergence.[41][42]

 
Comparison of Batesian and Müllerian mimicry, illustrated with a hoverfly, a wasp and a bee

The monarch butterfly (Danaus plexippus) is a member of a Müllerian complex with the viceroy butterfly (Limenitis archippus), sharing coloration patterns and display behaviour. The viceroy has subspecies with somewhat different coloration, each closely matching the local Danaus species. For example, in Florida, the pairing is of the viceroy and the queen butterfly, whereas in Mexico the viceroy resembles the soldier butterfly. The viceroy is thus involved in three different Müllerian pairs.[43] This example was long believed to be Batesian, with the viceroy mimicking the monarch, but the viceroy is actually more unpalatable than the Queen.[44] The genus Morpho is palatable, but some species (such as M. amathonte) are strong fliers; birds – even species that specialize in catching butterflies on the wing – find it hard to catch them.[45] The conspicuous blue coloration shared by most Morpho species may be Müllerian,[29] or may be "pursuit aposematism".[46] Since Morpho butterflies are sexually dimorphic, the males' iridescent coloration may also relate to sexual selection. The "orange complex" of distasteful butterfly species includes the heliconiines Agraulis vanillae, Dryadula phaetusa, and Dryas iulia.[29] At least seven species of millipedes in the genera Apheloria and Brachoria (Xystodesmidae) form a Müllerian mimicry ring in the eastern United States, in which unrelated polymorphic species converge on similar colour patterns where their range overlaps.[47]

Emsleyan/Mertensian edit

 
The deadly Texas coral snake, Micrurus tener (the Emsleyan/Mertensian mimic)
 
The harmless Mexican milk snake, Lampropeltis triangulum annulata (the Batesian mimic)

Emsleyan[9] or Mertensian mimicry describes the unusual case where a deadly prey mimics a less dangerous species. It was first proposed by M. G. Emsley[48] as a possible explanation for how a predator can learn to avoid a very dangerous aposematic animal, such as a coral snake, when the predator is very likely to die, making learning unlikely. The theory was developed by the German biologist Wolfgang Wickler[3] who named it after the German herpetologist Robert Mertens.[49][50][51]

The scenario is unusual, as it is usually the most harmful species that is the model. But if a predator dies on its first encounter with a deadly snake, it has no occasion to learn to recognize the snake's warning signals. There would then be no advantage for an extremely deadly snake in being aposematic: any predator that attacked it would be killed before it could learn to avoid the deadly prey, so the snake would be better off being camouflaged, to avoid attacks altogether. But if the predator first learnt to avoid a less deadly snake that had warning colours, the deadly species could then profit (be attacked less often) by mimicking the less dangerous snake.[50][51]

Some harmless milk snake (Lampropeltis triangulum) subspecies, the moderately toxic false coral snakes (genus Erythrolamprus), and the deadly coral snakes (genus Micrurus) all have a red background color with black and white / yellow rings. In this system, both the milk snakes and the deadly coral snakes are mimics, whereas the false coral snakes are the model.[48]

Wasmannian edit

In Wasmannian mimicry, the mimic resembles a model that it lives along with in a nest or colony. Most of the models here are social insects such as ants, termites, bees and wasps.[52]

Vavilovian edit

 
Rye is a secondary crop, originally being a mimetic weed of wheat.

Vavilovian mimicry is found in weeds that come to share characteristics with a domesticated plant through artificial selection.[9] It is named after Russian botanist and geneticist Nikolai Vavilov.[53] Selection against the weed may occur either by manually killing the weed, or by separating its seeds from those of the crop by winnowing.

Vavilovian mimicry presents an illustration of unintentional (or rather 'anti-intentional') selection by man. Weeders do not want to select weeds and their seeds that look increasingly like cultivated plants, yet there is no other option. For example, early barnyard grass, Echinochloa oryzoides, is a weed in rice fields and looks similar to rice; its seeds are often mixed in rice and have become difficult to separate through Vavilovian mimicry.[54] Vavilovian mimics may eventually be domesticated themselves, as in the case of rye in wheat; Vavilov called these weed-crops secondary crops.[53]

Vavilovian mimicry can be classified as defensive mimicry, in that the weed mimics a protected species. This bears strong similarity to Batesian mimicry in that the weed does not share the properties that give the model its protection, and both the model and the dupe (in this case people) are harmed by its presence. There are some key differences, though; in Batesian mimicry, the model and signal receiver are enemies (the predator would eat the protected species if it could), whereas here the crop and its human growers are in a mutualistic relationship: the crop benefits from being dispersed and protected by people, despite being eaten by them. In fact, the crop's only "protection" relevant here is its usefulness to humans. Secondly, the weed is not eaten, but simply destroyed. The only motivation for killing the weed is its effect on crop yields. Finally, this type of mimicry does not occur in ecosystems unaltered by humans.

Gilbertian edit

Gilbertian mimicry involves only two species. The potential host (or prey) drives away its parasite (or predator) by mimicking it, the reverse of host-parasite aggressive mimicry. It was coined by Pasteur as a phrase for such rare mimicry systems,[9] and is named after the American ecologist Lawrence E. Gilbert  [nl].[55]

Gilbertian mimicry occurs in the genus Passiflora. The leaves of this plant contain toxins that deter herbivorous animals. However, some Heliconius butterfly larvae have evolved enzymes that break down these toxins, allowing them to specialize on this genus. This has created further selection pressure on the host plants, which have evolved stipules that mimic mature Heliconius eggs near the point of hatching. These butterflies tend to avoid laying eggs near existing ones, which helps avoid exploitative intraspecific competition between caterpillars — those that lay on vacant leaves provide their offspring with a greater chance of survival. Most Heliconius larvae are cannibalistic, meaning that on leaves older eggs hatch first and eat the new arrivals. Thus, it seems that such plants have evolved egg dummies under selection pressure from these grazing herbivore enemies. In addition, the decoy eggs are also nectaries, attracting predators of the caterpillars such as ants and wasps as a further defence.[16]

Browerian edit

 
Monarch caterpillars, shown feeding, vary in toxicity depending on their diet.

Browerian mimicry,[9] named after Lincoln P. Brower and Jane Van Zandt Brower,[56][57] is a postulated form of automimicry; where the model belongs to the same species as the mimic. This is the analogue of Batesian mimicry within a single species, and occurs when there is a palatability spectrum within a population. Examples include the monarch and the queen from the subfamily Danainae, which feed on milkweed species of varying toxicity. These species store toxins from its host plant, which are maintained even in the adult (imago) form. As levels of toxin vary depending on diet during the larval stage, some individuals are more toxic than others. Less palatable organisms, therefore, mimic more dangerous individuals, with their likeness already perfected.

This is not always the case, however. In sexually dimorphic species, one sex may be more of a threat than the other, which could mimic the protected sex. Evidence for this possibility is provided by the behaviour of a monkey from Gabon, which regularly ate male moths of the genus Anaphe, but promptly stopped after it tasted a noxious female.[58]

Aggressive edit

Predators edit

Aggressive mimicry is found in predators or parasites that share some of the characteristics of a harmless species, allowing them to avoid detection by their prey or host; this can be compared with the story of the wolf in sheep's clothing as long as it is understood that no conscious deceptive intent is involved. The mimic may resemble the prey or host itself, or another organism that is either neutral or beneficial to the signal receiver. In this class of mimicry, the model may be affected negatively, positively or not at all. Just as parasites can be treated as a form of predator,[59] host-parasite mimicry is treated here as a subclass of aggressive mimicry.

The mimic may have a particular significance for duped prey. One such case is spiders, amongst which aggressive mimicry is quite common both in luring prey and disguising stealthily approaching predators.[60] One case is the golden orb weaver (Nephila clavipes), which spins a conspicuous golden colored web in well-lit areas. Experiments show that bees are able to associate the webs with danger when the yellow pigment is not present, as occurs in less well-lit areas where the web is much harder to see. Other colours were also learned and avoided, but bees seemed least able to effectively associate yellow-pigmented webs with danger. Yellow is the colour of many nectar-bearing flowers, however, so perhaps avoiding yellow is not worthwhile. Another form of mimicry is based not on colour but pattern. Species such as the silver argiope (Argiope argentata) employ prominent patterns in the middle of their webs, such as zigzags. These may reflect ultraviolet light, and mimic the pattern seen in many flowers known as nectar guides. Spiders change their web day to day, which can be explained by the ability of bees to remember web patterns. Bees are able to associate a certain pattern with a spatial location, meaning the spider must spin a new pattern regularly or suffer diminishing prey capture.[61]

Another case is where males are lured towards what seems to be a sexually receptive female. The model in this situation is the same species as the dupe. Beginning in the 1960s, James E. Lloyd's investigation of female fireflies of the genus Photuris revealed they emit the same light signals that females of the genus Photinus use as a mating signal.[62] Further research showed male fireflies from several different genera are attracted to these "femmes fatales", and are subsequently captured and eaten. Female signals are based on that received from the male, each female having a repertoire of signals matching the delay and duration of the female of the corresponding species. This mimicry may have evolved from non-mating signals that have become modified for predation.[63]

 
The spotted predatory katydid (Chlorobalius leucoviridis), an acoustic aggressive mimic of cicadas

The listrosceline katydid Chlorobalius leucoviridis of inland Australia is capable of attracting male cicadas of the tribe Cicadettini by imitating the species-specific reply clicks of sexually receptive female cicadas. This example of acoustic aggressive mimicry is similar to the Photuris firefly case in that the predator's mimicry is remarkably versatile – playback experiments show that C. leucoviridis is able to attract males of many cicada species, including cicadettine cicadas from other continents, even though cicada mating signals are species-specific.[64]

Some carnivorous plants may also be able to increase their rate of capture through mimicry.[65]

Luring is not a necessary condition however, as the predator still has a significant advantage simply by not being identified as such. They may resemble a mutualistic symbiont or a species of little relevance to the prey.

 
Two bluestreak cleaner wrasse cleaning a potato grouper, Epinephelus tukula

A case of the latter situation is a species of cleaner fish and its mimic, though in this example the model is greatly disadvantaged by the presence of the mimic. Cleaner fish are the allies of many other species, which allow them to eat their parasites and dead skin. Some allow the cleaner to venture inside their body to hunt these parasites. However, one species of cleaner, the bluestreak cleaner wrasse (Labroides dimidiatus), is the unknowing model of a mimetic species, the sabre-toothed blenny (Aspidontus taeniatus). This wrasse resides in coral reefs in the Indian and the Pacific Oceans, and is recognized by other fishes that then let it clean them. Its imposter, a species of blenny, lives in the Indian Ocean—and not only looks like it in terms of size and coloration, but even mimics the cleaner's "dance". Having fooled its prey into letting its guard down, it then bites it, tearing off a piece of its fin before fleeing. Fish grazed on in this fashion soon learn to distinguish mimic from model, but because the similarity is close between the two they become much more cautious of the model as well, so both are affected. Due to victims' ability to discriminate between foe and helper, the blennies have evolved close similarity, right down to the regional level.[66]

Another interesting example that does not involve any luring is the zone-tailed hawk, which resembles the turkey vulture. It flies amongst the vultures, suddenly breaking from the formation and ambushing its prey.[67] Here the hawk's presence is of no evident significance to the vultures, affecting them neither negatively or positively.

Parasites edit

 
Mimicry in a brood parasite: Cuckoo adult mimics sparrowhawk, alarming small birds enough to give female cuckoo time to lay eggs in their nests.[68]

Parasites can also be aggressive mimics, though the situation is somewhat different from those outlined previously. Some predators have a feature that draws prey; parasites can also mimic their hosts' natural prey, but are eaten themselves, a pathway into their host. Leucochloridium, a genus of flatworm, matures in the digestive system of songbirds, their eggs then passing out of the bird in the faeces. They are then taken up by Succinea, a terrestrial snail. The eggs develop in this intermediate host, and must then find a suitable bird to mature in. Since the host birds do not eat snails, the sporocyst has another strategy to reach its host's intestine. They are brightly coloured and move in a pulsating fashion. A sporocyst-sac pulsates in the snail's eye stalks,[69][70] coming to resemble an irresistible meal for a songbird. In this way, it can bridge the gap between hosts, allowing it to complete its life cycle.[3] A nematode (Myrmeconema neotropicum) changes the colour of the abdomen of workers of the canopy ant Cephalotes atratus to make it appear like the ripe fruits of Hyeronima alchorneoides. It also changes the behaviour of the ant so that the gaster (rear part) is held raised. This presumably increases the chances of the ant being eaten by birds. The droppings of birds are collected by other ants and fed to their brood, thereby helping to spread the nematode.[71]

In an unusual case, planidium larvae of some beetles of the genus Meloe form a group and produce a pheromone that mimics the sex attractant of its host bee species. When a male bee arrives and attempts to mate with the mass of larvae, they climb onto his abdomen. From there, they transfer to a female bee, and from there to the bee nest to parasitize the bee larvae.[72]

 
Egg mimicry: cuckoo eggs (larger) mimic many species of host birds' eggs, in this case of reed warbler.

Host-parasite mimicry is a two species system where a parasite mimics its own host. Cuckoos are a canonical example of brood parasitism, a form of parasitism where the mother has its offspring raised by another unwitting individual, often from a different species, cutting down the biological mother's parental investment in the process. The ability to lay eggs that mimic the host eggs is the key adaptation. The adaptation to different hosts is inherited through the female line in so-called gentes (gens, singular). Cases of intraspecific brood parasitism, where a female lays in a conspecific's nest, as illustrated by the goldeneye duck (Bucephala clangula),[73] do not represent a case of mimicry. A different mechanism is chemical mimicry, as seen in the parasitic butterfly Phengaris rebeli, which parasitizes the ant species Myrmica schencki by releasing chemicals that fool the worker ants to believe that the caterpillar larvae are ant larvae, and enable the P. rebeli larvae to be brought directly into the M. schencki nest.[74] Parasitic (cuckoo) bumblebees (formerly Psithyrus, now included in Bombus) resemble their hosts more closely than would be expected by chance, at least in areas like Europe where parasite-host co-speciation is common. However, this is explainable as Müllerian mimicry, rather than requiring the parasite's coloration to deceive the host and thus constitute aggressive mimicry.[75]

Reproductive edit

Reproductive mimicry occurs when the actions of the dupe directly aid in the mimic's reproduction. This is common in plants with deceptive flowers that do not provide the reward they seem to offer and it may occur in Papua New Guinea fireflies, in which the signal of Pteroptyx effulgens is used by P. tarsalis to form aggregations to attract females.[76] Other forms of mimicry have a reproductive component, such as Vavilovian mimicry involving seeds, vocal mimicry in birds,[77][78][79] and aggressive and Batesian mimicry in brood parasite-host systems.[80]

Bakerian and Dodsonian edit

Bakerian mimicry, named after Herbert G. Baker,[81] is a form of automimicry where female flowers mimic male flowers of their own species, cheating pollinators out of a reward. This reproductive mimicry may not be readily apparent as members of the same species may still exhibit some degree of sexual dimorphism. It is common in many species of Caricaceae.[82]

Dodsonian mimicry, named after Calaway H. Dodson, is a form of reproductive floral mimicry where the model belongs to a different species than the mimic.[83] By providing similar sensory signals as the model flower, it can lure its pollinators. Like Bakerian mimics, no nectar is provided. Epidendrum ibaguense (Orchidaceae) resembles flowers of Lantana camara and Asclepias curassavica, and is pollinated by monarch butterflies and perhaps hummingbirds.[84] Similar cases are seen in some other species of the same family. The mimetic species may still have pollinators of its own though. For example, a lamellicorn beetle, which usually pollinates correspondingly colored Cistus flowers, is also known to aid in pollination of Ophrys species that are normally pollinated by bees.[85]

Pseudocopulation edit

 
Dasyscolia ciliata, a scoliid wasp, attempting to copulate with a flower of the orchid Ophrys speculum

Pseudocopulation occurs when a flower mimics a female of a certain insect species, inducing the males to try to copulate with the flower. This is much like the aggressive mimicry in fireflies described previously, but with a more benign outcome for the pollinator. This form of mimicry has been called Pouyannian mimicry,[9] after Maurice-Alexandre Pouyanne, who first described the phenomenon.[86][87] It is most common in orchids, which mimic females of the order Hymenoptera (generally bees and wasps), and may account for around 60% of pollinations.[88] Depending on the morphology of the flower, a pollen sac called a pollinium is attached to the head or abdomen of the male. This is then transferred to the stigma of the next flower the male tries to inseminate, resulting in pollination. Visual mimicry is the most obvious sign of this deception for humans, but the visual aspect may be minor or non-existent. It is the senses of touch and olfaction that are most important, so this is at least partly chemical mimicry.[88]

Mimicry of rewards edit

Flowering plants, sometimes in conjunction with fungi, mimic rewards which attract insects.[89] Some "deceptive plants" imitate a pollinator's food source, with flower structures that resemble fruits.[90][91]

In a complex case, floral mimicry is induced by the discomycete fungus Monilinia vaccinii-corymbosi.[92] This fungal plant pathogen infects leaves of blueberries, causing them to secrete sugars, in effect mimicking the nectar of flowers. To the naked eye the leaves do not look like flowers, yet they still attract pollinating insects like bees using an ultraviolet signal. This case is unusual, in that the fungus benefits from the deception but it is the leaves that act as mimics, being harmed in the process. It is similar to host-parasite mimicry, but the host does not receive the signal. It has something in common with automimicry, but the plant does not benefit from the mimicry, and the action of the pathogen is required to produce it.[92]

Inter-sexual mimicry edit

Inter-sexual mimicry occurs when individuals of one sex in a species mimic members of the opposite sex to facilitate sneak mating. An example is the three male forms of the marine isopod Paracerceis sculpta. Alpha males are the largest and guard a harem of females. Beta males mimic females and manage to enter the harem of females without being detected by the alpha males allowing them to mate. Gamma males are the smallest males and mimic juveniles. This also allows them to mate with the females without the alpha males detecting them.[93] Similarly, among common side-blotched lizards, some males mimic the yellow throat coloration and even mating rejection behaviour of the other sex to sneak matings with guarded females. These males look and behave like unreceptive females. This strategy is effective against "usurper" males with orange throats, but ineffective against blue throated "guarder" males, which chase them away.[94][95] Female spotted hyenas have pseudo-penises that make them look like males.[96]

Automimicry edit

 
Eyespots of foureye butterflyfish (Chaetodon capistratus) mimic its own eyes, deflecting attacks from the vulnerable head.

Automimicry or intraspecific mimicry occurs within a single species. One form of such mimicry is where one part of an organism's body resembles another part. For example, the tails of some snakes resemble their heads; they move backwards when threatened and present the predator with the tail, improving their chances of escape without fatal harm. Some fishes have eyespots near their tails, and when mildly alarmed swim slowly backwards, presenting the tail as a head. Some insects such as some lycaenid butterflies have tail patterns and appendages of various degrees of sophistication that promote attacks at the rear rather than at the head. Several species of pygmy owl bear "false eyes" on the back of the head, misleading predators into reacting as though they were the subject of an aggressive stare.[97]

Some writers use the term "automimicry" when the mimic imitates other morphs within the same species. For example, in a species where males mimic females or vice versa, this may be an instance of sexual mimicry in evolutionary game theory. Examples are found in some species of birds, fishes, and lizards.[98] Quite elaborate strategies along these lines are known, such as the well-known "scissors, paper, rock" mimicry in Uta stansburiana,[99] but there are qualitatively different examples in many other species, such as some Platysaurus.[100]

Many species of insects are toxic or distasteful when they have fed on certain plants that contain chemicals of particular classes, but not when they have fed on plants that lack those chemicals. For instance, some species of the subfamily Danainae feed on various species of the Asclepiadoideae in the family Apocynaceae, which render them poisonous and emetic to most predators. Such insects frequently are aposematically coloured and patterned. When feeding on innocuous plants however, they are harmless and nutritious, but a bird that once has sampled a toxic specimen is unlikely to eat harmless specimens that have the same aposematic coloration. When regarded as mimicry of toxic members of the same species, this too may be seen as automimicry.[101]

 
Automimicry: many blue butterflies (Lycaenidae) such as this gray hairstreak (Strymon melinus) have a false head at the rear, held upwards at rest.

Many insects have filamentous "tails" at the ends of their wings and patterns of markings on the wings themselves. These combine to create a "false head". This misdirects predators such as birds and jumping spiders (Salticidae). Spectacular examples occur in the hairstreak butterflies; when perching on a twig or flower, they commonly do so upside down and shift their rear wings repeatedly, causing antenna-like movements of the "tails" on their wings. Studies of rear-wing damage support the hypothesis that this strategy is effective in deflecting attacks from the insect's head.[102][103]

Evolution edit

It is widely accepted that mimicry evolves as a positive adaptation. The lepidopterist and novelist Vladimir Nabokov however argued that although natural selection might stabilize a "mimic" form, it would not be necessary to create it.[104]

The most widely accepted model used to explain the evolution of mimicry in butterflies is the two-step hypothesis. The first step involves mutation in modifier genes that regulate a complex cluster of linked genes that cause large changes in morphology. The second step consists of selections on genes with smaller phenotypic effects, creating an increasingly close resemblance. This model is supported by empirical evidence that suggests that a few single point mutations cause large phenotypic effects, while numerous others produce smaller effects. Some regulatory elements collaborate to form a supergene for the development of butterfly color patterns. The model is supported by computational simulations of population genetics.[105] The Batesian mimicry in Papilio polytes is controlled by the doublesex gene.[106]

Some mimicry is imperfect. Natural selection drives mimicry only far enough to deceive predators. For example, when predators avoid a mimic that imperfectly resembles a coral snake, the mimic is sufficiently protected.[107][108][109]

Convergent evolution is an alternative explanation for why organisms such as coral reef fish[110][111] and benthic marine invertebrates such as sponges and nudibranchs have come to resemble each other.[112]

See also edit

Notes edit

References edit

  1. ^ King, R. C.; Stansfield, W. D.; Mulligan, P. K. (2006). A dictionary of genetics (7th ed.). Oxford University Press. p. 278. ISBN 978-0-19-530762-7.
  2. ^ a b Dalziell, Anastasia H.; Welbergen, Justin A. (27 April 2016). "Mimicry for all modalities". Ecology Letters. 19 (6): 609–619. Bibcode:2016EcolL..19..609D. doi:10.1111/ele.12602. PMID 27117779.
  3. ^ a b c d e f Wickler, Wolfgang (1968). Mimicry in plants and animals. McGraw-Hill.
  4. ^ Wickler, Wolfgang (1965). "Mimicry and the Evolution of Animal Communication". Nature. 208 (5010): 519–21. Bibcode:1965Natur.208..519W. doi:10.1038/208519a0. S2CID 37649827.
  5. ^ Radford, Benjamin; Frazier, Kendrick (January 2017). "Cheats and Deceits: How Animals and Plants Exploit and Mislead". Skeptical Inquirer. 41 (1): 60.
  6. ^ a b c d Ruxton, Graeme D.; Sherratt, T. N.; Speed, M. P. (2004). Avoiding Attack: the Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis, Warning Signals, and Mimicry. Oxford University Press.
  7. ^ Kikuchi, D. W.; Pfennig, D. W. (2013). "Imperfect Mimicry and the Limits of Natural Selection". Quarterly Review of Biology. 88 (4): 297–315. doi:10.1086/673758. PMID 24552099. S2CID 11436992.
  8. ^ Skelhorn, John; Rowland, Hannah M.; Ruxton, Graeme D. (2010). "The Evolution and Ecology of Masquerade". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 99: 1–8. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2009.01347.x.
  9. ^ a b c d e f Pasteur, G. (1982). "A Classificatory Review of Mimicry Systems". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 13: 169–199. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125.
  10. ^ Wiklund, Christer; Tullberg, Birgitta S. (September 2004). "Seasonal polyphenism and leaf mimicry in the comma butterfly". Animal Behaviour. 68 (3): 621–627. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.12.008. S2CID 54270418.
  11. ^ Endler, John A. (August 1981). "An Overview of the Relationships Between Mimicry and Crypsis". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 16 (1): 25–31. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x.
  12. ^ Stevens, Martin; Hopkins, Elinor; Hinde, William; Adcock, Amabel; Connolly, Yvonne; Troscianko, Tom; Cuthill, Innes C. (November 2007). "Field Experiments on the effectiveness of 'eyespots' as predator deterrents". Animal Behaviour. 74 (5): 1215–1227. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.01.031. S2CID 53186893.
  13. ^ Stevens, Martin (22 June 2007). "Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration". Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 274 (1617): 1457–1464. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0220. PMC 1950298. PMID 17426012.
  14. ^ Stevens, Martin; Stubbins, Claire L.; Hardman, Chloe J. (30 May 2008). "The anti-predator function of 'eyespots' on camouflaged and conspicuous prey". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 62 (11): 1787–1793. doi:10.1007/s00265-008-0607-3. S2CID 28288920.
  15. ^ Hossie, Thomas John; Sherratt, Thomas N. (August 2013). "Defensive posture and eyespots deter avian predators from attacking caterpillar models". Animal Behaviour. 86 (2): 383–389. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.029. S2CID 53263767.
  16. ^ a b Campbell, N. A. (1996). Biology (4th ed.). Benjamin Cummings. Chapter 50. ISBN 0-8053-1957-3.
  17. ^ Boyden, T. C. (1980). "Floral mimicry by Epidendrum ibaguense (Orchidaceae) in Panama". Evolution. 34 (1): 135–136. doi:10.2307/2408322. JSTOR 2408322. PMID 28563205.
  18. ^ Roy, B. A. (1994). "The effects of pathogen-induced pseudoflowers and buttercups on each other's insect visitation". Ecology. 75 (2): 352–358. Bibcode:1994Ecol...75..352R. doi:10.2307/1939539. JSTOR 1939539.
  19. ^ Wickler, Wolfgang, 1998. "Mimicry". Encyclopædia Britannica, 15th edition. Macropædia 24, 144–151. https://www.britannica.com/eb/article-11910
  20. ^ Johnson, Steven D.; Schiestl, Florian P. (2016). Floral Mimicry. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-104723-7.
  21. ^ Harper, Douglas. "Online Etymology Dictionary". Retrieved 23 February 2022.
  22. ^ Endler, John A. (1981). "An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 16: 25–31. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1981.tb01840.x.
  23. ^ Allen, J. A.; Cooper, J. M. (2010). "Crypsis and masquerade". Journal of Biological Education. 19 (4): 268. doi:10.1080/00219266.1985.9654747.
  24. ^ Boppré, Michael; Vane-Wright, Richard I.; Wickler, Wolfgang (1 January 2017). "A hypothesis to explain accuracy of wasp resemblances". Ecology and Evolution. 7 (1): 73–81. Bibcode:2017EcoEv...7...73B. doi:10.1002/ece3.2586. PMC 5214283. PMID 28070276.
  25. ^ Davies, N. B.; Welbergen, J. A. (2008). "Cuckoo–hawk mimicry? An experimental test". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 275 (1644): 1817–1822. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0331. PMC 2587796. PMID 18467298.
  26. ^ Bates, Henry W. (1863). The naturalist on the river Amazons. Murray.
  27. ^ Bates, Henry W. (1861). "Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley. Lepidoptera: Heliconidae". Transactions of the Linnean Society. 23 (3): 495–566. doi:10.1111/j.1096-3642.1860.tb00146.x.
  28. ^ Stearns, S. C.; Hoekstra, Rolf F. (2000). Evolution: An Introduction (5th ed.). Oxford University Press. p. 464. ISBN 978-0-19-854968-0.
  29. ^ a b c Pinheiro, Carlos E. G. (1996). "Palatability and escaping ability in Neotropical butterflies: tests with wild kingbirds (Tyrannus melancholicus, Tyrannidae)". Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 59 (4): 351–365. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1996.tb01471.x.
  30. ^ Barber, J. R.; Conner, W. E. (2007). "Acoustic mimicry in a predator–prey interaction". PNAS. 104 (22): 9331–9334. Bibcode:2007PNAS..104.9331B. doi:10.1073/pnas.0703627104. PMC 1890494. PMID 17517637.
  31. ^ Barber, Jesse R.; Chadwell, Brad A.; Garrett, Nick; Schmidt-French, Barbara; Conner, William E. (July 2009). "Naïve bats discriminate arctiid moth warning sounds but generalize their aposematic meaning". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (Pt 14): 2141–2148. doi:10.1242/jeb.029991. ISSN 0022-0949. PMID 19561203. S2CID 1303252.
  32. ^ Barber, Jesse R.; Plotkin, David; Rubin, Juliette J.; Homziak, Nicholas T.; Leavell, Brian C.; Houlihan, Peter R.; Miner, Krystie A.; Breinholt, Jesse W.; Quirk-Royal, Brandt; Padrón, Pablo Sebastián; Nunez, Matias; Kawahara, Akito Y. (21 June 2022). "Anti-bat ultrasound production in moths is globally and phylogenetically widespread". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 119 (25): e2117485119. Bibcode:2022PNAS..11917485B. doi:10.1073/pnas.2117485119. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 9231501. PMID 35704762.
  33. ^ Kawahara, Akito Y.; Barber, Jesse R. (19 May 2015). "Tempo and mode of antibat ultrasound production and sonar jamming in the diverse hawkmoth radiation". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 112 (20): 6407–6412. Bibcode:2015PNAS..112.6407K. doi:10.1073/pnas.1416679112. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 4443353. PMID 25941377.
  34. ^ . Archived from the original on 18 July 2017. Retrieved 9 June 2007.
  35. ^ "Deceptive Woodpecker Uses Mimicry to Avoid Competition". AMNH. Retrieved 12 August 2015.
  36. ^ Gianoli, Ernesto (2014). "Leaf Mimicry in a Climbing Plant Protects against Herbivory". Cell. 24 (9): 984–987. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.03.010. PMID 24768053.
  37. ^ Meyer, A. (2006). "Repeating Patterns of Mimicry". PLOS Biol. 4 (10): e341. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040341. PMC 1617347. PMID 17048984.
  38. ^ Müller, Fritz (1878). "Ueber die Vortheile der Mimicry bei Schmetterlingen". Zoologischer Anzeiger. 1: 54–55.
  39. ^ Müller, F. (1879). "Ituna and Thyridia; a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies". Proclamations of the Entomological Society of London. 1879. Translated by Meldola, R.: 20–29.
  40. ^ Flannery, T. F. (2007). "Community ecology: Mimicry complexes". Encyclopædia Britannica Online.
  41. ^ Huheey, James E. (1976). "Studies in warning coloration and mimicry VII — Evolutionary consequences of a Batesian–Müllerian spectrum: A model for Müllerian mimicry". Evolution. 30 (1): 86–93. doi:10.2307/2407675. JSTOR 2407675. PMID 28565050.
  42. ^ Benson, W. W. (1977). "On the Supposed Spectrum Between Batesian and Mullerian Mimicry". Evolution. 31 (2): 454–455. doi:10.2307/2407770. JSTOR 2407770. PMID 28563231.
  43. ^ Ritland, D. B. (1995). "Comparative unpalatability of mimetic viceroy butterflies (Limenitis archippus) from four south-eastern United States populations". Oecologia. 103 (3): 327–336. Bibcode:1995Oecol.103..327R. doi:10.1007/BF00328621. PMID 28306826. S2CID 13436225.
  44. ^ Ritland, D. B; Brower, Lincoln P. (1991). "The viceroy butterfly is not a Batesian mimic". Nature. 350 (6318): 497–498. Bibcode:1991Natur.350..497R. doi:10.1038/350497a0. S2CID 28667520. Viceroys are as unpalatable as monarchs, and significantly more unpalatable than queens from representative Florida populations.
  45. ^ Young, A. M. (1971). "Wing colouration and reflectance in Morpho butterflies as related to reproductive behaviour and escape from avian predators". Oecologia. 7 (3): 209–222. Bibcode:1971Oecol...7..209Y. doi:10.1007/BF00345212. PMID 28311247. S2CID 25970574.
  46. ^ Edmunds, M. (1974). Defence in Animals: a survey of anti-predator defences. Longman. pp. 255–256. ISBN 0-582-44132-3.
  47. ^ Marek, P. E.; Bond, J. E. (2009). "A Mullerian mimicry ring in Appalachian millipedes". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 106 (24): 9755–9760. Bibcode:2009PNAS..106.9755M. doi:10.1073/pnas.0810408106. PMC 2700981. PMID 19487663.
  48. ^ a b Emsley, M. G. (1966). "The mimetic significance of Erythrolamprus aesculapii ocellatus Peters from Tobago". Evolution. 20 (4): 663–64. doi:10.2307/2406599. JSTOR 2406599. PMID 28562911.
  49. ^ Mertens, Robert (1956). "Das Problem der Mimikry bei Korallenschlangen". Zool. Jahrb. Syst (in German). 84: 541–76.
  50. ^ a b Hecht, M. K.; Marien, D. (1956). "The coral snake mimic problem: a reinterpretation". Journal of Morphology. 98 (2): 335–365. doi:10.1002/jmor.1050980207. S2CID 83825414.
  51. ^ a b Sheppard, P. M.; Wickler, Wolfgang (1969). "Review of Mimicry in plants and animals by Wolfgang Wickler". Journal of Animal Ecology. 38 (1): 243. doi:10.2307/2762. JSTOR 2762.
  52. ^ Wasmann, E. 1894. Kritisches Verzeichniss der myrmecophilin und termitophilen Arthropoden. Felix Dames, Berlin xi + 231 pp.
  53. ^ a b Vavilov, N. I. (1951). "The origin, variation, immunity and breeding of cultivated plants (translation by K. S. Chester)". Chronica Botanica. 13: 1–366.
  54. ^ Barrett, S. (1983). "Mimicry in Plants". Scientific American. 257 (3): 76–83. Bibcode:1987SciAm.257c..76B. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0987-76.
  55. ^ Gilbert, Lawrence E. (1975). "Ecological consequences of a coevolved mutualism between butterflies and plants". In L. E. Gilbert; P. H. Raven (eds.). Coevolution of Animals and Plants. University of Texas Press. pp. 210–240. OCLC 636384400.
  56. ^ Brower, Lincoln P. (1970). "Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain and implications for mimicry theory". In Chambers, K. L. (ed.). Biochemical Coevolution. Corvallis, Oregon, USA: Oregon State Univ. pp. 69–82.
  57. ^ Brower, Lincoln P.; Van Brower, J. V. Z.; Corvino, J. M. (1967). "Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 57 (4): 893–98. Bibcode:1967PNAS...57..893B. doi:10.1073/pnas.57.4.893. PMC 224631. PMID 5231352.
  58. ^ Bigot, L.; Jouventin, P. (1974). "Quelques expériences de comestibilité de Lépidoptères gabonais faites avec le mandrill, le cercocèbe à joues grises et le garde-bœufs". Terre Vie (in French). 28: 521–43.
  59. ^ Begon, M.; Townsend, C.; Harper, J. (1996) Ecology: Individuals, populations and communities (third edition) Blackwell Science, London
  60. ^ Jackson, R. R. (1995). "Eight-legged tricksters: Spiders that specialize at catching other spiders". BioScience. 42 (8): 590–98. doi:10.2307/1311924. JSTOR 1311924.
  61. ^ Craig, C. L. (1995). "Webs of Deceit". Natural History. 104 (3): 32–35.
  62. ^ Lloyd, J. E. (1965) Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris: Firefly Femmes Fatales Science 149:653–654.
  63. ^ Lloyd, J. E. (1975). "Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris Fireflies: Signal Repertoires by Femmes Fatales". Science. 187 (4175): 452–453. Bibcode:1975Sci...187..452L. doi:10.1126/science.187.4175.452. PMID 17835312. S2CID 26761854.
  64. ^ Marshall, D. C.; Hill, K. B. R. (2009). Chippindale, Adam K (ed.). "Versatile aggressive mimicry of cicadas by an Australian predatory katydid". PLOS ONE. 4 (1): e4185. Bibcode:2009PLoSO...4.4185M. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0004185. PMC 2615208. PMID 19142230.
  65. ^ Moran, Jonathan A. (1996). "Pitcher dimorphism, prey composition and the mechanisms of prey attraction in the pitcher plant Nepenthes rafflesiana in Borneo". Journal of Ecology. 84 (4): 515–525. Bibcode:1996JEcol..84..515M. doi:10.2307/2261474. JSTOR 2261474.
  66. ^ Wickler, W. (1966). "Mimicry in Tropical Fishes". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 251 (772): 473–474. Bibcode:1966RSPTB.251..473W. doi:10.1098/rstb.1966.0036. S2CID 83609965.
  67. ^ Willis, E. O. (1963). "Is the Zone-Tailed Hawk a Mimic of the Turkey Vulture?". The Condor. 65 (4): 313–317. doi:10.2307/1365357. JSTOR 1365357.
  68. ^ Welbergen, Justin A.; Davies, Nicholas B. (2011). "A parasite in wolf's clothing: hawk mimicry reduces mobbing of cuckoos by hosts". Behavioral Ecology. 22 (3): 574–579. doi:10.1093/beheco/arr008. ISSN 1465-7279.
  69. ^ See here for a photo.
  70. ^ Moore, J. (2002). Parasites and the behavior of animals. Oxford University Press.
  71. ^ Yanoviak, S. P.; Kaspari, M.; Dudley, R.; Poinar, G. Jr (2008). "Parasite-induced fruit mimicry in a tropical canopy ant" (PDF). The American Naturalist. 171 (4): 536–44. doi:10.1086/528968. PMID 18279076. S2CID 23857167.
  72. ^ Saul-Gershenz, L. S.; Millar, J. G. (2006). "Phoretic nest parasites use sexual deception to obtain transport to their host's nest". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 103 (38): 14039–14044. Bibcode:2006PNAS..10314039S. doi:10.1073/pnas.0603901103. PMC 1599908. PMID 16966608.
  73. ^ Andersson, M.; Eriksson, M. O. G. (1982). "Nest parasitism in Goldeneyes Bucephala clangula: some evolutionary aspects". American Naturalist. 120: 1–16. doi:10.1086/283965. S2CID 86699716.
  74. ^ Barbero, Francesca; Thomas JA; Bonelli S; Balletto E; Schonrogge K (2009). "Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants". Journal of Experimental Biology. 212 (Pt 24): 4084–4090. doi:10.1242/jeb.032912. PMID 19946088. Retrieved 28 September 2013.
  75. ^ Williams, Paul H. (2008). "Do the parasitic Psithyrus resemble their host bumblebees in colour pattern?" (PDF). Apidologie. 39 (6): 637–649. doi:10.1051/apido:2008048. S2CID 27702692.
  76. ^ Ohba, N.; Shimoyama, Ayu (2009). Meyer-Rochow, V. B. (ed.). Bioluminescence in Focus - a collection of illuminating essays. Research Signpost; Trivandrum, Kerala, India. pp. 229–242.
  77. ^ Dalziell, Anastasia H.; Welbergen, Justin A.; Igic, Branislav; Magrath, Robert D. (30 July 2014). "Avian vocal mimicry: a unified conceptual framework". Biological Reviews. 90 (2): 643–668. doi:10.1111/brv.12129. PMID 25079896. S2CID 207101926.
  78. ^ Kelley, Laura A.; Coe, Rebecca L.; Madden, Joah R.; Healy, Susan D. (1 September 2008). "Vocal mimicry in songbirds". Animal Behaviour. 76 (3): 521–528. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.012. S2CID 53192695.
  79. ^ Goller, Maria; Shizuka, Daizaburo (22 June 2018). "Evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry in songbirds". Evolution Letters. 2 (4): 417–426. doi:10.1002/evl3.62. PMC 6121844. PMID 30283692.
  80. ^ Davies, Nick (2015). Cuckoo: Cheating by Nature. Bloomsbury. ISBN 978-1-4088-5656-7.
  81. ^ Baker, Herbert G. 1976. "Mistake" pollination as a reproductive system, with special reference to the Caricaceae. Pp 161–169 in J. Burley and B. T. Styles, eds. [clarification needed] Variation, breeding, and conservation of tropical trees. Academic Press, London, U.K.
  82. ^ Bawa, K. S. (1980). "Mimicry of male by female flowers and intrasexual competition for pollinators in Jacaratia dolichaula (D. Smith) Woodson (Caricaceae)". Evolution. 34 (3): 467–74. doi:10.2307/2408216. JSTOR 2408216. PMID 28568703.
  83. ^ Dodson, C. H.; Frymire, G. P. (1961). "Natural pollination of orchids". Missouri Botanical Garden Bulletin. 49: 133–39.
  84. ^ Boyden, T. C. (1980). "Floral mimicry by Epidendrurn ibaguense (Orchidaceae) in Panama". Evolution. 34 (1): 135–36. doi:10.2307/2408322. JSTOR 2408322. PMID 28563205.
  85. ^ Kullenberg, B. (1961). "Studies in Ophrys pollination". Zool. Bidr. Uppsala. 34: 1–340.
  86. ^ Correvon H., Pouyanne M. (1916) Un curieux cas de mimetisme chez les Ophrydées. J. Soc. Nat. Hortic. Fr. 17: 29–31, 41–42, 84.
  87. ^ Pouyanne, M. (1917). "La fécondation des Ophrys par les insectes". Bull. Soc. Hist. Nat. Afr. Nord. 8: 1–2.
  88. ^ a b Van der Pijl, L., Dodson, C. H. (1966) Orchid Flowers; Their Pollination and Evolution. Coral Gables, Florida, USA, Univ. Miami Press.
  89. ^ Johnson, Steven D.; Schiestl, Florian P. (2016). Floral Mimicry. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-104723-7.
  90. ^ Ellis, Allan G.; Johnson, Steven D. (2010). "Floral Mimicry Enhances Pollen Export: The Evolution of Pollination by Sexual Deceit Outside of the Orchidaceae". The American Naturalist. 176 (5): E143–E151. doi:10.1086/656487. ISSN 0003-0147. PMID 20843263. S2CID 45076899.
  91. ^ Goodrich, Katherine R.; Jürgens, Andreas (2018). "Pollination systems involving floral mimicry of fruit: aspects of their ecology and evolution". New Phytologist. 217 (1): 74–81. doi:10.1111/nph.14821.
  92. ^ a b Batra, L. R.; Batra, S. (1985). "Floral Mimicry Induced by Mummy-Berry Fungus Exploits Host's Pollinators as Vectors". Science. 228 (4702): 1011–1013. Bibcode:1985Sci...228.1011B. doi:10.1126/science.228.4702.1011. PMID 17797664. S2CID 21422865.
  93. ^ Shuster, Stephen (May 1987). "Alternative Reproductive Behaviors: Three Discrete Male Morphs in Paracerceis sculpta, an Intertidal Isopod from the Northern Gulf of California". Journal of Crustacean Biology. 7 (2): 318–327. doi:10.2307/1548612. JSTOR 1548612.
  94. ^ Sinervo, B.; C. M. Lively (1996). "The rock–paper–scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies". Nature. 380 (6571): 240–243. Bibcode:1996Natur.380..240S. doi:10.1038/380240a0. S2CID 205026253.
  95. ^ Sinervo, B.; Miles, D. B.; Frankino, W. A.; Klukowski, M.; Denardo, D. F. (2000). "Testosterone, Endurance, and Darwinian Fitness: Natural and Sexual Selection on the Physiological Bases of Alternative Male Behaviors in Side-Blotched Lizards". Hormones and Behavior. 38 (4): 222–233. doi:10.1006/hbeh.2000.1622. PMID 11104640. S2CID 5759575.
  96. ^ Muller, M. N.; Wrangham, R. (2002). "Sexual Mimicry in Hyenas". The Quarterly Review of Biology. 77 (1): 3–16. doi:10.1086/339199. PMID 11963460. S2CID 43440407.
  97. ^ . Owl Research Institute. Archived from the original on 28 December 2015. Retrieved 23 August 2015.
  98. ^ Plaistow, Stewart J.; Johnstone, Rufus A.; Colegrave, Nick; Spencer, Matthew (2004). "Evolution of alternative mating tactics: conditional versus mixed strategies". Behavioral Ecology. 15 (4): 534–542. doi:10.1093/beheco/arh029.
  99. ^ Schell, Robert; Dettman, Jessica. (PDF). Center for Watershed Sciences. University of California, Davis. Archived from the original (PDF) on 8 August 2014.
  100. ^ Lewis, Belinda Ann (2006). Sexual Selection and Signalling in the Lizard Platysaurus minor (PDF) (Masters thesis).
  101. ^ Svennungsen, Thomas Owens; Holen, Øistein Haugsten (2007). "The evolutionary stability of automimicry". Proc. R. Soc. B. 274 (1621): 2055–2063. doi:10.1098/rspb.2007.0456. PMC 2275178. PMID 17567561.
  102. ^ Sourakov, Andrei (2013): Two heads are better than one: false head allows Calycopis cecrops (Lycaenidae) to escape predation by a Jumping Spider, Phidippus pulcherrimus (Salticidae), Journal of Natural History, 47:15-16, 1047-1054
  103. ^ Robbins, Robert K. The "False Head" Hypothesis: Predation and Wing Pattern Variation of Lycaenid Butterflies. The American Naturalist Vol. 118, No. 5 (Nov., 1981), pp. 770-775
  104. ^ Alexander, Victoria N. (2002). "Nabokov, Teleology and Insect Mimicry". Nabokov Studies. 7: 177–213. doi:10.1353/nab.2010.0004. S2CID 42675699.
  105. ^ Holmgren, N. M. A.; Enquist, M. (1999). "Dynamics of mimicry evolution" (PDF). Biological Journal of the Linnean Society. 66 (2): 145–158. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1999.tb01880.x.
  106. ^ K. Kunte; W. Zhang; A. Tenger-Trolander; D. H. Palmer; A. Martin; R. D. Reed; S. P. Mullen; M. R. Kronforst (2014). "Doublesex is a mimicry supergene". Nature. 507 (7491): 229–232. Bibcode:2014Natur.507..229K. doi:10.1038/nature13112. PMID 24598547. S2CID 4448793.
  107. ^ Wilson, J.; Jahner, J.; Williams, K.; Forister, M. (2013). "Ecological and Evolutionary Processes Drive the Origin and Maintenance of Imperfect Mimicry". PLOS ONE. 8 (4): e61610. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...861610W. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061610. PMC 3625143. PMID 23593490.
  108. ^ Kikuchi, D.; Pfenning, D. (2010). "Predator Cognition Permits Imperfect Coral Snake Mimicry". The American Naturalist. 176 (6): 830–834. doi:10.1086/657041. PMID 20950143. S2CID 35411437.
  109. ^ Howse, P. E.; Allen, J. A. (1994). "Satyric Mimicry: The Evolution of Apparent Imperfection". Proceedings of the Royal Society B. 257 (1349): 111–114. Bibcode:1994RSPSB.257..111H. doi:10.1098/rspb.1994.0102. S2CID 84458742.
  110. ^ Robertson, D. Ross (2013). "Who resembles whom? Mimetic and coincidental look-alikes among tropical reef fishes". PLOS ONE. 8 (1): e54939. Bibcode:2013PLoSO...854939R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054939. PMC 3556028. PMID 23372795.
  111. ^ Robertson, D. Ross (2015). "Coincidental resemblances among coral reef fishes from different oceans". Coral Reefs. 34 (3): 977. Bibcode:2015CorRe..34..977R. doi:10.1007/s00338-015-1309-8.
  112. ^ Pawlik, J.R. (2012). "12". In Fattorusso, E.; Gerwick, W.H.; Taglialatela-Scafati, O. (eds.). Antipredatory defensive roles of natural products from marine invertebrates. Springer. pp. 677–710. ISBN 978-90-481-3833-3.

Further reading edit

  • Brower, L. P., ed. (1988). Mimicry and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 0-226-07608-3. (a supplement of volume 131 of the journal American Naturalist dedicated to E. B. Ford).
  • Carpenter, G. D. Hale; Ford, E. B. (1933). Mimicry. London: Methuen.
  • Cott, H. B. (1940) Adaptive Coloration in Animals. Methuen and Co, London, ISBN 0-416-30050-2
  • Dafni, A. (1984). "Mimicry and Deception in Pollination". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 15: 259–278. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.15.110184.001355.
  • Edmunds, M. 1974. Defence in Animals: a survey of anti-predator defences. Harlow, Essex and New York, Longman. ISBN 0-582-44132-3.
  • Evans, M. A. (1965). "Mimicry and the Darwinian Heritage". Journal of the History of Ideas. 26 (2): 211–220. doi:10.2307/2708228. JSTOR 2708228.
  • Owen, D. (1980) Camouflage and Mimicry. Oxford University Press, ISBN 0-19-217683-8.
  • Pasteur, Georges (1982). "A classificatory review of mimicry systems". Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics. 13: 169–199. doi:10.1146/annurev.es.13.110182.001125.
  • Stevens, M. (2016). Cheats and deceits: how animals and plants exploit and mislead. Oxford University Press, ISBN 978-0-19-870789-9
  • Wiens, D. (1978). "Mimicry in Plants". In Max K. Hecht; William C. Steere; Bruce Wallace (eds.). Evolutionary Biology. Vol. 11. pp. 365–403. doi:10.1007/978-1-4615-6956-5_6. ISBN 978-1-4615-6958-9. PMC 3282713. PMID 22182416. {{cite book}}: |journal= ignored (help)
  • Vane-Wright, R. I. (1976). "A unified classification of mimetic resemblances". Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 8: 25–56. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.1976.tb00240.x.
  • Wickler, W. (1968) Mimicry in Plants and Animals (translated from the German), McGraw-Hill, New York. ISBN 0-07-070100-8.

Children's edit

  • Hoff, M. K. (2003) Mimicry and Camouflage. Creative Education. Mankato, Minnesota, USA, Great Britain. ISBN 1-58341-237-9.

External links edit

mimicry, mimic, redirects, here, other, uses, mimic, disambiguation, evolutionary, biology, mimicry, evolved, resemblance, between, organism, another, object, often, organism, another, species, evolve, between, different, species, between, individuals, same, s. Mimic redirects here For other uses see Mimic disambiguation In evolutionary biology mimicry is an evolved resemblance between an organism and another object often an organism of another species Mimicry may evolve between different species or between individuals of the same species Often mimicry functions to protect a species from predators making it an anti predator adaptation 1 Mimicry evolves if a receiver such as a predator perceives the similarity between a mimic the organism that has a resemblance and a model the organism it resembles and as a result changes its behaviour in a way that provides a selective advantage to the mimic 2 The resemblances that evolve in mimicry can be visual acoustic chemical tactile or electric or combinations of these sensory modalities 2 3 Mimicry may be to the advantage of both organisms that share a resemblance in which case it is a form of mutualism or mimicry can be to the detriment of one making it parasitic or competitive The evolutionary convergence between groups is driven by the selective action of a signal receiver or dupe 4 Birds for example use sight to identify palatable insects and butterflies 5 whilst avoiding the noxious ones Over time palatable insects may evolve to resemble noxious ones making them mimics and the noxious ones models In the case of mutualism sometimes both groups are referred to as co mimics It is often thought that models must be more abundant than mimics but this is not so 6 Mimicry may involve numerous species many harmless species such as hoverflies are Batesian mimics of strongly defended species such as wasps while many such well defended species form Mullerian mimicry rings all resembling each other Mimicry between prey species and their predators often involves three or more species 7 Plate from Henry Walter Bates 1862 illustrating Batesian mimicry between Dismorphia species top row third row and various Ithomiini Nymphalidae second row bottom row In its broadest definition mimicry can include non living models The specific terms masquerade and mimesis are sometimes used when the models are inanimate 8 3 9 For example animals such as flower mantises planthoppers comma and geometer moth caterpillars resemble twigs bark leaves bird droppings or flowers 3 6 10 11 Many animals bear eyespots which are hypothesized to resemble the eyes of larger animals They may not resemble any specific organism s eyes and whether or not animals respond to them as eyes is also unclear 12 Nonetheless eyespots are the subject of a rich contemporary literature 13 14 15 The model is usually another species except in automimicry where members of the species mimic other members or other parts of their own bodies and in inter sexual mimicry where members of one sex mimic members of the other 6 Mimesis in Ctenomorphodes chronus camouflaged as a eucalyptus twig Mimicry can result in an evolutionary arms race if mimicry negatively affects the model and the model can evolve a different appearance from the mimic 6 p161 Mimicry should not be confused with other forms of convergent evolution that occurs when species come to resemble each other by adapting to similar lifestyles that have nothing to do with a common signal receiver Mimics may have different models for different life cycle stages or they may be polymorphic with different individuals imitating different models such as in Heliconius butterflies Models themselves may have more than one mimic though frequency dependent selection favours mimicry where models outnumber mimics Models tend to be relatively closely related organisms 16 but mimicry of vastly different species is also known Most known mimics are insects 3 though many other examples including vertebrates are also known Plants and fungi may also be mimics though less research has been carried out in this area 17 18 19 20 Contents 1 Etymology 2 Classification 2 1 Defensive 2 1 1 Batesian 2 1 2 Mullerian 2 1 3 Emsleyan Mertensian 2 1 4 Wasmannian 2 1 5 Vavilovian 2 1 6 Gilbertian 2 1 7 Browerian 2 2 Aggressive 2 2 1 Predators 2 2 2 Parasites 2 3 Reproductive 2 3 1 Bakerian and Dodsonian 2 3 2 Pseudocopulation 2 3 3 Mimicry of rewards 2 3 4 Inter sexual mimicry 2 4 Automimicry 3 Evolution 4 See also 5 Notes 6 References 7 Further reading 7 1 Children s 8 External linksEtymology editUse of the word mimicry dates to 1637 It derives from the Greek term mimetikos imitative in turn from mimetos the verbal adjective of mimeisthai to imitate Originally used to describe people mimetic was used in zoology from 1851 mimicry from 1861 21 Classification editMany types of mimicry have been described An overview of each follows highlighting the similarities and differences between the various forms Classification is often based on function with respect to the mimic e g avoiding harm Some cases may belong to more than one class e g automimicry and aggressive mimicry are not mutually exclusive as one describes the species relationship between model and mimic while the other describes the function for the mimic obtaining food The terminology used is not without debate and attempts to clarify have led to new terms being included The term masquerade is sometimes used when the model is inanimate but it is differentiated from crypsis in its strict sense 22 by the potential response of the signal receiver In crypsis the receiver is assumed to not respond while a masquerader confuses the recognition system of the receiver that would otherwise seek the signaller In the other forms of mimicry the signal is not filtered out by the sensory system of the receiver 23 These are not mutually exclusive and in the evolution of wasp like appearance it has been argued that insects evolve to masquerade wasps since predatory wasps do not attack each other but this mimetic resemblance also deters vertebrate predators 24 Defensive edit nbsp Macroxiphus sp katydid mimics an antDefensive or protective mimicry takes place when organisms are able to avoid harmful encounters by deceiving enemies into treating them as something else The first three such cases discussed here entail mimicry of animals protected by warning coloration Batesian mimicry where a harmless mimic poses as harmful Mullerian mimicry where two or more harmful species mutually advertise themselves as harmful Mertensian mimicry where a deadly mimic resembles a less harmful but lesson teaching model The fourth case Vavilovian mimicry where weeds resemble crops involves humans as the agent of selection Batesian edit Main article Batesian mimicry nbsp Common hawk cuckoo resembles a predator the shikra 25 In Batesian mimicry the mimic shares signals similar to the model but does not have the attribute that makes it unprofitable to predators e g unpalatability In other words a Batesian mimic is a sheep in wolf s clothing It is named after Henry Walter Bates an English naturalist whose work on butterflies in the Amazon rainforest described in The Naturalist on the River Amazons was pioneering in this field of study 26 27 Mimics are less likely to be found out for example by predators when in low proportion to their model This phenomenon is called negative frequency dependent selection and it applies in most forms of mimicry Batesian mimicry can only be maintained if the harm caused to the predator by eating a model outweighs the benefit of eating a mimic The nature of learning is weighted in favor of the mimics for a predator that has a bad first experience with a model tends to avoid anything that looks like it for a long time and does not re sample soon to see whether the initial experience was a false negative However if mimics become more abundant than models then the probability of a young predator having a first experience with a mimic increases Such systems are therefore most likely to be stable where both the model and the mimic occur and where the model is more abundant than the mimic 28 This is not the case in Mullerian mimicry which is described next nbsp Many insects including hoverflies and the wasp beetle are Batesian mimics of stinging wasps There are many Batesian mimics in the order Lepidoptera Consul fabius and Eresia eunice imitate unpalatable Heliconius butterflies such as H ismenius 29 Limenitis arthemis imitate the poisonous pipevine swallowtail Battus philenor Several palatable moths produce ultrasonic click calls to mimic unpalatable tiger moths 30 31 32 33 Octopuses of the genus Thaumoctopus the mimic octopus are able to intentionally alter their body shape and coloration to resemble dangerous sea snakes or lionfish 34 In the Amazon the helmeted woodpecker Dryocopus galeatus a rare species which lives in the Atlantic Forest of Brazil Paraguay and Argentina has a similar red crest black back and barred underside to two larger woodpeckers Dryocopus lineatus and Campephilus robustus This mimicry reduces attacks on Dryocopus galeatus from other animals Scientists had falsely believed that D galeatus was a close cousin of the other two species because of the visual similarity and because the three species live in the same habitat and eat similar food 35 Batesian mimicry also occurs in the plant kingdom such as the chameleon vine which adapts its leaf shape and colour to match that of the plant it is climbing such that its edible leaves appear to be the less desirable leaves of its host 36 Mullerian edit Main article Mullerian mimicry nbsp The Heliconius butterflies from the tropics of the Western Hemisphere are the classical model for Mullerian mimicry 37 Mullerian mimicry named for the German naturalist Fritz Muller describes a situation where two or more species have similar warning or aposematic signals and both share genuine anti predation attributes e g being unpalatable At first Bates could not explain why this should be so if both were harmful why did one need to mimic another Muller put forward the first explanation and mathematical model for this phenomenon if a common predator confuses two species individuals in both those species are more likely to survive 38 39 This type of mimicry is unique in several respects Firstly both the mimic and the model benefit from the interaction which could thus be classified as mutualism The signal receiver also benefits by this system despite being deceived about species identity as it is able to generalize the pattern to potentially harmful encounters The distinction between mimic and model that is clear in Batesian mimicry is also blurred Where one species is scarce and another abundant the rare species can be said to be the mimic When both are present in similar numbers however it makes more sense to speak of each as a co mimic than of distinct mimic and model species as their warning signals tend to converge 40 Also the mimetic species may exist on a continuum from harmless to highly noxious so Batesian mimicry grades smoothly into Mullerian convergence 41 42 nbsp Comparison of Batesian and Mullerian mimicry illustrated with a hoverfly a wasp and a bee The monarch butterfly Danaus plexippus is a member of a Mullerian complex with the viceroy butterfly Limenitis archippus sharing coloration patterns and display behaviour The viceroy has subspecies with somewhat different coloration each closely matching the local Danaus species For example in Florida the pairing is of the viceroy and the queen butterfly whereas in Mexico the viceroy resembles the soldier butterfly The viceroy is thus involved in three different Mullerian pairs 43 This example was long believed to be Batesian with the viceroy mimicking the monarch but the viceroy is actually more unpalatable than the Queen 44 The genus Morpho is palatable but some species such as M amathonte are strong fliers birds even species that specialize in catching butterflies on the wing find it hard to catch them 45 The conspicuous blue coloration shared by most Morpho species may be Mullerian 29 or may be pursuit aposematism 46 Since Morpho butterflies are sexually dimorphic the males iridescent coloration may also relate to sexual selection The orange complex of distasteful butterfly species includes the heliconiines Agraulis vanillae Dryadula phaetusa and Dryas iulia 29 At least seven species of millipedes in the genera Apheloria and Brachoria Xystodesmidae form a Mullerian mimicry ring in the eastern United States in which unrelated polymorphic species converge on similar colour patterns where their range overlaps 47 Emsleyan Mertensian edit nbsp The deadly Texas coral snake Micrurus tener the Emsleyan Mertensian mimic nbsp The harmless Mexican milk snake Lampropeltis triangulum annulata the Batesian mimic Main article Emsleyan mimicry Emsleyan 9 or Mertensian mimicry describes the unusual case where a deadly prey mimics a less dangerous species It was first proposed by M G Emsley 48 as a possible explanation for how a predator can learn to avoid a very dangerous aposematic animal such as a coral snake when the predator is very likely to die making learning unlikely The theory was developed by the German biologist Wolfgang Wickler 3 who named it after the German herpetologist Robert Mertens 49 50 51 The scenario is unusual as it is usually the most harmful species that is the model But if a predator dies on its first encounter with a deadly snake it has no occasion to learn to recognize the snake s warning signals There would then be no advantage for an extremely deadly snake in being aposematic any predator that attacked it would be killed before it could learn to avoid the deadly prey so the snake would be better off being camouflaged to avoid attacks altogether But if the predator first learnt to avoid a less deadly snake that had warning colours the deadly species could then profit be attacked less often by mimicking the less dangerous snake 50 51 Some harmless milk snake Lampropeltis triangulum subspecies the moderately toxic false coral snakes genus Erythrolamprus and the deadly coral snakes genus Micrurus all have a red background color with black and white yellow rings In this system both the milk snakes and the deadly coral snakes are mimics whereas the false coral snakes are the model 48 Wasmannian edit Further information ant mimicry In Wasmannian mimicry the mimic resembles a model that it lives along with in a nest or colony Most of the models here are social insects such as ants termites bees and wasps 52 Vavilovian edit Main article Vavilovian mimicry nbsp Rye is a secondary crop originally being a mimetic weed of wheat Vavilovian mimicry is found in weeds that come to share characteristics with a domesticated plant through artificial selection 9 It is named after Russian botanist and geneticist Nikolai Vavilov 53 Selection against the weed may occur either by manually killing the weed or by separating its seeds from those of the crop by winnowing Vavilovian mimicry presents an illustration of unintentional or rather anti intentional selection by man Weeders do not want to select weeds and their seeds that look increasingly like cultivated plants yet there is no other option For example early barnyard grass Echinochloa oryzoides is a weed in rice fields and looks similar to rice its seeds are often mixed in rice and have become difficult to separate through Vavilovian mimicry 54 Vavilovian mimics may eventually be domesticated themselves as in the case of rye in wheat Vavilov called these weed crops secondary crops 53 Vavilovian mimicry can be classified as defensive mimicry in that the weed mimics a protected species This bears strong similarity to Batesian mimicry in that the weed does not share the properties that give the model its protection and both the model and the dupe in this case people are harmed by its presence There are some key differences though in Batesian mimicry the model and signal receiver are enemies the predator would eat the protected species if it could whereas here the crop and its human growers are in a mutualistic relationship the crop benefits from being dispersed and protected by people despite being eaten by them In fact the crop s only protection relevant here is its usefulness to humans Secondly the weed is not eaten but simply destroyed The only motivation for killing the weed is its effect on crop yields Finally this type of mimicry does not occur in ecosystems unaltered by humans Gilbertian edit Gilbertian mimicry involves only two species The potential host or prey drives away its parasite or predator by mimicking it the reverse of host parasite aggressive mimicry It was coined by Pasteur as a phrase for such rare mimicry systems 9 and is named after the American ecologist Lawrence E Gilbert nl 55 Gilbertian mimicry occurs in the genus Passiflora The leaves of this plant contain toxins that deter herbivorous animals However some Heliconius butterfly larvae have evolved enzymes that break down these toxins allowing them to specialize on this genus This has created further selection pressure on the host plants which have evolved stipules that mimic mature Heliconius eggs near the point of hatching These butterflies tend to avoid laying eggs near existing ones which helps avoid exploitative intraspecific competition between caterpillars those that lay on vacant leaves provide their offspring with a greater chance of survival Most Heliconius larvae are cannibalistic meaning that on leaves older eggs hatch first and eat the new arrivals Thus it seems that such plants have evolved egg dummies under selection pressure from these grazing herbivore enemies In addition the decoy eggs are also nectaries attracting predators of the caterpillars such as ants and wasps as a further defence 16 Browerian edit nbsp Monarch caterpillars shown feeding vary in toxicity depending on their diet Browerian mimicry 9 named after Lincoln P Brower and Jane Van Zandt Brower 56 57 is a postulated form of automimicry where the model belongs to the same species as the mimic This is the analogue of Batesian mimicry within a single species and occurs when there is a palatability spectrum within a population Examples include the monarch and the queen from the subfamily Danainae which feed on milkweed species of varying toxicity These species store toxins from its host plant which are maintained even in the adult imago form As levels of toxin vary depending on diet during the larval stage some individuals are more toxic than others Less palatable organisms therefore mimic more dangerous individuals with their likeness already perfected This is not always the case however In sexually dimorphic species one sex may be more of a threat than the other which could mimic the protected sex Evidence for this possibility is provided by the behaviour of a monkey from Gabon which regularly ate male moths of the genus Anaphe but promptly stopped after it tasted a noxious female 58 Aggressive edit Main article Aggressive mimicry Predators edit Aggressive mimicry is found in predators or parasites that share some of the characteristics of a harmless species allowing them to avoid detection by their prey or host this can be compared with the story of the wolf in sheep s clothing as long as it is understood that no conscious deceptive intent is involved The mimic may resemble the prey or host itself or another organism that is either neutral or beneficial to the signal receiver In this class of mimicry the model may be affected negatively positively or not at all Just as parasites can be treated as a form of predator 59 host parasite mimicry is treated here as a subclass of aggressive mimicry The mimic may have a particular significance for duped prey One such case is spiders amongst which aggressive mimicry is quite common both in luring prey and disguising stealthily approaching predators 60 One case is the golden orb weaver Nephila clavipes which spins a conspicuous golden colored web in well lit areas Experiments show that bees are able to associate the webs with danger when the yellow pigment is not present as occurs in less well lit areas where the web is much harder to see Other colours were also learned and avoided but bees seemed least able to effectively associate yellow pigmented webs with danger Yellow is the colour of many nectar bearing flowers however so perhaps avoiding yellow is not worthwhile Another form of mimicry is based not on colour but pattern Species such as the silver argiope Argiope argentata employ prominent patterns in the middle of their webs such as zigzags These may reflect ultraviolet light and mimic the pattern seen in many flowers known as nectar guides Spiders change their web day to day which can be explained by the ability of bees to remember web patterns Bees are able to associate a certain pattern with a spatial location meaning the spider must spin a new pattern regularly or suffer diminishing prey capture 61 nbsp Chlorobalius leucoviridis mimicry of Kobonga oxleyi source source Kobonga oxleyi cicada song with reply clicks from a Chlorobalius leucoviridisChlorobalius leucoviridis mimicry of Pauropsalta sp source source Pauropsalta sp Sandstone song with reply clicks from a Chlorobalius leucoviridis Problems playing these files See media help Another case is where males are lured towards what seems to be a sexually receptive female The model in this situation is the same species as the dupe Beginning in the 1960s James E Lloyd s investigation of female fireflies of the genus Photuris revealed they emit the same light signals that females of the genus Photinus use as a mating signal 62 Further research showed male fireflies from several different genera are attracted to these femmes fatales and are subsequently captured and eaten Female signals are based on that received from the male each female having a repertoire of signals matching the delay and duration of the female of the corresponding species This mimicry may have evolved from non mating signals that have become modified for predation 63 nbsp The spotted predatory katydid Chlorobalius leucoviridis an acoustic aggressive mimic of cicadas The listrosceline katydid Chlorobalius leucoviridis of inland Australia is capable of attracting male cicadas of the tribe Cicadettini by imitating the species specific reply clicks of sexually receptive female cicadas This example of acoustic aggressive mimicry is similar to the Photuris firefly case in that the predator s mimicry is remarkably versatile playback experiments show that C leucoviridis is able to attract males of many cicada species including cicadettine cicadas from other continents even though cicada mating signals are species specific 64 Some carnivorous plants may also be able to increase their rate of capture through mimicry 65 Luring is not a necessary condition however as the predator still has a significant advantage simply by not being identified as such They may resemble a mutualistic symbiont or a species of little relevance to the prey nbsp Two bluestreak cleaner wrasse cleaning a potato grouper Epinephelus tukula A case of the latter situation is a species of cleaner fish and its mimic though in this example the model is greatly disadvantaged by the presence of the mimic Cleaner fish are the allies of many other species which allow them to eat their parasites and dead skin Some allow the cleaner to venture inside their body to hunt these parasites However one species of cleaner the bluestreak cleaner wrasse Labroides dimidiatus is the unknowing model of a mimetic species the sabre toothed blenny Aspidontus taeniatus This wrasse resides in coral reefs in the Indian and the Pacific Oceans and is recognized by other fishes that then let it clean them Its imposter a species of blenny lives in the Indian Ocean and not only looks like it in terms of size and coloration but even mimics the cleaner s dance Having fooled its prey into letting its guard down it then bites it tearing off a piece of its fin before fleeing Fish grazed on in this fashion soon learn to distinguish mimic from model but because the similarity is close between the two they become much more cautious of the model as well so both are affected Due to victims ability to discriminate between foe and helper the blennies have evolved close similarity right down to the regional level 66 Another interesting example that does not involve any luring is the zone tailed hawk which resembles the turkey vulture It flies amongst the vultures suddenly breaking from the formation and ambushing its prey 67 Here the hawk s presence is of no evident significance to the vultures affecting them neither negatively or positively Parasites edit nbsp Mimicry in a brood parasite Cuckoo adult mimics sparrowhawk alarming small birds enough to give female cuckoo time to lay eggs in their nests 68 Parasites can also be aggressive mimics though the situation is somewhat different from those outlined previously Some predators have a feature that draws prey parasites can also mimic their hosts natural prey but are eaten themselves a pathway into their host Leucochloridium a genus of flatworm matures in the digestive system of songbirds their eggs then passing out of the bird in the faeces They are then taken up by Succinea a terrestrial snail The eggs develop in this intermediate host and must then find a suitable bird to mature in Since the host birds do not eat snails the sporocyst has another strategy to reach its host s intestine They are brightly coloured and move in a pulsating fashion A sporocyst sac pulsates in the snail s eye stalks 69 70 coming to resemble an irresistible meal for a songbird In this way it can bridge the gap between hosts allowing it to complete its life cycle 3 A nematode Myrmeconema neotropicum changes the colour of the abdomen of workers of the canopy ant Cephalotes atratus to make it appear like the ripe fruits of Hyeronima alchorneoides It also changes the behaviour of the ant so that the gaster rear part is held raised This presumably increases the chances of the ant being eaten by birds The droppings of birds are collected by other ants and fed to their brood thereby helping to spread the nematode 71 In an unusual case planidium larvae of some beetles of the genus Meloe form a group and produce a pheromone that mimics the sex attractant of its host bee species When a male bee arrives and attempts to mate with the mass of larvae they climb onto his abdomen From there they transfer to a female bee and from there to the bee nest to parasitize the bee larvae 72 nbsp Egg mimicry cuckoo eggs larger mimic many species of host birds eggs in this case of reed warbler Host parasite mimicry is a two species system where a parasite mimics its own host Cuckoos are a canonical example of brood parasitism a form of parasitism where the mother has its offspring raised by another unwitting individual often from a different species cutting down the biological mother s parental investment in the process The ability to lay eggs that mimic the host eggs is the key adaptation The adaptation to different hosts is inherited through the female line in so called gentes gens singular Cases of intraspecific brood parasitism where a female lays in a conspecific s nest as illustrated by the goldeneye duck Bucephala clangula 73 do not represent a case of mimicry A different mechanism is chemical mimicry as seen in the parasitic butterfly Phengaris rebeli which parasitizes the ant species Myrmica schencki by releasing chemicals that fool the worker ants to believe that the caterpillar larvae are ant larvae and enable the P rebeli larvae to be brought directly into the M schencki nest 74 Parasitic cuckoo bumblebees formerly Psithyrus now included in Bombus resemble their hosts more closely than would be expected by chance at least in areas like Europe where parasite host co speciation is common However this is explainable as Mullerian mimicry rather than requiring the parasite s coloration to deceive the host and thus constitute aggressive mimicry 75 Reproductive edit Reproductive mimicry occurs when the actions of the dupe directly aid in the mimic s reproduction This is common in plants with deceptive flowers that do not provide the reward they seem to offer and it may occur in Papua New Guinea fireflies in which the signal of Pteroptyx effulgens is used by P tarsalis to form aggregations to attract females 76 Other forms of mimicry have a reproductive component such as Vavilovian mimicry involving seeds vocal mimicry in birds 77 78 79 and aggressive and Batesian mimicry in brood parasite host systems 80 Bakerian and Dodsonian edit Main article Mimicry in plants Bakerian mimicry named after Herbert G Baker 81 is a form of automimicry where female flowers mimic male flowers of their own species cheating pollinators out of a reward This reproductive mimicry may not be readily apparent as members of the same species may still exhibit some degree of sexual dimorphism It is common in many species of Caricaceae 82 Dodsonian mimicry named after Calaway H Dodson is a form of reproductive floral mimicry where the model belongs to a different species than the mimic 83 By providing similar sensory signals as the model flower it can lure its pollinators Like Bakerian mimics no nectar is provided Epidendrum ibaguense Orchidaceae resembles flowers of Lantana camara and Asclepias curassavica and is pollinated by monarch butterflies and perhaps hummingbirds 84 Similar cases are seen in some other species of the same family The mimetic species may still have pollinators of its own though For example a lamellicorn beetle which usually pollinates correspondingly colored Cistus flowers is also known to aid in pollination of Ophrys species that are normally pollinated by bees 85 Pseudocopulation edit Further information Pseudocopulation nbsp Dasyscolia ciliata a scoliid wasp attempting to copulate with a flower of the orchid Ophrys speculum Pseudocopulation occurs when a flower mimics a female of a certain insect species inducing the males to try to copulate with the flower This is much like the aggressive mimicry in fireflies described previously but with a more benign outcome for the pollinator This form of mimicry has been called Pouyannian mimicry 9 after Maurice Alexandre Pouyanne who first described the phenomenon 86 87 It is most common in orchids which mimic females of the order Hymenoptera generally bees and wasps and may account for around 60 of pollinations 88 Depending on the morphology of the flower a pollen sac called a pollinium is attached to the head or abdomen of the male This is then transferred to the stigma of the next flower the male tries to inseminate resulting in pollination Visual mimicry is the most obvious sign of this deception for humans but the visual aspect may be minor or non existent It is the senses of touch and olfaction that are most important so this is at least partly chemical mimicry 88 Mimicry of rewards edit Flowering plants sometimes in conjunction with fungi mimic rewards which attract insects 89 Some deceptive plants imitate a pollinator s food source with flower structures that resemble fruits 90 91 In a complex case floral mimicry is induced by the discomycete fungus Monilinia vaccinii corymbosi 92 This fungal plant pathogen infects leaves of blueberries causing them to secrete sugars in effect mimicking the nectar of flowers To the naked eye the leaves do not look like flowers yet they still attract pollinating insects like bees using an ultraviolet signal This case is unusual in that the fungus benefits from the deception but it is the leaves that act as mimics being harmed in the process It is similar to host parasite mimicry but the host does not receive the signal It has something in common with automimicry but the plant does not benefit from the mimicry and the action of the pathogen is required to produce it 92 Inter sexual mimicry edit Main article Sexual mimicry Inter sexual mimicry occurs when individuals of one sex in a species mimic members of the opposite sex to facilitate sneak mating An example is the three male forms of the marine isopod Paracerceis sculpta Alpha males are the largest and guard a harem of females Beta males mimic females and manage to enter the harem of females without being detected by the alpha males allowing them to mate Gamma males are the smallest males and mimic juveniles This also allows them to mate with the females without the alpha males detecting them 93 Similarly among common side blotched lizards some males mimic the yellow throat coloration and even mating rejection behaviour of the other sex to sneak matings with guarded females These males look and behave like unreceptive females This strategy is effective against usurper males with orange throats but ineffective against blue throated guarder males which chase them away 94 95 Female spotted hyenas have pseudo penises that make them look like males 96 Automimicry edit nbsp Eyespots of foureye butterflyfish Chaetodon capistratus mimic its own eyes deflecting attacks from the vulnerable head Main article Automimicry Automimicry or intraspecific mimicry occurs within a single species One form of such mimicry is where one part of an organism s body resembles another part For example the tails of some snakes resemble their heads they move backwards when threatened and present the predator with the tail improving their chances of escape without fatal harm Some fishes have eyespots near their tails and when mildly alarmed swim slowly backwards presenting the tail as a head Some insects such as some lycaenid butterflies have tail patterns and appendages of various degrees of sophistication that promote attacks at the rear rather than at the head Several species of pygmy owl bear false eyes on the back of the head misleading predators into reacting as though they were the subject of an aggressive stare 97 Some writers use the term automimicry when the mimic imitates other morphs within the same species For example in a species where males mimic females or vice versa this may be an instance of sexual mimicry in evolutionary game theory Examples are found in some species of birds fishes and lizards 98 Quite elaborate strategies along these lines are known such as the well known scissors paper rock mimicry in Uta stansburiana 99 but there are qualitatively different examples in many other species such as some Platysaurus 100 Many species of insects are toxic or distasteful when they have fed on certain plants that contain chemicals of particular classes but not when they have fed on plants that lack those chemicals For instance some species of the subfamily Danainae feed on various species of the Asclepiadoideae in the family Apocynaceae which render them poisonous and emetic to most predators Such insects frequently are aposematically coloured and patterned When feeding on innocuous plants however they are harmless and nutritious but a bird that once has sampled a toxic specimen is unlikely to eat harmless specimens that have the same aposematic coloration When regarded as mimicry of toxic members of the same species this too may be seen as automimicry 101 nbsp Automimicry many blue butterflies Lycaenidae such as this gray hairstreak Strymon melinus have a false head at the rear held upwards at rest Many insects have filamentous tails at the ends of their wings and patterns of markings on the wings themselves These combine to create a false head This misdirects predators such as birds and jumping spiders Salticidae Spectacular examples occur in the hairstreak butterflies when perching on a twig or flower they commonly do so upside down and shift their rear wings repeatedly causing antenna like movements of the tails on their wings Studies of rear wing damage support the hypothesis that this strategy is effective in deflecting attacks from the insect s head 102 103 Evolution editFurther information Evolution and adaptation It is widely accepted that mimicry evolves as a positive adaptation The lepidopterist and novelist Vladimir Nabokov however argued that although natural selection might stabilize a mimic form it would not be necessary to create it 104 The most widely accepted model used to explain the evolution of mimicry in butterflies is the two step hypothesis The first step involves mutation in modifier genes that regulate a complex cluster of linked genes that cause large changes in morphology The second step consists of selections on genes with smaller phenotypic effects creating an increasingly close resemblance This model is supported by empirical evidence that suggests that a few single point mutations cause large phenotypic effects while numerous others produce smaller effects Some regulatory elements collaborate to form a supergene for the development of butterfly color patterns The model is supported by computational simulations of population genetics 105 The Batesian mimicry in Papilio polytes is controlled by the doublesex gene 106 Some mimicry is imperfect Natural selection drives mimicry only far enough to deceive predators For example when predators avoid a mimic that imperfectly resembles a coral snake the mimic is sufficiently protected 107 108 109 Convergent evolution is an alternative explanation for why organisms such as coral reef fish 110 111 and benthic marine invertebrates such as sponges and nudibranchs have come to resemble each other 112 See also editBiomimicry Chemical mimicry Locomotor mimicry Mimic octopus Molecular mimicry Preadaptation SemioticsNotes editReferences edit King R C Stansfield W D Mulligan P K 2006 A dictionary of genetics 7th ed Oxford University Press p 278 ISBN 978 0 19 530762 7 a b Dalziell Anastasia H Welbergen Justin A 27 April 2016 Mimicry for all modalities Ecology Letters 19 6 609 619 Bibcode 2016EcolL 19 609D doi 10 1111 ele 12602 PMID 27117779 a b c d e f Wickler Wolfgang 1968 Mimicry in plants and animals McGraw Hill Wickler Wolfgang 1965 Mimicry and the Evolution of Animal Communication Nature 208 5010 519 21 Bibcode 1965Natur 208 519W doi 10 1038 208519a0 S2CID 37649827 Radford Benjamin Frazier Kendrick January 2017 Cheats and Deceits How Animals and Plants Exploit and Mislead Skeptical Inquirer 41 1 60 a b c d Ruxton Graeme D Sherratt T N Speed M P 2004 Avoiding Attack the Evolutionary Ecology of Crypsis Warning Signals and Mimicry Oxford University Press Kikuchi D W Pfennig D W 2013 Imperfect Mimicry and the Limits of Natural Selection Quarterly Review of Biology 88 4 297 315 doi 10 1086 673758 PMID 24552099 S2CID 11436992 Skelhorn John Rowland Hannah M Ruxton Graeme D 2010 The Evolution and Ecology of Masquerade Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 99 1 8 doi 10 1111 j 1095 8312 2009 01347 x a b c d e f Pasteur G 1982 A Classificatory Review of Mimicry Systems Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 13 169 199 doi 10 1146 annurev es 13 110182 001125 Wiklund Christer Tullberg Birgitta S September 2004 Seasonal polyphenism and leaf mimicry in the comma butterfly Animal Behaviour 68 3 621 627 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2003 12 008 S2CID 54270418 Endler John A August 1981 An Overview of the Relationships Between Mimicry and Crypsis Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 16 1 25 31 doi 10 1111 j 1095 8312 1981 tb01840 x Stevens Martin Hopkins Elinor Hinde William Adcock Amabel Connolly Yvonne Troscianko Tom Cuthill Innes C November 2007 Field Experiments on the effectiveness of eyespots as predator deterrents Animal Behaviour 74 5 1215 1227 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2007 01 031 S2CID 53186893 Stevens Martin 22 June 2007 Predator perception and the interrelation between different forms of protective coloration Proceedings of the Royal Society B Biological Sciences 274 1617 1457 1464 doi 10 1098 rspb 2007 0220 PMC 1950298 PMID 17426012 Stevens Martin Stubbins Claire L Hardman Chloe J 30 May 2008 The anti predator function of eyespots on camouflaged and conspicuous prey Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 62 11 1787 1793 doi 10 1007 s00265 008 0607 3 S2CID 28288920 Hossie Thomas John Sherratt Thomas N August 2013 Defensive posture and eyespots deter avian predators from attacking caterpillar models Animal Behaviour 86 2 383 389 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2013 05 029 S2CID 53263767 a b Campbell N A 1996 Biology 4th ed Benjamin Cummings Chapter 50 ISBN 0 8053 1957 3 Boyden T C 1980 Floral mimicry by Epidendrum ibaguense Orchidaceae in Panama Evolution 34 1 135 136 doi 10 2307 2408322 JSTOR 2408322 PMID 28563205 Roy B A 1994 The effects of pathogen induced pseudoflowers and buttercups on each other s insect visitation Ecology 75 2 352 358 Bibcode 1994Ecol 75 352R doi 10 2307 1939539 JSTOR 1939539 Wickler Wolfgang 1998 Mimicry Encyclopaedia Britannica 15th edition Macropaedia 24 144 151 https www britannica com eb article 11910 Johnson Steven D Schiestl Florian P 2016 Floral Mimicry Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 104723 7 Harper Douglas Online Etymology Dictionary Retrieved 23 February 2022 Endler John A 1981 An overview of the relationships between mimicry and crypsis Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 16 25 31 doi 10 1111 j 1095 8312 1981 tb01840 x Allen J A Cooper J M 2010 Crypsis and masquerade Journal of Biological Education 19 4 268 doi 10 1080 00219266 1985 9654747 Boppre Michael Vane Wright Richard I Wickler Wolfgang 1 January 2017 A hypothesis to explain accuracy of wasp resemblances Ecology and Evolution 7 1 73 81 Bibcode 2017EcoEv 7 73B doi 10 1002 ece3 2586 PMC 5214283 PMID 28070276 Davies N B Welbergen J A 2008 Cuckoo hawk mimicry An experimental test Proceedings of the Royal Society B 275 1644 1817 1822 doi 10 1098 rspb 2008 0331 PMC 2587796 PMID 18467298 Bates Henry W 1863 The naturalist on the river Amazons Murray Bates Henry W 1861 Contributions to an insect fauna of the Amazon valley Lepidoptera Heliconidae Transactions of the Linnean Society 23 3 495 566 doi 10 1111 j 1096 3642 1860 tb00146 x Stearns S C Hoekstra Rolf F 2000 Evolution An Introduction 5th ed Oxford University Press p 464 ISBN 978 0 19 854968 0 a b c Pinheiro Carlos E G 1996 Palatability and escaping ability in Neotropical butterflies tests with wild kingbirds Tyrannus melancholicus Tyrannidae Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 59 4 351 365 doi 10 1111 j 1095 8312 1996 tb01471 x Barber J R Conner W E 2007 Acoustic mimicry in a predator prey interaction PNAS 104 22 9331 9334 Bibcode 2007PNAS 104 9331B doi 10 1073 pnas 0703627104 PMC 1890494 PMID 17517637 Barber Jesse R Chadwell Brad A Garrett Nick Schmidt French Barbara Conner William E July 2009 Naive bats discriminate arctiid moth warning sounds but generalize their aposematic meaning The Journal of Experimental Biology 212 Pt 14 2141 2148 doi 10 1242 jeb 029991 ISSN 0022 0949 PMID 19561203 S2CID 1303252 Barber Jesse R Plotkin David Rubin Juliette J Homziak Nicholas T Leavell Brian C Houlihan Peter R Miner Krystie A Breinholt Jesse W Quirk Royal Brandt Padron Pablo Sebastian Nunez Matias Kawahara Akito Y 21 June 2022 Anti bat ultrasound production in moths is globally and phylogenetically widespread Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 119 25 e2117485119 Bibcode 2022PNAS 11917485B doi 10 1073 pnas 2117485119 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 9231501 PMID 35704762 Kawahara Akito Y Barber Jesse R 19 May 2015 Tempo and mode of antibat ultrasound production and sonar jamming in the diverse hawkmoth radiation Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112 20 6407 6412 Bibcode 2015PNAS 112 6407K doi 10 1073 pnas 1416679112 ISSN 0027 8424 PMC 4443353 PMID 25941377 Mimic Octopus Thaumoctopus mimicus at MarineBio org Archived from the original on 18 July 2017 Retrieved 9 June 2007 Deceptive Woodpecker Uses Mimicry to Avoid Competition AMNH Retrieved 12 August 2015 Gianoli Ernesto 2014 Leaf Mimicry in a Climbing Plant Protects against Herbivory Cell 24 9 984 987 doi 10 1016 j cub 2014 03 010 PMID 24768053 Meyer A 2006 Repeating Patterns of Mimicry PLOS Biol 4 10 e341 doi 10 1371 journal pbio 0040341 PMC 1617347 PMID 17048984 Muller Fritz 1878 Ueber die Vortheile der Mimicry bei Schmetterlingen Zoologischer Anzeiger 1 54 55 Muller F 1879 Ituna and Thyridia a remarkable case of mimicry in butterflies Proclamations of the Entomological Society of London 1879 Translated by Meldola R 20 29 Flannery T F 2007 Community ecology Mimicry complexes Encyclopaedia Britannica Online Huheey James E 1976 Studies in warning coloration and mimicry VII Evolutionary consequences of a Batesian Mullerian spectrum A model for Mullerian mimicry Evolution 30 1 86 93 doi 10 2307 2407675 JSTOR 2407675 PMID 28565050 Benson W W 1977 On the Supposed Spectrum Between Batesian and Mullerian Mimicry Evolution 31 2 454 455 doi 10 2307 2407770 JSTOR 2407770 PMID 28563231 Ritland D B 1995 Comparative unpalatability of mimetic viceroy butterflies Limenitis archippus from four south eastern United States populations Oecologia 103 3 327 336 Bibcode 1995Oecol 103 327R doi 10 1007 BF00328621 PMID 28306826 S2CID 13436225 Ritland D B Brower Lincoln P 1991 The viceroy butterfly is not a Batesian mimic Nature 350 6318 497 498 Bibcode 1991Natur 350 497R doi 10 1038 350497a0 S2CID 28667520 Viceroys are as unpalatable as monarchs and significantly more unpalatable than queens from representative Florida populations Young A M 1971 Wing colouration and reflectance in Morpho butterflies as related to reproductive behaviour and escape from avian predators Oecologia 7 3 209 222 Bibcode 1971Oecol 7 209Y doi 10 1007 BF00345212 PMID 28311247 S2CID 25970574 Edmunds M 1974 Defence in Animals a survey of anti predator defences Longman pp 255 256 ISBN 0 582 44132 3 Marek P E Bond J E 2009 A Mullerian mimicry ring in Appalachian millipedes Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106 24 9755 9760 Bibcode 2009PNAS 106 9755M doi 10 1073 pnas 0810408106 PMC 2700981 PMID 19487663 a b Emsley M G 1966 The mimetic significance of Erythrolamprus aesculapii ocellatus Peters from Tobago Evolution 20 4 663 64 doi 10 2307 2406599 JSTOR 2406599 PMID 28562911 Mertens Robert 1956 Das Problem der Mimikry bei Korallenschlangen Zool Jahrb Syst in German 84 541 76 a b Hecht M K Marien D 1956 The coral snake mimic problem a reinterpretation Journal of Morphology 98 2 335 365 doi 10 1002 jmor 1050980207 S2CID 83825414 a b Sheppard P M Wickler Wolfgang 1969 Review of Mimicry in plants and animals by Wolfgang Wickler Journal of Animal Ecology 38 1 243 doi 10 2307 2762 JSTOR 2762 Wasmann E 1894 Kritisches Verzeichniss der myrmecophilin und termitophilen Arthropoden Felix Dames Berlin xi 231 pp a b Vavilov N I 1951 The origin variation immunity and breeding of cultivated plants translation by K S Chester Chronica Botanica 13 1 366 Barrett S 1983 Mimicry in Plants Scientific American 257 3 76 83 Bibcode 1987SciAm 257c 76B doi 10 1038 scientificamerican0987 76 Gilbert Lawrence E 1975 Ecological consequences of a coevolved mutualism between butterflies and plants In L E Gilbert P H Raven eds Coevolution of Animals and Plants University of Texas Press pp 210 240 OCLC 636384400 Brower Lincoln P 1970 Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain and implications for mimicry theory In Chambers K L ed Biochemical Coevolution Corvallis Oregon USA Oregon State Univ pp 69 82 Brower Lincoln P Van Brower J V Z Corvino J M 1967 Plant poisons in a terrestrial food chain Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 57 4 893 98 Bibcode 1967PNAS 57 893B doi 10 1073 pnas 57 4 893 PMC 224631 PMID 5231352 Bigot L Jouventin P 1974 Quelques experiences de comestibilite de Lepidopteres gabonais faites avec le mandrill le cercocebe a joues grises et le garde bœufs Terre Vie in French 28 521 43 Begon M Townsend C Harper J 1996 Ecology Individuals populations and communities third edition Blackwell Science London Jackson R R 1995 Eight legged tricksters Spiders that specialize at catching other spiders BioScience 42 8 590 98 doi 10 2307 1311924 JSTOR 1311924 Craig C L 1995 Webs of Deceit Natural History 104 3 32 35 Lloyd J E 1965 Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris Firefly Femmes Fatales Science 149 653 654 Lloyd J E 1975 Aggressive Mimicry in Photuris Fireflies Signal Repertoires by Femmes Fatales Science 187 4175 452 453 Bibcode 1975Sci 187 452L doi 10 1126 science 187 4175 452 PMID 17835312 S2CID 26761854 Marshall D C Hill K B R 2009 Chippindale Adam K ed Versatile aggressive mimicry of cicadas by an Australian predatory katydid PLOS ONE 4 1 e4185 Bibcode 2009PLoSO 4 4185M doi 10 1371 journal pone 0004185 PMC 2615208 PMID 19142230 Moran Jonathan A 1996 Pitcher dimorphism prey composition and the mechanisms of prey attraction in the pitcher plant Nepenthes rafflesiana in Borneo Journal of Ecology 84 4 515 525 Bibcode 1996JEcol 84 515M doi 10 2307 2261474 JSTOR 2261474 Wickler W 1966 Mimicry in Tropical Fishes Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London Series B Biological Sciences 251 772 473 474 Bibcode 1966RSPTB 251 473W doi 10 1098 rstb 1966 0036 S2CID 83609965 Willis E O 1963 Is the Zone Tailed Hawk a Mimic of the Turkey Vulture The Condor 65 4 313 317 doi 10 2307 1365357 JSTOR 1365357 Welbergen Justin A Davies Nicholas B 2011 A parasite in wolf s clothing hawk mimicry reduces mobbing of cuckoos by hosts Behavioral Ecology 22 3 574 579 doi 10 1093 beheco arr008 ISSN 1465 7279 See here for a photo Moore J 2002 Parasites and the behavior of animals Oxford University Press Yanoviak S P Kaspari M Dudley R Poinar G Jr 2008 Parasite induced fruit mimicry in a tropical canopy ant PDF The American Naturalist 171 4 536 44 doi 10 1086 528968 PMID 18279076 S2CID 23857167 Saul Gershenz L S Millar J G 2006 Phoretic nest parasites use sexual deception to obtain transport to their host s nest Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103 38 14039 14044 Bibcode 2006PNAS 10314039S doi 10 1073 pnas 0603901103 PMC 1599908 PMID 16966608 Andersson M Eriksson M O G 1982 Nest parasitism in Goldeneyes Bucephala clangula some evolutionary aspects American Naturalist 120 1 16 doi 10 1086 283965 S2CID 86699716 Barbero Francesca Thomas JA Bonelli S Balletto E Schonrogge K 2009 Acoustical mimicry in a predatory social parasite of ants Journal of Experimental Biology 212 Pt 24 4084 4090 doi 10 1242 jeb 032912 PMID 19946088 Retrieved 28 September 2013 Williams Paul H 2008 Do the parasitic Psithyrus resemble their host bumblebees in colour pattern PDF Apidologie 39 6 637 649 doi 10 1051 apido 2008048 S2CID 27702692 Ohba N Shimoyama Ayu 2009 Meyer Rochow V B ed Bioluminescence in Focus a collection of illuminating essays Research Signpost Trivandrum Kerala India pp 229 242 Dalziell Anastasia H Welbergen Justin A Igic Branislav Magrath Robert D 30 July 2014 Avian vocal mimicry a unified conceptual framework Biological Reviews 90 2 643 668 doi 10 1111 brv 12129 PMID 25079896 S2CID 207101926 Kelley Laura A Coe Rebecca L Madden Joah R Healy Susan D 1 September 2008 Vocal mimicry in songbirds Animal Behaviour 76 3 521 528 doi 10 1016 j anbehav 2008 04 012 S2CID 53192695 Goller Maria Shizuka Daizaburo 22 June 2018 Evolutionary origins of vocal mimicry in songbirds Evolution Letters 2 4 417 426 doi 10 1002 evl3 62 PMC 6121844 PMID 30283692 Davies Nick 2015 Cuckoo Cheating by Nature Bloomsbury ISBN 978 1 4088 5656 7 Baker Herbert G 1976 Mistake pollination as a reproductive system with special reference to the Caricaceae Pp 161 169 in J Burley and B T Styles eds clarification needed Variation breeding and conservation of tropical trees Academic Press London U K Bawa K S 1980 Mimicry of male by female flowers and intrasexual competition for pollinators in Jacaratia dolichaula D Smith Woodson Caricaceae Evolution 34 3 467 74 doi 10 2307 2408216 JSTOR 2408216 PMID 28568703 Dodson C H Frymire G P 1961 Natural pollination of orchids Missouri Botanical Garden Bulletin 49 133 39 Boyden T C 1980 Floral mimicry by Epidendrurn ibaguense Orchidaceae in Panama Evolution 34 1 135 36 doi 10 2307 2408322 JSTOR 2408322 PMID 28563205 Kullenberg B 1961 Studies in Ophrys pollination Zool Bidr Uppsala 34 1 340 Correvon H Pouyanne M 1916 Un curieux cas de mimetisme chez les Ophrydees J Soc Nat Hortic Fr 17 29 31 41 42 84 Pouyanne M 1917 La fecondation des Ophrys par les insectes Bull Soc Hist Nat Afr Nord 8 1 2 a b Van der Pijl L Dodson C H 1966 Orchid Flowers Their Pollination and Evolution Coral Gables Florida USA Univ Miami Press Johnson Steven D Schiestl Florian P 2016 Floral Mimicry Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 104723 7 Ellis Allan G Johnson Steven D 2010 Floral Mimicry Enhances Pollen Export The Evolution of Pollination by Sexual Deceit Outside of the Orchidaceae The American Naturalist 176 5 E143 E151 doi 10 1086 656487 ISSN 0003 0147 PMID 20843263 S2CID 45076899 Goodrich Katherine R Jurgens Andreas 2018 Pollination systems involving floral mimicry of fruit aspects of their ecology and evolution New Phytologist 217 1 74 81 doi 10 1111 nph 14821 a b Batra L R Batra S 1985 Floral Mimicry Induced by Mummy Berry Fungus Exploits Host s Pollinators as Vectors Science 228 4702 1011 1013 Bibcode 1985Sci 228 1011B doi 10 1126 science 228 4702 1011 PMID 17797664 S2CID 21422865 Shuster Stephen May 1987 Alternative Reproductive Behaviors Three Discrete Male Morphs in Paracerceis sculpta an Intertidal Isopod from the Northern Gulf of California Journal of Crustacean Biology 7 2 318 327 doi 10 2307 1548612 JSTOR 1548612 Sinervo B C M Lively 1996 The rock paper scissors game and the evolution of alternative male strategies Nature 380 6571 240 243 Bibcode 1996Natur 380 240S doi 10 1038 380240a0 S2CID 205026253 Sinervo B Miles D B Frankino W A Klukowski M Denardo D F 2000 Testosterone Endurance and Darwinian Fitness Natural and Sexual Selection on the Physiological Bases of Alternative Male Behaviors in Side Blotched Lizards Hormones and Behavior 38 4 222 233 doi 10 1006 hbeh 2000 1622 PMID 11104640 S2CID 5759575 Muller M N Wrangham R 2002 Sexual Mimicry in Hyenas The Quarterly Review of Biology 77 1 3 16 doi 10 1086 339199 PMID 11963460 S2CID 43440407 Northern Pygmy Owl Glaucidium californicum Owl Research Institute Archived from the original on 28 December 2015 Retrieved 23 August 2015 Plaistow Stewart J Johnstone Rufus A Colegrave Nick Spencer Matthew 2004 Evolution of alternative mating tactics conditional versus mixed strategies Behavioral Ecology 15 4 534 542 doi 10 1093 beheco arh029 Schell Robert Dettman Jessica Ecology and breeding colors of the side blotched lizard Uta stansburiana in the Grand Canyon PDF Center for Watershed Sciences University of California Davis Archived from the original PDF on 8 August 2014 Lewis Belinda Ann 2006 Sexual Selection and Signalling in the LizardPlatysaurus minor PDF Masters thesis Svennungsen Thomas Owens Holen Oistein Haugsten 2007 The evolutionary stability of automimicry Proc R Soc B 274 1621 2055 2063 doi 10 1098 rspb 2007 0456 PMC 2275178 PMID 17567561 Sourakov Andrei 2013 Two heads are better than one false head allows Calycopis cecrops Lycaenidae to escape predation by a Jumping Spider Phidippus pulcherrimus Salticidae Journal of Natural History 47 15 16 1047 1054 Robbins Robert K The False Head Hypothesis Predation and Wing Pattern Variation of Lycaenid Butterflies The American Naturalist Vol 118 No 5 Nov 1981 pp 770 775 Alexander Victoria N 2002 Nabokov Teleology and Insect Mimicry Nabokov Studies 7 177 213 doi 10 1353 nab 2010 0004 S2CID 42675699 Holmgren N M A Enquist M 1999 Dynamics of mimicry evolution PDF Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 66 2 145 158 doi 10 1111 j 1095 8312 1999 tb01880 x K Kunte W Zhang A Tenger Trolander D H Palmer A Martin R D Reed S P Mullen M R Kronforst 2014 Doublesex is a mimicry supergene Nature 507 7491 229 232 Bibcode 2014Natur 507 229K doi 10 1038 nature13112 PMID 24598547 S2CID 4448793 Wilson J Jahner J Williams K Forister M 2013 Ecological and Evolutionary Processes Drive the Origin and Maintenance of Imperfect Mimicry PLOS ONE 8 4 e61610 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 861610W doi 10 1371 journal pone 0061610 PMC 3625143 PMID 23593490 Kikuchi D Pfenning D 2010 Predator Cognition Permits Imperfect Coral Snake Mimicry The American Naturalist 176 6 830 834 doi 10 1086 657041 PMID 20950143 S2CID 35411437 Howse P E Allen J A 1994 Satyric Mimicry The Evolution of Apparent Imperfection Proceedings of the Royal Society B 257 1349 111 114 Bibcode 1994RSPSB 257 111H doi 10 1098 rspb 1994 0102 S2CID 84458742 Robertson D Ross 2013 Who resembles whom Mimetic and coincidental look alikes among tropical reef fishes PLOS ONE 8 1 e54939 Bibcode 2013PLoSO 854939R doi 10 1371 journal pone 0054939 PMC 3556028 PMID 23372795 Robertson D Ross 2015 Coincidental resemblances among coral reef fishes from different oceans Coral Reefs 34 3 977 Bibcode 2015CorRe 34 977R doi 10 1007 s00338 015 1309 8 Pawlik J R 2012 12 In Fattorusso E Gerwick W H Taglialatela Scafati O eds Antipredatory defensive roles of natural products from marine invertebrates Springer pp 677 710 ISBN 978 90 481 3833 3 Further reading editBrower L P ed 1988 Mimicry and the evolutionary process Chicago University of Chicago Press ISBN 0 226 07608 3 a supplement of volume 131 of the journal American Naturalist dedicated to E B Ford Carpenter G D Hale Ford E B 1933 Mimicry London Methuen Cott H B 1940 Adaptive Coloration in Animals Methuen and Co London ISBN 0 416 30050 2 Dafni A 1984 Mimicry and Deception in Pollination Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 15 259 278 doi 10 1146 annurev es 15 110184 001355 Edmunds M 1974 Defence in Animals a survey of anti predator defences Harlow Essex and New York Longman ISBN 0 582 44132 3 Evans M A 1965 Mimicry and the Darwinian Heritage Journal of the History of Ideas 26 2 211 220 doi 10 2307 2708228 JSTOR 2708228 Owen D 1980 Camouflage and Mimicry Oxford University Press ISBN 0 19 217683 8 Pasteur Georges 1982 A classificatory review of mimicry systems Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 13 169 199 doi 10 1146 annurev es 13 110182 001125 Stevens M 2016 Cheats and deceits how animals and plants exploit and mislead Oxford University Press ISBN 978 0 19 870789 9 Wiens D 1978 Mimicry in Plants In Max K Hecht William C Steere Bruce Wallace eds Evolutionary Biology Vol 11 pp 365 403 doi 10 1007 978 1 4615 6956 5 6 ISBN 978 1 4615 6958 9 PMC 3282713 PMID 22182416 a href Template Cite book html title Template Cite book cite book a journal ignored help Vane Wright R I 1976 A unified classification of mimetic resemblances Biol J Linn Soc 8 25 56 doi 10 1111 j 1095 8312 1976 tb00240 x Wickler W 1968 Mimicry in Plants and Animals translated from the German McGraw Hill New York ISBN 0 07 070100 8 Children s edit Hoff M K 2003 Mimicry and Camouflage Creative Education Mankato Minnesota USA Great Britain ISBN 1 58341 237 9 External links edit nbsp Wikimedia Commons has media related to Mimicry nbsp Wikisource has original text related to this article Mimicry in Butterflies nbsp Wikisource has the text of the 1920 Encyclopedia Americana article Mimicry in Animals nbsp Wikisource has the text of the 1920 Encyclopedia Americana article Imitation in Animals Warning colour and mimicry Lecture outline from University College London Camouflage and Mimicry in Fossils Retrieved from https en wikipedia org w index php title Mimicry amp oldid 1193682382 Wasmannian, wikipedia, wiki, book, books, library,

article

, read, download, free, free download, mp3, video, mp4, 3gp, jpg, jpeg, gif, png, picture, music, song, movie, book, game, games.